0% found this document useful (0 votes)
37 views14 pages

The Effect of A Specialized Dyslexia Font, Opendyslexic, On Reading Rate and Accuracy

This study investigated the impact of the OpenDyslexic font on reading rate and accuracy compared to Arial and Times New Roman fonts. Elementary students with dyslexia participated in tasks involving letter naming, word reading, and nonsense word reading presented in the three fonts. Results from the alternating treatment experiment showed no improvement in reading rate or accuracy for individual students or the group as a whole when using the OpenDyslexic font. While some students noted it was different, none reported a preference for it. The findings indicate the specialized dyslexia font may provide no benefit over typical fonts.

Uploaded by

Estante de Cores
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
37 views14 pages

The Effect of A Specialized Dyslexia Font, Opendyslexic, On Reading Rate and Accuracy

This study investigated the impact of the OpenDyslexic font on reading rate and accuracy compared to Arial and Times New Roman fonts. Elementary students with dyslexia participated in tasks involving letter naming, word reading, and nonsense word reading presented in the three fonts. Results from the alternating treatment experiment showed no improvement in reading rate or accuracy for individual students or the group as a whole when using the OpenDyslexic font. While some students noted it was different, none reported a preference for it. The findings indicate the specialized dyslexia font may provide no benefit over typical fonts.

Uploaded by

Estante de Cores
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 14

Ann.

of Dyslexia
DOI 10.1007/s11881-016-0127-1

The effect of a specialized dyslexia font, OpenDyslexic,


on reading rate and accuracy

Jessica J. Wery 1 & Jennifer A. Diliberto 2

Received: 21 October 2015 / Accepted: 19 February 2016


# The Author(s) 2016. This article is published with open access at Springerlink.com

Abstract A single-subject alternating treatment design was used to investigate the extent to
which a specialized dyslexia font, OpenDyslexic, impacted reading rate or accuracy compared
to two commonly used fonts when used with elementary students identified as having
dyslexia. OpenDyslexic was compared to Arial and Times New Roman in three reading tasks:
(a) letter naming, (b) word reading, and (c) nonsense word reading. Data were analyzed
through visual analysis and improvement rate difference, a nonparametric measure of
nonoverlap for comparing treatments. Results from this alternating treatment experiment show
no improvement in reading rate or accuracy for individual students with dyslexia, as well as
the group as a whole. While some students commented that the font was Bnew^ or Bdifferent^,
none of the participants reported preferring to read material presented in that font. These results
indicate there may be no benefit for translating print materials to this font.

Keywords Decoding . Dyslexia . Fluency . Font . Learning disabilities . OpenDyslexic . Reading

Introduction

An estimated 15–20 % of English-speaking school-aged children experience difficulty learn-


ing to read (International Dyslexia Association (IDA), 2007; Lyon, Shaywitz, & Shaywitz,
2003). Within the USA, the prevalence rate of dyslexia is estimated to be between 10 and 15 %
(Fletcher, Lyon, Fuchs, & Barnes, 2007; Eden & Moats, 2002). Since its discovery, dyslexia
has been highly researched and debated (Washburn, Binks‐Cantrell, & Joshi, 2013).

* Jessica J. Wery
[email protected]

Jennifer A. Diliberto
[email protected]

1
School of Education, Elon University, 2105 Campus Box, Elon, NC 27244, USA
2
School of Education, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, CB#3500, 201F Peabody, Chapel
Hill, NC 27599-3500, USA
J.J. Wery, J.A. Diliberto

Researchers in a variety of disciplines, including medicine, psychology, and education, have


