Walter Anthony Cook Introduction To Tagmemic Analysis
Walter Anthony Cook Introduction To Tagmemic Analysis
Tagmemic Analysis
Introduction to
Tagmemic Analysis
Washington, D.C.
May 1969
Contents
Preface v
2 SENTENCE LEVEL 39
Sentence Level Sorting 41
Major Sentences 47
Minor Sentences 54
3 CLAUSE LEVEL 65
Independent Clauses 67
Dependent Clauses 73
Clause Level Analysis 79
4 PHRASE LEVEL 91
Relater-Axis Phrases 93
Multiple Head Phrases 99
Head-Modifier Phrases 106
Index 205
In human speech, different sounds
have different meanings. To study this
coordination of certain sounds with
certain meanings is to study
language.
Leonard Bloomfield, Language, 1933
INTRODUCTION: METHOD MODELS AND PRACTICE
'This refers to the author, date, and page number of the reading in the Selected Bibliog-
raphy at the end of the text.
2 INTRODUCTION TO TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS
Models of Grammar
SENTENCE LEVEL
Base + Intonation
CLAUSE LEVEL
Subject + Predicate + Object + Adjuncts
PHRASE LEVEL
Relater + Axis, and Endocentric Word Groups
WORD LEVEL
Stems + Derivations + Inflections
MORPHEME LEVKL
The ultimate level of analysis
1 TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS
13
14 INTRODUCTION TO TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS
word sequences are grouped into units, and the string is cut
simultaneously into its functional parts at a given level in
the grammar.
In practice, every tagma is considered, in the first analysis,
as if it were an invariant unit. One works as if every tagma were
a tagmeme, that is, as if every tagmeme had one and only one
allotagma. This is parallel to the science of morphetics where
the units isolated are morphs, but initially considered as if they
were the only manifestations of the morpheme. Form and mean-
ing are clearly isolated.
Once the slot-class correlatives have been initially formu-
lated it is possible to institute a grouping process and group
together those tagmas which seem to have the same functional
meaning, even though the fillers of the slots may be different.
Thus, given such structures as S:N, a subject slot filled by a noun
phrase, and S:pn, a subject slot filled by a pronoun, these may be
combined as S:N/pn, a subject slot filled by either a noun phrase
or a pronoun. Alternate filler classes are represented by the slash
symbol (/) and repeated as often as needed. When variants of a
tagmeme seem to have different grammatical meaning, the
grouping process becomes more difficult.
Allotagmas of a Tagmeme. The tagmeme is the essential unit
of grammar, but may be represented by one or more allotagmas.
Since the unit is a function-form unit, the variants of a tagmeme
may differ in function, or in form, or in both. Since the function-
form unit has a distribution in the language, there may also be
differences in distribution. The linguistic sign (LS) is fully de-
scribed in terms of meaning, form, and distribution (MFD); the
variants of the tagmeme will be variants: (1) of meaning, (2) of
form, (3) of distribution or some combination of these features.
If tagmas are totally different in form and meaning, and also
in distribution, they belong to different tagmemes. If tagmas are
partially different, that is, if they differ in one of these features
the following norms may be useful:
1. Tagmas differing in form alone are tagmas with the same
functional meaning and the same position in the string.
These are easily grouped as allotagmas of the same
tagmeme, by listing the alternate fillers in the one
tagmemic slot.
2. Tagmas differing in meaning alone may belong to the
same tagmeme. Although the structural meaning of the
tagmeme is the principal identifying feature, it is pos-
sible to carry over meanings from one's own native
language into the target language. Therefore, unless
the meaning difference is correlated with a parallel
TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS 21
Kinds of Syntagmemes
The syntagmeme, or construction, is a potential string of
tagmemes. All of the strings are not of the same kind. Strings
of various kinds occur at all levels of grammatical analysis.
These construction types include: (1) exocentric and endocentric
constructions, (2) closed and open-ended constructions and
(3) recursive and nonrecursive layering.
Exocentric versus Endocentric Constructions. Constructions
may be of the exocentric or endocentric type. An endocentric
construction is centered about one or more head tagmemes. An
exocentiie construction is not centered. In endocentric construc-
tionsi the whole construction may be replaced by a form similar
m form class to the head of the construction; in exocentric con-
structions, where there is no head tagmeme, the whole construc-
tion does not fill the same slot as one of its parts.
In string-type analysis, endocentric constructions include
multiple head constructions, such as coordinate phrase and
appositive phrase, as well as single headed phrases, composed of
nouns verbs, adjectives, and adverbs with their modifiers; they
24 INTRODUCTION TO TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS
sider only the sentence level and levels below the sentence. This
includes: (1) the five typical levels of grammar, (2) atypical
mapping of levels, and (3) the systematic organization of levels
in a field structure of hierarchy.
Typical Levels of Grammar. The five typical levels of gram-
mar are the sentence, clause, phrase, word, and morpheme levels.
These correspond to the sentence, clause, group, word, and mor-
pheme levels of M. A. K. Halliday's scale-and-category grammar.
The first four levels are levels at which constructions occur. The
final, or morpheme level, is not a level of construction, but an
ultimate point of reference.
1. The sentence level is that level of the grammar at which
the major and minor sentences of language are broken
down into dependent and independent clauses, together
with their intonation patterns.
2. The clause level is that level of the grammar at which
the clauses are broken down into their subjects, predi-
cates, objects, and various clause adjuncts such as time,
place, manner, and circumstance.
3. The phrase level is that level of the grammar at which the
structured word groups which are not clauses are
broken down into words.
4. The word level is that level of the grammar at which the
words of the language are broken down into their con-
stituent morphemes, including the analysis of the pro-
cesses of inflection, derivation, and compounding.
5. The morpheme level is that level of the grammar at
which the morphemes are seen as the ultimate mean-
ingful constituents of which the utterances of a lan-
guage are composed. Each morpheme is listed in a
lexicon accompanying the grammar, with its form
class, and gloss.
A tagmemic grammar will therefore consist of a set of for-
mulas ranging from the .sentence through the clause, phrase
and word levels, and this grammar will be accompanied by a
lexicon of constituent morphemes. The five levels as listed are
typical of most languages. It may be possible to establish more
or fewer levels within a given language, but strong points of
contrast between levels would have to be established in order
to set up new levels in the grammar. A priori, the five-level sys-
tem seems to correspond to natural units of language — found in
most grammars—which native speakers feel intuitively are
sentences, clauses, phrases, and words (see Pike, 1967:444).
Atypical Mapping. Although the normal processes of gram-
mar call for a mapping of lower level constructions into higher
TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS 31
At the phrase level, the word groups that fill clause level
slots are analyzed into constituent words. Slot names are marked
with capitals; fillers have capital letters for word groups and
small letters for words.
Phrase Level Construction:
RA = +R:rel +Ax:N across + (the prairie)
Read: A relater-axis phrase consists of a relater slot filled
by a relater (preposition), and an axis slot filled by a noun
phrase.
N = +Det:det±Pos:pos +H:n the + pioneer's + family
Read: A noun phrase consists of a determiner slot filled by a
determiner, an optional possessive slot filled by a possessive,
and a head slot filled by a noun.
Tern = ±Int:int +H:tem just + yesterday
Read: A temporal phrase consists of an optional intensifier
slot filled by an intensifier and a head slot filled by a tem-
poral word.
