The Neurobiology of Attachment From Infancy To Clinical Outcomes
The Neurobiology of Attachment From Infancy To Clinical Outcomes
The Neurobiology of Attachment From Infancy To Clinical Outcomes
NEUROBIOLOGY OF ATTACHMENT
Joanna Chambers
The parent–infant bond has arguably been the most important pro-
cess for human survival. While the importance of the mother–infant
attachment has been understood for centuries, it was not studied until
John Bowlby developed attachment theory in the 1950s. In order to put
attachment theory in proper context, one must begin with a reminder
of Sigmund Freud’s drive theory. According to drive theory, we at-
tached to our mothers because they fed us and gratified our oral yearn-
ings (Freud, 1923). In other words, our attachments were derived from
our libidinal drives and did not exist independently. Drive theory was
largely a one-person psychology focusing on the drives and conflicts of
the individual; our mother was simply an instigator who either grati-
fied or displeased our internal drives and wishes.
Melanie Klein led us to a two-person psychology where the other
person was more than an instigator of drives and wishes. The mother,
in particular, shaped the psychology of the infant through representa-
tions of the good and the bad breast, the merger of the two, and the
hope for reparation that allows us to stay attached to our mothers.
Klein introduced the idea that we need to feel hope for reparation to
stay connected to our loved ones in spite of our own aggressive drives
(Kristeva, 2001). For the first time, the object had relevance in our psy-
chology, though Klein believed that the emotional problems of children
still came largely from the fantasies generated by internal conflict re-
lated to the object.
John Bowlby was a supervisee of Melanie Klein and was influenced
by her, however he argued that children’s emotional problems do not
stem from their own internal conflicts based on a fantasy of the care-
taker, but rather their emotional problems stem from actual experiences
with their caretakers (Bretherton, 1992). Furthermore, Bowlby sug-
gested that the need for social bonds is independent of feeding and
sexual needs but equal in significance (Bowlby, 1977). Attachment
theory suggests that through the earliest relationship with one’s par-
ents, one develops an internal working model of relationships, which
affects the capacity for relationships later in life. These difficulties in
the earliest relationships can lead to marital difficulties, difficulties re-
lating to one’s children, neuroses, and personality disorders (Bowlby,
1977). Since then, insecure attachment has also been shown to lead to
difficulties with other psychiatric illnesses, including depression, anxi-
ety disorders, substance abuse disorders, and several medical illnesses
(Davies, Macfarlane, McBeth, Morriss, & Dickens, 2009; McWilliams &
Bailey, 2010; Puig, Englund, Simpson, & Collins, 2013).
It is clear from the previous section that the first three months of life
are important with respect to the attachment process through modu-
lation of the HPA axis. This is also the time when the hippocampus
fully matures. The hippocampus is a part of the limbic system and is
involved in spatial and emotional memory. It is instrumental in consoli-
548 CHAMBERS
The Amygdala
Even before birth, the fetal amygdala is affected by his mother’s affect
(Qiu et al., 2015). Specifically, both depression and cortisol spikes in the
mother during pregnancy have been shown to decrease the size of the
amygdala later in the infant (Qiu et al., 2015). The amygdala matures at
six to seven months of age (Vela, 2014). With this development, we also
see the beginnings of fear and salience, two important functions of the
550 CHAMBERS
amygdala. The infant at this age will show stranger anxiety and protest
separation from his mother. Youths who have been institutionalized
and suffer emotional deprivation, who are later adopted by families in
the U.S., show a difference in amygdala response when compared to
youths who had been raised by their biological parents without emo-
tional deprivation. The youths, who were 4–17 years of age at the time
of the imaging study, were shown pictures of their mothers (biological
or adopted mothers in the case of the adopted youths; Olavsky et al.,
2013). The youths who were raised by their mothers from birth showed
significant amygdala responses to their mothers but not to strangers.
The youths who had been institutionalized prior to adoption demon-
strated a significant amygdala response to both their mothers and the
strangers. The lack of differentiation became more significant with in-
creased age of the youth at the time of adoption. In other words, the
older the child’s age at adoption, the less likely their amygdala was to
discriminate between the mother and the stranger. This study demon-
strates that while the amygdala is important for a fear response, it is
also important for the determination of salience. The study also vali-
dates the notion that there is a sensitive period of attachment develop-
ment that has life-long consequences.
