Body Image Among Women With Physical Disabilities Internalization of Norms and Reactions To Nonconformity
Body Image Among Women With Physical Disabilities Internalization of Norms and Reactions To Nonconformity
Body Image Among Women With Physical Disabilities Internalization of Norms and Reactions To Nonconformity
To cite this article: Diane E. Taub , Patricia L. Fanflik & Penelope A. McLorg (2003) Body Image
among Women with Physical Disabilities: Internalization of Norms and Reactions to Nonconformity,
Sociological Focus, 36:2, 159-176, DOI: 10.1080/00380237.2003.10570722
Body norms for women are based on stereotypical gender expectations regarding physical
appearance. Although women in general are exposed to societal standards of the ideal body, women
with physical disabilities encounter unique circumstances in meeting these expectations. Women with
physical disabilities are often stigmatixed because they violate norms of body aesthetics and body
shape. This research examines the extent to which women with physical disabilities internalise body
norms and the reactions these women have to their nonconformity to societal body standards. In-
depth tape-recorded interviews, investigating a variety of social and interpersonal issues, were
conducted with 21 female university students with physical disabilities. Using content analysis, the
researchers examined the interviews for common themes and patterns relating to body image. The
major categories that emerged were (1) awareness of body norms, (2) compliance with body norms,
and (3) reactions to nonconformity to body norms. The study illustrates that women with physical
disabilities have two general reactions to societal expectations of the ideal female body: emotional
responses and stigma management. The lack of questioning of cultural body norms by respondents is
discussed as it relates to the rural, small town study location.
ΜΓ hysical attractiveness and attainment of the ideal body are highly valued in
Western cultures (Cash 1990; Raudenbush and Zellner 1997). Individuals who are
aesthetically pleasing typically receive social rewards and privileges during their daily
interactions (Henderson-King and Henderson-King 1997). Attractive people are often
viewed as possessing such admirable traits as sociability, popularity, happiness, and
confidence (Cash 1990; Fallon 1990). For both males and females, specific qualities of
age and race/ethnicity contribute to the cultural standards of beauty (Cash, Anas, and
Strachan 1997; Molloy and Herzberger 1998). Further, appearance expectations are
related to characteristics of strength, energy, and control over body movements (Abell
and Richards 1996; Cullari, Rohrer, and Bahm 1998; Wendell 1996,1997). Such views
can particularly affect individuals with physical disabilities. In this paper we
illustrate ways that women with physical disabilities respond to body norms, using
Contact Diane E. Taub, Department of Sociology, Southern Illinois University Carbondale, Carbondale, IL
62901-4624. E-mail: dtaub9siu.edu. This research was supported by a University Priorities and
Interdisciplinary Initiative Grant, Southern Illinois University Carbondale Graduate School and Office of
Research Development and Administration, to Elaine M. Blinde and Diane E. Taub. Much appreciation is
extended to Ann Herda-Rapp for her assistance in interviewing, and we gratefully acknowledge the helpful
suggestions and comments of Michelle Hughes Miller.
169
160 SOCIOLOGICAL FOCUS
data from a qualitative study of female college students. To provide a foundation for
our analysis, we review literature on societal views of physical appearance.
Both men and women are affected by the pervasive societal messages regarding
physical appearance (Brodie, Bagley, and Slade 1994). Television and mnganiy» adver-
tisements frequently display body images that reflect a standard that is unattainable
for the vast majority of people (Molloy and Herzberger 1998). Nevertheless, messages
from social interactions and the mass media assign the individual responsibility for
maintaining appearance expectations (Henderson-King and Henderson-King 1997).
Women are more vulnerable to body norms as they are more likely than men to be
judged by their appearance and sexual appeal (Fallon 1990). Cultural pressures thus
encourage females to be overly invested in physical aesthetics (Feingold and Mazzella
1998; Muth and Cash 1997).
Because the ideal body for women stipulates notable thinness, women are at a
greater risk of developing a distorted self-concept (Raudenbush and Zellner 1997). The
degree of satisfaction females feel about their bodies is influenced by the verbal and
nonverbal reactions they receive from others regarding their appearance (Monteath
and McCabe 1997). Body self-perceptions of women also are affected by their attitudes
about appearance, sexuality, health, and physical skills (Cullari et al. 1998; Smart
2001). Further, body dissatisfaction for females increases with weight gain, especially
when they feel the gain occurs in one area of their bodies, for example, on their
stomach, buttocks, or hips (Cash and Henry 1995; Monteath and McCabe 1997).
