Task Based Language Teaching
Task Based Language Teaching
Task-Based
Language
About the Series Series Editors
Teaching
This Elements series aims to close the gap Heath Rose
between researchers and practitioners by Linacre College,
Daniel O. Jackson
allying research with language teaching University of
practices, in its exploration of research- Oxford
informed teaching, and teaching- Jim McKinley
TASK-BASED LANGUAGE
TEACHING
Daniel O. Jackson
Kanda University of International Studies
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Shaftesbury Road, Cambridge CB2 8EA, United Kingdom
One Liberty Plaza, 20th Floor, New York, NY 10006, USA
477 Williamstown Road, Port Melbourne, VIC 3207, Australia
314–321, 3rd Floor, Plot 3, Splendor Forum, Jasola District Centre,
New Delhi – 110025, India
103 Penang Road, #05–06/07, Visioncrest Commercial, Singapore 238467
www.cambridge.org
Information on this title: www.cambridge.org/9781009068413
DOI: 10.1017/9781009067973
© Daniel O. Jackson 2022
This publication is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception and to the provisions
of relevant collective licensing agreements, no reproduction of any part may take
place without the written permission of Cambridge University Press & Assessment.
First published 2022
A catalogue record for this publication is available from the British Library.
ISBN 978-1-009-06841-3 Paperback
ISSN 2632-4415 (online)
ISSN 2632-4407 (print)
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Cambridge University Press & Assessment has no responsibility for the persistence
or accuracy of URLs for external or third-party internet websites referred to in this
publication and does not guarantee that any content on such websites is, or will
remain, accurate or appropriate.
Task-Based Language Teaching
DOI: 10.1017/9781009067973
First published online: September 2022
Daniel O. Jackson
Kanda University of International Studies
Author for correspondence: Daniel O. Jackson, [email protected]
1 What Is TBLT? 1
References 58
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Task-Based Language Teaching 1
1 What Is TBLT?
1.1 A Framework for Language Teaching
As an approach to communicative language teaching, task-based language
teaching (TBLT) originated in the mid-1980s. It has grown to become one
of the most widely recognized options for designing and implementing
language instruction today. As a field of academic inquiry, TBLT has
achieved a number of milestones, including the inauguration of the
International Conference on TBLT in 2005, since organized every two
years under the auspices of the International Association for Task-Based
Language Teaching (IATBLT), a book series published by John Benjamins
since 2009, and the launch of TASK: Journal on Task-Based Language
Teaching and Learning in 2021. In terms of its implementation, TBLT has
matured from an alternative approach to a mainstream educational policy
initiative encouraged or adopted in schools in Belgium, Hong Kong, and
New Zealand, among other regions. Increasingly, it is offered as a subject
in language teacher education programs, featured at teaching conferences
and in professional workshops, and is carried out by teachers with stu-
dents, during face-to-face or online lessons.
Thus, TBLT is a way of teaching languages and a robust area of inquiry.
In practice, language educators around the world use tasks to coherently
frame their teaching. This coherence can be seen from various perspec-
tives. First, ‘task’ provides a useful concept for framing the reasons why
languages are taught, what to teach (the particular content), and how to
teach (the classroom procedures). Second, in a practical sense, the litera-
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
ture on TBLT offers guidance on using the concept of task to link elements
of curriculum design such as materials, teaching, and testing. Lastly, and
most importantly, TBLT epitomizes the notion that classroom instruction
should be responsive to learners’ needs for using language in the real
world.
Tasks enable learners to acquire communicative abilities and to partici-
pate in social activities relevant to their present or future goals. There has
been much discussion and debate regarding the proposal that real-world
tasks should form the basis of language teaching, beginning with Long
(1985). The appeal of TBLT is that it seeks to identify and utilize activities
valued by learners as the impetus for curriculum development. How
the use of tasks facilitates acquisition of language and fosters participation
in society is a matter of considerable theoretical and practical interest. It
furthermore involves reconsideration of the teacher’s role, which in TBLT
2 Language Teaching
The present introductory section orients readers to TBLT and provides key
definitions and examples, as well as offering commentary on communication
task design. Section 2 guides readers through the familiar elements of
a language curriculum (needs analysis, sequencing of content, materials devel-
opment, teaching, testing, and evaluation) to illustrate how each can be
informed by tasks. Section 3 then adopts a case study approach to demonstrate
how teachers of diverse languages have found TBLT useful in their particular
contexts. The longest section of the Element is Section 4, which presents
a review of recent empirical studies divided into two distinct aspects that
concern practitioners: task design (i.e., complexity and modality) and task
implementation (i.e., preparation, interaction, and repetition). Section 5 then
provides an overview of some of the central issues faced by teachers in
understanding and using tasks. In the epilogue in Section 6, I offer a brief
critique of the potential of TBLT to bring about positive change in classrooms,
institutions, and societies. The Element concludes with an appendix of ques-
tions designed to facilitate discussion after each of the aforementioned sections
has been read.
Why use tasks in the first place? There are many answers, which will become
apparent throughout this text. In this opening section, the following rationales
will be presented. In short, among the clearest benefits of using tasks are that
they can be designed to offer students:
• practice to attain fluency and utilize specific features of language that may be
challenging to learn;
• choices regarding lesson content and procedures and thus more meaningful
and engaging learning experiences.
As described in this section, tasks are compatible with a wide range of teaching
approaches. Subsequently, from Section 2 onwards, further advantages gained
from entirely task-based approaches will be considered.
1.3 Definitions
There is a difference between target tasks, or real-world activities learners
ultimately aim to accomplish in their target language, and pedagogic tasks,
which are instructional activities derived from target tasks. During engagement
in pedagogic tasks, learners “use language, with an emphasis on meaning, to
attain an objective” (Bygate, Skehan, & Swain, 2001, p. 11). This basic
4 Language Teaching
definition incorporates many others that have been offered over the years.
According to it, the following practices would not fittingly be described as
tasks: (1) learning about the target language without actually using it, such as
when listening to an explanation of it in one’s first language; (2) using the
language mechanically rather than meaningfully, as in the memorized dialogues
or choral repetition associated with the audio-lingual method; and (3) using
language meaningfully but without any overt goal, as in free conversation. Of
course, one might benefit minimally from such activities, but they also illustrate
an essential categorical distinction.
Besides the disregard for learners’ needs in these examples of what is not
a task, it is worth briefly considering how each of Bygate and colleagues’
criteria is compatible with recent assumptions regarding learning and language.
Namely, the specification that tasks must involve language use acknowledges
that learning accrues gradually through practice in comprehending and produ-
cing oral and written discourse. The prioritization of meaning is supported by
various functional theories of language, which view it as a tool for communica-
tion. Lastly, establishing objectives helps fuel learner engagement and clarify
expected outcomes. A wide range of theoretical support for TBLT, often sharing
an emphasis on learning by doing, has been described elsewhere (see Ahmadian
& García Mayo, 2018; East, 2021; Ellis et al., 2019; Jackson & Burch, 2017;
Long, 2015; Norris, 2009; Samuda & Bygate, 2008).
Moving from theory to practice, a crucial aspect of using tasks involves the
difference between the task-as-workplan and the task-in-process (Breen, 1987).
Importantly, the design of a task can predict neither entirely how it should be
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
implemented for a given group of learners nor its outcomes. The original plan
for the task, including its stated objective and procedures, unfolds according to
the teacher’s implementation and learner responses. The potential of the task to
shape learning emerges from psycholinguistic and social activity during this
task-in-process. The terms retask and detask (Samuda, 2015) have been used to
refer to how teachers, as well as students, may alter plans during instruction.
Further useful distinctions include those between written versus oral tasks, as
well as monologic (narrative) versus dialogic (interactive) tasks. The examples
in Section 1.4 are oral, dialogic tasks.
popular songs in their L2, a teacher might consider how this target task could be
modified for them in a way that fosters learning through interaction. One
possibility is to distribute two sets of lyrics for a given song wherein missing
words in each set are present in the other, have the students exchange informa-
tion verbally to complete the lyrics, and then practice singing the song together.
In this example, the underlying task type is called a jigsaw task. Pedagogic task
types are accounts of classroom tasks in terms of abstract categories (e.g., Pica,
Kanagy, & Falodun, 1993; Prabhu, 1987; Robinson, 2001; Skehan, 1996;
Willis, 1996). Typological descriptions are helpful to researchers, designers,
and teachers because they may be used to classify tasks, discern their similar-
ities and differences, and rank them according to their learning potential, among
other uses.
