Teaching First-Year Civil Procedure and Other Introductory Course
Teaching First-Year Civil Procedure and Other Introductory Course
Teaching First-Year Civil Procedure and Other Introductory Course
12-2000
Recommended Citation
Teaching First-year Civil Procedure and Other Introductory Courses by the Problem Method, 34 Creighton L. Rev. 245 (2001)
This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Faculty Scholarship at ScholarWorks@University of Baltimore School of Law. It has been
accepted for inclusion in All Faculty Scholarship by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@University of Baltimore School of Law. For more
information, please contact [email protected].
245
PROBLEM METHOD
STl~PIl[EN J. SHAPIRot
I. INTRODUCTION
I have been teaching the first-year course in Civil Procedure for
twenty years, first for five years at Ohio Northern University, and for
the last fifteen years at the University of Baltimore, where I also teach
a required second-year course in Evidence. When I first started teach
ing Civil Procedure, I used a fairly typical case method. 1 I was never
very happy with this approach for teaching a course in which one of
my major goals was getting the students to learn to read, interpret
and apply the Federal Rules of Civil Procedure ("Federal Rules").
Gradually, I began to develop sets of my own problems which I used to
teach some of the classes. Eventually, I developed enough problems,
so that I could teach the entire first semester of the two-semester
course by the problem method. 2 Within the last several years I have
developed enough additional problems to teach both semesters of Civil
Procedure and also the Evidence course entirely by the problem
method.
There has been an ongoing debate within legal education as to the
relative merits of various teaching methods, especially the case
method and the problem method. 3 Yet even some supporters of the
problem method believe that it is more suited to smaller, advanced
dissect, defend and/or criticize the court's opinion. When the discus
sion of the first case is finished, the professor moves on to the second
case, usually involving the same or related subject matter, sometimes
by the same court and sometimes not. The professor then proceeds to
have the class discuss the second case, much like the first, sometimes
with the additional task of trying to rationalize any difference of re
sults between the two cases.
The benefits of this approach are said to be that it teaches stu
dents to read and think carefully, logically and critically-Le., to
"think like a lawyer." It requires students to learn actively (compared
to the textbookllecture format which preceded it). In class, this means
the students learn to think on their feet, and make and defend an ar
gument. The case method also supposedly teaches students to learn to
recognize the important facts and issues in a case and to separate
these issues from red herrings and makeweight arguments. It also
requires students to individually glean the substantive law in a partic
ular field from the cases, rather than spoon feeding the law to stu
dents through lecture or text. It also requires the students to
recognize that the law is a growing, changing body of doctrine. s
The case method, and the extent to which law faculty have come
to rely on it, has also been subject to criticism. Critics, while admit
ting that the case method might do a good job of teaching students to
understand and work with appellate opinions,1O have noted that this
skill forms only a small part of what lawyers actually do. Most law
yers do not get involved with a case at the appellate level, but rather
most become involved at the beginning of the case. The client brings a
problem to the lawyer, and the lawyer's job is to determine the rele
vant facts, and find and apply the appropriate law in order to either
advise the client or help solve the client's problem. l l
Students who have been taught by the case method usually get
some exposure to problem solving, but often not until they take their
exams at the end of the semester. These exams typically involve a set
of hypothetical facts constituting a legal problem, and one or more
questions testing the student's ability to recognize the legal issues in
volved in the problem and requiring the students to discuss how the
law (or a lawyer or judge) would handle these issues. The divergence
between how students are taught and tested has lead to further criti
cism that the case method is not only ignoring the skills that lawyers
need in practice, but also the skills that students need to succeed in
law school. 12 The case method has also been criticized because it puts
too much emphasis on cases as the source of substantive law, when
more and more law is governed by statutes, rules and regulations. 13
One proposed solution has been to turn, in whole or in part, to the
problem method. 14 In the problem method, the students are given a
set of facts, similar to a real life legal dispute (or a law school exam).
Although students might still read (among other sources) some appel
late cases to learn the law to be applied, the problems, rather than the
cases, become the focus of the class discussion. 15
The basic characteristic of the problem method ... lies in the task it poses for
the student. The committee sees the method as requiring the student, in pre
paring for class, to focus his study on a problem or problems posed in advance of
the class. His task is to wrestle with each problem, drawing on whatever mate
rial may have been assigned to be studied in connection with it.... It also calls
for the direction of class discussion to some or all of the assigned problems and
to the student's solutions to these.
1966 Annual Meeting, 1966 AsS'N AM. L. SCH. at 202-04. To the committee and to some
writers, other variations, such as the complexity of the problems, whether a written
answer must be prepared, and the type ofin-class discussion are secondary to the main
requirements that there be (1) a fact-based problem, (2) assigned to be prepared out of
class, and (3) it be a main focus of class discussion. Ogden, 34 J. LEGAL EDUC. at 655.
Some writers exclude problems based on simple, skeletal facts from their definition and
require "several issues cutting across several cases and statutes. It is meant to resem
ble a complex situation that a lawyer might face in practice." Moskovitz, 42 J. LEGAL
EDUC. at 250. See also, Hawkins-Leon, 1998 BYU Enuc. & L. J. at 8-9.
