Learning A First Language
Learning A First Language
■ Their language reflects the word order of the language that they are hearing. The
combination of the words has a meaning relationship (learning through imitation).
■ Their language also shows they are able to apply the rules of the language to make
sentences which they have never heard before (creativity).
Patterns in L1 Development
Before First Words -
■ The earliest vocalizations
■ Involuntary crying (when they feel hungry or uncomfortable)
■ Cooing and gurgling – showing satisfaction or happiness
■ “Babbling”
■ Babies use sounds to reflect the characteristics of the different language they are
learning.
Patterns in L1 Development
First Words –
■ Around 12 months (“one-word” stage):
■ Babies begin to produce one or two recognizable words (esp. content word); producing
single-word sentences.
■ Acquisition of Questions
Acquisition of
Grammatical morphemes
Roger Brown’s study (1973):
- approximate order of acquiring grammatical morphemes
• Present progressive –ing (running)
• Plural –s (books)
• Irregular past forms (went)
• Possessive -’s (daddy’s hat)
• Copula (am/is/are)
• Articles (a/an/the)
• Regular past –ed (walked)
• Third person singular simple present –s (he runs)
• Auxiliary ‘be’ (He is coming)
Acquisition of
Grammatical morphemes
e.g., “wug test” –
• Order of events
• You took all the towels away because I can’t dry my hands.
Imitation and practice alone cannot explain some of the forms created by children.
Children appear to pick out patterns and then generalize or overgeneralize them to new
contexts. They create new forms or new uses of words.
Innatism: It’s all in your mind
■ Chomsky’s viewpoints:
■ Children are biologically programmed for language and language develops in the child
in just the same way that other biological functions develop.
■ The environment makes only a basic contribution, that is, the availability of people who
speak to the child. Therefore, the child’s biological endowment (LAD) will do the rest.
■ Children are born with a specific innate ability to discover for themselves the underlying
rules of a language system on the basis of the samples of a natural language they are
exposed to.
Innatism: It’s all in your mind
■ Chomsky argues that behaviorism cannot provide sufficient explanations for children’s
language acquisition for the following reasons:
1. Children come to know more about the structure of their language than they could
be expected to learn on the basis of the samples of language they hear.
2. The language children are exposed to includes false starts, incomplete sentences and
slips of the tongue, and yet they learn to distinguish between grammatical and
ungrammatical sentences.
3. Children are by no means systematically corrected or instructed on language by
parents.
Innatism: It’s all in your mind
■ Problems of Innatism:
■ The innatists placed too much emphasis on the “final state” (i.e. the linguistic
competence of adult native speakers), but not enough on the developmental
aspects of language acquisition.
■ Language acquisition is an example of children’s ability to learn from
experience. What children need to know is essentially available in the language
they are exposed to.
Interactionist/developmental Perspectives:
Learning from inside and out
■ This position views that language develops as a result of the interplay between the innate
learning ability of children and the environment in which they develop.
■ Developmental psychologists attribute more importance to the environment than the
innatists, though they also recognize a powerful learning mechanism in the human brain.
■ They see language acquisition as similar to and influenced by the acquisition of other kinds
of skill and knowledge, rather than as something that is largely independent of the child’s
experience and cognitive development.
The Interactionist Position
■ Piaget: Language is dependent upon and springs from cognitive development. That is,
children’s cognitive development determines their language development.
(e.g., the use of words as “bigger” or “more” depends on children’s understanding of the
concepts they represent.)
■ He argued that the developing cognitive understanding is built on the interaction between
the child and the things which can be observed, touched, and manipulated.
■ For him, language was one of a number of symbol systems developed in childhood, rather
than a separate module of the mind. Language can be used to represent knowledge that
children have acquired through physical interaction with the environment.
The Interactionist Position
• Vygotsky: sociocultural theory of human mental processing. He argued that language
develops primarily from social interaction.
• Zone of proximal development (ZPD): a level that a child is able to do when there is
support from interaction with a more advanced interlocutor. That is, a supportive
interactive environment enables children to advance to a higher level of knowledge
and performance than s/he would be able to do independently.
• He observed the importance of conversations which children have with adults and
with other children and saw in these conversations the origins of both language and
thought.
The Interactionist Position
■ How Piaget’s view differs from Vygotsky’s:
■ Piaget hypothesized that language developed as a symbol system to express
knowledge acquired through interaction with the physical world.
■ Vygotsky hypothesized that thought was essentially internalized speech, and
speech emerged in social interaction.
The Interactionist Position
■ Language socialization framework: observed from childrearing patterns
(parent-child interaction)
■ “Sequential bilinguals”
■ Children who begin to learn a second language after they have acquired
the first language.
Childhood bilingualism
■ Is it difficult for children to cope with 2 language?
1. There is little support for the myth that learning more than one language in early childhood
slows down the child’s linguistic development or interferes with cognitive and academic
development.
2. Bilingualism can have positive effects on abilities that are related to academic success,
such as metalinguistic awareness.
3. The learning of languages for bilingual children is more related to the circumstances in
which each language is learned than to any limitation in the human capacity to learn more
than one language.
Childhood bilingualism
■ Language attrition for bilinguals -
“Subtractive bilingualism” (Lambert, 1987)
▪ When children are “submerged” in a second language for long periods in early schooling,
they may begin to lose their native language (L1) before they have developed an age-
appropriate mastery of the L2.
▪ It can have negative consequences for children’s self-esteem.
▪ In some cases, children continue to be caught between two languages; not having
mastered the L2, but not having continued to develop the L1.
Childhood bilingualism
■ Solution for “subtractive bilingualism”:
to strive for “additive bilingualism”
■ Parents should continue speaking the L1 to their children to maintain the home
language, while the L2 is being learned at school.
■ Maintaining the family language also creates opportunities for the children to continue
both cognitive and affective development in a language they understand easily while
they are still learning the L2.
Summary
■ Each of the three theoretical approaches explains a different aspect of
first language acquisition.
1. Behaviorists (learning through imitation, practice, reinforcement, habit-
formation) – the acquisition of vocabulary and grammatical morphemes.
2. Innatists (LAD/UG/CPH) – the acquisition of complex grammar (structure of
the language).
3. Interactionists (social interaction) – the acquisition of how form and meaning
are related, how communicative functions are carried out, and how language
is used appropriately.