This section offers examples of each type of task in the typology put forth by
Pica, Kanagy, and Falodun (1993). Being one of several possible choices, this
typology was selected for the following reasons. First, Pica and colleagues
covered five pedagogic task types, thereby incorporating earlier discussions
that are helpful but made fewer distinctions (e.g., Prabhu, 1987). Second, rather
than mainly describing the activity associated with tasks (e.g., Willis, 1996),
their stated purpose was to present a “typology which can be used to differenti-
ate tasks according to their contributions to language learning” (Pica, Kanagy,
& Falodun, 1993, p. 10), for both teachers and researchers. Third, related to this
goal, even though recent frameworks offer more fine-grained detail regarding
the psycholinguistic demands of tasks and are augmented by task sequencing
principles (e.g., Robinson, 2015), Pica and colleagues’ application of their
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Table 1 Pedagogic task types (adapted from Pica, Kanagy, & Falodun, 1993)
1.4.1 Jigsaw
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Figure 1 Two nutrition facts labels: white versus wheat bread (amounts are
a composite based on actual products)
fat, sodium, and sugar). To reach this conclusion, the learners verbally share
their information.
The main advantage of the jigsaw task derives from the need for both
participants to interact in order to converge on one solution. To compare all of
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
the data, participants must sustain their interaction over multiple turns, incorp-
orating lexical items that may be new or unfamiliar. They may also engage in
further discussion to weigh the importance of any differences uncovered. For
these reasons, Pica, Kanagy, and Falodun (1993, p. 21) claimed the jigsaw to be,
“the type of task most likely to generate opportunities for interactants to work
toward comprehension, feedback, and interlanguage modification processes
related to successful SLA [second language acquisition].” This claim has
been supported by face-to-face studies as well as those involving text-based
computer-mediated communication (Blake, 2000).
Like jigsaw tasks, information gap tasks also require messages to be exchanged.
However, they need only involve a one-way exchange: one person requests the
information while the other provides it. A two-way exchange can happen if the
listener actively seeks confirmation of the information received, or if the listener
8 Language Teaching
and speaker alternate roles. The goal of each person in the interaction is the
same (+ Convergent) and there is one fixed outcome according to the input
provided. As an example, the teacher could first have students write down their
favorite recipe. This can follow a simple formula: the name and origin of the
dish, the ingredients, and a list of steps. Once this material has been prepared,
the first student in the pair describes their recipe to a partner, who takes notes.
Then, they switch roles and repeat the task. Having students write down each
other’s recipes would benefit their interaction, as that can prompt them to seek
clarification and confirmation. Doing so would also allow the students and
teacher to check the accuracy of the exchange.
Alternatively, if the teacher rather than the students prepares the input, it is
possible to design information gap tasks drawing attention to specific language
features that are difficult to acquire due to low salience. Research on such tasks
by Pica, Kang, and Sauro (2006) found a strong association between inter-
actional processes and the noticing of specifically targeted forms. For example,
while working in pairs to complete tasks requiring them to discuss and make
choices about English articles, pronouns, determiners, and verb morphology,
intermediate-level learners’ interactions often showed evidence of noticing
these targeted forms. In Schmidt’s (1990) account, noticing, or conscious
registration of language, is necessary for the acquisition of an L2. Although
many tasks do not require such close attention to language input, Pica and
colleagues assumed on the basis of their evidence that task-based interaction
can prompt learners to notice. Maps, drawings, texts, and other materials can
provide content for information gap tasks.
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
1.4.3 Problem-Solving
directions, but if either student does not possess the requisite confidence,
knowledge, or skill, then the other may lead throughout the discussion.
Because the information requester versus provider roles are unspecified, the
design does not strictly require interaction. These problems also apply to
decision-making and opinion exchange tasks (see Sections 1.4.4 and 1.4.5). In
the example, it may turn out that only one individual dominates the discussion.
To promote more equal participation, the teacher might instruct students to take
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
turns giving their answers and provide reasons for them. However, the fact
remains that the amount and quality of interaction may be limited in comparison
to jigsaw tasks, in which the discourse is more predictable. On the other hand,
this design, like the previous two, has an advantage because its shared, fixed
goal provides a clear direction and endpoint for the discussion.
1.4.4 Decision-Making
The decision-making task encourages learners to discuss a given topic and agree
upon one of a finite number of acceptable outcomes. Other possible outcomes
suggested by the input may be unacceptable. To illustrate, the teacher could
provide the class with copies of a restaurant menu (Figure 3) to read. The task
involves a scenario in which students are at lunch with a friend who needs
assistance to understand the menu. This friend would prefer a meal that contains
protein and vegetables, but no dairy. The teacher asks pairs of students to look
over the menu in order to help choose a suitable option. Based on the criteria
10 Language Teaching
Figure 3 Menu
students given a number of objects found in someone’s coat pockets are asked to
reach a consensus on the owner’s identity. These authors made the point that the
discourse emerging from such tasks has important qualities such as the potential
for social engagement and collaborative thinking.
does not provide an inherent goal for the discussion to converge on. If students
express disagreement, their goal orientation would be considered divergent.
Besides, the goal is relatively simple: state any local food product or combin-
ation thereof, or none at all. For all of these reasons, exchanging opinions is
unlikely to guarantee learners equal opportunities for conversational interaction
to the extent seen in jigsaw and information gap tasks. Nonetheless, opinion
exchange would be appropriate for different aspects of L2 development
(Skehan, 1998). In fact, divergent tasks, in which learners produce additional
clauses to support their arguments, have been shown to generate more syntac-
tically complex discourse than convergent ones, in face-to-face (Duff, 1986)
and computer-mediated (Jackson, 2011) settings. To communicate effectively
in an L2, one must share opinions. Tasks that promote this ability also provide
valuable opportunities for students to raise issues or concerns that might not
otherwise come to light.
Second, more recently, the learners’ level of engagement has been recognized
as a major consideration in task-based learning. Philp and Duchesne (2016)
described engagement in terms of its cognitive (e.g., attention), behavioral (e.g.,
time on task), social (e.g., affiliation), and emotional (e.g., feelings) facets.
Researchers have measured engagement in various ways. With regard to task
design, findings suggest that key dimensions of engagement are enhanced when
using learner-generated as opposed to teacher-generated content (Lambert,
Philp, & Nakamura, 2017; Phung, Nakamura, & Reinders, 2021). In other
words, giving learners some control over the content appears to make tasks
more meaningful and engaging. Among the examples provided, the information
gap task in Section 1.4.2 does this by inviting learners to exchange their favorite
recipes. It is sometimes easy to make minor adjustments to existing tasks in
order to allow creativity and promote engagement. For example, the decision-
making task (Section 1.4.4) could be redesigned so that learners first write down
menu items individually, pool them to create their own menu, and then discuss
which ones would make appropriate choices based on certain dietary
restrictions.
These views are helpful for understanding the value of tasks, though in
a broader sense, TBLT offers even more than conversational interaction, lan-
guage practice, and learner engagement. As the following sections demonstrate,
the outcomes can extend far beyond even these important goals.
1
As a reviewer helpfully pointed out, ‘curriculum’ has the same meaning as ‘syllabus’ in some
parts of the world.
Task-Based Language Teaching 13
tasks. Other recent examples have focused on the language needs of medical
students using isiZulu (Gokool & Visser, 2021) and Syrian refugee parents
using Turkish (Toker & Sağıç, 2022).
Needs analysis is one of the features distinguishing a strong version of TBLT
from its weaker variants. Indeed, considering that L2 learning can be a choice or
a necessity, some argue that general approaches to curriculum development, as
often seen in commercial English as a second language (ESL) and EFL text-
books, are “particularly detrimental” (Serafini, 2022, p. 75) when learners need
assistance in integrating into society. The nature and scope of learner needs are
highly differentiated, as these three studies illustrate. In Park’s study, they
included academic and social needs, in Malicka’s study, they involved highly
specific occupational duties, and in Oliver’s study, they encompassed work-
place and social skills. Detailed knowledge of the sectors relevant to learners’
future success is the first step in selecting and sequencing appropriate tasks for
instruction.
Task-Based Language Teaching 15
will necessitate continual revision of materials as long as these new tools are
required to fulfill learners’ needs.