16. A 1966 survey by the AALS found relatively little use of the problem method in
the first year and the greatest use in third and fourth year courses, with second-year
courses falling in the middle. Its use in second-year courses was more likely to be on a
limited scale, for special purposes, rather than for a majority of class time. 1966 Annual
Meeting, 1966 AsS'N AM. L. SCH. at 221. More recently, some writers have advocated
extending the problem method into first-year courses. See Charles Calleros, Variations
on the Problem Method in First-Year and Upper-Division Classes, 20 U. SAN FRAN. L.
REV. 455 (1986); Moskovitz, 42 J. LEGAL EDUC. at 261-63.
17. Ogden states that "mastery of case analysis is a prerequisite to use of problems
which require case materials to find solutions." Ogden, 34 J. LEGAL Enuc. at 655; see
also Moskovitz, 42 J. LEGAL EDUC. at 262, quoting from a letter from Professor Nathan
Crystal, "lawyers face problems with a conceptual and linguistic framework developed
from law school and practical experience. First year students don't have this frame
work. Thus, I think it is sounder pedagogically to develop the framework first and then
proceed to the problems." ld. at 262.
18. 1966 Annual Meeting, AsS'N AM. L. SCH. at 210-11; Ogden, 34 J. LEGAL EDUC.
at 664.
19. The intensity ofthe problem method and the development of skills that can
result from its use require small classes for the sake of both teacher and stu
dent .... When the problem method is used in courses that are much larger
than 40 students, the experience of developing problem-solving skills is a vica
rious one for most students. It is also less workable to require all students to
250 CREIGHTON LAW REVIEW [Vol. 34
There has also been a wider choice of published materials using the
problem approach for advanced courses. 20 Another contributing fac
tor is that first year students do not have the basic knowledge of sev
eral areas of the law, which is very helpful in working out complex
problems that cut across several areas and issues. There may also be
a feeling (not necessarily correct) among those accustomed to teaching
by the case method, that the problem method is less efficient than the
case method for teaching legal doctrine. 21 Many teachers of first-year
subjects feel a tension between trying to acquaint the students with a
vast body of substantive law and teaching the students skills, such as
legal reasoning and problem solving. These faculty members are less
likely to use the problem method if they view it as more time
consuming.
Many law professors who use the case method also employ a tech
nique somewhat akin to the problem method: the in-class hypotheti
cal. The in-class hypothetical is usually a very short, simplified
problem, presented to the students in class by the professor. It is usu
ally devised by the professor, either in advance or on the spur of the
moment, but presented to the students in class rather than before
class. The in-class hypothetical is generally designed either to illus
trate a specific point raised during the class or to show how the results
might differ if the facts of the particular case under discussion were
slightly different.
The in-class hypothetical does give the students at least some of
the benefits of the problem method. Students are required to take the
legal doctrine learned from the case law and apply it to a different set
of facts. There are, however, some limitations. In-class hypotheticals,
both by necessity and design, are usually based on very simplified
facts and focused on one narrow issue. 22 The hypotheticals do not,
therefore, provide the students practice with analyzing the more com
turn in written answers to the problems if the teacher has to read 50 to 150
answers for each problem session.
Ogden, 34 J. LEGAL Enuc. at 664-65: See also 1966 Annual Meeting, 1966 AsS'N AM. L.
SCH. at 213-14.
20. A 1984 survey of West and Foundation Press publications found thirty-four
sets of problem materials for fifteen courses normally considered part of the upper-level
curriculum and only seven for five first-year courses (one each in criminal law, property,
and torts, and two each in civil procedure and contracts. Ogden, 34 J. LEGAL EDuc. at
670-73.
21. The 1966 AALS study found that although course coverage was viewed as a
concern by many of those who did not use the problem method and by some who did, a
majority of the faculty who actually used the method found it superior in this regard.
1966 Annual Meeting, 1966 AsS'N AM. L. SCH. at 211-12.
22. "[AJ hypo is not a problem. A hypo usually raises only one or two issues. A
problem raises several issues, which must be organized before each can be separately
analyzed." Moskovitz, 42 J. LEGAL Enuc. at 246.
2000] PROBLEM METHOD 251
23. A 1942 AALS committee stated that "the practice obtained by the students
with "hypos" is necessarily grounded in shallow consideration for want of opportunity to
wrestle with the problem before class ...." Handbook, 1942 AsS'N A'I1. L. SOH. at 88.
24. Most Civil Procedure texts use the Federal Rules of Civil Procedure as the
erning law for the purposes of study. Not only are these rules applicable in all
District Courts no matter where the students might end up practicing, but most states
have now adopted rules of civil procedure very similar to the Federal Rules. If state
procedure varies from federal practice, the former is often taught in a separate
upper-level course.
25. Most Civil Procedure texts and the standard Civil Procedure curriculum cover
the following topics: Conduct oflitigation (pleadings, discovery, resolution without trial,
trial, post trial motions and appeals); Jurisdiction (both subject matter and personal);
Choice of Law in Federal Courts (known as the Erie problem) and issues of Former
Adjudication (often called res judicata). This is a pretty ambitious undertaking, partic
ularly for those teaching a one-semester course, and not all faculty teach all subject
areas fully. I have always had the luxury of teaching a two-semester course and have
been able to cover all of these topics pretty thoroughly.
26. For many first-year students, Civil Procedure is the only course that provides
significant exposure to statutory or rule-based law. Torts, criminal law and property
252 CREIGHTON LAW REVIEW [Vol. 34
are mostly common-law courses. Contracts can be taught either as a common law
course, or with significant reliance on Article II of the Uniform Commercial Code.
