Financial Socialization-Financial Behavior - Dimension
Financial Socialization-Financial Behavior - Dimension
by
AN ABSTRACT OF A DISSERTATION
DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY
2018
Abstract
College is a time when many young adults are beginning to make financial decisions on
their own. The financial behaviors they engage in can have effects on their academic success, life
satisfaction, relationship quality, physical and mental well-being, and financial well-being. This
dissertation examined the direct and indirect relationships between financial socialization,
financial knowledge, financial self-efficacy, and financial behaviors in college students using
data from the 2014 National Student Financial Wellness Study (NSFWS). The sample consisted
of 12,598 college students from 52 college institutions. Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) was
conducted with the tested model guided by Gudmunson and Danes’ (2014) Family Financial
socialization and financial behaviors through its association with financial self-efficacy was also
found. Alternative models discovered neither parental financial socialization nor formal financial
education alone impacted financial knowledge, but when combined, their influence became
significant, suggesting a possible interaction effect between formal financial education and
parental financial socialization. Objective financial knowledge was not found to influence
financial self-efficacy or financial behaviors in college students. Results showed financial self-
efficacy to be the strongest predictor of students engaging in positive financial behaviors. A one
standard deviation increase in financial self-efficacy was associated with a 90% increase in the
Financial counselors, advisors, and therapists can use these findings to educate their clients on
the importance of financial socialization of their children. Furthermore, results reinforce the need
for mandatory formal financial education and infer the importance of parents and educators
by
A DISSERTATION
DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY
2018
College is a time when many young adults are beginning to make financial decisions on
their own. The financial behaviors they engage in can have effects on their academic success, life
satisfaction, relationship quality, physical and mental well-being, and financial well-being. This
dissertation examined the direct and indirect relationships between financial socialization,
financial knowledge, financial self-efficacy, and financial behaviors in college students using
data from the 2014 National Student Financial Wellness Study (NSFWS). The sample consisted
of 12,598 college students from 52 college institutions. Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) was
conducted with the tested model guided by Gudmunson and Danes’ (2014) Family Financial
socialization and financial behaviors through its association with financial self-efficacy was also
found. Alternative models discovered neither parental financial socialization nor formal financial
education alone impacted financial knowledge, but when combined, their influence became
significant, suggesting a possible interaction effect between formal financial education and
parental financial socialization. Objective financial knowledge was not found to influence
financial self-efficacy or financial behaviors in college students. Results showed financial self-
efficacy to be the strongest predictor of students engaging in positive financial behaviors. A one
standard deviation increase in financial self-efficacy was associated with a 90% increase in the
Financial counselors, advisors, and financial therapists can use these findings to educate their
reinforce the need for mandatory formal financial education and infer the importance of parents
viii
Demographics/Characteristics .......................................................................................... 31
Chapter 4 - Results ........................................................................................................................ 35
Descriptive Statistics................................................................................................................. 35
Confirmatory Factor Analysis .................................................................................................. 42
Model Fit ............................................................................................................................... 45
Structural Model Results....................................................................................................... 45
Direct Effects .................................................................................................................... 48
Indirect Effects .................................................................................................................. 50
Alternative Models............................................................................................................ 50
Chapter 5 - Discussion .................................................................................................................. 53
Financial Socialization .............................................................................................................. 53
Demographics and Financial Socialization ........................................................................... 53
Financial Socialization and Financial Knowledge ................................................................ 55
Financial Socialization and Financial Self-Efficacy ............................................................. 56
Financial Socialization and Financial Behavior ................................................................... 57
Financial Knowledge ................................................................................................................ 58
Financial Knowledge and Financial Self-Efficacy ............................................................... 58
Financial Knowledge and Financial Behavior ...................................................................... 59
Financial Self-Efficacy ............................................................................................................. 59
Financial Self-Efficacy and Financial Behavior ................................................................... 59
Family Financial Socialization Conceptual Model ................................................................... 60
Contributions ............................................................................................................................ 61
Implications .............................................................................................................................. 62
Strengths and Limitations ......................................................................................................... 65
Future Research ........................................................................................................................ 66
References ..................................................................................................................................... 68
Appendix A - Supplemental Tables and Figures .......................................................................... 75
Appendix B - SAS and Mplus Code ............................................................................................. 86
ix
List of Figures
x
List of Tables
xi
Acknowledgements
They say “A journey of a thousand miles begins with a single step” and this degree has
definitely felt like a thousand mile journey! And I could not have completed this part of my
journey without the help of many kind souls. So I must thank God for putting these people in my
life and for helping me achieve this dream. Thank you God, my Lord and Saviour!
Stu Heckman, thank you for your guidance on this dissertation and other research
projects. Your unique perspective and vast knowledge has always had me in awe. Sonya Lutter
and Kristy Archuleta, you both have been such an inspiration to me, personally and
professionally. It has been an honor to learn from such talented researchers and to have you both
as role models. Catherine Montalto, at The Ohio State University, thank you for being my “grand
advisor” and allowing me to work with your data. I appreciate the suggestions and resources you
provided. Christy Craft, thank you for being a part of my committee. I enjoyed getting to know
more about your research and appreciate your thoughtful feedback. Tierra Dimond, thank you for
being a great advisor and making things seem less daunting. Martin Seay and Morey
MacDonald, you both have provided me with wonderful words of wisdom along the way, and
I’m grateful you are a part of my journey. I have learned so much from all of the faculty at
To my crazy cohort members: Kate (the craziest of them all), David, Julie, Ben, Vincent,
and Dennis, thank you for your friendship and support over these past four years. United we
stand (and eventually graduate)! Derek Lawson, you have been such a great friend and peer in
this experience. I don’t know what I would have done without your guidance on SEM! Sarah
Asebedo, thank you for cheering me on and hooking me up with Stats Camp to learn SEM. You
xii
To Dawn and Phil Anderson at the Anderson Bed and Breakfast, I could not have
survived my summers at “boot camp” without the comforts of home you provided. You have
become my Manhattan family and home away from home. If only those chocolate chip cookies
I must also thank my friends and colleagues at Fort Hays State University (especially
Nanette Fitzhugh, Rachel Dolecheck, and Linda Kepka) for listening to me rejoice and complain
and encouraging me along the way. I also can’t forget to thank the professor whose stories and
teaching planted the seed that made me want to get my doctorate, Micol Maughan. Micol, I love
you!
And last, but certainly not least, thank you Mom and Bob! Without you guys, this
wouldn’t be possible.
xiii
Dedication
To quote my favorite president, Abraham Lincoln, “All that I am, or hope to be, I owe to
my angel mother.” For that reason, this dissertation is dedicated to my amazing mother, Elaine,
whose mental, spiritual, and financial support over the years has allowed me to pursue my
dreams. Thank you, Mom, for all you have done for me. My love for you is unmeasurable.
xiv
Chapter 1 - Introduction
College is a time when many young adults are leaving home for the first time, meeting
new people, and trying to establish the delicate balance of school, friends, work, and finances. As
college students make their way into the adult world, they find themselves facing new financial
challenges – paying rent and bills, managing student loan debt, applying for credit cards, and
establishing financial behaviors that they may continue as they grow older. The financial
behaviors these college students engage in can profoundly impact their well-being. Positive
financial behaviors have been linked to higher GPA, greater academic satisfaction, and greater
life satisfaction (Xiao, Tang, & Shim, 2009). Financial behavior is fundamental to financial well-
being (Brüggen, Hogreve, Holmlund, Kabadayi, & Löfgren, 2017; Gutter & Copur, 2011), with
financial well-being in turn being positively associated with psychological and physical health
(Shim, Xiao, Barber, & Lyons, 2010), relationship quality (Dew & Xiao, 2013), and overall life
satisfaction (Netemeyer, Warmeth, Fernandes, & Lynch, 2018). Engaging in positive financial
behaviors is associated with lower financial stress, higher subjective well-being, and lower
With financial behavior having such a strong influence on many areas of well-being,
teaching children to manage their finances should be a major priority for parents. Yet nearly 52%
of parents reported reluctance to discuss financial matters with their children, and only 33% of
parents reported discussing financial topics with their kids once a week or more (T. Rowe Price,
2016). Parents may be unsure how to teach their children to engage in positive financial
behaviors or which financial socialization practices are effective. Family financial socialization
is the process by which children acquire and develop financial knowledge, skills, attitudes, and
behaviors over time (Gudmunson & Danes, 2011). Parents are often considered the primary
1
socialization agent for their children. Financial socialization can result from conversations
parents have with their children about money, children observing their parents’ financial
behaviors, formal education at school, working at a job, and through direct experience with
“Whether you think you can or you cannot, you are right”. First postulated by Albert Bandura
their ability to produce a desired result. The greater one’s self-efficacy, the more likely they will
attempt a behavior, as well as persist in the face of difficulties (Bandura, 1977). Financial self-
efficacy is domain specific, reflecting confidence in one’s ability to manage their personal
finances. Studies have found individuals with higher levels of financial self-efficacy experience
lower levels of financial stress (Lapp, 2010), improved financial behavior, and greater financial
well-being (Serido, Shim, & Tang, 2013). Furthermore, the influence of self-efficacy appears to
be enduring. In a longitudinal study conducted over nine years, higher levels of economic self-
efficacy in high school students was associated with increased odds of completing college by age
24, higher income in young adulthood, and early adult financial independence (Lee & Mortimer,
2009). Despite the positive influences of having greater financial self-efficacy, limited research
has been conducted to determine the antecedents of financial self-efficacy. This study seeks to
add to the literature by examining the direct and indirect effects of financial socialization on
financial self-efficacy, financial knowledge, and financial behaviors. (For those not familiar with
2
The current study tests the family financial socialization model developed by Gudmunson
and Danes (2011), which suggests implicit and explicit socialization directly influence financial
capabilities (Gudmunson & Danes). Financial behaviors are thought to be directly influenced by
modeling was used to examine the association between financial socialization, financial self-
efficacy, financial knowledge, and financial behaviors in college students using data from the
Research Questions
Following the paths for the revised financial socialization conceptual model shown in
Figure 1.1, this study sought to answer the following research questions:
R1: What is the relationship between financial socialization and objective financial knowledge?
R2: What is the relationship between financial socialization and financial self-efficacy?
R3: What is the relationship between financial socialization and financial behaviors?
R4: Is there an indirect relationship between financial socialization and financial behaviors
R5: Is there an indirect relationship between financial socialization and financial behaviors
R6: What is the relationship between financial knowledge and financial self-efficacy?
R7: What is the relationship between financial knowledge and financial behaviors?
R8: What is the relationship between financial self-efficacy and financial behaviors?
3
Figure 1.1 Revised Family Financial Socialization Conceptual Model
Conceptual Framework
Financial literacy research conducted over the last 40 years has used a variety of
management, and systems theories to explore the variables related to financial behaviors
(Gudmunson & Danes, 2011). These studies have primarily focused on the individual as the unit
of analysis, without consideration of how family has influenced the individual through
socialization (Gudmunson & Danes). Through their examination of over 100 research articles,
Gudmunson and Danes created the family financial socialization (FFS) conceptual model to
outline the relationship between family socialization processes and the financial socialization
outcomes of financial attitudes, knowledge, capabilities, behavior, and well-being. The current
study utilizes the FFS conceptual model to examine if explicit and implicit socialization
4
processes influence financial self-efficacy in college students, as self-efficacy is considered to be
a motivation source for a person to attempt what they are capable of doing (Danes & Yang,
2014), and also for behavior change (Bandura, 1977). Further analysis was conducted to explore
the associations between financial socialization, financial self-efficacy, financial knowledge, and
The FFS model proposes using the demographic variables of gender, age, and race, and
family characteristics, such as family size and socioeconomic status, as predictors of financial
outcomes through their association with family socialization processes. Since the exact nature of
these relationships is unknown (Danes & Yang, 2014; Gudmunson & Danes, 2011) and not the
focus of the present study, demographics were treated as control variables for the purpose of this
paper.
Family interactions and relationships are representative of family dynamics and implicit
socialization processes. Families interact as a system, with individual behaviors influencing and
eliciting feedback from others in the family system. Communication and relationship quality
impact how implicit and explicit messages are received. Implicit financial socialization occurs
from children observing parent financial behaviors and daily interactions. Explicit financial
socialization involves purposive efforts to teach, model, and practice financial knowledge and
behaviors. Purposive financial socialization can occur bi-directionally, and within any family
relationship, not just from parent to child (Danes & Yang, 2014)
Family interactions and relationships are evaluated separately from purposive financial
socialization in Gudmunson and Danes (2011) model, in order to gain a greater understanding of
how these constructs influence attitudes, knowledge, and capabilities. Capabilities define what an
individual is able to do, rather than what is done proficiently, and also includes internal sources
5
of motivation such as self-efficacy, values, perceived needs, and living standards. Self-efficacy
may explain variations in what individuals of similar circumstances are capable of achieving.
Through its interaction with knowledge, self-efficacy may help produce behavior change
In the FFS model, financial attitudes, knowledge, and capabilities influence financial
behavior and financial well-being. The financial behavior construct consists of two types of
financial behaviors. The first type of financial behaviors represents patterns of action over time.
The second type of behaviors is related to decision making and financial turning points, such as
setting up a retirement account, or establishing automatic savings from one’s paycheck. The
relationship between financial attitudes, knowledge, and capabilities and financial behaviors and
well-being requires further research to determine if the influences are enhancing or constraining.
objective measures, such as income, savings, net worth, asset accumulation, and financial ratios,
as well as subjective measures, such as financial satisfaction (Danes & Yang, 2014). Because
most college students have not yet had time to acquire many assets, and many are acquiring
student loans, any objective evaluation of financial well-being in college students would be
futile, and therefore, is not included in this study. Instead the focus is on the financial behaviors
of college students, with the presumption that they may continue their financial behaviors into
adulthood.
The present study examines the influence of implicit and explicit socialization agents,
such as direct teaching about money, parents as financial role models, and formal education on
financial self-efficacy, financial knowledge, and financial behaviors in college students. As self-
6
socialization processes will be positively associated with financial self-efficacy. As parents and
educators socialize children on positive financial norms, confidence in one’s ability to manage
their finances should increase. It is posited higher financial self-efficacy will result in
performance of a greater number of positive financial behaviors. By using the FFS conceptual
model, the present study provides an opportunity to test an emerging theory specifically
Hypotheses
Based on the Family Financial Socialization conceptual model, the following hypotheses
will be tested:
H3: Financial socialization will have a positive and direct relationship with financial behaviors.