This leads to the question: “What happens if we remove the amygda-
la?” Bauman, Lavener, Mason, Capitanio, and Amaral (2004) answered
that exact question in a study with macaque monkeys. The macaques’
amygdalae were lesioned at two weeks of age and some interesting
results were found. The monkeys without their amygdala spent more
time with their mothers in the first six months. They were separated
from their mothers at six months of age and placed in a holding box
with equal proximity to their mothers and to a familiar female who
was not their mother. In this box, the control (non-lesioned) monkeys
spent all their time in proximity to their mother. The lesioned monkeys
spent an equal amount of time in proximity to the familiar female as
to their mother. Furthermore, the control monkeys screamed in pro-
test at not being able to be held by their mother, while the amygdala-
lesioned monkeys showed no distress at having limited proximity to
their mother. So while the lesioned monkeys were content with their
mothers in the first six months of life, they had no ability to understand
the salience of their mother when compared to a familiar female with-
out their amygdala.
In humans, amygdala lesions have been described in Urbach–Wiethe
syndrome, an autosomal recessively inherited syndrome that causes
bilateral calcifications of the amygdala in 50% of patients. In an inter-
esting report, the psychoanalytic findings in a 38-year-old male patient
NEUROBIOLOGY OF ATTACHMENT 551
The role of the HPA axis and cortisol is complicated and likely of
great importance in the relationship between attachment security and
medical and psychiatric comorbidity. Individuals with insecure attach-
ment show a greater perception of stress when compared to securely
attached individuals (Kidd, Hamer, & Steptoe, 2011). In general, corti-
sol levels are known to be altered in patients with depression, anxiety,
posttraumatic stress disorder, as well as in insecure attachment (Kidd,
Hamer, & Steptoe, 2010; Quirin, Pruessner, & Kuhl, 2008). It is therefore
reasonable to assume that modulation of the hypothalamus early in de-
velopment has significant effects on the individual later in life.
Interpersonal stress is increased in insecurely attached individuals
and this has several neurobiological causes. In interpersonal stress,
the prefrontal cortex is downregulated and its inhibitory control of the
amygdala decreases (Gold, 2015). In addition, the HPA axis is activated
during interpersonal stress through increased activation of the amyg-
dala, as well as through the lack of inhibition by the PFC, leading to
increased cortisol production. Cortisol further increases amygdala acti-
vation. In addition, corticotropin releasing hormone (CRH) is increased
by interpersonal stress, further increasing fear and anxiety through its
effect on the sympathetic nervous system and the amygdala (Gold,
2015). While the hippocampus generally has an inhibitory effect the
HPA stress response (Radley & Sawchenko, 2011), if the hippocampus
556 CHAMBERS
NEUROCIRCUITRY
The effects that abuse and neglect have on the developing brain as
described in this article are significant. Understanding how poor attach-
ment leads to changes in neurocircuitry later in life is still in progress.
For instance, in individuals with a history of child abuse, the amyg-
dala is hyper-responsive to angry faces (Teicher, Samson, Anderson, &
Ohashi, 2016). This hyper-responsivity may be due partly to a failure to
develop appropriate inhibitory mechanisms during the first year of life.
If the amygdala is faced with an aggressive, abusive, or unpredictable
attachment figure in the first year of life, without appropriate modula-
tion through oxytocin and other neural connections, hyperactivity may
be the result. The reward neurocircuitry also appears to be affected by
attachment, which may have implications for psychopathology later
in life. For example, in depression the nucleus accumbens is less active
which is largely responsible for the sense of anhedonia in depression
(Satterthwaite et al., 2015). It is possible that the altered function of the
nucleus accumbens developed in the early stages of life as a response
to poor attachment increases the risk for anhedonia and depression. An
altered reward neurocircuitry may also lead to a vulnerability to addic-
tions. Decreased function in the PFC, which may be caused by attach-
ment insecurity, also has a negative effect on the reward process by de-
creasing activity in the nucleus accumbens (Gold 2015). Given that the
striatum and PFC are both involved in the development of attachment,
it seems possible that insecure attachment may lead to a dysfunction in
these neurocircuits, leading to later development of psychopathology.