27: 1966 Annual Meeting, 1966 AsS'N AM. L. SCH. at 209.
28. Students oftenview «a case as the solution to a past problem or for its contribu
tion to a body of doctrine." 1966 Annual Meeting, 1966 AsS'N AM. L. SCH. at 203.
29. Most decisions interpreting the Federal Rules of Civil Procedure do not make it
to the United States Supreme Court. In other areas of Civil Procedure, especially Juris
diction and the Erie Problem the cases in the textbooks are mainly Supreme Court
cases. In the sections involving conduct of litigation, more of the cases are lower court
opinions.
30. "[T]he Socratic Method was accepted without attention to the fact that it re
quires active learning by law students who will have practiced passive learning tech
niques for the majority oftheir educational lives." Hawkins-Leon, 1998 BYU EDUC. & L.
J. at 7. See generally, Michael Richmond, Teaching Law to Passive Learners: The Con
temporary Dilemma of Legal Education, 26 CUMB. L. REV. 943 (1995-96). While the
problem method also requires active learning, it provides the student a task to help in
preparation for class.
2000] PROBLEM METHOD 253
Mter a few years, I found myself using more and more in-class
hypotheticals and putting less emphasis on the cases, at least in the
first semester. Gradually, I weeded out the hypotheticals that did not
work, or tinkered with the hypotheticals until they worked better.
Eventually, the hypotheticals evolved into sets of problems, and I had
enough problem sets that I could teach all or most of certain classes
with the problems. At that point, I began handing out sets of
problems in advance for certain classes, asking the students to pre
pare answers to the problems before arriving to class. I found that the
classes in which I used the problems seemed to be more interesting
and lively than the ones using the case method. Additionally, these
classes provided more and better student responses. I therefore began
a several year process of developing enough problem sets to teach al
most the entire first semester by this method. I have been using this
method ever since, and have expanded it to include second semester
Civil Procedure and my Evidence course. In the next section of this
article, I will describe, in more detail, the kind of problems I write and
how I use the problems to teach my classes.
syllabus and during the first class that I do not normally collect and
review the written answers, and never grade the problems. I do, how
ever, inform the students that I reserve the right to collect and review
their answers for sufficiency if I feel they are not making a serious
effort at completing the problems. I have never felt the need to under
take such a review.
In class, I proceed through the problem assigned for that day, call
ing on students at random and asking the students to give me their
answer for each problem. Many of the problems are designed so that
the average student will not usually get each question completely
right. Much of the class is spent in a modified Socratic dialogue with
the selected student and others as to whether, and how, the answer
could be revised to be more correct, more complete, or more
sophisticated.
Each section of each problem is usually designed to illustrate only
one or perhaps two points. The questions normally start out fairly
easy, with a definite right answer. I use these introductory questions
to teach the basics and to start a simple policy discussion of how that
specific rule is designed to work. Later questions, although remaining
simple in form, become more difficult. Each problem set often ends
with a problem that does not have one correct answer. It contains an
issue (either of the meaning or the specific application of a rule) that
can reasonably be argued in more than one way.
A good example of this format is the problem set on "Counter
claims," which comes fairly early in the first semester.37 The first
problem describes a case in which the defendant has a possible coun
terclaim which is completely unrelated to the plaintiff's claim and is,
therefore, a permissive counterclaim. 38 The second problem describes
another case with a clearly related, and therefore compulsory, coun
terclaim. The questions not only ask the students whether the defen
dant mayor must include the counterclaims, but also ask follow-up
questions designed to get students to think about the policies behind
the rule and the ramifications of the rule for both the legal system and
practicing attorneys.
leged).44 Third, I want to give students a feel for the process of discov
ery (how it proceeds through time and the roles of attorneys and
judges).
The first question asks students if and when they would be able to
obtain, from the corporate Defendant (1) all eyewitnesses known to
the Defendant, (2) which eyewitness the Defendant intends to call at
trial, and (3) what expert witnesses the Defendant intends to call. 45
As it turns out, Plaintiff is entitled to all of this information, but each
piece of the information is discoverable under a different section of the
Federal Rules and at a different time in the process. 46 This requires
the students to read the Federal Rules, especially Rule 26, very care
fully, including several cross-references to other rules. 47
By the time students get to class, most have correctly determined
which section of the Federal Rules makes each of the required pieces
of information discoverable. Usually, however, students have not de
termined exactly how the discovery rules work, or why the Federal
Rules were written and timed the way they are. For example, Rule
26(a) requires that the names of all eyewitnesses be disclosed within
ten days of the initial discovery conference. 48 When I ask exactly
when that conference would take place, the students (who usually
have not thought this through), must then find and apply the rule on
discovery conferences, which in turn refers students to the rule for
scheduling conferences, Rule 16.49 Since each piece of information
must be turned over at a different time in the process, I am able to
question the students as to why a certain order is prescribed. For ex
ample, why must the names of expert witnesses to be called at trial be
disclosed considerably earlier than the names of eyewitnesses who will
be called?
The second question in this introductory set asks how the plain
tiffs attorney would obtain a version of the accident from several
44. "Parties may obtain discovery regarding any matter, not privileged, which is
relevant to the subject matter involved in the pending action .... " FEn. R. Orv. P.
