Cognitve Notes
Cognitve Notes
Memories contribute to who we are and it is a complex system and function (Almaraz-Espinoza and
Grider, 2021)
• Memory is defined in psychology as the faculty of encoding, storing, and retrieving information
(Squire, 2009; Zlotnik & Vansintjan, 2019)
Short term memory: Information held very briefly in memory
• Digit-span task - “7 plus or minus 2” (Miller, 1956)
• Chunking concept - (Miller, 1956; Simon, 1974; Cowen, 2000)
Long term memory
Information stored in our brains for period of time, running into months or years (Eysenck, 2009)
• Differs from short-term memory with regards to storage and capacity (Cowan, 2008)
• There are types and sub-types of long term memory
MEMORY MODELS
THE MULTI-STORE MEMORY MODEL (ATKINSON & SHIFFRIN, 1968)
Atkinson and Shiffrin (1968) highlighted modality-specific stores
• Stimulation from the environment is received by sensory stores, some of the information is then
processed into the
short-term store, and then some information is transferred into the long-term store
CRITICAL EVALUATION
The assumption of separate stores have been influential
✓ There is evidence for modality specific stores (brain damage studies -
including the classic KF study, also Spiers et al., 2001)
✓ It has enabled researchers to expand research on the separate stores
The model is oversimplified (e.g. a single short term memory store and
only a single long term memory store)
- Learning can occur without conscious awareness and without rehearsal
- Processing order limitations exists
THE WORKING MEMORY MODEL (BADDELEY AND HITCH,
1974; BADDELEY, 1986; BADDELEY, 2001)
There has been support that short-term memory is comprised of sub-systems
✓ It reports how information is both processed and stored
✓ Evidence for separate short-term components come from brain damage studies
(KM study) and experimental research (dual-task studies)
✓ The model highlights that rehearsal is optional
It is oversimplified
- Subsystems should be further separated (Liberman, 1980)
- Questions remain regarding some components, e.g. the central executive,
and interaction between components
AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY
Autobiographical memory encompasses our recollections of specific, personal events -
Holland & Kessinger (2010)
• The territory of autobiographical memory includes:
✓Everyday memories
✓Self-defining memories
✓Flashbulb memories
✓Memories for traumatic experiences
EVERYDAY MEMORIES
• Known as ‘real world’ memory
• It is based on how individuals use their memory to get on with their daily lives
SELF-DEFINING MEMORIES
• These are based on personal events which can evoke a range of emotions during retrieval
• These memories could be (El-Haj and Gallouj, 2019):
1. vivid
2. emotionally intense
3. repetitively recalled
4. focus on enduring concerns
5. Include unresolved conflicts
FLASH-BULB MEMORIES
• Where were you when you first heard about the September 11 attacks?
• Flashbulb memories are vivid and detailed memories of dramatic events (Brown & Kulik, 1977)
- Examples include; assassinations, natural disasters, terrorist attacks
• Features of flash-bulb memories include:
- Place
- Ongoing activity
- Informant
- Own feelings
- Others’ feelings
- Aftermath
Lead to activation of special neural mechanisms
Why are they memorable?
• Subjected to rehearsal
• They can have a major impact in our lives
• They are associated with very strong emotions
• They do not get confused with other events
• Lead to activation of special neural mechanisms
• However, they may not be as accurate as initially predicted by Brown & Kulik (1977)
Evidence
• Lack of accuracy was supported by Pezdek (2003) - asked students specific questions about the
September 11th attacks
and students had incorrect responses
• Talarico & Rubin (2003) - flashbulb memories are subject to change
• However, accuracy is related to the intensity of the emotional experience (Sharot et al., 2007) - the more
intense the
experience, the more accurate the memory
• MRI studies support the role of emotional experience in creating accurate flashbulb memories (Sharot et
al., 2007)
• Positive events can also elicit FBMs (Bohn & Berntsen, 2007)
TRAUMATIC MEMORIES
• Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is an anxiety related condition that arises from a very distressing
event
• Neuroanatomy impacted in PTSD (hippocampus - reduced volume) - Gilbertson et al., (2002); Logue et
al., (2018)
How does it relate to memory?
• PTSD results in traumatic memory formation
• In such conditions, individuals experience the traumatic event through nightmares and flashbacks
(involuntary
memories) which come through via intrusive thoughts - Reynolds & Brewin (1998)
• Cues can trigger retrieval - which disrupts cognitive functioning
• Leads to avoidance strategies
Explanations for flashbacks:
- Reapperance hypothesis (Neisser, 1967) - the same memory can appear and disappear
without being altered
- Cognitive memory explanations - differences in working memory, with greater working
memory capacity leading to better suppression of unwanted thoughts (Brewin & Beaton,
2002)
- Cognitive model explanation (Ehlers & Clark, 2000)
CHARACTERISTICS OF AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY
Autobiographical
✓ Emphasises personal history recollection and its significance
NEUROANATOMY OF AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL
MEMORY
The neuroanatomical basis of autobiographical memories, include:
Posterior cingulate cortex, Hippocampus, Medial prefrontal cortex, Inferior parietal lobule
When examining the life span distribution of autobiographical memories, three phenomena are revealed:
INFANTILE AMNESIA
• Also known as childhood amnesia
• Shown by the almost total lack of memories from the first 3 years of life; recalling very few memories
from before the age
of 3 and limited recall for events occurring between the ages of 3 and 6.
• Found in adolescence, young adults as well as the elderly
REMINISCENCE BUMP
• Surprisingly a large number of memories are remembered from the years of adolescence and early
adulthood (15-25 years)
• Increased amount of autobiographical memories from youth and early adulthood observed in adults
over 40 – Koppel and Rubin (2016)
Personality continued
Neuroticism
- correlates positively with self function aspects of AM (Ramassen & Berntsen, 2010)
Remaining big 5
- less clear roles (Ramassen & Berntsen, 2010)
• Culture
- Western children and adults often recall more detailed, more specific, and more self- focused memories
- Euro-American adults are able to access more distant/more detailed very-long- term memories Wang,
2001, 2006;
Wang & Conway, 2004).
