Cognitive Evolution From Single Cells To The Human Mind
Cognitive Evolution From Single Cells To The Human Mind
Cognitive Evolution From Single Cells To The Human Mind
cognitive neuroscience researchers. There remains nothing else like it in its scope as it
describes the evolution of the neural systems relevant to cognition from single organisms
to modern humans. The second edition of Cognitive Evolution is an essential read for
anyone who wants to be current in their understanding of cognitive evolution.
Arnold L. Glass, Professor of Psychology, Rutgers University, USA
Rich in detail, engagingly written and nicely illustrated, the book is an excellent resource
for upper-level undergraduates and graduate students in the cognitive sciences. It serves
as a reminder of how even simple cognitive tasks depend on a brain architecture and
circuitry of immense complexity with deep evolutionary roots.
Thurston C. Lacalli, Biology Department, University of Victoria, Canada
Cognitive Evolution
Cognitive Evolution provides an in-depth exploration of the natural history of cognition, from
the beginning of life on Earth to present-day humans. Drawing together evolutionary,
comparative, and neuroscience research, the book brings a unique cognitive perspective to
evolutionary psychology.
The second edition features the latest research and illustrations on emerging topics,
making it a true update of the field. After introducing evolution, Boles adopts an information
processing perspective – from inputs to outputs, with all the mental processes in between to
provide a systematic overview of the evolution of cognition, including its sensory, motoric,
perceptual, and cognitive components. The combination of evolutionary, comparative, and
neuroscience perspectives provides insights on topics like vision, handedness, tools and
planning, spatial perception, pattern recognition, memory, language, and consciousness.
Cognitive Evolution is a comprehensive, essential read for advanced undergraduate and
postgraduate students of cognitive and evolutionary psychology. Researchers will find it a
useful and insightful synthesis of the field, yet even the curious public will find in it much
that is surprising and enlightening.
David B. Boles
Cover image: Getty
Second edition published 2022
by Routledge
605 Third Avenue, New York, NY 10158
and by Routledge
4 Park Square, Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon, OX14 4RN
Routledge is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business
© 2022 David B. Boles
The right of David B. Boles to be identified as author of this work has been asserted in
accordance with sections 77 and 78 of the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988.
All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reprinted or reproduced or utilised in any form
or by any electronic, mechanical, or other means, now known or hereafter invented, including
photocopying and recording, or in any information storage or retrieval system, without
permission in writing from the publishers.
Trademark notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks or registered trademarks, and
are used only for identification and explanation without intent to infringe.
First edition published by Routledge 2019
Library of Congress Cataloguing-in-Publication Data
A catalog record has been requested for this book
DOI: 10.4324/9781003137863
Typeset in Bembo
by MPS Limited, Dehradun
For Joan, Walter, and Madeline.
Introduction xi
Acknowledgments xiv
SECTION I
Introduction to evolution 1
1 Life begins 3
2 Life gets complicated 17
3 Vertebrates to early mammals 30
4 Later mammals through primates 43
5 Humans 65
SECTION II
Sensation and movement 93
SECTION III
Perception and cognition 161
Glossary 304
References 315
Additional picture credits 369
Author index 370
Subject index 376
Introduction
Cognitive Evolution grew out of the needs of instructors and students for a textbook explaining
how we as a species acquired our mental lives. On the one hand, cognitive psychology has
had a rich history of developing descriptions of human mental processes and their
organization, placing them in an information processing framework covering inputs,
outputs, and all the processes in between. On the other hand, evolutionary scientists have
developed a large literature using fossils, artifacts, and comparisons between species to explore
the evolution of such processes. However, for the most part, the two hands have not worked
together to create a cognitive evolution textbook.
When I began teaching an evolutionary psychology course at the University of Alabama in
2001, I noticed the absence of such a textbook among others that took a social approach to
evolutionary psychology. Over the years, it has become clear to me that most evolutionary
courses have covered primarily social materials such as the evolutionary mechanisms relating
to mating, kinship, and competition and cooperation. While there is certainly nothing wrong
with that approach – in fact, it can be quite informative and engaging – it only incorporates a
fraction of the existing knowledge base of evolutionary psychology. A whole world of
cognitive evolution research has not been covered.
Existing in parallel, I came to suspect, was another world of college and university
professors coming from cognitive, perceptual, or neuroscience backgrounds, who might like
to teach an evolutionary psychology course from a different perspective to a universe of
students who could profit from it. In effect, what was needed was a book-length treatment
of “evolutionary cognitive psychology”, recognized by David Buss (2012) as one of several
approaches that together define a unified evolutionary psychology.
Thus, Cognitive Evolution was born, with most chapters of the book debuting in my
classroom in 2007. They have been continuously updated since. This edition cites a number
of references as late as 2021, as up-to-date as possible within the constraints placed by
production. I think readers will find that Cognitive Evolution offers a complete, consistent, and
coherent approach to the evolution of our mental lives that is unmatched by any other
textbook.
Before describing how this book can best be used, I would like to add a bit about its
reception. For several years, I conducted anonymous surveys of my students on a chapter-
wise basis, having a graduate student collect and keep them until grading was complete.
Although most of the survey items (questions) were open-ended, one was a 1-to-7 rating
as to how satisfied the student was with the chapter. Most recently, across the first
16 chapters of the book, the mean rating has been 5.7 and the median 6.0. It has been a
well-received book.
xii Introduction
How to use this book
The book is intended to be used by upper-level undergraduate and graduate students, as
well as by researchers and educators interested in a synthesis of the field. It is organized into
three major sections: Introduction to Evolution, Sensation and Movement, and Perception
and Cognition. Examples of courses in which it could be the primary text include
Evolutionary Psychology, Human Evolutionary Psychology, Evolution of Cognition,
Human Cognitive Evolution, Mind in Evolution, Evolution of Mind and Intelligence, and
Evolution of the Brain.
When I vetted the chapters with colleagues, one biology professor questioned whether it
was really necessary to include the Introduction to Evolution. The answer is an unequivocal
yes. Unlike biology courses, nearly every one of which is permeated by evolutionary
background, most psychology courses have developed for decades with hardly a nod to
evolution. Worse, the state of our public education system, not to mention our religious
education system, is such that in many districts, students receive little or no evolutionary
content. Without the introduction to evolution chapters, many students have insufficient
intellectual background to contend with the misconceptions about evolution that abound in
popular society.
The good news, however, is that the introductory chapters can be covered quickly in the
classroom. I have typically spent only 2 or 2-1/2 weeks to cover all five, and that even
includes a handful of supplementary readings added for good measure. The content is
sufficiently straightforward to allow that.
The second section of the book, on Sensation and Movement, consists of four chapters. In
information processing terms, it covers inputs and outputs, including the mechanical,
chemical, and visual senses, and postural, hand, and leg movements. When covering these
along with supplementary materials, I have allowed 2-1/2 to 3-1/2 weeks.
The real meat of the book, however, is in the seven chapters on Perception and
Cognition. By the time students tackle this material, they understand not only the
principles of evolution, but how information enters the human processing system and is
acted upon. They are fully ready to consider what happens in between. This is the section
that takes best advantage of the explosion of cognitive-related evolution research over the
past 25 years. For that reason, and because of the plethora of supplementary readings that
can be brought to bear, I have typically taken 7 to 7-1/2 weeks to cover this material.
Finally, a 17th chapter summarizes the book’s major findings in nine “firsts”. This requires
only a single session to cover.
Pedagogical mileage will likely vary, particularly with the nature of the course. Having
taught the material as a seminar, I often leave time at the end for student project reports, but
that time could instead be accounted for by interspersing additional supplementary material.
A lecture-based course, on the other hand, will have different needs and different timing.
Although I have not personally done it, I can easily envision developing a more general
lecture course on evolutionary psychology using this textbook supplemented with social
cognition chapters from other sources.
Mileage will also vary with the level of the student. Although the book was written with
upper level undergraduates in mind, I have also used it with PhD-level graduate students
with no complaints or problems. Yet, I have also tried to write it with sufficiently accessible
language that any reasonably well-educated person should be able to use it profitably. I would
be pleased to learn that all who use the book will, to some degree, expand their
understanding of how evolution has guided our thinking and perceiving.
Introduction xiii
I should note that this is a psychology book for those interested in human psychology.
Most of the questions it asks fit under the umbrella of “how did you and I come to
be?” I think that is a reasonable question, and it is assuredly the question that psychology
students with only limited exposure to the topic want answered. However, it is not
shy about taking on comparisons between animal species. Fish, bird, rodent, and
especially primate findings are prominent throughout, illuminating our emergence from
an animal background.
Acknowledgments
I would like to thank my wife, Joan Barth, and children Walter and Madeline for their
unbounded patience and support while I have repeatedly written and revised this text. My
daughter Madeline deserves special recognition for contributing her art.
I would also like to express my appreciation of my late mentor Steve Keele, and of
Mike Posner, Mary Rothbart, and Russell Fernald, then of the University of Oregon, for
supporting the unorthodox collection of major prelim topics that set me on the path
toward writing this book. I also thank the now-forgotten fellow student, a year or two
ahead of me in graduate school (it may have been Tram Neill or Terry Hines), who
convinced me to follow my own star in that endeavor, instead of taking a more orthodox,
“mainstream” cognitive psychology approach. Without that little push, all of this may
never have happened.
I would like to thank a number of Alabama colleagues, both from the university and from
the Geological Survey, who read preliminary drafts of some of the chapters and provided
feedback. They included, in alphabetical order, Fred Andrus, Jim Bindon, Joe Chandler, Fran
Conners, Michael Dillard, David Kopaska-Merkel, Tony Loewald, Ed Merrill, Brad Okdie,
Kelly Pivik, Leslie Rissler, Jason Scofield, Ed Stevenson, Carl Stock, and Emily Wakeman.
I also appreciate the more recent chapter reviews written by Terry Hines and by numerous
anonymous members of my undergraduate and graduate seminars.
Also deserving of thanks are those who helped develop the book for publication. For the
first edition, these included Elena Bellaart, Sophie Crowe, Chris Johnson, Ceri McLardy,
Beverly Peavler, and three anonymous experts who contributed full-book reviews through
Routledge. For the second edition they also include April Peake, Lucy Kennedy, Alex
Howard, Chloe Holland, Salonia Singhania, Helen Evans, and Sumit Kumar.
Finally, I would like to express gratitude to the personnel connected with two resource
repositories. First are the library staff at the University of Alabama. This book was only
made possible by their provision of extensive electronic journal resources covering
virtually every need for a PDF of an article and by their prompt interlibrary loan deliveries
of articles that weren’t available electronically. Second are the staff of Creative Commons,
the online repository of public domain and freely licensable graphics. Just a few years ago,
before their efforts produced explosive growth in useable files, this would have been a
much drabber and less informative book. It goes without saying, of course, that my thanks
extend to the multitude of persons who have made their graphic works available through
the Creative Commons website.
Section I
Introduction to evolution
1 Life begins
No one knows how it started. No one is sure exactly what started. But science is led by
best guesses, and a good guess is that at some point a little over 4 billion years ago, almost a
half billion years after Earth’s formation, a complex molecule became self-replicating.
From that humble, nearly invisible beginning, increased complexity was inevitable due
to descent with modification, acted upon by natural selection. We’ll return to that idea
later to consider it more carefully. First, though, let’s look at the question of how life on
Earth originated.
a protocell? Somehow chemicals must have traversed the protocell’s membrane, as we will
soon consider.
The beginning of life was a sufficiently complex process that scientific best guesses as to
where and how it originated are incomplete at best. Only recently, after millions of years
of human cognitive evolution, have we had the intellectual tools to identify a range of
possibilities. First, what if life originated…
Did life start with a dose of cyanide? In recent years the favorite poison of
screenwriters has undertaken a new, more productive role, starring in one scenario
for the origin of life.
On early Earth, hydrogen cyanide (HCN) was generated in the reducing
atmosphere, and condensed in water sources such as rain, lakes, and oceans ( Das,
Ghule, & Vanka, 2019). When sufficiently concentrated, as in repeatedly
evaporated pools associated with alkaline lakes ( Toner & Catling, 2019), the
HCN reacted with hydrogen sulfide and ultraviolet light. Given natural variation
between pools in available metals, the result was a web of chemical reactions
capable of producing an impressive array of lipids (fatty or waxy molecules), amino
acids (the building blocks of proteins), and nucleic acids ( Patel, Percivalle, Ritson,
Duffy, & Sutherland, 2015).
All of these components could then have come together when rain caused
overflows, combining the ingredients for the emergence of life ( Service, 2015). For
the most part, its high reactivity with the biological domain having played its role,
cyanide then stepped out of the picture – to achieve fame again with the much later
invention of the murder mystery.
Is this how life began? As in the world of film, a plausible story isn’t necessarily a true
one. Already alternative scripts have emerged, involving cyanide-based chemical
reactions in the atmosphere itself, aided by energy imparted by meteors ( Ferus et al.,
2020), or in meteor impact craters ( Osinski, Cockell, Pontefract, & Sapers, 2020).
… On clay surfaces?
Another proposal is that chemicals were concentrated on wet clay, which was abundantly
produced by early volcanic activity. Clay’s advantage over ponds and pools is that it not only
concentrates but also scaffolds: That is, it allows molecules to organize on its surface.
Experiments find that volcano-derived clay aids the formation of short chemical chains.
Amino acids, RNA, and proteins bind efficiently to it, especially when the clay carries a
negative charge as it typically does (Ferris, 2005; Zhu et al., 2019). Short RNA chains can
also grow much longer on clay surfaces (Ferris, Joshi, Wang, Miyakawa, & Huang, 2004). In
addition, clay provides protection against reactive chemicals, and it aids RNA self-replication
(Franchi & Gallori, 2005).
Or did life begin…
… In space?
Reports of organic compounds in meteorites first surfaced in the 19th century, but were
discredited because of contamination due to handling and storage. By the second half of the
8 Introduction to evolution
20th century, however, sophisticated analyses showed that some uncontaminated meteorites
contain amino acids as well as long-chain hydrocarbons. These molecules are assumed to
have been created by nonbiological processes.
As three-dimensional chemical structures, most amino acids are asymmetric and can exist
equally well in both “left-handed” and “right-handed” forms. Yet on Earth, they are almost
universally left-handed. One advantage of space-based explanations of amino acid creation is
that certain stellar mechanisms can selectively produce left-handedness. Thus, the mind-
boggling possibility exists that a past merging of two neutron stars provided both isotopes and
energy to nearby meteoroids, triggering the formation of predominantly left-handed amino
acids that later found their way to Earth (Famiano, Boyd, Kajino, Onaka, & Mo, 2019).
Although amino acids are not RNA, the formation of complex organic molecules in
extraterrestrial matter allows us to speculate that RNA or its precursors could likewise have
fallen to Earth in meteors, dust grains, or comets. Nucleobases have in fact been found in
meteorites, including uracil, an important component of RNA but not DNA (Pearce &
Pudritz, 2015). Thus, space-originated nucleobases could have helped create RNA world. As
an added bonus, meteorites may have been a major source of phosphate, an important
component in the chemistry of early life (Ritson, Mojzsis, & Sutherland, 2020), and as we
have seen, in the structure of RNA.
Experiments exposing common space materials to radiation like that found in nebulae
have produced a rich array of organic compounds, including amino acids and the full set of
RNA and DNA nucleobases (adenine, cytosine, guanine, thymine, and uracil). The space
materials are powdered meteorites from which organic compounds had been removed, and
formamide, an amide of formic acid. The implication is that important compounds needed to
originate life may have been synthesized in space and then fallen to Earth (Saladino et al.,
2015, 2017).
Even more intriguingly, in 1996, NASA scientists announced fossil evidence of microbial
life in a meteorite discovered 12 years prior on Antarctic ice. The meteorite had presumably
been blasted from the planet Mars by an asteroid impact, for it chemically resembled other
meteorites in which the levels of the inert gases argon, krypton, neon, and xenon matched
concentrations in Mars’ atmosphere (Gibson, McKay, Thomas-Keptra, & Romanek, 2000;
Norton, 1998). The apparent fossils were segmented bodies (Figure 1.2) resembling fossil
bacteria on Earth, but smaller (Gibson et al., 2000).
This extraordinary claim was immediately subjected to intense scientific scrutiny. Some
findings, such as organic material in the meteorite and bacteria-like bodies near its surface,
were found to be attributable to terrestrial contamination. Specifically, the bodies resembled
a bacterium, called Actinomycetales, commonly found in Antarctica (Steele et al., 2000).
Certainly, terrestrial contamination seemed quite possible, since the meteorite had lain in
Antarctica an estimated 13,000 years before its discovery (Gibson et al., 2000).
In a 2008 article, Schulze-Makuch and colleagues reviewed the evidence. They noted that
two other Martian meteorites contain evidence of borehole-like structures, on a microbial
scale, similar to those found in basaltic rocks on Earth, and that the Earth structures test
positive for cellular material. However, terrestrial contamination could still not be ruled out
(Schulze-Makuch, Fairen, & Davila, 2008).
These observations counsel caution in interpreting Martian meteorites, but another ob-
servation is equally important. As others have noted, even if life came from space in cellular
form, that simply pushes the question of life’s origins to an older time and another place. It
seems at least as plausible that life emerged here by building on amino acids and other
complex molecules delivered from space.
So, at the end of the day, how did life emerge? It’s clear from the diverse views just
presented that no one really knows. Certainly, there is no scientific consensus. A diversity of
Life begins 9
Figure 1.2 Evidence for life on Mars? Some have claimed that this 380-nanometer object is a fossilized
bacterium.
Source: ALH84001 structures.jpg, public domain, NASA.
views does not mean that anything is possible, however. Warm little ponds, clay surfaces,
black smokers, nuclear geysers, and space are among a very few possibilities that sensibly
could have sparked life. Possibly, some combination generated it – e.g., with space providing
some of the hydrocarbons that evaporation concentrated in ponds, and with clay or mica
scaffolding the first RNA.
DNA world
Although they had a membrane and a means of metabolism, the first protocells were still
RNA-based. At that point, RNA’s role was one of information storage, its sequences of
nucleobases providing the instructions for manufacturing proteins. But soon, RNA world
gave way to DNA world, with all life thereafter based on the strands of the famous double
helix.
During the transition to DNA world, RNA gave up much of its information storage role
to a molecule that was chemically more stable, less subject to copying errors, and, due to its
length, able to carry instructions for the creation of more complex proteins. The DNA-
protein partnership, combining superior information encoding and superior energy pro-
duction, outcompeted RNA-based life (Lunine, 2005). RNA was, thenceforth, relegated to
a helper role, mediating protein creation based on DNA instructions.
While there is no evidence indicating how RNA gave rise to DNA, an evolutionary
succession is logical because of the similarity of the two molecules. Both have backbones of
phosphate and sugar, and while the sugars differ, they are chemically closely related. To the
backbones of each are attached only four types of nucleobases, and three of the four are
identical between RNA and DNA. It was not a major evolutionary trick to convert RNA or
its chemical precursors into DNA.
Whatever scenario transpired, a period of rapid evolution must have followed in order to
specify the huge number of proteins used subsequently in cell metabolism (Trevors, 2003). At
some point, in some branches of life, light-absorbing pigments were introduced into the cell
as a means of capturing solar energy to drive chemical reactions (Botta, 2004).
One-celled life
Today’s one-celled organisms are either prokaryotes (literally “before kernels” – cells having
no nuclei) or eukaryotes (“true kernels” – cells having nuclei). Based upon similarities and
differences among genes appearing in bacteria, archaea, and eukarya, there is a general
consensus that prokaryotes evolved first (Betts et al., 2018; Boeckmann et al., 2015).
Life begins 11
However, there is no consensus about whether the first cells were bacteria or archaea, the
two prokaryote domains. Some studies favor bacteria (Baldauf et al., 2004; Williams et al.,
2015) and others archaea (Caetano-Anollés et al., 2014; Raymann, Brochier-Armanet, &
Gribaldo, 2015). It may be that there was so much horizontal gene transfer (movement of
genetic material from one organism to another without a parent-child relationship) between
the early domains that a resolution will never be possible based on genes (Thiergart, Landan,
& Martin, 2014). Horizontal gene transfer is also a problem in working out the possible
relationship of viruses to one-celled life (see Box 1.2).
In round figures, prokaryotes are thought to have emerged a little less than 4 billion years
ago, and eukaryotes about 2 billion years ago (Betts et al., 2018; Doglioni, Pignatti, &
Coleman, 2016). The eukaryotes are of particular interest to us because they were ancestral
to animals and then humans.
Viruses are noncellular structures ranging from the small and simple to the large
and complex. Their protein shell can surround either a short strand of nucleic acid
(RNA or DNA) encoding one or two proteins, or long strands incorporating over a
thousand genes, some of which are shared by unicellular life ( Figure 1.3).
Figure 1.3 Structure of the tobacco mosaic virus. Some of the protein subunits have been removed to
reveal the coiled RNA.
Source: TMV structure simple.png, CC BY-SA 3.0, Thomas Splettstoesser.
The historical debate over whether viruses are alive, or are instead just chemical
entities, continues even as efforts are made to incorporate them into accounts of the
evolution of life ( Tennant, Fermin, & Foster, 2018). One particularly interesting
attempt was made by Nasir, Kim, and Caetano-Anollés (2017). Recognizing that virus
genes show high rates of both mutation and horizontal gene transfer, they instead
examined a slowly-changing group of proteins. This allowed deep evolutionary
relationships to be explored, based on protein similarities and differences. In
remarkably orderly results, viruses were found to have evolved first, followed
successively by archaea, bacteria, and eukaryotes ( Nasir et al., 2017).
These results call for replication using other approaches, especially since they appear
to resolve not just the relationship of viruses to life’s domains, but the emergence order
of the domains themselves. Yet at minimum, they contribute to increasing recognition
that viruses must be reckoned with when accounting for the early evolution of life
( Tennant et al., 2018).
12 Introduction to evolution
The eukaryotes, by definition, have nuclei, composed of an inner double membrane
enclosing DNA and proteins. Nuclei thus emerged around 2 billion years ago. It is
thought that another early addition to eukaryotes were mitochondria, small structures re-
sponsible for energy production that exist in multitudes inside today’s cells. Mitochondria
bear a strong resemblance to miniature bacteria and are believed to have initially been
incorporated into eukaryotes in a symbiotic (mutually beneficial) relationship: Mitochondria
provided energy, and eukaryotes provided nutrition and protection (Lane & Martin,
2010). The relationship stuck, and mitochondria became heritable along with the rest of
the cell’s components.
What did the earliest cells look like? It is difficult to be sure, but filaments and strands are
apparent within the first large-scale evidence of life on Earth (Baumgartner et al., 2019).
These are stromatolites – mats of sediment formed by primitive bacteria, dating to 3.5 billion
years ago and found in chert. The mats formed in very shallow water, and similar living
structures can be seen today in the shallow waters of Western Australia (Figure 1.4), the same
place where the fossil ones are found (Cowen, 2005; Westall, 2004).
The earliest stromatolites show no evidence of exposure to oxygen and some evidence
of reduction, suggesting that the microorganisms that created them evolved in a hydrogen-
enriched, reducing atmosphere (Westall, 2004). A likely scenario is that the earliest cells
forming the mats were anaerobic; but, at a later date, such cells emitted oxygen as a by-
product, similar to today’s cyanobacteria. Initially, the oxygen they emitted was scrubbed out
of the atmosphere by dissolved iron in the oceans, which oxidized (rusted) and precipitated
Speciation
Why, then, isn’t there only one species of animal, conqueror of all competitors and supre-
mely fit to rule its environment? The primary reason is that there is no single environment.
At a macro level, water, land, and air make wildly differing demands, and within each, there
are a multitude of niches, each of which favors some animals over others. A koala chewing
Australian gum tree leaves, a gazelle browsing on African grasses, a tiger consuming an Asian
deer, and a grizzly bear feeding on North American salmon have all adapted to particular
environments affording different types of food. Within each of these geographical areas,
countless other species grasp different threads of the web of life.
Speciation, the creation of new species, often occurs due to physical separation between
two parts of an animal population. During the voyage of the ship Beagle, Charles Darwin
collected numerous variations of finches inhabiting different islands of the Galapagos chain
west of South America. The islands provided similar environments, but separation by water
had prevented interbreeding between their inhabitants. Over time, natural selection had
created different variations on different islands. Today, a number of them are considered
separate species.
This kind of speciation is called allopatric, from Greek roots literally meaning “other
native village”. It results in animals well adapted to specific environments and therefore
often “unconquerable” by other species. Allopatric speciation is believed to account for
most of the speciation that has occurred. Besides the water separating islands, geographic
barriers that can divide populations and lead to speciation include mountains, rivers, and
energy-impoverished zones of ocean water such as those separating black smokers from
the surface.
At the other extreme, sympatric (literally “together native village”) speciation occurs in
populations not physically separated. Sympatric speciation can happen if different in-
dividuals in the population feed on differing food sources and mating is linked to those
sources. For example, the apple maggot fly has evolved from a North American fly in-
festing hawthorn fruit. When apple trees were introduced to the continent in the early
19th century, some flies that normally infested hawthorns instead laid their eggs in apples,
probably because of a preexisting geographically related genetic variation that was not
yet a difference between species ( Jiggins & Bridle, 2004). Subsequently, “hawthorn” and
“apple” females showed a preference for mating with males feeding on their preferred
Life begins 15
fruit. This mating preference largely isolated the two groups from one another. Today, the
groups show only low levels of genetic exchange, strongly suggesting the beginnings of
speciation (Feder et al., 1994). Furthermore, a wasp that is parasitic of the flies shows
corresponding change, with apple wasps genetically diverging from hawthorn wasps
(Forbes, Powell, Stelinski, Smith, & Feder, 2009).
A female preference for mating with some males over others is an important aspect of
sexual selection, an adjunct to natural selection. Selection of desirable qualities in mates
makes them more prevalent over successive generations. A classic example is the pre-
ference of many female birds, such as pea hens, for extremes in feather coloration and
form, like those in peacock tails. This is not just a frivolous form of selection, because more
substantial traits often accompany decorative ones. More colorful and robust males tend to
be healthier and better fed, and therefore tend to pass down health-related and possibly
foraging-related traits as well (Boogert, Fawcett, & Lefebvre, 2011).
Is evolution random?
To what extent is evolution a random process? The answer to this depends on which
component of evolution is considered, descent with modification or selection. The
modification part of descent with modification is largely random in that mutations occur at
random places in the genome (the set of genetic instructions embodied in DNA). Although
some of the nucleobases in DNA are more susceptible to change than others, there is no
biochemical equivalent of “pick me!” signaling an intention to change in a particular
direction. Genetic recombinations are also random.
Selection, however, is anything but random. To argue otherwise is to argue, for example,
that fish were equally likely to evolve on land or sea. The truth is that the environment places
strong directional (i.e., nonrandom) demands on organisms. Over time, natural selection,
aided by sexual selection, creates animals that fit their environment, whether it involves the
creation of fin-like structures in both fish and whales, or the creation of wing-like structures
in both birds and bats. Although our ability to predict the specific products of selection is
very limited, general predictions are quite possible. Thus, while no one predicted that
hawthorn maggot flies would like apples, it was inevitable that some North American insect
would come to occupy the new high-energy environmental niche provided by the apple’s
introduction.
Although life began as single cells within a half billion years of Earth’s formation, it took
over 3 billion years more for descent with modification, acted upon by natural selection, to
produce multicellular life. Presumably, speciation played a large role by creating a multitude
of “experiments” that ultimately led to more complex organisms. From that point in history,
we leave the realm of weak evidence and speculation behind, and can rely on a rich fossil
record to trace life’s course. Naturally, we need to accurately date the fossils to reconstruct
the path, and to that topic we soon turn.
Conclusion
Science can only guess at the ultimate origins of life, but an understanding of the natural
processes permitting it is beginning to take shape. A first requirement was replication, a
process implying chemical synthesis involving not only raw materials, but also energy
(metabolism) and a protective envelope (a membrane). The first self-replicating molecule
may have been a short RNA strand formed in a “warm little pond”, on a clay surface,
around a “black smoker”, in a nuclear geyser system, or in space, all representing locations
where energy was available if not abundant. However it originated, the molecule soon
16 Introduction to evolution
needed a membrane to protect it from the environment, perhaps a bubble based on lipids
that could enclose other compounds.
Although RNA catalyzes a number of chemical reactions, it is not an efficient energy
producer. At some point, RNA was relegated to a support role, with the chemically similar
DNA molecule taking over information coding and, through its coding of proteins, the
production of energy.
The earliest true organisms were prokaryotes, emerging about 4 billion years ago,
although it is unclear whether bacteria or archaea came first. Stromatolites dating
to 3.5 billion years ago, formed in shallow water, record the past presence of primitive
bacteria. Eukaryotes, cells with nuclei, emerged around 2 billion years ago and were the
ancestors of animals including humans. Mitochondria were incorporated into eukaryotes
in the role of energy producer, probably at first in a symbiotic relationship but later in
a heritable one.
The engine of evolution is descent with modification, acted upon by natural selection.
Modification is largely random, involving reassortment and recombination of genetic
material inherited from parents, or mutations occurring to chromosomes prior to or during
copying. Natural selection, however, is driven by environmental characteristics and is
decidedly nonrandom. Even small fitness differences can produce large effects over time,
as in the elimination of all but one ancestral human Y chromosome line within the last
200,000 years or so.
Speciation usually occurs allopatrically, with geographic barriers such as water expanses or
mountains separating two groups, which then evolve independently. However, it may also
occur sympatrically, in the same location, if there are other barriers to interbreeding, such as
differing food preferences. The evolution of complex organisms is the product of countless
genetic “experiments” producing species that could better exploit their environments.
Science stands on far firmer ground with evolution than it does with life’s origins. As we
shall see, evolving life left abundant evidence of descent with modification, acted upon by
natural selection. Some of it would be in the rock around us, and some in every living cell.
2 Life gets complicated
The single-cell aspect of Earth’s earliest life imposed severe limits on the complexity that
could be attained through evolution. Organisms would have to become multicellular for life
to become complicated.
Early sponges were the first true multicellular organisms (called metazoa, or metazoans)
that have left living descendants. Their existence about 650 million years ago is evidenced by
a specific cholesterol-related chemical in rocks of that age from Oman (Gold et al., 2016;
Love et al., 2009). Its principal secretor is a class of sponges called demosponges. The only
other known secretors are algae whose ancestors acquired the characteristic much later, and
therefore cannot account for the earlier deposits (Gold et al., 2016).
As immobile filter feeders, ancestral sponges lacked tissues such as muscles or nervous
systems (Peterson & Butterfield, 2005). However, they possessed specialized cells, including
tubular cells with large pores that allowed water to pass into the animal, and cells with flagella
that pumped it out. Other cells provided structural support through embedded stiff rods
called spicules.
Eumetazoa, the broad group of all animals that are not sponges, are first evidenced by their
apparent effect on acritarchs (tiny acid-resistant fossils showing evidence of a cell wall). Many
acritarchs are believed to have been similar to today’s algae as found in plankton, certainly a
food source for small multicellular animals. Significantly, the form of acritarchs remained
virtually unchanged in the fossil record for hundreds of millions of years – until about
635 million years ago. They then began a rapid evolution of form, with some developing
outward spines, probably as a defense against consumption. The implication seems clear that
eumetazoan consumers had recently evolved, creating a selective pressure for creation of
defensive structures. The dating of these events at 635 million years ago corresponds to
reasonable expectations for the first large populations of motile (independently moving)
metazoans, as opposed to the earlier sessile (attached) sponges. Motility presumably enabled
the consumption of acritarchs (Peterson & Butterfield, 2005).
Unfortunately from a dietary preference alone it is difficult to picture what early eu-
metazoa looked like. Cnidarians (pronounced nigh-dare’-ee-ans), comprised of the jellyfish,
hydras, sea anemones, and related species, are a little more accessible to the imagination.
They first diversified about 605 million years ago(Peterson & Butterfield, 2005), and pre-
sumably originated before that. Like other eumetazoa, they had true tissue serving as skin on
the outside, and a digestive cavity on the inside, the forerunner of the gut (Cowen, 2005;
Steinmetz, Arman, Kraus, & Technau, 2017).
The earliest body fossils of possible eumetazoans date from about 600 million years ago.
Fossils of the genus Vernanimalcula (“small spring animal”) are found in rock known as the
Doushantuo Formation in southwest China. Averaging only 150 micrometers across, they
are bilaterally symmetrical (having mirror-imaged sides) and reportedly have an identifiable
mouth and digestive tract (Petryshyn, Bottjer, Chen, & Gao, 2013). However, these
DOI: 10.4324/9781003137863-3
18 Introduction to evolution
interpretations are controversial, and Vernanimalcula’s status as a metazoan is not universally
accepted (Cunningham, Vargas, Yin, Bengtson, & Donohue, 2017).
Vernanimalcula is almost exactly 600 million years old. The confident assignment of age is
a key issue in evolutionary science. In this chapter we consider how fossils form, their
dating using geological and molecular clocks, and their role in helping to construct the tree
of life. We also examine the proliferation of species during the Cambrian explosion, in-
cluding the emergence of chordates, which were the ancestors of all vertebrates including
humans.
Fossil formation
A fossil most commonly forms when an organism is covered by sediment, shutting out
oxygen and protecting some parts – most often bones and teeth, but sometimes soft tissues as
well – from rapid decay. Initial burial is followed by a long period in which the parts are
gradually replaced by minerals precipitating from water permeating the sediment. It is not
uncommon, however, for original bones and teeth to survive as fossils. Very rarely, soft
tissues themselves survive without mineralization, as possibly in the cases of preserved tissue
from the dinosaurs Tyrannosaurus rex and Brachylophosaurus canadensis (San Antonio et al.,
2011; Schweitzer et al., 2009). Fossils are usually discovered in sedimentary rocks and vol-
canic ashfalls, and only uncommonly in volcanic lava, which typically consumes organic
material. However and wherever they form, however, fossils are rare and their record should
be assumed incomplete (Box 2.1).
In a world ideal for paleontologists, all animals would fossilize, and assembling their
record would only involve digging down to the right level in the right place to see
what is there. The discoveries from such a dig, arranged in time, could be likened to a
movie, recording a continuous series of changes in species across time. Unfortunately,
the world is not ideal, and the fossil record more resembles a series of snap-shots than it
does a movie. The snap-shots are widely spaced in time, and the series is almost always
grossly incomplete.
There are many causes of the incomplete fossil record. Most importantly, animals
usually decay without leaving a recognizable trace. It’s not even the case that some
constant percentage of individuals fossilize, because many animals live in environments
disfavoring rapid burial and mineral replacement of body parts. Chimpanzees provide
a good example. Ancestral chimpanzees left almost no fossils, and none of great age,
because most lived in hot, damp forest environments with acidic soils, factors
promoting rapid decay ( McBrearty & Jablonski, 2005; Orwant, 2005).
Nevertheless, an incomplete record can still yield good science. For instance, finding
jawless fishes in older deposits, and jawed fishes in younger ones, suggests that jawed
fishes evolved from jawless ones and not vice versa. Hypotheses formed in this way can
then be tested through new fossil discoveries. If the hypothesis is correct, new fossils
will likely be discovered over time to fill gaps in the record. Yet the approach is
flexible: Should newly discovered fossils prove inconsistent with a hypothesis, it can be
revised or replaced, and tested with future discoveries.
Life gets complicated 19
Geological clocks
Dating a fossil found in rock starts with considering its position on site. A basic consideration is
that rock is generally found in layers, with the oldest on the bottom and the newest on top. Of
course, layers can become inverted from folding of the earth’s crust. Newer rock can also be at
the same level as older rock, as when a river cuts through and deposits sediment, or when
molten rock is injected into sedimentary layers. Careful geological study can identify such cases,
however, and ascertain whether the oldest-on-bottom, newest-on-top relationship holds. But
how do we date the layers? That is the fundamental problem solved by geological clocks.
Assuming undisturbed geology, dating a fossil is straightforward if it is found in rock
sandwiched between layers of volcanic rock or ash. Potassium-argon dating can be used on the
two volcanic layers, and the fossil’s age must then lie between those dates. This method is
possible because minerals forming in a volcanic layer must once have been liquid and
therefore heated enough to drive off any argon gas. However potassium remained behind as a
solid, nongaseous element, with a tiny percentage (about one in 10,000 atoms) existing
naturally in the form of potassium-40, a radioactive isotope. The isotope began decaying over
time into the gas argon-40, becoming trapped in the cooled, solidified material. Thus, by
measuring the existing ratio of potassium-40 to argon-40 and using its known conversion
rate, the age of the volcanic layer can be determined. Because the radioactive half-life of
potasssium-40 is 1.25 billion years, the method is appropriate even for the oldest volcanic
materials on earth (Brown, 1992). However, a number of alternative techniques exist, based
on other radioactive isotopes (for brief, partial reviews, see Brown, 1992, and Levin, 2010).
Dating the Doushantuo Formation in which Vernanimalcula was found proved more
problematic because it is sedimentary, with no closely-spaced volcanic layers allowing precise
“sandwich” dating. Initial attempts to date it used volcanic layers well above and below the
formation, providing a wide range of possible ages, somewhere between 539 and 748 million
years (Barfod et al., 2002).
The problem was eventually solved by determining the age of the sediment itself. When
water deposits certain minerals such as phosphorite, minute quantities of uranium are in-
cluded. The radioactive decay of uranium follows a set sequence producing predictable levels
of lead isotopes. Thus, dating proceeds by measuring the levels of the isotopes. When the
method was applied to Doushantuo phosphorite, the precise age of 599.3 (±4.2) million
years was obtained (Barfod et al., 2002).
Geological clocks are essential tools for dating newly discovered fossil remains. Even when
a rock layer is unsuitable for isotope methods of dating, it is often possible to indirectly derive
a date by identifying ordinary fossil plants and animals within it. If those species have been
dated elsewhere using isotope methods, the date can often be applied to the layer of interest.
Of course, the value of geological clocks increases if more than one method is applied and
the results converge on similar values. But they are not the only dating methods.
Molecular clocks
One of the most exciting developments in late 20th century paleontology was the invention
of molecular clocks. Of great benefit when the fossil record is poor, these estimate when the
ancestors of two species diverged from one another based on quantified differences in DNA.
The forerunners of molecular clocks were clocks based on blood protein differences. These
scored an impressive early victory by showing that primate divergences were much more
recent than most paleontologists assumed (Pilbeam, 1972).
The fundamental assumption of molecular clocks is that genetic differences between
animals are directly related to when they branched off a common ancestor. Closely related
20 Introduction to evolution
descendants from a common ancestor should show relatively few differences in their genes;
a thousand generations later more differences should be apparent; and a million generations
later differences should be pronounced. This expectation is a direct outcome of the natural
processes that over time damage and miscopy DNA. The fundamental assumption is
therefore so well grounded as to be beyond serious dispute. Instead, the controversial
aspect of molecular clocks is how to calibrate them. In other words, how can genetic
differences be translated into a time scale that can be used to estimate how long ago
species diverged?
20
millions of years ago
15
Orangutans
10
Gorillas
5 Chimps
0
0 1 2 3 4
% genetic difference
Figure 2.1 Example of a linear clock using two calibration points (Old World monkeys and orangutans) to
estimate the divergence times of gorillas and chimpanzees from the human ancestral line, based on
genetic differences.
Source: Original figure.
Life gets complicated 21
Table 2.1 The major biological taxa, which can be remembered using the
mnemonic “Kings play cards on fat green stools”, as applied to
modern humans. Note that lower levels are members of higher levels,
so, e.g., all primates are also chordates (phylum Chordata)
Calibration aside, comparison to established portions of the primate tree show that a linear
clock provides a fairly good fit to primate data (Drummond, Ho, Phillips, & Rambaut,
2006). That is probably because primate species diverged over a relatively short period of
time, within a single biological order.
More problematic are attempts to extend linear clocks across broad biological taxa (clas-
sifications of organisms based on relatedness; taxon is the singular form) such as a phylum or
kingdom (Table 2.1). Extended across a kingdom, linear clock estimates in excess of one
billion years have been obtained for the common ancestor of all animals (Wang, Kumar, &
Hedges, 1999). Such “deep” estimates are rejected by most scientists because, as we have
seen, the oldest traces of metazoa are only about 650 million years old.
By convention, molecular clock studies usually date the age of a taxon as that of its first
diversification into two or more subgroups that have living descendants. Each such
subgroup is called a crown group. However, a taxon’s origin precedes diversification, and
the pre-diversification members are called the stem group. Thus, there are actually two
different ages of a taxon, reflecting the first appearance of crown and stem groups,
respectively ( Renner, 2005).
The two ages bear important implications for which species to compare for molecular
clock purposes. A crown group comparison, the usual type, uses species that are in
different living groups within the taxon. For example, for the African ape taxon
(Homininae), a crown group comparison would be between gorillas and chimpanzees. In
contrast, what might be called a “pre-stem” group comparison pairs a species from the
taxon of interest, with a species from the next earlier taxon that is available along the line
of descent: For example, a comparison between gorillas and orangutans (i.e., non-African
members of the Hominidae). This overestimates the age of the stem group ( Figure 2.2).
Figure 2.2 The crown and stem ages of a taxon. Comparing species marked by the completely or
partially white boxes allows a molecular clock estimate of the crown age. A stem age
comparison, however, is not available because of extinctions. The next best achievable
comparison, between the completely or partially black boxes, is to the next-most-related
species, providing a “pre-stem” age that overestimates the stem age.
Source: Original figure.
Life gets complicated 23
A little thought reveals that the true age of a taxon must lie between the dates
yielded by the two comparisons. The “pre-stem” age is an overestimation, but the
crown group age is an underestimation, because the taxon is older than its first
diversification.
The discrepancy with the beginning of physical metazoan evidence 650 million years ago
remains to be explained. Rocks older than that certainly exist, even sedimentary ones, yet
extensive searches have failed to yield unambiguous evidence of metazoan fossils within
them. Initially promising “fossils” of metazoans, and of their relatives the fungi, have been
discounted because they were possibly caused by geological and not biological processes
(Budd & Jensen, 2004; McMahon, 2019). Possibly this is itself part of the problem; if early
metazoan fossils always resemble other things, then unambiguous fossils of the requisite age
may never be identified.
It is also true that fossils are generally younger than an animal’s first appearance, which
likely accounts for at least some of the discrepancy. Metazoan populations would have been
small in the beginning, rendering improbable the preservation and future discovery of their
earliest remains (Budd & Jensen, 2004). Later, as metazoans became abundant, they left a
more extensive and thus more discoverable fossil record. However, it remains to be seen
whether this principle can be stretched sufficiently to cover a gap of 130 million years or so
between the first existence of metazoa and their first physical evidence.
Finally, we should keep in mind that while molecular clocks are valuable, they are only
approximate. Within the bounds of their calibration points, based on uncontroversial
fossils, they appear to yield reasonably accurate estimates of the age of interpolated events.
It is when they are used to extrapolate ancient events outside their calibration points, that
results become controversial. In contrast, geological clocks are usually much more precise,
as exemplified by the dating of the Doushantuo Formation within 4.2 million years.
Nevertheless, both kinds of clocks have their uses. Geological clocks date fossils, while
molecular clocks date the first appearance of new categories of life. Neither substitutes for the
other.
Figure 2.3 The eukaryote tree of life, showing some commonly known plant and animal groups. Origin at
top left, progressing to existing phyla on the right. Note that the bracket widths are not meant to
directly reflect time from a common origin.
Source: Copyright © 2004 by Oxford University Press, Inc. Adapted and reproduced with permission of the Licensor
through PLSclear.
demonstrate that soft body parts, and in the case of trails, imprints of soft body parts, did in
fact fossilize. But few are to be found.
What is notable is the lack of hard-body evidence until the 3 to 6 million years immediately
preceding the Cambrian explosion (Shu, Isozaki, Zhang, Han, & Maruyama, 2014;
Valentine, Jablonski, & Erwin, 1999). After that, the proliferation of relatively large animals
with hard-body parts does seem to qualify as an explosion (Butterfield, 2003). If the emerging
phyla became ancestral to most living animals, it is because hard body parts proved exquisitely
adaptable. A skeleton, whether external as in the shells of ancestral insects and crustaceans, or
internal as in sponges or ancestral vertebrates, is invaluable in providing protection from
predators, firm attachment points for muscles, and a framework for repair after damage
(Cowen, 2005).
Early hypotheses on the explosion’s cause cited rising oxygen levels as enabling the
evolution of large, complex bodies. Because oxygen penetrates poorly into external shells,
large-body evolution could not occur until it reached a sufficient concentration that it could
be brought into the body through small openings such as gills (Cowen, 2005). This sup-
position is supported by a correspondence between increasing metazoan consumption of
oxygen, and increasing oxygen levels. Thus, mollusks appeared before arthropods, which
appeared before fishes. Oxygen consumption increased across the three groups, reflecting
atmospheric increases in oxygen (Zhang & Cui, 2016).
It is also possible that hard parts simply took time to evolve, and once they began, were
so successful that hard bodies radiated worldwide. By this account the Cambrian explosion
is self-explanatory. Unfortunately for this explanation, the explosion was not limited
to “hard” animals but also included a proliferation of soft-body forms (Erwin et al., 2011;
Shu et al., 2014).
An ecological view was expressed by Logan, Hayes, Hieshima, and Summons (1995).
They suggested that the earlier proliferation of small metazoans feeding on plankton-like life
forms created a seabed environment that could be exploited by new phyla. Fecal pellets
dropping to the seabed would have created a carpet of high-energy foodstuff while at the
same time improving the oxygenation of the water due to the removal of carbon (which was
therefore not incorporated into carbon dioxide). The strength of this hypothesis is that it
provides one reason why most of the new life forms of the Cambrian explosion had benthic
(bottom-dwelling) lifestyles (Valentine et al., 1999).
Chordates
The Cambrian explosion phylum leading to vertebrates was Chordata, the chordates. These
relatively complex animals have a hollow neural tube behind a notochord (supporting rod)
which stiffens the body and provides muscle attachment points. The neural tube is segmented
into three compartments, namely a forebrain, a hindbrain, and probably a midbrain, the
beginnings of a modular nervous system (Holland & Chen, 2001). A living example thought
to closely model the chordate ancestor is amphioxus (genus Branchiostoma), also known as the
lancelet, a sea-bottom burrowing filter feeder. It is a member of the subphylum cepha-
lochordates. Its importance is its descent from a common ancestor shared by another chordate
Life gets complicated 27
subphylum, the vertebrates, but not by other chordate subphyla (Hickman, Roberts, &
Larson, 1997). Thus, its anatomical and behavioral characteristics are important in re-
constructing early vertebrates.
The cephalochordate-vertebrate common ancestor is generally accepted as having evolved
from earlier chordates by neoteny, the retention of juvenile characteristics in adulthood
(Lacalli, 1994). Most invertebrates pass through a larval stage before metamorphosing into
adults. Very often, the larvae swim but the adults are sessile. Thus, assuming it results in a
viable organism, a mutation producing sexual maturity prior to metamorphosis would result
in a swimming, breeding adult, accounting for swimming in the common ancestor.
Amphioxus resembles a translucent fish (sectioned in Figure 2.4), a few centimeters in
length, with no jaw, minimal fins, and with a single eye (Holland & Chen, 2001). Marked
behavioral and structural asymmetries are present despite membership of the chordates in
the Bilateria. As seen from behind, newly hatched amphioxus move by spiraling counter-
clockwise. Among other anatomical asymmetries, the mouth opens on the left and the
mid-gut diverticulum runs along the body’s right side. These asymmetries, and similar ones
in other chordates, indicate that lateral differences, so important to primates as we shall see,
first emerged in chordates’ immediate ancestors, and may be considered a chordate
character (Li et al., 2017). As a result, turning and feeding behaviors in the descendants of
early chordates – i.e., vertebrates – tend to be asymmetric as well, although at least in the
case of fish, different species may show either left or right biases (Stancher, Sovrano, &
Vallortigara, 2020).
By the Cambrian explosion’s end, the chordate phylum was well established. Over
about 250 million years, the animal ancestors of humans had evolved from microscopic
collections of cells to small fishlike creatures. Their neural tubes and anatomical asym-
metries would have important implications for the evolution of sensation, perception, and
cognition. After another 150 million years, some of their descendants would start leaving
the water.
Conclusion
The earliest metazoa are estimated to have emerged about 780 million years ago, although
the first physical evidence in the form of sponge secretions dates to 650 million years ago.
Defense-related changes in the shape of tiny fossil acritarchs 15 million years later suggests a
response to predation, presumably by eumetazoa. After another 30 million years, true tissues
appeared in the cnidarian ancestors of modern jellyfish.
Accurate dating is a key issue in evolutionary science. Geological clocks typically exploit
relative levels of radioactive isotopes to date rock. Molecular clocks use genetic differences
between species to date ancestral divergences. In primates, a linear molecular clock is rea-
sonably accurate. But it breaks down when extended across broad biological taxa, with the
clock found to run at different rates for different animals. Thus, a “relaxed” or “sloppy” clock
is often used, employing fossil evidence to calibrate different branches of the ancestral tree.
Even so controversies emerge, as in the dating of the earliest metazoa. Relaxed clock dates
are only approximate, and decrease in accuracy when used to extrapolate dates outside their
calibration points.
The tree of life is reconstructed using both cladistics (the grouping of species by novel
characteristics), and genetic similarity to map its branches. Its details are constantly revised as
research accrues.
Most living metazoan phyla suddenly appeared during the Cambrian explosion (roughly
520 or 530 to 540 million years ago). Proposed explanations include an inadequate fossil
record preceding the explosion; rising oxygen levels enabling the existence of large, complex
28
Introduction to evolution
The evolution of chordates (phylum Chordata) during the Cambrian explosion paved the
way for vertebrates (subphylum Vertebrata), or animals with a brain and a vertebral column
composed of cartilage or bone. In this chapter, we first consider early vertebrates and their
characteristics, before describing the emergence of tetrapods. Their descendants included the
amniotes, as well as the diapsid and synapsid lines of the dinosaurs and mammals.
Early vertebrates
Early vertebrates were fish-like. The Chenjiang rock layers of southern China, dating to
about 520 million years ago, provide one of the most complete fossils of a probable one called
Myllokunmingia fengjiaoa, illustrated in Figure 3.1 (Benton, 2005; Shu et al., 1999). It clearly
shows the presence of upper and lower fins.
Identification of Myllokunmingia as a vertebrate is largely based on evidence of gill pouches as
well as a distinct head with what may have been a cartilaginous skull (Holland & Chen, 2001;
McMenamin, 2019). However, some doubt remains because there is no evidence that the
notochord extended to the head, or that there was a distinct brain. These absences leave open
the possibility that the animal was a prevertebrate craniate (an animal with a skull, in early forms
made of cartilage). Yet either way it must be very close in form to an ancestral vertebrate.
Metaspriggina walcotti, from the Burgess Shale in Canada, likewise had a head, with cranial
cartilage, paired eyes, gill bars, and a notochord. Some identify it as an early vertebrate
(Morris & Caron, 2014) but others as a nonvertebrate chordate (McMenamin, 2019). That it
came from the Walcott Quarry section of the Burgess Shale tightly dates it to 505 million
years ago, give or take a couple of million years (Caron & Rudkin, 2009).
In whatever specific animal a brain first formed, it was a highly significant event, for the
vertebrate brain is what made high-level cognition possible.
Biomineralization
“Harder” evidence of vertebrate status might be taken as biomineralization, the incorporation
of minerals into structures such as bones or teeth. The earliest vertebrates, including those
already described but also the ancestors of hagfish and lampreys, had only cartilage. However,
in quick order dentine (bonelike tissue, but without cells) and true bone emerged, so quickly
in fact that hard parts were an innovation that must have carried a substantial selective ad-
vantage (Keating, Marquart, Marone, & Donoghue, 2018). About 493 million years ago
Anatolepis had primitive teeth, formed from skin that incorporated minerals to form dentine.
That clearly identifies it as a vertebrate, in which both teeth and skin develop from epidermal
cells. Thus, within about 40 million years, natural selection worked from the chordate body
plan to produce identifiable hard parts in early vertebrates (Benton, 2005).
DOI: 10.4324/9781003137863-4
Vertebrates to early mammals 31
All of these complex animals were sea-dwelling, as evolution had not solved the problems
raised by land habitation. The earliest fossils of land-dwelling, air-breathing animals –
centipedes, millipedes, and arachnids – date back only to about 415 to 425 million years
ago (Rodriguez et al., 2018; Suarez, Brookfield, Catlos, & Stöckli, 2017). Earlier, about
435 millon years ago, arachnids may have gone through a transitional state allowing them to
venture onto land even though they were primarily aquatic (Wendruff, Babcock, Wirkner,
Kluessendorf, & Mikulic, 2020).
By the time of Anatolepis, evolution produced several important adaptations that verte-
brates would build upon. First, the significance of hard parts cannot be overestimated. Teeth
allowed more effective predation, and the later development of bone increased body integrity
while protecting the nervous system and providing a ready supply of calcium (Maisey, 1996).
The notochord, the chordate innovation that supported the neural tube, was incorporated
by vertebrates into the spinal column where both became surrounded by bone. In later-
evolving vertebrates such as reptiles, birds, and mammals, the notochord is most apparent
during embryonic development, transforming to a pulpy mass within the spinal column in
adulthood.
Housed within the spinal column of even the earliest vertebrates was the spinal cord – an
important nervous system innovation, as it served as the conduit for information passing from
peripheral sensors to the brain, and from the brain to the muscles. These would become key
adjuncts to a knowing and acting cognitive brain.
Hox genes
Another major vertebrate adaptation was a multiplication of genes controlling the devel-
opment of body segments, or as has been written, “distinguishing anterior from posterior,
where segments will form in the embryo, where limbs or fins will form, and which way is
up” (Maisey, 1996, p. 31). These Hox genes, an abbreviation of homeobox (i.e., “like a box”,
so named because the DNA sequence is short and can be enclosed by a box in written form)
32 Introduction to evolution
are believed to have originated no later than the emergence of cnidarians (Ferrier, 2016;
Reddy, Unni, Gungi, Agarwal, & Galande, 2015). In early jellyfish, they may have helped
specify the “bell” at one end, and the mouth and tentacles at the other. Later, during ver-
tebrate evolution, Hox coding was responsible for several innovations including the transi-
tion between fins and limbs, the formation of ribs, and variations in the number of vertebrae
(Pascual-Anaya, D’Aniello, Kuratani, & Garcia-Fernàndez, 2013).
Nonvertebrate chordates have a single cluster of Hox genes, strung like beads on a string
along a single chromosome (or more accurately, because chromosomes are paired, along each
of a single pair of chromosomes). Vertebrates have four clusters, each strung along a different
chromosome. In humans, they are chromosomes 2, 7, 12, and 17 (Hughes, da Silva, &
Friedman, 2001). Within each cluster, a Hox gene’s position determines its influence on a
corresponding body segment. Thus, in each of the four chromosomes involved in verte-
brates, the Hox gene affecting head development is at one end, while the other end has genes
affecting the body’s hind parts (Hickman et al., 1997).
Having four vertebrate clusters instead of one allowed for increased structural complexity
because it permitted more elaborate developmental instructions for different body segments.
One way to quadruple the clusters would have been for the entire genome to double not just
once but twice, a “two-round” theory of genome duplication. Thus, a single cluster on one
chromosome would have become four clusters on different chromosomes (Cañestro, Albalat,
Irimia, & Garcia-Fernàndez, 2013).
Although the two-round theory has had its skeptics (Pervaiz et al., 2019), recent genetic
analysis has strongly supported it. Thus, Simakov et al. (2020) identified 17 ancient chro-
mosomal units shared by widespread animal groups, among others scallops, amphioxus (the
model chordate), and vertebrates (e.g., lampreys, garfish, and chickens). Tracking duplica-
tions across the groups identified two whole-genome duplications in our ancestral line: One
about 490 million years ago, before the emergence of jawed vertebrates, and the other
roughly 450 million years ago, before the emergence of bony fish. However Hox genes
multiplied, mutation followed by natural selection would have caused divergence among the
duplicates, allowing a corresponding increase in the complexity of body structure.
A related vertebrate innovation was the appearance of a neural crest during development,
regulated by Hox genes. In the embryo, the crest consists of a cluster of cells arranged in
ridge-like formations to the sides of the neural tube. Cells from it migrate to various parts of
the body during development to form the brain and many of the nerves and ganglia (func-
tional groupings of nerve cells) – as well as skull bones, teeth, connective tissue, smooth
muscles, and the heart among other organs and tissues (Trainor, Melton, & Manzanares,
2003). The result is a more complex design, including a more complex nervous system to
serve a more complex body plan.
Tetrapods
In 2004, a crew member on a paleontological expedition to the Canadian Arctic found a
snout sticking out of a cliff. It was Tiktaalik, an animal with thick, fleshy, almost hand-like
fins (Figure 3.2). Although not classified among tetrapods – vertebrates with four feet or legs –
it seemed a likely near ancestor of them. Remarkably, the expedition had been mounted to
discover just such a creature. The search, begun five years earlier, ended with the excavation
of three nearly complete fossils, the largest almost nine feet in length (Morelle, 2006).
Scientists had long known that such an animal must have existed. Previously discovered
fossils of Panderichthys (Figure 3.3), dating to 385 million years ago, revealed a creature with
bony gill covers that facilitated the flow of water, indicating a predominantly aquatic lifestyle.
In effect it was a predatory fish. It was lobe-finned, and from such fleshy structures “hands” –
including our own – are believed to have evolved (Ahlberg & Clack, 2006). In comparison,
Acanthostega (365 million years ago) lacked bony gill covers though it retained internal gills,
indicating less water flow and implying that the animal breathed partially out of the water.
This conclusion matches the limb structure, in which the limbs were broad and flat, suitable
Figure 3.3 The transition from fish (bottom left) to tetrapods (upper right), with Tiktaalik in a transitional
role. A lobe-finned coelacanth fish is also shown for comparison.
Source: Fishapods.png, CC BY-SA 4.0, dave souza.
for propping up the head in shallow water. Eight digits per limb are clearly visible (Laurin,
Girondot, & de Ricqlès, 2000; Shubin, Daeschler, & Coates, 2004). With the earlier animal
finned and the later one not, there must have been a transition between the two forms: A
“predicted Tiktaalik” that was searched for, found, and subsequently described (Daeschler,
Shubin, & Jenkins, 2006).
At 375 million years old, Tiktaalik is a transitional form between fish and tetrapods.
Traditionally, tetrapod evolution was attributed to a shift from water to land, but modern
research points to an intermediate phase in which animals propped themselves up with fleshy
fins in shallow water and mud flats. That gave access to the small prey available there, while
providing protection from large, deeper-water predators. The intermediate animals, the
fossils tell us, were still fish even though they were partially air breathing.
Besides prop fins, transitional forms are believed to have had a prominent tail fin used
when free swimming. Unfortunately the tail of Tiktaalik was not preserved in the fossils,
despite the fanciful ones shown in Figures 3.2 and 3.3. Nevertheless a tail fin was later present
on the true tetrapod Acanthostega (365 million years ago). That animal also had separated
digits, although it was still not a permanent land dweller.
The lineage represented by these species was a freshwater one, evolving in and beside lakes
and streams (Ahlberg & Clack, 2006; Shubin, 2008, pp. 40–41). There is little support for the
alternative, that the water-to-land transition was first made by sea-dwelling animals.
Exactly when hands and feet first appeared is not clear, although the existence of Tiktaalik
and Acanthostega seem to constrain their appearance to between 375 and 365 million years
ago. Unfortunately, fossils of the possible tetrapod Parmastega, dated tightly to 372 million
years ago, lack remains of the hands and feet (Beznosov, Clack, Luksevics, Ruta, & Ahlberg,
2019). Arguments for earlier dates (385–395 million years ago) are based on putative tetrapod
trackways, not remains. Unfortunately, in the absence of independent evidence of hand and
foot structure, their interpretation seems very subjective (e.g., Lucas, 2015) and in some
cases, vulnerable to alternative explanations (King, Shubin, Coates, & Hale, 2011).
The early tetrapods were amphibians, living a larval phase in water and an adult phase on
land like tadpoles and frogs do today. The problems involved in the transition to land were
formidable. Besides the obvious one of breathing air, there were also those of supporting the
body without the buoyancy of immersion, moving across land, feeding, and retaining water.
Vertebrates to early mammals 35
Lobe-finned fishes had a “leg up” in making the transition because their fleshy fins could
serve as both body supports and thrusters.
Other adaptations were useful as well. Early tetrapods were heavily scaled, aiding water
retention (Cowen, 2005), and they may already have been capable of some air breathing.
Among lobe-finned fishes, lungfish as opposed to coelacanths are the closest relatives of
tetrapods, suggesting that tetrapods evolved from something very similar – and lungfish can
breathe some air (Irisarri & Meyer, 2016; Takezaki & Nishihara, 2017). Finally, lobe-finned
fishes could raise their upper jaw and not just lower their lower jaw, a preadaptation (an
evolutionary change later built upon by other changes) for feeding in the shallows up to
water’s edge without scooping up mud (Cowen, 2005).
During the transition prey were initially captured through water suction, the feeding
method favored by most aquatic vertebrates. Obviously, however, this method is useless for
hunting out of water, so biting eventually evolved. We know roughly when this occurred,
because it is visible in the fossil record: The adjoining edges of skull bones show different
strain patterns in sucking versus biting species. Thus, Eusthenopteron (the earliest species in
Figure 3.3) was a sucker, and Acanthostega was a biter (Markey & Marshall, 2007). Tiktaalik
was also a biter, using a sideways snapping motion to capture prey much like modern cro-
codiles (Hohn-Schulte, Preuschoft, Witzel, & Distler-Hoffmann, 2013).
One peculiarity of early tetrapods is that the modern plan of five digits per limb did not
predominate. More common were six, seven, or eight digits per limb (Bradshaw, 1997;
Coates, Jeffery, & Ruta, 2002; Saxena, Towers, & Cooper, 2017), a condition known as
polydactyly (having more than five digits per limb). The five-a-limb plan did not become
fixed in most tetrapods until it became specified by Hox genes (Kherdjemil et al., 2016). As
Bradshaw (1997) pointed out, if the number of digits had not been reduced in the human
ancestral line we might now be counting in base 12 – or 14, or 16! But regardless of their
number, the evolution of digits would eventually have profound implications for the ma-
nipulation of objects, as well as their creation using memory representations, spatial trans-
formations, and other cognitive tools.
Why did tetrapod ancestors begin to venture onto land? The reason is similar to that
famously given by trailblazing mountaineer George Mallory when asked why he wanted to
scale Mount Everest: “Because it’s there”. Land was an environment lacking predators,
unexploited by animals larger than millipedes, and covered with potentially nutritious ve-
getation. It therefore represented an environmental niche – indeed many niches – waiting to
be filled. Venturing onto land had huge value to the first pioneers.
Amniotes
As advanced as tetrapod amphibians otherwise were, they resembled their fish ancestors by
laying eggs in water. The major innovation of their descendants the amniotes was the
development of embryos within a membrane. In egg-laying species the membrane and
associated shell allow gases to be exchanged with the environment but the embryo to
remain hydrated. That permits the eggs to be hatched on land rather than in water
(Cowen, 2005). In fact it appears that the early amniotes were fully terrestrial, having
relatively complex, multichambered lungs for breathing air (Lambertz, Grommes,
Kohlsdorf, & Perry, 2015).
The first-known amniotes date from about 325 million years ago. This estimate is based on
characteristics other than fossilized eggs, which are missing from the record for well over a
hundred million years after amniotes had to have evolved. It is thought that early eggs
fossilized very poorly because they had little or no mineral content (Piñeiro, Ferigolo,
Meneghel, & Laurin, 2012). Thus, the earliest known eggs, from dinosaurs living about
36 Introduction to evolution
Figure 3.4 Top to bottom: Anapsid (no-holed), Synapsid (one-holed), and Diapsid (two-holed) skulls.
Source: Adaptation of Skull anapsida 1.svg, Skull anapsida 1.png, and Skull diapsida 1.png, CC BY-SA 3.0, Preto(m).
190 million years ago, had calcium-based shells less than 100 micrometers (0.004 inches)
thick (Stein et al., 2019), about the width of the average human hair.
Instead, dating amniote origins depends on finding evidence of other characteristics of
known amniotes. Those include an anapsid skull (a skull lacking openings behind the eyes; see
Figure 3.4) with a narrow snout; vertebrae allowing head swiveling; and upright rather than
sprawling limbs. Based on these and other characteristics, one of the earliest amniotes was
Westlothiana lizziae (Figure 3.5), found in West Lothian, Scotland (Prothero, 2007,
pp. 234–237; Smithson, Carroll, Panchen, & Andrews, 1994). It inhabited a freshwater lake
environment but was terrestrial and dated to about 325 million years ago (van Tuinen &
Hadly, 2004). These were much smaller animals than the early transitional tetrapods. They
resembled small lizards and were an evolutionary step away from amphibians, although they
were not quite reptiles. They were likely cold-blooded and ate small invertebrates including
insects (Oftedal, 2002), using a weak bite to disable their prey (Kemp, 2005).
Figure 3.6 Amniote, synapsid, and diapsid relationships. Lines reaching fully to the right are living taxa.
Source: Original figure.
38 Introduction to evolution
After the synapsids split off, the remaining amniotes can be regarded as primitive reptiles of
which the diapsids were only one group – though a spectacularly successful one. Thus, there
were other early reptiles (e.g., parareptiles) with anapsid skulls (Lee et al., 2004). Figure 3.6
illustrates these relationships.
Dinosaurs
Our main narrative concerns synapsids, but brief consideration of the diapsids, and more
particularly the dinosaurs, is in order. As Figure 3.6 indicates, dinosaurs were a relatively late-
emerging group, first appearing about 225 million years ago (Cowen, 2005). In the popular
mind modern crocodiles and alligators are often imagined to be the remnants of the dinosaurs
but are actually part of a group that split from the dinosaurs’ ancestors a few million years
earlier. It is nevertheless correct to refer to them as the dinosaurs’ nearest living relatives. That
claim pales, though, in comparison to the recent discovery of actual living, breathing
dinosaurs. They are called birds.
Discovery of the feathered Archaeopteryx in 1860 gave firm evidence of a transitional form
between earlier dinosaurs and birds (see Figure 3.7). However, it was Thomas Huxley,
Darwin’s early supporter, who made the first systematic argument that birds and dinosaurs are
related (Ibrahim & Kutschera, 2013). Examining the pelvic bones of a dinosaur in 1868, he
quickly concluded that they were birdlike in character. Other similarities were found in
the vertebrae, leg bones, and toes. Huxley (1870) stated:
there can be no doubt that the hind quarters of the Dinosauria wonderfully approached
those of birds in their general structure, and, therefore, that these extinct reptiles were
more closely allied to birds than any which now live.
It wasn’t until the late-20th-century discovery of numerous feathered dinosaurs in China that
a consensus emerged: Birds are not just related to dinosaurs, but descendants of them and
indeed, cladistically one and the same as them (Cracraft et al., 2004). This affinity appeared
to receive striking confirmation when collagen protein sequences were isolated from
Early mammals
Mammal-like synapsids evolved at nearly the same time as the dinosaurs, about 225 million
years ago (Oftedal, 2002). Unfortunately, identifying the first true mammal is difficult despite
numerous fossils covering the transition. That is because mammalian characteristics evolved
gradually at varying times, producing a smooth transition with no clearly marked border
between mammal and nonmammal (Sidor & Hopson, 1998).
For example, endothermy (“warm-bloodedness”, the internal generation of heat to maintain
body temperature) probably evolved in mammalian ancestors as early as 250 million years
ago. Certain fossils of the time reveal ribs shaped to accommodate a diaphragm, nasal bones
associated with warming air before it entered the lungs, and the skeletal build of an active
lifestyle, all signs of endothermy. Less clear is the origin of mammary glands and the nursing
of offspring. However, judging from similarities in lactation across monotreme, marsupial,
and placental mammals, the secretion of milk probably derived from their common ancestor
(Oftedal, 2002). If so it may date from about 220 million years ago (Foley, Springer, &
Teeling, 2016).
Other mammalian characteristics, such as the pairing of a large brain relative to body size –
with its implied consequences for cognition – as well as middle ear bones separated from the
jaw, did not appear until 195 million years ago in the tiny two-gram creature Hadrocodium
(Kemp, 2005). For its part fur is first known with certainty from a “halo” around the fossil
skeleton of Megaconus, a mammal-like animal from 165 million years ago (Zhou, Wu,
Martin, & Luo, 2013).
The multiple-character nature of mammals, accompanied by uncertainty over the onset of
specific characters, makes identification of the first mammal difficult if not impossible. In any
case, our ancestral line depended specifically on the emergence of placental mammals, the
eutheria (“true beasts”). These have fetuses that develop with the aid of a fully formed placenta
and are born live, unlike marsupials that develop without a fully formed one and in most
species without a placenta of any kind. Eutherian ancestors branched off from marsupial
mammals perhaps 170 million years ago (Foley et al., 2016).
The earliest known eutherian was Juramaia, a small climbing mammal weighing a little
over half an ounce, from 160 million years ago (Luo, Yuan, Meng, & Ji, 2011). Closely
related was Eomaia from 125 million years ago. Its fossil is extraordinary in preserving a
pronounced fur halo (Figure 3.8) that makes its mammalian nature instantly clear.
Vertebrates to early mammals 41
Eomaia was probably partly terrestrial (living on the ground) and partly arboreal (living in trees)
and was adapted for grasping and climbing. Of course, as an internal soft tissue, the placenta did
not fossilize; but the animal can be classified based on similarities to living eutheria in dentition
(the form and patterning of teeth), wrist bones, and ankle bones (Ji et al., 2002).
Like Juramaia, Eomaia was very small, weighing just under an ounce (Ji et al., 2002). It is no
accident that it and other early mammals were tiny. Archosaurs (the dinosaurs and their kin)
ruled the large-animal habitats, while smaller lizard-like reptiles dominated the small-animal
niches. But small warm-blooded mammals could and did thrive in the cool of night. Indeed,
environmental pressure from a nocturnal existence encouraged the evolution of both in-
telligence and sharp senses (Cowen, 2005), including improvements in the senses of smell and
hearing, and in whisker-based touch sensitivity (Maor, Dayan, Ferguson-Gow, & Jones,
2017). When large land animals disappeared after the extinction event 66 million years ago,
new niches opened for the survivors.
Conclusion
Vertebrates originated about 520 million years ago as sea-dwelling animals with brains and
mineralized body parts. Teeth allowed more effective predation, and bones increased body
integrity and protected the nervous system. The emergence of Hox genes, aided by two full-
genome duplications, allowed body segmentation and a new avenue for evolutionary change,
in that one segment could undergo modification without affecting others. Another factor
increasing vertebrate complexity was the neural crest, from which cells migrate during de-
velopment to form nerves, bones, smooth muscles, and other localized structures. Other
vertebrate adaptations included external eye muscles, a macula, and eye pairing, all substantial
improvements of vision.
Tetrapods in the form of four-legged amphibians emerged from fresh water over a period
of about 20 million years. They lived a primarily aquatic existence 385 million years ago but
42 Introduction to evolution
breathed at least partially out of water 365 million years ago. “Hands” evolved from fleshy
lobe fins, initially with six-to-eight digits each, but becoming fixed at five by Hox genes.
Jaws evolved to open upward, allowing feeding at water’s edge without ingesting mud.
Tiktaalik, a transitional form, captured prey with a sideways snapping motion much like
modern crocodiles.
Amniotes appeared by about 325 million years ago. Their major innovation was to lay eggs
incorporating a membrane, which allowed gas exchange while keeping the embryo hydrated.
This permitted eggs to hatch on land so that from this point, many vertebrates could be
terrestrial in lifestyle. The earliest known shelled eggs date only to about 190 million years
ago, and had shells the width of a human hair. Amniote skulls were anapsid, having no holes
behind the eye.
Synapsids and diapsids, creatures with one versus two holes behind the eye, emerged
shortly afterward and were the ancestors of mammals and dinosaurs respectively. Although
dinosaurs were once thought to be extinct, we now know that birds are their living re-
presentatives. The large dinosaurs disappeared in a cataclysmic extinction 66 million years ago
when a six-mile diameter asteroid collided with the earth. Its dust combined with that from
coinciding volcanic eruptions to create a years-long blockage of sunlight and a calamitous
decline in global temperatures. The ensuing die-off of plants doomed large land animals.
Many smaller animals survived, however, including mammals. Identifying the first
mammals is difficult because of their multiple-character nature, but warm-bloodedness and
lactation probably emerged by 250 and 220 million years ago, respectively. The pairing of a
relatively large brain with middle ear bones that had separated from the jaw likely evolved by
195 million years ago. Fur is known from 165 million years ago.
Eutheria or “true beasts” evolved by 160 million years ago. Eutheria have fully formed
placentas and undergo live birth. Like other early mammals the initial ones were tiny, an
ounce or less in weight, but from such slight beginnings would evolve the large mammals of
post-dinosaur earth.
4 Later mammals through primates
The extinction of large land animals 66 million years ago left a vacuum to be filled by
evolution. Smaller animals survived, including among others lizards, salamanders, birds,
insects, and mammals. The increasing size of mammals would prove to be a major theme
of the postextinction period. Yet the event did not cause an explosive diversification.
Relaxed clock models indicate that all of today’s eutherian orders had emerged long before
the large dinosaurs’ demise. Although diversity did increase in the immediate period
afterward (Close et al., 2020; see Box 4.1), some of it was within orders now extinct
(Springer et al., 2017).
DOI: 10.4324/9781003137863-5
44 Introduction to evolution
Before the extinction mammals were already a varied class. Their teeth confirm a diversity
of diets across species, with foods varying between insects, meat, fruit, and seeds. But they
were very small, averaging only 150 g or a little over five ounces before the extinction. That
increased within a million years to one kg (2.2 pounds), about seven times heavier than
before (Novacek, 1999).
In this chapter we examine the emergence of primates from this mammalian background,
and their subsequent diversification into today’s taxa. Those include strepsirrhines; tarsiers;
and the large anthropoid group including New World monkeys, Old World monkeys, and
both the lesser apes and great apes. It should be kept in mind as these species are described
that all have undergone evolution since their ancestors diverged from the primate ancestral
tree. As a result, none is a true representation of its earlier ancestors, any more than a
kangaroo resembles early mammals. However, just as it is possible to deduce characteristics
possessed by early mammals from common characteristics among living mammals (e.g., fur
and endothermy), it is often possible to deduce from common characteristics among a group
of related primates, characteristics possessed by their common ancestor.
Early primates
Only in cartoon worlds did cavemen and large dinosaurs coexist. The reality is that they were
separated by tens of millions of years. Primates, however, did overlap the end of the large
dinosaurs. According to relaxed clock models they originated about 74 million years ago,
give or take four million years, with nearly all estimates dating before the dinosaur extinction
(e.g., Dos Reis et al., 2018; Herrera & Dávalos, 2016; Pozzi et al., 2014; Springer et al., 2017;
Tarver et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2019).
Despite an origin well inside the geological epoch known as the Late Cretaceous, primates
have fossils only dating to just outside that epoch’s boundary with the Paleocene (Table 4.1),
suggesting a slow emergence. The earliest known primate was Purgatorius, from 66 million
years ago (Mantilla et al., 2021; Silcox & López-Torres, 2017). That earlier primates existed is
implied not only by relaxed clock age estimates, but by the fact that the oldest fossils of
Purgatorius date to about 200,000 years after the dinosaur extinction, and represent two
different species. Because primate speciation is believed to take longer than that, the earliest
representatives of Purgatorius must have predated the extinction (Mantilla et al., 2021). It is
possible that fossils of even earlier primates have been collected, but have not been re-
cognized as primates because of their primitive form (Silcox & López-Torres, 2017).
The recent discovery of tarsal (foot) bones from Purgatorius shows that it had mobile,
grasping feet, as expected of an arboreal, primitive primate but unlike primates’ closest re-
latives, the flying lemurs and tree shrews (Chester, Bloch, Boyer, & Clemens, 2015). Its small
Epoch Dates
Figure 4.2 Cladogram illustrating early branching of the primates. Earlier splits are to the lower left; later
ones to the upper right.
Source: Original figure.
lemur-like animals were among the Strepsirrhini, and indeed, these were the ancestors of living
strepsirrhines, among them lemurs (Figure 4.3), sikafas, pottos, and galagos (Figure 4.4).
Strepsirrhines are found in continental Africa, Madagascar and nearby islands, and
southeast Asia. With few exceptions, all species have a dental comb (outward-projecting
lower canines and incisors, used as a comb while grooming), a divided lower jaw with a
joint at the chin (as in most mammals), elongated noses, and a divided upper lip closely
attached to the gums. Many are nocturnal and have a breeding season as opposed to in-
dividual cycles. Across species their food sources include leaves and other vegetation,
insects, and fruit (Cartmill, 1992).
The split of the Strepsirrhini from other primates is thought to have occurred about
68 million years ago (Table 4.2), although their earliest fossils date from several million years
later (Shoshani, Groves, Simons, & Gunnell, 1996). The Eocene yields evidence of about
40 genera of strepsirrhines. By that time there were both nocturnal and diurnal (active in
daytime) species. They moved by leaping and jumping (Simons, 1992), which were probably
inherited from their immediate primate ancestors (Boyer, Toussaint, & Godinot, 2017).
Table 4.2 Approximate dates of primate divergences from the human ancestral line (which follows the
remainder clades). (Derived in part from Perelman et al., 2011; Pozzi et al., 2014; Schrago &
Voloch, 2013; Tarver et al., 2016; and Zhang et al., 2019.)
Figure 4.7 First related group of New World monkeys: Common marmoset.
Source: Common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus).jpg, CC BY 2.0, Leszek Leszczynski.
Later mammals through primates 51
the floating island would likely prolong the survival period (Houle, 1999). A transatlantic
journey from Africa to South America is therefore the most likely scenario for the origin of
platyrrhines, the New World monkeys.
A migration from Africa need have happened only once across many millions of years to
introduce primates into South America. There is evidence, however, that it actually
happened twice. The now-extinct Ucayalipithecus, a genus of anthropoids, left fossils in
South America dating to about 32 million years ago. It split from an African lineage
35 million years ago, so that a second rafting must have occurred within that three-
million-year gap (Seiffert et al., 2020). That is about 10 million years later than the original
rafting of the ancestors of the other New World primates, the platyrrhines, implicating a
second migration.
As their name indicates, living platyrrhines have flat noses with widely separated
nostrils. They are generally small and arboreal, and most are diurnal. They typically have
long tails that are often prehensile (able to grasp objects). A wide range of locomotion
occurs across species, including clinging and leaping, quadrupedal branch-walking, and
suspension and brachiation (swinging from branch to branch). Food sources also vary
widely across species, and include leaves, fruit, tree gum, and insects (Rosenberger &
Hartwig, 2002).
The classification of the New World species has been controversial in recent years and has
given rise to competing sets of family names. However, molecular clock approaches make it
clear that the group containing marmosets and tamarins, and the group containing capuchins
and squirrel monkeys, are more closely related to each other than either are to a third group
containing spider monkeys, muriquis, and howler monkeys (Chatterjee, Ho, Barnes, &
Groves, 2009; Pozzi et al., 2014). Several species are illustrated in Figures 4.7 to 4.12.
Figure 4.8 First related group of New World monkeys: Emperor tamarin.
Source: Tamarin portrait 2 edit3.jpg, CC BY-SA 4.0, Brocken Inaglory.
52 Introduction to evolution
Figure 4.9 Second related group of New World monkeys: Capuchin monkey.
Source: Capuchin Costa Rica.jpg, CC BY-SA 3.0, David M. Jensen (Storkk).
Figure 4.10 Second related group of New World monkeys: Common squirrel monkey.
Source: Saimiri sciureus-1 Luc Viatour.jpg, CC BY-SA 3.0, Luc Viatour/ www.Lucnix.be.
Later mammals through primates 53
Figure 4.11 Third group of New World monkeys: Ornate spider monkey.
Source: Panama spider monkey, Costa Rica.jpg, CC BY-SA 3.0, Steven G. Johnson.
Early catarrhines
Ancestral Catarrhini meanwhile, were continuing their radiation (expansion in the number of
species) in the Old World regions of Africa, Asia, and Europe. We are fortunate to have substantial
fossils of an early catarrhine, Aegyptopithecus, dating from 29 to 33 million years ago (Simons,
Seiffert, Ryan, & Attia, 2007; Rasmussen, 2002a). A nearly complete male skull was found in
Egypt in 1966 (Figure 4.13). It was from an individual estimated to weigh 3 to 6 kg (7 to 14 pounds),
possessing the heavy limbs of a slowly moving arboreal quadruped. Its upper teeth were ape-like as
were its feet, and it was probably frugivorous (eating fruit; Cowen, 2005; Rasmussen, 2002a).
Early catarrhines existed for millions of years before their descendants split into two groups, one
ancestral to the living Old World monkeys, and one ancestral to the apes including humans.
Aegyptopithecus is a potential candidate for a common ancestor of both groups. In support of this,
the shape of its femur places it almost exactly midway between those of living Old World
monkeys and apes, suggesting that it was the form from which both groups diverged (Almécija
et al., 2019).
54 Introduction to evolution
Figure 4.12 Third group of New World monkeys: Black howler monkey.
Source: Black Howler Monkey (adult male) 4.jpg, CC BY 2.0, Ryan E. Poplin.
The Colobinae comprise nearly 60 species living in Africa and Asia. They are primarily
folivores (leaf eaters) and have a specially adapted stomach with multiple chambers to ferment
food. Unlike cercopithecines, who have well-developed thumbs, colobines have small or
vestigial thumbs. Nearly all are arboreal, and while they inhabit diverse climates they do not
live in dry or desert areas as do some cercopithecines. They usually have very long tails.
Figure 4.16 shows one such species.
Early apes
Apes diverged from monkeys about 29 million years ago, but their first substantial fossils
date to about 20 million years ago. The skeleton of Proconsul (Figure 4.17), from Kenya,
reveals a much larger body than earlier primates, ranging from 9 to 90 kg (20 to
200 pounds) depending on the species and gender (Harrison, 2002). It and related early
Later mammals through primates 57
Miocene genuses, such as Ekembo (McNulty, Begun, Kelley, Manthi, & Mbua, 2015),
were adapted to living in trees, and were probably slow climbers and above-branch ar-
boreal quadrupeds (Andrews, 2020). Proconsul had curved fingers and an opposable
thumb, and it was tailless (Andrews, 2020; McNulty et al., 2015).
Although Proconsul is a strong candidate for an ancestor of the living apes including
man, its unspecialized skeleton has led some to propose an alternative. Morotopithecus,
from Uganda, was a large-bodied (40 kg, 90 pound) ape also dating to a little over
20 million years ago. Its shoulder was highly mobile, consistent with brachiation or
arm-hanging but contrasting with the quadrupedalism of Proconsul (Gebo et al., 1997;
MacLatchy, 2004). Certainly brachiation or arm-hanging is a plausible character for an
ancestral ape. Phylogenetic analysis, largely based on teeth, does suggest that
Morotopithecus was a closer relative than Proconsul of living apes including humans (Stevens
et al., 2013). However, nothing like a complete skull is available to make the case more
convincing.
It is worth asking why apes arose at all, when Old World monkeys already existed in
similar arboreal environments. Wouldn’t the smaller bodies of monkeys allow them to
outcompete any emerging apes? Kevin Hunt (2016) pointed to a key ape adaptation,
58 Introduction to evolution
namely the ability to assume eccentric grasping postures. That allows them to suspend
from multiple slim supports to forage at the extreme ends of fruit tree branches
(Figure 4.18). When this adaptation began to appear, ape and monkey body sizes were
likely similar, but apes thrived because of it. According to Hunt, the ensuing 20-million
year competition resulted in a splitting of environmental resources. Monkeys began
consuming most of the fruit near the core of trees, and developed the ability to consume
unripe fruit. Apes, on the other hand, consumed ripe fruit at the tree periphery. A
difference in body size evolved, with monkeys remaining small so they could feed
while balanced on branches in sitting postures (e.g., Figure 4.17). Apes became large not
only to physically displace monkeys, but to reach slender branch supports at greater
distances.
Lesser apes
Among living species, the small-bodied gibbons and siamangs (5 to 11 kg, 12 to 25
pounds) are considered “lesser” apes as opposed to the great apes (orangutans, gorillas,
chimpanzees, and humans). Their divergence from the human ancestral line was about 20
million years ago (see Table 4.2). The four genuses are composed of agile brachiators.
Gibbons (Figure 4.19) live in portions of an area roughly defined by northeast India, to
southern China, to the island of Java. Siamangs live in part of the same range, specifically in
Later mammals through primates 59
Great apes
Our understanding of relationships between the great apes, including humans, has un-
dergone a revolution since the advent of molecular testing. Previously a single branch was
assumed to have diverged from the human ancestral line, representing a common ancestor
of orangutans, gorillas, and chimpanzees. According to that view, none would be more
closely related to us than another. However in the late 1960s, testing of blood proteins
showed chimpanzees to be more closely related to us than are orangutans and gorillas
(Pilbeam, 1972). DNA testing further clarified the relationships, demonstrating a genus-
by-genus divergence from the human line.
The first great ape divergence was of the genus Pongo, the orangutans (or orangutangs),
about 15 million years ago (Table 4.2 and Figure 4.18). There are three similar species,
namely the Bornean and Sumatran orangutans, their names betraying where they live, and
the recently identified Tapanuli orangutan, also a Sumatran species (Nater et al., 2017).
They are the only Asian great apes other than humans. Forest dwellers, they are highly
arboreal (the Tapanuli orangutan perhaps exclusively so), engaging in hanging and branch-
swinging behaviors and often supporting part of their weight (up to 90 kg, or 200 pounds)
with their feet (Figure 4.18). When on the ground, they walk quadrupedally, putting
weight on their fists instead of knuckles as in other great apes. They are generally solitary
but also strongly territorial, and are primarily frugivorous although they do eat other plant
material, insects, and fungi.
The genus Gorilla was the next to branch off the human ancestral line, about 9 million
years ago (Table 4.2). There are two living species, the eastern gorilla and the western gorilla
(Figure 4.20).
Gorillas live exclusively in the “mid-belt” region of Africa, east and west, usually in
forested areas. They are largely terrestrial, walking by putting weight on their knuckles,
although female and young animals do climb trees to sleep. They are chiefly folivorous but
also eat fruit, insects, and seeds. Males can naturally weigh up to 200 kg (440 pounds),
making them the largest living primates.
The last primate genus to diverge from the human ancestral line was Pan, the chim-
panzees, about 7.5 million years ago (Table 4.2). There are two living chimpanzee species
(Figures 4.21 and 4.22), the common chimp Pan troglodytes, and the pygmy chimp Pan
paniscus, also known as the bonobo (pronounced buh-NO-boh). Common chimps weigh
40 to 65 kg (90 to 145 pounds) in the wild, while bonobos are somewhat smaller,
weighing 30 to 45 kg (65 to 100 pounds).
A question that sometimes arises is whether common chimpanzees or bonobos better
model our last common ancestor. Since it was their joint ancestor that broke away from
our line, they are in fact equally related to us, a supposition confirmed by genetic com-
parisons (Prüfer et al., 2012). Thus, they and us are co-equal representatives of our
common ancestor.
The genetic comparisons also indicate that the two species diverged from each other about
two million years ago (Raaum, Sterner, Noviello, Stewart, & Disotell, 2005; Stone et al.,
2010). That split may coincide with a lowering of the Congo River due to dry conditions,
allowing bonobo ancestors to cross to the south of the river while common chimpanzee
ancestors remained to its north. If so, geographic isolation occurred when full river flow
Later mammals through primates 61
Conclusion
The dinosaur die-off 66 million years ago produced mammalian diversification, easily
noticeable in the fossil record even though it was not explosive in magnitude. The largest
effect on the ancestors of living mammals was to allow an increase in size, about sevenfold
within a million years. But there were also knock-on effects. In particular, mammalian
diversification produced a diversification of lice, and in turn new species of lice acquired
different species of bacteria.
62 Introduction to evolution
Most closely related to flying lemurs and tree shrews, primates originated some eight
million years prior to the extinction. Early ones were nocturnal, probably aiding their sur-
vival alongside dinosaurs. Purgatorius, dating to 66 million years ago, had a mobile, grasping
foot. Other primate features emerging early on included opposable thumbs, stereoscopic
vision, and relatively large brains. Carpolestes, from 58 million years ago, had a nail in place of
a claw on its big toe. It probably fed at the slender ends of branches, on a mixed diet
emphasizing fruit.
Initial divergences from the human ancestral line included the Strepsirrhini (e.g., lemurs
and galagos) about 68 million years ago, and tarsiers about 66 million years ago. Today
strepsirrhines live in continental Africa, Madagascar and vicinity, and southeast Asia. They
have a dental comb and a varied diet, and many are nocturnal. Tarsiers live on islands in
southeast Asia, are nocturnal, and eat insects and fruit.
The remainder clade, the anthropoids, had adaptations that included more convergent eyes
and a largely diurnal lifestyle. Two species of the extinct genus Eosimias are known from
about 45 million years ago. They were better adapted to horizontal postures, yet still had a
grasping big toe more similar to tarsiers than to living anthropoids. Others among early
anthropoids gave rise to the Old World and New World monkeys.
The platyrrhini or New World monkeys (e.g., marmosets, capuchins, spider monkeys)
diverged from the human ancestral line about 44 million years ago. As their name implies,
Later mammals through primates 63
they have flat noses. They usually have long tails that are often prehensile, and across species
show a wide range of locomotion and feeding styles. How they got to the Americas is an
enduring problem, with current thinking favoring a crossing from Africa on natural rafts of
vegetation.