contributed to our understanding of dyslexia, as well as the methods and interventions that are
effective for students with this disability.
Several authorities have put forth definitions of dyslexia. IDA has adopted the definition of
dyslexia published by Lyon et al. (2003):
Dyslexia is a specific learning disability that is neurological in origin. It is characterized
by difficulties with accurate and/or fluent word recognition and by poor spelling and
decoding abilities. These difficulties typically result from a deficit in the phonological
component of language that is often unexpected in relation to other cognitive abilities
and the provision of effective classroom instruction. Secondary consequences may
include problems in reading comprehension and reduced reading experience that can
impede growth of vocabulary and background knowledge (p.2).
Dyslexia is also included within the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA,
2004), within Bspecific learning disability (SLD)^, one of the 13 disability categories. The
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-V; American Psychiatric
Association, 2013) includes dyslexia within Blearning disorder.^ Unfortunately, the inconsis-
tency in terminology and the lack of one universally agreed upon definition of dyslexia has
caused some confusion among special educators, administrators, and parents. However, most
agree that dyslexia is a distinct type of SLD that presents in a difficulty with phonological
coding (Shaywitz et al., 2004; Snowling, 2009).
As reading and writing have become increasingly crucial for success in and out of school,
students with dyslexia are often at-risk for academic failure, lower reading self-efficacy
(Burden, 2008), and lower self-esteem (Alexander-Passe, 2006) as well as an increased risk
of dropping out of school (U.S. Department of Education, Office of Special Education and
Rehabilitative Services, Office of Special Education Programs, 2006). Due to the potential for
poor school and post-school outcomes, teachers, parents, and advocates often feel desperate to
locate and employ accommodations and interventions to help students with dyslexia read.
Recently, two specialized typefaces or fonts BOpenDyslexic^ (OD; Gonzalez, 2012) and
BDyslexie^ (Boer, 2008) have been developed that purport to increase readability for those
with dyslexia. These fonts differ from the other, more traditional fonts because the letters have
been designed to have thicker or Bheavier^ lines near the bottom of the letters (See Fig. 1). The
typeface developers of these fonts claim that this Bheaviness^ prevents the letters from turning
upside down for readers with dyslexia, and makes it easier for people with dyslexia to
distinguish individual letters while reducing reading errors and the effort it takes to read text
(http://www.studiostudio.nl/en/information/).
Perhaps, the typeface developers developed this font based on the same misconception that
dyslexia is characterized by letter reversals, what is commonly held by teachers (Washburn et al.,
2013). However, four decades of research on dyslexia suggests reading difficulties stem from more
basic deficits in alphabetic and phonological coding are the probable causes of the disorder rather
than visual, semantic, or syntactic deficits (Vellutino, Fletcher, Snowling, & Scanlon, 2004).
Despite this erroneous foundation, these newly designed fonts have captured public attention
across the world. National Public Radio, Scientific American, and eschool news (http://www.
eschoolnews.com/2015/09/04/dyslexia-online-font-593/comment-page-1/#comment-230792)
have featured interviews with one of the developers, and several websites contain numerous
testimonials touting the benefits of the specialized fonts. Additionally, many of these websites
highlight these testimonials on social media sites such as Facebook, heightening its visibility.
The effect of specialized font, OpenDyslexic

Fig. 1 OpenDyslexic font.


Source: BBC.com

Today, the U.S. Department of Education places an increased emphasis on greater rigor in
educational research (Institute of Educational Sciences, 2010), and legislative mandates call for
the use of research-based practices in schools (NCLB, 2001; ESRA, 2002; IDEIA, 2004).
These legal and policy changes as well as calls from the research academy (e.g., Horner et al.,
2005; Gersten et al., 2005) further movements for evidence-based interventions, practices, or
treatments in the field of special education (Odom, 2009; Odom et al., 2005), thus highlighting
the importance of investigations into the efficacy of specialized fonts. Yet, no empirical
research could be identified that investigated the effectiveness of the font with English readers,
and very little research exists on its effectiveness in readers of other languages.

Extant research

Two studies have investigated the effect of specialized fonts used with students with
dyslexia. Rello and Baeza-Yates (2013) measured eye-tracking recordings of Spanish
readers with dyslexia (aged 11–50) and found that OD did not significantly improve
reading time nor shorten eye fixation. In her master’s thesis, de Leeuw (2010) compared
Arial and Dyslexie with 21 Dutch students with dyslexia and found Dyslexie did not lead
to faster reading, but may help with some dyslexic-related errors. To date, no peer-
reviewed research studies report the use of a Bdyslexia-friendly^ font with English-
speaking and English-reading students. Thus, research is needed to determine the effec-
tiveness of a dyslexia-friendly font on reading rate and accuracy in English-speaking
students identified with dyslexia prior to its widespread use.
Therefore, the purpose of the current investigation is to determine the effectiveness of a
specialized dyslexia font on reading speed and accuracy on three reading tasks: (a) letter
naming, (b) word reading, and (c) nonsense word reading. These three types of reading tasks
were selected based on their strong correlation to reading achievement and ability to measure
both reading accuracy and speed. The independent variables compared were the fonts (a) Arial,
a san serif font; (b) Times New Roman (TNR), a serif font; and (c) OD, a specialized dyslexia
font. Here, Arial and TNR both commonly used fonts are baseline conditions 1 and 2,
respectively. OD is the treatment condition.
J.J. Wery, J.A. Diliberto

Method

The current investigation utilized a single-subject alternating treatment design to determine the
differences among three separate fonts within three reading tasks in elementary school students
identified with dyslexia. Single-subject research is considered a rigorous, scientific experi-
mental methodology used to establish evidence-based practices. The purpose of single-subject
designs is to document functional relationships between independent and dependent variables
thus focusing on practical significance as an outcome (Horner et al., 2005). Single-subject
research data is typically compared systematically thought visual representations within and
across conditions of a study using graphic representations (Alberto & Troutman, 2009).