H4: Financial socialization will have an indirect relationship with financial behavior through
financial self-efficacy.
H5: Financial socialization will have an indirect relationship with financial behavior through
financial knowledge.
7
Chapter 2 - Review of Literature
An indirect link between financial socialization and financial self-efficacy has been
demonstrated through its association with financial knowledge—financial socialization has been
shown to influence financial knowledge (Grohmann, Kouwenberg, & Menkoff, 2015; Shim,
Barber, Card, Xiao, & Serido, 2009; Shim et al., 2009) and others have shown that financial
knowledge influences financial self-efficacy (Danes, Huddleston-Casas, & Boyce, 1999; Lapp,
2010; Sanders, Weaver, & Schnabel, 2007; Serido et al., 2013). Both financial knowledge and
self-efficacy have a positive relationship with financial behaviors (Bandura, 1977; Babiarz &
Robb, 2013; Henager & Cude, 2016; Lown, Kim, Gutter, & Hunt, 2015; Montford & Goldsmith,
2016). Socialization tends to influence financial behaviors, though results vary when examining
specific socialization activities (Jorgensen, Rappleyea, Schweichler, Fang, & Moran, 2017; Kim
& Chatterjee, 2013; Kim, LaTaillade, & Kim, 2011; Serido et al., 2010). The following literature
review provides support for the need for further study on the influence of financial socialization
Financial Socialization
Parents are commonly considered the primary socialization agents for their children
(Gudmunson & Danes, 2011). With regard to finances, parents socialize their children by
modeling consumer behaviors, establishing the rules and norms for their children’s financial
behaviors, and directly discussing financial related topics with their children (Allen, 2010). By
encouraging their children to open a bank account, save, and/or invest money, parents are
providing direct teaching of their financial attitudes and beliefs. Parents sometimes pay their
8
children an allowance as a way for the child to learn how to manage their own money (Hira,
1997). Financial socialization can also occur through formal financial education, such as a class
or workshop in high school and/or college, as well as experience gained from working and
earning money. According to the FFS model, purposive financial socialization can either
promote or inhibit financial knowledge, attitudes, and capabilities (Danes & Yang, 2014).
parental financial teaching, parental financial behavior, high school financial education, and high
school work experience on financial knowledge (Shim et al., 2010). Financial knowledge was
modeled as a common latent construct, using both objective and subjective measures of
knowledge. A single item question asked students to rate their overall understanding of money
questions provided the measurement for objective financial knowledge. Results of the study
found direct parental teaching, high school financial education, and high school work experience
undergraduate and graduate college students and found parental socialization (measured by
spending, their own spending, and use of credit) and formal financial education were
significantly related to perceived financial knowledge. In this study, gender was also included as
students were significantly less likely to perceive themselves as financial knowledgeable (Shim
et al.). Research has suggested there may be differences in financial socialization between
genders. Garrison and Gutter (2010) found significant differences in social learning opportunities
9
between males and females in regards to finances. Females were found to have more
opportunities to discuss financial matters with their parents and with their peers, and had more
opportunities to observe their parents and their peers engage in positive financial behaviors
(Garrison & Gutter, 2010). Formal financial education, however, may be more beneficial for
females, as suggested by Danes and Haberman (2007) who found when high school students
received formal financial education, the female students had a greater increase in financial
knowledge than the male students. Danes and Haberman attributed this difference to the
possibility that female and male students may experience differences in context and degree of
socialization within the family. As illustrated in Figure 1.1, the FFS model suggests demographic
variables such as gender, age, race, and family income influence socialization processes, which
Other studies have found differences in how explicit and implicit socialization practices
influence financial knowledge (Grohmann et al., 2015; Jorgensen & Savla, 2010). In a study of
instruction on budgeting and saving had a positive relationship to financial literacy, while
socialization through work experience and early money experience had a negative influence on
financial literacy (Grohmann et al., 2015). Jorgensen and Savla (2010) found that perceived
parental influence had a negative association with objective financial knowledge in college
students; however these results were not significant. Using different measurements for
socialization may provide different results. The current study examines parental financial
10
Financial Socialization and Financial Self-Efficacy
personal finances. Using path analysis, Heckman and Grable (2011) examined the direct
relationship of parental debt attitudes, student income, and dependency status on financial
knowledge, and explored the indirect effect of these variables on financial self-efficacy. Data
was collected from a small sample (N = 80) of university students via online and paper survey.
Perceived parental debt attitudes were measured using nine survey items, the sum of which
indicated either positive attitudes (higher scores) or negative attitudes (lower scores) toward
self-efficacy was measured using a single item question asking how strongly the participant
agreed or disagreed with the statement, “I feel confident about making decisions that deal with
money.” A 20-item personal finance quiz measured objective financial knowledge, with the
number of correct items summed, and higher scores reflective of greater financial knowledge.
Results of the study showed students with higher levels of income had greater financial
knowledge and higher financial self-efficacy. Students who relied on their parents financially
showed lower levels of financial knowledge and lower self-efficacy; however, these results were
not significant. A significant positive relationship was found between students with higher
financial knowledge and financial self-efficacy. This lends support for further examination of the
relationship between objective financial knowledge and financial self-efficacy. An indirect path
from financial socialization to financial self-efficacy showed a weak yet insignificant association
between the two. The weakness of the association could be due to the small sample size
11
Studies examining the direct link between financial socialization and financial self-
efficacy have found mixed results. One study followed high school students from their freshmen
year until six years after their scheduled graduation from high school to determine if family
socialization influenced economic self-efficacy (Lee & Mortimer, 2009). Economic self-efficacy
was measured during their senior year of high school by asking respondents to respond to three
questions about how they viewed the future. Questions asked “what are the chances that you will
have a job that pays well?,” “what are the chances you will have a job that you enjoy doing?,”
and “what are the chances you will be able to own your own home?” Economic self-efficacy was
similar between boys and girls, yet girls were more optimistic about having a job they enjoy
doing in the future. A baseline model examined the influence of family background and
academic performance on self-efficacy. Family income and grades were found to have a
significant positive influence on economic self-efficacy in high school. Parents’ education level
also had an influence on the child’s economic self-efficacy. A second model tested the influence
of socialization on self-efficacy. Children whose parents talked with them about their work had
greater economic self-efficacy; however, receiving an allowance resulted in lower levels of self-
efficacy. A third model tested the combined effects of family background and socialization on
economic self-efficacy. Only parent-child discussions related to work and receiving an allowance
were shown to significantly influence economic self-efficacy in the third model, suggesting
background differences and academic performance cannot fully explain differences in self-
The direct link between financial socialization and financial self-efficacy was also
examined in a longitudinal study of college students, conducted during their first and fourth year
of college (Shim, Serido, Tang, & Card, 2015). Perceived parental financial role modeling,
12
perceived parental communication about finances, perceived parental financial expectations,
perceived friends’ financial behaviors, formal classroom learning, and informal self-learning
were analyzed to determine if changes in the socialization agents influenced change in financial
self-efficacy. Only changes in parental communication and changes in formal classroom learning
significantly influenced change in financial self-efficacy (Shim et al., 2015). The results of Shim
and associates’ study are based on changes in socialization agents. It does not address
socialization from a previous point in time. If parents did not change their socialization behaviors
during the four years their child was in college, then it is unlikely it would produce a change in
financial self-efficacy. The present research analyzes socialization practices from a prior point in
Several studies have examined the relationship between socialization and financial
behavior. Financial communication between parents and children has been shown to have a
positive association with budgeting and cash management, credit management, saving and
investment behavior, and long-term planning (Jorgensen et al., 2017). Similar results were found
by Serido et al. (2010) in a study examining financial parenting, coping behaviors, and financial
budget) and proactive financial coping behaviors (saving money and investing in long-term
financial goals; Serido et al.). A study of adults, ages 24-66 with low-and moderate-income, also
Gutter, Kim, & Mauldin, 2012). Respondents whose parents discussed the importance of saving,
13
using credit, and having a spending plan were more likely to report having a spending plan and
written financial goals than respondents whose parents did not have financial discussions with
Parental financial behavior has been linked to children’s financial behavior (Cho et al.,
2012; Webley & Nyhus, 2006). Adult children who categorized their parents as savers were
more likely to engage in financial planning behaviors than those whose parents were not
categorized as savers (Cho et al., 2012). Categorizing one’s parents as savers or non-savers
suggested a level of implicit or explicit socialization, either through perceived financial behavior
or direct financial conversations about saving from the parent. In a Dutch study examining
parental influence on children’s saving behavior, the amount of money saved by parents was
found to be positively related to the amount of money children (ages 16-21) saved (Webley &
Nyhus, 2006).
Webley and Nyhus (2006) also found that financial socialization through means of
financial communication, earning money as a teenager, and being encouraged to have a bank
account was found to be associated with children having a preference for saving left-over money
rather than spending it. Some socialization processes may be more effective than others at
financial socialization and young adult (ages 18-21) financial management found that children
who had a savings account as a child, whose parents monitored their spending, and those who
worked for pay were more likely to have bonds, CD’s, or other non-bank account related liquid
assets (Kim & Chatterjee, 2013). Receiving an allowance has been shown to have mixed results
(Kim & Chatterjee; Kim et al., 2011). In a study examining adolescents’ financial behaviors,
receiving an allowance was not related to having a savings account, nor was it related to saving
14
for future schooling among those who had savings accounts (Kim et al.). Adult children ages 18
to 21 who reported receiving an allowance were more likely to carry a credit card balance;
however, they were also less likely to report financial anxiety and to be fully responsible for
In a meta-analysis of 126 studies, Kaiser and Menkhoff (2017) found financial education
budgeting, saving, and retirement saving; however, there was not a significant difference in
borrowing and debt management, insurance and risk mitigation, or bank account behavior. The
analysis also examined how the setting/type of financial education (classroom versus non-
classroom, online, counseling, and informational nudge) influenced knowledge and behavior.
While classroom learning significantly increased financial knowledge, there were no significant
Financial Knowledge
the influence of financial knowledge. In a study conducted by Serido et al. (2013), college
students were surveyed during their first and fourth year of college to determine if changes in
Subjective financial knowledge was measured using a single-item question asking the respondent
to rate their overall level of understanding of personal finance concepts and practices. Objective
financial knowledge was measured using a 15-item true/false quiz on financial topics. Students
15
showed significant increases in objective and subjective financial knowledge, as well as financial
self-efficacy between their first and fourth year of college. Change in subjective knowledge was
objective financial knowledge did not influence change in self-efficacy. This suggests that what
individuals think they know is more influential on self-efficacy than what they actually know
(Serido et al., 2013). Results for objective financial knowledge could potentially differ,
depending on variable measurement. True/false responses provide for a 50% chance of the
respondent guessing the correct answer, when actually they may not know the answer.
Other studies have examined the influence of financial education on financial knowledge
and financial self-efficacy. Participants of a financial training program were surveyed on their
subjective financial knowledge and financial self-efficacy both before they participated in the
training and one year later (Lapp, 2010). Results showed subjective financial knowledge
significantly increased one year after the training. While the study did not indicate if there was a
results were found in a study of high-school students (Danes et al., 1999). Financial education
was found to significantly increase both subjective financial knowledge and financial self-
efficacy, although the relationship between financial knowledge and financial self-efficacy was
The review of the literature highlights the important difference between objective and
subjective financial knowledge. Objective financial knowledge represents what one actually
knows about personal finance, while subjective financial knowledge represents what one thinks
they know about personal finance. Results of these studies suggest instilling financial self-
16
efficacy might not be a result of actual knowledge, but rather a result of encouragement and
experience, something socialization can provide. The present study examines the relationship
financial knowledge using four multiple choice questions in order to explore if a different
A relationship between financial knowledge and financial behaviors has been identified
in many studies. Both objective and subjective financial knowledge has been found to be
positively associated with having an emergency fund, planning for retirement (Angrisani,
Kapteyen, & Lusardi, 2016; Chatterjee, Fan, Jacobs, & Haas, 2017; de Bassa Scheresberg;
2013), spending less than income, paying credit cards in full (Angrisani et al.); having
investments, and not overdrawing one’s checking account (Henager & Cude, 2016). Objective
financial knowledge has also been found to be negatively associated with high cost borrowing
(de Bassa Scheresberg). In a study examining financial knowledge and best financial practices,
Robb and Woodyard (2011) found a significant correlation between both objective and
subjective financial knowledge and positive financial behaviors. Positive financial behavior was
measured using a scale that summed the number of best practice financial activities performed
out of six possible activities: having an emergency fund to cover 3 months of expenses, checking
one’s credit report in the past 12 months, not overdrawing one’s checking account, having a
retirement account, and having insurance. The study did not examine the activities on an
A two-time period longitudinal study conducted by Serido et al. (2013) investigated the
association between change in financial knowledge over time and change in financial behaviors
17
through a survey of college students. Students were surveyed during their first year of college
and again during their fourth year of college. Objective financial knowledge was measured using
a 15-item true/false quiz. While a change in objective financial knowledge did not have a
between change in subjective knowledge and change in financial behaviors did exist. Financial
behaviors consisted of a scale of six items, including tracking monthly expenses, spending with
their budget, paying credit cards in full each month, saving money each month, and investing for
long-term financial goals. Change in specific behaviors were not examined in the study (Serido
et al.).
Objective financial knowledge has also been found to decrease negative financial
behaviors (Nghia & Scott, 2018; Xiao, Chen, & Chen, 2014). While examining the influence of
financial knowledge and behavior on financial distress, Nghia and Scott (2018) found higher
levels of financial knowledge lowered the likelihood of not saving enough for retirement, and
reduced the probability of making late mortgage payments. Similarly, Xiao, Chen, and Chen
(2014) found a strong negative correlation between objective financial knowledge and negative
financial behaviors, indicating the higher a person’s financial knowledge, the less likely they
were to engage in negative behaviors, such as spending more than income, carrying a credit card
balance, making late credit card or mortgage payments, and taking a 401(k) loan. Xiao and
associates also found objective financial knowledge to be positively correlated with positive
financial behaviors, such as having an emergency fund, calculating retirement needs, requesting
a credit report, and comparing loan offers for autos, mortgages, and/or credit cards. In both
examining knowledge on interest rates, inflation, bond prices, mortgage payments, and stocks.
