Attraction and Attachment - Understanding Styles of Relationships
Attraction and Attachment - Understanding Styles of Relationships
• •
Understanding Styles
of Relationships
This page intentionally left blank
Attraction
and Attachment:
Understanding Styles
of Relationships
Barbara Jo Brothers
Editor
First Published 1993 by The Haworth Press, Inc.
Published 2013 by Routledge
711 Third Avenue, New York, NY 10017 USA
2 Park Square, Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon OX14 4RN
Attraction and Attachment: Understanding Styles o f Relationships has also been published as Jour
nal o f Couples Therapy, Volume 4, Numbers 1/2 1993.
© 1993 by Taylor & Francis. All rights reserved. No part of this work may be reproduced or
utilized in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, microfilm
and recording, or by any information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing
from the publisher. Reprint - 2007
The development, preparation, and publication of this work has been undertaken with great care.
However, the publisher, employees, editors, and agents of The Haworth Press and all imprints of
The Haworth Press, Inc., including The Haworth Medical Press and Pharmaceutical Products Press,
are not responsible for any errors contained herein or for consequences that may ensue from use of
materials or information contained in this work. Opinions expressed by the author(s) are not neces
sarily those of The Haworth Press, Inc.
CONTENTS
Her point was that all six of the illustrated ways inhibit indepen
dent action and growth and mitigate against “standing on one’s own
two feet.” In all six, the balance of one depends on the other.
FIGURE 2. SUFFOCATION
4A
HB
FIGURE 5. VICTIM/VICTIMIZER
5h
5F
Oo FIGURE 6. So-Called “SPIRITUAL” RELATIONSHIP
Barbara Jo Brothers 9
REFERENCES
Satir, V. (Speaker). (1982, August). Lecture presented at Process Community II,
Crested Butte, Colorado.
Satir, V. & Banmen, J. (1983). Virginia Satir verbatim 1984. Transcription of
Process Community HI, 1983, Crested Butte, Colorado. (Available from Delta
Psychological Services, 11213 Canyon Crescent, N. Delta, British Columbia,
Canada, V4E2R6.)
Satir, V. (Speaker). (1987, October). [Videotape]. International Human Learning
Resources Network. October Meeting. Hacienda Vista Hermosa, Cuernavaca,
Mexico. (Available from Morris Gordon, 3705 George Mason Dr. #C35, Falls
Church, Virginia 22041).
Satir, V. (1988). The new peoplemaking. Mountain View, California: Science and
Behavior Books.
Satir, V., Banmen, J., Gerber, J., Gomori, M. (1991). The Satir model. Mountain
View, California: Science and Behavior Books.
This page intentionally left blank
Marriage and Attachment:
An Exploration of Ten Long-Term
Marriages
Alyce Jackson
[Hawoith co-indexing entry note]: “Marriage and Attachment: An Exploration of Ten Long-Teim
Marriages,” Jackson, Alyce. Co-published simultaneously in the Journal of Couples Therapy (The
Haworth Press, Inc.) Vol. 4, No. 1/2, 1993, pp. 13-30; and: Attraction and Attachment: Understanding
Styles of Relationships (ed: Barbara Jo Brothers) The Haworth Press, Inc., 1993, pp. 13-30. Multiple
copies of this article/chapter may be purchased from The Haworth Document Delivery Center
[I-8OO-3-HAWORTH; 9:00 a.m. - 5:00 p.m. (EST)].
OVERVIEW OF RESULTS
The research on which this article is based explored the attach
ment histories as well as the current attachment experiences of
currently married adults. Ten happy couples in long-term marriages
(mean = 27 years) who were nominated by clergy and mental health
professionals and screened with the Dyadic Adjustment Scale
(Spanier, 1976) were interviewed in depth using a semi-structured
inventory. Eight major themes of attachment were explored: avail
ability, understanding, separation and reunion behavior, loss, sup
port receiving, support giving, and autonomy.
What were the subjects’ recollections of their early attachment
experiences? How did they describe their marital relationships? The
results will be briefly summarized due to space restraints. Eighteen
subjects grew up in homes where at least one parent was physically
available. None of the subjects experienced a parental death during
childhood, nor were any of them separated from their caretakers for
an indeterminate period of time. Nine participants indicated that one
parent provided understanding and compassion during childhood. In
adulthood, however, 18 subjects had at least one relationship which
enabled them to feel understood. As children, eight subjects turned to
others when they felt emotionally distressed. Yet, in adult life, 16
subjects turned to their spouses when they felt upset, and all of them
attempted to provide support to their partners when needed. While
14 subjects described themselves as compliant children, all of the
spouses indicated that their marital relationship included reciprocal
tolerance of autonomous ideas and/or behaviors.
The data from these categories revealed some striking differ
ences between childhood and adult life. Despite the fact that the
subjects were all happily married, none of them described an “ Oz-
zie and Harriet” type childhood home life where both parents were
physically and emotionally available. Similarly, none of the sub
Alyce Jackson 15
CONTINUITY
DISCONTINUITY
In the last book he authored before his death Bowlby wrote about
change throughout the life span. He indicated that it was a human
being’s inherent ability to change that laid at the heart of therapy.
Those who can not remember the past are doomed to repeat it
-George Santayana
scribed how their interactions with and attitudes toward their par
ents shifted as the latter became elderly and confronted death. Mrs.
Stanford (married 11 years) spoke about her current relationship
with her mother and father.
I am really happy with [my relationship with my parents as an
adult]. Since my dad retired we are really having conversa
tions. . . . There was that time when we didn’t talk that much
[college and young adulthood], and then I think when I Anally
decided to have kids and could share that, being pregnant, my
mom and I talked about it a lot. That was the beginning of us
getting back on course together and sharing experiences as an
adult. I really felt like we could be adults together.. . . I know
there are things they don’t like about what we do. . . . I feel
really fortunate. I take them for granted.
Several subjects reported that their relationships with their par
ents had changed dramatically over time disconfirming to some
degree their “earlier acquired models” (Epstein cited in Ricks,
1985). In addition, all of the subjects experienced a strong emotion
al experience within the single relationship of marriage. Two pos
sible consequences of “ a strong emotional experience within a
single relationship” are altered self-perception and increased self
esteem. Thus, as we shall see in the next section, the subjects’
improved relationships with their parents may in part have been due
to the impact of the subjects’ marriages on themselves and their
perceptions of others.
Mrs. Walton’s analysis of the role marriage has played in her life
has significant theoretical and empirical support. Mattison and
Sinclair (1979), Napier and Whitaker (1978), Kirschner and Kirsch-
24 Attraction and Attachment: Understanding Styles of Relationships
ner (1986), and others have posited that marriage offers individuals
a second chance to form an attachment relationship. They also
believe that for many individuals the marital relationship can ame
liorate, at least in part, the effects of earlier less satisfying relation
ships.
The Kirschners (1986) posited that in all marriages there is a
natural drive to create a healing relationship, one that not only
replicates, but also transcends transactional gestalts that the spouses
experienced in their families of origin. Mattison and Sinclair (1979)
expressed a similar concept. “Marriage holds the promise of being
able to make better what was felt to be wrong in the past and to
make partnership more satisfying than is dimly remembered from
childhood” (p. 52).
