Botoom Up Top Down Processing
Botoom Up Top Down Processing
Botoom Up Top Down Processing
Theory and Practice in Language Studies, Vol. 4, No. 4, pp. 686-693, April 2014
© 2014 ACADEMY PUBLISHER Manufactured in Finland.
doi:10.4304/tpls.4.4.686-693
Abstract—The present study aimed at exploring the effects of top-down/bottom-up processing and
field-dependent/field-independent cognitive style on Iranian EFL learners’ reading comprehension. That is, it
was attempted to find firstly, whether FI learners would perform better when taught through top-down or
bottom-up reading instruction model; secondly, whether FD learners would perform better when taught
through top-down or bottom-up reading instruction model. Two intact classes including 40 Iranian freshmen
EFL students with the same level of reading proficiency tested through reading section of TOEFL test
participated in this study. One class was randomly assigned to top- down reading instruction model and the
other to bottom- up approach. The study consisted of three stages. At first stage, Group Embedded Figure
Test (GEFT) was administered to determine the distribution of subjects along the spectrum of FD/ FI in each
group. Then, the treatment was run and finally, both groups received a reading comprehension posttest. The
data were analyzed using two independent samples t-tests. The results revealed that FI learners outperform
their FD counterparts in bottom- up group. Moreover, the results showed that FD learners were more
successful than FI ones when taught through top- down reading instruction model. In light of these findings,
the researchers offered recommendations for further research and for EFL reading educators.
I. INTRODUCTION
The most important skill for second language learners in academic context is reading (Lynch& Hudson, 1991, cited in
Grabe, 1991). Thus, it can be concluded that perhaps the most fundamental skill to be taught in Iranian academic context,
where English is taught as a foreign language, is reading comprehension. Therefore, there is a need for EFL teachers to
know about the different approaches they can use while teaching reading comprehension.
Top-down and bottom-up processing are two approaches discussed in reading research and literature (Abraham,
1985; Field, 2004). Top-down processing, According to Paran (1996), top-down processing also known as concept-driven
model emphasizes on contextual factors such as socio-cultural knowledge and proceeds from whole to part. In other
words, top-down processing happens when the reader activates his/her world knowledge to facilitate comprehending the
text. On the other hand, in bottom-up reading model, the written or printed text is the centre of attention and reading
proceeds from part to the whole. Readers usually use their knowledge of lexical items, structural points and phonological
patterns to decode the text meaning. According to Gough (1972), in bottom-up model, the reading process proceeds in
serial fashion, from letter to sound, to words, to meaning.
Due to the importance and significance of top-down and bottom-up processing as revealed by previous studies (e.g.
Abraham, 1985; Field, 2004; Tsui & Fullilove, 1998); a great deal of studies have been conducted to investigate the effect
of top-down and/or bottom-up processing in learning different skills, and their application in EFL classroom. One of the
most interesting features discussed in relation to top-down and bottom-up processing is cognitive style.
The field-independence/field-dependence (FI/FD) cognitive style has received the greatest attention in second/foreign
language research. Zhang (2004) defined FI/FD as a reflection of the extent to which an individual uses external or
internal cues for performing tasks. FI/FD is typically referred to as a variable of cognitive style - a pervasive, stable, and
bipolar characteristic affecting the process of perception, thinking, and problem solving (Chapelle, 1988). A FI person
perceives analytically, analyzes and isolates relevant details, detects patterns, and critically evaluates data; while FD
one perceives holistically, tends to get lost in the stimuli and is unable to distinguish salient points. FI/FD describes two
contrasting ways of processing information along a continuum from extreme field-dependence to extreme
field-independence. “FI subjects trust internal cues, and this is associated with a greater aptitude for restructuring, i.e.
for imposing organization on received information. FD subjects, on the other hand, place their trust in external cues, and
tend to accept percents of symbolic representations at face value” (Tinajero & Paramo, 1997, p. 199).
The studies conducted in the domain of cognitive styles (field-dependent and/or field-independent) have considered
the effect of cognitive styles on academic achievement (Dwyer & Moore, 1995); the relationship between attitudes
toward computers and cognitive styles (Altun, 2003); and the effect of cognitive styles on reading comprehension tasks
(Davey, 1990).
Although some studies have been conducted on top-down / bottom-up processing, and cognitive styles separately,
relatively few efforts have been made in considering both variables in learning specific skill. This study attempted to fill
this gap and find whether there are any statistically significant differences between reading comprehension scores of
field-dependent and field-independent learners who are exposed to top-down vs. bottom-up reading instruction model.
To fulfill the purpose of this study, the following research questions were addressed:
1. Is there any statistically significant difference in reading comprehension scores of field-dependent and
field-independent learners who are exposed to top-down reading instruction model?
2. Is there any statistically significant difference in reading comprehension scores of field-dependent and
field-independent learners who are exposed to bottom-up reading instruction model?
the relationship between field dependence/field independence cognitive style and second/foreign language learning
(S/FLL) had demonstrated that, while field independent individuals were successful at analytic and deductive language
learning activities, field dependent learners showed their superiority at induction and communication. Shalbafan (1996)
aimed at investigating whether the findings of previous research could be generalized to Iranian EFL learners' writing
ability. The results indicated that, regarding the form of a writing task, field independent students outperformed their
field dependent counterparts. The results also revealed that regarding the content of writing, field dependent students
performed better than field independent students.
III. METHOD
A. Participants
The participants of the present study were 80 Iranian male and female EFL freshman students chosen out of 100
students based on their scores in reading section of TOEFL test. The EFL students were from two Islamic Azad
universities. The participants having passed reading posttest 1 and 2 shared the same linguistic background. The
participants aged from 18 to 24 years old. In this study, the participants were randomly assigned into two groups. There
were 40 students in one group benefiting from top-down reading instruction model and 40 students in the other group
benefiting from bottom-up reading instruction one. Classes were held 2 hours a week for the duration of 4 weeks. All
classes had the same material, namely the same reading texts were used for both groups.
B. Instrumentation
a. Reading Proficiency Test
In order to determine the reading proficiency level of the participants for the sake of homogeneity, the reading
section of TOEFL test was used.
b. The Group Embedded Figure Test (GEFT)
To identify participants' FD/ FI cognitive style, the GEFT instrument developed by Witkin, Oltman, Raskin, and
Karp (1971) was used. The reported reliability of GEFT is 0.89 (Foel & Fritz, 1994). " Among all the various
instruments that have been introduced for assessing the cognitive style of field–dependence/ independence, this test has
been the most common instrument applied by the researchers" (Alavi & Kavyanpanah, 2009; Yousefi, 2011, cited in
Hamed Mahvelati & Mukundan, 2012, p. 108).
This test consists of three sections; the first section includes 7 figures and is designed for practice. The second and
third section each has 9 figures. The students were required to recognize the common geometric shapes hidden in a
larger complex design. The cut- off point was determined based on Case's (1974, cited in Hamed Mahvelati &
Mukundan, 2012) criteria. That is, the students scored 1/4 SD below the group mean score were accounted as field-
dependent and those scored 1/4 SD above the group mean score were called field- independent. The learners whose
mean score was between 1/4 SD below and 1/4 SD above the mean were labeled as field- intermediate subjects and thus
were excluded.
c. Reading Comprehension Test
A reading comprehension test was used in this study, one as a pretest to assure the equality of two groups prior to the
instructional treatment and the second time, as post- test to determine the impact of two instructional treatments after
their implementation.
C. Procedure
In the present study, the data were collected through different stages. At the beginning of the experiment, the reading
section of a TOEFL test was administered to determine the learners' level of reading proficiency and select highly
homogeneous students. On the basis of this test, two groups of 40 participants were selected, one group received
top-down and the second one bottom-up reading instruction model. Then, the participants were asked to answer GEFT
to determine their distribution along the continuum of FD/ FI. Moreover, the reading comprehension posttest was pre-
administered to assure that the learners in each group had the same reading proficiency level. During the experiment,
the two groups received the instruction for 4 weeks, which was aligned with the ongoing university program. Both
groups were taught by the same teacher since the teachers' individual and methodological characteristics might affect
the results. After the tutorial, the reading comprehension test was administered to the two groups (top- down/ bottom-
up). The results of this posttest, once gathered and analyzed statistically, provided the answer to the proposed research
questions.
D. Data Analysis
In the present study, students' cognitive style (FD/ FI) and the reading instruction models (top- down vs. bottom- up)
were considered as independent variables; while the participants' reading comprehension scores were accounted as
dependent variable. Through using descriptive and inferential statistics, the data were analyzed. To address the first and
second research questions, two independent samples t- tests were used.
IV. RESULTS
The current study attempted to investigate the effects of top-down/bottom-up processing and
field-dependent/field-independent cognitive style on Iranian EFL learners’ reading comprehension.
To answer the research questions, after scoring the tests and tabulating the scores for each subject, the data were put
under a series of statistical analysis.
First of all, to show the distribution of participants along the spectrum of FI/ FD in each group, the results of the
administration of GEFT are presented in Table 1.
TABLE 1:
THE DISTRIBUTION OF PARTICIPANTS ALONG THE SPECTRUM OF FI/FD
Group 1 ( top- down reading instruction) Group 2 ( bottom- up reading instruction)
FI FD FI FD
22 18 17 23
Based on the above classification, the participants were divided into two main groups (top-down/bottom-up reading
instruction), each group included field independent/field-dependent learners.
Additionally, in order to show that subjects were homogeneous in two main groups and there was no significant
difference between them prior to the experiment, the reading achievement test was pre- administered before they stated
the experiment. The results of Homogeneity of Variance for each group are summarized in Table 2.
TABLE 2
TEST OF HOMOGENIETY OF VARIANCES FOR TOP - DOWN/ BOTTOM - UP GROUP
Levene Statistic df1 df2 Sig.
Top- down group .002 1 38 .967
Bottom- up group .623 1 38 .435
As it is clear, the performance of FI subjects was not statistically different from the FD ones in top- down reading
group (P= .96> .05) and bottom- up reading group (P=.43> .05). This means that subjects in each group started out the
experiment with equivalent reading background knowledge.
To answer the first research question of the study stating that whether there is any statistically significant difference
in reading comprehension scores of field-dependent and independent learners who are exposed to top-down reading
instruction, the participants' scores on the reading comprehension posttest were gathered and tabulated. Table 3 and
Figure 1 represent a better picture of the performance of FD/ FI learners in top- down group.
TABLE 3
DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS OF THE PERFORMANCE OF FD/FI LEARNERS IN TOP _DOWN READING GROUP
Std. 95% Confidence Interval for Mean
N Mean Deviation Std. Error Lower Bound Upper Bound Minimum Maximum
field independence 22 10.90 1.90 .40 10.06 11.75 8.00 14.00
field dependence 18 12.44 2.09 .49 11.40 13.48 9.00 15.00
Total 40 11.60 2.10 .33 10.92 12.27 8.00 15.00
According to the above descriptive results, the mean score of FI subjects differs from the FD ones'. To see whether
this difference is statistically significant or not, an independent sample t- test was also run (Table 4).
TABLE 4
INDEPENDENT SAMPLES T-TEST FOR EQUALITY OF MEANS (TOP -DOWN GROUP)
t-test for Equality of Means( top-down group)
95% Confidence Interval of the Difference
T df Sig. (2-tailed) Mean Difference Std. Error Difference Lower Upper
reading score -2.42 38 .020 -1.53 .63 -2.81 -.25
The results revealed that FD learners performed significantly better than FI ones in top- down reading instruction
group (t=2.42, p= .02< 05).
To answer the second research question of the study stating that whether there is any statistically significant
difference in reading comprehension scores of field-dependent and field-independent learners who were exposed to
bottom-up reading instruction model, the descriptive statistics are shown in Table 5 and Figure 2.
TABLE 5
DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS OF THE PERFORMANCE OF FD/ FI LEARNERS IN BOTTOM -UP READING GROUP
95% Confidence Interval for Mean
N Mean Std. Deviation Std. Error Lower Bound Upper Bound Minimum Maximum
field independence 17 13.05 1.02 .24 12.52 13.58 10.00 14.00
field dependence 23 9.82 1.23 .25 9.29 10.35 8.00 13.00
Total 40 11.20 1.97 .31 10.56 11.83 8.00 15.00
On inspection of mean scores, one can infer that FD/ FI learners performed differently on reading achievement
posttest. Its implication is that FI learners, as it is envisaged performed better than their FD counterparts. To check
whether this difference is statistically significant, an independent sample t- test was performed (Table 6).
TABLE 6
INDEPENDENT SAMPLES T-TEST FOR EQUALITY OF MEANS (BOTTOM - UP GROUP)
The results indicated that there was a statistically significant difference between the mean scores of FD/ FI learners
(t=8.790) at the end of the experiment meaning that FI subjects did outperform the FD ones in the reading
comprehension posttest.
V. DISCUSSION
The present study was an effort to investigate the possible effect that different types of reading instruction (top-down
vs. bottom-up) and cognitive style of field dependence vs. field independence might have on learners reading
performance during the course of the study. It attempted to empirically reveal that the type of instruction and classroom
tasks can be very influential on accelerating learners' reading achievement especially when they are interwoven with
individuals' cognitive style.
To achieve the purpose of the study, two research questions were raised at the onset.
Regarding the first research question, it was found that FD subjects outperformed their FI counterparts taught through
top- down reading instruction. One justification can be that according to Brown (1994, p.160), FD learners have " the
tendency to be dependent on the total field such that the parts embedded within the field is perceived more clearly as a
unified whole". This is in line with top- down model which processing initiates with general knowledge of subject and
continues to connect new information with previous ones. It might explain why FD learners seem to be less successful
in tasks where concentration on small details is required.
This finding is consistent with Salmani Nodoushan's (2006) view that learners' cognitive style (FD/ FI) is crucial in
benefitting from a specific teaching method. It is in sharp contrast with the theoretical view of those researchers who
claim that there is no significant relationship between learners' cognitive style (FD/ FI) and L2 learning (Hamed
Mahvelati & Mukundan, 2012). In addition, the above finding is not also in line with Dyer and Osborne's (1999) view
so far as they claim that the students' cognitive style of FD/ FI does not affect the effectiveness of teaching methods.
The second result of this study showed that FI learners gained higher scores in reading achievement test in
comparison with their FD counterparts when exposed to bottom- up reading instruction. one explanation can be that FI
individuals have the ability of " perceiving a particular, relevant item or factor in a field of distracting factors" (Brown,
1994, p.160). Hence, FI learners can easily focus on details in a given task/ text. This might be one reason for the
success of FI learners in bottom- up reading instruction where the brain begins with externally received information and
analyze them in detail.
Concerning the superiority of FI learners to FD ones, the finding is consistent with some researchers' view (Clark &
Roof, 1988; Salmani Nodoushan, 2006) that FI learners perform better than FD ones in the learning tasks which require
analysis and attention to details.
On the whole, the findings of the present study supported the findings of Sudzina (1993), Abu Romman (2005)
researches. They reported that the academic achievements of learners can be enhanced when students' cognitive style is
matched with the teaching methods. They are also consistent with Daoud's (2008) claim that the only factor affecting
individuals' language learning is matching instruction with learners' cognitive style. However, the findings of the
present study is not in line with El-Koumy's (2001) study which showed that students' achievement in reading
comprehension increases when there is a mismatch between learners' cognitive style and teaching method.
Furthermore, it should be born in mind that the contradictory findings of some studies regarding the effectiveness of
top- down and/ or bottom- up reading instruction can be due to the fact that learners' other individual differences have
been neglected in most of these studies.
VI. CONCLUSION
Concerning the first research question which dealt with investigating whether there is any statistically significant
difference in reading comprehension scores of field-dependent and field-independent learners who were exposed to
top-down reading instruction model, the obtained findings through independent samples t-test revealed that FD learners
performed significantly better than FI ones in top-down reading instruction group.
This result supports the findings of Simonson (1985) and Miller (1997). In their studies, they found out FI learners
were more proactive and usually had a strong self-concept, tended to solve problems through intuition and analyze data
in details, As opposed to FD learners, who perceived objects as a whole.
Concerning the second research question, stating that whether there is any statistically significant difference in
reading comprehension scores of field-dependent and field-independent learners who were exposed to bottom-up
reading instruction model, an independent sample t-test was conducted. The result revealed that FI learners performed
significantly better than FD ones in bottom-up reading instruction model.
Consistent with interactionist and cognitive views of language development, the above findings highlight the
important role of attention. Put it simply, when a text is given to both field-dependent and field-independent learners,
while the field-independent ones consider it analytically and focus on details, the field-dependent learners mostly pay
attention to its global aspect and focus on comprehension of the content.
The findings of this study are in line with a study done by Shalbafan’s (1996) who examined the relationship between
field-dependence/field-independence cognitive style and second/foreign language learning revealing that while
field-independent learners were successful at analytic and deductive language learning activities, field-dependent ones
showed their advantage at induction, communication, and perception of materials holistically.
These findings seem to have important implications for L2 teachers and learners. As for teachers, if they know their
students’ cognitive style of field dependence/independence and learn more about the effective ways to help both groups
improve their learning processes, they can change and adapt their teaching methods to the needs of the learners more
effectively.
The findings of this study can also help language learners by encouraging them to learn more about their own
individual cognitive styles and hence, improve their strengths and overcome their weaknesses.
There are some limitations need to be considered when interpreting the findings of this study. First, this study
included only the Iranian EFL learners. A more comprehensive study including other nationalities and/or learners will
enhance our understanding of the effects of top-down /bottom-up processing and cognitive styles on reading
comprehension. Secondly, to increase the external validity of the study findings, replication is needed in different
settings with diverse populations. It also needs to be emphasized that this study used only Witkin, Oltman, Raskin, and
Karp (1971) FD/FI as an indicator of cognitive style. Other cognitive style inventories could be used to examine the
interrelationship between reading skill and cognitive style in a broader context.
REFERENCES
[1] Abraham, R. G. (1985). Field independence /dependence and the teaching of grammar. TESOl Quarterly, 19, 689-702.
[2] Abu Romman, S. (2005). The effect of match/mismatch of teaching –learning styles on secondary stage students’ achievement
in English in Jordan. Unpublished Ph. D. dissertation, Amman Arab University for Graduate Studies, Jordan.
[3] Altun, A. (2003). The relationship between teacher trainees’ attitudes toward computers and their cognitive styles. Turkish
Online Journal of Educational Technology, 2(1). Retrieved July 5, 2013 from
http://www.tojet.sakarya.edu.tr/archive/v1i2/arif3.htm.
[4] Brown, H. D. (1994). Principles of language learning and teaching. Sanfrancisco: Prentice Hall.
[5] Clark, H. T., & Roof, K. D. (1988). Field dependence and strategy use. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 66, 303-307.
[6] Carrell, P. L., & Eisterhold, J. C. (1983) "Schema theory and ESL reading pedagogy”. In P. L. Carrell, J., Devine, & D. E.
Eskey (Eds.), Interactive approaches to second language reading. Cambridge: CUP.
[7] Chaplle, C. (1988). Field independence: A source of language test variance. Language Testing, 5(1), 62-82.
[8] Daoud, J. (2008). The effect of students’ perception and matching instruction with cognitive style on secondary stage students’
achievement in English literacy skills in Jordan. Unpublished Ph. D. dissertation, Amman Arab University for Graduate Studies,
Jordan.
[9] Davey, B. (1990). Field dependence-independence and reading comprehension questions: Task and reader interactions,
Contemporary of Experimental Education, 52, 119-206.
[10] Dooley, P. B. (1976). The relation of field independence, m space, and intelligence in efficient and inefficient readers.
Dissertation abstracts international, 37, 1465- A.
[11] Dwyer, F. M., & Moore, D. M. (1995). Effect of color coding and test type (visual/verbal) on students identified as possessing
different field dependence levels. The Journal of Psychology, 125(6), 677-680.
[12] Dyer, J. E., & Osborne, E. W. (1996). Effects of teaching approach on achievement of agricultural education students with
varying learning style. Journal of Agricultural Education, 3, 43-51.
[13] El-Koumy, A. (2001). The effect of matching versus mismatching instructional approaches with cognitive styles on EAP
students' achievement in reading comprehension. Proceedings of the 7th EFL Skills Conference. The American University in
Cairo: CACE.
[14] Fehrenbach, C. R. (1994). Cognitive style of gifted and average readers. Roeper Review, 16(4), 290-292.
[15] Field, J. (2004). An insight into listeners’ problems: Too much bottom-up or too much top-down? System, 32, 363-367.
[16] Foel, N. A., & Fritz, R. L. (1994). Association of cognitive style and satisfaction with distance learning. Journal of Industrial
Teacher Education, 33, 46-59.
[17] Gough, P. B. (1972). One second of reading. In J. F. Kawanagh & I. G. Mattingley (Eds.), Language by ear and by eye.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
[18] Grabe, W. (1991). Current developments in second language reading research. TESOL Quarterly, 25, 375-405.
[19] Guisande, M. A. Paramo, M. F. Tinajero, C., & Almeida, L. S. (2007).” Field dependence independence (FDI) cognitive style:
An analysis of ntentional functioning”. Psicothema, 19 (4), 572-577.
[20] Hamed Mahvelati, E., & Mukundan, J. (2012). The role of cognitive style in the collocation knowledge development of Iranian
EFL learners through input flood treatment. English Language Teaching, 5(10), 105-117.
[21] Joffe, R. T. (1987). Reflective- impulsivity and field dependence/ independence as factors in the reading achievement of
children with reading difficulties. Dissertation abstracts international, 48, 876- A.
[22] Liu, M., & Reed, W. M. (1994). The relationship between the learning strategies and learning styles in a hypermedia
environment. Computers in Human Behavior, 10, 419-434.
[23] Miller, G. (1997). Are distance education programs more acceptable to field-independent learners? (ERIC Document
Reproduction Service No. ED 409854.)
[24] Paran, A. (1996). Reading in EFL: Facts and fictions. ELT Journal, 5(1), 25-34.
[25] Salmani-Nodoushan, M. A. (2006). Does field independence relate to performance on communicative language tests?
Manager’s Journal of Educational Technology, 3(3), 79-85.
[26] Shalbafan, K. (1996). On the role field dependence/field independence in Iranian EFL learners’ writing ability. Unpublished
master’s thesis, University of Tehran.
[27] Simonson, M. R. (1985) Persuasion: Five studies dealing with the relationships between media, attitudes, and learning style .
(ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 256 337.)
[28] Škudienė, V. (2002). A comparison of reading models, their application to the classroom and their impact on comprehension.
Studies about Languages, 2, 94-98.
[29] Sudzina, M. (1993). Educational psychology applications. In T. Graves (Ed.), Cooperative learning and preservice teacher
education. Cooperative Learning, 13(3), 32-35.
[30] Swaffar, J. K., Arans, K. M., & Byrnes, H. (1991). Reading for meaning. Integrated approach to language learning. N Jersey:
Prentice Hall.
[31] Tinajero, C., & Paramo, M. F. (1997). “Field dependence / independence and academic achievement: A re-examination of their
relationship”, Educational Psychology, 67, 199-212.
[32] Tsui, A. B. M., & Fullilove, J. (1998). Bottom-up or top-down processing as a discriminator of L2 listening performance.
Applied Linguistics, 19(4), 432-451.
[33] Wasilewski, J. (2009). Contemporary understanding of the reading process and reading strategies used by ESOL learners while
reading a written discourse. Humanising Language Teaching, 4, 145-159.
[34] Witkin, H. A., Oltman, P., Raskin, E., & Karp, S. (1971). A manual for the embedded figures test. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
Psychologist Press.
[35] Xu. W. (2011). Learning styles and their implications in learning and teaching. Theory and Practice in Language Studies, 1(4),
413-416.
[36] Yaghoubi, R. (1994). The relationship between field-independent/field-dependent cognitive style Persian students and their
English language proficiency. Unpublished master’s thesis, Allameh Tabatabaii University, Iran.
[37] Zhang, L. F. (2004). Field-dependence/independence: Cognitive style or perceptual ability? Validating against thinking styles
and academic achievement”. Personality and Individual Differences, 3, 1295-1311.
Azar Hosseini Fatemi, assistant professor in TEFL, is the head of Department of English, Ferdowsi University of Mashhad,
Mashhad, Iran. Her areas of interest include issues in second language teaching and learning.
Vahideh Sadat Vahedi is the faculty member at Islamic Azad University, Qaenat branch, Qaenat, Iran. She is a PhD candidate in
Applied Linguistics at Ferdowsi University, Mashhad, Iran. She received her M.A. degree in English language teaching from Alzahre
University, Tehran, Iran in 2006, and she also got the B.A. degree in English language and literature from Ferdowsi University of
Mashhad, Mashhad, Iran in 2006. Her areas of research include applied linguistics, Anxiety in EFL learning, and CALL. She has
presented and published several articles in international conferences and journals.
Zari Sadat Seyyedrezaie received her BA in English language and literature from Zahedan University, Iran. Then she received
her MA in TEFL from Islamic Azad University, Science and Research Branch in Iran. She started her Ph.D at the University of
Mashhad in 2012. She has presented some papers in International conferences in different countries and also published some papers
in international journals. Her major areas of interest are English testing, evaluation, and language teaching methodology.