18
The present study explores the relationship between objective financial knowledge and financial
behaviors, including following a budget, tracking spending, paying bills on time, and saving
regularly.
Financial Self-Efficacy
The relationship between self-efficacy and behavior has been examined from two points
of view. As posited by Albert Bandura (1977), efficacy expectations – whether one believes they
can successfully perform a behavior – determines the amount of effort and persistence an
individual will expend to achieve behavior success. Efficacy expectations are influenced by
previous successful attempts at the behavior, seeing others succeed at the behavior, verbal
persuasion, and emotional arousal (Bandura, 1977). Socialization experiences, such as parental
encouragement to open a bank account, save and invest money, as well as opportunities to
practice money management with income received either from working or as an allowance, may
budgeting, tracking expenses, paying bills on time, and regularly saving could boost financial
Many studies have examined the relationship from self-efficacy to behavior (Asebedo et al.,
2018; Asebedo & Browning, 2017 ; Farrell, Fry, & Risse, 2016; Lapp, 2010; Lown et al., 2015;
Montford & Goldsmith, 2016). Montford and Goldsmith found that higher financial self-efficacy
was associated with willingness to make riskier investment decisions. Women with higher levels
of financial self-efficacy were more likely to hold a mortgage, a savings account, or investments,
and less likely to hold debt in the form of a credit card or loan (Farrell et al.). Self-efficacy has
19
also been shown to have a positive relationship with saving (Asebedo et al.; Lapp; Lown et al.),
lower levels of debt, fewer financial problems (Lapp), and lower portfolio withdrawal rates
(Asebedo & Browning). Gutter, Copur, and Garrison (2009) found that students who used a
budget to avoid overspending, checked their credit report in the last year, and regularly saved
had higher levels of financial self-efficacy than student who did not perform these behaviors.
The FFS model takes the position that socialization influences financial attitudes and capabilities
such as financial self-efficacy, which in turn influences financial behaviors. This study explores
the indirect effect of financial self-efficacy on the relationship between financial socialization
Summary
The current study expands upon the body of literature by exploring the direct and indirect
knowledge, and financial behaviors using the FFS conceptual model. Financial socialization
practices studied include parents talking to their children about money, children observing their
parents’ financial behaviors, formal education at school, working at a job, and direct experience
with money. Financial behaviors explored include following a budget, tracking spending, paying
bills on time, saving regularly, spending more than earned, and purchasing expensive items they
did not need. The family financial socialization model suggests self-efficacy will be influenced
by implicit and explicit socialization practices; however, further research is needed to determine
whether that influence is positive or negative (Danes & Yang, 2014). Objective financial
knowledge was tested as a possible influence on financial self-efficacy. Previous studies have
shown mixed results as to whether or not objective financial knowledge influences self-efficacy
(Heckman & Grable, 2011; Serido et al., 2013). The family financial socialization model
20
suggests both implicit and explicit socialization will influence financial knowledge; however,
more research is needed to determine whether that influence is enhancing or constraining (Danes
According to the FFS model, financial knowledge interacts with self-efficacy to influence
financial capabilities; and financial self-efficacy is posited to directly and indirectly influence
financial behaviors through its association with socialization (Danes & Yang, 2014; Gudmunson
& Danes, 2011). Figure 2.1 shows the revised conceptual model in which the financial
knowledge, attitudes, and capabilities construct was divided into separate constructs for financial
knowledge and financial self-efficacy to reflect the interaction between socialization, financial
directly influence both financial knowledge and financial self-efficacy and through these
financial knowledge will positively influence financial self-efficacy and both financial
21
Figure 2.1 Revised Family Financial Socialization Conceptual Model
22
Chapter 3 - Methods
Data
The current study utilized data from the 2014 National Student Financial Wellness Study
(NSFWS, 2014) to perform structural equation modeling (SEM) on the variables of interest.
Online surveys were administered to a random sample of college students during the fall 2014
and winter 2015 semesters. The response rate for all institutions that participated in the study was
11.5%, with a total of 18,795 responses. The survey asked a total of 11 questions about financial
socialization, allowing for this data to be useful in analyzing explicit and implicit financial
socialization processes. Information was also collected on financial knowledge, financial self-
efficacy, financial behaviors, and personal and family demographics, which provides the
financial self-efficacy and financial behaviors, as well as the appropriate variables associated
The NSFWS data file was prepared using SAS® 9.4 and converted to an Excel comma
separated values file in order for SEM to be conducted with Mplus version 8.0. Due to the use of
latent constructs, SEM was deemed the appropriate method of analysis for this study.
Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted to determine the relationship between items
and the constructs they represent. All factors for constructs were created based on item
Missing Data
In examining the data, a total of 2,390 responses for the 11 financial socialization survey
items were coded “missing-not asked,” as were 4,044 responses for mother’s education, father’s
23
education, respondent’s income, and gender, and 5,635 for GPA. Responses coded as “missing
not asked” are due to respondents not completing the questionnaire and were listwise deleted,
continuous variable with “asked but missing” and “prefer not to answer” responses estimated
using full information maximum likelihood (FIML), categorically for “prefer not answer” and
“answered” responses, and categorically for “do not know” and “answered” to explore if
respondents who prefer not to answer income or do not know income are different than those
who did specify their income range. For the categorical measurements of income, “asked but
missing” responses were placed in a category with the “prefer not to answer” responses. For all
other variables “asked but missing” responses were estimated using the full information
Operationalization of Variables
The following section outlines the operationalization of variables for the current study.
Financial Socialization
The 2014 NSFWS asked a total of 11 questions on financial socialization which will
serve as the measurements for financial socialization. The 11 financial socialization questions
were parceled based on correlations and using the facet method, resulting in four factors to
measure different facets of financial socialization. According to Danes and Yang (2014), family
demonstrated by parents, are considered implicit socialization. For this study, three survey items
were used to represent implicit socialization. Students were asked to rate whether they strongly
24
“Prior to college/university: My parent(s) or guardian(s) were comfortable talking
management.”
If parents were comfortable talking with the student about money, this represents positive family
dynamics and communication. If parents were thought to be good role models of financial
management, this represents the behavior the student observed in the household. Point values
were assigned to responses ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). The implicit
socialization factor had a standardized lambda loading of .51 for the construct of financial
socialization. A standardized lambda represents the amount each indicator or parcel contributes
skills (Danes & Yang, 2014). For this study, explicit socialization was measured using responses
to three survey items. Students were asked to answer “yes” or “no” to the following questions:
Prior to college/university:
Point values were assigned to responses with “no” equal to 0, and “yes” equal to 1 point. The
factor explicit socialization had a standardized lambda loading of .53 on the construct of
financial socialization.
25
A factor measuring financial education was created using responses to two items.
Students were asked if they had attended a personal finance class or workshop (1) while in high
school or (2) while in college. Responses included: (a) no, (b) yes, one-time event(s), or (c) yes,
term long course(s) or repeated sessions. Point values were assigned to responses with “no” = 1
point, “yes, one-time event” = 2 points, and “yes, term long course” = 3 points. The factor
financial education had a standardized lambda loading of .34 on the construct financial
socialization.
A factor measuring experience with money was created using responses the following
three items:
Point values were assigned to responses with “no” = 0, and “yes” = 1 point. The factor money
experience had a standardized lambda loading of .60 on the construct financial socialization. See
Table 3.1 for a full list of items in parcels for all variables. The latent construct financial
socialization has a Cronbach’s alpha of .68, boarding on the acceptable range of .70 or higher,
Financial Knowledge
1) “Imagine that the interest rate on your savings account is 1% per year and
inflation is 2% per year. After 1 year, would you be able to buy more than today,
26
a) More than today
d) Don’t know
2) “Suppose you have $100 in a savings account and the interest rate was 2% per
year. After 5 years, how much would you have in the account if you left the
money to grow?”
b) Exactly $102
d) Don’t know
3) “All paycheck stubs show your gross pay (the total amount you earned before any
taxes were taken out for the pay period) and your net pay (the amount of your
check after all taxes). The taxes that are commonly taken out include federal,
state, and local income tax, Social Security tax, and Medicare tax. On average,
what percentage of your income would you expect to receive as take-home pay?”
a) 100%
b) 99-99%
c) 80-89%
d) 70-79%*
e) Don’t know
4) “Suppose you borrowed $5,000 to help cover college expenses for the coming
year. You can choose to repay this loan over 10 years, 20 years, or 30 years.
27
Which of these repayment options will cost you the least amount of money over
d) Don’t know
a) Amounts owed*
b) New credit
e) Payment history*
f) Don’t know
An exploratory factor analysis of the five items produced one factor with an Eigenvalue
of 1.80 that included all items except for the question on the two components of a credit score.
Removing the credit score question from the scale improved the Cronbach’s alpha from a .53 to
a .57. A confirmatory factor analysis resulted in a low standardized lambda loading of .21 for the
credit score question as well, indicating this item should not be part of the financial knowledge
construct. The present study measured the construct objective financial knowledge using the
remaining four financial knowledge questions (interest, inflation, take-home pay, and loan
Standardized lambda loadings for the remaining four financial knowledge questions are as
28
follows: interest (.61), inflation (.77), take-home pay (.74), loan repayment (.38). While the final
Cronbach’s alpha was low (.57), it will not be problematic with the use of Structural Equation
Modeling (SEM). Little, Lindenberger, and Nesselroade (1999) found that confirmatory analyses
techniques are better suited for fitting measurement models than exploratory analyses techniques,
Financial Self-Efficacy
personal finances. For the present study, financial self-efficacy was measured using responses to
two survey questions. Respondents were asked to rate if they strongly disagree, disagree, agree,
Point values were assigned to responses ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly
agree). The confidence item had a standardized lambda loading of .71, and the manage money
well item had a standardized lambda loading of .66, indicating they are both good measurements
for the construct of financial self-efficacy. The Cronbach’s alpha for the financial self-efficacy
scale is .77, supporting the validity of the construct. With latent constructs that have only two
indicators, Heywood cases may occur. Heywood cases are present when a parameter estimate
has an illogical value, such as a negative variance or are greater than 1.0 (Kline, 2016). Results
were checked and negative variance was not found, indicating this was not an issue.
Financial Behavior
the respondent. For the present study, financial behavior was measured using seven survey items.
29
Three factors were created to measure the construct of financial behavior based on correlations
and using the facet method of parceling. An accounting factor was created using the following
three items. Respondents were asked to rate how often they perform the following activities:
Responses included never, sometimes, frequently, and always. Point values were assigned
Responses included never, sometimes, frequently, and always. Point values were assigned
A factor measuring “lives within means” was also created using responses to two survey
items. Respondents were asked to rate how strongly they agree with the following statements:
“In the past three months, I purchased something expensive that I wanted, but
Responses included strongly disagree, disagree, agree, and strongly agree. As these are
considered negative financial behaviors, the responses were reverse coded, with point values
assigned as follows: strongly disagree equal to 4, disagree equal to 3, agree equal to 2, and
30
A confirmatory factor analysis indicated all parcels belong to the financial behavior
construct, with the following standardized lambda loadings for the factors: accounting (.38),
timeliness (.41), and living with means (.62). The latent construct financial behavior has a
Cronbach’s alpha of .59; however, based on the findings of Little, Lindenberger, and
Nesselroade (1999), exploratory analyses techniques should not be heavily relied upon for
indicator selection in latent constructs, but rather practical and theoretical considerations should
Demographics/Characteristics
In the family financial socialization conceptual model, personal and family characteristics
are thought to influence implicit and explicit socialization (Danes & Yang, 2014). The present
study included the highest level of parent education, respondent’s current annual income, gender,
race, age, grade point average, number of years enrolled in school, and institution type as control
variables. Parent(s)/guardian(s) education level were treated as a continuous variable in order for
the model to converge in Mplus. Responses for education level included: (a) less than high
school, (b) high school diploma or the equivalent, (c) attended college but did not earn a degree,
(d) associate’s degree, (e) bachelor’s degree, (f) master’s degree, (g) professional degree, (h)
doctorate, and (i) don’t know. Professional degree and doctorate were combined into one
category, representing professional degrees. Point values were assigned for categories ranging
from 1 (less than high school) to 7 (professional degree) in order to create a continuous variable
representing highest level of parent education. Parent education was used as a proxy for parent
income as nearly 32% of the sample responded “don’t know” or “prefer not to answer” for the
survey item regarding parent income. The parent income variable may also be unreliable for
those who did answer, as students may have only guessed their parents’ income and not actually
31
know for certain. Respondent’s current annual income was treated as a continuous variable.
Current annual income responses included: (a) $0; (b) $1-$2,499; (c) $2,500-$4,999; (d) $5,000-
$7,499; (e) $7,500-$9,999; (f) $10,000-$14,999; (g) $15,000-$19,999; (h) $20,000-$24,999; (i)
$25,000-$29,999; (j) $30,000 or higher; (k) don’t know; (l) prefer not to answer. Point values
were assigned to categories, ranging from 1 (no income) to 10 ($30,000 or higher) in order to
treat respondent income as a continuous variable. As more than 12% of the sample responded
“don’t know” or “prefer not to answer,” these responses were treated as separate categories of
missing income to determine if these groups vary significantly from those with reported income.
Race was categorized as White, Black, Hispanic, Asian, mixed, and other. Prefer not to answer
responses to race were treated as “asked but missing” responses and estimated using FIML.