At the word level, words that consist of more than one mor-
pheme are analyzed into constituent morphemes. Slots have
small letters; fillers have small letters if they are either mor-
phemes or words. (For possessive noun phrase, see formula 4.13.)
Word Level Construction:
pos = +nnuc:ns ±num:plm +pos:posm pioneer + -'s
Read: A possessive consists of a noun nuclear slot filled by a
noun stem, an optional number slot filled by a plural marker,
and a possessive slot filled by a possessive marker.
tv = +vnuc:tvs ±t:tm pull + -ed
Read: A transitive verb consists of a verb nuclear slot filled
by a transitive verb stem and an optional tense slot filled
by a tense marker.
aj = +core:ivs/tvs +ajzr:{-ing}/{-ed} cover + -ed
Read: One type of adjective consists of a core slot filled by
a verb stem (transitive or intransitive), and an adjectivizer
slot filled by the (derivational) suffixes, {-ing} and {-ed}.
36 INTRODUCTION TO TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS
SUPPLEMENTARY READINGS 1
Cook, Walter A., S.J., On Tagmemes and Transforms, Washington, D.C.
Georgetown University Press, 1964. A summary of the tagmemic
system, including the use of transforms and generative potential.
Elson, Benjamin, and Pickett, Velma, An Introduction to Morphology
and Syntax, Santa Ana, Summer Institute of Linguistics, 1964.
Standard text for beginning morphology and syntax in tagmemics.
Longacre, Robert E., "String Constituent Analysis," Language, 36:63-
88, (1960). Compares tagmemic string construction with IC analy-
sis.
, Grammar Discovery Procedures, The Hague, Mouton & Co.,
1964. For procedures of tagmemics, including the use of transforms
and the generative potential in terms of a set of rewrite operations.
, "Some Fundamental Insights of Tagmemics," Language, 41:
65-76. For tagmeme, syntagmeme, hierarchy, and field structure.
Monograph Series on Languages and Linguistics, 18th Annual Round
Table, No. 20 (1967), E. L. Blansitt, Jr, ed. Conference with panels
on tagmemic theory, current research in tagmemic description
and grammatical analysis.
Pike, Kenneth L., Language in Relation to a Unified Theory of the Struc-
ture of Human Behaviour, Glendale, California, Summer Institute
of Linguistics, Part I (1954), includes Chaps. 1-7, on behavior,
morphemes, Part II (1955), includes Chaps. 8-10, phonemes, syl-
lables, Part III (1960), includes Chaps. 11-17, tagmeme, syntag-
meme. Reprinted in one volume with updated bibliographies and
footnotes on current developments. The Hague, Mouton & Co.,
1967.
"On Tagmemes nee Gramemes," UAL, 24:273-278, (1958). For
the concept of tagmeme and its discovery.
, "Language as Particle, Wave and Field." The Texas Quarterly,
2:37-54, (1959). For the development of the trimodal nature of the
unit.
"Dimensions of Grammatical Structure." Language, 38:221-
244, (1962). For the exposition of matrix theory as applied to gram-
mar,
"A Syntactic Paradigm." Language, 39:216-230, (1963). For
matrix theory applied to a practical example.
"A Guide to Publications Related to Tagmemic Theory." Cur-
rent Trends in Linguistics, Thomas A. Sebeok, ed., The Hague,
Mouton & Co., Vol. Ill (1966) 365-394. A survey and critical ap-
praisal of literature on tagmemic theory and practice.
TABLE 2: SENTENCE LEVEL ANALYSIS
According to the According to the According to the According to the
Type of Intonation Type of Base Type of Clauses Type of Situation
1. Compound Sentence 1. Statement (S)
two independent clauses formed to relay
with conjoining information
Major Types 2. Complex Sentence 2. Question (Q)
with independent and dependent clause formed to elicit
Complete Base with embedding answer response
3. Simple Sentence 3. Command (C)
one independent clause, formed to elicit
Sentences no dependent clause action response
with
Final Intonation
1. Sequential Sentence 1. Addition (A)
from compound with statements
2. Marginal Sentence 2. Response (R)
Minor Types from complex with questions
with 3. Elliptical Sentence 3. Exclamation (E)
Incomplete Base from simple in any context
Nonelliptical Types Vocatives, greetings,
no clause structure calls, titles, mottoes
SENTENCE LEVEL
Kernel Sentences
Within a given language, certain sentences belong to a set
of basic structures, and all other structures may be expressed
as derived from these basic structures. The basic structures
are called kernel sentences. We first identify the distinctive
features of kernel sentences; then consider the derived sen-
tences opposed to these basic structures; and finally show how
the nonkernel sentences are derived from kernel sentences.
Distinctive Features of the Kernel. Traditionally, language
analysis has focused upon the declarative sentence as of primary
importance. With the introduction of transformational grammar,
analysis has once more concentrated upon this basic type. In
response to the question of which set of sentences belongs to the
kernel, Chomsky answers, that for English at least, "the kernel
consists of simple, declarative, active sentences" (1957:80). How-
ever since both negative transformations and deletion trans-
formations are included in the grammar, we assume that the
kernel is also affirmative and nonelliptical.
A kernel sentence is defined as a sentence of the language
42 INTRODUCTION TO TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS
that is (1) simple, (2) complete, (3) statement, (4) active and
(5) affirmative. Any sentence that simultaneously has these
five distinctive features is a kernel sentence; any sentence that
lacks even one of these five features is a derived sentence. In
our analysis, we first institute a sort of all sentences in the
corpus to separate the kernel set from the nonkernel or de-
rived set. Later on, we attempt to describe the derived sentences
in terms of the kernel set, using either transformational rules
or matrix displays, to show how nonkernel sentences are derived.
In this way we concentrate attention on the basic sentence
structures.
Derived Sentences. The notion of kernel sentences is more
clearly understood when kernel and nonkernel sentences are
compared. The derived sentences lack at least one of the five
distinctive features of kernel sentences. Kernel and derived
sentences may be contrasted as follows:
Kernel Sentences Derived Sentences
Simple versus Complex, Compound
Complete versus Incomplete, Elliptical
Statement versus Question, Command
Active versus Middle, Passive
Affirmative versus Negative
In a sentence level sort, kernel sentences are separated from
derived sentences, and primary attention is focused upon kernel
sentences. The analysis of derived sentences is then related to
the simple structures from which they are derived.
Transformation. The transformational rule is simply a rule
of change. This rule has an input string, a rule of change, and an
output string. With kernel sentences as input, it is possible to
set up a series of optional rules that will produce the output,
the derived sentences:
INPUT STRING T-RULE OUTPUT STRING
Kernel sentence + transformation = derived sentence
Tagmemic grammars formerly described all the sentences of a
language by describing both kernel and derived sentences. The
resulting description was a complete description, but often failed
to show the relationships between similar sentences. With the
introduction of transformational rules or matrix devices to show
the relationships between sentences, it is still necessary to
describe both kernel and derived sentences in order to discover
the differences between structures. However, the final grammar
may be considerably simplified by employing some type of trans-
SENTENCE LEVEL 43
CLAUSE
TYPE STATEMENT QUESTION COMMAND
S-iCl Q-iCl C-iCl
intransitive j o hn went. DidJohn go? Go, John!
S-tCl Q-tCl C-tCl
iransmve John ate it. Did John eat it? Eat it, John!
E uational S-eqCl Q-eqCl C-eqCl
qua lona j o n n is good. Is John good? Be good, John!