26(b)(1).
45. See app. B, problem 7, question 1.
46. The names of all known eyewitnesses must be turned over without request,
fairly early in the process, pursuant to Rule 26(a)(1)(A). FED. R. Orv. P. 26(a)(1)(A). The
names of which witnesses the party intends to call must be provided, but not until
thirty days before trial. FED. R. Orv. P. 26(a)(3)(A). The names of expert witnesses to be
called must be disclosed ninety days before trial. FED. R. Orv. P. 26(a)(2).
47. FED. R. Orv. P. 26.
48. "[T]hese disclosures shall be made at or within 10 days after the meeting ofthe
parties under subdivision (f)." FED. R. Orv. P. 26(a)(1).
49. Rule 26(0 requires the parties to meet and develop a discovery plan "as soon as
practicable and in any event at least 14 days before a scheduling conference is held or a
scheduling order is due under Rule 16(b)." FED. R. Orv. P. 26(0. Rule 16(b) requires
that the scheduling order be issued within ninety days after the appearance of the de
fendant. FED. R. Orv. P. 16(b).
258 CREIGHTON LAW REVIEW [Vol. 34
sources, including the police, an eyewitness, the Plaintiff and the De
fendant. 50 This question focuses more on the tactics of discovery than
on interpretation of the Federal Rules. I use it to encourage the stu
dents to think not only about whether a formal discovery request (i.e.,
deposition, interrogatory, request for documents) would be allowed,
but whether it might be better to use an informal request or interview
outside the rules of discovery. We also discuss which of the various
discovery methods (i.e., deposition versus interrogatories) would be
better in certain circumstances.
The final discovery questions in the first set introduce simple ex
amples of what is relevant and what is privileged. The questions also
illustrate the difference between relevance and admissibility. The
first problem set on discovery usually takes somewhat longer than one
class period and leads directly to the next set of questions which deals
with depositions.
The first part of the deposition problem set involves a more de
tailed look at the mechanics of taking a deposition and how mechanics
vary depending on the identity of the deponent. 51 The second part of
the deposition problem set focuses on Rule 32, which governs when a
deposition may be introduced at trial. The problem posits the deposi
tion of four different persons and asks whether each deposition would
be admissible.52 The problem set is designed to get the students to
make a careful and precise application of a difficult and technical rule,
Rule 32(a).53
The final question in the deposition problem set goes one step fur
ther by asking whether the corporate Defendant could have the co
Defendant driver's deposition admitted. 54 Since Rule 32 allows a
party's deposition to be admitted by "an adverse party,"55 the problem
requires students to grapple with the conceptually difficult issue of
whether, and in what situations, two co-Defendants can be considered
"adverse" parties. The final question is again designed to give stu
dents practice dealing with questions that have no clear right or
wrong answer.
The third problem set covers mental and physical examinations. 56
It explores how and why the procedures and standards for mental and
physical examinations differ from all the other discovery devices. This
also allows for a fuller discussion of privilege: how and under what law
a privilege can be created or waived.
The fourth and final problem set on discovery addresses what
kind of protection from discovery is provided for ''work-product,'' that
is documents prepared in anticipation oflitigation. 57 This last prob
lem, on work product, clearly illustrates the difference between the
case method and the problem method. The main case on work product
is Hickman v. Taylor,58 a United States Supreme Court opinion from
1947. Hickman determined just how much protection would be
granted to work product material, and in what situations. In 1970,
the Federal Rules were amended to codify Hickman. 59 When I taught
this material by the case method, I began with an extended discussion
of Hickman, and then concluded the class with a shorter discussion
and some hypotheticals designed to show how the law of this case was
codified into the Federal Rules, and how the rule applies in different
situations. Now, although I have the students read Hickman, the
problem (and our class discussion) asks the students to address how
certain requests for attorney work-product would be handled under
the Federal Rules. Although Hickman comes into play, it is mostly an
aid to help define the purpose and meaning of the Federal Rules when
there is some question regarding their application. This more closely
replicates how an attorney would go about solving a work-product is
sue in real life.
A. BENEFITS
For me, the greatest benefit of the problem method is that classes
are more lively and interesting. First, students are more willing and
able to participate. Students almost never ask to "pass" because they
are unprepared. For years I bemoaned the fact that although my first
year students answered my questions quite willingly, if not always en
thusiastically, trying to get responses from most of my Evidence stu
dents seemed like pulling teeth. Getting students to respond was so
difficult that in some years I abandoned my normal practice of calling
on students at random and began relying mainly on volunteers. I at
tributed this difference to the eagerness of first-year students and the
fact that by the time students reach second year, most students have
been beaten down by the law school system and are much less
enthusiastic.
61. Most of my explanations so far focus on how and why I use the problem method
for teaching Civil Procedure, rather than Evidence. Most, if not all of what I have said
also applies to the Evidence course. Although Evidence is a second, rather than first
year course, in most other respects it is more similar to a first-year than to most other
upper-class courses. It is a required course, not an elective; it is usually taught in large
class sections; it is the first (introductory) course in the area; and the substantive law is
mostly governed by a set of federal rules. The problems I have developed for teaching
Evidence and how I use the problems in class are very similar to those I use to teach
Civil Procedure. Unfortunately, for the two years covered by my study, due to a quirk in
scheduling, my Evidence classes were much smaller (twenty-six and twelve students
respectively) than the usual number of fifty-seventy.