Gender was reduced to males and females, as less than one percent of the sample identified as
transgender or self-defined. Age and unweighted grade point average (GPA) were treated as
continuous variables. First-year students may have reported high school GPA, anticipated GPA,
or zero GPA if asked prior completing their first semester at the institution. To account for
potential inconsistency in GPA reporting for first-year students, the analyses was conducted both
with and without first-year students to determine if there were significant differences in the
models. Number of years in school responses included: 1 year, 2 years, 3 years, 4 years, or 5 or
more years. In order to compare first-year students to the rest of the sample, number of years in
school was treated as a binary categorical variable with first-year students = 1, and other cohorts
= 0. Institution type was treated as a categorical variable with categories (a) four-year public; (b)
32
Table 3.1 Items in Parcels
33
All paycheck stubs show your gross pay and your net
pay….On average, what percentage of your income
FQ4 would you expect to receive as take-home pay? 70-79% (1), else (0)
Financial Self-Efficacy
FSE1 I am confident I can manage my finances. Strongly disagree (1), disagree (2), agree (3), strongly agree (4)
FSE2 I manage my money well. Strongly disagree (1), disagree (2), agree (3), strongly agree (4)
Financial Behavior
Timeliness I pay my bills on time every month. Never (1), Sometimes (2), Frequently (3), Always (4)
I add to my savings on a regular basis. Never (1), Sometimes (2), Frequently (3), Always (4)
Accounting I have a weekly or monthly budget that I follow. Never (1), Sometimes (2), Frequently (3), Always (4)
I track my spending in order to stay within my budget. Never (1), Sometimes (2), Frequently (3), Always (4)
I track all debit card transactions/checks to balance my
account. Never (1), Sometimes (2), Frequently (3), Always (4)
34
Chapter 4 - Results
Descriptive Statistics
Table 4.1 provides the summary statistics for the sample. The sample consisted of 12,598
college students from the fall 2014 and winter 2015 semesters. The majority of the sample was
female (68%), White (74%), and between ages 18 and 23 (71%). This varies slightly from
national post-secondary education statistics for 2014, which reported approximately 57% of
college students were female, 60% were White, and 61% were between the ages of 18 and 24
(Snyder, de Brey, & Dillow, 2016). Black, Hispanic, and Asian students were underrepresented
in the NSFWS 2014 sample compared to the national college student population. The NSFWS
2014 sample consisted of approximately 5% Black, 5% Hispanic, and 2% Asian students, while
the national college student population in 2014 consisted of approximately 15% Black, 15%
Hispanic, and 6% Asian students (Snyder et al., 2016). Respondents had lower income levels,
with 10% reporting having no income, 15% having income less than $2,500, and almost 14%
having income between $2,500 and $4,999. The sample consisted of a relatively equal
distribution of students in their first year, second year, third year, fourth year, and five-plus years
enrolled. Most respondents had a relatively high GPA, with 71% reporting a GPA between 3.00
and 3.99 and nearly 6% reporting a perfect 4.0 GPA. The high GPA of respondents may be an
indication of self-selection bias, in which better performing students opted to participate in the
survey, while other students may have chosen not to participate. Approximately 29% of
respondents reported the highest level of education obtained by a parent was a Bachelor’s
35
With regard to financial socialization, the majority of the sample reported their parents
encouraged them to save (86%) and to open a bank account (88%), but very few indicated their
parents encouraged them to invest (30%). Less than half of the sample reported receiving an
allowance as a child (44%) or as a teenager (37%), but most reported working for pay in high
school (76%). Only 31% attended some sort of financial education in high school, and even
fewer attended financial education in college (24%). A moderate level of implicit financial
socialization was reported with a mean of 3.08 (on a scale of 1 to 4) for “My parents were
High levels of financial self-efficacy were reported with a mean of 3.12 for “I am
confident I can manage my finances” on a scale of 1 to 4. The financial knowledge level of the
respondents was average, with a mean of 2.64 on a scale of 1 to 4. The majority of the sample
were able to answer the repayment options question correctly (80%) and the interest rate
question correctly (80%), while less than half could answer the question on take-home pay
correctly (44%). Most reported positive financial behaviors, with a mean of 3.63 for pays bills on
time, a mean of 2.93 for track spending to stay within budget, and a mean of 2.6 for follows a
budget (all on a scale of 1 to 4). See Table 4.1 for full results.
36
Table 4.1 Summary Statistics
Full Sample First Year Other Cohorts
Financial Knowledge
Inflation correct 59.76% 52.57% 61.37%
Interest correct 79.78% 74.04% 81.06%
Repayment options correct 80.11% 73.91% 81.50%
Percent take home pay correct 44.44% 40.42% 45.33%
Credit score correct 38.30% 40.81% 37.74%
37
Financial Self-efficacy
Confident can manage finances 3.12 42 3.07 7 3.13 35
Manage money well 3.03 49 3.03 8 3.03 41
Financial Behaviors
Personal Accounting Factor
Follows budget (1-4) 2.6 40 2.5 5 2.62 35
Tracks spending (1-4) 2.93 69 2.92 14 2.93 55
Tracks checks/debits (1-4) 3.12 84 3.17 9 3.1 75
Timeliness Factor
Pay bills on time (1-4) 3.63 84 3.57 23 3.64 61
Saves regularly (1-4) 2.38 76 2.48 12 2.36 64
Lives within Means Factor
Regularly spends more than income w/credit
(RC 1-4) 3.36 50 3.53 12 3.32 43
Purchased something expensive wanted but didn't
need (RC 1-4) 2.85 55 2.82 8 2.86 42
Gender
Male 32.12% 29.92% 32.62%
Female 67.66% 69.77% 67.19%
Missing 0.22% 0.31% 0.19%
Age
Age 18-23 71.27% 87.76% 67.59%
Age 24-29 13.82% 3.70% 16.07%
Age 30-39 8.44% 3.92% 9.44%
Age 40 or older 6.29% 3.79% 6.84%
Missing 0.19% 0.83% 0.05%
38
Race
White 73.73% 71.78% 74.17%
Black 4.73% 5.18% 4.63%
Hispanic 5.31% 5.27% 5.32%
Asian 1.54% 5.58% 5.22%
Mixed race 7.76% 9.67% 7.33%
Other 1.54% 0.92% 1.68%
Missing 5.39% 1.60% 1.65%
GPA
0.00 - .99 0.31% 1.13% 0.13%
1.00 - 1.99 0.80% 0.91% 0.78%
2.00 - 2.99 22.49% 15.42% 24.06%
3.00 - 3.99 70.79% 69.03% 71.18%
4.00 5.61% 13.50% 3.85%
Respondent's Income
No income 10.44% 24.31% 9.29%
Less than $2,500 15.02% 28.28% 14.76%
BT $2,500 - $4,999 13.51% 17.77% 14.88%
BT $5,000 - $7,499 9.46% 9.04% 11.14%
BT $7,500 - $9,999 7.87% 5.23% 9.75%
BT $10,000 - $14,999 10.14% 4.97% 12.92%
BT $15,000 - $19,999 5.18% 2.20% 6.67%
BT $20,000 - $24,999 4.52% 2.61% 5.67%
BT $25,000 - $29,999 2.83% 1.46% 3.58%
GT $30,000 8.87% 4.13% 11.34%
Don't know 6.26% 9.32% 5.58%
39
Prefer not to answer/Missing 5.91% 7.36% 5.60%
Parent's Income
LT $15,000 4.64% 6.96% 6.80%
BT $15,000 - $29,999 6.60% 9.99% 9.66%
BT $30,000 - $39,999 6.90% 9.99% 10.20%
BT $40,000 - $59,999 10.37% 15.27% 15.29%
BT $60,000 - $79,999 10.60% 13.85% 16.02%
BT $80,000 - $99,999 9.20% 14.05% 13.44%
BT $100,000 - $149,999 11.03% 16.56% 16.19%
GT $150,000 8.53% 13.34% 12.40%
Don't know 22.57% 22.69% 22.54%
Prefer not to answer/Missing 9.56% 9.71% 9.52%
Institution Type
2 year public 8.20% 11.54% 7.45%
4 year public 82.59% 78.57% 83.49%
4 year private 9.21% 9.89% 9.06%
40
Number of years enrolled
First year 18.23% 100.00%
Second year 19.57% 23.94%
Third year 22.25% 27.21%
Fourth year 20.51% 25.08%
Five or more years 19.20% 23.48%
Missing 0.24% 0.29%
41
Confirmatory Factor Analysis
Using the fixed factor method of scale setting with the scale set to 1, a confirmatory
factor analysis (CFA) was first conducted with individual items to determine how the items were
correlated with the constructs they were intended to represent. Setting the scale to 1 allows for
the relationship between latent factors to be estimated in a standardized metric, as well as for the
estimates between latent constructs to be interpreted as correlations (Little, 2013). Table 4.2
displays the CFA correlation matrix for items pre-parcel. Items with standardized lambdas lower
than .30 were removed from analysis, as this was an indication that the item did not correlate
well with the construct it was intended to measure. Only one item, the financial knowledge
question regarding components of a credit score, had a lambda loading of less than .30. This
question differed from all other survey items as it required students to correctly identify more
Table 4.2 Confirmatory Factor Analysis Results for Latent Constructs without Parcels (N =
12,598)
Unstandardized Standardized
Parameter b SE β SE
42
FS→FS9: Encouraged to Open Bank Account 2.99 0.017 0.70 0.002
FS→FS10: HS Financial Education 2.48 0.036 0.32 0.003
FS→FS11: College Financial Education 2.50 0.038 0.34 0.003
A correlation matrix was run in SAS® to check for multicollinearity, and to identify
items that may parcel well together. Correlations greater than .80 are an indication of
multicollinearity (Field and Miles, 2010). No multicollinearity issues were present in the data as
all correlations were less than .65. Table A.1 in the Appendix presents the full correlation matrix
for all variables used in the model. Items were then parceled for each construct based on
correlations and the facets of the construct they represent. Items in parcels were averaged to
retain the original metrics of the scale and for ease in comparing means and variances (Little,
2013). Financial socialization had 11 items parceled into four factors: implicit socialization,
explicit socialization, financial education, and experience with money. Financial knowledge
43
consisted of only four items and was not parceled. Financial self-efficacy consisted of two items
and was not parceled; however, because the construct consisted of only two items, the variances
between the two items were set to equal. Financial behavior consisted of seven items and was
parceled into three factors: accounting, timeliness, and living within means. A second CFA was
conducted after parceling to check that the parcels had acceptable lambda loadings for each
construct. Table 4.3 presents the parceled CFA correlation matrix. A third CFA was conducted
with covariates included with the parceled items to test the proposed model in Figure 2.1. Delta
parameterization was used, which sets the total variance of the latent variables to one and
requires parameter coefficients to be interpreted as the change in standard deviation for a given
latent variable, relative to a one standard deviation change in a common factor (Kline, 2016).
Table 4.3 Confirmatory Factor Analysis Results for Latent Constructs with Parcels (N =
12,598)
Unstandardized Standardized
Parameter b SE β SE
44
FK→FK4: Take-home Pay 0.36 0.02 0.36 0.02 0.13***
Model Fit
The model chi-square exact fit indices was significant, suggesting that the model should
be rejected (χ2 [ 216] = 4385.80, p < .001). The chi-square test estimates are extremely sensitive
to a large sample size and a large number of degrees of freedom and will be significant for most
models (Little, 2013). As this sample is large at 12,598 observations, it is not surprising that the
chi-square test is significant since models with a sample size greater than 400 typically always
have a statistically significant chi-square value (Kenny, 2015). The Root Mean Square Error of
Approximation (RMSEA) measures absolute fit by comparing the saturated model to the
estimated model (Little, 2013). RMSEA for the model is .039, indicating the model is a close fit
(90% CI = .038, .040). The comparative fit index (CFI) for the model is .87 indicating a
mediocre fit, with the model being an 87% improvement over the null model. The Tucker –
Lewis Index (TLI) for the model is .85, also indicating a mediocre fit. Based on the RMSEA,
Figure 4.1 presents a diagram of the statistically significant direct effects from the tested
model. The model provides some support for the Family Financial Socialization conceptual
model developed by Gudmunson and Danes (2011). Both direct and indirect relationships were
found between financial socialization and financial behaviors in college students. Overall the
model explains 81% of the variance in financial behavior (R2 = .81), 2% of the variance in
45
financial socialization (R2 = .02), none of the variance in objective financial knowledge (R2 =
46
Figure 4.1 Significant Pathways in Structural Model Predicting Financial Behavior (N = 12,598)*
*Note: *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001. Model Fit Indices: χ2(216) = 4,385.80, p = <.001; RMSEA = .039, 90% CI [.038, .040], CFI = .87, TLI = .85.
All results were computed with Mplus in delta parameterization and STDYX standardization. The structural model was estimated with indicators
from the measurement model for the latent variables and controls for gender, age, race, income, parent education, GPA, years enrolled in school,
and institution type.
47
Direct Effects
Supporting Hypothesis 1, a direct relationship between financial socialization and
objective financial knowledge was found. A one standard deviation increase in financial
(β = .03, p < .01). Financial socialization was also found to have a direct and positive affect on
financial self-efficacy, supporting Hypothesis 2. For every one standard deviation increase in
.16). Hypothesis 3 was also supported, with a direct and positive relationship found between
financial socialization and financial behavior. For every one standard deviation increase in
.020). Financial self-efficacy was found to have the greatest influence on financial behaviors,
with a 90% (β = .90) increase in the standard deviation of positive financial behavior for every
one standard deviation increase in self-efficacy. The direct and positive relationship between
self-efficacy and behavior supports Hypothesis 8. Objective financial knowledge was not found
No significant differences were found between males and females with regard to financial
socialization. Significant differences among races were found in financial socialization, with
Blacks, Asians, Hispanics, and other races receiving less financial socialization than Whites.
Respondent income, parent education, and institution type had no significant associations with
financial socialization. Age was negatively associated with financial socialization, while GPA
was positively associated with financial socialization. First-year students as compared to students
enrolled two or more years reported significantly less financial socialization (β = -.03).