Matrix Operators. The analogy of a mathematical matrix
may be carried one step further by the introduction of matrix op-
erators. In matrix algebra, the matrix may be used as a unit in
SENTENCE LEVEL 51
Nonclause Structures
The minor sentences of language include single words or
short phrases that do not have the underlying structure of a
clause. These structures are characterized, in general, by the
absence of a predicate or predicate-like tagmeme in the string,
which defines a clause. Yet such forms do occur with final intona-
tion and fall within the general class of minor sentences.
Calls, Greetings, Interjections. Minor sentences of this group
have no clause structure and are generally limited to one or two
words. They are functionally of the exclamatory type, syntac-
tically independent.
1. Calls, or vocatives, are generally the names of persons
or titles of address designating persons. Many case-
marked language systems have a vocative case used
with or without exclamatory particles.
John! Waiter! Mr. Secretary!
2. Greetings in most languages are stereotyped expres-
sions, used in a ritual for meeting people, initiating
conversation, or leaving. Where the individual words
had meaning, this original meaning is often lost.
Hello! Goodbye! Be seeing you!
3. Interjections are usually short and expressive, not ex-
pecting a particular response. They are used particu-
larly to express strong emotion, such as pain, surprise,
enthusiasm but not limited to these uses.
Ouch! Oh boy! Doggone it!
Most of these types need no grammatical formulation. They are
simply listed in the lexicon of the language for use in particular
context situations; and they are listed as single lexical entries
without further analysis.
Titles, Mottoes, Inscriptions. This group of minor sentences
is generally of greater length and indicates some of the phrase
structures of the language. This type may overlap with favorite
sentence types.
SENTENCE LEVEL 59
1. Titles, when they consist of more than one word are
generally a legitimate phrase structure, with the author
included as an agentive. In reading, the structure is
spoken with a single final intonation pattern.
Alice in Wonderland by Lewis Carroll
2. Mottoes consisting of more than one word, show regular
phrase structure. The type of phrase used may differ
from language to language, as each culture has its own
favorite phra.se types in this use.
Latin: In caritate et iustitia (An adverbial phrase)
French: Liberti e*galite,fraternite (A nominal phrase)
3. Inscriptions, when limited to phrase structure, as well
as spoken toasts, often begin, in English, with To mean-
ing "dedicated to," followed by nominals often with
embedded modifying clauses.
Nontypical Structures. There are sentences in language use
which do not conform to any of the major or minor sentence
types. They are special uses of language which may be pitfalls
for the unwary analyst.
Metalanguage is language about language. In this usage,
some of the forms of language become the topic of conversation,
and thereby become nominal in use, losing their original func-
tional class, for example:
The is a definite article. {the = a noun)
Using metalanguage, it is possible to string together lists of
conjunctions, auxiliaries, and other parts of speech, m ways
normally ungrammatical.
Between hotdogs and and and and and hamburgers
there is the same spacing given on the neon sign.
Once the forms in metalinguistic use are recognized, the gram-
matical structures appear to be quite regular.
Abbreviated language occurs in common use in headlines in
the writing of telegrams, and in some types of radio announce-
mentSi The structure is shortened by eliminating many of the
function words, with the result that the message is cryptic and
often becomes ambiguous.
Army camps in the open Is camps noun or verb?
Officers Flying Home Is officers plural, possessive?
Anyone who has tried writing a ten-word telegram has experi-
enced the difficulties of using this abbreviated form of language.
60 INTRODUCTION TO TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS
+R +S +P ±0
1. as the sun set
2. he remembered the time in the tavern at
Casablanca
3. to give himself more confidence
4. when he had played the hand game with the great
negro from Cienfuegos
5. X who was the strongest man on the docks
Clauses within Sentences. After reduction of the corpus, com-
plete formulas for complex and compound sentences can be re-
corded with intonation pattern. If clause (2) main and clause
(3) purpose, combine:
(Complex) Sent = ±Marg:F-tCl +Base:tCl -Into:ICF
Read: A complex sentence consists of an optional margin
slot, filled by a purpose clause (F), a base slot filled by a
transitive clause, and an intonation slot filled by final in-
tonation contour.
F-tCl = +P:tvmt +IO:refl.pn +D0:N (Purpose)
Read: A purpose clause consists of a predicate slot filled by a
transitive verb infinitive, an indirect object slot filled by a
reflexive pronoun, and a direct object slot filled by a noun
phrase.
Alternate Solution: If the complex sentence formula is not a
useful pattern in the overall structure of the language, the pur-
pose clause may be interpreted as filling a purpose slot within
the main clause:
tCl = +S:pn +P:tv ±F:F-tCl ±0:N id (Main Clause)
Read: The (main) transitive clause consists of a subject
slot filled by a pronoun, a predicate slot filled by a transitive
verb an optional purpose slot filled by a purpose clause
and an optional object slot filled by an identified noun phrase.
Clauses within Clauses. Some dependent clauses will be
found as fillers of clause level slots, with one clause layered
within another clause. The temporal clause (1) fills a clause
level temporal slot.
tCl = ±T:T-iCl +S:pn +P:tv ±0:Nld (Main Clause)
Read: The (main) transitive clause consists of an optional
temporal slot filled by a temporal clause and so on.
T-iCl = +R:rel +Ax:iCl (Temporal)
Read: A temporal clause consists of a relater slot filled by
a relater and an axis slot filled by an intransitive clause.
62 INTRODUCTION TO TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS
SUPPLEMENTARY READINGS 2
Bloomfield, Leonard, Language, New York, Holt, Rinehart and Winston,
Inc., 1933. See Chap. 11, "Sentence Types," 170-177, for the classi-
cal definition of sentence. Favorite (= major) types are distin-
guished from minor. The minor are completive or exclamatory.
Chomsky, Noam, Syntactic Structures, The Hague, Mouton & Co., 1957.
Kernel sentences are the sentences which result when we apply
only obligatory and no optional transformations (46). In English,
the kernel sentences are simple declarative active (80) with no
complex noun or verb phrases (107). All other sentences are derived.
Elson, Benjamin, and Pickett, Velma, An Introduction to Morphology
and Syntax, Santa Ana, Summer Institute of Linguistics, 1962.
Chap. 11, "Tagmemes and Constructions at the Sentence Level,"
82-83, and Chap. 18, "Survey of Senten.e Types," 121-128. The
levels above the sentence are treated briefly in Chap. 19,127-128.
Fries, Charles C, The Structure of English, New York, Harcourt, Brace
and World, Inc., 1952. See Chap. 2, "What is a Sentence?" 9-28, for
use of Bloomfield's definition versus traditional definitions. Chap.
3, "Kinds of Sentences," 29-53, based on situation and response.
Gleason, Henry A. Jr, Linguistics and English Grammar, New York,
Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1965. Chap. 14, "Joining Clauses,"
329-350. Rejects division of sentences into simple, complex, and
compound, in favor of IC coordination and subordination of
clauses.
Liem, Nguyen Dang, English Grammar, A Combined Tagmemic and
Transformational Approach, Linguistic Circle of Canberra, 1966.
A Contrastive Analysis of Vietnamese and English, vol. I, Chap.
5, "English Sentence Types and Sentence Level Structures," 139-
149. Lists major independent, major dependent, and minor types.
SENTENCE LEVEL 83
Longacre, Robert E., Grammar Discovery Procedures, The Hague,
Mouton & Co., 1964. Chap. 4, "Sentence Level Procedures," 125-
161. Treats sentence level after the levels of clause, phrase, and
word.
