0% found this document useful (0 votes)
62 views9 pages

Subject-Verb Inversion in English - Wikipedia

Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
62 views9 pages

Subject-Verb Inversion in English - Wikipedia

Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 9

Subject–verb

inversion in English

Subject–verb inversion in English is a t ype of inversion where t he subject and verb (or chain
of verbs, verb cat ena) swit ch t heir canonical order of appearance so t hat t he subject follows
t he verb(s), e.g. A lamp stood beside the bed → Beside the bed stood a lamp. Subject –verb
inversion is dist inct from subject –auxiliary inversion because t he verb involved is not an
auxiliary verb.

Overview

The following sent ences illust rat e subject -verb inversion. They compare canonical order wit h
t he non-st andard inversion order, and t hey demonst rat e t hat subject -verb inversion is unlikely
if t he subject is a weak (non-st ressed) definit e pronoun:

a. Jim sat under t he t ree.


b. Under t he t ree sat Jim. - Subject -verb inversion
c. *Under t he t ree sat he. - Subject -verb inversion unlikely wit h weak definit e subject
pronoun
a. The dog came down t he st airs.
b. Down t he st airs came the dog. - Subject -verb inversion
c. *Down t he st airs came it. - Subject -verb inversion unlikely wit h weak definit e subject
pronoun
a. Some flowers are in t he vase.
b. In t he vase are some flowers. - Subject -verb inversion wit h t he copula
c. *In t he vase are they. - Subject -verb inversion unlikely wit h weak definit e subject
pronoun
a. Bill said, "I am hungry."
b. "I am hungry," said Bill. - Subject -verb-object inversion
c. "I am hungry," said he. - Subject -verb-object inversion here possible, but less likely, wit h
weak definit e subject pronoun

Subject -verb inversion has occurred in t he b-sent ences t o emphasize t he post -verb subject .
The emphasis may occur, for inst ance, t o est ablish a cont rast of t he subject wit h anot her
ent it y in t he discourse cont ext .

Types of subject-verb inversion

A number of t ypes of subject -verb inversion can be acknowledged based upon t he nat ure of
phrase t hat precede t he verb and t he nat ure of t he verb(s) involved. The following
subsect ions enumerat e four dist inct t ypes of subject -verb inversion: locative inversion,
directive inversion, copular inversion, and quotative inversion.

Locative inversion

Locat ive inversion also occurs in many languages, including Brazilian Port uguese, Mandarin
Chinese, Ot jiherero, Chichewa, and a number of Germanic and Bant u languages. An adjunct
phrase is swit ched from it s default post verbal posit ion t o a posit ion preceding t he verb,
which causes t he subject and t he finit e verb t o invert . For example:[1]

a. A lamp lay in t he corner.


b. In t he corner lay a lamp. - Locat ive inversion
c. *In t he corner lay it. - Locat ive inversion unlikely wit h a weak pronoun subject
a. Only Larry sleeps under t hat t ree.
b. Under t hat t ree sleeps only Larry. - Locat ive inversion
c. *Under t hat t ree sleeps he. - Locat ive inversion unlikely wit h a weak pronoun subject

The front ed expression t hat evokes locat ive inversion is an adjunct of locat ion. Locat ive
inversion in modern English is a vest ige of t he V2 order associat ed wit h earlier st ages of t he
language.

Directive inversion

Direct ive inversion is closely relat ed t o locat ive inversion insofar as t he pre-verb expression
denot es a locat ion, t he only difference being t hat t he verb is now a verb of movement .
Typical verbs t hat allow direct ive inversion in English are come, go, run, et c.[2]

a. Two students came int o t he room.


b. Int o t he room came two students. - Direct ive inversion
c. *Int o t he room came they. - Direct ive inversion unlikely wit h a weak pronoun subject
a. The squirrel fell out of t he t ree.
b. Out of t he t ree fell the squirrel. - Direct ive inversion
c. *Out of t he t ree fell it. - Direct ive inversion unlikely wit h a weak pronoun subject

The front ed expression t hat evokes inversion is a direct ive expression; it helps express
movement t oward a dest inat ion. The following sent ence may also be an inst ance of direct ive
inversion, alt hough t he front ed expression expresses t ime rat her t han direct ion:

a. The toasts came aft er t he speeches.


b. Aft er t he speeches came the toasts. - Inversion aft er a t ime expression

Like locat ive inversion, direct ive inversion is undoubt edly a vest ige of t he V2 word order
associat ed wit h earlier st ages of t he language.

