Analyzing Qualitative Data
Analyzing Qualitative Data
Analyzing Qualitative Data
In 1993, Bradley and Sutton wrote that “the serious cultivation of the potential
of qualitative research has yet to emerge” (1993, p. 405). Four years later,
Vakkari (1997, p. 458) concluded that the development in library and informa-
tion studies (LIS) is methodologic proliferation: both quantitative and qualita-
tive techniques are applied, although qualitative techniques have become more
popular, at least for information needs and seeking research. A recent trend in
qualitative research in LIS is the shift of attention from data-gathering methods
to the methods of data description, analysis, and interpretation. Qualitative
methods are increasingly being understood as explicitly theory-dependent ways
of describing, analyzing, and interpreting data. Such approaches as discourse
analysis (Frohmann, 1994), frame analysis (Chelton, 1998), phenomenography
(Bruce & Klaus, 1998), conversation analysis (Solomon, 1997), and deconstruc-
tionism (Olson, 1997) require specific norms and guidelines for data analysis;
they do not use a generally descriptive, hermeneutic, or naturalistic method.
In the 1980s and early 1990s, methodologic discussion in the social sciences was
still essentially bound up in the contrasting of qualitative and quantitative meth-
ods. Although the contrastive model was “neither accurate nor particularly help-
ful” (Sutton, 1993, p. 411), it is the reason why assessments of the merits and weak-
Direct all correspondence to: Sanna Talja, Department of Information Studies, University of Tampere,
P.O. Box 607, FIN-33101, Tampere, Finland ,lisaka@uta.fi..
Library & Information Science Research, Volume 21, Number 4, pages 459–477.
Copyright © 1999 by Elsevier Science Inc.
All rights of reproduction in any form reserved. ISSN: 0740-8188
459
460 Talja
nesses of qualitative research have been almost entirely connected to the data-
gathering phase, whereas the methods of data analysis and interpretation have
often not been discussed in similar detail. The contrastive model is based on the
concept that researchers have to choose between a “humanistic,” subject-centered
approach that aims to capture participants’ indigenous meanings and experiences,
or a “hard,” statistical approach that describes concrete facts or society’s larger
structural processes. In LIS, qualitative methods have mainly been viewed as a
welcome alternative to survey-dominated user studies conducted from the infor-
mation systems’ or institutional point of view (see, e.g., Dervin & Nilan, 1986).
This article introduces a method of qualitative analysis in which the basic ana-
lytic unit is the interpretative repertoire (see Gilbert & Mulkay, 1984; Potter &
Wetherell, 1987; Wetherell & Potter, 1988) and attempts to show the value of a
method of qualitative analysis that does not aim at capturing participants’ au-
thentic intentions, meanings, or experiences. In discourse analysis, interview data
are analyzed at a macrosociologic level, as social texts. Discourse analysis is an
approach that surpasses the dichotomy between subjective meanings and objec-
tive reality, as well as the dichotomy between user-centered and system-centered
research (see Talja, 1997). It concentrates on the analysis of knowledge forma-
tions, which organize institutional practices and societal reality on a large scale.
Discourse analysis is a part of the linguistic turn in the social sciences and the hu-
manities that emphasizes the role of language in the construction of social reality. It
is one of the dominant or mainstream research approaches in communication, soci-
ology, social psychology, and psychology. Although several articles have discussed
the application of discourse analysis in information studies (e.g., Budd & Raber,
1996; Frohmann, 1994; Talja, 1997; Talja et al., 1997; Tuominen & Savolainen,
1997), it has, thus far, been used relatively little as a concrete research method. This
article demonstrates, with examples from a study of library users’ library talk
(Talja, 1998), how the discourses existing in a particular field can be identified.
Some critics have argued that discourse analysis directs LIS researchers’ at-
tention to “private language use.” It should be noted, however, that the term
“discourse” is used within diverse research approaches that do not necessarily
have common theoretical footings. The version of discourse analysis described
in this article differs in focus from the interest in interaction and mundane talk
dominant in discursive social psychology and conversation analysis. As Froh-
mann (1994, p. 120) emphasized, Foucault-influenced discourse analysis does
not study the rules and conventions of mundane talk; rather, it examines “seri-
ous speech acts,” institutionalized talk or practices. This does not mean that the
participants of the study should be institutionally privileged speakers. Instead,
regardless of the roles and positions of the participants, talk is studied as an ex-
ample of more general interpretative practices.