, "The Notion of Sentence," Monograph Series on Languages and
Linguisties, No. 20 (Washington, D.C., Georgetown University
Press, 1967>, 15-25. Defines sentence level as a level of clause com-
bination, and classifies sentences by conjunction, alternation, im-
plication, and negation.
TABLE 3: CLAUSE LEVEL ANALYSIS
The clause level of grammar is that level that is below the sen-
tence level and above the phrase level. The clause is composed of
words and phrases and, in turn, fills slots at the sentence level.
The clause is a unit of grammar. It is a construction in which the
constitute is a potential sentence base and in which the con-
stituents are the subjects, predicates, objects, and adjuncts that
combine to form this base. Clause level analysis is central to the
system.
Clause Defined. The clause is "a string of tagmemes that con-
sists of or includes one arid only one predicate, or predicate-like
tagmeme, in the string, and whose manifesting morpheme se-
quence typically fills slots at the sentence level" (Elson and
Pickett, 1962H34). In the wording of this definition, the following
essential features should be noted:
1. Clauses typically fill slots on the sentence level. In typical
mapping of lower constructions into higher levels
clauses combine to form sentences, or combine with in-
tonation to form sentences. However, atypical mapping
of clause within clause and clause within phrase occurs.
2. Clauses consist of or include one and only one predicate.
Accordingly, there are as many clauses as there are
predicate tagmemes. A single clause may, however, have
a compound verb-form in the predicate slot. Some lan-
guages have verb forms which, in themselves, constitute
whatLongacre calls a "clause-in-miniature," containing
both subject and predicate; other languages require
subject and predicate tagmemes.
3. Clauses may have a predicate-like tagmeme. Particularly
65
66 INTRODUCTION TO TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS
INDEPENDENT CLAUSES
The clause level in tagmemics is the heart of the analytic process.
The sentence level provides a sorting ground for reducing sen-
tences to homogeneous sets of clauses, apart from their intona-
tion. After sorting, the underlying structure of the sentence is
analyzed by taking apart its clause base, in terms of subject-
predicate-object-adjunct tagmemes that form the underlying
structure. The work done here is comparable to the upper level
branching rules of a transformational grammar.
different elements in focus; but this does not indicate the need
for a new clause type. The emphatic clause must differ from
others by two structural differences. For example in Cashina-
hua (#164), the object is moved and marked with a particle.
3.10 E-tCl = +O:Nm/pnm ±M:RA +S:N/pn +P:tv/tV
The object slot normally occurs immediately before the predi-
cate. In the emphatic type clause, the object slot is moved to
first position, and the noun phrase or pronoun is marked with a
special emphatic particle {-ra}. There is a corresponding change
of meaning to strong emphasis. In the Laboratory Manual by
Merrifield, emphatic stress is indicated by underlining the forms
stressed. Palantla Chinantec (#125), Northern Tepehuan (#126),
Kalagan (#168, #170), and Bukidnon Manobo (#169) are sample
problems dealing with emphatic clause structures.
DEPENDENT CLAUSES
Dependent clauses are clauses that may not stand alone as major
sentences, though they occur, with final intonation, as minor
sentences. To understand the use of dependent clauses within
major sentence structure, we consider the external distribution
of the clause, the functional meaning of the clause and its in-
ternal structure.
each heading list the words or phrases that fill these particular
slots. Peripheral elements may initially be labeled as T: tem-
poral, L: Locational, I: introducer, and M: manner. Elements
m the string should be listed, in the chart, roughly in the order
m which they occur in the data. If the order is not fixed use the
statistically predominant order. If only one element is movable,
it may be listed in both positions. A partial chart of the data in
Sierra Popoluca (#165, Laboratory Manual, Merrifield, 1967) for
transitive clauses, showing subject, predicate, object, and loca-
tional slots, is given below.
±S +P ±0 ±L
Tagmemic Grammar
Tagmemic Lexicon
The lexicon is the "total stock of morphemes in a language"
Bloomneld (1933:162). In a particular language problem, the
lexicon is the totallist of morphemes in the particular language,
84 INTRODUCTION TO TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS
SUPPLEMENTARY READINGS 3
Elson, Benjamin, and Pickett, Velma, An Introduction to Morphology
and Syntax, Santa Ana, Summer Institute of Linguistics, 1962.
Chap. 7, "Tagmemes and Constructions at the Clause Level,"
64-72; Chap. 17, "Survey of Clause Types," 108-121, active, reflex-
ive, passive, indefinite, stative, imperative, optative, interrogative,
emphatic, and various types of dependent clauses.
Fries, Charles C, The Structure of English, New York, Harcourt, Brace
and World, Inc., 1952. Chap. 9, "Structural Meanings, Subjects
and Objects," 173-201; Chap. 12, "Immediate Constituents, Layers
of Structure," 256-273, with branching rules given 271-272. For
word classes, Class 1, 76; Class 2, 80; Class 3, 82; Class 4 83.
Gleason, Henry A., Jr., Linguistics and English Grammar, New York,
Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1965. Chap. 7, "Syntactic rela-
tions," 138-167, with traditional and IC diagrams; Chap. 13,
"Clause Patterns," 299-328, with transformations, 304-306.
Greenberg, Joseph H., Univeraals of Language, Cambridge, Mass., The
M.I.T. Press 1963. Chap. 5, "Some Universals of Grammar with
Particular Reference to the Order of Meaningful Elements,"
73-113, treats SPO order in language and prepositional/postposi-
tional contrast.
Koutsoudas, Andre, Writing Transformational Grammars, New York,
McGraw-Hill, Inc., 1966. Chap. 7, "Conjoining," 231-232; Chap.
8, "Embedding," 269-270, in transformational grammars.
Liem, Nguyen Dang, English Grammar, A Combined Tagmemtc and
Transformational Approach, Linguistic Circle of Canberra, 1966.
88 INTRODUCTION TO TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS
Chap. 1, "Independent Declarative Clause Types," 1-20, with sum-
mary chart 1.2, listing 10 independent declarative types. Minimum
and expanded formulas are given, with examples.
Longacre, Robert E., Grammar Discovery Procedures, The Hague,
Mouton & Co., 1964. Chap. 1, "Clause Level," 35-73, including
drama analogy, 35; rule of two, 47; and clause matrix, 70.
, and Williams, Ann F., "Popoluca Clause Types," Ada Linguis-
tica Hafniensia, vol. X, no. 2, 161-186 (Copenhagen, 1962). Dis-
tinguishes 6 kernel (statement) clause types and 12 derived (in-
terrogative) clause types, based on formal criteria.
TABLE 4: PHRASE LEVEL ANALYSIS
According1 to the According to the According to the Characteristics
Type of Grouping Type of Structure Internal Structure of Phrase Type
Exocentric Relater-Axis (RA) Recognized by
Noncentered relater and phrase relater class
1. Coordinate Phrase Recognized as
similar phrases, same type phrases
Phrase different referents with conjunction
Endocentric
as a 2. Item-Appositive (IA) Recognized as
Multiple Head
Structured similar phrases, same type phrases
Word Group same referent without conjunction
91
92 INTRODUCTION TO TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS
RELATER-AXIS PHRASES
The phrase level of grammar deals with structured word groups
which are not clauses. Since these word groups are of various
kinds we find within the phrase level, various geological strata
or layers. One such layer is the layer of relation. Words or word
groups, together with a phrase relater, act as functioning units.