Perusing existing, published task-based materials or lesson plans is often
a useful way to understand task-based teaching. In Section 2.4, an account of
task-based instruction is described and briefly critiqued.
2.4 Teaching
The use of tasks in classroom practice affords opportunities to redefine teaching
and reshape the learning environment. The most influential framework for task-
based teaching comes from Willis (1996), who divided instructor roles into
three phases that promote increased student involvement and reposition the
teacher as a guide. First, in the pretask phase, the teacher introduces the topic
and, along with the class, explores the content of the task by, for example,
brainstorming vocabulary. Here, learners are expected to orient receptively to
and activate language that will be useful in performing the task. Models may
optionally be provided in the form of teacher demonstrations, audio- or video-
recordings of the task being done, or relevant written texts. This phase culmin-
ates with the teacher providing instructions about what the students should do in
the main phase, including a clear statement of the goal. If applicable, the teacher
can announce the amount of time allocated to performance. As an example, in
Newton and Bui’s (2018) implementation study of TBLT in primary school EFL
lessons in Vietnam, pretask work involved (1) brainstorming school subjects to
prime relevant vocabulary and (2) listening to a conversation modeling the task
while completing a handout.
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
In the second phase, called the task cycle, the students carry out the task as the
teacher monitors them. There are three distinct stages to this cycle: task,
planning, and report. During the task stage, the teacher must step out of the
limelight to allow students to independently perform the task. At this stage, only
minimal teacher action is called for, which may include encouraging students
who need it, noticing the particular dynamics of student or group performance
for later reference, and keeping time. The planning stage follows the task and
provides students with time and resources to prepare a report on their task
performance. This report may concern the process or outcome of the task. Here,
the teacher needs to provide further instructions and advice on language.
Finally, during the report stage, the teacher manages the process by selecting
groups, taking notes as students share their reports, and then summarizing the
key points. In the case of Newton and Bui (2018), during the main phase,
students undertook an information gap task to share details about two timetables
and then had to identify three differences and two similarities across the
Task-Based Language Teaching 19
2.5 Assessment
In task-based language assessment (TBLA), “learners have to use their second
language (L2) abilities to get things done” (Norris & East, 2022, p. 507; see
also Norris, 2016). This approach is a radical departure from traditional tests
that require learners only to demonstrate their second language skills, often
rather differently from the ways we ordinarily use language. For instance,
a multiple-choice vocabulary test that asks students to read a word, then
choose the equivalent item in their first language (L1) is not a task-based
test. Alternatively, in TBLA, tests can be designed to measure learners’
abilities to use the language they have been learning in class in situations
20 Language Teaching
Two further examples from the literature illustrate how, like other types of
assessment, the uses of TBLA can be described as either summative or forma-
tive. Formative refers to assessment for learning during ongoing instruction,
while summative refers to high-stakes assessment of learning after instruction.
First, Weaver (2012) described the application of a formative assessment
cycle in a task-based business presentation course for university students in
Japan. The task was for students to deliver a PowerPoint presentation of a stock
listed on the New York Stock Exchange that they thought would be a good
investment. Across five class meetings, the instructor led forty-six students to
complete a number of steps, including: (1) listening to a description of the
assessment cycle; (2) watching a video of a student performing the task; (3)
collaboratively discussing the task definition and developing rating criteria (i.e.,
English speaking skills and presentation design skills); (4) evaluating additional
task videos using these criteria; and (5) delivering their own video-recorded
presentations, which were rated by their classmates. After this, the teacher (6)
analyzed these scores to provide an overall summary of the performances; (7)
Task-Based Language Teaching 21
met with each student to discuss their scores on the criteria; (8) had the students
transcribe their performances; and, finally (9) provided additional feedback on
these transcriptions.
Second, as an example of research oriented to summative purposes, Youn
(2018) employed roleplay tasks to gauge how much pragmatic competence
examinees displayed. In this study, 102 English as a second language (ESL)
students in the United States carried out five roleplay tasks based on scenarios
encountered in academic settings with a trained interlocutor who took the role of
a professor or classmate. These audio-recorded performances were then judged
using five rating criteria: content delivery, language use, sensitivity to the
situation, engaging in interaction, and turn organization (i.e., when and how
participants organize conversational turns). The analysis successfully distin-
guished examinees according to six levels of pragmatic ability. The author
recommended that care be taken in designing roleplays so that various, relevant
interactional behaviors can be elicited and assessed and also urged that rating
criteria explicitly incorporate descriptions of such behavior. For an earlier
example of developing and researching TBLA, see Norris and colleagues’
(1998) work plus the companion volume by Brown and colleagues (2002).
2.6 Evaluation
Having considered the main elements of a carefully designed TBLT program
(i.e., needs analysis, task sequencing, materials, teaching, and assessment), this
section describes program evaluation. Despite numerous studies focused solely
on its constituent elements, strong TBLT entails a programmatic view whereby
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
all of these elements are taken into consideration when seeking to determine the
effectiveness of TBLT (Norris & Davis, 2022). Program evaluation involves
systematically gathering data to judge the effectiveness the program as a whole.
It uses an array of research methods suited to the purpose of broadly under-
standing program outcomes. It also differs from individual research studies
because its primary aim is to guide decisions about a program. Norris and Davis
(2022, pp. 536–537) delineated several ways in which an evaluation can be
focused. These highlighted the following unique aspects of task-based pro-
grams: the learning sequences, materials, assessments, teacher and student
responses, and alignment with the local context.
For example, Markee (1997) reported on the design, implementation, and
evaluation of the curricular and teacher innovation (CATI) project, which
promoted TBLT as an innovation within a university EFL program in the
United States. Taking a participatory and formative approach to the evaluation
stage, the author gathered data from action research, teaching journals, and
22 Language Teaching
surveys to address the issue of how instructors, who were teaching assistants
enrolled in a teacher education program, responded to the innovation by devel-
oping new materials, skills, and values. Among the many detailed results
Markee provided, teachers contributed to an eventually large bank of tasks
and some of them developed new skills relevant to task-based teaching.
However, these teachers were not uniformly in agreement regarding the most
efficient classroom discourse strategies for promoting student talk during tasks,
which suggested that further realignment of pedagogic values might assist in
implementing TBLT. Markee also noted that turnover created difficulty in this
context because experienced teachers often help to transmit knowledge and
skills to new teachers.
The evaluation by Markee represented one approach of many. Other types of
evaluation may be viewed as a matter of comparing TBLT to existing, often
traditional, approaches such as PPP (see Shintani, 2016, described in
Section 3.3). Having offered a general account of each stage in a task-based
curriculum in this section, the next section will describe concrete examples of
how practitioners developed task-based programs in specific settings.
3.1 Chinese
As an example of the feasibility of introducing TBLT via the development of
instructional modules, Hill and Tschudi (2011) applied a task-based approach in
a blended university program on conversational Mandarin Chinese at the
Task-Based Language Teaching 23
met face-to-face to perform a map gap (one kind of information gap) task in
pairs. They then completed additional cooperative tasks online to practice
understanding and giving directions. Regarding assessment, the final exam
was based on interactive test tasks with different partners.
Hill and Tschudi (2011) reported an evaluation of this module consisting of
formative and summative dimensions. The formative evaluation pinpointed
a need for additional technology resources to support an assessment of real-
time interactions in online courses. The summative evaluation included
research concerning students’ uptake of linguistic features and their attitudes
toward the course. The uptake analysis targeted topicalization, renomination,
repetition, and modal verbs as features of natural discourse. Based on data
from two direction-giving pedagogic tasks used in courses for first-
and second-year students, the results showed greater use of these target
features among second-year students, but also revealed underuse of certain
features (topicalization and repetition). Finally, there was some variation
across the two tasks used, which interacted with the student year.
24 Language Teaching
On the whole, these results were viewed positively, because they were
achieved through completing meaningful tasks and indicated use of naturally
occurring discourse features. Students also expressed satisfaction with various
elements of the program, including the integration of educational technology,
cultural knowledge, authentic discourse, and their own increased awareness of
conversational Chinese. In sum, this was an exemplary study of the teacher-led
development of a web-based language course informed by student needs. Given
the intense effort across its different stages and the varying nature of the
expertise required, Hill and Tschudi recommended collaborating with col-
leagues to undertake such projects.