The remaining catarrhines or narrow-nosed monkeys resided in the Old World (Africa,
Asia, and Europe). Aegyptopithecus, from 29 to 33 million years ago, weighed 7 to 14 pounds,
had heavy limbs, and probably moved slowly in trees. Around that time Old World monkeys
diverged from the human line, and now comprise the Cercopithecinae (e.g., macaques and
baboons) and the Colobinae (e.g., langurs and proboscis monkeys). Both are very diverse
subgroups, defying simple characterization. However, Cercopithecinae tend to have short to
medium length tails, relatively large thumbs, and ischial callosities, while Colobinae usually
have long tails, small or vestigial thumbs, and lack ischial callosities.
Apes diverged from monkeys about 29 million years ago, but their first substantial fossils,
of Proconsul, Ekembo, Morotopithecus, and related genuses, are several million years younger.
They were mostly large-bodied yet were adapted to living in trees. Lesser apes (gibbons and
siamangs) split off about the same time, 20 million years ago. They live in Asia and are agile
brachiators.
In contrast to an older view that our fellow great apes diverged from our ancestral line as a
group, molecular data are conclusive that multiple divergences occurred. Thus, orangutans
did so about 15 million years ago, gorillas about nine million years ago, and chimpanzees
64 Introduction to evolution
7.5 million years ago. Orangutans live in Borneo and Sumatra in southeast Asia. They
are almost completely arboreal, and have a varied diet emphasizing fruit. Gorillas live in the
“mid-belt” of Africa, and are largely terrestrial. They also have a varied diet but emphasize
leaves. The chimpanzees (the common chimp and the bonobo) live in central and west
Africa. They are partly arboreal and partly terrestrial, and are omnivorous. They are
also equally related to us.
5 Humans
Through much of the 20th century, the term hominid was reserved for humans and our
immediate ancestors and relations. That made for a very uncertain clade, because the oldest
indisputably hominid fossils dated from three million years ago (Pilbeam, 1972, p. 104), while
the living apes were thought to be descended from a common ancestor that broke from the
human line 14 to 17 million years ago (Pilbeam, 1972, pp. 88, 98). When during that period
did we transition from a more apelike to a more human form?
Recent advances have provided both a more satisfying and a more troubling way of
defining the human clade. Recognition that the ape genera broke off one-by-one from our
ancestral line, with chimpanzee ancestors last doing so 7.5 million years ago, has allowed
tight definition of the clade as consisting of humans, our ancestors, and our nearest rela-
tions back to the chimpanzee split. With a date that recent – and as we shall see, with fossils
nearly that old – it seems likely that human characteristics emerged relatively soon
thereafter.
However, using the definition “since the chimpanzee split”, while reducing uncertainty, is
also troubling. The complete genomes of humans and chimpanzees have been decoded, and
it is clear that we are genetically very similar. Thus, we share over 99% of our coding
for amino acid sequences contained within proteins (Suntsova & Buzdin, 2020; Wildman,
Uddin, Liu, Grossman, & Goodman, 2003). Responding to this similarity, some proposed
that we rename the two chimpanzee species as Homo troglodytes and Homo paniscus, in effect
admitting them to humanity (Wildman et al., 2003).
In contrast, others downplayed any special similarity between humans and chimpanzees
and instead advocated creating a new hierarchy of taxa taking into account the separate
divergences of the Asian and African apes. Under this scheme the family Hominidae would
refer to all great apes including humans (and orangutans), the subfamily Homininae to
all African apes including humans (excluding orangutans), and the tribe Hominini to all
bipedal apes.
Whatever its merits, that proposal has now been widely accepted and the terminology
settled. Accordingly, for our purposes the term hominin (derived from Hominini) refers to
humans and their bipedal ancestors and relatives back to the chimpanzee-human split.
Nevertheless it should be kept in mind that historically, the term hominid was commonly
used, even recently, to refer to the identical classification.
In this chapter we will consider the characteristics of hominins from the Miocene through
the Pliocene, Pleistocene, and today, with special attention to key species. Recent discoveries
of late-living, small-brained hominins will be described. Trends over time will be identified,
including a generalized, disproportionate increase in brain size relative to body size. Finally,
possible ancestral connections will be explored.
DOI: 10.4324/9781003137863-6
66 Introduction to evolution
The first (Miocene) hominins
Within the past quarter century four spectacular finds have taken the human lineage back
almost to the chimpanzee-human divergence, placing it firmly within the Miocene geolo-
gical epoch. The earliest species, named Sahelanthropus tchadensis, is remarkable for the pre-
servation of a crushed but nearly complete skull nicknamed “Toumai” (Figure 5.1). The
nickname means “Hope of life” in the Goran language used in the area of Chad, north
central Africa, where the fossil was found.
Whether Sahelanthropus should be considered an early hominin or a nonhominin ape has
been controversial (Wolpoff, Hawks, Senut, Pickford, & Ahern, 2006). That is not surprising
considering the 7-million-year age of the specimen. Close to the chimpanzee-human divide,
species on either side would be nearly identical. Arguments for hominin status include the
presence of a relatively vertical, short face, a prominent brow ridge similar to that found in
most later hominins, reduced canine teeth, thickened tooth enamel, and a downward-
opening, oval-shaped foramen magnum, the opening on the bottom of the skull allowing entry
of the spinal column (Zollikofer et al., 2005). The tooth roots likewise suggest that Toumai
was a hominin, for they share some characters with later hominins that are imperfectly shared
with African apes, and not shared at all with orangutans or with Proconsul (Emonet, Andossa,
Mackaye, & Brunet, 2014).
That constellation of traits has produced general though incomplete consensus that
Sahelanthropus was a hominin. The downward opening foramen seems particularly sig-
nificant, as a possible indication of bipedalism. In quadrupedal apes, and even in brachiating
and clambering apes like orangutans, it opens angled a bit toward the rear, allowing the head
to more easily tilt upward relative to the spine. The foramen is also better centered on the
underneath surface of the cranium than it is in apes, another indication of bipedalism. Indeed,
its location falls well within the range of modern humans (Neaux et al., 2017).
Ardipithecus
The second major Miocene find was of Ardipithecus, shown by the cladogram as branching
off the human ancestral line soon after Sahelanthropus. The oldest examples date to about
6.3 million years ago and are from Ethiopia. Initially only one species was described,
Ardipithecus ramidus (White, 2002). Subsequently a case was made that the earliest examples
should be designated a separate species called Ardipithecus kadabba, with ramidus reserved for
later ones (Haile-Selassie, Suwa, & White, 2004). Others however consider these subspecies,
not separate species (Harcourt-Smith & Aiello, 2004).
The Miocene fossils of Ardipithecus consist of a jawbone, teeth, parts of an upper limb, and
a foot bone. Later ones, about 4.3 million years old, include a partial skeleton including a
pelvis, femurs, and portions of a cranium. Many believe bipedalism is indicated (Lovejoy,
Suwa, Spurlock, Asfaw, & White, 2009), but others are skeptical (Gibbons, 2009). Analysis of
the pelvis suggests that if it was bipedal, it likely walked with a bent-hip, bent-knee posture;
i.e., without a modern stride (Lewton & Scott, 2017).
Figure 5.2 Cladogram of living apes, fossil hominins, and humans, based on cranial and dental characteristics.
Source: Licensed from Elsevier under STM Permissions Guidelines.
68 Introduction to evolution
Figure 5.3 The foot of Ardipithecus, showing its splayed big toe.
Source: Ardipithecus Fuß.jpg, CC BY-SA 3.0, Tobias Fluegel.
Orrorin
The third spectacular find from the Miocene was of a species named Orrorin tugenensis. It is
missing from the analysis of Strait and Grine because a cranium has not been found. Instead
Humans 69
Figure 5.4 Top portion of a modern human femur, showing the short femoral neck.
Source: Adaptation of Fumur Posterior annoted.png, CC BY SA-3.0, Frank Gaillard.
its fossils consist primarily of limb bones and teeth from several individuals. Discovered near
Tugen, Kenya, in a tuff (a layer of rock formed from compacted volcanic ash), they are dated
at between 5.7 and 6.0 million years.
Senut (2006) summarized several bipedal features of Orrorin’s femur, the thigh bone
that forms a ball joint with the pelvis. One such feature concerns the femoral neck, the
short span of bone between the shaft and the femoral head (Figure 5.4). Orrorin’s
neck is noticeably longer than that of chimpanzees. Its length is characteristic of early
hominin bipedalism because it allowed the bottom of the femur to angle back toward the
body’s midline, centering body weight in a more columnlike fashion over the legs.
Interestingly, following modification of the pelvis during human evolution, the femoral
neck no longer needs to be so long to achieve the same effect and so it has again
shortened in modern Homo sapiens (Holliday, Hutchinson, Morrow, & Livesay, 2010;
Richmond & Jungers, 2008). This reflects a trend that began in mid-Pleistocene Homo
(Marchi et al., 2017).
Some unknown hominin species – with only chronology leading to the speculation it
might be Orrorin – is thought to have been responsible for the fourth spectacular Miocene
discovery, a possible trackway of footprints recently found near Trachilos, Crete. Dating to
5.7 million years, the impressions show only modest splay of the big toe, much less than that
of Ardipithecus. The find is notable not only for its antiquity but its location, the island of
Crete not having previously been implicated in hominin evolution. Its island status is not
problematic, though, in that it was likely connected to the Greek mainland at the time, and
could have been reached by walking (Gierlinski et al., 2017).
However, Meldrum and Sarmiento (2018) have been critical of Gierlinksi et al.’s inter-
pretation of the Trachilos impressions. Among other factors, they point to ambiguities in
shape that raise questions not only about the prints’ hominin origins, but whether they are
footprints of any kind. For their part, in an exhaustive review of research on hominin feet,
DeSilva, McNutt, Benoit, and Zipfel (2018) seemingly accept the impressions as footprints,
but call for additional information before concluding that they were made by hominins. That
may be forthcoming, in that additional footprints are rumored to have been discovered on
Crete (Chung, 2018).
Taken together, the Miocene hominin discoveries have been nothing short of revolu-
tionary, and they settle an old controversy (Box 5.1).
70 Introduction to evolution
The term missing link was coined in the 19th century, soon after the publication of
Charles Darwin’s Origin of Species ( Lewin & Foley, 2004). It was recognized that if the
nonhuman apes split off from the human ancestral line millions of years ago, there
should surely be fossil evidence of it. The lack of such evidence led to the conclusion
that there was a missing transitional form that sooner or later would be discovered by
scientists working in the field. However, for some members of the public who doubted
evolutionary accounts of human origins, the missing link became a rallying point. As
long as it remained unknown, the gap between apes and humans was something
science had not fully explained.
For scientists, on the other hand, the missing link proved a means of capturing the
public imagination, and a succession of fossil finds were popularly billed as the missing
link ( Reader, 1981). Thus, the discovery of “Java man” (now called Homo erectus) in
1892 provided what then appeared to be the missing link. Yet over the next century,
further discoveries were made, e.g., Australopithecus africanus in 1924, Australopithecus
afarensis in the 1970s, and Ardipithecus ramidus in 1994 ( Lewin & Foley, 2004). Early in
this process, William Jennings Bryan, the famous anti-evolution politician and orator,
is said to have asked, “If the missing link has been found, why are they still looking for
it?” ( BBN International, 2008). In truth, each of these was a missing link that pushed
human origins ever earlier. But the missing link, the earliest possible transitional form
with human characteristics, remained elusive.
For some researchers during the early 1970s, these discoveries seemed to only
nibble at a much deeper problem. The australopithecines, the earliest hominins
known at that time, dated back only three million years. The view then current, that
the ancestor of all nonhuman apes diverged from our ancestral line prior to 14 million
years ago, set off debate over which fossils of that age were closest to representing
the missing link. Pilbeam (1972) argued that the closest was Ramapithecus (now
considered Sivapithecus; Simons, 1992), an orangutan-like creature dating from
14 million years ago. However, the remains were fragmentary, and it was assumed
that future fossil discoveries would more firmly establish the human-ape split as
having occurred around that time.
Today, the reason those fossils have never been found is clear: They do not exist.
Because the ancestor of chimpanzees split from our ancestral line about 7.5 million
years ago, the first fossils with unmistakably human characteristics must be younger
than that split. The Miocene hominins appear to neatly fill the role. The missing link is
no longer missing.
Anamensis
Anamensis is known from a cranium, jaws, teeth, and a partial femur found in Kenya and
Ethiopia, dated to 3.8 to 4.2 million years ago. The recently discovered cranium shows a
sagittal crest (a ridge of bone running front-to-back on the top of the skull), serving as an
additional attachment point for jaw muscles). From associated fossils anamensis is believed to
have lived in a woodland environment. Given its location and characteristics an anamensis to
afarensis descent is considered plausible, although other possibilities exist (Haile-Selassie,
Melillo, Vazzana, Benazzi, & Ryan, 2019; Wood & Boyle, 2016).
Afarensis
Afarensis has the next oldest set of fossils, dating to 3.0 to 3.7 million years ago, with a
3.9 million-year-old skull bone only tentatively identified as coming from the species.
The fossils come from Tanzania, Kenya, and Ethiopia (White, 2002; Wood & Boyle,
2016). The most famous example is the partial skeleton of “Lucy”, including a femur
with an early biped’s distinctively long neck and a shaft that angled in to a horizontal
base (Figure 5.5). The shape of the pelvis is also consistent with bipedalism. Any re-
maining doubt is all but eliminated by Mary Leakey’s discovery of footprints at Laetoli,
Tanzania (Figure 5.6). Imprinted in ash, allowing firm dating at 3.66 million years
ago, the footprints comprise a trackway of two bipedal individuals apparently walking
side-by-side, with a smaller individual stepping in their footprints (Potts, 1992; Masao
et al., 2016). Like the 5.7 million year old Trachilos prints, but in contrast to ramidus
anatomy, the big toe showed relatively little splay. The Laetoli prints are attributed to
afarensis because that species is associated with the geological setting in which the
prints were found, and because its foot was moderately arched like the ones that made
the prints.
72 Introduction to evolution
Afarensis is one of the best known hominins due to extensive fossil remains. It is es-
timated to have stood 1–1.5 m (3–5 feet) in height depending on gender (with males
larger), and to have averaged 35–40 kg (75–90 pounds) in weight. Its brain is estimated
at somewhat over 400 cc (almost a pint) in volume, although cranial remains are in-
complete. It had noticeable brow ridges, and some individuals had a small sagittal crest
(Figures 5.7–5.8). Relatively small, unspecialized teeth suggest an omnivorous diet, and
fossil pollen collected at Hadar, Ethiopia, one of the main sites, indicate it was able to live
in a varied environment that included grassland, temperate woodland, tropical woodland,
Humans 73
swamp, and evergreen forest among others (Villaseñor, Bobe, & Behrensmeyer, 2020). Its
hand was remarkably modern-looking, with a thumb extending beyond the knuckles
(unlike chimpanzees) but with narrow fingertips (like chimpanzees). Its long arms suggest
it may have been partially arboreal, perhaps sleeping in trees at night like chimpanzees
(Fleagle, 1992; Trinkaus, 1992; Wood, 1992). This supposition is supported by the fossil
remains of a juvenile, showing evidence of bipedalism in the foot and leg, but of arboreal
existence in the shoulder blade and hand (Alemseged et al., 2006). This species, or
possibly Kenyanthropus platyops, is currently believed to be the earliest known hominin
stone tool maker (Lewis & Harmand, 2016).
74 Introduction to evolution
Garhi
Garhi, the next australopithecine to split from the hominin line according to the cladogram
(Figure 5.2), is known from a relatively late fossil cranium and palate found in Ethiopia,
about 2.5 million years old. It differs from other species of the period in having large teeth,
especially in the back of the jaw. Its brain size is estimated at 450 cc, about a pint in size
(Asfaw et al., 1999). Garhi may have manufactured stone tools, found in the same area
of Ethiopia and dated to about 2.6 million years ago, but the connection is not certain
(Semaw, 2000).
Africanus
Africanus is the first-discovered australopithecine, described by Raymond Dart in 1925 from
South African fossils. Next to branch off our ancestral line according to the cladogram, it
dates to 2.4 to 3.0 million years ago (White, 2002; Wood & Boyle, 2016). Together, africanus
and afarensis comprise what traditionally have been called the gracile (slender) forms of
Australopithecus, as opposed to the robust (heavily built) forms of robustus and boisei. However,
as we will shortly see, this division is a bit of a misconception.
Africanus stood 1.1-1.5 m (3.5-5 ft) in height, averaged 35-40 kg (75-90 pounds), and
had a brain 420-515 cc in volume. Its face was shorter than that of afarensis. It was certainly
bipedal. In all likelihood it ate fruit and leaves, but carbon isotope analysis suggests that it
also consumed the roots of grass or grasslike plants (or perhaps animals feeding on them).
Humans 75
In turn that suggests that africanus lived in a grassland or woodland environment (Wood,
1992; Wood & Aiello, 1998).
rhesus monkeys > squirrel monkeys) and the reduction of sex differences. Williams found
that an exponent of 0.28, the lowest of the values tested, best satisfied both criteria.
Subsequently others similarly found that an exponent of 0.3 fit better than exponents
exceeding 0.6, based on a more sophisticated measure of cognitive ability integrating
nine behavioral categories (e.g., detour, tool use, and object discrimination; Deaner, Isler,
Burkart, & van Schaik, 2007).
Williams (2002) recommended using the following relationship, which is adopted here:
In practice brain size in cubic centimeters (cc) can be substituted for brain mass in grams, as
the brain is about the density of water (i.e., one gram per cc). Of course when calculating
EQ it must be kept in mind that sample sizes are generally small and within-species
variability large. Further imprecision comes from estimating brain size using skulls re-
constructed from crushed or incomplete states, and in estimating body mass from mea-
surements of leg bones or even of correlated structures like the orbit of the eye. The
resulting EQ values must therefore be considered imprecise. Nevertheless, general evo-
lutionary trends are obvious (Table 5.1).
78 Introduction to evolution
Table 5.1 Eocene primates, Aegyptopithecus, Proconsul, and the common chimpanzee compared to hominin
species, arranged by genus and geological period. EQ = encephalization quotient (see text), kya=
thousands of years ago, mya = millions of years ago
Comparison primates
Eocene primates 43 mya 8.3 cc 2.5 kg 1.0 3 species
Aegyptopithecus zeuxis 30 mya 30 cc 6 kg 2.6
Proconsul nyanzae 18 mya 167 cc 15 kg 11.3
Pan troglodytes present 366 cc 45 kg 18.2
Miocene hominins (5.3-23 mya)
Sahelanthropus tchadensis 6.8-7.2 mya 365 cc 45 kg 18.2
Ardipithecus ramidus 4.3-6.3 mya 325 cc 50 kg 15.7 also kadabba
Orrorin tugenensis 5.7-6.0 mya n/a 35 kg n/a
Pliocene hominins (2.6-5.3 mya)
Australopithecus anamensis 3.8-4.2 mya 368 cc 42 kg 18.7
Australopithecus afarensis 3.0-3.7 mya 444 cc 37 kg 23.3
Kenyanthropus platyops 3.4-3.5 mya n/a n/a n/a
Australopithecus africanus 2.4-3.0 mya 455 cc 36 kg 24.1
Early Pleistocene hominins (0.8-2.6 mya)
Australopithecus aethiopicus 2.3-2.7 mya 410 cc 38 kg 21.4
Australopithecus garhi 2.5 mya 450 cc n/a n/a
Homo habilis 1.7-2.4 mya 616 cc 35 kg 32.9
Australopithecus boisei 1.3-2.3 mya 521 cc 42 kg 26.4
Australopithecus sediba 2.0 mya 420 cc 33 kg 22.8
Homo rudolfensis 2.0 mya 789 cc 56 kg 36.9
Australopithecus robustus 1.0-2.0 mya 530 cc 36 kg 28.1
Homo erectus 0.3-2.0 mya 987 cc 63 kg 44.7 mainly Asian
Homo ergaster 1.4-1.7 mya 871 cc 61 kg 39.8 African
Middle and Late Pleistocene hominins (0 kya-0.8 mya)
Homo heidelbergensis 200-700 kya 1158 cc 54 kg 54.8
Homo floresiensis 50-700 kya 426 cc 26 kg 24.7 Indonesian island
Homo naledi 253 kya 545 cc 46 kg 27.0
Homo neanderthalensis 40-130/400 kya 1431 cc 76 kg 61.5
Homo sapiens 0-195 kya 1350 cc 54 kg 63.9
Notes: Ages largely from Wood & Boyle (2016); for Eocene primates from http://eol.org; for floresiensis from van den
Bergh et al. (2016) and Sutikna et al., (2016); for erectus from Herries et al., (2020); for naledi from Dirks et al. (2017). Size
and mass estimates for Eocene primates from Harrington, Silcox, Yapunich, Boyer, & Bloch (2016); for Aegyptopithecus
and Proconsul from Begun and Kordos (2004); for aethiopicus from Kappelman (1996); for sediba, near-adult brain size
estimate from Berger et al. (2010), and adult body size retrieved from http://www.profleeberger.com/AusedibaQandA.
html; for erectus and heidelbergensis from Rightmire (2004). Brain size estimates for afarensis, africanus, habilis, rudolfensis,
floresiensis, and naledi from Hawks et al. (2017), for anamensis from Haile-Selassie et al. (2019), and for neanderthalensis from
Wood and Constantino (2004). Body mass estimate for Pan from Williams (2002); for tchadensis from Brunet et al. (2004);
for ramidus from Suwa, Asfaw, Kono, Kubo, Lovejoy, and White (2009), and White et al. (2009); for floresiensis from Falk
et al. (2006); for naledi from Berger, et al. (2015); for neanderthalensis from Cameron & Groves (2004). Most other values
from McHenry & Coffing (2000).
When EQs are calculated in this way, it is clear that habilis did have a large brain relative to
the australopithecines. Thus, while australopithecine EQs are in the range of 21.4-28.1, the
value for habilis was 32.7. The species packed a big brain for its size. The single known
specimen of rudolfensis did as well: It had a substantially larger body but also a larger brain,
resulting in an even greater EQ of 35.2.
In any case the general consensus is that Homo commenced with habilis, meaning “able,
handy, mentally skillful”, a notable example of assignment of a species name because of cultural
Humans 79
capabilities (Dunsworth & Walker, 2002). Of course mental skills are synonymous with cog-
nitive capabilities, and are reflected in the abundant stone tools associated with habilis.
Naledi
Homo naledi is a very recently described form, discovered by spelunkers exploring a South
African cave system. Reached by a torturous underground path, the fossil chamber was
nearly inaccessible due to a narrow entrance chute. Its excavation waited on the results of a
worldwide call for petite scientists with the requisite skills, and six women ultimately ac-
complished the job (Berger et al., 2015; Shreve, 2015).
An assessment of the fossils’ relatively primitive characteristics led some to think
that they dated back as far as two million years ago. Researchers were stunned, then,
when a variety of dating techniques showed that they are only 253,000 years old (Dirks
et al., 2017). Even more shockingly, naledi had a very small brain, averaging 545 cc across
three crania, 40% of the modern human average (Hawks et al., 2017). Even after taking
into account naledi’s relatively small body, its EQ of 27.0 was only 42% of ours
(Table 5.1).
One primitive characteristic of naledi was the marked curvature of its fingers, approx-
imating that of early hominins and nonhuman apes. The condition, which is likely genetic
(Wallace, Burgess, & Patel, 2020), is an adaptation for climbing and suspension from branches
and is therefore diagnostic of arboreality (Feuerriegel et al., 2017; Kivell et al., 2015). That is
an extraordinary conclusion given the species’ recent existence. Nevertheless naledi was a
proficient biped, and had a human-like stride on the ground. In that regard another primitive
characteristic was a long femoral neck, typical of early hominins but not of Homo species
contemporaneous to naledi (Marchi et al., 2017).
Sapiens
Modern Homo sapiens (Figure 5.12) is held to have emerged about 195,000 years ago
(Wood & Boyle, 2016), with Africa our point of origin. The earliest undisputed specimens
are known from Ethiopia and Israel. Although an origin as early as 350,000 years ago
has been claimed for fossils collected from a Moroccan site (Hublin et al., 2017), their status
as Homo sapiens has been questioned (Callaway, 2017). They do appear to have some features
that are not modern (i.e., the presence of a brow ridge and an undeveloped chin).
One problem is that the boundary between heidelbergensis and sapiens is arbitrary, as there is a
continual gradation. It has been argued nevertheless that the range of variation is sufficient to
justify a division into two species (Wood & Constantino, 2004). The emergence of modern
sapiens was accompanied by substantial cultural and cognitive developments which will be
outlined in subsequent chapters. The average brain size has been 1350 cc (nearly three pints).
Neanderthalensis
The middle Pleistocene is also known for the closely related Homo neanderthalensis
(Figures 5.13–5.14). The earliest unambiguous Neanderthal fossils date from about
130,000 years, and ambiguous ones from 400,000 years ago. The species survived until
about 40,000 years ago (Meyer et al., 2016; Wood & Boyle, 2016). Remains largely
come from Europe but some are from western and central Asia.
For many years there was a controversy regarding the Neanderthals’ status, with some
designating them a subspecies of modern humans (White, Gowlett, & Grove, 2014).
Species status seemed confirmed by the earliest research that examined mitochondrial
82 Introduction to evolution
and chromosomal DNA from Neanderthal remains. It reported little affinity to living
humans (Green et al., 2006; Orlando, Darlu, Toussaint, Bonjean, Otte, & Hanni, 2006).
However, subsequent study of the full Neanderthal genome did find similarities.
Specifically, Europeans and Asians were found to be more similar to Neanderthals than
were Africans, strongly implying that interbreeding occurred in areas of Neanderthal
habitation. While the species designation remains, it may not represent the final word
in classification given that up to 2% of the genome of non-Africans derives from
Neanderthals (see Box 5.2).
In that regard a distinction should be made between coalescence time, the latest date at
which Neanderthals and humans had completely common ancestry (and thus the date
at which there was no distinction at all between the species), and split time, the latest date at
which there was any genetic interchange between the species. Coalescence time averages
about 580,000 years across five studies using DNA from the Y chromosome (Mendez,
Poznik, Castellano, & Bustamante, 2016), the autosomes (chromosomes other than X and Y;
Green et al., 2010; Noonan et al., 2006), the whole genome (Prüfer et al., 2014), and
mitochondria (Green et al., 2008).
It is unsurprising that coalescence time predates the earliest Neanderthal fossils (130,000-
400,000 years old, depending on their ambiguity). First, as previously indicated, fossils
postdate the first appearance of a species for probabilistic reasons: The initial population is so
Humans 83
Ever since the initial discovery of Neanderthals in 1856, there has been controversy
over how the group should be placed in the human family tree. Hermann
Schaafhausen, the German anatomist who analyzed the first Neanderthal remains,
concluded that in spite of the presence of a heavy brow ridge, they were human – a
race, perhaps, within Homo sapiens ( Goodrum, 2013). However, William King, after
his own examination of the fossils, concluded that they represented a new species,
Homo neanderthalensis. A few years later he went further, stating “I now feel strongly
inclined to believe that it is not only specifically but generically distinct from Man”;
i.e., falling not just outside sapiens but even outside Homo ( King, 1864).
The placement debate continues to the present day. As an undergraduate in the
1970s, I saw the group referred to as both Homo neanderthalensis, a species, and Homo
sapiens neanderthalensis, a subspecies. In 2014, the issue was still alive when Suzanna
White and colleagues reviewed what was meant by “species” and concluded that
Neanderthals fulfilled the major requirements ( White, Gowlett, & Grove, 2014).
Among others, those included the maintenance of distinct anatomical and genetic
differences even after interbreeding.
Yet the issue is still not dead. If anything it is increasingly coming to the fore in
debates over phylogenetic status. Largely this is because of increasing evidence
that sapiens interbred with not just one but multiple archaic groups. Thus, about
2% of the genome of non-Africans and about 0.3% of that of Africans derives
from Neanderthals ( Price, 2020; Skov et al., 2020). But among Oceanics (e.g.,
Melanesians, aboriginal Australians, and Papuans) another 2-4% comes from
Denisovans ( Vernot et al., 2016). The Denisovan group, whose ancestors split
off the Neanderthal line about 420,000 years ago ( Prüfer et al., 2014), and which
survived as late as 29,000 years ago, was first identified by a finger bone and
teeth recovered from a Siberian cave ( Slatkin & Racimo, 2016; Wood & Boyle,
2016). Many stone and bone tools were found as well. Recently, Denisovan genetic
influence has even been found in modern Icelanders ( Skov et al., 2020). Its
contributions to the genome of Homo sapiens, and that of Neanderthals, are illustrated
in Figure 5.15.
Nor does it end there. By accounting for genetic material provided by ancient
groups of sapiens, Neanderthals, and Denisovans, it has proved possible to infer
the existence of another, older interbreeding group whose identity is unknown. This
group, called “superarchaics”, may have directly descended from the hominins
who first migrated from Africa into Europe – possibly early Homo ergaster or erectus
1.9 million years ago ( Rogers, Harris, & Achenbach, 2020) or an even earlier, more
habilis-like species as much as 2.5 million years ago ( Scardia et al., 2020).
For awhile the possibility was entertained that Homo antecessor, the proposed species
name of a one-million-year-old hominid found in Spain, might have been the last
common ancestor of all three groups ( de Castro, Martinón-Torres, Arsuaga, &
Carbonell, 2017; Wood & Constantino, 2004). On that basis it might have proved
to be the superarchaic species. However, analysis of proteins from an antecessor tooth
indicates that the hominin was related to, but not the same as, the last common
ancestor of the three groups ( Welker et al., 2020). For now, the superarchaic species is
unidentified, and the status of antecessor is ambiguous.
Humans 85
Figure 5.15 Denisovan and Neanderthal contributions to the ancestry of Homo sapiens. The numbers
at the left indicate millions of years.
Source: Homo-Stammbaum, Version Stringer.jpg, CC BY-SA 3.0 DE, Chris Stringer.
Given extensive evidence of interbreeding, one criterion for species status – that
groups are genetically isolated so that interbreeding is either impossible or produces
offspring that are not themselves reproductively viable – could lead to the conclusion that
the more recent groups, including Neanderthals and Denisovans, were not separate
species but rather subspecies of sapiens. However some, such as White et al. (2014), have
not considered genetic isolation to be a necessary criterion of species status.
Small wonder, then, that the phylogenetic status of Neanderthals - and indeed, of all of
these groups – is in flux. With respect to Neanderthals specifically, some accept species
status (e.g., Nielsen, Langley, Shipton, & Kapitány, 2020; Terhune, Ritzman, &
Robinson, 2018), others subspecies status (e.g., Abdelhady, Elewa, & El-Dawy, 2019;
Cserhati et al., 2018), and some avoid the matter entirely (e.g., Rogers et al., 2020). This
old debate will likely continue for awhile before scientific consensus is reached.
tiny that finding the first fossils is practically an impossibility. Second, as noted in the dis-
cussion of Toumai, following their split two species are so similar that it may not be possible
to distinguish between them. Thus, older fossils are not only difficult to find, but even if
found might not appear unambiguously Neanderthal.
For its part split time was initially estimated at 370,000 years ago, based on an incomplete
Neanderthal genome (Noonan et al., 2006). However, that conclusion has yielded to re-
cognition that late interbreeding must have occurred between sapiens and neanderthalensis,
possibly as recently as 50,000 years ago (Simonti et al., 2016; Wall & Brandt, 2016).
Neanderthals typically lived in cold, “marginal” environments. They had a larger brain
than sapiens in absolute terms, but a slightly smaller EQ because of their substantially larger
body mass (Table 5.1). They were powerfully built, and their skulls had thick, continuous
brow ridges (Figure 5.13). They left cultural remains including evidence of intentional burials
(Cowen, 2005; Wood & Constantino, 2004).
86 Introduction to evolution
Floresiensis
The final hominin is also arguably the most bizarre, and not just because it was a late
survival in a world populated by sapiens. A skeleton including a skull of Homo floresiensis
was discovered in 2004 in a cave on the island of Flores, Indonesia, by a team of Australian
and Indonesian researchers. The relatively recent remains were in a prefossilized, extremely
fragile state. Remarkably, even though the specimen was an adult it stood only slightly
over one meter (3 ft) tall and would have weighed only about 22 kg (50 pounds). The
nickname “The Hobbit” was accordingly bestowed because of its diminutive size. The
skull was correspondingly small and had a low cranium (Figures 5.16–5.17), producing an
estimated brain size of only 426 cc, a little less than a pint (Brown et al., 2004; Falk et al.,
2006). The resulting EQ is only 24.7, the smallest of all Middle and Late Pleistocene
species (Table 5.1).
The find was initially greeted by skepticism and a counterproposal that the single in-
dividual might have been a microcephalic (abnormally small-brained) dwarf but nevertheless a
sapiens. Its diminutive stature was subsequently confirmed, however, following the discovery
of partial remains of eight other individuals. Height could be estimated for one of them,
which turned out to be slightly less than that of the original find (Morwood et al., 2005).
Furthermore, measurements based on the skull were found to much more closely match
normal humans than a sample of microcephalics (Falk, et al., 2007). Other analyses based on
the wrist (Tocheri et al., 2007) and the shoulder, foot, pelvis, and femur (Aiello, 2010)
likewise indicate that the Hobbit was not a pathological sapiens.
Initial dating estimates based on charcoal found in the cave found the youngest of the
Flores fossils to be only 12,000 years old. However, this date was discovered to be false,
the result of inadvertent sampling from a different geological section than the fossils. An array
of dating methods now firmly date the youngest of the floresiensis-related deposits at
50,000 years old (Sutikna et al., 2016).
More recently, teeth and a jaw bone of floresiensis excavated in 2014 were described and
dated to the much older period of 700,000 years ago. They were just as small as the later
remains, and if anything a bit smaller. Although they do not address the size or state of the
cranium at that period, they do establish the antiquity of the species on the island of Flores
(van den Bergh et al., 2016).
In 2019, 67-thousand-year-old hominin fossils from Luzon, Philippines, were described as
a new species, Homo luzonensis (Détroit et al., 2019). Aspects of its teeth, and of its foot and
hand bones, variously resembled australopithecines, Homo erectus, Homo sapiens, Homo nean-
derthalensis – and Homo floresiensis. Although believed to have been small-bodied, there are
insufficient skeletal remains to allow either a weight estimate or a close comparison with
floresiensis. It therefore seems inadvisable at this early stage to attribute new species status to
the remains (Gerbis, 2019). The same might be said of the even more recently described
“Homo longi”, of 150,000 years ago, that pairs massive, continuous brow ridges with a very
Neanderthal- and sapiens-like brain size of 1420 cc (Ji, Wu, Ji, Li, & Ni, 2021).
Skull trends
Additional trends over the course of hominin evolution are apparent from skulls. The brow
ridges of tchadensis seven million years ago (Figure 5.1) waned, waxed, and waned again
in size over successive species but were consistently present until the emergence of
modern sapiens. At the same time, the face became flatter and more vertical, and the rear
teeth generally decreased in size as diets included more meat and less fibrous vegetable matter.
All of these characters, like brain size, have figured prominently in the reconstruction of
our phylogeny.
Hominin phylogeny
What then was the structure of the human family tree? Before addressing this question it is
helpful to consider some influences on phylogenetic inferences. One already referred to is the
occasional tendency of the discoverers of a fossil to overweight its importance. That can
easily affect how that particular team reconstructs phylogeny. However, the beauty of science
is that others are free to reconsider the evidence to potentially come to more balanced
conclusions.
A second influence is that the age of a species is rarely precisely known even if it left
accurately dated fossils. Earlier and later fossils may never have formed, or may still be in the
ground waiting to be discovered. Thus, the species may have had a longer evolutionary
lifespan than is apparent from fossil evidence.
Third, the assignment of fossils to particular species is error-prone. There are many his-
torical examples of the reassignment of a misassigned fossil following the acquisition of ad-
ditional information. That obviously affects the reconstruction of phylogeny.
Finally, a fourth influence on phylogenetic inferences should be inserted as a cautionary
note. This is that it is generally impossible to determine whether any fossil is from a direct
human ancestor in a strict genetic sense. The best we can say is that any given fossilized
individual, and every human alive today, traces back to a common ancestor at some un-
known point during or prior to the existence of the fossilized individual. This raises questions
as to whether we can say that we descend from a particular species as defined by known
fossils. However, in many cases we can infer that we must descend from an individual very
much like that species. With that proviso it is nevertheless common practice, as a kind of
shortcut, to refer to specific species defined by known fossils when reconstructing phylogeny.
We can now consider the likelihoods, tracing backwards. Homo sapiens (0-195,000
years ago) is almost universally recognized as a descendant of Homo heidelbergensis (0.2 to
0.7 million years ago. In turn, heidelbergensis is assumed to have descended from Homo
erectus (0.3 to 2.0 million years ago), most likely the early African forms since later Asian
ones are probably too derived to be ancestors of modern humans (Wood & Constantino,
2004). Erectus likely descended from the more primitive ergaster, even though at present
its oldest fossils, dated at 2.0 million years ago, predate the oldest ergaster fossils, at
1.7 million years ago.
90 Introduction to evolution
What path is traced backward from ergaster depends on which characters are emphasized. A
strict chronology-and-encephalization approach traces the line back through Homo habilis.
However, others emphasize the postcranial skeleton (the skeleton below the head) and suggest
that habilis had limb proportions that were too ape-like to make it a likely human ancestor
(Richmond, Aiello, & Wood, 2002). Nevertheless increasing encephalization was clearly a
major hominin theme, and that plus the cultural remains associated with habilis do seem to
justify its placement in the ancestral tree.
There is also controversy with respect to the link between habilis and earlier forms. There
is near-consensus that Australopithecus afarensis (3.0 to 3.7 million years ago) was ancestral,
but less agreement that the 600,000 year gap between habilis and afarensis was filled by
Australopithecus africanus (2.4 to 3.0 million years ago). Some nominate Australopithecus garhi
(2.5 million years ago) for the intermediate role, but remains seem too fragmentary at this
point to draw that conclusion with any certainty.
As earlier indicated, a descent of afarensis from anamensis (3.8 to 4.2 million years ago) is
considered plausible, and in fact the across-three-species ancestral connections between
habilis, afarensis, and anamensis have been accepted by Donald Johanson, one of the dis-
coverers of “Lucy”, based on derived characters that seemingly passed from one to the next
( Johanson, 2017).
Although a connection from anamensis to ramidus (4.3 to 6.3 million years ago) has been
proposed, ramidus’ widely splayed big toe is problematic. For that reason it seems wiser to
place a speculative link from anamensis to tugenensis. What relationship tugenensis – or for that
matter, ramidus – had to Sahelanthropus tchadensis remains to be seen. Wood and Boyle (2016)
raised the possibility that all three should be assigned to the same genus, or even collapsed
into one or two species instead of three, in recognition of their general similarity. But for
now tchadensis (7 mya) can be tentatively placed at the head of the hominin ancestral line of
descent, as the earliest of the three forms.
A well-pruned bush
What is most notable about Table 5.1 is that while many hominin species have existed across
7.5 million years, only one survives. Our portion of the tree of life more resembles a heavily
pruned bush that it does a tree, with a single branch emerging from the crown and all others
trimmed away.
Heavy pruning is not unusual in evolutionary history, another example being the survival
of birds as the only representatives of a once-numerous superorder of dinosaurs. By its
nature evolution often takes a crooked path from past to present. The randomness of mu-
tation combined with diverse and changing environments results in many unsuccessful
“experiments”, a useful term as long as we recognize that no intentionality is involved.
One educated guess is that of all the species ever generated by evolution, 99.9% are now
extinct (Raup, 1991, pp. 3-4).
Of course one difference between birds and humans is that birds radiated to become
thousands of species worldwide, while there is now only one human species. Not only
can any population of humans interbreed with all others, but genetic analyses show close
relatedness in evolutionary terms. For example, studies have examined the similarity of
populations with respect to mitochondrial DNA, the genetic instructions accompanying
the mitochondria in our cells that are not part of our 46 chromosomes. Because mi-
tochondria are inherited as part of the egg cell rather than the sperm, their genetic
content follows the maternal line of descent, mother to daughter to child. (Although boys
don’t have egg cells, they nevertheless derive their mitochondria from their mother’s
egg cell.) Taking a molecular clock approach suggests that living human populations trace
Humans 91
to a common female ancestor, a “mitochondrial Eve”. She is estimated to have lived
sometime between 125,000 and 200,000 years ago. Furthermore, Africans show the
greatest variability in mitochondrial DNA and therefore the deepest convergence on that
ancestor, strongly arguing that she lived in Africa (Cyran & Kimmel, 2010; Ingman,
Kaessmann, Pääbo, & Gyllensten, 2000). In fact, comparison with living populations
suggest she may more specifically have lived in present-day Botswana, in southern Africa
(Chan et al., 2019).
A second line of research has examined population similarities in the sequencing of the
Y chromosome, which is inherited in the paternal line, father to son to son. (Girls do not
have Y chromosomes.) Application of molecular clock principles indicates that a common
ancestor of all men lived 70,000 to 200,000 years ago, but possibly as much as 340,000 years
ago. Furthermore, like Eve this “Adam” is believed to have lived in Africa, possibly in what is
now western Cameroon in the continent’s northwest quarter (Mendez et al, 2013; Poznik
et al., 2013; Scozzari et al., 2014). Furthermore, remains of Neanderthals less than 55,000
years old were recently discovered to have carried an “Adam-like” Y chromosome! The
implication is that the interbreeding that resulted in our Neanderthal genes also allowed
Neanderthals to inherit our apparently advantageous Y chromosome (Petr et al., 2020).
It should be stressed that African origins apply to all of our genetic material, not just
mitochondrial and Y chromosomal DNA. The DNA from our autosomes similarly shows
greatest variability in African populations, implying that it originated there and thus has had
maximal time to develop diversity. Yet even the least diverse population globally – the Suruí,
a Brazilian tribe – captures 60% of total worldwide diversity within its members (Hunley,
Cabana, & Long, 2016), reflecting the relatedness of human populations.
Thus, 7.5 million years of human evolution have come down to just one interrelated
human species. This certainly was not always the case, as hominin ancestry has been bushy in
the past. At least six species were alive two million years ago, and three species as little as
50,000 years ago (Table 5.1).
What accounts for such severe pruning? Undoubtedly part of the answer is that all bipedal
apes have occupied very similar environmental niches. They have been omnivorous, able to
travel distances while carrying babies or tools, and large enough to defend against predators
and doubtless other bipedal apes. Such similarity in the demands placed on the environment,
and in responses to it, created direct competition for resources when different species came
into contact.
Part of the answer, too, must be that the playing field implied by competition became
radically tilted when human cultural innovations became commonplace. In turn cultural
innovations were produced by cognitive innovations, ensuring the selective survival of “wise
man”, or in Latin, Homo sapiens.
Conclusion
The term “hominin” refers to humans, their ancestors, and their immediate relatives, back to
the chimpanzee-human split 7.5 million years ago. The Sahelanthropus tchadensis (“Toumai”)
fossil dates back seven million years, almost to the split itself. Signs of bipedalism in the skull
are echoed in Orrorin fossils a million years later, showing the long femoral neck characteristic
of early bipeds. Ardipithecus is a third Miocene hominin dating to 6.3 million years ago.
However, the species had a splayed, grasping big toe, postdating a recently discovered
trackway in Crete lacking that feature, and thereby suggesting that it may not have been
directly ancestral to modern humans.
Hominin species emerging in the Pliocene included Australopithecus anamensis, afarensis,
and africanus, and Kenyapithecus platyops. Anamensis, dating to 3.8 to 4.2 million years ago,
92 Introduction to evolution
represents a plausible transition from earlier forms. “Lucy” is the most famous example of
afarensis, consisting of a partial skeleton including a long-necked, bipedal femur. This species
likely made the trackway at Laetoli. Afarensis may also have been partially arboreal, judging
from the shoulder blade and hand, but there is a good chance that either it or Kenyapithecus
made the earliest known stone tools.
Australopithecus garhi (2.5 million years ago) was possibly also an early maker of stone tools.
Its contemporary Africanus was the first-discovered australopithecine, and probably lived in a
grassland or woodland environment in South Africa. The australopithecines robustus, boisei,
and aethiopicus are sometimes assigned a separate genus, Paranthropus, in recognition of dis-
tinctive features including massive jaws and a sagittal crest.
The genus Homo emerged about 2.4 million years ago in the form of Homo habilis, a species
marked by a noticeable increase in encephalization. It was probably the ancestor of Homo
ergaster, itself giving rise after a very brief time to Homo erectus, with the latter surviving in Asia
until relatively recently (about 310,000 years ago). Ergaster was an African hominin that for
the first time showed an absence of arboreal adaptations, indicating full-time bipedalism.
Erectus was a likely ancestor of Homo heidelbergensis, or archaic Homo sapiens, dating as far
back as one million years ago. Modern humans emerged about 195,000 years ago in Africa.
Three other relatively late species were Homo neanderthalensis, naledi, and floresiensis, the
former of which contributed to the biological ancestry of modern sapiens. Naledi and flor-
esiensis were small-brained species with low EQs, and floresiensis also had a very small body,
possibly a case of island dwarfism. A long-standing controversy over the species versus
subspecies status of Neanderthals is receiving new impetus from genetic studies implicating
interbreeding of sapiens with neanderthalensis, with a group called Denisovans, and with yet
another group of “superarchaics”.
Throughout humankind’s descent, brain size has generally increased both in absolute terms
and in relation to body size. EQ values more than tripled between “Toumai” and today, a
trend aided from the onset of archaic sapiens by a reduction in body size. Another trend has
been a consistent trimming of the hominin “bush”, from at least five coexistent species two
million years ago to today’s single species. There is strong evidence that all humans descend
from a common female ancestor, “mitochondrial Eve” living 125,000-200,000 years ago,
and all men from a Y-chromosome “Adam” living 70,000-200,000 years ago.
Determining the hominin line of descent is subject to biases, imprecise dates, and the im-
possibility of determining whether any fossil is a direct ancestor in a genetic sense. The like-
lihood is that modern Homo sapiens descends from Homo heidelbergensis, itself descending from
Homo erectus and then in turn from Homo ergaster and Homo habilis. Habilis was likely descended
from Australopithecus afarensis and Australopithecus anamensis. The remaining connections are
more conjectural, but links to Orrorin tugenensis and finally Sahelanthropus tchadensis are possible.
Section II
A large brain serves as only a crude indicator of the sensory, motor, and cognitive abilities
Homo sapiens acquired over 7.5 million years of hominin evolution. Our ancestors used
brain specializations, as do we, to collect inputs from the environment, to understand
meaning, and to produce outputs in response. How sensation and movement evolved
provides important context for understanding the evolution of the cognitive faculties that
bridge them. In this chapter we primarily consider the mechanical and chemical senses of
touch, balance, hearing, smell, and taste, and the evolutionary trajectory shared by some
of them.
Early sensing
Even early single-cell organisms needed a way to interact with their environment. Life
depended on it, for metabolism within the cell was contingent on obtaining raw materials
from outside and on eliminating waste products from within. Both these needs required the
ability to sense the presence of substances outside or inside the cell.
The simplest early sense mechanism was mechanical. When pressure inside increased
past tolerable limits, large pores in the cell membrane opened, pouring out some of the
cell’s contents until pressure was lowered. This mechanical gating (the controlled passing of
substances through a membrane) exists in similar form across all three domains of bacteria,
archaea, and eukaryotes, testifying to an ancient evolutionary origin (Balleza, 2011).
At a very early evolutionary point, cell membranes acquired the ability to pass some
molecules but not others. The ability to distinguish between what to pass and what to
block from entering the cell was a crude form of external sensing. Early on, naturally
occurring amino acids and phosphates were candidates for transportation into the cell
(Trevors, 2003).
There is evidence that the cross-membrane transport of ions (electrically charged
atoms), occurred early in the tree of life. Potassium ions (symbolized K+) form when
potassium salts dissolve in water, and are actively transported across cell membranes in all
three biological domains (Hänelt, Tholema, Kröning, Vor der Brüggen, & Wunnicke,
2011). Maintaining appropriate K+ levels within the cell is critical to phosphate transport,
as well as to enzyme-mediated chemical reactions. Derst and Karschin (1998) constructed
a phylogenetic tree of genes involved in K+ transport and tentatively concluded that
some originated in prokaryotes, later descending to eukaryotes about two billion
years ago.
A proton pump – a membrane pump of hydrogen ions (H+) – was probably an additional
early development. Membrane proteins capable of pumping H+ out of a cell are believed to
DOI: 10.4324/9781003137863-8
96 Sensation and movement
have existed in the last common ancestor of all life (Deamer & Weber, 2010; Wei &
Pohorille, 2015). Using an external source of energy such as thermal or light energy from the
sun, H+ was pumped out of the cell. Then when the energy source was no longer available,
such as at night, H+ was let back into the cell, freeing energy to be used in chemical reactions
such as phosphate synthesis.
Other chemical transport mechanisms believed to have had prokaryote precursors are
those for the transport of sodium (Na+) and calcium (Ca+) ions, evolutionary descendants of
the K+ mechanism. By about 600 million years ago, most of today’s ion transport channels
had evolved (Derst & Karschin, 1998).
Even at the unicellular level, the ability to sense compounds outside the cell can have
behavioral consequences. Today’s bacteria are capable of surprisingly sophisticated chemical
signaling techniques to enhance mass action. Quorum sensing refers to the ability to detect
increasing numbers of other bacteria by sensing a building concentration of chemical ex-
cretions. It is used to coordinate the release of toxins when attacking a host, and to establish
biofilms, which are cooperative communities of bacteria, each contributing some component
to the matrix (Bassler & Losick, 2006). It is also used to respond to the deaths of community
members, for example by signaling survivors to move away from antibiotics or to expel them
(Bhattacharyya, Walker, & Harshey, 2020).
An early origin for quorum sensing is suggested by the fact that rhomboid protein, a
signaling chemical, is found across the life domains (Henke & Bassler, 2004). It is even
possible that chemical communication was essential for the emergence of unicellular life.
Communal living would have allowed massed numbers of individual cells, avoiding “ex-
tinction by dilution”, and chemical communication would have provided group protection.
These may be reasons early life is evidenced by the huge colonies of bacteria comprising
stromatolites (Combarnous & Nguyen, 2020; see Figure 1.3).
Touch
Mechanical gating and ion transport probably served as sufficient sense mechanisms for many
early single-cell organisms. However, metazoa, with complex body plans and in many cases
improved mobility, required more complicated sensory systems. These included the presence
of specialized touch receptors, and in most phyla a nervous system to carry touch information
beyond the limits of the cell.
Mechanoreceptors
Mechanoreceptors (specialized sensory receptor cells that react to mechanical forces) predated
nervous systems and possibly contributed to their evolution. The earliest mechanoreceptors
are thought to have been in the form of thread-like cellular outgrowths. These were either
small (cilia) or large (flagella), surrounded by microvilli (tiny projections of cell membrane) that
were used in feeding. The outgrowths had both sensory and motor function, so that their
stimulation caused movement. Their possession by choanoflagellates dates their origin to 1.0
to 1.5 billion years ago (Arendt, Benito-Gutierrez, Brunet, & Marlow, 2015; Fritzsch,
Pauley, & Beisel, 2006).
Hair-based mechanoreceptors may descend from ancestral choanoflagellates, but more
certainly from ancestral metazoans over 600 million years ago. These had a central hair
surrounded by microvilli, and were probably the origin of later touch receptors (Fritzsch
et al., 2006). The ancestors of early cnidarians probably also had hair-based mechanoreceptors
in the form of stinging cells, possibly the origin of neurons (Box 6.1).
The mechanical and chemical senses 97
Miljkovic-Licina, Gauchat, and Galliot (2004) proposed that neurons (nerve cells)
had their origin as mechanoreceptors, specifically in the immediate ancestors of
early cnidarians. Along with some ctenophores (comb jellies), cnidarians were
the first metazoans to have identifiable neural nets. The earlier-emerging sponges
did not.
But how could neurons have evolved from mechanoreceptors? Miljkovic-Licina
and colleagues pointed out that cnidarian stinging cells sense mechanical dis-
turbances. They respond by ejecting barbed tubes or threads that inject venom to
immobilize both attackers and prey ( Figure 6.1). Evolution might have caused
these cells to differentiate, with one line of descending cells evolving into neurons.
In turn these might have connected with the stinging cells, possibly bidirectionally
to allow signals to pass back and forth, improving coordination during attacks
( Liebeskind, Hillis, Zakon, & Hofman, 2016). Partially supporting this supposition,
stinging cells and neurons share the same regulatory genes ( Miljkovic-Licina et al.,
2004).
Figure 6.1 Representation of a cnidarian stinging cell firing sequence (left to right), with hair “trigger”
(cnidocil) and coiled thread ready for ejection.
Source: Nematocyst discharge.png, public domain, Spaully.
98 Sensation and movement
However, there is by no means a consensus that neurons evolved from stinging cells.
Some believe they instead evolved in conjunction with muscles, quickly building into
diffuse neural nets controlling movement. Certainly cnidarians and ctenophores both
have nets directly connected to muscle fibers. This arrangement could then have
extended to control the muscles involved in feeding and digestion ( Arendt, Tosches, &
Marlow, 2016).
Whatever view of neuronal origins is correct, continuity between cnidarians and the
bilateria (including chordates and vertebrates) is indicated by the use of the same genes
to specify cnidarian neural nets and bilaterian nervous systems ( Arendt et al., 2016).
Similarities between mechanical sensors found in cnidarians, chordates, and the
vertebrate inner ear likewise suggest a common origin. Thus, the nervous system in
the crude form of a neural net, and its support of mechanical sensing, originated in
shared ancestry a little over 605 million years ago ( Burgihel et al., 2003).
Figure 6.3 The lobes of the brain, and the location of the postcentral gyrus.
Source: Blausen 0101 Brain LateralView.png, CC BY-SA 3.0, BruceBlaus.
Balance
Like touch, the vestibular sense (balance) also involves hair cells. Some are located at the base
of each of three semicircular canals (ducts) in the inner ear, in structures called ampullae
(Figure 6.4). The canals are filled with fluid and are set nearly (but not quite) at right angles to
one another. Because of the slightly off-kilter angles, any rotational acceleration results
in fluid movement in at least two of the canals. That causes hair bending and thus sensing
in at least two planes, enabling the precise neural computation of acceleration information.
Other hair cells are located in fluid-filled cavities called the utricle and saccule, and respond to
vertical and horizontal acceleration (Lundberg, Xu, Thiessen, & Kramer, 2015).
The base of the hair cells synapses onto neurons, which coalesce into the vestibular nerve,
in turn connecting to nuclei in the brain stem (Cronin, Arshad, & Seemungal, 2017). This
pathway produces the vestibular reflex that allows us to keep our eyes fixated on an object
while our heads move. Another pathway terminates, like somatosensation, in SI of the
parietal lobe (D. Schneider, L. Schneider, Claussen, & Kolchev, 2001).
It was once believed that vestibular hair cells originated in the lateral line system of fish. This
system uses hair cells arranged in a line from gills to tail, detecting water currents, the pre-
sence of prey, and possibly the location of sounds. However, the lateral line system and the
inner ear differ in form, development, and function. Therefore, the current view is that both
evolved in parallel from loosely arrayed somatosensory cells, rather than one from the other
(Duncan & Fritzsch, 2012).
The earliest emerging taxa with an identifiable inner ear were the hagfish and lampreys,
placing its origin about 520 million years ago. However, those cartilaginous fish have only
one and two semicircular canals, respectively. The third canal emerged about 430 million
years ago with the evolution of bony fishes (Beisel, Wang-Lundberg, Maklad, &
Fritzsch, 2005).
The mechanical and chemical senses 101
Figure 6.4 Structures of the inner ear, including the semicircular ducts (canals), utricle, and saccule.
Source: Blausen 0329 EarAnatomy InternalEar.png, CC BY-SA 3.0, BruceBlaus.
A striking aspect of the vestibular apparatus is its extraordinarily restricted size range
across mammalian species. The size ratio of the smallest to largest is less than 1:10, in
contrast to a ratio of possible body lengths of nearly 1:1,000. Possibly its size is constrained
by physical properties such as the inertia of its fluids and the range of hair displacement
in the receptors. When these and other properties are mathematically modeled, a narrow
range of sizes is indeed possible, and the human apparatus is neatly positioned in midrange
(Squires, 2004).
Nevertheless, some size variation is present across species, although its meaning is unclear.
We might expect highly active species like gibbons to have large semicircular canals to
provide more precise acceleration information. However, there is no consistent relationship
between activity/agility and canal size (Kemp & Kirk, 2014; Malinzak, Kay, & Hullar, 2012).
Larger canals may instead benefit species with more acute vision, in order to better stabilize
vision during movement via the vestibular reflex (Kemp & Kirk, 2014).
In agile species, large canals may be less useful than canals oriented 90° to one another.
Across mammalian species, the more the canals are at right angles to each other, the greater
the estimated sensitivity to angular acceleration (Berlin, Kirk, & Rowe, 2013). Even within a
specific group of primates, i.e., strepsirrhines, actual measurements of the speed of head
rotation in a species is correlated to the perpendicularity of its semicircular canals (Malinzak
et al., 2012). The implication is that evolution has reoriented the canals to be more per-
pendicular to one another, in species emphasizing speed and agility. However, the limited
102 Sensation and movement
species examined so far do not permit a phylogenetic analysis across primates, let alone
animals more generally.
Hearing
Hearing, or the auditory sense, likewise depends on hair cells to detect mechanical disturbances,
in this case in the air or water medium surrounding the organism. Sound propagates pressure
waves through the medium, and their amplification and detection result in hearing.
However, even at the basic mechanical level, auditory processes are complex. A sound
entering the ear is transformed by the physical structure of the pinna (outer ear), enhancing
middle frequencies in the range of 2,000 to 5,000 Hz. It then impinges on and causes
vibration of the eardrum (tympanic membrane), which transmits the vibration through
three connected ossicles (small bones in the middle ear called the malleus, incus, and stapes) to
the oval window of the inner ear’s cochlea (Figure 6.5).
The sound causes the basilar membrane inside the cochlea to vibrate, and hair cells located
on the membrane detect the vibration (Varin & Petrov, 2009). Thus, before the first neural
response to sound, a series of complex mechanical events takes place. Adding to the com-
plexity, some people can modify the transmission of sound through voluntary control of
middle-ear muscles (Box 6.2).
Can you tense your middle ears to produce an apparent roaring or rumbling sound? If
so, you are among what is believed to be a minority of individuals who can voluntarily
contract the tensor tympani muscles, one in each ear ( Angeli, Lise, Tabajara, & Maffacioli,
2013). These muscles end in a ligament connected to the midsection of the malleus
( Figure 6.5). Tensing them therefore dampens the movement of the three-ossicle
assemblage that transmits sound from the eardrum to the inner ear.
Contraction of the tensor tympani muscles substantially lowers the perceived
volume of sound, especially sounds of low pitch. The likely advantage is that when
a loud sound is anticipated, the muscles can be tensed, reducing sound transmission
through the middle ear to the inner ear and thus providing protection against physical
damage. Another probable function of the muscles is involuntary, with moment-to-
moment contraction suppressing the self-produced sounds of chewing and speaking
( Angeli et al., 2013).
The tensor tympani muscles have a long evolutionary history. They existed in
mammal-like therapsids ( Panciroli, Schultz, & Luo, 2019) and are characteristic of
mammals generally ( Allin & Hopson, 1992), dating their origins to at least 250 million
years ago. However, early uses of the muscles were probably involuntary in nature,
with voluntary control emerging in some individuals at a much later (but unknown)
date.
If you are not able to voluntarily contract the tensor tympani muscles, you may still
gain a sense of what it is like. Simply yawn widely in a quiet environment. If you hear a
faint trembling sound, it is a low-loudness version of the much more intense
experience resulting from voluntary control.
As we have seen, hair-based mechanoreceptors are very ancient. As with the sense of
balance, auditory hair cells of the inner ear likely evolved from loosely arrayed receptors
of that type. While nonvertebrate chordates have hair cells, they are not located in an ear but
rather in the oral region. Thus, the co-option of hair cells for hearing must have postdated
the chordate emergence 535 million years ago. Probably it was long after, as will become
apparent.
As the primary receptor surface, the basilar membrane is of central interest in evolutionary
analyses. Its initial form was as basilar papillae (a papilla being a rounded projection), one in
each ear, each containing a few hair cells. Coelacanth fish have them, as presumably did their
ancestors about 395 million years ago as a vestibular system modification (Fritzsch, 1987).
However, the lungfish lineage, not the coelacanth lineage, is believed ancestral to tetrapods,
including humans – and lungfish lack basilar papillae. Why do tetrapods share them with
their more distant coelacanth relations, but not with their closer lungfish cousins?
The likely answer is that basilar papillae were inherited from the last common ancestor of
all three groups but were subsequently lost in lungfish. It has been suggested that the
common ancestor was a lobe-finned fish whose papillae were used to assess pressure dif-
ferences between its primitive lungs and its membrane-covered middle ears, allowing the
monitoring of depth (Fritzsch et al., 2013). The shallows-dwelling lungfish presumably had
little need for depth information and so eventually lost the papillae, but not before giving rise
to tetrapod descendants who incorporated them into a more elaborate hearing mechanism.
In tetrapods, a few hair cells became many hair cells. Ultimately the “carpet” of hair cells
104 Sensation and movement
became continuous over a lengthened basilar membrane, achieving the mammalian form that
humans have today.
Another evolutionary step coiled the basilar membrane inside a cochlea, packing a max-
imum of membrane surface into minimum space (Figure 6.4). Comparative analyses indicate
that full coiling evolved after monotremes emerged about 195 million years ago, but before
marsupial and placental mammals diverged about 170 million years ago (Manley, 2000; Vater,
Meng, & Fox, 2004). Nevertheless, the earliest known fossil showing coiling only dates back
about 80 million years (Vater et al., 2004).
Frequency selectivity
With these modifications came frequency selectivity, the ability to distinguish between
sounds of differing pitch. Turtles and lizards have little, and birds an intermediate amount,
but mammals including man have substantial selectivity (Manley, 2000). In mammals, se-
lectivity is mostly achieved through organization of the basilar membrane. Hair cells at its
base (nearer to the bones of the middle ear) detect high frequencies, while those near the
membrane’s apex (on the opposite end) detect low frequencies (Breedlove, Watson, &
Rosenzweig, 2010). Frequencies can then be distinguished based on the location at which
hair cells are stimulated.
Lizards lack basilar membrane selectivity but have evolved frequency-related modifications
of the hair cells themselves so that particular cells are more responsive to certain frequencies
than others. Birds also use hair cell modifications, as even mammals do to supplement the use
of location on the basilar membrane (Manley, 2000).
In humans, the modification of vestibular structures for auditory function has been ac-
companied by a substantial shift in the ratio of hair cells to connecting neurons. Vestibular
hair cells show an approximately 5:1 ratio to neurons, indicating some funneling of in-
formation, but auditory hair cells show a remarkable 1:8 ratio – in effect a reverse funneling
or elaboration of processing at the first neural juncture. The elaboration is particularly
pronounced in the case of high-frequency hair cells toward the base of the basilar membrane
(Fritzsch et al., 2002). Indeed, “high end” expansion has occurred over some 350 million
years. Turtles, which are reptiles, may be limited to hearing frequencies under 1000 Hz,
while birds are capable of detecting frequencies up to about 10,000 Hz. Humans, at least
when young, can hear up to about 20,000 Hz, and some other mammals can do so up to
100,000 Hz! (Manley, 2000).
Within primates there is evidence that high-frequency sound detection is related to the
amount of social calling. Species living in larger groups, and therefore employing a wider
variety of social calls, have greater sound sensitivity overall but especially greater high-
frequency sensitivity. This exploits the fact that in the natural environment, background
noise tends to be pitched low. Thus, having social calls pitched at high frequencies makes
them easier to distinguish from the background (Ramsier, Cunningham, Finneran, &
Dominy, 2012).
For many species, another advantage of a higher frequency range is improved sound lo-
calization (Masterton, Heffner, & Ravizza, 1969). Blockage of sound by the head is an
important location cue, and high frequencies (short wavelengths) experience much greater
blockage than low frequencies (long wavelengths). The greater blockage of high frequencies
thus allows a computation of location using differences between the ears in perceived fre-
quency (Breedlove et al., 2010). The same differential blockage is true of other obstacles as
well. That is why we are usually more annoyed by a neighbor’s bass, which tends to pass
through walls, than by their treble, which doesn’t.
The mechanical and chemical senses 105
However, if an animal’s head is large, there is a more powerful cue than differential
blockage, based on timing. Sound reaches the near ear earlier than the far ear, and the larger
the head, the larger the timing difference. Timing differences tend to be less affected than
frequency differences by other objects in the environment, posing an advantage for animals
large enough to use them. Thus, we should look to large mammals to have a reduced “high
end”, because they have less need for high frequency hearing. That is certainly true of
elephants, which top out at only 11,000 Hz. It is also true of humans, topping out at
20,000 Hz.
Other primates, which tend to be smaller, typically have higher “high ends” than hu-
mans, with nearly all being able to hear over 32,000 Hz. Within land mammals generally
(Heffner, 2004), and primates specifically (Ramsier et al., 2012), evolution appears to have
optimized frequency response to match body size. The range shifts toward higher fre-
quencies in small-bodied species, and toward lower frequencies in large-bodied species.
The between-ear distance and the highest frequency that can be heard show an imperfect
correlation in primates, but is nevertheless substantial (r = −0.63; i.e., with smaller heads
hearing higher frequencies). Even with humans excluded, the correlation remains,
although it is smaller (r = −0.40; Ramsier, Vinyard, & Dominy, 2017).
Although humans have reduced high-frequency hearing compared to our fellow primates,
we do have other unusual auditory abilities. Specifically, we have lower “low ends” than
most primates, being able to routinely detect frequencies close to 30 Hz, or even 20 Hz
under ideal conditions. At our most sensitive frequency we can also hear fainter sounds than
most (Coleman & Ross, 2004; Heffner, 2004).
Smell
As with mechanical sensing, chemical sensing began with simple cellular transport me-
chanisms. Yet today we enjoy a complex ability to smell and to taste. While the olfactory
(smell) and gustatory (taste) sensory systems are closely related in function, more is currently
known about the evolution of the former.
In humans, olfaction begins when a molecule of an odorant binds to the membrane
of a cell in the olfactory epithelium, a layer of neurons lining the upper portion of the nasal
passageway. When binding occurs, a complex chain of reactions is triggered that opens some of
the Na+ channels in the cell membrane. The same scenario plays out as with somatosensation,
with a spike potential propagating throughout the cell and into the axon on its far end.
Taken together, the axons from millions of receptor neurons make up the olfactory nerve,
which travels a very short distance to an olfactory bulb of the brain (Figure 6.6). There is one
bulb on the left and another on the right of the brain’s midline, with an olfactory nerve
connecting to each in ipsilateral (same-sided) fashion. This is the only sensory system which
does not predominantly cross over with each side of the body linked to the opposite side
of the brain.
From the olfactory bulbs, the olfactory tract courses backward in the brain to connect with
cortex on the medial (inner) surface of the temporal lobes. Pathways then run through the
midbrain to connect with prefontal cortex, the forward portion of the frontal lobes (Figure 6.3).
Together these comprise the brain’s olfactory sensory system.
Across at least the first three levels of this system, size is correlated. Examining six species of
small mammals, Srinivasan and Stevens (2019) reported a strong relationship between the size
of the olfactory epithelium and the size of the olfactory bulb. Furthermore, the size of the
olfactory bulb was highly correlated to the size of the medial cortical area it projected to.
Assuming that a larger epithelium requires greater processing, the olfactory system therefore
follows the same “principle of proper mass” that applies to the sense of touch.
During prenatal development, the olfactory epithelium develops from a placode (a spe-
cialized cell cluster), visible as a thickening of the ectoderm (the cells giving rise to external
features like the skin, nails, and hair, and counterintuitively, the brain and nervous system).
Placodes exist for other senses as well, developing into structures of the eye and ear among
others (Steventon, Mayor, & Streit, 2016).
Comparing species suggests that the olfactory placode originated shortly after the
emergence of chordates, but before the emergence of vertebrates. Thus, while it exists
across the full spectrum of vertebrates, its existence in chordates depends on the species.
Amphioxus, considered representative of early ancestral chordates, either shows no evi-
dence at all for placodes (Schlosser, 2017) or the possibility of tiny ones consisting of
only one or two cells (Lacalli, 2004). Furthermore, evidence for either smell or taste in
The mechanical and chemical senses 107
Figure 6.6 The olfactory bulbs (upper part of I), and olfactory tract (lower part of I) on the underneath side
of the human brain. Also shown is the optic nerve (II) among other numbered cranial nerves.
Source: Blausen 0284 CranialNerves.png, CC BY-SA 3.0, BruceBlaus.
Olfactory genes
Research on the genes underlying olfaction has yielded new insight into the evolutionary
rise, and in our case fall, of this chemical sense. A few olfactory genes trace back to the early
ancestors of chordates (Niimura, 2012), just before evolution of the olfactory placode.
The evolution of tetrapods was accompanied by a tremendous expansion of olfactory
genes, so that amphibians and terrestrial vertebrates now have several hundreds to thou-
sands of these. The expansion greatly increased the receptors sensitive to airborne odors. In
contrast, coelacanth fish lack airborne odor receptors entirely, and in some whales and seals
whose ancestors shifted from land to sea, their numbers are dramatically reduced. Thus,
there is good reason to believe that it was the transition to land some 370 million years ago,
and the availability of airborne odors, that provided the impetus for olfactory expansion
(Bear, Lassance, Hoekstra, & Datta, 2016).
108 Sensation and movement
Today’s terrestrial mammals have an average of about 1,500 olfactory receptor genes. They
are believed to be directly related to the development of specialized odor detectors in the
olfactory epithelium, so that the more genes there are, the larger the range of detectable
odors. Actual behavioral data demonstrating such a relationship are quite limited, however
(Kay, Campbell, Rossie, Colbert, & Rowe, 2004).
A mammalian expansion in olfactory sensitivity is also observable in the form of larger
olfactory bulbs. Larger bulbs service larger numbers of receptors (Heritage, 2014). Because
the bulbs sometimes make impressions on the inside of fossil skulls, their volume can be
measured in extinct species. It is found that a three-stage increase occurred during
the evolution of mammals, all within a 30-million-year span beginning about 225 million
years ago (Rowe, Macrini, & Luo, 2011). Thus, there has been a strong, general mam-
malian trend toward evolving larger olfactory bulbs.
It is all the more striking, then, that the more recent primate trend has been the exact
opposite, toward olfactory bulb reduction. Controlling for overall brain size, the olfactory
bulbs of strepsirrhines are sized about the same as those of close relatives like tree shrews.
But from there on down the line, it was all reduction: The olfactory bulbs got smaller and
smaller, relative to brain size, from haplorhines (splitting off 68 million years ago) to an-
thropoids (66 million years ago) to catarrhines (44 million years ago) to apes (29 million
years ago; Heritage, 2014).
Taste
Gustation, or the sense of taste, is a second chemical sense. The receptors are in the form of
cells clustered inside taste buds, in turn located in papillae on the tongue’s surface. The tastes
of sweet, sour, and umami (meaty or savory) are detected much as odors are detected, with
complex molecular binding on the membranes of the cells. Salty and sour tastes, however,
involve simpler mechanisms of ion transport through receptor membranes, with the sodium
(Na+) ions of salt directly transported and with the hydrogen (H+) ions of sour acids
blocking the transport of potassium (K+) ions.
A receptor cell responds to taste by releasing neurotransmitter that crosses a synapse and
activates a neuron in one of three cranial nerves (sensory and motor nerves completely located
in the head), specifically those designated VII, IX, and X (Figure 6.6). These nerves run to
the brainstem, forming a tract that, after a couple of synapses, reaches the somatosensory
cortex in the postcentral gyrus of the parietal lobe (Figure 6.3).
Comparative analyses indicate that all vertebrates including lampreys, but not hagfish, have
taste buds, and that even hagfish have chemically sensitive cells in the mouth area that are
sensitive to tastes (Kirino, Parnes, Hansen, Kiyohara, & Finger, 2013). Thus, the gustatory
system must date from about 520 million years ago with the emergence of ancestral hagfish.
Much of the recent research on gustation’s evolution has focused on bitter tastes, in part
because bitterness characterizes many toxins, and in part because of the discovery of a family
of bitter taste receptor genes designated T2R. The common ancestor of all vertebrates is
reconstructed as having less than 10 T2R genes. Today the number varies widely over species
(Li & Zhang, 2013). Mammals have many more, between 19 and 41 in the species that have
been studied (humans, mice, dogs, cattle, and opossums), while zebrafish, pufferfish, and
chickens have only three or four (Go, 2006).
110 Sensation and movement
As in the case of olfaction, there has also been notable transformation to pseudogenes
in some lineages. Although under 10% have been transformed in nonmammalian species,
20% to 40% have been in mammals. Humans occupy the midpoint of the mammalian
range (Go, 2006).
An average level of gustatory pseudogene formation in humans raises questions about
toxin-based explanations, both in gustation and olfaction. If either cooking or raw meat-
eating reduced the toxicity of food, for instance, and that led to olfactory pseudogene for-
mation, there should have been at least as much impetus to convert gustatory genes to
pseudogenes. Thus, the failure to find a higher level of gustatory pseudogenes in humans than
in other mammals casts doubt on the toxin explanation. Unfortunately, it is not yet clear
what alternative can take its place.
Complicating the issue is that when assessed widely across mammalian species, there is a
positive correlation between the amount of vegetable matter in the diet and the number of
T2R bitter taste genes (Li & Zhang, 2013). This is exactly what would be expected of a
toxin-based explanation of pseudogene formation, because carnivores wouldn’t need sensi-
tivity to plant toxins and thus should have fewer functional T2R genes. Herbivores should
have a greatly increased need and thus more functional genes. However, the size of the
correlation is quite modest, r = +0.43, indicating that other unknown factors are in play.
Perhaps if they were known, they would account for the failure to find increased gustatory
pseudogene formation in humans relative to other mammals.
Human ancestors, of course, emerged from a primate background about 7.5 million
years ago. We might expect, therefore, that we would show taste sensitivities similar to
those of our close cousins the chimpanzees. Sweet tastes are a good vehicle for examining
this, because while chimpanzees are omnivorous, their diet is about 55% to 75% fruit, and
they appear to judge fruit from the first bite on the basis of sweetness. When given a
choice between a sugar solution and plain water, chimpanzees are most sensitive in
detecting sucrose, the predominant sugar in many fruits. Fructose, another fruit sugar,
comes in a close second, distantly followed by glucose, maltose, and lactose. Humans
show similar sensitivity for sucrose, but maltose is second (Norlén, Sjöström, Hjelm,
Hård, & Laska, 2019). That is probably meaningful in that maltose is extracted from the
chewing of starches, such as those contained in tubers, a common component of the
human but not chimpanzee diet. Unfortunately it is unclear whether sugar sensitivities are
selected in the evolutionary sense, or merely reflect what is typically available. Perhaps,
for example, both chimpanzees and humans would show increased sensitivity to glucose if
it was a larger part of their diets.
One of our most important senses does not depend on either mechanical or chemical
stimulation. Although vision involves a chemical reaction, the chemistry involved is internal
to the cell, and stimulation involves light-based rather than mechanical energy transfer. In
this chapter we will first consider how vision began and led to the evolution of eyes. We will
examine how day vision came to sense colors, and how it gave rise to highly sensitive night
vision. We will also see how visual acuity increased in the human ancestral line, producing
improvement in vision that went hand in hand with changes in the brain.
Vision began with single-cell organisms that were neither animal nor plant. Among single-
cell organisms, photomotility (movement in response to light) is widespread and involves
light-absorbing pigments from several chemical classes. These photopigments are embedded in
the cell membrane, in cytoplasm, or even at the base of a flagellum, as in the single-celled
Euglena. In Euglena, rotational movement of the cell causes the light-sensitive pigment to be
shaded by a nearby red eyespot, allowing the direction of light to be determined. This alters
the flagellum’s activity, keeping the organism in an area where light levels are suitable for
photosynthesis (Ogawa et al., 2016).
Other indications of the multiple origins of eyes include differences in (a) the organization
of photopigments in the receptors, (b) the receptors’ ion transport channels, and (c) the
development of the eye in embryos. On the other hand, arguing for a common origin is that
all eyes use variations of vitamin A-based photopigments even though other light-sensitive
pigments exist (Fernald, 2000).
Many sponges, which have only PaxB, sense light with rings of pigment-containing receptor
cells (Rivera et al., 2012). Some are known to withdraw their oscula (mouth-like openings)
when exposed to light, and their larvae swim directionally in response to light. They are most
sensitive to violet light with a wavelength of 440 nanometers (nm), a response that is consistent
with a class of photopigments called flavins or cryptochromes (called “crypto” because for many
years their identity was unknown; Leys, Cronin, Degnan, & Marshall, 2002).
Perhaps as our legacy from such ancient ancestors, flavin-based photopigment today un-
derlies the human circadian rhythm. This rhythm governs our sensitivity to the 24-hour day,
raising respiration, temperature, and activity levels during the day and lowering them at
night. Light helps set this rhythm and, at least in mammals, does so through visual sensing by
some of the retina’s ganglion cells. Output from the ganglion cells then projects to the
suprachiasmatic nucleus, which controls the circadian rhythm (Do & Yau, 2010).
However, in cnidarians, Pax genes are involved in the production of a different family of
photopigments called opsins. Such genes have been found in cnidarians, but are absent in
sponges (Plachetzki, Degnan, & Oakley, 2007). Consistent with this, peak visual sensitivity in
the cnidarian genus Hydra falls within the range of wavelengths to which opsins respond.
Specifically, hydra are most sensitive to blue-green light, at wavelengths between 480 and
530 nm (Leys et al., 2002).
Putting these observations together, it seems that sponges’ photosensitivity is based on
flavin-based photoreceptors, using a receptor chemistry that is repeated in our regulation of
the circadian rhythm. Receptors based on opsins, which led evolutionarily to complex eyes,
emerged later in cnidarians.
The bilateria appeared soon after cnidarians (see Figure 2.3), about 580 million years ago.
These are animals showing bilateral symmetry, and they include humans. Arendt and
Wittbrodt (2001) concluded that the immediate ancestors of bilateria probably had paired,
pigmented cup-eyes as larvae, each possibly as simple as a single photoreceptor containing a
light-sensitive pigment, with a second pigment close by. The second pigment functioned like
the eyespot in Euglena, providing shading and thus an indication of the directionality of light.
The Pax6 gene and opsin figured into the chemistry of these receptors (Arendt & Wittbrodt,
2001; Nilsson, 2013).
Vision 119
Chordate and early vertebrate eyes
Early chordates emerged after cnidarians and bilaterians about 535 million years ago, and may
have been similar to today’s amphioxus. Amphioxus larvae have a single Pax-expressed
frontal eye involving a multicellular pigment cup backed by rows of neurons. It is suited to
determining the direction of light, perhaps sufficient to escape predators (Lacalli, 2004, 2018).
The salp, a primitive jelly-like chordate, likewise has a single eye during the solitary phase of
its life (Braun & Stach, 2017). Accordingly, it seems possible that even though bilaterian
ancestors had paired eyes, it was a single eye that passed from chordates to the ancestors of
vertebrates (Lacalli, 2018).
The earliest fossil evidence of eyes among possible vertebrate ancestors is in Haikouella, a
chordate resembling amphioxus. A Cambrian species dating to about 530 million years ago,
several of its fossils unambiguously show paired eyes (Mallatt & Chen, 2003). The basic
structure of the human eye may thus have been set over a half billion years ago during the
chordate-vertebrate transition.
Hagfish, which emerged shortly after, also imply the reappearance of paired eyes.
Living hagfish have two eyes hidden under skin. They are lensless even though lens
placodes are present in embryos, suggesting that the eyes degenerated during evolution.
Consistent with this supposition, hagfish fossils, dating to about 300 million years ago,
show a pair of dark eye spots including small white dots that may represent lenses
(Bardack, 1991; Gabbott et al., 2016). If so, the lenses were subsequently lost. It is
possible that hagfish eyes are now used not for vision, but for circadian rhythm regulation
(Sun et al., 2014).
It is important to note that the ancestral hagfish eye reversed images left-to-right (and up-
to-down). That is because of the geometry involved in its pit structure, as illustrated in
Figure 7.2 (Lamb, Collin, & Pugh, 2007). Whether or not it had a lens did not impact this,
and images would have reversed in either case. However, if present, a lens would have
strongly improved image focusing.
Lampreys, the next group to diverge, definitely have paired, reversing eyes, with lenses
that in the adult produce focused images (Kröger, Gustafsson, & Tuminaite, 2014). This
argues that lenses originated at least 520 million years ago, although they evolved con-
vergently in multiple taxa. Some of the transparent proteins forming the lens are specific to
vertebrates and presumably derive from a common ancestor (Bloemendal et al., 2004).
Color vision
Although the abilities to sense light and to form images from it are fundamental to vision, the
ability to see color is also important to the recognition and evaluation of objects. Some
objects, such as fruit, carry color cues signaling suitability for consumption. Others carry
warnings, as in distinctively colored poisonous snakes, or advertising, as in the colorful
plumage of male birds seeking mates.
In humans, and in all apes and Old World monkeys, normal daytime color vision is ac-
complished with a system of three retinal receptor types called cones. Each type of cone
responds to a broad range of wavelengths and thus colors, but each shows a peak response at a
particular wavelength. Our short-wavelength cones show a peak at 420 nm, in the violet
portion of the spectrum; medium-wavelength cones at 530 nm, a green color; and long-
wavelength cones at 560 nm, a yellow-green. However, these three cone types are often
referred to as “blue”, “green”, and “red” cones, respectively, because each is sensitive to
longer wavelengths as well that include those primary colors. An opsin photopigment is
involved in each.
120 Sensation and movement
Figure 7.2 A pit eye. Light from the left projects to the right side of the retina, and vice versa.
Source: Adaptation of Pit eye.svg, CC BY-SA 3.0, Gagea.
It may seem curious to you that we see only a narrow range of wavelengths in an elec-
tromagnetic spectrum running from gamma rays (wavelengths on the order of a thousandth
of a nm), up to the emissions of AC circuits (on the order of 10 to the 15th power nm).
Why is our vision limited to the tiny range of about 400 to 700 nm?
A large part of the answer is that our sensitivity reflects the predominant wavelengths
available on earth’s surface in daytime (Figure 7.3). Above and below visible wavelengths,
there is relatively less to detect. That can only mean that evolution tuned the response of our
cones to the radiation characteristics of our environment.
However, the greater availability of some wavelengths over others only tells part of the
story. While our maximum daytime sensitivity is at 555 nm, fairly close to the availability
peak (Delgado-Bonal & Martín-Torres, 2016), this may not be the case for animals
generally. Bees, for example, show maximum sensitivity at 344 nm, in the ultraviolet range
of light (de Ibarra, Vorobyev, & Menzel, 2014). At first consideration that seems odd in
that most flowers absorb ultraviolet light (de Ibarra, Vorobyev, Brandt, & Giurfa, 2000).
However, some do reflect it, and counterintuitively, ultraviolet reflection specifically
makes red flowers more attractive to bees, most of which lack red color receptors
Vision 121
Figure 7.3 Solar radiation above the atmosphere (yellow) and at sea level (red).
Source: Solar spectrum en.svg, CC BY-SA 3.0, Nick84.
(Chen, Liu, Sun, & Niu, 2020). Therefore, it may be the need to detect red flowers that
has led to peak sensitivity in the ultraviolet range.
The daytime system is not very sensitive to low levels of light, and so there are additional
receptors called rods which we rely on at night to see dim objects. These receptors are
exquisitely sensitive: A single photon or quantum of light is sufficient to trigger a rod re-
sponse, and 5 to 7 responding rods are sufficient to result in the perception of light (Field &
Sampath, 2017). However, because only one receptor type is involved in dim nighttime
vision, there is virtually no ability to perceive colors unless an object is bright enough to be
sensed by the cones.
The rods and cones are located in the retina, shown in Figure 7.4. The receptors in the
retina operate in backward fashion compared with other senses in two different ways, the first
involving their position and the second involving their effect on neurotransmitters.
First, rods and cones are literally backward in position: The light-capturing ends of the
receptors are oriented away from the light, so that light has to pass through neural ma-
chinery to be captured by photopigment. In fact, as Figure 7.4 illustrates, light must pass
through the ganglion cells, the connecting bipolar cells, and the rod and cone nuclei and cell
bodies before it reaches the light-sensitive disks of the receptors. Fortunately, neurons are
nearly transparent, limiting the absorption and scattering of light. Also, the problem is
minimized in the fovea (the central area of acute vision) because the connecting neurons
bend away from it, taking many of their cell bodies out of the direct light path
(Figure 7.5).
Why the system is arranged this way is an interesting evolutionary question in its own
right. One possibility is that the tight packing of the receptors so severely limits space that the
connecting neurons and major blood vessels cannot both be on the side away from the light.
If so, they must be separated. In that case, it is better to have the mostly transparent con-
necting neurons in the light path and the dark, occluding blood vessels away from it, rather
than the other way around. Even so, except in the fovea, some capillaries are located in front
of the receptors. Normally, we adapt to their presence and are unaware of them. But under
artificial circumstances such as the beaming of a narrow light shaft at the corner of the closed
eye (a cell phone light works well with some fiddling), we may suddenly see the shadow cast
by the network of capillaries.
122 Sensation and movement
Figure 7.4 The retina. Light from the bottom must pass through several layers of cells before reaching
the rods and cones.
Source: Adaptation of 1414 Rods and Cones.jpg, CC BY-SA 3.0, OpenStax College.
The second backward aspect of the visual system is that light stimulation slows the flow of
neurotransmitter from the receptors instead of initiating it. When light strikes a receptor,
Na+ channels close, and the receptor hyperpolarizes, becoming negatively charged. This
slows the release of neurotransmitter in a graded manner related to the intensity of the light:
The brighter the light, the less transmitter that is released (Breedlove et al., 2010). This use of
receptor hyperpolarization appears to date back to ancestral urochordates, now exemplified
by sea squirts, some 535 million years ago (Lamb et al., 2007).
Trichromacy
Our possession of three cone types – “blue”, “green”, and “red” – is the origin of the term
trichromacy (the condition of possessing three types of cones). Oddly, trichromacy is not an
advanced evolutionary feature. Genetic analyses indicate that the ancestral state in vertebrates
was tetrachromacy, the possession of four cone types. The “extra” type was tuned to very short
Vision 123
Figure 7.5 Cross-section of the retina, showing the bending of the nerve fiber layer away from the fovea.
Source: Adaptation of Three Internal chambers of the Eye.png, CC BY-SA 3.0, Holly Fischer, and Retina-OCT800.
png, CC BY 2.0, origin medOCT-group.
wavelengths, allowing sensitivity to ultraviolet light. Consistent with this, some lampreys
have four cone types. Many reptiles, birds, and bony fish do as well, and some species have
cone subtypes, resulting in more than four spectral peaks (Bowmaker & Hunt, 2006).
What happened to reduce the range of color vision in apes and Old World monkeys? The
story is even more dramatic than it appears, because our ancestors passed through an even
tighter bottleneck of only two cone types (dichromacy), meaning that we lost not just one but
two of the original four. This is believed to have occurred in the earliest emerging mammals,
by 215 million years ago (Borges et al., 2018). Today, most mammals continue to have only
two cone types.
These losses were connected to the nocturnal nature of early mammals. Nocturnal animals
generally have fewer cone types than diurnal ones, as well as a smaller cone population
relative to rods. Early mammals lost first one and then a second cone type as their lives
became increasingly dependent on rod-based night vision, following a well-known “use it or
lose it” evolutionary principle. Biological structures require metabolic expenditures to de-
velop and maintain, and so if they fail to convey a compensating selective advantage in
behavior they are selected against. In the case of cones, losing two types reduced metabolic
costs while maintaining visual sensitivity to a range of wavelengths beyond those detected
by rods. The loss was accomplished when random mutations converted cone genes to
124 Sensation and movement
pseudogenes, rendering them inactive (Arrese, Hart, Thomas, Beazley, & Shand, 2002;
Bowmaker & Hunt, 2006).
Although the ancestral primate state is somewhat uncertain, it appears more likely than not
that early primates followed the general mammalian pattern and were therefore dichromatic
(Jacobs et al., 2019). That is true of most living lemurs, which, as strepsirrhines, represent the
first branching point of primate phylogeny (see Figure 4.2).
As explained in Chapter 6, Gilad et al. (2004) identified a link between the re-evolution of
trichromacy and olfactory pseudogene formation, with Old World monkeys and the howler
monkey developing three-cone color vision along with a large percentage of olfactory
pseudogenes. With these monkeys’ diurnal lifestyle came the opportunity to judge food
sources at a distance through reddish color cues, whether in fruit or leaves (Melin, Chiou,
et al., 2017). Re-evolving trichromacy, by allowing the detection of ripe fruit through red
coloration, then rendered some olfactory receptors unnecessary and led to their loss.
Interestingly, Madagascar, home to many of the world’s two-cone strepsirrhine species, has
fruit that more often ranges along yellow-blue than along red-green distinctions (Nevo et al.,
2018). That raises a chicken-and-egg question: Did red fruit come first, pressuring toward the
evolution of trichromacy, or did trichromacy come first, pressuring toward the evolution of
red fruit that monkeys would eat, depositing its seeds with ready-made fertilizer? As with
many chicken-and-egg questions, the answer may be “both”, with each ratcheting up
pressure for the other.