Mr. Oppenheimer and Mrs. Walton (married 28 years) indicated
that, at least in their lives, this was indeed the case.
Mr. White, who was married 12 years, believed that his relation
ship with his wife has had a positive effect on his physical and
psychological well being.
rium for a spastic colon which was totally anxiety related.. . . I’d
say the wedding was the last time I had any. I don’t feel there is
any risk of getting back in that scenario with the possible excep
tion of losing one of my immediate family.
The Kirschners’ (1986) described how marriage, a “strong emo
tional relationship,” fosters change. The Kirschners believe that
attachment is one of the primary needs throughout the life cycle.
They posited that spouses transfer their attachment needs from their
parents to each another in the beginning of marriage.
In a regressive burst, termed falling in love, mates idealize
their partners in a manner resembling that of a young child
with a parent. This process sets the stage for each spouse to
accept the other’s suggestions and ideas about himself or her
self and the nature of reality, much as a child does with a
parent. The new bond replaces the old parental relationship
and thus promotes individuation from the family, one of the
first tasks of marriage. (1986, p. 4)
There is also empirical support for the belief that stable mar
riages improve mental health and buffer the individuals involved
from at least some of the deleterious effects of stress (Dunn, 1988,
p. 198). Kotler’s (1985) research indicated that strong marital
relations were therapeutic for a partner whose early experiences of
care were less than optimal. Kotler and Omodei (1988) found that
later marital quality was strongly related to the mental health of
both partners. Similarly, Gove, Hughes, and Style (1983) posited
that “ . . . it is the quality of a marriage and not marriage per se that
links marriage to positive mental health” (p. 122). Valliant’s (1977)
longitudinal research concluded that the variable which best pre
dicted mental health was the subject’s capacity to remain happily
married over time. Egeland, Jacobvitz and Stroufe (1988) discov
ered that an emotionally supportive, satisfying marriage helped
women who were abused during childhood to break the cycle of
abuse when raising their own children. Rutter and Quinton (1984)
echoed their results. In Long-term follow-up of women institutiona
lized in childhood: Factors promoting good functioning in adult life
they stated that “the findings . . . suggested that the spouses’ good
26 Attraction and Attachment: Understanding Styles of Relationships
CONCLUSIONS
This study, like Kotler and Omodei’s (1988), Brooks’ (1981) and
others, offers moderate support for a continuity model to the degree
that early attachment relationships appear to have been influential
in many of the subjects’ lives. The finding that discontinuity might
better describe the majority of the subjects’ development echoes
several of the conclusions that Valiant (1977) reached in the Grant
study and those of Egeland, Jacobvitz and Stroufe (1988).
The results from these studies and others also indicated that rela
tionships with others, specifically marital and psychotherapeutic
relationships, modified to some extent the deleterious effects of un
fortunate childhood experiences. The marital relationship, whether
legalized or not, is for the majority of adults, one of life’s most
important and consequential experiences. Additional research could
shed more light on how, for some individuals, it is also the context
for emotionally corrective change. Future research should focus on
the circumstances in which the marriage relationship is healing.
The findings from this study should provide fiuther encourage
ment to those members of the therapeutic community who espouse
the possibility of characterological change. They particularly sup
port those marriage and family therapists who believe that positive
marital and family relationships ameliorate to some degree the
childhood developmental immaturities of both spouses and, there
fore, pave the way for their children’s healthy development (Kirsch-
ner and Kirschner, 1986).
Bowlby (1988) believed that therapeutic change occurred when
the client was able to use the therapist as a secure base from which
to explore the self and his or her interactions in the world. The
present results indicate that if troubled spouses can learn from the
therapist to provide that resource for one another, the healing pro-
Alyce Jackson 29
cess will continue long after the psychotherapy sessions have end
ed. Each spouse’s development or personal growth will enhance the
modification of the partner’s working model.
The current findings to a large degree confirm the beliefs of
Bowlby, Valliant, and others. Tragic events and early vicissitudes
did not fully determine the subjects’ paths in life. While early inter
personal difficulties can set the stage for later developmental prob
lems, the subjects in this study also found that relationships were
healing.
REFERENCES
Ainsworth, M. S. D. (1972). Attachment and dependency: A comparison. In J. L.
Gewirtz (Ed). Attachment and dependency. New York: Wiley and Sons.
Ainsworth, M.S.D., & Wittig, B. A. (1969). Attachment and exploratory behavior
of one year olds in a strange situation. In B. M. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of
infant behavior (Vol. 4, pp. 111-136), London: Methuen.
Belsky, J., & Nezworski, T. (1988). Clinical implications of attachment. Hillsdale,
New Jersey: L. Erlbaum and Associates. (Ed), Psychoanalysis and contempo
rary thought. (pp. 33-57). London: Hogarth Press.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and Anger.
New York: Basic.
Bowlby, J. (1979). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. London: Tavi
stock.
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base. New York: Basic.
Brown, G. W., & Harris, T. (1978). The social origins of depression. London:
Tavistock.
Brooks, J. (1981). Social maturity in middle-life and its developmental anteced
ents. In D. Eichom, J. Clausen, N. Haan, M. Honzik, and P. Muzen (Eds.),
Present and past in middle life. New York: Academic Press.
Caspi, A., and Elder, G. H. (1987). Emergent family patterns: The intergenera-
tional construction of problem behavior and relationships. In R. A. Hinde and
J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Relations between relationships. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Chess, S., Thomas, A., Korn, S., Mittleman, M., & Cohen, J. (1983). Early parental
attitudes, divorce and separation, and young adult outcome: Findings of a longi
tudinal study. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 22,47-51.
Cohen, L. J. (1974). The operational definition of human attachment Psychologi
cal Bulletin, 81 y 207-217.
Dunn, J. (1988). Relations among relationships. In S. W. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of
personal relationships. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Egeland, B., Jacobvitz, D., & Stroufe, L.A. (1988). Breaking the cycle of abuse.
Child Development, 59,1080-1088.
30 Attraction and Attachment: Understanding Styles of Relationships
infant is bom in a state of total need and literally does not survive in
the absence of those needs being met. Having been bom with a total
expectancy of and need for their needs to be met, there is a corre
spondingly total vulnerability. Each failure to meet their needs has
an impact on the developing ego and the more severe the depriva
tion, the more significant the impact on the developing sense of self.
The experience of not being received and loved creates deformation
of the loving/loved expectancy as well as of the sense of self. This is
a deformation of the expectancy and need for love not an elimina
tion of it.
In examining the potential distortions in our lover/beloved sense
of self it is of value to think of the self that needs to be loved, the
expressions of that need, and the lover that will exactly satisfy the
need as a functional unit. Failure to satisfy any aspect of the unit
effects the other aspects. For example, if the need is to be loved, the
expression of it may be reaching out with arms or lips, and the
satisfying lover may be the mother. If the mother is not available, the
child will go to the next available person to meet the need. No matter
how loving that substitute for the mother is, there will be incomplete
satisfaction as it is a substitute for the mother, not the mother. If the
need is repeatedly unmet at the human level, the child will seek
satisfaction with non-human objects. With continued frustration,
awareness of the need and its original satisfying object are sub
merged and satisfaction is sought with a new substituted object. Over
time, with repeated partial satisfaction the child begins to identify the
need as related to the substituted object If, for example, food was the
partial solution to the need to be loved then when the loving impulse
arises it would be experienced as a need for food. No matter how
sated with food, since the need was for love, there remains a sense of
being unfulfilled, that something is missing.
Linus and his blanket, from the Charlie Brown cartoons, provides
a good visual example of the need satisfaction expressed with a
non-human object, the blanket. Linus is always seen with the blan
ket, which illustrates a consequence of the substitute love object not
being the original satisfying object. Incomplete satisfaction of the
need increases the attachment to the object which is expressed in a
compulsive relationship with it. So we could consider the blanket
for Linus as his solution to the absence of, for example, an available
34 Attraction and Attachment: Understanding Styles of Relationships
able to his current relationship. The steps involved were those re
quired to remain true to his internal reality and to his sense of
personal safety. The effect was for him to discover more of a sense
of possibility in the world, to feel safer, and to have an increased
capacity to love and be loved in his relationship to Judy.
We have looked at one consequence of a person’s early environ
ment forcing a substitute solution to the love need. That solution left
Harry present but removed love from the relationship. In the next
case we shall examine, the love remains but in an essential way the
person is removed from the relationship. It is arguable that unless
you are fully present the love expressed, is at least partially false.
Durang (1985) in The Marriage o f Bette and Boo expressed it this
way;
Unless you go through all the genuine anguish that you feel,
both justified and unjustified, the feeling of love that you do
have will not have any legitimate base and will be at least
partially false. Plus, eventually you will go crazy.
In general, to love the other and to express it requires a trust that
our love will be accepted. Where there is excessive concern over its
reception we become timid in its expression and vigilant concerning
its reception. For intimacy and love to be present and experienced
between two people, two selves must be present. If one or both
people are overly focused on how they are received then there is a
tendency to lose track of the self in focusing on the other’s reaction.
This eventuates in the other becoming a judge of our love, resulting
in an exchange between a superior and an inferior.
Our primary information about the expression of love is the way
we and our intimates were treated. Helen, 38, and Luke, 39, have
been married for 20 years. They entered psychotherapy after she
read some of the adult children of alcoholic’s material. Helen came
from a home in which her father was a frequently absent alcoholic
and her mother was very controlling and deprecating of her. Luke’s
father was a religious fanatic and an absolute ruler of his wife and
children. His mother was warm, loving, submissive to his father,
and died when he was 16.
They both learned, in the name of love, to vigilantly watch the
other. She watched in the hope of pleasing and thereby earning his
38 Attraction and Attachment: Understanding Styles of Relationships
REFERENCES
Durang, C. (1985). The marriage of Bette and Boo. New York: Dramatist Play
Service Inc.
Pesso, A. (1973). Experience in action. New York: New York University Press.
Schell, B. (1992. Elements of couple psychotherapy and awakening. Journal of
Couples Therapy, 3(1).
Response to Bruce J. Schell’s Article:
“Objects of Heart’s Desire”
Aliya Alexana
Loving is the purpose of life. One is bom being pure love rather
than learning love; the rejection of this love creates a lifelong striv
ing to reattain the state of loving bliss-loving for the joy and awe of
loving. B. Schell’s description of love development with its distor
tions is excellent although I believe the infant has only love to give
and the learning process is how to have it received rather than how
to obtain it. The same distortions occur in either event. I do disagree
with his idea that one loves power, beauty, pleasure, etc.
Love is the natural human state. Beauty and ideas may touch a
person deeply to open up the innate love within. One appreciates
beauty which releases the loving energy, rather than loving beauty.
Many positive experiences and emotions may unlock one’s capacity
for love and thus begin the path of achieving the purpose of life.
The drive for power is the result of an absence of love. It is an
ego distortion with the purpose being to obtain love, not to love.
Melanie Klein (Envy and Gratitude, 1965) describes greed as the
fear that one will not obtain sufficient love, so one tries to get as
much as possible before it is withdrawn. This, as an adult ego, is so
insecure that it believes power will convince others to love it. We
are loved for being, not for achieving. The latter is a distortion of
love resulting in the oxymoron of “conditional love.” Envy, greed
and jealousy are derived from the concept of scarcity-that there will
[Haworth co-indexing entry note]: “Response to Bruce J. Schell’s Article: “Objects of Heart’s
Desire,” Alexana, Aliya. Co-published simultaneously in the Journal of Couples Therapy (The Ha
worth Press, Inc.) Vol. 4, No. 1/2, 1993, pp. 39-40; and: Attraction and Attachment: Understanding
Styles of Relationships (ed: Barbara Jo Brothers) The Haworth Press, Inc., 1993, pp. 39-40. Multiple
copies of this article/chapter may be purchased from The Haworth Document Delivery Center
[1-800-3-HAWORTH; 9:00 a.m. - 5:00 p.m. (EST)].
© 1993 by The Haworth Press, Inc. All rights reserved. 39
40 Attraction and Attachment: Understanding Styles of Relationships
not be enough love. All other scarcities are substitutes for insuffi
cient love.
Unfortunately a majorfactor o f the human condition is that one’s
capacity for loving is cut off from awareness and expression
through fear and the pain of life. I agree with Schell that we respond
to emotional fears with a primal reality, but emotional losses are
more confusing and stressful to a developing ego than the direct
fear of a known physical threat. All fear is the direct or indirect
anxiety of the loss of love. The fear in turn causes insecurity, doubt,
mistrust, hopelessness, pain, resentment, anguish and despair.
Transformation of the fear into pure love and acceptance is neces
sary to begin loving. This is the purpose of the therapy process.
People seek love from others and believe in error that their appreci
ation of being loved is actual loving. It is true that various types of
appreciation and gratitude can open the heart to experience true
loving of other.
We seek human love in an effort to tap the love energy locked
within us. In fact, most relationships become so bound with expecta
tions that true loving is not achieved-the primal fear of loss while
risking intimacy interfering with that possibility. If the relationship
can achieve enough security, one can succeed in opening the channel
of love from within and the experience of loving another can begin.
As this loving expands, one gradually experiences divine as well as
human love. Increased divine love increases human love in turn. This
process continues until one attains again the capacity for pure loving
that was lost at birth. Allowing oneself to know such loving and love
(lover and beloved-Schell) enables a communion with man and God
that fills the soul with awe and joy-the natural state of being.
Sexual Intimacy-
Towards Equal Relationships
Between Men and Women
(with Treatment Assistance
of a Computer Program)
Louis Sommeling
a
r
0
u
s
a
I
time
processes, but above all by ideas we’ve been talked into in the
years that we’ve been living.
The fact that not only the clients but also many professionals still
tend to think in terms of a merely biological model of sexuality too,
has far-reaching consequences. To name some examples: unneces
sary medical operations are still performed (e.g., for vaginism or a
tight foreskin). Mythical prejudices are perpetuated, e.g., about
asexuality of older women and men. Also, there are many no-sense
making intake questions for clients, such as: “How many times do
you do it?” There are more important questions to be asked than,
e.g., the latter, as we will see in the next section.
What are we looking for in sex? When people come with sex
problems it is important for therapists to invite them to think about
their needs by asking such a question. But giving an answer to this
question would suggest that people have an awareness of why they
want something. Most of the time however, unconscious needs play
a role in the background.
If we ask a client ‘What is it you are looking for in sex, why do
you do it?’, the obvious answer is usually something like: ‘Because
I love it.’ If you keep on asking however, often there turns out to be
a good deal more to it. Possible hidden motivations could be: ‘to
relieve my sexual tension,’ ‘to make another person feel nice and
comfortable,’ ‘to prevent a fight at home,’ ‘otherwise I wouldn’t
live up to the standard coitus frequency of the average man and that
makes me feel sure,’ etc. People tend to sexualize their human
needs. In those cases, they may use an ‘improper’ way to express
Louis Sommeling 47
those needs. They would have been better served, had they been
able to express this some other way.
Inviting clients to think about their sexual motivations will sup
ply alternatives. It creates possibilities to act in a more adequate and
personal way and to better meet our needs. Especially male thera
pists (Aghassy, 1984, 1990) can sometimes make mistakes in the
interpretation of the behavior of their clients: behavior which looks
on the surface ‘sexual’ can often also be interpreted as testing
behavior or as a need for approval, support or affection.
come bonus.’ In reverse the female rule can be: ‘As a rule I
don’t climax during intercourse. I do, if we pet to climax; if he
comes too while petting, good for him.’
The couple comes in and he may say: “She never feels like i t ”
She nods “yes” and feels guilty.
He points at her and thinks: “She is the patient.”
1. “ Most likely there’s something wrong with her. For that rea
son she’ll need a psychological or physical examination.”
2. “We’ll start a therapy to teach her to own her sexual feelings,
for that’s what she lacks.”
3. “We’ll try and teach him how to lessen his libido.”
4. “We don’t speak about sexuality at all, because our focus is
on relationship problems; relationship problems are always
“ deeper” than sexual problems.”
5. “We are going to talk about their different expectations and
belief systems regarding sex. . . ”
of more autonomy. Now is the time for the men to slowly start
identifying with die (encouraging) male therapist and with the
other men in the room, the famous “male bond” (Tripp, 1976).
Masculine Spirituality
The difficulty which men co-experience in staying alone for
sometime without looking outside themselves for another symbiotic
confusion, is beautifully pictured in Nelson’s lecture Male Sexuality
and Masculine Spirituality (1991):
Women tend to experience their sexuality as internal, deep and
mysterious. As a male on the other hand, I am often inclined to
experience my sexuality as more instrumental. My penis is an
instrument for penetrating and exploring a mystery which is
essentially external to me. And the linearity, the hardness, the
straightness of the prized erection all are important to my
understanding of reality. Then spiritually ( . . . ) I am prone to
believe that mystery is “out there” rather than sensing the
mystery dwelling within me ( . . . . ) .
The flaccid penis is empty of the engorging blood which
brings hard excitement to the phallus. Flaccidity is letting go
of all urgency; the spiritual experience of sinking, letting go
and emptying is an experience of divine grace, as interpreted
in the Christian tradition. It means trusting God that we do not
need to do, that our being is enough. It means trusting our
selves to the darkness.. . .
I would like to conclude here that autonomy is a condition for
real sexual intimacy. It seems that especially men have to learn to be
alone (which is something essentially different from withdrawal!).
Instead of waiting until the woman will love him, a man has to learn
the secret that only in a position of true autonomy is he able to be
attractive. In that position of autonomy a man can take at the same
time responsibility for the sexual and the communicative problems
he and his partner are having as a couple. In such a position he will
be less emotionally dependent on his partner and consequently
claim her less as well.
When one of the male sex symbols of the eighties, William Hurt,
won an Oscar, I asked some female clients what made him so
Louis Sommeling 55
cognition and traditional myths about sexuality (like, e.g., the fa
mous male myths of Zilbergeld (1978). Information screens on
modem contraceptives, sex problems and literature are available as
well as simple exercises for treatment of anoigasmia, vaginismus,
erectile failure, premature ejaculation and a tight foreskin.
Especially for professionals there is a second program: Sexper
tise Professional. This computer program offers support to thera
pists, social workers and general practitioners in the field of intake
assessment and treatment of sexual problems. Also more “recent”
problems, like incest, sexual abuse, AIDS and problems related to
female emancipation are talked about here. Of course, a therapist
can never be replaced by a computer.
Without reducing the therapeutic encounter however, the pro
gram may prevent the therapist from overlooking some essential
questions. Some reflections about options for treatment are added
(individual, partner, sex- or communication-therapy). Reported ef
fects of medication on sexual dysfunctions are pointed out.
Installed on data-bank or TV-teletext, the information in these
computer programs is easily accessible, twenty-four hours a day
(Figure 3). Already available comparative examples in the field of
sexology are the Human Sexuality Computer Service in New York
GENERAL INFORMATION
F1= CONTRACEPTIVES
F2= IF SOMETHING GOES WRONG
F3= SEXUALLY TRANSMITTED DISEASES (i.e., AIDS)
F4= EFFECTS OF MEDICINE AND DRUGS
F5= USEFULADDRESSES
F6= LITERATURE
F7= SEXOLOGICAL SOFTWARE
NOTES
INFORMATION
Publisher of the Sexpertise computer programs: Mediware, Groningen, the
Netherlands. More information: see note 11.
AUTHOR NOTE
Louis Sommeling is author of Volumes in the Computer Assisted Humanistic
Psychology Applications Series (Sexpertise and Dreamanalysis). At present he is
working on a book about Male Desire.
REFERENCES
Aghassy, G. (1984). Sexual contact between client and psychotherapist. Universi
ty, Amsterdam.
Aghassy, G. and Noot, M. (1990). Sexuele kontakten binnen psychologische hulpver-
leningsrelaties. Ministerie van Sociale Zaken en Werkgelegenheid. Den Haag.
Binik, Y. (1988). Intelligent computer-base assessment and psychotherapy. An
expert system for sexual dysfunction. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease,
176, 387-400. Williams and Wilkins. Baltimore: Preston.
Louis Sommeling 59
1. Males and females generally many those who are similar in age.
When there is a discrepancy, males are usually slightly older.
2. Males and females generally marry those with similar educa
tion. As with age, when there is a discrepancy, males usually
are more educated.
3. Males and females usually many those of a similar race although
this is changing with slightly more interracial marriages occurring.
4. Males and females generally marry those of a similar religion
although this is changing with more interreligious marriages
occurring.
68 Attraction and Attachment: Understanding Styles of Relationships
DISCUSSION
In order to present the reader with a general flavor for the re
search findings and theorizing in the area of mate selection, the
biological theories of mate selection, the social psychological vari
ables associated with mate selection, along with a discussion of the
psychological theories associated with mate selection was pre
sented. In addition, the basic assumption of family therapy theory-
persons of equal levels of differentiation and psychological maturi
ty marry-was presented. From the above discussion it is obvious
that family therapy theorists hold certain assumptions around mate
selection processes. Some of these major assumptions are:
1. Spouses in a relationship tend to be at equal levels of psychologi
cal maturity (Meissner, 1978; and Sonne and Swirsky, 1981).