3.2 Dutch
The duration and scope of task-based teaching in Flanders, in the north of
Belgium, makes it one of the best-known examples of a regional implementa-
tion of TBLT. Beginning in the 1990s, educators have transformed Flemish
schools at the primary, secondary, and adult levels in order to support immi-
grants, refugees, and their families in their efforts to learn Dutch and integrate
into society. This initiative has been fueled by government education policy and
the work of the Centre for Language and Education at the Katholieke
Universiteit Leuven. The Centre for Language and Education has assisted
hundreds of school teams in the region in implementing task-based syllabuses
to meet the language needs of a wide variety of students, including speakers of
Dutch as an L1 or L2. This process has resulted in a trove of research offering
teacher perspectives on tasks in classroom practice (see, e.g., Van den Branden,
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
2006, 2015, 2016; Van den Branden, Van Gorp, & Verhelst, 2007; Vandommele,
Van den Branden, & Van Gorp, 2018). Over the years, that emphasis has
expanded from individual in-service teachers’ use of tasks in their own lessons
to preservice teacher education and school-wide language policies encouraging
collaboration (Van den Branden & Van Gorp, 2021).
Van den Branden (2015) provided a vivid snapshot of the theoretical back-
ground to this implementation of TBLT, as well as of three theory-to-practice
accounts based on classroom research in primary school settings. The first study
described how three teachers all modified, in their own ways, a task in which
learners were asked to plan and deliver a radio news program for a fictitious,
multilingual country. One teacher emphasized creativity and thus prohibited
students from referring to real events. Another took the opposite approach,
modeling the task using an authentic news broadcast and expecting students to
incorporate features of this genre. Whereas both of these teachers disallowed
multiple languages, the third one invited students to decide for themselves
Task-Based Language Teaching 25
whether to go along with the original task’s encouragement to use their own
languages and relied on students to cooperate in groups. In the second study,
student–teacher interactions were observed in order to relate them to students’
writing development over one year. Writing was scored in several ways,
including communicative effectiveness, accuracy, and complexity. Student
progress in writing varied both within and across individuals and some of this
variation was attributed to the teacher’s individualization of instruction.
Namely, the teacher encouraged L1 learners to be creative while asking L2
learners to write shorter, more accurate sentences. The third study dealt with the
use of tasks to support Dutch learners in a science project on DNA. The study
used a pretest-posttest research design with a control group to measure out-
comes and also closely examined classroom processes. A key insight here was
that two individual students who made large gains varied considerably in their
classroom behavior, or the extent to which they engaged with the teacher.
From these studies, Van den Branden (2015) concluded that what matters for
learning is not the task design, or workplan, but instead the interaction that
emerges between teachers and students in the classroom, as well as students’
own unique motivation, goals, and self-regulation.
3.3 English
There have been questions about the applicability of TBLT in contexts where
the target language is not widely spoken, and about its appropriateness for
beginning, younger learners. In response to these issues, Shintani (2016)
reported on a study carried out with six-year-old, novice English learners in
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
(IRF) exchanges, but these were longer in the TBLT lessons. The types of questions
varied, with more teacher-initiated display questions in PPP and more student-
initiated referential questions in TBLT. Whereas the teacher had control of the floor
in PPP, students had more control over turn-taking in TBLT. Finally, as might be
expected, turns occurred in chorus in PPP but not in TBLT.
Second, moving on to the quantitative results, the study employed a battery of
pretests before the treatments followed by immediate and delayed posttests to
compare outcomes across the groups. Regarding vocabulary, there were four
tests covering nouns and adjectives introduced in the TBLT and PPP lessons.
The nouns were acquired equally well by learners in both groups. However, the
TBLT group outperformed the PPP group on adjectives. In accordance with its
focus on communication, in the TBLT lessons adjectives were not pretaught but
learned incidentally through meaningful exchanges. Concerning grammar, five
tests were used to assess incidental learning of two features: the plural –s and
copula be. In this case, the results were less robust. Neither group improved in
their production of these features. Nonetheless, those in the TBLT classes
showed improvement in comprehending plural –s. Shintani (2016, p. 136)
suggested that this positive result could be explained by the relevance of the
plural to completing the task, as illustrated here:
S3: two?
T: two?
S2: three?
T: three, yes. okay? ready? three, two, one, go.
Ss: (show the correct card).
3.4 German
A commendable case of grounding curricular thinking in notions of task and
genre can be found in Byrnes’ work within the Georgetown University German
Department (see Byrnes, 2014, 2015; Byrnes, Maxim, & Norris, 2010; Byrnes
et al., 2006). Systemic functional linguistics provided the basis for this unique
curriculum. This theory views language as a semiotic resource that enacts and
construes social contexts by presenting choices that enable users to express (1)
what they experience, (2) who is taking part (and their social relations), and (3)
the role of language itself. Such expression comprises frequently occurring
genres, which are recognizable to communities of language users. Based on
the academic and professional needs of its students, the department’s curricu-
lum aims to promote longitudinal development to advanced levels through
a focus on oral and written genres. This is achieved through genre-oriented,
task-based teaching, the gist of which is described by Byrnes (2014, p. 243) as
follows:
3.5 Spanish
The context for González-Lloret and Nielson (2015) was the US Border Patrol
Academy, which provides language training tailored to the needs of agents who
must speak fluent Spanish in order to offer assistance, communicate legal rights,
and safely resolve conflicts. The TBLT program resulted from dissatisfaction
with a previous grammar-based course that left agents-in-training underpre-
pared for these responsibilities. This is a highly informative case as it illustrates
how linking all six, interconnected curricular components to the notion of task
can improve training programs. First, a needs analysis identified seven target
tasks, or job duties likely requiring the L2. Second, these tasks were sequenced
to form an eight-week series of increasingly complex modules. Third, materials
were developed, including audio- and video-recordings, roleplay scenarios, and
an interactive video game. Fourth, regarding teaching, native speakers partici-
pated in roleplaying and unscripted practice activities, and instructors were
encouraged to use focus on form. Fifth, task-based assessment was used. At
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
the course. Over the duration of the program, their scores significantly increased
on overall spoken proficiency, as well as on sentence mastery, vocabulary,
fluency, and pronunciation. Further analyses demonstrated that the increase in
their ability was unrelated to starting proficiency. Study 3 gauged opinions of
the program using a questionnaire methodology. Respondents included those
who were enrolled in the task-based course, as well as agents who had graduated
from it. All of these individuals generally found the program useful, interesting,
and relevant. Particularly worth noting is that graduates agreed that they could
use the Spanish they learned in the field.
3.6 Zapotec
South of the Mexico–US border, in a very different context, Riestenberg and
colleagues (Riestenberg & Sherris, 2018; Riestenberg & Manzano, 2019) have
applied task-based principles in teaching Macuiltianguis Zapotec to children
living in the community in Oaxaca where this language is traditionally spoken.
Because there are fewer younger than older speakers, needs include the preser-
vation of the culture and revitalization of the language. Therefore, the project
highlighted the transmission of traditional knowledge and creation of new
domains of language use through tasks. Riestenberg and Sherris (2018) elabor-
ated on the role of investment (encompassing identity and ideology) and task-
based methodological principles (Long, 2015) in Indigenous language educa-
tion. Pedagogic tasks in the Zapotec classroom concerned greetings, small talk,
and shopping for food. During instruction, teachers used elaborated spoken
input (i.e., repetition and paraphrasing) and provided limited, contextually
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
examined how tasks support the development of literacy within this setting,
viewing writing as a set of processes embedded within sociocultural contexts,
according to Ivanič’s model (cited in Riestenberg & Manzano, 2019). This
effort built upon those of a revitalization group that has standardized the
alphabet and created print resources: a word list, various games, and booklets
containing songs, stories, and local history. The following examples illustrate
how writing was used as a scaffold for speaking tasks. First, to prepare students
for the task of asking a speaker how to prepare a traditional drink, the teacher
asked them to listen to the ingredients, write them down, and compare their
spelling with a classmate’s. The teacher then gave them the spelling and had
them label the ingredients on a whiteboard. Second, to prepare the students to
introduce themselves at a community event, they first memorized and then were
provided with a written sample of a self-introduction, from which they bor-
rowed chunks of language to write their own personalized introduction. In
a final example, the students made use of Spanish and Zapotec on signs that
they created with slogans promoting Earth Day, which were posted on a nearby
highway. As such, in addition to developing phonological and orthographic
knowledge, new arenas for the everyday use of Zapotec emerged from the
integration of written language and task-based teaching.