Setting and participants

Students attending a K-12 independent urban school for students with dyslexia and
related disorders, located in the southeast USA, participated in this study, with a total
school enrollment of 160. Students who were invited to participate were those who were
(a) of elementary age, (b) with confirmed and current diagnoses of dyslexia (or a specific
learning disability in the areas of phonological processing or decoding based on psycho-
educational assessments conducted by an outside evaluator required for admission to the
school), (c) with normal vision, and (d) no comorbid diagnoses (e.g., attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder, autism). Of the 13 students who were identified and invited, 12
returned, signed parental consent forms and participated in this study. Table 1 provides
information about the age, grade, gender, ethnicity, and lexile levels of the participants.
Of students that participated in the study, seven were female and five were male. Three
students were in third grade, three in fourth grade, two in fifth grade, and four were in
sixth grade. Their lexile levels ranged from 120 to 1137. The probes created by the
authors were based on the lower lexile scores of the participants.

Table 1 Participants

Pseudonym Age Grade Sex Race/ethnicity Lexile

Randy 12–4 6th M C 450–600


Steve 12–8 6th M C 250–400
Chris 12–5 6th M C 900–1050
Elizabeth 12–5 6th F C 800–950
Kelly 11–6 5th F C 500–660
Audrey 10–11 5th F C 987–1137
Michael 10–7 4th M C 500–650
Bethany 10–4 4th F C 339–489
Jake 10–2 4th M C 250–400
Ellen 9–0 3rd F C 120–350
Lilly 10–4 3rd F A 159–309
Madison 10–4 3rd F C 519–669

M male, F female, A Asian, C Caucasian


The effect of specialized font, OpenDyslexic

Measures/dependent variables

Within single case research design, dependent variables should be selected for their social
significance (Horner et al., 2005). Therefore, three types of reading tasks were selected because
of their strong correlation to reading achievement and sensitivity to both reading accuracy and
speed: (a) letter naming (Fuchs, Compton, Fuchs, Bouton, & Caffrey, 2011), (b) real word
decoding, and (c) nonsense word decoding (Nunes, Bryant, & Barros, 2012; Kendeou, van den
Broek, White, & Lynch, 2009). Further, these tasks directly measure the anecdotal claims made
by the font developers. The letter-naming probes acted as a measure of rapid automatic naming
and letter identification in isolation. The real word list provided a measure of word reading or
decoding and the nonsense word list allowed for a measure of sound-symbol correspondence that
was free from effects of previous reading learning and memorization. A measure of reading
comprehension was not selected because reading comprehension can be influenced by many
other reading and executive functioning skills unrelated to decoding of print.

Stimulus material Three sets of research-created probes were created, one set for each
reading task: (a) letter-naming, (b) real word decoding, and (c) nonsense word decoding
(See Fig. 2). The letter-naming probes contained a list of randomly ordered upper and
lowercase letters. The real word list contained phonetically regular one- and two-syllable
words. The nonsense word list contained nonreal words following typical orthographic
patterns. From each set, seven randomly ordered lists were generated. Each list was then
printed in each of the three fonts (Arial 12, OD 10, TNR 12). The nominal font sizes varied in
order to keep the physical size of the font consistent across probes. Each list consisted three
columns of double-spaced rows and was printed on standard white copy paper.

Fig. 2 Probe examples: letter naming in TNR, words in OD, and nonsense words in Arial
J.J. Wery, J.A. Diliberto

Procedure

For each session, participants read a letter-naming list, a real word list, and a nonsense word
list each for one minute. If a student hesitated for 4 s, he or she was prompted to skip that item
and move to the next one. A digital timer with an audible alarm/bell was used to time each 1-
min reading session. The researchers recorded student responses on identical observer copies
of the student probes. A random-number calculator (Microsoft Excel) was used in order to
randomize the font sequence to ensure the order of presentation did not affect the decoding rate
or accuracy.