Overlooked in standards of appearance are variations in body type, height, age, race/
ethnicity, and physical competence. Rather than including diverse characteristics, the
ideal body represents a woman who is young, medium height, slim, nondisabled, and
white (Begum 1992; Monteath and McCabe 1997).
Although women in general are influenced by body norms, women with physical
disabilities encounter unique circumstances in attempting to conform to these ideals
(Asch and Fine 1988, 1997; Stone 1995). In particular, the use of accommodating
devices such as wheelchairs, braces, and crutches counters appearance expectations
(Asch and Fine 1997; Begum 1992). In Bogle and Shaul (1981), accommodating
devices are described as "metal . . . hard, cold, angular, and usually ugly" (p. 93).
Incorporating these devices as an aspect of a woman's physical self may affect her
development of a positive body concept (Stone 1995). In addition, movement restric-
tions and the body shape of women with physical disabilities complicate their con-
formity to ideal body aesthetics (Frank 2000).
Daily interactions for women with physical disabilities can be impeded when
able-bodied individuals believe these women are not complying with appearance
expectations (Smart 2001; Thomson 1997). Deviation from such cultural norms may
result in their being viewed as "defects and undesirable" (Begum 1992, p. 77), as well
as asexual, unfeminine, and childlike (Healey 1993; Lakoff 1989; Lloyd 1992).
Thomson's work (1997) suggested that the body of a woman with a physical disability
BODY IMAGE AMONG WOMEN WITH PHYSICAL DISABILITIES 161
METHODOLOGY
SAMPLE
with physical disabilities. The age range for respondents was 19 to 56 years with a
median age of 35. Nineteen individuals were white; one participant was African
American; and the other respondent was Asian. While 7 individuals had congenital
disabilities, 14 participants had an acquired disability. The length of time with an
acquired disability spanned 2 to 38 years, with an average duration of 14 years.
Disabilities of respondents included mobility impairment (n = 6), post polio (re = 3),
spinal cord injury (re = 2), muscular dystrophy (re = 2), cerebral palsy (re = 2), multiple
sclerosis (re = 1), a cardiac condition (re = 1), spina bifida (re = 1), fibromyalgia, a
condition characterized by chronic musculoskeletal pain (re = 1), achondroplasia, a
condition characterized by delayed bone growth and short stature (re = 1), and
osteogenesis imperfecta, or brittle bone disease (n = 1). Seven individuals did not
require movement accommodations; however, they had limited mobility and often
limped or walked very slowly. Seven of the women utilized wheelchairs, and one
respondent used an ankle brace. The remaining six individuals used a combination of
at least two accommodating devices, such as ankle, leg, and back braces, crutches,
wheelchair, cane, scooter, and walker.
DATA COLLECTION
DATA ANALYSIS
RESULTS
The women with physical disabilities who were interviewed for this study hold
beliefe similar to those of their able-bodied counterparts regarding societal standards
of the ideal female body. Respondents, regardless of age or whether they had a con-
genital or acquired disability, indicated their belief that the body of the ideal woman is
lean. Responses referring to the muscularity of an ideal body included "not mis-
shapen," "firm," "looks stronger, has muscle tone," "not full-figured," "perfect little
bodies," "muscular definition," and "good athletic shape, toned op, looks like a model."
BODY IMAGE AMONG WOMEN WITH PHYSICAL DISABILITIES 165
Even though these women with physical disabilities are aware of body norms,
they believe they cannot achieve the ideal body type because of how they think they
look and move. While participants acknowledged that nearly all women encounter
difficulties meeting societal appearance standards, women with physical disabilities
are unique in the ways that they attempt to attain the ideal body. Respondents,
regardless of type of disability, stated that their lack of compliance with body norms is
related to their accommodating devices (e.g., wheelchairs, braces, crutches, and
scooters), restricted physical movement, and atypical body appearance.
These women indicated that attempts to comply with a lean physique are
hindered by the appearance and effects of accommodating devices. Their comments
linked lack of compliance with body norms to accommodating devices: "I'm heavier,
society's more thin. Since I'm in a wheelchair . . . I'm the opposite of the whole total
picture;" "the back brace, it's an albatross. I mean it's there; you can't miss it;" and
"it's hard to lose weight when you're a female in a wheelchair... you don't get a lot of
activity." Casey, a 45-year-old woman with a mobility impairment, reflected on how
her body image has changed since being in a wheelchair after a falling accident 12
years ago. Prior to this incident, Casey enjoyed an active lifestyle that included roller-
skating, aerobics, and swimming. Distraught about her inability to exercise to lose
weight, Casey commented, "For someone who was very athletic and very active most
of their life — to be in a position where your body won't allow you to do whatever you
want, ifs very frustrating and very demeaning."