Another result of trichromacy’s advantage in detecting reddish food may have been a
preference for red foods. Presented with a choice of artificially dyed sweetbreads, macaques
(which are trichromatic Old World monkeys) choose red ones far more often than they do
green ones. This preference is specific to food itself and does not extend to coverings put
over food (Skalníková, Frynta, Abramjan, Rokyta, & Nekovárova, 2020).
Trichromacy probably became characteristic of a significant number of primate species
only after the New World – Old World monkey split 44 million years ago. The re-creation
of trichromacy in some primates, from an ancestral dichromatic state, beautifully illustrates
the role of chance in evolution, because different primate groups accomplished it in varying
ways with a varying quality of outcome. Old World monkeys and apes accomplished it by
duplicating the gene for the long-wave receptor, and then modifying the duplicate to be-
come sensitive to middle wavelengths (Bowmaker & Hunt, 2006; Davies et al., 2012). The
duplication created different gene sites, so all individuals normally inherit genes for all three
cone types. In Old World monkeys and apes, this solution must have occurred sometime
between 20 and 44 million years ago.
New World monkeys, on the other hand, generally accomplished trichromacy by evolving
different alleles of the same gene and shifting the sensitivity of one toward middle wave-
lengths. Furthermore, the gene involved is located on the X chromosome. This means that
females often show trichromacy, if they inherit differing alleles on their two X chromosomes.
In that case, one X chromosome is randomly inactivated in every cell, resulting in a mosaic of
photoreceptors, with half being the middle-wavelength type and half the long-wavelength
type. That mosaic, together with the third cone type, creates three-color vision. But a price is
paid: Females who inherit the same allele on both X chromosomes are dichromatic. So are all
males, because they have only one X chromosome and therefore only one of the alleles.
Thus, most New World monkeys are only trichromatic as a species, not as all individuals
within the species (Bowmaker & Hunt, 2006; Davies et al., 2012).
The howler monkey, as noted earlier, is exceptional among New World monkeys. After
the evolution of the alternate alleles, it underwent an actual gene duplication, allowing the
two cone variations to become fixed in the population (Surridge, Osorio, & Mundy, 2003).
Molecular-clock estimates place the divergence of howler monkeys from other New World
Vision 125
monkeys at about 7 million years ago, so the duplication was a fairly recent event (Cortés-
Ortiz et al., 2003; Perelman, et al., 2011).
Night vision
There is strong evidence that the rods used in night vision evolved after cones. Hoke and
Fernald (1997) cited analyses of DNA sequences from humans, chickens, and fish indicating
that rhodopsin, the specific opsin found in rods, emerged prior to divergences among these
groups, but after the appearance of color-sensitive opsins. Furthermore, these researchers
pointed out that rod output converges on cone pathways for transmission from the retina, not
the other way around, and that during gestation, rods develop later than cones. All of these
considerations suggest that rods evolved after cones (Kim et al., 2016).
This conclusion seems surprising because as the superficially less specialized system, in-
volving only one type of receptor, the rod system appears more primitive than the cone
system. Yet in truth, the rod system is highly specialized, having evolved to allow an exquisite
sensitivity to low levels of light. As we have seen, a single photon is sufficient to cause a
response in a rod containing rhodopsin.
The cone-then-rod order makes sense from a broader evolutionary perspective as well.
The largest selective pressure in the history of vision was no doubt toward evolving any
sensitivity to light. Because light is mostly present in the daytime, selective pressure was
maximal for daytime sensitivity, and so it emerged first. Later, more subtle selective pressures
fine-tuned it so that many creatures became sensitive to the low nighttime levels of light.
More recent work on gene sequences agrees with the conclusion that cones came first, and it
dates the cone-rod divergence to a vertebrate common ancestor about 520 million years ago.
Furthermore, it appears that the first of the cone types to evolve was the long-wave (“red”)
type, followed by short-wavelength (“blue”) cones, then medium-wavelength (“green”)
cones, and finally rods (Pisani, Mohun, Harris, McInerny, & Wilkinson, 2006; Wilson, 2007).
The opsins underlying multiple cone types may have emerged in urochordates, perhaps
535 million years ago before the two-round genome duplication described in Chapter 3.
Rhodopsin then emerged at the time of the duplication. However, it is possible that there
was already a dim-light photopigment, perhaps pinopsin, that rhodopsin replaced (Lamb &
Hunt, 2018).
Acuity
The funneling of rods and cones has important consequences for acuity (the ability to resolve
fine visual details). The human fovea’s 1:2 or 1:3 ratio of cones to ganglion cells preserves
high acuity, while the 1,000:1 ratio for human rods does not. Day vision is therefore more
acute than night vision. Similarly, whether a primate species is diurnal or nocturnal has much
to do with their visual acuity. Among New World monkeys, diurnal species have been found
to have two to five times better resolution than nocturnal species (Ross, 2000).
Correlations (r) of optic foramen area to brain volume were +.66 in living primates and +.67
in fossil primates, indicating that about 45% of the variation in the brain size of living and
extinct primates is explained by variation in optic nerve size.
Although correlation is not causation, the robustness of these relationships is suggestive. As
Kirk noted, they are consistent with Jerison’s “principle of proper mass”. This principle,
introduced in Chapter 6, holds that there is a relationship between the neural mass devoted to
a function and the amount of processing involved. Those primates relying more on cone
receptors, with increased processing needs, have expanded cortical areas devoted to vision
and thus larger brains.
The most interesting results of the study emerged from the fossil data. Two groups of fossil
primates were included, adapiformes and haplorhines. Of the two, the adapiformes were
more ancient in terms of divergences from the human ancestral line (see Figure 4.2). They
were found to have small optic nerves and small brains—in fact, outside the range of living
primates. In contrast, the haplorhines had relatively large optic nerves and large brains, well
within that range (Kirk, 2006).
These results are telling, for haplorhines with their large brains emerged about 68 million
years ago, while anthropoids with their perfected foveas emerged about 66 million years ago
(Table 4.2). In other words, the evolutionary threshold for larger optic nerves – and bigger
brains – appears to be located at about same place in primate phylogeny as the threshold for a
well-developed fovea. As such the results support a link between acute vision and brain
expansion in primate evolution.
A link between eye and brain continues to the present day. Pearce and Dunbar (2012)
reasoned that in modern populations, eye size (and thus brain size) should be affected by
geographic latitude. At higher latitudes, low-light days are more frequent than at lower
latitudes. Thus, larger eyes should be selected for, because larger eyes have larger corneas that
collect more light. In turn, larger eyes have more ganglion cells, requiring a larger brain to
Vision 129
support visual processing. Pearce and Dunbar therefore examined modern human skulls
curated in a British museum. They found, as predicted, that both eye size and brain
size increased with increases in the geographic latitude of the population that a skull was
drawn from.
Of course acuity-related brain expansion, starting in the Paleocene, was only the first of
several brain expansions in the primate line leading to modern humans. Later ones were to
have other causes.
Conclusion
Vision originated in single-cell organisms in the form of photopigments allowing the or-
ganisms to orient to light. Possibly it was introduced into multicellular animals through
symbiosis, with an initial cooperative relationship with a light-sensitive unicellular organism
eventually becoming genetic.
The first true eyes feeding into neurons were probably in cnidarians, specifically jellyfish.
Today’s jellyfish commonly use pigment cells as a navigation and migration aid, and box
jellyfish use complex lensed eyes to avoid obstacles such as mangrove roots. Along with the
earlier-emerging sponges, cnidarians have a PaxB gene known to play a role in eye devel-
opment. It was probably ancestral to a number of other Pax genes involved in expression of
the eyes, nervous system, and other organs. Thus, the basic control mechanism for eye
development was likely present by 650 million years ago, although the later appearance of
photoreceptor cells and true eyes involved independent evolution in multiple taxa.
Opsin photopigments may originally have been regulated by PaxB. Flavins (crypto-
chromes) are another class of photopigments involved in setting the circadian rhythm via
retinal ganglion cells and the suprachiasmatic nucleus. Ancestral eyes were probably simple
light sensors in early cnidarians, cup eyes in bilaterians and chordates, and paired lensed eyes
in 520 million-year-old vertebrates.
The human visual system is “backward” in two respects. First, the light-capturing end of
the receptors is located away from the light, behind several layers of cells, a design that may
actually minimize the blocking of light, since the cells are nearly transparent while blood
vessels behind the retina are not. Second, when light is detected, receptors hyperpolarize
instead of depolarize. That slows the flow of neurotransmitter, a feature tracing back to
ancestral chordates.
Old World monkeys and apes, including humans, have three retinal cone receptors
with differing wavelength sensitivities, which are used in daylight conditions. The cones
are tuned to the predominant wavelengths available at the earth’s surface. The ancestral
state in vertebrates was four cone types, only two of which were retained in our noc-
turnal mammalian ancestors. A third type, sensitive to middle wavelengths, was regained
independently in New World and Old World monkeys, though in different manners.
This independent development led to individual-level trichromacy in the Old World
branch but in only species-level trichromacy in most New World monkeys. An exception
is the New World howler monkey, which recently evolved species-level trichromacy.
The selection pressure toward trichromacy likely came from red-green distinctions in
food sources.
The cones of the retina are the eye’s original receptors, and rods later evolved from cones.
Rods are sensitive to low levels of light, with a single rod capable of responding to a single
photon, and with sensitivity of the system as a whole increased by substantial funneling of
information from the rods to the cortex. Nocturnal species show more funneling than diurnal
ones, and often have other low-light adaptations, such as a large corneal light-gathering area,
a reflective tapetum lucidum, and a dense packing of rods in the retina.
130 Sensation and movement
The difference between the rods and cones in the amount of funneling affects acuity; the
rod system in the periphery of vision has low acuity while the cone system at the macula
has very high acuity. The area of the visual field with the highest acuity has decreased
throughout primate evolution and is only 1.5° in extent in humans. There is a strong
correlation between optic nerve size and brain size in primates, even after correcting for
body size, suggesting that part of the primate brain expansion may be accounted for by
specialization in vision.
8 The origins of motion
As we have seen, the earliest form of sensation allowed substances to pass through cell
membranes. However, early organisms had only a limited ability to exploit such resources,
because they depended on whatever was brought to them by water. Acquiring movement
was a quantum leap in evolution, freeing single-cell life from relative immobility and at least
to some extent from the vagaries of currents. Motion meant that by trial and error, organisms
could shift from areas of scarcity to areas of plenty.
Movement would prove to be a key building block in cognitive evolution. The ability to
shift between locations imposed navigational requirements calling for highly developed
spatial processing abilities. Favored locations needed to be remembered so that movement
could be directed back to them, imposing a need for improved memory. Pattern recognition
processes were required to recognize known locations, and to direct movement toward new
ones affording familiar types of food as well as water and shelter. In humans, movement
also led to bipedalism and a resulting freeing of the hands, allowing the evolution of more
dexterous reaching and grasping – as well as what those imply for tool use and manufacture.
In this chapter, we lay the groundwork for those developments. First, we will look briefly at
the movement of single-cell organisms, before examining early metazoan motion. We then
consider movement innovations introduced in the human line of descent by chordates, tet-
rapods, amniotes, mammals, and primates. The neural mechanisms of movement are con-
sidered, as is the origin of contralateral organization in both the motor and sensory systems.
Single-cell organisms
Locomotion (movement from place to place) originated in early flagellated bacteria. Rotation
of the flagella propelled a bacterium on a straight course. In a beneficial microenvironment,
repeated momentary reversals kept the bacterium in place. Entering a harmful micro-
environment, however, caused abrupt reversal of the rotation, introducing a tumbling
motion and a random change of direction (Machemer, 2001).
Some archaea are also mobile, using structures similar in shape to bacterial flagella.
Although their underlying proteins are similar, there are enough structural differences that
thread-based motility in bacteria and archaea is best thought of as a matter of convergent
evolution. Given a water environment, and the use of proteins during movement, physical
factors like the viscosity of water and the stiffness of proteins led to superficially similar
solutions (Miyata et al., 2020).
As cell sizes increased, gravity posed an increasing challenge, because cells were denser
than water and thus subject to the deadly consequences of sedimentation. In response, eu-
karyote membranes evolved mechanical sensitivity to the force exerted by water displace-
ment on their undersides. This triggered upward swimming, supplemented by photomotility
in light-sensitive organisms (Machemer, 2001).
DOI: 10.4324/9781003137863-10
132 Sensation and movement
Metazoa
Early metazoans encountered similar challenges to those faced by single cells, but magnified
by increased mass. Sponge larvae swam, using the beating of cilia on their surface and
end (Leys et al., 2002). Thus, since sponges were the earliest-emerging metazoan phylum
with living representatives, dating to about 650 to 780 million years ago, movement likely
appeared near the outset of animal history. Ctenophores, next to emerge, reached a similar
solution. They used columns of cilia (“combs”) beating in unison, propelling the animal
through water.
Many early metazoans adopted a benthic lifestyle, surrendering to gravity instead of
counteracting it. However, a benthic lifestyle alone could not cope with local variations
in nutrient concentrations. In times of plenty a stationary animal might thrive, but in
times of scarcity there was a strong selective pressure favoring mobility and a seeking of
new resources.
Trace fossils (fossils of behavior as opposed to physical form) depicting trails about 1 mm in
width implicate a surface-grazing form of locomotion in some benthic metazoans of
the Ediacaran (also known as Vendian) period 570 million years ago. Beginning about
545 million years ago, near the onset of the Cambrian period, trails increased to 2.5–5 cm in
width, and burrowing became common at depths up to 5 cm (Chen, Zhou, Yuan, & Xiao,
2019; Collins, Lipps, & Valentine, 2000; Fedonkin, 2003).
Judging from living animals, many of the trails were caused by creeping movements. In
many cases, secreted mucus provided lubrication, and cilia propelled the animal forward.
Some sea anemones, classified as cnidarians, move in this way. They minimize their
“footprint” by keeping only a tiny fraction of their body surface in contact with the seabed,
thus leaving trails only 2–3 mm wide from bodies up to 10 times wider. The secreted mucus
binds the substrate material, leaving a trail that can fossilize. Some flatworms likewise creep
using mucus secretions and cilia, leaving ridged trails resembling some Precambrian trace
fossils (Collins et al., 2000). Cilia-based benthic movement achieves speeds of 1–2 mm/sec,
or .002–.004 miles per hour.
Other animals, however, may have had other means of movement. Yilingia spiciformis,
which resembled a greatly elongated ear of wheat (in fact, spiciformis refers to its spiky ap-
pearance), was a 545-million-year-old species that may have had rows of paired podomeres
(leg segments). If so, their presence presumably implies the use of muscles rather than cilia for
propulsion. A fossil of the creature heading a clearly visible path leaves little doubt that it left
sea-bottom trails up to 2.5 cm in width (Chen et al., 2019). Movement accomplished by
peristaltic waves, similar to that of earthworms, evolved a little later, providing increased
speed as bodies became larger.
Flatworms are notable not just for creeping, but for swimming, involving full-body un-
dulation using muscles. The later-emerging chordates also undulated to swim, but in a more
fish-like fashion due to their stiff notochord. Thus, one possibility is that the cilia used
in swimming by early sponge larvae and ctenophores were adapted in benthic species for
creeping movements, in turn followed by the evolution of muscle tissue that allowed a
reacquisition of swimming.
However, a complication is that muscles may have first evolved for spawning rather than
locomotion purposes. Seipel and Schmid (2005) proposed that an early swimming metazoan
(using flagella or cilia) evolved muscle cells for reproductive purposes. Its descendants then
included benthic bilateria as well as swimming ctenophores and cnidarians. All inherited
muscles, which began to be used for movement. This inheritance became particularly sig-
nificant to the bilateria, whose bottom-dwelling lifestyle led to differentiation between dorsal
(top) and ventral (bottom) sides. That set the stage for the evolution of body segments,
The origins of motion 133
allowing more complex muscle arrangements capable of more complicated movements
(Finnerty, 2005). Yilingia was just such a bilaterian (Chen et al., 2019).
It is indisputable that body segments permitted more complicated movements. In seg-
mented chordates like amphioxus, swimming involves head-to-tail waves of body contrac-
tion controlled by the nerve cord. A swimming rhythm is maintained by alternating cord
segments, a movement pattern that descended to later fish, and from them to tetrapods and
eventually to mammals (Viala, 2006).
Tetrapods
A major feature of the fish-to-tetrapod transition was a shift in emphasis from power supplied
by the forward fins or limbs, in the pectoral region, to the rear ones in the pelvic region
(Boisvert, 2005; Molnar, Hutchinson, Diogo, Clack, & Pierce, 2021). The Panderichthys-
Acanthostega-Ichthyostega evolutionary sequence (Figure 3.3) illustrates the changes. The early
lobe-finned Panderichthys, in the words of Boisvert (2005), was a “front-wheel drive” model,
pairing fish-like undulation of the front part of the body with anchoring pelvic fins.
In contrast, to varying degree Acanthostega and Ichthyostega were “rear-wheel drive”
models. Their appendages shifted toward the side, both front and back; their muscles and
bones enlarged for body support; and the “fin rays” became digits. All limbs lengthened, but
the increase was greater in the rear, where the pelvis also enlarged for increased support.
Thus, the transformation of fins to limbs began in the front, spread to the rear, and ultimately
resulted in transferring power to the rear (Boisvert, 2005).
This shift toward “rear-wheel drive” is significant in light of later locomotor developments
in dinosaurs including birds. But what caused it? The initial “front-wheel drive” state sensibly
used anchoring rear fins to stabilize an undulating body. But once tetrapod ancestors were
free of the mud, what caused the shift toward rear propulsion?
The explanation is that in land animals, the tail became critically important to propulsion.
This is visible in the archosaurs. Ancestors of the crocodilians and dinosaurs, they left ex-
tensive fossils allowing visualization of muscle attachments to bone. All three groups had a
massive muscle, the caudofemoralis longus, located in the tail but connected to the rear limbs.
Although the muscle helped move the tail, biomechanical analysis reveals a more important
opposite function. Specifically, the muscle provided power to the rear limbs, using the heavy
tail as an anchoring point (Gatesy, 1990; Persons & Currie, 2017). Figure 8.1 illustrates this
aspect of its use.
The relevance to tetrapod evolution is that the caudofemoralis longus is a tetrapod
character. It is found even in the early-diverging salamanders, where it does help power
strides (Ashley-Ross, 1994).
Thus, the shift to “rear-wheel drive” involved propulsory use of the tail. It is a beautiful
example of evolution using an existing structure for new purposes, much as jaw bones were
used in the auditory evolution of mammals. The tail, finned and directly propulsive in water,
could no longer be used that way on land. But it could be used indirectly, by anchoring
muscles powering newly evolved rear limbs.
Another tetrapod characteristic was a “step cycle” that moved the limbs in a particular
order. The sides alternated, specifically right hind - right front - left hind - left front. It is
noteworthy that the same step cycle is observed in lobe-finned fishes, indicating that gait (the
cyclical means by which the limbs move the body) traces back to the immediate ancestors of
tetrapods (Prothero, 2007, p. 220).
As a result of these changes, tetrapods enjoyed a leap forward in traveling speed over a solid
surface. One species of salamander, an early-emerging tetrapod lineage, has been clocked
trotting at 0.6 miles per hour (Ashley-Ross, 1994), a 200-fold increase over cilia-based
134 Sensation and movement
Figure 8.1 The tail as anchoring point, dorsal view. The caudofemoralis longus is in reddish brown, con-
necting the tail to the femur of a Tyrannosaurus.
Source: Tyrannosaurus muscle mass.png, CC BY-SA 4.0, John R. Hutchinson, Karl T. Bates, Julia Molnar, Vivian Allen,
and Peter J. Makovicky.
movement. However, early tetrapods sprawled severely. Limbs were held outward and the
belly was supported by the ground.
left-right differences.) Often both gaits are available, as in the horse’s symmetrical trot but
asymmetrical gallop (Gasc, 2001).
The absence of asymmetrical gaits in early tetrapods was due to their sprawling posture.
Without support directly under the body, sprawling compromises the balance needed for
an asymmetric gait. Even today, sprawling species like salamanders use a symmetrical gait,
nearly always with two or three limbs in contact with the ground even when trotting.
In contrast horses do not sprawl, and their asymmetric gallop can take all four limbs off
the ground simultaneously (Figure 8.3). Although salamanders can support themselves
momentarily on a single leg, this is maintained for only one or two strides before they flop
on their bellies (Ashley-Ross, 1994). Thus, the evolution of a fully upright quadrupedal
posture in mammals, putting weight directly over the legs, was an important milestone in
making fast movement possible.
Figure 8.3 Asymmetrical gallop of a horse, leaving all four limbs off the ground at once (the two leftmost
frames on the bottom).
Source: Muybridge horse gallop.jpg, public domain, Eadweard Muybridge (1830–1904).
Figure 8.4 The neuromuscular junction. 1 = axon, 2 = synaptic junction, 3 = muscle fiber, 4 = myofibrils.
Source: Synapse diag3.png, CC BY-SA 3.0, Dake.
The origins of motion 139
medulla. The tract continues to descend to a segment of the spinal cord, where it synapses
with motor neurons that travel to the muscles.
Damage to the primary motor cortex therefore affects movement on the opposite side of
the body due to the crossover. Because the tract largely controls movement of the distal
(farther from the core) musculature, damage to it is more likely to affect the hands and lower
limbs than it is the trunk muscles.
The other tract is called the extrapyramidal tract, although there are actually several distinct
pathways. What they have in common is that crossover is only partial. Thus, the pathways
from the brain divide, with part going down the same side, and part crossing over to the
opposite side. This allows either side of the brain to control both sides of the body. However,
unlike the corticospinal tract, the extrapyramidal tract largely innervates proximal (nearer to
the core) musculature such as trunk muscles that maintain posture.
With this background, we can easily address the question of why damage to one hemi-
sphere often impacts movement on the opposite side of the body. What is observed in such
cases is the effect of the corticospinal system, which crosses over. With one hemisphere
damaged, cortical control of the opposite distal musculature may be disrupted. Proximal
musculature is much less affected because the bilateral (two-sided) extrapyramidal system
allows those muscles to be controlled by the undamaged hemisphere.
That answers the “why” question of opposite-side effects of hemispheric damage. But how
did such a crossover evolve? That is both more interesting and more difficult to answer.
Cajal’s proposal
The famous neurophysiologist Santiago Ramón y Cajal (1852–1934) argued that the origin
of crossover was the reversal of images by the eyes, a simple optical property (Vulliemoz,
Raineteau, & Jabaudon, 2005). Given that images reverse, the problem faced by the nervous
system is how to achieve a continuous representation in the brain. According to Cajal, an
uncrossed system results in discontinuity, as at the bottom of Figure 8.5 where the word
“BEAR” is represented across the cerebral hemispheres as “ARBE”. A crossed system, in
contrast, achieves continuity and reproduces the two halves of the visual scene in the correct
order (top of the figure). When the two sides of the midline are bound together using the
between-hemisphere connections of the corpus callosum, a fully coherent representation is
achieved and “BEAR” is represented as “BEAR”.
Cajal assumed that with the left side of the visual world represented in the right
hemisphere, and vice versa, the other senses would need to be arranged similarly. That
would allow a coherent global representation of the world in which all input from one
side would be integrated in the same hemisphere. The motor system would also need to
follow suit, to control responses on the same side on which stimulation is received
(Vulliemoz et al., 2005).
140 Sensation and movement
Figure 8.5 Image continuity in a crossed system (top), and discontinuity in an uncrossed system (bottom).
The two curved segments at the bottom of each diagram represent the left and right cerebral
hemispheres.
Source: Original figure.
Figure 8.6 Illustration of the proposed origin of the visual crossover in paired reversing eyes. Left: Single eye,
with same-side projection of each retinal half. Right: Paired eyes, following the same projection
pattern.
Source: Original figure.
The seed of contralateral representation that was proposed by Cajal is present in the single
amphioxus eye. The visual receptors are arranged in cup form and are shaded by the cup’s
sides (Lacalli, 1996). The eye is therefore a reversing eye, as in Figure 7.2. Neural output
from the receptors largely project back along the same side (although receptors at the midline
of the eye project to both sides; Lacalli, 1996). As a result, while the neural pathways
themselves do not cross over, representation of the visual world is mostly contralateral
because of image reversal (Figure 8.6, left).
A modified proposal
There have been other proposals for the origin of crossover, e.g., that it resulted from a
physical “twist” that occurred in a proposed flatfish-like ancestor of vertebrates (de Lussanet
& Osse, 2010). However, Cajal’s reversing eye seems compelling as a proposed point of
origin. It is proposed here that the emergence of paired eyes, which occurred no later than
ancestral hagfish (Chapter 7), is what resulted in the full crossover of neural representation
illustrated on the right side of Figure 8.6.
Evolutionarily, what is proposed is the following. First, a single reversing eye appeared.
At roughly the same time, a partial motor crossover evolved in order to coordinate the
two sides of an undulating tail. Contraction on one side of that midline structure needed
to be paired with relaxation on the other, creating a need for motor commands to affect
muscles on both sides. Partial crossover probably also allowed the use of timed opposite
contractions, allowing the tail to cyclically flip back and forth (Lacalli & Kelly, 2003).
Notice, though, that the effect of a partial motor crossover is to allow both sides of the
body to be moved by either side of the nervous system. This is the arrangement of the
extrapyramidal tract.
Next, the eye duplicated, an evolutionary success because the eyes provided somewhat
differing views and thus improved surveillance. The duplicated eyes, it is proposed, simply
followed the same projection pattern of the single eye: The left side of each eye back along
the left side, and the right side of each back along the right side. The result was essentially the
modern crossover of projections (Figure 8.6, right).
142 Sensation and movement
Given this simple beginning, the remaining puzzle pieces fall into place much as Cajal
proposed. Because each half of the visual world was mapped onto the opposite side of the
nervous system, selection also favored crossover in the auditory and tactile sensory modalities.
That kept the global view of the world consistent. As motor responses came to be made not
just by midline structures but by distal ones away from the midline (e.g., fins and limbs),
increasing crossover continued to be favored in order to bring the motor and sensory worlds
into congruence. Therefore, the corticospinal tract is contralateral.
Finally, paired eyes and crossover led to a divided brain with left and right cerebral
hemispheres. Both were present in vertebrates as an early evolutionary emergence after
chordates (Kapoor & Khanna, 2004), before the evolution of bony fish (Vigouroux et al.,
2020) and at least as early as the evolution of lampreys (Pombal, Álvarez-Otero, Pérez-
Fernández, Solveira, & Megías, 2011; Suryanarayana, Robertson, Wallén, & Grillner, 2017).
Thus, it is proposed that a cascade of mutually reinforcing evolutionary changes caused
crossover to become a primary organizing principle of the vertebrate world, all of it traceable
to a reversing eye.
Conclusion
Movement originated with flagellum-propelled bacteria. However, early free-swimming
metazoa used cilia for thrust. Cilia were probably also used by benthic species for grazing,
made possible by lubricating mucus secretions. Trace fossils indicate this type of locomotion
existed about 570 million years ago.
Later muscles powered movement, by moving leg segments or by causing peristaltic waves
that pulled animals forward. However, it is possible that muscles were initially used for
spawning rather than locomotion. Hox gene-based segmentation of the body beginning with
chordates allowed swimming by way of head-to-tail waves of body contraction.
The transformation from fish to tetrapods produced a shift in power from the pectoral fins
to the rear limbs. Once bodies could clear the substrate, the rear limbs continued to power
strides because of their anchoring to the tail. One tetrapod characteristic was a “step cycle”
that moved the limbs in a particular order, the cycle having its origin in lobe-finned fishes.
Early amniotes had a sprawling posture that nevertheless held their bellies off the ground.
About 125 to 150 million years ago the sprawl gave way to an erect posture in mammals,
spreading power more evenly between the limbs. Limbs evolved a third segment, forming
a zigzag pattern that produced an up-and-down rather than side-to-side oscillation, an
adaptation to uneven ground. A symmetric gait characterized by the simple alternation of
sides, transformed to asymmetrical walking and trotting in which forelimbs and hindlimbs
had different timing cycles. An example is the horse’s gallop, which can leave all four limbs
off the ground.
Judging from ankle fossils, early primates were vertical climbers and leapers. Later ones,
however, evolved a quadrupedal stride with diagonal sequencing and forearm extension,
both of which are adaptations for an arboreal, fine-branch environment. In many species
heavy tails help maintain balance by producing a counterrotational shift of the body when
moved to one side. Lifting the tail also helps stabilize the body during leaps. However, apes
lost their tails, probably because they passed through a large-body phase of evolution in
which a counterbalancing tail was of little help. Primates including apes also have an “amble”
that allows continuous engagement with the substrate.
Knuckle walking, characteristic of gorillas and chimpanzees, has been thought by some a
human ancestral trait. More recent evidence, however, has found features of the hominin wrist
and humerus to be inconsistent with knuckle walking. More likely our hominin ancestors
engaged in “generalized arboreality” with both above-branch and below-branch locomotion.
The origins of motion 143
One prominent feature of the neurology of movement is that control of distal
movement crosses over, with the left hemisphere controlling the right limbs and vice
versa. This may have occurred to make motor control congruent with sensory re-
presentation. That also largely crosses over, an arrangement that probably traces to optical
image reversal by the eyes. It is proposed that neuronal arrangement in the single-eyed
amphioxus, in which each half of the retina projects straight back along the left and right
sides, was simply duplicated when the eyes duplicated, producing true crossover of neural
representation. Other senses followed crossover schemes to produce consistency in the
global view of the world.
9 Bipedalism
At the onset of the hominin clade, primate quadrupedalism gave way to bipedalism. How
and why this occurred have always been central issues in human paleontology. Many sub-
sequent developments depended on a bipedal stance, for it freed the hands. In turn our hands
manufactured tools as well as the other multivaried artifacts of culture, giving form to human
cognition.
As we have seen, the cranium of “Toumai”, the 7-million-year old hominin close to the
chimpanzee-human divergence, shows signs of bipedalism in a downward-opening, rela-
tively centered foramen magnum. In a recent report (still undergoing critical review as of this
writing), a number of bipedal features have also been identified in a femur associated with
Toumai, and thought to be from the same Sahelanthropus tchadensis species. Among others,
the reported features include a hominin-like orientation and compression of the femoral neck
(although nearly all of it is missing), a flattening that in other hominins indicates that the neck
was a long one; and a pattern of localized thickening and thinning of the shaft that is human-
like and not ape-like. Its describers concluded that Sahelanthropus was a biped, though one
capable of careful climbing (Guy et al., 2020).
However, some doubt remains because of the curvature of the femur. Guy et al. re-
ported their impression that the curvature resembles that of more certain bipeds like
Australopithecus and Orrorin. Yet actual measurements of the curvature suggest greater
similarity to apes, leading one team of researchers to conclude that it was not an
habitual biped and possibly not even a hominin (Macchiarelli, Bergeret-Medina, Marchi,
& Wood, 2020).
In contrast Orrorin tugenensis, a million years younger, leaves little doubt as to its mode of
locomotion because of a number of bipedal features including a long femoral neck (Almécija
et al., 2013; Kuperavage, Pokrajac, Chavanaves, & Eckhardt, 2018). Certainly by 3.4 million
years ago, the strongly bipedal characteristics of Australopithecus afarensis can leave no doubt
that full bipedalism had evolved (Ward, Kimbel, & Johanson, 2011).
The most parsimonious conclusion is that bipedalism was present in some form since the
onset of the human clade. Indeed, it may very well define it. If so, we need to know why it
began, how it evolved, and what consequences came of it.
DOI: 10.4324/9781003137863-11
Bipedalism 145
bipedalism, while other apes did not. This will be a good question to keep in mind as we
consider several possible causes of our bipedalism.
Efficiency
Could it have evolved because it was a more efficient means of locomotion? For many years
it was assumed, based on running, that it is energetically inferior to quadrupedalism. For
example, Alexander (1992) reported that when running, humans are less efficient than a
typical mammal of similar mass. It was concluded, therefore, that increased efficiency could
not be bipedalism’s cause.
Analyses since then have modified that conclusion. Steudel-Numbers (2001) found that at
three miles per hour, human walking saves 20% of the energy used by a typical similar-sized
mammal. Nevertheless Steudel-Numbers discounted this difference because a third of
similar-sized quadrupedal mammals proved more efficient than humans, and because early
bipedal hominins would have walked less efficiently than modern ones. Halsey and White
(2012) took a similar stance. They reported that walking is more efficient (and running less
efficient) in humans than in the average similarly sized mammal, but deemphasized the
outcome because humans fell within the overall range of comparison species.
However, Leonard and Robertson (1997) correctly pointed out that the proper com-
parison group is not mammals generally, but apes specifically. Thus, the walking efficiency of
the human ape need only have exceeded that of its ancestors for increased efficiency to help
explain bipedalism’s appearance. As it happens, apes are notably inefficient walkers: The
human energy savings over their quadrupedalism falls in the astounding range of 50% to 75%
(Leonard & Robertson, 1997; Sockol, Raichlen, & Pontzer, 2007).
Also under reconsideration is the assumption that early bipeds were notably less efficient
walkers than modern ones. Initially Australopithecus afarensis was thought to have moved with
a shuffling bipedal gait (Hunt, 1994), or even quadrupedally like a baboon but with occa-
sional bipedalism (Wood, 1992). Further research soon indicated, however, that afarensis was
a habitual biped (Ward, 2002). A notable afarensis feature is a long femoral neck (Figure 9.1),
permitting an angling-in of the thighs to place body weight directly over the knees in a
bipedal stance (Lovejoy, Meindl, Ohman, Heiple, & White, 2002). Chimpanzees have
a short neck, while a long neck is characteristic of the australopithecines and early Homo
species (Marchi et al., 2017).
Biomechanical simulations indicate that afarensis was capable of walking efficiently in an
upright position (Wang & Crompton, 2004). Furthermore, it was most efficient at a speed of
1.0 m/sec (2.2 mi/hr), matching the speed inferred from adult footprint trails at Laetoli (see
Figure 5.6). Although afarensis was slower than modern humans, who walk most efficiently at
1.5 m/sec (3.4 mi/hr), it was also shorter. But in any case, its gait was notably more efficient
than chimpanzee quadrupedalism, allowing the conclusion that it was “a fully competent
biped” (Sellers, Cain, Wang, & Crompton, 2005, p. 439).
This conclusion is reinforced by close analysis of Australopithecus africanus femurs. The un-
apelike greater thickness of cortex on the inferior as opposed to superior surface of the
femoral neck is consistent with bipedalism. However, the femoral neck angle was reduced
compared to modern humans, probably implying some side-to-side rocking, and thus a
somewhat less efficient gait (Ruff & Higgins, 2013).
One problem with australopithecine efficiency analyses, however, is that they focus on a
period postdating the appearance of bipedalism by some three to five million years. The
limited fossil remains do not allow a determination of whether bipedalism in its earliest form
was notably more efficient than ape quadrupedalism. Thus, the efficiency issue cannot be
fully addressed until more extensive fossils are found of the earliest hominin species.
146 Sensation and movement
Figure 9.1 Femur with long femoral neck (top) in Australopithecus afarensis.
Source: Adaptation of Lucy blackbg.jpg, CC BY-SA 3.0, 120.
Carrying
A final possibility is that a need to carry objects produced a selective pressure toward
bipedalism. One version of the hypothesis is that bipedalism allowed mothers to carry their
infants (Hodges, 2017). Others, however, emphasize the role of inanimate objects. For
example, chimpanzees carry rocks to nut trees, at distances up to several hundred meters,
where they are used as hammers and anvils to crack tough shells (Mercader, Panger, &
Boesch, 2002).
Even food itself is carried. Providing captive chimpanzees with piles of fruit results in
upper limb carries using unassisted bipedality, i.e., with full weight on the legs (Videan &
McGrew, 2002). If nuts are provided requiring a hammer stone, both are carried bipedally
(Carvalho et al., 2012). Thus, prized food items are carried sufficiently far that they can be
consumed without interference.
Although the carrying explanation of bipedalism is intuitively appealing, it is not entirely
problem-free. First, early hand-carried loads must have been very restricted in weight.
Simulations suggest that loads were limited to 50% of upper limb mass in Australopithecus
afarensis, in contrast to 200%-300% in Homo ergaster and nearly 800% in modern humans
(Wang & Crompton, 2004). In species preceding afarensis, hand-carriable loads may have
been even more limited. A second problem with the carrying explanation is that it seems
most applicable to open savanna, where widely-spaced trees might have required the
transport of stones from one to another. Yet the earliest known hominin, “Toumai”, lived in
a mosaic environment consisting not only of savanna, but also woodland, grassland, and forest
(Brunet et al., 2004).
A closer look, however, suggests these may not be problems for the carrying explanation
so much as they are important features of it. First, perhaps only light loads were involved,
well within the severe weight restriction on hand carrying. Such light loads might have
included simple vegetation-based tools and hunting weapons. Second, a mosaic environment
may have necessitated carrying items into areas where they were otherwise unavailable. For
example, a spear might be difficult to acquire in grassland. Thus, a mosaic environment may
itself have been an important influence on the adoption of bipedalism.
Chimpanzees certainly hunt small animals for meat, so it seems reasonable to assume that
the last chimpanzee-human common ancestor did as well. Chimpanzees encountering a tree
with red colobus monkeys follow a strategy in which several individuals drive monkeys into
part of the tree crown or to the ground where they can be caught. Once caught the monkey
is killed either with a bite to the cranium using the canine teeth or by flailing it against the
ground or a limb. Adult males are the primary hunters, and they succeed the majority of
the time (Gilby et al., 2017; Stanford, 1996; Watts & Mitani, 2002). Other encounters are
more spontaneous, as when a young pig or antelope is encountered and quickly grabbed. The
annual caloric intake from hunting is not insubstantial, by one estimate approaching 8%
to 9% of total calories (Stanford, 1996).
Of course this style of hunting is mostly uninformative with respect to a carrying ex-
planation of bipedalism, since no weapons are involved. Recently, however, a second type of
hunt has been observed that is more directly relevant. Chimpanzees in Senegal have been
found to prepare branches for use as spears, involving breaking off a living branch, stripping it
148 Sensation and movement
of leaves and twigs, and modifying one end to sharpen it. It is then used to forcefully probe
tree holes for galagos sleeping during the day (Pruetz et al., 2015). It seems quite possible that
early hominins engaged in similar behavior, advantaging the carrying of a prepared spear
from one place to another.
Hominin vegetation-based weapons would be consistent with weight-limited carries in
that neither spears nor clubs need be heavy. They are also consistent with an observed
shrinkage of canines, the fang-like teeth used by chimpanzees in aggressive encounters in-
cluding hunting. Canine reduction occurred as far back as seven million years ago in
“Toumai” (Brunet et al., 2004). If weapons were used, they may have eliminated the need
for large canines, leading to their reduction according to the “use it or lose it” principle.
A hunting-carrying contribution to the evolution of bipedalism is thus consistent with a
wide range of evidence, including observations of hunting in chimpanzees; a variable,
mosaic habitat; limitations in upper limb strength; and reductions in canine size. However,
it remains controversial. Some resistance to the idea can be accounted for by the absence of
stone tools, including weapon points, until later when bipedalism was highly advanced.
The rejoinder, of course, is that the earliest weapons would have been crafted as chim-
panzees craft them, from perishable materials such as sticks. These would likely not be
preserved in the fossil record.
Consequences of bipedalism
Once bipedalism became reasonably efficient, hominins could afford to embark on hunts that
included the opportunistic collection of fruit and nuts as a secondary activity. This reversed
the quadrupedal ape practice of foraging for fruit and nuts accompanied by opportunistic
hunting. To the chimpanzee-human common ancestor, fruit was an imperative and meat a
luxury; to later hominins, meat was an imperative and fruit a luxury. The long-distance hunt
may have perfected human bipedalism (Stanford, 2003).
The achievement of habitual bipedalism was accompanied by significant changes in
anatomy. Virtually all body segments were affected, head to toe. As we have already seen, the
head moved to a more balanced position with respect to the trunk, visible in fossil skulls as a
better-centered foramen magnum that opened straight downward rather than angling slightly
backward (Figure 9.3). At the same time, the snout shortened which improved head stabi-
lization (Bramble & Lieberman, 2004).
Figure 9.3 Underneath side of the gorilla and modern human skulls. The foramen magnum is posteriorly
shifted in the gorilla relative to the more central one in the human. Note that the two skulls are
not equally to scale.
Credits: Adaptation of Gorilla Male skull base.png, CC BY-SA 3.0, Didier Descouens, and Sobo 1909 41.png, public
domain, Dr. Johannes Sobotta.
The pelvis
One of the most substantial areas of change, long-recognized and carrying high significance
for childbearing, was in the pelvis. Our pelvis is lower and broader than that of a nonhuman
ape. The pelvic opening opens more distinctly downward, and the ilium (each of the bla-
delike extensions at the back of the pelvis) wraps further forward around the sides, providing
a broad attachment surface for the hugely expanded gluteus maximus (butt) muscles. These
muscles allow us to walk stably, with only one foot periodically in contact with the surface,
unlike the side-to-side rocking of apes walking bipedally. In general the shape of our pelvis is
more cup-like, cradling our internal organs (Martin, 1992; Stanford, 2003). These changes
were well advanced by the time of the australopithecines (Figure 9.6, top).
By the time of Homo erectus about a million years ago, the pelvis’ birth canal had enlarged
sufficiently to pass a baby’s head sized within the modern range (Simpson et al., 2008).
Modern humans have a pelvic opening that is more circular than that of australopithecines,
better accommodating the shape of the newborn skull (Figure 9.6, bottom; Claxton,
Hammond, Romano, Oleinik, & DeSilva, 2016; Gruss & Schmitt, 2015). However,
clearances are tighter, and compared to other mammals the pelvic bones do not spread apart
as flexibly, typically resulting in more difficult childbirth. The inflexibility may itself be an
adaptation for bipedalism, by allowing attached muscles to exert greater force (Pavlicev,
Romero, & Mitteroecker, 2020).
152 Sensation and movement
Figure 9.4 The modern human spinal column. C = cervical vertebrae, T = thoracic vertebrae, L = lumbar
vertebrae.
Credit: The human spinal column, CC BY 4.0, Sketchfab.
Bipedalism 153
Figure 9.5 Human versus gorilla skeleton, showing cylindrical versus cone-shaped ribcages.
Source: Primatenskelett-drawing.jpg, public domain, unknown.
Figure 9.6 Pelvis in Australopithecus sediba (top) and a modern human (bottom). Note: Not to scale.
Credits: Adaptation of Pelvis MH2 Australopithecus sediba.jpg, CC BY-SA 3.0, Profberger, and Pelvis (male) 03-superior
view.png, CC BY-SA 2.1 JP, BodyParts3D.
et al., 2020). The hallux (big toe) also changed. In apes it is opposable and can be used to
grasp limbs when climbing. In our ancestors this function was lost, and the hallux moved into
the same plane as the other toes (Harcourt-Smith & Aiello, 2004).
Better documented is the angle of the hallux to the other toes, within that plane. In
quadupedally walking bonobos, the mean angle is about 32°, and one measured while
walking bipedally showed an angle of 28° (Vereecke, D’Août, De Clercq, Van Elsacker, &
Aerts, 2003). In the 3.66-million-year-old Laetoli footprints, believed to have been made by
Australopithecus afarensis, the hallux was at a bonobo-like 25° angle – which, as noted in
Chapter 5, is indistinguishable from that in a 5.7 million-year-old putative hominin
trackway. In marked contrast, a 4.4-million-year-old Ardipithecus ramidus specimen had the
hallux splayed away from the other toes at a whopping 68° angle, as approximated in
Figure 5.3 (Lovejoy, Latimer, Suwa, Asfaw, & White, 2009). In this, it appears to have much
more resembled gorillas than Homo sapiens (Prang, 2019).
It is interesting, and relevant to the evolution of bipedalism, that even a 25° angle could
produce dragging of the hallux, as in some prints from the Laetoli trackway (Figure 9.8). In
1.5-million-year-old fossilized footprints in Kenya, thought to have been made by Homo
ergaster or Homo erectus, the hallux was at a 14° angle (Bennett et al., 2009) and would
have been less susceptible to dragging. In Homo neanderthalensis, the angle was 11°. Now, in
Homo sapiens, it is only about 7° (Bennett et al., 2009; Pablos et al., 2019), and we certainly
do not drag our hallux.
156 Sensation and movement
Figure 9.8 Detail of newly uncovered prints at Laetoli. The middle print reveals a dragging hallux as the
individual stepped forward, evidenced as a trail behind the footprint.
Source: Adaptation of Southern part of the hominin trackway in test-pit L8, CC BY-SA 4.0, Fidelis T. Masao, Elgidius B.
Ichumbaki, Marco Cherin, Angelo Barili, Giovanni Boschian, Dawid A. Iurino, et al.
The substantial departure of the ramidus measurement from the bonobo and afarensis results
give strong reason to doubt that ramidus was in the direct human ancestral line. Otherwise,
the last four million years of hominin evolution has seen a smoothly progressing realignment
of the big toe, increasing its efficiency in working with the other toes to promote bipedal
locomotion.
Late anatomical developments in Homo helped improve bipedal performance when
running. These included shortened toes commencing with habilis and lengthened legs
beginning with erectus. Today, while we are relatively poor short-distance runners, we
are fairly impressive at long distances. The speeds obtainable in endurance running,
about 2.3 to 6.5 m/sec (5.1 to 14.5 miles/hr), manage to bracket the average 5.8 m/sec
(12.9 miles/hr) speed of long-distance postal horses. However, this type of running is still
metabolically more expensive, pound for pound, than it is for typical quadrupeds
(Bramble & Lieberman, 2004).
Nevertheless endurance running may tie in with long-distance hunting. Although modern
hunter-gatherers rarely engage in endurance running, it may have been an obligatory aspect
of hunting before the invention of at-a-distance weapons like spear throwers and arrows.
Endurance running allowed animals to be run down through exhaustion, permitting hunters
to approach and make a kill (Bramble & Lieberman, 2004). Even though the method in-
volves a large outlay of energy, if a large animal is killed and consumed, the ratio of energy
gains to losses exceeds 25:1 (Glaub & Hall, 2017).
Conclusion
All indications are that bipedalism has been a hominin feature for about as long as there have
been hominins. It was definitely present in Orrorin six million years ago, and very likely
present in “Toumai” seven million years ago. Nevertheless early hominins were simulta-
neously specialized for more than one mode of locomotion. Besides being well adapted for
bipedal walking using a remarkably modern stiff, extended stride, Australopithecus afarensis also
showed clear signs of arboreal adaptation in its cone-shaped thorax and curved fingers.
Both modes of locomotion make sense in terms of our evolutionary antecedents. The
last common ancestor of chimpanzees and humans could probably stand and take a few
steps bipedally, most often while foraging. Yet like chimpanzees, that ancestor was also
comfortable in trees. Human bipedal walking is more efficient than ape quadrupedal
walking, providing one reason for the evolution of bipedalism, but other factors must have
been at work.
The factors pushing us into habitual bipedalism have become clearer although they are not
yet a matter of consensus. Naturalistic observation of our closest relatives indicates some
capacity for bipedally carrying objects, a conclusion confirmed by experimental observations
of food carrying. A reduction in the size of the canines 6 to 7 million years ago in human
ancestors likely indicates a reduced need for them in aggressive encounters. Together these
observations suggest that carrying, including the carrying of simple weapons and other tools,
provided impetus for us to walk upright. Other possible influences include improvements in
upright foraging and in thermoregulation.
It appears that the split between hominins and chimpanzees was sympatric in that the
environments they occupied overlapped. However, the absence of chimpanzee fossils of any
significant age suggests that ancient chimpanzees were limited to hot, damp forests while
hominins exploited a mosaic of woodland, grassland, forest, and savanna. If so, hominins may
have had to carry tools and weapons to ensure that they had them in shifting environments,
providing a possible reason why humans but not chimpanzees became bipedal.
An initially compliant bipedalism yielded over three million years ago to a stiffened gait
that recovered energy from one step to the next. Then further changes beginning about two
million years ago adapted us for endurance running.
Throughout this evolutionary process, changes occurred in the hominin skeleton. The
foramen magnum became more centered in the skull, allowing the head to balance during
bipedal locomotion. The vertebral column became a curved spring. As hanging from
Bipedalism 159
branches became less common, the ribcage became more cylindrical than the cone shape
found in nonhuman apes and early hominins, and curved fingers straightened. The pelvis
became lower and broader, and its opening more circular. However, it also became less
flexible to allow greater muscle forces, resulting in more difficult childbirth.
The legs lengthened, and a longer femoral neck allowed them to angle in to distribute
weight more directly over the feet (though this was to shorten again following changes to the
pelvis). The Achilles tendon lengthened at least as early as Australopithecus afarensis 3.2 million
years ago, improving running. The feet developed prominent arches, stiffening them. The
hallux came into the same plane as the other toes, and became much less divergent over
several million years. These changes improved both balance and propulsion.
So much for our legs and feet. The next question was, what would we do with our
freed-up hands?
Section III
Whether or not carrying objects led to bipedalism, bipedalism certainly had profound effects
on carrying objects. Standing upright freed the hands from any direct role in locomotion.
From that point onward they could either wither away as an unnecessary metabolic expense,
like the forelimbs of some theropod dinosaurs, or adopt new roles in the hominin lifestyle.
That they took the latter course profoundly affected the history of cognition as it relates to
the creation of objects and language.
In this chapter, we first consider the nature of praxis and its underpinnings in the brain, before
examining handedness and its primate origins and human components. The genetic foundations
of handedness are considered, as is archaeological evidence attesting to its antiquity. Finally, we
ponder why handedness exists, in both a right-handed majority and a left-handed minority.
Praxis
Consideration of how we voluntarily move, especially the hands but not limited to them,
begins with praxis – the ability to conceive, initiate, and complete movement. Although this
ability partially depends on motor pathways described earlier, brain mechanisms play a
substantial role by allowing us to conceptualize and initiate movement.
Figure 10.1 Motor areas of the frontal lobe. The primary motor cortex is in red, the premotor cortex is in
green, and the supplementary motor area is in blue. The red and blue areas also extend onto the
medial surface of the cortex.
Source: Adaptation of Brodmann area 4 lateral.jpg, CC BY-SA 2.1 JP, BodyParts3D.
In the right hemisphere particularly, the area is involved when the integration of spatial
information is demanding, as in the early phases of motor learning using visual guidance.
Activation may switch to the left hemisphere later in learning (Halsband & Lange, 2006).
A notable aspect of the ventral portion of the premotor cortex is that it contains cells that
fire rapidly when their possessor observes the actions of others. They also fire when he or she
makes the same motions. Such mirror cells undoubtedly play a major role in skill learning
through the observation and mimicry of others (Rizzolatti, Fogassi, & Gallese, 2002). In
effect, they serve as a kind of preverbal communication system, with a teacher triggering
neural activity in the learner that aids in copying behaviors.
The parietal lobe (Figure 6.3) plays an indirect role in praxis through spatial computation
and the evaluation of sensory feedback. Although these could be viewed as perceptual, there
is increasing recognition that the parietal lobe can play a more direct role in movement.
Specifically, the intraparietal sulcus (Figure 10.3) helps direct eye movement, and in the left
hemisphere helps produce gestures. It becomes active when hand motions are targeted to
particular locations. Also, lesions in or near it interfere with reaching movements (Grefkes &
Fink, 2005; Mühlau et al., 2005).
The functions served by the premotor cortex in the frontal lobe and the intraparietal sulcus
in the parietal lobe are sufficiently similar that they are probably linked as parts of a larger
system for the control of movement. Damage to either can result in disordered gestures due
to spatial and timing errors. Although their functional differences are somewhat unclear, it
may be that the premotor area is more involved in memory aspects of movement, and the
Praxis and handedness 165
Figure 10.2 Body part representations in the primary motor cortex. The size of each part reflects the amount
of cortex devoted to it.
Source: 1421 Sensory Homunculus.jpg, CC BY 3.0, OpenStax College.
parietal area in spatial orienting and the positioning of body parts (Haaland, Harrington, &
Knight, 2000).
A final brain area involved in praxis is the cerebellum. This large structure at the rear and
base of the brain (Figure 6.3) plays a role in motor programming, specifically in “packaging”
movements as a timed sequence of components. The cerebellum also directs real-time
correction of movement due to its possession of overlaid sensory and motor maps of the
body, permitting it to detect discrepancies and issue error messages (Kolb & Whishaw, 2003).
supplementary, premotor, and parietal motor areas date approximately from the origin of
primates 74 million years ago (Stepniewska, Preuss, & Kaas, 2006). Tree shrews, close re-
latives of primates, do not have them (Kaas, 2004a).
There have also been changes within the primate lineage. Even before the divergence of
New World from Old World monkeys about 44 million years ago, control of the forelimbs
by premotor cortex had expanded from proximal to distal musculature, and stronger con-
nections had formed between premotor cortex and surrounding brain areas. These adapta-
tions are thought to have been important to the evolution of reaching and grasping by
primate forelimbs (Stepniewska et al., 2006; Sustaita et al., 2013).
In contrast, the cerebellum is a truly ancient structure. Its first clear appearance was in
jawed fish, a group including the sharks and rays (see Figure 2.3). This places the origin of the
cerebellum about 445 million years ago. It may be even older, consisting of a few cells in
lampreys (Sugahara, Murakami, Pascual-Anaya, & Kuratani, 2017). If this homology (a simi-
larity in traits traceable to common ancestry) is accepted, the origin of the cerebellum is
pushed back to over 500 million years ago.
Evolutionary investigation of the cerebellum in primates has focused on its size relative to
the rest of the brain. An enlargement in apes relative to monkeys suggests a size shift be-
ginning about 29 million years ago. The enlargement may have been related to increases in
movement complexity and motor planning (Rilling, 2006).
Table 10.1 Three components of PRH, as defined by factor analysis, with inferred hominin trends
since the last chimpanzee-human common ancestor. aft = after, bef = before, diff = difference,
mya = million years ago
Family resemblances
If handedness is under at least some genetic control, there should be resemblances between
family members. Chimpanzee studies generally support this. Thus, in wild chimpanzees using
tools, or performing the tube task, an individual’s handedness is moderately related to that of
its mother (Hopkins, 2006; Hopkins, Wesley, Russell, & Schapiro, 2006). Sibling resem-
blances pertain as well: For handedness assessed by throwing hand, siblings show concordant
(same-sided) handedness to an extent greater than chance (Hopkins, Russell, Cantalupo,
Freeman, & Schapiro, 2005).
These results are certainly consistent with a genetic influence on handedness. Caution
should be observed, however, because they may also be consistent with nongenetic
factors, i.e., mothers or siblings may introduce environmental pressures toward similar
handedness.
In human handedness research, twin studies have a long history, with the first handedness
comparison of monozygotic (identical) and dizygotic (nonidentical or fraternal) twins dating
back to research by Siemens in 1924 (Medland, Duffy, Wright, Geffen, and Martin, 2006).
Greater concordance in handedness between monozygotic twins is reported, an outcome
consistent with a genetic influence although interpretation is clouded by greater similarity in
the prenatal and postnatal environments of such twins. Medland et al. (2006) reviewed 34
studies, beginning with Siemens’ work, and concluded that about 25% of variation in
handedness can be attributed to genetic factors – or in other words, that handedness is 25%
heritable. The remaining 75% is due to unknown environmental factors.
Thus, the scientific literature examining family resemblances is certainly consistent with a
genetic influence on handedness, although it cannot be regarded as completely conclusive.
Genetic models
Several specific genetic models of handedness have been proposed. The most influential is
the right shift theory of Marian Annett, a version of which was first proposed in the 1970s.
This states that human handedness is the product of a bell-shaped distribution in com-
bination with a right-shift allele that is present in some individuals and not others. By
itself, the bell-shaped distribution would lead to equal numbers of left- and right-handers,
as well as a large proportion of persons with mixed handedness. However, according to
the model the distribution is shifted rightward in those with the allele, resulting in a
predominance of right-handedness. Furthermore, depending on the task, the sub-
population of individuals with two copies of the allele, one on each of a pair of chro-
mosomes, may be shifted more strongly rightward than those with only one copy
(Annett, 1998). More recently, Annett addressed the issue of how the model might
account for the reduced PRH in chimpanzees, reporting a right-shift factor similar to that
in humans but reduced in strength (Annett, 2006).
The right shift theory makes no distinction between genders, and so the involved gene is
presumed to be an autosomal one – i.e., a gene located elsewhere than on the sex-determining
X and Y chromosomes. McManus and Bryden (1992), however, proposed that in addition
172 Perception and cognition
there is a recessive modifier gene located on the X chromosome that can suppress the effect
of the autosomal gene. Such a gene, they argued, could account for the existence of a
“maternal effect” in which left-handed mothers tend to produce more left-handed offspring
than do left-handed fathers (McKeever, 2000; McManus & Bryden, 1992).
A problem with both the autosomal and X-chromosome right shift theories is that testing
them largely depends on fitting handedness frequency data to the models. While the fits can
be quite good, such data could be viewed as insufficiently weighty to establish the models’
validity. This is particularly the case when different studies produce different frequencies. For
example, McKeever (2000) reported data showing that in families with one left- and one
right-handed parent, left-handed mothers produced left-handed sons (a) more frequently
than did left-handed fathers, and (b) more frequently than they did left-handed daughters.
Both observations are consistent with a handedness-related gene on the X chromosome, but
as McKeever pointed out, earlier studies failed to produce similar findings.
Individual genes
Other evidence emerges from studies relating specific genes to handedness. Laval et al.,
(1998) measured handedness by the speed of movement of pegs across a pegboard, and found
an allele on the X chromosome to be associated with left-handedness. However, the result is
not as supportive of the McManus and Bryden version of right shift theory, as initially
appears, because right shift theory states that there is no genetic left-handedness, only genetic
right-handedness.
A second discovery was that a pegboard measure of handedness correlated to the presence
of a specific gene on chromosome 2 (Francks et al., 2003), since designated LRRTM1. So
far, though, the relationship has been found only in clinical, and not nonclinical populations
(Beste et al., 2018). Also, the gene acts in a manner quite unlike that predicted by the right
shift theories, with greater father-to-child than mother-to-child genetic similarity across
multiple samples.
A third gene affecting handedness appears to be located on chromosome 12. It is related to
writing and drawing – i.e., the skilled component of handedness (Warren, Stern, Duggirala,
Dyer, & Almasy, 2006). That is one of the autosomal chromosomes, which might favor
Annett’s right shift theory. However, it remains to be seen whether a specific genetic locus
can be identified and if so, whether the gene works in right-shift fashion.
Additionally, an allele of a gene on chromosome 15, designated PCSK6, has been found to
be correlated to handedness as measured by a pegboard task. In an individual, each copy of
the allele shifts handedness rightward a small amount. Yet so far, the correlation has only been
found in reading disabled individuals, not in normal readers (Brandler et al., 2013).
Finally, Wiberg and colleagues have examined linkages between genes and handedness in
what is by far the largest sample to date, about 38,000 left-handers and nearly 360,000 right-
handers. Four genes were identified, but none were the same ones identified in the other
studies. Although one was on chromosome 2 like the LRRTM1 gene of Francks et al.
(2003), it was a different gene designated MAP2, at a different location. The other three
genes were on chromosomes not previously implicated in handedness – chromosomes 6, 17,
and 22 (Wiberg et al., 2019).
It should be noted that if there are several genes influencing handedness, all operating in
different fashion, it is unlikely that a simple genetic model of handedness will be found to
apply. In that regard some studies of either the entire genome or exome (the reduced portion
of the genome that codes for proteins) have failed to identify any genes with a major in-
fluence on handedness (Armour, Davison, & McManus, 2014; Kavaklioglu, Ajmal, Hameed,
& Francks, 2016).
Praxis and handedness 173
What accounts for the weakness of single-gene effects on handedness? It appears to be an
instance of what had been called the missing heritability problem – the observation that even
when single gene effects are cumulated, they usually don’t account for the full heritability of a
behavior or characteristic (Bourrat & Lu, 2017). Even in the massive whole-genome study of
Wiberg et al. (2019), the four identified genes together only accounted for about 1% of the
variability in handedness – comparing unfavorably to the 25% heritability deduced from twin
studies. Thus, one reason for “missing heritability” is that each relevant gene often has only a
tiny effect on the characteristic. If so, then it can only be hoped that after a sufficient number
of massive studies have been conducted, a fuller picture will emerge. But more funda-
mentally, the problem may trace to the interactivity of genetic effects: The impact of a gene
may depend on the status of other genes, on the environment, or on the carrier’s stage of
development. All can lead to underestimating the size of genetic effects.
Nevertheless, indirect evidence from family resemblances as well as direct evidence from
studies of specific genes does support the existence of genetic influences on human hand-
edness. These discoveries lend weight to the outcome of comparative analysis indicating that
human handedness is an elaboration of earlier forms of primate handedness, and has been
subject to evolutionary forces.
Archaeological evidence
Having considered the evolution and genetics of handedness, and more specifically, of PRH,
a logical next question is to ask how far back hominins showed evidence of it. As it happens,
there is a rich and continuous record of PRH dating back nearly two million years.
The fossils of two hominins from 1.5 to 1.8 million years ago both suggest the presence of
right-handedness, though in different ways. An analysis of teeth from a specimen attributed
to Homo habilis, dating from 1.8 million years ago, indicates an increased number of scratches
on the individual’s right. Presumably the scratches resulted from holding material in the teeth
while cutting with a stone tool held by one hand, usually the right, resulting in occasional
accidental contact with the teeth (Frayer et al., 2016). In the other case, a 1.5-million-year
old Homo erectus individual, it was a larger deltoid (shoulder) muscle attachment groove on
the right clavicle that led to an assessment of right-handedness (Cashmore, Uomini, &
Chapelain, 2008). While only two cases are not conclusive evidence of PRH within their
time period, they are suggestive.
The oldest artifactual evidence of PRH in humans derives from the flakes and cores of stone
tools. When modern tool knappers work stone, they tend to hold the core (the stone from
which the tool is made) in the left hand, striking flakes (stone fragments) from it using a
striking stone held in the right hand. This distribution of effort undoubtedly traces to the
greater skill and precision of the right hand of most people.
The largely left-right distribution of core and striking stone has consequences for the
flakes struck from the core. Examination shows that the flakes have a characteristic form
involving the striking platform (the small flat surface where the blow landed). According to
Toth (1985), as the core is rotated clockwise for repeated striking, the striking platform of
a flake struck off by the right hand has a scar to the left and a weathered outer surface to
the right, when oriented upward. A left-handed knapper would presumably show the
opposite orientation of flakes, resulting from holding the core in the right hand, striking
flakes off with the left, and rotating the core counter-clockwise. Thus, collecting flakes,
and analyzing the location of the scar relative to the striking platform, may allow an
inference about the handedness of the knapper.
Modern-day counts of flakes produced by right-handed knappers show that the numerical
difference between “left-handed” and “right-handed” flakes is not large, perhaps because
174 Perception and cognition
these tendencies are inherently small in size, but perhaps also because a fatiguing knapper may
strike differently or even change hands from time to time. Nicholas Toth, an experienced
right-handed knapper, counted his own directional flakes and found that 56% of them were
right-handed while 44% were left-handed (Toth, 1985). Similarly, Pobiner (1999) reported
that seven right-handed students produced right-left flakes in a 60%-40% split. Less strikingly
(no pun intended), Bargalló and Mosquera (2014) reported knapping results from eight right-
handed and seven left-handed university students in Spain, which can be characterized as a
51%-49% right-left split for right handers and a 50%-50% split for left-handers. However, the
Spanish splits are based on median values across participants, which may minimize differences
compared to the overall frequency counts used by Toth and Pobiner.
The relevance, of course, is that the same analysis can be applied to archaeological col-
lections of flakes. The oldest that have been analyzed were discovered at Koobi Fora, Kenya,
and date to 1.5 million years ago. When classified, 57% were found to be right-handed
(Toth, 1985), a significant deviation from 50% given the sample size. The figure is not
significantly deviant from the 60% proportion produced by the modern right-handed
knappers of Pobiner. The Koobi Fora knappers therefore appear to have been predominantly
right-handed.
The second oldest sample is from Ambrona, Spain, dating to about 350,000 years ago,
where 61% of the flakes were right-handed (Toth, 1985), again significantly divergent from a
50% value. A sample of Neanderthal flakes, dating to about 180,000 years ago, also showed
PRH (Cornford, 1986). While an archaeological sample of flakes produced by Australian
aborigines, of uncertain age but less than 50,000 years old, showed a right-handed percentage
of only 51% (Pobiner, 1999), that did not differ significantly from the modern 60% value.
Thus, the simplest interpretation of the combined results is that humans have shown con-
sistent PRH from the time of the oldest stone tools that have been assessed, dating from 1.5
million years ago.
Nevertheless, some caution should be observed before accepting these results. Uomini
(2006) argued for the rejection of Toth’s methodology, in part because she observed no
consistency across modern knappers in the direction of rotation of the core. It is not clear,
however, how the repeated observations of asymmetry in archaeological assemblies of flakes
are otherwise to be explained, except by predominant right-handedness.
Uomini did accept as valid another study of asymmetry in lithic assemblages, that of
Phillipson (1997). After analyzing modifications to the edges of 54 handaxes from Kenya,
dated at about one million years old, Phillipson concluded that 51 were probably intended to
be used by one hand or the other. Of those, 45 (88%) were intended for right-handers, and
11 (12%) for left-handers. Note that besides enjoying a potentially better-grounded meth-
odology than that of Toth, this approach has the additional advantage of specifying specific
handedness percentages rather than a general population tendency.
As already indicated, teeth can also indicate handedness through asymmetric scratching,
pitting, and chipping by tools. You can simulate this by using your dominant hand to place a
pen cap between your teeth, and carefully guiding the pen into it. Then imagine that you
missed with an edged stone!
A sample of damaged teeth from Homo heidelbergensis in Spain, dating to before 530,000
years ago, revealed that all 20 individuals were right-handed (Lozano, Mosquera, de Castro,
Arsuaga, & Carbonell, 2009). Neanderthals, too, show handedness with this methodology,
with 29 of 31 individuals (94%) right-handed across two samples (Fox and Frayer, 1997;
Estalrrich & Rosas, 2013). It therefore seems that both Homo heidelbergensis and Homo
neanderthalensis showed strong PRH.
Tools also figured in an assessment of handedness by Semenov (1970), reporting that stone
scrapers from upper Paleolithic sites (10,000–40,000 years ago) showed traces of greater wear
Praxis and handedness 175
on the right side, indicating right-handed use. Some 80% of scrapers were classified as right-
handed, a trend claimed to be visible even in published depictions.
Cave art supports the existence of PRH in Homo sapiens 10,000–30,000 years ago in the
form of negative hands, each of which is the outline of a hand held against the rock and painted
by blowing pigment through a tube held by the other, usually dominant hand. Of 507
known negative hands from caves in France and Spain, 77% are outlines of the left hand,
implying PRH. Confirming this, the identical 77% figure has been observed in a large sample
of university students producing negative hands with a similar technique (who, of course,
would be expected to be about 90% right-handed). Furthermore, in these modern would-be
troglodytes, throwing and writing handedness was correlated with which hand held the tube
(Faurie & Raymond, 2004).
Finally, a study of artistic representation of hand usage over the past 5000 years reveals a
stable PRH over that period. Instances of one-handed use of a tool or weapon were tallied on
the assumption that models held these artifacts in their preferred hands. Across the years, with
only minor and seemingly random variation, 93% were right-handed (Coren & Porac, 1977).
In summary, the archaeological evidence pertaining to human handedness is consistent:
PRH has existed over at least the past 1.5 to 1.8 million years, probably on the order of 90%
of individuals. In their general direction these conclusions conform to expectations from the
observed PRH in apes, and they suggest that the quantitative shift from the approximately
60% PRH found in apes to 90% in humans, dates back at least to the beginning of the genus
Homo. It may date even further back, as archaeology is silent on the matter before 1.8 million
years ago.
Why handedness?
Perhaps the most important question about handedness, specifically PRH, is why it exists at
all. The answer is likely to imply something important about the evolution of human
cognition as expressed through hand movement.
It is proposed here that PRH reflects the evolution of mechanisms of praxis that brought
the timing and sequencing of movement under control of the left hemisphere of the brain
(Leinen, Panzer, & Shea, 2016; Serrian & Sovijärvi-Spapé, 2015). Having begun as a simple
mechanism underlying the relatively gross movements of reaching, gesturing, and pointing, it
became progressively differentiated into a more skilled component supporting precision
movement about 12 million years ago, and then sometime after 7.5 million years ago, one
showing sidedness in the complex insertion and turning manipulations of tools. Throughout
this evolutionary history, operation of left hemisphere mechanisms of praxis presumably
drove a preferential use of the right hand simply because of the predominance of contralateral
motor connections. The advantage was a system that operated more smoothly and with less
delay than one requiring cross-hemisphere coordination.
At present it is difficult to reach definite conclusions about the evolution of brain changes
that supported increasing manual skill in general and PRH in particular. However, three
brain areas have been identified that will very likely prove relevant in future research.
First, monkey and human unskilled movements have in common an activation of primary
motor cortex opposite the hand (Rizzolatti et al., 2002). It may be, therefore, that the basis of
the most primitive form of PRH is asymmetry in the hand area of the primary motor cortex.
This asymmetric anatomy appears to exert influence even over skilled handedness, although
that likely involves other anatomical regions as well. Thus, in chimpanzees, not only reaching
and feeding but also tube task handedness, all appear to be related to asymmetry in the
volume of the hand area of the primary motor cortex (Dadda, Cantalupo, & Hopkins, 2006;
Taglialatela, Cantalupo, & Hopkins, 2006).
176 Perception and cognition
Figure 10.4 Brodmann areas of the human brain. Lobes: F = frontal, P = parietal, T = temporal, O =
occipital.
Source: Brodmann areas and lobes of the left hemisphere, licensed from Elsevier under STM Permissions Guidelines.
A second highly relevant brain area is a portion of the ventral premotor area in Old World
monkeys known as F5. This area appears to be anatomically homologous to human
Brodmann area 44 (Figure 10.4), located in the inferior frontal gyrus (Rizzolatti et al., 2002).
While area 44 exists on both sides of the brain, in the left hemisphere it is part of Broca’s area
and plays a major role in the production of speech. Notably, gesture, hammering, and termite
fishing handedness – representing all three of the handedness components in Table 10.1 – are
related to asymmetry in the chimpanzee inferior frontal gyrus, with it tending to be larger on
the left (Hopkins, Russell, & Cantalupo, 2007; Taglialatela et al., 2006).
F5 is of note because it is the location of mirror cells, which in the monkey are active
during the production of specific actions such as grasping, holding, and tearing. They are also
active when the same actions are observed in others. In humans area 44 becomes active
not only during speech production, but also when manipulating objects and especially when
executing precision grips, as we often do when using tools. Simply observing tool use, or
even tools themselves, also results in area 44 activation (Rizzolatti et al., 2002). Thus, it
appears to be one location of mirror cells in humans, with neighboring area 6 likely re-
presenting another (Cerri et al., 2015; Cook, Bird, Catmure, Press, & Heyes, 2014; de la
Rosa, Schillinger, Bülthoff, Schulz, & Uludag, 2016).
Significantly, in chimpanzees, use of a precision index finger-thumb grip is more fre-
quently performed by the right hand than by the left (Hopkins et al., 2005). This seems
similar to human right-handedness in object manipulation and tool use. Therefore it may be
that the evolution of asymmetry in area F5/44, favoring the left hemisphere, is what sup-
ported the fuller emergence of the more skilled form of PRH that is observed in primate
communicative gestures, and that in humans supports the use of simple tools. As indicated
above, comparative analysis suggests this development occurred about 12 million years ago.
However, the area itself is obviously older than its asymmetry. Macaques have an area
F5/44 (Petrides & Pandya, 2009), suggesting that it emerged no later than the time of
the Old World monkey divergence 29 million years ago. That put a critical anatomical
substrate into place long before the development of skilled handedness. Its seeds may date
back even further than that, because mirror neurons have been found in the marmoset
Praxis and handedness 177
homolog of area F5 (Suzuki et al., 2015), and the New World monkey divergence was
44 million years ago.
A final brain area that has been implicated in primate handedness is the “pli-de-passage
fronto-parietal moyen parietale” (PPFM). This is a gyrus buried along the central sulcus,
connecting the precentral and postcentral gyri; i.e., under the dark line between
Brodmann areas 4 and 1–2-3 in Figure 10.4. In chimpanzees, differences in speed of
performance of a simulated termite fishing task correlate to anatomical asymmetry of the
PPFM, such that individuals who are faster with the right hand show a larger PPFM as well
as a larger inferior frontal gyrus in the left hemisphere. Individuals faster with the left hand
show the opposite, a larger PPFM and inferior frontal gyrus in the right hemisphere
(Hopkins et al., 2017). Unfortunately it is not yet known whether this result is specific to
the insertion-turning component of handedness, or generalizes to the unskilled and skilled
components as well.
While asymmetries of the primary motor cortex, F5, and PPFM are correlated to primate
handedness, it is not yet clear how they can account for mutual independence of the three
components of handedness (unskilled, skilled, and insertion-turning). That is an issue that
must remain to future research.
Why left-handedness?
In spite of the overwhelming right-handedness of human populations, nearly 10% of people
remain left-handed. That has probably been the case for well over a million years. What
accounts for the maintenance of this minority?
One popular view is that creativity is more often associated with left-handedness than
right-handedness. Could it be that a “creative edge” is what maintains left-handers in the
population? Some studies report increased percentages of left-handers in music or the visual
arts, or higher scores on tests of creativity or divergent thinking in left-handers relative to
right-handers. However, a literature review shows that on balance, there is no evidence for
increased creative excellence in left-handers in music, the visual arts, or literature (Heilman,
2005). It therefore seems unlikely that the maintenance of left-handedness can be accounted
for by increased creativity, if there has been no increased production of creative products by
left-handers.
In contrast, one hypothesis receiving empirical support is that in violent societies, left-
handers enjoy a survival advantage because of their unorthodox style in confrontational
fighting. The offensive and defensive hands, which in most right-handers are the right and
left respectively, are more often left and right in left-handers. Unfamiliarity with this reversal
puts right-handers at a disadvantage compared to their left-handed opponents, who are more
likely to have seen instances of their adversaries’ opposite style. Consistent with the un-
orthodox style hypothesis, sizeable increases in the incidence of left-handers are observed in
interactive sports like martial arts, table tennis, and ice hockey, relative to noninteractive
sports like swimming, skiing, and archery (Raymond, Pontier, Dufour, & Møller, 1996;
Richardson & Gilman, 2019).
The analysis of homicide rates and the incidence of left-handedness in eight traditional
societies has lent further support to the hypothesis by finding a strong correlation between
the two, as shown in Figure 10.5 (Faurie & Raymond, 2005). Left-handedness increases as
homicide rates increase, as predicted by the hypothesis that left-handers have an advantage in
confrontational conflict.
By any reasonable interpretation of the daily news, even in developed societies old-
fashioned arms-length murder remains a popular means of dispensing with adversaries.
Of the 63,661 murders committed in the United States in 2011–2015, 23% were by way of
178 Perception and cognition
Figure 10.5 The correlation (r = +.83) between homicide and left-handedness, across eight traditional
societies.
Source: Original figure.
knives, blunt objects, beatings, drownings, stranglings, or asphyxiations – and that does not
even include the 48% committed with handguns, many of which were surely arms-length
confrontations (FBI, 2018). Thus, it seems possible that confrontational conflict continues to
play a role in maintaining left-handedness even in modern western society.
Presumably the strength of this effect has outweighed any disadvantage that left-handers
suffer in having motor timing and sequencing represented in the left hemisphere of the brain,
but control of the preferred hand in the right hemisphere. As discussed earlier, this should
result in a need for cross-hemisphere coordination, resulting in a system that works less
smoothly and with more delay.
Conclusion
The view developed in this chapter is that handedness has multiple components whose causes
ultimately trace to the same roots, namely a preponderance of left hemisphere involvement in
praxis. Taking an anatomical point of view, lesions to the primary cortex in the left hemi-
sphere can cause a paralysis of the right hand. Damage to the supplementary motor cortex,
particularly in the left hemisphere, can lead to disruption of movement rhythms. Although
the premotor cortex of the right hemisphere integrates spatial cues into movement, later in
movement learning left hemisphere influence predominates. Notably, the ventral portion of
premotor cortex is one important site of mirror cells that respond to observation of the
actions of others, and that fire when the observer makes the same motions. The parietal
lobe plays an indirect role in movement through spatial computation and sensory feedback
evaluation, but also a more direct role, with the intraparietal sulcus integrating spatial cues
with motion control. Finally, the cerebellum corrects movement errors.
The aforementioned areas emerged over many millions of years of evolution in the human
ancestral line. Comparative approaches suggest the beginnings of the cerebellum date back at
least 445 million years. Primary motor cortex dates from over 160 million years ago, and
interconnections between supplementary, premotor, and parietal motor areas date from
74 million years ago. By 44 million years ago the premotor cortex exerted control over distal
Praxis and handedness 179
musculature. Finally, enlargement of the cerebellum relative to the rest of the brain likely
started about 29 million years ago.
A striking aspect of human praxis is the existence of population right-handedness (PRH),
with about a 90% incidence. Its roots extend at least 15 million years ago, and possibly
further. PRH likely began with a simple anatomical imbalance, perhaps in the hand area of
the primary motor cortex, that produced a slight handedness effect in reaching, gesturing,
and feeding.
Later developments, it is proposed, built upon the initial imbalance, exploiting areas
increasingly working in tandem with primary cortex. At some point in the human-ape
line, subsequent to the divergence of orangutans 15 million years ago but prior to that of
gorillas nine million years ago, a more skilled component of praxis emerged in the left
hemisphere. It likely exploited a neurological substrate, namely ventral premotor cortex, in
the production of communication gestures as well as in tool-related learning and ma-
nipulation. Further developments in manual praxis involved the kinds of spatial manip-
ulations involved in insertion and turning motions. Their control may have started in the
right hemisphere in the common ancestor of humans and chimpanzees prior to 7.5 million
years ago, but transferred to the left in humans as part of the motor programming functions
of the human left hemisphere.
Investigations of genetic influences on human handedness have a long history, but have
most recently focused on specific genes. So far, it appears that genetic determination of
handedness is controlled by a number of genes, each having a very small impact. Such studies
lend weight to the susceptibility of handedness to evolutionary influences even though we
are far from identifying the full set of genes that make it 25% heritable.
Archaeological evidence, while sometimes controversial, consistently shows hominin
PRH as far back as inferences can be made, about 1.5 to 1.8 million years ago. It consists of
lateralized tooth scratches, asymmetric muscle attachments to bone, directional flaking of
stone tools, and depictions of hands in caves and historical art.
PRH probably exists because praxis mechanisms evolved to bring the timing and se-
quencing of movement under control of the left hemisphere, eliminating the need for their
cross-hemisphere coordination. Both monkey and human simple reaching involves the
activation of primary motor cortex opposite the hand, and in chimpanzees, reaching and
feeding handedness is related to primary cortex asymmetry. Premotor cortex plays a critical
role, with all components of PRH in chimpanzees proving to be related to inferior frontal
gyrus asymmetry. Notably, the gyrus is also a site of mirror neurons.
Left-handedness, however, has been consistently maintained at roughly 10% of the human
population, begging the question of what advantage it confers. The preliminary view that it
affords a “creative edge” appears not to be true. Its maintenance may depend instead on the
existence of confrontational fighting. By using an unfamiliar, unorthodox style, left-handers
enjoy an advantage over right-handers. Thus, the higher the level of homicide in traditional
societies, the higher the incidence of left-handedness. Left-handers also tend to predominate
in confrontational sports like martial arts and ice hockey.
11 Tools and planning
Tools were once considered uniquely human artifacts. However, scientists have known
since 1917 that chimpanzees both use and manufacture tools. In that year Wolfgang
Köhler reported that captive chimpanzees at Tenerife in the Canary Islands used sticks to
retrieve food. They sometimes inserted a thinner tube inside a thicker one to extend their
reach, or even chewed the sides off a plank of wood so it could be used as the insert. The
animals also stacked boxes to reach suspended food, and developed a “climbing jump”
using a long stick, holding it vertically and climbing quickly before it tipped over
(Köhler, 1927).
Such behaviors imply the use of a number of cognitive faculties: Spatial processes and
memory to be sure, but possibly also planning and the understanding of causality. In this
chapter we consider the use of tools by great apes, and their development of tool cultures
through social learning. We examine the relationship between brain size and tool use, as
well as the evolution of grip. Hominin tools and tool cultures are described, and ape and
human tool behaviors are compared. Last considered are the role of planning in tool
manufacturing and the understanding of causality, and changes in the brain that evolved to
support them.
DOI: 10.4324/9781003137863-14
Tools and planning 181
Might such capabilities have existed even earlier? If so they should be observed in other
apes. In this regard gorillas seem enigmatic, in captivity using and sometimes modifying sticks
into probes, weapons, and rakes, but almost never employing tools in the wild (McGrew,
1992; Parker, Kerr, Markowitz, & Gould, 1999). A notable exception is a female observed
using a stick as a depth-tester when wading a pool, and as a prop while collecting aquatic
plants (Breuer, Ndoundou-Hockemba, & Fishlock, 2005; see Figure 11.1).
Tool use by orangutans is rare in the wild (Byrne, 2004), but common and even creative in
captivity where it is perhaps unleashed by boredom (McGrew, 1992), or under a more
positive view, by curiosity resulting from contact with humans and conspecifics in a risk-free
environment (Damerius, Graber, Willems, & van Schaik, 2017). Nevertheless wild or-
angutans do use sticks to probe bees’ nests for honey, and to extract insects from nests and
seeds from fruit (Fox, Sitompul, & van Schaik, 1999).
The use of tools by the great apes, even if limited, might lead us to conclude that tool use is
phylogenetic. However, before we make that leap we might consider a warning by W.C.
McGrew (1992, p. 196):
Termite fishing may just as well have been invented in 1959, the year before Jane
Goodall arrived, or a million years ago. It may well have been invented only once by
some chimpanzee Edison… and then diffused across Africa, or it may have been re-
invented 100 or 1000 times.
McGrew’s point seems well taken. To what extent might primate tool use be cultural and
recent, and not phylogenetic and ancient?
Defining culture
Whether or not chimpanzees show evidence of culture may depend on how culture is
defined. A full definition might involve assessing a behavior’s innovation, dissemination,
standardization, durability, diffusion, and tradition, among other attributes (Davidson &
McGrew, 2005). Andrew Whiten and colleagues, however, have focused on just two
characteristics: Cultural variation, the extent to which a behavior differs between ape com-
munities, and diffusion, the extent to which behavioral similarities across communities are
attributable to social transmission (Whiten et al., 2001).
With respect to the first of these, Whiten et al. identified 39 behaviors qualifying as
cultural variants in chimpanzee communities stretching across Africa. Each variant was ha-
bitual in at least one location and absent in at least one other. While not all involved tool use
(e.g., the “rain dance”, a display at the start of heavy rain), many did (e.g., using a pestle to
pound palm crowns, or leaves to clean the body).
Diffusion, the second characteristic, is difficult to assess. As Davidson and McGrew (2005)
stated, “Processes of social transmission are extraordinarily difficult to document for animals
that cannot speak to us” (p. 806). Nevertheless, the availability of chimpanzee communities
stretching from West to East Africa allowed Whiten et al. the opportunity to identify possible
instances by examining whether contiguous communities showed similar behaviors. Of
several tool use behaviors, nut hammering was found to be consistent with a single origin
followed by diffusion to contiguous communities. Other tool behaviors were found in
noncontiguous communities and thus were more consistent with multiple origins. For ex-
ample, the use of sticks in ant dipping, clubbing, levering, and termite-fishing were judged
likely to have been invented in more than one location (Whiten et al., 2001). It is notable
that these behaviors are simpler than nut hammering, which involves assembling multiple
parts and sequencing actions. Perhaps simple behaviors are reinvented, while complex ones
tend to be invented once and socially transmitted.
Consistent with this, when naïve chimpanzees were provided with potatoes accessible only
by pounding a small opening in a container with the end of a stick, a simple behavior not
requiring sequencing or multiple parts, “stick pounding” was spontaneously invented by one
chimpanzee in each of three of four groups. No teaching was involved (Bandini & Tennie,
2019). In contrast, nut cracking, a complex skill found to develop slowly over ten years or
more, involving controlled striking of a carefully positioned nut relative to anvil and
hammer, was accompanied by active teaching of young chimpanzees by adults. Thus, a
mother might reposition a nut for a child, provide a hammer, or demonstrate the nut
cracking sequence. However, no evidence was found that chimpanzees use pointing, or
Tools and planning 183
repositioning of a child’s body parts as part of their teaching repertoire (Boesch, Bombjaková,
Meier, & Mundry, 2019).
In orangutans, cultural diffusion has been reported by van Schaik and colleagues across
locations in Sumatra and Borneo. They identified 24 likely cultural variants, a number of
them involving tools (e.g., wiping the face with squashed leaves and using a stick to scratch
the body). There proved to be a pronounced correlation with distance: The closer two
locations were, the more they resembled one another with respect to cultural variants. That is
exactly the result expected of diffusion (van Schaik et al., 2003). Similar results have been
reported in gorillas (Robbins et al., 2016).
Social learning
Diffusion presumably occurs through social learning, the acquisition of new behaviors by
observing others. A detailed example was provided by a study of wild chimpanzees at Bossou
in Guinea (Biro et al., 2003). The community was already familiar with palm nuts, and
cracking with hammer and anvil was customary. When unfamiliar coula and panda nuts were
provided, it was primarily juveniles who initially undertook their cracking, although a
number of adults eventually did as well. Notably, nut-cracking attracted interest among
others, with juveniles frequently observing their peers.
Juvenile-led innovation has been observed in several other contexts, including potato and
wheat grain washing by Japanese macaques (Choleris & Kavaliers, 1999; Schofield, McGrew,
Takahashi, & Hirata, 2018). While not a general primate characteristic, it notably exists in the
social and investigative activities of chimpanzees, and in investigative uses of objects by
capuchins (Perry, Barrett, & Godoy, 2017). Yet in the Bossou study, learning how to crack
the new nuts mostly involved individuals observing their peers or older animals, not younger
ones (Biro et al., 2003). Together these observations suggest that juveniles are more likely
than adults to introduce object-related cultural innovations, but are also more often ignored.
Perhaps the combination encourages “prudent innovation” – change is allowed, but only
slowly in case of pitfalls. In any case, the parallel with the introduction of new human fads
and customs seems irresistible.
The acquisition of tool use in an orangutan was described by Fox et al. (1999). Andai, a
juvenile, often observed her mother use sticks to probe tree holes. Two years later, she made
her own tools, which she used to probe holes that her mother had just left. Two years after
that, she was a fully competent tool maker and user who found her own holes to probe.
Significantly, during the learning phase, the mother was once observed to make her probes in
an inefficient way, by shortening them on both ends rather than one end, and Andai pro-
ceeded to make a tool in the same manner.
The propagation of an inefficient technique seems a sure tip-off to its acquisition through
observational learning (Fox et al., 1999). A similar example is the imitation by younger
chimpanzees of a manually disabled adult’s inefficient technique for self-grooming, involving
rubbing against a vine held fast by the feet (Hobaiter & Byrne, 2010). Another is the adoption
by migrating female chimpanzees, of the nut-cracking techniques of their new group (e.g.,
the use of wooden hammers), even though they previously used more efficient ones (e.g., the
use of stone hammers; Luncz, Sirianni, Mundry, & Boesch, 2018).
Nevertheless social learning can be expected to more frequently spread efficient techniques
within a group than to inhibit them. Presumably this accounts for the observation that
termite fishing behaviors are more similar among members of the same group, than they are
between groups (Boesch et al., 2020). The spread of an efficient, novel tool behavior has
even been directly observed in a chimpanzee community in Uganda. A flooded waterhole
was apparently the impetus for the dominant male to craft a moss-sponge, which he then
184 Perception and cognition
used to drink water. Over six days, seven more chimpanzees were observed doing the same.
Six had been explicitly observed watching the male doing so, and the seventh acquired the
crafting behavior immediately after using a discarded moss-sponge (Hobaiter, Poisot,
Zuberbühler, Hoppitt, & Gruber, 2014).
Such examples represent relatively passive social learning: One individual engaged in a
behavior, and another learned by watching. Active teaching, involving one individual in-
tervening to further the learning of another, is much rarer in primates. However, it has been
seen in chimpanzees in the form of “tool transfers”. Skilled females have been observed
preparing termite-fishing probes before handing them to their child or sibling. Subsequently,
the recipient “fishes” for a longer period, with more nest insertions, than in periods before
they were handed a tool. The presumption is that at minimum, providing an expertly
prepared tool affords an enhanced opportunity to learn termite fishing. But it may also
transfer knowledge about the appropriate raw materials from which to make a probe
(Musgrave, Morgan, Lonsdorf, Mundry, & Sanz, 2016). It appears that mother-to-child tool
transfers more often occur with complex tool sets than simple ones, indicating that teaching is
more intense when greater knowledge must be imparted (Musgrave et al., 2020).
