10 CH4 Social and Emotional Development
10 CH4 Social and Emotional Development
10 CH4 Social and Emotional Development
79
An introduction to childhood studies and child psychology
Learning outcomes
When you have studied this chapter, you should be able to:
Introduction
The first year of life is a time of rapid change. Children begin to
acquire new skills and gain a better understanding of the social world
around them. First relationships, especially with primary caregivers, are
crucial for development and for infants to learn about their social
world. Although infants are born with rather limited capabilities, they
actively engage in developing early relationships and have an innate set
of capacities to do so. Over time, these early social relationships
become extended to include a wider range of individuals, usually
including other key caregivers as well as grandparents, siblings and
peers. Some children may also experience caregiving from people
beyond the family, such as foster carers or childminders. These
relationships are central to ensuring a child’s healthy social and
emotional development, and can influence social relationships in later
childhood.
In this chapter we will focus on exploring how different social
relationships can influence the lives of children and young people. This
chapter will briefly introduce the child’s first relationships and the
importance of establishing those relationships for promoting secure
attachment bonds. The later sections will then begin to consider
children’s social and emotional development by exploring not only how
they develop a sense of ‘self ’ but also how they come to be able to
communicate with others. The importance of forming social
80
Chapter 4 Social and emotional development
First relationships
Chapter 3 described the influential suggestion, which came from the
writings of the seventeenth-century British philosopher John Locke
(1632–1704), that children were born as a blank slate or ‘empty vessel’
with no innate ideas or knowledge about the world. Certainly, infants
rely on parents and other caregivers to meet their primary needs to
ensure survival. However, the way in which we viewed infancy,
especially during the first months of life, underwent significant shifts in
the twentieth century, not least with growing scientific evidence that
infants enter the world with innate capabilities that enable them to
develop early social relationships.
Note that these important early relationships can develop with any key
caregiver. However, due to the gendered nature of caregiving practices
in many societies, psychological research has often focused on the role
of the mother. Research has shown that, from a young age, infants are
able to recognise and respond to familiar faces and familiar voices and
to engage in early social interaction. For instance, infants are born
recognising their own mother’s voice and will very soon after birth
prefer to look at their own mother’s face rather than any other
woman’s (Grossmann, 2010). It is suggested that in this sense young
infants are biologically predisposed to interact with others and this, in
part, may be due to the strong primary need for food, warmth and
protection. When an infant cries this provides a strong emotional
signal to the caregiver that the child requires, for example, physical
comfort from a change of nappy, or sustenance, or warmth and
affection. Caregivers need to be highly sensitive to these behavioural
signals and respond to the needs of the newborn.
Social development is fundamentally important, not just for children’s
social relationships but also for their cognitive and even physical
development, and researchers have been astonished by the impact of
early social deprivation on children’s language and psychological
development, even when physical needs have been met
(Gerhardt, 2006).
81
An introduction to childhood studies and child psychology
82
Chapter 4 Social and emotional development
Figure 1 Close proximity and physical contact between infant and mother
are often considered important for developing a strong emotional bond
We also know that the caregiver and the infant contribute equally to
the quality of these dyadic interactions. Important features of these
early caregiver–infant interactions often include mutuality, reciprocity,
engagement and affective sharing. This is one way in which infants
learn about the basis of communication. Turn-taking in conversations
is an important aspect of establishing early social relationships and
over time the type and frequency of infants’ face-to-face greetings
change from reactive forms of interaction (responding solely to
gestures or speech) to more proactive forms (such as initiating an
interaction with the mother). One way to think about these early
relationships is to see them as a series of reciprocal interactions that
occur between the child, the caregiver and their environment
(Sameroff, 2009).
83
An introduction to childhood studies and child psychology
Key points
. Early interactions between infant and primary carer(s) are crucial for
a child’s social and emotional development.
. Infants are born with a readiness to relate to others.
. Early relationships can develop through a series of verbal and non-
verbal forms of parent–infant interactions.
. Relationships can often be seen as a series of causal interactions
between the infant and the parent.
. The two-way process between caregiver and infant illustrates the
importance of communication and children’s agency.