48
Table 4.4 Direct Effects with Financial Behavior (N = 12,598)
Unstandardized Standardized*
Parameter b SE β SE p
Financial Self-Efficacy→Financial Behavior 2.028 0.03 0.896 0.00 .00***
Financial Knowledge→Financial Behavior 0.046 0.03 0.020 0.02 0.18
Financial Socialization→Financial Behavior 0.050 0.01 0.022 0.00 .00***
Financial Self-Efficacy→Fin. Knowledge -.003 0.01 -.003 0.01 0.8
Financial Self-Efficacy→Fin. Socialization 0.164 0.00 0.163 0.00 .00***
Financial Knowledge→Fin. Socialization 0.028 0.01 0.029 0.01 .01**
Female→Financial Socialization -.041 0.03 -0.019 0.01 0.15
Black→Financial Socialization -.277 0.05 -0.058 0.01 .00***
Asian→Financial Socialization -.199 0.05 -0.044 0.01 .00***
Hispanic→Financial Socialization -.148 0.06 -0.033 0.01 .01**
Mixed Race→Financial Socialization -.049 0.05 -0.013 0.01 0.32
Other Race→Financial Socialization -.389 0.07 -0.047 0.01 .00***
Income→Financial Socialization 0.000 0.00 0.013 0.01 0.34
Parent Education→Financial Socialization -.001 0.09 0.000 0.01 0.99
Four-Year Private→Financial Socialization -.031 0.04 -.009 0.01 0.43
Two-Year Public→Financial Socialization -.020 0.04 -0.005 0.01 0.65
Age→Financial Socialization -.012 0.00 -.087 0.01 .00***
GPA→Financial Socialization 0.075 0.03 0.039 0.01 .00**
First Year Students→Financial Socialization -.070 0.03 -.027 0.01 .02*
R-Squared .81
*Note: *p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001 All results were computed with Mplus in delta parameterization and STDYX standardization.
Overall Model Fit Indices: χ2(216) = 4,385.80, p = <.001; RMSEA = .039, 90% CI [.038, .040], CFI = .87, TLI = .85.
Reference Groups: male, White, four-year public, students enrolled two or more
years
49
Indirect Effects
financial knowledge was proposed in Hypothesis 5 but was not supported, as there was no direct
relationship found between financial knowledge and financial behavior. Hypothesis 4 was
supported, with a positive indirect relationship found between financial socialization, financial
self-efficacy, and financial behavior. A one standard deviation increase in financial socialization
was associated with a 15% increase in the standard deviation of positive financial behaviors
Alternative Models
Due to the low CFI of the original model and lower lambda loadings for financial
education with financial socialization, an alternative model splitting out financial socialization
into two constructs, parental financial socialization and financial education, was conducted. The
alternative model did not improve model fit [(χ2diff [10] = 1,531); (χ2 (df 226) = 5917.360, p =
<.001; CFI = .86; RMSEA = .045, CI .044, .046)], and was therefore rejected; however, it did
yield some interesting results. Shown in Figure A.1 in the Appendix, the path from financial
socialization to financial education was found to have a positive and significant relationship. For
every one standard deviation increase in parent financial socialization, the standard deviation of
financial education increased by almost 53% (β = .525, p<.001). Both parent financial
socialization and financial education were found to increase financial self-efficacy, with a one
13% (β = .131) and financial education increasing the standard deviation of self-efficacy by
50
socialization significantly increased financial knowledge. A one standard deviation increase in
financial education was associated with a 3% (β = .031) standard deviation increase in positive
financial behaviors, as was parent financial socialization (β = .029). Results of covariates with
parent financial socialization were similar to the original model; however, only gender and age
were significantly associated with financial education. Women were less likely to attend
financial education (β = -.042), while age was positively associated with attending financial
education (β = .031). Table A.2 and Figure A.1 in the Appendix show the full results of direct
effects for the alternative split socialization model, while Table A.3 in the Appendix shows the
full results of the indirect effects of the alternative split socialization model.
Because the reliability of GPA reporting for first-year students was questionable, the
model was conducted both with and without first-year students to test for model sensitivity for
GPA. No differences in direction or significance was found in the model when first-year students
were excluded. Results for the model excluding first-year students can be found in the Appendix,
Table A.3.
Approximately 6% of the sample indicated they did not know their income. Not knowing
one’s income suggests a lack of financial awareness and may be an indication of socialization
socialization for those who indicated they did not know their income compared to those who did
know their income. No significant differences in financial socialization were found between
these two groups. See Table A.4 in the Appendix for full results.
Six percent of the sample also answered “prefer not to answer” for the income item. A
preference to keep income private may be a result of financial socialization, in which parents
may have taught the child not to discuss income or finances with others. A separate model was
51
conducted to determine if there were differences in socialization for those who preferred not to
answer their income as compared to those who did answer the income item. No significant
differences in financial socialization were found between these two groups. See Table A.5 in the
52
Chapter 5 - Discussion
This study examined the direct and indirect relationships between financial socialization,
financial knowledge, financial self-efficacy, and financial behavior using Gudmunson and
Danes’ (2011) Family Financial Socialization conceptual model (FFS) to guide the hypotheses.
Analysis was conducted using Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) due to the use of latent
constructs and the need to explore the direct and indirect effects among the variables. Results of
the study show strong support for the FFS conceptual model, along with insight into the
Financial Socialization
While not the focus of this study, the relationship between the demographic variables and
financial socialization revealed some intriguing results. In the combined financial socialization
model, there were no significant differences in financial socialization between males and
females. These results contradict previous findings which indicate females are more likely to
have discussions with parents’ and peers’ regarding finances, and more likely to observe parents’
and peers’ financial behaviors (Gutter et al., 2009; Gutter & Garrison, 2010). The difference in
results could be attributed to socialization measurements, as the present study included a broader
range of financial socialization factors. In the split socialization model, the results indicate there
is a difference in financial socialization between males and females with regard to financial
education. Being female was negatively associated with participating in financial education.
There were no significant differences in parental socialization between males and females in the
split socialization model. Looking more closely at the split model, there was a strong, positive
53
association between parental financial socialization and formal financial education, suggesting
that parental financial socialization is an important factor in whether or not a student participates
in a personal finance class or workshop while in high school or college. Gender differences in
financial education could possibly be attributed to parents suggesting to their sons to enroll in
financial education courses, but not their daughters. More research is needed to determine if
Difference in financial socialization were also found among races. Compared to White
respondents, Black, Asian, Hispanic, and other races reported less financial socialization. This
finding opposes Gutter, Copur, and Garrison’s (2009) study, which found that White students
were less likely to discuss finances with their parents or peers than students of other races. The
financial socialization, but not in formal financial education, according to the split socialization
model. This suggests there may be cultural differences in willingness of parents to discuss
Age was negatively related to financial socialization, indicating the older a student was,
the less financial socialization they received. The relationship between age and financial
socialization supports Gutter, Copur, and Garrison’s (2009) findings that older students were less
likely to discuss personal finances with parents or to observe parents’ financial behaviors. The
negative relationship between age and financial socialization might be attributed to generational
differences in financial socialization. It could be that parents of older students did not discuss
finances with their child growing up because it was not socially acceptable to talk about financial
matters. Another possible explanation is that as the student ages, the more financially
54
independent they become, and the less need for financial socialization from their parents. Results
of the split socialization model indicate that age is negatively associated with parental financial
socialization, but positively associated with formal financial education. The number of years a
student was enrolled was also significantly related to financial socialization. Compared to other
cohorts, first year students received less financial socialization. First year students are often
living in the dorm and participating in meal plans, or still living at home with their parents,
which removes the immediate responsibility of paying rent and bills, and the need to budget
finances. Parents may wait to engage in certain financial conversations with their child such as
how to create a budget, timeliness of bill payments, and use of credit, until the child moves off-
campus. The split model of socialization supports this possibility, as first year students did
receive significantly less parental financial socialization than other cohorts. No difference was
found between first year students and other cohorts in regards to formal financial education.
The retained model showed a positive relationship between financial socialization and
financial knowledge, consistent with previous research (Lapp, 2010; Shim et al., 2009; Shim et
al., 2010); however, in the alternative split model of financial socialization, neither parental
financial socialization nor formal financial education alone were significantly related to financial
knowledge. Formal financial education bordered on statistical significance at the p<.10 level, yet
parental financial socialization was no where near statistical significance. This suggests that
there may be an interacting relationship between formal financial education and parental
financial socialization that influences financial knowledge. Children who receive formal
financial education at school may discuss what they are learning with their parents, providing
parents with the opportunity to discuss their own financial attitudes and beliefs with their
55
children, and thereby teaching their children more about managing their finances and
strengthening their financial knowledge. The fact that formal financial education borders on
statistical significance provides support for findings from Shim and associates (2010), Lapp
(2010), and Kaiser and Menkhoff (2017), all of which found financial education to have a
positive relationship with financial knowledge. Differences in the strength and level of
Within the model, the relationship between financial socialization and financial self-
efficacy was the second strongest association between variables. While other studies have found
weak or mixed results (Heckman & Grable, 2011; Lee & Mortimer, 2009), the present study
alternative split model of financial socialization shows that parental financial socialization has a
much stronger influence on financial self-efficacy than formal financial education, though both
included in this study, the experience with money parcel (λ = .60) and two of the indicators
within this parcel (working for pay while in high school, λ = .96; receiving an allowance as a
child, λ = .90) contained the highest correlations with the financial socialization construct.
Providing children with experience in earning and managing money is an important factor in
increasing their financial self-efficacy and teaching them positive financial behavior. These
results support Bandura’s (1977) theory of self-efficacy, which posits that self-efficacy is
inferred from our experiences mastering a task and comparison of our capabilities to others.
What parents teach their children from direct conversations about finances, experiences the child
has managing money earned from working or allowance, and the financial behaviors parents
56
model around their children instill confidence in the child’s belief that they can successfully
In accordance with previous research, financial socialization was found to have a direct,
positive influence on financial behaviors (Cho et al., 2012; Kim & Chatterjee, 2013; Webley &
Nyhus, 2006). Among the individual activities included in the financial socialization factors,
working for pay while in high school yielded the strongest correlation at β = .96, suggesting this
is a compelling way for young adults to gain experience with money. Receiving an allowance as
a child (younger than age 12) also produced a high correlation at β = .90, further highlighting the
well, as conversations about saving, investing, and using credit can help guide children toward
better financial behaviors (Cho et al., 2012). The alternative split model of financial socialization
shows both parental financial socialization and formal financial education have similar levels of
influence on financial behavior (β = .03 for each). At this time, there are few, if any studies
exploring the effect of parental financial socialization on participation in financial education. The
findings in the split model of financial socialization suggest that parental financial socialization
has a strong, positive association with children enrolling in formal financial education. Given
that other studies have found a positive relationship between financial education and financial
behaviors (Kaiser & Menkhoff, 2017), this finding further emphasizes the importance of parents
and self-efficacy, in turn, influences financial behavior. As parents model good financial
57
behaviors, discuss financial topics with their children, and provide their children with
opportunities to make their own financial decisions, this instills higher efficacy beliefs in the
child regarding his or her own abilities to perform positive financial behaviors. Since financial
self-efficacy is a key factor in implementing positive financial behaviors, this underscores the
Financial Knowledge
The results of the model show that objective financial knowledge has no significant
relationship with financial self-efficacy. This could be due to how objective financial knowledge
was measured, as this is one limitation of the study. Heckman and Grable (2011) found objective
financial knowledge to increase financial self-efficacy when using a more thorough measurement
of objective financial knowledge that consisted of a 20-item personal finance quiz. Yet, Serido
and associates (2013) found objective financial knowledge (measured with 15 true/false
questions) was not an important predictor of financial self-efficacy. However, Serido and
colleagues found subjective financial knowledge to have a positive and significant influence on
noted that self-efficacy depends on a level of competence, or at the very least perceived
competence, regarding the behavior. Given that limited research has been conducted as to the
antecedents of financial self-efficacy, both objective and subjective knowledge could be equally
important. More research is needed to determine how objective and subjective financial
58
Financial Knowledge and Financial Behavior
Considering the numerous studies that have found a strong relationship between financial
knowledge and behavior (Angrisani et al., 2016; Chatterjee et al., 2017; de Bassa Scheresberg,
2013; Henager & Cude, 2016; Nghia & Scott, 2018; Xiao et al., 2014), it was surprising to find
no significant relationship between objective financial knowledge and financial behavior in the
model. The most likely explanation for this non-association is the measurement for objective
financial knowledge is fallacious. The construct for objective financial knowledge contained 4
multiple-choice items, covering inflation, interest rate, loan repayment, and take-home pay. Two
items – interest rate and loan repayment – are essentially the same concept, with the loan
repayment question requiring a little more extrapolation of general interest rate knowledge. A
more thorough measurement of objective financial knowledge may yield different results.
Financial Self-Efficacy
The direct, positive relationship between financial self-efficacy and financial behavior in
the model was congruent with previous findings (Asededo et al., 2018; Asebedo & Browning,
2017; Farrell et al., 2016; Lapp, 2010; Lown et al., 2015; Montford & Goldsmith, 2016). As
predicated by Bandura (1977) and considered in the FFS conceptual model, the greater one’s
self-efficacy, the more likely the person will attempt a behavior. The magnitude of this
behaviors. Results of this study show that increasing financial self-efficacy can be achieved
through financial socialization. Both parental financial socialization and formal financial
education are predictors of financial self-efficacy; however, parental financial socialization has a
much stronger influence on financial self-efficacy. Bandura (1977) indicated that self-efficacy
59
can be increased through successful attempts at a behavior. As experience with money had the
strongest correlation with financial socialization, parents should find ways to provide their child
with practice managing their own money, most commonly provided through the child earning an
Overall, results of this study provide support for many of the propositions presented in
Gudmunson and Dane’s Family Financial Socialization (FFS) conceptual model. Differences
were found in age, race, gender, and GPA with regards to receipt of financial socialization;
however, other personal and family characteristics, such as income and parent’s education level
were not related to financial socialization. This study only focused on personal and family
exploration in this area is needed. An association between financial socialization and objective
financial knowledge was supported by the findings. However, the results of the split model of
socialization and formal financial education that may need to be further explored in the FFS
framework. Financial socialization was found to influence financial self-efficacy, which the FFS
framework included as a motivating factor of capabilities. While the present study did not find an
measurement of objective financial knowledge may yield different results. The relationship
examined in more detail to determine how financial socialization influences these constructs
individually, as well as how their direct and indirect relationships may influence financial
60
Contributions
This study makes three meaningful contributions to the literature. First, it tests the Family
conceptual framework is the first step in theory development. As a conceptual model is tested
over time, its propositions can be justified, and with consistent results, eventually develop into a
mature theory (Danes & Yang, 2014). Research in the field of personal finance often borrows
theory from other disciplines. By testing the FFS conceptual model, this study contributes to the
justification of the framework, and offers support toward theory development in our discipline.