Debbie, a 24-year-old woman with cerebral palsy, stated that self-perceptions of
her body are related to the use of both a wheelchair and braces. She indicated that one
effect of cerebral palsy is that she has minimal control of her arm, leg, and trunk
muscles.
When you're sitting down moat of the time, your metabolism isn't a· great, and you work harder to
stay thinner or «tay more normal looking.... I wore brace« on my lege . . . and I need to go on my
walker and walk down the hall and my braces would just clink together, so that aflecta your body
image. You think you're more weird looking.
166 SOCIOLOGICAL FOCUS
Women who do not use accommodating devices reported they could not achieve
the ideal body because of their restricted physical mobility. Respondents who were
older than 30 years were more likely than younger women to indicate that physical
limitations hindered their attempts to comply with body norms. As two such women
noted, "Sometimes I hate my body because I can't get out of bed." "How are you
supposed to stay in shape if you hurt all the t i m e . . . . It's something that's on my mind
sometimes throughout the day that my body doesn't work like I would like it to." With
the onset of muscular dystrophy, Vicky became troubled over her inability to meet
appearance expectations. While striving for a lean body, she admitted to being
hindered by chronic fatigue and a lack of muscle strength. April, a 41-year-old woman
with a spinal cord injury resulting from an automobile accident five years ago,
discussed how her "activity level has changed from sports-oriented affairs to more
cultural activities." Describing how she compares her body to those of other women
her age, she remarked, "I'm not holding my own. . . . I can't run the steps anymore. I
think I could, even at my age, if I didn't have the hip pain, the back pain, afraid of
falling . . . it preoccupies my mind and body."
Other respondents expressed the belief that they cannot attain the ideal body
because of their atypical appearance. However, they continue to strive toward societal
expectations regarding the body. Women younger than 30 years of age were more
likely than women who were older to mention an atypical body appearance as an
explanation for their lack of compliance with body norms. Describing how she feels
about her body, Linda, a 25-year-old woman with osteogenesis imperfecta, stated, "I'm
about the size of a 3-year-old . . . my body proportions — I'm not shapely. . . . I don't
have the perfect shape. . . . I don't like my shoulders, probably because I don't really
have any." In a discussion about the appearance of her disability, Josephine, a 19-
year-old woman with post polio, stated that she cannot meet societal standards
because "both my legs don't look the same and it's different looking."
These women with physical disabilities reported that they are aware of body
norms for women. However, they feel it is difficult for them to comply with such
appearance standards. In their discussions about body norms, these women indicated
two general reactions to their nonconformity to an ideal body: emotional responses
and stigma management.
Emotional Responses
Perry, a 27-year-old woman with cerebral palsy, said, "I get really mad, and that just
makes me want to try all those diets even more." She believes the ideal body is
becoming increasingly "bone-thin" and that her body is "completely deformed . . . a
giant zigzag." Perry, who has been continually irritated about her weight gain over
the past 10 years, remarked, "I don't like my body. . . . I don't want to go through life
overweight the whole time . . . I just want to be thinner."
The pressure to be slim is so intense that several respondents constantly try to
lose weight. Mary, a 25-year-old woman with achondroplasia, has perpetually used fad
diets. Reflecting resentment of body standards, she stated, "I get mad; they're trying
to make us think that we have to look like that and that's ridiculous. I just think
that's wrong." Similar to other respondents, Mary considers her body to be "different
than other people's" and dislikes the portrayal of the ideal female body in magazines
and on television. Although Mary expressed her anger over societal images, she still
insisted, "I wish I was a little skinnier. I wish I could lose weight."
Casey becomes angry about not meeting societal standards because she can no
longer maintain the ideal slim and fit body she had before her accident. In fact, she
noted, "Being overweight bothers me a lot worse than the back injury." In the past,
Casey had engaged in a regular exercise regime, whereas now she is sedentary.
I get really disgusted because, naturally, when you can't exercise or be athletic anymore, you start
putting on weight. And I'd say I'm going to lose this, I'm going to exercise. I'm going to get rid of it.
And I would exercise every day or three days a week for maybe one week or two, and then I would
end up sick and down and back in pain again.
