Huo Binning Compass 2008
Huo Binning Compass 2008
Huo Binning Compass 2008
Abstract
The psychological experience of respect has implications for the nature and
quality of group life and for the individual’s psychological and physical well-
being. However, the manner in which respect has been studied and defined has
frequently differed among researchers, making it difficult to connect the various
findings. Whereas some researchers have focused on the implications of respectful
treatment from group members (e.g., authorities, peers), others have focused on
individuals’ perceptions of how they are generally evaluated by the group. We
present the dual pathway model of respect in which these various lines of research
are integrated within a single framework. Organized around two basic social
motives – the need for status and the need to belong – the model describes two
pathways (status evaluation and liking) through which respect from the group
shapes social engagement, self-esteem, and health. These evaluative dimensions
are informed by interactions with group authorities and peers and differentially
predict social psychological outcomes.
What respect is and who deserves it are central questions in social life.
Those who have experienced social exclusion, a loss of standing within a
community, or endured undignified treatment by others fully understand
the significance of the psychological experience of respect. Normative ideals
of what respect is and who deserves it have been central questions in the
discipline of philosophy (e.g., Rawls, 1971). However, only in recent years
has empirical research begun to map out the phenomenological experience
of respect and its social–behavioral consequences. What is now clear is
that the giving and receiving of respect are important both in regulating
group dynamics and in influencing personal well-being. Alluding to this
point, the sociologist Richard Sennett (2003, p. 3) poignantly asked,
‘Unlike food, respect costs nothing. Why then should it be in short supply?’
The observation that respect is withheld and doled out sparingly highlights
its significance in social relationships. Although it cannot be quantified in
the same way that money or other concrete resources can be, it is valuable
and can be thought of as a form of social currency. Our goal is to review
© 2008 The Authors
Journal Compilation © 2008 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
2 The Psychology of Respect
Just as some argue that respect reflects the status motive, others argue
that it reflects another basic human motive – the need to belong
(Baumeister & Leary, 1995). As such, respect serves a critical function by
communicating information about a person’s inclusion within a social
group. There are several lines of work in support of respect as serving the
need to belong. One investigation found that the effect of respectful
treatment on willingness to contribute to group welfare was more
pronounced among peripheral members than among more central members
(De Cremer, 2002). Another found that self-reports of belongingness
mediated the relationship between perceived respect and contributions to
the group (De Cremer, 2003). A third study found that respectful treatment
increased individuals’ perception that they are welcomed or accepted within
a group in which they are a member (Simon & Sturmer, 2005). These
findings are consistent with the idea that respect is rooted in a fundamental
need for acknowledgment that one is an accepted member of the group
and that one belongs.
Together, the need for status and the need to belong represent core social
needs that can potentially be met when individuals participate in group life.
By recognizing that they are conceptually distinct, we can then explore their
antecedents as well as their effects on different social psychological outcomes.
Social engagement
Social engagement refers to a broad cluster of attitudes and behavioral
intentions that are associated with the desire to actively participate in, and
to maintain and improve upon, the collective experience in groups and
communities. Much of the research on respect has focused on its effect
on the internal dynamics of groups. We now have clear evidence that
© 2008 The Authors Social and Personality Psychology Compass 2 (2008): 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00129.x
Journal Compilation © 2008 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
6 The Psychology of Respect
Self-esteem
Feelings of respect are important not only for the well-being of the
collective; they also affect the individual. One of the most robust effects
in the research on respect is the relationship between respect and personal
self-esteem (how the individual feels about him or herself) as predicted by
the group-value model (Lind & Tyler, 1988). A review of ten datasets
found positive associations between perceptions of being respected by
other group members and self-esteem with an average effect size of 0.36
(Smith, Tyler, & Huo, 2003). These associations are documented in data
from correlational studies of experiences in real groups (e.g., Tyler et al.,
1996) and in experimental studies in the laboratory (e.g., Smith et al.,
1998). Other studies have found similar effects for respect on collective
self-esteem or the aspect of one’s self-concept associated with group
identity (Ellemers et al., 2004). As a form of social evaluation, respect
seems to play an important role in shaping the self-concept.
Health
Although the evidence is more tentative, there are hints in the literature
suggesting an intriguing and potentially important link between experiences
with respect and another aspect of personal well-being – the individual’s
mental and physical health. The epidemiologist Michael Marmot (2004)
coined the term, ‘status syndrome,’ to describe the idea that social
evaluations have fundamental effects on our health. Drawing on large-scale
© 2008 The Authors Social and Personality Psychology Compass 2 (2008): 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00129.x
Journal Compilation © 2008 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
8 The Psychology of Respect
a beauty pageant but not the pageant title itself. Similarly, a high-status
individual within a group may not be well liked (e.g., the CEO of a
company). In this way, the respect accorded an individual can alternately
be informed by how well one is liked by other group members and by
perceptions of one’s status in the eyes of the same group members.
The works by Ellemers, Spears, and their colleagues offer insight into
the important distinction between the liking and status aspects of respect.
How well one is liked by other group members as a basis for respect has
been implicated in a number of social phenomena including intergroup
discrimination (Branscombe et al., 2002) and willingness to work on behalf
of the group (Ellemers et al., 2004). There is also evidence that competence
judgments reflecting status concerns has similar effects as liking on both
the desire to exert effort on behalf of the group and on self-evaluations
(Spears et al., 2005). Interestingly, when the two dimensions are considered
jointly in an experimental context, warmth or liking emerges as the
primary influence on the person’s self-perceptions and perceptions of the
group (Spears et al., 2005).
Spears et al. (2005) have also found evidence of an interaction between
the two aspects of respect such that individuals who are judged to be highly
competent (reflecting status) but not well-liked by other group members
report the highest level of negative emotions although not lower commitment
to the group. Thus, although not being liked affects the internal emotional
life of highly competent group members, it does not negatively affect group
commitment. A field study of adolescents similarly show that the two forms
of evaluations are empirically distinct, and that they independently predict
perceptions of the extent to which an individual report feeling respected
by the community as a whole (Huo, Binning, & Molina, 2008). Thus, there
is already some empirical evidence to support the conceptual distinction
between status evaluation and liking. These distinctions are important in
designing future research to flush out the two paths through which respect
affects different outcomes variables (i.e., social engagement, self-esteem,
and health).
Conclusion
Short Biographies
Yuen J. Huo is associate professor of psychology at the University of
California, Los Angeles. Her research explores how fairness concerns
and social identity processes jointly operate to influence efforts at conflict
management as well as the exercise of authority. Her publications on
these topics have appeared in numerous journals and three co-authored
books: Social Justice in a Diverse Society (Westview Press, 1997), How Different
Ethnic Groups React to Legal Authority (2000, PPIC), and Trust in the Law
(Russell Sage, 2002). Her current research efforts focus on the role of justice-
based emotions on political attitudes and on the relationship between
perceptions of (in)justice and health outcomes. Her research examining
the influence of group identities on ethnic conflicts in diverse settings was
recognized by the Otto Klineberg Intercultural and International Relations
Award from the Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues
(SPSSI). She currently sits on the Board of Directors for SPSSI. Before
joining the UCLA faculty, she was a research fellow at the Public Policy
© 2008 The Authors Social and Personality Psychology Compass 2 (2008): 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00129.x
Journal Compilation © 2008 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
14 The Psychology of Respect
Institute of California. She holds BA, MA, and PhD degrees from the
University of California, Berkeley.
Kevin R. Binning is a doctoral candidate in social psychology at the
University of California, Los Angeles. He received a master’s in social
psychology from the same institution in 2003, has won several awards for
research and teaching, and is currently funded by the Frank Joseph McGuigan
Dissertation Year Fellowship. His research focuses on the psychology of
intragroup and intergroup relations, including issues related to respect,
discrimination, and the beliefs and ideologies underpin tolerance of inhumane
treatment of outgroups. Binning expects to defend his dissertation in
spring of 2008 and looks forward to a career of teaching and research.
Endnotes
* Correspondence address: Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles,
4625 Franz Hall, Box 951563, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1563, USA. Email:
[email protected].
References
Adler, N. E., Epel, E., Castellazzo, G., & Ickovics, J. (2000). Relationship of subjective and
objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: Preliminary data in
healthy White women. Health Psychology, 1, 586–592.
Anderson, E. (1994). The code of the streets. The Atlantic Monthly, 273, 81–94.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal
attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
117, 497–529.