EPIGENETICS
SUMMARY
While much work has been done to increase the understanding of the
neurobiology of attachment, we are left with many unanswered ques-
tions that require more research. For instance, can we affect attachment
style later in life? Can insecure attachment be treated? If so, how? In
addition, can psychotherapy be enhanced with direct neurobiological
modification, such as oxytocin or cortisol modulation? It might be in-
NEUROBIOLOGY OF ATTACHMENT 559
teresting to check both cortisol and oxytocin levels in patients who are
undergoing psychoanalysis or psychodynamic psychotherapy. Would
attachment treatment be a part of the overall treatment to improve oth-
er psychiatric and medical morbidities? Given that we already have
evidence that attachment treatment to postpartum mothers improves
postpartum depression (Hoffman, Marvin, Cooper, & Powell, 2006), it
may be worth considering for other illnesses as well.
Given what we know about the role of oxytocin and cortisol spe-
cifically, are pregnancy and the postpartum periods a natural time to
intervene in mothers with poor attachment? Knowing the importance
of mother’s attachment process for the success of secure attachment
in the infant, it may be that nature has provided us with an additional
sensitive period in the attachment neurocircuitry. This would neces-
sarily lead to significant changes in how we approach women in the
obstetricians’ offices as we would want to check all pregnant mothers
for insecure attachments, oxytocin levels, early childhood trauma, in
addition to the current monitoring of fetal heart rates, etc. Furthermore,
our understanding of insecure attachment in psychiatric and medical
diagnoses may change how we understand and treat these illnesses.
REFERENCES
Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Bell, S. M. (1970). Attachment, exploration, and separation:
Illustrated by the behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situation. Child De-
velopment, 41, 49-67.
Alim, T. N., Lawson, W. B., Feder, A., Iacoviello, B. M., Saxena, S., Bailey, C. R.,
Greene, A. M., & Neumeister, A. (2012). Resilience to meet the challenge of
addiction: Psychobiology and clinical considerations. Alcohol Research: Cur-
rent Reviews, 34(4), 506-515.
Atzil, S., Hendler, T., & Feldman, R. (2011). Specifying the neurobiological basis of
human attachment: Brain, hormones, and behavior in synchronous and intru-
sive mothers. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36, 2603-2615.
Bauman, M. D., Lavener, P., Mason, W. A., Capitanio, J. P., & Amaral, D. G. (2004).
The development of mother-infant interactions after neonatal amygdala le-
sions in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(3), 711-721.
Beebe, B., Lachmann, F., Markese, S., Buck, K. A., Bahrick, L. E., Chen, H., Cohen, P.,
Andrews, H., Feldstein, S., & Jaffe, J. (2012). On the origins of disorganized at-
tachment and internal working models: Paper II. An empirical microanalysis
of 4-month mother-infant interaction. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 22(3), 352-374.
Bowlby, J. (1977). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. I. Aetiology and
psychopathology in the light of attachment theory. An expanded version of
the fiftieth Maudsley lecture, delivered before the Royal College of Psychia-
trists, 19 November 1976. British Journal of Psychiatry, 130, 201-209.
560 CHAMBERS
Bretherton, I. (1992). The origins of attachment theory: John Bowlby and Mary Ain-
sworth. Developmental Psychology, 28, 759-775.
Campbell, S., & MacQueen, G. (2004). The role of the hippocampus in the patho-
physiology of depression. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 29(6), 417-426.
Caspers, K. M., Yucuis, R., Troutman, B., & Spinks, R. (2006). Attachment as an orga-
nizier of behavior: Implications for substance abuse problems and willingness
to seek treatment. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy, 1, 32-41.
Davies, K. A., Macfarlane, G. J., McBeth, J., Morriss, R., & Dickens, C. (2009). In-
secure attachment style is associated with chronic widespread pain. Pain,
143(3), 200-205.
Debiec, J., & Sullivan, R. M. (2014). Intergenerational transmission of emotional
trauma through amygdala-dependent mother-to-infant transfer of specific
fear. Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences, 111(33), 12222-12227.
DeWall, C. N., Masten, C. L., Powell, C., Combs, D., Schurtz, D. R., & Eisenberger,
N. I. (2012). Do neural responses to rejection depend on attachment style? An
fMRI study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(2), 184-192.
Duman, R. (2004). Role of neurotrophic factors in the etiology and treatment of
mood disorders. Neuromolecular Medicine, 5(1), 11-25.