The second emotional reaction of women with physical disabilities toward not
achieving the ideal body is discontentment. Throughout the interviews, the respond
ents' comments about body size and shape revealed feelings of inadequacy about their
bodies and low self-esteem. Women who were older than 30 years of age were more
likely than younger women to mention feelings of discontentment over not attaining
an ideal body. When discussing how she cannot meet societal expectations of body
norms, April stated that having a disability negatively affects her self-image,
especially as a woman. She said, "[the disability] interferes with your ability to feel
your femininity, your motherhood, what I feel makes a woman a woman. Yes, I feel
like it violates or [that] it does interfere with [being a woman]." Marsha, a 41-year-old
woman with a mobility impairment, described how feelings about her body have
changed after her car accident. She now walks with a distinct limp and believes her
body is incapable of meeting both physical demands and appearance expectations.
I feel bad; it doesn't work anymore. I feel my body's bad; I have a negative perception of i t I don't
have the type of body that society classifies as desirable which makes me feel undesirable . . . so,
yeah, I feel bad. I don't fit the standard norm. I guese I have a hard time being okay with the way I
am.
Vicky discussed how negative self-perceptions about her body affected her inter
actions with other individuals. Avoiding social situations, she believes her disability
"really sent my self-esteem way down, and I'd stay in the house all week long for
months." Similar to Vicky, Sara, a 41-year-old woman with post polio, described her
apprehension at forming relationships or appearing in public. Believing she is judged
168 SOCIOLOGICAL FOCUS
by her appearance, Sara considers herself to be shy and "distant . . . afraid to get
close." Sara commented on how she particularly disliked going to the mall:
I always felt like a zombie ii bow I used to describe it to myself. I need to hate if we would go
■hopping and there waa full-length mirrors, or whenever yon had to eee yourself paaa by. I waa just
really repulsed by seeing how I walked or how I looked and how I must look to other people. I never
accepted it well.
Responding to whether her weight gain has adversely affected her self-image,
Holly admitted, "I've been wanting to lose weight for a long time, and nothing has
completely worked. The more it [weight] goes on, the worse I feel." While attempting
to meet societal standards, she acknowledged that "the ideal body and my [body]...
just don't go together, and that frustrates me and that bothers me. And I don't like it."
Stigma Manageaient
A second response toward not meeting expectations of the ideal body is stigma
management, techniques used in an effort to control negative reactions or conse
quences from a discrediting attribute (Goffman 1963). These women with physical
disabilities engage in strategies that offset unfavorable perceptions by others that
their bodies vary from societal standards. As all the respondents have a disability that
is immediately apparent, these women exhibit a discredited stigma. This group of
women, who internalize body norms, attempt to manage tension by minimising
disparaging responses to their body shape and size. The most commonly used stigma
management strategies relate to concealment of their disability, deflection of attention
from their disability, and normalization of their disability.
Women with physical disabilities make efforts to bide or disguise their dis
ability. Concealment strategies include self-segregation, passing, and use of disidenti-
fiers (Elliott et al. 1990; Goffman 1963). These women do not use the technique of self-
segregation because removal from social interactions with able-bodied individuals is
impractical. Similarly, passing, a means by which individuals attempt to manage
undisclosed discrediting information about themselves, is not evident from the
responses of these women. The strategy of passing is not chosen primarily because the
physical disability of all respondents is easily noticed. The degree to which individuals
can pass is dependent upon the visibility, obtrusiveness, and centrality of their stigma
(Elliott et al. 1990; Goffinan 1963).
These women with physical disabilities employ disidentifiers to separate them
selves from visible attributes and other aspects most negatively related to a physical
disability (Goffinan 1963). Disidentifiers are used as a strategy of concealment: The
body is consciously monitored or altered in an attempt to conform to appropriate body
norms. Acts of concealment for respondents involve the strategic placement of their
body and clothing deliberately chosen to hide their disability.
Respondents with acquired disabilities report using the technique of conceal
ment more often than women with congenital disabilities. For example, Megan, a 28-
year-old woman who sustained a spinal cord injury five years ago, attempts to die-
guise her disability during social events by purposefully placing her wheelchair away
from her. Megan explained that she engages in this behavior "to feel like more part of
things. You're not separated by this big piece of equipment." She feels hindered by
BODY IMAGE AMONG WOMEN WITH PHYSICAL DISABILITIES 169
If I go to a party, 111 wear heels and stay off my feet all day long and walk in very slowly and wait
till nobody's looking and lean on something or slowly sit down. Act like I don't have a problem, BO
that I can look good.
be my eyes. I know I have nice eyes. I emphasize them too much, to try and draw
attention away from everything else."