In conclusion, there can be little doubt that cultural transmission through social learning
helps spread ape tool culture. However, it seems equally clear that phylogeny must play a role
as well. First, while social learning may be widespread among animal species, apes excel at it
and may be the only animals to understand requests to “do this” (Zentall, 2006). The im-
plication is that the biologically based intelligence of apes has much to do with cultural
transmission. Second, as the Whiten et al. study indicates, and as other studies have docu-
mented, many tool behaviors are continually reinvented (Bandini & Tennie, 2017, 2019;
Laumer, Call, Bugnyar, & Auersperg, 2018). That too grounds tool behaviors in the basic
intelligence of apes. When these considerations are paired with the evolutionary changes in
praxis and handedness outlined in the previous chapter, the conclusion appears unavoidable
that primate tool use is ultimately phylogenetic – and cultural.
Although apes provide the most applicable comparisons to human tool use, we now
know that many nonprimates use tools as well. Among birds, corvids (crows, ravens,
jays, magpies, etc.) show diverse tool behaviors. These include holding food with a
skewer, dropping prey on hard surfaces, using sticks to extract food from holes, and
crafting and using hooks to retrieve food ( Lefebvre et al., 2002; Rutz, Klump, et al.,
2016; Rutz, Sugasawa, van der Wal, Klump, & St. Clair, 2016).
Tools and planning 185
Tool use has also been found in about 30 aquatic species, including cephalopods,
crustaceans, and fish. The use of coconut shells for protection by octopuses has been
observed, much like hermit crabs use gastropod shells as temporary housing. Hermit
crabs also sometimes carry sea anemones on the shells, using their stinging cells to
repel predators and their weight to help maintain balance. The squirting of water jets
can be viewed as tool use and is employed by many fish and squid to attack prey,
burrow, or to attach eggs. Some fish are also known to use stone or coral anvils to
crack shellfish or to remove spines from sea urchins. Others lay their eggs on leaves,
then protect them by subsequently moving the leaves from danger. Sea otters use
stone hammers and anvils to extract the meat of shellfish, and bottlenose dolphins
sometimes “wear” sponges as protection while foraging ( Brown, 2012; Mann &
Patterson, 2013).
Among tool-using terrestrial mammals are elephants, which sometimes throw
objects at others as part of aggressive displays ( Bentley-Condit & Smith, 2010), and
which modify and use branches to ward off flies ( Deecke, 2012). A brown bear has
been observed picking up a barnacle-encrusted rock in its paws, and using it to rub its
face, perhaps to relieve itching or to remove food debris ( Deecke, 2012).
Even insects have been observed using tools. Some ants use leaves as carriers for food
( Brown, 2012). Sawfly larvae defend themselves by applying tree-derived acids to their
attackers ( Bentley-Condit & Smith, 2010).
Nonprimate animals may offer fertile ground for examining the relationship
between brain size and tool use. Corvids, which we have seen show diverse tool
behaviors, are notably large-brained among birds ( Emery, 2006). On the other hand,
preliminary analysis suggests that in fish, there is little relationship between tool
use and “residual” brain size after correcting for body weight ( Brown, 2012). In the
future, determining the conditions under which brain size predicts, or does not
predict, tool use may provide further insight into the evolutionary origins of our
own tool usage.
Intriguingly, brain size also appears informative in the case of capuchins. As New
World monkeys they seem unlikely candidates for high intelligence, but nevertheless
show EQs equivalent to those of macaques (Table 11.1). Wild capuchins of both the
Sapajus and Cebus genuses use stone hammers and anvils to crack nuts (Barrett et al.,
2018). This is accomplished not with a free-fall drop of the hammer, but with a full
follow-through of the arms and shoulders, sometimes recruiting muscles of the lower
body. The behavior is known to predate provisioning by humans, using the nuts of wild
palms (Fragaszy, Izar, Visalberghi, Ottoni, & de Oliviera, 2004). Capuchins also hammer
hard-shelled fruit, seeds, crabs, and snails (Barrett et al., 2018; De Moraes, Souto, &
Schiel, 2018).
Termite fishing affords a second behavioral resemblance between capuchins and
chimpanzees. Wild capuchins have repeatedly been observed tapping a termite nest, in-
serting a stick while rotating it, and removing the stick to eat the attached termites.
Impressively, the tapping appears to have the function of rousing the termites, increasing
the yield, while the rotation reduces stick breakage. Neither of these elaborations of
technique have been reported in chimpanzee termite fishing (Souto et al., 2011).
Generally speaking, brain size fairly well predicts tool use in primates. Macaques, the
encephalization equivalent to capuchins, have been observed using hammerstones to open
oysters and, more recently, to crack palm nuts (Proffitt et al., 2018). They also use leaves to
186 Perception and cognition
Table 11.1 Comparative encephalization quotients (EQs) among living primates, two extinct Eocene pri-
mates, and a living, related tree shrew. mya = millions of years ago
Note: Based on a 0.28 exponent. Homo sapiens value from Table 5.1. Microsyops brain and body masses from Silcox,
Benham, and Bloch (2010). Ignacius brain and body masses from Silcox, Dalmyn, and Bloch (2009). All other values
derived from Williams (2002), using means of male and female brain and body masses where available.
wrap and rub food items covered with thorns, stingers, or mud, and grass blades and plant
needles to floss teeth (Pal, Kumara, Mishra, Velankar, & Singh, 2018; Trébouet, Reichard,
Pinkaew, & Malaivijitnond, 2018). Spider monkeys, with slightly higher EQs, have been
reported using sticks as body scratchers (Lindshield & Rodrigues, 2009). Baboons, still higher
in EQ, throw sticks at rivals during aggressive displays (Cheney & Seyfarth, 2007, p. 187).
Perhaps we should be impressed that a gross measure of encephalization predicts as well
as it does, in that tool-relevant brain mechanisms will almost certainly be found to also vary
across species.
A final indication that EQ is related to cognitive performance, of which tool-related
behaviors are part, is afforded by a meta-analysis performed by Deaner, van Schaik, and
Johnson (2006). Using the results of previous studies, they derived a global measure of in-
telligence for each of 24 primate genuses. The measure was based on the abilities to detour,
problem-solve, learn, sort, delay response, and use tools, among others. Across 13 genuses in
Table 11.1 for which both the learning and EQ measures are available, the measures correlate
at a whopping r = .87 level. Genuses with higher EQs show higher cognitive performance.
In summary a case can be made that in primates at least, tool use is influenced by a species’
general intelligence and that this is related to encephalization. As we saw in Chapter 10, an
additional role is played by the evolution of specific brain mechanisms relative to praxis. But
as the next section outlines, the physical hand itself is a third factor affecting the evolution of
tool use and manufacture.
Tools and planning 187
Figure 11.2 Orangutan using a precision grip involving the forefinger and thumb tip.
Source: Orangutan using precision grip.jpg, CC BY-SA 4.0, William H. Calvin.
The Gona tools resemble the Oldowan toolkit, named for Olduvai Gorge in Tanzania
where 1.9 million-year-old tools were found and attributed either to australopithecines or to
Homo habilis. In recent years, however, Oldowan-like tools have also been found in the
Republic of Georgia in Eurasia. They date to about 1.8 million years ago (Mgeladze et al.,
2011). They are thought to have been produced by Homo erectus. Assemblages of Oldowan
tools, dating to 1.3 to 1.6 million years ago, have also been found associated with Homo erectus
crania in Ethiopia (Semaw et al., 2020).
Roughly contemporaneous with some of the older tools are cut marks on bone, from
Bouri, Ethiopia, dating to 2.5 million years ago. Hammerstone impact marks on a shattered
bovine tibia also indicate the extraction of bone marrow. These discoveries are from layers
associated with fossil remains of Australopithecus garhi, suggesting that it was the toolmaker
(de Heinzelin et al., 1999). A claim of even earlier cut marks dating to 3.4 million years
ago (McPherron et al., 2010), was initially criticized on the basis that they may have
resulted from trampling (Domínguez-Rodrigo, Pickering, & Bunn, 2010). However, the
claim has been made plausible by the subsequent reporting of actual stone tools from 3.3
million years ago, and studies indicating that trampled bones do not show similar cut marks
(Thompson et al., 2015).
Tools and planning 191
That early hominin tool kits were effective in allowing the consumption of meat and
marrow is suggested by an analysis of carnivore extinctions in East Africa (Faurby,
Silvestro, Werdkin, & Antonelli, 2020). Large carnivores underwent progressive ex-
tinction there, from four million years ago to the present time. As the researchers pointed
out, the beginning of the decline corresponded to the first achievement of hominin brain
volumes above 500 cc, and indeed, the first EQs over 20 (see Table 5.1). The pre-
sumption is that increasing intelligence, and an increasingly effective toolkit, allowed
hominins to compete with large carnivores, either through direct hunting, scavenging, or
the stealing of kills. Importantly, this effect was not seen in small African carnivores, or in
carnivores in North America, where hominins were absent until recently. Competing
hypotheses, such as changes in climate or forest cover, also did not account for the effect
(Faurby et al., 2020).
Later tools
Tools evolved much as species did, with more successful ones replacing less successful ones.
In general, later stone tools are characterized by increasing complexity and diversity.
192 Perception and cognition
Acheulean tools
After Lomekwian and Oldowan tools, the next tool culture to arise was the Acheulean, be-
ginning about 1.8 million years ago (Table 11.2) and named for the archaeological site of St.
Acheul in France. Handaxes first arose in this culture (Kooyman, 2000). The Ethiopian sites
where Oldowan tool assemblages are found, also host Acheulian tools in the 1.3 to 1.6
million-year-old range, and are likewise associated with Homo erectus (Semaw et al., 2020).
However, caution should be observed when identifying particular tool industries with
particular periods of time, because the period during which an industry existed depends on
the geographic location. Nevertheless it is useful to give approximate dates, as in Table 11.2,
in order to interpret other cultural remains (e.g., artwork, needles, and hearths) associated
with the cultures.
The names of the tool cultures generally reflect the geographic locations from which they
were first or best known. Thus, the Oldowan industry is named for the Olduvai Gorge in
Tanzania. Nevertheless, it should not be assumed that an industry is found only in the lo-
cation for which it was named, because most spread widely. As for why the Lower, Middle,
and Upper Paleolithic periods are so named, “think dig”. When digging down, the upper
layers are the youngest, the middle layers are the next youngest, and the lower layers are the
oldest.
Acheulean tools originated in Africa, where they are first known from 1.8 million years
ago (Diez-Martin et al., 2015; Lepre et al., 2011). However, they spread widely throughout
Africa, Europe, and Asia as far as India and Korea (Gowlett, 1992b). There is evidence that
the spread was literal, because variability in the tools is highest in Africa and lowest in areas
furthest from Africa, exactly as one would expect from transmission through repeated mi-
grations (Lycett & von Cramon-Taubadel, 2008).
These tools were substantially more complex than their predecessors, incorporating
symmetry around one or two axes, and were often heavily worked so that little or no raw
stone surface was left exposed (Gowlett, 1992a; Kooyman, 2000). When Acheulean tools
from a particular location are compared over time, skill is found to increase over hundreds of
thousands of years, improving both symmetry and the straightness of edges (Shipton, 2018).
The high degree of symmetry of some later Acheulean handaxes strikes many modern
viewers as quite beautiful, appealing to an aesthetic that their makers may have felt as well
(Figure 11.4). Yet they were also efficient tools, probably notably more efficient at butchery
than the previously used stone flakes (Galán & Domínguez-Rodrigo, 2014). Because such
highly symmetric tools are not produced in modern experiments that randomly remove
Table 11.2 Some prominent tool cultures, with very approximate ages and geological periods (kya =
thousands of years ago). Ages largely derived from Gowlett (1992b) and Jones, Martin, and
Pilbeam (1992)
protrusions from a core, there is good reason to believe that top-down, planful processing
was employed during their manufacture (Moore, 2020).
One late development in the Acheulean tradition was the Levallois technique, involving
removing many flakes from a core in preparation for striking off one desired flake that with
minimal or no retouching (secondary flaking along the edge) became the sought-after tool
(Gowlett, 1992b; Figure 11.5).
The advantage to this technique was that through careful preparation of the core, a tool
exactly the desired size and shape could be obtained, with a sharper edge than was possible
using earlier techniques (Kooyman, 2000). Achieving this provides another instance of the
use of top-down, planful processing (Moore, 2020).
What Acheulean tools were used for, besides butchery, has been a point of con-
troversy. However, analysis of phytoliths (microscopic mineral particles formed by plants)
found on the working surfaces of Acheulean handaxes from about 1.5 million years ago
indicate that they were used for chopping wood (Dominguez-Rodrigo, Serrallonga,
Juan-Tresserras, Alcala, & Luque, 2001). The finding is important in revealing that
woodworking occurred at a remote time from which wooden artifacts themselves have
194 Perception and cognition
Figure 11.5 Levallois technique, showing the desired flake removed from the core.
Source: Adaptation of Production of points & spearheads from a flint stone core, Levallois technique, Mousterian Culture,
Tabun Cave, 250,000–50,000 BP (detail).jpg, public domain, Gary Todd.
not survived. However there is also evidence that points of Acheulean age, in this case
500,000 years ago, were hafted to shafts to make spears, becoming the first known in-
stances of composite tools (tools made of more than one material). Presumably they were
used for hunting (Wilkins, Schoville, Brown, & Chazan, 2012). The varied uses of the
Acheulean handaxe have led to its being described as the “Swiss army knife” of its age
(Calvin, 2002).
While surviving Acheulean tools are almost always made of stone, recent analyses of ar-
tifacts recovered from Acheulean sites in the Olduvai Gorge show that a tiny percentage
were made of bone. Most were apparently used as stone reworkers, anvils, choppers, or
cutting tools, One, however, was in the shape of a hunting point with three barbs, It was in
excess of 700,000 years old, the earliest bone point known, and was mostly likely made by
Homo erectus (Pante, de la Torre, d’Errico, Njau, & Blumenschine, 2020).
Mousterian tools
The Mousterian toolkit, named for Le Moustier, France, is largely though not exclusively
associated with Neanderthals. The emphasis was on discoids and on Levallois flakes
used as scrapers. Small stone points with true tangs, suggesting hafting onto a shaft,
developed out of this tradition and are found in northwest Africa, dating to about 22,000
to 45,000 years ago (Gowlett, 1992b; Straus, 2001). Also, arrow points made of bone,
more than 60,000 years old, have been reported in South Africa. They are thought to
have been inserted in a shaft and secured with a wrapped collar (Backwell, d’Errico, &
Wadley, 2008).
Tools and planning 195
Aurignacian tools
Aurignacian culture, which extended from Spain to Russia (Gowlett, 1992b), was marked
by an increase in microblades (small blades less than 5 cm in length) and microliths (small
blades with at least three distinct sides). The latter were mounted in series to form a
continuous edge. By thus combining stone with a mounting material, a composite tool was
created (Kooyman, 2000). Aurignacian culture also created bone points. Bone and antler
were common toolmaking materials although wood was undoubtedly used as well
(Gowlett, 1992b).
Magdalenian tools
Magdalenian culture introduced harpoons, lance-heads, and spearthrowers, made from bone and
antler. The spearthrowers were often decorated with carvings of animals, birds, and fish. Bone
needles, complete with eyes, attest to the wearing of clothing (Clark, 1967; Gowlett, 1992b;
Figure 11.8), although the invention of clothing must have substantially predated this culture
(see Box 11.2). Microlithic backed stone blades were also common (Kooyman, 2000).
Important tool uses have included the preparation of hides and textiles for clothing and
shelter, and the creation of fire. To some extent, these thermoregulatory uses of tools
must be linked to the reduction of body hair in the hominin line, or in other words to
what Markus Rantala has picturesquely called “the great denudation”. While the
density of hair follicles does not differ significantly between humans and other apes,
hair size does, and as a result humans have nakeder skin. Thus, in our ancestry there has
been hair loss that can be characterized as a reduction in coat thickness ( Rantala, 2007).
In the past there has been little consensus on whether the use of clothing, fire, and
portable shelters accompanied the loss of hair, or followed it much later as hominins
moved into colder climates. To begin addressing this question, Rogers, Iltis, and
Wooding (2004) examined changes in the MC1R gene. The gene affects skin color
and was selected on the assumption that the loss of body hair must have been
accompanied by darkening skin as protection against the sun. Using a molecular-clock
approach, the researchers concluded that the great denudation occurred about 1.2
million years ago. Interestingly, this date corresponds to a split in the human louse line,
with different subspecies of lice beginning to diverge at about that time ( Light & Reed,
2009). Certainly the loss of human body hair would have had implications for body
parasites.
198 Perception and cognition
But at what point was clothing adopted? Based on a later genetic divergence
between human head lice and body lice, it has been argued that clothing first appeared
83,000 to 170,000 years ago. Thus, the much earlier denudation is assumed to have
confined lice to the head, which then moved to the body and genetically diverged
once clothing was adopted. The 83,000 to 170,000-year-old date is believed to be
prior to the first permanent passage of modern human beings into Europe, suggesting
that clothing was invented before the successful removal of Homo sapiens to cold
climates ( Toups, Kitchen, Light, & Reed, 2010). However, this leaves unclear how
earlier hominins were able to survive in the cool parts of Europe before 500,000 years
ago ( Hosfield, 2016).
Direct evidence of clothing is very limited beyond a few thousand years ago. The
oldest known clothing dates back 20,000 years, and dyed flax fibers about 33,000 years.
Less direct evidence includes eyed needles at 30,000 to 35,000 years, and bone awls at
76,000 years or more ( Hoffecker, 2005; Kvavadze et al., 2009; Rantala, 2007).
Although scraped hides likely extend back about 300,000 years ( Kvavadze et al.,
2009; Rantala, 2007), hides can be used for either clothing or shelter ( Toups et al.,
2010). If they were then used for clothing, it may have been in a simple form involving
draping a single layer loosely over the body, providing only a small amount of warmth.
Complex clothing, consisting of fitted pieces sewn together to more tightly fit the
arms, legs, and torso, might then be inferred from the much later awls and needles.
They would have been warmer, and they would have allowed layering for even greater
warmth ( Gilligan, 2010). Perhaps it was this form of clothing that appeared 83,000 to
170,000 years ago.
The material evidence relating to the use of fire is also limited. In Africa, it may
date as far back as Homo ergaster or erectus 1.5 to 1.7 million years ago. Yet a heating
purpose cannot necessarily be assumed because fire can also be used for light,
protection against predators, cooking, and socializing ( Beaumont, 2011; Gowlett,
2016). Although there is some evidence from an Israeli site that fire was used to roast
grains and fruit 780,000 years ago ( Melamed, Kislev, Geffen, Lev-Yadun, & Goren-
Inbar, 2016), in colder European climates there is no evidence of any intentional use
of fire until after 450,000 years ago ( Roebroeks & Villa, 2011). Thus, the story of
the great denudation, and its implications for clothing, shelter, and heating in colder
parts of the world, is likely to prove a complicated one to unravel.
Clovis tools
Finally, the Clovis culture is specific to central North America and is named for a site near
the town of Clovis, New Mexico. It is known for elaborately worked stone points with a
Tools and planning 199
fluted base that allowed firmer attachment to a shaft. Clovis hunters took down large game
including mammoths, mastodons, camels, and bison (Gowlett, 1992b; Waguespack &
Surovell, 2003).
Figure 11.9 View of the floresiensis endocast from the (a) side, (b) back, (c) front, and (d) top. Note the
substantial bulges, labeled as feature 6, at the frontal pole.
Source: Virtual endocast of LB1, licensed from Elsevier under STM Permissions Guidelines.
Armstrong, Schleicher, Zilles, & Van Hoesen, 2001). They found that humans have a
greatly enlarged area 10, comprising 1.2% of brain volume compared to 0.5% to 0.7% in
the great apes and only 0.2% in gibbons. Combining these findings with other data,
Semendeferi et al. proposed that in an initial ancestral state now approximated by gibbons,
area 10 occupied a limited area nearest the eyes. This was then followed by two expan-
sions: One in a common ancestor of great apes and humans, and the other more specifically
in humans when area 10 greatly expanded in size and connectivity with other brain areas
(Semendeferi et al., 2001). In concert with this, area 10 matures more slowly than other
brain areas in both chimpanzees and humans, and more slowly in humans than in chim-
panzees (Teffer et al., 2013).
The expansion’s nature is clarified by the discovery that while area 10 comprises 1.2% of
our brain volume, it has only 0.3% of the brain’s neurons (Barton & Venditti, 2013). That
means there must be unusually profuse connections between area 10 neurons, taking up
volume that might otherwise be filled by cell bodies. Indeed, pyramidal cells in area 10 are
found to have larger dendritic arbors (the treelike branchings of dendrites) relative to several
comparison areas (3, 18, and 4; see Figure 10.4 for these areas). The same is also true of
chimpanzees (Bianchi et al., 2013). It appears, therefore, that area 10 has long been under
selective pressure for increased cellular interconnectivity, going back at least to the
chimpanzee-human common ancestor.
202 Perception and cognition
The cognitive role of area 10
If area 10 expanded in connectivity during ape evolution and in size during hominin evo-
lution, we might expect that it serves a critically important role in cognition. In fact that
appears to be true. For one thing it is involved in human working memory, a form of memory
that holds and manipulates information over the short term. Thus, a review of brain-imaging
studies found that working memory activates the lateral portions of area 10 (Gilbert et al.,
2006). While other cortical areas may play an overall larger role in working memory (Cabeza
& Nyberg, 2000), area 10 more specifically evaluates its contents (Leung, Gore, & Goldman-
Rakic, 2005). In addition the anterior portion of the area is involved in rapidly switching
between tasks (Gilbert et al., 2006). Consistent with that, it appears to play a role in eval-
uating alternative goals and then allocating cognitive resources accordingly (Mansouri,
Koechlin, Rosa, & Buckley, 2017).
The role of area 10 in evaluating the content of working memory and in task switching has
clear applicability to the manufacturing of stone tools. Knapping involves keeping in mind a
goal state, evaluating the emerging tool in terms of that state, and repeated switching between
evaluative and action-oriented phases. The area 10 processing load would have been parti-
cularly demanding in a small-brained species like floresiensis (and perhaps not in modern large-
brained knappers, as suggested by the silence of area 10 in the brain imaging results of Stout,
Toth, Schick, & Chaminade, 2008). Together, the evaluation and task switching functions of
area 10 seem almost synonymous with planning, if planning is understood to include not just
the initial plan but its updating over time.
Other brain-imaging research confirms a role of area 10 in planning. In human experi-
ments, the Tower of London task is one of the most commonly used planning tasks, in-
volving moving beads one-at-a-time to transform a beginning state to a desired goal state
(Figure 11.10). Problems vary in complexity, e.g., from one to seven moves. While area 10
activation is not always apparent overall when the task is performed (Boghi et al., 2006),
research usually finds that its activation increases with increasing difficulty (Schall et al., 2003;
van den Heuvel et al., 2003). Also, better performance is associated with increased activation
in the area (Unterrainer et al., 2004). Both findings indicate area involvement in the task.
Further supporting the role of area 10 in planning, Roca and colleagues found that damage
to it impaired the ability to multitask while operating a virtual hotel; specifically, to perform
several subgoals while keeping in mind an overall goal. Patients with damage elsewhere in the
brain had less difficulty (Roca et al., 2011).
Understanding causality
Much of the planning using area 10 may only involve heuristics (rules of thumb that tend
toward the solution of a problem). The similarity heuristic, for example, entails making the
current state more similar to the desired end state. Followed to its conclusion, this often
results in a correctly crafted tool or a solved Tower of London problem. However, more
complex problem solving can be achieved if an understanding of causality is also brought to
bear. For example, in some of the more complex Tower of London problems, a move must
be made that decreases the similarity between the current and end states, in the interest of
allowing future moves that will again increase similarity. This requires understanding causal
relationships across multiple moves: “If I do this, then I can do that and that”.
Wolpert (2007) has argued that compared to us, apes have a very limited notion of
causality. To be sure, they can learn to associate an action with an outcome, but often lack
insight into why one leads to the other. Thus, Wolpert cited work by Povinelli, Reaux,
Theall, and Giambrone (2000) indicating that a chimpanzee may understand that tool
contact is necessary to reach food, but not understand that a hook at the end of the tool can
be used to pull the food within reach. Also, when raking food toward them with a pit
obstructing the path, few chimpanzees reliably select a pathway that avoids the food falling
into the pit. These failures suggest a very limited understanding of causality. Yet Wolpert
(2007) did allow that there is occasionally some understanding, “at the edge, even just over
the edge” as he put it (p. 59), as in the assembly of hammers and anvils and the leveling of
the anvils for nut-cracking.
The same “just over the edge” conclusion may apply to the solving of mazes, which
younger chimpanzees can plan out to two steps (Völter & Call, 2014). Some notion of
causality must be present, as the animal must understand that the solution of one step affects
the next. However, this does not detract from the general conclusion that their causal
understanding is limited.
It is not clear that area 10 supports causal understanding, so much as it supports the
comparison of the current and end states of a problem in working memory, an evaluation of
the discrepancy, and any task switching needed to reach a solution. There is, however,
another brain area that seems more clearly related to the understanding of causality. In
humans, but not in macaques, a portion of the inferior parietal lobe becomes active in the left
hemisphere during the observation of tool use. More importantly, its activity is contingent on
understanding the goal of using the tool. For example, the area is activated when participants
view a small rake dragging an object, but not when they view the rake moving alone without
an object. Thus, the area, more specifically the anterior portion of the supramarginal gyrus
(Brodmann area 40, see Figure 10.4), appears to grasp causal relations between the intended
use of the tool and the results of using it (Peeters et al., 2009).
Furthermore, tasks requiring the understanding of spatial causality without the use of tools,
such as deciding whether a ball striking another caused it to move, activate the supramarginal
gyrus on the right side (Straube & Chatterjee, 2010; Wende et al., 2013). Thus, the gyrus
204 Perception and cognition
seems generally involved in understanding physical causality, with its left-versus-right
manifestation depending on specific aspects of the task.
An understanding of causality is important to planning because it produces solutions when
simple heuristics fail. Thus, we have no difficulty solving the rake-and-pit problem failed by
chimpanzees, because we understand that dragging the rake straight toward us will cause the
food to fall into the pit, and that moving the rake sideways at the right place will avoid that.
Understanding causal relations is one of the things allowing us to innovate when solving
problems.
The fact that humans, but not macaques, have the localized parietal lobe mechanism for
understanding physical causality, dovetails with the observation that beginning with the
genus Homo about 2.4 million years ago, inferior parietal cortex visibly acquired new gyri.
They were specifically the supramarginal gyrus and the angular gyrus, Brodmann areas
40 and 39 (Glover, 2004; Tobias, 1995). It appears, therefore, that humans acquired a new
brain area to allow a deeper appreciation of physical causal relationships. Together with the
planning functions of area 10, these changes helped launch us on the pathway to ever more
sophisticated technologies.
Conclusion
Apes and some monkeys show remarkably diverse tool manufacture and use, which can take
on a cultural dimension and show evidence of diffusion across neighboring communities.
Social learning is the likely mechanism of diffusion, with innovations often introduced by
younger animals. However, most copying of behavior proceeds either peer-to-peer or from
older to younger individuals. In apes, social learning is aided by an ability to understand
requests to “do this”. Actual teaching behaviors are observed for complex skills like nut
cracking, but simpler tasks may involve repeated reinvention instead of teaching.
Across primate species, there is some indication of a positive relationship between
encephalization and the ability to use tools. Thus, primate tool use appears to be both
phylogenetic and cultural in nature.
While precision grip is more widespread among species than was previously appreciated,
humans unlike other apes have a truly opposable thumb that can make pad-to-pad contact
with the index finger. The relatively long thumbs that are possibly a precondition for this,
may date back as far as our common ancestor with gorillas 10 million years ago.
Compared to other primates, humans have evolved both greater hand dexterity and more
forceful grips. Specific evolutionary acquisitions include a hand more capable of cupping
various shapes of objects, a longer thumb relative to the fingers, a more hinge-like thumb
joint, tip-stabilized finger pads that are otherwise mobile, and a horseshoe-shaped crest in the
distal thumb that increases its gripping power.
The result, in part attributable to such influences, was the emergence of worked stone
tools by 3.3 million years ago, developing out of what was presumably an earlier vegetation-
and hammer-and-anvil-based tool tradition. Their maker was probably either Australopithecus
afarensis or Kenyanthropus platyops. In contrast Australopithecus garhi may have manufactured
the tools found at Gona, Ethiopia, dating from 2.6 million years ago.
It seems likely that the biggest factors in the emergence of worked stone tools were
cognitive in nature. Stone tool design progressed from the relatively crude Lomekwian
and Oldowan styles to the highly symmetrical and heavily flaked Acheulean style about
1.8 million years ago. Some later Acheulean tools were made of bone, and include a
barbed hunting point over 700,000 years old. Composite tools adopting other materials
are first known to have appeared about 500,000 years ago, and by about 45,000 years ago
incorporated a variety of forms and materials.
Tools and planning 205
The mental model of a stone tool requires a high degree of three-dimensional spatial
processing, and achieving a satisfactory product requires a mental script including planning
and plan revision. Although apes follow scripts in their tool manufacture and use, human
scripts are more complex, involve more spatial planning, and are more likely to defer success
or failure into the future. Here there appears to be a strong influence of encephalization, with
nearly the same EQ achieved by the likely makers of Lomekwian tools and by the floresiensis
toolmakers on the island of Flores. The relative preservation of Brodmann area 10 in the
dwarfed brain of the latter likely reflects its importance in planning, through its evaluation of
the contents of working memory and its roles in task switching, evaluation of alternative
goals, and allocation of cognitive resources.
In summary, then, it appears that tool use and manufacture on the one hand, and cognitive
evolution on the other hand, have long had intertwined histories. Primate tool use may
depend in part on reaching macaque or capuchin levels of encephalization. However,
changes of brain function and associated structure, and not just size, are likely to have been
equally important. In humans, the development of brain areas such as prefrontal cortex and
the supramarginal gyrus was probably key. The latter in particular seems to be involved in
comprehending physical causality. It and neighboring areas in the inferior parietal lobe
became visible in the human brain beginning with the genus Homo, presumably aiding in
the manufacture of increasingly sophisticated tools.
12 Spatial perception
When an early Euglena-like organism first chased the sun by altering movement of its
flagellum in response to the shading of a photoreceptor, a long evolution of spatial be-
havior was begun. Today, our capacities to identify the location, depth, motion, and
quantity of objects represent some of our most important perceptual abilities. The result
is that we can successfully navigate an environment much more complex than anything
encountered by Euglena.
In this chapter we examine several kinds of spatial information, consider how it is pro-
cessed by complex organisms with emphasis on humans and other primates, and trace
evolutionary changes as far back as data permit us.
Location
The first kind of spatial information is location. Early on, it was sensed using the simple
optical properties of the eye. Thus, a lensless pit eye was capable of determining the general
direction of light through the alignment of photoreceptors with both the light source and the
eye’s aperture (see Figure 7.2). As the pit deepened and the aperture shrunk, approximating a
pinhole eye, localization became more precise because of a substantially improved image
on the retina. Finally, in the lensed eye there was a sharply imaged, one-to-one mapping of
source points to retinal points. Thus, in a straightforward evolutionary sequence, successive
improvements in the structure and physics of the eye led to a more finely tuned ability to
locate a light source.
Of course the eye tells only part of the story, and we must look to its projections to
the brain and the brain itself to complete it. In the human eye, the output of some
130 million receptors are funneled to the brain over 1.2 million optic nerve axons, an
average compression ratio of over 100:1. As we have seen, much of the compression is
accomplished by compromising peripheral vision. Thus, massive numbers of receptors
in the periphery funnel down to relatively few ganglion cells, producing substantial in-
formation compression but poor acuity. In comparison there is little funneling from
receptors in the fovea to a relatively large number of ganglion cells, producing little
compression and high acuity.
These patterns are repeated at V1, the primary visual cortex, where a cortical magnification
factor is apparent. This is a mathematical relationship between the acuteness of an area of
vision and the amount of cortex serving it, so that more cortex is devoted to sharper areas of
vision. As a result much larger numbers of cortical neurons are devoted to an area of the fovea
than to a same-size area of peripheral vision (Born, Trott, & Hartmann, 2015).
In turn, acuity determines the precision of spatial localization. This can be measured by
Vernier acuity, the ability to detect whether two line segments are continuations of the same
line or rather are offset from one another. Vernier acuity is much worse in the periphery than
DOI: 10.4324/9781003137863-15
Spatial perception 207
in central vision. An impression of this can be gained by averting central vision away from the
lines in Figure 12.1, which makes detection of the offset much more difficult. Thus, the
ability to spatially localize the lines is positively related to the degree of cortical magnification
for their location on the retina (Greenwood, Szinte, Sayim, & Cavanagh, 2017).
Figure 12.1 Demonstration of Vernier acuity. The left lines are aligned, while the right ones are slightly offset.
Source: Original figure.
208 Perception and cognition
Relative acuity across primate species is fairly well understood, using a version of Vernier
acuity in which two lines are perceived if the gap is discriminable but one line if not.
Measured in that way, acuity in apes (including man) and macaques is nearly twice as good as
it is in strepsirrhines and New World monkeys (Ross, 2000).
Evolutionarily speaking, what happened is that in diurnal anthropoids – including apes,
humans, and macaques – the cornea and lens became reduced in size relative to the rest of the
eye. That allowed their positioning further from the retina, increasing the focal length
(the distance required for an image to come to a focus). In turn that magnified the image on
the retina (Ross & Kirk, 2007). The more magnification, the greater the acuity, because any
given part of the image was spread over more receptors. Night vision degraded, however,
because smaller corneas and lenses meant less light-gathering power. But in diurnal an-
thropoids that did not pose a significant problem, because life’s critical activities were per-
formed in daylight (Ross & Kirk, 2007). These changes to the cornea and lens must have
taken place close to the origin of anthropoids 66 million years ago.
Located in the medial temporal lobe, the hippocampus is a ridge of gray matter
roughly in the shape of a sea horse: Thus, its name, which is Latin for sea horse. It
makes up part of the ventral stream of processing, and is the site of place cells, neurons
that respond to particular locations in the environment. Place cells in turn connect to
grid cells, neurons that respond to locations in an overall grid representation of the
environment. Because the representation is environmental – the grid is in effect a
map – it is allocentric. Grid cells can be found both in the hippocampus and in
neighboring cortex ( Jacobs et al., 2013).
Both place and grid cells have been found in humans undergoing electrode
placements in their brains to locate the source of epilepsy prior to possible surgical
treatment ( Ekstrom et al., 2003; Jacobs et al., 2013). They have also been found in
rhesus macaques ( Killian, Jutras, & Buffalo, 2012), rats and mice ( Fyhn, Hafting,
Witter, Moser, & Moser, 2008), and bats ( Yartsev, Witter, & Ulanovsky, 2011).
Furthermore the grid cells of bats are very similar to those of rats, e.g., in grid layout,
cell layering, and the correlation of cell firing with movement velocity ( Yartsev et al.,
2011). That suggests that they, and presumably place cells as well, have a very long
evolutionary history dating back beyond the last common ancestor of bats, rats, and
humans – which lived a bit in excess of 80 million years ago ( Springer et al. 2017;
Tarver et al., 2016).
There is evidence that the size of the hippocampus is related to how much it is used
in spatial processing of the environment. Birds that cache food for later retrieval, such
as the marsh tit and black-capped chickadee, have larger hippocampi than related
species that do not cache food (for the examples given, the bridled titmouse and the
Mexican chickadee). Similar results apply in rodents ( Roth, Brodin, Smulders,
LaDage, & Pravosudov, 2010). They have even been found in humans: University
students who better learned a mental map of campus, as evidenced in a task requiring
them to point to relative locations while blindfolded, had larger posterior hippocampi
than those who performed more poorly ( Schinazi, Nardi, Newcombe, Shipley, &
Epstein, 2013). This confirmed other studies examining navigation in taxi drivers
( Woollett & Maguire, 2011).
Other studies indicate that the area of activation involved in egocentric location judgments
may be narrowed down to area 7 and the bordering posterior intraparietal sulcus (Zaehle
et al., 2007). Furthermore, when hemispheric differences are found, the right hemisphere is
more involved than the left (Iachini, Ruggiero, Conson, & Trojano, 2009). Right hemi-
sphere lateralization is also evident in visual field studies, in which a dot is presented in the
left or right visual field and the task requires subsequent report of the location. Accuracy is
210 Perception and cognition
generally higher in the left visual field, projecting to the right hemisphere (Boles, Barth, &
Merrill, 2008).
It is no accident that egocentric location processing emphasizes the dorsal pathway and the
parietal lobe, while allocentric processing emphasizes the ventral pathway and the temporal
lobe. Recall that the parietal lobe is also the principal location of somesthetic processing.
In representing the body’s surface, SI is highly egocentric. Thus, evolution appears to have
unified egocentric representation by placing the tactile and visual frames of reference in
proximity within the same lobe. Indeed, in the macaque many neurons in the superior
parietal lobe respond to both visual and somesthetic stimulation (Gamberini et al., 2018;
Gamberini, Galletti, Bosco, Breveglieri, & Fattori, 2011). In contrast, allocentric location
processing depends heavily on referencing objects in the environment. As we shall see in
the next chapter, object recognition is largely performed in the temporal lobe as part of the
“what” pathway (Figure 12.2).
In the macaque, the intraparietal sulcus (Figure 12.3) has neurons that code the coordinates
of visual stimuli in a head-centered, egocentric frame of reference. There is substantial
sensitivity of the sulcus to motion of both the stimulus and the body, providing spatial
updating that prepares the animal for movement (Grefkes & Fink, 2005).
The use of area 7 and the intraparietal sulcus for egocentric processing may trace to early
primates. Galagos, which are strepsirrhines, have a large posterior parietal area, while our
close nonprimate relatives the tree shrews do not (Kaas, 2013). However, egocentric
processing is performed by species as diverse as mice, birds, and insects (Benhamou &
Poucet, 1996; Bruck, Allen, Brass, Horn, & Campbell, 2017), presumably using other
neural mechanisms.
Depth
Other processes are devoted to localization in a dimension not yet considered, the one in-
volving positions nearer to or farther from the observer. Depth can be computed from many
cues, but four are particularly powerful. Three are monocular cues because they can be seen
by a single eye, as opposed to the fourth, which is a binocular cue (requiring two eyes to see).
The monocular cues are occlusion, in which an object whose image partially blocks another
is judged closer; relative size, in which the images of two similar objects are compared and the
1,2,3 7
intraparietal sulcus
40 39
43
Figure 12.3 Major features of the parietal lobe, referenced to numbered Brodmann areas.
Source: Adaptation of Gray726-Brodman.svg, public domain, Henry Vandyke Carter and Tkgd2007.
Spatial perception 211
larger is judged closer; and height in the visual field, in which the images of objects lower in the
visual field are usually judged closer than those higher in the visual field (Glueck, Crane,
Anderson, Rutnik, & Khan, 2009). All emerge from location processing, as well as more
general pattern recognition processes covered in the next chapter.
Here, attention focuses on the binocular cue specifically. It is called binocular disparity,
the discrepancy between images from the two eyes. In order to compare images across eyes
for the purpose of detecting disparities, it is necessary for them to come together in the
primary visual cortex (V1). As we have seen, after the optic nerves leave the eyes, they split
with the inner halves crossing over and the outer ones passing straight back (Figure 7.6). As
a result the inner half of the one eye and the outer half of the other eye, both surveilling
the same visual field, project to the same hemisphere of the brain. The outputs from their
images come together and are compared in V1, which has cells that directly detect bi-
nocular disparity.
Across species, the use of binocular disparity as a depth cue depends heavily on the spatial
arrangement of the eyes. A minimal requirement is that the eyes be capable of imaging the
same object at the same time, achieving binocular convergence. This is not as trivial as it seems
since many species (e.g., horses) have eyes located on the sides of the head nearly opposite
one another. Because such species have only a small overlap between what the two eyes see,
binocular disparity cannot be used as a depth cue in most of the visual field. Other species,
however, including primates, have their eyes shifted forward so they face the same direction.
Images with much greater overlap are produced, providing disparity cues throughout the
visual field.
Binocular vision has a deep history within primate ancestry. Even the earliest primates
showed substantial binocular convergence. Convergence can be measured as the binocular field
of view (BFOV), i.e., the opening angle of the joint field of view. If you close your left eye
and look straight ahead, you can move a fingertip left until it disappears behind your nose
about 70° left of center. If you repeat the process with the right eye, the fingertip disappears
about 70° right of center. Both eyes can see everything within those limits, a joint field of
view of about 140°.
Rats at 40–60° and tree shrews (close relatives of primates) at 60° have BFOVs less than
half that amount. In contrast the early Eocene primate species, Teilhardina asiatica, dating to
55 million years ago, had an estimated BFOV of about 110–120°. That value is not much less
than the 127–128° value of tarsiers, the 135° value of galagos, the 138–146° values of New
World owl and squirrel monkeys, and even the 140° to 160° values of macaques and modern
humans (Heesy, 2004; Ross, 2000).
Thus, binocular convergence was largely in place during an early phase of primate evo-
lution, increasing only modestly during our descent. It testifies to an origin in nocturnal
predation, as it is believed to have been used by early primates to counter camouflage by
detecting depth disparities between an insect and its surroundings (Heesy, 2008; Ni, Wang,
Hu, & Li, 2004). In fact predators in general, not just primates, tend to have front-facing
vision, because the overlap between the eyes provides good depth perception to discern and
attack prey. In contrast prey animals tend to have side-facing vision with little binocular
overlap, for maximal surveillance of the environment.
Another factor is that larger bodies have greater separation between the eyes, increasing
disparity cues and improving depth perception. In fact in our ancestral line, eye separation
increased dramatically as body sizes increased. Thus, Teilhardina asiatica weighed only about
1 ounce and had an inner interorbital distance (the space between the eyes, as measured
between their nearest edges inside the skull) measuring only 4 mm (Ni et al., 2004). In
contrast the typical interorbital distance in modern humans is on the order of 20–30 mm
(Mafee et al., 1986).
212 Perception and cognition
Figure 12.4 A random-dot stereogram. Slightly crossing the eyes so that the squares lay atop one another and
fuse, produces a perception of abstract shapes in depth.
Source: Adaptation of Stereogram Tut Random Dot Shark.png, CC BY-SA 3.0, Fred Hsu.
Depth perception, however, requires further cortical processing beyond mere con-
vergence between the eyes in V1. Tsao and colleagues (2003) used random-dot stereograms
(simpler but otherwise similar in structure to Figure 12.4) to examine brain mechanisms of
depth perception in both humans and macaques. Binocular fusion of two stereogram views
resulted either in a perception of depth or of no depth. When depth was present, there were
widespread increases in cortical activation that were strongest in both species in area V3a and
the caudal (rear) intraparietal sulcus (Tsao et al., 2003). Area V3a is in the occipital lobe
bordering on the parietal lobe, and like the intraparietal sulcus is thus part of the dorsal
“where” stream of processing.
However, to some extent depth is also processed along the ventral “what” pathway (see
Figure 12.2; Georgieva, Peeters, Kolster, Todd, & Orban, 2009). There it is used to re-
cognize objects in depth rather than their location as such (Verhoef, Vogels, & Janssen,
2016). Perceiving 3D shapes uses this pathway.
Motion
Other cues for perceiving depth come from motion. If one object moves to occlude another,
partially or completely hiding it, then the occluding object is presumed to be closer to the
observer. More subtly, as we move toward objects, ones that are closer show substantial
moment-to-moment change in the size and position of their image at the retina, while those
farther away show relatively little change. Vehicle occupants notice this in the form of optic
flow, the experience of objects rushing past in the periphery while the view of the road far
ahead seems to change much more slowly. However, optic flow is more than just a depth
cue, because it also provides essential information as to heading, i.e., the direction of
movement. Given stationary objects, eyes, and head, the point in space from which flow
seems to emanate is the direction in which the observer is moving.
Spatial perception 213
Both monkeys and humans use optic flow, and show at least somewhat similar patterns of
cerebral activation. In humans, studies using two different brain imaging technologies, po-
sitron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI; for both, see
Box 12.2) found that human observers passively viewing optic flow showed activation in area
MT/V5 (see Figure 13.1), located in part of the inferior temporal sulcus (Sunaert, Van
Hecke, Marchal, & Orban, 1999). The same was found during the active determination of
heading, although parietal activation was also observed (Peuskens, Sunaert, Dupont, Van
Hecke, & Orban, 2001). These results indicate that MT/V5 is sensitive to the movement
present in optic flow, while the parietal lobe performs the spatial computations needed to
determine heading.
For their part, macaques have an equivalent of human area MT/V5 that is sensitive to
motion. It responds most to coherent motion in one direction, although incoherent motion
provokes a response of lesser degree (Zaksas & Pasternak, 2005). Comparative studies suggest
that this area exists in all primates including galagos, but not in species related to primates
such as tree shrews. It therefore appears to have been a primate innovation (Kaas, 2004b).
However, the ventral parietal cortex of monkeys is also involved in perceiving optic flow,
with most neurons responding to expansion but some to contraction (Bremmer, 2005). This
214 Perception and cognition
area may differ somewhat from that used by humans to perceive heading, which is more
dorsal than ventral in location (Peuskens et al., 2001).
Quantity
The ability to judge quantities has clear adaptive value, for it can help determine which tree
has more fruit, or which way to run when escaping groups of predators. In the absence of
verbal counting, which is presumably a recent addition to hominin capabilities, quantity
perception must rely on some type of spatial representation.
Studies of quantification in modern humans indicate the existence of three separate
processes. When varying numbers of stimuli such as dots are presented briefly, and observers
are asked to specify their number as quickly as possible, a three-limbed function is produced
relating reaction time (RT) to numerosity (Figure 12.5). The first limb is from 1 to 3 or
4 dots, showing a modest increase in RT with each additional dot. The second is from
3 or 4 to about 7 dots, with a steep increase per dot. Finally, the third limb is marked by
negligible increases in RT from about 7 dots on.
There has been little controversy over the nature of the second and third limbs. The
second has been held to be due to an explicit counting process that involves verbalization and
working memory, two time-intensive components that account for the substantial increase in
RT as the number of dots increases. The third limb is due to an estimation process that kicks
in when the number of dots is too large to be counted within the brief memory of the
display. In fact when the display is left on until response, RTs continue to rise with an
increasing number of dots – i.e., estimation does not occur – because counting continues to
completion. Because brief display of a large number of dots involves a single numerical
estimation, RT is not much affected by the number of dots (Mandler & Shebo, 1982).
Subitizing in humans
Greater controversy has surrounded the first limb of the numerosity curve. Although held by
consensus to reflect a subitizing process (an early perceptual process that rapidly enumerates
small numbers of items), attempts to further characterize it have ranged widely. They have
Figure 12.5 The numerosity curve, relating the number of dot stimuli to the time required to enumerate
them.
Source: Original figure.
Spatial perception 215
included explanations based on the large proportional differences that exist among small
numbers, making them more discriminable than differences among large numbers (Averbach,
1963); the ability of small numbers of items to form patterns such as triangles and lines
(Mandler & Shebo, 1982); a “fast counting” process specific to small numbers (Gallistel &
Gelman, 1992); and a preattentive tagging of item locations that is limited in capacity and
therefore specific to small numbers (Trick & Pylyshyn, 1993).
However, more recent research strongly supports the “fast counting” interpretation of
subitizing (Boles, Phillips, & Givens, 2007). The research was motivated by the observation
that extracting number information from a specific type of bargraph involves the same
perceptual process as extracting number from dot clusters (Figure 12.6). Thus, both are better
recognized in the left visual field (LVF) as opposed to the right visual field (RVF), indicating
a right hemisphere processing advantage. Significantly, the visual field differences are cor-
related so that observers with a large LVF advantage on one also tend to show a large LVF
advantage on the other (Boles, 1991). Furthermore, trainees who practice bargraphs show
substantial transfer of training to dot clusters but not to other numerical tasks (Boles, 1997),
and simultaneous performance of the two tasks produces more interference than pairing
either with a different task (Boles & Law, 1998). All of these observations indicate that the
two tasks share a common process.
Figure 12.6 Examples of dot clusters and bargraphs that use the same perceptual process. The horizontal
reference lines for the bargraph are at the 0, 4, and 8 levels, with the example reflecting the
value “2”.
Source: Original figure.
216 Perception and cognition
Figure 12.7 RTs to dot clusters and bargraphs in Boles et al. (2007). Closed symbols LVF, open sym-
bols RVF.
Source: Response times from Study 1, reprinted by permission from Springer Nature Customer Service Centre GmbH,
David B. Boles, Jeffrey B. Phillips, Somer M. Givens.
The experiments of Boles et al. (2007) examined the relationship between the tasks in
more detail, beginning with the plotting of numerosity curves. The dot clusters showed
the classic multi-limbed function (Figure 12.7). The bargraphs, in contrast, showed an
intriguing scallop-shaped function in which small increases in RT occurred at values
starting at 0, 4, and 8 – i.e., the values represented by the horizontal reference lines
(Figures 12.6 and 12.7). This pattern of small RT changes with increasing number sug-
gested that the same subitizing process underlies the enumeration of small numbers of dots
and the enumeration of bargraphs. The suggestion was strongly confirmed by other re-
lationships in the results between the bargraphs and the subitizing range (1–4) of dot
clusters that were not found between bargraphs and the higher range (5–8) of dot clusters
(Boles et al., 2007).
The importance of these observations is that by establishing that subitizing underlies
bargraph recognition, the nature of subitizing is revealed by the pattern of bargraph RTs.
The scallop-shaped RT function is a telltale indication that subitizing is a rapid, nonverbal
counting process operating on analog quantities. In the case of bargraphs the analog quantities
are imaginary intervals or steps above or below the reference lines, while in the case of dot
clusters they are the dots themselves. Because subitizing uses analog quantities, it seems
reasonable to classify it as a spatial process. Furthermore, consistent with subitizing’s spatial
nature, brain imaging studies implicate right parietal lobe involvement, involving either the
intraparietal sulcus (Cutini, Scatturin, Moro, & Zorzi, 2014) or area 39 at its junction with
the temporal lobe (He, Zuo, Chen, & Humphrey, 2013).
Figure 12.8 Numerosity curves for a chimpanzee responding to 1–7 dots. RT = Reaction time.
Source: Original figure.
The results indicate that chimpanzees likely engage in the same analog-based subitizing
process as humans. What they do in the “counting” range is less clear since they presumably
do not have the same verbal processes as humans. Murofushi observed that with the larger
numbers Ai’s eyes tended to move back and forth between the display and the response
numerals, as if taking additional decision time – a possible cautionary note for research on
human enumeration in suggesting that large increases in RT can in fact be due to processes
other than verbal counting.
There are indications that macaques show much the same behavioral pattern of results
when enumerating items, as well as at least some parietal lobe involvement. Nieder,
Freedman, and Miller (2002) used a somewhat different experimental design requiring rhesus
monkeys to compare quantities of successive patterns of 1–5 dots. The resulting numerosity
curve nevertheless showed a strong resemblance to that found for humans and chimpanzees,
except that the subitizing range was limited to 1 to 2 dots rather than 1 to 3 or 4 dots (Nieder
et al., 2002.)
Nieder et al. also recorded from single neurons in monkeys performing the task, and
found that 7% of inferior parietal cells, and about a third of randomly selected prefrontal
cells, showed activation that varied with the number of dots. Furthermore, individual
neurons were “tuned” to a particular number, so that some responded with peak activity
to one dot, others to two dots, and so on, indicating an individual-neuron based sensitivity
to numerosity. The involvement of inferior parietal cells is generally consistent with the
human literature cited earlier.
Because there is at least some inferior parietal involvement in subitizing in macaques, it
appears that use of the area for that purpose traces back at least to the origin of Old World
monkeys about 29 million years ago. Thus, basic numerical ability far predates the symbolic
representation of number in hominin artifacts (Box 12.3).
Numerous examples have been discovered of hominin artifacts of relatively late date
that may – or may not – symbolize quantity. Specifically, bones bearing roughly
parallel cut marks have been found, most dating to 10,000–42,000 years ago but some
218 Perception and cognition
earlier. The challenge lies in distinguishing markings used as symbolic quantities, from
those merely appealing to a sense of aesthetics ( d’Errico et al., 2018). Thus, a row of
vertical marks on a bone might represent a kind of tally, or it might just be a pleasing
pattern (see Figure 12.9).
Figure 12.9 A bone artifact displaying five cut marks of ambiguous significance.
Source: Detail of cut marks (FindID 96513).jpg, CC BY-SA 4.0, The Portable Antiquities Scheme/The Trustees of the
British Museum.
D’Errico and colleagues have suggested clever criteria to distinguish between the
possibilities. An aesthetic artifact might be expected to have equidistant markings, and
might be embedded in a larger pattern. A symbolic use of cut marks, in contrast, would
not be embedded, and the marks would not be equidistant because its maker would be
interested only in the tally, not in the spatial precision of the marks. By these criteria,
the artifact in Figure 12.9 might be interpreted as a tally. Judging from the oldest
examples, a shift from exclusively aesthetic uses of cut marks to more symbolic ones
may have occurred by 60,000 years ago ( d’Errico et al., 2018). This, of course, long
postdates the acquisition of basic numerical abilities in our ancestral line – but it long
predates the development of mathematics.
How much further than 29 million years ago it extends is presently unclear. Like primates,
crows have neurons sensitive to number, but it is uncertain whether that is due to convergent
evolution, or to shared ancestry dating to over 300 million years ago (Wagener, Loconsole,
Ditz, & Nieder, 2018).
Orienting
Spatial perception is enhanced by our ability to shift eyes and attention toward a source of
stimulation, in other words orienting to objects. This allows more precise estimates of location
and depth. The parietal lobe plays a major role in orienting. In humans, both saccades (eye
movements toward a source of stimulation) and pursuit eye movements (eye movements fol-
lowing a moving object) involve an area in the middle of the intraparietal sulcus including a
portion of Brodmann area 7 (Culham, Cavina-Pratesi, & Singhal, 2006; Olson, Gatenby,
Leung, Skudlarski, & Gore, 2003). It is sometimes called the parietal eye field, and one of its roles
is to update spatial information in response to movement (Culham et al., 2006). In addition, it
Spatial perception 219
may actually initiate eye movements (Pierrot-Deseilligny, Ploner, Müri, Gaymard, & Rivaud-
Péchoux, 2002), although others take the position that it maintains eye movement accuracy
(Rafal, 2006). As we have seen, galagos have a large posterior parietal lobe, and so the in-
volvement of the intraparietal sulcus and area 7 may trace back to the early history of primates.
Eye movements also involve a second important area, the frontal eye field, located in the
frontal lobe. There is increasing consensus that it is located near the rear of the middle frontal
gyrus (see Figure 10.3), more specifically at or close to the intersection of the superior frontal
sulcus with the precentral sulcus (Petit & Pouget, 2019; Rafal, 2006). Research suggests it
plays a larger role in triggering voluntary saccades (i.e., to a stimulus long present) than
in triggering reflexive saccades (i.e., to the sudden appearance of a stimulus; Rafal, 2006).
This may be a matter of degree since bilateral lesions of both the frontal and parietal eye fields
appear necessary to cause a severe, long-lasting effect on both voluntary and reflexive sac-
cades (Pierrot-Deseilligny et al., 2002).
Spatial attention, the ability to attend to objects or locations independent of eye position,
involves parietal areas that overlap with those controlling eye movements. For humans,
posterior parietal cortex (Brodmann area 7), was highlighted as critical to spatial attention by
Posner and colleagues (Posner, Walker, Friedrich, & Rafal, 1984). They presented lesion
evidence indicating that the area is involved in the engagement and disengagement of at-
tention to positions in space. The bordering intraparietal sulcus is also activated by the
preparation and movement of attention to the periphery, even if the eyes are kept fixated in a
central location (Astafiev et al., 2003).
A second parietal area involved in spatial attention is the inferior parietal lobe. When it is
damaged, a syndrome called visuospatial neglect (or visual neglect, or spatial neglect) can result.
The patient fails to orient to objects opposite the side of the lesion. The syndrome most often
(but not exclusively) follows right hemisphere damage, thus producing a left-sided neglect.
The symptoms can be quite striking, as when patients draw objects with their left sides
missing, or bisect lines far to the right of their actual midpoints. A companion phenom-
enon, pseudoneglect, can be observed in normal individuals by requiring the bisection of
lines staggered down a page. Bisections are typically made slightly to the left of true center,
opposite to the effect seen in most neglect patients. This result is attributable to an intact
right hemisphere attentional mechanism that overattends to its half of space, resulting
in overestimation of line length on that side (Boles, Adair, & Joubert, 2009; McCourt,
Garlinghouse, & Reuter-Lorenz, 2005).
Several studies have mapped the lesions of patients with visuospatial neglect. Taken
together they find that the critical damage is either in the posterior portion of area 40 or
the adjoining area 39 (e.g., Mort et al., 2003; Ptak & Schnider, 2011; Verdon, Schwartz,
Lovblad, Hauert, & Vuilleumier, 2010). Since both area 40 (the supramarginal gyrus) and
area 39 (the angular gyrus) border on and are inferior to the intraparietal sulcus (see
Figure 12.4), it is an interesting question whether they represent the same or a different
area than that identified in attention shifting. The available evidence suggests that it is
in fact different. Astafiev et al. (2003) found that after accounting for areas involved in
saccades, a small portion of Brodmann area 7 could be attributed to spatial attention
specifically. In contrast, the lesions involved in neglect appear to lie in the ventral portions
of areas 39 and 40 and thus at some remove from area 7 and the intraparietal sulcus.
Therefore areas 40/39 and 7 may well support separate components of spatial attention.
Navigation
As perceptual information is progressively processed in the “where” pathway in the parietal
lobe and then in the frontal lobe, its coding becomes more action-oriented, matching percept
to motion. All of this activity comes together in our ability to navigate our environment.
As we learn to navigate, memory becomes increasingly important. It allows us to recall the
locations of landmarks, and to associate with them actions such as turning left or right.
Generally speaking, when we find ourselves in a new environment, whether it is a new
building or a new city, most of us initially build route knowledge, assembling knowledge of
landmarks and the associated turns to learn pathways from one point to another. As learning
progresses, we gradually develop more sophisticated survey knowledge, which is the knowledge
Spatial perception 221
of how landmarks are configured relative to one another. At this point a true cognitive map
develops, allowing us to deviate from established pathways and find new routes (Wolbers,
Weiller, & Büchel, 2004). However, there are individual differences in the formation of
cognitive maps. Some individuals begin to acquire information about spatial relationships and
start building a cognitive map on first exposure to a new environment, essentially bypassing
the route knowledge phase. Others never proceed beyond route knowledge to build a
cognitive map (Ishikawa & Montello, 2006).
There is good evidence that many chimpanzees develop true cognitive maps. In African
forest habitats they follow nearly linear paths to food trees scattered among many thousands
of other trees, with the vector varying depending on the relative locations of the tree and
themselves. They also decelerate when nearing the goal. The implication is that the
chimpanzees “know” where they are going on a mental map (Normand & Boesch, 2009).
Similar results were found by Janmaat, Ban and Boesch (2013), who followed the foraging
behavior of five female chimpanzees for several weeks. They reported that 13% of close
inspections of food trees were preceded by vector-like goal-directed approaches.
Counterintuitively, the use of linear routes increases when territory is less familiar, perhaps
because the animals minimize travel time through territory shared with other, often
aggressive, groups ( Jang, Boesch, Mundry, Ban, & Janmaat, 2019).
Throughout our evolutionary history, survival has depended on distinguishing among objects
in the environment, and within the class of objects that are living things, among predators,
prey, and conspecifics (other members of our species). We depend particularly heavily on visual
recognition, which is achieved by a series of processing stages along the ventral “what”
pathways of the brain (Figure 13.1).
In this chapter, we trace the progress of information processing along these pathways,
achieving the recognition of features and contours on the way to developing a re-
presentation of general form, and ultimately of objects and their classification. Similarities
and differences between ourselves and other species are highlighted, allowing inferences
about the evolutionary origins of our pattern recognition processes.
Localized processes
Once visual information enters the cerebral cortex in area V1, it proceeds in “feed forward”
fashion through a series of locations in the occipital and temporal lobes. However, anatomical
studies indicate that every such location is directly linked to most others (Kolb & Whishaw,
2009), resulting in connections that are partially serial (e.g., V1 to V2 to VP to V4, as
suggested by Figure 13.1) but are also often capable of bypassing intermediate levels (e.g., V1
directly to V4).
Nevertheless the system has a “stages of processing” aspect to it. Single-cell recordings
reveal that information arriving at V1 in the macaque produces activity serially in V2, V4,
posterior inferotemporal cortex (in humans, located just anterior to LO in Figure 13.1), and
anterior inferotemporal cortex (just forward of that), in phases each about 10 msec apart.
About 20 msec after that, prefrontal cortex activates (Thorpe & Fabre-Thorpe, 2001).
Humans show similar “feed forward” processing, although the timing differs (Isik, Meyers,
Leibo, & Poggio, 2014).
All eutherians have multiple visual areas in the cortex, with the number and arrangement
differing dramatically across species. Hedgehogs, with low reliance on vision, have only two
or three (Rosa & Krubitzer, 1999). Galagos have six (Wong & Kaas, 2010), while cats and
apes, both highly visual, have 20 to 30 or more (Rosa & Krubitzer, 1999). In the case of
humans, the locations shown in Figure 13.1 are only the initial ones in a larger network
of visual areas.
There is strong evidence that in the early stages of cortical processing, primate neurons
respond to features of a visual stimulus. These could be a particular line slant or color
at a specific location. At later stages, neurons respond to specific configurations such as a
particular face or object.
DOI: 10.4324/9781003137863-16
Pattern recognition 227
Figure 13.1 Major visual areas in the “what” pathway of the posterior lateral surface of the brain. Some
extend to the medial surface as well.
Source: Adaptation of Human-brain.SVG, CC BY-SA 3.0, Hugh Guiney.
David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel won a Nobel Prize in 1981 for their investigations
of visual information processing. Recording from the visual cortex of cats and
monkeys, they reported feature detection by simple cells. Specifically, the cells
were found to respond to an edge or bar stimulus at a particular orientation at a
particular point in space. They also discovered complex and hypercomplex cells
( Spillmann, 2014).
However, in subsequent years a competing view has strongly emerged. This view
maintains that the cells involved in early stages of visual pattern recognition can better
be characterized as spatial frequency detectors than as feature detectors. The optimal
stimulus for such detectors is a repeating sine wave pattern at a particular orientation – a
grating, not a bar ( Figure 13.2).
228 Perception and cognition
Figure 13.2 A spatial frequency stimulus (grating), representing a sine wave pattern at a particular
orientation. The frequency is specified by the number of repetitions per degree of
visual angle.
Source: Sine pattern.png, CC BY-SA 3.0, Setreset.
Figure 13.3 Cells in V2 extract the contour of the implied (but not physically present) borderless triangle.
Source: Kanizsa triangle.svg, CC BY-SA 3.0, Fibonacci.
230 Perception and cognition
Similarities across species in the anatomy of V1 and V2 lead to the conclusion that both
derived from an early mammalian ancestor (Rosa & Manger, 2005). The distribution of V1 is
very wide among mammals including monotremes and marsupials, indicating an origin about
220 million years ago. V2 may have had later eutherian origins, about 170 million years ago
(Rosa & Krubitzer, 1999). Unfortunately, for the most part we do not know whether the
two areas play the same functional roles across distantly related mammalian species as they do
in primates. An exception is that since cats were used in the feature analysis experiments of
Hubel and Wiesel, it can probably be assumed that the sensitivity of V1 neurons to line
orientations has largely been preserved over the last 85 million years. However, an initially
disordered layout of neurons in V1 became orderly only about 80 million years ago in the
common ancestor of tree shrews and primates (Kaas, 2012).
General forms
From V2 a further processing step brings visual information to VP and V3 (also known as
V3v and V3d to emphasize their locations in the ventral and dorsal streams), as well as to V4.
All are involved in the processing of form, with evidence linking VP and V3 to dynamic
form, and V4 to color form (Kolb & Whishaw, 2009). Neurons in areas VP and V3 respond
to specific shapes at depths corresponding to the eyes’ fixation point and beyond, and also to
small objects embedded in clutter (Rosa & Manger, 2005).
The VP/V3 complex is found in both primates and non-primates, with at least traces
of it found in tree shrews, ferrets, and cats, but perhaps not in marsupials like the opossum.
At present, the best interpretation of the evidence is that like V2, VP/V3 originated with
eutherian mammals about 170 million years ago (Rosa & Manger, 2005).
Studies of form recognition are hampered by the virtually infinite variety of forms that
could be used as stimuli. Finding a shape that evokes a strong neuronal response doesn’t
preclude the possibility that another shape might evoke an even stronger response (Ponce
et al., 2019). Nevertheless using a large variety of shapes often enables the identification of
feature combinations to which a cell is sensitive. In macaque area V4, some cells are sensitive
to particular curvatures at particular orientations relative to an object’s center. An example
might be a concave curve to the left of center.
However, other cells in V4 are sensitive to certain combinations of multiple curvatures, that
can include other features. These cells respond to fairly specific objects such as an enclosed
form of a particular color and texture (Pasupathy et al., 2019). A simple example is a red
“squashed raindrop” (Pasupathy & Connor, 2002). More complex objects detected in V4
can combine many features and be quite difficult to characterize (Ponce et al., 2019).
Even so, it is probably the combination of features that evokes the response, not the object
itself, so that other objects with similar features would evoke a similar response. Thus, there is
nothing special about a red squashed raindrop beyond its particular assemblage of features.
Other objects with different features would evoke a response in other V4 cells. If nothing
else, this illustrates the subtlety of information coding in the ventral stream: At any particular
point in processing, representations may not match up closely with what we end up per-
ceiving, i.e., specific objects, but rather with abstract combinations of features.
V4 builds upon earlier contour processing by combining parts of an object (Pasupathy &
Connor, 2002), thus achieving a generalized, abstract representation of it. The area also
resolves depth information passed along from areas V1 and V2. Presumably this is in the
service of developing a 3D representation so that the abstract object can be perceived in
depth (Verhoef, Vogels, & Janssen, 2016).
Tracing the evolutionary origins of V4 is complicated by uncertainty in identifying cor-
responding locations in monkeys and humans. While a color-sensitive V4 exists in both
Pattern recognition 231
humans (Hong & Tong, 2017) and macaques (Pasupathy & Connor, 2002), the evidence for
homology is ambiguous at a neuroanatomical level (Orban, Van Essen, & Vanduffel, 2004).
In any case, little if any effort has been devoted to uncovering the origins of V4 prior to the
divergence of Old World monkeys.
A stronger case for homology can be made for the next cortical stage of pattern re-
cognition beyond V4. This involves a correspondence between inferotemporal (IT) cortex in
macaques and the “lateral occipital complex” (LOC) in humans. Human lateral occipital
(LO) cortex, illustrated in Figure 13.1, is usually considered part of LOC (Denys et al., 2004).
However, LOC also includes posterior portions of the inferior temporal gyrus (see
Figure 10.3) as well as the middle fusiform gyrus on the underneath side of the cortex. That
allows it to be characterized as a large area straddling the border between the occipital and
temporal lobes (Denys, et al., 2004).
The human LOC appears to be homologous with the monkey IT, both anatomically
(Denys et al., 2004; Orban et al., 2004) and functionally. Functionally, both areas respond
similarly to two-dimensional, three-dimensional, and moving patterns. Also, both show a
tendency to respond less in their anterior portions than in their posterior portions to
scrambled patterns, indicating increasing sensitivity to complex combinations of features as
processing moves forward in the brain (Denys et al., 2004). In this respect, IT and LOC both
resemble the function of area V4, differing from it in that larger proportions of their cells
show sensitivity to complex feature combinations (Tanaka, 1997).
Assuming that IT and LOC really are homologous, what is their evolutionary history?
Human LOC appears to have a greater monopoly over form processing than macaque IT, in
that the macaque parietal lobe also participates in recognizing visual form (Denys et al., 2004).
While this could indicate decreased specialization of brain areas in macaques relative to
humans – thereby “spreading out” processing to a larger degree – contrary observations apply
to the processing of motion and form. There monkeys show less overlap between motion and
form processing areas than do humans, suggesting increased specialization of brain areas.
The answer to this apparent contradiction may lie with tools. Specifically, habitual tool use
requires the integration of motion with two- and three-dimensional representations of tools,
potentially accounting for the greater overlap of motion and form processing in humans
(Denys et al., 2004). Viewed in this way the greater overlap is actually an indication of
increased specialization for tool use. Of course even if this explanation is correct, it is difficult
to say to what extent the species difference is due to human evolution, or to dissimilar tool
traditions – i.e., differences in learning.
Objects
We have seen that information accumulates about objects as visual input passes in a forward
direction from V1 on through the monkey IT and human LOC areas. The next step is to
achieve specific neural responses to specific concrete objects.
One possibility is that a given neuron in monkey IT cortex uniquely responds to a specific
object. To early researchers this seemed quite plausible, because one of the most striking
characteristics of the ventral system was the specificity that many cells seemed to exhibit. In
the early years of research on the ventral “what” pathway, such neurons were called gnostic
cells. Subsequently they became popularly known as grandmother cells because a single cell
might specifically respond to one’s grandmother (Bowers, 2017; Gross, 2002).
An alternative to grandmother cells is a distributed network of neurons that produces a
specific response to a particular object only as a population. According to this formulation,
each cell within the network responds to a wide variety of objects, but every object produces
a unique pattern of activation of some cells and not others.
232 Perception and cognition
In fact both of these possibilities are problematic. For grandmother cells, one problem is
that there would need to be a specific cell for every possible object we can recognize, itself a
mind-boggling prospect. In addition, the cell would somehow need to respond to that object
regardless of orientation, size, and color (Gross, 2002). Perceptual invariance is the ability to
recognize an object regardless of those and other physical transformations. While it is an
important achievement of the ventral “what” system operating as a whole, it seems unlikely
that a single cell recognizes a single object in all of its possible transformations.
For distributed networks, the major problem is that a network by itself does not seem well-
suited to assess its own state. For example, how can the network decide which specific object
corresponds to a particular pattern of activation across cells, unless there is a cell to recognize
each pattern of activation – in other words, a grandmother cell?
No current solution provides neat answers to these problems, but there are suggestions that
the key to object recognition lies somewhere between the extremes of grandmother cells and
fully distributed networks. Hung, Kreiman, Poggio, and DiCarlo (2005) combined an ar-
tificial intelligence application and single-cell recordings to estimate the number of IT
neurons needed to recognize an object. They recorded responses to 77 different objects from
300 neurons and fed them in varying numbers into a mathematics-based computer program
that in turn “learned” to recognize the objects from the neural responses. It was found that
reasonably high performance (72% correct) could be achieved by feeding 256 responses into
the program. This result suggests that assemblies of IT neurons numbering in the hundreds
are sufficient to achieve object recognition (Hung et al., 2005).
Even in this approach, however, the “grandmother” problem was alive and well, because
in the artificial intelligence implementation, each of the 77 objects had a “classifier” – in
effect, a grandmother cell – to determine whether an identification could be made. On the
other hand, an important feature of the study was that good performance was obtained even
when object position or size changed (Hung et al., 2005). Together these observations in-
dicate the potential feasibility of a hybrid solution that combines a distributed network to
achieve perceptual invariance, thereby greatly reducing the number of object instances that
must be recognized, together with grandmother cells to identify that reduced number.
It seems intuitively obvious that perceptual invariance must be achieved at a relatively
“high” level of the system, following substantial integration of neural activity. Studies of
brain activity support this intuition. Within both macaque IT and human LOC, the more
anterior sectors, at the furthest remove from earlier stages of processing, show the greatest
perceptual invariance. However, only a subset of cells in these areas achieve invariance,
indicating that the object recognition system maintains both invariant and variant re-
presentations of objects (Kovács et al., 2003; Sawamura, Georgieva, Vogels, Vanduffel, &
Orban, 2005). Again this makes intuitive sense, because we can clearly differentiate among
views while simultaneously recognizing that they are of the same object.
The increased integration of neural activity as processing proceeds forward within
IT/LOC cortex leads to an expectation that information should become increasingly
integrated across sensory modalities as well. In fact cells in the LOC area have been
found to respond to objects presented not just visually but also tactilely (Hernández-Pérez
et al., 2017). However, they are in the minority, and it seems more appropriate to
characterize LOC as making available a wide range of representations of objects. These
include (a) specific objects of specific position, size, viewing angle, contrast, and/or
texture; (b) specific objects independent of such factors or even of modality; (c) generic
objects of a general shape and color; and even (d) generic objects of no particular shape
and color (Desimone et al., 1984). Presumably the development of a wide variety of
representations is in the service of ultimately achieving, by way of their integration,
unambiguous recognition of the object.
Pattern recognition 233
Processes within IT/LOC appear to be evolutionarily well conserved, with an origin
sometime prior to 29 million years ago. However, the usefulness of this conclusion is limited
by the absence of other comparative data that could put a more definitive limit on the
emergence of the area.
Object classes
Recognizing an object as a unique instance of a larger class, e.g., your grandmother as
a member of the class of people, is aided by specializations of the ventral stream that are
class-specific. Particular subareas within the occipito-temporal cortex appear to be sensitive
to particular object classes such as faces or letters. Thus, the human fusiform gyrus, par-
ticularly in the right hemisphere, is sensitive to faces. In part this is known because of
single-cell recordings from patients in whom electrodes were implanted to localize the
source of epileptic seizures. A face recognition “patch” was found to be located largely,
though not exclusively, in the lateral and posterior portion of the fusiform gyrus (Allison,
Puce, Spencer, & McCarthy, 1999). A similar area appeared to be involved in recognizing
facial expressions such as happy, sad, and fearful (Li, Richardson, & Ghuman, 2019). Other
patches in the fusiform gyrus and its vicinity proved responsive to objects such as cars,
butterflies, flowers, and letters (Allison, et al., 1999).
The important question, of course, is whether particular patches are sensitive to specific
object classes and not others. This seemed to be the case in the study by Allison et al. Across
participants, face patches were generally located anterior to object patches, and letter patches
were located near the occipitotemporal sulcus as compared to the fusiform location of face
patches. Additional evidence that face patches are face-specific was reported in an fMRI
imaging study by Rhodes, Byatt, Michie, and Puce (2004), using lepidoptera (butterfly and
moth) experts. They found little overlap in the specific locations activated during lepidoptera
recognition and face recognition tasks, even though both tasks used the fusiform gyrus.
McCarthy, Puce, Belger, and Allison (1999) examined the effects of altering faces on the
response of face-specific patches in epileptic patients. Patch responses didn’t vary across color
versus black-and-white renditions of faces, or to larger versus smaller sizes, indicating that
they responded to faces irrespective of color or size. The patches also responded to cat
and dog faces, with reduced sensitivity. Inverting human faces (turning them upside down)
likewise reduced their response, more so in the right hemisphere than the left. Because
inversion presumably impacts the configuration of a face more than its features, the result was
viewed by the authors as implying configural processing of faces in the right hemisphere, but
piecemeal processing of faces in the left hemisphere. Finally, there were strong indications
that internal parts of faces like the eyes, nose, and mouth, were separately processed from
the full face, at sites close to the face patches.
Visual agnosia
Varieties of visual agnosia, the inability to recognize objects presented visually, likewise
support the existence of cortical patches that are sensitive to particular object classes. Cases
have been reported in which the recognition of living things is deficient even though
nonliving things are identified normally. The opposite has also been reported (Humphreys &
Forde, 2001). Other object classes sometimes showing deficient recognition, while other
classes are preserved, include faces, tools, animals, and fruits and vegetables (Capitani,
Laiacona, Mahon, & Caramazza, 2003; Tranel, Damasio, & Damasio, 1997).
Prosopagnosia is the name given to face-specific visual agnosia. It is typically due to lesions
of the fusiform gyrus, usually in the right hemisphere (Kanwisher & Yovel, 2006). Tranel and
234 Perception and cognition
colleagues have reported lesion data indicating that deficits in recognizing tools, faces, and
animals involve damage to differing portions of temporal cortex, although in some cases
occipital and parietal areas are also involved (Tranel et al., 1997).
Figure 13.4 The Thatcher illusion, so named because the original illusion used pictures of British Prime
Minister Margaret Thatcher. Examine the pictures, then turn the page upside down and re-
examine them.
Source: Thatcher.PNG, public domain, Albert Kok.
236 Perception and cognition
H
A A + + H
A A + + H
A A + + H
A A A A A A + + + + + + H H H H H H H
A A + + H
A A + + H
A A + + H
H
Figure 13.5 Illustration of hierarchical stimuli. Left: Conflicting, Center: Global neutral, Right: Local
neutral. As shown, in the neutral conditions a nonletter plus sign substitutes at one level.
Source: Licensed from Elsevier under STM Permissions Guidelines.
from the faster processing of global information. Thus, if the global level provides conflicting
information, it interferes more because of its earlier availability. These basic results have been
replicated many times (e.g., Blanca & López-Montiel, 2009; Boles, 1984; Boles & Karner,
1996; Dale & Arnell, 2013).
Conclusion
The “what” pathway supports much of our visual pattern recognition. Located in the
ventral portions of the occipital and temporal lobes, it initially uses both spatial frequency
and feature analyses. Cells in the superficial layer of V1 appear to combine features so that
they are sensitive to complex angles and junction types. By the time information has “fed
forward” and been processed in V2, figure has separated from ground, and contours have
been extracted. Areas VP and V3 (also known as V3v and V3d) enhance boundaries and
color them in.
Comparative analyses indicate that V1 originated about 220 million years ago with the
emergence of mammals, while V2 as well as VP/V3 originated about 170 million years ago
with eutherian mammals.
Pattern recognition 239
Neurons in V4 are sensitive to combinations of curvatures organized relative to an object’s
center, thus achieving a representation of an object’s combined parts. It also incorporates
depth so that a 3D representation is achieved. The next stage, involving the homologous
areas of the LOC in humans and IT cortex in macaques, seems to build on these re-
presentations through sensitivity to complex feature combinations.
The LOC/IT areas trace at least as far back as the emergence of Old World monkeys
29 million years ago, but little is known about their earlier origins. There seems to have been
some divergence between macaques and humans in that humans show an increased spe-
cialization of LOC relative to monkey IT, yet more integration of motion and form in-
formation. This may reflect increased specialization for tool use.
Recent research has estimated the number of inferotemporal neurons necessary to re-
cognize an object in the hundreds rather than the much larger numbers of a fully distributed
network. However, they don’t completely escape a problem associated with “grandmother”
cells, namely the need for a dedicated unit to recognize each object. On the other hand, a
strength is that networks can achieve a substantial degree of invariance, representing the same
object regardless of transformation in such physical characteristics as position or size.
Neurons at this level are organized into “patches” that are specific to object classes such as
faces, cars, butterflies, flowers, and letters. Face patches are largely though not exclusively
located in the fusiform gyrus, especially in the right hemisphere. Damage to this area can
cause prosopagnosia, a visual agnosia for faces. New World and Old World monkeys,
chimpanzees, humans, and even sheep all appear to have the same configuration-based,
orientation-specific mechanism for recognizing faces, something not shared by pigeons.
This suggests an origin at least 85 million years ago, but how much more recently than our
300-million-year-old common ancestor with birds is unclear.
Experiments using hierarchical stimuli indicate that humans are more sensitive to global
configurations than to features. We respond faster to the global level, and conflicting in-
formation at that level interferes more with feature processing than the reverse, two phe-
nomena that together are called the global precedence effect. Chimpanzees appear to show a
weaker form of the effect, while Old World and New World monkeys show an opposite,
local precedence effect. Thus, an evolutionary shift from local to global precedence may
have started about 29 million years ago, down to later than 7.5 million years ago. It may be
that bigger, more interconnected brains are better able to use the proximity of small elements
and their apparent continuity to extract global form.
14 Memory
Skilled handedness, tool use and manufacture, spatial perception, and object recognition have
something in common: They require a highly evolved ability to remember. In our day-to-
day lives, we constantly access information in memory that represents the meaning of
environmental stimuli. We use memory to select action and to execute it after recalling
learned motor patterns. In a real sense, memory can be viewed as a prerequisite for all
perceptual and cognitive activity.
In general, memory is defined as the ability to retain information from past events. It is
phylogenetically ancient in the form of habituation, a decline in response to repeated sti-
mulation. Even single-cell organisms show declining responses on repetition, e.g., by
ceasing contraction in response to repeated pressure changes in the surrounding water
(van Duijn, 2017). Because habituation involves retaining information from past events –
i.e., an event has occurred repeatedly in the past and so response is reduced – it qualifies as
a memory phenomenon, although a very primitive one (Moore, 2004; Szabo, Noble, &
Whiting, 2021).
A more restrictive definition of memory requires association, the arbitrary pairing of a
stimulus with a response. In classical conditioning, associations are learned when a condi-
tioned stimulus (e.g., a ringing bell) shortly precedes an unconditioned stimulus (e.g., meat),
which evokes a reflexive response (e.g., salivation in Pavlov’s dogs). Soon, the conditioned
stimulus itself produces the response without any intervening unconditioned stimulus,
showing that an arbitrary association has been learned between the stimulus and the response.
In theory, the same stimulus could arbitrarily be paired with any other response and con-
ditioned in similar fashion.
Association is what most people mean by memory. For example, when we learn a new
face, we learn arbitrary facts associated to the face: A name, that this is our new boss, that this
face’s office is down the hall. There is nothing “built in” or reflexive about any of those
associations; they have to be learned as arbitrary bits of information attached to a stimulus.
Like habituation, conditioning in various forms is phylogenetically ancient. It has been
observed in snails, wasps, and birds, as well as in kittens, lambs, rats, monkeys, and a host of
other mammals (Moore, 2004). It has even been observed in planaria (Hutchinson, Prados, &
Davidson, 2015), a flatworm whose common ancestry with our own traces back almost to
the origin of bilateria during the Cambrian explosion. It probably extends even before that,
as sea anemones show conditioning to both food and shock stimuli (Cheng, 2021). Because
sea anemones are cnidarians, conditioning likely traces at least to the cnidarian divergence
over 605 million years ago.
These observations may indicate the importance of nerve cells and their organization to
conditioning. Cnidarians, including sea anemones, have a diffuse neural net using un-
specialized neurons (Havrilak et al., 2017; Cheng, 2021). Planaria and some other flatworms
show greater neural organization, as well as a differentiation of neurons into sensory,
DOI: 10.4324/9781003137863-17
Memory 241
association, and motor types (Hickman, Roberts, & Larson, 1997). Thus, the presence of
nerve cells having at least net-like organization – but not necessarily specialization into
functional types – may be a prerequisite for conditioning.
One problem in uncovering the evolutionary origins of memory is the sheer variety in
forms of learning that have been investigated. Moore (2004) suggested that there are dozens
of biologically distinct kinds of learning, including ten different forms of conditioning.
However, his analysis proceeded largely on logical grounds, and it is unclear to what extent
so many different kinds of learning actually involve different abilities.
In any case, it seems indirect to trace memory’s evolutionary origins by examining learning
abilities rather than types of memory. Whether or not an animal learns using a particular
method is likely to depend on a number of factors. They include sensitivity to the stimuli
used, the ability to make the required response, and even the species’ predisposition toward
associating certain stimuli to certain responses and not others (Seligman, 1970).
Cognitive psychology offers a more direct approach by positing the existence of several
empirically dissociable memory stores, or kinds of memory. This structural approach eliminates
the problems of a learning-based approach by focusing on the nature and fate of the in-
formation that is remembered, not the method by which it became represented in memory.
For the most part, the approach taken here will be to describe each component of
memory from the human perspective, accompanied by a comparative analysis to identify
evolutionary trends.
Sensory memory
Sensory memory is a very short-lived trace that preserves much of the physical form of
stimuli and lasts at most several seconds. The visual form of sensory memory, known as
the icon, may be the best understood. It is a copy of visual stimulation that lasts half a
second or less in brightly lit conditions but up to four seconds in the dark (Sperling,
1963). An example is a sparkler waved in a circle in the dark (Figure 14.1). The resulting
smear makes the circle appear complete when in actuality, at any instant the tip of the
sparkler only emits a point of light on the circle. The icon is thus a very brief memory
that forms a bridge from one instant of time to the next, creating a continuous visual
experience. It is responsible for our sense of continuity when watching a 24-frame-per-
second Hollywood movie.
Figure 14.1 A circle created with a sparkler. Points in the circle don’t persist over time, but instead are held
in sensory memory (or in this case, during the exposure interval of the camera) to create the
illusion of a circle.
Source: Sparkler Fun, CC BY-SA 2.0, Kirt Edblom.
to fade rapidly over 400 msec. Presumably, the cones in the retina were responsible for the
icon in this condition.
In the other half of the experiment, the rod contrast condition, the task was made difficult
for the cones by adjusting the intensities so that the dot and background appeared equally
bright to the cones. Although the cones could still detect a color difference, this condition
made it more likely that the rods would be involved in the task, and it certainly should have
been easier for the rods than the cones. In this condition, performance again declined over
400 msec, but to a much smaller extent implying that the icon was holding information for a
much longer time (Figure 14.2).
The beauty of these results is that they accord with what is otherwise known about the
duration of the icon. As already indicated, in brightly lit (daytime) conditions, it lasts only half
a second or less, while in dark (nighttime) conditions, it can last up to four seconds. Daytime
vision depends on the cones and nighttime vision on the rods, so the difference between the
cone contrast and rod contrast conditions accords well with that difference. Most im-
portantly, the results indicate that one component of the icon is located at the retina: The
cones are responsible for a rapidly fading version of it while the rods are responsible for a
more slowly fading version. Of course, the usefulness of the rod version is limited by the low
resolution of the rod system (Long & McCarthy, 1982).
Memory 243
Figure 14.2 Decay of the icon (with the curves smoothed for clarity) under rod and cone contrast conditions.
Source: Original figure.
A pattern mask is most effective when it is briefly delayed relative to the stimulus, a
result interpreted as the mask “catching up” to the stimulus at a time-consuming stage
of processing. But how is that possible? Logically, it seems that a delay should be
maintained throughout the processing system, so that the stimulus always stays ahead of
the pursuing mask.
The answer lies in a two-phase response of the visual system. The magnocellular
response is a rapid response to stimulation that preserves only fuzzy detail but serves to
alert the system to the appearance and location of the stimulus. The parvocellular response
is much slower, but contains detailed information that allows feature extraction and
identification of the stimulus. The two responses are transmitted by different, but
roughly parallel, systems of cells beginning in the retina.
244 Perception and cognition
It is the faster speed of the magnocellular system that allows a pattern mask to
“catch up” to a stimulus. The fast magnocellular response to the mask reaches feature
analyzers in the cortex at the same time as the slower parvocellular response to the
stimulus, even though the mask was presented after the stimulus. Consistent with this
explanation, if the task is simply to indicate the location of the stimulus without
identifying its features, a delayed pattern mask is not effective. In that case, the
magnocellular response to the stimulus does indeed stay ahead of the pursuing mask
( Öğmen, Breitmeyer, Todd, & Mardon, 2006).
Other forms of sensory memory that have been investigated include auditory sensory
memory, the so-called echo lasting about two to ten seconds (Bijsterveld, 2015), and a
somesthetic sensory memory lasting about one to two seconds (Bliss, Crane, Mansfield, &
Townsend, 1966).
The general interpretation that has been offered for the existence of all of these is that
they represent aftereffects of neural activity. With respect to the icon, the cones and rods of
the retina remain in a heightened state of activity for a brief period of time following
stimulation, preserving a sensory trace of the stimulus. The same is true of visual feature
detectors beginning with V1 in the cortex. With respect to the echo, durations toward the
lower end of the two to ten second range appear to be associated with residual activity in
the primary auditory cortex, while longer durations reflect activity in association auditory
cortex (Lu et al., 1992).
Short-term memory
Short-term memory has a much smaller capacity but a much longer duration than sensory
memory. Whereas sensory memory preserves a copy of all stimulation for a few seconds at
most, short-term memory holds a maximum of about nine items for 20 seconds or so.
However, it may be refreshed indefinitely by rehearsal, e.g., when vocally repeating a phone
number from a contact list (Baddeley, 1986; Klatzky, 1980).
The capacity of nine items is a generalization that greatly depends on the type of item held
in memory. Capacity is measured by requiring that varying numbers of items be recalled in
the order presented. The number of items that can consistently be recalled in correct order is
the memory span. Strings of random digits (e.g., 3, 6, 1, 7) are subject to the “seven, plus or
Memory 245
minus two” dictum made famous by George Miller (1956). Five to nine digits can be re-
membered in order, depending on the individual and the conditions of the experiment.
Cavanagh (1972) reviewed the literature and found that average memory span varied de-
pending on the type of item: E.g., about eight for colors, five to six for random words, and
three for nonsense syllables.
Thus, short-term memory does not have a fixed number of “slots” but rather a fixed
capacity for information. Items requiring more detailed memory (e.g., random words and
nonsense syllables) have fewer slots than items requiring less-detailed memory (e.g., digits and
colors; Alvarez & Cavanagh, 2004; Cavanagh, 1972). Conversely, stretching capacity over
ever-increasing numbers of items results in a progressive degradation of memory content,
indicating that a limited information resource is being stretched too thin (Bays, 2018).