These are called relater-axis phrases.
Relater-Axis Phrase Defined
A relater-axis phrase is a structured word group with two
immediate constituents, one of which is a phrase relater and the
other a word or word group governed by the relater and called
the axis. This construction type is exocentric because neither the
relater alone, nor the axis alone, may fill the same clause level
slots as the relater-axis group.
Relational Structure. In our discussion of the features which
might be found in emic levels of structure within a language
(see p. 32), we suggested that levels may be characterized by
three geological strata: the layer of relation, the layer of co-
ordination, and the layer of subordination. At the phrase level,
the layer of relation is exemplified by the relater-axis type con-
struction which is a phrase, but of a type different from other
phrases.
The function of the relater, in this type of structure, is to
relate the whole constitute of the axis whether word or phrase,
to the grammatical structure of which it is a part. The relater
acts as a sky hook, which takes the word or word group that is
its axis and ties it into the structure. The function of the relater
here is analogous to the clause level relater which subordinates
clauses to sentence structure and to inflectional endings at the
word level which relate the word form to syntactic use.
This structure should always be treated as an immediate
constituent structure with two and only two constituents the
relater and its axis. The reason for this is that the axis in turn
94 INTRODUCTION TO TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS
such as: and; (2) disjunctive, but not; (3) alternative, either/or;
(4) comparative, rather/than.
In contrast, the appositive multiple head phrase is char-
acterized by zero connector and the heads of construction have
both the same grammatical function and the same referent in
the extra-linguistic world:
Apposition: Read Tolstoy, the author of War and Peace
If connectors are introduced, the phrase is interpreted as co-
ordinate and the referents of the two heads of construction are
presumed different:
here in Washington Apposition: here = in Washington
here and in Washington Coordinate: here 5* in Washington
Connector class. The connector class used in conjoining at
the phrase level, is also used at the sentence level in combining
clauses to form compound sentences (see p. 45). The following
list of connectors is common in English; it is listed by Fries
(1952:94-95) as Group E:
and both .. .. and
but, but not not ..but
not neither. ..nor
or either .. . .or
rather than rather . ..than
In the listing above, Fries notes that the connector, but used
with words and phrases, is more common with adjectivals, poor
but honest or with adverbials, slowly but carefully. With nomin-
als this connector is common only with indefinite pronouns,
in such expressions as everything but, nothing but. It is not com-
mon elsewhere with nominals or verbals. In tagmemic formula-
tions, the first list is the list of single connectors and the second
list is the list of double connectors which fill the first (cj and
second (Cj.) connector slots respectively. The connector slot as
part of phrase level, is marked with a capital letter, as C:; the
fillers, being words, are represented by small letters, as c or as
Cj . . . C 2 ,
HEAD-MODIFIER PHRASES
The first layer at phrase level concerns the relater-axis phrase,
the second layer the multiple head phrases. The third level deals
with the single headed phrases and the structure of modifica-
tion. This latter layer is also endocentric, but is endocentric and
subordinate. All of the elements of the phrase are subordinate
to the single head tagmeme.
within the brackets are chosen. These are "etic variants of the
emic construction" (Pike, 1962:236). The formulation in 4.11 may
be made explicit in the following formulation:
rel -j "m.sg. "—o
4.12 N=+Det: la +H:n f.sg. ±Mod:aj —a
los m.pl. —OS
.las_ Lf.pi. J .—as.
T h e m e c h a n i c s of concord a r e outlined by Elson a n d P i c k e t t
(1962:87-91), developed by Pike as " t a g m a s in reciprocally-con-
ditioned v a r i a t i o n , " (1962:236), a n d specified by D a n M. M a t s o n
in " T a g m e m i c Description of A g r e e m e n t " (18th A n n u a l R o u n d
Table, Georgetown, 1967:103-108).
In a g e n e r a t i v e t a g m e m i c model we view t h e concord m a r k -
ing as calling up a subroutine that will select the proper modi-
fiers according to the head word selected. The rule, as Matson
has pointed out, is context-restrictive, and exponentiation (cate-
sian multiplication) is not uniformly possible. The output must
be broken down into four types of output as in the above example
with no cross-multiplication. Thus the output may be calculated
separately for the noun phrases:
NnNfsic N mpl Nfl
The subroutine would identify the head noun with the selective
feature for example, casa, n.f.; (note that it can be marked for
plural) and generate the phrases, la casa blanca and las casas
blancas while excluding the masculine articles and the adjec-
tives with masculine endings. The output generated contains
all grammatical and no ungrammatical sequences.
SUPPLEMENTARY READINGS 4
Becker, Alton L., "Conjoining in a Tagmemic Grammar of English,"
Monograph Series on Languages and Linguistics, No. 20, 109-121.
Washington, D.C., Georgetown University Press, 1967.
Elson, Benjamin, and Pickett, Velma, An Introduction to Morphology
and Syntax, Santa Ana, Summer Institute of Linguistics, 1962.
Chap. 8, "Tagmemes and Constructions at the Phrase Level "
73-75; Chap. 16, "Survey of Phrase Types," 103-108, with head-
modifier, coordinate, appositional, relater-axis, and close-knit
verb phrase.
Fries, Charles C, The Structure of English, New York, Harcourt, Brace
and World, Inc., 1952. Chap. 10, "Structural Meanings, Modifiers,"
202-239, and Chap. 6, "Function Words," 87-109.
Gleason, Henry A., Jr., Linguistics and English Grammar, New York,
Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1965. Chap. 8, "Structure Sig-
nals," see "Parts of Speech and Function Words," 186-194, use of
meaning.
Hockett, Charles F., A Course in Modern Linguistics, New York, Mac-
millan Company, 1958. Chap. 21, "Endocentric Constructions,"
183-190 and Chap. 22, "Exocentric Constructions," 191-198.
Huddleston, R. D., "Rank and Depth," Language, 41: 574-586 (1965).
Proposes a coordinate layer for all levels of the grammar, in a
scale-and-category grammar of five (natural) levels.
Law, Howard W., "The Use of Function-Set in English Adverbial Classi-
fication, Monograph Series on Languages and Linguistics, No. 20
93-102. Washington, D.C., Georgetown University Press, 1967.
Sorting adverbials by form, function, and position.
Matson Dan M., "Tagmemic Description of Agreement," Monograph
Series on Languages and Linguistics, 18th Annual Round Table
No. 20, 103-108. Washington, D.C., Georgetown University Press
1967. Tagmemic agreement reduced to subroutine using square
bracket notation.
Warriner, John E., English Grammar and Composition, New York,
Harcourt, Brace and World, Inc., 1951, revised 1965. Chap. 2 "The
Phrase," 35-51, including traditional definition of phrase (36)
and explanation of prepositional, verbal, and appositive phrases.
TABLE 5: WORD LEVEL ANALYSIS
According to Its According to the According to Its Characteristics
Freedom in Use Formation Process Internal Structure of the Word Type
The word level of grammar is that level which is below the phrase
level and above the morpheme level. The word is composed of
morphemes and typically fills slots at the phrase level. It is a con-
struction in which the constitute is a minimally free form in the
language and whlose constituents are morphemes. Words are
composed of one or more morphemes; the morphemes are the
ultimate grammatical constituents, the minimum meaningful
forms in the language.