Copular inversion

Copular inversion occurs when a predicat ive nominal swit ches posit ions wit h t he subject in a
clause where t he copula be is t he finit e verb. The result of t his inversion is known as an
inverse copular const ruct ion, e.g.[3]

a. Bill is our represent at ive.


b. Our represent at ive is Bill. - Copular inversion
c. *Our represent at ive is he. - Copular inversion unlikely wit h weak pronoun subject
a. The objection was a concern.
b. A concern was the objection. - Copular inversion
c. *A concern was it. - Copular inversion unlikely wit h weak pronoun subject

This t ype of inversion occurs wit h a finit e form of t he copula be. Since English predominant ly
has SV order, it will t end t o view whichever noun phrase immediat ely precedes t he finit e verb
as t he subject . Thus in t he second b-sent ence, A concern is t aken as t he subject , and the
objection as t he predicat e. But if one acknowledges t hat copular inversion has occurred, one
can argue t hat the objection is t he subject ; and A concern, t he predicat e. This confusion has
led t o focused st udy of t hese t ypes of copular clauses.[4] Where t here is a difference in
number, t he verb agrees wit h t he noun phrase t hat precedes it :

a. Jack and Jill are t he problem.


b. The problem is Jack and Jill. - On an inversion analysis, t he verb agrees wit h t he
apparent predicat e.

Quotative inversion

In lit erat ure, subject -verb inversion occurs wit h verbs of speaking. The speech being report ed
is produced in it s direct form, usually wit h quot at ion marks:[5]

a. "We are going t o win," Bill said.


b. "We are going t o win," said Bill. - Quot at ive inversion
c. *"We are going t o win," said he. - Quot at ive inversion less likely wit h weak subject
pronoun
a. "What was t he problem?" Larry asked.
b. "What was t he problem?" asked Larry. - Quot at ive inversion
c. *"What was t he problem?" asked he. - Quot at ive inversion less likely wit h weak subject
pronoun

This sort of inversion is absent from everyday speech. It occurs almost exclusively in lit erary
cont ext s.

Multiple verbs

Subject -verb inversion can somet imes involve more t han one verb. In t hese cases, t he subject
follows all of t he verbs, t he finit e as well as non-finit e ones, e.g.

a. Remnants of marijuana consumption have been found under her bed t wice.
b. Under her bed have been found remnants of marijuana consumption t wice.
c. Under her bed have been found t wice remnants of marijuana consumption.

Sent ence b and sent ence c, where t he subject follows all t he verbs, st and in st ark cont rast
t o what occurs in cases of subject -auxiliary inversion, which have t he subject appearing
bet ween t he finit e auxiliary verb and t he non-finit e verb(s), e.g.

d. Has anyt hing been found under her bed?

Furt her, t he flexibilit y across sent ence b and sent ence c demonst rat es t hat t here is some
freedom of word order in t he post -verb domain. This freedom is consist ent wit h an analysis in
t erms of right wards shift ing of t he subject , where heavier const it uent s t end t o follow light er
ones. Evidence for t his claim comes from t he observat ion t hat equivalent s of sent ence c
above are not as good wit h a light subject :

e. ?? Under her bed has been found t wice marijuana.


f. * Under her bed has been found t wice it.
These fact s clearly dist inguish t his kind of inversion from simple subject -auxiliary inversion,
which applies regardless of t he weight of t he subject :

g. Has it been found under her bed?

Thus, it is not clear from t hese examples if subject -auxiliary inversion is a unified grammat ical
phenomenon wit h t he ot her cases discussed above.

Structural analysis

Like most t ypes of inversion, subject -verb inversion is a phenomenon t hat challenges t heories
of sent ence st ruct ure. In part icular, t he t radit ional subject -predicat e division of t he clause (S
→ NP VP) is difficult t o maint ain in light of inst ances of subject -verb inversion such as Into
the room will come a unicorn. Such sent ences are more consist ent wit h a t heory t hat t akes
sent ence st ruct ure t o be relat ively flat , lacking a finit e verb phrase const it uent , i.e. lacking
t he VP of S → NP VP.

In order t o maint ain t he t radit ional subject -predicat e division, one has t o assume movement
(or copying) on a massive scale. The basic difficult y is suggest ed by t he following t rees
represent ing t he phrase st ruct ures of t he sent ences:

The convent ion is used here where t he words t hemselves appear as t he labels on t he nodes
in t he t rees. The t ree on t he left shows t he canonical analysis of t he clause, whereby t he
sent ence is divided int o t wo immediat e const it uent s, t he subject Bill and t he finit e VP
crouched in the bush. To maint ain t he int egrit y of t he finit e VP const it uent crouched in the
bush, one can assume a rearranging of t he const it uent s in t he second sent ence on t he right ,
whereby bot h crouched and in the bush move out of t he VP and up t he st ruct ure. The
account suggest ed wit h t he second t ree is t he sort of analysis t hat one is likely t o find in
Government and Binding Theory or t he Minimalist Program. It is a phrase st ruct ure account
t hat relies on unseen movement /copying mechanisms below t he surface.
The unseen mechanisms must perform an even great er job for t he marijuana-example above.
That sent ence (sent ence c in t he previous sect ion) would necessit at e at least five inst ances
of movement /copying in order t o maint ain t he presence of an underlying finit e VP
const it uent .