The informational view is lent weight by an orderly relationship between memory span
and search time through short-term memory. When several items are held in memory
(the memory set), reaction time (RT) to decide whether a subsequent probe was among the
memory set increases linearly with the number of items in the set. For digits, RT increases
about 33 msec per digit, implying that it takes that amount of time to compare the probe to
a single memory item. As it turns out, plotting the comparison time for a particular kind
of memory item against the memory span for that item results in a linear relationship. For
example, colors on average show a 38 msec comparison time and a 7.1 item memory span,
random words a 47 msec comparison time and a 5.5 item memory span, and nonsense
syllables a 73 msec comparison time and a 3.4 item memory span (Cavanagh, 1972). In other
words, memory span systematically decreases as comparison time increases. This result
supports the informational view of short-term memory because it implies that the greater
the detail required to remember an item, as reflected in the memory span, the more time is
required to compare the probe to an item in memory.
At least if held in auditory form, information in short-term memory generally lasts 10 to
30 seconds if it is not refreshed through rehearsal. That makes 20 seconds a reasonable
estimate of the duration of short-term memory (Klatzky, 1980).
Working memory
But what about the area 6, 44, and 10 activations? These may best be understood by con-
sidering the concept of working memory developed by Alan Baddeley (1986). Working
memory incorporates the storage aspects of short-term memory by proposing the existence of
an “articulatory loop” in which information may be held through the process of verbal
rehearsal (i.e., area 40), and a “visuo-spatial sketch pad” in which information may be held in
a visual format (i.e., area 7). Both are subordinate to a reasoning, active component of
working memory called the “executive control system”, that can consciously manipulate
their contents. By this view, the area 6 activation corresponds to activation in the executive
control system. Support for this role comes from research linking area 6 activity to problem
solving (Fincham et al., 2002), reasoning (Monti & Osherson, 2012), and short-term in-
formation updating (Nee & Brown, 2013). Area 6 is part of a larger frontal lobe network
Memory 247
involved in executive control, for as we have seen, area 10 appears to evaluate the contents of
working memory (see Chapter 11). Left area 44, on the other hand, constitutes a portion
of Broca’s area. Activation there likely corresponds to the verbal rehearsal component of
working memory (Cabeza & Nyberg, 2000).
In contrast to area 6 and 44 activations in humans, macaque working memory activations
are generally in areas 9 and 46 (Petrides, 2005; Petrides & Pandya, 1999). Frontal lobe
homologies between humans and macaques are close (Figure 14.3), so these really appear to
be different areas of activation than those most frequently observed in humans. However,
areas 9 and 46 do activate in a fair number of human working memory studies (Cabeza &
Nyberg, 2000). It seems possible that they comprise an evolutionarily older substrate for
working memory, dating back to at least a macaque-human common ancestor 29 million
years ago, and that during human (or possibly ape) evolution, adjacent areas (i.e., 10 and 44)
and nearby areas (i.e., 6) have been recruited.
In any case it is clear that in monkeys and humans, short-term memory is based in the
neocortex. Birds including pigeons do not have a structure homologous to the neocortex of
mammals (Krauzlis, Bogadhi, Herman, & Bollimunta, 2018). Thus, short-term memory in
birds on the one hand, and primates on the other, is best regarded as a matter of convergent
evolution.
Intermediate-term memory
Mark Rosenzweig and colleagues have called attention to an intermediate-term memory
in certain animals (Rosenzweig, Bennett, Colombo, Lee, & Serrano, 1993). For example,
after chicks are trained to avoid pecking a normally attractive shiny bead, their avoidance
initially dips over time, then recovers (Figure 14.4). The result is consistent with the
phasing out of an intermediate term memory, succeeded by long-term memory
(Rosenzweig et al., 1993).
Somewhat similar findings have been reported from the nautilus, a deep-water shelled
mollusk. The animals were conditioned to extend their tentacles following presentation of
a signal light, and they were subsequently tested at intervals. Memory declined smoothly
over one hour, but dramatically rebounded after six hours (Crook & Basil, 2008).
Intermediate memories with varying temporal characteristics have also been proposed for
fruit flies, honeybees, roundworms, snails, and sea hares, a type of sea slug (Braun &
Likowiak, 2011).
The idea of an hours-long memory intermediate to what in humans is a seconds-long
short-term memory and a decades-long long-term memory is certainly appealing.
However, to this point there is no evidence for it in people. Human memory follows a
smoothly declining trajectory (Figure 14.5), to the extent that detecting any discontinuity
at all, let alone a dramatic rebound, is somewhere between difficult and impossible (Rubin,
Hinton, & Wenzel, 1999; Wickelgren, 1973). In addition, it smoothly declines regardless
of whether the memory items are nonsense syllables, words, pictures, or geometric forms
(Gilliland, 1948; Wixted & Ebbesen, 1997). That means that the human–animal difference
cannot be attributed to the predominant use of verbal memory items in human research on
forgetting.
Therefore, it appears that intermediate-term memory has no parallel in humans. Perhaps
the necessary mutations never occurred, or perhaps they occurred and were subsequently
selected against. Presumably a smooth forgetting function improves estimates of when events
occurred and in what order, an important capability for a species dependent on experiential
recollection. That could be a possible reason for deselection of an intermediate-term
memory.
248 Perception and cognition
Figure 14.3 Brodmann areas in human frontal cortex (a), and corresponding areas in macaques (b).
Source: Architectonic map of the human and macaque monkey prefrontal cortex, CC BY-SA 4.0, M. Petrides, D.N.
Pandya.
Memory 249
80
60
% Avoidance
40
20
0
1 15 30 60 90 120
Interval since training (min)
Figure 14.4 Avoidance in chicks following training, showing recovery of memory over 120 minutes and
thus implying the existence of an intermediate term memory.
Source: Original figure.
Figure 14.5 Example of the human forgetting function, showing a smooth decline of percent correct recalls
over time. Note the contrast with Figure 14.4.
Source: Original figure.
250 Perception and cognition
Long-term memory and its divisions
If association is what most people think of when they think of memory, long-term memory
is its primary repository. In this form of memory, knowledge and experiences can be retained
more or less permanently. Models of human cognitive processing hold that information
enters long-term memory from short-term memory through rehearsal. Maintenance rehearsal
(rote repetition) suffices to increase a sense of familiarity of memory items, but elaborative
rehearsal is required to embed memory items firmly enough in long-term memory to allow
their recall. Elaborative rehearsal involves processing the meaning of the items, thus asso-
ciating them to the existing knowledge structure (Klatzky, 1980).
Long-term memory actually refers to a family of memory systems. Most comparative
research, however, has explored two divisions of long-term memory called episodic memory
and semantic memory. Episodic memory is literally memory for episodes or particular events in
the past, while semantic memory is memory for the facts that make up our knowledge
structure.
A distinction between these systems is strongly indicated on neurological grounds.
Amnesic patients who have suffered damage to the hippocampus and neighboring areas of
the brain show severely impaired encoding of new information into episodic memory, while
simultaneously showing a relative sparing of the acquisition of new semantic information
(Rosenbaum, Gilboa, & Moscovitch, 2014). For example, a patient with amnesia may not
remember a trip to Florida taken last year, while simultaneously recalling that the capital of
Florida is Tallahassee, a fact learned during the trip.
Brain imaging and lesion studies also indicate a difference between episodic and semantic
memory. For episodic memory, encoding a new event most often activates the medial
temporal cortex, where the hippocampus is located, but retrieving an event most often in-
volves area 10 of the frontal lobe (Cabeza & Nyberg, 2000). Lesions affecting episodic
memory also typically involve the medial temporal lobe, especially the hippocampus
(McCormick, Ciaramelli, De Luca, & Maguire, 2018).
In contrast, semantic memory retrieval involves the lateral temporal cortex. It also in-
volves area 45 of the frontal lobe in both encoding and retrieval; however, this may be
specific to tests that are verbal in nature, calling on language-related structures (Cabeza &
Nyberg, 2000).
Lateral temporal involvement was confirmed in a study relating activation in normal brains
responding to semantic tasks, to the site of brain atrophy (shrinkage) in semantic dementia. In
semantic dementia, use of semantic knowledge is impaired while episodic memory is spared.
The study found the activation and atrophy sites to be the same: The anterior lateral temporal
lobe, in both hemispheres. More specifically, the activation data implicate the anterior
portions of the middle and superior temporal gyri (see Figure 10.3; Rogers et al., 2006).
Others have confirmed that this area is among those affected in semantic dementia (Landin-
Romero, Tan, Hodges, & Kumfor, 2016).
Episodic memory
Do animals have episodic memories? Hampton and Schwartz (2004) have emphasized how
difficult they are to demonstrate. Because animals lack language, memory is generally tested
with recognition tasks in which previously seen items are picked from an array of such items
intermixed with distractors. Because the items were learned as an episode, it is true that
episodic memory could be responsible for correct recognitions. But unfortunately, mere
familiarity could also support recognition. Good performance might therefore result not from
recall of the learning episode, but from a vague feeling that items have been seen before.
Memory 251
A second problem was noted by Schwartz, Hoffman, and Evans (2005) in regard to
previous research by Menzel (1999). In Menzel’s study, a chimpanzee indicated the nature
and location of hidden food to human caregivers, having observed it being hidden up to
16 hours previously. While that could certainly be an indication of episodic memory, it could
also be that semantic memory was employed, and that the chimpanzee merely indicated what
it knew to be true (i.e., that there is a particular food item hidden over there), without any
specific recall of the episode of the food being hidden.
Therefore, turning this observation on its head, one way of demonstrating episodic
memory might be to require the recall of something that was once true and was learned
through a single observation, but which should be known by the recaller to no longer be
true. For example, as I write this, I can recall being at the library earlier in the day and seeing
a particular book on a shelf; it was true then but is no longer true, because I checked it out
from the library. My memory must be of an episode, since it is not a truth represented in
semantic memory.
This kind of strategy has been employed in a series of studies of a lowland gorilla named
King. In the initial study, King was trained to respond to the English question, “What did
you eat?” by handing over a card symbolizing one of five kinds of fruit. Subsequently he was
given the corresponding piece of fruit, and at either a short delay (5–65 minutes) or a long
delay (24–96 hours) was asked what he had eaten. At both delays, he was 70–80% correct,
well above chance performance. In a second study, he was able to indicate both the food
eaten and the person who had given it to him, at both short and long delays. Thus, in both
studies, King recalled information learned in a single trial, that he should have known to no
longer be true: He had eaten the food, so it existed only as a memory (Schwartz, Colon,
Sanchez, Rodriguez, & Evans, 2002).
Of course, it is always possible that King based his responses not on recalled episodes, but
rather on a sense of familiarity evoked by the most recently-presented fruit or the most recent
human benefactor. Familiarity was perhaps most convincingly ruled out in one of a later set
of studies (Schwartz et al., 2005). King was given three pieces of fruit one at a time, at five-
minute intervals, e.g., apple-grapes-pear. During training, after five minutes he had to
produce the three cards that symbolized the fruit, in reverse order, i.e., pear-grapes-apple,
and if successful, he was given raisins. Subsequently, once this procedure had been learned,
King was tested at longer intervals (5–23 minutes) and was successful (Schwartz et al., 2005).
Mere familiarity could not have been the basis for his performance, because it could not
plausibly have provided sufficient information to correctly order multiple cards. Thus, King
appears to have an episodic memory.
Different researchers have adopted different criteria for demonstrations of episodic
memory, but the requirements that learning (a) be achieved in one trial, (b) reflect cir-
cumstances the animal should know are no longer true, (c) persist over intervals of at least
several minutes, and (d) produce recognition that cannot plausibly be attributed to familiarity
alone, would seem sufficient to convince most observers that episodic and not semantic
memory is involved. Unfortunately, it is a very difficult combination to satisfy.
A revealing study, but one that only partially satisfied the four conditions, was con-
ducted by Lewis, Call, and Bemtsen (2017) using a relatively large sample of 33 apes
(chimpanzees, orangutans, and bonobos). The apes witnessed an experimenter showing
them food that was not part of their regular diet, and then observed the experimenter
hiding it in a concealed location after climbing a ladder. The animals were given im-
mediate access to the enclosure, and those that succeeded in climbing up to and finding the
food in less than five minutes were included in the subsequent study. In that study, at a
delay of 2, 10, or 50 weeks, the ape entered the enclosure and discovered the same kind of
food on the ground below the hiding place. The question was whether the animal would
252 Perception and cognition
Figure 14.6 Percentage of climbs in the hidden food study, as a function of retention interval.
Source: Original figure.
then climb the ladder to search the previous hiding place. The answer was a resounding
yes, but with an apparent delay effect such that the longer the delay, the fewer the climbs
(Figure 14.6; note, however, that the delay effect was nonsignificant). Furthermore,
control animals that had not been exposed to the initial food hiding did not climb
the ladder.
The study certainly satisfied condition (a), one-trial learning, and (c), persistence. It
probably satisfied condition (b), because the animals had previously eaten the food stored in
the hiding place. It did not satisfy (d), however, because familiarity alone could plausibly have
led animals to climb the ladder. In spite of that, the results are revealing because one-trial
learning persisted at a respectable level even 50 weeks after the episode. In addition,
something resembling the human forgetting function was seemingly obtained.
Scrub jays show evidence of memory satisfying all four of the episodic memory criteria, as
well as correctly recalling “what”, “where”, and “when” information about hidden food
(Hampton & Schwartz, 2004). The “when” aspect is particularly intriguing because the jays
avoid looking for food hidden sufficiently long ago that it should be spoiled.
In one of the best designed of these studies, the birds were first taught that cached
worms (a preferred food) degraded over time, while nuts remained intact (Clayton, Bussey,
& Dickinson, 2003). They then were allowed to cache worms and nuts, on separate sides
of a tray and on separate days. Later, they were required to choose a side, with the food
removed. At short delays, the task was to choose the side in which the worms had been
cached, but at long delays the side in which the nuts had been cached. The jays were able
to perform this task successfully, seeming to satisfy the criteria developed earlier. Thus,
caching a particular food was a one-time act; preferentially avoiding worm locations after a
long delay indicated an understanding that circumstances had changed (i.e., the worms had
degraded); the delay of hours-to-days showed long persistence; and familiarity could not
account for the results since the worm and nut locations were equally familiar. Such
memories indeed seem episodic.
Subsequent to the research on jays, a similar experiment was conducted with a mixed
group of bonobos, common chimpanzees, and orangutans. Frozen juice and grapes were
the perishable and nonperishable foods, with the juice irretrievably melting through a net
over the space of an hour. After training, the animals were shown both being hidden, and
were then allowed to choose and access one location at 5- or 60-minute delays. It was
found that a strong preference for the frozen juice location at the short delay was
Memory 253
substantially reduced at the longer delay, similar to the scrub jay results. Interestingly, there
was a relationship between delay effect and the animals’ age, with only those of middling
age (10–14 years) showing a substantial reduction in frozen juice choices at 60 minutes
(Martin-Ordas, Haun, Colmenares, & Call, 2010).
Somewhat similar studies have been carried out in rats, and they implicate the hippo-
campus in animal episodic memory (Eichenbaum and Fortin, 2005; Naqshbandi, Feeney,
McKenzie, & Roberts, 2007). Thus, Eichenbaum and Fortin (2005) reported studies in
which rats were exposed to five odors, then were required to discriminate which of two test
odors had been presented prior to the other. Normal rats succeeded, but rats with hippo-
campal lesions failed. Importantly, both the normal and lesioned rats were able to perform the
simpler task of recognizing which of two odors had been presented in the five-odor series,
with performance declining for odors presented earlier in the series. This result indicates that
both groups of rats had access to a memory trace that faded over time, but only the normal
rats were able to use the trace in episode-like fashion: They correctly picked the earlier-
presented odor in a pair even though more forgetting had occurred for it. While the degree
of control over familiarity is not as good in this paradigm as in the jay paradigm – after all, the
task could conceivably be performed by selecting the less familiar alternative – the in-
volvement of the hippocampus provides reasonably convincing evidence that episodic, or at
least episodic-like, memory is involved.
In fact, “following the hippocampus” may be a way to explore the evolutionary origins
of episodic memory. The assumption would be that if a hippocampus is present, then
episodic memory, or important elements thereof, may also be present. Such a strategy
avoids the complicated behavioral methods necessary to identify episodic memory.
However, it is potentially problematic for exactly the same reason: It may be possible for
an animal to have a hippocampus without showing all the behavioral features of such a
memory.
Birds have a hippocampus, part of the paleocortex (ancient cortex), even though they do not
have a structure homologous to the mammalian neocortex (Clayton et al., 2003; Vargas,
Bingman, Portavella, & López, 2006). Even fish have a hippocampus, or more accurately, a
brain structure called the lateral pallium that is anatomically, genetically, and functionally
homologous to the avian and mammalian hippocampus (Vargas et al., 2006). Fish use it to
represent memory of the spatial environment, and it incorporates timing information
(Broglio et al., 2005; Murray, Wise, & Graham, 2018). Indeed, as we saw in Chapter 12, the
hippocampus is one site of grid cells that play an important role in representing environ-
mental locations even in bats, rodents, and humans. That is certainly episodic in that the cells
determine position and timing relative to the present environment, even though the en-
vironmental representation itself is built up over many different exposures and thus has
semantic qualities as well.
These observations suggest that key components of episodic memory originated by the
first appearance of bony fishes about 430 million years ago. However, whether these
components have always supported the kind of recollective experience we think of as epi-
sodic memory is doubtful. It will take a great deal of clever experimentation to determine
whether or not the episodic memory evident in gorillas, rats, and scrub jays shows continuity
over nearly 80% of the timespan of vertebrate evolution.
Also to be determined is the significance of discontinuities in the evolution of the hippo-
campus. Humans have a disproportionately large hippocampus compared to other primates,
and its posterior portion substantially enlarged relatively recently, i.e., during hominin
evolution. But, was this to support improvements in episodic memory, or alternatively, in
executive control or spatial ability? (Schilder, Petry, & Hof, 2019).
254 Perception and cognition
Semantic memory
How episodic memory gives way to semantic memory seems a bit mysterious. Logically,
every piece of knowledge that we possess began as an episode. For example, there was a
specific time and place that we first learned that 8 times 6 equals 48. At one time in our lives,
soon after our first exposure to that fact, we might well have remembered the specific cir-
cumstances under which we learned it, an indication of reliance on episodic memory. Now,
many years later, we do not. Somehow, multiple episodes have become something we simply
know to be true.
One possibility is that episodic memory is required for encoding into semantic memory, so
that in due course, multiple episodes build our knowledge structure. However, this does not
seem to be the case in that some amnesiacs have severely impaired episodic memory yet still
acquire new semantic memories (Rosenbaum et al., 2014; Verfaellie, Koseff, & Alexander,
2000). The alternate possibility, which appears more likely, is that the two memory systems
exist at least partially in parallel. Faulty operation of one would therefore not necessarily affect
the other. As we have seen, there is anatomical evidence for this view, with episodic memory
calling on the hippocampus and semantic memory on anterior temporal cortex.
Superficially, semantic memory should be much easier to demonstrate than episodic
memory, seeming to require only that information be recalled in a way that reflects some
understanding of meaning, and at a sufficient delay to represent long-term memory.
However, the situation is more complex than that, because conditioning also produces
behavior that appears meaning-laden. Thus, in the classic Pavlovian study, when a dog
salivates on hearing a bell, it appears that the dog understands the meaning of the bell: Meat is
coming. But does it? Remember that conditioning is a form of learning that is found in
flatworms and likely derives, at minimum, from the first emergence of neural nets
605 million years ago. A strengthening of simple neural connections through repeated
association seems more plausible than “understanding” at such an evolutionary remove.
One possible way around the conundrum may be to adopt an anatomical criterion.
Semantic memory involves the lateral temporal lobe, and thus its evolution might be revealed
by tracing the emergence of lateral temporal mechanisms of memory. Functional imaging
evidence suggests this may be a workable solution. Thus, while semantic memory involves
the lateral temporal lobe, conditioning involves widely scattered brain areas (Cabeza &
Nyberg, 2000), as might be expected from its arbitrary, associational nature.
Why would semantic memory draw on a specific region of the cortex such as the lateral
temporal lobe? Rogers et al. (2006) viewed the area as a hub that links modality-specific areas
of the brain. Thus, a semantic memory consists of a collection of facts linked together as a
concept. For example, a grapefruit has a waxy-feeling skin, is yellow, and emits a sour odor
as well as a satisfying squirting sound when dug into by a spoon. All of these features are
modality-specific – tactile, visual, olfactory, and auditory – and together comprise the
modality-free concept of a grapefruit. The lateral temporal lobe appears to be the cortical
region that links component features into an overall representation in semantic memory.
The use of lateral temporal cortex as a modality-free memory hub may have originated
in humans. There appears to be little evidence that macaques use the area in the same way.
To be sure, the tip of their temporal lobe – called the temporal pole – is involved in dis-
criminating between familiar and unfamiliar faces (Landi, Viswanathan, Serene, & Freiwald,
2021), and nearby perirhinal cortex (literally cortex “near the nose”) located in the medial
temporal lobe combines visual features in order to assign objects to classes (Bussey, Saksida, &
Murray, 2002). But because they are both visual, these operations are modality-specific.
One strong possibility is that the temporal pole and perirhinal cortex were the evolu-
tionary “kernels” of a semantic memory system that now primarily involves lateral cortex in
Memory 255
humans. Semantic dementia affects medial as well as lateral temporal areas (Landin-Romero
et al., 2016), although medial damage alone does not produce the full syndrome. For that,
lateral damage is required (Rogers et al., 2006). In humans, relative to monkeys and
chimpanzees, lateral temporal areas are greatly expanded in size and have become connected
to frontal lobe language areas, consistent with the input of meaning (semantic memory) into
language (Rilling et al., 2008).
However, it must be stressed that semantic memory is in part dependent on “local” levels
of the cortex where perceptual processes are conducted. Thus, damage to particular areas of
the brain can cause an inability to perceive particular classes of things. For example, fusiform
cortex lesions can affect face recognition (Iidaka, 2014), occipital lesions color recognition
(Bouvier & Engel, 2006), occipitotemporal lesions the recognition of living things
(Humphreys & Riddoch, 2003), superior temporal lesions music recognition (Sihvonen
et al., 2016), and so on. In a broad sense, such perceptual deficits can be viewed as semantic:
The meaning of a class of objects is lost because they cannot be recognized.
The lateral temporal cortex should therefore be viewed as only one level, although the
most general level, of a broader semantic memory system. The implication is that other
animals have the local elements of such a system even though they may lack the more general
component. Or, in evolutionary terms, human semantic memory may be much older in its
local levels than it is in its general one.
Conclusion
Truly comparative research in memory is surprisingly meager given that animals have
figured prominently as memory research subjects. Nevertheless, habituation, a decline in
response to repeated stimulation, can be regarded as a universal memory phenomenon,
with origins tracing to single-cell organisms. Association in the form of conditioning
is nearly as universal, extending at least as far back as the cnidarian divergence over
605 million years ago.
A case for universality, at least for vertebrates, can also be made for sensory memory.
Sensory memory is a short-lived trace that preserves much of the physical form of stimuli. It
lasts less than half a second up to ten seconds depending on the sensory modality. In the case
of human visual sensory memory, there is strong evidence of both a peripheral retinal
component and a central brain component, although in both cases the “icon” is best viewed
as an aftereffect of nervous system activity. Because other vertebrates have similar sensory
modalities and receptors, it is reasonable to believe they have the peripheral component of
sensory memory.
However, the universality assumption may not be valid in the case of the central com-
ponent, at least for the visual modality, because the brain area involved (V1) is a late
mammalian innovation dating to about 220 million years ago. Rhesus monkeys appear to
have the central component, as indicated by the existence of delayed masking.
Unfortunately, we do not have data on earlier-diverging taxa.
Comparative research is also sparse with respect to short-term memory, a small-capacity
form of memory that holds information for about 20 seconds in the absence of rehearsal.
Short-term memory capacity and search time are linked, such that fewer “slots” are
available for items requiring more memory, while search time is longer. Limited research
indicates that pigeons, monkeys, and humans all show evidence of a recency effect, in
which the most recently encoding items show increased recall, indicating the widespread
existence of short-term memory. However, its duration is haphazard across taxa, and it
appears that its neocortical mechanisms as found in primates are absent in birds. Thus,
primate and bird short-term memories appear to represent convergent evolution.
256 Perception and cognition
For the present, there is no evidence of intermediate memory in humans. It has been
found in birds and several invertebrates, with a measured duration on the order of one to six
hours depending on the species.
Long-term memory has been a more fruitful area for comparative study. Episodic
memory, which is memory for particular events, has been demonstrated in the lowland
gorilla King. King can reproduce the order of closely spaced events at intervals well beyond
the duration of short-term memory. His performance appears to demonstrate one-trial
learning of circumstances he knows are no longer true, that persist for a substantial length of
time, and that cannot be attributed to familiarity alone, together indicating the presence of
episodic memory.
Scrub jays and apes also exhibit episodic memory by searching for either of two types of
food cached in single events, contingent on delay which affects the integrity of the food.
Studies of rats likewise suggest the existence of episodic memory, and implicate the hip-
pocampus, the same anatomical substrate of episodic memory that is found in primates in-
cluding humans. Because all of these species, including birds, have a hippocampus, there may
well be a common evolutionary origin for episodic memory. However, it is unclear to what
extent it has supported a recollective quality of memory throughout its history.
Finally, semantic memory (memory for facts) may have had its origins in local brain areas
where particular classes of stimuli are recognized. If so, its history is likely to prove very
complicated, as each local area may have had a separate evolutionary trajectory. However,
the most general level of semantic memory may only have emerged in humans, involving
modality-free lateral temporal cortex mechanisms. It is possible that this was an outgrowth
of primate processing of multiple visual features in nearby perirhinal cortex as well as the
temporal pole.
15 Language
One day in 1995, a New York Times reporter visited a primate research facility in Georgia
and witnessed an interesting interaction. A bonobo named Panbanisha, walking with her
trainer Dr. Sue Savage-Rumbaugh, abruptly pressed three symbols on a portable keyboard,
multiple times in different orders. The symbols signified “Mad”, “Fight”, and “Austin”.
Savage-Rumbaugh surmised that there had been a fight at the residence of Austin, a
chimpanzee at the facility, which Panbanisha confirmed. Subsequently, it was discovered that
there had indeed been a fight between two chimpanzees over the use of computer equip-
ment. It had apparently been heard by Panbanisha in a different building some 200 feet
distant (Johnson, 1995).
Copious anecdotal evidence of a wide variety of cognitive skills in general and language
skills in particular have been provided by apes trained in the use of symbols or sign language.
From the Panbanisha anecdote, it seems straightforward to infer the existence of episodic
memory (recollection of a previous incident), semantic memory (identification of Austin
through sound alone), representational capacity (mapping the concept of Austin on to a
symbol), vocabulary (employment of a set of symbols to convey meaning), and syntax or
grammar (organization of symbols in orders that succeed in conveying meaning). To say that
studies of ape language have revolutionized our thinking about the mental capacities of our
nearest relatives would be an understatement.
But how much of the inferred mental repertoire of apes is based in science, and how much
in wishful thinking? As we have seen, there is reason to believe that apes do have episodic
memories. At minimum, they have elements of semantic memory in being able to assign
meaning to stimuli, even if they lack the most general, modality-free aspect of human se-
mantic memory. In this chapter, the remaining inferences from the Panbanisha anecdote are
examined, and we consider the extent to which apes show language capabilities in the form
of representational capacity, vocabulary, and grammar. We then consider how these capa-
cities led to human language, first considering evolutionary changes in the brain, and then the
chronology of changes – both physiological and behavioral – that created modern language.
Representational capacity
The ability to mentally represent the world in a realistic, literal way has been termed primary
representation. It refers to a direct semantic relationship between what is in the head and what
is in the environment (Leslie, 1987). Human infants show evidence of primary representation
from a very early age. By 4-1/2 months, they reach for suspended objects, indicating that
they use boundary information to separate objects from the surrounding environment. At this
age, infants appear to extract a considerable amount of information about objects, in that
distance, size, and movement all affect reaching. Even earlier, by six weeks of age, infants can
DOI: 10.4324/9781003137863-18
258 Perception and cognition
detect the difference between an object that is partially hidden behind a second object, and
one with the hidden part actually missing (Spelke, 1982).
Primary representation is clearly a prerequisite for language in that an object must be
mentally represented before it can be named. However, the ability to perceptually separate
objects from the environment, and even understand their meaning, is an ancient one and as
such sheds little light on language evolution. More complex representations, on the other
hand, yield more interesting relationships to language.
Secondary representation
Secondary representation refers to the ability to separate a primary representation from its en-
vironmental reference for hypothetical purposes (Suddendorf & Whiten, 2001). For example,
using a banana as a play telephone implies secondary representation because in the real world,
bananas are not telephones.
The relevance of secondary representation to language is that it allows names to be used
without their environmental references being present (Suddendorf & Whiten, 2001). Before
cell phones, it was common to say “I have a telephone in my office” without being in the
office, a displacement in space, or “I used to have a telephone”, a displacement in time, or “I
need to find a telephone”, a reference to the entire universe of telephones. The name could
even be used metaphorically, as in “I played telephone”, the child’s game in which state-
ments become progressively distorted as they are transmitted in a chain from one individual
to another.
Several phenomena indicate fairly strongly that apes are capable of secondary re-
presentation. As we have seen in previous chapters, chimpanzees can plan the creation of
tools, selecting an appropriate twig from which the leaves are stripped to become a “fishing”
tool, or a stick whose end is sharpened to become a spear. Secondary representation must be
present to allow the raw materials to be visualized as tools, which before their creation are not
literally present in the environment. It is also present when home-reared chimpanzees play
with dolls, bathing them or giving them something to “eat” (Tomasello & Call, 1997). Wild
chimpanzees, especially female ones, have been observed treating a log or stick as a baby, e.g.,
making a nest for it, or carrying it for periods of several hours (Kahlenberg & Wrangham,
2011).
The ape ability to perform hidden displacement tasks has also been interpreted as in-
dicating secondary representation. In one such task, an object is hidden in a small box, which
is then concealed in a larger box. The smaller box is then removed but no longer contains the
object. On discovering this absence, if test subjects look for the object in the larger box they
are considered to show secondary representation because the hidden object has been inferred
to have a hypothetical existence inside the larger box. Human one-year olds fail this test but
two-year olds typically pass. Chimpanzees, gorillas, and orangutans also pass (Collier-Baker,
Davis, Nielsen, & Suddendorf, 2006; Jaakkola, 2014; Suddendorf & Whiten, 2001).
Some of the most striking evidence of secondary representation comes from studies of
mirror-image recognition, in which apes demonstrate that they recognize themselves in a
mirror by using it to view parts of their body they otherwise cannot see. For example, many
chimpanzees will reach up to touch a mark placed on their brow, having used the mirror to
discover the mark (Anderson & Gallup, 2015). This is secondary representation because the
animal recognizes that the image is not itself an actual chimpanzee, but rather a hypothetical
chimpanzee representing them.
Is there evidence of secondary representation in more distant relatives of humans, such as
monkeys? Both New World and Old World monkeys generally fail hidden displacement
tasks, with their few apparent successes perhaps attributable to low-level strategies such as
Language 259
“pick the last-touched box” and not to secondary representation. The same may be true of
lesser apes (Jaakkola, 2014). Monkeys generally, including capuchins, squirrel monkeys,
macaques, and baboons, do not pass the mirror “mark” test, nor do lesser apes as a rule
(Anderson & Gallup, 2015). Thus, among primates, secondary representation appears to be
largely a great ape capacity.
Vocabulary
The creation of a secondary representation is basically the creation of a symbol. A banana is
not an actual telephone, but it can be a symbol of a telephone. Indeed, it has been argued that
even so simple a cognitive act as creating a mental image is symbolic, because the image is not
itself what it represents (Russon, 2004). In effect, some thing has been parsed from its en-
vironment, isolated, and given independent status as a mental entity.
Conceptually, it seems a small step to give such entities names and begin building a vo-
cabulary. Evolutionarily, however, it is a huge step mastered by only one living species and
dabbled in, with prodding, by a handful of others. A little thought gives some idea why the
step is so large. First, a concrete means of reference must become established, both to transmit
symbols and to receive them. Will the symbols be transmitted visually, e.g., through hand
gestures? Or will they be transmitted auditorily, e.g., through vocalizations? Also, how is
such a system bootstrapped from nothing, so that a symbol is both known by its transmitter to
be producible, and by its receiver to be meaningful? Finally, there must be sufficient mental
capacity on the part of both the transmitter and the receiver that multiple symbols can be
combined to convey actions and to flexibly cover new situations. For example, it might do
no good to tell hunters to “throw” if they are not sure when to throw, or what, or where.
Thus, communication is likely to be ineffective if both the transmitter and the receiver have
difficulty handling multiple symbols.
Of these varied parts of the larger problem, reception of symbols seems much the easiest to
achieve. A wide variety of species – including, it is safe to say, all primates – can readily learn
that one symbol represents a food reward and another does not. Discrimination learning has
been claimed to be a fundamental ability of all vertebrates (Moore, 2004). However, Russon
(2004) pointed out that the symbolic abilities of apes go well beyond mere associative
processes. For example, some chimpanzees understand how a scale model or photograph of a
room relates to its full-scale counterpart, and can use it to find a hidden soda. Others,
however, fail to master this task (Kuhlmeier, Boysen, & Mukobi, 1999). These results suggest
that as a species, chimpanzees are on the edge of understanding that a representation of a
space is symbolic of it, an ability acquired in humans’ third year of life (Russon, 2004).
Then, too, the use of multiple internalized symbols seems within the capabilities of
chimpanzees. For example, the idea of using a hammer to hit a nut placed on an anvil leveled
by a wedge represents a hierarchy in three levels, which chimpanzees routinely master. In
contrast monkeys have not demonstrated a capacity to hierarchically represent multiple levels
(Russon, 2004).
From the standpoint of great apes including hominins, therefore, the biggest problems in
establishing a vocabulary do not appear to be receiving or combining multiple symbols.
Instead, they are the establishment of a transmission system, and its bootstrapping so that the
transmitter knows the vocabulary is producible and the receiver knows its meaning.
Grammar
In my undergraduate classes, after describing some of the vocabulary-related findings of sign
and lexigram studies with apes, I ask my students to debate whether the capabilities reflect
262 Perception and cognition
language. Usually I get no takers on the “not language” side of the debate. Typically, every
student – or at least every student willing to express an opinion – endorses the conclusion that
apes can learn and use a rudimentary language.
But then, virtually all of my students are psychology students. To many psychologists
meaning is everything, or a big chunk of it. If a chimpanzee signs “give X” when “X”
is available, and the chimpanzee seems satisfied on receiving it, the intent for commu-
nication seems clear, the meaning definite, and the use of vocabulary in a language-like
way undeniable.
Linguists, in contrast, are often much more skeptical of the language nature of these
capabilities. To a linguist, the hallmark of language is grammar, not meaning. Grammar is
what allows speakers to communicate (and listeners to distinguish) the meaning of sen-
tences that use similar wording. For example, I might say, “The dog that the child is
chasing is wet”, or “The dog is chasing the child that is wet”. The two sentences use
identical words but have very different interpretations. Using grammar, we can generate
meanings that are not present in the vocabulary alone, greatly magnifying both the
complexity and subtlety of our communications.
On the grammar criterion, apes fall short. This first became apparent in the signing of the
chimpanzee Neam Chimpsky, “Nim” for short, playfully named after the famous linguist
Noam Chomsky. In his first four years, Nim learned 125 signs. Initial analysis of Nim’s two-
sign combinations showed some evidence for the consistent ordering that would be expected
if ape signing followed grammatical rules. For example, “more X” and “give X” combi-
nations outnumbered “X more” and “X give” combinations by nearly a 2:1 ratio. However,
this trend toward ordering broke down completely with combinations of three or more
signs, leading to the conclusion that grammatical rules were not present (Terrace, Petitto,
Sanders, & Bever, 1979).
Thus was ignited an academic controversy. On the pro-language side, Greenfield and
Savage-Rumbaugh (1991) produced data showing that Kanzi ordered lexigrams in a gram-
matical way. For example, two-sign combinations of action-agent (e.g., “carry person) and
action-object (e.g., “keepaway balloon”) outnumbered agent-action and object-action com-
binations by nearly a 6:1 ratio. Subsequent data from two bonobos, including Kanzi, and one
chimpanzee, confirmed frequent ordering preferences in two-lexigram combinations, and in
combinations of a lexigram with a gesture (Lyn, Greenfield, & Savage-Rumbaugh, 2011).
Furthermore, in the 1991 data, three-sign combinations of action-action-agent (e.g.,
“chase bite P”, where “P” was the designation of a human caretaker) outnumbered other
orderings by a 7:1 ratio. Unfortunately, however, this particular observation itself proved
problematic, as these were the only three-sign combinations sufficiently frequent to be
analyzable. Even then only eight instances had been observed among all combinations of
three or more signs, which themselves constituted only 10% of Kanzi’s corpus of 13,000
utterances (Greenfield & Savage-Rumbaugh, 1991).
In truth, the average length of ape utterances remains at about two vocabulary items, or a
little less, regardless of length of training (Lyn et al., 2011; Miles, 1990; also Terrace et al.,
1979, idealized data in Figure 15.1). This renders arguments over grammar rather sterile; it
may not make sense to argue whether grammar is observed in multiple-sign combinations,
when utterance length itself falls so short of human capabilities.
Clearly, apes have minimal access to complex grammatical structures. Nevertheless the
simpler trends in two-sign combinations may be important, particularly given parallels ob-
served in primate gesture and vocalization. In their natural gestures, chimpanzees commu-
nicate actions that also imply an actor, a simple two-element grammar (Fouts & Waters,
2003). With respect to vocalization, in both bonobos and common chimpanzees, a kind of
phonological syntax (ordering of meaningless sound elements) is observed in long-distance calls.
Language 263
Figure 15.1 Mean length of utterance over time, comparing Nim to three human children using oral lan-
guage. Nim remained “stuck” at fewer than two words per utterance, unlike human children.
Source: Original figure.
For example, a “pant hoot” may be altered by inserting a “bark” into the sequence of
vocalizations. Such variations may be related to status, territoriality, and the need to maintain
contact with others (Ujhelyi, 1998). As in signing, and in natural gestures, they give a hint of
the beginnings of grammar. However, the grammatical structures are not complex.
Broca’s area
Broca’s area, consisting of Brodmann areas 44 and 45, is another frontal area involved in
language. In Chapter 10 the roles of the area in human speech production and object
264 Perception and cognition
manipulationa were highlighted. Both actions have grammatical qualities, requiring the rule-
based sequencing of component movements. Brain lesions in the vicinity of Broca’s area
often result in agrammatism, the disordering of grammar. Thus, speech becomes telegraphic,
for example, with the intended sentence “Yesterday I went to the movies” possibly be-
coming “Yesterday go movie”. Also, the decoding of grammatically complex sentences fails.
For example, upon hearing the sentence, “The boy that the girl is chasing is tall”, agram-
matism patients are typically at a loss in judging which person is doing the chasing, and which
person is tall (Miceli, 1999).
Recent work suggests that area 44, comprising a portion of Broca’s area, is particularly
involved in the production of the complex phrase structures characteristic of natural human
language. When syntactical processing is complicated, the posterior temporal cortex is also
recruited by way of connecting tracts, especially the arcuate fasciculus. Arcuate fasciculus
connections are much stronger in humans than in other primates, potentially accounting for
the weak to nonexistent grammar of ape utterances. They are also more lateralized, to the left
hemisphere, in humans relative to chimpanzees and macaques (Balezeau et al., 2020; Eichert
et al., 2019).
As noted in Chapter 10, primate area F5 is homologous to the area 44 portion of Broca’s
area. F5 contains mirror cells that participate in the copying of sequential actions modeled by
others, implicating it in gesture. Furthermore in chimpanzees, asymmetry favoring the left
inferior frontal gyrus (the location of F5) correlates to the strength of right-handedness in
manual gestures, but not to handedness as evidenced in reaching (Hopkins & Cantalupo,
2004; Taglialatela, et al., 2006). Significantly, the correlation also extends to the insertion-
turning behaviors we examined in the handedness chapter (Hopkins, et al., 2017). Those
behaviors can be viewed as having a sequential, grammar-like structure (e.g., grasp an object,
insert it into an opening, turn it, and pull it toward oneself). Furthermore, a link to speech is
suggested by the fact that chimpanzee communication gestures are more right-handed when
accompanied by vocalization, than when emitted alone (Hopkins & Cantero, 2003; Hopkins
et al., 2005). Together these findings strongly suggest that in the chimpanzee, vocalization
and gesture are both at least partially controlled by F5 in the left hemisphere.
The implication is that the involvement in grammar of what in humans is Broca’s area has
roots tracing back to F5 in our common ancestor with chimpanzees. But should we attribute
the origin of those roots to gesture, or to vocalization? The answer is of critical importance,
because the difference is between a gestural origin or a vocal origin of human language.
On one hand, Taglialatela, Russell, Schaeffer, and Hopkins (2008) argued that lesioning
the Broca’s area homolog in monkeys does not affect their vocalizations. That observation
could be taken as supporting an early evolutionary role for the area specifically in gesture
rather than vocalization.
However, on the other hand, F5 in macaques activates during the production of lips-
macking, a form of vocalization (Shepherd & Freiwald, 2018). Furthermore, both macaques
and chimpanzees produce lip-smacks in a rhythm averaging four times a second – the same as
in human speech, reflecting the production of syllables with an open-closed mouth cycle
(Pereira, Kavanagh, Hobaiter, Slocombe, & Lameira, 2020). Together these results imply an
evolutionary continuity of F5/Broca’s area involvement in vocalization, dating back to or
prior to the divergence of Old World monkeys from our ancestral line.
In truth, the most likely scenario for the origin of language is that it was both gestural and
vocal. In comparison to using only one modality, using both improves signal detection,
disambiguates meaning, and increases the amount of information transmitted (Wilke et al.,
2017). Bonobos and common chimpanzees often pair gestures and vocalizations when
communicating, and pairing improves the chances of obtaining the desired end compared to
the use of either alone (Pollick & de Waal, 2007; Wilke et al., 2017). A further indication of
Language 265
Figure 15.2 Movement of the mouth and tongue accompanying the fine motor movements of a child.
Source: Wesley and his tongue, CC BY 2.0, Christopher Cornelius.
the intimate link between gesture and vocalization is that when chimpanzees make fine
motor movements with their hands, their mouths often make accompanying movements
(Wacewicz, Zywickzynski, & Orzechowski (2016). Human children, of course, do much the
same (Figure 15.2).
Interestingly, in contrast to common chimpanzees and bonobos, the pairing of vocalization
and gesture in gorillas and orangutans appears to be rare, even though each behavior alone is
fairly common (Knox et al., 2019; Poss, Kuhar, Stoinski, & Hopkins, 2006). This suggests
that coproduction of vocalizations and gestures may have originated, in large part, with the
chimpanzee-human common ancestor. Thus, it seems reasonable to suppose that at the very
beginning of human language, we made use of all of the communication tools at our disposal
in order to make ourselves understood. Gesturing and vocalization together were very likely
those tools.
Figure 15.3 The Sylvian fissure in relation to Wernicke’s and Broca’s areas.
Source: Adaptation of Brain Surface Gyri.SVG, CC BY-SA 4.0, James.mcd.nz.
sounds. Above the Sylvian fissure is inferior parietal cortex, which is involved in the ongoing
storage of phonology (language phonetics), necessary to understanding connected speech.
Figure 15.4 Human (top) and macaque (bottom) brains. Note that the inferior parietal cortex (PCi) of the
macaque lacks gyri present in the human.
Source: The RM was used in order to construct the connectivity matrices of the two species, CC BY-SA 4.0, A. Goulas,
M. Bastiani, G. Bezgin, H.B.M. Uylings, A. Roebroeck, et al.
phonological coding, and phonological working memory. Thus, the supramarginal gyrus
(area 40) plays a role in all of these (Frey et al., 2005; Jax et al., 2006; Liebenthal et al., 2013),
while the angular gyrus (area 39) aids the copying of hand postures by differentiating between
fingers and discriminating right from left (Denes, 1999). They are clearly structures one
might expect evolution to have altered if early language was both gestural and vocal in origin,
and evolution has done so.
Broca’s cap
Following on that beginning, it can probably be assumed that attaining speech-like utterances
depended at least in part on the conversion of F5 to more resemble the modern Broca’s area.
An indirect indicator of Broca’s area is Broca’s cap, an outward bulge of cortex in that vicinity.
It first appears with consistency in Homo habilis endocasts, having had variable expression in
apes and in australopithecines (Broadfield et al., 2001; Tobias & Campbell, 1981). Recently,
it was discovered that a well-defined Broca’s cap in endocasts counterintuitively depends on
the cap in the left hemisphere having shrunk relative to the corresponding area in the right
hemisphere, bringing its borders into better relief (Balzeau, Gilissen, Holloway, Prima, &
Grimaud-Hervé, 2014). Thus, if the cap is first clearly visible in Homo habilis, the shrinkage
may have begun by 2.4 million years ago. At about the same time, the gyri more directly
marking Broca’s area may have first made impressions on endocasts (Falk, 1983).
Falk (2014) reported that chimpanzees and humans have different sulcal patterns in the
region, affecting the positioning of Broca’s cap with respect to Brodmann areas.
Nevertheless, there were possible indications in an endocast of the 2-million-year-old species
Australopithecus sediba of a transitional state between chimpanzees and modern humans.
Recently, another study found that in 1.7-million-year-old hominins, the ventral end of the
precentral sulcus (see Figure 10.3) had shifted in a posterior direction, suggesting a late
expansion of inferior frontal cortex (Ponce de León et al., 2021). Taken together, these
results suggest reorganization of the region in hominins following the last chimpanzee-
human common ancestor.
Another discontinuity across species is apparent when the area of the third frontal con-
volution is measured as an approximation of Broca’s area, a quantity that can be assessed using
endocasts. In early hominins, the area was smaller in smaller brains, but beginning with late
Homo species (rhodensiensis/heidelbergensis, neanderthalensis, and sapiens), the size of Broca’s area
has been the same irrespective of brain size, even in relatively small-brained individuals
(Balzeau, et al., 2014). This seems reminiscent of the preservation of area 10 during the
dwarfing of the brain of Homo floresiensis, which was interpreted in Chapter 11 as suggesting
the area’s critical importance to that species. Thus, in completion of the analogy, Broca’s area
270 Perception and cognition
may have assumed critical functional significance by the onset of the later Homo species about
one million years ago.
By 370,000 years ago, according to a molecular clock estimate, an important genetic
change occurred that further enabled language. A gene known as FOXP2, located on
chromosome 7, has been linked to language capacity via an extended family with a de-
fective allele. Affected members of the family cannot properly combine component oral
movements such as protruding the tongue while closing the lips, and have trouble re-
peating even short word sequences (Lieberman, 2007). Imaging indicates that their Broca’s
area has reduced gray matter but increased activation, in the left hemisphere specifically
(Vargha-Khadem et al., 1998). The significance is that changes to the gene preceded the
split between Neanderthals and modern humans, although it postdated the chimpanzee-
human divergence. Thus, DNA analysis indicates that Neanderthals shared our form of
the gene (Krause et al., 2007), placing its origin somewhere in excess of 370,000 years
ago (Noonan, et al., 2006). To the extent that it is a “language gene” (a cautionary note
is that intelligence is affected as well; Lieberman, 2012), the result implies the existence of
language in that time frame.
Wernicke’s area
Although changes to Wernicke’s area in the temporal lobe are difficult to date, at some point
that area’s cellular microstructure changed. In chimpanzees, the cell bodies of neurons in the
planum temporale line up in minicolumns that are about 36 micrometers (0.036 millimeters)
wide in both hemispheres. In contrast, in humans there is a noticeable asymmetry, with
wider columns existing in the left hemisphere (50 micrometers) than in the right (43 mi-
crometers). Thus there appears to have been a “rewiring” of the planum temporale, pre-
sumably related to the left hemisphere comprehension of language (Buxhoeveden, Switala,
Litaker, Roy, & Casanova, 2001). Unfortunately, there is no evidence bearing on exactly
when this change occurred.
The lungs
One substantial species distinction is that humans can control vocal air flow to a much
greater extent than apes. Our abilities are easily self-observed by monitoring breathing
while speaking out loud. Clearly, we can produce either sound bursts or continuous
vocalizations at will. This allows the sequencing of phonetic information, to the extent that
human sequencing of vocalizations can be ten times faster than that of apes (MacLarnon &
Hewitt, 2004).
An important element in the control of air flow is the control of breathing by the lungs
themselves. While speaking, exhalations become extended in time in order to produce the air
movement required for modification by the throat and mouth. Air pressure can also be
regulated by abdominal muscles controlling the lungs, to produce either loud or soft voca-
lizations. In contrast, other primates have much less control over breathing and cannot
produce vocalizations of anything like human complexity. In them, the regulation of vo-
calization much more involves the diaphragm that it does the abdominal muscles, the op-
posite of humans (MacLarnon & Hewitt, 2004).
Nevertheless in humans, some control over the breathing musculature is exerted by the
chest. It involves certain nerves exiting from the spinal cord at the thoracic vertebrae, those at
the level of the thorax. The nerves partially comprise the spinal cord, and so the vertebral canal
(the opening in the vertebrae through which the cord passes) must be wide enough to
accommodate them. This observation leads to a simple prediction: Species with the fine
breathing control needed for rapidly sequenced vocalizations should have enlarged vertebral
canals at the thoracic level, because of the increased size of nerves needed to control the
lungs. Of course, when looking for enlargement, it is necessary to take body size into
account, because larger species will have larger canals for that reason alone.
Putting this prediction to the test, MacLarnon and Hewitt showed that when thoracic
canal size is plotted against body size, all nonhuman primates track very closely to the
same line. In other words, when body weight is accounted for, they all have similar-sized
canals. However, modern humans place significantly above the line, indicating an en-
larged canal. Most critical to present purposes, though, were measurements of the size of
the thoracic canal in fossil hominin vertebrae. Neanderthals and early modern humans
likewise tracked above the primate line, while Australopithecus afarensis, Australopithecus
africanus, and a single Homo ergaster specimen fell almost exactly on it. The results strongly
suggest that precise control over breathing was achieved sometime between 1.6 million
years ago and 100,000 years ago, the age limits of the Homo ergaster specimen and the
more modern hominins. Unfortunately, at present we do not have sufficient vertebral
remains of intervening hominins to narrow this range further (MacLarnon & Hewitt,
1999, 2004).
272 Perception and cognition
The ear
A recent modeling of the auditory capabilities of reconstructed hominin ears suggests that
compared to modern humans, 430,000-year-old Homo heidelbergensis individuals had reduced
sensitivity in the 3,000–5,000 Hz range. That range is particularly important to the com-
prehension of speech generally, and consonants such as “t”, “f”, and “s” in particular. The
implication is that the structure of the ear changed at some point in our descent from hei-
delbergensis, to better accommodate the perception of modern speech sounds (Conde-
Valverde et al., 2021).
Strikingly, 130,000-year-old Neanderthals showed sensitivity in the 3,000–5,000 Hz range
that was nearly identical to modern humans. This result indicates that whatever occurred to
bring extended sensitivity to both species, operated sometime in the time window of 130,000
to 430,000 years ago (Conde-Valverde et al., 2021).
The jaw
Turning to the jaw, unlike Homo erectus we have a protruding, flat-surfaced chin, sometimes
described as an “inverted T” (Figure 15.5). Analysis of a series of jaws of modern Homo
sapiens, Neanderthals, and earlier specimens often attributed to Homo heidelbergensis, suggest
that it emerged about 90,000 years ago in modern humans (Schwartz & Tattersall, 2000).
However, that may only have been the final visible manifestation of a trend that began as
early as Australopithecus afarensis 3.7 million years ago, progressively pushing the chin forward
of the teeth (Pampush, Scott, Robinson, & Delezene, 2018).
The protruding chin has long been an evolutionary enigma, as its appearance does not
appear related to evolutionary changes in chewing. However, analysis of forces exerted by
Figure 15.5 A human chin with an “inverted T” (added for emphasis) formed from the mental protuberance
in the center, and the mental tubercle on each side.
Source: Adaptation of Sobo 1909 90.png, public domain, Dr. Johannes Sobotta.
Language 273
the tongue during speech do indicate that protruding chins reduce stress (Ichim, Kieser, &
Swain, 2007). Thus, its appearance about 90,000 years ago may be a sign of increased ar-
ticulatory ability. Whether that explanation can be stretched to a protrusion beginning as far
back as the australopithecines, of course, remains to be seen.
Figure 15.6 The “verticalization” of the modern human vocal tract, which is much more horizontal in apes,
Homo heidelbergensis, and Neanderthals.
Source: Adaptation of Midsagittal diagram unlabeled.svg, public domain, Wugapodes.
274 Perception and cognition
the full modern range of sounds would not have become possible until the vocal cavities
became fully vertical in modern Homo sapiens (Barney, Martelli, Serrurier, & Steele, 2012;
Lieberman, 2007). The date has been suggested as somewhere between 40,000 and
90,000 years ago (Lieberman, 2006). At the same time, the root of the tongue descended
deeper in the throat, becoming thicker and more rounded, and better able to control
air flow during vocalization (Lieberman, 2007).
Also consistent with change between Neanderthals and modern humans, is the finding that
genes influencing the development of our vocal tract – the vocal cords, pharynx, and larynx –
are overrepresented among genes that differ between the two species (Weiss et al., 2021).
Clearly something was working to favor the selection of more modern vocal tract char-
acteristics, and that something was no doubt language.
“Proto-World” language
However, there is additional behavioral evidence – though highly controversial – of a de-
velopment in language on the tens-of-thousands-of-years scale. It emerges from the com-
parative study of languages, suggesting that all existing language families may partly derive
from an original language spoken about 45,000 years ago (Ruhlen, 1994b). Specifically,
tracing the common roots of words has led to a claimed convergence on a small vocabulary
reflective of a hunter-gatherer society, with words that mean “ashes”, “bone”, “dog”,
“earth”, “water”, and so on. A reconstruction of 27 of the original words has been proposed
(Ruhlen, 1994a). For example, “kuan” is taken to mean “dog” and is proposed as the root of
the word “hound” in English, while “aq’wa” is thought to mean “water” and is proposed as
the origin of the Latin and English word “aqua” (Bradshaw, 1997; Ruhlen, 1994a).
This hypothetical “Proto-World” language is quite controversial. Evidence of it has been
called subjective, poorly repeatable, and even unscientific (McMahon, 2004). Notable
problems are that apparent correspondences between languages can be coincidental, and that
words can not only be inherited from an earlier language, but can also be borrowed from
unrelated ones (Nettle, 1999). It also is not clear how a 45,000 year-old language would have
spread across the globe.
However, it is worth pointing out that the Proto-World hypothesis places something of an
upper limit on the emergence of modern language. If it is untrue, then modern languages
Language 275
have even more recent origins. Either way, it must be the case that earlier languages, as-
suming they existed, were less successful. The reason why that might be can only be a matter
of speculation. However, the 45,000 year old date roughly coincides with the lower date
limit on the verticalization of the vocal tract, suggesting that modern language may have
depended on evolving a novel capability to produce a wide range of sounds.
Writing systems date back only about 5,500 years, yet reading is a nearly universal
human capability. The short time frame in which reading emerged, and its rapid
spread, imply that it is not a direct product of natural selection.
Instead, reading must have emerged as a co-option of other capabilities. Dehaene
and Cohen (2007) have suggested that one of the brain sites involved in reading
occupies an intersection of cortical areas sensitive to object identity and foveal
stimulation. The implication is that this visual word form area (VWFA), in or near the
occipito-temporal sulcus, is located where it is because recognizing words presented
visually calls for sensitivity to particular objects (words) at a particular location (the
fovea). In other words, reading has co-opted preexisting capabilities, and a preexisting
cortical map, for new purposes.
Even so, however, co-option requires the preexisting circuitry to be altered during
development. Consistent with this, the predominant role of the left hemisphere in
reading does not emerge until several years after reading begins. Thus visual field
studies of word recognition show no visual field difference – and therefore no
hemispheric difference – from the beginning of reading at about age 5 to around ages 8
to 10, when a transition to increasing left hemisphere representation begins. By age 11,
the leftward shift is complete ( Boles, et al., 2008). Similarly, reading-related evoked
potentials also shift leftward during development as reading finds its “optimal location”
in the left ventral visual system ( Dehaene and Cohen, 2007). Presumably it is optimal
because other language processes are located in the left hemisphere as well.
A second reading-relevant location is the left angular gyrus (Brodmann area 39),
involved in recoding visual words into phonological forms ( Boles, 2002; Pugh et al.,
2000). The angular gyrus adjoins Wernicke’s area, yet how “left hemisphered” an
individual is in reading, fails to positively predict how left hemisphered that individual
is in recognizing auditory language. In fact, there is actually a slight negative
relationship between the two, so that a person with an especially strong left hemisphere
involvement in one tends to have a reduced left hemisphere involvement in the other
( Boles, 1998). This pattern is consistent with the co-option view: The more left
276 Perception and cognition
parieto-temporal cortex is co-opted by either reading or auditory language compre-
hension, the less is available for the other.
It may be more appropriate to ask, “Will reading evolve?” Given its importance to
education and economic security, and the impact those have on reproduction, the
answer is almost certainly “yes”. However, as with natural selection more generally,
there is no way to predict the time frame or the precise nature of future adaptations.
Conclusion
Language did not spring forward in one great leap, like Athena from the forehead of her
father Zeus in Greek mythology. Instead it followed the course of other complex abilities,
starting crudely and acquiring increased sophistication by degrees over many millions of years
(Table 15.1). It was prefigured by the increased larynx size of primates over 68 million years
ago, and by the appearance of frontal lobe mirror cells prior to the Old World monkey
divergence 29 million years ago. These assisted the copying of others’ actions, an operation
that would later prove critically important in learning communication gestures.
Table 15.1 Milestones in the evolution of language. Mya = millions of years ago
Bef 68 mya Change in larynx size In primates: Increased size relative to size of
the body.
Bef 29 mya Frontal lobe mirror cells In living monkeys: Activity during grasping,
etc., and observations of same by others.
Asymmetric planum temporale In living monkeys: Larger on the left side.
Superior temporal gyrus involvement In living monkeys: Greater left-side
in recognizing vocalizations involvement.
Inferior parietal cortex asymmetry In living monkeys: Longer Sylvian fissure
on the left, indicating a larger left side.
Four times per second vocal rhythm In living monkeys: Lip-smacking. In
humans: Syllables.
Bef 15 mya Secondary representation In living apes: Tool manufacture; hidden
(displacements and metaphors) displacement; mirror-image recognition.
Symbolic capacity In living apes: Play objects; hierarchical
planning; learning of externally provided
languages (signs and lexigrams).
Simple grammar In living apes: Tendency to order two-
element sequences in externally provided
languages and in natural gestures;
vocalization insertions.
A “vocabulary” of natural In individual living apes: Repertoires of
communication gestures 10–30 gestures.
Shift from mostly body and head In living apes: Mostly manual (hand, finger,
gestures and arm) gestures
~12 mya Skilled handedness, possibly In living gorillas and chimps: Population
indicating left hemisphere area F5 right-handedness in tube task,
development hammering.
Bef 7.5 mya F5 co-production of communication In living chimps: Activation of F5 during
gestures and vocalizations co-occurrence of right-handed
communication gestures and
vocalizations; mouth movements
accompanying fine hand movements.
(Continued)
Language 277
Table 15.1 (Continued)
Aft 7.5 mya Increased laryngeal motor control In hominins: Laryngeal motor cortex
(otherwise recruitment of area 4, with stronger
uncertain) parietal connections and direct
connections to the brainstem.
Increased connections between In hominins: Relative to chimps.
Broca’s area and the temporal lobe,
especially on the left
Asymmetric minicolumns in In hominins: Found in humans but not
Wernicke’s area chimps.
Aft 3 mya Loss of air sacs, allowing speech-like In hominins: Hyoid bones suggest air sacs
vocalizations in Australopithecus africanus 3 mya, not
in Homo heidelbergensis 0.5 mya
2.4 mya Appearance of a well-defined Broca’s In hominins: First found in Homo habilis.
cap and gyri marking Broca’s area
(homologous to F5)
Appearance of supramarginal and In hominins: First found in Homo habilis.
angular gyri; “rounded fullness”
of inferior parietal lobe
~1.9 mya Face flattening and jaw shortening In hominins: Occurred between Homo
noticeable; shortening and habilis and Homo ergaster.
reorienting of tongue
~1 mya Stabilization of size of Broca’s area In hominins: Size of third frontal
convolution stabilized beginning with
Homo heidelbergensis.
0.1–1.6 mya Increased breathing control In hominins: Enlarged thoracic vertebral
canal.
Bef 0.37 mya FOXP2 allele affecting Broca’s area In hominins: Shared by Neanderthals and
Homo sapiens.
Bef 0.13 mya Increased sensitivity to speech In hominins: In the 3000–5000 Hz range,
frequencies shared by Neanderthals and Homo sapiens.
~.09 mya Protruding, flat-surfaced chin In hominins: First appeared in modern
supporting speech stresses humans as the culmination of a trend
beginning 3.7 mya.
.04–.09 mya Selection of modern vocal tract; In hominins: Verticalization and
full range of vocalizations overrepresentation of the vocal tract in
gene changes in modern humans.
.045 mya Appearance of modern vocabulary?
Our human cognitive nature produces a number of complex behaviors such as tool-making
and language. However, in the opinion of many, a nonbehavior, consciousness, is the
supreme achievement of the human mind.
Daniel Dennett (1981) likened the popular view of consciousness to an “inner light” that is
either on or off, and that is obvious from the inside. The problem, Dennett noted, is iden-
tifying consciousness from the outside. Are animals conscious? Could a machine be conscious?
Now, we must define external criteria that denote consciousness.
Dennett’s description, however, only scratches the surface of the mystery. Many psy-
chologists closely identify consciousness with attention, taking the view that whatever we are
attending to, we must be conscious of (e.g., Posner, 1978). However, that assumes a unitary
nature of attention that may not exist. Attention can be split between information sources
(Scharlau, 2004) and according to some theories, between tasks (Boles & Dillard, 2015). But,
are we really splitting consciousness when we split attention?
Even so, attention is only a beginning point in the universe of thought about con-
sciousness. In ever-expanding circles, consciousness has been equated with intellect
(Ornstein, 1972), the sum total of cognitive processes (Titchener, 1909), matter (Miller,
1962), reality (Eccles, 1966), and even unity with God (White, 1974). Some have linked it to
quantum physics (Mukhopadhyay, 2014). Small wonder, then, that others have argued that
consciousness cannot be defined (Granit, 1977).
The approach taken in this chapter is to examine behaviors that have been interpreted as
implying consciousness. This is a functional approach to the topic, emphasizing what
consciousness brings to behavior. Even so, some readers will demur. Thus, while sup-
porting functional approaches, Allen and Bekoff (2007) noted that some philosophers
consider consciousness an epiphenomenon of other mental processes, itself having no effect
on behavior.
It is likely that some of the behaviors that have been interpreted as implying consciousness
will be viewed as more compelling than others. But perhaps somewhere in the darkness, we
can catch a glimpse of the “inner light”.
Sensory awareness
An initial function of consciousness to consider is that of sensory awareness, the subjective
experience that we have sensed something external to us. At first glance, this may seem too
simplistic and self-evident to lend value to evolutionary analyses. However, certain failures of
sensory awareness in fact prove informative.
DOI: 10.4324/9781003137863-19
280 Perception and cognition
Blindsight and deaf hearing
Blindsight is a phenomenon in which an observer claims to be blind, yet shows unexpected
residual visual abilities. It results from partial or complete destruction of the primary visual
cortex (area 17; see Figures 16.1 and 10.4), producing blindness for part or all of visual space.
The basic demonstration of blindsight begins with the presentation of a visual stimulus in the
blinded area, to which the patient fails to respond. On prompting, the patient states that they
are blind and see nothing. However, if they are asked to point toward the place where the
stimulus was shown, they often are able to do so with surprising accuracy. Some patients can
even make discriminations of shape (e.g., squares from rectangles), identify line drawings of
animals or other objects, or discriminate colors, while simultaneously stating that they are
guessing (Ajina & Bridge, 2017; Trevethan, Sahraie, & Weiskrantz, 2007).
A similar phenomenon, deaf hearing, likewise occurs due to destruction of the primary
sensory cortex, this time Heschl’s gyrus or Brodmann area 41 bilaterally (see Figures 10.4
and 16.2). The patient denies hearing anything, but nevertheless orients to loud sounds like
hands clapping or fingers snapping (Garde & Cowey, 2000).
Blindsight and deaf hearing demonstrate that while sensory information entering the
brain through the primary cortex routinely reaches consciousness, when that route is
blocked it does not. The major visual route is the geniculostriate route, which proceeds from
the eye to the lateral geniculate nucleus to the primary visual cortex (area 17). It is this
route that produces visual awareness and is disrupted in blindsight. However, there is an
alternate tectopulvinar pathway that proceeds from the eye to the superior colliculus, then to
the pulvinar (a nucleus in the thalamus), and finally to the secondary visual areas of the
cortex (see Figure 16.1). This route bypasses area 17 altogether, providing cortical visual
input in the absence of area 17 function.
Is it the tectopulvinar pathway that supports the residual capabilities of blindsight?
Though long thought so, confirmatory evidence only emerged relatively recently. Leh,
Mullen, and Ptito (2006) studied hemispherectomy patients in which an entire cerebral
hemisphere had been surgically removed, producing profound blindness in one visual field.
They found that responses to visual stimuli in the sighted field were speeded by the re-
dundant presentation of the same stimuli in the blind field, a demonstration of blindsight.
However, the speeding of responses only occurred if the redundant stimuli were achro-
matic (i.e., in shades of white, gray, and black) and thus visible to the medium- and long-
wavelength cones that have inputs into the superior colliculus. If they were in colors
distinguishable only to short-wavelength cones, which have no input into the superior
colliculus, no speeding occurred. A subsequent fMRI study reported activation of visual
cortex beyond V1 and V2, when the redundant stimulus was achromatic but not when it
Figure 16.1 The geniculostriate and tectopulvinar visual pathways to the cortex.
Source: Original figure.
Consciousness 281
could be seen only by short-wavelength cones (Leh, Ptito, Schönwiesner, Chakravarty, &
Mullen, 2010). Together these results argue strongly that the intact pathway in blindsight is
routed through the superior colliculus, as is true of the tectopulvinar pathway. This route,
of course, does not appear to provide visual awareness.
In the auditory modality the major pathway is from the ear to the inferior colliculus, and
then to the ventral portion of the medial geniculate nucleus, before ending up in Brodmann
area 41, the primary auditory cortex (see Figure 16.2). Destruction of area 41 on both sides
can result in the deaf hearing phenomenon. The apparent cause is a secondary pathway
leading from the inferior colliculus to the dorsal portion of the medial geniculate nucleus,
then bypassing area 41 before ending up in secondary auditory cortex (area 42). No auditory
awareness appears possible through this route, although as indicated, the patient orients to
loud sounds (Garde & Cowey, 2000).
What can blindsight and deaf hearing tell us about the neural substrate of consciousness?
On one hand, it seems clear that under normal circumstances, stimulation of primary cortex is
a prerequisite for sensory awareness. If such stimulation doesn’t occur as in blindsight and deaf
hearing, sensory awareness does not occur. The most obvious possibility, then, is that stimu-
lation of primary cortex directly generates awareness. However, this can quickly be rejected on
the basis of a study of macaques by Nakamura and Mishkin (1980). They surgically isolated
visual cortex in one hemisphere – striate, inferotemporal, and other higher visual areas – from all
other cortexes in that hemisphere. They also blinded the other hemisphere by disconnecting
optic nerve input to it, and they disconnected the hemispheres by severing the cerebral com-
missures. The result was behavioral blindness. The monkeys could not perform a previously
trained visual discrimination, bumped into objects, and failed to react to fear-inducing objects.
In other words, even though the visual system seemed to be fully intact in one hemisphere –
though disconnected from the rest of the cortex – the monkeys showed no evidence of visual
awareness. Awareness therefore is not a property of visual cortical stimulation alone.
A second possibility is that information from the primary cortex travels to some other brain
structure that itself produces awareness. While this is difficult to rule out – a definitive test
would require ruling out all other brain structures, one by one – it seems unlikely. That’s
because in blindsight and deaf hearing, the residual sensory capabilities enter the cortex in
secondary areas just barely removed from the primary cortex. It seems very likely that they
have access to all the brain structures that primary cortex can access, yet awareness does not
result. Note that this also means the secondary areas, taken alone, do not produce awareness.
If primary and secondary sensory areas are not alone responsible for consciousness, what is?
There appears to be something unique about primary cortex in interaction with other areas of
the brain that results in awareness.
282 Perception and cognition
The role of reentry
A promising idea is that neurologically speaking, reentrant connections support such inter-
actions. In other words, the primary cortex stimulates higher-order cortical sites that in turn
send connections back to primary cortex. Perhaps that is somehow what results in sensory
awareness (Lamme, 2003; Rees & Frith, 2007).
There is in fact direct experimental evidence that supports the role of reentry. When
Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) to V1 is used to evoke an experience of phosphenes
(imaginary points of light) in intact brains, awareness of the phosphenes can be prevented
by V1 restimulation at a lower intensity that does not itself generate phosphenes, 30 msec or
so after the initial stimulation (Pascual-Leone & Walsh, 2001). This strongly suggests that if
reentry is prevented using subthreshold TMS, there is no awareness. Delayed TMS has also
been found to degrade V1 function compared to no delay, again consistent with reentry
although any effects on subjective awareness were not examined (Camprodon, Zohary,
Brodbeck, & Pascual-Leone, 2010).
Of course, the reentry solution does not completely resolve the problem, for we still have
little idea how it is that reentry results in sensory awareness (Lamme, 2003). In particular we
have little understanding how we generate the ineffable sensory qualities that baffle even the
most dedicated introspective observers (e.g., why is yellow yellow, or sour sour?). However,
reentry does at least allow us to begin an evolutionary analysis of the origins of sensory
awareness, by tracing the history of reentrant connections.
In this regard, reentrant connections into V1 certainly exist in macaques, where they have
been most extensively studied (Hupé et al., 2001). They also exist in squirrel monkeys, a
New World species (Rockland & Knutson, 2000), as well as in rats (Johnson & Burkhalter,
1996), ferrets, and cats (Cantone, Xiao, McFarlane, & Levitt, 2005). Indeed, feedback
connections are a fundamental characteristic of the mammalian brain (Johnson & Burkhalter,
1996). Thus, their evolutionary history dates as far back as the origin of mammals about
220 million years ago.
However, it is not clear whether they have supported sensory awareness across that entire
time span. Although cats have residual visual abilities following destruction of primary visual
cortex (Payne, Lomber, MacNeil, & Cornwell, 1996), which are consistent with blindsight,
we do not know whether the animals experience reduced visual awareness. What we do
know, however, is that monkeys experience blindsight, while simultaneously believing
themselves blind. Thus, Cowey and colleagues have found that macaques can localize stimuli
in their blind field. Yet if the same stimuli are shown in the same blind field, and the choice is
whether or not a stimulus was presented, the monkeys choose to respond that none appeared
(Cowey & Alexander, 2012). Thus, the monkeys appear to inform us that they believe
themselves blind. Monkeys can also be trained to respond to the location of stimuli in a blind
area even though they do not spontaneously shift their eyes to them (Moore, Rodman,
Repp, & Gross, 1995). These performances are very similar to those of human blindsight
patients who deny that a stimulus is present but who can localize it when encouraged to.
Putting these observations together it seems clear that sensory awareness dates back at least
to the human-macaque common ancestor about 29 million years ago. It may date back to the
origin of mammals 220 million years ago, if reentrant connections from higher to lower brain
areas are diagnostic of sensory awareness.
Attention
William James is widely credited with establishing a close correspondence between attention
and consciousness. In his book, Psychology (1892), he maintained that attention accentuates
Consciousness 283
and emphasizes perceptions that become the objects of consciousness, and that it ignores
(and thus causes consciousness to ignore) the rest of the many stimuli that constantly besiege
us. Later, authors equated consciousness with awareness and likewise emphasized the role of
attention in creating it (Posner, 1978; Taylor & Fragopanagos, 2007).
Metacognition
One possible function of consciousness is that it permits metacognition, the awareness of one’s
own cognitive states and processes. For example, it might involve a judgment of how much
one knows in a particular circumstance. This aids decision making by distinguishing between
situations where action can be taken because knowledge is sufficient, and situations where it
should be withheld because knowledge is insufficient. Such “feelings-of-knowing” are vague
and error-prone, as are other products of metacognition like judgments of how easily one
will learn something, or of the quality of one’s retrieved answers (Nelson, 1996). They can
therefore be viewed as a type of “fringe consciousness” that is constructed from the outcomes
of unconscious processes (Norman, 2017).
Even young children have metacognitive skills. In one study, picture-number pairs such as
a dog and the number “2” were presented for learning. Then in the testing phase, a number
was presented and the child could either choose to decline answering, or to view an array
of pictures with the aim of choosing the correct partner – e.g., the dog if a “2” had been
presented. If correct, a highly rewarding visual display was shown. If incorrect none was
shown, while declining to respond received a less rewarding display. The results showed that
4.5- to 5-year-old children declined to respond to pictures that were difficult to remember
40% of the time, but those that were easy to remember only 21% of the time. However
younger children, 4–4.5 years old, did not do this (34% versus 33%). The implication is that
“feeling of knowing” is a metacognitive faculty that develops beginning at about 4.5 years of
age (Liu, Su, Xu, & Pei, 2018).
In a second study, children always responded by trying to pick out a studied picture from a
larger array, but could indicate afterward whether or not they felt confident in their decision.
Children age 4–5 showed much higher accuracy when they felt confident than when they
did not (82% versus 28%), a difference that wasn’t found in children age 3–4 (56% versus
46%, a nonsignificant difference). Thus, the “confidence in retrieved answers” form of
metacognition (Nelson, 1996) appears to begin developing around age 4 (Liu et al., 2018).
Monkeys, too, show evidence of metacognition. Shields and colleagues tested two
rhesus macaques on their confidence in retrieved answers. A rectangle that was filled either
densely or sparsely with pixels was presented, and the task was to respond by pressing an
“X” to a dense rectangle or “O” to a sparse one. Difficulty was varied by changing the
density within each of these categories. Thus a dense rectangle could be relatively sparse,
or a sparse rectangle relatively dense, with either of these representing a difficult trial. After
learning the task, the monkeys were next introduced to confidence ratings, learning to
press a blue bar if they had confidence in their just-made response or a pink bar if not.
Food rewards and time-out punishments were arranged to appropriately incentivize the
monkeys. It was found that accuracy was higher when the monkeys expressed confidence
in their judgment that when they did not, by 14%. This was quite similar to the outcome
of a parallel experiment using adult humans, also finding a 14% differential (Shields, Smith,
Guttmannova, & Washburn, 2005). The outcome was also similar to that from a later study
testing event order judgments in macaques, showing that when the animals were allowed
Consciousness 285
to select which trials to respond to, accuracy was higher than when selection was not
allowed (Templer, Brown, & Hampton, 2018).
Are New World monkeys also aware of what they know? Tests administering a version of
the sparsity task to capuchins produced ambiguous results. Monkeys were allowed to make
an opt-out response when presented with the rectangle, with the expectation that they
would make the response more frequently in the most difficult trials. Five monkeys com-
pletely failed to do so in spite of appropriate incentives. However, a sixth monkey did so after
extensive experience with the task (Beran, Smith, Coutinho, Couchman, & Boomer, 2009).
A later set of studies used the opt-out procedure with a different discrimination task
involving picking the largest of a set of squares. Macaques were found to regularly use the
opt-out response, reaching 40% of the most difficult trials. Capuchins were much less likely
to use it, opting out of only 10% of the most difficult trials. A second experiment shrunk this
difference between species but did not eliminate it (Beran, Perdue, & Smith, 2014).
Other studies using varying methods have supported the existence of metacognition in all
of the great ape species (including humans) and in Old World monkeys (i.e., macaques).
Evidence for it is mixed in New World monkeys (i.e., capuchins), with some studies failing
to find it, and others finding either reduced proficiency or proficiency only in some in-
dividuals and not others (Beran et al., 2014). While including other monkey species in future
research would be desirable, the tentative conclusion is that New World monkeys are “on
the cusp” with respect to metacognitive abilities – and thus on the cusp with respect to
“fringe consciousness” as well. Thus metacognition may have barely begun to evolve in our
common ancestor with New World monkeys 44 million years ago.
Self-recognition
Another possible function of consciousness is that it allows self-recognition. Gordon
Gallup (1968) first reported self-directed behaviors in a chimpanzee following brief
exposure to a mirror:
The chimp’s initial responses to the mirror consisted of the typical other-directed
behaviors such as lip smacking, vigorous head bobbing, a few threats, and intense
inspection of the mirror image. [Afterward,] however, the other-directed orientation
was abruptly displaced…. He was observed to stick out his tongue at the mirror, and
shake his brass neck tag in front of the mirror. Perhaps the most significant evidence of
self-directed behavior was the fact that the chimp would actively groom various parts of
his body otherwise not visually accessible except in the presence of a mirror (for
example, the forehead and eyebrows).
The unique feature of mirror-image stimulation is that the identity of the observer and
his reflection in a mirror are necessarily one and the same. The capacity to correctly infer
the identity of the reflection must, therefore, presuppose an already existent identity on
the part of the organism making this inference. Without an identity of your own it
would be impossible to recognize yourself. And therein may lie the basic difference
between monkeys and great apes. The monkey’s inability to recognize himself may be
due to the absence of a sufficiently well-integrated self-concept.
In other words, chimpanzees, who unlike monkeys can self-recognize, have a self-concept and
are thus self-aware. Furthermore, Gallup argued, self-awareness implies self-consciousness.
Species EQ
pig 0.27
sheep 0.54
horse 0.73
rat 0.84
European cat 1.14
fox squirrel 1.43
ring-tailed lemur 1.45
gorilla 1.54
African elephant 1.67
Asian elephant 2.14
chimpanzee 2.32
bottlenose dolphin 3.93
human 7.51
Conclusion
There are many conceptions of consciousness, ranging from an identification with attention,
to considering it a byproduct of large-scale systems like the totality of cognitive processes or
even of reality itself. The comparative study of consciousness introduces further uncertainties
because external behaviors are used to infer the functions of consciousness, and disagreements
often arise over the meaning of such behaviors.
Sensory awareness, while a seemingly simplistic function of consciousness, in fact is
quite informative when impaired, as in blindsight and deaf hearing. Patients who deny
seeing anything but who can localize stimuli, and others who deny hearing anything but
who orient to loud sounds, have in common a destruction of primary sensory cortex
accompanied by a lack of sensory awareness. Experiments using TMS in humans, and
disconnection of visual cortex from higher order cortex in monkeys, suggest that sensory
awareness is the result of reentrant connections from higher order cortex back to primary
cortex. These connections appear to be characteristic of mammals in general, and thus have
a 220 million year history. While it is unknown whether they supported sensory awareness
that long ago, macaques experience blindsight, putting a lower limit of about 29 million
years on the origin of sensory awareness.
In contrast, the idea that attention connotes consciousness is more problematic, in part
because it is not a unitary concept. Attention as selection, as recognition of meaning, and as
alertness are all fair characterizations, but such a subdivision greatly complicates the task of
tracing the evolution of consciousness. One of these, selection, seems especially problematic
in that it may have its origins in the first appearance of nervous systems.
294 Perception and cognition
Metacognition, the awareness of one’s own cognitive states and processes, includes such
phenomena as “feelings of knowing” and the ability to judge the quality of one’s retrieved
answers. Because these feelings and judgments are vague and error-prone, they can be
viewed as a kind of “fringe consciousness” constructed from unconscious processes.
Human research suggests that some forms of metacognition begin developing between 4
and 5 years of age. All great apes as well as an Old World monkey (the rhesus macaque)
show evidence of metacognition, but a New World species (the capuchin monkey) is “on
the cusp”, with studies finding either no proficiency, reduced proficiency, or proficiency
only in some individuals.
Self-recognition seems a fairly straightforward function of consciousness, in the sense that
it is easily demonstrated through self-directed responses using mirrors. It has been found in
apes, Asian elephants, and horses, and perhaps in siamangs, dolphins, corvids, and cleaner
wrasses. Apes, which have been most extensively tested, show individual differences in that
about a quarter of adolescents and young adults, and three-quarters of older adults, fail to self-
recognize. This effect appears to be at least partially developmental in that some animals that
self-recognize earlier in life fail to do so later. Many individuals never succeed in recognizing
themselves. These observations suggest that apes may be barely over an evolutionary divide
that excludes some individuals as well as Old and New World monkeys.
Encephalization may well play a role, in that most (but not all) self-recognizing species
have relatively high encephalization quotients. Research on chimpanzees suggests that there
may also be important roles for brain connectivity, specifically between the parietal and
frontal lobes; and sociality, with self-recognizing individuals showing greater social ability.
Theory of mind is often taken as a manifestation of self-awareness, on the grounds that
imputing a state of knowledge in others presupposes understanding similar states in oneself.
Some simple behaviors that might be taken as implying a theory of mind, such as gaze
following, helping behaviors, and deception, are widespread among animal species, but are
also potentially interpretable in stimulus-response or instinctual terms. More complex
behaviors, such as approaching and looking around a barrier to see what a human is staring
at, or selectively targeting foods that a dominant rival cannot see, seem more convincing of
a theory of mind. Certainly a number of primate species perform one or both of these,
suggesting that theory of mind is a general anthropoid feature. However, scrub jays also
engage in surprisingly complex alterations of behavior when food-caching, depending on
whether or not they are being observed by other jays. This again suggests a possible link to
encephalization.
Thus, in the end, evolutionary analyses appear to confirm the multifaceted nature of
consciousness. Selective attention may be as ancient as nervous systems, and sensory
awareness possibly traces to the first mammals. But, in any case, sensory awareness certainly
originated earlier than self-recognition.
17 A summary in nine firsts
The roots of cognition date nearly from the origin of life a little over four billion years ago.
When the first cell responded to interior pressure and expelled waste products, a primitive
type of sensation was born. Emerging about the same time was the ability to pass useful
substances into the cell while excluding less useful ones. Soon, the addition of flagella or cilia
allowed movement away from less- to more-productive areas. Together, internal mechanical
sensing, external chemical sensing, and the ability to move constituted the first building
blocks of cognition.
Over 600 million years ago, hair-based mechanoreceptors were incorporated into the
stinging cells of jellyfish and other cnidarians. Neurons may have evolved from them, with
neural nets emerging by 605 million years ago. A little later, early chordates had sensing hair
cells and a nervous system. The nervous system in turn enabled the selection of one of
multiple stimuli, an early form of selective attention. Early nervous systems, however, de-
pended on primary sensory cells with sensors and an axon on opposite ends, limiting their
architectural complexity.
Metazoan vision likely appeared first in cnidarians. Whether vertebrate eyes descended
directly from them is unclear, but all eyes use variations of vitamin A-based photopigment.
An additional photopigment class, the flavins, originated even before cnidarians and was later
used to regulate circadian rhythms.
Paired, pigmented though likely lensless cup eyes probably emerged with the first bi-
lateria by about 580 million years ago. Early chordates likely had only one eye, used for
sensing direction. Optical reversal in the eye resulted in a nervous system crossover, with
the left side of the world represented on the right side of the nervous system, and vice
versa. It seems likely that this scheme was simply duplicated when eyes again paired in
vertebrates.
One “backward” aspect of the visual system, namely the slowing rather than initiation of
neurotransmitter flow in response to light, was probably in place in chordates. Multiple cone
types likely date to the vertebrate emergence.
The ability to move is thought to have been characteristic of early metazoa as early as
650–780 million years ago, specifically in swimming sponge larvae. It was accomplished with
beating cilia, propelling free-swimming forms through water, or a little later, in benthic
species, over a mucus-lubricated trail. Ancestral flatworms both crept on the sea bottom and
used muscles for full-body undulation when swimming, prior to 540 million years ago.
The ability to remember was probably present even in early single-cell organisms, in the
form of habituation to repeated stimuli. Conditioning dates at least from the cnidarian
emergence over 605 million years ago. It was the product of a central nervous system or-
ganized to allow the arbitrary pairing of stimuli and responses. Visual sensory memory,
produced as an aftereffect of neural activity, likely originated with the first cnidarian eyes and
in all probability was present in chordates.
DOI: 10.4324/9781003137863-20
296 Perception and cognition
These characteristics – receptors linking to nervous systems enabling sensory selectivity;
the ability to move through water either in suspension or on the ocean bottom; and the
beginnings of sensory memory – were the birthright of chordates. From those beginnings
sprang numerous cognitive mechanisms and cognitive-influenced behaviors. These may be
loosely organized under nine “firsts”.