Word Defined; The word has been defined by Bloomfield
(1933:178) as "a minimum free form; a free form which does not
consist entirely of two or more lesser free forms." The phrase
minimum free form" is to be understood as a minimally free
form, that is, a free form which does not consist entirely of other
free forms. The word "entirely" must be emphasized. If free
forms are compounded, then something else enters into the
composition of the! compound to form the word.
In tagmemic analysis, word level construction is defined as a
construction which consists, potentially, of two or more word level
tagmemes, filled by morphemes. Word level includes the layers
of (1) inflection, (2) derivation, and (3) compounding, and parallels
the phrase level in which there are layers of relation coordina-
tion, and subordination.
Elson and Pickett distinguish between word level construc-
tions, which consists of a stem (or root) and an (inflectional) affix
(1962:76), and stem; level constructions, composed of two or more
tagmemes which form the stem and fill the nuclear slot in words
(1962:79). The disadvantage of separating a word and stem level
is that the parallelism with the phrase level is destroyed, and
117
118 INTRODUCTION TO TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS
2. The word may have more than one inflection. If so, one
inflectional affix will appear in final position, and an-
other inflectional affix in nonfinal position. Therefore
it is not true to say that inflectional affixes occur only
in word final position.
3. Considering word formation as a whole the set of in-
flectional affixes tends to be outer and the set of deriva-
tional affixes tends to be inner. In one analysis of Latin
(O'Brien, 1965:29), the Latin word is characterized by
a stem, followed by three derivational affixes and three
inflectional affixes, in that order. The three inflectional
affixes are outer and the three derivational affixes are
inside the inflections.
It is generally true for English that no derivational affix ever
occurs outside of an inflectional affix. Forms like spoonfuls,
noun+derivation+inflection, are preferred over spoonsful,
noun+inflection+derivation. Forms like betterment are regular
verb+derivation formations, not adjective-(-inflection+deri-
vation.
Derivation Defined
Derivational affixes are affixes which are not inflections. If
the inflections of language are denned as affixes which do not
change the form class of the word, but fit the word for use in syn-
tax in the sense explained above, then derivational affixes are
affixes which can change the form class and merely establish
words as members of the various form classes.
Compounding Denned
Compound stems are stems consisting of more than one root.
Roots are monomorphic forms, which carry the basic meaning of
words. Monomorphic stems are roots; polymorphic stems may
consist of more than one root. Stems of more than one root are
reduced to single roots at the compounding or coordinate layer
of word formation.
Compound Recognition. When compounds are discovered
composed of two roots, it is not difficult to analyze them as having
two parts, or two roots. What is difficult is to recognize the dis-
tinctive features of compounds so that compounds are distin-
guished from phrases. Some features are:
1. Phonological Features. This includes such features as the
patterns for consonants and vowels in the phonology of
the language, and suprasegmental features like stress.
In English, words are characterized by a single primary
stress, so that compounds are often recognized by stress
pattern, and lack of juncture. For example: black bird
has primary stress on each word, and a juncture. Black-
bird, the compound, has one primary stress and no
juncture.
2. Syntactic Features. Compounds are distinguished from
phrases, in that they have asyntactic features, which
are contrary to phrase patterns:
a. Word Order. In compounds, unusual orders may be
found and usual orders are not regular even for a
particular compounded pattern. For example: sea-
sick noun followed by adjective, is not a usual
phrase pattern. Verb + particle, as in splash-down, is
regular for verbs, not for nouns.
b. Interruptibility. The parts of a compound are not
interrupted, but form a rigid noninterruptible pat-
tern; the form is inseparable. For example: dare-
devil cannot be used as dare-the-devil which is a
phrase.
c. Modification. Elements of the compound cannot be
separately expanded with modifiers, although the
whole compound may be modified. For example:
sea-sick may not occur as deep seasick, with deep
modifying sea.
WORD LEVEL 133
cuts, in order to determine the pattern for the compound and its
underlying forms.
1. Forma in -ER, used as second root, fill the same function
in the compound as simple forms. The construction is
Noun + Noun (-ER). For example: house-keeper and
line-backer are simple noun + noun compounds. The
second noun is a derived form with verb root + deriva-
tion.
2. Forms in -ED used as second root, are generally phrase
derivatives with the -ED governing the whole phrase.
(Noun + Noun) + -ED. Thus, for example, red-haired in
which red modifies hair, and the whole phrase fills
the core slot in a derivational formula, governed by -ED.
is always the same as the second root, except for the verb + adverb
construction, which is listed here as a compound noun with
single primary stress. The chart is based on form alone in a
manner analogous to the charting of consonant clusters in pho-
nology. Ones form classes have been sorted, they may be further
differentiated according to functional norms.
Compound + Noun -t-Verb + Adj. + Adv.
1. Noun N+N N+V N+Aj N + Av
text-book baby-sit home-sick No case
2. Verb •V+ N v +v V + Aj V + Av
turn-key No case = Noun
3. Adjective Aj+N dive-bomb
Aj+V Aj+Aj Aj +Av
blue-bird white-wash blue-green No case
4. Adverb Av + N Av + V Av + Aj Av +Av
back-talk out-shine off-white through-out
SUPPLEMENTARY READINGS S
Bloomfield, Leonard, Language, New York, Holt, Rinehart and Winston,
Inc., 193S. Definition of lexicon, 162, and word, 178. Chap. 13, "Mor-
phology," 207-226; Chap. 14, "Morphologic Types," 227-246.
Elson, Benjamin, and Pickett, Velma, An Introduction to Morphology
and Syntax, Santa Ana, Summer Institute of Linguistics, 1962.
Chaps. 9 and 1Q, "Tagmemes and Constructions at Word (and Stem)
Level," 75-81; Chaps. 14 and 15, "Survey of Stem Formation, and
Word Types," 05-102. Word level includes inflection, stem level,
derivation, and so on.
Fries, Charles C, The Structure of English, New York, Harcourt, Brace
and World, Inci, 1952. Chap. 7, "Parts of Speech, Formal Charac-
teristics,'' 110-141, including lists of derivational affixes.
Hockett, Charles F., A Course in Modern Linguistics, New York, Mac-
millari Company, 1958. Chap. 24, "Inflection," 209-213, and Chap.
28, "Derivations," 240-245, restrictive and governing types.
Lees, Robert B., The Gramrnar of English Nominalizations, The Hague,
Mouton & Co., reprinted 1960. For the deep structure under-
lying nominal constructions in English.
140 INTRODUCTION TO TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS
1. Sentence Level
2. Clause Level
3. Phrase Level
4. Word Level
5. Morpheme Level
143
144 INTRODUCTION TO TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS
GRAMMATICAL PATTERNS
Tagmemic analysis) begins with the sentence and analyzes all of
the utterances of the data as far as the ultimate morpheme
level, The formal statement of the resultant grammar estab-
lishes a set of well-defined relationships between grammatical
functions, between filler classes, and between the morphemes
themselves.
Functional Slots Related
g grammars consist of a set of formulas at the five
levels of the grammar: sentence, clause, phrase, word, and mor-
pheme. The units of (this system are tagmeme units, correlations
of functional slot and filler class. These units are strung into
construction patterns, and the patterns occur at definite levels
in a grammatical system.