Second, results of this study provide new insight into the relationship between parental
socialization and financial education. The strong, positive association between parental financial
socialization and financial education indicates that children whose parents teach them about
finances are significantly more likely to take a personal finance class or workshop. With the push
to educate more people on personal finance topics, this provides a fresh observation into what
factors might influence a person’s decision to enroll in financial education. While one might
consider parental financial socialization adequate, the results of this study indicate there may be
an interaction effect between parental financial socialization and financial education that leads to
greater financial knowledge. As noted in the discussion section, when financial socialization was
tested as one combined construct that included facets for both parental financial socialization and
However, in the split model of financial socialization, neither parental socialization nor financial
Third, this study adds to existing literature by exploring the direct and indirect
61
financial behavior in college students, using both Structural Equation Modeling and a large,
national dataset. While other studies have explored these relationships directly, few have
explored the indirect effects of financial socialization on self-efficacy through its association
with financial knowledge. Of equal importance, many studies that involve college students are
limited to samples from one or two institutions, making it difficult to generalize the results to
college students throughout the United States. The present study uses data collected by The Ohio
State University from 52 institutions, including two-year public, four-year public, and four-year
private schools. The large number of institutions and participants allows for less bias in the
Implications
Findings from this study show strong support for the importance of financial socialization
of children and young adults. Both parental financial socialization and financial education are
equally important in the development of healthy financial behaviors. With the unique finding of
this study showing parental financial socialization to be an important predictor of young adults
participating in formal financial education while in high school or in college, it magnifies the
importance of parents teaching and role modeling positive financial behaviors to their children.
Results of this study indicate there may be an interaction effect between parental financial
socialization and financial education that produces a significant increase in financial knowledge.
Parents should be aware of the financial behaviors they are modeling in front of their children,
and take time to engage in every day conversations pertaining to finances. Providing
opportunities for children to earn an allowance or work a part-time job in high school can
provide children with valuable money experience in a low-stakes environment, before they start
living on their own in college. Successful experiences managing their own spending money can
62
lead to great financial self-efficacy and improved financial behavior in college. Discussing the
importance of saving, investing, and budgeting, are also critical to a child’s development of
With only 57% of Americans being financially literate (Klapper, Lusardi, & van
Oudheusden, 2015), parents may need community support to learn good financial practices for
themselves, as well as how to instill positive financial behaviors in their children. Financial
institutions, such as banks and credit unions, could present workshops for families and children
to learn about budgeting, saving, and investing, as well as responsible use of credit and ways to
improve credit scores. Research and extension offices may also want to be involved in
developing personal financial education programs for the community. Resources and training
materials should be created and made available for interested parties to easily access for use in
Currently only five states (i.e., Alabama, Missouri, Tennessee, Utah, and Virginia)
require high school students to participate in at least a half of a year of personal finance
education (Carrns, 2018). With financial education shown to improve financial knowledge
(Lapp, 2010; Shim et al., 2010) and financial behavior (Kaiser & Menkhoff, 2017), policy
makers should push to mandate financial education for all students. The findings in the split
financial socialization model of this study suggest a co-financial socialization approach may
strengthen financial knowledge in young adults. Educators in K-12 may find it useful to inform
parents about the financial curriculum taught in the classroom, in order for the parents to engage
in conversations and experiences at home that coincide with school lessons. By working
together, parents and educators can provide children with the necessary knowledge they need to
63
perform positive financial behaviors, as well as instill a level of self-efficacy in young adults that
Financial counselors, advisors, and therapists have the opportunity to encourage their
clients with children of all ages to provide opportunities for their children to manage an
allowance or earnings from a job, and to engage in regular discussions about savings, budgeting,
investing, and appropriate use of credit with their children. Financial advisors may find that
having a family financial education day with their clients will provide them opportunities to
teach all members of the family about personal finance, as well as establish relationships with
their clients’ young adult children that could carry on into the future. Financial therapists can
discuss ways that parents can interact with their children to establish healthy financial behaviors
at a young age, and avoid troubling family financial patterns that have been established by
previous generations.
Outcomes from this study suggest the FFS conceptual framework may be enhanced by
further exploration of the direct and indirect relationships of financial attitudes, knowledge, and
capabilities, and concepts, such as self-efficacy, that are included in this category. Understanding
how these concepts interact can provide a clearer understanding of how we can improve upon
and develop the necessary attitudes, skills, and capabilities that lead to better financial behavior.
While the FFS conceptual model breaks apart the financial socialization construct into implicit
and explicit financial socialization, this study examined financial socialization as whole, and as a
split construct, examining parental financial socialization and formal financial education
separately. The outcome from splitting financial socialization into these two constructs indicated
there are differences in how parental financial socialization and formal financial education
64
associated with the decision to enroll in formal financial education. The FFS conceptual model
may benefit from further exploration into how these two types of financial socialization interact.
As with all research, this study faced some limitations. Of primary concern, the
measurement used for objective financial knowledge was mediocre. Consisting of four multiple-
choice items, it was limited in the personal finance topics it tested. A more thorough
measurement for objective financial knowledge may have yielded different results, as there were
some unexpected relationships found in the model, or lack thereof, with objective financial
knowledge. Another concern was the lack of a subjective financial knowledge measurement in
the data. Subjective financial knowledge has been found to have associations with financial self-
efficacy (Lapp, 2010; Serido et al., 2013) and financial behavior (Serido et al.), and the lack of
With the use of SEM, there are some constraints as well. Income had to be measured as a
continuous variable in order for the model to converge in Mplus. Separating income into
categories would have provided more detail into differences between respondents of different
income levels, and may have provided different results. Similarly, parents’ education level had to
be measured as a continuous variable in order for the model to converge in Mplus, which
based on parent education level. Use of latent constructs also limits interpretation of results to the
constructs themselves, without much detail as to how the individual indicators influence one
another. Interpretation of how specific financial socialization activities influence other variables
can only be inferred from indicator correlations as to their importance in the financial
socialization construct.
65
While SEM does limit some types of interpretation, it does allow for examination of the
direct and indirect effects among the variables. By examining indirect relationships, we gain
more knowledge into how and why variables influence one another. This study shows financial
through its association with financial self-efficacy. This indirect relationship heightens financial
self-efficacy’s relevance to financial behavior. Finally, a major strength of this study is its use of
a large, national dataset. By analyzing a larger and more diverse sample of college students, we
can generalize our findings to a larger population. While the data set is not nationally
representative, it does include students from 52 college institutions throughout the United States,
as well as students enrolled at different types of college institutions (two-year public, four-year
public, and four-year private). Most studies are limited to one or two institutions, which restricts
their application to only students in that particular region or institution type. The results of this
study are generalizable to college students with similar demographics as the sample, but is not
Future Research
While this study provides some unique findings, more research is needed to determine the
exact nature of some relationships. Specifically, the association between parental financial
socialization and financial education should be explored more to determine how parental
parental financial socialization and formal financial education interact to influence financial
knowledge and behavior. Future studies should examine how subjective financial knowledge fits
into the FFS conceptual framework, as well as test the relationship between objective financial
knowledge and self-efficacy using a different measure for objective financial knowledge. With
66
the strong association found between financial self-efficacy and positive financial behaviors,
subsequent research should focus on the antecedents of financial self-efficacy. The present study
research study would provide greater discernment into the long-term impact of financial
socialization on financial behaviors, beyond just the college years. Finally, it is important to
investigate the disparity in financial socialization between genders and races. With financial
behavior, it is essential that people of all races and genders have equal opportunities to learn
67
References
Handbook of Consumer Finance Research (pp. 351-362). New York, New York:
Springer.
Angrisani, M., Kapteyn, A., & Lusardi, A. (2016). The National Financial Capability Study:
Empirical findings from the American Life Panel Survey. Retrieved from
http://gflec.org/wp-content/uploads/2016/11/NFCS_ALP_Report_Final_UPDATED.pdf.
Asebedo, S. D., Wilmarth, M., Seay, M. C., Archuleta, K. L., Brase, G., & MacDonald, M.
(2018). Personality and saving behavior among older adults. Journal of Consumer
https://www.depts.ttu.edu/hs/pfp/cfr/documents/WorkingPaper2017-04.pdf
Babiarz, P., & Robb, C. A. (2013). Financial literacy and emergency saving. Journal of Family
Brüggen, E. C., Hogreve, J., Holmlund, M., Kabadayi, S., & Löfgren, M. (2017). Financial well-
being: A conceptualization and research agenda. Journal of Business Research, 79, 228-
237.
Carrns, A. (2018). Most states don’t require specific financial literacy classes. The New York
financial-literacy-classes.html
68
Chatterjee, S., Fan, L., Jacobs, B., & Haas, R. (2017). Risk tolerance and goals-based savings
https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2918014.
Cho, S. H., Gutter, M., Kim, J., & Mauldin, T. (2012). The effect of socialization and
income adults. Family & Consumer Sciences Research Journal, 40(4), 417-430.
Danes, S. M., & Haberman, H. R. (2007). Teen financial knowledge, self-efficacy, and behavior:
Danes, S. M., & Yang, Y. (2014). Assessment of the use of theories within the Journal of
Financial Counseling and Planning and the contribution of the family financial
53-68.
Danes, S. M., Huddleston-Casas, C., & Boyce, L. (1999). Financial planning curriculum for
teens: Impact evaluation. Association for Financial Counseling and Planning Education,
26-39.
De Bassa Scheresberg, C. (2013). Financial literacy and financial behavior among young adults:
Dew, J. P., & Xiao, J. J. (2013). Financial declines, financial behaviors, and relationship
Farrell, L., Fry, T.R.L., & Risse, L. (2016). The significance of financial self-efficacy in
85-99.
69
Fields, A. & Miles, J. (2010). Discovering statistics using SAS. London: SAGE Publications,
Ltd.
Garrison, S. T., & Gutter, M. S. (2010). Gender differences in financial socialization and
willingness to take financial risks. Journal of Financial Counseling and Planning, 21(2),
60-72.
Grohmann, A., Kouwenberg, R., & Menkhoff, L. (2015). Childhood roots of financial literacy.
Gudmunson, C. G., & Danes, S. M. (2011). Family financial socialization: Theory and critical
Gutter, M., & Copur, Z. (2011). Financial behaviors and financial well-being of college students:
Evidence from a national survey. Journal of Family and Economic Issues, 32(4), 699-
714.
Gutter, M., Copur, Z., & Garrison, S. (2009). Which students are more likely to experience
https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=1432523.
Heckman, S. J., & Grable, J. E. (2011). Testing the role of parental debt attitudes, student
income, dependency status, and financial knowledge have in shaping financial self-
Henager, R., & Cude, B. J. (2016). Financial literacy and long- and short-term financial behavior
in different age groups. Journal of Financial Counseling and Planning, 27(1), 3-19.
Hira, T. K. (1997). Financial attidues, beliefs and behaviours: Differences by age. Journal of
70
Huston, S. (2010). Measuring financial literacy. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 44(2), 296-316.
Jorgensen, B. L., Rappleyea, D. L., Schweichler, J.T., Fang, X., & Moran, M.E. (2017). The
Jorgensen, B., & Savla, J. (2010, October). Financial literacy of young adults: The importance of
3729.2010.00616.x
Kaiser, T., & Menkhoff, L. (2017). Does financial education impact financial literacy and
financial behavior, and if so, when? The World Bank Economic Review, 31(3), 611-630.
Kazdin, A. E. (1978). Conceptual and assessment issues raised by self-efficacy theory. Advances
Kim, J., LaTaillade, J., & Kim, H. (2011). Family processes and adolescents’ financial
Kim, J., & Chatterjee, S. (2013). Childhood financial socialization and young adults’ financial
Klapper, L., Lusardi, A., & van Oudheusden, P. (2015). Financial literacy around the world:
Insight from the Standard and Poor’s ratings services global financial literacy survey.
Finlit_paper_17_F3_SINGLES.pdf
Kline, R. B. (2016). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling (4th ed.). New
71
Lapp, W. M. (2010, November). Behavioral models for prosperity: A statistical assessment of
savings and behavioral change. Retrieved February 20, 2016, from EARN:
https://www.earn.org/wp-content/uploads/2015/03/5_-
_Behavioral_Models_for_Prosperity-
_A_Statistical_Assessment_of_Savings_and_Behavioral_Change-1.pdf
Lee, J. C., & Mortimer, J. T. (2009). Family socialization, economic self-efficacy, and the
Studies: International Journal, 1(1), 45-62. Retrieved March 25, 2016, from
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3198812/
Lim, H., Heckman, S. J., Letkiewicz, J. C., & Montalto, C. P. (2014). Financial stress, self-
Little, T. D. (2013). Longitudinal Structural Equation Modeling. New York, New York: The
Guliford Press.
Little, T. D., Lindenberger, U., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1999). On selecting indicators for
multivariate measurement and modeling with latent variables: When “good” indicators
are bad and “bad” indicators are good. Psychological Methods, 4(2), 192-211.
Lown, J. M., Kim, J., Gutter, M. S., & Hunt, A. T. (2015). Self-efficacy and savings among
middle and low income households. Journal of Family and Economic Issues, 36(4), 491-
502.
Montford, W., & Goldsmith, R. E. (2016). How gender and financial self-efficacy influence
72
National Study on Collegiate Financial Wellness (NSCFW). (2014). The Ohio State University.
Netemeyer, R. G., Warmeth, D., Fernandes, D., & Lynch Jr., J. G. (2018). How am I doing?
Perceived financial well-being, its potential antecedents, and its relation to overall well-
Nghia, N. V., & Scott, J. K. (2018). Financial capability: Literacy, behavior, and distress.
https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=3178367
Robb, C. A., Babiarz, P., Woodyard, A., & Seay, M. C. (2015). Bounded rationality and use of
Robb, C. A., & Woodyard, A. S. (2011). Financial knowledge and best practive behavior.
Sanders, C. K., Weaver, T. L., & Schnabel, M. (2007). Economic education for battered women.