Inter-observer agreement Each session was audio-recorded. Later, a graduate student not
familiar with the research question compared the researcher-marked observer copy to the audio
recording. Inter-observer agreement was calculated with the following formula: agreements/
(agreements + disagreements). Inter-observer agreement was conducted on at least 96 % of all
administrations with a median agreement of 100 % (range = 99.3–100 %) for letter naming,
100 % (range = of 98.2–100 %) for real word reading, and 100 % (range = of 97.9–100 %) for
nonsense word reading.

Experimental design

This study utilized an alternating treatment design (ATD), a form of single-subject


research, to investigate the effects of the specialized dyslexia font, OD, compared to
Arial and Times New Roman fonts on reading accuracy and speed in elementary-aged
readers with dyslexia. ATD is an ideal design for comparing the effects of two or more
treatments in applied research, and can identify the presence or absence of a causal
relationship between the independent variable (e.g., font type) and a change in the
dependent variable (e.g., reading speed and accuracy (Smith, 2012). ATDs control for
internal threats to validity related to inter-subject variability by essentially dividing each
participant into multiple identical participants receiving each treatment (Martella, Nelson,
& Marchand-Manella, 1999). Further, ATDs that include random assignment of treatment
order and replication across multiple participants rule out rival hypotheses, resulting in
elegant control of internal threats to validity (Horner et al., 2005).

Data analysis

The results of this study were analyzed through visual analysis, as well as nonparametric
statistical analysis.
Visual analysis was used to evaluate the outcomes of this study. A positive effect is present
when there is (a) a consistent level, trend, and variability within each phase or condition; (b) an
immediate effect, proportion of overlap, consistent data across phases, and projected patterns
of the dependent variable to determine the presence of an intervention effect; and (c) absence
of anomalies within the data (e.g., sudden changes in level or trend).
While visual analysis has been shown to be effective in detecting large, practically
important and clinically significant participant outcomes, it can be insensitive to smaller effects
(Glass, 1997; Parsonson & Baer, 1992). Therefore, we also conducted statistical tests to
identify and summarize the effect of each font.
The effect of specialized font, OpenDyslexic

Because the data did not meet the parametric assumptions (e.g., parametric assumptions of
normally distributed data, homogeneity of variance of the residuals, and the independence of
the distribution of the residuals; Campbell, 2004; West & Hepworth, 1991; Hersen & Barlow,
1976), a nonparametric effect size calculation was used. Distribution-free nonparametric
models are not impeded by these parametric assumption violations (Parker & Vannest,
2009) and are a measure of practical significance (Parker, Vannest, & Brown, 2009).
While there are several nonparametric methods for calculating effect size for SCDs,
improvement rate difference (IRD) was selected for use here because it is an intuitive approach
that makes use of established effect sizes (e.g., Phi, Cohen’s Kappa and Cramer’s V). IRD is
the difference in improvement rates between baseline and treatment phases (Parker et al.,
2009) and is commonly used in medical research under the terms Brisk difference^ or Brisk
reduction^ (Vannest, Harrison, Temple-Harvey, Ramsey, & Parker, 2010). Confidence inter-
vals and p values for IRD can also be calculated using commonly available statistics modules.
The strengths of IRD for single case research are described at length in Parker et al. (2009).
IRD is calculated by finding the difference between the improvement rate in baseline and
the improvement rate in treatment. The improvement rate is for each phase is the number of
Bimproved data points^ divided by the total data points in that phase. In a treatment phase, an
improved data point is any point that exceeds all data points in the baseline phase. Within a
baseline phase, an improved data point is one that ties or exceeds any points in the treatment
phase. IRD is then calculated from these two independent proportions. For this analysis we
used the online calculator (Relative Risk and Risk Difference Confidence Intervals, version
1.0, Buchan, 2004; available at http://www.phsim.man.ac.uk/risk/Default.aspx).
The maximum score is 100 % IRD and occurs when there is no overlap between treatment
and baseline. A 50 % IRD, or chance levels of improvement, occurs when half of the scores
overlap between phases. A negative IRD score occurs when the treatment deteriorates below
baseline levels (Parker et al., 2009).
Confidence intervals were also calculated around IRD to provide a measure of confidence,
so that we can conclude with 95 % confidence that the true difference between the two
conditions (Arial vs OD, TNR vs OD) lies within the calculated interval.

Results

Eleven of the 12 students completed each of the three reading task probes (i.e., letter-naming,
word reading, and nonsense word reading). One male student was removed from the nonsense
word reading on the second day of data collection because his articulation patterns interfered
with accurate scoring. His nonsense word reading results were not included in this analysis.