Holistic accounts of instruction (such as those above) are invaluable for under-
standing how various features of educational practice come together when tasks are
viewed as primary. To offer another, complementary perspective, Section 4 will
introduce several discrete approaches to pedagogic research on tasks.
This section reviews twenty-five studies of TBLT published in the most recent
decade (between 2011 and 2021). First, some good news. Studies comparing
TBLT programs to traditional, non-TBLT programs indicate that using tasks
leads to stronger gains on L2 outcomes, as well as positive attitudes toward
TBLT programs by teachers and learners (Bryfonski & McKay, 2017).
Accordingly, this section will focus on studies illustrating the use of tasks in
practice, rather than methods comparison studies (see Section 3.3 for instance;
for a recent example, see Borro, 2022).
The review is organized into several branches, based on the notion that tasks
can be viewed as either static workplans constructed by materials designers or
as a series of processes implemented by teachers and learners (e.g., Breen, 1987;
Samuda, 2015; Van den Branden, 2016). The main features of the task workplan
are design and mode. The primary task processes are preparation, interaction,
and repetition. These branches (Figure 4) are each explained, reviewed, and
Task-Based Language Teaching 31
4.1 Design
As already indicated in Section 1.4, much attention has been paid to the inherent
features of tasks as workplans. Present research in this area is an outgrowth of
decades of work, beginning in the 1980s, which described and classified tasks
according to their essential characteristics (e.g., Nunan, 1989; Prabhu, 1987;
Yule, 1997). In the 1990s, researchers began to consolidate various pedagogic
task types into systematic frameworks (Pica, Kanagy, & Falodun, 1993;
Skehan, 1996) which referenced key considerations in SLA, such as the essen-
tial roles of negotiated interaction and psycholinguistic processing. The turn of
the millennium brought with it research seeking to refine and test theoretical models
of the influence of task design on L2 performance and learning, especially Skehan’s
Limited Attentional Capacity Model and Robinson’s Cognition Hypothesis
(for state-of-the-art commentaries, see Skehan, 2018 and Robinson, 2015).
32 Language Teaching
The latter approach, focused on here due to space limitations, is based on Robinson’s
Triadic Componential Framework, which differentiates between:
task complexity: number of elements, reasoning demands, and whether the task was
to be performed in here-and-now or there-and-then conditions. Importantly, these
studies also bring to light small, yet meaningful effects of task complexity on L2
production across the literature. For instance, increasing task complexity influences
oral narrative production by raising accuracy (d = 0.28; CI = ± 0.12) and lowering
fluency (d = -0.16; CI = ± 0.09) (Jackson & Suethanapornkul, 2013).3 The focus
here on interactive tasks is intentional, as this line of research has received less
attention despite its relevance to instruction.
The findings from several studies into the effects of task design on learner–
learner interaction in pairs or groups (see Table 2) are briefly summarized here. To
begin with face-to-face studies, Révész (2011) showed that learners
2
These factors include resource-directing variables, which make conceptual/cognitive demands, as
well as contrasting resource-dispersing variables (e.g., planning time) which make performative/
procedural demands (Robinson, 2022, p. 211).
3
Cohen’s d is a measure of effect size that indicates how large or small a difference was found
between groups or across time.
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
• Over time, the challenges posed by complex tasks may be more suited than
simple tasks and traditional instruction to the development of syntactic and
pragmatic features.
4.2 Mode
Mode refers to whether communication is oral or written, and includes
hybrid modes having characteristics of both speech and writing, such as
CMC. In practice, tasks may be conducted entirely in a single mode (e.g.,
oral discussions versus written essays) or they may involve more than one
mode (e.g., note-taking based on a lecture). This might be considered a part
of the task’s design, but it is also independent of it, because a given task can
sometimes be carried out in different modes. For example, L2 learners in
a business program might practice offering a position to a successful job
candidate via either a spoken message left on an answering machine or
a written email, with a given number of details required to be conveyed
regardless of the mode. Problematically, the language teaching field has
strongly tended to associate tasks with oral communication, as shown by
much of the literature dating back to the 1990s and earlier. According to
Byrnes and Manchón (2014), this dominant emphasis on the oral mode
continues to permeate task-based theory, research, and practice. It is there-
fore highly appropriate that recent research has shifted to look at the
contribution of the written mode to learning in tasks, as well as to how task-
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Gilabert, and Manchón (2017) found main effects for both task complexity
and mode. The written task performances were higher in lexical, structural,
and propositional complexity, whereas no mode-related difference was found
in accuracy. Lastly, considering the influence of mode during interaction in
CMC, Ziegler and Phung (2019) compared four modes available via Skype:
text, audio, video, and multimodal. Tasks performed using multimodal chat
showed the greatest percentage of all interactional features, including negoti-
ation, recasts, and explicit feedback. The video mode had the second highest
percentage for nearly all features.
Mode is a relevant consideration in language classrooms. To recap, the
findings suggest the following:
• In narrative tasks, the written mode tends to show more syntactic and lexical
complexity than the oral mode.
• Learners may be aware of differences in these modes and use their L2
differently in speaking versus writing tasks.
• In interactive CMC tasks, oral modes supported by video technology are
more suited to the negotiation of meaning as they afford access to nonverbal
signals.
These results should encourage teachers to think carefully about mode when
planning task-based instruction. In addition, they suggest that learners should
have opportunities to demonstrate their L2 abilities in different modes, at
different times. However, there are certain limitations to note, as well. First, it
should be kept in mind that challenges arise when seeking to compare perform-
ance across the distinctive modes of speaking and writing, particularly with
regard to the amount of time taken. Second, it is of utmost importance in TBLT
to remain faithful to the mode in which the task is most likely to be performed in
the real world, including tasks which blend modes (Gilabert, Manchón, &
Vasylets, 2016). In light of these issues, teachers might find it easier to adjust
the degree of task complexity than to render a task typically done in speaking as
a writing task, or vice versa. Despite these caveats, the studies reviewed here
generally reflect bottom-up concerns because the teaching of writing and liter-
acy skills is a major area of language instruction. Fortunately, the lack of
attention to this area in TBLT has begun to be addressed (Byrnes & Manchón,
2014). Moreover, these studies were generally clear about task design, content,
and procedures and forthcoming as regards pedagogic implications. Put suc-
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
4.3 Preparation
The discussion of task processes, or those factors that play out directly in the
classroom, begins with the question of how teachers and learners can prepare
together before a task. Section 2.4 has already introduced the framework offered
by Willis (1996), noting that the pretask phase may involve discussing the task’s
topic and content, brainstorming useful language, analyzing models of success-
ful task performance, and establishing expectations for the main task phase,
among other forms of preparation. A related, useful concept from recent
research is that of task readiness.
As proposed by Bui (2014), readiness for a task consists of implicit and
explicit types of planning. Implicit planning includes a learner’s inherent
familiarity with content (e.g., professional knowledge), schema (e.g., routines
Task-Based Language Teaching 39
used during the task), and the task itself. Learners may gain certain advantages
when their knowledge of these areas is activated. Teachers should therefore try
to leverage student familiarity with task content and schema, as Willis (1996)
recommended. Otherwise, the benefit of this type of readiness may not extend to
task performance. This view is also consistent with the use of motivational
strategies as preparation. During the preactional stage of task motivation
(Dörnyei, 2002), for instance, the teacher helps to orient students to goals,
convey intentions behind the task, and initiate purposeful action.
Another kind of readiness according to Bui (2014) is explicit planning, which
involves externally imposed forms of planning. The options here include pre-
task planning and within-task planning. Based on Ellis (2005), pretask planning,
or planning that ensues prior to task performance, includes rehearsal and
strategic planning. The former involves practicing a task. The latter encourages
students to carefully think about what they want to say and how they will say it.
Pretask planning differs from within-task planning, which concerns the amount
of time allocated to the task. Providing more or less time for performance
naturally influences the amount of planning time available to learners within
a task. As shown in Section 4.2, there are differences across the oral and written
mode in this respect. In practice, these two broad types of explicit planning can
be combined to yield lessons using distinct combinations of planning conditions
(e.g., +/- pretask planning and +/- within-task planning).