Acheulean a hominid tool culture that began about 1.8 million years ago, marked by the
rise of handaxes
acritarchs tiny acid-resistant fossils showing evidence of a cell wall
acuity the ability to resolve fine visual details
adductors in the leg, muscles on the inside of the thigh that move it inward
agonist a drug that temporarily binds to neural receptors and prevents their normal
function
agrammatism a clinical disorder marked by the disordering of syntax (or grammar)
Ahrensburgian a human tool culture known for extensive organic remains, and for
showing evidence of hunting using spear-throwers or bows and arrows
allele a variation of a gene
allocentric frame of reference a mental framework allowing judgment of location
relative to the environment
allopatric speciation the evolution of new species by physical separation
amino acids the building blocks of proteins
amniotes animals whose embryos develop within a membrane
anaerobic living without oxygen
anapsids tetrapods with no openings in the side of the skull behind the eyes
anterior forward
Anthropoidea anthropoids, i.e., monkeys, apes, and man
arboreal living in trees
archaea a domain and kingdom of single-cell organism
artifactual pertaining to an artifact, such as a stone tool
artiodactyls a clade within mammals, consisting of mammals that are hooved and have an
even number of toes
association the arbitrary pairing of a stimulus with a response in memory
asymmetrical gait a gait with a movement cycle taking a different length of time for the
forelimbs and hindlimbs
atrophy shrinkage
auditory sense the sense of hearing
Aurignacian a human tool culture marked by an increase in microblades and microliths
autosomal pertaining to the autosomes
autosomes all of the chromosomes other than X and Y
axon a long thread-like projection of a neuron
backed blade a stone blade blunted on one edge
basilar membrane a membrane with hair cells, located inside the cochlea
basilar papillae rounded projections in the inner ear, each containing a few hair cells
benthic bottom-dwelling
Glossary 305
bilateral two-sided
Bilateria bilaterally symmetric animals
bilayers films
binocular convergence the state of both eyes imaging the same object at the same time
binocular cue a depth cue requiring two eyes to see
binocular disparity a binocular depth cue using the discrepancy between images from
the two eyes
binocular field of view (BFOV) the opening angle of the joint field of view of the two
eyes
biofilms cooperative communities of bacteria
biomineralization the incorporation of minerals into structures such as bones or teeth
bipedal two-legged; walking on two legs
bipolar cells a class of neurons located in front of the retina that connect the ganglion cells
to the rods and cones
blindsight a phenomenon in which an observer claims to be blind, yet shows residual
visual abilities
brachiation a form of locomotion characterized by swinging from branch to branch
Broca’s cap an outward bulge of cortex in Broca’s area
Cacatuidae the family of cockatoos
Cambrian explosion the rapid expansion of metazoan phyla in a portion of the Cambrian
period, roughly 520 or 530 to 540 million years ago
catalyze accelerate, especially a chemical reaction
Catarrhini narrow-nosed monkeys; also known as Old World monkeys
caudal rear
caudofemoralis longus in most tailed animals, a muscle located largely in the tail and
connected to bones of the rear limb
Cercopithecidae a family of catarrhines consisting of two subfamilies, the
Cercopithecinae and the Colobinae
Cercopithecinae a subfamily of Cercopithecidae consisting of macaques, baboons, and
vervet monkeys
Chordata, or chordates relatively complex animals with a hollow neural tube behind a
notochord
cilia small hairlike cellular outgrowths
circadian rhythm a biological rhythm governing our sensitivity to the 24-hour day
clade a branch on the tree of life defined by a novel charateristic
cladistics the grouping of species by novel characteristics not present in other less-related
species
Clovis a North American tool culture involving hunting large game
cnidarians a phylum of animals composed of jellyfish, hydras, sea anemones, etc.
coalescence time the last date at which two later species had completely common an-
cestry
cochlea a spiral-shaped cavity of the inner ear
Colobinae a diverse subfamily of Cercopithecidae that includes colobus and proboscis
monkeys, doucs, langurs, etc.
complex cells in the visual system, neurons that respond to orientation over a large area,
and often to movement in a particular direction
composite tools tools made of more than one material
concordant using the same side, as in two right-handed siblings
cones a class of receptors in the retina, chiefly responsible for color vision
conspecifics other members of the same species
306 Glossary
contralateral opposite-sided
convergent evolution the evolution of similar form or behavior in relatively unrelated
species, due to similar environmental pressures
core in the making of stone tools, the stone from which the tool is made
coronal organ a vibration-sensing organ in some chordates, located in the oral region
cortical magnification factor a mathematical relationship between the acuteness of an
area of vision and the amount of cortex that processes it
corticospinal tract a motor tract originating in the precentral gyrus of the brain that
crosses over to the opposite side of the body; also known as the pyramidal or dorsolateral
tract
Corvidae the family of corvids, which includes crows, ravens, and jays among others
cranial nerves sensory and motor nerves completely located in the head
craniates animals with skulls
cranium bony braincase
crown group one of at least two subgroups with living descendants
cryptochromes a class of photopigments, more recently called flavins
ctenophores comb jellies
cubozoans box jellyfish
cultural variation in ape communities, the extent to which a behavior differs between
communities
cutaneous nerves nerves serving somatosensation, which enter the spinal cord
deaf hearing a phenomenon in which the patient denies hearing anything, but never-
theless orients to loud sounds
dendritic arbor the treelike branching of dendrites
dental comb outward-projecting lower canines and incisors, used as a comb while
grooming
dentine bonelike tissue without cells
dentition the form and patterning of teeth
derived character a characteristic possessed by one subgroup but not all subgroups within
a clade
descent with modification the intergenerational transfer of genetic instructions that
differ between parents and offspring
diapsids tetrapods with two openings in the skull behind the eye
dichromacy the state of having two types of cones
diffusion in ape communities, the social transmission of behaviors across communities
dinoflagellates ocean-dwelling unicellular organisms with two flagella
distal farther from the core
diurnal active in daytime
dizygotic twins nonidentical (fraternal) twins
dorsal top
dorsal visual stream the visual pathway from brain area V1 to the parietal lobe
dorsolateral tract a motor tract originating in the precentral gyrus of the brain, that
crosses over to the opposite side of the body; also known as the corticospinal or pyr-
amidal tract
echo the auditory form of sensory memory
ectoderm in vertebrate prenatal development, the cells giving rise to external features like
the skin, nails, and hair, as well as the brain and nervous system
egocentric frame of reference a mental framework allowing judgment of location re-
lative to the body
Glossary 307
elaborative rehearsal processing the meaning of items to be remembered, thus associating
them to the existing knowledge structure
encephalization quotient (EQ) a calculated quantity expressing brain size in a form that
takes the brain-body size relationship into account
endocast a latex rubber molding of the inside of a skull; or more recently, a virtual
rendition of the inside of a skull using imaging data
endothermy a state characterized by the internal generation of heat to maintain body
temperature, i.e., “warm-bloodedness”
episodic memory memory for episodes or particular events in the past
eukaryotes literally, “true kernels”, i.e., cells having nuclei
eumetazoa all animals that are not sponges
eutheria literally, “true beasts”, i.e., placental mammals
exome the reduced portion of the genome that codes for proteins
extralemniscal system somatosensory neural pathways that run upward to secondary
somatosensory cortex
extrapyramidal tract a collective term for motor pathways with only partial crossover
between the brain and the body
factor analysis a statistical technique that groups tasks or measures according to their
interrelationships
fatty acids a class of lipids
femur the thigh bone that forms a ball joint with a socket in the pelvis
figure-ground segregation in vision, the process of extracting an object from its back-
ground
flagella whip-like cell structures
flakes in the making of stone tools, the stone fragments struck from the core
flavins a class of photopigments
flexed bent
focal length the distance required for an image to come to a focus
folivores leaf eaters
foramen magnum the opening on the bottom of the skull allowing entry of the spinal
column
fovea the central area of maximally acute vision located within the macula
frontal eye field a location in the frontal lobe of the brain involved in making voluntary
saccades
frontal pole the foremost part of the frontal lobe
frugivorous eating fruit
functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) a method for imaging brain activity
involving detection of electrical currents influenced by proton spin
gait the cyclical means by which the limbs move the body
ganglia functional groupings of nerve cells
ganglion cells a class of neurons located in front of the retina, whose axons form the optic
nerve
gating the controlled passing of substances through a membrane
generator potential a net positive electrical charge inside a neuron, that has increased past
a threshold value, opening sodium channels in the cell membrane
genetic recombination an error during the reproduction of chromosomes resulting in
the movement of genetic instructions from one chromosome to another
geniculostriate route a visual pathway that proceeds from the eye to the lateral geniculate
nucleus to the primary visual cortex
genome the set of genetic instructions; all the chromosomes together
308 Glossary
gill arches thin support structures for the gills, made of cartilage or bone
global precedence effect in its first sense, faster response to global (broad) than to local
(narrow) aspects of a scene; in its second sense, the greater effect of conflicting in-
formation at the global level on the local level, than vice versa
gluteus maximus muscles large muscles in the butt that allow us to walk stably
gnostic cell, or grandmother cell in vision, a neuron that responds to a specific object,
such as one’s grandmother
gracile slender
grammar, or syntax the rule-based organization of symbols in orders that succeed in
conveying meaning
grandmother cell, or gnostic cell in vision, a neuron that responds to a specific object,
such as one’s grandmother
Gravettian (called Périgordian in France) a human tool culture characterized by
backed blades and end-scrapers
grid cells neurons that respond to locations in an overall grid representation of the en-
vironment
gustatory sense, or gustation the sense of taste
habituation a decline in response to repeated stimulation
hair follicle receptors nerve endings wrapped around the base of a hair, that respond to
its bending
hallux big toe
Hamburgian a human tool culture known for extensive organic remains, and for showing
evidence of hunting using spear-throwers or bows and arrows
hamstrings muscles on the back of the thigh that move it backward
Haplorhini, or haplorhines dry-nosed primates
heading the direction of movement; bearing
height in the visual field a monocular depth cue in which the images of objects lower in
the visual field are usually judged closer than those higher in the visual field
Heschl’s gyrus a gyrus on the superior surface of the temporal lobe that constitutes the
primary auditory area
heuristics rules of thumb that tend toward the solution of a problem
Homeobox genes genes “like a box”, because the DNA sequence is short and can be
enclosed by a box in written form
hominid in outmoded usage, humans and their immediate ancestors; now used synony-
mously with Hominidae
Hominidae a family consisting of all great apes including humans
Homininae a subfamily consisting of all African apes including humans
Hominini, or hominins a tribe consisting of all bipedal apes including humans
Hominoidea, or hominoids the superfamily consisting of apes including humans
homologous traceable from common origins
homology a similarity in traits traceable to common ancestry
horizontal gene transfer the movement of genetic material from one organism to an-
other in the absence of a parent–child relationship
Hox abbreviation of Homeobox
humerus the upper arm bone
hyoid a horseshoe-shaped bone in the neck
hypercomplex cells in the visual system, neurons that respond to edges having both an
orientation and an end; some also respond to curvature
hyperpolarize to become more negatively charged
icon the visual form of sensory memory
Glossary 309
ilium each of the bladelike extensions at the back of the pelvis
impedance the amount of pressure required to produce a given displacement
incus literally, “anvil”; one of the ossicles of the middle ear
insectivores insect eaters
interneurons small, laterally connecting neurons
interorbital distance the space between the eyes, measured between their nearest edges
ions electrically charged atoms
ipsilateral same-sided
ischial callosities callused skin patches on the buttocks that serve as padding while sitting
island dwarfism the state of having reduced size, due to the species’ isolation on an island
over time
kinesthetic sense a mechanical sense that assesses joint position and muscle tension
knapping the act of shaping a stone core by breaking off flakes
lactase persistence the presence of lactase beyond childhood and into adulthood, al-
lowing the digestion of the sugar lactose, present in milk
laryngeal motor cortex cortex involved in regulating the larynx
lateral line system in fish, hair cells arranged in a line from gills to tail, detecting pressure
gradients
lateral pallium a brain structure in fish that is anatomically, genetically, and functionally
homologous to the avian and mammalian hippocampus
lesions areas of physical damage, in the brain usually resulting from strokes or penetrating
wounds
Levallois technique a stone toolmaking technique involving removing many flakes from
a core in preparation for striking off one desired flake
lexigram a type of visual symbol conveying meaning
lipids fatty or waxy molecules
locomotion movement from place to place
macula a small spot of relatively acute central vision, containing the fovea
Magdalenian a human tool culture that made many types of tools from bone and antler,
some decorated with carvings
magnocellular response a rapid response to visual stimulation that preserves only fuzzy
detail but serves to alert the system to the appearance and location of the stimulus
maintenance rehearsal rote repetition
malleus literally, “mallet”; one of the ossicles of the middle ear
mechanoreceptors specialized sensory receptor cells that react to mechanical forces
medial inner; toward the middle
medial lemniscal system somatosensory neural pathways that run upward to the medulla
of the brain, and then via synapses to primary somatosensory cortex
Meissner’s corpuscles touch receptors below the outer layers of skin, detecting flutter
memory set several items held in memory
memory span the number of items that can consistently be recalled in correct order
memory stores kinds of memory
Merkel’s receptors (or discs) touch receptors below the outer layers of skin, detecting
fine details
metabolism the cellular process by which energy is made available
metacognition the awareness of one’s own cognitive states and processes
metazoa multicellular animals
microblades small stone blades less than 5 cm in length
microcephalic abnormally small-brained
microliths small stone blades with at least three distinct sides
310 Glossary
microvilli tiny projections of cell membrane
minicolumns columns of neurons in the cortex, oriented vertically to the cortical surface
and extending through most of the cell layers
mirror cells neurons that rapidly fire when their possessor observes the actions of others,
and when he or she makes the same motions
missing heritability problem the observation that when single gene effects are cumu-
lated, they fail to account for the full heritability of a behavior or characteristic
mitochondria small structures responsible for energy production in a cell
molecular clock a method of dating based on genetic differences between species
monocular cues depth cues that can be seen by a single eye
monozygotic twins identical twins
motile independently moving
Mousterian a hominid tool culture usually associated with Neanderthals
mutation a copying error within a chromosome resulting in an altered set of instructions
myofibrils chains of protein in muscle fibers that contract when stimulated
natural selection the environmental favoring of traits, improving survival and re-
production
negative hand the outline of a hand held against a rock and painted by blowing pigment
through a tube held by the other
neoteny the retention of juvenile characteristics in adulthood
neural crest a cluster of embryonic cells arranged in ridge-like formations to the sides of
the neural tube
neuromuscular junction the location where a motor neuron synapses onto muscle fiber
neurons nerve cells
New World monkeys monkeys native to the Americas
nocturnal active at night
notocord a supporting rod in chordates, located in front of a hollow neural tube
nucleotide a molecule composed of a nucleobase, a sugar, and a phosphate
occlusion a monocular depth cue in which an object whose image partially blocks another
is judged closer
Old World monkeys monkeys native to Africa, Asia, or Europe
olfactory epithelium a layer of neurons lining the upper portion of the nasal passageway
olfactory sense, or olfaction sense of smell
omnivore an animal that eats both animals and plants
opposable thumb a thumb that can rotate along its long axis, allowing its pad to contact
the pads of other fingers
opsins a family of photopigments
optic flow the experience of objects rushing past in the periphery while the view far ahead
changes much more slowly
optic foramen a small opening in the bone of the eye socket through which the optic
nerve passes
orienting the shifting of eyes, attention, or both toward a source of stimulation
oscula mouth-like openings
ossicles three small bones in the middle ear
Pacinian corpuscles touch receptors, deeply embedded in the skin, that respond to vi-
bration
paleocortex ancient cortex
parietal eye field a location in the parietal lobe of the brain that updates spatial in-
formation in response to movement
Glossary 311
parvocellular response a relatively slow response to visual stimulation that contains
detailed information
pattern mask a stimulus composed of jumbled features that spatially covers another
stimulus
peptides short chains of amino acids
perceptual invariance the ability to recognize an object regardless of physical transfor-
mations of characteristics like position, depth, size, viewing angle, contrast, and texture
Périgordian (called Gravettian outside France) a human tool culture characterized by
backed blades and end-scrapers
perirhinal cortex cortex located in the medial temporal lobe, literally “near the nose”
perisylvian cortex cortex located around the Sylvian fissure
phonological syntax the ordering of meaningless sound elements
phonology language phonetics
phosphenes imaginary points of light
photomotility movement in response to light
photopigments pigments that react chemically to light
phylogeny evolutionary relationships
phytoliths microscopic mineral particles formed by plants
pinna outer ear
place cells neurons that respond to particular locations in the environment
placode a specialized cell cluster, representing a thickening of the ectoderm during ver-
tebrate prenatal development that develops into a structure
planum temporale, or temporal plane a small surface of cortex located lateral and
posterior to Heschl’s gyrus
Platyrrhini literally, “broad-nosed” monkeys, also known as New World monkeys
“pli-de-passage fronto-parietal moyen parietale” (PPFM) a gyrus in the brain that
is buried along the central sulcus, connecting the precentral and postcentral gyri
podomeres leg segments
polydactyly the condition of having more than five digits per limb
population left-handedness (PLH) a predominance of left-handedness in a population
population right-handedness (PRH) a predominance of right-handedness in a popu-
lation
positron emission tomography (PET) a method for imaging brain activity that involves
injecting a radioactive substance into the bloodstream, followed by the detection of
decay products immediately outside of the head
postcranial pertaining to the skeleton below the head
potassium-argon dating a geological clock method that assigns dates according to the
ratio of potassium-40 to argon-40
praxis the ability to conceive, initiate, and complete movement
preadaptation an evolutionary change later built upon by other changes
precision grip the delicate holding of a small object between the index finger and thumb
precuneus the medial portion of area 7
prefontal cortex the forward portion of the frontal lobes
prehensile able to grasp objects
primary representation the ability to mentally represent the world in a realistic, literal way
primary sensory cells neurons with sensors at one end and an axon out the other end
primates an order of eutherians with highly mobile feet and hands, and opposable digits
primitive character a characteristic possessed by the earliest evolving member of a clade
primordial soup the thin solution of chemicals comprising the early ocean
prokaryotes literally, “before kernels”, i.e., cells without nuclei
312 Glossary
prosimians outmoded term for primates “before apes”, consisting of the Strepsirrhini and
tarsiers
prosopagnosia the inability to recognize faces presented visually
protocell a self-replicating compartment enclosing chemical processes
proximal nearer to the core
pseudogenes genes that have become deactivated because of disruptive mutations
pseudoneglect overattention to one side of space due to an intact attentional mechanism
on the opposite side of the brain
Psittacidae the family of parrots
pulvinar a nucleus in the thalamus
pursuit eye movements eye movements that follow a moving object
pyramidal tract a motor tract originating in the precentral gyrus of the brain, that crosses
over to the opposite side of the body; also known as the corticospinal or dorsolateral tract
quadriceps muscles on the front of the thigh that extend it
quadrupedal four-legged; moving on four legs
quorum sensing the ability of some bacteria to detect the presence of increasing numbers
of other bacteria, by sensing a building concentration of chemical excretions
radiation expansion in the number of species
recency effect a memory phenomenon in which the most recent item or items in a list are
recalled better than the items immediately preceding
reentrant connections connections from higher-order cortical areas back to primary
cortex
relative size a monocular depth cue in which the images of two similar objects are
compared and the larger is judged closer
residual brain size the amount of brain over and above that predicted by body weight
retouching secondary flaking along the edge of a stone tool
retrosplenial cortex cortex immediately posterior to the corpus callosum
right-shift theory the theory stating that human handedness is the product of a bell-
shaped distribution in combination with a right-shift allele that is present in some in-
dividuals and not others
robust heavily built
rods a class of receptors in the retina, chiefly responsible for night vision
root hair plexus nerve endings wrapped around the base of a hair
rostral forward
route knowledge knowledge of landmarks and associated turns, used in learning pathways
from one point to another
Ruffini’s endings (or corpuscles) touch receptors, deeply embedded in the skin, that
respond to skin stretching
saccades eye movements toward a source of stimulation
saccule a fluid-filled cavity in the inner ear with sensors that respond to acceleration
sagittal crest a ridge of bone running front-to-back on the top of the skull
savanna grassland with sparse stands of trees
script a contextually dependent series of actions leading to a goal
secondary representation the ability to separate a primary representation from its en-
vironmental reference for hypothetical purposes
secondary sensory cells neurons with a sensory end and a broad synaptic surface on the
other end
semantic dementia a disorder in which use of semantic knowledge is impaired even
though episodic memory is spared
semantic memory memory for facts that make up our knowledge structure
Glossary 313
sensory memory a very short-lived memory trace that preserves much of the physical
form of stimuli
sessile attached, as to the sea bottom
sexual selection mate selection by desirable qualities, e.g., particular colorations or forms
simple cells in the visual system, neurons that perform a feature analysis by representing a
scene in terms of small segments of contours, and that may incorporate color
social learning the acquisition of new behaviors by observing others
Solutrean a human tool culture known for creating the laurel-leaf point through pressure
flaking
somatosensation the sense of touch, or the tactile sense
spatial attention the ability to attend to objects or locations independent of eye position
spatial neglect, or visual neglect difficulty in orienting to objects opposite the side of
a brain lesion
speciation the creation of new species
spike potential a sudden increase in positivity that travels up a neuron’s axon, opening
sodium channels
split time the latest date at which there was any genetic interchange between two species
stapes literally, “stirrup”; one of the ossicles of the middle ear
stem group a group prior to diversification into crown groups
Strepsirrhini strepsirrhines, literally “wet-nosed” primates
striking platform in the making of a stone tool, a small flat surface where a blow landed
stromatolites mats of sediment formed by bacteria
subitizing a perceptual process that rapidly enumerates small numbers of items
superficial layer of V1 the layer of V1 cells closest to the surface of the brain, also known
as layer 1
suprachiasmatic nucleus a nucleus of neurons connected to the ganglion cells of the
retina, involved in maintaining the circadian rhythm
survey knowledge knowledge of how landmarks are configured relative to one another
symbiotic mutually beneficial
symmetrical gait a gait with a movement cycle taking the same length of time for the
forelimbs as for the hindlimbs
sympatric speciation the evolution of new species within an unseparated population
synapsids tetrapods with one opening in the skull behind the eye
syntax, or grammar the rule-based organization of symbols in orders that succeed in
conveying meaning
tactile pertaining to touch
Talairach space a set of X, Y, and Z coordinates allowing the mapping of brain activity
to structures in an “average” brain
tapetum lucidum literally “silvery carpet”; the reflective layer behind the retinas of
certain animals such as cats and dogs
taxa classifications of organisms based on relatedness
tectopulvinar pathway a visual pathway that proceeds from the eye to the superior
colliculus, then to the pulvinar, and finally to the secondary visual areas of the cortex
tensor tympani muscles muscles in the middle ear, connected by a ligament to the
malleus
termite fishing a behavior in which a twig is prepared and then inserted into termite
mounds, allowing the withdrawal of termites
terrestrial living on the ground
tetrachromacy the state of having four types of cones
tetrapods vertebrates with four feet or legs
314 Glossary
theory of mind the ability to ascribe mental and emotional states to others
thermophilic heat-loving
theropods large carnivorous dinosaurs with short forelimbs and large jaws
thoracic vertebrae vertebrae at the level of the thorax or chest
trace fossils fossils of behavior as opposed to physical form
trichromacy the state of having three types of cones
trichromatic vision three-color vision
tuff a layer of rock formed from compacted volcanic ash
umami a meaty or savory flavor
utricle a fluid-filled cavity in the inner ear with receptors that respond to acceleration
ventral bottom
ventral visual stream the visual pathway from brain area V1 to the temporal lobe
Vernier acuity the ability to determine whether two aligned line segments are con-
tinuations of the same line or rather are offset from one another
vertebral canal the opening in the vertebrae through which the spinal cord passes
vertebrates animals with a brain and a vertebral column
vesicles sacs
vestibular sense the sense of balance
visual agnosia the inability to recognize a class of objects presented visually
Visual neglect, visuospatial neglect, or spatial neglect difficulty in orienting to objects
opposite the side of a brain lesion
visual word form area (VWFA) a brain area in or near the occipito-temporal sulcus,
involved in recognizing words presented visually
visuospatial neglect, visual neglect, or spatial neglect difficulty in orienting to objects
opposite the side of a brain lesion
vomeronasal sense a chemical sense that detects pheromones using receptors in the nasal
passages
Wernicke’s area the posterior portion of the superior temporal gyrus implicated in the
decoding of speech sounds
working memory a form of memory that holds and manipulates information over the
short term
References
Abdelhady, A. A., Elewa, A. M. T., & El-Dawy, M. H. (2019). The position of Neandertal and Homo erectus
within the hominid clade based on craniodental morphology and whole mtDNA genomes. HOMO –
Journal of Comparative Human Biology, 70, 305–323.
Afraz, S-R., Kiani, R., & Esteky, H. (2006). Microstimulation of inferotemporal cortex influences face
categorization. Nature, 442, 692–695.
Ahlberg, P. E., & Clack, J. A. (2006). A firm step from water to land. Nature, 440, 747–749.
Aiello, L. C. (2010). Five years of Homo floresiensis. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 142, 167–179.
Ajina, S., & Bridge, H. (2017). Blindsight and unconscious vision: What they teach us about the human
visual system. Neuroscientist, 23, 529–541.
Albrecht, D. G., De Valois, R. L., & Thorell, L. G. (1980). Visual cortical neurons: Are bars or gratings the
optimal stimuli? Science, 207, 88–90.
Aldridge, K. (2011). Patterns of differences in brain morphology in humans as compared to extant apes.
Journal of Human Evolution, 60, 94–105.
Alemseged, Z., Spoor, F., Kimbel, W. H., Bobe, R., Geraads, D., Reed, D., & Wynn, J. G. (2006). A
juvenile early hominin skeleton from Dikika, Ethiopia. Nature, 443, 296–301.
Alexander, R. M. (1992). Human locomotion. In S. Jones, R. Martin, & D. Pilbeam (Eds.), The Cambridge
encyclopedia of human evolution (pp. 80–85). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Allen, C., & Bekoff, M. (2007). Animal consciousness. In M. Velmans & S. Schneider (Eds.), The Blackwell
companion to consciousness (pp. 58–71). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
Allen, J. M., Reed, D. L., Perotti, M. A., & Braig, H. R. (2007). Evolutionary relationships of “Candidatus
Riesia spp.,” endosymbotic Enterobacteriaceae living within hematophagous primate lice. Applied and
Environmental Microbiology, 73, 1659–1664.
Allin, E. F., & Hopson, J. A. (1992). Evolution of the auditory system in Synapsida (“mammal-like reptiles”
and primitive mammals) as seen in the fossil record. In D. B. Webster, A. N. Popper, & R. R. Fay (Eds.),
The Evolutionary Biology of Hearing (pp. 587–614). New York, NY: Springer.
Allison, T., Puce, A., Spencer, D. D., & McCarthy, G. (1999). Electrophysiological studies of human face
perception. I: Potentials generated in occipitotemporal cortex by face and non-face stimuli. Cerebral
Cortex, 9, 415–430.
Almé cija, S., Shrewsbury, M., Rook, L., & Moyà -Solà , S. (2014). The morphology of Oreopithecus bombolii
pollical distal phalanx. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 153, 582–597.
Almé cija, S., Smaers, J. B., & Jungers, W. L. (2015). The evolution of human and ape hand proportions.
Nature Communications, 6, article 7717.
Almé cija, S., Tallman, M., Alba, D. M., Pina, M., Moyà -Solà , S., & Jungers, W. L. (2013). The femur of
Orrorin tugenensis exhibits morphometric affinities with both Miocene apes and later hominins. Nature
Communications, 4, article 2888.
Almécija, S., Tallman, M., Sallam, H. M., Fleagle, J. G., Hammond, A. S., & Seiffert, E. R. (2019). Early
anthropoid femora reveal divergent adaptive trajectories in catarrhine hind-limb evolution. Nature
Communications, 10, article 4778.
Alvarez, G. A., & Cavanagh, P. (2004). The capacity of visual short-term memory is set both by visual
information load and by number of objects. Psychological Science, 15, 106–111.
Amodio, P., Farrar, B. G., Krupenye, C., Ostojić, L., & Clayton, N. S. (2021). Little evidence that Eurasian
316 References
jays protect their caches by responding to cues about a conspecific’s desire and visual perspective. eLife, 10,
article e69647.
Anderson, J. R., & Gallup, G. G. (2015). Mirror self-recognition: A review and critique of attempts to
promote and engineer self-recognition in primates. Primates, 56, 317–326.
Andre, J., Picchioni, M., Zhang, R., & Toulopoulou, T. (2016). Working memory circuit as a function of
increasing age in healthy adolescence: A systematic review and meta-analyses. NeuroImage: Clinical, 12,
940–948.
Andrews, P. (2020). Last common ancestor of apes and humans: Morphology and environment. Folia
Primatologica, 91, 122–148.
Angeli, R. D., Lise, M., Tabajara, C. C., & Maffacioli, T. B. (2013). Voluntary contraction of the tensor
tympani muscle and its audiometric effects. The Journal of Laryngology & Otology, 127, 1235–1237.
Annett, M. (1998). Handedness and cerebral dominance: The right shift theory. Journal of Neuropsychiatry, 10,
459–469.
Annett, M. (2006). The distribution of handedness in chimpanzees: Estimating right shift in Hopkins’
sample. Laterality, 11, 101–109.
Antcliffe, J. B. (2012). Patterns in paleontology: The Cambrian explosion – Paradoxes and possible worlds.
Paleontology (online), retrieved 25 July 2020 from http://www.palaeontologyonline.com.
Antó n, S. C. (2003). Natural history of Homo erectus. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 46, 126–170.
Arcila, D., Ortí, G., Vari, R., Armbruster, J. W., Stiassny, M. L. J., Ko, K. D., … & Betancur-R, R. (2017).
Genome-wide interrogation advances resolution of recalcitrant groups in the tree of life. Nature Ecology &
Evolution, 1, article 20.
Arendt, D., & Wittbrodt, J. (2001). Reconstructing the eyes of Urbilateria. Philosophical Transactions of the
Royal Society of London, B, 356, 1545–1563.
Arendt, D., Benito-Gutierrez, E., Brunet, T., & Marlow, H. (2015). Gastric pouches and the mucociliary
sole: Setting the stage for nervous system evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 370,
article 20150286.
Arendt, D., Tosches, M. A., & Marlow, H. (2016). From nerve net to nerve ring, nerve cord and brain –
evolution of the nervous system. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17, 61–72.
Argue, D., Groves, C. P., Lee, M. S. Y., & Jungers, W. L. (2017). The affinities of Homo floresiensis based on
phylogenetic analyses of cranial, dental, and postcranial characters. Journal of Human Evolution, 107,
107–133.
Argue, D., Morwood, M. J., Sutinka, T., Jatmiko, & Saptomo, E. W. (2009). Homo floresiensis: A cladistic
analysis. Journal of Human Evolution, 57, 623–639.
Arias-Martorell, J., Potau, J. M., Bello-Hellegouarch, G., Pastor, J. F., & Pé rez-Pé rez, A. (2012). 3D
geometric morphometric analysis of the proximal epiphysis of the hominoid humerus. Journal of Anatomy,
221, 394–405.
Arias-Martorell, J., Potau, J. M., Bello-Hellegouarch, G., Pastor, J. F., & Pé rez-Pé rez, A. (2015). Like father,
like son: Assessment of the morphological affinities of A.L. 288-1 (A. afarensis), Sts 7 (A. africanus) and
Omo 119-73-2718 (Australopithecus sp.) through a three-dimensional shape analysis of the shoulder joint.
PLoS ONE, 10(2), e0117408.
Armour, J. A. L., Davison, A., & McManus, I. C. (2014). Genome-wide association study of handedness
excludes simple genetic models. Heredity, 112, 221–225.
Arrese, C. A., Hart, N. S., Thomas, N., Beazley, L. D., & Shand, J. (2002). Trichromacy in Australian
marsupials. Current Biology, 12, 657–660.
Arroyo, A., Harmand, S., Roche, H., & Taylor, N. (2020). Searching for hidden activities: Percussive tools
from the Oldowan and Acheulean of West Turkana, Kenya (2.3-1.76 Ma). Journal of Archaeological Science,
123, article 105238.
Asara, J. M., Schweitzer, M. H., Freimark, L. M., Phillips, M., & Cantley, L. C. (2007). Protein sequences
from mastodon and Tyrannosaurus rex revealed by mass spectrometry. Science, 316, 280–285.
Asfaw, B., White, T., Lovejoy, O., Latimer, B., Simpson, S., & Suwa, G. (1999). Australopithecus garhi: A
new species of early hominid from Ethiopia. Science, 284, 629–635.
Ashley-Ross, M. A. (1994). Hindlimb kinematics during terrestrial locomotion in a salamander (Dicamptodon
tenebrosus). Journal of Experimental Biology, 193, 255–283.
Astafiev, S. V., Shulman, G. L., Stanley, C. M., Snyder, A. Z., Van Essen, D. C., & Corbetta, M. (2003).
References 317
Functional organization of human intraparietal and frontal cortex for attending, looking, and pointing.
The Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 4689–4699.
Averbach, E. (1963). The span of apprehension as a function of exposure duration. Journal of Verbal Learning
and Verbal Behavior, 2, 60–64.
Baab, K. L., & McNulty, K. P. (2009). Size, shape, and asymmetry in fossil hominins: The status of the LB1
cranium based on 3D morphometric analyses. Journal of Human Evolution, 57, 608–622.
Babiloni, C., Vecchio, F., Rossi, S., De Capua, A., Bartalini, S., Ulivelli, M., & Rossini, P. M. (2007).
Human ventral parietal cortex plays a functional role on visuospatial attention and primary consciousness.
A repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 1486–1492.
Backwell, L., d’Errico, F., & Wadley, L. (2008). Middle stone age bone tools from the Howiesons Poort
layers, Sibudu Cave, South Africa. Journal of Archaeological Science, 35, 1566–1580.
Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Bagnara, S., Boles, D. B., Simion, F., & Umiltà , C. (1982). Can an analytic/holistic dichotomy explain
hemispheric asymmetries? Cortex, 18, 67–78.
Baldauf, S. L., Bhattacharya, D., Cockrill, J., Hugenholtz, P., Pawlowski, J., & Simpson, A. G. B. (2004).
The tree of life: An overview. In J. Cracraft & M. J. Donohue (Eds.), Assembling the tree of life (pp. 43–75).
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Balezeau, F., Wilson, B., Gallardo, G., Dick, F., Hopkins, W., Anwander, A., … Petkov, C. I. (2020).
Primate auditory prototypes in the evolution of the arcuate fasciculus. Nature Neuroscience, 23, 611–614.
Balleza, D. (2011). Toward understanding protocell mechanosensation. Origins of Life and Evolution of
Biospheres, 41, 281–304.
Balzeau, A., Gilissen, E., Holloway, R. L., Prima, S., & Grimaud-Herve, D. (2014). Variations in size, shape
and asymmetries of the third frontal convolution in hominids: Paleoneurological implications for hominin
evolution and the origin of language. Journal of Human Evolution, 76, 116–128.
Bandini, E., & Tennie, C. (2017). Spontaneous reoccurrence of ‘‘scooping’’, a wild tool-use behaviour,
in naï ve chimpanzees. PeerJ, 5, article e3814.
Bandini, E., & Tennie, C. (2019). Individual acquisition of “stick pounding” behavior by naïve chim-
panzees. American Journal of Primatology, 81, article e22987.
Baragli, P., Scopa, C., Maglieri, V., & Palagi, E. (2021). If horses had toes: Demonstrating mirror self
recognition at group level in Equus caballus. Animal Cognition, 24, 1099–1108.
Bard, J. (2017). Principles of evolution: Systems, species, and the history of life. London: Garland Science.
Bardack, D. (1991). First fossil hagfish (Myxinoidea): A record from the Pennsylvanian of Illinois. Science, 254,
701–703.
Barfod, G. H., Albarè de, F., Knoll, A. H., Xiao, S., Té louk, P., Frie, R., & Baker, J. (2002). New Lu-Hf and
Pb-Pb age constraints on the earliest animal fossils. Earth and Planetary Science Letters, 201, 203–212.
Bargalló , A., & Mosquera, M. (2014). Can hand laterality be identified through lithic technology? Laterality,
19, 37–63.
Barney, A., Martelli, S., Serrurier, A., & Steele, J. (2012). Articulatory capacity of Neanderthals, a very
recent and human-like fossil hominin. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 367, 88–102.
Barrett, B. J., Monteza-Moreno, C. M., Dogandzic, T., Zwyns, N., Ibáñez, A., & Crofoot, M. C. (2018).
Habitual stone-tool-aided extractive foraging in white-faced capuchins, Cebus capucinus. Royal Society
Open Science, 5, article 181002.
Barton, R. A., & Venditti, C. (2013). Human frontal lobes are not relatively large. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 110, 9001–9006.
Bassler, B. L., & Losick, R. (2006). Bacterially speaking. Cell, 125, 237–246.
Battaglia-Mayer, A., Mascaro, M., & Caminiti, R. (2007). Temporal evolution and strength of neural ac-
tivity in parietal cortex during eye and hand movements. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 1350–1363.
Baumgartner, R. J., Van Kranendonk, M. J., Wacey, D., Fiorentini, M. L., Saunders, M., Caruso, S., …
Guagliardo, P. (2019). Nano-porous pyrite and organic matter in 3.5-billion-year-old stromatolites record
primordial life. Geology, 20, article g46365.
Bays, P. M. (2018). Reassessing the evidence for capacity limits in neural signals related to working memory.
Cerebral Cortex, 28, 1432–1438.
BBN International. (2008). Anthropology – The doctrine of man. Retrieved February 15, 2019 from
https://bbn1.bbnradio.org/english/tools-home/bible-doctrines/bible-doctrines-anthropology.
318 References
Bear, D. M., Lassance, J-M., Hoekstra, H. E., & Datta, S. R. (2016). The evolving neural and genetic
architecture of vertebrate olfaction. Current Biology, 26, R1039–R1049.
Beaumont, P. B. (2011). The edge: More on fire-making by about 1.7 million years ago at Wonderwerk
Cave in South Africa. Current Anthropology, 52, 585–595.
Begun, D. R. (2010). Miocene hominids and the origins of the African apes and humans. Annual Review of
Anthropology, 39, 67–84.
Begun, D. R., & Kordos, L. (2004). Cranial evidence of the evolution of intelligence in fossil apes. In A. E.
Russon & D. R. Begun (Eds.), The evolution of thought: Evolutionary origins of great ape intelligence
(pp. 260–279). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Beisel, K. W., Wang-Lundberg, Y., Maklad, A., & Fritzsch, B. (2005). Development and evolution of the
vestibular sensory apparatus of the mammalian. Journal of Vestibular Research, 15, 225–241.
Bell, E. A., Boehnke, P., Harrison, T. M., & Mao, W. L. (2015). Potentially biogenic carbon preserved in a
4.1 billion-year-old zircon. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112, 14518–14521.
Benhamou, S., & Poucet, B. (1996). A comparative analysis of spatial memory processes. Behavioural
Processes, 35, 113–126.
Bennett, M. R., Harris, J. W. K., Richmond, B. G., Braun, D. R., Mbua, E., Kiura, P., … Gonzalez, S.
(2009). Early hominin foot morphology based on 1.5-million-year-old footprints from Ileret, Kenya.
Science, 323, 1197–1201.
Bennett, M. R., Reynolds, S. C., Morse, S. A., & Budka, M. (2016). Footprints and human evolution:
Homeostasis in foot function? Palaeogeography, Palaeoclimatology, Palaeoecology, 461, 214–223.
Bentley-Condit, V. K., & Smith, E. O. (2010). Animal tool use: Current definitions and an updated
comprehensive catalog. Behaviour, 147, 185–221.
Benton, M. J. (2005). Vertebrate palaeontology (3rd ed.). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
Beran, M. J., Perdue, B. M., & Smith, J. D. (2014). What are my chances? Closing the gap in uncertainty
monitoring between rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition, 40, 303–316.
Beran, M. J., Smith, J. D., Coutinho, M. V. C., Couchman, J. J., & Boomer, J. (2009). The psychological
organization of “uncertainty” responses and “middle” responses: A dissociation in capuchin monkeys
(Cebus apella). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 35, 371–381.
Berger, L. R., de Ruiter, D. J., Churchill, S. E., Schmid, P., Carlson, K. J., Dirks, P. H. G. M., &
Kibii, J. M. (2010). Australopithecus sediba: A new species of Homo-like Australopith from South Africa.
Science, 328, 195–204.
Berger, L. R., Hawks, J., de Ruiter, D. J., Churchill, S. E., Schmid, P., Delezene, L. K., … Walker, C. S.
(2015). Homo naledi, a new species of the genus Homo from the Dinaledi chamber, South Africa. eLife, 4,
article e09560.
Berlin, J. C., Kirk, E. C., & Rowe, T. B. (2013). Functional implications of ubiquitous semicircular canal
non-orthogonality in mammals. PLoS One, 8, article e79585.
Bernhardt, H. S. (2012). The RNA world hypothesis: The worst theory of the early evolution of life (except
for all the others). Biology Direct, 7, 23.
Beste, C., Arning, L., Gerding, W. M., Epplen, J. T., Mertins, A., Rö der, M. C., … Ocklenburg, S. (2018).
Cognitive control processes and functional cerebral asymmetries: Association with variation in the
handedness-associated gene LRRTM1. Molecular Neurobiology, 55, 2268–2274.
Betts, H. C., Puttick, M. N., Clark, J. W., Williams, T. A., Donoghue, P. C. J., & Pisani, D. (2018).
Integrated genomic and fossil evidence illuminates life’s early evolution and eukaryote origins. Nature
Ecology & Evolution, 2, 1556–1562.
Beznosov, P. A., Clack, J. A., Luksevics, E., Ruta, M., & Ahlberg, P. E. (2019). Morphology of the earliest
reconstructable tetrapod Parmastega aelidae. Nature, 574, 527–531.
Bhattacharyya, S., Walker, D. M., & Harshey, R. M. (2020). Dead cells release a ’necrosignal’ that activates
antibiotic survival pathways in bacterial swarms. Nature Communications, 11, article 4157.
Bianchi, S., Stimpson, C. D., Bauernfeind, A. L., Schapiro, S. J., Baze, W. B., McArthur, M. J., …
Sherwood, C. C. (2013). Dendritic morphology of pyramidal neurons in the chimpanzee neocortex:
Regional specializations and comparison to humans. Cerebral Cortex, 23, 2429–2436.
Bijsterveld, K. (2015). Beyond echoic memory: Introduction to the special issue on auditory history. The
Public Historian, 37, 7–13.
References 319
Biro, D., Inoue-Nakamura, N., Tonooka, R., Yamakoshi, G., Sousa, C., & Matsuzawa, T. (2003). Cultural
innovation and transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees: Evidence from field experiments. Animal
Cognition, 6, 213–223.
Blake, J. (2004). Gestural communication in the great apes. In A. E. Russon & D. R. Begun (Eds.), The
evolution of thought: Evolutionary origins of great ape intelligence (pp. 61–75). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Blanca, M. J., & Ló pez-Montiel, G. (2009). Hemispheric differences for global and local processing: Effect of
stimulus size and sparsity. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 12, 21–31.
Bliss, J. C., Crane, H. D., Mansfield, P. K., & Townsend, J. T. (1966). Information available in brief tactile
presentations. Perception & Psychophysics, 1, 273–283.
Blob, R. W. (2001). Evolution of hindlimb posture in nonmammalian therapsids: Biomechanical tests of
paleontological hypotheses. Paleobiology, 27, 14–38.
Bloch, J. I., & Wilcox, M. T. (2006). Cranial anatomy of the Paleocene plesiadapiform Carpolestes simpsoni
(Mammalia, Primates) using ultra high-resolution X-ray computed tomography, and the relationships of
plesiadapiforms to Euprimates. Journal of Human Evolution, 50, 1–35.
Bloemendal, H., de Jong, W., Jaenicke, R., Lubsen, N. H., Slingsby, C., & Tardieu, A. (2004). Ageing and
vision: Structure, stability and function of lens crystallins. Progress in Biophysics & Molecular Biology, 86,
407–485.
Boccia, M. L. (1994). Mirror behavior in macaques. In S. T. Parker, R. W. Mitchell, & M. L. Boccia (Eds.),
Self-awareness in animals and humans (pp. 350–360). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Boë, L.-J., Sawallis, T. R., Fagot, J., Badin, P., Barbier, G., Captier, G., … Schwartz, J.-L. (2019). Which
way to the dawn of speech?: Reanalyzing half a century of debates and data in light of speech science.
Science Advances, 5, article eaaw3916.
Boeckmann, B., Marcet-Houben, M., Rees, J. A., Forslund, K., Huerta-Cepas, J., Muffato, M., …
Gabaldó n, T. (2015). Quest for orthologs entails quest for Tree of Life: In search of the gene stream.
Genome Biology and Evolution, 7, 1988–1999.
Boesch, C., Bombjaková, D., Meier, A., & Mundry, R. (2019). Learning curves and teaching when ac-
quiring nut-cracking in humans and chimpanzees. Scientific Reports, 9, article 1515.
Boesch, C., Kalan, A. K., Mundry, R., Arandjelovic, M., Pika, S., Dieguez, P., … Kuhl, H. S. (2020).
Chimpanzee ethnography reveals unexpected cultural diversity. Nature Human Behaviour, 4, 910–916.
Boghi, A., Rampado, O., Bergui, M., Avidano, F., Manzone, C., Coriasco, M., … Bradac, G. B. (2006).
Functional MR study of a motor task and the Tower of London task at 1.0 T. Neuroradiology, 48,
763–771.
Boisvert, C. A. (2005). The pelvic fin and girdle of Panderichthys and the origin of tetrapod locomotion.
Nature, 438, 1145–1147.
Boles, D. B. (1984). Global versus local processing: Is there a hemispheric dichotomy? Neuropsychologia, 22,
445–455.
Boles, D. B. (1991). Factor analysis and the cerebral hemispheres: Pilot study and parietal functions.
Neuropsychologia, 29, 59–91.
Boles, D. B. (1997). Multiple resource contributions to training. Proceedings of the Human Factors and
Ergonomics Society, 41st Annual Meeting, 1176–1179.
Boles, D. B. (1998). Relationships among multiple task asymmetries: II. A large-sample factor analysis. Brain
and Cognition, 36, 268–289.
Boles, D. B. (2002). Lateralized spatial processes and their lexical implications. Neuropsychologia, 40,
2125–2135.
Boles, D. B., & Dillard, M. (2015). The measurement of perceptual resources and workload. In R. R.
Hoffman, P. A. Hancock, M. W. Scerbo, R. Parasuraman, & J. L. Szalma (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of
applied perception research (Vol. 1, pp. 39–59). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Boles, D. B., & Karner, T. A. (1996). Hemispheric differences in global versus local processing: Still unclear.
Brain and Cognition, 30, 232–243.
Boles, D. B., & Law, M. B. (1998). A simultaneous task comparison of differentiated and undifferentiated
hemispheric resource theories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24,
204–215.
320 References
Boles, D. B., Adair, L. P., & Joubert, A-M. (2009). A preliminary study of lateralized processing in attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of General Psychology, 136, 243–258.
Boles, D. B., Barth, J. M., & Merrill, E. C. (2008). Asymmetry and performance: Toward a neurodeve-
lopmental theory. Brain & Cognition, 66, 124–139.
Boles, D. B., Phillips, J. B., & Givens, S. M. (2007). What dot clusters and bargraphs reveal: Subitizing is fast
counting and subtraction. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 913–922.
Bonini, L. (2017). The extended mirror neuron network: Anatomy, origin, and functions. The Neuroscientist,
23, 56–67.
Boogert, N. J., Fawcett, T. W., & Lefebvre, L. (2011). Mate choice for cognitive traits: A review of the
evidence in nonhuman vertebrates. Behavioral Ecology, 22, 447–459.
Borges, R., Johnson, W. E., O’Brien, S. J., Gomes, C., Heesy, C. P., & Antunes, A. (2018). Adaptive
genomic evolution of opsins reveals that early mammals flourished in nocturnal environments. BMC
Genomics, 19, article 121.
Born, R. T., Trott, A. R., & Hartmann, T. S. (2015). Cortical magnification plus cortical plasticity equals
vision? Vision Research, 111, 161–169.
Botella, J., Privado, J., de Liañ o, B. G., & Suero, M. (2011). Illusory conjunctions reflect the time course of
the attentional blink. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 73, 1361–1373.
Botta, O. (2004). The chemistry of the origin of life. In P. Ehrenfreund, W. Irvine, T. Owen, L. Becker, J.
Blank, J. Brucato, … F. Robert (Eds.), Astrobiology: Future perspectives (pp. 359–391). Dordrecht: Kluwer
Academic Publishers.
Bourrat, P., & Lu, Q. (2017). Dissolving the missing heritability problem. Philosophy of Science, 84,
1055–1067.
Boussau, B., Blanquart, S., Necsulea, A., Lartillot, N., & Gouy, M. (2008). Parallel adaptations to high
temperatures in the Archaean eon. Nature, 456, 942–946.
Bouvier, S. E., & Engel, S. A. (2006). Behavioral deficits and cortical damage loci in cerebral achromatopsia.
Cerebral Cortex, 16, 183–191.
Bowers, J. S. (2017). Parallel distributed processing theory in the age of deep networks. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 21, 950–961.
Bowling, D. L., Dunn, J. C., Smaers, J. B., Garcia, M., Sato, A., Hantke, G., … Fitch, W. T. (2020). Rapid
evolution of the primate larynx? PLoS Biology, 18, article e3000764.
Bowmaker, J. K., & Hunt, D. M. (2006). Evolution of vertebrate visual pigments. Current Biology, 16,
R484–R489.
Boyd, B. M., Allen, J. M., Nguyen, N.-P., Vachaspati, P., Quicksall, Z. S., Warnow, T., … Reed, D. L.
(2017). Primates, lice and bacteria: Speciation and genome evolution in the symbionts of hominid lice.
Molecular Biology and Evolution, 34, 1743–1757.
Boyer, D. M., Toussaint, S., & Godinot, M. (2017). Postcrania of the most primitive euprimate and im-
plications for primate origins. Journal of Human Evolution, 111, 202–215.
Bradshaw, J. (1997). Human evolution: A neuropsychological perspective. East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.
Bramble, D. M., & Lieberman, D. E. (2004). Endurance running and the evolution of Homo. Nature, 432,
345–352.
Brandler, W. M., Morris, A. P., Evans, D. M., Scerri, T. S., Kemp, J. P., Timpson, N. J., … Paracchini, S.
(2013). Common variants in left/right asymmetry genes and pathways are associated with relative hand
skill. PLoS Genetics, 9, article e1003751.
Brä uer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). All great ape species follow gaze to distant locations and around
barriers. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119, 145–154.
Brä uer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Chimpanzees really know what others can see in a competitive
situation. Animal Cognition, 10, 439–448.
Braun, D. R., Plummer, T., Ferraro, J. V., Ditchfield, P., & Bishop, L. C. (2009). Raw material quality and
Oldowan hominin toolstone preferences: Evidence from Kanjera South, Kenya. Journal of Archaeological
Science, 36, 1605–1614.
Braun, K., & Stach, T. (2017). Structure and ultrastructure of eyes and brains of Thalia democratica (Thaliacea,
Tunicata, Chordata). Journal of Morphology, 278, 1421–1437.
Braun, M. H., & Likowiak, K. (2011). Intermediate and long-term memory are different at the neuronal
level in Lymnaea stagnalis (L.) Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 96, 403–416.
References 321
Bray, E. E., Gnanadesikan, G. E., Horschler, D. J., Levy, K. M., Kennedy, B. S., Famula, T. R., MacLean,
E. L. (2021). Early-emerging and highly heritable sensitivity to human communication in dogs. Current
Biology, 31, 1–5.
Brazeau, M. D., & Ahlberg, P. E. (2006). Tetrapod-like middle ear architecture in a Devonian fish. Nature,
439, 318–321.
Brazeau, M. D., & Friedman, M. (2015). The origin and early phylogenetic history of jawed vertebrates.
Nature, 520, 490–497.
Brecht, K. F., Wagener, L., Ostojic, L., Clayton, N. S., & Nieder, A. (2017). Comparing the face inversion
effect in crows and humans. Journal of Comparative Physiology, A, 203, 1017–1027.
Breedlove, S. M., Watson, N. V., & Rosenzweig, M. R. (2010). Biological psychology. Sunderland, MA:
Sinauer Associates.
Breitmeyer, B. G. (1984). Visual masking: An integrative approach. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Bremmer, F. (2005). Navigation in space – the role of the macaque ventral intraparietal area. Journal of
Physiology, 566, 29–35.
Breuer, T., Ndoundou-Hockemba, M., & Fishlock, V. (2005). First observation of tool use in wild gorillas.
PLoS Biology, 3, 2041–2043.
Broadfield, D. C., Holloway, R. L., Mowbray, K., Silvers, A., Yuan, M. S., & Má rquez, S. (2001). Endocast
of Sambungmacan 3 (Sm 3): A new Homo erectus from Indonesia. The Anatomical Record, 262, 369–379.
Broglio, C., Gó mez, A., Durá n, E., Ocañ a, F. M., Jimé nez-Moya, F., Rodríguez, F., & Salas, C. (2005).
Hallmarks of a common forebrain vertebrate plan: Specialized pallial areas for spatial, temporal and
emotional memory in actinopterygian fish. Brain Research Bulletin, 66, 277–281.
Brown, C. (2012). Tool use in fishes. Fish and Fisheries, 13, 105–115.
Brown, F. H. (1992). Methods of dating. In S. Jones, R. Martin, & D. Pilbeam (Eds.), The Cambridge
encyclopedia of human evolution (pp. 179–186). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Brown, J. C., & Golston, C. (2006). Embedded structure and the evolution of phonology. Interaction Studies,
7, 17–41.
Brown, P., Sutikna, T., Morwood, M. J., Soejono, R. P., Jatmiko, Saptomo, E. W., & Due, R. A. (2004). A
new small-bodied hominin from the Late Pleistocene of Flores, Indonesia. Nature, 431, 1055–1061.
Bruck, J. N., Allen, N. A., Brass, K. E., Horn, B. A., & Campbell, P. (2017). Species differences in ego-
centric navigation: The effect of burrowing ecology on a spatial cognitive trait in mice. Animal Behaviour,
127, 67–73.
Brugger, J., Feulner, G., & Petri, S. (2017). Baby, it’s cold outside: Climate model simulations of the effects
of the asteroid impact at the end of the Cretaceous. Geophysical Research Letters, 44, 419–427.
Brü ne, M., & Brü ne-Cohrs, U. (2006). Theory of mind – evolution, ontogeny, brain mechanisms and
psychopathology. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 437–455.
Bruner, E. (2004). Geometric morphometrics and paleoneurology: Brain shape evolution in the genus
Homo. Journal of Human Evolution, 47, 279–303.
Bruner, E. (2010). Morphological differences in the parietal lobes within the human genus. Current
Anthropology, 51, S77–S88.
Bruner, E., Preuss, T. M., Chen, X., & Rilling, J. K. (2017). Evidence for expansion of the precuneus in
human evolution. Brain Structure and Function, 222, 1053–1060.
Brunet, M., Guy, F., Boisserie, J.-R., Djimdoumalbaye, A., Lehmann, T., Lihoreau, F., … Zol-Likofer, C.
(2004). “Toumaï ”, Miocè ne supé rieur du Tchad, le nouveau doyen du Rameau humain. Comptes Rendus
Palevol, 3, 277–285.
Buckley, M., Warwood, S., van Dongen, B., Kitchener, A. C., & Manning, P. L. (2017). A fossil protein
chimera; difficulties in discriminating dinosaur peptide sequences from modern cross-contamination.
Proceedings of the Royal Society, B, 284, article 20170544.
Budd, G. E., & Jensen, S. (2004). The limitations of the fossil record and the dating of the origin of the
Bilateria. In P. C. J. Donoghue & M. P. Smith (Eds.), Telling the evolutionary time: Molecular clocks and the
fossil record (pp. 166–189). Boca Raton: CRC Press.
Bugnyar, T., Stö we, M., & Heinrich, B. (2004). Ravens, Corvus corax, follow gaze direction of humans
around obstacles. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B., 271, 1331–1336.
Buniyaadi, A., Taufique, S. K. T., & Kumar, V. (2020). Self-recognition in corvids: Evidence from the
mirror-mark test in Indian house crows (Corvus splendens). Journal of Ornithology, 161, 341–350.
322 References
Burgihel, P., Lane, N. J., Fabio, G., Stefano, T., Zaniolo, G., Carnevali, M. D. C., & Manni, L. (2003).
Novel, secondary sensory cell organ in ascidians: In search of the ancestor of the vertebrate lateral line. The
Journal of Comparative Neurology, 461, 236–249.
Burkart, J. M., & Heschl, A. (2007). Understanding visual access in common marmosets, Callithrix jacchus:
Perspective taking or behaviour reading? Animal Behaviour, 73, 457–469.
Buss, D. M. (2012). Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind. Boston: Allyn & Bacon, 4th edition.
Bussey, T. J., Saksida, L. M., & Murray, E. A. (2002). Perirhinal cortex resolves feature ambiguity in
complex visual discriminations. European Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 365–374.
Butler, A. B. (2000). Chordate evolution and origin of craniates: An old brain in a new head. The Anatomical
Record Part B, The New Anatomist, 261, 111–125.
Butterfield, N. J. (2003). Exceptional fossil preservation and the Cambrian explosion. Integrative and
Comparative Biology, 43, 166–177.
Buxhoeveden, D. P., Switala, A. E., Litaker, M., Roy, E., & Casanova, M. F. (2001). Lateralization of
minicolumns in human planum temporale is absent in nonhuman primate cortex. Brain, Behavior, and
Evolution, 57, 349–358.
Byrne, R. W. (2004). The manual skills and cognition that lie behind hominid tool use. In A. E. Russon &
D. R. Begun (Eds.), The evolution of thought: Evolutionary origins of great ape intelligence (pp. 31–33).
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Cabeza, R., & Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 1–47.
Caetano-Anollé s, G., Nasir, A., Zhou, K., Castano-Anollé s, D., Mittenthal, J. E., Sun, F-J., & Kim, K. M.
(2014). Archaea: The first domain of diversified life. Archaea, 2014, article 590214.
Calcutt, S. E., Rubin, T. L., Pokorny, J. J., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2017). Discrimination of emotional facial
expressions by tufted capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 131, 40–49.
Call, J., Brä uer, J., Kaminski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2003). Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) are sensitive to the
attentional state of humans. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117, 257–263.
Callaway, E. (2017). Oldest Homo sapiens fossil claim rewrites our species’ history. Nature, retrieved
21 September 2019 from https://www.nature.com/news/oldest-homo-sapiens-fossil-claim-rewrites-
our-species-history-1.22114.
Calvin, W. H. (2002). Rediscovery and the cognitive aspects of toolmaking: Lessons from the handaxe.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 403–404.
Cameron, D. W., & Groves, C. P. (2004). Bones, stones, and molecules: “Out of Africa” and human origins.
Amsterdam: Elsevier Academic Press.
Camprodon, J. A., Zohary, E., Brodbeck, V., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2010). Two phases of V1 activity for
visual recognition of natural images. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 1262–1269.
Cañestro, C., Albalat, R., Irimia, M., & Garcia-Fernàndez, J. (2013). Impact of gene gains, losses and
duplication modes on the origin and diversification of vertebrates. Seminars in Cell & Developmental Biology,
24, 83–94.
Cantalupo, C., Pilcher, D. L., & Hopkins, W. D. (2003). Are planum temporale and sylvian fissure
asymmetries directly related?: A MRI study in great apes. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1975–1981.
Canteloup, C., Piraux, E., Poulin, N., & Meunier, H. (2016). Do Tonkean macaques (Macaca tonkeana)
perceive what conspecifics do and do not see? PeerJ, 4, article e1693.
Cantine, M. D., & Fournier, G. P. (2018). Environmental adaptation from the origin of life to the last
universal common ancestor. Origins of Life and Evolution of Biospheres, 48, 35–54.
Cantone, G., Xiao, J., McFarlane, N., & Levitt, J. B. (2005). Feedback connections to ferret striate cortex:
Direct evidence for visuotopic convergence of feedback inputs. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 487,
312–331.
Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., Mahon, B., & Caramazza, A. (2003). What are the facts of semantic category-
specific deficits? A critical review of the clinical evidence. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 213–261.
Caron, J. B., & Rudkin, D. (2009). A burgess shale primer: History, geology, and research highlights. Toronto: The
Burgess Shale Consortium.
Cartmill, E. A., & Byrne, R. W. (2007). Orangutans modify their gestural signaling according to their
audience’s comprehension. Current Biology, 17, 1345–1348.
References 323
Cartmill, E. A., & Byrne, R. W. (2010). Semantics of primate gestures: Intentional meanings of orangutan
gestures. Animal Cognition, 13, 793–804.
Cartmill, M. (1992). Non-human primates. In S. Jones, R. Martin, & D. Pilbeam (Eds.), The Cambridge
encyclopedia of human evolution (pp. 24–32). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Carvalho, S., Biro, D., Cunha, E., Hockings, K., Richmond, B. G., & Matsuzawa, T. (2012). Chimpanzee
carrying behaviour and the origins of human bipedality. Current Biology, 22, R180–R181.
Cashmore, L., Uomini, N., & Chapelain, A. (2008). The evolution of handedness in humans and great apes:
A review and current issues. Journal of Anthropological Sciences, 86, 7–35.
Catania, K. C., & Henry, E. C. (2006). Touching on somatosensory specializations in mammals. Current
Opinion in Neurobiology, 16, 467–473.
Catchpole, R. J., & Forterre, P. (2019). The evolution of reverse gyrase suggests a nonhyperthermophilic last
universal common ancestor. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 36, 2737–2747.
Cauchoix, M., Crouzet, S. M., Fize, D., & Serre, T. (2016). Fast ventral stream neural activity enables rapid
visual categorization. NeuroImage, 125, 280–290.
Cavanagh, J. P. (1972). Relation between the immediate memory span and the memory search rate.
Psychological Review, 79, 525–530.
Censky, E. J., Hodge, K., & Dudley, J. (1998). Over-water dispersal of lizards due to hurricanes. Nature,
395, 556.
Cerri, G., Cabinio, M., Blasi, V., Borroni, P., Iadanza, A., Fava, E., … Bello, L. (2015). The mirror neuron
system and the strange case of Broca’s area. Human Brain Mapping, 36, 1010–1027.
Chan, E. K. F., Timmermann, A., Baldi, B. F., Moore, A. E., Lyons, R. J., Lee, S.-S., … Hayes, V. M.
(2019). Human origins in a southern African palaeo-wetland and first migrations. Nature, 575, 185–189.
Chang, L., Fang, Q., Zhang, S., Poo, M., & Gong, N. (2015). Mirror-induced self-directed behaviors in
rhesus monkeys after visual-somatosensory training. Current Biology, 25, 212–217.
Chapman, K. M., Weiss, D. J., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2010). Evolutionary roots of motor planning: The
end-state comfort effect in lemurs. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 124, 229–232.
Chatterjee, H. J., Ho, S. Y. W., Barnes, I., & Groves, C. (2009). Estimating the phylogeny and divergence
times of primates using a supermatrix approach. BMC Evolutionary Biology, 9, 259.
Chen, Z., & Wiens, J. J. (2020). The origins of acoustic communication in vertebrates. Nature
Communications, 11, article 369.
Chen, Z., Liu, C.-Q., Sun, H., & Niu, Y. (2020). The ultraviolet colour component enhances the at-
tractiveness of red flowers of a bee-pollinated plant. Journal of Plant Ecology, 13, 354–360.
Chen, Z., Zhou, C., Yuan, X., & Xiao, S. (2019). Death march of a segmented and trilobate bilaterian
elucidates early animal evolution. Nature, 573, 412–415.
Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (2007). Baboon metaphysics: The evolution of a social mind. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Cheng, K. (2021). Learning in cnidaria: A systematic review. Learning & Behavior, 49, 175–189.
Chester, S. G. B., Bloch, J. I., Boyer, D. M., & Clemens, W. A. (2015). Oldest known euarchontan tarsals
and affinities of Paleocene Purgatorius to primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112,
1487–1492.
Chirchir, H., Kivell, T. L., Ruff, C. B., Hublin, J-J., Carlson, K. J., Zipfel, B., & Richmond, B. G. (2015).
Recent origin of low trabecular bone density in modern humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 112, 366–371.
Choleris, E., & Kavaliers, M. (1999). Social learning in animals: Sex differences and neurobiological analysis.
Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 64, 767–776.
Chow, R. L., & Lang, R. A. (2001). Early eye development in vertebrates. Annual Review of Cell and
Developmental Biology, 17, 255–296.
Chung, E. (2018). One hell of an impression. Retrieved 18 Oct 2018 from https://newsinteractives.cbc.ca/
longform/human-footprints-greece.
Clark, G. (1967). The stone age hunters. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Clark, M., Ferrara, T., Jones, D., Marion, A., Rose, K., & Yaeger, L. (1990). Koko’s Mac II: A preliminary
report. In B. Laurel (Ed.), The art of human-computer interface design (pp. 95–102). Reading, MA: Addison
Wesley.
324 References
Clary, D., & Kelly, D. M. (2016). Graded mirror self-recognition by Clark’s nutcrackers. Scientific Reports, 6,
article 36459.
Clary, D., Stow, M. K., Vernouillet, A., & Kelly, D. M. (2019). Mirror-mediated responses of California
scrub jays (Aphelocoma californica) during a caching task and the mark test. Ethology, 126, 140–152.
Claxton, A. G., Hammond, A. S., Romano, J., Oleinik, E., & DeSilva, J. M. (2016). Virtual reconstruction
of the Australopithecus africanus pelvis Sts 65 with implications for obstetrics and locomotion. Journal of
Human Evolution, 99, 10–24.
Clayton, N. S., Bussey, T. J., & Dickinson, A. (2003). Can animals recall the past and plan for the future?
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 685–691.
Close, R. A., Benson, R. B. J., Alroy, J., Carrano, M. T., Cleary, T. J., Dunne, E. M., … Butler, R. J.
(2020). The apparent exponential radiation of Phanerozoic land vertebrates is an artefact of spatial sam-
pling biases. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B, 287, article 20200372.
Coates, M. I., Jeffery, J. E., & Ruta, M. (2002). Fish to limbs: What the fossils say. Evolution & Development,
4, 390–401.
Cochet, H., & Byrne, R. W. (2013). Evolutionary origins of human handedness: Evaluating contrasting
hypotheses. Animal Cognition, 16, 531–542.
Coleman, M. N., & Ross, C. F. (2004). Primate auditory diversity and its influence on hearing performance.
The Anatomical Record Part A, 281A, 1123–1137.
Collier-Baker, E., Davis, J. M., Nielsen, M., & Suddendorf, T. (2006). Do chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)
understand single invisible displacement? Animal Cognition, 9, 55–61.
Collins, A. G., Lipps, J. H., & Valentine, J. W. (2000). Modern mucociliary creeping trails and the bodyplans
of neoproterozoic trace-makers. Paleobiology, 26, 47–55.
Combarnous, Y., & Nguyen, T. M. D. (2020). Cell communications among microorganisms, plants, and
animals: Origin, evolution, and interplays. International Journal of Molecular Sciences, 21, article 8052.
Condamine, F. L., Guinot, G., Benton, M. J., & Currie, P. J. (2021). Dinosaur biodiversity declined well
before the asteroid impact, influenced by ecological and environmental pressures. Nature Communications,
12, article 3833.
Conde-Valverde, M., Martínez, I., Quam, R. M., Rosa, M., Velez, A. D., Lorenzo, C., … Arsuaga, J. L.
(2021). Neanderthals and Homo sapiens had similar auditory and speech capacities. Nature Ecology &
Evolution, 5, 609–615.
Cook, R., Bird, G., Catmure, C., Press, C., & Heyes, C. (2014). Mirror neurons: From origin to function.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37, 177–192.
Corballis, M. C. (2003). From mouth to hand: Gesture, speech, and the evolution of right-handedness.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26, 199–260.
Coren, S., & Porac, C. (1977). Fifty centuries of right-handedness: The historical record. Science, 198,
631–632.
Cornford, J. M. (1986). Specialized resharpening techniques and evidence of handedness. In P. Callow &
J. M. Cornford (Eds.), La Cotte de St. Brelade 1961–1978 (pp. 337–351). Norwich, England: Geo Books.
Corté s-Ortiz, L., Bermingham, E., Rico, C., Rodríguez-Luna, E., Sampaio, I., & Ruiz-García, M. (2003).
Molecular systematics and biogeography of the neotropical monkey genus, Alouatta. Molecular Phylogenetics
and Evolution, 26, 64–81.
Coull, J. T., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Frith, C. D. (1998). Monitoring for target objects: Activation of right
frontal and parietal cortices with increasing time on task. Neuropsychologia, 36, 1325–1334.
Cowen, R. (2005). History of life (4th ed.). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.
Cowey, A., & Alexander, I. (2012). Are hemianopic monkeys and a human hemianope aware of visual
events in the blind field? Experimental Brain Research, 219, 47–57.
Coyne, J. (2009). Why evolution is true. New York, NY: Viking Press.
Cozzi, B., Povinelli, M., Ballarin, C., & Granato, A. (2014). The brain of the horse: Weight and cepha-
lization quotients. Brain, Behaviour, and Evolution, 83, 9–16.
Cracraft, J., Feinstein, J., García-Moreno, J., Barker, F. K., Stanley, S., Sorenson, M. D., … Mindell, D. P.
(2004). Phylogenetic relationships among modern birds (neornithes): Toward an avian tree of life.
In J. Cracraft & M. J. Donohue (Eds.), Assembling the tree of life (pp. 468–489). Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
References 325
Cronin, T., Arshad, Q., & Seemungal, B. M. (2017). Vestibular deficits in neurodegenerative disorders:
Balance, dizziness, and spatial disorientation. Frontiers in Neurology, 8, article 538.
Crook, R., & Basil, J. (2008). A biphasic memory curve in the chambered nautilus, Nautilus pompilius L.
(Cephalopoda: Nautiloidea). The Journal of Experimental Biology, 211, 1992–1998.
Cserhati, M. F., Mooter, M.-E., Peterson, L., Wicks, B., Xiao, P., Pauley, M., & Guda, C. (2018).
Motifome comparison between modern human, Neanderthal and Denisovan. BMC Genomics, 19, ar-
ticle 472.
Culham, J. C., Cavina-Pratesi, C., & Singhal, A. (2006). The role of parietal cortex in visuomotor control:
What have we learned from neuroimaging? Neuropsychologia, 44, 2668–2684.
Cunningham, J. A., Vargas, K., Yin, Z., Bengtson, S., & Donohue, P. C. J. (2017). The Weng’an biota
(Doushantuo Formation): An Ediacaran window on soft-bodied and multicellular micro-organisms.
Journal of the Geological Society, 174, 793–802.
Custance, D., & Bard, K. A. (1994). The comparative and developmental study of self-recognition and
imitation: The importance of social factors. In S. T. Parker, R. W. Mitchell, & M. L. Boccia (Eds.), Self-
awareness in animals and humans (pp. 207–226). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Cutini, S., Scatturin, P., Moro, S. B., & Zorzi, M. (2014). Are the neural correlates of subitizing and
estimation dissociable? An fNIRS investigation. NeuroImage, 85, 391–399.
Cyran, K. A., & Kimmel, M. (2010). Alternatives to the Wright-Fisher model: The robustness of mi-
tochondrial Eve dating. Theoretical Population Biology, 78, 165–172.
d’Errico, F., Doyon, L., Colagé, I., Queffelec A., Le Vraux, E., Giacobini, G., … Maureille, B. (2018).
From number sense to number symbols. An archaeological perspective. Philosophical Transactions of the
Royal Society, B, 373, article 20160518.
D’Aoû t, K., Vereecke, E., Schoonaert, K., De Clercq, D., Van Elsacker, L., & Aerts, P. (2004). Locomotion
in bonobos (Pan paniscus): Differences and similarities between bipedal and quadrupedal terrestrial
walking, and a comparison with other locomotor modes. Journal of Anatomy, 204, 353–361.
d’Errico, F., Henshilwood, C., Vanhaeren, M., & van Niekerk, K. (2005). Nassarius kraussianus shell beads
from Blombos Cave: Evidence for symbolic behaviour in the middle stone age. Journal of Human Evolution,
48, 3–24.
Dadda, M., Cantalupo, C., & Hopkins, W. D. (2006). Further evidence of an association between hand-
edness and neuroanatomical asymmetries in the primary motor cortex of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).
Neuropsychologia, 44, 2582–2586.
Daeschler, E. B., Shubin, N. H., & Jenkins, F. A. (2006). A Devonian tetrapod-like fish and the evolution of
the tetrapod body plan. Nature, 440, 757–763.
Dagosto, M. (2002). The origins and diversification of anthropoid primates: Introduction. In W. C. Hartwig
(Ed.), The primate fossil record (pp. 125–132). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Dale, G., & Arnell, K. M. (2013). Investigating the stability of and relationships among global/local pro-
cessing measures. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 394–406.
Damerius, L. A., Graber, S. M., Willems, E. P., & van Schaik, C. P. (2017). Curiosity boosts orang-utan
problem-solving ability. Animal Behaviour, 134, 57–70.
Danchin, E. G. J., Gouret, P., & Pontarotti, P. (2006). Eleven ancestral gene families lost in mammals and
vertebrates while otherwise universally conserved in animals. BMC Evolutionary Biology, 6, 5.
Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection. London: John Murray.
Das, T., Ghule, S., & Vanka, K. (2019). Insights into the origin of life: Did it begin from HCN and H2O?
ACS Central Science, 5, 1532–1540.
Dá vid-Barrett, T., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2016). Bipedality and hair loss in human evolution revisited: The
impact of altitude and activity scheduling. Journal of Human Evolution, 94, 72–82.
Davidson, I., & McGrew, W. C. (2005). Stone tools and the uniqueness of human culture. Journal of the
Royal Anthropological Institute, 11, 793–817.
Davies, W. I. L., Collin, S. P., & Hunt, D. M. (2012). Molecular ecology and adaptation of visual pho-
topigments in craniates. Molecular Ecology, 21, 3121–3158.
de Beaune, S. A. (2004). The invention of technology: Prehistory and cognition. Current Anthropology, 45,
139–151.
de Boer, B. (2012). Loss of air sacs improved hominin speech abilities. Journal of Human Evolution, 62, 1–6.
326 References
de Castro, J. M. B., Martinón-Torres, M., Arsuaga, J. L., & Carbonell, E. (2017). Twentieth anniversary of
Homo antecessor (1997–2017): A review. Evolutionary Anthropology, 26, 157–171.
de Heinzelin, J., Clark, J. D., White, T., Hart, W., Renne, P., WoldeGabriel, G., … Vrba, E. (1999).
Environment and behavior of 2.5-million-year-old Bouri hominids. Science, 284, 625–629.
de Ibarra, N. H., Vorobyev, M., & Menzel, R. (2014). Mechanisms, functions and ecology of colour vision
in the honeybee. Journal of Comparative Physiology, A, 200, 411–433.
de Ibarra, N. H., Vorobyev, M., Brandt, R., & Giurfa, M. (2000). Detection of bright and dim colours by
honeybees. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 203, 3289–3298.
de la Rosa, S., Schillinger, F. L., Bü lthoff, H. H., Schulz, J., & Uludag, K. (2016). fMRI adaptation between
action observation and action execution reveals cortical areas with mirror neuron properties in human BA
44/45. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, article 78.
De Lillo, C., Palumbo, M., Spinozzi, G., & Giustino, G. (2012). Effects of pattern redundancy and hier-
archical grouping on global-local visual processing in monkeys (Cebus apella) and humans (Homo sapiens).
Behavioural Brain Research, 226, 445–455.
de Lussanet, M. H. E., & Osse, J. W. M. (2010). An ancestral axial twist explains the contralateral forebrain
and the optic chiasm in vertebrates. Animal Biology, 62, 193–216.
de Manuel, M., Kuhlwilm, M., Frandsen, P., Sousa, V. C., Desai, T., Prado-Martinez, J., … Marques-
Bonet, T. (2016). Chimpanzee genomic diversity reveals ancient admixture with bonobos. Science, 354,
477–481.
De Moraes, B., Souto, A., & Schiel, N. (2018). Adaptability in stone tool use by wild capuchin monkeys.
American Journal of Primatology, 76, 967–977.
de Oliveira, F. B., Molina, E. C., & Marroig, G. (2009). Paleogeography of the South Atlantic: A route for
primates and rodents into the new world? In P. A. Garber, A. Estrada, J. C. Bicca- Marques, E. W.
Heymann, & K. B. Strier (Eds.), South American primates, developments in primatology: Progress and prospects
(pp. 55–68). New York, NY: Springer.
de Sousa, A. A., Sherwood, C. C., Mohlberg, H., Amunts, K., Schleicher, A., MacLeod, C. E., … Zilles, K.
(2010). Hominoid visual brain structure volumes and the position of the lunate sulcus. Journal of Human
Evolution, 58, 281–292.
De Valois, R. L., & De Valois, K. K. (1990). Spatial vision. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
de Veer, M. W., Gallup, G. G., Theall, L. A., van den Bos, R., & Povinelli, D. J. (2003). An 8-year
longitudinal study of mirror self-recognition in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Neuropsychologia, 41,
229–234.
De Waal, F. (2019). Fish, mirrors, and a gradualist perspective on self-awareness. PLoS Biology, 17, article
e3000112.
de Waal, F. B. M. (2003). Darwin’s legacy and the study of primate visual communication. Annals of the New
York Academy of Sciences, 1000, 7–31.
de Waal, F. B. M., Dindo, M., Freeman, C. A., & Hall, M. J. (2005). The monkey in the mirror: Hardly a
stranger. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 11140–11147.
Deacon, T. W. (1992b). The human brain. In S. Jones, R. Martin, & D. Pilbeam (Eds.), The Cambridge
encyclopedia of human evolution (pp. 115–123). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Deamer, D., & Weber, A. L. (2010). Bioenergetics and life’s origins. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology,
2, article a004929.
Deaner, R. O., Isler, K., Burkart, J., & van Schaik, C. (2007). Overall brain size, and not encephalization
quotient, best predicts cognitive ability across non-human primates. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 70,
115–124.
Deaner, R. O., van Schaik, C. P., & Johnson, V. (2006). Do some taxa have better domain-general cog-
nition than others? A meta-analysis of nonhuman primate studies. Evolutionary Psychology, 4, 149–196.
Deecke, V. B. (2012). Tool-use in the brown bear (Ursus arctos). Animal Cognition, 15, 725–730.
Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (2007). Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron, 56, 384–398.
Delgado-Bonal, A., & Martín-Torres, J. (2016). Human vision is determined based on information theory.
Scientific Reports, 6, article 36038.
Dembo, M., Matzke, N. J., Mooers, A. O., & Collard, M. (2015). Bayesian analysis of a morphological
supermatrix sheds light on controversial fossil hominin relationships. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B, 282,
article 20150943.
References 327
Dembo, M., Radovcic, D., Garvin, H. M., Laird, M. F., Schroeder, L., Scott, J. E., … Collard, M. (2016).
The evolutionary relationships and age of Homo naledi: An assessment using dated Bayesian phylogenetic
methods. Journal of Human Evolution, 97, 17–26.
Denes, G. (1999). Disorders of body awareness and body knowledge. In G. Denes & L. Pizzamiglio (Eds.),
Handbook of clinical and experimental neuropsychology (pp. 497–506). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Dennett, D. C. (1981). What is it like to be me? New Scientist, 91, 806–809.
Denys, K., Vanduffel, W., Fize, D., Nelissen, K., Peuskens, H., Van Essen, D., & Orban, G. A. (2004). The
processing of visual shape in the cerebral cortex of human and nonhuman primates: A functional magnetic
resonance imaging study. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 2551–2565.
Derst, C., & Karschin, A. (1998). Evolutionary link between prokaryotic and eukaryotic K+ channels. The
Journal of Experimental Biology, 201, 2791–2799.
DeSilva, J., McNutt, E., Benoit, J., & Zipfel, B. (2018). One small step: A review of Plio-Pleistocene
hominin foot evolution. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 168, 63–140.
Desimone, R., Albright, T. D., Gross, C. G., & Bruce, C. (1984). Stimulus-selective properties of inferior
temporal neurons in the macaque. The Journal of Neuroscience, 4, 2051–2062.
Détroit, F., Mijares, A. S., Corny, J., Daver, G., Zanolli, C., Dizon, E., Robles, E., Grün, R., &
Piper, P. J. (2019). A new species of Homo from the late Pleistocene of the Philippines. Nature, 568,
181–188.
Diez-Martin, F., Yustos, P. S., Domínguez-Rodrigo, M., Mabulla, A. Z. P., Bunn, H. T., Ashley, G. M., …
Baquedano, E. (2010). New insights into hominin lithic activities at FLK North Bed I, Olduvai Gorge,
Tanzania. Quaternary Research, 74, 376–387.
Diez-Martin, F., Yustos, P. S., Uribelarrea, D., Baquedano, E., Mark, D. F., Mabulla, A., … Domínguez-
Rodrigo, M. (2015). The origin of the Acheulean: The 1.7 million-year-old site of FLK West, Olduvai
Gorge (Tanzania). Scientific Reports, 5, article 17839.
Dirks, P. H. G. M., Roberts, E. M., Hilbert-Wolf, H., Kramers, J. D., Hawks, J., Dosseto, A., …
Berger, L. R. (2017). The age of Homo naledi and associated sediments in the Rising Star Cave, South
Africa. eLife, 6, article e24231.
Do, M. T. H., & Yau, K-W. (2010). Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells. Physiological Reviews,
90, 1547–1581.
Doglioni, C., Pignatti, J., & Coleman, M. (2016). Why did life develop on the surface of the earth in the
Cambrian? Geoscience Frontiers, 7, 865–873.
Dohrmann, M., & Wö rheide, G. (2017). Dating early animal evolution using phylogenomic data. Scientific
Reports, 7, article 3599.
Domalain, M., Bertin, A., & Daver, G. (2017). Was Australopithecus afarensis able to make the Lomekwian
stone tools? Towards a realistic biomechanical simulation of hand force capability in fossil hominins and
new insights on the role of the fifth digit. Comptes Rendus Palevol, 16, 572–584.
Domínguez-Rodrigo, M., Pickering, T. R., & Bunn, H. T. (2010). Configurational approach to
identifying the earliest hominin butchers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107,
20929–20934.
Dominguez-Rodrigo, M., Serrallonga, J., Juan-Tresserras, J., Alcala, L., & Luque, L. (2001). Woodworking
activities by early humans: A plant residue analysis on Acheulian stone tools from Peninj (Tanzania).
Journal of Human Evolution, 40, 289–299.
Dominy, N. J. (2004). Fruits, fingers, and fermentation: The sensory cues available to foraging primates.
Integrative and Comparative Biology, 44, 295–303.
Dominy, N. J., & Lucas, P. W. (2001). Ecological importance of trichromatic vision to primates. Nature,
410, 363–366.
Dos Reis, M., Gunnell, G. F., Barba-Montoya, J., Wilkins, A., Yang, Z., & Yoder, A. D. (2018). Using
phylogenomic data to explore the effects of relaxed clocks and calibration strategies on divergence time
estimation: Primates as a test case. Systematic Biology, 67, 594–615.
dos Reis, M., Thawomwattana, Y., Angelis, K., Telford, M. J., Donoghue, P. C. J., & Yang, Z. (2015).
Uncertainty in the timing of origin of animals and the limits of precision in molecular timescales. Current
Biology, 25, 2929–2950.
Drapeau, M. S. M. (2015). Metacarpal torsion in apes, humans, and early Australopithecus: Implications for
manipulatory abilities. PeerJ, 3, article e1311.
328 References
Drasdo, N., Millican, C. L., Katholi, C. R., & Curcio, C. A. (2007). The length of Henle fibers in the
human retina and a model of ganglion receptive field density in the visual field. Vision Research, 47,
2901–2911.
Drayton, L. A., & Santos, L. R. (2017). Do rhesus macaques, Macaca mulatta, understand what others know
when gaze following? Animal Behaviour, 134, 193–199.
Drummond, A. J., Ho, S. Y. W., Phillips, M. J., & Rambaut, A. (2006). Relaxed phylogenetics and dating
with confidence. PLoS Biology, 4, 699–710.
Dumoncel, J., Subsol, G., Durrleman, S., Bertrand, A., de Jager, E., Oettlé, A. C., … Beaudet, A. (2020).
Are endocasts reliable proxies for brains? A 3D quantitative comparison of the extant human brain and
endocast. Journal of Anatomy, 238, 480–488.
Duncan, J. S., & Fritzsch, B. (2012). Evolution of sound and balance perception: Innovations that aggregate
single hair cells into the ear and transform a gravistatic sensor into the Organ of Corti. The Anatomical
Record, 295, 1760–1774.
Dunham, N. T., McNamara, A., Shapiro, L. J., Hieronymus, T. L., Phelps, T., & Young, J. W. (2019).
Effects of substrate and phylogeny on quadrupedal gait in free-ranging platyrrhines. American Journal of
Physical Anthropology, 170, 565–578.
Dunn, J. C. (2018). Sexual selection and the loss of laryngeal air sacs during the evolution of speech.
Anthropological Science, 126, 29–34.
Dunsworth, H., & Walker, A. (2002). Early genus Homo. In W. C. Hartwig (Ed.), The primate fossil record
(pp. 419–435). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Eccles, J. C. (1966). Conscious experience and memory. In J. E. Eccles (Ed.), Brain and conscious experience.
New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
Eichenbaum, H., & Fortin, N. J. (2005). Bridging the gap between brain and behavior: Cognitive and neural
mechanisms of episodic memory. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 84, 619–629.
Eichert, N., Verhagen, L. Folloni, D., Jbabdi, S., Khrapitchev, A. A., Sibson, N. R., … Mars, R. B. (2019).
What is special about the human arcuate fasciculus? Lateralization, projections, and expansion. Cortex,
118, 107–115.
Ekstrom, A. D., Kahana, M. J., Caplan, J. B., Fields, T. A., Isham, E. A., Newman, E. L., & Fried, I. (2003).
Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation. Nature, 425, 184–187.
Elton, S. (2008). The environmental context of human evolutionary history in Eurasia and Africa. Journal of
Anatomy, 212, 377–393.
Emery, N. J. (2006). Cognitive ornithology: The evolution of avian intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of
the Royal Society, B, 361, 23–43.
Emonet, E-G., Andossa, L., Mackaye, H. T., & Brunet, M. (2014). Subocclusal dental morphology of
Sahelanthropus tchadensis and the evolution of teeth in hominins. American Journal of Physical Anthropology,
153, 116–123.
Erwin, D. H., Laflamme, M., Tweedt, S. M., Sperling, E. A., Pisani, D., & Peterson, K. J. (2011). The
Cambrian conundrum: Early divergence and later ecological success in the early history of animals. Science,
334, 1091–1097.
Estalrrich, A., & Rosas, A. (2013). Handedness in Neandertals from the El Sidró n (Asturias, Spain): Evidence
from instrumental striations with ontogenetic inferences. PLoS One, 8, article e62797.
Eyun, S. (2019). Accelerated pseudogenization of trace amine-associated receptor genes in primates. Genes,
Brain and Behavior, 18, article e12543.
Fagot, J., & De Lillo, C. (2011). A comparative study of working memory: Immediate serial spatial recall in
baboons (Papio papio) and humans. Neuropsychologia, 49, 3870–3880.
Fagot, J., & Deruelle, C. (1997). Processing of global and local visual information and hemispheric spe-
cialization in humans (Homo sapiens) and baboons (Papio papio). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Perception and Performance, 23, 429–442.
Fagot, J., & Tomonaga, M. (1999). Global and local processing in humans (Homo sapiens) and chimpanzees
(Pan troglodytes): Use of a visual search task with compound stimuli. Journal of Comparative Psychology,
113, 3–12.
Falk, D. (1983). Cerebral cortices of East African early hominids. Science, 221, 1072–1074.
Falk, D. (2014). Interpreting sulci on hominin endocasts: Old hypotheses and new findings. Frontiers in
Human Neuroscience, 8, article 134.
References 329
Falk, D., Hildebolt, C., Smith, K., Morwood, M. J., Sutikna, T., Brown, P., … Prior, F. (2005). The brain
of LB1. Homo floresiensis. Science, 308, 242–245.
Falk, D., Hildebolt, C., Smith, K., Morwood, M. J., Sutikna, T., Jatmiko., … Prior, F. (2006). Response to
comment on “The Brain of LB1, Homo floresiensis”. Science, 312, 999c.
Falk, D., Hildebolt, C., Smith, K., Morwood, M. J., Sutikna, T., Jatmiko., … Prior, F. (2007). Brain shape
in human microcephalics and Homo floresiensis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104,
2513–2518.
Famiano, M., Boyd, R., Kajino, T., Onaka, T., & Mo, Y. (2019). Astrophysical sites that can produce
enantiomeric amino acids. Symmetry, 11, article 23.
Faurby, S., Silvestro, D., Werdkin, L., & Antonelli, A. (2020). Brain expansion in early hominins predicts
carnivore extinctions in East Africa. Ecology Letters, 23, 537–544.
Faurie, C., & Raymond, M. (2004). Handedness frequency over more than ten thousand years. Proceedings of
the Royal Society of London, B, 271, S43–S45.
Faurie, C., & Raymond, M. (2005). Handedness, homicide and negative frequency-dependent selection.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B, 272, 25–28.
FBI. (2018). Retrieved May 1, 2018, from https://ucr.fbi.gov/crime-in-the-u.s/2015/crime-in-the-u.s.-
2015/tables/expanded_homicide_data_table_8_murder_victims_by_weapon_2011-2015.xls.
Feder, J. L., Opp, S. B., Wlazlo, B., Reynolds, K., Go, W., & Spisak, S. (1994). Host fidelity is an effective
premating barrier between sympatric races of the apple maggot fly. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 91, 7990–7994.
Fedonkin, M. A. (2003). The origin of the Metazoa in the light of the Proterozoic fossil record.
Paleontological Research, 7, 9–41.
Fernald, R. D. (2000). Evolution of eyes. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 10, 444–450.
Ferrier, D. E. K. (2016). The origin of the Hox/Parahox genes, the Ghost Locus hypothesis and the
complexity of the first animal. Briefings in Functional Genomics, 15, 333–341.
Ferris, J. P. (2005). Mineral catalysis and prebiotic synthesis: Montmorillonite-catalyzed formation of RNA.
Elements, 1, 145–149.
Ferris, J. P., Joshi, P. C., Wang, K-J., Miyakawa, S., & Huang, W. (2004). Catalysis in prebiotic chemistry:
Application to the synthesis of RNA oligomers. Advances in Space Research, 33, 100–105.
Ferus, M., Rimmer, P., Cassone, G., Knízek, A., Civis, S., Šponer, J. E. … Krus, M. (2020). One-pot
hydrogen cyanide-based prebiotic synthesis of canonical nucleobases and glycine initiated by high-velocity
impacts on early Earth. Astrobiology, 20, 1476–1488.
Feuerriegel, E. M., Green, D. J., Walker, C. S., Schmid, P., Hawks, J., Berger, L. R., & Churchill, S. E.
(2017). The upper limb of Homo naledi. Journal of Human Evolution, 104, 155–173.
Field, G. D., & Sampath, A. P. (2017). Behavioural and physiological limits to vision in mammals.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 372, article 20160072.
Field, Y., Boyle, E. A., Telis, N., Gao, Z., Gaulton, K. J., Golan, D., … Pritchard, J. K. (2016). Direction of
human adaptation during the past 2000 years. Science, 354, 760–764.
Fincham, J. M., Carter, C. S., van Veen, V., Stenger, V. A., & Anderson, J. R. (2002). Neural mechanisms
of planning: A computational analysis using event-related fMRI. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 99, 3346–3351.
Finestone, E. M., Brown, M. H., Ross, S. R., & Pontzer, H. (2018). Great ape walking kinematics:
Implications for hominoid evolution. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 166, 43–55.
Finnerty, J. R. (2005). Did internal transport, rather than directed locomotion, favor the evolution of
bilateral symmetry in animals? BioEssays, 27, 1174–1180.
Fischer, M. S., Schilling, N., Schmidt, M., Haarhaus, D., & Witte, H. (2002). Basic limb kinematics of small
therian mammals. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 205, 1315–1338.
Fleagle, J. G. (1992). Primate locomotion and posture. In S. Jones, R. Martin, & D. Pilbeam (Eds.), The
Cambridge encyclopedia of human evolution (pp. 75–79). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Foley, N. M., Springer, M. S., & Teeling, E. C. (2016). Mammal madness: Is the mammal tree of life not yet
resolved? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 371, article 20150140.
Follmann, H., & Brownson, C. (2009). Darwin’s warm little pond revisited: From molecules to the origin of
life. Naturwissenschaften, 96, 1265–1292.
330 References
Forbes, A. A., Powell, T. H. Q., Stelinski, L. L., Smith, J. J., & Feder, J. L. (2009). Sequential sympatric
speciation across trophic levels. Science, 323, 776–779.
Foundas, A. L., Leonard, C. M., Gilmore, R., Fennell, E., & Heilman, K. M. (1994). Planum temporale
asymmetry and language dominance. Neuropsychologia, 32, 1225–1231.
Fouts, R. S., & Waters, G. (2003). Unbalanced human apes and syntax. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26,
221–222.
Fouts, R. S., Fouts, D. H., & Van Cantfort, T. E. (1989). The infant Loulis learns signs from cross- fostered
chimpanzees. In R. A. Gardner, B. T. Gardner, & T. E. Van Cantfort (Eds.), Teaching sign language to
chimpanzees (pp. 280–292). Albany: State University of New York Press.
Fouts, R. S., Jensvold, M. L. A., & Fouts, D. H. (2002). Chimpanzee signing: Darwinian realities and
Cartesian delusions. In M. Beckoff, C. Allen, & G. M. Burghardt (Eds.), The cognitive animal: Empirical and
theoretical perspectives on animal cognition (pp. 285–291). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Fox, C. L., & Frayer, D. W. (1997). Non-dietary marks in the anterior dentition of the Krapina
Neanderthals. International Journal of Osteoarchaeology, 7, 133–149.