Serido, J., Shim, S., Mishra, A., & Tang, C. (2010). Financial parenting, financial coping
Serido, J., Shim, S., & Tang, C. (2013). A developmental model of financial capability: A
Shim, S., Barber, B. L., Card, N. A., Xiao, J. J., & Serido, J. (2010). Financial socialization of
first-year college students: The roles of parents, work, and education. Journal of Youth
73
Shim, S., Serido, J., Tang, C., & Card, N. (2015). Socialization processes and pathways to
Shim, S., Xiao, J. J., Barber, B. L., & Lyons, A. C. (2009). Pathways to life success: A
Snyder, T. D., de Brey, C., and Dillow, S. A. (2016). Digest of Education Statistics 2014 (NCES
2016-006). National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S.
https://nces.ed.gov/pubs2016/2016006.pdf
T. Row Price. (2016). Detailed results: Eighth annual parents, kids, and money survey. Retrieved
from https://corporate.troweprice.com/Money-Confident-
Kids/images/emk/2016pkmresultsdeckfinal-160322181149.pdf
Webley, P., & Nyhus, E. K. (2006). Parents’ influence on children’s future orientation and
Xiao, J.J. (2008). Applying behavior theories to financial behavior. In Xiao, J.J. (Ed.), Handbook
Xiao, J. J., Chen, C., & Chen, F. (2014). Consumer financial capability and financial satisfaction.
Xiao, J. J., Tang, C., & Shim, S. (2009). Acting for happiness: Financial behavior and life
74
Appendix A - Supplemental Tables and Figures
75
Variables 8 9 10 11 12 13 14
8. Comfortable Talking - .72*** .57*** .11*** .10*** -.01 .37***
9. Taught Money Mgmt. .72*** - .68*** .13*** .12*** -.01 .44***
10. Role Model .57*** .68*** - .14*** .15*** -.01 .43***
11. Child Allow .11*** .13*** .14*** - .54*** .03** .13***
12. Teen Allow .10*** .12*** .15*** .54*** - -.05*** .11***
13. HS Work -.01 -.01 -.01 .03** -.05*** - .04***
14. Save .37*** .44*** .43*** .13*** .11*** .04*** -
15. Bank .31*** .034*** .32*** .11*** .08*** .11*** .50***
16. Invest .23*** .28*** .28*** .09*** .09*** .01 .22***
17. HS Fin Ed .09*** .12*** .09*** .02* -.01 .05*** .09***
18. Coll Fin Ed 0.01 0.01 0.01 .03* .02* .03** 0
19. Confident .15*** .17*** .10*** 0.01 0 .06*** .04***
20. Manage Money Well .16*** .20*** .15*** 0.02 -.01 .02* .09***
21. FKQ 1 -.02 -.02** 0 .03** 0.01 .03*** -.02*
22. FKQ 2 0.01 -.01 .02* 0.01 .02* .03** 0.01
23. FKQ 3 -.02* -.04*** -.02* 0 0 .07*** -.02*
24. FKQ 4 -.02* -.02 0 0.01 .02* .07*** -.02*
25. Income -.15*** -.18*** -.17*** .02* 0.01 .18*** -.17***
26. Parent Income .19*** .23*** .36*** .15*** .12*** .06*** .19***
27. Race -.02* -.02** -.04*** -.03*** .02* -.12*** -.01
28. Gender 0.02 0 0.01 -.01 -.02 0 0
29. Years Enrolled -.03** -.04*** -.03** .02* 0 .03** -.03**
30. GPA .06*** .06*** .10*** .04*** 0.01 -.01 .05***
31. Age -.07*** -.07*** -.06*** 0 0 .04*** -.05***
32. Parent Ed. .18*** .20*** .27*** .11*** .08*** -.01 .18***
33. Institution Type -.08*** -.08*** -.09*** 0 .03** 0.01 -.09***
Variables 15 16 17 18 19 20 21
15. Bank - .19*** .09*** 0 .05*** .08*** -.01
16. Invest .19*** - .06*** .05*** .10*** .12*** 0
17. HS Fin Ed .09*** .06*** - .16*** .05*** .06*** .04***
18. Coll Fin Ed 0 .05*** .16*** - .08*** .05*** .07***
19. Confident .05*** .10*** .05*** .08*** - .64*** .08***
20. Manage Money Well .08*** .12*** .06*** .05*** .64*** - .04***
21. FKQ 1 -.01 0 .04*** .07*** .08*** .04*** -
22. FKQ 2 .02* 0 .02* .06*** .08*** .06*** .32***
23. FKQ 3 0.01 -.03** .03** .05*** .10*** .06*** .28***
24. FKQ 4 0 .02* -.03** .03** .05*** .03** .18***
25. Income -.10*** -.03** -.06*** .08*** .17*** .05*** .10***
26. Parent Income .21*** .17*** .04** 0 0.02 .04*** .06***
76
27. Race -.05*** 0.01 -.06*** 0 -.04*** -.04*** -.03***
28. Gender 0 -.01 -.01 -.02* -.01 0.1 -.02
29. Years Enrolled 0.01 -.02* -.02* .03** -.01 -.02* .04***
30. GPA .04*** .02* .02* 0 .08*** .12*** .09***
31. Age -.06*** -.03** -.01 .03** 0.02 -.01 .02**
32. Parent Ed. .19*** .13*** .05*** 0.01 -.03** 0.01 .06***
33. Institution Type -.08*** -.01 -.07*** -.03** .03** 0 -.04***
Variables 22 23 24 25 26 27 28
22. FKQ 2 - .37*** .16*** .10*** .06*** -.02 -.02*
23. FKQ 3 .37*** - .18*** .14*** .05*** -.05*** -.01
24. FKQ 4 .16*** .18*** - .12*** .06*** -.05*** -.04***
25. Income .10*** .14*** .12*** - -.04** -.09*** -.01
26. Parent Income .06*** .05*** .06*** -.04** - -.11*** 0.01
27. Race -.02 -.05*** -.05*** -.09*** -.11*** - -.01
28. Gender -.02* -.01 -.04*** -.01 0.01 -.01 -
29. Years Enrolled .04*** .05*** .02* .13*** 0.02 -.02 -.02
30. GPA .06*** .06*** .03** -.04*** .08*** -.04*** 0.01
31. Age .02* .04*** .04*** .16*** -.03* -.03** -.01
32. Parent Ed. .05*** 0.01 0.01 -.16*** .45*** -.04*** 0
33. Institution Type -.02* -.01 .03** .13*** -.13*** -.01 0.01
Variables 29 30 31 32 33
29. Years Enrolled - -.03** .03** -.01 -.05***
30. GPA -.03** - -.02 .09*** 0.01
31. Age .03** -.02 - -.07*** .05***
32. Parent Ed. -.01 .09*** -.07*** - -.15***
33. Institution Type -.05*** 0.01 .05*** -.15*** -
77
Table A.2 Split Socialization Model Direct Effects with Financial Behavior (N = 12,598)*
Unstandardized Standardized
Parameter b SE β SE p
Financial Self-Efficacy→Fin. Behavior 1.407 0.02 0.812 0.00 0.00***
Financial Knowledge→Fin. Behavior 0.034 0.03 0.019 0.02 0.18
Financial Socialization→Fin. Behavior 0.051 0.01 0.029 0.01 .00***
Financial Education→Fin. Behavior 0.046 0.01 0.031 0.01 0.00***
Financial Self-Efficacy→Fin. Knowledge -.002 0.01 -.002 0.01 0.88
Financial Self-Efficacy→Fin. Socialization 0.131 0.00 0.131 0.00 0.00***
Financial Self-Efficacy→Fin. Education 0.022 0.00 0.025 0.00 .00***
Financial Knowledge→Fin. Socialization -.001 0.02 -.001 0.02 0.94
Financial Knowledge→Fin. Education 0.031 0.02 0.037 0.02 0.10
Financial Socialization→Fin. Education 0.610 0.01 0.525 0.01 0.00***
Female→Financial Socialization -.001 0.03 0.000 0.01 0.98
Black→Financial Socialization -.76 0.05 -.058 0.01 0.00***
Asian→Financial Socialization -.212 0.05 -.047 0.01 0.00***
Hispanic→Financial Socialization -.151 0.05 -.034 0.01 0.00**
Mixed Race→Financial Socialization -.057 0.05 -.015 0.01 0.24
Other Race→Financial Socialization -.385 0.07 -.047 0.01 .00***
Income→Financial Socialization 0.000 0.00 0.011 0.01 0.44
Parent Education→Financial Socialization 0.011 0.10 0.002 0.02 0.91
Four-Year Private→Fin. Socialization -.020 0.04 -.006 0.01 0.64
Two-Year Public→Fin. Socialization -.005 0.05 -.001 0.01 0.91
Age→Financial Socialization -.013 0.00 -.096 0.01 .00***
GPA→Financial Socialization 0.067 0.03 0.035 0.01 0.01**
First Year Students→Fin. Socialization -.068 0.03 -.026 0.01 .03*
Female→Financial Education -.105 0.04 -.042 0.02 0.01**
Black→Financial Education 0.053 0.06 0.010 0.01 0.36
Asian→Financial Education 0.078 0.08 0.015 0.01 0.30
Hispanic→Financial Education 0.077 0.06 0.015 0.01 0.19
Mixed Race→Financial Education 0.081 0.07 0.019 0.02 0.23
Other Race→Financial Education 0.137 0.13 0.014 0.01 0.30
Income→Financial Education 0.000 0.00 0.008 0.01 0.51
Parent Education→Financial Education 0.015 0.12 0.002 0.02 0.90
Four-Year Private→Financial Education -.030 0.06 -.007 0.01 0.59
Two-Year Public→Financial Education -.046 0.06 -.011 0.01 0.40
Age→Financial Education 0.005 0.00 0.031 0.01 0.01**
GPA→Financial Education -.002 0.03 -.001 0.01 0.95
First Year Students→Financial Education 0.008 0.04 0.003 0.01 0.84
78
R-Squared .67
*Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001 All results were computed with Mplus in delta
parameterization and STDYX standardization.
Overall Model Fit Indices: χ2(226) = 5,917.36, p = <.001; RMSEA = .045, 90% CI [.044, .046],
CFI = .86, TLI = .83.
Reference Groups: male, White, four-year public, students enrolled two or more years
79
Table A.4 Direct Effects with Financial Behavior without First Year Students (N = 10,302)*
Unstandardized Standardized
Parameter b SE β SE p
Financial Self-Efficacy→Fin. Behavior 2.191 0.04 0.907 0.00 .00***
Financial Knowledge→Fin. Behavior 0.058 0.04 0.024 0.02 0.16
Financial Socialization→Fin. Behavior 0.075 0.01 0.031 0.00 .00***
Financial Self-Efficacy→Fin. Knowledge -.008 0.02 -.008 0.02 0.59
Fin. Self-Efficacy→Fin. Socialization 0.201 0.00 0.200 0.00 .00***
Financial Knowledge→Fin. Socialization 0.025 0.01 0.025 0.01 .03**
Female→Financial Socialization -.028 0.03 -0.013 0.02 0.4
Black→Financial Socialization -.349 0.05 -0.073 0.01 .00***
Asian→Financial Socialization -.132 0.06 -0.029 0.01 .03**
Hispanic→Financial Socialization -.144 0.06 -0.032 0.01 .02**
Mixed Race→Financial Socialization -.042 0.06 -0.011 0.02 0.49
Other Race→Financial Socialization -.476 0.06 -0.060 0.01 .00***
Income→Financial Socialization 0.001 0.00 0.022 0.01 0.11
Parent Education→Fin. Socialization -.024 0.09 -.004 0.01 0.78
Four-Year Private→Fin. Socialization -.023 0.04 -0.007 0.01 0.60
Two-Year Public→Fin. Socialization -.046 0.05 -0.012 0.01 0.34
Age→Financial Socialization -.012 0.00 -.088 0.01 .00***
GPA→Financial Socialization 0.075 0.03 0.037 0.01 .01**
R-Squared .84
*Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001 All results were computed with Mplus in delta parameterization and
STDYX standardization. Overall Model Fit Indices: χ2(204) = 4,234.14, p = <.001; RMSEA = .044, 90% CI [.043,
.045], CFI = .86, TLI = .84.
Reference Groups: male, White, four-year public, students enrolled two or more years
80
Table A.5 Direct Effect with Financial Behavior for Do Not Know Income (N = 12,598)*
Unstandardized Standardized
Parameter b SE β SE p
Financial Self-Efficacy→Financial Behavior 2.038 0.03 0.897 0.00 .00***
Financial Knowledge→Financial Behavior 0.050 0.04 0.022 0.02 0.23
Financial Socialization→Financial Behavior 0.042 0.01 0.018 0.01 .00***
Financial Self-Efficacy→Financial Knowledge -.002 0.01 -.002 0.01 0.87
Financial Self-Efficacy→Financial Socialization 0.166 0.00 0.165 0.00 .00***
Financial Knowledge→Financial Socialization 0.030 0.01 0.030 0.01 .00**
Female→Financial Socialization -.040 0.03 -.018 0.01 0.17
Black→Financial Socialization -.267 0.04 -.056 0.01 .00***
Asian→Financial Socialization -.201 0.05 -.045 0.01 .00***
Hispanic→Financial Socialization -.150 0.06 -.033 0.01 .01**
Mixed Race→Financial Socialization -.049 0.05 -.013 0.01 0.32
Other Race→Financial Socialization -.379 0.06 -.046 0.01 .00***
Do Not Know Income→Financial Socialization -.151 0.05 -.036 0.01 0.00**
Parent Education→Financial Socialization -.004 0.09 -.001 0.01 0.96
Four-Year Private→Financial Socialization -.032 0.04 -0.009 0.01 0.41
Two-Year Public→Financial Socialization -.019 0.04 -0.005 0.01 0.66
Age→Financial Socialization -.011 0.00 -.084 0.01 .00***
GPA→Financial Socialization 0.082 0.03 0.043 0.01 .00**
First Year Students→Financial Socialization -.070 0.03 -.027 0.01 .02*
R-Squared .81
* Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001 All results were computed with Mplus in delta parameterization and
STDYX standardization. Overall Model Fit Indices: χ2(216) = 4,616.87, p = <.001; RMSEA = .040, 90% CI
[.039, .041], CFI = .87, TLI = .85.
Reference Groups: male, White, know income, four-year public, students enrolled two or more
years
81
Table A.6 Direct Effects with Financial Behavior for Prefer Not to Answer (N = 12,598)*
Unstandardized Standardized
Parameter b SE β SE p
Financial Self-Efficacy→Financial Behavior 2.011 0.03 0.894 0.00 .00***
Financial Knowledge→Financial Behavior 0.050 0.03 0.022 0.02 0.14
Financial Socialization→Financial Behavior 0.053 0.01 0.023 0.00 .00***
Financial Self-Efficacy→Financial Knowledge -.004 0.01 -.004 0.01 0.78
Financial Self-Efficacy→Financial Socialization 0.164 0.00 0.164 0.00 .00***
Financial Knowledge→Financial Socialization 0.029 0.01 0.030 0.01 .00***
Female→Financial Socialization -.043 0.03 -0.020 0.01 0.12
Black→Financial Socialization -.274 0.05 -0.058 0.01 .00***
Asian→Financial Socialization -.206 0.05 -0.046 0.01 .00***
Hispanic→Financial Socialization -.148 0.06 -0.033 0.01 .01**
Mixed Race→Financial Socialization -.050 0.05 -0.013 0.01 0.31
Other Race→Financial Socialization -.398 0.07 -0.049 0.01 .00***
Prefer Not to Answer Inc.→Fin. Socialization -.013 0.07 -.003 0.02 0.86
Parent Education→Financial Socialization -.011 0.08 -.002 0.01 0.9
Four-Year Private→Financial Socialization -.033 0.04 -0.009 0.01 0.39
Two-Year Public→Financial Socialization -.019 0.04 -0.005 0.01 0.66
Age→Financial Socialization -.011 0.00 -.085 0.01 .00***
GPA→Financial Socialization 0.072 0.02 0.037 0.01 .00**
First Year Students→Financial Socialization -.073 0.03 -.028 0.01 .02*
R-Squared .81
* Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001 All results were computed with Mplus in delta parameterization
and STDYX standardization. Overall Model Fit Indices: χ2(216) = 4,607.70, p = <.001; RMSEA = .040,
90% CI [.039, .041], CFI = .87, TLI = .85.
Reference Groups: male, White, answered income, four-year public, students enrolled two or
more years
82
Table A.7 Glossary
83
Figure A.1 Significant Pathways in Structural Model Predicting Financial Behavior with Split Socialization Construct (N =
12,598)*
84
_____________________________________________________________________________________________________________________
*Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. Model Fit Indices: χ2(226) = 5,917.36, p = <.001; RMSEA = .045, 90% CI [.044, .046], CFI = .86, TLI =
.83. All results were computed with Mplus in delta parameterization and STDYX standardization. The structural model was estimated with
indicators from the measurement model for the latent variables and controls for gender, age, race, income, parent education, GPA, years enrolled
in school, and institution type.
85
Appendix B - SAS and Mplus Code
SAS Code
*Financial behaviors;
86
if paybills = -99 then bills_miss =1; else bills_miss = 0;
*Financial Socialization;
87
if parents_rolemodel =2 then pmodel = 2;
if parents_rolemodel = 3 then pmodel = 3;
if parents_rolemodel = 4 then pmodel = 4;
if parents_rolemodel = -99 then pmodel_miss = 1; else pmodel_miss =0;
if parents_rolemodel = . then delete;
88
if financeclass_highschool = 1 then HSfin = 1;
if financeclass_highschool = 2 then HSfin = 2;
if financeclass_highschool = 3 then HSfin = 3;
if financeclass_highschool = -99 then HSfin_miss = 1; else HSfin_miss =0;
if financeclass_highschool = . then delete;
income=annualincome_self;
if income = 1 then noinc=1; else noinc=0;
if income = 2 then inc2500=1; else inc2500=0;
if income = 3 then inc5000=1; else inc5000=0;
if income = 4 then inc7500=1; else inc7500=0;
if income = 5 then inc10k=1; else inc10k=0;
if income = 6 then inc15k=1; else inc15k=0;
if income = 7 then inc20k=1; else inc20k=0;
if income = 8 then inc25k=1; else inc25k=0;
if income = 9 then inc30k=1; else inc30k=0;
if income = 10 then incGT30K=1; else incGT30K=0;
*income as continuous;
89
if annualincome_self = 2 then inc=2;
if annualincome_self = 3 then inc=3;
if annualincome_self=4 then inc=4;
if annualincome_self =5 then inc=5;
if annualincome_self =6 then inc=6;
if annualincome_self =7 then inc=7;
if annualincome_self =8 then inc=8;
if annualincome_self =9 then inc=9;
if annualincome_self = 10 then inc=10;
if annualincome_self = 11 then DKinc=1; else DKinc=0;
if annualincome_self in (-99, 12) then PNAinc=1; else PNAinc=0;
if annualincome_self = . then delete;
* Financial Self Effiacy;
if confidentfinances = 1 then confident =1;
if confidentfinances = 2 then confident = 2;
if confidentfinances = 3 then confident =3;
if confidentfinances = 4 then confident = 4;
if confidentfinances = -99 then confidentfinances_miss =1; else confidentfinances_miss=0;
if confidentfinances = . then delete;
* financial knowledge;
if finknowledge_SCORE = . then delete;
fin1=finknowledge_1correct ;
fin2=finknowledge_2correct;
fin3=finknowledge_3correct;
fin4=finknowledge_4correct;
fin5=finknowledge_5correct;
90
finknow = (sum(fin1 + fin2 + fin3 +fin4)/4);
* GPA;
if gpa_recode = . then delete;
gpacat = gpa_recode;
* Age;
if age_category = . then delete;
agecat = age_category;
* Gender;
* Race;
if race = 1 then white=1; else white =0;
if race = 2 then black = 1; else black =0;
if race = 3 then hispanic = 1; else hispanic = 0;
if race = 4 then asian = 1; else asian = 0;
if race in (5,6,7,9) then other =1; else other = 0;
if race in (-99,10) then race_miss=1; else race_miss= 0;
if race = 8 then mixed=1; else mixed = 0;
if race = . then delete;
*parent education;
if education_mother = 1 then momLTHS=1; else momLTHS=0;
if education_mother = 2 then momHS = 1; else momHS=0;
if education_mother = 3 then momsocol=1; else momsocol =0;
if education_mother = 4 then momassoc = 1; else momassoc =0;
if education_mother = 5 then mombach=1; else mombach=0;
91
if education_mother = 6 then momgrad=1; else momgrad=0;
if education_mother in (7,8) then momprof=1; else momprof=0;
if education_mother = 9 then momDKed =1 ;else momDKed=0;
if education_mother in (., -99) then mother_ed_miss =1; else mother_ed_miss=0;
highestEd=max(education_mother, education_father);
* instiution type;
92
if InstType = 1 then fourpub = 1; else fourpub = 0;
if InstType = 2 then fourpriv = 1; else fourpriv =0;
if InstType = 3 then twopub = 1; else twopub =0;
VARIABLE:
NAMES ARE
studentid instcode InstType race yearsenrolled gpa_value
age
spendmor want budget spending bills savings checks
accting timely beyond
pcomfort pmoneymg
pmodel childall teenall HSwork save bank invest
HSfin Collfin impsoc fined expsoc monexp
inc DKinc PNAinc pinc
93
pDKinc pincPNA
confident mmwell FSE fin1 fin2 fin3 fin4 fin5
finknow
gpacat agecat male female
white black hispanic asian other mixed
parEd parDKed fourpub fourpriv twopub first_year;
MODEL:
Cfinsoc ON female
94
black
asian
hispanic
mixed
other
inc
pared
fourpriv
twopub
age
gpa_value
first_year;
Cfinsoc BY
impsoc*
fined
expsoc
monexp;
95
Cfinbehav BY accting*
timely
beyond;
Cfinknow BY fin1*
fin2
fin3
fin4;
CFSE@1;
Cfinsoc@1;
Cfinbehav@1;
Cfinknow@1;
[CFSE@0];
[Cfinsoc@0];
[Cfinbehav@0];
[Cfinknow@0];
Cfinbehav ON Cfinknow
CFSE
96
Cfinsoc;
CFSE ON Cfinknow
CFinsoc;
Cfinknow ON Cfinsoc;
97
!BY: define latent variables
! the first factor loading is fixed at 1.0 by default
!: free a parameter
!estimate factor loading of Great, Cheerful, and Happy
[email protected];
!@: fix a parameter at a specific value
!fix the variance of Positive at 1.0
OUTPUT:
modindices
TECH1
!request parameter specifications and starting values
STANDARDIZED;
!request two standardized coefficient
Mplus Code Split Model
TITLE:
Initial CFA
DATA:
FILE IS C:\Users\Christina Glenn\Dropbox\Dissertation\Data\NSFW2014Mplus.csv;
VARIABLE:
98
NAMES ARE
studentid instcode InstType race yearsenrolled gpa_value
age
spendmor want budget spending bills savings checks
accting timely beyond
pcomfort pmoneymg
pmodel childall teenall HSwork save bank invest
HSfin Collfin impsoc fined expsoc monexp
inc DKinc PNAinc pinc
pDKinc pincPNA
confident mmwell FSE fin1 fin2 fin3 fin4 fin5
finknow
gpacat agecat male female
white black hispanic asian other mixed
parEd parDKed fourpub fourpriv twopub first_year;
!names of variables in the data set
USEVARIABLES = gpa_value age
accting timely beyond HSfin Collfin
impsoc expsoc monexp inc
confident mmwell
fin1 fin2 fin3 fin4 female
black hispanic asian other mixed
99
parEd fourpriv twopub first_year;
MODEL:
Cfinsoc ON female
black
asian
hispanic
mixed
other
inc
pared
fourpriv
twopub
age
gpa_value
first_year;
100
Cfined ON
female
black
asian
hispanic
mixed
other
inc
pared
fourpriv
twopub
age
gpa_value
first_year;
Cfinsoc BY
impsoc*
101
expsoc
monexp;
Cfined BY
HSfin* (L1)
Collfin (L1);
Cfinbehav BY accting*
timely
beyond;
Cfinknow BY fin1*
fin2
fin3
fin4;
CFSE@1;
Cfinsoc@1;
Cfinbehav@1;
Cfinknow@1;
Cfined@1;
102
[CFSE@0];
[Cfinsoc@0];
[Cfinbehav@0];
[Cfinknow@0];
[Cfined@0];
Cfinbehav ON Cfinknow
CFSE
Cfinsoc
Cfined;
CFSE ON Cfinknow
CFinsoc
Cfined;
Cfinknow ON Cfinsoc
Cfined;
Cfined ON Cfinsoc;
103
! Construc BY race* yearsenrolled
! spendmor want budget spending bills savings checks finbehav
! pcomfort pmoneymg pmodel childall teenall
! HSwork save bank invest HSfin Collfin expsoc impsoc finsoc noinc
! inc2500 inc5000 inc7500 inc10k inc15k inc20k inc25k inc30k incGT30k DKinc PNAinc
! pincLT15k pinc30k pinc40k pinc60k pinc80k pinc100k pinc150k
! pinc200k pincG200 pDKinc pincPNA confident mmwell
! FSE finknow fin1 fin2 fin3 fin4 fin5
! gpacat agecat male female
! white black hispanic asian other mixed momLTHS momHS momsocol momassoc
! mombach momgrad momprof
! momDKed dadLTHS dadHS dadsocol dadassoc dadbach dadgrad dadprof dadDKed
! highestEd parLTHS parHS parsocol parassoc parbach pargrad parprof parDKed
! paredmis fourpub fourpriv;
104
!fix the variance of Positive at 1.0
OUTPUT:
modindices
TECH1
!request parameter specifications and starting values
STANDARDIZED;
!request two standardized coefficient
VARIABLE:
NAMES ARE
studentid instcode InstType race yearsenrolled gpa_value
age
spendmor want budget spending bills savings checks
accting timely beyond
pcomfort pmoneymg
105
pmodel childall teenall HSwork save bank invest
HSfin Collfin impsoc fined expsoc monexp
inc DKinc PNAinc pinc
pDKinc pincPNA
confident mmwell FSE fin1 fin2 fin3 fin4 fin5
finknow
gpacat agecat male female
white black hispanic asian other mixed
parEd parDKed fourpub fourpriv twopub first_year;
!names of variables in the data set
USEVARIABLES = gpa_value age
accting timely beyond
impsoc fined expsoc monexp inc
confident mmwell
fin1 fin2 fin3 fin4 female
black hispanic asian other mixed
parEd fourpriv twopub first_year ;
106
MODEL:
Cfinsoc ON female
black
asian
hispanic
mixed
other
inc
pared
fourpriv
twopub
age
gpa_value
first_year;
Cfinsoc BY
impsoc*
107
expsoc
monexp
fined;
Cfinbehav BY accting*
timely
beyond;
Cfinknow BY fin1*
fin2
fin3
fin4;
CFSE@1;
Cfinsoc@1;
Cfinbehav@1;
Cfinknow@1;
[CFSE@0];
[Cfinsoc@0];
108
[Cfinbehav@0];
[Cfinknow@0];
Cfinbehav ON Cfinknow
CFSE
Cfinsoc;
CFSE ON Cfinknow
CFinsoc;
Cfinknow ON Cfinsoc;
MODEL INDIRECT:
Cfinbehav IND Cfinknow Cfinsoc;
Cfinbehav IND CFSE Cfinsoc;
109
! pcomfort pmoneymg pmodel childall teenall
! HSwork save bank invest HSfin Collfin expsoc impsoc finsoc noinc
! inc2500 inc5000 inc7500 inc10k inc15k inc20k inc25k inc30k incGT30k DKinc PNAinc
! pincLT15k pinc30k pinc40k pinc60k pinc80k pinc100k pinc150k
! pinc200k pincG200 pDKinc pincPNA confident mmwell
! FSE finknow fin1 fin2 fin3 fin4 fin5
! gpacat agecat male female
! white black hispanic asian other mixed momLTHS momHS momsocol momassoc
! mombach momgrad momprof
! momDKed dadLTHS dadHS dadsocol dadassoc dadbach dadgrad dadprof dadDKed
! highestEd parLTHS parHS parsocol parassoc parbach pargrad parprof parDKed
! paredmis fourpub fourpriv;
110
OUTPUT:
modindices
TECH1
!request parameter specifications and starting values
STANDARDIZED;
!request two standardized coefficient
111