2000] PROBLEM METHOD 261
the second time through. It also makes sense that students are more
willing to answer a question in class if they have had the opportunity
to test that answer with a small group of their colleagues first.
It is also likely that students learning by the problem method
spend more time preparing for class. 64 With the case method, stu
dents read the assigned cases, but then do not have any specific as
signment. Students may re-read the case or brief it, but at that point,
most students consider their preparation complete. With the problem
method, however, a student's main task still lies ahead after having
completed the assigned reading. The most significant results I found
in my study of the differences between my Evidence classes taught by
the two methods was in out-of-class preparation time. I asked both
classes to keep an anonymous daily record of the amount of time spent
preparing for class. Students in the class taught by the problem
method spent more than twice as long preparing for class, which pre
sumably helped the learning process and improved their in-class
performance.65
This brings us to the question of what the students think about
the problem method. Do students consider this extra work helpful, or
is it an unwanted burden? The latter is certainly possible, since in
most oftheir other substantive courses, students are asked to do little
or no outside work beyond than reading the assigned material. Year
after year, student response to the problem method has been over
whelmingly positive. On the official student evaluation forms, one
question asks the student to comment on the teaching materials. An
overwhelming majority of students comment on how helpful the
problems have been,66 often using superlatives that you do not typi
cally hear from law students, such as the problems were "fabulous" or
"wonderful," and that they "loved" the problems. 67 There are virtually
64. "All this [preparing for class under the problem method] takes more time and
effort than studying for class under the case method ...." Moskovitz, 42 J. LEGAL
EDuc. at 254.
65. Students in the class taught by the case method reported spending an average
of 103 minutes per week preparing for class, while students in the problem-method
class reported spending an average of 234 minutes per week. This difference was signif
icant to the .05 level. See tbl. 1.
66. In my Fall 1998 Civil Procedure class, of the fifty-four students who gave any
written comments at all, forty-five commented positively about the problems and only
one negatively. In my Fall 1999 Civil Procedure class, fifty-three of sixty-five students
commented positively about the problems and none negatively. In my Spring 1999 Evi
dence class, twelve of thirteen commented positively about the problems.
67. Student Civil Procedure Evaluations from 1997-1999 (anonymous). Some
other typical comments about the problems in the evaluations were: "very effective,"
"very helpful," "very useful," "great," "an excellent way to learn the material and be
able to test your knowledge by answering the questions," and "the problems really al
lowed me to be able to explore the material and learn the subject matter." My personal
favorite, however was "Problems rock!"
264 CREIGHTON LAW REVIEW [Vol. 34
At this point, I can sense that the reader might be saying: "O.K.,
so classes seem more interesting, students are working more and per
forming better in class, but are students learning the material and
skills any better than with the case method?" For better or worse, we
generally measure student performance in law school, especially in
large first-year courses, by their grades on a final examination. So the
question becomes whether teaching the same material by the problem
method produces higher exam scores than teaching the same material
by the case method. I certainly expected that it would. One of the
criticisms of the case method is that it does not provide students with
sufficient practice at the skills needed for law school exams or for the
practice oflaw. It is my intention when I write the problems to pro
vide students with as much practice as possible at the same tasks they
will be asked to perform on the final exam. My final exams are practi
cally identical in format and very similar in content to the problems
the students have worked on all semester. I do not hide this fact from
the students, but rather I make it clear to the students right from the
first day of class. While I change the facts just enough so that stu
dents cannot merely recognize the problem and spit back the same
answer as in class, my exam questions are often not much more than
the issues from two or three problems put together into one fact pat
tern. It seemed intuitively obvious, therefore, that the students
taught by the problem method would outperform students taught by
the case method. In fact, the students who had worked all semester
on problems so similar to the exam would seem to have had such an
advantage that any performance enhancement might not fairly be at
tributed to the superiority of the teaching method, but merely famili
arity with the exam material and format.
68. I have received only three negative comments in the last three years on the
Student Evaluations from 1997-1999. Each student commented that the problems were
"circular," "too difficult," and "vague." Student Evaluations from 1997-1999.
69. Some students complain that I sometimes change the facts of a problem while
we are working on it in class. Although I try not to do this too often, it often seems
necessary to illustrate a point or answer a student's question. When I switched from
the case method to the problem method, my overall performance rating for teaching
Evidence went up from 3.29 to 3.67 (on a 4.0 scale) This difference was not quite statis
tically significant, possibly due to the unusually low number of students in the class
these years. See tbl. 2.
2000] PROBLEM METHOD 265
70. The mean exam score (out of 100 points) was 54.9 for the case method-class and
57.5 for the problem-method class. This small difference was not statistically signifi
cant. See tbl. 3.
71. The no-feedback class had a mean exam score of 62.2, which was actually
higher that the feedback class mean of 58.9, but this small difference was not statisti
cally significant. See tbl. 4.
72. I compared the first-year law school grade point average for the students in the
two evidence classes, and found that the averages were virtually identical. For the first
year students, I compared their Law School Admission Test ("LSAT") scores and their
undergraduate grade point averages. Again, these were virtually identical for the two
classes. I also tried to control for any differences in the way I graded from one year to
the next by mixing in blindly and re-grading a number of the first year's exams while I
was grading the second year. I realize, of course, that proper experimental design
would have had students randomly assigned to the two classes in the same year. This
was just not practicable under the circumstances.
73. For an excellent article describing and explaining this phenomenon, see Teich,
36 J. LEGAL Enuc. at 168-69, who states:
Recent research results concerning law teaching have been consistent with re
sults in higher education generally. Both traditional and specially developed
experimental group law-teaching systems have recently been shown to function
equivalently in terms of teaching effectiveness in several studies were effective
ness has been evaluated by a method's impact on group wide achievement.
Tiech, 36 J. LEGAL Enuc. at 168-69. Teich also reported that a limited number of stud
ies have shown, however, that highly individualized instructional methods such as com
puter-aided instruction can have a positive effect on student test achievement. ld. at
184. This is consistent with the results of an earlier study I conducted showing that use
of CALI computer programs by students in my Evidence class increased their scores on
my exam. Stephen Shapiro, The Use and Effectiveness of Various Learning Materials in
an Evidence Class, 46 J. LEGAL Enuc. 101 (1996).
266 CREIGHTON LAW REVIEW [Vol. 34
74. John Blackburn & Edward Niedzwiedz, Do Teaching Methods Matter? A Field
Study of an Integrative Teaching Technique, 18 AM. Bus. L. J. 525 (1981).
75. Blackburn & Niedzwiedz, 18 AM. Bus. L. J. at 525.
2000] PROBLEM METHOD 267
pIe facts and one issue. 78 My problems do not help students learn the
important skills of sifting through the facts to separate the relevant
from the irrelevant in solving a more complex, multi-faceted problem.
This is certainly true. I think the kind of integrational skills taught
by more complex problems are important ones for law students. I
agree with those who advocate the use of such problems in advanced
courses. I do not, however, think that a first-year Civil Procedure
course, where students still have not learned more basic skills of legal
reasoning and statutory construction, is a good place to teach these
more complex skills. It is possible that, as a matter of personal prefer
ence, my problems are simpler and more compartmentalized than
they need to be. I can imagine someone else using somewhat more
complex problems to accomplish many of the same goals. I do try to
include at least of few of these kinds of problems, especially at the
conclusion of some units, like the Erie doctrine and personal jurisdic
tion. I also use a few problems with fuller facts in areas of the law like
declaratory judgments, where the legal rule is simple, yet leaves a
great deal of room for argument about whether a specific situation sits
on one side of the line or the other.
I do realize that by keeping my problems simple and directed at
only one rule or area of the law (and with that area identified at the
top of the problem), I may not be giving my students enough practice
at recognizing when a certain issue needs to be addressed. I some
times see this deficiency on their final exams. Here is an example:
As I described earlier, one of the problems is supposed to teach
the difference between permissive and compulsory counterclaims. 79
On a recent exam, I gave the students a question in which the plaintiff
sued the defendant in state court. Defendant had a claim that was
somewhat related to the original claim (and could arguably have been
a compulsory counterclaim). Instead of bringing it in the state court
lawsuit, however, defendant brought it as a new lawsuit in federal
court. The question posited that the plaintiff asked the federal judge
to dismiss the defendant's suit, arguing that the claim could only be
brought in the ongoing state court lawsuit. A significant number of
students wrote only about whether federal subject matter jurisdiction
(diversity) existed for the claim and ignored the real issue of whether
it was closely related enough to have been a compulsory counterclaim
in the original lawsuit. Most ofthese students probably knew the dif
ference between a compulsory and permissive counterclaim, and had I
78. "A lawyer in practice does not receive a list of hypos from the client. The law
yer gets a story, and must sort out interrelated issues based the on the questions to be
resolved and the rules oflaw that apply." Moskovitz, 42 J. LEGAL EDUC. at 256.
79. See supra notes 37-38 and accompanying text.
2000] PROBLEM METHOD 269
asked students directly which the defendant's claim was, could have
given a reasonable answer. However, students were not able to iden
tify that this was the issue. The reason is perhaps that students did
not have enough practice at the skill of finding the correct rule to
apply.
I note, however, that among the students who did realize that the
issue was whether the counterclaim was permissive or compulsory, a
significant number merely concluded that it was one or the other,
when the facts clearly gave the students enough leeway to explain
why it could reasonably be argued either way. This mistake was cer
tainly not caused by a lack of familiarity with spotting this kind of
situation where there are two possible results, since this was a central
theme of many of the problems students had done, including the one
on counterclaims. It may be that certain skills are difficult for stu
dents to master no matter how much practice we give them.
Another problem that I sometimes worry about is whether my
classes are a little too programmed and predictable. The problems are
so directive, and at this point so well-tuned, that there is sometimes
little variance from year to year in the class discussion. This in itself
may not be too bad, since the students in each class do the problems
only once and there are enough differences and challenges that I do
not get bored doing the problems once a year. I do wonder, however,
whether the students should have a little more opportunity to think
outside the box and to be a little more creative. First year law school
might do too good ajob at forcing students to think linearly and deduc
tively. I suppose that another teacher with more tolerance for unpre
dictability could write problems that allowed the students more
opportunity for creativity and still accomplish many of the same goals
that I seek to achieve.