Visual analysis

The whole group data and graphs were evaluated by the visual analysis procedures described
above. For both measures (i.e., speed and accuracy) of each reading task (i.e., letter naming,
word reading, and nonsense word reading), the visual analysis of the data does indicate an
overall increasing trend over time, as one would expect with the effect of practice (see Figs. 3,
4, 5, 6, 7, and 8). However, no individual trend line for any of the specific fonts demonstrated a
stronger increasing trend than any other. Further, the significant amount of overlap between
each of the individual font data lines indicates that no one font lead to significantly better or
J.J. Wery, J.A. Diliberto

Overall Letter Naming Fluency


100

80
Letters per Minute

60

40

20

0
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Session
Arial Dyslexie Times New Roman
Fig. 3 Overall average letter naming fluency (letters correct per minute). Accuracy

worse reading accuracy or speed. Further, individual student graphs and data were also
analyzed to investigate individual effects, none of which demonstrate a positive effect (Please
contact the first author for copies of individual student graphs).

Effect size calculation

IRD Arial, OD, and TNR were evaluated to determine if differences between the font in area
of reading fluency and accuracy for each of the reading tasks (See Tables 2 and 3). The
confidence intervals take into account the number of participants and observations, describe
how reliable survey results are, and provide a range within which the parameter is likely to
lie.On measures of reading fluency, OD produced negative results, or decreased students’
outcomes compared to both Arial and TNR, on all three reading tasks (i.e., letter naming, word
decoding, nonsense word decoding). ES ranged from −88.65 %, CI95 [−94.45, −77.57] Arial
compared to OD on word reading, to −49.65 %, CI95 [−63.33, −32.98] Arial compared to OD
on letter naming.

Fig. 4 Overall average letter Overall Letter Naming Accuracy


naming accuracy (letters correct/ 100%
total attempts)
80%
Percent Correct

60%

40%

20%

0%
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17
Session
Arial Dyslexie Times
The effect of specialized font, OpenDyslexic

Fig. 5 Overall average reading Overall Word Reading Fluency


fluency (words correct per minute) 60

Words per Minute


50
40
30
20
10
0
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Session
Arial Dyslexie Times New Roman

On measures of reading accuracy, OD also produced negative results, or decreased


students’ outcomes compared to both Arial and TNR, on all three reading tasks (i.e., letter
naming, word decoding, nonsense word decoding). ES ranged from −73.53 %, CI95 [−83.38,
−59.28] TNR compared to OD on word reading, to −63.62 %, CI95 [−74.96, −48.66] TNR
compared to OD on letter naming.
Based on visual and statistical analysis, there is Bno evidence^ of OD having a positive
effect on reading speed or accuracy.
While not the focus of this study, it is interesting to note that there appears to be no
practically significant difference between the use of TNR and Arial fonts, even though some
have opined a preference for one of the other (British Dyslexia Association, n.d).

Discussion

The development of the OD font may have been developed as the result of a common
misunderstanding of dyslexia. Many new teachers believe dyslexia is caused by a deficit in
visual perception (Allington, 1982; Bell, McPhillips, & Doveston, 2009; Hudson, High, & Al
Otaiba, 2007; Wadlington & Wadlington, 2005; Washburn, Joshi, & Binks, 2011a, Washburn,

Word Reading Accuracy


100%
90%
80%
Percent Correct

70%
60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
Session
Arial Dyslexie Times New Roman
Fig. 6 Overall average reading accuracy (letters correct/total attempts)
J.J. Wery, J.A. Diliberto

Overall Nonsense Word Reading Fluency


40

35
Nonsense Word per Minute

30

25

20

15

10

0
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19
Session
Arial Dyslexie Times New Roman
Fig. 7 Overall average nonsense word reading fluency (pseudo-words correct per minute)

Joshi, & Binks-Cantrell, 2011b; Washburn et al., 2013), which may have originated in a very
early use of the term Bword blindness^ (Das & Das, 2009) and perpetuated by the fact that
many students with dyslexia have letter reversals. While some with dyslexia do report
difficulty with vision, there is little evidence to support that this is related to dyslexia
(Christenson, Griffin, & Taylor, 2001; Fletcher, Foorman, Shaywitz, & Shaywitz, 1999). In
fact, emerging readers commonly reverse letters as they consolidate and make sense of the
sound-symbol system (Adams, 1998).
With the poor outcomes and personal struggles associated with dyslexia, teachers and
parents are eager to find interventions to improve reading outcomes. Consequently, people in a
variety of fields are seeking to help find solutions. However, to truly meet the needs of students