Functional Slot. Traditional grammar has always dealt with
grammatical functions. From the earliest grammars, we find
terms I such &a subject, predicate, modifier, and object. Chomsky
(Aspects, 1965:60) recognizes these functional terms and calls
them grammatical relationships. In Chomsky's view, these
"grammatical relationships" need not be explicitly stated in the
146 INTRODUCTION TO TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS
Type 1: 2 X 2 = 4
Type 2: 2 X 2 X 2 = 8
Type 3: 2 X 2 X 2 x 2 = 16
Type 4: 2 X 2 X 2 = 8
Type 5: 2 X 2 X 2 x 2 = 16
Type 6: 2 X 2 X 2 x 2 x 2 = 32
Type 7: 2 X 2 X 2 = 8
Type 8: 2 X 2 X 2 x 2 = 16
Type 9: 2 X 2 X 2 x 2 x 2 = 32
Type 10: 2 X 2 X 2 x 2 = 16
Type 11: 2 X 2 X 2 x 2 x 2 = 32
Type 12: 2 X 2 X 2 x 2 x 2 x 2 = 64
Total number of sentences generated by all patterns together
would be equal to 252, which coincides with the MGP of the tag-
memic grammar.
Sentence Generators
After the underlying tree markers have been established
and the structure reduced to a set of preterminal strings, show-
ing the same structure in formula form the concrete sentences
of the language can be generated by arranging the elements of
sentences in a matrix form. These matrices, arrays of rows and
columns are called sentence generators.
n=l 1 1 C = 21 = 2 = 1+1
n=2 1 2 1 C = 22 = 4 = 1+2+1
n=3 1 3 3 1 C = 23 = 8 = 1+3+3+1
n=4 1 4 6 4 1 C = 24 = 16 = 1+4+6+4+1
If for example, the number of combinations is 16 the kinds of
combinations are broken down into 1+4+6+4+1, that is, 1 C with
five elements KABCD, 4 C with four elements, 6 C with three
elements, 4 C with two elements, and 1 combination with one
element K. These 16 combinations are: KABCD; KABC, KABD,
KACD, KBCD; KAB, KAC, KAD, KBC, KBD, KCD; KA, KB, KC
KD and the minimum string K.
Solution:
Let m = the total number of elements in the string
Then
P = m!
P==4 x 3 X2 x 1
P = 24, Answer
Check: Write the 24 permutations of the given formula.
KABC KACB, KBAC, KBCA, KCAB KCBA
AKBC AKCB, ABKC, ABCK, ACKB ACBK
BKAC, BKCA, BAKC, BACK, BCKA, BCAK
CKAB CKBA, CAKB, CABK, CBKA, CBAK
In tagmemics, the Law of Combinations is first applied to all
formulas which have optional elements (conflated or condensed
formulas) to reduce these to many simple formulas with only
obligatory elements. Second, the law of permutations is applied
to any simple formula which has elements that are freely mova-
ble. Note that in applying the Law of Combinations n = the
number of optional elements whereas in the Law of Permuta-
tions m = the total number of elements present. This total
includes the constant element K as well as A, B, C and so on.
This Law of Permutation may be found in the already cited
College Algebra (Schaum's Outline Series, Murray R. Spiegel
1956, p. 229), where the formula represents a special case of
permutations—of m elements, taken m at a time, for example:
5 elements, taken 5 at a time.
VIETNAMESE
1. cho sem cum to
'The dog sees the big bird.'
2. cho to xawng sem cho nyo
'The big dog does not see the little dog.'
3. cho nyo thay chim nyo
'The little dog perceives the little bird.'
4. chim ku?ng sem cho
'The bird also sees the dog.
5. chim ku?ng thay
'The bird also perceives.
6. chim xawng thay
'The bird does not perceive.
7. cho thay chim
'The dog perceives the bird.'
8. cho sem
'The dog sees.
MORPHEME LEVEL 165
GRAMMAR LEXICON
Sent = +Base:tCl -Into:ICF chim n. 'bird'
252 252 x 1 cho n. 'dog'
tCl=+S:N+P:tV±O:N nyo aj. 'little'
252 6 x 6 x (6 4-1) to aj. 'big'
N = +H:n ±Mod:»j ku?ng av. 'also'
6 2 x(2 + l) xawng av. 'not'
tV = ±Mod:av +H:tv sem tv. 'see
6 (2+1) x 2 thay tv. 'perce
STRUCTURAL DESCRIPTION
Sent
PRETERMINAL STRINGS
SENTENCE GENERATOR
I J K L M N
1. chim nyo ku?ng sem chim nyo
2. cho to xawng thay cho to
SUPPLEMENTARY READINGS 6
Chomsky, Noam, Syntactic Structures. The Hague, Mouton & Co.,
1957. Grammar denned as generative, 13; all and only requirement,
18; phrase structure rules, derivations, and structural trees, 26-
27.
, Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. The M.I.T. Press, 1965. Struc-
tural descriptions assigned, 9; branching vs. lexical rule, 68; ter-
minal vs. preterminal string, 84; grammatical relations vs. gram-
matical category, 68. Extra-node tree, 69, not acceptable.
Cook, Walter A., S. J., On Tagmemes and Transforms. Washington, D.C.,
Georgetown University Press, 1964. Rules for generation, 53-56
developed from prepublication edition of Longacre, 1964 (infra).
, "The Generative Power of a Tagmemic Grammar," Monograph
Series on Languages and Linguistics, No. 20, 27-41. Washington
D.C., Georgetown University Press, 1967. Methods for generating
sentences and assigning structural descriptions.
Katz, Jerrold J., and Postal, Paul, An Integrated Theory of Linguistic
Description. The M.I.T. Press, 1964. For semantic component re-
strictions on concurrence based on semantic features.
Koutsoudas, Andre, Writing Transformational Grammars. New York,
McGraw-Hill, Inc., 1966. For notion of the conflated rule 9 flf. and
the construction of P-markers and PS rules, 14 ff.
Longacre, Robert E., Grammar Discovery Procedures. The Hague,
Mouton & Co., 1964. "Introduction: Symbols and Rewrite Opera-
tions," 24-34, in terms of readings, permutations, and manifesta-
tions.
"Some Fundamental Insights of Tagmemics." Language, 41:
65-76 (1965). Generative power of tagmemic grammar, 71 ff.
, "Reply to Postal's Review of Grammar Procedures," UAL,
33:323-328 (1967), with sample tagmemic trees, 325.
Merrifield, William R., "On the Form of Rules in a Generative Gram-
mar," Monograph Series on Languages and Linguistics, No. 20,
43-55. Washington, D.C., Georgetown University Press, 1967.
Postal Paul M., "Constituent Structure: A Study of Contemporary
Models of Syntactic Description," UAL, vol. 39. no. 1, Part III,
33-51 (1964).
, "Review of Robert E. Longacre's Grammar Discovery Pro-
cedures," UAL, 32: 93-98 (1966). For objections to the tagmemic
system.
TABLE 7: LANGUAGE DESCRIPTION
I 1 2 3
PHONOLOGICAL ALLOPHONES PHONEME PHONEMIC SYSTEM
COMPONENT Set of etic sounds Emic units of sound Paradigmatic
(Phonology) Phoneme variants Differential function Matrix of Phonemes
II 4 5 6
LEXICAL ALLOMORPHS MORPHEME MORPHEMIC SYSTEM
COMPONENT Set of etic forms Emic units of form Paradigmatic
(Lexicon) Morpheme variants Referential function Matrix of Morphemes
III 7 8 9
SYNTACTIC ALLOTAGMAS TAGMEME TAGMEMIC SYSTEM
COMPONENT Set of etic patterns Emic pattern units Paradigmatic
(Grammar) Tagmeme variants Syntactic function Matrix of Tagmemes
7 LANGUAGE DESCRIPTION
/ i / J + I /n/
In this representation, the overlapping characteristics of the
sounds are ignored, /p/ is a voiceless bilabial stop. The feature
LANGUAGE DESCRIPTION 175
though within any one feature there may be variation. Each tag-
meme will contrast with others in the system, according to these
contrastive features.