For me there is no turning back. At this point I could not imagine
teaching Civil Procedure or Evidence by anything other than the prob
lem approach. I have already begun thinking about changing my last
remaining course, Civil Rights Litigation, to a problem format. If af
ter reading this far, you think you would like to try this approach in
some of your classes, I will present, in the next section, some practical
tips for getting started.
Once you have written some problems, how should you use them?
I have found that the problems work best when I give them out to the
students at least one class period before we cover the material. I also
find that it is necessary to require the students to bring in a written
answer to the questions. Whether and to what extent you allow or
require the students to work together on the problems is another ques-
tion you need to decide. I always encourage the students to work in
small groups, although I have never measured to what extent stu-
dents have done so. For several years, I required the students to form
themselves into groups of three. Students were supposed to work on
the problems together, come up with one answer for the whole group
and bring this to class, where they all sat together. I gave this up
because the University of Baltimore is a commuter school, with many
students driving from different areas. Many students also have signif-
icant outside responsibilities including parenting and working part
time. It seemed to be a hardship for many of the students to find the
time to meet with the same group of their classmates on a regular
basis. I still think, however, that this is an interesting idea and might
work better in another setting.
I would not encourage grading, or even collecting the original an-
swers that the students bring to class. First of all, you cannot expect
most students to come up with very good answers when they prepare
answers before the material is covered in class. Second, the problems
are much better received when the students view the problems as
learning aids for their own benefit rather than a graded assignment.
Also, if you grade the problem, you probably cannot allow students to
work together and you have to worry about cheating (either working
with each other or obtaining a copy of another student's answers).
Obviously, I do not worry about students working together, since I
encourage students to do this. I would be concerned if students were
merely copying other students' answers (either from their class or a
previous year). At the beginning of the semester, I explain to students
why this would only be cheating themselves of an important learning
tool, since neither their written answers nor their class performance
has any effect on their grade. I also tell students that I would consider
it an honor code offense if they did obtain and copy another student's
answer. I am reasonably certain that students are doing their own
work.
I always encourage the students to go back and rework the
problems after class. I tell students to do this as soon after class as
possible, and not to wait until the end of a unit or the end of the se-
mester. The procedure I suggest to students is after each class, and
before the next problem is prepared, the student should review the
2000] PROBLEM METHOD 273
class notes and the answer to the previous problem. Students should
then put both their original answers and their class notes aside and
answer the problem from scratch using only the Federal Rules. I do
not know how many students are following this regimen and to what
extent. Several years ago, when I required students to revise and turn
in their answers, I got about eighty percent compliance, even though I
told students that there would be no negative consequences for non
compliance.
Whether you should require the students to revise and turn in
their answers is another choice. If you require the students to turn
the problems in, then I think you owe it to the students to give the
problems back with at least some comments and suggestions. I found
it very enlightening the year I did this, but it was an incredible
amount of work: twenty problem sets, times three to five problems per
set, times seventy-five students. Had this significantly improved their
performance I might have felt some obligation to continue, but it did
not seem to have any such effect.83 Now, I merely collect and review
one or two selected problems near the beginning of the semester so the
students can have some feedback. In addition, I have always been
willing to a review student's work if they are industrious enough to
ask me to do so, but this does not often happen.
VI. CONCLUSION
I have developed my own variation of the problem method which
seems to work very well for me and my students. I have described my
approach in this article with the hope that it might start others think
ing about how to integrate some form of the problem method into their
law school teaching method. The form may turn out to be quite differ
ent from the one I use. The point is that I have, after some trial and
error, and a lot of work, found an approach with which both the stu
dents and I are reasonably satisfied. I encourage other faculty, espe
cially those who are not satisfied with the case method, to try using
some version of the problem method.
APPENDICES:
(APP. A) LIST OF PROBLEM SETS
Problem 1: The Complaint
Problem 2: The Answer
Problem 3: Service and Time Limits
Problem 4: Raising and Waiving Defenses
Problem 5: Counterclaims
Problem 6: Amendments
Problem 7: General Rules of Discovery
Problem 8: Depositions
Problem 9: Mental and Physical Exams
Problem 10: Work Product
Problem 11: Summary Judgment
Problem 12: Provisional Remedies
Problem 13: Trial by Jury
Problem 14: Motions During and Mter Trial
Problem 15: Amount of Judgment
Problem 16: Declaratory Judgments
Problem 17: Appeals
Problem 18: Introduction to Federal Subject Matter Jurisdiction
Problem 19: Federal Question Jurisdiction
Problem 20: Diversity Jurisdiction
Problem 21: Joinder of Parties Under the Rules
Problem 22: Permissive v. Compulsory Joinder
Problem 23: Interpleader
Problem 24: Supplemental Jurisdiction
Problem 25: Class Actions
Problem 26: Erie R.R. v. Tompkins
Problem 27: Applying the Erie Rule
Problem 28: York, Byrd, and Hanna
Problem 29: A Modern Day Erie Case
Problem 30: Pennoyer v. Neff
Problem 31: Challenging Personal Jurisdiction
Problem 32: Quasi-in-rem Jurisdiction
Problem 33: International Shoe Co. v. Washington
Problem 34: Long Arm Statutes
Problem 35: Mullane, Hanson, and Shaffer
Problem 36: World Wide Volkswagen v. Woodson
Problem 37: Burger King and Burnham
Problem 38: Federal Venue
Problem 39: Transfer of Venue
Problem 40: Former Adjudication Generally
Problem 41: Claim Preclusion
2000] PROBLEM METHOD· 275
PROBLEM 6: AMENDMENTS
1. P flies and serves a complaint on day one. D files and serves an
answer on day fifteen, containing a defense of failure to state a
claim. On day twenty-five, P, realizing his complaint is faulty,
wishes to file an amended complaint.