Culture
- Han et al., (1998) - provides evidence for cultural differences in AM
- Both European and Asian American adults recalled memories that focused less on social interactions
(Wang, 2011)
AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY - THEORETICAL VIEWS
Self Memory System Model - Conway & Pleydell-Pearce (2000)
Autobiographical memory depends on:
1) Autobiographical memory base
- Lifetime periods
- General events
- Event specific knowledge
2. The working self
- The self
- Influence the memories stored within the autobiographical knowledge base
• According to the theory, autobiographical memories can be accessed through generative or direct
retrieval
Self Memory System Model (further developed) - Conway (2005)
• Event specific knowledge is now divided into the
conceptual-self and episodic memories
• This is a hierarchical structure of an overall life story,
including
- Broad themes
- Lifetime periods
- General events
- Episodic memories
CRITICAL EVALUATION
Support for:
- Major goals influence our autobiographical memory (Wioke et al., 1999; Newman & Lindsay,
2009)
- Autobiographical memory has a hierarchical structure - brain damaged patients (Conway &
Rubin, 1993)
- Autobiographical memories and the self are closely connected (Conway & Holmes, 2004)
✓ Neural evidence for the distinctions between the retrieval types as stated in the model (Addis et al.,
2012)
AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY DISTURBANCES
• Mental health
- Depression:
Diminished (and slower) access to positively experienced autobiographical past events (Dalgeish &
Wener-Seidler, 2014)
Do not experience mood improvements following recollection of positive autobiographical memories
(Joormann, Siemer & Gotlib, 2007)
Less vivid and emotionally intense recall compared to controls ( Wener-Seidler & Moulds, 2012)
There have been mixed findings - (Lui et al., 2013)
- Obsessive compulsive disorder:
OCD patients have difficulty retrieving autobiographical memories Vs. Healthy control group (Wilhelm et
al., 1997)
OCD patients showed longer retrieval latencies when retrieving memories (Wilhelm et al., 1997)
May reflect co-morbid symptoms rather than OCD per se
Mental health
- Boderline personality disorder (BPD):
BPD patients demonstrate impaired autobiographical memory functioning (Beran, Richman & Unoka,
2018)
Recall greater negative life events compared to healthy controls (Renneberg et al., 2005)
Autobiographical memories emphasise rejection (Rosenbach & Renneberg, 2015)
• Cognitive conditions; Amnesia
- Dissociative Fuge
Loss of autobiographical memories; Patient NN - Hennig-Fast et al., (2008)
Severe loss and uniform loss of memories for facts and events across all time period (Harrison et al.,
2017)
Neuropsychiatrics conditions
- Confabulation
Generating false memories without intending to deceit (Wiggins & Bunin, 2020)
Associated with frontal lobe diseases (AD and Korsakoff’s Syndrome) and can also effect the average
person too
Impacts autobiographical memories the most (Barba & Denes 1990)
Types include: provoked type & spontaneous type
Dorsal stream:
- Stretches from the primary visual cortex (V1) in the occipital lobe and flows to the parietal lobe
- Guides actions by recognising where objects are in space
- Known as the ‘Where’ pathway
Ventral stream:
- Stretches from the primary visual cortex (V1) to the inferior temporal lobe
- It is involved in object identity (Sheth & Young, 2016)
- Known as the ‘what’ pathway
- Plays a crucial role in processing
EVIDENCE FOR THE TWO STREAMS AND THEIR ROLES
PROCESSING DIRECTIONS
• Top-down processing
• Bottom-up processing
Bottom-up processing begins with the retrieval of sensory information from our external environment to
build perceptions based on the current input of sensory information. Top-down processing is the
interpretation of incoming information based on prior knowledge, experiences, and expectations.
PATTERN RECOGNITION
A pattern is an object
• During recognition, given objects are assigned to prescribed classes
• Pattern recognition is the ability to identify two-dimensional patterns (e.g. letters, symbols)
• It is also the process of matching perceptual info with long term memory info
• Includes serial processing and parallel processing
• Our perceptual system is flexible
PERCEPTUAL ORGANISATION
• This is a fundamental issue in visual perception
• Eysenck (2008) noted that it is based on our ability to accurately work out:
1. Which parts of visual information presented to us belong together and thus form objects
2. Which parts should be nearer and which further away
• Based on the process of grouping visual elements to form a whole
• Studied by the Gestaltist’s (Koffka, Kohler & Wertheimer) who led the Gestalt Psychology movement,
and their primary principle for perceptual organisation was the law of Pragnanz.
LAW OF PRAGNANZ
• Law of Pragnanz: “Of several geometrically possible organisations that one will actually occur which
possesses the best, simplest and most stable shape” - Koffka, 1953, p.138
• Humans will naturally perceive the simplest organisation of their visual environment, which is a whole
pattern, rather than the individual elements (Law of Prä gnanz).
• There are predictable ways in which our brain tries to perceive a whole, these have been translated into
a set of laws/ principles to explain perceptual organization
Points to consider:
- There is a lot of overlap between problem solving and expertise
- Knowledge seems to be more important in research in expertise, rather than problem solving. And
individual differences seem to be of interest in the expertise research, rather than problem solving
- Expertise research mostly focuses on what the differences are between experts and novices regarding
their knowledge and problem solving skills
PROBLEM SOLVING
• Problem solving is vital because it is “a crossroads, where many different processes come together in
the service of the needs and goals of an individual” (Weisberg, 2018, p. 607).
• There are 3 major aspects to problem solving:
1) it is purposeful
2) It involves controlled processes; doesn’t always rely on “automatic” processes.
3) A problem exists when someone lacks the relevant knowledge to produce an immediate solution.
An example of an insight problem is the mutilated draftboard (or chessboard, or checkerboard) problem:
INSIGHT - EVIDENCE
The research surrounding insight:
• Metcalfe and Wiebe (1987) – measured feelings of “warmth” in insight and non-insight problems.
• Kounios & Beeman (2014) – insight associated with anterior cingulate cortex and pre-frontal cortex.
• Ellis at al. (2011) – used an eye tracker to measure insight during an anagram task.
Some heuristics and strategies that humans use during problem solving include:
1. Means-end analysis (Newell & Simon, 1972) - based on creating a subgoal to reduce the difference
between the
current state and the goal state.
2. Hill climbing (Newell & Simon, 1972) - focus on making moves that bring you closer to the goal
3. Meta-reasoning - monitoring processes that influence the time, effort and strategies used during
reasoning and
problem solving.