Berger, J., Cohen, B. P., & Zeltich, M. Jr. (1972). Status characteristics and social interactions.
American Sociological Review, 37, 241–255.
Blascovich, J., Spencer, S. J., Quinn, D., & Steele, C. (2001). (African Americans and high
blood pressure: The role of stereotype threat. Psychological Science, 12, 225–229.
Boeckmann, R. J., & Tyler, T. R. (2002). Trust, respect and the psychology of political
engagement. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 2067–2088.
Branscombe, N. R., Spears, R., Ellemers, N., & Doosje, B. (2002). Intragroup and intergroup
evaluation effects on group behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 744–753.
Clark, R., Anderson, N. B., Clark, V. R., & Williams, D. R. (1999). Racism as a stressor for
African Americans. American Psychologist, 54, 805 –816.
Cohen, D., Nisbett, R., Bowdle, B., & Schwarz, N. (1996). Insult, aggression, and the southern
culture of honor: An experimental ethnography. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 70,
945– 60.
Cohen, G. L., Garcia, J., Apfel, N., & Master, A. (2006). Reducing the racial achievement gap:
A social-psychological intervention. Science, 313, 1307–1310.
De Cremer, D. (2002). Respect and cooperation in social dilemmas: The importance of feeling
included. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1335–1341.
De Cremer, D. (2003). Noneconomic motives predicting cooperation in public good dilemmas:
The effect of received respect on contributions. Social Justice Research, 16 (4), 367–377.
Ellemers, N., Doosje, B., & Spears, R. (2004). Sources of respect: The effects of being liked
by ingroups and outgroups. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 155 –172.
Elovainio, M., van den Bos, K., Linna, A., Kivimäki, M., Ala-Mursula, L., & Pentti, J. et al.
(2005). Combined effects of uncertainty and organizational justice on employee health:
Testing the uncertainty management model of fairness judgments among Finnish public
sector employees. Social Science & Medicine, 61 (12), 2501–2512.
© 2008 The Authors Social and Personality Psychology Compass 2 (2008): 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00129.x
Journal Compilation © 2008 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
The Psychology of Respect 15
Emler, N., & Hopkins, N. (1990). Reputation, social identity and the self. In D. Abrams &
M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Social Identity Theory: Constructive and Critical Advances (pp. 113–130).
New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
Fang, C. Y., & Myers, H. F. (2001). The effects of racial stressors and hostility on cardiovascular
reactivity in African American and Caucasian men. Health Psychology, 20, 64 –70.
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition:
Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11 (2), 77–83.
Fiske, S., Xu, J., Cuddy, A., & Glick, P. (1999). (dis)respecting versus (dis)liking: Status and
interdependence predict ambivalent stereotypes of competence and warmth. Journal of Social
Issues, 55, 473 – 489.
Frank, R. H. (1985). Choosing the Right Pond: Human Behavior and the Quest for Status. New
York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Greenberg, J. (2006). Losing sleep over organizational injustice: attenuating insomniac reactions
to underpayment inequity with supervisory training in interactional justice. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 91, 58 – 69.
Hendrick, S. S., & Hendrick, C. (2006). Measuring respect in close relationships. Journal of
Social and Personal Relationships, 23, 881–899.
Hogan, R., & Hogan, J. (1991). Personality and status. In D. G. Gilbert & J. J. Connolly (Eds.),
Personality, Social Skills, and Psychopathology: An Individual Differences Approach (pp. 137–154).
New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Huo, Y. J., & Molina, L. E. (2006). Is pluralism a viable model of diversity? The benefits and
limits of subgroup respect. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 9, 359–376.
Huo, Y. J., Binning, K. R., & Molina, L. E. (2008). Liking vs. Status Evaluation as Sources of
Generalized Respect. Manuscript in preparation.
Huo, Y. J., Molina, L. E., Binning, K. R., & Funge, S. (2008). Subgroup Respect, Social
Engagement, and Well-Being: A Field Study of an Ethnically Diverse High School. Manuscript
under review.
Langdon, S. W. (2007). Conceptualizations of respect: Qualitative and quantitative evidence of
four (five) themes. The Journal of Psychology, 141, 469–484.