A further distinction for instructors to be aware of is whether strategic pretask
planning is guided or unguided. For instance, Mochizuki and Ortega (2008)
described a study carried out with groups of EFL learners at a high school in
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Japan who were asked to retell a picture story. The unguided and guided
planning groups were given the same amount of time to prepare; however, the
guided planning group was also given a handout on relative clause formation
and told that it might be useful for the task, but they could only use it during
planning. The results showed large effects in favor of the guided planners on
both the quantity and quality of relative clauses produced, with no significant
difference in fluency between groups. This study shows that the processes
undertaken during planning are essential. From the perspective of the class-
room, an obvious question is how learners may support each other during the
process of preparing for a task. Finally, another approach to preparation
involves metacognitive instruction to enhance students’ learning opportunities
during interactive tasks (Fujii, Ziegler, & Mackey, 2016).
Table 4 summarizes details from a handful of studies on learners preparing
for tasks. The first two studies looked into individual planning. Bui (2014) gave
students no planning time versus ten-minutes planning time before speaking
about familiar versus unfamiliar topics. Planning time improved complexity
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Given the wide range of options, these findings are tentative conclusions which,
ideally, will be augmented by future research. Metacognitive instruction (Sato,
2020; see also Fujii, Ziegler, & Mackey, 2016) appears to be one of the most
promising avenues, particularly in foreign language settings. Planning studies
highlight learner involvement in TBLT, given the clear effects it has on improv-
ing language performance, comprehensibility, and interaction.
4.4 Interaction
Conversational interaction assists the development of L2 knowledge because it
fosters comprehension, provides feedback, and encourages output (see, e.g.,
Behney & Gass, 2021; Long, 1996; Mackey, 2007, 2012, 2020; Philp, Adams,
& Iwashita, 2013; Pica, 1994). In L2 classroom settings, particularly during pair
and group work, interaction research has focused on the negotiation of meaning,
which occurs during conversations where a listener works to repair understand-
ing of a speaker’s intended meaning by using clarification requests (what do you
mean?) or confirmation checks (you said ten-fifteen not ten-fifty, right?), or
when a speaker attempts to find out whether an utterance is understood by the
listener using a comprehension check (do you understand?). These speaker/
listener roles may be adopted by teachers or learners. Interaction research has
also examined the negotiation of form through corrective feedback provided to
learners by teachers using recasts, elicitations, or metalinguistic explanations to
follow up on erroneous L2 production. Tasks create opportunities for these
forms of interaction and studies viewing interaction as a catalyst for L2 acqui-
sition have often relied on tasks.
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
For instance, important work by Keck and colleagues (2006) summarized the
results of fourteen studies that had investigated the impact that communication
tasks have on the acquisition of specific grammatical or lexical features by adult
L2 learners. The tasks used in these studies included the five types described by
Pica, Kanagy, and Falodun (1993; see Section 1.4 of this Element), as well as
narrative/story-telling tasks incorporating interaction. In these quantitative
studies, the impact of task-based interaction was assessed using test scores.
Keck and colleagues’ meta-analytic study used standardized measures of these
scores to combine results from the entire set of primary studies. There were
clear outcomes in favor of task-based interaction. In terms of its immediate
effectiveness, the average effect when comparing task-based versus control
groups was large (d = 0.92; CI = ± 0.24). Considering the duration of these
effects, results varied according to the delay between the treatment and the test.
A short delay (eight to twenty-nine days later) yielded a large average effect
within a narrow range (d = 1.12; CI = ± 0.31). A longer delay (thirty to sixty days
Task-Based Language Teaching 43
later) yielded a similar, large average effect, within a wider range (d = 1.18; CI =
± 0.83). That is, the observed positive effects remained large but became less
precise as time went on. This pattern could be due to the fact that fewer studies
investigated the long-term effects of task-based interaction on SLA.
Nonetheless, the findings clearly demonstrate the contribution of task-based
interaction to grammar and lexis.
Given the effectiveness of interaction, new research directions have emerged.
In terms of explanatory variables, the interactionist agenda has looked to the
unique contributions of learners, instructors, and other interlocutors
(Gurzynski-Weiss, 2017) for insight, including studies on cognitive individual
differences (see Mackey, 2020 for a review). Another recent development,
exemplified by studies using conversation analysis (e.g., Kunitz & Skogmyr
Marian, 2017; Ro, 2018) or those grounded in engagement (e.g., Baralt,
Gurzynski-Weiss, & Kim, 2016; Philp & Duchesne, 2016), is increased atten-
tion to the interplay between task-based interaction and social or psychological
processes. In light of these trends, the studies reviewed in Table 5 focused on
what learners bring to their interactions and how they experience them.
Factors such as age and proficiency make a difference in how students
interact, as do their roles. Among groups of younger (five-to-seven-year-old)
and older (eleven-to-twelve-year-old) children, Oliver, Philp, and Duchesne
(2017) found that primary students supported each other to complete a set of
information gap tasks. Several age-related differences emerged, with more
language play among the younger group and more cooperation and on-task
behavior among the older group, though task effects were also found to influ-
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
ence these outcomes. Turning to proficiency, Dao and McDonough (2017) had
adult learners do a task in mixed-level pairs. They gave either the lower- or
higher-proficiency student the role of information holder versus information
receiver. When the less proficient partner had to convey the information, pairs
engaged in more LREs, though no difference occurred in how these were
resolved. These pairs also demonstrated higher mutuality, which refers to
collaborative or expert/novice dynamics thought to be beneficial for L2 learning
(see Storch, 2002). Roles were also the focus of Le’s (2021) study, in which
university students joined an academic reading circle in a different role each
week: leader, notetaker, luminary (responsible for teaching words from the
text), or contextualizer (responsible for making connections based on the
text). Analyses showed how students used their roles to orient to performance
and problematize interactions. Suggestions for minimizing the challenges of
certain roles included simplifying or modeling the task, as well as checking to
ensure progress.
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
• Age and maturity influence children’s social worlds and their interactions.
Tasks should be designed specifically for children in order to sustain their
interest. Younger learners engage in play and conflict more than older peers.
• Proficiency will naturally influence the amount of L2 input, feedback, and
output generated. Mixed proficiency dyads can be effective when the lower
proficiency partner is required to share information and the higher proficiency
partner must request it.
• Roles create expectations about the task which can guide performance and
lead to group management. Teachers can assist by assigning or helping
learners choose roles, modeling them, and checking that they are carried
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
out efficiently.
4.5 Repetition
Unlike the areas reviewed up to this point, task repetition is a relatively recent
area of empirical interest. Bygate (2018) presented the first edited collection
covering the topic. In the editor’s introductory chapter, it is pointed out that
46 Language Teaching
a task can never be precisely repeated. There will always be at least some slight
variation in a learner’s performance owing to the fluctuating psychological and
social conditions under which it is carried out. For this reason, Larsen-Freeman
(2018) proposed replacing the term repetition with iteration, in order to better
convey that the rationale for doing tasks again is, indeed, to promote change and
L2 development, rather than to have learners reproduce their speech or writing
verbatim.
Bygate (2018) also helpfully summarized early research on task repetition,
drawing three conclusions from a spate of studies that began in the early 1990s.
Specifically, all studies had found some significant differences based on repeat-
ing tasks. It was found that these effects occurred regardless of age or profi-
ciency. And it was not possible to predict which of three dimensions of L2
production (complexity, accuracy, or fluency) would mainly be affected by
repetition. There is thus an ongoing need for research into this key area of
TBLT, which can have practical implications. To illustrate how much ground
there is to cover, Bygate also identified various types of partial repetition
occurring in educational contexts: tasks can hold some feature (such as the
design or material) constant, but vary in terms of the interlocutor, the arrange-
ment of the material, the number of contents, the response, or the mode, and so
forth. For example, a map gap task can be redone with students navigating
different routes or locations, addressing different partners, using maps of the
same location at a different scale, adding obstacles to the path, or reducing the
time allowed, and so on. The studies summarized in Table 6 were recent
attempts to address the panoply of ways that tasks can be repeated.