As the third form of stigma management, normalization is a strategy that
attempts to redefine the stigma and reeducate "normals." Stigmatized individuals
may engage in normalization when the strategies of concealment and deflection are
not practical (Elliott et al. 1990). Thus, women with physical disabilities may choose
to confront the stigma directly. The optimal outcome of normalization is a condition in
which the discrediting attribute loses its stigmatizing quality. Although not widely
used among respondents, attempts at redefinition generally emphasize the positive
attributes of the bodies of women with physical disabilities, while reeducation consists
of the women informing others that body norms should be a non-issue. These women
with physical disabilities engage in normalization in an effort to manage the stigma of
not complying with societal standards regarding the ideal body for women.
Respondents who are older than 30 years and have an acquired disability are most
likely to employ techniques of normalization. Strategies of normalization include
being resigned to the appearance of their body and feigning disinterest in the pursuit
of an ideal body.
Some respondents stated that they have learned to adjust to not meeting
appearance expectations. Sally, a 50-year-old woman with a mobility impairment,
believes that self-perceptions of her body have changed since the onset of Lyme
Disease three years ago. She is now more cognizant of her body as she realizes the
physical effects of her disability. Sally commented, "I'm still happy with myself. . .
sometimes I compare myself to ads and stuff.... I don't like to get down on myself but
I want to be realistic."
Like Sally, Josephine expressed resignation about her physical appearance. In
contrast to when she was a child and self-conscious about wearing a leg brace,
Josephine no longer tries to conceal her disability and is more reconciled to the
appearance of her body. She stated, "I like my body. I don't think everybody should be
like that [the ideal]. It's not healthy. I'm not out to please everybody anymore."
However, when discussing societal portrayal of women's bodies, Josephine acknow
ledged that she adheres to body norms. As she has become older, Josephine realizes
she will never achieve "a 10 body" and that her body "doesn't fit up to most average
women's bodies, society's average women." Further, she "feels the pressures of being
the ideal thing," [but] "sometimes I just don't care."
Among these participants, feigned disinterest, or outward expressions that body
appearance does not concern them, is the second type of normalization strategy
employed. Some respondents state that they simply do not care if they meet body
expectations, while others contend that body standards are not of interest to them
because they know they cannot embody the ideal. In a discussion regarding how
comfortable she is about her body, Janet, a 35-year-old woman with multiple sclerosis,
revealed that surgery to correct her limited sight resulted in "a droopy cheek and an
eye that turns out." However, she believes that "beauty comes from within and . . . I'm
doing the best that I can, to make use of what I've got." Janet nevertheless maintain«
that body self-perceptions may affect how other individuals treat her. "If you feel
crummy about your looks... the way you are, other people aren't going to treat you as
well."
BODY IMAGE AMONG WOMEN WITH PHYSICAL DISABILITIES 171
Similarly, Laney has "learned to live with her body" and believes that "what's
inside a person . . . is more important than physical looks or physical movement."
Laney, 45, has ruptured discs and developed bone spurs from a car accident six years
ago. She reflected on how she has "gained about 80 pounds and . . . can't do exercises
to take the weight off because of the hernia and the back injury." Rather than striving
for a slender physique, Laney believes that "the most important thing is the mind and
what you do and how you help people." However, she admitted, "I don't think 111 ever
be satisfied with my body again, even with a weight loss."
Other women profess to be indifferent to appearance norms because they
acknowledge that they cannot meet body standards. However, the women revealed
that they do believe the norms are relevant. Respondents attributed their inability to
achieve an ideal body to their disability. Rose, a 31-year-old woman with muscular
dystrophy, indicated that not achieving an ideal body "doesn't really bother me that
much because I know I'll never be like them." Although she stated that she is not
troubled by appearance expectations, Rose believes she "has gotten fatter" and admits
she has to "worry a lot more about being skinny."
Kat, a 20-year-old woman with a congenital mobility impairment, insisted that
she is "more settled with it [her body], more used to it. It's like, 'this is the way it is.
We'll deal with it.' There ain't much you can do about it, so . . . I've never thought
about it really." However, Kat stated that she should lose weight. Kat appears to
ignore societal pressures and be accepting of her body; nonetheless, she remains
aware of the pressure to conform to body expectations.
DISCUSSION
(Abell and Richards 1996; Feingold and Mazzella 1998). Interestingly, this age-related
motivation may be ameliorated if these women have a feminist orientation. Such a
finding was noted by Tiggemann and Stevens (1999), who reported that when women
between the ages of 30 and 49 internalize a feminist ideology, their self-esteem is less
associated with their adherence to body norms. Respondents who were older than 30
were more likely than younger women to express discontentment in not achieving the
ideal body and to indicate that restricted physical movement hinders their ability to
comply. Demonstrating some concern for compliance with body norms, the younger
participants in this study were more likely than older women to indicate that they
could not attain the ideal body because of atypical appearance.