Leary, J. D., Brennan, E. M., & Briggs, H. E. (2005). The African American adolescent respect
scale: A measure of a prosocial attitude. Research on Social Work Practice, 15, 462– 469.
Leary, M. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). The nature and function of self-esteem: Sociometer
theory. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 1– 62.
Liebkind, K., & Jasinskaja-Lahti, I. (2000). Acculturation and psychological well-being among
immigrant adolescents in Finland: A comparative study of adolescents from different cultural
backgrounds. Journal of Adolescent Research, 15, 446–469.
Lind, E. A., & Tyler, T. R. (1988). The Social Psychology of Procedural Justice. New York, NY:
Plenum Press.
Marmot, M. (2004). The Status Syndrome. New York, NY: Henry Holt and Company.
Rawls, J. (1971). A Theory of Justice. Cambridge, MA: Belknap.
Schmitt, M., & Dörfel, M. (1999). Procedural injustice at work, justice sensitivity, job satisfaction
and psychosomatic well-being. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29 (4), 443– 453.
Sennett, R. (2003). Respect in a World of Inequality. New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company, Inc.
Simon, B., & Sturmer, S. (2003). Respect for group members: Intragroup determinants of
collective identification and group-serving behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
29, 183–193.
Simon, B., & Stürmer, S. (2005). In search of the active ingredient of respect: A closer look at
the role of acceptance. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35 (6), 809– 818.
Simon, B., Lücken, M., & Stürmer, S. (2006). The added value of respect: Reaching across
inequality. British Journal of Social Psychology, 45 (3), 535 –546.
Smith, H. J., & Tyler, T. R. (1997). Choosing the right pond: The impact of group membership
on self-esteem and group-oriented behavior. Journal of Experimental and Social Psychology, 33,
146 –170.
Smith, H. J., Tyler, T. R., & Huo, Y. J. (2003). Interpersonal treatment, social identity and
organizational behavior. In S. A. Haslam, D. van Knippenberg, M. J. Platow, & N. Ellemers
(Eds.), Social Identity at Work: Developing Theory for Organizational Practice (pp. 155–171).
Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.
© 2008 The Authors Social and Personality Psychology Compass 2 (2008): 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00129.x
Journal Compilation © 2008 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
16 The Psychology of Respect
Smith, H. J., Tyler, T. R., Huo, Y. J., Ortiz, D., & Lind, E. A. (1998). The self–relevant
implications of the group-value model: Group membership, self-worth, and treatment quality.
Journal of Experimental and Social Psychology, 34, 470 – 493.
Spears, R., Ellemers, N., & Doosje, B. (2005). Let me count the ways in which I respect thee:
Does competence compensate or compromise lack of liking from the group? European Journal
of Social Psychology, 35 (2), 263–279.
Spears, R., Ellemers, N., Doosje, B., & Branscombe, N. (2006). The Individual Within the
Group: Respect! Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.
Srivastava, S., & Beer, J. S. (2005). How self-evaluations relate to being liked by others:
Integrating sociometer and attachment perspectives. Journal of personality and social psychology,
89 (6), 966 –977.
Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and
performance. American Psychologist, 52, 613– 629.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In
S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of Intergroup Relations (pp. 7–24). Chicago, IL:
Nelson-Hall.
Thibaut, J. W., & Kelley, H. H. (1959). The Social Psychology of Groups. New York, NY: Wiley.
Tyler, T. R., & Smith, H. J. (1999). Justice, Social Identity, and Group Processes. Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Tyler, T. R., Degoey, P., & Smith, H. (1996). Understanding why the justice of group procedures
matters: A test of the psychological dynamics of the group-value model. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 70, 913 –930.
Tyler, T. R., & Lind, A. E. (1992). A relational model of authority in groups. Advances in
Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 115 –191.
Williams, K. D., Forgas, J. P., von Hippel, W., & Zadro, L. (2005). The social outcast: An
overview. In K. D. Williams, J. P. Forgas, & W. von Hippel (Eds.), The Social Outcast:
Ostracism, Social Exclusion, Rejection, and Bullying (pp. 1–16). New York, NY: Psychology
Press.
© 2008 The Authors Social and Personality Psychology Compass 2 (2008): 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00129.x
Journal Compilation © 2008 Blackwell Publishing Ltd