Overall Nonsense Word Reading Accuracy


100%
90%
80%
Percent Correct

70%
60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19
Session
Arial Dyslexie Times New Roman
Fig. 8 Overall average nonsense word reading accuracy (pseudo-words correct/total attempts)
The effect of specialized font, OpenDyslexic

Table 2 Fluency effect (correct letters or words per minute)

Reading task Contrast IRD ES (%) 95 % Confidence interval

Letter naming Arial vs OD −49.65 % −63.33, −32.98


TNR vs OD −67.73 % −73.60, −46.17
Word reading Arial vs OD −88.65 % −94.45, −77.57
TNR vs OD −82.81 % −90.01, −71.74
Nonsense word reading Arial vs OD −69.70 % −79.90, −55.74
TNR vs OD −77.24 % −85.99, −64.10.

OD OpenDyslexic, TNR Times New Roman

who struggle to learn to read, they need to be provided interventions that are empirically
proven to be effective. Given the press and popular support of using a specialized font as a
remedy for dyslexia, it is critical to highlight that results from this study failed to identify any
positive effect for using it. Currently, there is no documentation to support a specialized font is
an evidence-based practice. Teachers, administrators, and parents need to be aware of the lack
of empirical data supporting any positive effects of OD on reading before altering all written
material into a dyslexia-friendly font. If fact, using a font with claims to improve reading for
individuals with dyslexia without evidence to support this claim could result in further
frustrations by teachers, parents, and individuals with dyslexia when no difference is observed
after changing fonts used. Teachers and other practitioners need to be able to discriminate
between those interventions that have been empirically shown to be effective from those that
have not. While some may conclude that an intervention that fails to produce a positive effect
may not do good, but probably does not do harm, others disagree.
Inert interventions may in fact cause other forms of harm, in depriving resources (time and
financial) away from those interventions that have demonstrated efficacy. While the interven-
tion studied here, a freely available font, does not have costs associated with purchasing it,
there are financial and time cost associated with downloading it and transferring print materials
to the new font. That time and resource could be use on other interventions that are more likely
to improve students’ reading ability. Further, the use of unsubstantiated interventions can
impact the credibility of the profession, and lead to the public losing trust in special educators
(Lilienfeld, Lynn, & Lohr, 2015).
Finally, the most harm may come when students who have already experienced significant
struggle and academic failures related to learning to read, have yet another experience with

Table 3 Accuracy effect (correct/total attempts)

Reading task Contrast IRD ES (%) 95 % Confidence interval

Letter naming Arial vs OD −68.18 % −78.64, −54.10


TNR vs OD −63.62 % −74.96, −48.66
Word reading Arial vs OD −53.89 % −67.52, −36.70
TNR vs OD −73.53 % −83.38, −59.28
Nonsense word reading Arial vs OD −67.19 % −78.28, −52.02
TNR vs OD −75.81 % −85.09, −61.90

OD OpenDyslexic, TNR Times New Roman


J.J. Wery, J.A. Diliberto

failure when they are not able to read significantly better in a font designed to do so. A
repeated failure experience can further damage students’ self-efficacy and academic self-
esteem. Instead, students with dyslexia need well-qualified teachers and interventionists,
who can skillfully implement intensive instruction (Moats, 2009). This intensive systematic
intervention is likely to include direct multisensory instruction in the areas of phonological
awareness, phonics, and fluency (Mather & Wendling, 2012; Shaywitz, 2003; Snowling &
Hulme, 2012) that is both sequential and cumulative, and taught to automaticity (Moats, 2009).

Limitations and future research

While all three reading tasks were selected because of their sensitivity to small changes, and
nonsense word reading was selected because it reduces the influence of memorization, we did
not measure comprehension of connected text—the end goal of reading. However, this study
failed to find any positive effect of the specialized dyslexia font on the reading accuracy and
speed, we can assume it will also have no effect on reading comprehension, this study did not
directly measure that variable.
While single case research designs are empirical research designs, multiple independent
studies with similar results are needed to deem any intervention or practice to be Bevidence-
based^ (Kratochwill et al., 2010) Future research is needed to verify the results of this study.
While OD has not been shown to be an effective intervention for students with dyslexia,
that is not to say the field should not continue to search for and develop new and innovative
ways to improve outcomes for students. Since students with dyslexia are already behind their
nondisabled peers in regards to reading achievement, it become more critical that educators use
interventions empirically proven to be effective. Educators cannot waste time with interven-
tions having no empirical support. Our children do not need to experience another failure.