Forming attachments
An infant’s very survival depends on the presence of some other
person who can provide the basic needs of warmth, shelter and food
that infants are unable to provide for themselves. However, attachment
theory has provided one of the best theoretical and most influential
accounts for understanding that infant–caregiver relationships have
value beyond physical sustenance. John Bowlby, a British psychiatrist,
was one of the first to highlight the importance of the relationship that
infants have with their primary caregivers. His theory of attachment
was influenced by his early experiences working during the 1930s with
boys who had adjustment problems (Bowlby, 1969). He began to
formulate his idea that disruption to the maternal bond during early
childhood is detrimental to later adjustment in childhood. He
suggested that under normal circumstances, young children form
strong mutual attachments with their primary caregivers, which provide
a strong sense of physical and emotional security. Bowlby believed that
a strong, healthy attachment is based on a long-term, stable
relationship, and at first believed that a single maternal attachment
figure was the best way of achieving this. However, he later believed
that although early attachments are usually formed with the mother,
attachments could also be formed with other prominent individuals
within the infant’s immediate environment.
According to Bowlby (1969), attachment is a deep and enduring
emotional bond that connects one person to another. It is
characterised by specific behaviours, such as children seeking closeness
84
Chapter 4 Social and emotional development
with the attachment figure when upset or threatened and the adult’s
ability to respond sensitively and appropriately to the child’s emotional
needs. Bowlby made a clear distinction between two forms of
attachment – secure and insecure – that have an influence on how
infants and children develop and grow both socially and emotionally.
According to Bowlby, children who are securely attached to their
primary caregiver use this as a secure base for exploration and a safe
figure to return to in times of distress. This theory provides an
explanation of how the parent–child relationship emerges and
influences development, particularly a child’s later social, emotional and
psychological development.
Almost all children will form attachments, whether secure or otherwise.
Mary Ainsworth and colleagues (1978) carried out influential empirical
research on attachment theory by looking at the types of attachment-
related behaviours that occur across infancy and childhood by mothers
and children. Following her observations of many children within the
UK and Uganda, Ainsworth formed the conclusion that, irrespective of
culture, all children show similar patterns of reaction on separation and
reunion with the mother, depending on whether they were securely or
insecurely attached. This led her to develop a classification of different
attachment formations and their accompanying behaviours for securely
and insecurely attached infants. Ainsworth used laboratory
observations, known as the ‘strange situation’ procedure, to assess the
kinds of behaviours and emotional reactions shown by infants and
young children after a series of events, such as separation and reunion
with the mother or a stranger entering the room. She concluded that
sensitive care was the crucial factor in allowing a secure attachment to
form.
In considering the quality and nature of these attachment relationships
between the young infant and the primary caregiver, it is important to
consider the wider social and cultural contexts that can shape these
relationships. For instance, across cultures children lead very different
lives and their parents have very different ideas or expectations of
childhood and the nature of adult–child relationships. In Western
societies we tend to observe intense, high-quality exchanges between
caregivers and infants, which include cuddling, singing or talking in an
attempt to provide stimulation and encourage social exchanges.
However, Schieffelin and Ochs (1986) remind us that these types of
interactions are dependent on cultural assumptions and ideologies.
Among Kaluli mothers in Papua New Guinea, for example, infants are
85
An introduction to childhood studies and child psychology
Figure 2 A woman from the Samburu people with her infant. Different
cultures have different cultural ideologies with regard to parenting
86
Chapter 4 Social and emotional development
87
An introduction to childhood studies and child psychology
88
Chapter 4 Social and emotional development
But it is not just the mother and father who can support a child’s early
social and emotional development; there is evidence for a positive
influence of sibling relations on a child’s later adjustment and
psychological well-being (Kreppner and Lerner, 2013). Interactions
with siblings and other family members are also seen as a critical part
of developing social skills during mid to late childhood.
Figure 3 Siblings can provide a wealth of support and act as role models
for young children through the different ages of childhood
89
An introduction to childhood studies and child psychology
Key points
. Secure attachment is characterised by an enduring emotional bond
between infant and primary caregiver, which lasts over time.
. This early attachment can have a strong influence on a child’s later
social, emotional and psychological development.
. A lack of ‘sensitive parenting’, whether in caregiving or during play,
can affect the quality of these interactions and lead to insecure
attachment behaviours between infant and caregiver.
. Other caregivers and family members including siblings can have a
positive influence on children’s social adjustment and psychological
well-being.