Fox, E. A., Sitompul, A. F., & Van Schaik, C. P. (1999). Intelligent tool use in wild Sumatran orangutans. In
S. T. Parker, R. W. Mitchell, & H. L. Miles (Eds.), The mentalities of gorillas and orangutans: Comparative
perspectives (pp. 99–116). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Fragaszy, D., Izar, P., Visalberghi, E., Ottoni, E. B., & de Oliviera, M. G. (2004). Wild capuchin monkeys
(Cebus libidinosus) use anvils and stone pounding tools. American Journal of Primatology, 64, 359–366.
Franchi, M., & Gallori, E. (2005). A surface-mediated origin of the RNA world: Biogenic activities of clay-
adsorbed RNA molecules. Gene, 346, 205–214.
Francks, C., DeLisi, L. E., Shaw, S. H., Fisher, S. E., Richardson, A. J., Stein, J. F., & Monaco, A. P. (2003).
Parent-of-origin effects on handedness and schizophrenia susceptibility on chromosome 2p12-q11.
Human Molecular Genetics, 12, 3225–3230.
Frayer, D. W., Clarke, R. J., Fiore, I., Blumenschine, R. J., Pé rez-Pé rez, A., Martinez, L. M., …. Bondioli,
L. (2016). OH-65: The earliest evidence for right-handedness in the fossil record. Journal of Human
Evolution, 100, 65–72.
Frey, S. H., Vinton, D., Norlund, R., & Grafton, S. T. (2005). Cortical topography of human anterio
intraparietal cortex active during visually guided grasping. Cognitive Brain Research, 23, 397–405.
Fritzsch, B. (1987). Inner ear of the coelacanth fish Latimeria has tetrapod affinities. Nature, 327, 153–154.
Fritzsch, B., Beisel, K. W., Jones, K., Fariñ as, I., Maklad, A., Lee, J., & Reichardt, L. F. (2002).
Development and evolution of inner ear sensory epithelia and their innervation. Journal of Neurobiology,
53, 143–156.
Fritzsch, B., Pan, N., Jahan, I., Duncan, J. S., Kopecky, B. J., Elliott, K. L., … Yang, T. (2013). Evolution
and development of the tetrapod auditory system: An organ of corti-centric perspective. Evolution &
Development, 15, 63–79.
Fritzsch, B., Pauley, S., & Beisel, K. W. (2006). Cells, molecules and morphogenesis: The making of the
vertebrate ear. Brain Research, 1091, 151–171.
Fu, W.-W., Wang, X.-W., Wang, C.-L., Zhao, H.-T., Ren, Y., & Li, B.-G. (2019). Effects of age, sex and
manual task on hand preferences in wild Rhinopithecus roxellana. Zoological Research, 40, 129–138.
Fyhn, M., Hafting, T., Witter, M. P., Moser, E. I., & Moser, M-B. (2008). Grid cells in mice. Hippocampus,
18, 1230–1238.
Gabbott, S. E., Donoghue, P. C. J., Sansom, R. S., Vinther, J., Dolocan, A., & Purnell, M. A. (2016).
Pigmented anatomy in Carboniferous cyclostomes and the evolution of the vertebrate eye. Proceedings of
the Royal Society, B, 283, article 20161151.
Galá n, A. B., & Domínguez-Rodrigo, M. (2014). Testing the efficiency of simple flakes, retouched flakes
and small handaxes during butchery. Archaeometry, 56, 1054–1074.
Galletta, L., Stephens, N. B., Bardo, A., Kivell, T. L., & Marchi, D. (2019). Three-dimensional geometric
morphometric analysis of the first metacarpal distal articular surface in humans, great apes and fossil ho-
minins. Journal of Human Evolution, 132, 119–136.
Gallistel, C. R., & Gelman, R. (1992). Preverbal and verbal counting and computation. Cognition, 44,
43–74.
Gallup, G. G. (1968). Mirror-image stimulation. Psychological Bulletin, 70, 782–793.
Gallup, G. G. (1970). Chimpanzees: Self-recognition. Science, 167, 86–87.
References 331
Gallup, G. G. (1977). Self-recognition in primates. American Psychologist, 32, 329–338.
Gallup, G. G. (1982). Self-awareness and the emergence of mind in primates. American Journal of Primatology,
2, 237–248.
Gallup, G. G. (1998). Can animals empathize? Yes. Scientific American Presents, 9, 66–71.
Gallup, G. G., Anderson, J. R., & Shillito, D. J. (2002). The mirror test. In M. Bekoff, C. Allen, & G. M.
Burghardt (Eds.), The cognitive animal: Empirical and theoretical perspectives on animal cognition (pp. 325–334).
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gamberini, M., Bò , G. D., Breveglieri, R, Briganti, S., Passarelli, L., Fattori, P., & Galletti, C. (2018).
Sensory properties of the caudal aspect of the macaque’s superior parietal lobule. Brain Structure and
Function, 223, 1863–1879.
Gamberini, M., Galletti, C., Bosco, A., Breveglieri, R., & Fattori, P. (2011). Is the medial posterior parietal
area V6a a single functional area? The Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 5145–5157.
Gannon, P. J., Kheck, N., & Hof, P. R. (2008). Leftward interhemispheric asymmetry of macaque monkey
temporal lobe language area homolog is evident at the cytoarchitectural, but not gross anatomic level.
Brain Research, 1199, 62–73.
Garde, M. M., & Cowey, A. (2000). “Deaf hearing”: Unacknowledged detection of auditory stimuli in a
patient with cerebral deafness. Cortex, 2000, 36, 71–80.
Gardner, R. A., & Gardner, B. T. (1998). The structure of learning: From sign stimuli to sign language. Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Garm, A., Hedal, I., Islin, M., & Gurska, D. (2013). Pattern- and contrast-dependent visual response in the
box jellyfish Tripedalia cystophora. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 216, 4520–4529.
Garm, A., Oskarsson, M., & Nilsson, D-E. (2011). Box jellyfish use terrestrial visual cues for navigation.
Current Biology, 21, 798–803.
Gasc, J-P. (2001). Comparative aspects of gait, scaling and mechanics in mammals. Comparative Biochemistry
and Physiology Part A, 131, 121–133.
Gatesy, S. M. (1990). Caudefemoral musculature and the evolution of therapod locomotion. Paleobiology, 16,
170–186.
Gebo, D. L., Beard, K. C., Ni, X., & Dagosto, M. (2015). Distal phalanges of Eosimias and Hoanghonius.
Journal of Human Evolution, 86, 92–98.
Gebo, D. L., Dagosto, M., Beard, K. C., Qi, T., & Wang, J. (2000). The oldest known anthropoid
postcranial fossils and the early evolution of higher primates. Nature, 404, 276–278.
Gebo, D. L., MacLatchy, L., Kityo, A., Deino, A., Kingston, J., & Pilbeam, D. (1997). A hominoid genus
from the early Miocene of Uganda. Science, 276, 401–404.
Gehring, W. J. (2005). New perspectives on eye development and the evolution of eyes and photoreceptors.
Journal of Heredity, 96, 171–184.
Georgieva, S., Peeters, R., Kolster, H., Todd, J. T., & Orban, G. A. (2009). The processing of three-
dimensional shape from disparity in the human brain. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 727–742.
Gerbault, P., Liebert, A., Itan, Y., Powell, A., Currat, M., Burger, J., … Thomas, M. G. (2011). Evolution
of lactase persistence: An example of human niche construction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
Society, B, 366, 863–877.
Gerbis, N. (2019). ASU expert: Too soon to call Philippine remains new Homo species. Retrieved
September 24, 2019, from https://news.azpm.org/p/news-topical-sci/2019/4/15/149703-asu-expert-
too-soon-to-call-philippine-remains-new-homo-species.
Gibbons, A. (2009). A new kind of ancestor: Ardipithecus unveiled. Science, 326, 36–40
Gibson, E. K., McKay, D. S., Thomas-Keptra, K., & Romanek, C. S. (2000). The case for relic life on Mars.
In D. H. Levy (Ed.), The Scientific American book of the cosmos (pp. 283–291). New York, NY: St. Martin’s
Press.
Gierlinski, G. D., Niedzwiedzki, G., Lockley, M. G., Athanassiou, A., Fassoulas, C., Dubicka, Z., …
Ahlberg, P. E. (2017). Possible hominin footprints from the late Miocene (c. 5.7 Ma) of Crete? Proceedings
of the Geologists’ Association, 128, 697–710.
Gilad, Y., Bustamante, C. D., Lancet, D., & Pä ä bo, S. (2003). Natural selection on the olfactory receptor
gene family in humans and chimpanzees. American Journal of Human Genetics, 73, 489–501.
Gilad, Y., Man, O., & Glusman, G. (2005). A comparison of the human and chimpanzee olfactory receptor
gene repertoires. Genome Research, 15, 224–230.
332 References
Gilad, Y., Wiebe, V., Przeworski, M., Lancet, D., & Pä ä bo, S. (2004). Loss of olfactory receptor genes
coincides with the acquisition of full trichromatic vision in primates. PLoS Biology, 2, 120–125.
Gilbert, S. J., Spengler, S., Simons, J. S., Steele, J. D., Lawrie, S. M., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W. (2006).
Functional specialization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10): A meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 18, 932–948.
Gilby, I. C., Machanda, Z. P., O’Malley, R. C., Murray, C. M., Lonsdorf, E. V., Walker, K., …
Wrangham, R. W. (2017). Predation by female chimpanzees: Toward an understanding of sex differences
in meat acquisition in the last common ancestor of Pan and Homo. Journal of Human Evolution, 110, 82–94.
Gilligan, I. (2010). The prehistoric development of clothing: Archaeological implications of a thermal
model. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 17, 15–80.
Gilliland, A. R. (1948). The rate of forgetting. Journal of Educational Psychology, 39, 19–26.
Glaub, M., & Hall, C. A. S. (2017). Evolutionary implications of persistence hunting: an examination of
energy return on investment for!Kung hunting. Human Ecology, 45, 393–401.
Glezer, V. D. (1995). Vision and mind: Modeling mental functions. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Glover, S. (2004). Separate visual representations in the planning and control of action. Behavioral and Brain
Sciences, 27, 3–78.
Glueck, M., Crane, K., Anderson, S., Rutnik, A., & Khan, A. (2009). Multiscale 3D reference visualization.
Proceedings of I3D 2009: The 2009 ACM SIGGRAPH Symposium on Interactive 3D Graphics and Games,
225–232.
Go, Y. (2006). Lineage-specific expansions and contractions of the bitter taste receptor gene repertoire in
vertebrates. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 23, 964–972.
Go, Y., & Niimura, Y. (2008). Similar numbers but different repertoires of olfactory receptor genes in
humans and chimpanzees. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 25, 1897–1907.
Gold, D. A., Grabenstatter, J., de Mendoza, A., Riesgo, A. Ruiz-Trillo, I., & Summons, R. E. (2016). Sterol
and genomic analyses validate the sponge biomarker hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Science, 113, 2684–2689.
Gómez-Olivencia, A., Barash, A., García-Martínez, D., Arlegi, M., Kramer, P., Bastir, M., & Been, E.
(2018). 3D virtual reconstruction of the Kebara 2 Neanderthal thorax. Nature Communications, 9, article
4387.
Gommery, D., & Senut, B. (2006). La phalange distale du pouce d’Orrorin tugenensis (Miocè ne supé rieur du
Kenya). Geobios, 39, 372–384.
Goodchild, A. K., Ghosh, K. K., & Martin, P. R. (1996). Comparison of photoreceptor spatial density and
ganglion cell morphology in the retina of human, macaque monkey, cat, and the marmoset Callithrix
jacchus. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 366, 55–75.
Goodrum, M. R. (2013). Hermann Schaaffhausen (1816-1893). Retrieved August 18th, 2020, from https://
drive.google.com/file/d/13J3VE-jdNyUp_Sdc46Mz5Lv2foy1_UFl/view.
Gough, P. M., Connally, E. L., Howell, P., Ward, D., Chesters, J., & Watkins, K. E. (2018). Planum
temporale asymmetry in people who stutter. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 55, 94–105.
Goulas, A., Bastiani, M., Bezgin, G., Uylings, H. B. M., Roebroeck, A., et al. (2014). Comparative analysis
of the macroscale structural connectivity in the macaque and human brain. PLoS Computational Biology,
10(3), article e1003529.
Gowlett, J. A. J. (1992a). Early human mental abilities. In S. Jones, R. Martin, & D. Pilbeam (Eds.), The
Cambridge encyclopedia of human evolution (pp. 341–345). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Gowlett, J. A. J. (1992b). Tools – The Paleolithic record. In S. Jones, R. Martin, & D. Pilbeam (Eds.), The
Cambridge encyclopedia of human evolution (pp. 350–360). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Gowlett, J. A. J. (2016). The discovery of fire by humans: A long and convoluted process. Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 371, article 20150164.
Granatosky, M. C., Tripp, C. H., Fabre, A.-C., & Schmitt, D. (2016). Patterns of quadrupedal locomotion
in a vertical clinging and leaping primate (Propithecus coquereli) with implications for understanding the
functional demands of primate quadrupedal locomotion. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 160,
644–652.
Granit, R. (1977). The purposive brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Green, R. E., Krause, J., Briggs, A. W., Maricic, T., Stenzel, U., Kircher, M., … Pä ä bo, S. (2010). A draft
sequence of the Neandertal genome. Science, 328, 710–722.
References 333
Green, R. E., Krause, J., Ptak, S. E., Briggs, A. W., Ronan, M. T., Simons, J. F., … Pä ä bo, S. (2006).
Analysis of one million base pairs of Neanderthal DNA. Nature, 444, 330–336.
Green, R. E., Malaspinas, A-S., Krause, J., Briggs, A. W., Johnson, P. L. F., Uhler, C., … Pä ä bo, S. (2008).
A complete Neandertal mitochondrial genome sequence determined by high-throughput sequencing.
Cell, 134, 416–426.
Greenfield, P. M., & Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1991). Imitation, grammatical development, and the in-
vention of protogrammar by an ape. In N. A. Krasnegor, D. M. Rumbaugh, R. L. Schie-Felbusch, &
M. Studdert-Kennedy (Eds.), Biological and behavioral determinants of language development (pp. 235–258).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Greenwood, J. A., Szinte, M., Sayim, B., & Cavanagh, P. (2017). Variations in crowding, saccadic precision,
and spatial localization reveal the shared topology of spatial vision. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 114, E3573–E3582.
Grefkes, C., & Fink, G. R. (2005). The functional organization of the intraparietal sulcus in humans and
monkeys. Journal of Anatomy, 207, 3–17.
Grefkes, C., Weiss, P. H., Zilles, K., & Fink, G. R. (2002). Crossmodal processing of object features in
human anterior intraparietal cortex: An fMRI study implies equivalencies between humans and monkeys.
Neuron, 35, 173–184.
Grimm, R. E., & Marchi, S. (2018). Direct thermal effects of the Hadrean bombardment did not limit early
subsurface habitability. Earth and Planetary Science Letters, 485, 1–8.
Gross, C. G. (2002). Genealogy of the “grandmother cell”. The Neuroscientist, 8, 512–518.
Gruss, L.T., & Schmitt, D. (2015). The evolution of the human pelvis: Changing adaptations to bipedalism,
obstetrics and thermoregulation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 370, article 20140063.
Gulick, S. P. S., Bralower, T. J., Ormö, J., Hall, B., Grice, K., Schaefer, B., … Wittmann, A. (2019). The
first day of the Cenozoic. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 116, 19342–19351.
Gupta, S., & Sinha, A. (2019). Gestural communication of wild bonnet macaques in the Bandipur National
Park, Southern India. Behavioural Processes, 168, article 103956.
Guy, F., Daver, G., Mackaye, H. T., Likius, A., Boisserie, J.-R., Moussa, A., … Nekoulnang, C. (2020).
Postcranial evidence of late Miocene hominin bipedalism in Chad. Pre-review manuscript, retrieved 19
June 2021 from https://www.researchsquare.com/article/rs-69453/v1.
Haaland, K. Y., Harrington, D. L., & Knight, R. T. (2000). Neural representations of skilled movement.
Brain, 123, 2306–2313.
Haile-Selassie, Y., Melillo, S. M., Vazzana, A., Benazzi, S., & Ryan, T. M. (2019). A 3.8-million-year-old
hominin cranium from Woranso-Mille, Ethiopia. Nature, 573, 214–219.
Haile-Selassie, Y., Suwa, G., & White, T. D. (2004). Late Miocene teeth from Middle Awash, Ethiopia, and
early hominid dental evolution. Science, 303, 1503–1505.
Hallos, J. (2005). “15 minutes of fame”: Exploring the temporal dimension of Middle Pleistocene lithic
technology. Journal of Human Evolution, 49, 155–179.
Halsband, U., & Lange, R. K. (2006). Motor learning in man: A review of functional and clinical studies.
Journal of Physiology – Paris, 99, 414–424.
Halsey, L. G., & White, C. R. (2012). Comparative energetics of mammalian locomotion: Humans are not
different. Journal of Human Evolution, 63, 718–722.
Hampton, R. R., & Schwartz, B. L. (2004). Episodic memory in nonhumans: What, and where, is when?
Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14, 192–197.
Hä nelt, I., Tholema, N., Krö ning, N., Vor Der Brü ggen, M., & Wunnicke, D. (2011). KtrB, a member of
the superfamily of K+ transporters. European Journal of Cell Biology, 90, 696–704.
Hanna, J. B., Granatosky, M. C., Rana, P., & Schmitt, D. (2017). The evolution of vertical climbing in
primates: Evidence from reaction forces. Journal of Experimental Biology, 220, 3039–3052.
Hansma, H. G. (2010). Possible origin of life between mica sheets. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 266,
175–188.
Harcourt-Smith, W. E. H., & Aiello, L. C. (2004). Fossils, feet and the evolution of human bipedal lo-
comotion. Journal of Anatomy, 204, 403–416.
Hare, B., Addessi, E., Call, J., Tomasello, M., & Visalberghi, E. (2003). Do capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella,
know what conspecifics do and do not see? Animal Behaviour, 65, 131–142.
334 References
Harmand, S., Lewis, J. E., Feibel, C. S., Lepre, C. J., Prat, S., Lenoble, A., … Roche, H. (2015). 3.3-
million-year-old stone tools from Lomekwi 3, West Turkana, Kenya. Nature, 521, 310–316.
Harrington, A. R., Silcox, M. T., Yapunich, G. S., Boyer, D. M., & Bloch, J. I. (2016). First virtual
endocasts of adapiform primates. Journal of Human Evolution, 99, 52–78.
Harrison, T. (2002). Late Oligocene to middle Miocene catarrhines from Afro-Arabia. In W. C. Hartwig
(Ed.), The primate fossil record (pp. 311–338). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Hart, B. L., Hart, L. A., & Pinter-Wollman, N. (2008). Large brains and cognition: Where do elephants fit
in? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 86–98.
Hatala, K. G., Demes, B., & Richmond, B. G. (2016). Laetoli footprints reveal bipedal gait biomechanics
different from those of modern humans and chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B, 283, article
20160235.
Havrilak, J. A., Faltine-Gonzalez, D., Wen, Y., Fodera, D., Simpson, A. C., Magie, C. R., & Layden, M. J.
(2017). Characterization of NvLWamide-like neurons reveals stereotypy in Nematostella nerve net devel-
opment. Developmental Biology, 431, 336–346.
Hawks, J. (2009). Ardipithecus FAQ. Retrieved February 15, 2019 from http://johnhawks.net/weblog/
fossils/ardipithecus/ardipithecus-faq-2009.html.
Hawks, J., Elliott, M., Schmid, P., Churchill, S. E., de Ruiter, D. J., Roberts, E. M., … Berger, L. R.
(2017). New fossil remains of Homo naledi from the Lesedi Chamber, South Africa. eLife, 6, article e24232.
Hayden, B. (2015). Insights into early lithic technologies from ethnography. Philosophical Transactions of the
Royal Society, B, 370, article 20140356.
He, L., Zuo, Z., Chen, L., & Humphrey, G. (2013). Effects of number magnitude and notation at 7T:
Separating the neural response to small and large, symbolic and nonsymbolic number. Cerebral Cortex, 24,
2199–2209.
Hecht, E. E., Mahovetz, L. M., Preuss, T. M., & Hopkins, W. D. (2017). A neuroanatomical predictor of
mirror self-recognition in chimpanzees. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 37–48.
Heesy, C. P. (2004). On the relationship between orbit orientation and binocular visual field overlap in
mammals. The Anatomical Record Part A, 281A, 1104–1110.
Heesy, C. P. (2008). Ecomorphology of orbit orientation and the adaptive significance of binocular vision in
primates and other mammals. Brain, Behavior, and Evolution, 71, 54–67.
Heffner, R. S. (2004). Primate hearing from a mammalian perspective. The Anatomical Record Part A, 281A,
1111–1122.
Heilbroner, P. L., & Holloway, R. L. (1988). Anatomical brain asymmetries in new world and old world
monkeys: Stages of temporal lobe development in primate evolution. American Journal of Physical
Anthropology, 76, 39–48.
Heilman, K. M. (2005). Creativity and the brain. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Henke, J. M., & Bassler, B. L. (2004). Bacterial social engagements. Trends in Cell Biology, 14, 648–656.
Heritage, S. (2014). Modeling olfactory bulb evolution through primate phylogeny. PLoS One, 9, article
e113904.
Herná ndez-Pé rez, R., Cuaya, L. V., Rojas-Hortelano, E., Reyes-Aguilar, A., Concha, L., & DeLafuente, V.
(2017). Tactile object categories can be decoded from the parietal and lateral-occipital cortices.
Neuroscience, 352, 226–235.
Herrera, J. P., & Dá valos, L. M. (2016). Phylogeny and divergence times of lemurs inferred with recent and
ancient fossils in the tree. Systematic Biology, 65, 772–791.
Herries, A. I. R., Martin, J. M., Leece, A. B., Adams, J. W., Boschian, G., Joannes-Boyau, R., … Menter,
C. (2020). Contemporaneity of Australopithecus, Paranthropus, and early Homo erectus in South Africa.
Science, 368, article 47.
Hickman, C. P., Roberts, L. S., & Larson, A. (1997). Integrated principles of zoology. Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Hobaiter, C., & Byrne, R. W. (2010). Able-bodied chimpanzees imitate a motor procedure used by a
disabled individual to overcome handicap. PLoS One, 5, e11959.
Hobaiter, C., & Byrne, R. W. (2017). What is a gesture? A meaning-based approach to defining gestural
repertoires. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 82, 3–12.
Hobaiter, C., Poisot, T., Zuberbü hler, K., Hoppitt, W., & Gruber, T. (2014). Social network analysis shows
direct evidence for social transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees. PLoS Biology, 12, article e1001960.
References 335
Hodges, B. H. (2017). Carrying, caring, and conversing: Constraints on the emergence of cooperation,
conformity, and language. Interaction Studies, 18, 26–54.
Hoffecker, J. F. (2005). Innovation and technological knowledge in the Upper Paleolithic of northern
Eurasia. Evolutionary Anthropology, 14, 186–198.
Hoffman, J. N., Montag, A. G., & Dominy, N. J. (2004). Meissner corpuscles and somatosensory acuity:
The prehensile appendages of primates and elephants. Anatomical Record Part A, 281A, 1138–1147.
Hohn-Schulte, B., Preuschoft, H., Witzel, U., & Distler-Hoffmann, C. (2013). Biomechanics and func-
tional preconditions for terrestrial lifestyle in basal tetrapods, with special consideration of Tiktaalik roseae.
Historical Biology, 25, 167–181.
Hoke, K. L., & Fernald, R. D. (1997). Rod photoreceptor neurogenesis. Progress in Retinal and Eye Research,
16, 31–49.
Holdstock, J. S., Hocking, J., Notley, P., Devlin, J. T., & Price, C. J. (2009). Integrating visual and tactile
information in the perirhinal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 2993–3000.
Holland, N. D., & Chen, J. (2001). Origin and early evolution of the vertebrates: New insights from
advances in molecular biology, anatomy, and palaeontology. BioEssays, 23, 142–151.
Holliday, C. M., Porter, W. R., Vliet, K. A., & Witmer, L. M. (2019). The frontoparietal fossa and dor-
sotemporal fenestra of archosaurs and their significance for interpretations of vascular and muscular
anatomy in dinosaurs. The Anatomical Record, 303, 1060–1074.
Holliday, T. W., Hutchinson, V. T., Morrow, M. M. B., & Livesay, G. A. (2010). Geometric mor-
phometric analyses of hominid proximal femora: Taxonomic and phylogenetic considerations. HOMO –
Journal of Comparative Human Biology, 61, 3–15.
Hong, S. W., & Tong, F. (2017). Neural representation of form-contingent color filling-in in the early visual
cortex. Journal of Vision, 17, 1–10.
Hopkins, W. D. (2006). Comparative and familial analysis of handedness in great apes. Psychological Bulletin,
132, 538–559.
Hopkins, W. D., & Cantalupo, C. (2004). Handedness in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) is associated with
asymmetries of the primary motor cortex but not with homologous language areas. Behavioral Neuroscience,
118, 1176–1183.
Hopkins, W. D., & Cantero, M. (2003). From hand to mouth in the evolution of language: The influence of
vocal behavior on lateralized hand use in manual gestures by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Developmental
Science, 6, 55–61.
Hopkins, W. D., & Washburn, D. A. (2002). Matching visual stimuli on the basis of global and local
features by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Animal Cognition, 5,
27–31.
Hopkins, W. D., Hopkins, A. M., Misiura, M., Latash, E. M., Mareno, M. C., Schapiro, S. J., & Phillips, K.
A. (2016). Sex differences in the relationship between planum temporale asymmetry and corpus callosum
morphology in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): A combined MRI and DTI analysis. Neuropsychologia, 93(Pt.
B), 325–334.
Hopkins, W. D., Meguerditchian, A., Coulon, O., Misiura, M., Pope, S., Mareno, M. C., & Scha-Piro, S. J.
(2017). Motor skill for tool-use is associated with asymmetries in Broca’s area and the motor hand area of
the precentral gyrus in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Behavioural Brain Research, 318, 71–81.
Hopkins, W. D., Phillips, K. A., Bania, A., Calcutt, S. E., Gardner, M., Russell, J., … Schapiro, S. J. (2011).
Hand preferences for coordinated bimanual actions in 777 great apes: Implications for the evolution of
handedness in hominins. Journal of Human Evolution, 60, 605–611.
Hopkins, W. D., Pilcher, D. L., & MacGregor, L. (2000). Sylvian fissure asymmetries in nonhuman primates
revisited: A comparative MRI study. Brain, Behavior, and Evolution, 56, 293–299.
Hopkins, W. D., Russell, J. L., & Cantalupo, C. (2007). Neuroanatomical correlates of handedness for tool
use in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Psychological Science, 18, 971–977.
Hopkins, W. D., Russell, J. L., Cantalupo, C., Freeman, H., & Schapiro, S. J. (2005). Factors influencing the
prevalence and handedness for throwing in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative
Psychology, 119, 363–370.
Hopkins, W. D., Russell, J. L., Freeman, H., Buehler, N., Reynolds, E., & Schapiro, S. J. (2005). The
distribution and development of handedness for manual gestures in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).
Psychological Science, 16, 487–493.
336 References
Hopkins, W. D., Russell, J. L., Hostetter, A., Pilcher, D., & Dahl, J. F. (2005). Grip preference, derma-
toglyphics, and hand use in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Physical Anthropology,
128, 57–62.
Hopkins, W. D., Schaeffer, J., Russell, J. L., Bogart, S. L., Meguerditchian, A., & Coulon, O. (2015). A
comparative assessment of handedness and its potential neuroanatomical correlates in chimpanzees (Pan
troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus). Behaviour, 152, 461–492.
Hopkins, W. D., Wesley, M. J., Russell, J. L., & Schapiro, S. J. (2006). Parental and perinatal factors
influencing the development of handedness in captive chimpanzees. Developmental Psychobiology, 48,
428–435.
Hosfield, R. (2016). Walking in a winter wonderland? Strategies for early and middle Pleistocene survival in
midlatitude Europe. Current Anthropology, 57, 653–682.
Hotta, T., Komiyama, S, & Kohda, M. (2018). A social cichlid fish failed to pass the mark test. Animal
Cognition, 21, 127–136.
Houle, A. (1999). The origin of platyrrhines: An evaluation of the Antarctic scenario and the floating island
model. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 109, 541–559.
Hublin, J-J., Ben-Ncer, A., Bailey, S. E., Freidline, S. E., Neubauer, S., Skinnter, M. M., … Gunz, P.
(2017). New fossils from Jebel Irhoud, Morocco and the pan-African origin of Homo sapiens. Nature, 546,
289–292.
Hughes, A. L., da Silva, J., & Friedman, R. (2001). Ancient genome duplications did not structure the
human Hox-bearing chromosomes. Genome Research, 11, 771–780.
Humphreys, G. L., & Forde, E. M. E. (2001). Hierarchies, similarity, and interactivity in object recognition:
“Category-specific” neuropsychological deficits. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 453–476.
Humphreys, G. L., & Riddoch, M. J. (2003). A case series analysis of “category-specific” deficits of living
things: The HIT account. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 263–306.
Hung, C-C., Yen, C. C., Ciuchta, J. L., Papoti, D., Bock, N. A., Leopold, D. A., & Silva, A. C. (2015).
Functional mapping of face-selective regions in the extrastriate visual cortex of the marmoset. The Journal
of Neuroscience, 35, 1160–1172.
Hung, C. P., Kreiman, G., Poggio, T., & DiCarlo, J. J. (2005). Fast readout of object identity from macaque
inferior temporal cortex. Science, 310, 863–866.
Hunley, K. L., Cabana, G. S., & Long, J. C. (2016). The apportionment of human diversity revisited.
American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 160, 561–569.
Hunt, K. D. (1994). The evolution of human bipedality: Ecology and functional morphology. Journal of
Human Evolution, 26, 183–202.
Hunt, K. D. (2016). Why are there apes? Evidence for the co-evolution of ape and monkey ecomorphology.
Journal of Anatomy, 228, 630–685.
Hupé , J-M., James, A. C., Girard, P., Lomber, S. G., Payne, B. R., & Bullier, J. (2001). Feedback con-
nections act on the early part of the responses in monkey visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 85,
134–145.
Hutchinson, C. V., Prados, J., & Davidson, C. (2015). Persistent conditioned place preference to cocaine
and withdrawal hypo-locomotion to mephedrone in the flatworm planaria. Neuroscience Letters, 593,
19–23.
Hutchinson, J., Bates, K. T., Molnar, J., Allen, V., & Makovicky, P. J. (2011). A computational analysis of
limb and body dimensions in Tyrannosaurus Rex with implications for locomotion, ontogeny, and growth.
PLoS ONE, 6(10): e26037.
Huxley, T. H. (1870). Further evidence of the affinity between the dinosaurian reptiles and birds. Quarterly
Journal of the Geological Society. Retrieved July 24, 2006 from http://aleph0.clarku. edu/huxley/SM3/
Dino-boid.html
Hyatt, C. W. (1998). Responses of gibbons (Hylobates lar) to their mirror images. American Journal of
Primatology, 45, 307–311.
Iachini, T., Ruggiero, G., Conson, M., & Trojano, L. (2009). Lateralization of egocentric and allocentric
spatial processing after parietal brain lesions. Brain and Cognition, 69, 514–520.
Ibrahim, N., & Kutschera, U. (2013). The ornithologist Alfred Russel Wallace and the controversy sur-
rounding the dinosaurian origin of birds. Theory in Biosciences, 132, 267–275.
References 337
Ichim, I., Kieser, J., & Swain, M. (2007). Tongue contractions during speech may have led to the devel-
opment of the bony geometry of the chin following the evolution of human language: A mechan-
obiological hypothesis for the development of the human chin. Medical Hypotheses, 69, 20–24.
Ida, Y., & Bryden, M. P. (1996). A comparison of hand preference in Japan and Canada. Canadian Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 50, 234–239.
Iidaka, T. (2014). Role of the fusiform gyrus and superior temporal sulcus in face perception and re-
cognition: An empirical review. Japanese Psychological Research, 56, 33–45.
Indriati, E., Swisher, C. C., Lepre, C., Quinn, R. L., Suriyanto, R. A., Hascaryo, A. T., … Antó n, S. C.
(2011). The age of the 20 meter Solo River Terrace, Java, Indonesia and the survival of Homo erectus in
Asia. PLoS One, 6, e21562.
Ingman, M., Kaessmann, H., Pä ä bo, S., & Gyllensten, U. (2000). Mitochondrial genome variation and the
origin of modern humans. Nature, 408, 708–713.
Irisarri, I., & Meyer, A. (2016). The identification of the closest living relative(s) of tetrapods: Phylogenomic
lessons for resolving short ancient internodes. Systematic Biology, 65, 1057–1075.
Irish, J. D., Guatelli-Steinberg, D., Legge, S. S., de Ruiter, D. J., & Berger, L. R. (2013). Dental mor-
phology and the phylogenetic “place” of Australopithecus sediba. Science, 340, article 1233062.
Ishikawa, T., & Montello, D. R. (2006). Spatial knowledge acquisition from direct experience in the en-
vironment: Individual differences in the development of metric knowledge and the integration of sepa-
rately learned places. Cognitive Psychology, 52, 93–129.
Isik, L., Meyers, E. M., Leibo, J. Z., & Poggio, T. (2014). The dynamics of invariant object recognition in
the human visual system. Journal of Neurophysiology, 111, 91–102.
Islas, S., Velasco, A. M., Becerra, A., Delaye, L., & Lazcano, A. (2003). Hyperthermophily and the origin
and earliest evolution of life. International Microbiology, 6, 87–94.
Iwaniuk, A. N., & Nelson, J. E. (2003). Developmental differences are correlated with relative brain size in
birds: A comparative analysis. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 81, 1913–1928.
Jaakkola, K. (2014). Do animals understand invisible displacement? A critical review. Journal of Comparative
Psychology, 128, 225–239.
Jablonski, N. G., & Chaplin, G. (2017). The colours of humanity: The evolution of pigmentation in the
human lineage. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 372, article 20160349.
Jacobs, J., Weidermann, C. T., Miller, J. F., Solway, A., Burke, J. F., Wei, X-X., … Kahana, M. J. (2013).
Direct recordings of grid-like neuronal activity in human spatial navigation. Nature Neuroscience, 16,
1188–1191.
Jacobs, R. L., Veilleux, C. C., Louis, E. E., Herrera, J. P., Hiramatsu, C., Frankel, D. C., … Bradley, B. J.
(2019). Less is more: Lemurs (Eulemur spp.) may benefit from loss of trichromatic vision. Behavioral Ecology
and Sociobiology, 73, article 22.x
James, W. (1892). Psychology. New York, NY: Henry Holt.
Jang, H., Boesch, C., Mundry, R., Ban, S. D., & Janmaat, K. R. L. (2019). Travel linearity and speed of
human foragers and chimpanzees during their daily search for food in tropical rainforests. Scientific Reports,
9, article 11066.
Janmaat, K. R. L., Ban, S. D., & Boesch, C. (2013). Chimpanzees use long-term spatial memory to monitor
large fruit trees and remember feeding experiences across seasons. Animal Behaviour, 86, 1183–1205.
Jax, S. A., Buxbaum, L. J., & Moll, A. D. (2006). Deficits in movement planning and intrinsic coordinate
control in ideomotor apraxia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 2063–2076.
Jerison, H. J. (1973). Evolution of the brain and intelligence. New York, NY: Academic Press.
Ji, Q., Luo, Z.-X., Yuan, C-X., Wible, J. R., Zhang, J-P., & Georgi, J. A. (2002). The earliest known
eutherian mammal. Nature, 416, 816–822.
Ji, Q., Wu, W., Ji, Y., Li, Q., & Ni, X. (2021). Late Middle Pleistocene Harbin cranium represents a new
Homo species. The Innovation, article 100132.
Jiggins, C. D., & Bridle, J. R. (2004). Speciation in the apple maggot fly: A blend of vintages? Trends in
Ecology and Evolution, 19, 111–114.
Johanson, D. (2017). The paleoanthropology of Hadar, Ethiopia. Comptes Rendus Palevol, 16, 140–154.
Johnson, G. (1995, June 6). Chimp talk debate: Is it really language? New York Times, pp. C1, C10.
Johnson, K. P., Nguyen, N-P., Sweet, A. D., Boyd, B. M., Warnow, T., & Allen, J. M. (2018).
338 References
Simultaneous radiation of bird and mammal lice following the K-Pg boundary. Biology Letters, 14, article
20180141.
Johnson, R. R., & Burkhalter, A. (1996). Microcircuitry of forward and feedback connections within rat
visual cortex. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 368, 383–398.
Jones, S., Martin, R., & Pilbeam, D. (1992). Glossary. In The Cambridge encyclopedia of human evolution
(pp. 458–472). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Joordens, J. C. A., d’Errico, F., Wesselingh, F. P., Munro, S., de Vos, J., Wallinga, J., … Roe-Broeks, W.
(2015). Homo erectus at Trinil on Java used shells for tool production and engraving. Nature, 518, 228–231.
Jordan, S. F., Rammu, H., Zheludev, I. N., Hartley, A. M., Maréchal, A., & Lane, N. (2019). Promotion of
protocell self-assembly from mixed amphiphiles at the origin of life. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 3,
1705–1714.
Josse, G., Mazoyer, B., Crivello, F., & Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2003). Left planum temporale: An anatomical
marker of left hemispheric specialization for language comprehension. Cognitive Brain Research, 18, 1–14.
Kaas, J. H. (2004a). Evolution of somatosensory and motor cortex in primates. The Anatomical Record Part A,
281A, 1148–1156.
Kaas, J. H. (2004b). The evolution of the visual system in primates. In L. M. Chalupa & J. S. Werner (Eds.),
The visual neurosciences (Vol. 2, pp. 1563–1572). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kaas, J. H. (2012). The evolution of neocortex in primates. Progress in Brain Research, 195, 91–102.
Kaas, J. H. (2013). The evolution of brains from early mammals to humans. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews:
Cognitive Science, 4, 33–45.
Kahlenberg, S. M., & Wrangham, R. W. (2011). Sex differences in chimpanzees’ use of sticks as play objects
resemble those of children. Current Biology, 20, R1067–R1068.
Kalan, A. K., Carmignani, E., Kronland-Martinet, R., Ystad, S., Chatron, J., & Aramaki, M. (2019).
Chimpanzees use tree species with a resonant timbre for accumulative stone throwing. Biology Letters, 15,
article 20190747.
Kaltenbach, S. L., Yu, J-K., & Holland, N. D. (2009). The origin and migration of the earliest- developing
sensory neurons in the peripheral nervous system of amphioxus. Evolution & Development, 11, 142–151.
Kaminski, J., Riedel, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Domestic goats, Capra hircus, follow gaze direction
and use social cues in an object choice task. Animal Behaviour, 69, 11–18.
Kano, F., & Call, J. (2014). Cross-species variation in gaze following and conspecific preference among great
apes, human infants and adults. Animal Behaviour, 91, 137–150.
Kanwisher, N., & Yovel, G. (2006). The fusiform face area: A cortical region specialized for the perception
of faces. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 361, 2109–2128.
Kapoor, B. G., & Khanna, B. (2004). Ichthyology handbook. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
Kappelman, J. (1996). The evolution of body mass and relative brain size in fossil hominids. Journal of Human
Evolution, 30, 243–276.
Karakostis, F. A., Haeufle, D., Anastopoulou, I., Moraitis, K., Hotz, G., Tourloukis, V., & Harvati, K.
(2021). Biomechanics of the human thumb and the evolution of dexterity. Current Biology, 31,
1317–1325.
Kasting, J. F., & Catling, D. (2003). Evolution of a habitable planet. Annual Review of Astronomy and
Astrophysics, 41, 429–463.
Kavaklioglu, T., Ajmal, M., Hameed, A., & Francks, C. (2016). Whole exome sequencing for handedness in
a large and highly consanguineous family. Neuropsychologia, 93, 342–349.
Kay, R. F., Campbell, V. M., Rossie, J. B., Colbert, M. W., & Rowe, T. B. (2004). Olfactory fossa of
Tremacebus harringtoni (Platyrrhini, Early Miocene, Sacanana, Argentina): Implications for activity pattern.
The Anatomical Record Part A, 281A, 1157–1172.
Keating, J. N., Marquart, C. L., Marone, F., & Donoghue, P. C. J. (2018). The nature of aspidin and the
evolutionary origin of bone. Nature Ecology and Evolution, 2, 1501–1506.
Kemeny, R. (2019). Did hominins leave Africa 2.5 million years ago? Retrieved January 8, 2000, from
t https://www.realclearscience.com/articles/2019/09/21/did_hominins_leave_africa_25_million_years_
ago.html.
Kemp, A. D., & Kirk, E. C. (2014). Eye size and visual acuity influence vestibular anatomy in mammals. The
Anatomical Record, 297, 781–790.
Kemp, T. S. (2005). The origin and evolution of mammals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
References 339
Kersken, V., Gómez, J.-C., Liszkowski, U., Soldati, A., & Hobaiter, C. (2019). A gestural repertoire of 1- to
2-year-old human children: In search of the ape gestures. Animal Cognition, 22, 577–595.
Kherdjemil, Y., Lalonde, R. L., Sheth, R., Dumouchel, A., de Martino, G., Pineault, K. M., … Kmita, M.
(2016). Evolution of Hoxa11 regulation in vertebrates is linked to the pentadactyl state. Nature, 539,
89–92.
Kielan-Jaworowska, Z., & Hurum, J. H. (2006). Limb posture in early mammals: Sprawling or parasagittal.
Acta Paleontologica Polonica, 51, 393–406.
Killian, N. J., Jutras, M. J., & Buffalo, E. A. (2012). A map of visual space in the primate entorhinal cortex.
Nature, 491, 761–764.
Kim, J-W., Yang, H-J., Oel, A. P., Brooks, M. J., Jia, L., Plachetzki, D. C., … Swaroop, A. (2016).
Recruitment of rod photoreceptors from short-wavelength-sensitive cones during the evolution of
nocturnal vision in mammals. Developmental Cell, 37, 520–532.
Kimbel, W. H., & Rak, Y. (2017). Australopithecus sediba and the emergence of Homo: Questionable evi-
dence from the cranium of the juvenile holotype MH1. Journal of Human Evolution, 107, 94–106.
King, H. M., Shubin, N. H., Coates, M. I., & Hale, M. E. (2011). Behavioral evidence for the evolution of
walking and bounding before terrestriality in sarcopterygian fishes. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 108, 21146–21151.
King, W. (1864). The reputed fossil man of the Neanderthal. The Quarterly Journal of Science, 1, 88–96.
Kircher, T., Nagles, A., Kirner-Veselinovic, A., & Krach, S. (2011). Neural correlates of rhyming vs. lexical
and semantic fluency. Brain Research, 1391, 71–80.
Kirino, M., Parnes, J., Hansen, A., Kiyohara, S., & Finger, T. E. (2013). Evolutionary origins of taste buds:
Phylogenetic analysis of purinergic neurotransmission in epithelial chemosensors. Open Biology, 3, article
130015.
Kirk, E. C. (2006). Visual influences on primate encephalization. Journal of Human Evolution, 51, 76–90.
Kivell, T. L., & Schmitt, D. (2009). Independent evolution of knuckle-walking in African apes shows that
humans did not evolve from a knuckle-walking ancestor. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
106, 14241–14246.
Kivell, T. L., Deane, A. S., Tocheri, M. W., Orr, C. M., Schmid, P., Hawks, J., … Churchill, S. E. (2015).
The hand of Homo naledi. Nature Communications, 6, article 8431.
Klatzky, R. L. (1980). Human memory: Structures and processes. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman and Co.
Knox, A., Markx, J., How, E., Azis, A., Hobaiter, C., van Veen, F. J. F., & Morrogh-Bernard, H. (2019).
Gesture use in communication between mothers and offspring in wild orang-utans (Pongo pygmaeus
wurmbii) from the Sabangau peat-swamp forest, Borneo. International Journal of Primatology, 40, 393–416.
Kohda, M., Hotta, T., Takeyama, T., Awata, S., Tanaka, H., Asai, J., & Jordan, A. (2019). If a fish can pass
the mark test, what are the implications for consciousness and self-awareness testing in animals? PLoS
Biology, 17, article e3000021.
Kö hler, W. (1927). The mentality of apes. New York, NY: Harcourt, Brace & Co.
Kolb, B., & Whishaw, I. Q. (2003). Fundamentals of human neuropsychology. New York, NY: W.H. Freeman
and Co.
Kolb, B., & Whishaw, I. Q. (2009). Fundamentals of human neuropsychology. New York, NY: Worth
Publishers.
Kooyman, B. P. (2000). Understanding stone tools and archaeological sites. Calgary, Canada: University of
Calgary Press.
Koudouna, E., Winkler, M., Mikula, E., Juhasz, T., Brown, D. J., & Jester, J. V. (2018). Evolution of the
vertebrate corneal stroma. Progress in Retinal and Eye Research, 64, 65–76.
Ková cs, G., Sá ry, G., Kö teles, K., Chadaide, Z., Tompa, T., Vogels, R., & Benedek, G. (2003). Effects of
surface cues on macaque inferior temporal cortical responses. Cerebral Cortex, 13, 178–188.
Kozmik, Z. (2008). The role of Pax genes in eye evolution. Brain Research Bulletin, 75, 335–339.
Krachen, C., Lurz, R., Mahovetz, L. M., and Hopkins, W. D. (2019). Mirror self-recognition and its
relationship to social cognition in chimpanzees. Animal Cognition, 22, 1171–1183.
Krause, J., Lalueza-Fox, C., Enard, W., Green, R. E., Burbano, H. A., Hublin, J-J., … Pä ä bo, S. (2007).
The derived FOXP2 variant of modern humans was shared with Neanderthals. Current Biology, 17,
1908–1912.
340 References
Krauzlis, R. J., Bogadhi, A. R., Herman, J. P., & Bollimunta, A. (2018). Selective attention without a
neocortex. Cortex, 102, 161–175.
Krö ger, R. H. H., Gustafsson, O. S. E., & Tuminaite, I. (2014). Suspension and optical properties of the
crystalline lens in the eyes of basal vertebrates. Journal of Morphology, 275, 613–622.
Ksepka, D. T., Balanoff, A. M., Smith, N. A., Bever, G. S., Bhuller, B.-A. S., Bourdon, E., … Smaers, J. B.
(2020). Tempo and pattern of avian brain size evolution. Current Biology, 30, 2026–2036.
Kuczaj, S., Tranel, K., Trone, M., & Hill, H. (2001). Are animals capable of deception or empathy?
Implications for animal consciousness and animal welfare. Animal Welfare, 10, S161–S173.
Kü hl, H. S., Kalan, A. K., Arandjelovic, M., Aubert, F., D’Auvergne, L., Goedmakers, A., … Boesch, C.
(2016). Chimpanzee accumulative stone throwing. Scientific Reports, 6, article 22219.
Kuhlmeier, V. A., Boysen, S. T., & Mukobi, K. L. (1999). Scale-model comprehension by chimpanzees
(Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113, 396–402.
Kumar, V., Croxson, P. L., & Simonyan, K. (2016). Structural organization of the laryngeal motor cortical
network and its implication for evolution of speech production. The Journal of Neuroscience, 36,
4170–4181.
Kuperavage, A., Pokrajac, D., Chavanaves, S., & Eckhardt, R. B. (2018). Earliest known hominin calcar
femorale in Orrorin Tugenensis provides further internal anatomical evidence for origin of human bipedal
locomotion. The Anatomical Record, 301, 1834–1839.
Kvavadze, E., Bar-Yosef, O., Belfer-Cohen, A., Boaretto, E., Jakeli, N., Matskevich, Z., & Meshveliani, T.
(2009). 30,000-year-old wild flax fibers. Science, 325, 1359.
Lacalli, T. C. (1994). Apical organs, epithelial domains, and the origin of the chordate central nervous
system. American Zoologist, 34, 533–541.
Lacalli, T. C. (1996). Frontal eye circuitry, rostral sensory pathways and brain organization in amphioxus
larvae: Evidence from 3D reconstructions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B, 351,
243–263.
Lacalli, T. C. (2004). Sensory systems in amphioxus: A window on the ancestral chordate. Brain, Behavior,
and Evolution, 64, 148–162.
Lacalli, T. C. (2018). Amphioxus, motion detection, and the evolutionary origin of the vertebrate re-
tinotectal map. EvoDevo, 9, article 6.
Lacalli, T. C., & Kelly, S. J. (2003). Ventral neurons in the anterior nerve cord of amphioxus larvae. I. An
inventory of cell types and synaptic patterns. Journal of Morphology, 257, 190–211.
Lacey, S., Tal, N., Amedi, A., & Sathian, K. (2009). A putative model of multisensory object representation.
Brain Topography, 21, 269–274.
Lafer-Sousa, R., Conway, B. R., & Kanwisher, N. G. (2016). Color-biased regions of the ventral visual
pathway lie between face- and place-selective regions in humans, as in macaques. The Journal of
Neuroscience, 36, 1682–1697.
Lamb, T. D., Collin, S. P., & Pugh, E. N. (2007). Evolution of the vertebrate eye: Opsins, photoreceptors,
retina and eye cup. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 960–975.
Lamb, T. D., & Hunt, D. M. (2018). Evolution of the calcium feedback steps of vertebrate photo-
transduction. Open Biology, 8, article 180119.
Lambertz, M., Grommes, K., Kohlsdorf, T., & Perry, S. F. (2015). Lungs of the first amniotes: Why simple if
they can be complex? Biology Letters, 11, article 20130848.
Lamme, V. A. F. (2003). Why visual attention and awareness are different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7,
12–18.
Lamme, V. A. F., Rodriguez-Rodriguez, V., & Spekreijse, H. (1999). Separate processing dynamics for
texture elements, boundaries and surfaces in primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey. Cerebral
Cortex, 9, 406–413.
Land, M. F., & Nilsson, D-E. (2012). Animal eyes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Landi, S. M., Viswanathan, P., Serene, S., & Freiwald, W. A. (2021). A fast link between face perception and
memory in the temporal pole. Science, in press.
Landin-Romero, R., Tan, R., Hodges, J. R., & Kumfor, F. (2016). An update on semantic dementia:
Genetics, imaging, and pathology. Alzheimer’s Research & Therapy, 8, article 52.
Lane, N., & Martin, W. (2010). The energetics of genome complexity. Nature, 467, 929–934.
References 341
Larney, E., & Larson, S. G. (2004). Compliant walking in primates: Elbow and knee yield in primates
compared to other mammals. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 125, 42–50.
Larson, S. G., & Stern, J. T. (2006). Maintenance of above-branch balance during primate arboreal quad-
rupedalism: Coordinated use of forearm rotators and tail motion. American Journal of Physical Anthropology,
129, 71–81.
Laumer, I. B., Call, J., Bugnyar, T., & Auersperg, A. M. I. (2018). Spontaneous innovation of hook-bending
and unbending in orangutans (Pongo abelii). Scientific Reports, 8, article 16518.
Laurin, M., Girondot, M., & de Ricqlè s, A. (2000). Early tetrapod evolution. TREE, 15, 118–123.
Laval, S. H., Dann, J. C., Butler, R. J., Loftus, J., Rue, J., Leask, S. J., … Crow, T. J. (1998). Evidence for
linkage to psychosis and cerebral asymmetry (relative hand skill) on the X chromosome. American Journal of
Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics), 81, 420–427.
Lee, M. S. Y., Reeder, T. W., Slowinski, J. B., & Lawson, R. (2004). Resolving reptile relationships:
Molecular and morphological markers. In J. Cracraft & M. J. Donohue (Eds.), Assembling the tree of life
(pp. 451–467). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Lee, Y.-C., Chiang, C.-C., Huang, P.-Y., Chung, C.-Y., Huang, T. D., Wang, C.-C., … Reisz, R. R.
(2017). Evidence of preserved collagen in an Early Jurassic sauropodomorph dinosaur revealed by syn-
chrotron FTIR microspectroscopy. Nature Communications, 8, article 14220.
Lefebvre, L., Marino, L., Sol, D., Lemieux-Lefebvre, S., & Arshad, S. (2006). Large brains and lengthened
life history periods in Odontocetes. Brain, Behavior, and Evolution, 68, 218–228.
Lefebvre, L., Nicolakakis, N., & Boire, D. (2002). Tools and brains in birds. Behaviour, 139, 939–973.
Leh, S. E., Mullen, K. T., & Ptito, A. (2006). Absence of S-cone input in human blindsight following
hemispherectomy. European Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 2954–2960.
Leh, S. E., Ptito, A., Schö nwiesner, M., Chakravarty, M. M., & Mullen, K. T. (2010). Blindsight mediated
by an S-cone-independent colllicular pathway: An fMRI study in hemispherectomized patients. Journal of
Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 670–682.
Leinen, P., Panzer, S., & Shea, C. H. (2016). Hemispheric asymmetries of a motor memory in a recognition
test after learning a movement sequence. Acta Psychologica, 171, 36–46.
Leonard, W. R., & Robertson, M. L. (1997). Rethinking the energetics of bipedality. Current Anthropology,
38, 304–309.
Lepre, C. J., Roche, H., Kent, D. V., Harmand, S., Quinn, R. L., Brugal, J-P., … Feibel, C. (2011). An
earlier origin for the Acheulian. Nature, 477, 82–85.
Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “theory of mind”. Psychological Review, 94,
412–426.
Leung, H-C., Gore, J. C., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (2005). Differential anterior prefrontal activation during
the recognition stage of a spatial working memory task. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1742–1749.
Levin, H. L. (2010). The Earth through time. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Lewin, R., & Foley, R. A. (2004). Principles of human evolution. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
Lewis, A., Call, J., & Bemtsen, D. (2017). Non-goal-directed recall of specific events in apes after long
delays. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B, 284, article 20170518.
Lewis, J. E., & Harmand, S. (2016). An earlier origin for stone tool making: Implications for cognitive
evolution and the transition to Homo. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 371, article
20150233.
Lewton, K. L., & Scott, J. E. (2017). Ischial form as an indicator of bipedal kinematics in early hominins: A
test using extant anthropoids. The Anatomical Record, 300, 845–858.
Leys, S. P., Cronin, T. W., Degnan, B. M., & Marshall, J. N. (2002). Spectral sensitivity in a sponge larva.
Journal of Comparative Physiology A – Neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology, 188, 199–202.
Li, D., & Zhang, J. (2013). Diet shapes the evolution of the vertebrate bitter taste receptor gene repertoire.
Molecular Biology and Evolution, 31, 303–309.
Li, G., Liu, X., Xing, C., Zhang, H., Shimeld, S. M., & Wang, Y. (2017). Cerberus-Nodal-Lefty- Pitx
signaling cascade controls left-right asymmetry in amphioxus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
114, 3684–3689.
Li, Y., Richardson, R. M., & Ghuman, A. S. (2019). Posterior fusiform and midfusiform contribute to
distinct stages of facial expression processing. Cerebral Cortex, 29, 3209–3219.
342 References
Liang, R., Lau, M. C. Y., Saitta, E. T., Garvin, Z. K., & Onstott, T. C. (2020). Genome-centric resolution
of novel microbial lineages in an excavated Centrosaurus dinosaur fossil bone from the Late Cretaceous of
North America. Environmental Microbiome, 15, article 8.
Libby, T., Moore, T. Y., Chang-Siu, E., Li, D., Cohen, D. J., Jusufi, A., & Full, R. J. (2012). Tail- assisted
pitch control in lizards, robots and dinosaurs. Nature, 481, 181–186.
Liebenthal, E., Sabri, M., Beardsley, S. A., Mangalathu-Arumana, J., & Desai, A. (2013). Neural dynamics of
phonological processing in the dorsal auditory stream. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 15414–15424.
Lieberman, P. (2006). Toward an evolutionary biology of language. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard
University Press.
Lieberman, P. (2007). The evolution of human speech: Its anatomical and neural bases. Current Anthropology,
48, 39–66.
Lieberman, P. (2012). Vocal tract anatomy and the neural bases of talking. Journal of Phonetics, 40, 608–622.
Lieberman, P. (2017). Comment on “Monkey vocal tracts are speech-ready”. Science Advances, 3, article
e1700442.
Liebeskind, B. J., Hillis, D. M., Zakon, H. H., & Hofman, H. A. (2016). Complex homology and the
evolution of nervous systems. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 31, 127–135.
Light, J. E., & Reed, D. L. (2009). Multigene analysis of phylogenetic relationships and divergence times of
primate sucking lice (Phthiraptera: Anoplura). Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution, 50, 376–390.
Lincoln, T. A., & Joyce, G. F. (2009). Self-sustained replication of an RNA enzyme. Science, 323,
1229–1232.
Lind, J., Enquist, M., & Ghirlanda, S. (2015). Animal memory: A review of delayed matching-to- sample
data. Behavioural Processes, 117, 52–58.
Lindshield, S. M., & Rodrigues, M. A. (2009). Tool use in wild spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi). Primates,
50, 269–272.
Liu, Y., Su, Y., Xu, G., & Pei, M. (2018). When do you know what you know? The emergence of memory
monitoring. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 34–48.
Logan, G. A., Hayes, J. M., Hieshima, G. B., & Summons, R. E. (1995). Terminal Proterozoic re-
organization of biogeochemical cycles. Nature, 376, 53–56.
Long, G. M., & McCarthy, P. R. (1982). Rod persistence on a partial-report task with scotopic and
photopic backgrounds. American Journal of Psychology, 95, 309–322.
Lonsdorf, E. V., & Hopkins, W. D. (2005). Wild chimpanzees show population-level handedness for tool
use. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 12634–12638.
Lordkipanidze, D., de Leó n, M. S. P., Margvelashvili, A., Rak, Y., Rightmire, G. P., Vekua, A., &
Zollikofer, C. P. E. (2013). A complete skull from Dmanisi, Georgia, and the evolutionary biology of
early Homo. Science, 342, 326–331.
Love, G. D., Grosjean, E., Stalvies, C., Fike, D. A., Grotzinger, J. P., Bradley, A. S., … Summons, R. E.
(2009). Fossil steroids record the appearance of Demospongiae during the Cryogenian period. Nature,
457, 718–722.
Lovejoy, C. O., Latimer, B., Suwa, G., Asfaw, B., & White, T. D. (2009). Combining prehension and
propulsion: The foot of Ardipithecus ramidus. Science, 326, 72e1–72e8.
Lovejoy, C. O., Meindl, R. S., Ohman, J. C., Heiple, K. G., & White, T. D. (2002). The Maka femur and
its bearing on the antiquity of human walking: Applying contemporary concepts of morphogenesis to the
human fossil record. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 119, 97–133.
Lovejoy, C. O., Simpson, S. W., White, T. D., Asfaw, B., & Suwa, G. (2009). Careful climbing in the
Miocene: The forelimbs of Ardipithecus ramidus and humans are primitive. Science, 326, 70–70e6.
Lovejoy, C. O., Suwa, G., Spurlock, L., Asfaw, B., & White, T. D. (2009). The pelvis and femur of
Ardipithecus ramidus: The emergence of upright walking. Science, 326, 71.
Lozano, M., Mosquera, M., de Castro, J. M. B., Arsuaga, J. L., & Carbonell, E. (2009). Right handedness of
Homo heidelbergensis from Sima de los Huesos (Atapuerca, Spain) 500,000 years ago. Evolution and Human
Behavior, 30, 369–376.
Lu, Z., Williamson, S., & Kaufman, L. (1992). Behavioral lifetime of human auditory sensory memory
predicted by physiological measures. Science, 258, 1668–1670.
Lucas, S. F. (2015). Thinopus and a critical review of Devonian tetrapod footprints. Ichnos, 22, 136–154.
References 343
Luncz, L. V., Sirianni, G., Mundry, R., & Boesch, C. (2018). Costly culture: Differences in nut-cracking
efficiency between wild chimpanzee groups. Animal Behaviour, 137, 63–73.
Lundberg, Y. W., Xu, Y., Thiessen, K. D., & Kramer, K. L. (2015). Mechanisms of otoconia and otolith
development. Developmental Dynamics, 244, 239–253.
Lunine, J. I. (2005). Astrobiology: A multidisciplinary approach. San Francisco: Addison Wesley.
Luo, Z-X. (2001). A new mammaliaform from the early Jurassic and evolution of mammalian characteristics.
Science, 292, 1535–1540.
Luo, Z-X., Yuan, C-X., Meng, Q-J., & Ji, Q. (2011). A Jurassic eutherian mammal and divergence of
marsupials and placentals. Nature, 476, 442–445.
Lycett, S. J., & von Cramon-Taubadel, N. (2008). Acheulean variability and hominin dispersals: A model-
bound approach. Journal of Archaeological Science, 35, 553–562.
Lyn, H., & Savage-Rumbaugh, S. (2013). The use of emotion symbols in language-using apes. In
S. Watanabe & S. Kuczaj (Eds.), Emotions of animals and humans: Comparative perspectives (pp. 113–127).
Tokyo: Springer.
Lyn, H., Greenfield, P. M., & Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (2011). Semiotic combinations in Pan: A com-
parison of communication in a chimpanzee and two bonobos. First Language, 31, 300–325.
Lyn, H. (2007). Mental representation of symbols as revealed by vocabulary errors in two bonobos (Pan
paniscus). Animal Cognition, 10, 461–475.
Lyras, G. A., Dermitzakis, M. D., Van der Geer, A. A. E., Van der Geer, S. B., & De Vos, J. (2009). The
origin of Homo floresiensis and its relation to evolutionary processes under isolation. Anthropological Science,
117, 33–43.
Macchiarelli, R., Bergeret-Medina, A., Marchi, D., & Wood, B. (2020). Nature and relationships of
Sahelanthropus tchadensis. Journal of Human Evolution, 149, article 102898.
Machemer, H. (2001). The swimming cell and its world: Structures and mechanisms of orientation in
protists. European Journal of Protistology, 37, 3–14.
Macintyre, B. (2007, February 16). A large vocabulary isn’t just useful – it can be sexy too. The Times.
Retrieved February 15, 2019 from http://entertainment.timesonline.co.uk/tol/ arts_and_entertainment/
books/article2317122.ece.
MacLarnon, A., & Hewitt, G. P. (1999). The evolution of human speech: The role of enhanced breathing
control. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 109, 341–363.
MacLarnon, A., & Hewitt, G. P. (2004). Increased breathing control: Another factor in the evolution of
human language. Evolutionary Anthropology, 13, 181–197.
MacLatchy, L. (2004). The oldest ape. Evolutionary Anthropology, 13, 90–103.
Mafee, M. F., Pruzansky, S., Corrales, M. M., Phatak, M. G., Valvassori, G. E., Dobben, G. D., &
Capek, V. (1986). CT in the evaluation of the orbit and the bony interorbital distance. American Journal of
Neuroradiology, 7, 265–269.
Maisey, J. G. (1996). Discovering fossil fishes. New York, NY: Henry Holt and Co.
Malinzak, M. D., Kay, R. F., & Hullar, T. E. (2012). Locomotor head movements and semicircular canal
morphology in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 17914–17919.
Mallary, M. (2004). Our improbable universe. New York, NY: Thunder’s Mouth Press.
Mallatt, J., & Chen, J-Y. (2003). Fossil sister group of craniates: Predicted and found. Journal of Morphology,
258, 1–31.
Mandler, G., & Shebo, B. J. (1982). Subitizing: An analysis of its component processes. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General, 111, 1–22.
Manley, G. A. (2000). Cochlear mechanisms from a phylogenetic viewpoint. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 97, 11736–11743.
Manley, G. A., & Clack, J. A. (2004). An outline of the evolution of vertebrate hearing organs. In G. A.
Manley, A. N. Popper, & R. R. Fay (Eds.), Evolution of the vertebrate auditory system (pp. 1–26). New York,
NY: Springer-Verlag.
Mann, J., & Patterson, E. M. (2013). Tool use by aquatic animals. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
Society, B, 368, article 20120424.
Mansouri, F. A., Koechlin, E., Rosa, M. G. P., & Buckley, M. J. (2017). Managing competing goals – a key
role for the frontopolar cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18, 645–657.
344 References
Mansy, S. S., Schrum, J. P., Krishnamurthy, M., Tobé , S., Treco, D. A., & Szostak, J. W. (2008). Template-
directed synthesis of a genetic polymer in a model protocell. Nature, 454, 122–126.
Mantilla, G. P. W., Chester, S. G. B., Clemens, W. A., Moore, J. R., Sprain, C. J., Hovatter, B. T., …
Renne, P. R. (2021). Earliest Paleocene purgatoriids and the initial radiation of stem primates. Royal
Society Open Science, 8, article 210050.
Maor, R., Dayan, T., Ferguson-Gow, H., & Jones, K. E. (2017). Temporal niche expansion in mammals
from a nocturnal ancestor after dinosaur extinction. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 1, 1889–1895.
Marchant, L. F., & McGrew, W. C. (2007). Ant fishing by wild chimpanzees is not lateralised. Primates, 48,
22–26.
Marchi, D., Walker, C. S., Wei, P., Holliday, T. W., Churchill, S. E., Berger, L. R., & DeSilva, J. M.
(2017). The thigh and leg of Homo naledi. Journal of Human Evolution, 104, 174–204.
Marean, C. W., Bar-Matthews, M., Bernatchez, J., Fisher, E., Goldberg, P., Herries, A. I. R., … Williams,
H. M. (2007). Early human use of marine resources and pigment in South Africa during the Middle
Pleistocene. Nature, 449, 905–909.
Marguet, E., & Forterre, P. (1998). Protection of DNA by salts against thermodegration at temperatures
typical for hyperthermophiles. Extremophiles, 2, 115–122.
Marie, D., Roth, M., Lacoste, R., Nazarin, B., Bertello, A., Anton, J-L., … Meguerditchian, A. (2018). Left
brain asymmetry of the planum temporale in a nonhominid primate: Redefining the origin of brain
specialization for language. Cerebral Cortex, 28, 1808–1815.
Markey, M. J., & Marshall, C. R. (2007). Terrestrial-style feeding in a very early aquatic tetrapod is sup-
ported by evidence from experimental analysis of suture morphology. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 104, 7134–7138.
Marshall, J. W. B., Baker, H. F., & Ridley, R. M. (2002). Contralesional neglect in monkeys with small
unilateral parietal cortical ablations. Behavioural Brain Research, 136, 257–265.
Martin-Ordas, G., Haun, D., Colmenares, F., & Call, J. (2010). Keeping track of time: Evidence for
episodic-like memory in great apes. Animal Cognition, 13, 331–340.
Martin, R. D. (1981). Relative brain size and basal metabolic rate in terrestrial vertebrates. Nature, 293,
57–60.
Martin, R. D. (1992). Walking on two legs. In S. Jones, R. Martin, & D. Pilbeam (Eds.), The Cambridge
encyclopedia of human evolution(p. 78). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Martínez, I., Arsuaga, J. L., Quam, R., Carretero, J. M., Gracia, A., & Rodríguez, L. (2008). Human hyoid
bones from the middle Pleistocene site of the Sima de los Huesos (Sierra de Atapuerca, Spain). Journal of
Human Evolution, 54, 118–124.
Martínez, I., Rosa, M., Quam, R., Jarabo, P., Lorenzo, C., Bonmatí, A., … Arsuaga, J. L. (2013).
Communicative capacities in Middle Pleistocene humans from the Sierra de Atapuerca in Spain.
Quaternary International, 295, 94–101.
Maruyama, S., Kurokawa, K., Ebisuzaki, T., Sawaki, Y., Suda, K., & Santosh, M. (2019). Nine requirements
for the origin of Earth’s life: Not at the hydrothermal vent, but in a nuclear geyser system. Geoscience
Frontiers, 10, 1337–1357.
Marzke, M. W. (1997). Precision grips, hand morphology, and tools. American Journal of Physical
Anthropology, 102, 91–110.
Marzke, M. W., & Marzke, R. F. (2000). Evolution of the human hand: Approaches to acquiring, analysing
and interpreting the anatomical evidence. Journal of Anatomy, 197, 121–140.
Masao, F. T., Ichumbaki, E. B., Cherin, M., Barili, A., Boschian, G., Iurino, D. A., … Manzi, G. (2016).
New footprints from Laetoli (Tanzania) provide evidence for marked body size variation in early ho-
minins. eLife, 5, article 19568.
Masterton, B., Heffner, J., & Ravizza, R. (1969). The evolution of human hearing. The Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America, 45, 966–985.
Matsui, A., Go, Y., & Niimura, Y. (2010). Degeneration of olfactory receptor gene repertories in primates:
No direct link to full trichromatic vision. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 27, 1192–1200.
McBrearty, S., & Jablonski, N. G. (2005). First fossil chimpanzee. Nature, 437, 105–108.
McCarthy, G., Puce, A., Belger, A., & Allison, T. (1999). Electrophysiological studies of human face
perception. II: Response properties of face-specific potentials generated in occipitotemporal cortex.
Cerebral Cortex, 9, 431–444.
References 345
McCormick, C., Ciaramelli, E., De Luca, F., & Maguire, E. A. (2018). Comparing and contrasting the
cognitive effects of hippocampal and ventromedial prefrontal cortex damage: A review of human lesion
studies. Neuroscience, 374, 295–318.
McCourt, M. E., Garlinghouse, M., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2005). Unilateral visual cueing and asym-
metric line geometry share a common attentional origin in the modulation of pseudoneglect. Cortex, 41,
499–511.
McGrew, W. C. (1992). Chimpanzee material culture: Implications for human evolution. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
McHenry, H. M., & Coffing, K. (2000). Australopithecus to Homo: Transformations in body and mind.
Annual Review of Anthropology, 29, 125–146.
McKeever, W. F. (2000). A new family handedness sample with findings consistent with X-linked trans-
mission. British Journal of Psychology, 91, 21–39.
McMahon, R. (2004). Genes and language. Community Genetics, 7, 2–13.
McMahon, S. (2019). Earth’s earliest and deepest purported fossils may be iron-mineralized chemical gar-
dens. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B, 286, article 20192410.
McManus, I. C., & Bryden, M. P. (1992). The genetics of handedness, cerebral dominance and later-
alization. In I. Rapin & S. J. Segalowitz (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol. 6, pp. 115–144).
Amsterdam: Elsevier Academic Press.
McMenamin, M. A. S. (2019). Cambrian chordates and vetulicolians. Geosciences, 9, article 354.
McNulty, K. P., Begun, D. R., Kelley, J., Mnthi, F. K., & Mbua, E. N. (2015). A systematic revision of
Proconsul with the description of a new genus of early Miocene hominoid. Journal of Human Evolution, 84,
42–61.
McNutt, E. J., & DeSilva, J. M. (2020). Evidence for an elongated Achilles tendon in Australopithecus. The
Anatomical Record, 303, 2382–2391.
McPherron, S. P., Alemseged, Z., Marean, C. W., Wynn, J. G., Reed, D., Geraads, D., … Bé arat, H. A.
(2010). Evidence for stone-tool-assisted consumption of animal tissues before 3.39 million years ago at
Dikika, Ethiopia. Nature, 466, 857–860.
Medland, S. E., Duffy, D. L., Wright, M. J., Geffen, G. M., & Martin, N. G. (2006). Handedness in twins:
Joint analysis of data from 35 samples. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 46–53.
Meguerditchian, A., Phillips, K. A., Chapelain, A., Mahovetz, L. M., Milne, S., Stoinski, T., …
Hopkins, W. D. (2015). Handedness for unimanual grasping in 564 great apes: The effect on grip
morphology and a comparison with hand use for a bimanual coordinated task. Frontiers in Psychology,
6, article 1794.
Meisenzahl, E. M., Zetsche, T., Preuss, U., Frodl, T., Leinsinger, G., & Mö ller, H-J. (2002). Does the
definition of borders of the planum temporale influence the results in schizophrenia? American Journal of
Psychiatry, 159, 1198–1200.
Melamed, Y., Kislev, M. E., Geffen, E., Lev-Yadun, S., & Goren-Inbar, N. (2016). The plant component of
an Acheulian diet at Gesher Benot Ya’aqov, Israel. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113,
14674–14679.
Meldrum, J., & Sarmiento, E. (2018). Comments on possible Miocene hominin footprints. Proceedings of the
Geologists’ Association, 129, 577–580.
Melin, A. D., Chiou, K. L., Walco, E. R., Bergstrom, M. L., Kawamura, S., & Fedigan, L. M. (2017).
Trichromacy increases fruit intake rates of wild capuchins (Cebus capucinus imitator). Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences, 114, 10402–10407.
Melin, A. D., Khetpal, V., Matsushita, Y., Zhou, K., Campos, F. A., Welker, B., & Kawamura, S. (2017).
Howler monkey foraging ecology suggests convergent evolution of routine trichromacy as an adaptation
for folivory. Ecology and Evolution, 7, 1421–1434.
Mendez, F. L., Poznik, G. D., Castellano, S., & Bustamante, C. D. (2016). The divergence of neandertal and
modern human Y chromosomes. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 98, 728–734.
Mendez, F. L., Krahn, T., Schrack, B., Krahn, A.-M., Veeramah, K. R.,Woerner, A. E., … Hammer, M. F.
(2013). An African American paternal lineage adds an extremely ancient root to the human Y chro-
mosome phylogenetic tree. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 92, 454–459.
Menzel, C. R. (1999). Unprompted recall and reporting of hidden objects by a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes)
after extended delays. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113, 426–434.
346 References
Mercader, J., Panger, M., & Boesch, C. (2002). Excavation of a chimpanzee stone tool site in the African
rainforest. Science, 296, 1452–1455.
Merker, B. (2007). Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: A challenge for neuroscience and medicine.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 63–134.
Meunier, H., Fizet, J., & Vauclair, J. (2013). Tonkean macaques communicate with their right hand. Brain &
Language, 126, 181–187.
Meyer, K. W., Petersen, S. V., Lohmann, K. C., Blum, J. D., Washburn, S. J., Johnson, M. W., …
Winkelstern, I. Z. (2019). Biogenic carbonate mercury and marine temperature records reveal global
influence of Late Cretaceous Deccan Traps. Nature Communications, 10, article 5356.
Meyer, M., Arsuaga, J.-L., de Filippo, C., Nagel, S., Aximu-Petri, A., Nickel, B., … Pääbo, S. (2016).
Nuclear DNA sequences from the Middle Pleistocene Sima de los Huesos hominins. Nature, 531,
504–507.
Mgeladze, A., Lordkipanidze, D., Moncel, M-H., Despriee, J., Chagelishvili, R., Nioradze, M., &
Nioradze, G. (2011). Hominin occupations in the Dmanisi site, Georgia, Southern Causcasus: Raw
materials and technical behaviours of Europe’s first hominins. Journal of Human Evolution, 60, 571–596.
Miceli, G. (1999). Grammatical deficits in aphasia. In G. Denes & L. Pizzamiglio (Eds.), Handbook of clinical
and experimental neuropsychology (pp. 245–272). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Miles, H. L. W. (1990). The cognitive foundations for reference in a signing orangutan. In S. T. Parker & K.
R. Gibson (Eds.), “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes (pp. 511–539). Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Miljkovic-Licina, M., Gauchat, D., & Galliot, B. (2004). Neuronal evolution: Analysis of regulatory genes in
a first-evolved nervous system, the hydra nervous system. BioSystems, 76, 75–87.
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for
processing information. The Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.
Miller, G. A. (1962). Psychology: The science of mental life. New York, NY: Harper & Row.
Mincer, S. T., & Russo, G. A. (2020). Substrate use drives the macroevolution of mammalian tail length
diversity. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B, 287, article 20192885.
Miyata, M., Robinson, R. C., Uyeda, T. Q. P., Fukumori, Y., Fukushima, S., Haruta, S., … Wakabayashi,
K. (2020). Tree of motility – A proposed history of motility systems in the tree of life. Genes to Cells,
25, 6–21.
Moffat, S. D., Elkins, W., & Resnick, S. M. (2006). Age differences in the neural systems supporting human
allocentric spatial navigation. Neurobiology of Aging, 27, 965–972.
Molesti, S., Meguerditchian, A., & Bourjade, M. (2020). Gestural communication in olive baboons (Papio
anubis): Repertoire and intentionality. Animal Cognition, 23, 19–40.
Molnar, J. L., Hutchinson, J. R., Diogo, R., Clack, J. A., & Pierce, S. E. (2021). Evolution of forelimb
musculoskeletal function across the fish-to-tetrapod transition. Science Advances, 7, article eabd7457.
Mongle, C. S., Strait, D. S., & Grine, F. E. (2019). Expanded character sampling underscores phylogenetic
stability of Ardipithecus ramidus as a basal hominin. Journal of Human Evolution, 131, 28–39.
Montgomery, S. H. (2013). Primate brains, the ‘island rule’ and the evolution of Homo floresiensis. Journal of
Human Evolution, 65, 750–760.
Monti, M. M., & Osherson, D. N. (2012). Logic, language and the brain. Brain Research, 1428, 33–42.
Moore, B. R. (2004). The evolution of learning. Biological Reviews, 79, 301–335.
Moore, M. W. (2020). Hominin stone flaking and the emergence of ’top-down’ design in human evolution.
Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 30, 647–664.
Moore, T., Rodman, H. R., Repp, A. B., & Gross, C. G. (1995). Localization of visual stimuli after striate
cortex damage in monkeys: Parallels with human blindsight. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
92, 8215–8218.
Morelle, R. (2006). Arctic fossils mark move to land. Retrieved February 15, 2019 from http://news.bbc.
co.uk/2/hi/science/nature/4879672.stm.
Morino, L. (2011). Left-hand preference for a complex manual task in a population of wild siamangs
(Symphalangus syndactylus). International Journal of Primatology, 32, 793–800.
Morino, L., Uchikoshi, M., Bercovitch, F., & Hopkins, W. D. (2017). Tube task hand preference in captive
hylobatids. Primates, 58, 403–412.
References 347
Moriya-Ito, K., Hayakawa, T., Suzuki, H., Hagino-Yamagishi, K., & Nikaido, M. (2018). Evolution of
vomeronasal receptor 1 (V1R) genes in the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). Gene, 642, 343–353.
Morris, S. C., & Caron, J. B. (2014). A primitive fish from the Cambrian of North America. Nature, 512,
419–422.
Morrison, R., & Reiss, D. (2018). Precocious development of self-awareness in dolphins. PLoS One, 13,
article e0189813.
Mort, D. J., Malhotra, P., Mannan, S. K., Rorden, C., Pambakian, A., Kennard, C., & Husain, M. (2003).
The anatomy of visual neglect. Brain, 126, 1986–1997.
Morwood, M. J., & Jungers, W. L. (2009). Conclusions: Implications of the Liang Bua excavations for
hominin evolution and biogeography. Journal of Human Evolution, 57, 640–648.
Morwood, M. J., Brown, P., Jamitko, Sutikna, T., Saptomo, E. W., Westaway, K. E., … Djubian-Tono, T.
(2005). Further evidence for small-bodied hominins from the Late Pleistocene of Flores, Indonesia.
Nature, 437, 1012–1017.
Moulton, V., Gardner, P. P., Pointon, R. F., Creamer, L. K., Jameson, G. B., & Pennyl, D. (2000). RNA
folding argues against a hot-start origin of life. Journal of Molecular Evolution, 51, 416–421.
Mü hlau, M., Hermsdö rfer, J., Goldenberg, G., Wohlschlä ger, A. M., Castrop, F., Stahl, R., … Boecker, H.
(2005). Left inferior parietal dominance in gesture imitation: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 43,
1086–1098.
Mukhopadhyay, R. (2014). Quantum mechanics, objective reality, and the problem of consciousness. Journal
of Consciousness Studies, 21, 57–80.
Murofushi, K. (1997). Numerical matching behavior by a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): Subitizing and
analogue magnitude estimation. Japanese Psychological Research, 39, 140–153.
Murray, E. A., Wise, S. P., & Graham, K. S. (2018). Representational specializations of the hippocampus in
phylogenetic perspective. Neuroscience Letters, 680, 4–12.
Musgrave, S., Lonsdorf, E., Morgan, D., Prestipino, M., Bernstein-Kurtycz, L., Mundry, R., & Sanz, C.
(2020). Teaching varies with task complexity in wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 117, 969–976.
Musgrave, S., Morgan, D., Lonsdorf, E., Mundry, R., & Sanz, C. (2016). Tool transfers are a form of
teaching among chimpanzees. Scientific Reports, 6, article 34783.
Nakamura, R. K., & Mishkin, M. (1980). Blindness in monkeys following non-visual cortical lesions. Brain
Research, 188, 572–577.
Naqshbandi, M., Feeney, M. C., McKenzie, T. L. B., & Roberts, W. A. (2007). Testing for episodic-like
memory in rats in the absence of time of day cues: Replication of Babb and Crystal. Behavioural Processes,
74, 217–225.
Nasir, A., Kim, K. M., & Caetano-Anollés, G. (2017). Phylogenetic tracings of proteome size support the
gradual accretion of protein structural domains and the early origin of viruses from primordial cells.
Frontiers in Microbiology, 8, article 1178.
Nater, A., Mattle-Greminger, M. P., Nurcahyo, A., Nowak, M. G., de Manuel, M., Desai, T., … Krützen,
M. (2017). Morphometric, behavioral, and genomic evidence for a new orangutan species. Current
Biology, 27, 3487–3498.
Nation, P. (1993). Using dictionaries to estimate vocabulary size: Essential, but rarely followed, procedures.
Language Today, 10, 27–40.
Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive
Psychology, 9, 353–383.
Neaux, D., Bienvenu, T., Guy, F., Daver, G., Sansalone, G., Ledogar, J. A., … Brunet, M. (2017).
Relationship between foramen magnum position and locomotion in extant and extinct hominoids. Journal
of Human Evolution, 113, 1–9.
Nee, D. E., & Brown, J. W. (2013). Dissociable frontal-striatal and frontal-parietal networks involved in
updating hierarchical contexts in working memory. Cerebral Cortex, 23, 2146–2158.
Neggers, S. F. W., Van der Lubbe, R. H. J., Ramsey, N. F., & Postma, A. (2006). Interactions between ego-
and allocentric neuronal representations of space. NeuroImage, 31, 320–331.
Neiworth, J. J., Whillock, K. M., Kim, S. H., Greenberg, J. R., Jones, K. B., Patel, A. R., … Kudura, A. G.
(2014). Gestalt principle use in college students, children with autism, toddlers (Homo sapiens), and cotton
top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 128, 188–198.
348 References
Nelson, E. L., O’Karma, J. M., Ruperti, F. S., & Novak, M. A. (2009). Laterality in semi-free-ranging black
and white ruffed lemurs (Varecia variegata variegata): Head-tilt correlates with hand use during feeding.
American Journal of Primatology, 71, 1032–1040.
Nelson, T. O. (1996). Consciousness and metacognition. American Psychologist, 51, 102–116.
Nettle, D. (1999). Towards a future history of macrofamily research. In C. Renfrew & D. Nettle (Eds.),
Nostratic: Examining a linguistic macrofamily (pp. 403–419). Cambridge, UK: The McDonald Institute for
Archaeological Research.
Neufuss, J., Humle, T., Cremaschi, A., & Kivell, T. L. (2017). Nut-cracking behaviour in wild-born,
rehabilitated bonobos (Pan paniscus): A comprehensive study of hand-preference, hand grips and effi-
ciency. American Journal of Primatology, 79, article e22589.
Nevo, O., Valenta, K., Razafimandimby, D., Melin, A. D., Ayasse, M., & Chapman, C. A. (2018).
Frugivores and the evolution of fruit color. Biology Letters, 14, article 20180377.
Ni, X., Wang, Y., Hu, Y., & Li, C. (2004). A euprimate skull from the early Eocene of China. Nature, 427,
65–68.
Nieder, A., Freedman, D. J., & Miller, E. K. (2002). Representation of the quantity of visual items in the
primate prefrontal cortex. Science, 297, 1708–1711.
Nielsen, C. (2019). Early animal evolution: A morphologist’s view. Royal Society Open Science, 6, article
190638.
Nielsen, M., Langley, M. C., Shipton, C., & Kapitány, R. (2020). Homo neanderthalensis and the evo-
lutionary origins of ritual in Homo sapiens. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 375, article
20190424.
Nieuwenhuys, R. (2002). Deuterostome brains: Synopsis and commentary. Brain Research Bulletin, 57,
257–270.
Niimura, Y. (2012). Olfactory receptor multigene family in vertebrates: From the viewpoint of evolutionary
genomics. Current Genomics, 13, 103–114.
Nilsson, D-E. (2013). Eye evolution and its functional basis. Visual Neuroscience, 30, 5–20.
Nishimura, T. (2018). The descended larynx and the descending larynx. Anthropological Science, 126, 3–8.
Nishimura, T., Mikami, A., Suzuki, J., & Matsuzawa, T. (2003). Descent of the larynx in chimpanzee
infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 6930–6933.
Noonan, J. P., Coop, G., Kudaravalli, S., Smith, D., Krause, J., Alessi, J., … Rubin, E. M. (2006).
Sequencing and analysis of Neanderthal genomic DNA. Science, 314, 1113–1118.
Norlén, E., Sjöström, D., Hjelm, M., Hård, T., & Laska, M. (2019). Taste responsiveness of Western
chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) to five food-associated saccharides. Primates, 60, 29–39.
Norman, E. (2017). Metacognition and mindfulness: The role of fringe consciousness. Mindfulness, 8,
95–100.
Normand, E., & Boesch, C. (2009). Sophisticated Euclidean maps in forest chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour,
77, 1195–1201.
Norton, O. R. (1998). Rocks from space (2nd ed.). Missoula, MT: Mountain Press Publishing Company.
Novacek, M. J. (1999). 100 million years of land vertebrate evolution: The cretaceous-early tertiary tran-
sition. Annals of the Missouri Botanical Garden, 86, 230–258.
Nunez, V., Shapley, R. M., & Gordon, J. (2018). Cortical double-opponent cells in color perception:
Perceptual scaling and chromatic visual evoked potentials. i-Perception, Jan-Feb 2018, 9, 1–16.
Nyakatura, J. A., Allen, V. R., Lauströ er, J., Andikfar, A., Danczak, M., Ullrich, H-J., … Fischer, M. S.
(2015). A three-dimensional skeletal reconstruction of the stem amniote Orobates pabsti (Diadectidae):
Analyses of body mass, centre of mass position, and joint mobility. PLoS One, 10, article e0137284.
Oftedal, O. T. (2002). The mammary gland and its origin during synapsid evolution. Journal of Mammary
Gland Biology and Neoplasia, 7, 225–252.
Ogawa, T., Shoji, E., Suematsu, N. J., Nishimori, H., Izumi, S., Awazu, A., & Iima, M. (2016). The flux of
Euglena gracilis cells depends on the gradient of light intensity. PLoS One, 11, article e0168114.
Ö ğ men, H., Breitmeyer, B. G., Todd, S., & Mardon, L. (2006). Target recovery in metacontrast: The effect
of contrast. Vision Research, 46, 4726–4734.
Oh, Y-M., & Koh, E-J. (2009). Language lateralization in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy: A com-
parison between volumetric analysis and the Wada test. Journal of the Korean Neurosurgical Society, 45,
329–335.
References 349
Olkowicz, S., Kocourek, M., Lucan, R. K., Portes, M., Fitch, W. T., Herculano-Houzel, S., & Nemec, P.
(2016). Birds have primate-like numbers of neurons in the forebrain. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 113, 7255–7260.
Olson, I. R., Gatenby, J. C., Leung, H-C., Skudlarski, P., & Gore, J. C. (2003). Neuronal representation of
occluded objects in the human brain. Neuropsychologia, 42, 95–104.
Orban, G. A., Claeys, K., Nelissen, K., Smans, R., Sunaert, S., Todd, J. T., … Vanduffel, W. (2006).
Mapping the parietal cortex of human and non-human primates. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2647–2667.
Orban, G. A., Van Essen, D., & Vanduffel, W. (2004). Comparative mapping of higher visual areas in
monkeys and humans. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 315–324.
Organ, C. L., Schweitzer, M. H., Zheng, W., Freimark, L. M., Cantley, L. C., & Asara, J. M. (2008).
Molecular phylogenetics of mastodon and Tyrannosaurus rex. Science, 320, 499.
Orlando, L., Darlu, P., Toussaint, M., Bonjean, D., Otte, M., & Hanni, C. (2006). Revisiting Neandertal
diversity with a 100,000 year old mtDNA sequence. Current Biology, 16, R400–R401.
Ornstein, R. E. (1972). The psychology of consciousness. New York, NY: Viking Press.
Orwant, R. (2005). First convincing chimp fossil discovered. New Scientist, 187, 7.
Osinski, G. R., Cockell, C. S., Pontefract, A., & Sapers, H. M. (2020). The role of meteorite impacts in the
origin of life. Astrobiology, 20, 1121–1149.
Pablos, A., Gómez-Olivencia, A., Maureille, B., Holliday, T. W., Madelaine, S., Trinkaus, E., & Couture-
Veschambre, C. (2019). Neandertal foot remains from Regourdou 1 (Montignac-sur-Vézere, Dordogne,
France). Journal of Human Evolution, 128, 17–44.
Pal, A., Kumara, H. N., Mishra, P. S., Velankar, A. D., & Singh, M. (2018). Extractive foraging and tool-
aided behaviors in the wild Nicobar long-tailed macaque (Macaca fascicularis umbrosus). Primates, 59,
173–183.
Pampush, J. D., Scott, J. E., Robinson, C. A., & Delezene, L. K. (2018). Oblique human symphyseal angle is
associated with an evolutionary rate-shift early in the hominin clade. Journal of Human Evolution, 123,
84–95.