Compliance with ethical standards

Funding We received no financial support in conducting this research.

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and repro-
duction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a
link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

References

Adams, M. (1998). Beginning to read: thinking and learning about print. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Alberto, P.A., & Troutman, A. C. (2009). Applied behavior analysis for teachers. Pearson.
Alexander-Passe, N. (2006). How dyslexic teenagers cope: an investigation of self-esteem, coping and depres-
sion. Dyslexia, 12, 256–275.
Allington, R. L. (1982). The persistence of teacher beliefs in facets of the visual perceptual deficit hypothesis. The
Elementary School Journal, 82, 351–359.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
Washington, DC: Author.
Bell, S., McPhillips, T., & Doveston, M. (2009). How do teachers in Ireland and England conceptualise dyslexia?
Journal of Research in Reading, 34, 171–192.
Boer, C. (2008). Dyslexie font retrieved from http://www.dyslexiefont.com/
The effect of specialized font, OpenDyslexic

British Dyslexia Association (n.d.). Typefaces for dyslexia. Retrieved from http://bdatech.org/what-technology/
typefaces-for-dyslexia/
Buchan, I. (2004). Relative risk and risk difference confidence intervals, version 1.0.Public Health Informatics at
The University of Manchester/ http://www.phsim.man.ac.uk/risk/Default.aspx
Burden, R. (2008). Is dyslexia necessarily associated with negative feelings of self-worth? A review and
implications for future research. Dyslexia, 14(3), 188–196.
Campbell, J. M. (2004). Statistical comparison of four effect sizes for single-subject designs. Behavior
Modification, 28, 234–246. doi:10.1177/0145445503259264
Christenson, G. N., Griffin, J. R., & Taylor, M. (2001). Failure of blue-tinted lenses to change reading scores of
dyslexic individuals. Optometry, 71, 627–633.
Das, J. P., & Das, J. P. (2009). Reading difficulties and dyslexia: an interpretation for teachers. SAGE Publications.
de Leeuw, R., (2010). Special font for dyslexia? Retrieved from http://essay.utwente.nl/60474/
Eden, G., & Moats, L. (2002). The role of neuroscience in the remediation of students with dyslexia. Nature
Neuroscience, 5, 1080–1084.
Education Sciences Reform Act (ESRA). (2002).
Fletcher, J. M., Foorman, B. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1999). Conceptual and methodological
issues in dyslexia research: a lesson for develop mental disorders. In H. Tager-Flugsberg (Ed.),
Neurodevelopmental disorders (pp. 271–306). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Fletcher, J. M., Lyon, G. R., Fuchs, L. S., & Barnes, M. A. (2007). Learning disabilities: from identification to
intervention. New York: Guilford.
Fuchs, D., Compton, D. L., Fuchs, L. S., Bouton, B., & Caffrey, E. (2011). The construct and predictive validity
of a dynamic assessment of young children learning to read: implications for RTI frameworks. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 44, 339–347.
Gersten, R., Fuchs, L. S., Compton, D., Coyne, M., Greenwood, C., & Innocenti, M. S. (2005). Quality
indicators for group experimental and quasi-experimental research in special education. Exceptional
Children, 71(2), 149–164.
Glass, G. V. (1997). Education Policy Analysis Archives, 5, 1–22.
Gonzalez, A. (2014). OpenDyslexic: a font. Retrieved from http://opendyslexic.org/
Hersen, M., & Barlow, D. H. (1976). Single case experimental designs: strategies for studying behavior change.
New York: Pergamon Press.
Horner, R. H., Carr, E. G., Halle, J., McGee, G., Odom, S., & Wolery, M. (2005). The use of single-
subject research to identify evidence-based practice in special education. Exceptional Children,
71(2), 165–179.
Hudson, R. F., High, L., & Al Otaiba, S. (2007). Dyslexia and the brain: what does current research tell us? The
Reading Teacher, 60, 506–515.
Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act of 2004. 20 U. S. C. §1400 et seq. (2004).
Institute of Educational Sciences. (2010).
International Dyslexia Association. (2007). Dyslexia basics. Retrieved October 21, 2007 from http:// www.
interdys.org/ewebeditpro5/upload/Dyslexia_Basics_FS_-_final_81407.pdf
Kendeou, P., van den Broek, P., White, M. J., & Lynch, J. S. (2009). Predicting reading comprehension in early
elementary school: the independent contributions of oral language and decoding skills. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 101, 765–778. doi:10.1037/a0015956
Kratochwill, T. R., Hitchcock, J. J., Horner, R. H., Levin, J. R., Odom, S. L., Rindskopf, D., & Shadish, (2010).
What works clearinghouse: single-case designs technical documentation.
Lilienfeld, S. O., Lynn, S. J., & Lohr, J. M. (2015). Science and pseudoscience in clinical psychology, (2nd ed.).
Guilford Press.
Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53, 1–14.
Martella, R., Nelson, J. R., & Marchand-Manella, N. (1999). Research methods: learning to become a critical
research consumer. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Mather, N., & Wendling, B. (2012). Essentials of dyslexia: assessment and intervention. New York: Wiley.
Moats, L. (2009). Knowledge foundations for teaching reading and spelling. Reading and Writing, 22(4), 379–399.
No Child Left Behind (NCLB) Act of 2001, Public Law No. 107-110, § 115, Stat. 1425 (2002).
Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Barros, R. (2012). The development of word recognition and its significance for
comprehension and fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(4), 959–973.
Odom, S. L. (2009). The tie that binds: evidence-based implementation, science, and early intervention. Topics in
Early Childhood Special Education, 29, 53–61.
Odom, S. L., Brantlinger, E., Gersten, R., Horner, R. H., Thompson, B., & Harris, K. R. (2005). Research in
special education: scientific methods and evidence-based practices. Exceptional Children, 71, 137–148.
Parker, R. I., & Vannest, K. J. (2009). An improved effect size for single case research: non-overlap of all pairs
(NAP). Behavior Therapy, 40, 357–367. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2008.10.006
J.J. Wery, J.A. Diliberto