90
Chapter 4 Social and emotional development
91
An introduction to childhood studies and child psychology
92
Chapter 4 Social and emotional development
(‘boys should play with cars and girls should play with dolls’) and they
come to learn about their own cultural beliefs about gender (see
Chapter 12). Often children acquire gender-related prejudices or
stereotypes. But it’s not just gender that affects a child’s sense of
identity during middle childhood. There is also evidence that by the
age of 9 years children often see themselves as belonging to a
particular nationality and are able to assign to themselves the qualities
associated with a particular national identity (see Barrett, 2012). They
also begin to recognise their own ‘racial’ or ethnic background from as
early as the pre-school years and may come to absorb and reproduce
racist attitudes through their social interactions with others (see
Chapter 9).
The journey of self-discovery is complex and is guided not just by the
kinds of social interactions available, such as with family, siblings and
peers, but also by children’s exposure to different types of media,
including television, the internet and books, that represent the cultural
expectations of the particular time and place. As children grow and
develop, they form an understanding of the self and their own personal
identity and this is often associated with the child’s own development
of self-worth and self-esteem. There is also evidence that these early
emotional developments and growing personal identity provide the
building blocks for children’s psychological well-being.
Key points
. Children’s ability to recognise emotions and regulate their own
behaviours or emotional expressions are important for social
interactions.
. Children develop an early sense of self-identity.
. Throughout development, this awareness of self-identity extends to
include aspects of gender, nationality and ‘race’.
. Often the development of identity occurs through observational
learning and children’s exposure to media, television and books.
93
An introduction to childhood studies and child psychology
94
Chapter 4 Social and emotional development
Acts of kindness and support are not only dependent on the child’s
age and social experience, but are also influenced by social and cultural
expectations. In a classic study on cultural differences in prosocial
behaviours, Whiting and Whiting (1975) provided a detailed account of
the development of prosocial behaviours among groups of children
between the ages of 3 and 11 years across six different cultures –
Kenya, Mexico, the Philippines, Japan, India and the United States.
Despite observing children showing prosocial acts, such as cooperation
and sharing, they found that cultural expectations had a strong
influence on the kinds of help or support offered by children. They
found that children from Kenya, Mexico and the Philippines were
more likely to offer help or support and make responsible suggestions
to others than those from Japan, India and the United States. In part,
this can be explained by the child-rearing practices of the time and the
different social and cultural ideologies.
95
An introduction to childhood studies and child psychology
Key points
. Social cognition addresses how people think about themselves and
about others.
. Before or by the age of 5 years children develop a theory of mind
and recognise that others may hold different intentions, beliefs and
desires from their own.
. Recognising the mental states and emotional states of others
enables more complex social interactions to occur throughout
childhood.
. Prosocial behaviour, such as giving, sharing and cooperating, is
mediated by cultural expectations and ideologies and children’s own
social experiences.
96
Chapter 4 Social and emotional development
97
An introduction to childhood studies and child psychology
His findings revealed that for most boys, the important factor affecting
popularity and peer group status was their athletic ability and sporting
prowess. For many, football was often seen as a key feature of
maintaining a strong ‘heterosexual masculinity’ and one that held them
in high regard among their peers. Other indicators of popularity
included fashion and wearing certain clothes or trainers as well as
being dominant and outspoken. Overall, as Swain suggests, masculinity
is often linked to the status held within a particular peer group and
this is influenced by wider social pressures to behave in certain ways.
Psychologists have attempted to assess popularity using quantitative
approaches, such as ‘sociometric’ techniques. These provide measures
of a person’s social status within a group which are based on ratings
for answers to simple questions like ‘Who do you most play with?’ and
‘How much would you like to spend time with this child?’ as well as
observations of their interactions outside the classroom (Ladd
et al., 1999). Using these measures, psychologists have begun to
identify a number of distinct categories of status based on peer
acceptance, which include ‘popular’, ‘rejected’ or ‘neglected’. Each of
these three categories includes a set of naïve assumptions about
whether personality traits are externalised (such as happy, attractive,
aggressive, argumentative) or internalised (anxious, lonely, depressed).
Using this technique, psychologists have been able to identify who is
regarded as popular, what factors influence this popularity and how
peer group status can affect such ratings among different groups (see
the following box).
98
Chapter 4 Social and emotional development
Key points
. Social interaction with peers through middle to late childhood
promotes social and emotional development.
. Children form different types of relationship with peers during the
school years and this can influence their peer acceptance.
. Peer acceptance is an indicator of how much someone is liked or
disliked within a particular group.
. A child’s masculinity or femininity influences popularity and peer
group status.
. Sociometric techniques have been used by psychologists to assess
peer groups and popularity.
99
An introduction to childhood studies and child psychology
Conclusion
The development of social and emotional skills in young children is a
complex process and continues from infancy through to later
childhood. In this chapter you have considered the nature of children’s
early first relationships and how those caregiver–infant dyadic
interactions, whether through care or play, can provide important
building blocks for later social and emotional development. We know
that infants are born with a readiness to relate to others; they recognise
familiar faces and familiar voices from a very early age, and engaging
in reciprocal types of interaction allows for the development of secure
attachments to other individuals, who may include mothers, fathers and
siblings. Beyond early infancy, we have seen that children develop an
awareness of themselves as well as an understanding of the mental
states of others. The knowledge that other people hold different
desires and beliefs about the world (having a ‘theory of mind’) is an
important aspect of developing friendships and peer groups at school
and allows children to engage in more complex social relationships
beyond the family during mid to late childhood. In this sense, early
social and emotional development plays an important role throughout
the child’s life and continues to shape who they are and who they
become in future years.
100
Chapter 4 Social and emotional development
References
Ainsworth, M.D.S., Blehar, M.C., Waters, E. and Wall, S. (1978) Patterns of
Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation, New Jersey,
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Barrett, M. (2012) ‘Beliefs about other countries’, in Byram, M. and Hu, A.
(eds), Routledge Encyclopedia of Language Teaching and Learning , 2nd edn,
London, Routledge, pp. 78–83.
Boer, F., Dunn, J. and Dunn, J.F. (eds) (2013) Children’s Sibling Relationships:
Developmental and Clinical Issues, London, Psychology Press.
Bornstein, M.H. (2013) ‘Mother–infant attunement’, in Legerstee, M., Haley,
D.W. and Bornstein, M.H. (eds) The Infant Mind: Origins of the Social Brain,
New York, Guilford Press, pp. 266–300.
Bowlby, J. (1969) Attachment and Loss, vol. 1: Attachment, New York, Basic
Books.
Bretherton, I. (2010) ‘Fathers in attachment theory and research: a review’,
Early Child Development and Care, vol. 180, no. 1/2, pp. 9–23.
Bureau, J-F. Martin, J., Yurkowski, K., Schmiedel, S., Quan, J., Moss, E.,
Deneault, A.-A. and Pallanca , D. (2017) ‘Correlates of child–father and child–
mother attachment in the preschool years’, Attachment & Human
Development. vol. 19, no. 2, pp. 130–150.
Cassaniti, J. I. and Menon, U. (2017) Universalism without Uniformity:
Explorations in Mind and Culture, Chicago, University of Chicago Press.
Damian, R.I. and Robins, R.W. (2012) ‘Investigations into the human self: a
naturalist perspective’, Social Cognition, vol. 30, special issue, pp. 431–48.
Darwin, C. (1872) The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals,
London, John Murray.
Ekman, P. and Friesen, W.V. (1971) ‘Constants across cultures in the face and
emotion’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 17, no. 2, pp. 124–9.
Gerhardt, S. (2006) ‘Why love matters: how affection shapes a baby’s brain’,
Infant Observation, vol. 9, no. 3, pp. 305–9.
Golombok, S. (2000) Parenting: What Really Counts?, London, Routledge.
Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K.E., Fremmer-Bombik, E., Kindler, H.,
Scheuerer-Englisch, H. and Zimmermann, P. (2002) ‘The Uniqueness of the
Child–Father Attachment Relationship: Fathers’ Sensitive and Challenging Play
as a Pivotal Variable in a 16-year Longitudinal Study’. Social Development, vol
11, no. 3, pp. 307–331.
Grossmann, T. (2010) ‘The development of emotion perception in face and
voice during infancy’, Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, vol. 28,
pp. 219–36.
101
An introduction to childhood studies and child psychology
102