Women with physical disabilities are more likely to adhere to body expectations
if their disability is acquired than congenital (Smart 2001). The high degree of
reported efforts at stigma management demonstrated in this study could have
occurred because two thirds of the women had acquired a disability as an adult, with
the average duration of having a disability being 14 years. For women who have
acquired a disability, compliance with appearance norms can be more salient as their
bodies become objects of increased scrutiny. A negative body image for these women
might be reinforced by the stereotypes conveyed throughout their able-bodied lives
about women with physical disabilities (Smart 2001).
With an acquired disability, women become new members of a stigmatized group
and learn to manage any discrepancies between their former and new bodies (Wendell
1997). Women with acquired disabilities therefore may adhere to body standards to
divert attention from their disability. In contrast, women with congenital disabilities
might be more likely to accept their body shape and appearance as they place less
importance on the achievement of appearance standards. Compared to women with
acquired disabilities, women with congenital disabilities are more reconciled to their
bodies because they have experienced fewer identity transformations regarding their
appearance and physical abilities (Smart 2001).
Findings from these women with physical disabilities demonstrate their
adherence to the medical model, a view in which the disability is individualized and
defined in biological and medical terms. Such a framework highlights the functional
limitations of the body and the responsibility of the individual to make personal
accommodations and to develop coping strategies (Barton 1998; Darling 2001; Linton
1998). Although the disability is the defining characteristic of women with physical
disabilities, their devalued status is generalized to the entire person (Barnes et al.
1999). In following the medical model, respondents hold self-perceptions that focus on
their body deviations and lack of adherence to body norms. Thus, these women react
to stigmatization by being nonresistant and indicating a need for some appearance
modification and adjustment.
This stigma-based medical model is counter to the social or identity politics
model (Anspach 1979; Barton 1998; Darling 2001). In the latter model, emphasis is
placed on social, political, and cultural obstacles that limit opportunities and choices
for women with physical disabilities. An alternative set of societal values and stand
ards is enacted in attempts to remove such barriers. Rather than embodying
stigmatized identities, women with physical disabilities take a proactive stance to
eliminate biased societal expectations regarding appearance. The focus shifts from
changing women with physical disabilities to transforming societal structures and
174 SOCIOLOGICAL FOCUS
norms (Anspach 1979; Barton 1998; Darling 2001). Thus, appearance norms become
the object of social change in which body choice and differentness are highlighted.
Such new standards alter social interactions with women with physical disabilities
and, subsequently, their body self-perceptions.
The failure of these women with physical disabilities to adhere to the social
model may relate to their rural, midwestern background. Consistent with living in a
traditional and conservative environment, respondents readily internalized appear-
ance expectations and took responsibility for complying with these standards. That
community resources for accommodation are likely to be less common may also
reinforce the women's perception that it is they who must accommodate, not their
environment. The women interviewed indicated a lack of social interactions with other
women with physical disabilities; thus, they do not appear to have developed a
disability consciousness. For respondents, current body norms often emphasize a
disability status in which their disability is labeled as their most salient charac-
teristic. Findings that these women adhere to the medical model counter current
literature that shows a paradigm shift to increasing use of a social model.
Overall, the objectification of women with physical disabilities contributes to
their feeling of worthlessness and negative self-image (Begum 1992; Deegan 1985). By
internalizing societal definitions regarding the body, respondents disassociate
themselves from their disabled identity (Bogle and Shaul 1981; Stone 1995; Wendell
1996, 1997). As body norms highlight strength, attractiveness, and physical compe-
tence, the devalued status of a disability is reinforced and ableism is encouraged
(Lakoff 1989; Lisi 1993; Lloyd 1992). However, resistance to body standards at the
individual, group, and structural levels could promote cultural beliefs that disability
reflects diversity and variation (Wendell 1996, 1997). With a redefinition of disability,
differences in body appearance become valuable rather than deviant or resulting from
individual failure (Frank 2000; Thomson 1997).
Diane E. Taub is a professor of sociology and associate dean of the College of Liberal Arts at Southern Illinois
University Carbondale. She holds cross-appointments in the Department of Psychology, Women's Studies, and
the Department of Family and Community Medicine, School of Medicine. Her research focuses on eating
disorders and individuals with physical disabilities.
Patricia L. Fanflik is currently a senior research analyst, Office of Research and Evaluation, at the American
Prosecutors Research Institute in Alexandria, Virginia. Her research interests include domestic violence, gun
control policies, and workplace violence involving prosecutors.