4. Progress monitoring (McGregor et al., 2001) - enables one to change their strategy if needed
5. Planning is also critically involved in problem solving (Goel and Grafman, 1995)
EXPERTISE
• In expertise, knowledge and skills are developed over years of practice and people become experts in
their field
• Expertise involves extremely high level thinking
• Research on expertise has mainly focused on:
1) Chess experts
2) Medical experts
• Expertise is usually assessed using knowledge-rich problems
CHESS EXPERTISE
• It takes 10 years of intense practise to become a chess master - Chase & Simon (1973). Chess masters
recall the chess board positions much more accurately than less expert players (De Groot, 1965).
The template theory (Gobet & Waters, 2003)
• This theory proposes the notion that chunks that are used frequently develop into more complex data
structures known as templates:
- These are schematic structures consisting of a core and slots. They typically store information relating
to ten pieces or more. They are flexible and adaptable due to slots.
✓ The reason why expert chess players can complete games of chess very quickly is because of the
superior template knowledge they have and they also have better recall for random positions, than non-
experts.
Slow strategic search processes are used more by expert players than the theory assumes
- The precise nature of what is stored in long-term memory for chess experts is controversial
- Gregor and Howes (2002) showed that the attack and defence relationships between pieces may be
more
Important
MEDICAL EXPERTISE
• Medical experts use implicit reasoning; novices use explicit reasoning (Engel, 2008). Eye-tracking good
for determining the focus of attention while examining case slides. Krupinski et al. (2013) - found there
was a significant reduction in the number of fixations per slide and there was less examinations of non-
diagnostic regions. Kundel et al. (2007) – tracked eye movements during examination of mammograms
and found fast fixation was excellent predictor of high performance.
Gegenfurtner et al. (2011) identified several differences between experts and non-experts that were
common across domains:
- shorter fixations, faster first fixations on task-relevant information, more fixations on task-relevant
information, fewer fixations on task-irrelevant areas, longer saccades (rapid eye movements).
• Supports the information-reduction hypothesis (Haider & Frensch, 1999) and the holistic model
(Kundel et al., 2007).
• Melo et al. (2012) suggested that experts use visual strategy, whilst non-experts use a more analytical
approach.
READING
Reading is an important skill, and it is therefore important for us to understand the processes involved in
reading (Eysenck & Keane, 2020), which can include:
- Perceptual processes
- Cognitive processes
• It is important to note, that reading can sometimes be more difficult than speech perception because:
1) Reading lacks prosodic cues (pitch, intonation, stress and timing)
2) Reading lacks body language and gestures
Processes and structures involved in reading comprehension include (Perfetti & Stafura, 2014)
1. Various kinds of stored information;
- Lexicon
- Orthograpic knowledge
- Knowledge about the world
- Linguistic knowledge
Processes and structures involved in reading comprehension include (Perfetti & Stafura, 2014)
EEG methods and time-locked EEG activity (i.e. ERPs - event related potentials): monitoring brain activity
(Rabovsky et al., 2018)
READING - WORD RECOGNITION
The Processes involved in word recognition:
The Interactive Activation Model - proposed by McClelland & Rumelhart, (1981):
• Bottom up and top down processes interact to recognise words
• It consists of a feature level, letter level and word level
• This model has been very influential (Harley, 2013)
• The model accounts for effects including:
1. word superiority effect
2. effects of orthographic neighbours
Orthographic neighbours: With reference to the target word, the number of words that can be formed by
changing one of its letters.
• Large neighbourhood word: CAT (neighbours include: MAT, SAT, FAT)
• Small neighbourhood word: Hippopotamus (no neighbours)
• Orthographic neighbours influence recognition time.
• Orthographic neighbours facilitate word recognition, if they are less frequent in the language than the
target word itself. However, they have an inhibitory effect if they are more frequent than the target word
(Chen & Mirman, 2012).
CRITICAL EVALUATION
The theory has been very influential (Harley, 2013)
✓ Good example of how connectionist processing system can be applied to visual word recognition
✓ The model explains the word superiority effect and orthographic neighbours effect. What about
semantics? (Meyer & Schvaneveldt, 1971)
- What about phonological processing? (Rastle and Brysbaert, 2006)
- It does not account for the role of context (Kutas et al., 2011)
- Puts too much importance on letter order
- The model only accounts for 4 letter words
READING DEFICITS
DYSLEXIA
- Also known as a reading disability (Siegel, 2016)
- Causes impairments in speed and accuracy of word decoding
- Impacted comprehension
- It is a stable condition (Bruck, 1990)
Explanations:
• Visual difficulties or problems with hand-eye coordination (Orton, 1925)
• Dual route theory (Colthart et al., 1993) - Most dyslexics have difficulty with phonological route because
they lack phonological skills:
- Supported by Lundberg, Frost & Peterson (1998)
- Supported by Elbro & Petersen (2004)
Genetics play a role (Pennington, 1991; Seigel, 2006):
- Chromosomes 6
- Chromosome 15
SPEECH PERCEPTION
Most important form of auditory perception, and differs from other kinds of auditory perception (left-
hemisphere dominance).
• “The process in which a listener decodes, combines, and converts an incoming stream of otherwise
meaningless sound created by the speech production process into a meaningful sequence and
phonological representation” - APA, (2020)
• Speech perception can sometimes be more difficult than reading because:
- Reading can be done at own pace, whereas some people speak fast
- In reading words are spaced out; in speech hard to tell when one word ends and the other begins
- Background noises can be distracting in speech
- Words no longer available in speech – so demands are greater on STM
• Speech perception involves several stages:
1) Decoding speech signals
2) Identification of phonemes or syllables
3) Word identification
4) Interpretation
5) Integration with context
COPING WITH LISTENING PROBLEMS
Understanding speech not very straightforward (Mattys et al., 2012) Ways we cope:
1. Segmentation:
- Dividing the almost continuous sounds of speech into separate phonemes and words
- Relies on top-down and bottom-up cues
2. Lip reading:
- The McGurk Effect: auditory illusion that demonstrates interaction between hearing and vision in
speech perception (McGurk & McDonald, 1976)
- McGurk Effect occurs when crucial word presented in semantically congruent sentence (Windmann,
2004)
SPEECH PERCEPTION DISORDER
Auditory processing disorder
- These individuals have poor recognition of speech in noise (Lagace, Jutras & Gagne, 2010)
- Difficulty in complex listening situations
- Difficulty comprehending verbal instructions
- It is not a hearing problem (NHS, 2020)
• Auditory agnosia
- Impairments in sound perception despite intact hearing (Slevc & Shell, 2015)
- It can affect all types of sound perception or it can be specific to a particular domain
- Different types of therapy has been used in treatment, e.g. speech therapy and training with musical
sounds (Kim et al., 2018)
LANGUAGE COMPREHENSION
Higher level processes involved in language comprehension (e.g. sentences, stories) are generally the
same whether you are reading or listening.
• There are two main levels of analysis
1. Syntactical structure of a sentence (i.e. parsing):
- Syntax: involves the formation of sentences and pays close attention to the word order
- Grammar: concerned with the way in which words are combined, but also includes punctuation, etc
2. Meaning of sentence (i.e. semantics)
- Pragmatics: the study of intended meaning
- Sometimes the literal meaning is not intended (e.g. irony, sarcasm, and understatement)
PARSING
Parsing is analysis of the syntactical or grammatical structure of sentences.
• Research into parsing looks at the relationship between syntactic and semantic analysis:
- Syntactic analysis precedes semantic analysis
- Semantic analysis occurs prior to syntactic analysis
- Syntactic and semantic analysis occur simultaneously
- Syntax and semantics are very closely associated
• A major problem with research on parsing is that it has been based on the English language, which
methodologically impacts research findings
• Ambiguous sentences are used to study parsing.
PROSODIC CUES
Prosodic cues include stress, intonation, pauses and duration
• Prosodic cues are used by listeners to work out the grammatical structure of speech
- Example: John, go to the library for me (Hirotani et al., 2006)
• Studies have demonstrated that reading silently also involves prosodic cues (Steinhaur & Friederici,
2001)
PARSING MODELS
There are different models of parsing
1. Two-stage, serial processing theories
- Parser only entertains one syntactic structure at a time
2. One-stage, parallel processing theories
- Parser entertains multiple syntactic structures at a time
GARDEN PATH MODEL - FRAZIER & RAYNER (1982)
Readers and listeners can be “led up the garden path” (i.e. misled), by ambiguous sentences
• An example - “The horse raced past the barn fell”:
- This can be tricky to understand
- We already concluded that “the horse raced past the barn” was a complete sentence, then when we read
“fell” it didn’t make sense.
• The model assumes that meanings of words do not influence the initial assignment of grammatical
structure, only one meaning considered at a time and simplest syntactical structure chosen first.
CRITICAL EVALUATION
There is support for the principle of minimal attachment (Fraizer & Rayner, 1982):
- the girl knew the answer by heart
- The girl knew the answer was wrong
✓ Evidence that readers’ use of late closure was shown by van Gompel and Pickering (2001). Example:
“After the child had sneezed the doctor prescribed a course of injections”.
✓ Semantic dementia didn’t affect detecting grammatical violations (Bredin & Saffran, 1999)
Visual context is important in influencing initial parsing (Spivey et al., 2002)
- Meaning of words does influence the initial assignment of grammatical structure (Trueswell et al., 1994)
- Pauker et al. (2012) found that misleading prosody can prevent principles of minimal attachment and
late closure:
- “When a bear is approaching the people, the dogs come running” – coincides with sentence structure
- “When a bear is approaching, the people, the dogs come running” – doesn’t coincide with sentence
structure and contains misleading pauses
- Here, participants accepted meaning of 1 over 2
CONSTRAINT-BASED MODEL - MCDONALD ET AL., (1984)
All sources of information or constraints are available immediately to someone processing a sentence
(e.g.
syntactic, semantic, general world knowledge). These are called constraints as they limit, or constrain, the
number of possible interpretations
• The model assumes that all relevant information is immediately available to the parser
• The model assumes parallel processing
The model assumes flexibility in parsing decisions, because the information we attend to depends on past
linguistic experience
• According to the theory, the processing system uses four language characteristics to resolve sentence
ambiguities:
1. Grammatical knowledge constrains interpretations
2. Information associated with a word is typically not independent
3. A word may be more ambiguous in some ways than in others
4. Interpretations generally differ in probability on the basis of past experience
CRITICAL EVALUATION
More efficient to use all relevant information. Word meaning (Trueswell et al., 1994) and context (Spivey
et al., 2002) are used.
✓ Evidence shows we can consider more than one syntactic structure at a time (Cai et al., 2012).
✓ Wilson & Garnsey (2009) found that previous experience with verbs had an effect on sentence
processing.
✓ Brysbaert and Mitchell (1996) found there were individual differences among Dutch people in their
parsing decisions.
EYEWITNESS MISIDENTIFICATION
CONFIRMATION BIAS
Confirmation bias is the tendency to process information by looking for, or interpreting, information that
is consistent with one’s existing beliefs (Casad, 2019)
• Examples (VeryWell Mind, 2021): Not seeking out objective facts and Interpreting information to
support your own beliefs
Remembering details upholding your beliefs and Ignoring information challenging your beliefs.
Lindholm & Christianson (1998) showed that participants were more likely to pick innocent immigrant
over innocent Swede to be the culprit in a robbery simulation
SCHEMAS
Bartlett (1932) proposed that people have schemata, or unconscious mental structures, which store
general knowledge of the world in long-term memory
• Schemas stored in long-term memory can lead us to form certain expectations – can distort memory by
causing us to reconstruct an event’s details based on ‘what must be true’ (Barlett, 1932)
• Tuckey and Brewer (2003) found that eye-witnesses recalled information from a simulated bank
robbery only if it matched their existing schema.
CHANGE BLINDNESS
Change blindness has been defined as an inability to notice changes in the environment (Simons &
Chabris, 1995; Simons & Levin,1998)
Research shows that people can miss large changes in a scene and this can result in misidentifying
innocent
individuals as perpetrators of crimes (Davies & Hine, 2007; Davies et al., 2008).
• Davies & Hine (2007) showed that if eyewitnesses are primed to pay attention to a crime scene, then
they are more likely to detect change and make a successful identification.
• Nelson et al. (2011) found that change blindness led to misidentification. Crime severity also affected
eyewitness accuracy (more severe crimes, led to fewer errors).
WEAPONS FOCUS
Weapons focus is the increased likelihood to attend to the weapon than other details of the event (Loftus
et al., 1987).
• Theoretical explanations of weapons focus:
- The arousal hypothesis (Loftus, 1980; Peters, 1988) which is supported by the Cue-Utilisation model
(Easterbook, 1959)
- Threat superiority effect (Fox, 2006; Eastwood, 2001)
- Unusual objects hypothesis (Shaw & Skolnick, 1994; Mitchell et al.,1998)
Loftus et al. (1987) showed that participants focused on a gun more than a cheque and remembered less
details unrelated to the gun in the gun scenario.
• Biggs et al. (2013) found that participants looked more at guns than faces. But if they were holding guns
themselves, in a readily usable position, they looked at faces more than objects.
Biological research:
- Stress & arousal can strengthen memories due to epinephrine & cortisol release (Roozendaal &
McGaugh, 2011)
- However, too much stress can result in reduced memory (Adreano & Cahill, 2006). Depends on
individual’s stress response
• Meta-analysis (Deffenbacher et al., 2004) found:
- Culprit face identification: 54% in low-anxiety conditions vs. 42% in high-anxiety conditions
- Average proportion of details remembered: 64% in low-stress conditions vs. 52% in high-stress
conditions
MISINFORMATION EFFECT
The misinformation effect is the distorting effect on eye-witness memory of misleading information
presented after a crime
• Terminology: - Proactive interference - old memories interfere with retrieval of new memories
- Retroactive interference - new memories interfere with the retrieval of old memories and information
The wording used by interviewers can affect the memory of witnesses (e.g. “smashed” instead of
“bumped”; Loftus & Palmer, 1974) (retroactive interference).
• Ecker et al. (2011) - The misinformation effect was still present when participants were warned not to
be influenced by misleading information
• Lindsay et al. (2004) - Misinformation before an event can also lead to distortion of memory (proactive
interference).
• Dalton & Daneman (2006) argued that misinformation effect only occurs with peripheral information,
not central information.
Theoretical explanations:
• Source misattribution (aka source monitoring errors): the misidentification of the origin of a memory
(Johnson et al., 1993). Evidence: Lindsay et al. (2004)
• Reconsolidation: misleading information reactivates the original memory, making it open to change.
Evidence: Chan & Lapglia (2013)
Vacant slot information: related information from the original event was not stored in memory (Wright &
Loftus, 2008).
• Blend explanation: information from the original event and misinformation are combined together in
memory (Wright & Loftus, 2008).
SOURCE MONITORING ERRORS (OR MISATTRIBUTION ERRORS)
• Inaccuracies in eyewitness memories can result from misattributing where the information came from
(Johnson et al., 1993; Johnson, 1997; Zaragoza & Lane, 1994)
• Three types of source monitoring:
1. External source monitoring – distinguishing between two different external information sources.
2. Reality source monitoring – distinguishing between what happened and what one imagined; also
discriminating between what was experienced (internal source) and what was communicated (external
source).
3. Internal source monitoring – distinguishing between one’s actions and thoughts
UNCONSCIOUS TRANSFERENCE
• This refers to when an eyewitness misidentifies a familiar but innocent person as the culprit (Loftus,
1976).
• Read et al. (1990) defined the concept as “the transfer of one person’s identity to that of another person
from a different setting, time, or context”
• Ross et al. (1994) reported unconscious transference can occur as a result of:
1. Automatic processing
2. Deliberate source-monitoring at retrieval
3. Conscious inference at encoding
SOCIAL FACTORS
• Interaction with other eyewitnesses can lead to misinformation effect (Gabbert et al., 2004)
• Eyewitnesses communicating together after a crime can (Gabbert et al., 2003; Paterson & Kemp, 2006;
Takarangi et al., 2006):
1. Reinforce common memories
2. However, it can also contaminate each other’s memories
VERBAL SHADOWING
• Putting an experience into words can result in failures of memory about that experience, whether it be
the memory of a person’s face, the colour of an object, or the speed that a car was going (Schooler &
EngstlerSchooler, 1990)
• Meissner & Brigham (2001) found that eliciting elaborate and detailed descriptions of a previously seen
face were more likely to demonstrate verbal overshadowing.
Theoretical explanations:
1. Recoding interference or content account (Schooler & Engstler-Schooler, 1990; Meissner et al., 2001):
generate and later rely upon an inadequate verbal description of the content of their original non-verbal
memory of the face.
3. Transfer-inappropriate processing shift account (Schooler et al., 1997; Schooler, 2002): verbalisation
encourages featural processing, rather than configural processing of the face, which is less effective (e.g.,
Valentine, 1988; Tanaka & Farah, 1993).
5. Criterion shift account (Clare & Lawandowsky, 2004): verbalization induces caution in people’s
responses.
OWN-RACE BIAS (OR SAME-RACE BIAS)
• People show better recognition memory for their own race compared to other races.
• Majority of eyewitness misidentifications may be due to own-race bias (Innocence Project, 2020)
• Contact Hypothesis: argues that we recognise own race better as we have had more exposure to faces
from the same race (Brigham & Malpass, 1985; Slone, Brigham & Meissner, 2000; Hugenberg, Miller, &
Claypool, 2007)
More exposure with other races reduces the own-race bias (Brigham et al., 2007).
• Wong et al. (2020) found that level of exposure to other-race faces only accounts for small part of own-
race bias.
LINE-UP PRESENTATION
Simultaneous line-ups lead to more errors than sequential line-ups (Steblay et al., 2011).
• Misidentification is reduced when witnesses are given a ‘not-sure’ option (Steblay & Phillips, 2011).
• Telling the witness that the culprit might not be in line-up reduced mistaken identification (Steblay,
1997).
FROM THE LAB TO THE COURTROOM
Differences:
- Eyewitnesses likely to be victims in real life
- More stressful to be a witness to a violent crime
- Consequences of misidentification are much more serious in real life
• However:
- Lab conditions don’t distort findings (Ihlebaek et al., 2003)
- What we see in labs is an underestimate of what is happening in real life.
Evidence:
• Memon et al., (2010) conducted a meta-analysis comparing standard police interview with cognitive
interview, and found:
1. Large increase in number of details correctly recalled
2. Beneficial effect reduced in high arousing situation and long interval
3. Increased number of incorrect details
Evidence which supports the notion that faces are processed holistically include:
- Face inversion effect (McKone et al., 2006)
- Thatcher illusion (Thompson,1980)
- Part-whole effect (Tanaka & Farah, 1993)
- Composite-faces effect (Richler et al., 2011)
FACE INVERSION EFFECT
• Faces are harder to process when presented upside down
• McKone et al., (2006) found that same effect was not seen as strongly in objects
• Configural-processing hypothesis: inversion impairs the processing of configural information (the
spatial
relations between features), but doesn’t impair the processing of featural information (e.g., eyes, nose,
and
mouth) (Freire, Lee & Symons, 2000)
PART-WHOLE EFFECT
• The part-whole effect describes the difficulty people have in recognizing familiar faces from isolated
features, rather than when the features are within a face (Davidoff & Donnelly, 1990; Donnelly & Davidoff,
1999; Tanaka & Farah, 1993; Tanaka & Sengco, 1997)
• No part-whole effect found in objects (e.g. Tanaka & Farah, 1993).
COMPOSITE-FACE EFFECT
• Richler et al. (2011) showed that it is difficult to ignore bottom half of face
• Murphy, Gray & Cook (2016) composite-face effect diminishes when the two halves are misaligned
When aligned with different lower halves, it is surprisingly difficult to recognise that the upper regions of
the two composites are identical (left top). However, the illusion-induced interference is greatly
diminished when the composites are misaligned (right top). When composite arrangements are shown
upside-down,
little illusion induced interference is seen in either the aligned (left bottom) or misaligned (right bottom)
conditions
ARE FACES SPECIAL?
• The inversion, Thatcher, part–whole and composite effects provide evidence that faces undergo holistic
processing
• We have a lot of experience of faces more than any other object (for the majority of people)
• It is possible that holistic processing is found for any category of objects for which an individual
possesses expertise?
PROSOPAGNOSIA
• The APA (2020) defined prosopagnosia as "a form of visual agnosia in which the ability to perceive and
recognize faces is impaired, whereas the ability to recognize other objects may be relatively unaffected”
• The symptoms and severity of prosopagnosia can vary across individuals
• Often show signs of covert recognition (i.e. face processing without conscious awareness; Simon et al.,
2011).
Explanations include:
1) damage to specific face-processing brain regions.
2) Face recognition is harder, because it involves distinguishing between same category
• Busigny et al., (2010) tested both of these explanations (patient GG and controls)
• Moskovitch et al. (1997) found that patient (patient CK) with object agnosia performed as well as
controls
on face recognition tasks
EXPERTISE HYPOTHESIS
• The FFA is probably involved in expertise, rather than face processing (Gautier and Tarr, 2002)
• Expertise leads to holistic (configural) processing.
• Predictions:
1) Holistic processing occurs for any object in which one has expertise
2) The FFA should be activated for any object category in which one has expertise
3) Young children will have less evidence of holistic processing of faces than older children and adults.
4) If the processing of faces and of objects of expertise involves similar processes, then objects of
expertise should interfere with face processing.
GAZE DIRECTION
• Gives strong cues about intentions and future actions (Baron-Cohen, 1995) by providing information
about where the person is attending to (Clifford & Palmer, 2018).
• Newborns look at faces whose gaze direction is looking towards them, rather than away (Farroni et al.,
2002).
• Infants smile more at direct gaze (Hains & Muir, 1996; Symons et al., 1998). Mutual gaze important for
learning of faces (Blss & Camp, 2001)
Theory of Mind (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985) suggested that interpretation of gaze plays important role in
normal functioning of theory of mind (Baron-Cohen, 1997)
• Research shows that gaze direction detection less accurate in autism spectrum disorder (Forgeot et al.,
2016).
WOLLASTON ILLUSION - WOLLASTON (1824)
• A person’s gaze is pulled in the direction of the head’s rotation
• Suggests that perception of gaze relies on integrating information from the eyes and rotation of head
FACIAL ATTRACTIVENESS
• Facial attractiveness refers to the pleasant emotional experience and approaching intention induced by
a human face (Rhodes, 2006)
• Highly attractive faces attract visual attention (Sui & Liu, 2009; Lindell & Lindell, 2014)
• Memory for highly attractive faces is superior to that of low attractive faces (Zhang et al., 2016), while
another study found opposite results (Wiese et al., 2014)
Attractiveness halo’ or ‘beauty is good effect’: people with more attractive faces are judged more
positively on a host of dimensions (Berscheid & Walster, 1972; Dion et al., 1972; Eagly et al., 1991;
Feingold, 1992)
• Attractive people are given preferential treatment in many life domains (Langlois et al., 2000)
WHAT FACES ARE CONSIDERED ATTRACTIVE?
• Certain facial features are considered more attractive than others:
- Symmetry (Rhodes et al., 1998)
- Averageness (Langlois & Roggman, 1990; Grammer & Thornhill, 1994)
- Distinct faces (Perrett, May, & Yoshikawa, 1994)
- Happy faces (Otta et al., 1996; Ebner et al., 2018)
Certain facial features are considered more attractive than others:
- Sexually dimorphic cues (Perrett et al., 1998; Cunningham et al., 1990).
- Youthfulness (Mathes et al., 1985; Alley, 1988; Henss, 1991, Zebrowitz et al., 1993)
• Strong evidence exists to show that raters agree within and across cultures who is and who is not
attractive (Langlois et al., 2000)
Average faces denote genetic variation, which protect against disease (Lie et al., 2008)
- Sexually dimorphic cues indicate fertility (Perret et al., 1998) and good genes (Perret et al., 1998;
Penton-Voak, Jacobson & Trivers, 2004)
• However, there are some criticisms to consider, Some criticisms relating to the evolutionary perspective
of facial attractiveness includes:
- Good looks don’t always equal good health or fertility (Kalick et al., 1998)
- Many symmetrical, youthful, and sexually dimorphic faces are not attractive (Langlois et al., 1994;
Rubenstein et al., 2002).
The perceptual bias account (Enquist & Arak 1994; Enquist & Johnstone 1997; Enquist & Ghirlanda,
1998)
- Symmetry is found attractive as a by-product of the relative ease with which the perceptual system can
process all symmetrical stimuli
- May be a result of the symmetrical nature of the human visual system (Mach, 1897; Herbert &
Humohrey,
1996)
- We also prefer symmetrical objects (Rentch, 1963) and decorative art (Gombrich, 1984)
Prototypical faces are symmetrical (Jones et al., 2003)
- Predictions: 1) should prefer symmetry regardless of orientation (i.e. upright or inverted); 2) should
prefer
familiar faces over symmetrical ones.
- Jones, Little & Perret (2003) found symmetrical upright faces are more attractive, than inverted
symmetrical faces. Also, symmetrical faces preferred over familiar faces.
UNIT 7: Attention
Attention normally refers to selectivity of processing (James, 1890)
• “Attention is…taking possession of the mind, in clear and vivid form, of one out of what seem several
simultaneously possible objects or trains of thought. Focalisation, concentration, of consciousness are of
its essence” (James, 1890, pp.403-404).
• The American Psychological Association (2020) defined attention as “a state in which cognitive
resources are focused on certain aspects of the environment…"
Attention can be
1. Active (endogenous or top-down processing)
- E.g. When you are looking for your keys; Paying attention to this lecture
2. Passive (exogeneous or bottom-up processing)
- It is controlled by external stimuli
DISTINCTIONS AND DEFINITIONS
Focused attention or selective attention:
- Focusing on one stimulus, in the face of other distracting stimuli
• Divided attention (e.g. dual tasking or multitasking):
- Paying attention to more than one thing at the same time
External attention:
- Paying attention to things in the environment
• Internal attention:
- Paying attention to internally generated information
VISUAL SEARCH
Overt attention: Orienting attention via the movement of eyes towards the focus
- Measured by: eye-tracker (saccades and fixation points)
• Covert attention: Orienting attention to a different part of the visual field without moving the eyes or
body to do so
- Measured by: filtering tasks.
Feature Integration Theory (Treisman & Gelade, 1980; Treisman, 1998):
• There are two processing stages during visual search
1. Basic visual features (e.g. shape and colour) are processed rapidly and preattentively, in parallel across
the visual scene
2. Slower, serial process with focused attention providing ‘glue’ to form objects from features.
Illusory conjunction: mistakenly combining features from two different stimuli to perceive an object that
is not present (Eysenck & Keane, 2012)
• Prediction: Targets defined by single feature (e.g. a green letter or a T) should be detected rapidly,
whilst
conjunction of features are slower to detect and require attention.
Treisman & Gelade (1980) and Treisman (1998) visual search findings:
1. Items distinguished by a core feature (feature condition) pop out regardless of distractors
(bottom-up processing)
2. Conjunctive condition is more effortful as targets share features with distractors (top-down
processing)
3. As distractors increase in conjunction search, the reaction time increases, but no change in
feature search
• Lack of focused attention can produce illusory conjunctions (Friedman-Hill et al., 1995)
DEFINITIONS - CONCEPTS
Mental representations of categories (e.g. of objects or items) stored in semantic memory
• Basic units of cognition
• Combine sensory and motor experiences with the environment into categories (Humphreys et al.,
1988; Kiefer & Pulvermuller, 2012)
• Includes concrete objects and referents (e.g. abstract ideas) (Harpaintner et al., 2018)
HIERARCHIES OF CONCEPTS
• Rosch et al. (1976): Concepts are organised hierarchically:
We tend to use basic-level categories the most:
- Right balance of informativeness and distinctiveness (Rosch et al., 1976)
- Most general level at which we use similar motor movements to interact with category members
(Bourdais & Pecheux (2009)
- Children learn nouns at this level first (Bourdais & Pecheux, 2009)
METHODOLOGY
Category verification task - Participant has to respond to whether a word belongs to a certain category or
not
• Feature verification task - Participant decides if features belong to a word presented to them or not
• Sentence verification task - Participant is asked to make quick judgments about sentences
Familiarity:
- Anaki & Bentin (2009) found that familiarity with famous towers (e.g. Eiffel Tower) leads to fast
subordinate categorisation, as opposed to unfamiliar towers, which lead to basic-level categorisation
Press et al., (2013):
- Participants had to categorise photos of objects very quickly
- Categorisation was at superordinate (animal or vehicle), basic (cat or dog), or subordinate level
(Siamese cat or Persian cat)
- Performance most accurate and fastest at superordinate level
- Least accurate and slowest at subordinate level
- Categorisation at superordinate level faster than basic, because basic contains more information (Close
& Pathos, 2012).
When comparing two concepts (e.g. is a robin a bird?) the following decision-making process is used. All
features are compared to determine similarity
- If highly similar – quick decision - true
- If highly dissimilar – quick decision - false
- If medium similarity - compare defining features – true/false
HUB-AND-SPOKE MODEL
Patterson et al. (2007), Patterson (2016) and Probric et al. (2010) proposed hub-and spoke models of
semantic representation
• Spokes consist of modality-specific regions involving sensory and motor processing
• Hubs unify and integrate knowledge and are modality-independent
• Hubs are located in anterior temporal lobes (Patterson et al., 2007)
Evidence for hubs:
- Binder et al. (2009) found that ‘hubs’ are located in anterior temporal lobes and related regions
- Findings from semantic dementia patients (who have anterior temporal lobe deterioration) show that
they struggle with categorising atypical and pseudotypical items, suggesting hubs are more general
(Mayberry et al., 2011)
Evidence for spokes:
- Category-specific deficits: disorders caused by brain damage whereby semantic memory is disrupted for
certain semantic categories (Eysenck & Keane, 2015)
- Patients with category-specific deficits struggle with living things over non-living things and this may be
due to the different properties of living and non-living things (Marques et al., 2013).
- Cree & McRae (2003) identified 7 patterns of category specific deficits
- Pobric et al. (2010) found slowing in naming times to objects that could be manipulated when TMS
applied to inferior parietal lobule (involved in actions towards objects) to inhibit its function.
Limitations:
- Left anterior temporal lobe involved in language processing, so might not be as general as is assumed
(Mesulam et al., 2013)
- Unclear exactly what info is in hubs (Eysenck & Keane, 2015)
- Unclear how spokes integrate with hubs (Eysenck & Keane, 2015)
- Number and nature of spokes unknown (Eysenck & Keane, 2015)
SCHEMAS
In psychology and cognitive science, a schema describes a pattern of thought or behavior that organizes
categories of information and the relationships among them
SCHEMAS IMPACT MEMORY RETRIEVAL
• Bartlett (1932) argued that schemas play a crucial role in how we remember stories or events
• Brewer and Treyens (1981) found that participants remembered more schema-consistent objects than
schema inconsistent objects when participants were asked to remember objects in a ‘graduate student’s
room’
• Steyvers & Hemmer (2012) found that schema-inconsistent objects were recalled well too
• Van Kesteren et al. (2012) found that schema-inconsistent information is processed in the medial
temporal lobe
ASSUMPTIONS OF SEMANTIC MEMORY
• Semantic memory contains two types of information:
1. Abstract concepts generally correspond to individual words
2. Broader and more flexible organisation structures based on schemas
DISTINCT TYPES OF SEMANTIC MEMORY?