2. People tend to recreate their interpersonal world in relation
ships by recreating patterns of interaction that were estab
78 Attraction and Attachment: Understanding Styles of Relationships
REFERENCES
Abies, B. & Brandsma, J. (1977). Therapy for couples. San Francisco: Jossey-
Bass.
Ackerman, N. (1958). The psychodynamics of family life. New York: Basic.
Aldous, N. (1973). Mechanisms of stalemate in conjoint marital therapy. Cana
dian Psychiatric Association Journal, 1 8 ,191-197.
Alexander, R. & Norman, K. (1979). Concealment of ovulation, parental care and
80 Attraction and Attachment: Understanding Styles of Relationships
Toman, W. (1970). Never mind your horoscope: Birth order rules all. Psychology
Today, 4, 45-49.
Toman, W. (1976). Family constellation: Its effects on personality and social
behavior. New York: Springer.
Touhey, J. (1971). Birth order and mate selection. Psychological Reports, 29,618.
Townsend, J. & Levy, G. (1990). Effects of potential partners’ physical attractive
ness and socioeconomic status on sexuality and partner choice. Archives of
Sexual Behavior, 79, 149-164.
Trivers, R. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.)
Sexual selection and the descent of man. Aldine.
Udry, R. (1965). The influence of the ideal mate image on mate selection and
mate perception. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 27,477-482.
U.S. Bureau of Census. (1989). Washington, DC.
Vanderberg, S. (1972). Assortive mating, or who marries whom? Behavior Genet-
ics, 7, 59-71.
Ward, C.; Castro, M.; Wilcox, A. (1974). Birth order effects on mate selection and
parenthood. Journal of Social Psychology, 94, 57-64.
Watkins, M. & Meredith, W. (1981). Spouse similarity in newlyweds with respect
to specific cognitive abilities, socioeconomic status, and education. Behavior
Genetics, 77,1-21.
Watzlawick, P., Beavin, J. & Jackson, D. (1967). Pragmatics of human commu
nication. New York: Norton.
Weinberg, L. (1978). Differentiation andfusion in marital relations. Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Temple University, Philadelphia.
Weininger, O. (1906). Sex and character. New York: Putman.
Weinrich, J. (1987). Sexual landscapes: Why we are what we are, why we love
whom we love. New York: Scribner.
Westermarck, E. (1936). The future of marriage in western civilization. New
York: Macmillan.
White, G. (1980). Physical attractiveness and courtship progress. Journal of Per
sonality and Social Psychology, 59, 660-668.
White, S. & Hatcher, C. (1986). Couple complementarity and similarity: A review
of the literature. The American Journal of Family Therapy, 12, 15-25.
Willi, J. (1982). Couples in collusion. New York: Jason Aronson.
Willi, J. (1984). Dynamics of couples therapy. New York: Jason Aronson.
Winch, R. (1952). The modem family. New York: Holt.
Winch, R. (1954). Mate selection: A study of complementary needs. New York:
Harper & Row.
Winch, R. (1955). The theory of complementary needs in mate selection. Ameri
can Sociological Review, 20, 552-555.
Winch, R. (1958). Mate selection: A study of complementary needs. Harper & Row.
Winch, R., Ktones, T., Ktones, V. (1958). TTieory of complementary needs in mate
selection: An analytic and descriptive study. American Sociological Review,
19, 241-249.
Joan Atwood 87
Frank P. Troise, DSW, BCD, would like to thank Mr. Michael Aymar for his
research assistance in developing this paper.
[Haworth co-indexing entry note]: “An Overview of the Historical and Empirical Antecedents m
the Development of the Codependency Concept,” Troise, Frank P. Co-published simultaneously in the
Journal of Couples Therapy (The Haworth Press, Inc.) Vol. 4, No. 1/2,1993, pp. 89-104; and: Attraction
and Attachment: Understanding Styles of Relationships (ed: Barbara Jo Brothers) The Haworth Press,
Inc., 1993, pp. 89-104. Multiple copies of this article/chapter may be purchased from The Haworth
Document Delivery Center [I-8OO-3-HAWORTH; 9:00 a.m. - 5:00 p.m. (EST)].
INTRODUCTION
During World War II the nation applauded women for their par
ticipation in the national war effort. However, this applause rapidly
turned to criticism of women for having gone to work during the
war and thereby destroying the American family in the process.
Men returned from war and women were now defined as having
given up their femininity and maternal role to compete with men
causing their children to become delinquents and their husbands to
become alcoholics (Banner, 1974).
Freudianism, according to Banner, had begun to have a major
influence on American psychiatry and psychiatric social work prac
tice. As previously noted, it was Freudian psychiatrists and psycho
Frank P. Troise 95
DISCUSSION
This historical overview of the research which foreshadowed the
development of contemporary Codependency Theory reveals that
the basic theoretical construct, which informs Codependency
Theory, was abandoned by mental health researchers in the late
1950’s and early 1960’s. Attempts to link wives of alcoholics to
gether with a shared personality disturbance, which contributed to
their selection of an alcoholic mate and the perpetuation of the
alcoholic’s illness, were put to rest by the findings of the “ Reaction
to Stress” theorists. Furthermore, history reveals that the original
“ Disturbed Personality Theory” of wives of alcoholics, which
clearly shares a similar theoretical foundation with contemporary
Codependency Theory, was intimately connected to long since
abandoned Freudian concepts regarding innate masochism in
women. In addition Banner (1974) has noted that this theory was
developed within a social climate which blamed the assertiveness of
women for the psychological difficulties of men returning from
World WarH.
Most codependency theorists attribute the codependent’s behav
ior to a combination of failed coping attempts in dealing with the
alcoholic’s destructive behavior and a pre-existing psychopatholo
gy which attracts the codependent to a disturbed relationship there
by perpetuating and exploiting it for psychopathologic gain (i.e.,
repetition compulsion). Nevertheless the notion of codependents as
“enablers” in their symptomatic spouses’ illness, strongly suggests
a blaming of the victim who is married to someone who is creating
havoc in the marriage due to his alcoholism.
100 Attraction and Attachment: Understanding Styles of Relationships
Bowen (1978) has noted that the symptoms of one spouse may be
needed by the other mate in order to mask or defend the non-symp-
tomatic spouse against personality deficits. In such an instance the
symptomatic spouses’ illness is needed to maintain homeostasis in
the relationship. Merikangas (1982) found that individuals who are
symptomatic but have a vulnerability to develop a psychiatric disor
der (i.e., alcoholism) and therefore display behavioral anomalies,
withdrawal, and a lack of social skills are less likely to develop
mature intimate healthy relationships. Consequently, these individ
uals are either selected out of the “ marital pool” or marry individu
als who are similarly impaired. Clinical observation of married
couples in conflict often reveal that the florid symptoms of one
spouse are needed and exploited for psychopathologic gain by the
non-symptomatic spouse. However the widespread acceptance of
codependency as a distinguishable diagnostic entity, without the
benefit of empirical validation or recognition of previously related
research, encourages pre-conceived sweeping notions regarding
women who marry alcoholics by mental health professionals.
Kokin and Walker (1989) have stated that the concept of code
pendency is firmly grounded in antiquated and empirically un
founded Freudian beliefs that women enjoy their suffering based on
innate biologically grounded masochistic tendencies. They argue
that expert professionals find it difficult to diagnose and treat alco
holism yet when the wife of the alcoholic or codependent flounders
in dealing with her alcoholic spouse she is accused of perpetuating
his illness for psychopathologic gain. They further state that believ
ing that women are codependents takes the focus off the alcoholic
himself and provides some relief since alcoholism is so difficult to
treat and recovery rate is so low. Furthermore it encourages the
blaming of women for yet another problem. Women-blaming in our
culture is a widely accepted activity as it was in the 1940’s and
1950’s when the related disturbed “Personalty Theory” was bom.
Interestingly enough both time periods were characterized by in
creased female independence and demands for equal status with
men. Kokin and Walker suggest that Codependency Theory has
created a whole new set of jargon and counterintuitive principles
giving bewildered and frustrated professionals a sense of control,
power and efficacy as they now speak a new language that lay
Frank P. Troise 101
tently allow the alcoholic to continue drinking. Many wives are not
familiar with the alcoholic syndrome and unknowingly support the
alcoholic’s denial through various attempts to maintain marriage,
family and social or employment obligations. Such attempts often
prolong the alcoholic’s recognition of the erosive effects that alco
holism is having on his family, marriage, social and employment
life. However, to define the wife’s attempts at coping with such an
unfamiliar mental health problem as pathologically gratifying ob
fuscates the essence of the difficulty the non-alcoholic spouse is
having in coping with such a marital trauma. Given the almost all
inclusive diagnostic picture attributed to codependents and the will
ingness of many wives of alcoholics to assume some blame for their
husbands’ alcoholism many so-called codependents will readily
conform to and accept a codependent diagnosis. Kokin and Walker
offer a simple yet profound example of the basic adaptive function
of the codependent in dealing with the alcoholic’s destructive be
havior. They say that if the codependent assumes responsibility for
driving an automobile, with children in it, because the husband is
too drunk to drive she is merely protecting her family from potential
disaster. While this example is somewhat obvious it is metaphori
cally significant in terms of the so-called “enabling” behaviors
attributed to codependents. The psychopathology or “enabling”
behavior of the wife of the alcoholic, so defined by codependency
theory, is more often than not the only option available to a wife
who must assume responsibility for her and her family’s welfare
given her alcoholic husband’s inability to do so. While such behav
ior may inadvertently perpetuate the alcoholic’s disease it must be,
at first, considered as an attempt to legitimately and reasonably
cope with the trauma imposed on her by her alcoholic husband.
Clinical wisdom suggests that certain interactions between flor
idly symptomatic spouses and their “non-symptomatic” partners
are often perpetuated and exploited for psychopathologic gain by
the “non-symptomatic” spouse. Codependency Theory, however,
returns full circle to a theory, previously abandoned through empiri
cal research, which obliterates differential diagnosis, ignores the
impact of reality trauma of alcoholism on the non-alcoholic spouse
and is seemingly a thinly disguised version of previously aban
doned Freudian concepts which blame women for the difficulties of
Frank P. Troise 103
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Bailey, M.B., Haberman, P. and Alksne, H. (1962). Outcomes of alcoholic mar
riages; endurance, termination or recovery. Quart. J. Stud. Ale. 23: 610-623,
(CAAL, 9584).
Bailey, M.B. (1967). Psychophysiological impairment in wives of alcoholics as
related to their husbands’ drinking and sobriety. Pp. 134-144. In: Fox, R.,
(ed.), Alcoholism, behavioral research»therapeutic approaches. New York:
Springer.
Ballard, R. G. (1959). The interrelatedness of alcoholism and marital conflict
symposium: 3. The interaction between marital conflict and alcoholism as seen
through MMPI’s of marriage partners. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,
29,528-546.
Banner, L.W. (1974). Women in modem america: A brief history. Harcourt, Brace
and Jovanovich, 2nd Edition.
Beattie, M. (1987). Codependent no more. New York: Harper and Row.
Black, C. (1982). It will never happen to me. Denver: M.A.C.
Boggs, M. H. (1944). The role of social work in the treatment of inebriates.
Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 4, 557-567.
Bowen, M. (1978). Family therapy and clinical practice. New York/London:
Jason Aronson, Inc.
Cermak, T. L. (1986). Diagnosing and treating codependence. Minneapolis:
Johnson Institute.
Clifford, B. J. (1960). A study of the wives of rehabilitated and unrehabilitated
alcoholics. Social Casework, 41,457-460.
Edwards, P., Harvey, C., & Whitehead, P. C. (1972). Wives of alcoholics: A
critical review and analysis. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 34,
112-132.
Futterman, S. (1953). Personality trends in wives of alcoholics. Journal of Psy
chiatric Social Work, 23, 37-41.
Haberman, P.W. (1964) Psychological test score changes for wives of alcoholics
during periods of drinking and sobriety. J. Clin. Psychol 20: 230-232,
(CAAAL, 10418).
Jackson, J. K. (1954). The adjustment of the family to the crisis of alcoholism.
Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 15, 562-586.
Kokin, M. and Walker, I. (1989). Women married to alcoholics. New York: Wil
liam Morrow and Company, Inc.
Larsen, E. (1985). “Codependency Seminar,” Stillwater, MN.
104 Attraction and Attachment: Understanding Styles of Relationships
Betsey Backe, MS, MSW, is Director of the Divorce Center in Slidell and New
Orleans, LA.
Erin L. Bonck, MSW, is Director of Social Services and Field Operations for
Metro Home Health Care, Inc. in Marrero, LA.
Marie L. Riley, RN, LMSW, is affiliated with Clayton Country Mental Health
Center in Jonesboro, GA.
[Haworth co-indexing entry note): “Codependency and Depression: A Correlational Study/’ Backe,
Betsey, Erin L. Bonck, and Marie L. Riley. Co-published simultaneously in the Journal of Couples
Therapy (The Haworth Press, Inc.) Vol. 4, No. 1/2,1993, pp. 105-127; and: Attraction and Attachment:
Understanding Styles of Relationships (ed: Barbara Jo Brothers) The Haworth Press, Inc., 1993, pp.
105-127. Multiple copies of this article/chapter may be purchased from The Haworth Document Deliv
ery Center [l-8(X)-3-HAW0RTH; 9:00 a.m. - 5:00 p.m. (EST)].
© 1993 by The Haworth Press, Inc. All rights reserved. 105
106 Attraction and Attachment: Understanding Styles of Relationships
METHODOLOGY
Instruments
RESULTS
TABLE 1
Summary of Data
Note.
a Mean Value
FIGURE 1. FEMALE
8 -
25.0 30.0 35.0 40.0 45.0 50.0 55.0 60.0 65.0 70.0 75.0
RAW SCORES ON CODEPENDENCY INVENTORY
Frequency Distribution of Codependency for Females: Number of Women who Tested at the Codependent
Level Indicated.
DISCUSSION
FIGURE 2. MALE
*■r j i j 11 | Ti iTj i
21.0 26.0 31.0 36.0 41.0 46.0 51.0 56.0 61.066.071.0
RAW SCORES ON CODEPENDENCY INVENTORY
Frequency Distribution of Codependency for Males: Number of Males who Tested at the Codepen
dent Level Indicated.
79.0
69.0*
59.0*
49.0
39.0*
29.0-
19.0*
9.0-
0.0 5.0 10.0 15.0 20.0 25.0 30.0 35.0 40.0 45.0 50.0 55.0 60.0
RAW SCORES ON DEPRESSION INVENTORY
Codependency as a Function of Depression in both Genders:
Individual raw scores for both the Beck Depression Inventory and the Codependency Inventory.
FIGURE 4. MALE
69.0
59.0
49.0
39.0
29.0
19.0
9.0
0.0 5.0 10.0 15.0 20.0 25.0 30.0 35.0 40.0 45.0 50.0
This study may give further credence to Shaw et al. who reported
the idea that depression may not be a primary disorder but rather a
concomitant disorder; in this case depression in relation to codepen
dency. One area of further research may include looking at code
pendency levels in patients already diagnosed as being depressed;
the reverse of this may also prove useful.
Our study also tested the internal reliability and validity of The
Awareness Activity: How Codependent Are You? This study sup
ported the validity of this inventory. By empirically testing the
Backe, Bonck, and Riley 121
FIGURE 5. FEMALE
73.0
63.0
53.0
13.0
33.0
23.0
13.0
0.0 5.0 10.0 15.0 20.0 25.0 30.0 35.0 40.0 45.0 50.0 55.0 60.0
FIGURE 6. FEMALE
cUJ/>
_J
CL
O
0LUc
CD
FIGURE 7. MALE
5 -
co 3 -
2 -
1 -
0.0 5.0 10.0 15.0 20.0 25.0 30.0 35.0 40.0 45.0
men. However, our study did not confirm their findings which may
raise the question of why the results of our study were different.
Perhaps the variance is due to the constraints of our sampling tech
nique; the population of men tested being more health conscious
than that of the general population. Consequently, the outcome of
our study may have been different if a random sample had been
used. In our study, the sample was individuals who showed an
124 Attraction and Attachment: Understanding Styles of Relationships
AUTHOR NOTE
This article was submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the
degree of Master of Social Work.
REFERENCES
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC.: Author
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders (3rd ed.-revised). Washington, DC: Author
Billings, A. G. & Moos, R. H. (1985). Psychosocial stressors, coping and depression.
In E. E. Beckham & W. R. Leber (Eds.), Handbook of depression: Treatment\
assessment, and research (pp. 940-974). Homewood IL: The Dorsey Press.
Cermak, T. L. (1986). Diagnosing and treating co-dependence. Minneapolis:
Johnston Institute Books.
Cermak, T. L. (1986). Diagnostic criteria for codependency. Journal of Psychoac
tive Drugs. 18(1), 15-20.
Epstein, M. K., & Epstein, E. K. (1990). Codependence as social narrative. Read
ings: A Journal of Reviews and Commentary in Mental Health, 5(3), 4-7.
Grinker, R. R., Werble, B., Drye, R. C. (1968). Borderline Syndrome. New York:
Basic Books.
Homey, K. (1950). Neurosis in human growth. New York: Norton.
Lin, N., Woelfel, M., & Dumin, M. (1986). Gender of the confident and depression.
In N. Lin, A. Dean & W. Ensel (Eds.), Social support, life events, and depression
(pp. 283-306). Orlando, FL: Academic Press Inc.
Mendenhall, W. (1989). Co-dependency definitions and dynamics. Alcoholism
Treatment Quarterly. 6(1), 3-17.
Mendenhall, W. (1989). Co-dependency treatment. Alcoholism Treatment Quarter
ly. 6(1), 75-86.
Backe, Bonck, and Riley 125
Millon, T. (1981). Disorders of personality: DSM-III Axis II. New York: Wiley-In-
terscience.
Millon, T. & Kotik, D. (1985). The relationship of depression to disorders of
personality. In E. E. Beckham & W. R. Leber (Eds.), Handbook of depression:
Treatment, assessment, and research (pp. 700-744). Homewood IL: The Dorsey
Press.
Peterson, C. & Seligman, M. E. P. (1985). The learned helplessness model of
depression: Current status of theory and research. In E. E. Beckham & W. R.
Leber (Eds.), Handbook of depression: Treatment, assessment, and research (pp.
914-939). Homewood EL: The Dorsey Press.
Potter-Efron, R. T. & Potter-Efron, P. S. (1989). Assessment of co-dependency with
individuals from alcoholic and chemically dependent families. Alcoholism
Treatment Quarterly. 6(1), 37-57.
Sacco, W. P. & Beck, A. T. (1985). Cognitive therapy of depression. In E. E.
Beckham & W. R. Leber (Eds.), Handbook of depression: Treatment, assess
ment, and research (pp. 3-38). Homewood IL: The Dorsey Press.
Shaw, B. F., Vallis, M., & McCabe, S. B. (1985). The assessment of the severity and
symptom patterns in depression. In E. E. Beckham & W. R. Leber (Eds.),
Handbook of depression: Treatment, assessment, and research (pp. 372-407).
Homewood EL: The Dorsey Press.
Smally, S. & Coleman, E. (1987). Treating intimacy dysfunctions in dyadic rela
tionships among chemically dependent and codependent clients. Journal of
Chemical Dependency Treatment. 7(1), 229-243.
Subby, R. (1984). Inside the chemically dependent marriage: Denial and manipula
tion. In Co-dependency: An emerging issue (pp. 25-29). Pompano Beach, FL:
Health Communications Inc.
Wegscheider-Cruse, S. (1985). Choicemaking. Pompano Beach FL: Health Com
munications, Inc.
Whitfield, C. L. (1989). Co-dependence: Our most common addiction-some physi
cal, mental, emotional, and spiritual perspectives. Alcoholism Treatment Quar
terly, <5(1), 19-36.
Wing, J. K. & Bebbington, P. (1985). Epidemiology of depression. In E. E. Beckham
& W. R. Leber (Eds.), Handbook of depression: Treatment, assessment, and
research (pp. 700-744). Homewood EL.: The Dorsey Press.
126 Attraction and Attachment: Understanding Styles of Relationships
APPENDIX A
MARITAL STATUS
SEX
_____AGE
APPENDIX A (continued)
( ) I tend to value the opinion of others more than my own.
( ) My feelings of worth come from outside myself, through the
opinions of other people or from activities that seem to validate
my worth.
( ) Ifind it difficult to be vulnerable and to ask for help.
( ) Ideal with issue self-control by attempting to always be in
control or, the opposite, by being careful never to be in a posi
tion of responsibility.
( ) I am extremely loyal to others, even when that loyalty is unjusti
fied.
( ) I tend to view situations with “all or none” thinking.