REFERENCES
Abell, Steven C. and Maryse H. Richards. 1996. T h e Relationship Between Body Shape Satisfaction and Self-
Esteem: An Investigation of Gender and Class Differences.'* Journal of Youth and Adolescence 25:691-
703.
BODY IMAGE AMONG WOMEN WITH PHYSICAL DISABILITIES 175
Anspach, Renee R. 1979. "From Stigma to Identity Politics: Political Activism Among the Physically Disabled
and Former Mental Patients." Social Science and Medicine 13A:765-773.
Asch, Adrienne and Michelle Fine. 1997. "Nurturance, Sexuality and Women with Disabilities: The Example of
Women and Literature." Pp. 241-259 in The Disability Studies Reader, edited by Lennard J. Davis. New
York: Routledge.
. 1988. "Introduction: Beyond Pedestals." Pp. 1-37 in Women with Disabilities: Essays in Psychology,
Culture, and Politics, edited by Michelle Fine and Adrienne Asch. Philadelphia, PA: Temple Univereity
Press.
Baker, Therese L. 1999. Doing Social Research. 4th ed. Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.
Barnes, Colin, Geof Mercer, and Tom Shakespeare. 1999. Exploring Disability: A Sociological Introduction.
Maiden, MA: Blackwell.
Barton, Len. 1998. "Sociology, Disability Studies and Education: Some Observations." Pp. 53-64 in The
Disability Reader: Social Science Perspectives, edited by Tom Shakespeare. London: Cassell.
Begum, Nasa. 1992. "Disabled Women and the Feminist Agenda." Feminist Review 40:70-84.
Berg, Bruce L. 1998. Qualitative Research Methods for the Social Sciences. 3rd ed. Boston, MA: Allyn and
Bacon.
Bogle, Jane Elder and Susan L. Shaul. 1981. "Body Image and the Woman with a Disability." Pp. 91-96 in
Sexuality and Physical Disability: Personal Perspectives, edited by David G. Bullard and Susan E.
Knight. St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
Brodie, D. A., K. Bagley, and P. D. Slade. 1994. "Body-Image Perception in Pre- and Postadolescent Females."
Perceptual and Motor Skills 78:147-154.
Bushy, Angeline. 1990. "Rural U.S. Women: Traditions and Transitions Affecting Health Care." Health Care
for Women International 11:503-513.
Cash, Thomas F. 1990. "The Psychology of Physical Appearance: Aesthetics, Attributes, and Images." Pp. 6 1 -
79 in Body Images: Development, Deviance, and Change, edited by Thomas F. Cash and Thomas
Pruzinsky. New York: Guilford.
Cash, Thomas F., Julie R. Ancie, and Melissa D. Strachan. 1997. "Gender Attitudes, Feminist Identity, and
Body Image Among College Women." Sex Roles 36:433-447.
Cash, Thomas F. and Patricia E. Henry. 1995. "Women's Body Images: The Results of a National Survey in the
U.SA." Sex Roles 33:19-27.
Cullari, Salvatore, Jennifer M. Rohrer, and Christine Bahm. 1998. "Body-Image Perceptions Across Sex and
Age Groups." Perceptual and Motor Skills 87:839-847.
Darling, Rosalyn Benjamin. 2001. "Stigma of Disability." Pp. 482-485 in Encyclopedia of Criminology and
Deviant Behavior, edited by Clifton D. Bryant. Philadelphia, PA: Brunner-Routledge.
Deegan, Mary Jo. 1985. "Multiple Minority Groups: A Case Study of Physically Disabled Women." Pp. 37-55 in
Women and Disability: The Double Handicap, edited by Mary Jo Deegan and Nancy A. Brooke. New
Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Books.
Elliott, Gregory C, Herbert L. Ziegler, Barbara M. Altman, and Deborah R. Scott 1990. "Understanding
Stigma: Dimensions of Deviance and Coping." Pp. 423—443 in Deviant Behavior: Readings in the
Sociology of Norm Violation, edited by Clifton D. Bryant New York: Hemisphere.
Fallon, April. 1990. "Culture in the Mirror: Sociocultural Determinants of Body Image." Pp. 80-109 in Body
Images: Development, Deviance, and Change, edited by Thomas F. Cash and Thomas Pruzinsky. New
York: Guilford.
Feingold, Alan and Ronald Mazzella. 1998. "Gender Differences in Body Image are Increasing." Psychological
Science 9:190-195.
