Society For Research in Child Development, Wiley Child Development
Society For Research in Child Development, Wiley Child Development
Society For Research in Child Development, Wiley Child Development
Author(s): Robert B. McCall, Pamela S. Hogarty, Jayne S. Hamilton and John H. Vincent
Source: Child Development, Vol. 44, No. 2 (Jun., 1973), pp. 280-287
Published by: Wiley on behalf of the Society for Research in Child Development
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1128048
Accessed: 26-06-2016 11:46 UTC
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
http://about.jstor.org/terms
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted
digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about
JSTOR, please contact [email protected].
Society for Research in Child Development, Wiley are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize,
preserve and extend access to Child Development
This content downloaded from 128.205.114.91 on Sun, 26 Jun 2016 11:46:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Habituation Rate and the Infant's Response
to Visual Discrepancies
MCCALL, ROBERT B.; HOGARTY, PAMELA S.; HAMILTON, JAYNE S.; and VINCENT, JOHN H.
Habituation Rate and the Infant's Response to Visual Discrepancies. CHILD DEVELOPMENT,
1973, 44, 280-287. A simple visual stimulus was repeatedly presented to 120 infants 12 and 18
weeks of age until visual fixation reached a habituation criterion. A discrepant stimulus fol-
lowed that varied in its magnitude of discrepancy from the familiar standard. In contrast to
the age effects observed for the rate of habituation (though they interacted with specific
stimuli), there were no age differences in the distribution of fixation times to the several
magnitudes of discrepancy. Infants who habituated rapidly displayed an inverted-U curve of
fixation as a function of discrepancy in accord with the discrepancy hypothesis, whereas slow
habituators gave their maximum response to the largest discrepancy.
This content downloaded from 128.205.114.91 on Sun, 26 Jun 2016 11:46:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
McCall et al. 281
magnitude of discrepancy represented by the Cantor 1966; Pancratz & Cohen 1970), and
novel stimulus as a possible determinant of some have found sex differences in the response
attentional behavior. The discrepancy hypothe- to novel stimuli (e.g., Meyers & Cantor 1967;
sis (e.g., see McCall 1971) predicts attentional Pancratz & Cohen 1970).
behavior to be an inverted-U function of the
magnitude of discrepancy. Such a curvilinear Method
relationship presumably reflects the intersection
of two vectors: (1) increasing attention with Subjects
increasing amounts of information, and (2) Subjects, 12 and 18 weeks of age, were
decreasing attention as the stimulus information recruited by sending letters to parents whose
becomes more difficult to process by existing names appeared in the newspaper birth an-
cognitive structures and integrated with estab- nouncements. Approximately 12% of the let-
lished engrams. ters resulted in contacts, and a total of 157
infants were seen. However, 37 subjects were
Empirically, the inverted-U prediction is not included in the data analysis: 20 because
troublesome because there is no theoretical
of fretting and lack of cooperation, 10 due to
basis for deciding how much stimulus discrep- equipment and operator failure, and 7 because
ancy is sufficient to produce the downturn in of other circumstances (i.e., had abnormal
the response curve (Thomas 1971). Thus, al- medical histories, fell asleep, etc.). The re-
though some studies of infant attention have maining 120 infants formed the sample for
observed the complete inverted-U function these observations, and their characteristics are
(McCall 1972; McCall & Melson 1969), others presented in table 1.
have only hinted at it (McCall & Kagan 1967)
Procedure
or found a simple linearly increasing relation-
ship (McCall & Kagan 1970) between dis- Stimuli.-The stimuli are presented in
crepancy and response. All of these results figure 1. They were professionally photo-
might be consonant with the discrepancy hy- graphed (in color) and rear projected by an
pothesis, depending upon the subject's percep- Airequipt 350/EF projector with a 500-w
tion of the degree of discrepancy represented lamp. The diameter of the circular element of
by the set of experimental stimuli. A linear the stimulus was 10 inches, and was located
versus curvilinear result not only depends upon approximately 24 inches from the subject's
the particular stimuli-it might also depend eyes. A stimulus consisted of a black circle
upon the character of the memory engram, per- with a white interior on a yellow background.