( ) Ihave a high tolerance for inconsistency and mixedmessages.
( ) Ihave emotional crises and chaos in my life.
( ) I tend to find relationships in which I feel “needed” and attempt
to keep it that way.
Scoring: Add the numbers to get a total score. Use the following
ranges to help interpret your level of co-dependency:
METHODS
Subjects
One hundred and fifty premarital subjects (75 males and 75 fe
males) who were currently involved in intimate relationships were
utilized as participants in this study. Length of relationship ranged
from 2 months to 6 years (M = 1.59 years, SD = 1.14 years). The
median length of relationship across subjects was 1 year. These
subjects ranged in age from 17 to 39 years (M = 19.89, SD = 2.82).
Fifty subjects (33%) scored in the distressed range on the Dyadic
Adjustment Scale. Scores on the Dyadic Adjustment Scale ranged
from 77 to 151 points (M = 108.69, SD = 16.28). Nineteen percent of
the subjects had previously participated in therapy. Of those subjects
who had previous therapy experience, individual treatment was most
common (12%), followed by family treatment (10%). None of the
subjects had previously participated in relationship treatment.
Assessment
RESULTS
The scores of the TEI and the SD-Evaluative Scale were analyzed
as a summed score because they were found to be highly intercorre
Marian R. Flammang and Gregory L. Wilson 135
TEI/SD - Evaluative***
SD - Potency/Activity***
Mean scores that share a common superscript are not significantly different.
TEI/SD - Evaluative**
SD - Potency/Activity*
Mean scores that share a common superscript are not significantly different.
138 Attraction and Attachment: Understanding Styles of Relationships
TEI/SD - Evaluative*
Mean scores that share a common superscript are not significantly different.
DISCUSSION
The results of this study clearly indicate that: (a) conjoint treat
ment format was generally rated as most acceptable, followed by
group and individual interventions, respectively; (b) similarly,
when asked to choose between the three therapy formats, signifi
cantly more subjects chose the conjoint condition as the most pre
ferred format; (c) subjects who had been involved in a relationship
longer than one year rated group and conjoint treatment formats as
equal in acceptability; (d) additionally, subjects who had been in
volved in a relationship less than one year rated the individual
treatment format as significantly more acceptable than did all other
subjects; (e) educational information appears to influence accept
ability ratings in that subjects who received group-oriented in
formation rated group treatment as more acceptable than did sub
jects who received only descriptive information.
In previous acceptability studies regarding relationship treatment
formats, educational information did not influence acceptability
ratings (Wilson & Flammang, 1990; Wilson, Flammang, & Dehle,
in press). However, these studies employed college students as
Marian R. Flammang and Gregory L. Wilson 139
AUTHOR NOTE
This research is part of the dissertation project that Marian R. Flammang com
pleted as partial fulfillment for the doctor of philosophy degree at Washington State
University. Gratitude is expressed to Herb Cross, and James Whipple who served
on her committee. Address correspondence to Marian R. Flammang, M.S., Depart
ment of Psychology, Washington State University, Pullman, WA 99164-4820.
REFERENCES
Beutler, L. E., & Clarkin, J. F. (1990). Systematic treatment selection: Toward
targeted therapeutic interventions. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Bomstein, P. H., & Bomstein, M. T. (1986). Marital therapy: A behavioral-com-
munications approach. New York: Pergamon Press.
Jacobson, N. S., & Margolin, G. (1979). Marital therapy: Strategies based on
social learning and behavior exchange principles. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Kazdin, A.E. (1980a). Acceptability of alternative treatments for deviant child
behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 13, 259-273.
Kazdin, A. E. (1980b). Acceptability of time out from reinforcement procedures
for disruptive child behavior. Behavior Therapy, 77, 329-344.
Montag, K., & Wilson, G. L. (1991). An empirical evaluation of behavioral and
cognitive-behavioral group marital treatments with rural couples. Submitted
for publication.
140 Attraction and Attachment: Understanding Styles of Relationships
Sheldon Starr, PhD, was founder and director for 15 years of the Family Study
Unit, a family therapy training and treatment program at the V.A. Medical Center,
Palo Alto, CA when this interview was conducted. Dr. Starr is presently Professor
of Psychology (part time) at Pacific Graduate School of Psychology and is in
private practice, both in Palo Alto, CA. His association with Virginia Satir
spanned 25 years, first as student, then as associate and long-time friend. Corre
spondence may be sent to 770 Welch Road, #161, Palo Alto, CA 94304.
A highly condensed and edited summary of the entire interview appeared in
the AFTA Newsletter, Fall 1985 and that version consisted of less than 20% of the
interview.
[Haworth co-indexing entry note]: “Change, Vulnerability, Fear, and Taking Risks-An Interview
with Virgnia Satir,” Starr, Sheldon. Co-published simultaneously in the Journal of Couples Therapy
(The Haworth Press, Inc.) Vol. 4, No. 1/2, 1993, pp. 141-45; and: Attraction and Attachment: Under
standing Styles of Relationships (ed: Barbara Jo Brothers) The Haworth Press, Inc., 1993, pp. 141-145.
Multiple copies of this article/chapter may be purchased from The Haworth Document Delivery Center
[ 1-800-3-HAWORTH; 9:00 a.m. - 5:00 p.m. (EST)].
you know fits? See that’s exactly what happened in Germany. A lot
of people cared [but did nothing].
SATIR: But going on, well that’s what he did. He died so he could
live. Anyway, the point you’re making is a very important one. If I
violate my common sense and do something else because I think
that somebody will like it or I don’t want to be out there vulnerable,
then I’m in trouble. See, my history was I didn’t do that. I took
whatever steps I know how to take. But the reason that I could do
that were two. Number one is because I was sort of nice and I didn’t
make fun of people or put them down because they may have
questioned me and so they would come [to therapy]. They were
interested. And the other thing was that I worked with the people no
one else wanted, so it was too late by the time we got there. But fear
is everywhere, and when professionals say to me, “I can’t do it
because I’m afraid” and when “I’m afraid” translates into “What
will they say?” , I say, “I don’t know what they’ll say. Why don’t
you ask them? But in the meantime, what kind of prostitution are
you getting into yourself, to say you know something that fits but
you don’t do it?” And what difference is that?
SATIR: Sure.
STARR: Joan of Arc stuck with her principles and she burned at
the stake; Galileo recanted, but we still know that the earth is not the
center of the universe.
Sheldon Starr 145
SATIR: Right.
STARR: Well, okay, yeah, yeah. You’re not talking with an adver
sary because I feel that way also. The VA Family Study Unit in
1970 were treating homosexual couples. We just didn’t talk about it
publicly because at that point they’d (VA) have killed the program.
Now we can do that. You see, now that’s out in the open, but we
were doing it way back then.
SATIR: We’ve got more of that, more people need to take risks,
and learning how to take risks is important. Many people behave as
though they’ve only two possibilities, to attack and rebel or capitu
late. Those [certainly] are two you can use, but you can also do
something quite different. It’s the same thing as when you get a
double level message. You can respond to one or the other [level] or
simply say you know there are two things here and comment on the
presence of what is going on and ask for something else.