The first two studies in Table 6 used monologic tasks. Ahmadian and Tavakoli
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
(2011) compared four conditions based on planning type (careful vs. pressured)
and repetition (with vs. without). In the repetition conditions, the task was
repeated after one week. It was found that careful planning in combination
with repetition led to improved performance on measures of complexity, accur-
acy, and fluency. Thai and Boers (2016) compared learners who repeated
monologues in a constant time group (2/2/2 minutes) with a shrinking time
group (3/2/1 minutes). Despite the popularity of 3/2/1-type activities among
practitioners, this study revealed that although the shrinking time condition
benefitted fluency, it also seemed to inhibit planning to enhance complexity
and accuracy, which was evident under the constant time condition. The remain-
ing three studies used interactive tasks. Kim and Tracy-Ventura (2013) looked at
the differences between exact repetition (same procedure, same content) and
procedural repetition (same procedure, new content). Based on three iterations
spanning one week, they reported few significant differences between these
groups on measures of complexity, fluency, and accuracy. Procedural repeaters
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
• Monologic task repetition can benefit fluency, and these benefits may also
extend to complexity and accuracy when careful online planning is imple-
mented, such as in constant time conditions.
• Learners should be informed of the goal of repetition (e.g., improving
multiple dimensions of speech production) so as to draw their attention to it
during subsequent iterations.
• Interactive task repetition seems to yield benefits such as increased accuracy
and decreased reliance on L1, though any differences between exact and
procedural conditions remain somewhat unclear.
• Repeating tasks with different partners or audiences can help learners focus
on their delivery and alleviate any potential boredom associated with
repetition.
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
• More natural settings for repetition, such as the poster carousel task, offer
many social learning affordances.
was carefully described (see Tables 2–6). The authors did not merely theorize
and specify workplans. They also discussed excerpts of learner production or
interaction, highlighting the nature of the task-in-process. Nonetheless, it could
also be argued that space limitations imposed by academic journals may, at
times, discourage authors from offering detailed descriptions of the links
between learner’s needs, the tasks they research, and the wider curriculum
context. Furthermore, the value of TBLT sometimes seems in danger of being
obscured by technical discourse, which these studies contained.
Third, as for relevance, in many instances, the studies generated knowledge
relevant to understanding teaching. They show that the research often targets
areas of potentially overlapping interest to teachers and researchers (Spada,
2022). For instance, based on the bulleted summaries in each subsection in
Section 4, instructors could develop ideas about how to plan sequences of tasks,
guide performance using different modes, support learner preparation, match
students for group and pair work, or repeat tasks. The work summarized offers
practical insight drawn from a range of geographic locations and educational
50 Language Teaching
settings, with a focus on the needs of adult, and to a lesser extent, child L2
learners.
Thanks to an increasingly active research community, it is even truer now
than when first asserted that “TBLT is the closest thing to a researched language
pedagogy that exists” (Long, 2015, p. 343). Nevertheless, there is no guarantee
that research on tasks will always be understandable and meaningful to educa-
tors. Furthermore, for all the information it provides, in the classroom, a thriving
literature is no substitute for teacher expertise, gained through personal encoun-
ters with target and pedagogic tasks. TBLT research seems ideally positioned to
achieve its aims when it is located in schools in which teachers and researchers
are similarly invested, its realization is a process imbued with ethical values
shared by local communities, and its findings represent a dialogue about what
we want and need to know concerning language education in particular settings.
Thus, close attention has also been paid to teachers and tasks.
these studies is that they did not investigate the impact of these interventions
over the long-term (but see East, in press, which presents a longer-term per-
spective on preservice teacher education and TBLT).
Research has also been undertaken with in-service teachers. These studies
highlight the diversity of approaches to preparing teachers to create and/or use
tasks. Such approaches have included short-term intensive training programs
(Bryfonski, 2021), action research done in conjunction with task-based lessons
(Zhu, 2020), courses and workshops using loop approaches wherein teachers
experience and reflect on L2 tasks themselves (Hall, 2015; Sherris et al., 2013),
exploratory practice to better understand the competencies supporting teacher-
led focus on form (Müller-Hartmann & Schocker, 2018), and government-
sponsored faculty development programs (Cozonac, 2004). In one detailed
study, Bryfonski (2021, p. 16) reported “varied success in TBLT implementa-
tion after training” among first-year teachers assigned to English-Spanish
bilingual schools in Honduras. Lessons prepared by these teachers used input
52 Language Teaching
elaboration, but were not highly individualized. Teachers viewed their lessons
as promoting cooperative/collaborative learning, though not as providing nega-
tive feedback. In other words, these teachers partly succeeded in putting Long’s
(2015) methodological principles into practice. The author concluded by
recommending that more attention be paid to teachers’ prior experience and
more time be spent on educating teachers about principles that seem challenging
to implement.
construct a series of plans, spanning their lesson plan, their dynamic or in-class
plans, and their retrospective plan, which can be used in future teaching. Based on
these studies, effective instructors possess a sophisticated communicative reper-
toire, responsiveness to contingencies arising during tasks, and a forward-looking
orientation to their role as teacher based on reflective practice. For a recent study
with teachers that sought to bridge an in-service program with classroom imple-
mentation, see Erlam and Tolosa’s (2022) work.
between language users, the choices inherent in spoken versus written language,
and the influence of various resources, including technology, on meaning-
making. In the classroom, having learners adopt specific roles during tasks
builds their pragmatic knowledge.
Some of the processes in Table 7 were mentioned earlier (e.g., noticing,
repair), whereas others constitute areas for future study. In short, tasks transform
classrooms by creating contexts for primarily student-centered learning pro-
cesses. This consequently alters the teacher’s role, which emphasizes their
noticing (Jackson, 2021) of learners’ cognitive, behavioral, social, and emo-
tional engagement (Philp & Duchesne, 2016). Teachers in task-based settings
should try to notice and promote student involvement.
Samuda & Bygate, 2008, p. 60), its contribution to language programs can be
substantial (Long & Norris, 2000; Markee, 1997; Norris, 2009, 2015). At this
scale, TBLT cannot be understood without attention to complex social realities
(Byrnes, 2019). The types of questions then raised can be partly illustrated by
referring back to the curriculum framework introduced in Section 2:
1. Needs analysis – What sources and methods are used to determine needs?
How do past, present, and future students inform program developers’
understandings of relevant target tasks?
2. Task selection and sequencing – In what way(s) are pedagogic tasks organ-
ized across the curriculum and within courses? To what extent does their
arrangement promote ongoing learner development?
3. Materials development – Does the program rely on in-house or commercial
materials and are these resources task-based? Which materials may benefit
from further development?
4. Teaching – What qualifications, expertise, and support are needed for
teachers to engage in task-based instruction? How are teachers made
aware of the program’s alignment with TBLT?
5. Assessment – What roles do formative and summative assessment play?
Does the program utilize task-based language assessment and, if so, how
does it communicate its outcomes?
6. Evaluation – How, by whom, and for what purposes is each of the afore-
mentioned components evaluated? And how might evaluation improve the
program overall?
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
(da Silva, 2020). The affinity between task-based and critical perspectives also
extends to teacher education, as described by Vieira (2017), whose study of two
beginning teachers revealed how tasks enabled them to promote student choice,
authentic language use, and engagement with issues, including democratic
participation. She described the use of tasks by these teachers as providing
space “between reality and ideals”, where “possibilities for transformation are
explored” (Vieira, 2017, p. 711).
Seeking to align TBLT with critical language pedagogy links it to democracy
and raises questions of when and how it might enact those principles (Crookes,
2021). From this perspective, one might ask whether a given implementation of
tasks reflects the following democratic values:
Successful TBLT might address needs less fundamental than those expressed
above. Nonetheless, unlike some language teaching approaches, TBLT can
nurture an environment conducive to them, if carried out with an understanding
of – and a commitment to – its learner-centered and communicative nature. By
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
480–495. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168816654169.
Baralt, M., Gilabert, R., & Robinson, P. (2014). Task Sequencing and Instructed
Second Language Learning. London: Bloomsbury.
Baralt, M., Gurzynski-Weiss, L., & Kim, Y. (2016). Engagement with the
language: How examining learners’ affective and social engagement
explains successful learner-generated attention to form. In M. Sato &
S. Ballinger (Eds.), Peer Interaction and Second Language Learning:
Pedagogical Potential and Research Agenda (pp. 209–239). Amsterdam:
John Benjamins.
Baralt, M., Harmath-de Lemos, S., & Werfelli, S. (2014). Teachers’ application
of the Cognition Hypothesis when lesson planning: A case study. In
M. Baralt, R. Gilabert, & P. Robinson (Eds.), Task Sequencing and
Instructed Second Language Learning (pp. 179–206). London: Bloomsbury.