Frank, Gelya. 2000. Venus on Wheels: Two Decades of Dialogue on Disability, Biography, and Being Female in
America. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Gerschick, Thomas J. 2000. "Toward a Theory of Disability and Gender." Signs 25:1263-1268.
Gill, Carol J. 1996. "Becoming Visible: Personal Health Experiences of Women with Disabilities." Pp. 5-15 in
Women with Physical Disabilities: Achieving and Maintaining Health and Well-Being, edited by Danuta
M. Krotoski, Margaret A. Nosek, and Margaret A. Turk. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Goffman, Erving. 1963. Stigma: Notes on the Management of Spoiled Identity. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Gross, Susan M., Henry T. Ireys, and Stephen L. Kinsman. 2000. "Young Women with Physical Disabilities:
Risk Factors for Symptoms of Eating Disorders." Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics
21:87-96.
Healey, Shevy. 1993. The Common Agenda Between Old Women, Women with Disabilities and All Women."
Women & Therapy 14:65-77.
Henderson, Karla A., Leandra A. Bedini, and Lynn Hecht 1994. "Not Just a Wheelchair, Not Just a Woman':
Self-Identity and Leisure." Therapeutic Recreation Journal 28:73-86.
Henderson-King, Eaaron and Donna Henderson-King. 1997. "Media Effects on Women's Body Esteem: Social
and Individual Difference Factors." Journal ofApplied Social Psychology 27:399-417.
Johnson, Nan E. 1999. "Nonmetropolitan Sex-Role Ideologies: A Longitudinal Study." Rural Sociology 64:44-
65.
176 SOCIOLOGICAL FOCUS
Lakoff, Robin Tolmach. 1989. "Review Essay: Women and Disability.* Feminist Studies IS: 365-375.
Linton, Simi. 1998. Claiming Disability: Knowledge and Identity. New York: New York University Press.
Lisi, Deborah. 1993. "Found Voices: Women, Disability and Cultural Transformation.* Women A Therapy
14:195-209.
Lloyd, Margaret 1992. "Does She Boil Eggs? Toward a Feminist Model of Disability.* Disability, Handicap &
Society 7:207-221.
Molloy, Beth L. and Sharon D. Herzberger. 1998. "Body Image and Self-Esteem: A Comparison of African
American and Caucasian Women." Sex Roles 38:631-643.
Monteath, Sheryl and Marita P. McCabe. 1997. "The Influence of Societal Factors on Female Body Image.* 77«
Journal of Social Psychology 137:708-727.
Morris, Jenny. 1993. "Feminism and Disability.* Feminist Review 43:57-70.
Muth, Jennifer L. and Thomas F. Cash. 1997. "Body-Image Attitudes: What Difference Does Gender Make?"
Journal ofApplied Social Psychology 27:1438-1452.
Peters, S. L. 1993. "Having a Disability 'Sometimes'." Canadian Women Studies 13:26-27.
Raudenbush, Bryan and Debra A. Zellner. 1997. "Nobody's Satisfied: Effects of Abnormal Eating Behaviors
and Actual and Perceived Weight Statue on Body Image Satisfaction in Males and Females.* Journal of
Social and Clinical Psychology 16:95-110.
Rybarczyk, Bruce, David L. Nyenhuis, John J. Nicholas, Susan M. Cash, and James Kaiser. 1995. "Body
Image, Perceived Social Stigma, and the Prediction of Psychosocial Adjustment to Leg Amputation."
Rehabilitation Psychology 40:95-110.
Smart, Julie. 2001. Disability, Society, and the Individual. Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen.
Stone, Sharon Dale. 1995. The Myth of Bodily Perfection." Disability & Society 10:413-424.
Strauss, Aneelm and Juliet Corbin. 1998. Basics of Qualitative Research: Techniques and Procedures for
Developing Grounded Theory. 2d ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Thomson, Rosemarie Garland. 1997. Extraordinary Bodies: Figuring Physical Disability in American Culture
and Literature. New York: Columbia University Press.
Tiggemann, Marika. 1992. "Body-Size Dissatisfaction: Individual Differences in Age and Gender, and
Relationship with Self-Esteem." Personality and Individual Differences 13:39-13.
Tiggemann, Marika and Claire Stevens. 1999. "Weight Concern Across the Life-Span: Relationship to Self-
Esteem and Feminist Identity." International Journal of Eating Disorders 26:103-106.
Wendell, Susan. 1997. "Toward a Feminist Theory of Disability." Pp. 260-278 in The Disability Studies Reader,
edited by Lennard J. Davis. New York: Routledge.
. 1996. The Rejected Body: Feminist Philosophical Reflections on Disability. New York: Routledge.