• Bier et al. (2013) tested script memory in patients with semantic dementia. Findings: 2 struggled, 1
didn’t
• Sirigu et al. (1995) found patients with prefrontal damage couldn’t put event related actions of script in
order. Prefrontal cortex involved in goal-directed behaviour.
Consentino et al. (2006) found that patients with fronto-temporal dementia struggled with sequencing
errors, but not semantic knowledge of concepts
Conclusion:
- Research supports notion that there is a distinction between concept and script knowledge
- However, the two are related, thus semantic dementia patients can have problems with accessing script
knowledge.
REPRESENTATIVE HEURISTICS
• The assumption that an object or individual belongs to a specified category because it is typical of that
category (Tversky & Kahneman, 1974)
• We ignore base-rate information (the relative frequency with which an event occurs within a
population)
• Conjunction fallacy: The mistaken belief that the conjunction or combination of two events is more
likely
than one event on its own (Tversky & Kahneman, 1983)
- Linda is 31 years old, single, outspoken and very bright. She majored in philosophy. As a student, she
was deeply concerned with issues of discrimination and social justice, and also participated in anti-
nuclear demonstrations.
• Tentori el al., (2013) argued that the conjunction fallacy happens because the hypothesis is supported
by the description
AVAILABILITY HEURISTICS
The frequencies of events can be estimated accurately by the subjective ease with which they can be
retrieved (Tversky and Kahneman, 1974)
• Litchenstein et al., (1978) asked participants to judge the relative likelihood of different causes of death.
Participants judged murder to be more likely than suicide, because murder attracts more public attention
(the opposite is true)
Pachur el al., (2012) three possible explanations for the judgement on how likely each cause of death is:
1. Availability heuristic based on own experiences
2. Availability heuristic based on own experience and media coverage
3. Affect heuristic (emotion influencing judgement)
• Availability heuristic can be over-ridden using deliberate thought (Oppenheimer, 2004), but not when
under cognitive load (Oppenheimer & Monin, 2009).
FAST-AND-FRUGAL HEURISTICS
• “If we are so dumb, how come we’re so smart?” (Glymour, 2001, p. 8)
• Heuristics can often be very valuable (Gigerenzer & Gaissmaier, 2011)
• Take-the-best heuristic: ‘take-the-best, ignore the rest’
FAST-AND-FRUGAL HEURISTICS
With regards to ‘take-the-best heuristic: ‘take-the-best, ignore the rest’:
• Three components:
1. Search rule
2. Stopping rule
3. Decision rule
• The theory assumes that system 2 is needed for answers that reflect base-rate information, but this is
incorrect (Pennycook & Thompson, 2012)
• The theory has been criticised for being to neat and tidy, with no real evidence that the two systems are
distinct (Keren & Schul, 2009; Fielder and von Sydow, 2015)
Proposed to explain paradox of why people avoid gains when the odds are in their favour
It is assumed:
1. Individuals identify a reference point representing their current state
2. Individuals are more sensitive to potential losses than to potential gains; this is loss aversion
• People overweight low probability events and underweight high probability events
FRAMING EFFECT
• Irrelevant aspects of a situation can influence our decisions (e.g. wording)
• Tversky and Kahneman (1981) used the Asian disease problem to study this:
Imagine that the US is preparing for the outbreak of an unusual Asian disease, which is expected to kill
600 people. Two alternative programmes to combat the disease have been proposed. Assume that the
exact scientific estimate of the consequences of the programme are as follows
In gain-frame condition, participants chose between:
- If programme A is adopted, 200 people will be saved
- If programme B is adopted, there is a 1 in 3 probability 600 people will be saved and a 2 in 3 probability
that no people will be saved
• In loss-frame condition, participants chose between:
- If programme C is adopted, 400 people will die
- If programme D is adopted, there is a 1 in 3 probability that nobody will die, and 2 in 3 probability that
600 people will die. If programme A is adopted, 200 people will be saved
Results of Asian problem study (Tversky and Kahneman, 1981):
- 72% of participants chose the certain gain (programme A) even though the two programmes (if
implemented several times) would lead to saving 200 lives on average - 78% of participants chose
programme D, even though the two programmes would have the same effect if implemented several
times
This means how something is framed or worded can affect the decision
• When programmes were described in full framing effect disappeared (Mandel and Vartanian, 2011)
• Wang (1996) argued that social and moral factors also play a role
• Almashat et al., (2008) found framing effect disappeared when people weigh up pros and cons and
justify decision
SUNK-COST EFFECT
• “A tendency for people to pursue a course of action even after it has proved suboptimal, because
resources have been invested in that course of action” (Braverman & Blumenthal-Barby, 2012, p. 186)
• Dawes (1998) illustrates the sunk-cost effect by presenting participants with a scenario about a couple
who fall ill before going on holiday and have paid $100 non-refundable deposit – what should they do?
Balgia and Ely (2011) argue that participants who remember all information should be immune from this
effect
• Braverman & Blumenthal-Barby (2012) showed that expert healthcare providers who had to make
treatment recommendations did not exhibit a sunk-cost effect
Omission bias: a preference for risking harm through inaction compared to risking harm through action
- Brown et al., (2010) and Wroe et al., (2005) support this in studies of parents making decisions about
getting children vaccinated
• Status quo bias: a preference for maintaining status quo (present state) rather than acting to change
one’s decision
- Samuel and Zeckhauser (1988) supports this bias.
SOCIAL BIASES
• Tetlock (2002) proposed the social functionalist approach: people behave like intuitive politicians, as in
they need to justify their decisions to others
• Decisions with high accountability were more likely to show high sunk-cost effect (Simonson and Staw,
1992)
• Medical experts decision making is more biased when forced to be held accountable (Schwartz et al.,
2004)
• Limitations:
1. Individual differences not taken into account
2. Difficult to relate to real world