ceptual-cognitive style of the subject (i.e., In the center of each circle was an arrow,
rapid vs. slow habituator), age of the infant, which was either a saturated blue (stimulus
etc. Thus, it was of interest to explore the rela- A), blue green (stimulus B), saturated green
tive response to a dimension of discrepancy as (stimulus C), or yellow green (stimulus D).
a function of sex, age, and habituation rate. Unfortunately, the photography minimized the
distinctiveness of these color differentials. The
Another purpose was to focus on possible stimuli were conceived to form a dimension of
age differences in habituation and response to similarity in the alphabetical order presented in
discrepancy. Younger infants have been found figure 1.
to habituate more slowly than older subjects
(e.g., Fantz 1964; Lewis 1969; McGurk 1972). General procedure.-All observations were
Further, several studies indicate that infants do
TABLE 1
not respond to a discrepancy more than to a
familiar event until approximately 2 months SAMPLE CHARACTERISTICS (N = 120)
of age (see McCall 1971), but this depends
upon the assessment procedures (Siqueland Characteristic Males Females
1969). An experimental design that habituates Firstborn ............ ..... 25 20
infants to a criterion could assess age differ- No birth anesthetic ........ 7 13
ences in habituation separately from age differ- Abnormal birth positiona ... 6 5
ences in the response to several magnitudes Breast-fedb ................ 21 27
of discrepancy. Complicated pregnancy ..... 3 7
Birth weight (oz) .......... 124.9 118.1
Parental educatione ........ 14.4 14.5
Finally, some investigators have observed
sex differences in habituation to simple visual SBreach, transverse, other.
b Includes all subjects not totally bottle-fed.
stimuli (Horowitz & Paden 1969; Meyers & C 12 years equals high school graduate.
This content downloaded from 128.205.114.91 on Sun, 26 Jun 2016 11:46:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
282 Child Development
This content downloaded from 128.205.114.91 on Sun, 26 Jun 2016 11:46:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
McCall et al. 283
design was a sex X age (12, 18 weeks) X without time constraint, the data were trans-
standard stimulus (A, D) X magnitude of formed by X' = loglo X.
discrepancy (0,1,2,3) matrix.
The principal result of the above analysis
Results was an age X standard stimulus x trials in-
teraction, Fmult (6,107) = 2.16, p = .05,
Habituation which was largely a difference in quadratic
Although Ss were given as many familiar- trend in the habituation curves of these groups,
ization trials as necessary to reach criterion, all F (1,112) = 7.99, p = .006.
Ss were required to view at least seven presen-
These data are plotted in figure 2. Note
tations of the standard. Ordinarily, such data
that the two 18-week groups tended to have
might be analyzed with an age (12, 18) x shorter fixation times from the first trial and
sex x standard stimulus (vertical, horizontal more regular and shallower habituation curves,
arrow) X trials (7) repeated measures analysis
but the difference in general length of fixation
of variance. However, because fixation time
between the ages was much greater for Ss
across the seven trials was not homogeneously viewing the horizontal than the vertically ori-
correlated (i.e., there was heterogeneity of co-
ented standard stimulus, age x standard,
variance), this conventional analysis would F(1,112) = 8.82, p = .004. There was also a
yield results which were seriously biased in a
sex X standard x trials interaction, FmuIt
positive direction (see McCall & Appelbaum (6,107), p = .01, but this was a difference in
in press] for a nontechnical discussion of this
the sixth-degree trend contrast, F(1,112) =
issue and alternative procedures). Another ap-
15.11, p < .001, in which females viewing the
proach, not dependent upon the nature of the
horizontal standard showed an isolated dip in
covariances, involves analyzing the within-sub-
looking on the fourth trial. Apart from this
jects effects by submitting the six orthogonal unexplained deflection, there were no sex dif-
polynomial contrasts for trials for each S to an
ferences of the type reported by Pancratz and
age x sex X standard stimulus multivariate Cohen (1970).
analysis of variance (see McCall [1970] for a
brief description of multivariate analysis of The number of stimulus presentations
variance). Since Ss could look at the stimulus (trials) and the total time spent looking at the
0-0 horizontal
? ? 12 week AmA vertical
.70 0A EsA
18 week o . .6 horizontal
vertical
X
U6
.50
.60
Z .40 *
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
TRIALS
FIG. 2.-Log fixation time during the required seven-trial familiarization period as a function of age
and standard stimulus.