The functional meaning of the tagmeme is vaguely denned
and this is an advantage in initial analysis. Subject is an intui-
tive notion; but it is opposed to predicate, object, location and
so on, which occur in the same construction. Subjects which occur
in different constructions are only the same by analogy: active
subjects are to active predicates as passive subjects are to pas-
sive predicates. The relation is the same; the meanings of the
various subjects need not be univocal.
Tagmemes as Particles. The feature mode of the slot:class
correlative establishes the tagmeme as a particle. Tagmemes
are "structurally significant points within a given pattern"
(Longacre, 1964:17). In this static view, there is no overlapping,
but a clear-cut abstracted view of definite points in the construc-
tion pattern filled by definite filler classes. Further, the patterns
occur at levels, so that we may speak of clause level tagmemes
or phrase level tagmemes.
The points within the construction pattern are grammati-
cally significant, in that they assign grammatical meaning to
fillers of the slot. The grammatical meaning is determined by
subtracting the lexical meaning of occurring items from the total
linguistic meaning. This meaning is associated with the slot
not with the particular lexical item. By this method we isolate
such meanings as subject, predicate, object, and so on at the
clause level, or head and modifier at the phrase level.
There is correlativity between the slot meaning isolated and
the meanings of the other slots in the same string. Point and
pattern are correlatives; there is no construction without con-
stituent tagmemes, and no tagmemes independent of the con-
struction in which they occur. The "subject" is subject because it
is opposed to the predicates, objects, and adverbial adjuncts
within the same string.
SUPPLEMENTARY READINGS 7
Cook, Walter A., S;J., On Tagmemea and Transforms. Washington, D.C.,
Georgetown University Press, 1964. For grammatical hierarchy,
10-12; particle, wave, and field, 12-14; and discussion, 27-39.
194 INTRODUCTION TO TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS
Conklin, Harold C, "Lexicographical Treatment of Folk Taxonomies."
UAL, 28, Part IV (1962). Problems in Lexicography, 119-141.
Methods for establishing: distribution mode for lexical units.
Longacre, Robert E., Grammar Discovery Procedures. The Hague,
Mouton & Co., 1964. "Introduction: The Notion of Grammar," for
the triple hierarchy of "semiautonomous and interlocking" sys-
tems.
, "Prolegomena to Lexical Structure." Linguistics, An Interna-
tional Review, 5:5-24. The Hague, Mouton & Co., 1964. For a differ-
ent viewpoint on field structure, with a matrix showing phonology,
lexicon, and grammar, opposed to particle, string, field.
Pike, Kenneth L., Language in Relation to a Unified Theory of the Struc-
ture of Human Behaviour: The Hague, Mouton & Co., 1967. Lan-
guage unit, with feature, manifestation, distribution modes.
Chap. 6, Morpheme 6.4 (154) 6.5 (163) 6.6 (169)
Chap. 7, Tagmeme 7.3 (196) 7.4 (228) 7.5 (236)
Chap. 8, Phoneme 8.3(293) 8.4(306) 8.5(318)
."Language as Particle, Wave, and Field," The Texas Quarterly,
2:37-54 (1959). Original presentation of particle, wave, field theory
showing that the three views may be reconciled in single units.
, "Grammar as Wave." Monograph Series on Languages and
Linguistics, No. 20, 1-14. Washington, D.C., Georgetown Uni-
versity Press, 1967. Applied to Kasem language of Ghana.
, "Non-linear Order and Anti-Redundancy in German Morpho-
logical Matrices." Journal of Dialectology, 32 (1965), Wiesbaden.
For application of matrix theory in the lexical component.
, "Tagmemic and Matrix Linguistics Applied to Selected African
Languages." HEW Contract No. OE-5-U-065 Report (1966).
Sapir, Edward, Language: An Introduction to the Study of Speech. New
York, Harcourt, Brace and World, Inc., 1921. Chap. 2, "The Ele-
ments of Speech (Lexicon)," and Chap. 3, "The Sounds of Language
(Phonology)."
SUPPLEMENTARY EXERCISES
195
196 INTRODUCTION TO TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS
197
188 INTRODUCTION TO TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS
200
SELECTED BIBLIOGRAPHY 201
Cook, Walter A,,: On Tagmemes and Transforms. Washington, D.C.,
Georgetown; University Press; 1964.
, A Deseiiptive Analysis of Mundari. Ph.D. Dissertation,George-
town University, 1965.
, ""Hie Generative Power of a Tagmemic Grammar." Monograph
Series on Languages and Linguistics, No. 20, 27-41. Washington,
D.C., Georgetown University Press, 1967.
DeSaussure, Ferdinand, Course in General Linguistics, trans, by Wade
Baskin. Geneva, 1916. New York, Philosophical Library, Inc., 1959.
Dinneen, Francis; P., S.J., An Introduction to General Linguistics. New
York, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1967.
Elson, Benjamin, and Pickett, Velma, An Introduction to Morphology
and Syntax, Santa Ana, Summer Institute of Linguistics, 1964.
Frllmore, Charles J., "The Case for Case." Universal in Linguistic
Theory, eds.| Emmon Bach and Robert Harms. New York, Holt,
Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1968.
Fodor, Jerry A., and Katz, Jerrold J., The Structure of Language: Read-
ings in the Philosophy of Language. Englewood Cliffs, N.J., Pren-
tice-Hall, Inc., 1964.
Fries, Charles C.,The Structure of English. New York, Harcourt, Brace
and World, Inc., 1952. \
, Linguistics and Reading. New York, Holt, Rinehart and Win-
ston, Inc., 1962.
Gleason, Henry A., Jr., An Introduction to Descriptive Linguistics.
New York, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1955; revised, 1961.
, Linguistics and English Grammar. New York, Holt, Rinehart
and Winston, Inc., 1962,
, "Contrastive Analysis in Discourse Structure." Monograph
Series on Languages and Linguistics, No. 21, 39-63. Washington,
B.C., Georgetown University Press, 1968.
Greenfeerg, Joseph H., Universals of Language. Cambridge, Mass., The
M.I.T^Press> 1963.
Halliday, M. A. K|., "Categories of the Theory of Grammar." Word, vol.
17(1961).
, Mclntosh, Angus, and Strevens, Peter, The Linguistic Sciences
and Language Teaching. London, Longmans Green, Ltd., 1964.
, "Syntax iand the Consumer." Monograph Series on Languages
and Linguistics, No. 17,11-14. Washington, D.C., Georgetown Uni-
versity Presfe, 1964.
Harris, Zellig, String Analysis of Sentence Structure. The Hague,
Mouton & Co., 1962.
Hjelmslev, Louis, Prolegomena to a Theory of Language, trans, by
Francis J. Whitfield. Madison, Wise, The University of Wisconsin
Press, 1963.
Hockett, Charles F., "Problems of Morphemic Analysis." Language,
23:321-343 (1947).
202 INTRODUCTION TO TAGMEMIC ANALYSIS
207
208 INDEX