Discourse analysis studies practices of producing knowledge and meanings
in concrete contexts and institutions - be they in library organizations, informa-
tion studies, information society strategies, database interfaces, or the Dewey
Decimal Classification. Discourse analysis systematizes different ways of talk-
Analyzing Qualitative Interview Data 461
ing to make visible the perspectives and starting points on the basis of which
knowledge and meanings are produced in a particular historical moment. It
pays attention to the way in which discourses produce and transform social re-
ality, and makes it possible to evaluate the practical consequences of different
ways of approaching a particular phenomenon.
The article introduces the basic research strategy of the analysis of interpreta-
tive repertoires: the systematic examination of context-dependent variability in
talk and texts. Second, it explains how the discourses existing in a particular field
can be identified. The terms “discourse” and “interpretative repertoire” are syn-
onyms. The identification of interpretative repertoires is the endpoint of dis-
course analysis, and a repertoire is a named discourse. Third, the article explains
how discourse analysis differs from the hermeneutic reading of qualitative data.
Fourth, the criteria of validity and reliability in qualitative research are discussed.
Finally, the article discusses the application of discourse analysis in LIS research.
Example 1
A: Real people, quite simply. It should be the starting point that li-
brary services are for the ordinary people. Although I don’t listen to
mass music, to the Rolling Stones, or to Madonna, it does not mean
that I can deny others such music. If the majority of people like the
music, then the library should offer it. And if I want something spe-
cial, I can ask for it, and they can order it as a remote loan. The library
should take care of ordinary people and their needs, their cultural
needs, and recreational needs. It is the starting point that the library
should serve the majority of people, and it should not be turned into a
money machine, like our government is aiming to do. (Interviewee 8)
chine,” “people would be much poorer culturally and mentally”). Let us take
another example.
Example 2
In the first extract, the interviewee emphasizes that the library should not
strive to cultivate users’ tastes. Collection work should be systematically based
on demand or researched user needs. In the second extract, the library is an in-
stitution that encourages people to aim at higher goals and educates them by its
very atmosphere and the nature of its collections.
464 Talja
The kind of variation and inconsistency seen in the extracts is not an exception,
nor is it a product of the interview situation (for similar examples, see Gilbert &
Mulkay, 1984; Machin & Carrithers, 1996; Potter & Mulkay, 1985; Potter &
Wetherell, 1987). Inconsistencies can also easily be found in the answers to sur-
vey questionnaires. However, such variation is usually managed by analytic strat-
egies of restriction, for instance categorization, coding, and selective reading, be-
cause researchers are accustomed to regarding the individual as a coherent,
consistent unit and as the natural starting point for their investigations (Potter &
Wetherell, 1987; Wetherell & Potter, 1988). Variability between the accounts
that people give is not part of this picture. Moreover, it is customary to assume
that the object of talk, for instance the library, exists as a permanent and coherent
whole of which different people simply have different opinions and experiences.1
In the discourse analytic approach, the researcher abandons the assumption
that there is only one truly accurate version of participants’ action and belief. In-
terview talk is, by nature, interpretation work concerning the topic in question. It is
reflexive, theoretical, contextual, and textual, because the objects of talk (e.g., the li-
brary) are not abstract, ideal entities everyone sees in the same way. When talking
about the library, participants do not solely produce a neutral description and ex-
press their opinion. They produce a version of the library (“a cradle of counter cul-
ture,” or “an institution above shopping centers”), and this version contains an
evaluation. As Wittgenstein (1971) has noted, in normal language each expression
not only states, but also evaluates. The nature and essence of the library institution
are defined in interview talk, and speakers’ opinions concern these specific formu-
lations, context-dependent versions of the library. Descriptions, evaluations, and
large-scale cultural models of accounting are inseparably bound together (Potter
& Wetherell, 1987). A third example represents a library professional’s talk.
Example 3
• Users make the library what it is through their own presence. They also se-
lect the library’s collections, if libraries are user-centered.
• At this moment we in the library have to consider very carefully what is
most important for our customers. Is it education, research, and study; is it
recreation and entertainment; or is it the everyday following of events?
We have come to the conclusion that the first consideration is, by far, the
1 This is particularly evident when considering the tradition of social surveys and opinion
polls, which is based on the assumption that neither the questionnaire’s formulation and con-
textualization of the topic nor the persuasiveness and cultural status of particular statements
in a specific moment in time and space significantly affect the answering. It is assumed that
people actually hold such one-dimensional and context-independent opinions as described
by the conclusions the research enabled.
Analyzing Qualitative Interview Data 465
2 When interpreting things it is always possible to bring up several different aspects. The
chosen or privileged interpretation depends on the context of talk and to whom the talk is di-
rected. For instance, if an adolescent reader is asked to give her opinion about a Nancy Drew
book, description to her best friend is likely to be different from the one offered to a teacher
in a classroom setting. Both interpretations are equally authentic, because the reading expe-
rience is, in itself, seldom unambiguous. Usually, it consists of multiple, both positively and
negatively experienced, impulses. In the linear process of reading, the reader and the text are
in symbiosis. The reader identifies with the implicit reader of the book (Vainikkala, 1989).
The spoken description of the reading experience is necessarily an afterward rationalization
in which some of all the manifold impulses are chosen as important, and the reading experi-
ence itself receives a form and a meaning.
466 Talja
tion, the speakers are simultaneously able to retain in memory only two or
three of their latest turns (Gilbert & Mulkay, 1984). If, however, two different
viewpoints are taken into consideration in the same section of talk, the speak-
ers usually show an awareness of their possible inconsistencies and attempt to
resolve them.3 If this orientation toward resolving potential contradictions does
not appear when different versions of the topic are produced in different sec-
tions of talk, it is a clear sign of the existence of different interpretative reper-
toires (Gilbert & Mulkay, 1984; Wetherell & Potter, 1988).
4 Saussure (1983) has noted that the signs of language describe values, not real and permanent
“essences.” Language is basically a system of distinctions in which the meaning of a single word
depends on its difference from other words. Language consists of distinctions, but, in permanent
use, a word can become cemented as one-accented, so that it seems to have only one unchange-
able meaning (Volosinov, 1986). That is why it is not necessarily noted that talk about “quality,”
for instance, can be meaningful only if it has opposites, entities that are not considered to repre-
sent quality. However, “quality” must also be linked with attributes, which specify the meaning of
the term by their presence. One significant point of incompatibility in library talk can be found in
interpretations of what kinds of entities represent and which attributes define “quality.”
468 Talja
it, all the minuscule and passing moments of social changes are crystallized in
words.
There are simultaneously several more-or-less conflicting discourses existing
in a particular field of knowledge or a particular institution at a certain point in
time, because novel or alternative interpretations emerge as corrections to
prior discourses. These earlier discourses appear in some respects erroneous or
one sided. Changes in social experiences and possibilities slowly render histori-
cally strong discourses less valid and accurate. These discourses seem to misin-
terpret some of the essential features or the “true nature” of the discussed phe-
nomenon. Established ways of conceptualizing and approaching phenomena do
not, however, vanish as their validity begins to be questioned. Novel interpreta-
tions gradually become established, and alternative discourses exist side by side
in the same field of knowledge. That is why discourses are internally relatively
coherent, but mutually contradictory and alternative.
5 For instance, the “guide for higher destinations” library metaphor is based on the state-
ment that there are cultural products that educate, or lead to spiritual growth, and that
merely entertain. The “cradle of counter culture” metaphor is based on the classification of
cultural products to commercial and conventional “mainstream” and noncommercial, devi-
ant “counter culture” or “alternatives.”
Analyzing Qualitative Interview Data 469
aims at broadness, and that the function of school and academic libraries is to
support education and study. However, the facts legitimizing a particular ver-
sion of reality are rarely ever simply true or false (Foucault, 1972). The variabil-
ity in participants’ accounts results from the fact that even generally accepted
facts or empirically supported truths, and logical, well-founded opinions can be
in conflict with one another (Billig et al., 1988). The constitution, interpreta-
tion, and weighing of facts always depend on what is considered important,
valuable, or desirable. Volosinov (1986) emphasized it is always a particular
viewpoint, or horizon of evaluation, that brings “the facts” into speakers’ sight.
When made visible, the statements building a discourse are always relative,
susceptible to dispute and denial (Foucault, 1972). However, because statements
are a part of an established and naturalized way of speaking, they are not nor-
mally, in themselves, taken under scrutiny. The utterances produced on the basis
of established discourses are normally received simply as “grammatical,” that is,
as logical and believable descriptions of “how things are” (Foucault, 1972).