Behney, J., & Gass, S. (2021). Interaction. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
References 59
Gilabert, R., Manchón, R., & Vasylets, O. (2016). Mode in theoretical and empirical
TBLT research: Advancing research agendas. Annual Review of Applied
Linguistics, 36, 117–135. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0267190515000112.
Gokool, R., & Visser, M. (2021). IsiZulu task-based syllabus for medical
students: Grading and sequencing doctor-patient communication tasks.
South African Journal of African Languages, 41(2), 149–159. https://doi
.org/10.1080/02572117.2021.1948214.
Gurzynski-Weiss, L. (2016). Spanish instructors’ operationalization of task com-
plexity and task sequencing in foreign language lessons. The Language Learning
Journal, 44(4), 467–486. https://doi.org/10.1080/09571736.2015.1015151.
Gurzynski-Weiss, L. (2017). L2 instructor individual characteristics. In
S. Loewen & M. Sato (Eds.), The Routledge Handbook of Instructed
Second Language Acquisition (pp. 451–467). New York: Routledge.
Gurzynski-Weiss, L. (2021). Let’s talk tasks: A conversation between task-
based researchers, language teachers, and teacher trainers. TASK, 1(1),
138–149. https://doi.org/10.1075/task.00012.gur.
64 References
Gurzynski-Weiss, L., & IATBLT (n.d.). The TBLT Language Learning Task
Bank. https://tblt.indiana.edu
Jackson, D. O. (2011). Convergent and divergent computer-mediated commu-
nication tasks in an English for academic purposes course. TESL-EJ, 15(3),
1–18. http://tesl-ej.org/pdf/ej59/a1.pdf.
Jackson, D. O. (2012). Task-based language teacher education in an under-
graduate program in Japan. In A. Shehadeh & C. A. Coombe (Eds.), Task-
Based Language Teaching in Foreign Language Contexts: Research and
Implementation (pp. 267–285). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Jackson, D. O. (2018). Teacher autonomy. In J. Liontas & M. DelliCarpini
(Eds.), TESOL Encyclopedia of English Language Teaching (pp. 1–6).
Hoboken: Wiley.
Jackson, D. O. (2021). Language Teacher Noticing in Tasks. Bristol:
Multilingual Matters.
Jackson, D. O., & Burch, A. R. (2017). Complementary theoretical perspectives
on task-based classroom realities. TESOL Quarterly, 51(3), 493–506. https://
doi.org/10.1002/tesq.393.
Jackson, D. O., & Maie, R. (In press). Implicit statistical learning and second
language outcomes: A Bayesian meta-analysis. In Z. E. Wen, P. Skehan, &
R. Sparks (Eds.), Language Aptitude Theory and Practice. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Jackson, D. O., & Shirakawa, T. (2020). Identity, noticing, and emotion among
pre-service English language teachers. In B. Yazan & K. Lindahl (Eds.),
Language Teacher Identity in TESOL: Teacher Education and Practice as
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press
Long, M. H., & Ahmadian, M. J. (2022). Preface: The origins and growth of
task-based language teaching. In M. J. Ahmadian & M. H. Long (Eds.), The
Cambridge Handbook of Task-Based Language Teaching (pp. xxv–xxxii).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Long, M., & Crookes, G. (1992). Three approaches to task-based syllabus
design. TESOL Quarterly, 26(1), 27–56. https://doi.org/10.2307/3587368.
Long, M. H., & Norris, J. M. (2000). Task-based teaching and assessment. In
M. Byram (Ed.), Routledge Encyclopedia of Language Teaching and
Learning (pp. 597–603). London: Routledge.
Long, M. H., & Robinson, P. (1998). Focus on form: Theory, research, and
practice. In C. Doughty & J. Williams (Eds.), Focus on Form in Classroom
Second Language Acquisition (pp. 15–41). Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Mackey, A. (Ed.). (2007). Conversational Interaction in Second Language
Acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
References 67
Norris, J. M., Brown, J. D., Hudson, T., & Yoshioka, J. (1998). Designing
Second Language Performance Assessments. Honolulu: University of
Hawai‘i, Second Language Teaching & Curriculum Center.
Nunan, D. (1989). Designing Tasks for the Communicative Classroom.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Ogilvie, G., & Dunn, W. (2010). Taking teacher education to task: Exploring the
role of teacher education in promoting the utilization of task-based language
teaching. Language Teaching Research, 14(2), 161–181. https://doi.org/10
.1177/1362168809353875.
Oliver, R. (2020). Developing authentic tasks for the workplace using needs
analysis: A case study of Australian Aboriginal vocational students. In
C. Lambert & R. Oliver (Eds.), Using Tasks in Second Language Teaching:
Practice in Diverse Contexts (pp. 146–61). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Oliver, R., & Bogachenko, T. (2018). Teacher perceptions and use of tasks in
school ESL classrooms. In V. Samuda, K. Van den Branden, & M. Bygate
References 69
Philp, J., Adams, R., & Iwashita, N. (2013). Peer Interaction and Second
Language Learning. New York: Routledge.
Phung, L., Nakamura, S., & Reinders, H. (2021). The effect of choice on
affective engagement: Implications for task design. In P. Hiver, A. H. Al-
Hoorie, & S. Mercer (Eds.), Student Engagement in the Language Classroom
(pp. 163–181). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Pica, T. (1994). Research on negotiation: What does it reveal about
second-language learning conditions, processes, and outcomes? Language
Learning, 44(3), 493–527. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-1770.1994.tb01115.x.
Pica, T., Kanagy, R., & Falodun, J. (1993). Choosing and using communication
tasks for second language instruction and research. In S. Crookes &
S. M. Gass (Eds.), Tasks and Language Learning: Integrating Theory and
Practice (pp. 9–34). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Pica, T., Kang, H. S., & Sauro, S. (2006). Information gap tasks: Their
multiple roles and contributions to interaction research methodology.
70 References
Van den Branden, K., & Van Gorp, K. (2021). Implementing task-based lan-
guage education in primary education: Lessons learnt from the Flemish
experience. Language Teaching for Young Learners, 3(1), 3–27. https://doi
.org/10.1075/ltyl.20013.bra.
Van den Branden, K., Van Gorp, K., & Verhelst, M. (2007). Tasks in Action:
Task-Based Language Education from a Classroom-Based Perspective.
Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars.
Vandommele, G., Van den Branden, K., & Van Gorp, K. (2018). Task-based
language teaching: How task-based is it really? In V. Samuda, K. Van
den Branden, & M. Bygate (Eds.), TBLT as a Researched Pedagogy
(pp. 165–97). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Vasylets, O., Gilabert, R., & Manchón, R. M. (2017). The effects of mode and
task complexity on second language production. Language Learning, 67(2),
394–430. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12228.
Vieira, F. (2017). Task-based instruction for autonomy: Connections with
contexts of practice, conceptions of teaching, and professional development
74 References
Heath Rose
Linacre College, University of Oxford
Heath Rose is an Associate Professor of Applied Linguistics at the University of Oxford. At
Oxford, he is course director of the MSc in Applied Linguistics for Language Teaching.
Before moving into academia, Heath worked as a language teacher in Australia and Japan in
both school and university contexts. He is author of numerous books, such as Introducing
Global Englishes, The Japanese Writing System, Data Collection Research Methods in Applied
Linguistics, and Global Englishes for Language Teaching. Heath’s research interests are firmly
situated within the field of second language teaching, and includes work on Global
Englishes, teaching English as an international language, and English Medium Instruction.
Jim McKinley
University College London
Jim McKinley is an Associate Professor of Applied Linguistics and TESOL at UCL, Institute of
Education, where he serves as Academic Head of Learning and Teaching. His major research
areas are second language writing in global contexts, the internationalisation of higher
education, and the relationship between teaching and research. Jim has edited or authored
numerous books including the Routledge Handbook of Research Methods in Applied
Linguistics, Data Collection Research Methods in Applied Linguistics, and Doing Research in
Applied Linguistics. He is also an editor of the journal System. Before moving into academia,
Jim taught in a range of diverse contexts including the US, Australia, Japan, and Uganda.
Advisory Board
Brian Paltridge, University of Sydney
Gary Barkhuizen, University of Auckland
https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009067973 Published online by Cambridge University Press