This makes it unlikely t hat t he mechanism discussed above is t he correct analysis for t he
marijuana-examples, as t hese might be generat ed by t he same mechanisms t hat underlie
ext raposit ion and heavy-NP shift .

An alt ernat ive analysis of subject -verb inversion reject s t he exist ence of t he finit e VP
const it uent . Due t o t he absence of t his const it uent , t he st ruct ure is flat t er, which simplifies
mat t ers considerably. The sent ences wit h invert ed order will oft en not result in a
discont inuit y, which means t he basic hierarchy of const it uent s (t he vert ical order) does not
change across t he canonical and invert ed variant s. The following t rees illust rat e t his
alt ernat ive account . The first t wo t rees illust rat e t he analysis in an unort hodox phrase
st ruct ure grammar t hat reject s t he presence of t he finit e VP const it uent , and t he second
t wo t rees illust rat e t he analysis in a dependency grammar. Dependency grammar reject s t he
presence of a finit e VP const it uent .[6]

Because t here is no finit e VP const it uent in t hese t rees, t he basic hierarchy of const it uent s
remains consist ent . What changes is just t he linear order of t he const it uent s. The following
t rees illust rat es t he "flat " dependency-based analysis of t he marijuana-example.
Due t o t he lack of a finit e VP const it uent , t he basic hierarchy of const it uent s is not alt ered
by inversion. However, t his analysis does not capt ure t he obvious dependency bet ween t he
main verb and t he invert ed subject .

Notes

1. For more examples and discussions of locative inversion, see Quirk et al. (1979:478), Culicover
(1997:170f.) and Greenbaum and Quirk (1990:409).

2. For further examples of directive inversion, see Quirk et al. (1979:478), Greenbaum and Quirk
(1990:410), and Downing and Locke (1992:231).

3. For further examples and discussion of copular inversion, see Greenbaum and Quirk (1990:409).

4. Moro (1997) and Mikkelsen (2005) are two examples of detailed studies of copular inversion.

5. For more examples of quotative inversion, see for instance Greenbaum and Quirk (1990:410f.) and
Downing and Locke (1992:300f.).

6. Concerning the dependency grammar rejection of a finite VP constituent, see Tesnière (1959:103–
105), Matthews (2007:17ff.), Miller (2011:54ff.), and Osborne et al. (2011:323f.).

Literature

Culicover, P. 1997. Principles and paramet ers: An int roduct ion t o synt act ic t heory. Oxford,
UK: Oxford Universit y Press.

Downing, A. and Locke, P. 1992. English grammar: A universit y course, second edit ion.
London: Rout ledge.

Greenbaum, S. and R. Quirk. 1990. A st udent 's grammar of t he English language. Harlow,
Essex, England: Longman.

Groß, T. and T. Osborne 2009. Toward a pract ical dependency grammar t heory of
discont inuit ies. SKY Journal of Linguist ics 22, 43-90.
Mat t hews, P. H. (2007). Synt act ic Relat ions: a crit ical survey (1. publ. ed.). Cambridge:
Cambridge Universit y Press. ISBN 9780521608299. Ret rieved 24 August 2012.

Mikkelsen, Line 2005. Copular clauses: Specificat ion, predicat ion, and equat ion. Linguist ics
Today 85. Amst erdam: John Benjamins.

Miller, J. 2011. A crit ical int roduct ion t o synt ax (ht t ps://books.google.com/books?id=a0UQ
HXUzR9AC&print sec=front cover#v=onepage&q&f=false) . London: cont inuum.

Moro, A. 1997. The raising of predicat es: Predicat ive noun phrases and t he t heory of clause
st ruct ure. Cambridge: Cambridge Universit y Press.

Osborne, T., M. Put nam, and T. Groß 2011. Bare phrase st ruct ure, label-less t rees, and
specifier-less synt ax: Is Minimalism becoming a dependency grammar? The Linguist ic
Review 28, 315–364.

Quirk, R. S. Greenbaum, G. Leech, and J. Svart vik. 1979. A grammar of cont emporary English.
London: Longman.

Tesnière, L. 1959. Élemént s de synt axe st ruct urale. Paris: Klincksieck.

Tesnière, L. 1969. Élemént s de synt axe st ruct urale, 2nd edit ion. Paris: Klincksieck.

Retrieved from
"https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?
title=Subject–
verb_inversion_in_English&oldid=1067534228"


Last edited 6 days ago by 138.185.14.42

You might also like