Fan, A. P., Buka, S. L., Kosik, R. O., Chen, Y., Wang, S., Su, T., & Eaton, W. W. (2014).
Association between maternal behavior in infancy and adult mental health: A
30-year prospective study. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 55, 283-289.
Feldman, R. (2010). The relational basis of adolescent adjustment: Trajectories of
mother-child interactive behaviors from infancy to adolescence shape adoles-
cents’ adaptation. Attachment and Human Development, 12(1-2), 173-192.
Felitti, V. J., Anda, R. F., Nordenberg, D., Williamson, D. F., Spitz, A. M., Edwards,
V., Koss, M. P., & Marks, J. S. (1998). Relationship of childhood abuse and
household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The
Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) study. American Journal of Preventive
Medicine, 14(4), 245-258.
Fish, E. W., Shahrokh, D., Bagot, R., Caldji, C., Bredy, T., Szyf, M., & Meaney, M.
J. (2004). Epigenetic programming of stress responses through variations in
maternal care. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1036, 167-180.
Fonagy, P., Steele, H., & Steele, M. (1991). Maternal representations of attachment
during pregnancy predict the organization of infant-mother attachment at
one year of age. Child Development, 62, 891-905.
Fowler, J. C., Groat, M., & Ulanday, M. (2013). Attachment style and treatment com-
pletion among psychiatric inpatients with substance use disorders. American
Journal on Addictions, 22, 14-17.
Freud, S. (1923). The ego and the id. New York; London: Norton.
Giesbricht, G. F., Letourneau, H., Campbell, T. S., & the Alberta Pregnancy Out-
comes and Nutrition Study Team. (2017). Sexually dimorphic and interactive
effects of prenatal maternal cortisol and psychological distress on infant cor-
tisol reactivity. Development and Psychopathology, 29(3), 805-818.
Gold, P. W. (2015). The organization of the stress system and its dysregulation in
depressive illness. Molecular Psychiatry, 20, 32-47.
Gordon, I., Zagoory-Sharon, O., Leckman, J. F., & Feldman, R. (2010). Oxytocin and
the development of parenting in humans. Biological Psychiatry, 68, 377-382.
NEUROBIOLOGY OF ATTACHMENT 561
Hayes, L. J., Goodman, S. H., & Carlson, E. (2013). Maternal antenatal depression
and infant disorganized attachment at 12 months. Attachment and Human De-
velopment, 15(2), 133-153.
Heim, C., & Nemeroff, C. B. (2001). The role of childhood trauma in the neurobiol-
ogy of mood and anxiety disorders: Preclinical and clinical studies. Biological
Psychiatry, 49(12), 1023-1039.
Heim, C., Young, L. J., Newport, D. J., Mletzko, T., Miller, A. H., & Nemeroff, C. B.
(2009). Lower CSF oxytocin concentrations in women with a history of child-
hood abuse. Molecular Biology, 14, 954-958.
Heinrichs, M., Baumgartner, T., Kirschbaum, C., & Ehlert, U. (2003). Social support
and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psycho-
social stress. Biological Psychiatry, 54(12), 1389-1398.
Hoffman, K. T., Marvin, R. S., Cooper, G., & Powell, B. (2006). Changing toddlers’
and preschoolers’ attachment classifications: The Circle of Security interven-
tion. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74(6), 1017-1026.
Huot, R. L., Plotsky, P. M., Lenox, R. H., & McNamara, R. K. (2002). Neonatal mater-
nal separation reduces hippocampal mossy fiber density in adult Long Evans
rats. Brain Research, 950, 52-63.
Kidd, T., Hamer, M., & Steptoe, A. (2011). Examining the association between adult
attachment style and cortisol responses to acute stress. Psychoneuroendocrino-
logy, 36, 771-779.
Kolb, B., Mychasiuk, R., Muhammad, A., Li, Y., Frost, D. O., & Gibb, R. (2012). Expe-
rience and the developing prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Acade-
my of Sciences, 109(Suppl 2), 17186-17193.
Koulomzin, M., Beebe, B., Anderson, S., Jaffe, J., Feldstein, S., & Crown, C. (2002).
Infant gaze, head, face, and self-touch at 4 months differentiate secure vs.
avoidant attachment at 1 year: A microanalytic approach. Attachment and Hu-
man Development, 4(1), 3-24.
Kristeva, J. (2001). Melanie Klein. New York: Columbia University Press.
Levine A., Zagoory-Sharon, O., Feldman, R., & Weller, A. (2007). Oxytocin during
pregnancy and early postpartum: Individual patterns and maternal-fetal at-
tachment. Peptides, 28, 1162-1169.
Lungwitz, E. A., Stuber, G. D., Johnson, P. L., Dietrich, A. D., Schartz, N., Hanrahan,
B., Shekhar, A., & Truitt, W. A. (2014). The role of the medial prefrontal cortex
in regulating social familiarity-induced anxiolysis. Neuropsychopharmacology,
39, 1009-1019.
Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as disorganzied/
disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation. In M. T. Greenberg, D.
Cicchetti, & E. M. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years: Theory,
research and intervention (pp. 121-160). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
McGowan, P. O., Sasaki, A., D’Alessio, A. C., Dymov, S., Labonte, B., Szyf, M., Tu-
recki, G., & Meaney, M. J. (2009). Epigenetic regulation of the glucocorticoid
receptor in human brain associates with childhood abuse. Nature Neurosci-
ence, 1(3), 342-348.
McWilliams, L. A., & Bailey S. J. (2010). Associations between adult attachment rat-
ings and health conditions: Evidence from the National Comorbidity Survey
Replication. Health Psychology, 29(4), 446-453.
562 CHAMBERS
Meredith, P., Ownsworth, T., & Strong, J. (2008). A review of the evidence linking
adult attachment theory and chronic pain: Presenting a conceptual model.
Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 407-429.
Moffett, M. C., Vicentic, A., Kozel, M., Plotsky, P., Francis, D. D., & Kuhar, M. J.
(2007). Maternal separation alters drug intake patterns in adulthood in rats.
Biochemical Pharmacology, 73, 321-330.
Monk, C., Spicer, J., & Champagne, F. A. (2012). Linking prenatal maternal adversity
to developmental outcomes in infants: The role of epigenetic pathways. Deve-
lopmental Psychopathology, 24(2), 1361-1376.
Morgan, J. K., Shaw, D. S., & Forbes, E. E. (2014). Maternal depression and warmth
during childhood predict age 20 neural response to reward. Journal of the Ame-
rican Academy of Child ans Adolescent Psychiatry, 53(1), 108-117.
Murgatroyd, C. A., Pena, C. J., Podda, G., Nestler, E. J., & Nephew, B. C. (2016). Early
life social stress induced changes in depression and anxiety associated neural
pathways which are correlated with impaired maternal care. Neuropeptides,
52, 103-111.
Naber, F., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Deschamps, P., van Engeland H., & Bakermans-
Kranenburg, M. J. (2010). Intranasal oxytocin increases fathers’ observed re-
sponsiveness during play with their children: A double-blind within-subject
experiment. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35, 1583-1586.
Olavsky, A. K., Telzer, E. H., Shapiro, M., Humphreys, K. L., Flannery, J., Goff, B.,
& Tottenham, N. (2013). Indiscriminate amygdala response to mothers and
strangers after early maternal deprivation. Biological Psychiatry, 74, 853-860.
Pierrehumbert, B., Torrisi, R., Ansermet, F., Borghini, A., & Halfon, O. (2012). Adult
attachment representations predict cortisol and oxytocin responses to stress.
Attachment and Human Development, 14(5), 453-476.
Plant, D. T., Pawlby, S., Sharp, D., Zunszain, P. A., & Pariante, C. M. (2016). Prenatal
maternal depression is associated with offspring inflammation at 25 years: A
prospective longitudinal cohort study. Translational Psychiatry, 6(11), e936, 1-8.
Puig, J., Englund, M. M., Simpson, J. A., & Collins, W. A. (2013). Predicting adult
physical illness from infant attachment: A prospective longitudinal study.
Health Psychology, 32(4), 400-417.
Qiu, A., Anh, T. T., Li, Y., Chen, H., Rifkin-Grabol, A., Broekman, B. F. P., Kwek,
K., Saw, S.-M., Chong, Y.-S., Gluckman, P. D., Fortier, M. V., & Meaney, M. J.
(2015). Prenatal maternal depression alters amygdala functional connectivity
in 6-month old infants. Translational Psychiatry, 5, e508, 1-7.
Quirin, M., Pruessner, J., & Kuhl, J. (2008). HPA system regulation and adult at-
tachment anxiety: Individual differences in reactive and awakening cortisol.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33, 581-590.
Radley, J. J., & Sawchenko, P. E. (2011). A common substrate for prefrontal and hip-
pocampal inhibition of the neuroendocrine stress response. Journal of Neu-
roscience, 31(26), 9683-9695.
Rincon-Cortes, M., Barr, G. A., Mouly, A. M., Shionoya, K., Nunez, B. S., & Sullivan,
R. M. (2015). Enduring good memories of infant trauma: Rescue of adult neu-
robehavioral deficits via amygdala serotonin and corticosterone interaction.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(3), 881-886.
Roth, T. L., Lubin, F. D., Funk, A. J., & Sweatt, J. D. (2009). Lasting epigenetic influ-
ence of early-life adversity on the BDNF gene. Biological Psychiatry, 65, 760-
769.
NEUROBIOLOGY OF ATTACHMENT 563
Ruppenthal, G. C., Arling, G. L., Harlow, H. F., Sackett, G. P., & Suomi, S. J. (1976). A
10-year perspective of motherless monkey behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psy-
chology, 85(4), 341-349.
Sapolsky, R. Krey, L. C., & McEwen, B. S. (1986). The neuroendocrinology of stress
and aging: The glucocorticoid cascade hypothesis. Endocrine Reviews, 7(3),
284-301.
Satterthwaite, T. D., Kable, J. W., Vandekar, L, Katchmar, N., Bassett, D. S., Baldas-
sano, C. F., . . .Wolf, D. H. (2015). Common and dissciable dysfunction of the
reward system in bipolar and unipolar depression. Neuropsychopharmacology,
40, 2258-2268.
Strathearn, L. (2011). Maternal neglect: Oxytocin, dopamine and the neurobiology of
attachment. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 23(11), 1054-1065.
Strathearn, L., Fonagy, P., Amico, J., & Montague, P. R. (2009). Adult attachment
predicts maternal brain and oxytocin response to infant cues. Neuropsycho-
pharmacology, 34, 2655-2666.
Suomi, S. J., Harlow, H. F., & McKinney, W. T. (1972). Monkey psychiatrists. Ameri-
can Journal of Psychiatry, 128(8), 927-932.
Swain, J. (2008). Baby stimuli and the parent brain: Functional neuroimaging of the
neural substrates of parent-infant attachment. Psychiatry, 5(8), 28-36.
Teicher, M. H., Samson, J. A., Anderson, C. M., & Ohashi, K. (2016). The effects of
childhood maltreatment on brain structure, function, and connectivity. Nature
Reviews, Neuroscience, 17, 652-666.
Tops, M., Koole, S., IJzerman, H. I., & Buisman-Pijlman, F. T. A. (2014). Why social at-
tachment and oxytocin protect against addiction and stress: Insights from the
dynamics between ventral and dorsal corticostriatal systems. Pharmacology,
Biochemistry and Behavior, 119, 39-48.
Vaglio, S. (2009). Chemical communication and mother-infant recognition. Commu-
nicative and Integrative Biology, 2(3), 279-281.
Vela, R. (2014). The effect of severe stress on early brain development, attachment,
and emotions: A psychoanatomical formulation. Psychiatric Clinics of North
America, 37, 519-534.
Weaver, I. C. G., Cervoni, N., Champagne, F. A., D’Alessio, A. C., Sharma, S., Seckl,
J. R., Dymov, S., Szyf, M., . . . Meaney, M. J. (2004). Epigenitic programming
by maternal behavior. Nature Neuroscience, 7(8), 847-854.
Weaver, I. C. G., Meaney, M. J., & Szyf, M. (2006). Maternal care effects on the hippo-
campal transcriptome and anxiety-mediated behaviors in the offspring that
are reversible in adulthood. Proccedings of the National Academy of Sciences of
the United States of America, 103(9), 3480-3485.
Weisman, O., Zagoory-Sharon, O., & Feldman, R. (2012). Oxytocin administration
to parent enhances infant physiological and behavioral readiness for social
engagement. Biological Psychiatry, 72, 982-989.
Wiest, G., & Brainin, E. (2010). Neuropsychoanalytic findings in a patient with bilat-
eral lesions of the amygdala. Neuropsychoanalysis, 12(2), 193-200.