Panciroli, E., Schultz, J. A., & Luo, Z.-X. (2019). Morphology of the petrosal and stapes of Borealestes
(mammaliaformes, Docodonta) from the Middle Jurassic of Skye, Scotland. Papers in Palaeontology, 5,
139–156.
Pante, M., de la Torre, I., d’Errico, F., Njau, J., & Blumenschine, R. (2020). Bone tools from Beds II-IV,
Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania, and implications for the origins and evolution of bone technology. Journal of
Human Evolution, 148, article 102885.
Papademetriou, E., Sheu, C-F., & Michel, G. F. (2005). A meta-analysis of primate hand preferences,
particularly for reaching. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119, 33–48.
Papaleontiou-Louca, E. (2008). Metacognition and theory of mind. Newcastle, UK: Cambridge Scholars
Publishing.
Parker, E. T., Cleaves, H. J., Dworkin, J. P., Glavin, D. P., Callahan, M., Aubrey, A., … Bada, J. L. (2011).
Primordial synthesis of amines and amino acids in a 1958 Miller H2S-rich spark discharge experiment.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 5526–5531.
Parker, S. T. (2004). The cognitive complexity of social organization and socialization in wild baboons and
chimpanzees: Guided exploration, socializing interactions, and event representation. In A. E. Russon &
D. R. Begun (Eds.), The evolution of thought: Evolutionary origins of great ape intelligence (pp. 45–60).
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Parker, S. T., Kerr, M., Markowitz, H., & Gould, J. (1999). A survey of tool use in zoo gorillas. In S. T.
Parker, R. W. Mitchell, & H. L. Miles (Eds.), The mentalities of gorillas and orangutans (pp. 188–193).
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Parr, L. A. (2011). The evolution of face processing in primates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society,
B, 366, 1764–1777.
Pascual-Anaya, J., D’Aniello, S., Kuratani, S., & Garcia-Fernà ndez, J. (2013). Evolution of Hox gene clusters
in deuterostomes. BMC Developmental Biology, 13, 26.
Pascual-Leone, A., & Walsh, V. (2001). Fast backprojections from the motion to the primary visual area
necessary for visual awareness. Science, 292, 510–512.
Pasupathy, A., & Connor, C. E. (2002). Population coding of shape in area V4. Nature Neuroscience, 5,
1332–1338.
350 References
Pasupathy, A., Kim, T., & Popovkina, D. V. (2019). Object shape and surface properties are jointly encoded
in mid-level ventral visual cortex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 58, 199–208.
Patel, B. H., Percivalle, C., Ritson, D. J., Duffy, C. D., & Sutherland, J. D. (2015). Common
origins of RNA, protein and lipid precursors in a cyanosulfidic protometabolism. Nature Chemistry, 7,
301–307.
Paterson, J. R., García-Bellido, D. C., Lee, M. S. Y., Brock, G. A., Jago, J. B., & Edgecombe, G. D. (2011).
Acute vision in the giant Cambrian predator Anomalocaris and the origin of compound eyes. Nature, 480,
237–240.
Patrick, S. K., Noah, J. A., & Yang, J. F. (2009). Interlimb coordination in human crawling reveals simi-
larities in development and neural control with quadrupeds. Journal of Neurophysiology 101, 603–613.
Paulson, A. (2007). In Iowa, English 101 for bonobos. Christian Science Monitor. Retrieved February 16,
2008, from www.usatoday.com/tech/science/discoveries/2007-05-10-bonobo-studies_N.htm.
Pavlicev, M., Romero, R., & Mitteroecker, P. (2020). Evolution of the human pelvis and obstructed
labor: New explanations of an old obstetrical dilemma. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology,
222, 3–16.
Payne, B. R., Lomber, S. G., MacNeil, M. A., & Cornwell, P. (1996). Evidence for greater sight in
blindsight following damage of primary visual cortex early in life. Neuropsychologia, 34, 741–774.
Pearce, B. K. D., & Pudritz, R. E. (2015). Seeding the pregenetic earth: Meteoric abundances of nucleobases
and potential reaction pathways. The Astrophysical Journal, 807, article 85.
Pearce, E., & Dunbar, R. (2012). Latitudinal variation in light levels drives human visual system size.
Biological Letters, 8, 90–93.
Peeters, R., Simone, L., Nelissen, K., Fabbri-Destro, M., Vanduffel, W., Rizzolatti, G., & Orban, G. A.
(2009). The representation of tool use in human and monkeys: Common and uniquely human features.
The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 11523–11539.
Peirce, J. W., Leigh, A. E., & Kendrick, K. M. (2000). Configurational coding, familiarity and the right
hemisphere advantage for face recognition in sheep. Neuropsychologia, 38, 475–483.
Peirce, J. W., Leigh, A. E., daCosta, A. P. C., & Kendrick, K. M. (2001). Human face recognition in sheep:
Lack of configurational coding and right hemisphere advantage. Behavioural Processes, 55, 13–26.
Peng, K., Steele, S. C., Becerra, L., & Borsook, D. (2018). Brodmann area 10: Collating, integrating and
high level processing of nociception and pain. Progress in Neurobiology, 161, 1–22.
Pereira, A. S., Kavanagh, E., Hobaiter, C., Slocombe, K. E., & Lameira, A. R. (2020). Chimpanzee lip-
smacks confirm primate continuity for speech-rhythm evolution. Biology Letters, 16, article 20200232.
Perelman, P., Johnson, W. E., Roos, C., Seuá nez, H. N., Horvath, J. E., Moreira, M. A. M., … Pecon-
Slattery, J. (2011). A molecular phylogeny of living primates. PLoS Genetics, 7, e1001342.
Perry, S. E., Barrett, B. J., & Godoy, I. (2017). Older, sociable capuchins (Cebus capucinus) invent more social
behaviors, but younger monkeys innovate more in other contexts. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 114, 7806–7813.
Persons, W. S., & Currie, P. J. (2017). The functional origin of dinosaur bipedalism: Cumulative evidence
from bipedally inclined reptiles and disinclined mammals. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 420, 1–7.
Pervaiz, N., Shakeel, N., Qasim, A., Zehra, R., Anwar, S., Rana, N., … Abbasi, A. A. (2019). Evolutionary
history of the human multigene families reveals widespread gene duplications throughout the history of
animals. BMC Evolutionary Biology, 19, article 128.
Peterson, K. J., & Butterfield, N. J. (2005). Origin of the eumetazoa: Testing ecological predictions of
molecular clocks against the Proterozoic fossil record. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102,
9547–9552.
Peterson, K. J., Cotton, J. A., Gehling, J. G., & Pisani, D. (2008). The Ediacaran emergence of bilaterians:
Congruence between the genetic and the geological fossil records. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
Society, B., 363, 1435–1443.
Petit, L., & Pouget, P. (2019). The comparative anatomy of frontal eye fields in primates. Cortex, 118,
51–64.
Petr, M., Hajdinjak, M., Fu, Q., Essel, E., Rougier, H., Crevecoeur, I., … Kelso, J. (2020). The evolu-
tionary history of Neanderthal and Denisovan Y chromosomes. Science, 369, 1653–1656.
Petrides, M. (2005). Lateral prefrontal cortex: Architectonic and functional organization. Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B, 360, 781–795.
References 351
Petrides, M., & Pandya, D. N. (1999). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: Comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis
in the human and the macaque brain and corticocortical connections patterns. European Journal of
Neuroscience, 11, 1011–1036.
Petrides, M., & Pandya, D. N. (2009). Distinct parietal and temporal pathways to the homologues of Broca’s
area in the monkey. PLoS Biology, 7, e1000170.
Petryshyn, V. A., Bottjer, D. J., Chen, J-Y., & Gao, F. (2013). Petrographic analysis of new specimens of the
putative microfossil Vernanimalcula guizhouena (Doushantuo Formation, South China). Precambrian
Research, 225, 58–66.
Peuskens, H., Sunaert, S., Dupont, P., Van Hecke, P., & Orban, G. A. (2001). Human brain regions
involved in heading estimation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 2451–2461.
Phillipson, L. (1997). Edge modification as an indicator of function and handedness of Acheulian handaxes
from Kariandusi, Kenya. Lithic Technology, 22, 171–183.
Picciani, N., Kerlin, J. R., Sierra, N., Swafford, A. J. M., Ramirez, M. D., Roberts, N. G., … Oakley, T. H.
(2018). Prolific origination of eyes in Cnidaria with co-option of non-visual opsins. Current Biology, 28,
2413–2419.
Pichaud, F., & Desplan, C. (2002). Pax genes and eye organogenesis. Current Opinion in Genetics &
Development, 12, 430–434.
Pierrot-Deseilligny, C., Ploner, C. J., Mü ri, R. M., Gaymard, B., & Rivaud-Pé choux, S. (2002). Effects of
cortical lesions on saccadic eye movements in humans. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 956,
216–229.
Pilbeam, D. (1972). The ascent of man: An introduction to human evolution. New York, NY: Palgrave
Macmillan.
Piñ eiro, G., Ferigolo, J., Meneghel, M., & Laurin, M. (2012). The oldest known amniotic embryos suggest
viviparity in mesosaurs. Historical Biology, 24, 620–630.
Pisani, D., Mohun, S. M., Harris, S. R., McInerney, J. O., & Wilkinson, M. (2006). Molecular evidence for
dim-light vision in the last common ancestor of the vertebrates. Current Biology, 16, R318–R319.
Plachetzki, D. C., Degnan, B. M., & Oakley, T. H. (2007). The origins of novel protein interactions during
opsin evolution. PLoS One, 2, e1054.
Plotnik, J. M., de Waal, F. B. M., & Reiss, D. (2006). Self-recognition in an Asian elephant. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences, 103, 17053–17057.
Pobiner, B. L. (1999). The use of stone tools to determine handedness in hominids. Current Anthropology, 40,
90–92.
Pollick, A. S., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2007). Ape gestures and language evolution. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 104, 8184–8189.
Pombal, M. A., Álvarez-Otero, R., Pérez-Fernández, J., Solveira, C., & Megías, M. (2011). Development
and organization of the lamprey telencephalon with special reference to the GABAergic system. Frontiers
in Neuroanatomy, 5, article 20.
Ponce, C. R., Xiao, W., Schade, P. F., Hartmann, T. S., Kreiman, G., & Livingstone, M. S. (2019).
Evolving images for visual neurons using a deep generative network reveals coding principles and neu-
ronal preferences. Cell, 177, 999–1009.
Ponce de León, M. S., Bienvenu, T., Marom, A., Engel, S., Tafforeau, P., Warren, J. L. A., … Zollikofer,
C. P. E. (2021). The primitive brain of early Homo. Science, 372, 165–171.
Porter, M. L., & Crandall, K. A. (2003). Lost along the way: The significance of evolution in reverse. Trends
in Ecology and Evolution, 18, 541–547.
Posada, S., & Colell, M. (2007). Another gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) recognizes himself in a mirror. American
Journal of Primatology, 69, 576–583.
Posner, M. I. (1978). Chronometric explorations of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Posner, M. I. (1992). Attention as a cognitive and neural system. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1,
11–14.
Posner, M. I., Walker, J. A., Friedrich, F. J., & Rafal, R. D. (1984). Effects of parietal injury on covert
orienting of attention. The Journal of Neuroscience, 4, 1863–1874.
Poss, S. R., Kuhar, C., Stoinski, T. S., & Hopkins, W. D. (2006). Differential use of attentional and visual
communicative signaling by orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) and gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) in response to the
attentional status of a human. American Journal of Primatology, 68, 978–992.
352 References
Potts, R. (1992). The hominid way of life. In S. Jones, R. Martin, & D. Pilbeam (Eds.), The Cambridge
encyclopedia of human evolution (pp. 325–334). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Pouydebat, E., Coppens, Y., & Gorce, P. (2006). É volution de la pré hension chez les primates humains et
non humains: la pré cision et l’utilisation d’outils revisité es. L’anthropologie, 110, 687–697.
Povinelli, D. J. (1989). Failure to find self-recognition in Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) in contrast to
their use of mirror cues to discover hidden food. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 103, 122–131.
Povinelli, D. J., & Bering, J. M. (2003). The mentality of apes revisited. Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 11, 115–119.
Povinelli, D. J., Reaux, J. E., Theall, L. A., & Giambrone, S. (2000). Folk physics for apes. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Povinelli, D. J., Rulf, A. B., Landau, K. R., & Bierschwale, D. T. (1993). Self-recognition in chimpanzees
(Pan troglodytes): Distribution, ontogeny, and patterns of emergence. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 107,
347–372.
Poznik, G. D., Henn, B. M., Yee, M.-C., Sliwerska, E., Euskirchen, G. M., Lin, A. A., … Bus-Tamante,
C. D. (2013). Sequencing Y chromosomes resolves discrepancy in time to common ancestor of males
versus females. Science, 341, 562–565.
Pozzi, L., Hodgson, J. A., Burrell, A. S., Sterner, K. N., Rauum, R. L., & Distotell, T. R. (2014). Primate
phylogenetic relationships and divergence dates inferred from complete mitochondrial genomes. Molecular
Phylogenetics and Evolution, 75, 165–183.
Prang, T. C. (2019). The African ape-like foot of Ardipithecus ramidus and its implications for the origin of
bipedalism. eLife, 8, article e44433.
Prang, T. C., Ramirez, K., Grabowski, M., & Williams, S. A. (2021). Ardipithecus hand provides evidence
that humans and chimpanzees evolved from an ancestor with suspensory adaptations. Science Advances, 7,
article eabf 2474.
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? The Behavioral and Brain
Sciences, 4, 515–526.
Price, M. (2020). Africans, too, carry Neanderthal genetic legacy. Science, 367, 497.
Prieur, J., Barbu, S., & Blois-Heulin, C. (2018). Human laterality for manipulation and gestural commu-
nication related to 60 everyday activities: Impact of multiple individual-related factors. Cortex, 99,
118–134.
Prieur, J., Pika, S., Barbu, S., & Blois-Heulin, C. (2016a). A multifactorial investigation of captive chim-
panzees’ intraspecific gestural laterality. Animal Behaviour, 116, 31–43.
Prieur, J., Pika, S., Barbu, S., & Blois-Heulin, C. (2016b). Gorillas are right-handed for their most frequent
intraspecific gestures. Animal Behaviour, 118, 165–170.
Prinzmetal, W. (1981). Principles of feature integration in visual perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 30,
330–340.
Prior, H., Schwarz, A., & Gü ntü rkü n, O. (2008). Mirror-induced behavior in the magpie (Pica pica):
Evidence of self-recognition. PLoS Biology, 6, 1642–1650.
Proffitt, T., Luncz, V. L., Malaivijitnond, S., Gumert, M., Svensson, M. S., & Haslam, M. (2018). Analysis
of wild macaque stone tools used to crack oil palm nuts. Royal Society Open Science, 5, article 171904.
Proops, L., Rayner, J., Taylor, A. M., & McComb, K. (2013). The responses of young domestic horses to
human-given cues. PLoS One, 8, article e67000.
Prothero, D. R. (2007). Evolution: What the fossils say and why it matters. New York, NY: Columbia
University Press.
Pruetz, J. D., Bertolani, P., Ontl, K. B., Lindshield, S., Shelley, M., & Wessling, E. G. (2015). New
evidence on the tool-assisted hunting exhibited by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) in a savannah habitat
at Fongoli, Sé né gal. Royal Society Open Science, 2, article 140507.
Prü fer, K., Munch, K., Hellmann, I., Akagi, K., Miller, J. R., Walenz, B., …. Pä ä bo, S. (2012). The bonobo
genome compared with the chimpanzee and human genomes. Nature, 486, 527–531.
Prüfer, K., Racimo, F., Patterson, N., Jay, F., Sankararaman, S., Sawyer, S., … Pääbo, S. (2014). The
complete genome sequence of a Neanderthal from the Altai Mountains. Nature, 505, 43–49.
Ptak, R., & Schnider, A. (2011). The attention network of the human brain: Relating structural damage
associated with spatial neglect to functional imaging correlates of spatial attention. Neuropsychologia, 49,
3063–3070.
References 353
Pugh, K. R., Mencl, W. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Fulbright, R. K., Constable, R. T., … Gore, J.
C. (2000). The angular gyrus in developmental dyslexia: Task-specific differences in functional con-
nectivity within posterior cortex. Psychological Science, 11, 51–56.
Quraishi, S., Heider, B., & Siegel, R. M. (2007). Attentional modulation of receptive field structure in area
7a of the behaving monkey. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 1841–1857.
Raaum, R. L., Sterner, K. N., Noviello, C. M., Stewart, C-B., & Disotell, T. R. (2005). Catarrhine primate
divergence dates estimated from complete mitochondrial genomes: Concordance with fossil and nuclear
DNA evidence. Journal of Human Evolution, 48, 237–257.
Rafal, R. D. (2006). Oculomotor functions of the parietal lobe: Effects of chronic lesions in humans. Cortex,
42, 730–739.
Rajala, A. Z., Reininger, K. R., Lancaster, K. M., & Populin, L. C. (2010). Rhesus monkeys (Macaca
mulatta) do recognize themselves in the mirror: Implications for the evolution of self-recognition. PLoS
One, 5, e12865.
Ramsier, M. A., Cunningham, A. J., Finneran, J. J., & Dominy, N. J. (2012). Social drive and evolution of
primate hearing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 367, 1860–1868.
Ramsier, M. A., Vinyard, C. J., & Dominy, N. J. (2017). Auditory sensitivity of the tufted capuchin (Sapajus
apella), a test of allometric predictions. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 141, 4822–4831.
Rantala, M. J. (2007). Evolution of nakedness in Homo sapiens. Journal of Zoology, 273, 1–7.
Rasmussen, D. T. (2002a). Early catarrhines of the African Eocene and Oligocene. In W. C. Hartwig (Ed.),
The primate fossil record (pp. 203–220). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Rasmussen, D. T. (2002b). The origin of primates. In W. C. Hartwig (Ed.), The primate fossil record (pp. 5–9).
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Raup, D. M. (1991). Extinction: Bad genes or bad luck? New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company.
Rauschecker, J. P. (2018). Where did language come from? Precursor mechanisms in nonhuman primates.
Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 21, 195–204.
Raymann, K., Brochier-Armanet, C., & Gribaldo, S. (2015). The two-domain tree of life is linked to a new
root for the Archaea. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112, 6670–6675.
Raymond, M., & Pontier, D. (2004). Is there geographic variation in human handedness? Laterality, 9,
35–51.
Raymond, M., Pontier, D., Dufour, A-B., & Møller, A. P. (1996). Frequency-dependent maintenance of
left handedness in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B, 263, 1627–1633.
Reader, J. (1981). Missing links: The hunt for earliest man. Boston: Little, Brown and Company.
Reddy, P. C., Unni, M. K., Gungi, A., Agarwal, P., & Galande, S. (2015). Evolution of Hox-like genes in
Cnidaria: Study of Hydra Hox repertoire reveals tailor-made Hox-code for Cnidarians. Mechanisms of
Development, 138, 87–96.
Rees, G., & Frith, C. (2007). Methodologies for identifying the neural correlates of consciousness. In M.
Velmans & S. Schneider (Eds.), The Blackwell companion to consciousness (pp. 553–566). Malden, MA:
Blackwell Publishing.
Regaiolli, B., Spiezio, C., & Hopkins, W. D. (2016). Hand preference on unimanual and bimanual tasks in
strepsirrhines: The case of the ring-tailed lemur (Lemur catta). American Journal of Primatology, 78, 851–860.
Rein, T. R. (2019). A geometric morphometric examination of hominoid third metacarpal shape and its
implications for inferring the precursor to terrestrial bipedalism. The Anatomical Record, 302, 983–998.
Reiss, D., & Marino, L. (2001). Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: A case of cognitive
convergence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 5937–5942.
Renner, S. S. (2005). Relaxed molecular clocks for dating historical plant dispersal events. Trends in Plant
Science, 10, 550–558.
Rhodes, G., Byatt, G., Michie, P. T., & Puce, A. (2004). Is the fusiform face area specialized for faces,
individuation, or expert individuation? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 189–203.
Richardson, T., & Gilman, R. T. (2019). Left-handedness is associated with greater fighting success in
humans. Scientific Reports, 9, Article 15402.
Richmond, B. G., & Jungers, W. L. (2008). Orrorin tugenensis femoral morphology and the evolution of
hominin bipedalism. Science, 319, 1662–1665.
Richmond, B. G., Aiello, L. C., & Wood, B. A. (2002). Early hominin limb proportions. Journal of Human
Evolution, 43, 529–548.
354 References
Rightmire, G. P. (2004). Brain size and encephalization in early to mid-Pleistocene Homo. American Journal of
Physical Anthropology, 124, 109–123.
Rightmire, G. P., de León, M. S. P., Lordkipanidze, D., Margvelashvili, A., & Zollikofer, C. P. E. (2017).
Skull 5 from Dmanisi: Descriptive anatomy, comparative studies, and evolutionary significance. Journal of
Human Evolution, 104, 50–79.
Rilling, J. K. (2006). Human and nonhuman primate brains: Are they allometrically scaled versions of the
same design? Evolutionary Anthropology, 15, 65–77.
Rilling, J. K. (2014). Comparative primate neurobiology and the evolution of the brain language systems.
Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 28, 10–14.
Rilling, J. K., Glasser, M. F., Preuss, T. M., Ma, X., Zhao, T., Hu, X., & Behrens, T. E. J. (2008). The
evolution of the arcuate fasciculus revealed with comparative DTI. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 426–428.
Ritson, D. J., Mojzsis, S. J., & Sutherland, J. D. (2020). Supply of phosphate to early Earth by photo-
geochemistry after meteroritic weathering. Nature Geoscience, 13, 344–348.
Rivas, E. (2005). Recent use of signs by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in interactions with humans. Journal of
Comparative Psychology, 119, 404–417.
Rivera, A. S., Ozturk, N., Fahey, B., Plachetzki, D. C., Degnan, B. M., Sancar, A., & Oakley, T. H. (2012).
Blue-light-receptive cryptochrome is expressed in a sponge eye lacking neurons and opsin. The Journal of
Experimental Biology, 215, 1278–1286.
Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2002). Motor and cognitive functions of the ventral premotor
cortex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 12, 149–154.
Robbins, M. M., Ando, C., Fawcett, K. A., Grueter, C. C., Hedwig, D., Iwata, Y., … Yamagiwa, J. (2016).
Behavioral variation in gorillas: Evidence of potential cultural traits. PLoS ONE, 11, article e0160483.
Roberts, A. I., Roberts, S. G. B., & Vick, S-J. (2014). The repertoire and intentionality of gestural com-
munication in wild chimpanzees. Animal Cognition, 17, 317–336.
Robertson, M. P., & Joyce, G. F. (2014). Highly efficient self-replicating RNA enzymes. Chemistry &
Biology. 21, 238–245.
Roca, M., Torralva, T., Gleichgerrcht, E., Woolgar, A., Thompson, R., Duncan, J., & Manes, F. (2011).
The role of area 10 (BA10) in human multitasking and in social cognition: A lesion study.
Neuropsychologia, 49, 3525–3531.
Rockland, K. S., & Knutson, T. (2000). Feedback connections from area MT of the squirrel monkey to
areas V1 and V2. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 425, 345–368.
Rodriguez, J., Jones, T. H., Sierwald, P., Marek, P. E., Shear, W. A., Brewer, M. S., … Bond, J. E. (2018).
Step-wise evolution of complex chemical defenses in millipedes: A phylogenomic approach. Scientific
Reports, 8, article 3209.
Roebroeks, W., & Villa, P. (2011). On the earliest evidence for habitual use of fire in Europe. Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 5209–5214.
Rogers, A. R., Iltis, D., & Wooding, S. (2004). Genetic variation at the MC1R locus and the time since loss
of human body hair. Current Anthropology, 45, 105–108.
Rogers, A. R., Harris, N. S., & Achenbach, A. A. (2020). Neanderthal-Denisovan ancestors interbred with a
distantly related hominin. Science Advances, 6, article eaay5483.
Rogers, T. T., Hocking, J., Noppeney, U., Michelli, A., Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Patterson, K., & Price,
C. J. (2006). Anterior temporal cortex and semantic memory: Reconciling findings from neu-
ropsychology and functional imaging. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 6, 201–213.
Roma, P. G., Silberberg, A., Huntsberry, M. E., Christensen, C. J., Ruggiero, A. M., & Suomi, S. J. (2007).
Mark tests for mirror self-recognition in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) trained to touch marks. American
Journal of Primatology, 69, 989–1000.
Rosa, M. G. P., & Krubitzer, M. A. (1999). The evolution of visual cortex: Where is V2? Trends in
Neurosciences, 22, 242–248.
Rosa, M. G. P., & Manger, P. R. (2005). Clarifying homologies in the mammalian cerebral cortex: The case
of the third visual area (V3). Clinical and Experimental Pharmacology and Physiology, 32, 327–339.
Rosenbaum, R. S., Gilboa, A., & Moscovitch, M. (2014). Case studies continue to illuminate the cognitive
neuroscience of memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1316, 105–133.
Rosenberger, A. L., & Hartwig, W. C. (2002). New world monkeys. In Encyclopedia of life sciences (Vol. 13,
pp. 164–167). London: Nature Publishing Group.
References 355
Rosenzweig, M. R., Bennett, E. L., Colombo, P. J., Lee, D. W., & Serrano, P. A. (1993). Short-term,
intermediate-term, and long-term memories. Behavioural Brain Research, 57, 193–198.
Rosenzweig, M. R., Breedlove, S. M., & Watson, N. V. (2005). Biological psychology. Sunderland, MA:
Sinauer Associates.
Ross, C. F. (2000). Into the light: The origin of Anthropoidea. Annual Review of Anthropology, 29, 147–194.
Ross, C. F., & Kirk, E. C. (2007). Evolution of eye size and shape in primates. Journal of Human Evolution,
52, 294–313.
Roth, T. C., Brodin, A., Smulders, T. V., LaDage, L. D., & Pravosudov, V. V. (2010). Is bigger always
better? A critical appraisal of the use of volumetric analysis in the study of the hippocampus. Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 365, 915–931.
Rowe, T. B. (2004). Chordate phylogeny and development. In J. Cracraft & M. J. Donohue (Eds.),
Assembling the tree of life (pp. 384–409). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Rowe, T. B., Macrini, T. E., & Luo, Z-X. (2011). Fossil evidence on origin of the mammalian brain. Science,
332, 955–957.
Rubin, D. C., Hinton, S., & Wenzel, A. (1999). The precise time course of retention. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 1161–1176.
Ruff, C. B., & Higgins, R. (2013). Femoral neck structure and function in early hominins. American Journal
of Physical Anthropology, 150, 512–525.
Ruff, C. B., Holt, B., Niskanen, M., Sladek, V., Berner, M., Garofalo, E., … Whittey, E. (2015). Gradual
decline in mobility with the adoption of food production in Europe. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 112, 7147–7152.
Ruhlen, M. (1994a). On the origins of languages. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Ruhlen, M. (1994b). The origin of language. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
Ruiz, A., Gó mez, J. C., Roeder, J. J., & Byrne, R. W. (2009). Gaze following and gaze priming in lemurs.
Animal Cognition, 12, 427–434.
Russon, A. E. (2004). Great ape cognitive systems. In A. E. Russon & D. R. Begun (Eds.), The evolution of
thought: Evolutionary origins of great ape intelligence (pp. 76–100). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
Press.
Rutschmann, F. (2006). Molecular dating of phylogenetic trees: A brief review of current methods that
estimate divergence times. Diversity and Distributions, 12, 35–48.
Rutz, C., Klump, B. C., Komarczyk, L., Leighton, R., Kramer, J., Wischnewski, S., … Masuda, B. M.
(2016). Discovery of species-wide tool use in the Hawaiian crow. Nature, 537, 403–407.
Rutz, C., Sugasawa, S., van der Wal, J. E. M., Klump, B. C., & St. Clair, J. J. H. (2016). Tool bending in
New Caledonian crows. Royal Society Open Science, 3, article 160439.
Saad, N. Y. (2018). A ribonucleopeptide world at the origin of life. Journal of Systematics and Evolution,
56, 1–13.
Saha, R., Verbanic, S., & Chen, I. A. (2018). Lipid vesicles chaperone an encapsulated RNA aptamer. Nature
Communications, 9, article 2313.
Saitta, E. T., Liang, R., Lau, M. C. Y., Brown, C. M., Longrich, N. R., Kaye, T. G., … Onstott, T. (2019).
Cretaceous dinosaur bone contains recent organic material and provides an environment conducive to
microbial communities. eLife, 8, article e46205.
Sakamoto, M., Benton, M. J., & Venditti, C. (2016). Dinosaurs in decline tens of millions of years before
their final extinction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113, 5036–5040.
Sakitt, B., & Long, G. M. (1979). Spare the rod and spoil the icon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Perception and Performance, 5, 19–30.
Saladino, R., Bizzari, B. M., Botta, L., Sponer, J., Sponer, J. E., Georgelin, T., … & Di Mauro, E. (2017).
Proton irradiation: A key to the challenge of N-glycosidic bond formation in a prebiotic context. Scientific
Reports, 7, article 14709.
Saladino, R., Carota, E., Botta, G., Kapralov, M., Timoshenko, G. N., Rozanov, A. Y., … DiMauro, E.
(2015). Meteorite-catalyzed syntheses of nucleosides and of other prebiotic com- pounds from formamide
under proton irradiation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(21), E2746–E2755.
San Antonio, J. D., Schweitzer, M. H., Jensen, S. T., Kalluri, R., Buckley, M., & Orgel, J. P. R. O. (2011).
Dinosaur peptides suggest mechanisms of protein survival. PLoS One, 6, article e20381.
356 References
Sánchez-Amaro, A., Tan, J., Kaufhold, S. P., & Rossano, F. (2020). Gibbons exploit information about what
a competitor can see. Animal Cognition, 23, 289–299.
Sandel, A. A., MacLean, E. L., & Hare, B. (2011). Evidence from four lemur species that ring- tailed lemur
social cognition converges with that of haplorhine primates. Animal Behaviour, 81, 925–931.
Santini, L., Rojas, D., & Donati, G. (2015). Evolving through day and night: Origin and diversification of
activity pattern in modern primates. Behavioral Ecology, 26, 789–796.
Sanz, C. M., & Morgan, D. B. (2007). Chimpanzee tool technology in the Goualougo Triangle, Republic
of Congo. Journal of Human Evolution, 52, 420–433.
Sanz, C. M., Morgan, D. B., & Hopkins, W. D. (2016). Lateralization and performance asymmetries in the
termite fishing of wild chimpanzees in the Goualougo Triangle, Republic of Congo. American Journal of
Primatology, 78, 1190–1200.
Sato, N., Sakata, H., Tanaka, Y. L., & Taira, M. (2006). Navigation-associated medial parietal neurons in
monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103, 17001–17006.
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1992). Language training of apes. In S. Jones, R. Martin, & D. Pilbeam (Eds.),
The Cambridge encyclopedia of human evolution (pp. 138–141). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
Press.
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., McDonald, K., Sevcik, R. A., Hopkins, W. D., & Rubert, E. (1986).
Spontaneous symbol acquisition and communicative use by pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus). Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 211–235.
Sawamura, H., Georgieva, S., Vogels, R., Vanduffel, W., & Orban, G. A. (2005). Using functional magnetic
resonance imaging to assess adaptation and size invariance of shape processing by humans and monkeys.
The Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 4294–4306.
Saxena, A., Towers, M., & Cooper, K. L. (2017). The origins, scaling and loss of tetrapod digits. Philosophical
Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 372, article 20150482.
Scardia, G., Neves, W. A., Tattersall, I., & Blumrich, L. (2020). What kind of hominin first left Africa?
Evolutionary Anthropology, 2020, 1–6.
Scardia, G., Parenti, F., Miggins, D. P., Gerdes, A., Araujo, A. G. M., & Neves, W. A. (2019). Chronologic
constraints on hominin dispersal outside Africa since 2.48 Ma from the Zarqa Valley, Jordan. Quaternary
Science Reviews, 219, 1–19.
Schall, U., Johnston, P., Lagopoulos, J., Jü ptner, M., Jentzen, W., Thienel, R., … Ward, P. B. (2003).
Functional brain maps of tower of London performance: A positron emission tomography and functional
magnetic resonance imaging study. NeuroImage, 20, 1154–1161.
Scharlau, I. (2004). Evidence for split foci of attention in a priming paradigm. Perception & Psychophysics, 66,
988–1002.
Schilder, B. M., Petry, H. M., & Hof, P. R. (2019). Evolutionary shifts dramatically reorganized the human
hippocampal complex. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 528, 3143–3170.
Schilling, T. (2003). Making jaws. Heredity, 90, 3–5.
Schinazi, V. R., Nardi, D., Newcombe, N. S., Shipley, T. F., & Epstein, R. A. (2013). Hippocampal size
predicts rapid learning of a cognitive map in humans. Hippocampus, 23, 515–528.
Schlosser, G. (2017). From so simple a beginning – what amphioxus can teach us about placode evolution.
International Journal of Developmental Biology, 61, 633–648.
Schmid, P., Churchill, S. E., Nalla, S., Weissen, E., Carlson, K. J., de Ruiter, D. J., & Berger, L. R. (2013).
Mosaic morphology in the thorax of Australopithecus sediba. Science, 340, article 1234598.
Schmitt, D. (2003). Insights into the evolution of human bipedalism from experimental studies of humans
and other primates. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 206, 1437–1448.
Schmitt, D., & Lemelin, P. (2002). Origins of primate locomotion: Gait mechanics of the wooly opossum.
American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 118, 231–238.
Schmitt, D., Cartmill, M., Griffin, T. M., Hanna, J. B., & Lemelin, P. (2006). Adaptive value of ambling
gaits in primates and other mammals. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 209, 2042–2049.
Schmitz, L., & Motani, R. (2011). Nocturnality in dinosaurs inferred from scleral ring and orbit mor-
phology. Science, 332, 705–708.
Schmolesky, M. (2018). The primary visual cortex. Webvision, Moran Eye Center. Retrieved August 20,
2018, from https://webvision.med.utah.edu/book/part-ix-psychophysics-of-vision/ the-primary-visual-
cortex.
References 357
Schneider, D., Schneider, L., Claussen, C-F., & Kolchev, C. (2001). Cortical representation of the vestibular
system as evidenced by brain electrical activity mapping of vestibular late evoked potentials. Ear, Nose, &
Throat Journal, 80, 251–265.
Schoene, B., Samperton, K. M., Eddy, M. P., Keller, G., Adatte, T., Bowring, S. A., … Gertsch, B. (2015).
U-Pb geochronology of the Deccan traps and relation to the end-Cretaceous mass extinction. Science, 347,
182–184.
Schofield, D. P., McGrew, W. C., Takahashi, A., & Hirata, S. (2018). Cumulative culture in nonhumans:
Overlooked findings from Japanese monkeys? Primates, 59, 113–122.
Schrago, C. G., & Russo, C. A. M. (2003). Timing the origin of New World monkeys. Molecular Biology and
Evolution, 20, 1620–1625.
Schrago, C. G., & Voloch, C. M. (2013). The precision of the hominid timescale estimated by relaxed clock
methods. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 26, 746–755.
Schroeder, L., Scott, J. E., Garvin, H. M., Laird, M. F., Dembo, M., Radovcic, D., … Ackermann, R. R.
(2017). Skull diversity in the Homo lineage and the relative position of Homo naledi. Journal of Human
Evolution, 104, 124–135.
Schroeter, E. R., DeHart, C. J., Cleland, T. P., Zheng, W., Thomas, P. M., Kelleher, N. L., … Schweitzer,
M. H. (2017). Expansion for the Brachlyophosaurus canadensis collagen I sequence and additional evidence
of the preservation of cretaceous protein. Journal of Proteome Research, 16, 920–932.
Schulman, A. H., & Kaplowitz, C. (1977). Mirror-image response during the first two years of life.
Developmental Psychobiology, 10, 133–142.
Schulte, P., Alegret, L., Arenillas, I., Arz, J. A., Barton, P. J., Bown, P. R., … Willumsen, P. S. (2010). The
Chicxulub asteroid impact and mass extinction at the Cretaceous-Paleogene boundary. Science, 327,
1214–1218.
Schulze-Makuch, D., Fairen, A. G., & Davila, A. F. (2008). The case for life on Mars. International Journal of
Astrobiology, 7, 117–141.
Schwab, I. R. (2018). The evolution of eyes: Major steps. The Keeler lecture 2017: Centenary of Keeler Ltd.
Eye, 32, 302–313.
Schwartz, B. L., Colon, M. R., Sanchez, I. C., Rodriguez, I. A., & Evans, S. (2002). Single-trial learning of
“what” and “who” information in a gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla): Implications for episodic memory.
Animal Cognition, 5, 85–90.
Schwartz, B. L., Hoffman, M. L., & Evans, S. (2005). Episodic-like memory in a gorilla: A review and new
findings. Learning and Motivation, 36, 226–244.
Schwartz, J. H., & Tattersall, I. (2000). The human chin revisited: What is it and who has it? Journal of
Human Evolution, 38, 367–409.
Schweitzer, M. H., Zheng, W., Organ, C. L., Avci, R., Suo, Z., Freimark, L. M., … Asara, J. M. (2009).
Biomolecular characterization and protein sequences of the Campanian hadrosaur B. canadensis. Science,
324, 626–631.
Scozzari, R., Massaia, A., Trombetta, B., Bellusci, G., Myres, N. M., Novelletto, A., & Cru-Ciani, F.
(2014). An unbiased resource of novel SNP markers provides a new chronology for the human Y
chromosome and reveals a deep phylogenetic structure in Africa. Genome Research, 24, 535–544.
Sears, K. E., Finarelli, J. A., Flynn, J. J., & Wyss, A. R. (2008). Estimating body mass in New World
“monkeys” (Platyrrhini, Primates), with a consideration of the Miocene platyrrhine, Chilecebus carras-
coensis. American Museum Novitates, article 3617.
Seghier, M. L., Lazeyras, F., Pegna, A. J., Annoni, J-M., Zimine, I., Mayer, E., … Khateb, A. (2004).
Variability of fMRI activation during a phonological and semantic language task in healthy subjects.
Human Brain Mapping, 23, 140–155.
Segurel, L., Guarino-Vignon, P., Marchi, N., Lafosse, S., Laurent, R., Bon, C., … Heyer, E. (2020). Why
and when was lactase persistence selected for? Insights from Central Asian herders and ancient DNA. PLoS
Biology, 18, article e3000742.
Seiffert, E. R., Tejedor, M. F., Fleagle, J. G., Novo, N. M., Cornejo, F. M., Bond, M., … Campbell, K. E.
(2020). A parapithecid stem anthropoid of African origin in the Paleogene of South America. Science, 368,
194–197.
Seipel, K., & Schmid, V. (2005). Evolution of striated muscle: Jellyfish and origin of triploblasty.
Developmental Biology, 282, 14–26.
358 References
Selby, M. S., Simpson, S. W., & Lovejoy, C. O. (2016). The functional anatomy of the carpometacarpal
complex in anthropoids and its implications for the evolution of the hominoid hand. Anatomical Record,
299, 583–600.
Selby, M. S., & Lovejoy, C. O. (2017). Evolution of the hominoid scapula and its implications for earliest
hominid locomotion. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 162, 682–700.
Selig, K. R., Sargis, E. J., & Silcox, M. T. (2019). The frugivorous insectivores? Functional morphological
analysis of molar topography for inferring diet in extant treeshrews (Scandentia). Journal of Mammalogy,
100, 1901–1917.
Seligman, M. E. P. (1970). On the generality of the laws of learning. Psychological Review, 77, 406–418.
Sellers, W. I., Cain, G. M., Wang, W., & Crompton, R. H. (2005). Stride lengths, speed and energy costs in
walking of Australopithecus afarensis: Using evolutionary robotics to predict locomotion of early human
ancestors. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 2, 431–441.
Semaw, S. (2000). The world’s oldest stone artefacts from Gona, Ethiopia: Their implications for under-
standing stone technology and patterns of human evolution between 2.6–1.5 million years ago. Journal of
Archaeological Science, 27, 1197–1214.
Semaw, S., Rogers, M. J., Simpson, S. W., Levin, N. E., Quade, J., Dunbar, N., … Everett, M. (2020). Co-
occurrence of Acheulian and Oldowan artifacts with Homo erectus cranial fossils from Gona, Afar, Ethiopia.
Science Advances, 6, article eaaw4694.
Semendeferi, K., & Damasio, H. (2000). The brain and its main anatomical subdivisions in living hominoids
using magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Human Evolution, 38, 317–332.
Semendeferi, K., Armstrong, E., Schleicher, A., Zilles, K., & Van Hoesen, G. W. (2001). Prefrontal cortex
in humans and apes: A comparative study of area 10. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 114,
224–241.
Semenov, S. A. (1970). Prehistoric technology.Bath, UK: Adams & Dart.
Senut, B. (2006). Bipé die et climat. Comptes Rendus Palevol, 5, 89–98.
Serrian, D. J., & Sovijärvi-Spapé, M. M. (2015). Hemispheric asymmetries and the control of motor se-
quences. Behavioural Brain Research, 283, 30–36.
Service, R. F. (2015). Researchers may have solved origin-of-life conundrum. Retrieved 19 July 2020 from
https://www.sciencemag.org/news/2015/03/researchers-may-have-solved-origin-life-conundrum.
Seymour, K. J., Williams, M. A., & Rich, A. N. (2016). The representation of color across the human visual
cortex: Distinguishing chromatic signals contributing to object form versus surface color. Cerebral Cortex,
26, 1997–2005.
Shelton, A. L., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002). Neural correlates of encoding space from route and survey
perspectives. The Journal of Neuroscience, 22, 2711–2727.
Shepherd, S. V., & Platt, M. L. (2008). Spontaneous social orienting and gaze following in ring-tailed lemurs
(Lemur catta). Animal Cognition, 11, 13–20.
Shepherd, S. V., & Freiwald, W. A. (2018). Functional networks for social communication in the macaque
monkey. Neuron, 99, 413–420.
Shields, W. E., Smith, J. D., Guttmannova, K., & Washburn, D. A. (2005). Confidence judgments by
humans and rhesus monkeys. The Journal of General Psychology, 132, 165–186.
Shipton, C. (2018). Biface knapping skill in the East African Acheulean: Progressive trends and random
walks. African Archaeological Review, 35, 107–131.
Shoshani, J., Groves, C. P., Simons, E. L., & Gunnell, G. F. (1996). Primate phylogeny: Morphological vs
molecular results. Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution, 5, 102–154.
Shoshani, J., Kupsky, W. J., & Marchant, G. H. (2006). Elephant brain. Part 1: Gross morphology, func-
tions, comparative anatomy, and evolution. Brain Research Bulletin, 70, 124–157.
Shreve, J. (2015, October). Mystery man: A trove of fossils found deep in a South African cave adds a
baffling new branch to the human family tree. National Geographic, 30–57.
Shu, D-G., Luo, H-L., Morris, S. C., Zhang, X-L., Hu, S-X., Chen, L., … Chen, L-Z. (1999). Lower
Cambrian vertebrates from South China. Nature, 402, 42–46.
Shu, D., Isozaki, Y., Zhang, X., Han, J., & Maruyama, S. (2014). Birth and early evolution of metazoans.
Gondwana Research, 25, 884–895.
Shubin, N. H. (2008). Your inner fish. New York, NY: Pantheon Books.
References 359
Shubin, N. H., Daeschler, E. B., & Coates, M. I. (2004). The early evolution of the tetrapod humerus.
Science, 304, 90–93.
Sidor, C. A., & Hopson, J. A. (1998). Ghost lineages and “mammalness”: Assessing the temporal pattern of
character acquisition in the synapsida. Paleobiology, 24, 254–273.
Sihvonen, A. J., Ripollé s, P., Leo, V., Rodríguez-Fornells, A., Soinila, S., & Sä rkä mö , T. (2016). Neural
basis of acquired amusia and its recovery after stroke. The Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 8872–8881.
Silcox, M. T., Benham, A. E., & Bloch, J. I. (2010). Endocasts of Microsyops (Microsyopidae, Primates) and
the evolution of the brain in primitive primates. Journal of Human Evolution, 58, 505–521.
Silcox, M. T., Dalmyn, C. K., & Bloch, J. I. (2009). Virtual endocast of Ignacius graybullianus
(Paromomyidae, Primates) and brain evolution in early primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 106, 10987–10992.
Silcox, M. T., & López-Torres, S. (2017). Major questions in the study of primate origins. Annual Review of
Earth and Planetary Sciences, 45, 113–137.
Silvestro, D., Tejedor, M. F., Serrano-Serrano, M. L., Loiseau, O., Rossier, V., Rolland, J., … Salamin, N.
(2019). Early arrival and climatically-linked geographic expansion of New World monkeys from tiny
African ancestors. Systematic Biology, 68, 78–92.
Simakov, O., Marlétaz, F., Yue, J.-X., O’Connell, B., Jenkins, J., Brandt. A., … Rokhsar, D. S. (2020).
Deeply conserved synteny resolves early events in vertebrate evolution. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 4,
820–830.
Simion, P., Philippe, H., Baurain, D., Jager, M., Richter, D. J., DiFranco, A., … Manuel, M. (2017). A
large and consistent phylogenomic dataset supports sponges as the sister group to all other animals. Current
Biology, 27, 958–967.
Simons, E. L. (1992). The fossil history of primates. In S. Jones, R. Martin, & D. Pilbeam (Eds.), The
Cambridge encyclopedia of human evolution (pp. 199–208). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Simons, E. L., Seiffert, E. R., Ryan, T. M., & Attia, Y. (2007). A remarkable female cranium of the early
Oligocene anthropoid Aegyptopithecus zeuxis (Catarrhini, Propliopithecidae). Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 104, 8731–8736.
Simonti, C. N., Vernot, B., Bastarache, L., Bottinger, E., Carrell, D. S., Chisholm, R. L., … Capra, J. A.
(2016). The phenotypic legacy of admixture between modern humans and Neandertals. Science, 351,
737–741.
Simonyan, K. (2014). The laryngeal motor cortex: Its organization and connectivity. Current Opinion in
Neurobiology, 28, 15–21.
Simpson, S. W., Latimer, B., & Lovejoy, C. O. (2018). Why do knuckle-walking African apes knuckle-
walk? The Anatomical Record, 301, 496–514.
Simpson, S. W., Quade, J., Levin, N. E., Butler, R., Dupont-Nivet, G., Everett, M., & Semaw, S. (2008). A
female Homo erectus pelvis from Gona, Ethiopia. Science, 322, 1089–1092.
Skalníková, P., Frynta, D., Abramjan, A., Rokyta, R., & Nekovárova, T. (2020). Spontaneous color pre-
ferences in rhesus monkeys: What is the advantage of primate trichromacy? Behavioural Processes, 174,
article 104084.
Skottun, B. C., & Skoyles, J. R. (2010). Backward masking as a test of magnocellular sensitivity. Neuro-
ophthalmology, 34, 342–346.
Skov, L., Macià, M. C., Sveinbjörnsson, G., Mafessoni, F., Lucotte, E. A., Einarsdóttir, M. S., … Stefansson,
K. (2020). The nature of Neanderthal introgression revealed by 27,566 Icelandic genomes. Nature, 582,
78–83.
Slatkin, M., & Racimo, F. (2016). Ancient DNA and human history. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Science, 113, 6380–6387.
Smith, T. D., Rossie, J. B., & Bhatnagar, K. P. (2007). Evolution of the nose and nasal skeleton in primates.
Evolutionary Anthropology, 16, 132–146.
Smithson, T. R., Carroll, R. L., Panchen, A. L., & Andrews, S. M. (1994). Westlothiana lizziae from the
Visé an of East Kirkton, West Lothian, Scotland, and the amniote stem. Transactions of the Royal Society of
Edinburgh: Earth Sciences, 84, 383–412.
Sockol, M. D., Raichlen, D. A., & Pontzer, H. (2007). Chimpanzee locomotor energetics and the origin of
human bipedalism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 12265–12269.
Soler, M., Colmenero, J. M., Pérez-Contreras, T., & Peralta-Sánchez, J. M. (2020). Replication of the
360 References
mirror mark test experiment in the Magpie (Pic pica) does not provide evidence of self-recognition. Journal
of Comparative Psychology, 134, 363–371.
Soler, M., Pé rez-Contreras, T., & Peralta-Sá nchez, J. M. (2014). Mirror-mark tests performed on jackdaws
reveal potential methodological problems in the use of stickers in avian mark-test studies. PLoS One, 9,
article e86193.
Soligo, C., & Martin, R. D. (2006). Adaptive origins of primates revisited. Journal of Human Evolution, 50,
414–430.
Souto, A., Bione, C. B. C., Bastos, M., Bezerra, B. M., Fragaszy, D., & Schiel, N. (2011). Critically
endangered blonde capuchins fish for termites and use new techniques to accomplish the task. Biology
Letters, 7, 532–535.
Spelke, E. S. (1982). Perceptual knowledge of objects in infancy. In J. Mehler, E. C. T. Walker, &
M. Garrett (Eds.), Perspectives on mental representation (pp. 409–430). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Sperling, G. (1963). A model for visual memory tasks. Human Factors, 5, 19–31.
Spillmann, L. (2014). Receptive fields of visual neurons: The early years. Perception, 43, 1145–1176.
Spinozzi, G., De Lillo, C., & Salvi, V. (2006). Local advantage in the visual processing of hierarchical stimuli
following manipulations of stimulus size and element numerosity in monkeys (Cebus apella). Behavioural
Brain Research, 166, 45–54.
Spoor, F., Leakey, M. G., Gathogo, P. N., Brown, F. H., Antó n, S. C., McDougall, I., … Leakey, L. N.
(2007). Implications of new early Homo fossils from Ileret, east of lake Turkana, Kenya. Nature, 448,
688–691.
Springer, M. S., Emerling, C. A., Meredith, R. W., Janecka, J. E., Eizirik, E., & Murphy, W. J. (2017).
Waking the undead: Implications of a soft explosive model for the timing of placental mammal di-
versification. Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution, 106, 86–102.
Squires, T. M. (2004). Optimizing the vertebrate vestibular semicircular canal: Could we balance any better?
Physical Review Letters, 93, 198106, 1–4.
Srinivasan, S., & Stevens, C. F. (2019). Scaling principles of distributed circuits. Current Biology, 29,
2533–2540.
Stancher, G., Sovrano, V. A., & Vallortigara, G. (2020). Motor asymmetries in fishes, amphibians, and
reptiles. In G.S. Forrester, W. D. Hopkins, K. Hudry, & A. Lindell (Eds.), Cerebral lateralization and
cognition: Evolutionary and developmental investigations of behavioral biases (pp. 33–56). Cambridge, MA:
Academic Press.
Stanford, C. B. (1996). The hunting ecology of wild chimpanzees: Implications for the evolutionary ecology
of Pliocene hominids. American Anthropologist, 98, 96–113.
Stanford, C. B. (2003). Upright: The evolutionary key to becoming human. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co.
Stanford, C. B. (2006). Arboreal bipedalism in wild chimpanzees: Implications for the evolution of hominid
posture and locomotion. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 129, 225–231.
Steele, A., Goddard, D. T., Stapleton, D., Toporski, J. K. W., Peters, V., Bassinger, V., … McKay, D. S.
(2000). Investigations into an unknown organism on the Martian meteorite Allan Hills 84001. Meteoritics
& Planetary Science, 35, 237–241.
Steenhuis, R. E., & Bryden, M. P. (1989). Different dimensions of hand preference that relate to skilled and
unskilled activities. Cortex, 25, 289–304.
Stein, K., Prondvai, E., Huang, T., Baele, J.-M., Sander, P. M., & Reisz, R. (2019). Structure and evo-
lutionary implications of the earliest (Sinemurian, Early Jurassic) dinosaur eggs and eggshells. Scientific
Reports, 9, article 4424.
Steinmetz, P. R. H., Arman, A., Kraus, J. E. M., & Technau, U. (2017). Gut-like ectodermal tissue in a sea
anemone challenges germ layer homology. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 1, 1535–1542.
Stepniewska, I., Pouget, P., & Kaas, J. H. (2018). Frontal eye field in prosimian galagos: Intracortical mi-
crostimulation and tracing studies. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 526, 626–652.
Stepniewska, I., Preuss, T. M., & Kaas, J. H. (2006). Ipsilateral cortical connections of dorsal and
ventral premotor areas in new world owl monkeys. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 495,
691–708.
Steudel-Numbers, K. L. (2001). Role of locomotor economy in the origin of bipedal posture and gait.
American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 116, 171–173.
References 361
Stevens, N. J., Seiffert, E. R., O’Connor, P. M., Roberts, E. M., Schmitz, M. D., Krause, C., … Temu, J.
(2013). Paleontological evidence for an Oligocene divergence between old world monkeys and apes.
Nature, 497, 611–614.
Steventon, B., Mayor, R., & Streit, A. (2016). Directional cell movements downstream of Gbx2 and Otx2
control the assembly of sensory placodes. Biology Open, 5, 1620–1624.
Stone, A. C., Battistuzzi, F. U., Kubatko, L. S., Perry, G. H., Trudeau, E., Lin, H., & Kumar, S. (2010).
More reliable estimates of divergence times in Pan using complete mtDNA sequences and accounting for
population structure. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 365, 3277–3288.
Stout, D. (2002). Skill and cognition in stone tool production: An ethnographic case study from Irian Jaya.
Current Anthropology, 43, 693–715.
Stout, D., Toth, N., Schick, K., & Chaminade, T. (2008). Neural correlates of early stone age tool-making:
Technology, language and cognition in human evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B,
363, 1939–1949.
Strait, D. S., & Grine, F. E. (2004). Inferring hominoid and early hominid phylogeny using craniodental
characters: The role of fossil taxa. Journal of Human Evolution, 47, 399–452.
Straube, B., & Chatterjee, A. (2010). Space and time in perceptual causality. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience,
4, article 28.
Straus, L. G. (2001). Africa and Iberia in the Pleistocene. Quaternary International, 75, 91–102.
Stringer, C., & McKie, R. (1996). African exodus: The origins of modern humanity. New York, NY: Henry
Holt and Company.
Suá rez, R., Gobius, I., & Richards, L. J. (2014). Evolution and development of interhemispheric con-
nections in the vertebrate forebrain. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, article 497.
Suarez, S. E., Brookfield, M. E., Catlos, E. J., & Stöckli, D. F. (2017). A U-Pb zircon age constraint on the
oldest-recorded air-breathing land animal. PLoS ONE, 12, article e0179262.
Suddendorf, T., & Whiten, A. (2001). Mental evolution and development: Evidence for secondary re-
presentation in children, great apes, and other animals. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 629–650.
Sugahara, F., Murakami, Y., Pascual-Anaya, J., & Kuratani, S. (2017). Reconstructing the ancestral verte-
brate brain. Development, Growth, & Differentiation, 59, 163–174.
Sun, L., Kawano-Yamashita, E., Nagata, T., Tsukamoto, H., Furutani, Y., Koyanagi, M., & Ter-Akita, A.
(2014). Distribution of mammalian-like melanopsin in cyclostome retinas exhibiting a different extent of
visual functions. PLoS One, 9, article e108209.
Sunaert, S., Van Hecke, P., Marchal, G., & Orban, G. A. (1999). Motion-responsive regions of the human
brain. Experimental Brain Research, 127, 355–370.
Suntsova, M. V., & Buzdin, A. A. (2020). Differences between human and chimpanzee genomes and their
implications in gene expression, protein functions and biochemical properties of the two species. BMC
Genomics, 21 (Suppl 7), article 535.
Surridge, A. K., Osorio, D., & Mundy, N. I. (2003). Evolution and selection of trichromatic vision in
primates. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 18, 198–205.
Suryanarayana, S. M., Robertson, B., Wallén, P., & Grillner, S. (2017). The lamprey pallium provides a
blueprint of the mammalian layered cortex. Current Biology, 27, 3264–3277.
Sussman, R. W., Rasmussen, D. T., & Raven, P. H. (2013). Rethinking primate origins again. American
Journal of Primatology, 75, 95–106.
Sustaita, D., Pouydebat, E., Manzano, A., Abdala, V., Hertel, F., & Herrel, A. (2013). Getting a grip on
tetrapod grasping: Form, function, and evolution. Biological Reviews, 88, 380–405.
Sutikna, T., Tocheri, M. W., Morwood, M. J., Saptomo, E. W., Jatmiko, Awe, R. D., … Roberts, R. G.
(2016). Revised stratigraphy and chronology for Homo floresiensis at Liang Bua in Indonesia. Nature, 532,
366–369.
Suwa, G., Asfaw, B., Kono, R. T., Kubo, D., Lovejoy, C. O., & White, T. D. (2009). The Ardipithecus
ramidus skull and its implications for hominid origins. Science, 326, 68.
Suzuki, W., Banno, T., Miyakawa, N., Abe, H., Goda, N., & Ichinohe, N. (2015). Mirror neurons in a new
world monkey, common marmoset. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 9, article 459.
Swartz, K. B., Sarauw, D., & Evans, S. (1999). Comparative aspects of mirror self-recognition in great apes.
In S. T. Parker, R. W. Mitchell, & H. L. Miles (Eds.), The mentalities of gorillas and orangutans in comparative
perspective (pp. 283–294). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
362 References
Szabo, B., Noble, D. W. A., & Whiting, M. J. (2021). Learning in non-avian reptiles 40 years on: Advances
and promising new directions. Biological Reviews, 96, 331–356.
Szilá gyi, A., Kö nnyű , B., & Czá rá n, T. (2020). Dynamics and stability in prebiotic information integration:
An RNA World model from first principles. Scientific Reports, 10, article 51.
Tabiow, E., & Forrester, G. S. (2013). Structured bimanual actions and hand transfers reveal population-
level right-handedness in captive gorillas. Animal Behaviour, 86, 1049–1057.
Taglialatela, J. P., Cantalupo, C., & Hopkins, W. D. (2006). Gesture handedness predicts asymmetry in the
chimpanzee inferior frontal gyrus. NeuroReport, 17, 923–927.
Taglialatela, J. P., Russell, J. L., Schaeffer, J. A., & Hopkins, W. D. (2008). Communicative signaling
activates ‘Broca’s’ homolog in chimpanzees. Current Biology, 18, 1–6.
Takemoto, H., Kawamoto, Y., & Furuichi, T. (2015). How did bonobos come to range south of the Congo
River? Reconsideration of the divergence of Pan paniscus from other Pan populations. Evolutionary
Anthropology, 24, 170–184.
Takezaki, N., & Nishihara, H. (2017). Support for lungfish as the closest relative of tetrapods by using slowly
evolving ray-finned fish as the outgroup. Genome Biology and Evolution, 9, 93–101.
Tanaka, K. (1997). Mechanisms of visual object recognition: Monkey and human studies. Current Opinion in
Neurobiology, 7, 523–529.
Tang, S., Lee, T. S., Li, M., Zhang, Y., Xu, Y., Liu, F., … Jiang, H. (2018). Complex pattern selectivity in
macaque primary visual cortex revealed by large-scale two-photon imaging. Current Biology, 28, 38–48.
Tarver, J. E., dos Reis, M., Mirarab, S., Moran, R. J., Parker, S., O’Reilly. J. E., … Pisani, D. (2016). The
interrelationships of placental mammals and the limits of phylogenetic inference. Genome Biology and
Evolution, 8, 330–344.
Tashiro, T., Ishida, A., Hori, M., Igisu, M., Koike, M., Mejean, P., … Komiya, T. (2017). Early trace of life
from 3.95 Ga sedimentary rocks in Labrador, Canada. Nature, 549, 516–518.
Taubert, J., Van Belle, G., Vanduffel, W., Rossion, B., & Vogels, R. (2015). Neural correlate of the
Thatcher face illusion in a monkey face-selective patch. The Journal of Neuroscience, 35, 9872–9878.
Taylor, J. G., & Fragopanagos, M. (2007). Resolving some confusions over attention and consciousness.
Neural Networks, 20, 993–1003.
Teffer, K., Buxhoeveden, D. P., Stimpson, C. D., Fobbs, A. J., Schapiro, S. J., Baze, W. B., … Semendeferi,
K. (2013). Developmental changes in the spatial organization of neurons in the neocortex of humans and
common chimpanzees. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 521, 4249–4259.
Templer, V. L., Brown, E. K., & Hampton, R. R. (2018). Rhesus monkeys metacognitively monitor
memories of the order of events. Scientific Reports, 8, article 11541.
Tennant, P., Fermin, G., & Foster, J. E. (2018). Viruses: Molecular biology, host interactions, and applications to
biotechnology. San Diego: Academic Press.
Terberger, T. (2006). From the first humans to the Mesolithic hunters in the Northern German lowlands –
current results and trends. In K. M. Hansen & K. B. Pedersen (Eds.), Across the Western Baltic
(pp. 111–184). Vordingborg, Denmark: Sydsjællands Museum.
Terhune, C. E., Ritzman, T. B., & Robinson, C. A. (2018). Mandibular ramus shape variation and on-
togeny in Homo sapiens and Homo neanderthalensis. Journal of Human Evolution, 121, 55–71.
Terrace, H. S., Petitto, L. A., Sanders, R. J., & Bever, T. G. (1979). Can an ape create a sentence? Science,
206, 891–902.
Thiergart, T., Landan, G., & Martin, W. F. (2014). Concatenated alignments and the case of the dis-
appearing tree. BMC Evolutionary Biology, 14, 28–49.
Thompson, J. C., McPherron, S. P., Bobe, R., Reed, D., Barr, W. A., Wynn, J. G., … Alemseged, Z.
(2015). Taphonomy of fossils from the hominin-bearing deposits at Dikika, Ethiopia. Journal of Human
Evolution, 86, 112–135.
Thompson, R. L., & Boatright-Horowitz, S. L. (1994). The question of mirror-mediated self- recognition
in apes and monkeys: Some new results and reservations. In S. T. Parker, R. W. Mitchell, & M. L. Boccia
(Eds.), Self-awareness in animals and humans (pp. 330–349). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Thorpe, S. J., & Fabre-Thorpe, M. (2001). Seeking categories in the brain. Science, 291, 260–263.
Titchener, E. B. (1909). A text-book of psychology. New York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan.
Tobias, P. V. (1995). The brain of the first hominids. In J-P. Changeux & J. Chavaillon (Eds.), Origins of the
human brain (pp. 61–81). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
References 363
Tobias, P. V., & Campbell, B. (1981). The emergence of man in Africa and beyond. Philosophical Transactions
of the Royal Society of London, B, 292, 43–56.
Tocheri, M. W., Marzke, M. W., Liu, D., Bae, M., Jones, G. P., Williams, R. C., & Razdan, A. (2003).
Functional capabilities of modern and fossil hominid hands: Three-dimensional analysis of trapezia.
American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 122, 101–112.
Tocheri, M. W., Orr, C. M., Larson, S. G., Sutikna, T., Jatmiko, Saptomo, E. W., … Jungers, W. L. (2007).
The primitive wrist of Homo floresiensis and its implications for hominin evolution. Science, 317,
1743–1745.
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Tomasello, M., Call, J., & Hare, B. (1998). Five primate species follow the visual gaze of conspecifics.
Animal Behaviour, 55, 1063–1069.
Tomasello, M., Call, J., & Hare, B. (2003). Chimpanzees understand psychological states – the question is
which ones and to what extent. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 153–156.
Tomasello, M., Hare, B., & Agnetta, B. (1999). Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, follow gaze direction geo-
metrically. Animal Behaviour, 58, 769–777.
Toner, J. D., & Catling, D. C. (2019). Alkaline lake settings for concentrated prebiotic cyanide and the
origin of life. Geochimica et Cosmochimica Acta, 260, 124–132.
Topolski, R., & Inhoff, A. W. (1995). Loss of vision during the retinal stabilization of letters. Psychological
Research, 58, 155–162.
Toth, N. (1985). Archaeological evidence for preferential right-handedness in the lower and middle
Pleistocene, and its possible implications. Journal of Human Evolution, 14, 607–614.
Toth, N., & Schick, K. (2019). Why did the Acheulian happen? Experimental studies into the manufacture
and function of Acheulean artifacts. L’anthropologie, 123, 724–768.
Toups, M. A., Kitchen, A., Light, J. E., & Reed, D. L. (2010). Origin of clothing lice indicates early clothing
use by anatomically modern humans in Africa. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 28, 29–32.
Trainor, P. A., Melton, K. R., & Manzanares, M. (2003). Origins and plasticity of neural crest cells and their
roles in jaw and craniofacial evolution. International Journal of Developmental Biology, 47, 541–553.
Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (1997). A neural basis for the retrieval of conceptual knowledge.
Neuropsychologia, 35, 1319–1327.
Trébouet, F., Reichard, U. H., Pinkaew, N., & Malaivijitnond, S. (2018). Extractive foraging of toxic
caterpillars in wild northern pig-tailed macaques (Macaca leonina). Primates, 59, 185–196.
Trevethan, C. T., Sahraie, A., & Weiskrantz, L. (2007). Form discrimination in a case of blindsight.
Neuropsychologia, 45, 2092–2103.
Trevors, J. T. (2003). Early assembly of cellular life. Progress in Biophysics and Molecular Biology, 81, 201–217.
Trick, L. M., & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1993). What enumeration studies can show us about spatial attention:
Evidence for limited capacity preattentive processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception
and Performance, 19, 331–351.
Trinkaus, E. (1992). Evolution of human manipulation. In S. Jones, R. Martin, & D. Pilbeam (Eds.), The
Cambridge encyclopedia of human evolution (pp. 346–349). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Truppa, V., De Simone, D. A., & De Lillo, C. (2016). Short-term memory effects on visual global/ local
processing in tufted capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 130, 162–173.
Tsao, D. Y., Vanduffel, W., Sasaki, Y., Fize, D., Knutsen, T. A., Mandeville, J. B., … Tootell, R. B. H.
(2003). Stereopsis activates V3A and caudal intraparietal areas in macaques and humans. Neuron, 39,
555–568.
Tschudin, A., Call, J., Dunbar, R. I. M., Harris, G., & van der Elst, C. (2001). Comprehension of signs by
dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115, 100–105.
Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Crivello, F., & Mazoyer, B. (2018). Is the planum temporale surface area a marker of
hemispheric or regional language lateralization? Brain Structure and Function, 223, 1217–1228.
Ujhelyi, M. (1998). Long-call structure in apes as a possible precursor for language. In J. R. Hurford, M.
Studdert-Kennedy, & C. Knight (Eds.), Approaches to the evolution of language (pp. 177–189). Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge University Press.
Ujhelyi, M., Merker, B., Buk, P., & Geissmann, T. (2000). Observations on the behavior of gibbons
(Hylobates leucogenys, H. gabriellae, and H. lar) in the presence of mirrors. Journal of Comparative Psychology,
114, 253–262.
364 References
Unterrainer, J. M., Rahm, B., Kaller, C. P., Ruff, C. C., Spreer, J., Krause, B. J., … Halsband, U. (2004).
When planning fails: Individual differences and error-related brain activity in problem solving. Cerebral
Cortex, 14, 1390–1397.
Uomini, N. T. (2006). In the knapper’s hands: Testing markers of laterality in hominin lithic production,
with reference to the common substrate of language and handedness (Thesis submitted for the degree of
Doctor of Philosophy), University of Southampton.
Valentine, J. W., Jablonski, D., & Erwin, D. H. (1999). Fossils, molecules and embryos: New perspectives
on the Cambrian explosion. Development, 126, 851–859.
van Buuren, M., Auersperg, A., Gajdon, G., Tebbich, S., & von Bayern, A. (2019). No evidence of mirror
self-recognition in keas and Goffin’s cockatoos. Behaviour, 156, 763–786.
van den Bergh, G. D., Kaifu, Y., Kurniawan, I., Kono, R. T., Brumm, A., Setiyabudi, E., … Morwood, M. J.
(2016). Homo floresiensis-like fossils from the early Middle Pleistocene of Flores. Nature, 534, 245–248.
van den Heuvel, O. A., Groenewegen, H. J., Barkhof, F., Lazeron, R. H. C., van Dyck, R., & Veltman, D.
J. (2003). Frontostriatal system in planning complexity: A parametric functional magnetic resonance
version of Tower of London task. NeuroImage, 18, 367–374.
van Duijn, M. (2017). Phylogenetic origins of biological cognition: Convergent patterns in the early
evolution of learning. Interface Focus, 7, article 20160158.
Van Lawick-Goodall, J. (1971). In the shadow of man. New York, NY: Dell.
van Schaik, C. P., Ancrenaz, M., Borgen, G., Galdikas, B., Knott, C. D., Singleton, I., … Merrill, M.
(2003). Orangutan cultures and the evolution of material culture. Science, 299, 102–105.
van Tuinen, M., & Hadly, E. A. (2004). Error in estimation of rate and time inferred from the early amniote
fossil record and avian molecular clocks. Journal of Molecular Evolution, 59, 267–276.
Vanhooland, L.-C., Bugnyar, T., & Massen, J. J. M. (2020). Crows (Corvus corone ssp.) check contingency in
a mirror yet fail the mirror-mark test. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 134, 158–169.
Vargas, J. P., Bingman, V. P., Portavella, M., & Ló pez, J. C. (2006). Telencephalon and geometric space in
goldfish. European Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 2870–2878.
Vargha-Khadem, F., Watkins, K. E., Price, C. J., Ashburner, J., Alcock, K. J., Connelly, A., … Passingham,
R. E. (1998). Neural basis of an inherited speech and language disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences, 95, 12695–12700.
Varin, V. P., & Petrov, A. G. (2009). A hydrodynamic model of human cochlea. Computational Mathematics
and Mathematical Physics, 49, 1632–1647.
Vater, M., Meng, J., & Fox, R. C. (2004). Hearing organ evolution and specialization: Early and later
mammals. In G. A. Manley, A. N. Popper, & R. R. Fay (Eds.), Evolution of the vertebrate auditory system
(pp. 256–288). New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
Vá zques-Salazar, A., & Lazcano, A. (2018). Early life: Embracing the RNA world. Current Biology, 28,
R220–R222.
Venkadesan, M., Yawar, A., Eng, C.M., Dias, M. A., Singh, D. K., Tommasini, S. M., … Mandre, S.
(2020). Stiffness of the human foot and evolution of the transverse arch. Nature, 579, 97–100.
Verdon, V., Schwartz, S., Lovblad, K-O., Hauert, C-A., & Vuilleumier, P. (2010). Neuroanatomy of
hemispatial neglect and its functional components: A study using voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping.
Brain, 133, 880–894.
Vereecke, E., D’Aoû t, K., De Clercq, D., Van Elsacker, L., & Aerts, P. (2003). Dynamic plantar pressure
distribution during terrestrial locomotion of bonobos (Pan paniscus). American Journal of Physical
Anthropology, 120, 373–383.
Verfaellie, M., Koseff, P., & Alexander, M. P. (2000). Acquisition of novel semantic information in amnesia:
Effects of lesion location. Neuropsychologia, 38, 484–492.
Verhoef, B-E., Vogels, R., & Janssen, P. (2016). Binocular depth processing in the ventral visual pathway.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 371, article 2050259.
Vernot, B., Tucci, S., Kelso, J., Schraiber, J. G., Wolf, A. B., Gittelman, R. M., … Akey, J. M. (2016).
Excavating Neandertal and Denisovan DNA from the genomes of Melanesian individuals. Science, 352,
235–239.
Vesia, M., Barnett-Cowan, M., Elahi, B., Jegatheeswaran, G., Isayama, R., Neva, J. L., … Chen, R. (2017).
Human dorsomedial parieto-motor circuit specifies grasp during the planning of goal-directed hand ac-
tions. Cortex, 92, 175–186.
References 365
Viala, D. (2006). Evolution and behavioral adaptation of locomotor pattern generators in vertebrates.
Comptes Rendus Palevol, 5, 667–674.
Videan, E. N., & McGrew, W. C. (2002). Bipedality in chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and bonobo (Pan
paniscus): Testing hypotheses on the evolution of bipedalism. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 118,
184–190.
Vigouroux, R. J., Duroure, K., Vougny, J., Albadri, S., Kozulin, P., Herrera, E., … Chédotal, A. (2020).
Bilateral visual projections exist in non-teleost bony fish and predate the emergence of tetrapods. Science,
372, article eabe7790.
Villaseñor, A., Bobe, R., & Behrensmeyer, A. K. (2020). Middle Pliocene hominin distribution patterns in
Eastern Africa. Journal of Human Evolution, 147, article 102856.
Vingerhoets, G. (2014). Contribution of the posterior parietal cortex in reaching, grasping, and using objects
and tools. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, article 151.
Vö lter, C. J., & Call, J. (2014). Younger apes and human children plan their moves in a maze task. Cognition,
130, 186–203.
Völter, C. J., Mundry, R., Call, J., & Seed, A. M. (2019). Chimpanzees flexibly update working memory
contents and show susceptibility to distraction in the self-ordered search task. Proceedings of the Royal
Society, B, 286, article 20190715.
Vulliemoz, S., Raineteau, O., & Jabaudon, D. (2005). Reaching beyond the midline: Why are human brains
cross wired? Lancet Neurology, 4, 87–99.
Wacewicz, S., Zywickzynski, P., & Orzechowski, S. (2016). Visible movements of the orofacial area:
Evidence for gestural or multimodal theories of language evolution? Gesture, 15, 250–282.
Wagener, L., Loconsole, M., Ditz, H. M., & Nieder, A. (2018). Neurons in the endbrain of numerically
naïve crows spontaneously encode visual numerosity. Current Biology, 28, 1090–1094.
Waguespack, N. M., & Surovell, T. A. (2003). Clovis hunting strategies, or how to make out on plentiful
resources. American Antiquity, 68, 333–352.
Walker, A., & Shipman, P. (2005). The ape in the tree: An intellectual & natural history of Proconsul. Cambridge,
MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.
Wall, J. D., & Brandt, D. Y. C. (2016). Archaic admixture in human history. Current Opinion in Genetics &
Development, 41, 93–97.
Wallace, I. J., Burgess, M. L., & Patel, B. A. (2020). Phalangeal curvature in a chimpanzee raised like a
human: Implications for inferring arboreality in fossil hominins. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Science, 117, 11223–11225.
Wang, D. Y-C., Kumar, S., & Hedges, S. B. (1999). Divergence time estimates for the early history of
animal phyla and the origin of plants, animals and fungi. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B, 266,
163–171.
Wang, L., Luo, Y., Wang, H., Zou, Y., Yao, H., Ullah, S., & Li, Z. (2020). Azure-winged magpies fail to
understand the principle of mirror imaging. Behavioural Processes, 177, article 104155.
Wang, W-J., & Crompton, R. H. (2004). The role of load-carrying in the evolution of modern proportions.
Journal of Anatomy, 204, 417–430.
Ward, C. V. (2002). Interpreting the posture and locomotion of Australopithecus afarensis: Where do we
stand? Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 45, 185–215.
Ward, C. V., Kimbel, W. H., & Johanson, D. C. (2011). Complete fourth metatarsal and arches in the foot
of Australopithecus afarensis. Science, 331, 750–753.
Warren, D. M., Stern, M., Duggirala, R., Dyer, T. D., & Almasy, L. (2006). Heritability and linkage analysis
of hand, foot, and eye preference in Mexican Americans. Laterality, 11, 508–524.
Watts, D. P., & Mitani, J. C. (2002). Hunting behavior of chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park,
Uganda. International Journal of Primatology, 23, 1–28.
Wei, C., & Pohorille, A. (2015). M2 proton channel: Toward a model of a primitive proton pump. Origins of
Life and Evolution of Biospheres, 45, 241–248.
Weiss, C. V., Harshman, L., Inoue, F., Fraser, H. B., Petrov, D. A., Ahituv, N., & Gokhman, D. (2021).
The cis-regulatory effects of modern human-specific variants. eLife, 10, article e63713.
Welch, J. J., & Bromham, L. (2005). Molecular dating when rates vary. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 20,
320–327.
366 References
Welker, F., Ramos-Madrigal, J., Gutenbrunner, P., Mackie, M., Tiwary, S., Jersie-Christensen, …
Cappellini, E. (2020). The dental proteome of Homo antecessor. Nature, 580, 235–238.
Wende, K. C., Nagels, A., Blos, J., Stratmann, M., Chatterjee, A., Kircher, T., & Straube, B. (2013).
Differences and commonalities in the judgment of causality in physical and social contexts: An fMRI
study. Neuropsychologia, 51, 2572–2580.
Wendruff, A. J., Babcock, L. E., Wirkner, C. S., Kluessendorf, J., & Mikulic, D. J. (2020). A Silurian
ancestral scorpion with fossilized internal anatomy illustrating a pathway to arachnid terrestrialisation.
Scientific Reports, 10, article 14.
Wesley, M. J., Fernandez-Carriba, S., Hostetter, A., Pilcher, D., Poss, S., & Hopkins, W. D. (2002). Factor
analysis of multiple measures of hand use in captive chimpanzees: An alternative approach to the
assessment of handedness in nonhuman primates. International Journal of Primatology, 23, 1155–1168.
Westall, F. (2004). Early life on earth: The ancient fossil record. In P. Ehrenfreund et al. (Eds.), Astrobiology:
Future perspectives (pp. 287–316). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Whelan, N. V., Kocot, K. M., Moroz, L. L., & Halanych, K. M. (2015). Error, signal, and the placement of
Ctenophora sister to all other animals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112, 5773–5778.
White, J. (1974). The yogi in the lab. In J. White (Ed.), Frontiers of consciousness (pp. 77–93). New York, NY:
The Julian Press, Inc.
White, S., Gowlett, J. A. J., & Grove, M. (2014). The place of Neanderthals in hominin phylogeny. Journal
of Anthropological Archaeology, 35, 32–50.
White, T. D. (2002). Earliest hominids. In W. C. Hartwig (Ed.), The primate fossil record (pp. 407–417).
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
White, T. D., Asfaw, B., Beyene, Y., Haile-Selassie, Y., Lovejoy, C. O., Suwa, G., & WoldeGabriel, G.
(2009). Ardipithecus ramidus and the paleobiology of early hominids. Science, 326, 64.
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., … Boesch, C. (2001).
Charting cultural variation in chimpanzees. Behaviour, 138, 1481–1516.
Wiberg, A., Ng, M., Al Omran, Y., Alfaro-Almagro, F., McCarthy, P., Marchini, J., … Furniss, D. (2019).
Handedness, language areas and neuropsychiatric diseases: Insights from brain imaging and genetics. Brain,
142, 2938–2947.
Wickelgren, W. A. (1973). The long and the short of memory. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 425–438.
Wilde, S., Timpson, A., Kirsanow, K., Kaiser, E., Kayser, M., Unterlä nder, M., … Burger, J. (2014). Direct
evidence for positive selection of skin, hair, and eye pigmentation in Europeans during the last 5,000 y.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111, 4832–4837.
Wildman, D. E., Uddin, M., Liu, G., Grossman, L. I., & Goodman, M. (2003). Implications of natural
selection in shaping 99.4% nonsynonymous DNA identity between humans and chimpanzees: Enlarging
genus Homo. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 7181–7188.
Wilke, C., Kavanagh, E., Donnellan, E., Waller, B. M., Machanda, Z. P., & Slocombe, K. E. (2017).
Production of and responses to unimodal and multimodal signals in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes
schweinfurthii. Animal Behaviour, 123, 305–316.
Wilkins, J., Schoville, B. J., Brown, K. S., & Chazan, M. (2012). Evidence for early hafted hunting tech-
nology. Science, 338, 942–946.
Wilkinson, R. D., Steiper, M. E., Soligo, C., Martin, R. D., Yang, Z., & Tavare, S. (2011). Dating primate
divergences through an integrated analysis of palaeontological and molecular data. Systematic Biology, 60,
16–31.
Williams, M. F. (2002). Primate encephalization and intelligence. Medical Hypotheses, 58, 284–290.
Williams, S. A., & Russo, G. A. (2015). Evolution of the hominoid vertebral column: The long and the
short of it. Evolutionary Anthropology, 24, 15–32.
Williams, T. A., Heaps, S. E., Cherlin, S., Nye, T. M. W., Boys, R. J., & Embley, T. M. (2015). New
substitution models for rooting phylogenetic trees. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 370, article
20140336.
Wilson, A. C. V. (2007). The divergence of genes in ciliary photoreceptors (Master’s thesis), University of
California at Santa Barbara.
Wilson, D. A., & Tomonaga, M. (2018). Visual discrimination of primate species based on faces in chim-
panzees. Primates, 59, 243–251.
References 367
Wixted, J. T., & Ebbesen, E. B. (1997). Genuine power curves in forgetting: A quantitative analysis of
individual subject forgetting functions. Memory & Cognition, 25, 731–739.
Wokke, M. E., Vandenbroucke, A. R. E., Scholte, H. S., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2013). Confuse your illusion:
Feedback to early visual cortex contributes to perceptual completion. Psychological Science, 24, 63–71.
Wolbers, T., Weiller, C., & Bü chel, C. (2004). Neural foundations of emerging route knowledge in
complex spatial environments. Cognitive Brain Research, 21, 401–411.
Wolpert, L. (2007). Six impossible things before breakfast: The evolutionary origins of belief. New York, NY: W.
W. Norton & Company.
Wolpoff, M. H., Hawks, J., Senut, B., Pickford, M., & Ahern, J. (2006). An ape or the ape: Is the Toumaï
cranium TM 266 a hominid? PaleoAnthropology, 2006, 36–50.
Wong, P., & Kaas, J. H. (2010). Architectonic subdivisions of neocortex in the galago (Otolemur garnetti). The
Anatomical Record, 293, 1033–1069.
Wood, B. A. (1992). Evolution of australopithecines. In S. Jones, R. Martin, & D. Pilbeam (Eds.), The
Cambridge encyclopedia of human evolution (pp. 231–240). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Wood, B. A., & Aiello, L. C. (1998). Taxonomic and functional implications of mandibular scaling in early
hominins. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 105, 523–538.
Wood, B. A., & Boyle, E. K. (2016). Hominin taxic diversity: Fact or fantasy? Yearbook of Physical
Anthropology, 159, S37–S78.
Wood, B. A., & Constantino, P. (2004). Human origins: Life at the top of the tree. In J. Cracraft & M. J.
Donohue (Eds.), Assembling the tree of life (pp. 517–535). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Woollett, K., & Maguire, E. A. (2011). Acquiring “the Knowledge” of London’s layout drives structural
brain changes. Current Biology, 21, 2109–2114.
Wright, A. A., Santiago, H. C., Sands, S. F., Kendrick, D. F., & Cook, R. G. (1985). Memory processing of
serial lists by pigeons, monkeys, and people. Science, 229, 287–289.
Xing, J., Wang, H., Han, K., Ray, D. A., Huang, C. H., Chemnick, L. G., … Batzer, M. A. (2005). A
mobile element based phylogeny of old world monkeys. Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution, 37, 872–880.
Xue, M., Black, R. A., Cornell, C. E., Drobny, G. P., & Keller, S. L. (2020). A step toward molecular
evolution of RNA: Ribose binds to prebiotic fatty acid membranes, and nucleosides bind better than
individual bases do. ChemBioChem, 21, 2764–2767.
Yamada, E. S., Silveira, L. C. L., Perry, V. H., & Franco, E. C. S. (2001). M and P retinal ganglion cells of
the owl monkey: Morphology, size and photoreceptor convergence. Vision Research, 41, 119–131.
Yapuncich, G. S., Feng, H. J., Dunn, R. H., Seiffert, E. R., & Boyer, D. M. (2019). Vertical support use and
primate origins. Scientific Reports, 9, article 12341.
Yartsev, M. M., Witter, M. P., & Ulanovsky, N. (2011). Grid cells without theta oscillations in the en-
torhinal cortex of bats. Nature, 479, 103–107.
Young, N. M., Capellini, T. D., Roach, N. T., & Alemseged, Z. (2015). Fossil hominin shoulders support
an African ape-like last common ancestor of humans and chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences, 112, 11829–11834.
Yu, H. V., Tao, L., Llamas, J., Wang, X., Nguyen, J. D., Trecek, T., & Segil, N. (2021). POU4F3 pioneer
activity enables ATOH1 to drive diverse mechanoreceptor differentiation through a feed-forward epi-
genetic mechanism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118, article e2105137118.
Zachar, I., Szilá gyi, A., Szá mado, S., & Szathmá ry, E. (2018). Farming the mitochondrial ancestor as a model
of endosymbiotic establishment by natural selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115,
E1504–E1510.
Zaehle, T., Jordan, K., Wü stenberg, T., Baudewig, J., Dechent, P., & Mast, F. W. (2007). The neural basis
of the egocentric and allocentric spatial frame of reference. Brain Research, 1137, 92–103.
Zahnle, K. J., Catling, D. C., & Claire, M. W. (2013). The rise of oxygen and the hydrogen hourglass.
Chemical Geology, 362, 26–34.
Zahnle, K. J., Schaefer, L., & Fegley, B. (2010). Earth’s earliest atmospheres. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in
Biology, 2, article a004895.
Zaksas, D., & Pasternak, T. (2005). Area MT neurons respond to visual motion distant from their receptive
fields. Journal of Neurophysiology, 94, 4156–4167.
Zeitoun, V., Barriel, V., & Widianto, H. (2016). Phylogenetic analysis of the calvaria of Homo floresiensis.
Comptes Rendus Palevol, 15, 555–568.
368 References
Zentall, T. R. (2006). Imitation: Definitions, evidence, and mechanisms. Animal Cognition, 9, 335–353.
Zhang, H., Japee, S., Stacy, A., Flessert, M., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2020). Anterior superior temporal sulcus
is specialized for non-rigid facial motion in both monkeys and humans. NeuroImage, 218, article 116878.
Zhang, L., Peritz, A., & Meggers, E. (2005). A simple glycol nucleic acid. Journal of the American Chemical
Society, 127, 4174–4175.
Zhang, M.-L., Li, M.-L., Ayoola, A. O., Murphy, R. W., Wu, D.-D., & Shao, Y. (2019). Conserved
sequences identify the closest living relatives of primates. Zoological Research, 40, 532–540.
Zhang, Z., & Cui, L. (2016). Oxygen requirements for the Cambrian explosion. Journal of Earth Science, 27,
187–195.
Zhou, C., Wu, S., Martin, T., & Luo, Z-X. (2013). A Jurassic mammaliaform and the earliest mammalian
evolutionary adaptations. Nature, 500, 163–167.
Zhu, C., Wang, Q., Huang, X., Yun, J., Hu, Q., & Yang, G. (2019). Adsorption of amino acids at clay
surfaces and implication for biochemical reactions: Role and impact of surface charges. Colloids and Surfaces
B: Biointerfaces, 183, article 110458.
Zilhão, J. (2007). The emergence of ornaments and art: An archaeological perspective on the origins of
“behavioral modernity”. Journal of Archaeological Research, 15, 1–54.
Zollikofer, C. P. E., de Leó n, M. S. P., Lieberman, D. E., Guy, F., Pilbeam, D., Likius, A., … Brunet, M.
(2005). Virtual cranial reconstruction of Sahelanthropus tchadensis. Nature, 434, 755–759.
Additional picture credits
Most picture sources can be located by web searching the title-license-author triads shown
under pictures in the text. The following are additional credits for pictures requiring them.
Figure 5.4. (2004). Cro-magnons conquered Europe, but Left Neanderthals Alone. PLoS
Biology, 2(12), e449. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0020449.
Figures 6.2, 6.3, 6.4, 6.5, and 6.6. Blausen.com staff. (2014). Medical gallery of
Blausen Medical 2014. WikiJournal of Medicine, 1(2). DOI:10.15347/wjm/2014.010.
ISSN 2002-4436.
Figure 8.1. John R. Hutchinson, Karl T. Bates, Julia Molnar, Vivian Allen, and Peter J.
Makovicky. (2011). A computational analysis of limb and body dimensions in Tyrannosaurus
rex with implications for locomotion, ontogeny, and growth. PLoS ONE, 6(10), e26037.
Figure 9.8. Fidelis T. Masao, Elgidius B. Ichumbaki, Marco Cherin, Angelo Barili,
Giovanni Boschian, Dawid A. Iurino, et al. (2016). New footprints from Laetoli (Tanzania)
provide evidence for marked body size variation in early hominins. eLife, 2016(5), e19568.
Figure 11.1. (2005). Wild gorillas handy with a stick. PLoS Biology, e385. DOI:10.1371/
journal.pbio.0030385.
Figure 12.7. “Response times from Study 1”. Reprinted by permission from Springer
Nature Customer Service Centre GmbH: Springer Nature, Perception & Psychophysics, What
dot clusters and bar graphs reveal: Subitizing is fast counting and subtraction, David B. Boles,
Jeffrey B. Phillips, Somer M. Givens, 2007.
Figure 14.3. M. Petrides and D. N. Pandya. (2009). “Architectonic map of the human
and macaque monkey prefrontal cortex”, appearing in Distinct parietal and temporal
pathways to the homologues of Broca’s area in the monkey. PLoS Biology, 7(8), e1000170.
DOI:10.1371/journal.pbio.1000170, p. 2.
Figure 15.4. A. Goulas, M. Bastiani, G. Bezgin, H. B. M. Uylings, A. Roebroeck, et al.
(2014). Adaptation of “The RM was used in order to construct the connectivity matrices
of the two species”, appearing in Comparative analysis of the macroscale structural con-
nectivity in the macaque and human brain. PLoS Computational Biology, 10(3), e1003529.
DOI:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003529, p. 3.
Author Index