Discourses, in the context of their rules and conceptualizations, provide the
space in which things can be talked about (Hall, 1992, p. 291). On one hand,
they limit the ways in which it is possible to make sense of things, but on the
other hand, they provide conflicting and variable viewpoints. As discourses
provide the language for talking about a topic, for presenting knowledge and
views, in a profound sense, they construct the lived reality (Hall, 1992; Wether-
ell & Potter, 1988). The perspectives and horizons of interpretation that give
structure to social action, practices, and relationships are not normally con-
sciously reflected, because they are part of the everyday order of things. The
choices of perspectives also have practical consequences, but analyzing these
consequences does not mean speculating about individual speakers’ intentions.
It means exploring the connotations, allusions, and implications that particular
discursive forms evoke (Foucault, 1972, 1980; Parker, 1992). Discourses are not
individuals’ creations: they have taken their shape with the passage of time,
they reflect the whole history of the societal form, and they have effects that no
one has consciously intended.
Discourse analysts are not interested in processes taking place either in individ-
uals’ minds or in reality. They concentrate on the regularities of language use:
what kinds of descriptions and accounts of a topic are possible, what kinds of
evaluations are they based on, how do different modes of accounting construct
different versions of the topic or produce different kinds of truths, and what do
these versions accomplish (Wetherell & Potter, 1988). It is seen that discourses
are “historical facts,” but the speakers using them, topics to which they are ap-
plied, and attitudes toward them can, and do, change continuously (Foucault,
1972; Machin & Carrithers, 1996).
470 Talja
ternal, sovereign territory, he is wholly and always on the boundary; looking in-
side himself, he looks into the eyes of another or with the eyes of another.”
It may seem irrelevant whether the research object is defined as the subject’s
sensemaking or linguistic practices. Surely, talk expresses what is in an individ-
ual’s mind. In normal life people do have to act on the basis of the assumption
that language straightforwardly and accurately describes both mental events
and the outside reality. However, language is not just a tool to be taken up and
put down at will, when we have something to communicate (Williams, 1977).
Language is also an indispensable part of the subject’s self-understanding, be-
cause words are present in every act of interpretation (Volosinov, 1986).
According to Bakhtin (1981), the words of language are always half someone
else’s: when individuals use words, they formulate themselves and their
thoughts from the point of others, from the point of view of their community.
Words become speakers’ “own” as they use them for their own purposes, in-
clude their own intentions in them (Bakhtin, 1981). Individuals cannot, how-
ever, invent new words to express their intentions. They have to use the same
expressions that have been used countless times before (Volosinov, 1986). For
instance, libraries are routinely defined as providers of education and recre-
ation, even if at closer reflection people probably do not think that serious
reading could not be entertaining, or that entertainment could not educate. It is
as if the words called for quotation marks to be set around them. “Language’s
own talk” is, however, supreme in its power compared with individual speakers’
views, because preexisting conceptualizations and ways of classifying phenom-
ena have to be used even by speakers whose conscious intention it is to oppose
them (Hall, 1982). Individuals are not able to modify the resources of interpre-
tation freely because they are limited by the episteme of a specific cultural and
historical phase. However, discourses, like individuals, are variable, conflicting,
and continuously changing and developing.
pretative repertoires identified is then tested against a larger set of data. In this
process, the model also becomes more detailed and rich, and the researcher’s
understanding of the starting points and statements behind different ways of
talking increases.
In the factist approach, the reliability of research findings can be increased
by methodologic triangulation using multiple data-gathering methods (e.g., ob-
servation, interviews, and diary techniques). In the study of interpretative prac-
tices, the reliability and generalizability of research findings can be enhanced
by combining different types of research materials, interviews, and written
texts, and by contextual triangulation. According to Foucault (1972), one crite-
rion for the existence of a “discourse” is that it is used in a variety of contexts
and that it can be applied in the handling of a variety of themes. If the analysis
of interviews can be extended by making explicit comparisons between differ-
ent settings or contexts of discussion, the research does not comprise a case
study with restricted generalizability.
In the study used as an example in this article, the interpretative repertoires
found in users’ library talk were compared with handbooks of music library ac-
tivity and library professionals’ writings. The themes and concepts that were
disputed in professional discourse were also themes of discussion in users’ talk.
Next, library talk was compared with texts outlining broadcasting and radio
politics, as well as official reports outlining governmental cultural policy. All
these texts contained the same conflicting viewpoints (which were called the
general education repertoire, the alternative repertoire, and the demand reper-
toire). In this way, it could be shown how the formation of the library institu-
tion, often viewed as a product of library professionals’ action, depends on
more general, historical knowledge formations.
REFERENCES