This content downloaded from 128.205.114.91 on Sun, 26 Jun 2016 11:46:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
284 Child Development
slow
standard prior to reaching criterion were two
.70
additional indices of the rate of habituation.
These two measures were submitted to a multi-
.60
variate analysis of variance with sex, standard
stimulus, and age as independent variables
(N = 15 per cell). A multivariate analysis of S.0so
variance is an extension of univariate analysis
.4017
of variance to consider the weighted combina-
tion of several variables (in this case, two).
.30
0
There was a significant multivariate effect
for age, Fmnit (2,111) = 5.07, p = .008, in - .20
rapid
which the time measure reached univariate
significance, F(1,112) = 9.65, p = .002. .10
This content downloaded from 128.205.114.91 on Sun, 26 Jun 2016 11:46:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
McCall et al. 285
(12 vs. 18 weeks) X habituation group (rapid the curve for slow habituators, the only signifi-
vs. slow) X magnitude of discrepancy (0,1,2, cant trend was linear, F(1,104) = 9.01, p =
3) analysis of variance. Preliminary analyses .003.
indicated no effects or interactions of these
factors with sex or standard stimulus, and con- Discussion
sequently these variables were ignored in the
analyses that follow. The entire five-way anal- These data contain two major results.
ysis could not be performed because some cells First, infants who habituated rapidly to a
would have been vacant.
simple stimulus pattern looked maximally at a
As with the habituation data, the depen- pattern representing a moderate amount of
discrepancy from the familiarized stimulus,
dent variable was the log10 fixation time. Inas-
much as previous studies had indicated that while infants who required more trials to reach
stimulus differences were likely only on the an habituation criterion displayed their maxi-
response to the first presentation of a dis- mum looking to a relatively more discrepant
crepancy (e.g., McCall & Kagan 1970), log stimulus. Second, although older infants habitu-
fixation times to the first and second discrepant ated more rapidly than younger subjects (only
stimuli were analyzed separately. There were for the horizontal standard stimulus), there
no significant differences on the second dis- were no age differences or interactions with
crepancy. Because all Ss were habituated to a magnitude of discrepancy, F < 1, in the re-
common criterion prior to the introduction of a sponse to discrepancies.
discrepant stimulus, the response to the discrep- At first thought, the rapid-slow habitua-
ant stimulus alone was the dependent variable tion results appear to be in conflict with
(rather than a discrepant-minus-previous-stan- previous data (e.g., McCall & Kagan 1970)
dard change score). However, to insure that which indicated that rapid but not slow habitu-
results were not a function of differences in
ators responded positively to discrepant stimuli.
response style and "initial values," the analyses Because the previous studies used a fixed num-
were repeated using the response to the stan- ber of familiarization trials regardless of the
dard immediately preceding the first discrep- infant's behavior, these results can be viewed
ancy as a covariate as suggested by Cronbach as quite consistent. Collectively, they suggest
and Furby (1970). that if an infant has not habituated to a
The analysis of variance indicated three stimulus, he will not respond positively to a
effects: magnitude of discrepancy, F (3,104) = discrepant event and may even display relative
3.26, p = .025; habituation group, F(1,104) gaze avoidance to moderately discrepant
= 12.29, p < .001; and a habituation group- stimuli (e.g., McCall 1972; McCall & Kagen
magnitude of discrepancy interaction, F (3,104) 1970). However, if such infants are permitted
= 3.31, p = .023. Each of these effects was to view the standard until they reach a be-
havioral criterion of habituation, they will
also significant (at only slightly larger proba-
respond to discrepancies, and slow habituators
bilities) when the response to the previous
standard was covaried. There were no age may respond more to larger magnitudes of
effects or interactions (two of three F's < 1). discrepancy than rapid habituators.