a. May P amend "as of course," or must P obtain "leave of court or
written consent" of D?
b. If you were D's attorney and received a call from P's attorney
asking for consent to allow P to amend, would you give it?
c. If you were the judge and received a request from P for leave of
court to amend, would you grant it?
d. Same facts as above, but after receiving the Answer on day fif-
teen, P immediately tries to amend on day sixteen. May P
amend "as of course," or would he still need permission?
2. P serves D with a complaint on day one. D files and serves a Mo-
tion to Dismiss for Failure to State a Claim on day fifteen. On day
twenty-five, before the motion is ruled upon by the judge, P, realiz-
ing his complaint is faulty, wishes to file an amended complaint.
a. May he amend "as of course," or must he obtain "leave of court or
written consent" of D?
b. Compare the answers to l(a) and 2(a). Are these answers the
same or different? If the answers are different, should they be?
Is it fair to treat the two plaintiffs differently? Does the differ-
ence really matter?
3. D files and serves an answer to a complaint. The answer does not
contain a counterclaim. Fifteen days later D realizes he has left out
the defense of improper venue. May D, either with or without leave
of court, file an Amended Answer containing the defense?
4. D files and serves an answer to a complaint. The answer does not
contain a counterclaim. Thirty days later D realizes he has left out
the defense of improper venue. May D, either with or without leave
of court, file an Amended Answer containing the defense? Compare
this result with l(a) and 3. Is the difference fair and justified?
5. P serves a complaint on D on day one. On day five, P serves an
Amended Complaint on D. On what day must D plead to the
Amended Complaint?
PROBLEM 7-10: DISCOVERY
The following facts are applicable to Problems 7 through 10:
Paula is injured when her automobile is struck from behind by a
truck, owned by Mom's Catering Service Inc., driven by Denny, an em-
ployee of Mom's Catering. Paula sues Denny, alleging negligent driv-
ing. She also sues Mom's, on both a theory of (1) respondeat superior
(that as Denny's employer they are responsible for his negligence),
2000] PROBLEM METHOD 277
and (2) their own negligence in failing to maintain the truck properly.
Pawa claims to have severely injured her back, leaving her in con-
stant pain and unable to work. Denny denies negligence. Mom's de-
fends by denying that Denny was negligent, by denying that Denny
was engaged in his employment duties for Mom at the time ofthe acci-
dent, and by denying negligence in the maintenance of the truck.
PROBLEM 7: GENERAL RULES OF DISCOVERY
FED.R.ClY.P. 26(a), 26(b)(1) and (2), 30(a)(1), 33(a), 34(a)
Assume you are the attorney for Paula, and have determined that you
need the following information to prepare for trial. For each piece of
information listed, indicate:
A. Will either of the Defendants have to supply this information
to you without your having to make a request, and if so, when?
B. If not, how will you go about getting the information? What
methods will you use? Might the defendants argue that it is
not discoverable? What would be their anticipated argument,
and how would you respond to it?
1. The names of:
a. All eyewitnesses to the accident.
b. The eyewitnesses Mom's intends to call at trial.
c. The expert witnesses Mom's intends to call at trial.
2. The version of the accident of:
a. The police.
b. Bob Bystander (an eyewitness).
c. Paula.
d. Denny.
3. Maintenance records of the truck for the two year period before the
accident.
4. As to Mom's Catering:
a. Their net worth.
b. Whether they carry liability insurance.
5. Did Denny make a confession to his priest, asking to be absolved of
guilt for the accident?
6. Were the brakes on the truck replaced after the accident? (Under
the Federal Rules of Evidence, evidence of post accident repairs
are generally not admissible in a negligence action.)
PROBLEM 8: DEPOSITIONS
FED.R.ClV.P. 30 and 32
1. Paula would like to take an oral deposition of the following persons.
As to each, would a deposition be allowed? What steps must, or
should, the plaintiff take to procure the deposition?
a. Denny.
b. Bob Bystander (an eyewitness).
278 CREIGHTON LAW REVIEW [Vol. 34
TABLES:
(TBL.l)
Weekly Preparation Time per Student for Evidence Class
Teaching Method Minutes per Week
Case Method 103
(1998 Class)
Problem Method
(1999 Class) 234
t = .002
(TBL. 2)
Student Evaluations of Evidence Class
Teaching Method Score (on 4.0 Scale)
Case Method 3.29
(1998 Class)
Problem Method 3.67
(1999 Class)
t = .064
(TBL. 3)
Exam Scores for Evidence Class
Teaching Method Score (one 100 point scale)
Case Method 54.9
(1998 Class)
Problem Method 57.5
(1999 Class)
t = .27
(TEL. 4)
Exam Scores for Civil Procedure Class
Teaching Method Score (on 100 point scale)
Problem, No Feedback 62.2
(1997 class)
Problem, with Feedback 58.9
(1998 class)
t = .087
282 CREIGHTON LAW REVIEW [VoL 34