Parker, R. I., Vannest, K. J., & Brown, L. M. (2009). The improvement rate difference for single case research.
Exceptional Children, 75, 135–150.
Parsonson, B. S., & Baer, D. M. (1992). The visual analysis of data, and current research into the stimuli
controlling it. In T. R. Kratochwill & J. R. Levin (Eds.), Single-case research design and analysis: new
directions for psychology and education (pp. 15–40). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Rello, L., & Baeza-Yates, R. (2013). Good fonts for dyslexia. In Proceedings of the 15th International ACM
SIGACCESS Conference on Computers and Accessibility (p. 14). ACM.
Shaywitz, S. (2003). Overcoming Dyslexia. 53–58. Published by Alfred A.
Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Blachman, B., Pugh, K. R., Fulbright, R. K., Skudlarski, P., et al. (2004).
Development of left occipito-temporal systems for skilled reading in children after a phonologically-based
intervention. Biological Psychiatry, 55, 926–933.
Smith, J. D. (2012). Single-case experimental designs: a systematic review of published research and current
standards. Psychological Methods, 17(4). doi:10.1037/a0029312
Snowling, M. J. (2009). Changing concepts of dyslexia: nature, treatment and co-morbidity. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 609–618.
Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (2012). Annual research review: the nature and classification of reading
disorders—a commentary on proposals for DSM-5. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53,
593–607.
U.S. Department of Education, Office of Special Education and Rehabilitative Services, Office of Special
Education Programs. (2006). 26th annual (2004) report to congress on the implementation of the
Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (Vol. 1). Washington, DC: Author.
Vannest, K. J., Harrison, J. R., Temple-Harvey, K., Ramsey, L., & Parker, R. I. (2010). Improvement rate
differences of academic interventions for students with emotional and behavioral disorders. Remedial and
Special Education. doi:10.1177/0741932510362509
Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia):
what have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(1), 2–40.
Wadlington, E. M., & Wadlington, P. L. (2005). What educators really believe about dyslexia. Reading
Improvement, 42, 16–33.
Washburn, E. K., Joshi, R. M., & Binks-Cantrell, E. S. (2011). Are preservice teachers prepared to teach
struggling readers? Annals of Dyslexia, 61, 21–43.
Washburn, E., Joshi, R. M., & Binks, E. (2011). Teacher knowledge of basic language concepts and dyslexia.
Dyslexia, 17, 165–183.
Washburn, E. K., Binks-Cantrell, E. S., & Joshi, R. (2013). What do preservice teachers from the USA and the
UK know about dyslexia? Dyslexia, 20(1), 1–18.
West, S. G., & Hepworth, J. T. (1991). Statistical issues in the study of temporal data: daily experiences. Journal
of Personality, 59, 609–662.

You might also like