This content downloaded from 128.205.114.91 on Sun, 26 Jun 2016 11:46:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
286 Child Development
ceptual issue, the data have several implica- (Salapatek 1968). Thus, it may be that 12-
tions. First, the results are consistent with the week-old infants are as able as 18-weekers to
proposition that the nature of habituation may process a stimulus whose major feature presents
reflect some aspect of the internal process of its contours directly to such a horizontal scan-
acquiring a memory engram for the standard ning strategy. However, when the arrow is
stimulus. Infants who habituate, respond posi- horizontal, examination of its contours requires
tively to new stimuli; if the discrepancy is a more sophisticated scanning strategy, and
introduced before habituation is relatively com- age differences are more obvious.
plete, the subject does not respond positively No sex differences in habituation rate were
to discrepancies.1
observed, despite the fact that others have
A second feature of these data is the clear found more rapid habituation to simple visual
inverted-U result displayed by the rapid habitu- stimuli for boys than girls (e.g., Pancratz &
ators. The fact that two different standards Cohen 1970).
were used without an interaction with magni-
Finally, the fact that age differences were
tude of discrepancy accentuates the role of
present for habituation but not the response to
discrepancy per se, apart from specific physical
discrepancy suggests that whatever develop-
stimulus characteristics, as the causal factor.
mental processes transpire between 12 and 18
Moreover, the observation that the trend was
linear for slow habituators but curvilinear for weeks they may have relatively greater in-
fluence on encoding information into the mem-
rapid habituators agreed with other results ory system and related processes than on
(McCall 1972) in suggesting that the form of retrieving perceptual-cognitive information and
the obtained relationship will not only depend using it to interact with new stimuli. Other
upon the particular levels of discrepancy used data are consistent with such a hypothesis. For
but habituation and/or subject characteristics.
example, Papousek (1961) has shown that
This result joins with other data (McCall 1972;
while there were marked age differences in the
McCall & Melson 1969) in indicating that a acquisition rate of conditioning infant head
curvilinear function may be the basic under-
rotation, the process of extinction was remark-
lying relationship between attention and dis- ably similar across several ages in the first year
crepancy, and that observations of a linear of life. Campbell and Spear (1972) have re-
trend may derive from using stimuli which do
cently reviewed the animal literature on learn-
not represent a sufficient range of discrepancies.
ing and development and conclude that while
Unfortunately, there is no a priori theoretical
there are age differences in the retention of a
definition of "sufficient," but this conceptual
conditioned avoidance response there are no
inelegance does not obviate the potential em- differences in rate of extinction. Finally, Sique-
pirical validity of the inverted-U prediction land (1969) reported a study using 1- and 4-
(Thomas 1971).
month-old infants who were given pacifiers
While previous experiments have demon- which, when sucked, would present or termi-
strated that habituation rate is a function of age nate a visual stimulus. Once asymptotic re-
(e.g., Lewis 1969; McGurk 1972) and stimulus sponding was achieved, a new visual stimulus
complexity (e.g., Caron & Caron 1968), the was suddenly introduced as a reinforcer. While
results of the present study suggest a possible the general level of response differed for the
interaction between age and stimulus proper- two age groups, there was no interaction be-
ties. While an age x complexity dependency tween age and the relative increase in sucking
might be expected, the stimuli in the present in response to the new stimulus. Although
study did not obviously differ in complexity as there must be age differences under some cir-
usually defined. A speculative explanation of cumstances, these studies suggest a minimum
why age differences occurred more clearly for age effect within the first year of life for be-
the horizontal relative to the vertical arrow haviors requiring some comparison with an
might be rooted in the propensity of very young already learned perceptual or associative mem-
infants to scan horizontally more than vertically ory engram.
1 It should be noted that the "short-looking" group in McCall and Kagan (1970) which
apparently did not habituate in the graph presented in that paper actually did habituate.
Because the first stimulus appeared to scare subjects, it was not included in the habituation
analysis. If it were included, the "short lookers" would have habituated by the second trial,
the "rapid habituators" by the fifth trial, and the "slow habituators" not at all. Thus, these
groups should more properly be called "most rapid," "rapid," and "nonhabituators."
This content downloaded from 128.205.114.91 on Sun, 26 Jun 2016 11:46:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
McCall et al. 287
This content downloaded from 128.205.114.91 on Sun, 26 Jun 2016 11:46:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms