Strabismus and Amblyopia
Strabismus and Amblyopia
Strabismus and Amblyopia
VOLUME 49
Edited by
Gunnar Lennerstrand
Department of Ophthalmology
Karolinska Institute
Stockholm, Sweden
and
Emilio C. Campos
Department of Ophthalmology,
University of Modena
Modena, Italy
M
MACMILLAN
PRESS
Scientific & Medical
© Th e Wenner-Gren Center 1988
Softcover reprint of the hardcover 1st edition 1988 978-0-333-47173-9
Preface lX
Invited Contributors and Participants Xl
Opening Address: Bjorn Tengroth 1
Session I
Oculomotor Control and Strabismus
v
VI CONTENTS
Session II
Normal and Abnormal Visual Development
13. Amblyopia in Humans and Clinical Relevance of Animal
Models G.K. vonNoorden 169
Session III
Psychophysics Related to Strabismus and Amblyopia
24. Visual Perception in Strabismus E. C. Campos 311
Index 431
PREFACE
During the last two decades a large amount of experimental research has
been devoted to visual and oculomotor physiology. Normal visual
development in animals and humans is quite well established and the
electrophysiological and behavioural effects of different forms of visual
deprivation in animals are also extensively studied. Investigations on the
developmental aspects of ocular motility are just beginning.
The meeting had a workshop character with much time for discussion. It
was divided into three sessions. The first dealt with oculomotor control
aspects in strabismus, the second with normal and abnormal visual
development, and the third part with the psychophysics of strabismus and
amblyopia. Subsections of each part were reviewed by prominent workers
in each field. The presentations as well as the discussions of the findings
and the clinical ramifications are included in this publication.
We want to thank the participants for all the work they have invested in
the preparations and during the conference, and for the help that they have
given in editing the discussion parts. We are deeply indebted to Professor
David Ottoson for providing support on the scientific part and for the use
of the excellent conference facilities of the Wenner-Gren Center in
Stockholm. We also acknowledge the generous financial support of the
ix
X PREFACE
Wenner-Gren Center Foundation, the Swedish Medical Research Council
and the Swedish Government.
Gunnar Lennerstrand
Gunter K. von Noorden
Emilio C. Campos
INVITED CONTRIBUTORS AND PARTICIPANTS
Xl
xii INVITED CONTRIBUTORS and PARTICIPANTS
BJÖRN TENGROTH
Almost 250 years ago Chevalier John Taylor presented himself in this
city as the doctor who could put the eyes straight. From his coll-
eagues at the time as well as from the public in general, remarks
were made as to whether he was a humbug or not. Since then strabology
in this country as well as in the rest of the world has passed a
stage which more resembles theology than a biological science. With
our increased knowledge and understanding of the basic neurophysiolo-
gy of vision and the oculomotor system, an increasing knowledge of
the pathophysiology of strabismus and amblyopia has developed.
1
2 B.TENGROTH
With this perspective one wonders how the clinicians will ever be
able to grasp the theoretical basis and use this knowledge to create
modern methods for diagnosis and treatment. However, the scientifi-
cally trained clinical mind will always be able to use the clinical
signs in order to form the relevant questions. All scientists in-
volved are dependent upon these questions in order to enhance re-
search projects which might lead to the right answers.
GUNNAR LENNERSTRAND
GENERAL CONSIDERATIONS
Strabismus or squint is a condition that is
closely linked to the ability of binocular vision.
Binocular functions always suffer in strabismus,
but it is mainly in its effects on monocular vi-
sion, i.e. as a creator of amblyopia that strabis-
mus causes ophthalmological concern. These aspects
of strabismus will be treated extensively later at
this meeting. Strabismus is defined as a patho-
logical deviation of one eye with respect to the
other. In this eye position a visual object is
imaged on non-corresponding retinal areas, since
the visual axes of the two eyes do not intersect at
the fixation point (in horizontal and vertical
strabismus) or the vertical planes through the
visual axes are tilted with respect to each other
(in pure cyclotorsional strabismus).
Depending on the eye position, a strabismus
can be subdivided into the following pure forms:
esotropia or convergent strabismus, exotropia or
divergent strabismus, hyper- or hypotropia, i.e.
vertical strabismus, and incyclotropia and excyclo-
tropia or torsional strabismus. These forms can be
mixed in one and the same patient. Based on the
manner of fixation one can distinguish between
alternating strabismus, in which each eye can
fixate and the other be strabismic, or monolateral
strabismus where one eye fixates most of the time
and the other is deviated. Amblyopia is most pre-
valent in the latter form. Depending on the stra-
bismus situation in terms of time, a distinction is
made between permanent of manifest strabismus
(hetero-tropia) and intermittent squint. In the
latter condition, strabismus is seen at some times
and during other periods the eyes are straight.
Depending on the magnitude of squint angle in
various directions of gaze, a separation is made
between concomitant and incomitant strabismus. The
concomitant type infers that the angle of squint is
constant or nearly so, independent of the direction
of gaze, while the incomitant form shows an angle
of squint that varies with the gaze direction. The
cause of the latter form is usually ocular muscle
palsy and it is therefore often referred to as
paralytic strabismus.
In addition to the above mentioned manifest
types there are also forms of latent strabismus or
heterophoria. They occur mostly after the interup-
tion of binocular vision by occlusion. Depending
upon the direction of the deviation following oc-
clusion the terms exophoria, hyperphoria etc are
used. Heterophoria is very common and is regarded
MOTOR DYSFUNCTION IN STRABISMUS 7
Brain damage
In patients with brain abnormalities, for
instance those with Down's syndrome, there is a
high incidence or strabismus (40-60 % in different
materials). In children with cerebral palsy, hydro-
cephalus and other more general brain disease,
strabismus is common in addition to other abnor-
malities or ocular motility. Children suffering
from general disease, e.g. congenital heart le-
sions, have 4-6 times higher frequency or strabis-
mus than the general population. Dericiencis or
postural control has been reported in esotropic
children, but exotropics were normal in this re-
spect (Sandstedt et al, 1986). Thus, it is possible
that minor motor problems exist in children with
some types or strabismus, as a sign or general CNS
dysfunction. This may be postulated in cyclic
heterotropia with rhythmic variations on alternate
days between heterotropia and normal binocular
vision (von Noorden, 1985).
Neuromuscular anomalies
These will be described more extensively in
the following section and in other papers or this
session. They include pathophysiological factors
12 G.LENNERSTRAND
connected with orbital mechanics, eye muscle func-
tion, brain stem and cerebellar function, accom-
modation-convergence coupling, the eye movement
systems and the development of oculomotor func-
tions.
Reflexological theories
It has been suggested that strabismus is a
disturbance of the optomotor reflexes (Keiner,
1951). The position of the eyes during fetal life
would depend upon subcortical reflexes initiated by
stimulation of eye muscle proprioceptors (that in-
duce a monocular duction reflex) and the vestibular
organs (that induce a binocular reflex for ver-
sions). Light stimulation after birth initiates
the development of the optomotor reflexes, which
supersede the older, subcortical reflexes of pro-
prioception and vestibular action. Esotropia would
thus depend on an abnormal development of the opto-
motor reflexes and consists of a predominance of
the monocular adduction reflexes over those for
conjugate movements and abduction. These ideas have
been retaken by Mitsui and Tamura (1986) in their
work on the effects of muscle stretch on eye posi-
tion in esotropia and exotropia (see also a sub-
sequent paper by Lennerstrand, this symposium).
However, the disturbances in optomotor responses
could be secondary to changes in the sensory
systems, as suggested by studies on the development
of the optokinetic nystagmus (Schor, 1983;
Hoffmann, Kommerell, this symposium) and need not
be the primary cause of strabismus.
TABLE
Patient data, ocular motility observations and CNS deformations in four
patients with meningomyelocele. All had normal visual acuity and visual
fields.
Ocular Motility Deformation of
Pat. Sex Age Strab. Binoc. Gaze + Sacc Purs OKN HC Mec M.O. - Shunt
-
r-- - - - -Cer -
type func + nyst - - - - - QP_.
================' ================ p=~==================== =====================' ==========I
ELN F 20 ! ET-A none LNy N(?) p p ++ +++ + ++ 16y
I GP I
C'l
LC F 11 XF-V 240 GPNy p N p + ++ ++ +++ 3w
r
i I:Tl
p p
z
TJ M 10 ortho 60 GPNy N + + + + 4y zI:Tl
l (/)
SUMMARY
Motor characteristics of concomitant and in-
comitant strabismus and the different treatment
methods currently in use for alignment of the eyes
have been briefly reviewed. The hereditary aspects
and racial differences in the occurance of strabis-
mus were pointed out. The present understanding of
the general pathophysiology of strabismus was
briefly reported. A number of specific ocular motor
factors related to strabismus were reviewed and
suggested for further research.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
The research reported from the author's
laboratory has been supported by grants from the
Swedish Medical Research Council (No 4751) and
Karolinska institutets fonder.
REFERENCES
Berthoz, A, and Melvill Jones, G.M. (Eds.), (1985).
Adaptive Mechanisms in Gaze Control. Reveiws in
Oculomotor Research, Vol. I. Elsevier, Amsterdam.
20 G.LENNERSTRAND
Boothe, R.G., Dobson. V. and Teller, D.Y. (1985).
Postnatal development of vision in human and non-
human primate. Am. Rev. Neurosci., ~. 495-545.
Clement, R.A. (1986). A comparison of different
models of extraocular muscle cooperation. Ophthal.
Physiol. Opt., ~. 165-170.
Collins, c.c. and Jampolsky, A. (1982). Objective
calculation of strabismus surgery. In Functional
Basis of Ocular Motility Disorders. (eds.
G. Lennerstrand, D.S. Zee and E.L. Keller). Pp 185-
194, Pergamon Press, Oxford.
Fielder, A.R., Gresty, M.A., Dodd, K.L., Mellor,
D.H. and Levene, M.I. (1986). Congenital ocular
motor apraxia. Trans. Ophthalmol. Soc. UK., 105,
589-598.
Helveston, E.M. (1986). Esotropia in the first year
of life. In Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus.
Transactions of the New Orleans Academy of Ophthal-
mology. Raven Press, New York.
Huber, A. (1974). Electrophysiology of the retrac-
tion syndromes. Brit. J. Ophthalmol., 58, 293-300.
Keiner, G.B.J (1951). New Viewpoints on the Origin
of Squint. Martinus Nijhoff, The Hague.
Koorneef, L. and Zonneveld, F.W. (1985). Orbital
anatomy, the direct scanning of the orbit in three
planes and their bearings on the treatment of
motility disturbances of the eye after orbital
"blow-out" fractures. Acta. Morphol. Neerl. Scand.,
23, 229-246.
Leigh, R.J. and Zee, D.S. (1983). The Neurolo~y of
Eye Movements. F.A. Davies Co., Philadelphia.
Lennerstrand, G., Zee, D.S. and Keller, E.L. (Eds)
(1982). Functional Basis of Ocular Motility
Disorders, Pergamon Press, Oxford.
Metz, B.S. (1983). Restrictive factors in strabis-
mus. Surv. Ophthalmol., 28, 71-83.
Mitsui, Y, and Tamura, 0. (1986). Strabismus and
the sensory motor reflex. Excerpta Medica Clin.
Pract. Series no. 3, Amsterdam.
MOTOR DYSFUNCTION IN STRABISMUS 21
BRIAN HARCOURT
23
24 B. HARCOURT
~WITH NYSTAGMUS
ESSENTIAL INFANTILE ESOTROPIA
~WITHOUT NYSTAGMUS
NYSTAGMUS BLOCKAGE SYNDROME
EARLY ONSET ACCOMMODATIVE ESOTROPIA
CONGENITAL OR INFANTILE SIXTH CRANIAL NERVE PALSIES
DUANE'S SYNDROME
MOBIUS SYNDROME
STRABISMUS FIXUS
SYMPTOMATIC SENSORY STRABISMUS
a) with nystagmus:
as are those when both eyes are uncovered. These anomalous res-
ponses are not found in patients who have essential infantile
esotropia without DVD and they are not related to the presence or
absence of binocular vision (t1ein, 1983; Flynn et al, 1984).
b) without nystagmus:
TABLE I
metropia and the AC/A ratio was also high in some 50% of cases.
However, binocular vision was subnormal; half the patients reviewed
were at risk of amblyopia and the esotropia recurred after treatment
in almost half the cases. The outcome was therefore much inferior
to that which would be expected in a similar group of patients with
accommodative convergent strabismus of later onset. Whether this is
due to the immaturity of the binocular fixation reflexes at this
earlier age, or whether the conditions described are in fact essent-
ially different cannot at present be stated with any certainty.
DUANE'S SYNDROME
TABLE II
d) When and in what way does strabismus arise in the first few
months of life?
32 B. HARCOURT
Nixon et al (1985) have shown that, at least up to the age of
3 months, persistent esotropia is extremely rare, certainly by
comparison with the generally accepted 1% incidence of infantile
esotropia within the population (Helveston, 1986). De Decker (1987)
is reaching a similar conclusion. The inference is that many more
babies begin to squint between the ages of 3 and 6 months, around
the stage at which normal ocular motor responses become firmly est-
ablished. If so, what are the earliest features of the developing
strabismus? In particular, which comes first, the strabismus or the
nystagmus; and do skew deviations occur as a preparatory feature in
a significant proportion of patients as suggested by Hoyt et al
(1980).
REFERENCES
Baker, J.D. & Parks, M.M. (1980). Early onset accommodative eso-
tropia. American Journal of Ophthalmology, 2Q, 11-18.
Hotchkiss, M.G., Miller, N.R., Clark, A.W. & Green W.R. (1980).
Bilateral Duane's retraction syndrome. A clinico-pathologi~ case
report. Archives of Ophthalmology, ~' 870-4.
Hoyt, C.S., Mousel, D.K. & Weber, A.A. (1980). Transient Supra-
nuclear disturbances of gaze in healthy neonates. American Journal
of Ophthalmology, 89, 708-13.
Nixon, R.B., Helveston, E.M., Miller, K., Archer, S.M. & Ellis, F.D.
(1985). Incidence of Strabismus in Neonates. American Journal of
Ophthalmology, 100, 798-801.
INTRODUCTION
37
38 R.F. SPENCER and K.W. McNEER
CONCLUSION
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
REFERENCES
Ringel, S.P., Engel, W.K., Bender, A.N., Peters, N.D. and Yee, R.D.
(1978). Histochemistry and acetylcholine receptor distribution in
normal and denervated monkey extraocular muscles. Neurology
(Minn.),28, 55-63.
GUNNAR LENNERSTRAND
47
48 G.LENNERSTRAND
charateristics, innervation, histochemistry and
distribution within the eye muscle of the different
fiber types have just been described by Spencer
(this symposium). Also in lower vertebrates several
distinct fiber types have been reported (Morgan and
Proske, 1984; Lennerstrand and Baker, 1987).
In the physiological sense EOM is unique in
that it combines high contractile speed and rather
extreme fatigue resistance during continous acti-
vation (Fuchs and Binder, 1983). Both properties
are usually much more developed than in ordinary
skeletal muscle of the same species. Fatigue re-
sistance of EOM is higher in species with good bi-
nocular vision such as the cat, than in species
with poor binocular vision (rat, guinea pig, fish)
(Lennerstrand, 1982; Lennerstrand and Baker, 1987).
In the cat the fatigue resistance is higher in
animals with normal binocular vision than in cats
with congenitally abnormal binocularity (Siamese
cats) or acquired defects (Lennerstrand, 1982).
An EOM motor unit consist of the motoneuron in
the brain stem and the muscle fibers it innervates
in the muscle itself. All fibers in a unit are pre-
sumably of the same type (Burke, 1981; Gueritaud et
al, 1985). In the EOM a motoneuron innervates only
6-10 muscle fibers on an average while the inner-
vation ratio may be up to 1:1000 for large hind
limb muscles. Studies of EOM motor units have been
performed mainly in the cat, by recording the
mechanical and electrical responses in the muscle
to stimulation of single motoneurons or their axons
(Lennerstrand, 1975, Nelson et al, 1986). It has
been possible to differentiate 2 or 3 types of
motor units. The units with singly innervated
fibres (SI units) have rapid contractions, among
the fastest in the body. Their fatigue resistance
ranges from very low to intermediate values. The
fiber type that Spencer (this symposium) has sug-
gested to be involved in strabismus, i.e. the or-
bital, singly innervated fiber, is probably the
slowest and most fatigue resistant among the SI
units. The units with multiply innervated, non-
conduction fibers (MINC units) contract very slowly
and are extremely fatigue resistant. Properties of
units with multiple innervation but conducted
action potentials (MIC units) are intermediate to
SI and MINC units with regard to speed of contrac-
tion and fatigue resistance. Thus, it seems likely
that the rapid contractions of EOM are due to SI
unit properties and the high endurance to MIC and
MINC unit activation.
EOM MOTOR AND SENSORY FUNCTION 49
innervational programs.
Recent work by Nelson and collaborators (1986)
on trochlear motoneurons has shown that the size of
the EOM motoneuron is fairly well correlated with
the speed of contraction and force production of
its muscle unit, in the same way as in the limb
muscles. This suggests that the recruitment order
in EOM follows the size principle outlined for limb
muscles. It is likely that slow EOM motor units are
activated before fast units, but this recruitment
order may not be as strict as in limb muscles,
since there is evidence that some of the slow
units are innervated by large axons and motoneurons
(Lennerstrand, 1975), and they would be recruited
late during muscle activation. Possibly they supply
the steady contraction with low fatigue necessary
for maintenance of eye deviation in lateral gaze.
Support for the view that slow and fast units can
be recruited separately are supplied in the
experiments on human EOM done by Scott and Collins
(1973). It is therefore tempting to propose that
the slow units with characteristics of amphibian
and avian slow-tonic fibers, are used predominantly
for fixations in different positions of gaze and
for slow eye movements like vergences, while the
fast units are used mainly for faster eye movements
like persuit and saccades, although we know that
there is no selective activation of any type of
unit for a specific type of eye movement in the
monkey or the cat (Delgado-Garcia et al 1986a).
Spencer (this symposium) has suggested a role also
for the orbital, singly innervated fibers in
control of slow eye movements and fixation.
With respect to frequency modulation of motor
unit discharge, variations between EOM motor units
have been demonstrated in the relation between
firing rate and eye position (so called k-value)-
(Delgado-Garcia et al, 1986a). Generally the units
and motoneurons with low threshold of excitation
(mostly the slow units) have lower k-value than the
high-threshold units that usually contract faster.
Most low threshold units had lower maximal dis-
charge frequency than fast units, which probably is
related to the fact that the slow units reach fused
tensions at lower rates of activation than the fast
units. It would thus seem as if recruitment and
frequency modulation both play important parts in
the production of EOM force dynamics in the various
types of eye movements, but the relative contri-
bution of each of them for the different types of
motor units and eye movements have still to be
established. The possibilities of recruitment
disorders in different types of strabismus has not
52 G.LENNERSTRAND
yet been explored.
SUMMARY
The aim of the review has been to supply
material for further discussion on extraocular
muscle (EOM) dysfunction as a cause or a conse-
quence of strabismus. In general there is much less
data on humans than on animals and the majority of
the animal findings are from normal, non-strabismic
muscle. The following areas were covered: (i) Motor
unit properties, unit distribution in EOM and the
recruitment in eye movements; (ii) peri- and post-
natal development of eye muscle function and the
influence of defects of binocular vision in ani-
mals; (iii) properties of EOM after denervation and
reinnervation; (iv) EMG of human EOM under normal
conditions and in neuromuscular disease. Very
little has been reported on EMG of strabismic
muscle; (v) muscle receptors of EOM, the types and
their occurance within different species. The func-
tional properties of spindles and tendon organs;
(vi) the central connections of EOM receptor
afferents, their reflex actions in EOM and the
possible role in strabismus.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
The research reported from the authors:
laboratory has been supported by grants from the
Swedish Medical Research Council (No. 4751) and the
Karolinska Institute.
REFERENCES
Aichmair. H. (1977). Muscular neurotisation in
surgery of traumatic abducens paralysis. Jap. J.
Ophthalmol., ~. 477-487.
Alvarado-Mallart. R-M. and Pincon-Raymond. M.
(1979). The palisade endings of cat extraocular
muscles; a light and electron microscope study.
Tissue & Cell, !1. 567-584.
Ashton, J.A., Boddy, A. and Donaldsson, I.M.L.
(1984). Input from proprioceptors in the extrinsic
ocular muscles to the vestibular nuclei in the
giant toad, Bufo marinus.Exp.Brain Res. ,53,409-419
Asmussen, G. and Gaunitz, U. ( 1981 ) . Changes in
mechanical properties of the inferior oblique
muscle of the rabbit after denervation. Pfltigers
Arch., 392, 198-205.
60 G.LENNERSTRAND
HARLEY E. A. BICAS
Energy for the system comes from the extraocular muscles (in-
ternal forces) and is expressed by a state of"tonicity".This repre-
sents the consecutive functions of a central command for the gener-
ation of a signal, of its neural transmission and of the muscular
response to it. Variations of the neuromuscular function (innerv-
ational impulses and, or tonic factors) are directly related to the
amount of the resulting ("active") muscular force, a relative in-
crease of it being named contraction and the reverse, relaxation.As
the ocular insertion of the muscle may move relative to its other
extremity (the fixed orbital insertion), this active force, or
tension, causes an ocular rotation, with its consequent and inverse
variation of the muscular length (isotonic contraction or relax-
ation). In fact, it is possible experimentally to obtain the varia-
tion of the muscular length between a known (initial) and an expected
(final) ocular position and the respective developed force for such
a rotation. Therefore, the resultant length-tension curve of the
muscle is important for the determination of how a stimulus (hence
innervation and then force) is related to a rotation (response).
Meaningful maps of length-tension curves of each muscle at least
for some (cardinal, diagnostic) of all possible ocular rotational di
rections are then needed to complement the data already known of th;-
horizontal recti at the horizontal plane (Robinson et al., 1969).
However, the relationship ~et~een innervation and ocular ro-
65
66 H. E. A. BICAS
-tension state, the passively stretched muscle rotates the eye, op-
poses to the action of the antagonist muscle (becomes more resistive)
and, since it is shortened, absorbs less energy to further con-
tractions, that is, reacts more promptly to innervation (becomes more
propulsive). A recession slacks the muscle, with reverse effects.
(a l (b) (c l
namic "arc of rotation" must be considered (fig. 1 c). Note that the
relationship between the angle of the arc of rotation and its re-
spective length varies as function of d and therefore is not constant
for all muscles.
(a) (bl
FIGURE 2: Muscular action before (a) and after (b) section (CC') of
an intermuscular membrane. The direction of rotation changes
because of the displacement of the point where the force is applied
(from P 1 to P 2 ). R is the center of rotation (for the sake of simpli
fication made coincident with the center of the eye), 0 the orlgln
of the muscle and A the midpoint of its ocular (anatomic) insertion.
GENERAL BALANCE
The disposition of the six extra-ocular muscles makes reason-
able the assumption that they may be considered in pairs. So the hor
izontal muscles, laying exactly at the horizontal plane,are mutually
exclusive in their pure actions of adduction (the medial rectus) and
abduction (the lateral rectus). In primary position, the relative
values of the vectorial components for the vertical, torsional and
horizontal rotations may be taken respectively by the approximate
proportions of 8, 4 and 3 for the vertical recti and 6, 8 and 1 for
the obliques, such that all actions in each plane are mutually ex-
clusive for each pair of muscles (elevation and incycloduction of
the SR balanced by depression and excycloduction of the IR; ele-
vation, excycloduction and adduction of the IO balanced by depression,
incycloduction and abduction of the SO), except adduction due to the
vertical recti. This could be a possible hypothesis for the abduction
of the eye when all the tonus of the extra-ocular muscles is elimi-
nated. (Even if the oblique pair were considered to cause abduction,
that certainly would not compensate the adducting power of the ver-
tical recti).
Dynamic Balance
REFERENCES
Bicas, H.E.A. (1984). Principios teoricos de substitui~ao de
a~ao rotacional de musculo extra-ocular. I - Generalidades.Arq.
Bras.Oftalmol., ~. 154-159.
ACKNOWLEDEMENTS
ROBERT BAKER
77
78 R. BAKER
K.-P. HOFFMANN
1 Introduction
Numerous experiments dating back to the last century have shown
that visual structures in the midbrain of vertebrates play an
important role in visually guided behaviour. In addition to the
oculomotor nuclei themselves there are two other midbrain structures
with clearly defined visuomotor functions. The superior colliculus
(SC) is important for saccadic eye movements as well as head- and
body- movements in the visual grasp reflex and the nucleus of the
optic tract (NOT) is essential for slow eye movements in the
optokinetic reflex (OKR) (Akert 1949; Wurtz and Albano 1980; Precht
1982; Hoffmann 1983a; Vanegas 1984). Comparative neuroanatomical
studies of mammals representing an ascending sample from the
phylogenetic tree have shown that both the SC and the NOT in the
pretectum receive a continually increasing proportion of their input
from cortical visual areas as opposed to the well established direct
retinal projection. In relation to the well established rule that
ontogeny recapitulates in part mechanisms of this phylogeny
developmental studies of these structures may offer the opportunity
to examine the rules by which information from these different
sources is used and integrated to create the specific properties of
nerve cells in SC and NOT of modern mammals. In this chapter
examples will be reviewed to show that the specific filter
characteristics of retinal recipient midbrain nuclei are established
early in ontogeny by the retinal input and before the arrival of
cortical afferents. Thereafter information flow via cortical
connections is accepted in the midbrain only if it agrees with the
complements of the retinal imprint. Nevertheless, in adult cats and
monkeys analyses of the response properties of SC and NOT cells have
revealed the strong influence of the cortical input and the
dependence of some specific properties on that input. For example,
visual cells in the SC of the cat show a high degree of binocularity
and direction specificity. These properties are lost when the
cortical input is disrupted (Wickelgren and Sterling 1969). Visual
cells in the NOT of cats or monkeys are all direction specific over
89
90 K.-P. HOFFMANN
8 Conclusions
This model could explain why after the loss of visual cortex
the optokinetic reflex is much weaker and asymmetric in cats or
monkeys and absent in man. This model could explain why in
monocularly deprived cats suffering an occlusion amblyopia OKR is
asymmetric. The non-deprived eye can make no connections to the
ipsilateral NOT. The NOT cells are never sufficiently depolarized
by the sparse ipsilateral retinal input and the activity normally
relayed by the more massive contralateral retinal input is blocked
by the lid suture. The deprived eye (amblyopic eye) has lost its
cortical connections. This model could also explain why OKR is
asymmetric in strabismic cats. Only contralateral input controls
the NOT responses. The ipsilateral eye is connected to the NOT only
through binocular cortical cells and binocular cells are rare in
strabismic cats. All these inferences of course could also apply to
monkeys and man.
So far we have no information on the development of this
pathway in primates. The similarities in the ontogeny of the
optokinetic reflex and in the NOT response properties in adult cats
and monkeys suggests, however, that similar developmental mechanisms
may exist in all higher mammals, including man.
9 References
~ert, k. (1949) The visual grasp reflex. Helv. Physiol. Acta,
7. 112-134.
Ballas, I., Hoffmann, K.-P., Wagner, H. J. (1981) Retinal
projection to the nucleus of the optic tract in the cat as
revealed by retrograde transport of horseradish peroxidase.
Neurosci. Lett., 26, 197-202.
Changeux, J.-P., Danchin, A. (1976) Selective stabilization of
developing synapses as a mechanism for the specification of
neuronal networks. Nature, 264, 705-711.
NEURAL BASIS FOR CHANGES OF THE OPTOKINETIC REFLEX 97
GUNTRAM KOMMERELL
ABSTRACf
INTRODUCTION
99
100 G. KOMMERELL
Although a common cause for all four phenomena should be given consideration,
a causal interdependence between them appears to be more likely. Indeed, a wealth of
evidence indicates that strabismus is the primary abnormality which subsequently leads to
an asymmetry in the pursuit and OK systems, and it is a plausible hypothesis that the
asymmetry in these smooth tracking systems ultimately leads to LN. Less clear is the
causal relationship between DVD and strabismus, but DVD also seems to occur as a
consequence of the strabismus.
The asymmetry in pursuit and OKN is defined as a reduction of gain for uniocular
stimuli directed to the temporal side, while the gain for nasally-directed stimuli is normal
or only moderately reduced. Healthy infants show such asymmetry in the smooth
tracking systems (Atkinson 1979; Atkinson and Braddick 1981; Hainline et al. 1984;
Naegele and Held, 1982), but the asymmetry disappears by about six months of age if
signs of normal binocularity appear (Atkinson 1979; Naegele and Held 1982). In adults,
a slight nasally-directed preponderance is only observed if optokinetic stimulation is
confined to the temporal hemifield; this asymmetry is counterbalanced by a temporally-
directed preponderance of the nasal hernifield (Ohrni et al1986).
The reduced, though not necessarily absent (Flynn et al 1984, Sorsby 1931),
binocularity caused by strabismus could prevent maturation of the smooth tracking
systems (van Hof- van Duin 1978). This hypothesis is supported by the persistence of
nasal-temporal asymmetry in each eye in cats which had been deprived of binocular
vision by unilateral lid suture early in life (Hoffmann 1979; van Hof- van Duin 1976).
The crucial factor in producing asymmetry in pursuit and OKN appears to be the
reduced binocularity rather than monocular or binocular deprivation. Certainly as far as
strabismic humans are concerned, amblyopia is not a prerequisite for the asymmetry in
the smooth tracking systems (Mohn et al1986; Schor and Levi 1980; Tychsen et al1985;
Tychsen and Lisberger 1986; van Hof- van Duin and Mohn 1986). Cats rendered
exotropic, but not amblyopic, by early surgery, show a reduced OKN, predominantly of
the temporally-directed slow phases (Cynader and Harris 1980). In monkeys an adequate
model still seems to be lacking, although deprivation of binocular vision by alternating lid
suture may be promising (Tusa et al. 1987). In the monocularly deprived (i.e.,
amblyopic) monkey, nasal-temporal asymmetry in the OKN is present when the deprived
eye is stimulated with a rotating drum, whereas stimulation of the non-deprived eye
results in normal OKN (Sparks et al. 1986).
The neural mechanism of how the loss of binocularity leads to the asymmetry in
the smooth tracking systems could be that the cortical projection to the nucleus of the
optic tract (NOT) has not developed its normal function (Hoffmann, this symposium).
The NOT which is located in the pretectum is an important relay station of the optokinetic
system that receives direct input from the contralateral eye and indirect input from both
eyes via both occipital lobes (Hoffmann 1982, 1983). Lesions of the visual cortex
drastically reduce the optokinetic response to temporally-directed motion under conditions
of monocular viewing in the cat (Hoffmann 1982; Strong et al. 1984) and in the monkey
(Zee 1986). Dark rearing of cats also results in asymmetry in the smooth tracking systems
and may be equivalent to a surgical lesion of the visual cortex (Harris and Cynader 1981;
van Hof - van Duin 1978). The relative preservation of responses to nasally-directed
motion appears to be due to the direct connection from the retina to the contralateral NOT
both in the cat (Hoffmann 1982) and in the monkey (Hoffmann and Distler 1986). In the
normal adult human, the subcortical projection alone seems to be insufficient to drive the
NOT, as most cortically blind patients do not show any optokinetic response (Jung and
Kornhuber 1964). But the relative preservation of responses to nasally-directed stimuli in
patients with incomplete bilateral occipital lobe destruction (Mehdorn 1982) could be due
OCULAR MOTOR PHENOMENA IN INFANTILE STRABISMUS 101
to remnants of the subcortical projection to the NOT which might have been released from
cortical control.
Although a cortical defect of binocularity induced by infantile strabismus may be
responsible for the impairment of temporally-directed tracking, this defect does not imply
difficulties in motion perception. Indeed, patients are able to differentiate between various
stimulus velocities. We ascertained this in a patient with infantile esotropia who had a
marked asymmetry in the smooth tracking systems (Fig. 1). Using optokinetic stimuli
and Stevens' (1957) magnitude estimation, the patient could clearly distinguish between
fast and slow slip velocities of the retinal image. The performance was the same
regardless of whether the stimuli were nasally or temporally-directed (Fig. 2A and B).
Similarly, there was good velocity discrimination independent of whether the optokinetic
stimulus was applied in the nasal or temporal hemifield (Fig. 2C).
LE stimulation
temp. ~I n j a .temp.l +fit.- nas.
~- ~/~\.
./ ':1'\(:/V
N/"1;]~o·
, L
RE stimulation
nas. ~I temp.
This result is compatible with recent work conducted by Tychsen and Lisberger
(1986) who presented a nasally or temporally moving single target to patients with
infantile strabismus whilst they were fixating a central stationary target (technique of
McKee and Welch 1985). The ability to discriminate differences in velocity was normal
when nasally and temporally-directed motion were considered separately. Only when the
patients compared target speed in the two directions, did they judge temporally-directed
stimuli to be slightly slower than nasally-directed stimuli. The authors regard this
perceptual asymmetry as an indication of a defect in the cerebral pathways responsible for
velocity perception, but an alternative interpretation is possible. Patients may have
underestimated temporally-directed stimuli because of adaptation to unidirectional slip of
the retinal image in every-day life: their "latent" nystagmus may have been partly manifest
under natural viewing conditions, i.e., these patients may have had so-called manifest
latent nystagmus (Dell'Osso et al1979).
Schor and Levi (1980) measured the contrast sensitivity function for perceived
motion in strabismus patients with nasal-temporal asymmetry of the OKN. Contrast
thresholds for nasally and temporally-directed stimulus movements were the same. This
finding along with the patients' unimpaired ability to distinguish between fast and slow
slip velocities in both horizontal
102 G. KOMMERELL
i
-; 150
0 0
X @ X <I <I <I
0
li> X 0 0 0 E 100
-~
Q X
~ <I
0 0
~
·o
l!i> 1i> ijSO <I~IJJ>
......~ modulus
X X
X X
\modulus x 0 '-modulus ~
<l <I ..,
X li>
<I
&0
20 40 60 20 40 60 80 100"/sec
20 40 60 80 100"/sec
stripe-velocity in front of right eye stripe-velocity in front of left eye stripe-velocity in front of left eye
from nasal to temporal
x = from temporal to nasal x = from temporal to nasal
o = from nasal to temporal 0 = from nasal to temporal
• stimulus in ~~ hemifield
<l stimulus in nasal hemifield
LN is defined as a jerk nystagmus whose rapid phases are directed to the side of
the visually dominant eye. With the left eye occluded the slow phases are directed to the
left, and with the right eye occluded the slow phases are directed to the right. We
suggested the hypothesis that the asymmetry in pursuit and OKN typically present in
patients with infantile strabismus might be the cause of LN (Kommerell 1978; Kommerell
and Mehdorn 1982). According to this hypothesis, the nasally-directed vector of the
smooth tracking systems would be preponderant, even in the presence of stationary
patterns. This directional preponderance could drive the slow phases of LN if the visual
input is unbalanced in favour of one eye. Total occlusion of one eye causes the greatest
imbalance in visual input, but as most patients with LN also have strabismus, part of the
OCULAR MOTOR PHENOMENA IN INFANTILE STRABISMUS 103
Stimulation of LE Stimulation of RE
on~ -ttt-7t •
Fig. 3. Asymmetry in the
OKN in 6 patients with
LN. A full-field stimulus
with seven-degree wide
stripes was presented
monocularly for 15 s. The
average velocity of the ten
fastest slow phases that
occurred during this time
was taken as the response.
The velocity of the slow
0
E 100
phases of LN was
-~ measured when the patient
~ looked at stationary stripes
~
(response to zero
·o
ijSO velocity). The responses
0
E 100
to temporally-directed
-~ stimulus motion (filled
~ circles) are much weaker
~
than the responses to
·o
ijSO nasally-directed stimulus
motion (open circles), but
the responses to nasally-
directed stimulus motion
are also subnormal,
particularly at stimulus
velocities above 20° /s.
Responses with a gain of
1.0 would have appeared
on a 45-degree-line, either
above or below the zero
line. Patient codes:
AZ020874, UD110671,
0 15 30 60 90 120 0 15 :l) 60 90 120 KH170470, MK201072,
I I SP050469, AS291070.
LN LN The patient represented in
the uppermost panel is the
Stimulus velocity [•Js] same as in Fig. 4.
104 G. KOMMERELL
R
[ 10'
L
The cognitive influence is further exemplified by an unusual patient who was able
to manifest his LN at will. When asked to "wiggle" his eyes he evoked a strong
nystagmus with the fast phases directed to his leading left eye (Fig. 4). Binocular vision
was tested with Bagolini's striated glasses. In the every-day condition when the
nystagmus was not obvious (only occasionally was there a slight "manifest latent"
OCULAR MOTOR PHENOMENA IN INFANTILE STRABISMUS !OS
nystagmus), the patient reponed the streak of the squinting right eye to be faintly visible.
When he evoked the left-beating nystagmus at will, he completely suppressed the streak
of the squinting right eye. This result suggests that some residual binocularity was
available to the patient under normal circumstances, but that he was also able to fully
concentrate on his leading left eye. Concentrating on the left eye may have offset his
pursuit balance so much that a strong nystagmus appeared. Our patient required visual
contours to evoke the nystagmus at will. He could not produce it in darkness or when he
was looking at a contourless light screen.
A remarkable cognitive influence on LN in the dark was demonstrated by Abel et
al. ( 1986) in a patient who had been blind in the right eye from birth due to a
malformation of the eye. The right eye had been enucleated. In darkness, the patient's
"latent nystagmus" beat as though his right prosthesis were viewing (similar to the case
described by Ohm in 1928); he could, however, "look" with either eye at will, producing
the appropriate reversals of direction.
We do not believe that cognitive influences are at variance with our hypothesis
that LN is related to the asymmetry in the smooth tracking systems. One has to realize that
the smooth tracking systems of normal observers can also be influenced by cognitive
effort. Zikmund (1966) has shown this very convincingly: Trained subjects, studied in
total darkness, were able to evoke OKN by visual imagery of moving stimulus patterns.
Nobody would deny that OKN is a visual oculomotor phenomenon because it can be
evoked also in the dark. An analogous argument should apply to LN.
Summing up the arguments presented so far, we have suggested the following
causal relationship: Infantile strabismus impairs the development of binocularity in the
visual cortex. The reduced binocularity prevents oculomotor maturation in that the nasal-
temporal asymmetry in the smooth tracking systems which is a normal feature in the first
few months of life remains a permanent defect. Finally, the asymmetry in the smooth
tracking systems produces LN.
The reverse relationship suggested by Tychsen and Lisberger (1986), that LN
might constitute a tonic drive which leads to convergent strabismus appears unlikely to
us. It has to be borne in mind that the drift directed to the nose refers only to the viewing
eye. The drift is always conjugate, i.e., at the same time, the non-viewing eye drifts
temporally. It is hard to see how such a conjugate drift could lead to convergent
strabismus. Moreover, asymmetry in the smooth tracking systems and LN also occur in
patients who have had divergent strabismus from infancy (Roelofs 1928, present writers
experience).
Although early deprivation of binocular vision appears to be an important
pathogenetic factor of optokinetic asymmetry and LN, a lack of binocular vision is not an
absolute determinant of LN, and, conversely, the presence of binocular vision does not
preclude LN absolutely. We have seen a patient (SW250672) with a unilateral hypoplasia
of the optic nerve who had congenital squint and very likely never had binocular vision.
Nevertheless, we found only a subtle nasal-temporal asymmetry in the OKN, and LN
was absent on careful ophthalmoscopic examination. On the other hand we have seen
exceptional cases with LN who had binocular vision with only slightly reduced stereopsis
(JK051067). These observations show that factors other than defective binocular vision
play a role in the pathogenesis of LN. These factors can compensate, or aggravate the
condition.
In many patients with infantile strabismus, the strength of the nystagmus
increases with lateral gaze, in a few it does not. This means that a gaze-paretic component
of the nystagmus can be associated with the latent component, but there is no strict
quantitative correlation between the two. The latent component depending on the visual
dominance of the right or left eye can be marked while the gaze-paretic component is
subtle, and vice versa. Clinical impression suggests that the gaze-paretic component
decreases during the first four years of life, but oculographic data are lacking. The
106 G. KOMMERELL
pathogenetic link between the gaze-paretic component and infantile strabismus is not yet
clear.
Two alternative hypotheses on the pathogenesis of LN have been proposed
recently. Lang (1982) suggests that LN may be due to a dominance of the nasal half of
the retina over the temporal half. However, this hypothesis fails to explain why there are
slow drifts towards the allegedly dominant half of the retina. We would rather expect
rapid eye movements, in analogy to acquired lesions where the preserved area of the
retina is typically reached by saccades. Dell'Osso et al. (1979) advanced the hypothesis
that switching the egocentric localization from one eye to the other may cause the eyes to
drift in patients with LN. This explanation appears rather unlikely to us because there are
no other conditions, such as pastpointing in patients with an eye muscle palsy, where a
change of egocentric localization is associated with a drift of the eyes.
DVD occurs only rarely in subjects with otherwise normal binocularity, but is
much more frequent in patients with early onset strabismus. There is one similarity
between LN and DVD: Both of these ocular motor abnormalities depend on the balance of
visual inputs coming through the right and left eyes. A balance with real scales can be
used to illustrate the clinical findings in DVD (Kommerell and Mattheus 1984). If the
right eye is fixing a bright picture and the left eye is occluded, the balance of visual inputs
shifts strongly to the right eye. This imbalance forces the left eye up. In the classical
Bielschowsky test, the imbalance is decreased by placing a dark filter in front of the
fixing right eye, thus reducing the dominance of the input through the right eye and
allowing the left eye to move slightly down (Bielschowsky 1930). If the right eye is
completely occluded and the left eye takes over fixation, the imbalance is reversed to give
a dominance of the input through the left eye forcing the right eye up.
The balance of visual inputs can also be influenced by conscious effort. The more
the patient tries to resolve a difficult acuity task, the more the squinting eye is forced up
(Rlissmann 1986).
Obviously, scales are only a metaphor to demonstrate the phenomena of DVD.
They do not tell us very much about the neural mechanism which remains obscure. The
changes in vertical deviation on the alternate cover test are brought about by disjunctive
movements, and they are not combined with vertical nystagmus (Helveston 1980).
Therefore, we cannot see any relationship to a vertical asymmetry in the conjugate smooth
tracking systems as has been suggested by Tychsen and Lisberger (1986), although
preference for upward tracking does indeed occur in patients with DVD (Tychsen and
Lisberger 1986). Preference for downward tracking was found by Schor and Levi (1980)
in patients with strabismic amblyopia. It is possible, though not specifically reported, that
their patients had also DVD. We assume that asymmetries in the vertical smooth tracking
systems are most likely a coincident abnormality, not causally related to DVD.
DVD is probably not a variant of the so-called ocular tilt reaction (Westheimer and
Blair 1975) because the cyclorotation that accompanies the vertical deviation is opposite in
the two conditions. In DVD, there is excyclorotation in the upward moving eye, whereas
in the ocular tilt reaction, there is incyclorotation in the upward moving eye.
OCULAR MOTOR PHENOMENA IN INFANTILE STRABISMUS 107
REFERENCES
Abel LA, Grossman G, Dell'Osso LF, Thurston SE, Daroff RB (1986) Abstract. In:
Thurston SE (ed) Ocular motor minisymposium. Neuro-ophthalmology 6: 137-138
Atkinson J (1979) Development of optokinetic nystagmus in the human infant and
monkey infant. In: Freeman RD (ed) Developmental neurobiology of vision.
Plenum, New York, pp 277-287
Atkinson J, Braddick 0 (1981) Development of optokinetic nystagmus in infants: An
indicator of cortical binocularity? In: Fisher DF, Monty RA, Senders JW (eds) Eye
movements: Cognition and visual perception. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 53-64
Bielschowsky A (1930) Die einseitigen und gegensinnigen ("dissoziierten")
Vertikalbewegungen der Augen. Albrecht von Graefes Arch Ophthalmol125: 493-
553
Cynader M, Harris L (1980) Eye movement in strabismic cat. Nature 286:64-65
Dell'Osso LF, Schmidt D, Daroff RB (1979) Latent, manifest latent and congenital
nystagmus. Arch Ophthalmol97: 1877-1885
Doden W (1961) Latenter Nystagmus bei Strabismus concomitans alternans. Ber Dtsch
Ophthalmol Ges 63: 486-490
Flynn IT, Pritchard C, Lasley D (1984) Binocular vision and OKN asymmetry in
strabismic patients. In: Reinecke RD (ed) Strabismus II, Proceedings of the fourth
meeting of the international strabismological association, Grune & Stratton,
Orlando, pp 35-43
Hain TC, Kelman SE, Zee DS (1985) Pursuit and saccade asymmetries in latent
nystagmus. Soc Neuroscience Abstract
Hainline L, Lemerise E, Abramov I, et al (1984) Orientational asymmetries in small-field
optokinetic nystagmus in human infants. Behav Brain Res 13: 217-230
Harris LR, Cynader M (1981) The eye movements of the dark-reared cat. Exp Brain Res
44:57-70
Helveston EM (1980) Dissociated vertical deviation. A clinical and laboratory study.
Trans Amer Ophthalmol Soc 78: 734-779
Hoffmann KP (1979) Optokinetic nystagmus and single cell responses in the nucleus
tractus opticus after early monocular deprivation in the cat. In: Freeman RD (ed)
Developmental neurobiology of vision. Plenum Press, New York, pp 63-73
Hoffmann KP (1982) Cortical versus subcortical contribution to the optokinetic reflex in
the cat. In: Lennerstrand G, Zee DS, Keller EL (eds) Functional basis of ocular
motility disorders, Pergamon Press, Oxford, pp 303-310
Hoffmann KP (1983) Neuronal responses related to optokinetic nystagmus in the eat's
nucleus of the optic tract. In: Fuchs A, Becker W (eds) Progress in oculomotor
research, Elsevier North Holland, New York, pp 443-454
Hoffmann KP, Distler C (1986) The role of direction selective cells in the nucleus of the
optic tract of cat and monkey during optokinetic nystagmus. In: Keller EL, Zee DS
(eds) Adaptive processes in visual and oculomotor systems. Pergamon Press,
Oxford, pp 261-266
Jung R, Kornhuber HH (1964) Results of electronystagmography in man: The value of
optokinetic, vestibular, and spontaneous nystagmus for neurologic diagnosis and
research. In: Bender MB (ed) The ocular motor system. Harper and Row, New
York, pp 428-488
Keiner GBJ, Roelofs CO (1955) Optomotor reflexes and nystagmus. In: Niihoff M (ed)
La Haye, Vol I, p 224. Cited after Godde-Jolly D, Larmande A (1973) Les
nystagmus, Vol!. Masson and Cie, Paris, Chap VIII, p 632
Kestenbaum A (1961) Clinical methods of neuro-ophthalmologic examination. Grone and
Stratton, New York, London, p 366
108 G. KOMMERELL
Sharpe JA, Lo AW, Rabinovitch HE (1979) Control of the saccadic and smooth pursuit
systems after cerebral hemidecortication. Brain 102: 387-403
Sorsby A (1931) Latent nystagmus. Brit J Ophthalmol15: 1-18
Sparks DL, Gurski MR, Mays LE, Hickey TL (1986) Effects of long-term and short-
term monocular deprivation upon oculomotor function in the Rhesus monkey. In:
Keller EL, Zee DS (eds) Adaptive processes in visual and oculomotor systems.
Pergamon Press, Oxford, pp 191-197
Stevens S (1957) On the psychophysical law. Psycho! Rev 64: 153-184
Strong NP, Malach R, Lee P, van Sluyters RC (1984) Horizontal optokinetic nystagmus
in the cat. Recovery from cortical lesions. Dev Brain Res 13: 179-192
Tusa RJ, Smith CB, Herdman SJ (1987) The development of nystagmus in infant
monkeys following visual deprivation. Neuroscience abstract
Tychsen L, Hurtig RR, Scott WE (1985) Pursuit is impaired but the vestibulo-ocular
reflex is normal in infantile strabismus. Arch Ophthalmol103: 536-539
Tychsen L, Lisberger St G (1986) Maldevelopment of visual motion processing in
humans who had strabismus with onset in infancy. J Neuroscience 6: 2495-2508
van Hof- van Duin J (1976) Early and permanent effects of monocular deprivation on
pattern discrimination and visuomotor behaviour in cats. Brain Res 111: 261-276
van Hof- van Duin J (1978) Asymmetry in optokinetic nystagmus observed in normal
kittens and light deprived cats. In: Kommerell G (ed) Augenbewegungsstorungen,
Neurophysiologie und Klinik, Bergmann, Miinchen, pp 363-366
van Hof- van Duin J, Mohn G (1986) Monocular and binocular optokinetic nystagmus in
humans with defective stereopsis. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 27: 574-583
van Vliet AGM (1973) On the central mechanism of latent nystagmus. Acta Ophthalmol
(Copenh) 51:772-781
Westheimer G, Blair SM (1975) The ocular tilt reaction- a brainstem oculomotor routine.
Invest Ophthalmol 14: 833-839
Yamazaki A (1979) Abnormalities of smooth pursuit and vestibular eye movements in
congenital jerk nystagmus. In: Shimizu K (ed) Ophthalmology, vol 2, Excerpta
Medica, Amsterdam, pp 1162-1165
Zee DS, Tusa RJ, Herdmann SJ, Butler PH, Gucer G (1986) The acute and chronic
effects of bilateral occipital lobectomy upon eye movements in monkey. In: Keller
EL, Zee DS (eds) Adaptive processes in visual and oculomotor systems. Pergamon
Press, Oxford, pp 267-274
Zee DS, Friendlich AR, Robinson DA (1974) The mechanism of downbeat nystagmus.
Arch Neurol30: 227-237
Zikmund V (1966) Oculomotor activity during visual imagery of a moving stimulus
pattern. Stud Psychol8: 254-272
9
PHASIC-TONIC ORGANIZATION OF ACCOMMODATION
AND VERGENCE
CLIFTON M. SCHOR
INTRODUCTION
111
112 C.M. SCHOR
theory predicts that the linear sun of the responses by these two
mechanisms accounts for the total accommodative response (Kotulak
and Schor 1986). Similarly two separate processes are proposed to
underly vergence control, a fast (phasic) disparity vergence
component followed serially by a slower (tonic) adaptable component
(Schor 1983). The linear sun of the responses of these phasic and
tonic mechanisms is believed to account for the total vergence
response (Schor 1983). The second property underlying the effect of
adaptation on cross coupling interactions is that only phasic
components stimulate accommodative vergence and vergence
accommodation whereas the adaptable processes do not stimulate
cross-link interactions. This is demonstrated by aftereffects of
accommodation and vergence which decay independently of one another
in darkness (Kotulak and Schor, 1986). Additional evidence for this
comes from observed increases of the AC/ A and CA/ C ratios with the
speed or temporal frequency of the driving stimulus. Because
adaptation is a slow process, it responds more to low than high
temporal frequency stimuli. Consequently the adaptable tonic
response decreases with temporal frequency while phasic responses
ani their cross-link stimulation increase. At lower temporal
frequencies, more of the total response is controlled by the
adaptable component, which results in lower cross-link interactions.
At higher frequencies, adaptation is unable to keep pace with the
stimulus and the fast process controls the response. If only the
phasic process interact between the two motor systems then the
cross-link interactions become temporal frequency dependent. Thus,
both accommodative vergence and vergence accommodation are mainly
associated with rapid changes in their respective stimuli.
+
TARGET ACCOMMODATION
DISTANCE
VERGENCE
+_
~CCOMMOOATIVE VERGENCE
C. H.
I
L
LEFT EYE
10 P. d.
RICHT [¥£
ACCOiol.
SliM.
<0. OS Hzl
15 s.c •.
~CCOMMOOATIVE VERGENCE
(, M.
I
l
LEfT [Y(
10 P.d.
•
RIGHT 0£
ACCOM.
<,I /M.
<0. OS Hzl
,__ _ _____,
15 Seca.
VERGENCE ~CCOMMODAT!DN
J. T.
I•o
lEFT EYE
P.d.
•
R!GHT EYE
ACCOM. I 1.5 D
STIH.
<0. 05 Hz)
15 s.c•.
VERGENCE ~CCOMMODAT!ON
J. T.
I
LEFT EYE l
8 P.d.
•
RIGHT EYE
r~.~o
ACCDM.
ST JM.
(Q_ 06 Hz)
While these fatigue effects on tre AC/ A and CA/C ratios are
only temporary, they suggest a possible means of altering the cross
link interactions by methods which will produce prolonged changes in
adaptability of accommodation and vergence. Such long term changes
have been reported by North and Henson (1982) in the form of
enhanced adaptability of vergence following a standard orthoptic
exercise program. If this therapeutic approach were to bring about
a balance in adaptation of accommodation and vergence it would
perhaps return extreme AC/A and CA/C ratios to within normal ranges.
REFERENCES
Judge, S.J. and Cumming, B.G. (1986). Neurons in the monkey midbrain
with activity related to vergence eye movement and accomodation. J.
Neurophysiol. ,.2.2_, 915-930.
BRUNO BAGOLINI
121
122 B. BAGOLINI
Fig. 1 A Fig. 1 B
"
••
so ....
lo
lO
10
·10
REFERENCES
RICHARD N. ASLIN
INTRODUCTION
1
Supported by NIH research grant (EY-05976) to R.N.A. and by NIH
center grant (EY-01319) to the Center for Visual Science.
133
134 R.N. ASLIN
The foregoing argument does not imply that all sensory deficits
observed in normal infants can be attributed to retinal development.
There is compelling evidence that response properties of neurons
beyond the retinal level undergo significant postnatal development.
However, if we view the retina as a preliminary filter on visual
input, significant postnatal retinal developments would limit the
sensory information available at higher neural levels.
--.,
OJ
Q)
"0
5
'-
~ 0
OJ
c: 10
"(
Q)
(.,)
c:
Q)
2' 15 o Newborns
~ 0 1-month-olds
0 2- month -aids
• 3-month-olds
20
•
50 33 25 15 12.5
Target distance (em)
Figure 1. Vergence angle as a function of target viewing
distance in newborns (Slater & Findlay, 1975) and in older infants
(Aslin, 1977). Dotted line indicates expected value.
136 R.N. ASLIN
Accommodative vergence
Fusional vergence
(/)
•• 105 prism diopters
...J 100
<{ 0 0 (control)
a::
I-
I-
2
w
~~
wl-
>2 50
ow
~~
w
2a..
o~
!;:{
X
LL.
w
a::
0 o••
2 3 4.5 6
AGE (MONTHS)
Figure 2. The probability of a re-fixation response to prism-
induced binocular misalignment. Data replotted from Aslin (1977).
138 R.N. ASLIN
&-WEEK-OLD
z
0
i= 10-WEEK-OLD
Ui
0
a.
w
>-
w
-'
<(
~
z
2
a:0
:I:
ADULT
TIME
SUMMARY
REFERENCES
Shimojo, s., Bauer, J., O'Connell, K. M., and Held, R. (1986). Pre-
stereoptic binocular vision in infants. Vision Res.,~. 501-510.
Vilis, T., Yates, s., and Hore, J. (1985). Visual patching of one
eye produces changes in saccadic properties in the unseeing eye.
Dev. Brain Res., .!2_, 290-292.
143
144 G.LENNERSTRAND
affected, though the changes, if any, were subtle
in comparison. In other instances, such as the
clinically overacting inferior oblique muscle, we
observed changes associated specifically with
another fibre type in the global layer of the
muscle. Recent findings by Porter indicate that yet
another fibre type in the global layer is affected
by bupivacaine. The most significant concept to
emerge from these various findings is that
different fibre types may be selectively involved
in ocular motility deficits largely as a conse-
quence of not only their distribution in the
orbital or global layers of the muscle, but also
their distinct histochemical and ultrastructral
features that relate to oxidative/glycolytic
function and contractile properties. While all
motoneurones and the different muscle fibre types
that they innervate participate in all types of eye
movements, the orbital singly-innervated muscle
fibres may be one of the first recruited during
movement and, being the most fatigue resistant, are
ideally suited to maintaining fixation during
vergence or gaze.
Bagolini: Dr Spencer showed a photograph of a
section of muscle in which it was easy to
distinguish between the orbital and global layers.
If I understand correctly, one part is mainly
related to tonic or probably vergence movements,
while the other part is possibly related to
saccadic and therefore duction movements. From a
practical point of view, would it be possible to
selectively destroy the global part or the orbital
part by surgery or chemicals thus influencing more
the saccadic system or the vergence system?
Spencer: Although the division of labor concept, as
proposed by Scott and Collins, might imply that
different functions are associated with the orbital
versus global layers, it is unlikely that by
lesioning one or the other part you would interfere
with one type of eye movements as opposed to
another. In the case of the orbital layer, the
result might simply be difficulty in maintaining
the position of gaze without necessarily inter-
fering with the ability to perform a saccadic or
vergence eye movement.
Helveston: Clinically we use Oculinum for the
treatment of strabismus as well as blepharospasm.
An acute paresis is created, but after a while
function of the muscle returns. Do you have any
comments on the long term morphological changes of
DISCUSSION: OCULOMOTOR CONTROL AND STRABISMUS 145
References
1. Boothe, R.G., Dobson, V., and Teller, D.Y. (1985). Postnatal
development of vision in human and nonhuman primates. Annu.
Rev. Neurosci., 8, 495-545.
2. Eggers, H.M., Boothe, R.G. ( 1987). Naturally occurring
esotropia in macaques. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. (suppl)
28, 103.
3. Garey, L. (1987). Normal anatomic development of the primate
primary visual pathway. Wenner Gren Center Symposium
Strabismus and Amblyopia - Experimental Basis for Advances
in Clinical Management, Stockholm.
4. Guillery, R.W., Hickey, T.L., Kaas, J.H., Fellman, D.J.,
de Bruyn, E.J., Sparks, D.L. (1984). Abnormal visual pathways
in the brain of an albino green monkey (Cercopithecus
aethiops). J. Comp. Neurol., 226, 165-183.
5. Kaas, J.H., Guillery, R.W. (1974). The transfer of abnormal
visual field representatives from the dorsal lateral geniculate
nucleus to the visual cortex in Siamese cats. Brain Res., 59,
61-95.
6. Kiorpes, L., Boothe, R.G., Carlson, M.R., Alfi, D. (1985).
Frequency of naturally occurring strabismus in monkeys. J.
Pediatr. Ophthalmol. Strabismus, 22, 60-64.
7. von Noorden, G.K. (1985). Amblyopia: a multidisciplinary
approach. (Proctor Lecture) Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci., 26,
52-64.
172 G.K. von NOORDEN
L. GAREY
RETINA
In the foetal monkey there is a migration of neu-
rons away from the fovea, leading to a thinning out of
the bipolar and ganglion cells. At birth the retina
has a fairly mature appearance, even to the extent of
there being a true foveal pit (Samorajski et al., 1965;
Hendrickson and Kupfer, 1976). In spite of this, there
are still subtle changes that occur only postnatally,
especially concerning synapse formation (Smelser et
al., 1974; Nishimura and Rakic, 1985) and an increase
in length and numerical density of cone photoreceptors
(Hendrickson and Kupfer, 1976).
The peripheral portions of the human retina are
already fairly mature at birth. However, the central
173
174 L. GAREY
12 I
VOLUME
.r-·-·, .,.,
>-
I ., (J)
z
10
I-
./ 0
CY
(J)
z ./ ::::J
w ./ w
8 z
D
./ LJ._
0
w
:E 6 ./· 600 CY
w
::::J
_J
0
/
---------------~--~
-- --~f.l..M_I!~.Ef
------------
NEURONS CD
:E
400 ::::J
> 4 z
DENSITY
2 200
VISUAL CORTEX
As for the LGN, the human area 17 increases in
volume rapidly during the perinatal period. It quad-
ruples between 28 weeks of gestation and birthj when
the volume of a single area 17 is some 1. 5 em . It
quadruples again by about four months of age (Rutten-
locher et al., 1982), reaching a volume of about 6 cm3,
that is maintained into adulthood. The basic laminar
pattern is already visible at birth in area 17 of Old
and New World monkeys (Wiesel and Hubel, 1974; Blake-
more et al., 1979; Fritschy and Garey, 1986a) and man
(Leuba and Garey, 1987). In man, cortical lamination
is already recognizable before mid-gestation, but the
various sublaminae are only really distinguishable from
about 26 gestational weeks (Takashima et al., 1980;
Sauer et al., 1983). The stria of Gennari, in layer
IVb, and the cell dense granular layer IVc are visible
by mid-gestation (Leuba and Garey, 1987) thus making
area 17 easily distinguishable from area 18 in the
foetus.
In man there is no evidence for any loss of neu-
rons between mid-gestation and old age, for there are
compensatory decreases in cell density and increases in
cortical volume (Leuba and Garey, 1987). The neuronal
numerical density at mid-gestation is over one million
per mm3, decreasing to 90,000 by birth. There is a
further decrease to about 40, 000 by some four months
postnatally, a value that is approximately maintained
in the adult. However, 0 1 Kusky and Colonnier ( 1982)
did describe a moderate neuronal loss in macaque area
17 between birth and adulthood. As there is a more
178 L. GAREY
CONCLUSION
In both man and monkey the periods during which
visual function is developing most rapidly are also the
times when the visual system is most susceptible to
permanent damage if the visual environment is abnormal.
In the human, visual acuity improves during the first
half of the first postnatal year (Dobson and Teller,
1978; Pirchio et al., 1978; Held, 1979), a time when
interference with the normal visual environment is most
likely to cause amblyopia (Awaya et al. , 1973; Von
Noorden, 1977, 1981). This is just the period when the
changes described above are at their most dramatic.
Similar critical periods exist in the monkey, when
acuity is increasing and experimental visual depriva-
tion is most devastating (Vital-Durand et al., 1978;
Blakemore and Vital-Durand, 1979; Wiesel, 1982) but
these periods occur earlier than in man. It has been
suggested that the processes in man and monkey are
parallel but about four times faster in monkey (Teller,
1981).
REFERENCES
Abramov, I., Gordon, J., Hendrickson, A., Hainline, L.,
Dobson, V. and LaBossiere E. (1982). The retina of the
newborn human infant. Science, 217, 265-267.
Awaya, s., Miyake, Y., Imaizumi, Y., Shiose, Y., Kanda,
T. and Komuro, K. (1973). Amblyopia in man, suggestive
of stimulus deprivation amblyopia. Jap. J. Ophthalmol.,
17, 69-82.
INTRODUCriON
185
186 M.P. HEADON, J.J. SLOPER and T.P.S. POWELL
•
I
•
300 • •• •
• • ~~
• •
•
NE •
:J... • •
0
Cl!
....
0
200
•• • •
•
•
•
•
•
•
• .
~
•
Q;
u •
c
0
Cl! •- Magnocellular
~
100 •- Parvocellular :!::
:::J
"0
<{
os~~~----~~~~~~----~-L-L~~~----
10 50 100 500
Age(days)
Fig 1. Mean LGN cell area for 18 normal monkeys plottoo against
age. Cells in the parvocellular laminae do not grow between a week
of age and adulthood. From Headen et al. 1985a.
growth is also completoo early. In this the monkey differs from man
because growth in the human LGN continues until 6 months of age in
the parvocellular laminae and about two years of age in the
magnocellular laminae (Hickey 1977). There is also evidence from
the development of the detailoo structure of LGN cells (Garey and
Saini 1981, De Courten and Garey 1982) and the timing of the
appearance of cytochrome oxidase patches in the visual cortex
(Horton 1984, Horton and Hooley-White 1984), that the infant monkey
is considerably more mature at birth than the human, although the
basic pattern of cell growth appears to be similar (Headon et al.
1985a). In the cat the sensitive period corresponds to a period of
rapid LGN cell growth (Garey et al. 1973) and it has been suggestoo
that this may be a general principle (Hickey 1 977), but it is
clearly not so in the monkey (Headon et al 1985a).
g •- Undeprived
o- Deprived
"- Approx age at dosurP E " Approx.oge at closure
'/,
"
-10
or---------------~~~
t
£
g
E
"
-30
0 ~--------------~----v~
l
~
-10
-20
g
t
E
"
-3()
0
£ -10
g
"t£
~ -20
E
~
-30
l
-10
-20
-30 t
l
0 £
-10
-20
-30
=;~L
0o 100 200~---soo 600
<'---//---
2!
c Magnocellular
~ Parvocellular
~ .,,
i 0
''
£
~
-10 •
:e
~
-20 '~·
&" ...........,
- - #---
~c -30
~,.____._
~ -40
-50
•- - - - - • Undeprived magnocellular
'1.
Deprived magnocellular
- Undeprived parvocellular
2!
c
Deprived parvocellular
~
__ ..-----#'---- _ ..
~ - .#--
i
£
~
:e
~ -20
' 'c: '
.,.o-- - -- -o- - - - - #- - - - o, .... '
b--- -<>....
~c -30
~ -40
-soo~--~20~---4~0~--~67
o----~80~---1~oo~~--4-00L----4~2-o-*~18~4-0--~1860
Durat 1on (days)
that disuse may also play a part in causing the cell size changes
(Von Noorden and Middleditch 1975, Von Noorden et al. 1976).
Although confirming the smaller difference in size between deprived
and urrleprived monocular segment cells, comparisons with cells
sizes in the normal monocular segment have shown this difference to
be due to a small degree of hypertrophy of cells in the urrleprived
monocular segment (Sloper et al. 1987). Because there is no direct
influence from the deprived eye in this region this cannot be due
to disuse but probably indicates a weak indirect or diffuse
binocular effect influencing cells in the monocular segment.
ffiiTICAL PERIODS
CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS
REFERENCES
INTRODUCTION
The eye 1 s optical and axial components normally grow in a
regulated manner so that the eye maintains an approximately
emmetropic refractive condition throughout development. But, if the
potential for a clear retinal image is prevented during development
by an ocular abnormality (or by experimental manipulation in
laboratory animals), the coordinated growth of the eye is disrupted
resulting in an anomalous refractive error (Rabin et al., 1981; von
Noorden and Lewis, 1987; also see Criswell and Goss, 1983; and
Yinon, 1984, for recent reviews). Since these resulting refractive
errors can aggravate existing amblyopiogenic factors, optimal
treatment and management procedures for amblyopia require an
understanding of the vision-dependent mechanisms that influence the
emmetropization process.
Form deprivation produced by lid suture, the most common
manipulation employed to degrade the retinal image in experimental
animals, has consistently been shown to disrupt the normal
emmetropization process in young monkeys. There is, however, a
substantial amount of variability in the type and magnitude of the
refractive error produced by form deprivation in young monkeys
(Raviola and Wiesel, 1985; von Noorden and Crawford, 1978; Smith et
al., 1987). Since form-deprived monkeys potentially could serve as
useful animal models in investigations of the vision-dependent
mechanisms that influence the emmetropization process, it is
important to determine the primary reasons for the intersubject
variability in the effects of form deprivation on the monkey 1 s
refractive status. Identifying the experimental and subject factors
responsible for this variability should also provide insight into
the mechanisms regulating emmetropization. In the present report,
we have analyzed existing data on form-deprived monkeys in order to
*Supported in part by grants EY 03611, EY 01120, and EY 01139 from
the National Eye Institute.
197
198 E.L. SMITH III et al.
, o-............
A
A ,
.............""""'
A
·-·· 1
- 10
r-1
u
0
W€SE.l ..,~AAio'IOLA
(19711
.,~~.=~~~.~:,o
l.::1::
UJ G HARW'E.ATH lit ... f 1N.l)
u 0
z
UJ
-80 SMITH~.._ (111187)
cr
~ ~
~ ~ -60
cr
~ ii -40
i
UJ •
~ -~0
i= B ·.·.
u
<(
cr
lL
UJ
a: •2 0
0 5 10 15 20 2.5 30
MONOCULARLY DEPRIVED MONKEYS
"'w
u -12.0
•
•
VON NOOROEN & CRAWFORD (1978)
HARWERTH et al (1963)
z e SMITH et ul
w
a:
~ ~ -10.0
~~ ~ -8.0
• •
....
owu
~~0
w~~
•
..
we~
> a: -4.0
.
t=
u
...
a:
t:
-2.0
w
a: 0 - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - _ _ _ _. _ _ _
•2.0 •
•
30 100 300 1000
AGE AT ONSET (DAYS)
Ci)
a:
,_w
0.
• VON NO ORDEN & CRAWFORD (1977)
+4.0 I I I
•
B
·f .o-· ..... · i
INTERQC.UlA" AlCIAL UIIIG1H Ra,1iOS
....
•
•
...........
• WIII!KL AIOID ~WIOU 11• 11)
""'"'' ~"'
DISCUSSION
The results of our analysis demonstrate that when an
appropriate reference is available (eg., the nondepri ved eye of
monocularly lid-sutured monkeys), the effects of form deprivation
on the developing monkey's refractive status are extremely
consistent. In 45 of the 49 individual cases (92%) presented in the
literature, monocular lid suture caused the deprived eye to develop
a longer axial length and/or to become more myopic/less hyperopic
than the nondepri ved control eye. The magnitude of this relative
axial myopia depends on both the age at onset and the duration of
deprivation. It appears that much of the apparent intersubject
variability in the type and magnitude of these deprivation-induced
refractive errors can be attributed to fundamental experimental
EXPERIMENTAL MYOPIA 205
REFERENCES
Criswell, M.H. and Goss, D.A. (1983). Myopia development in
nonhuman primates -- a literature review. Am. J. Optom. Physiol.
Opt., 60, 250-268.
Greene, P.R. and Guyton, D.L. (1986). Time course of rhesus
lid-suture myopia. Exp. Eye Res., 42, 529-534.
Harwerth, R.S., Smith, E.L., Boltz, R.L., Crawford, M.L.J., and von
Noorden, G.K. (1983). Behavioral studies on the effect of abnormal
early visual experience in monkeys: spatial modulation sensitivity.
Vision Res., 23, 1501-1510.
206 E.L. SMITH III eta!.
M.L.J. CRAWFORD
207
208 M.L.J. CRAWFORD
...
, ,
•-....u
_.., .
c __ ......................
_ _ _ c . ...... . _ ... , ,. ruo .. ,,
·~(.11
;,
j
j ...
• !
'
!. L
:· .·...
~ • 4 • •• "
o•rOittOC• IC 11110 1
..
l~l!oOl
cs....-c, o
,.,,..,._,...,.,. , ,,,...r••
<1.1 . _ _ , _ . . . . . .. .. - •• • •• ••, , . ., .
........c,.,
, ,--.
~t
. . ..... ..........,
. ............... lo•WIO ....... . 0 00 I I to Uoo l
L
'
' ...
11 Jl 1111 • ~ •
o•o,ool ORIDIIII•C.• I( OUII
Normal
~
79%
A % D %
B
%tl=n E
~ Ofo
-LI::.n
0
1 4 7
F-LJjT-
60% 90/120 60% ED
c 40%
200fo
20% 11-N
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
ED F M B •
50
IIIllL853B090
45
D 0328MBD14
:
B NCliMI 01 = 440)
~
I
1!5 25
.o
... 15
10
R E F E R E N C E S
COLIN BLAKEMORE
INTRODUCTION
219
220 C. BLAKEMORE
X
w
0
z
z
0
i= 05
u
6z 04
02
-SENSITIVE PERIOD-
0 1
OL-------L-------L-------L-------L-------L---
0 5 10 15 20 25
AGE (weeks)
geniculate
This diagram summarizes Hubel and Wiesel's experiments
demonstrating a sensitive period for the modification of ocular
dominance of cells in area 17 of the kitten. Each bar represents
results from one animal. The length of the bar shows the period of
deprivation of one eye (always the contralateral eye). The
"induction index" is the ratio of the number of neurons dominated
by the experienced eye to the total number of responsive cells.
The interrupted line indicates the value of this index for a large
sample from normal cats. (Reproduced, by permission, from
Blakemore, 1974).
SENSITIVE PERIODS OF THE MONKEY'S VISUAL CORTEX 221
Thus, from this work in the cat, it seems that the balance of
input to the cortex from the axons of right-eye and left-eye
geniculate cells is controlled by the relative activity of those
axons during a fairly distinct but quite long sensitive period
after birth. At the height of this period, as little as a few
hours of monocular deprivation has a detectable effect on cortical
ocular dominance.
MONOCULAR DEPRIVATION
100
D CJ
20
11-16 MONTHS
10
ADULT FOR 10
61;2 MONTHS
geniculate
In these histograms, the ocular dominance of neurons in the
primate striate cortex is expressed according to the seven-group
system of Rubel and Wiesel (1962), arranged in such a way that
cells monocularly dominated by the left eye fall in the first bin
of the histogram, while those totally dominated by the right eye
lie in the seventh bin. Between these two extremes there are five
categories of binocular neurons, covering a spectrum of relative
dominance between the two eyes. The histograms with unfilled
blocks on the left refer to samples of non-oriented cells recorded
within layer IVc. Those on the right are for cells recorded in all
other layers (filled blocks for orientation selective cells,
unfilled for non-oriented), The ordinate represents the number of
neurons in each bin. The three monkeys whose data are represented
by these histograms were each monocularly deprived by suturing the
lids of the right eye during the period of time indicated on the
left. The eye was re-opened at the end of the period of
deprivation and recordings were immediately made from the cortex.
Very few cells were recorded in layer IVc of the adult animal and
therefore no histogram is shown for that unrepresentative sample.
(Results of C. Blakemore, L.J. Garey and F. Vital-Durand.)
224 C. BLAKEMORE
Results like these led Rubel et al. (1977) to suggest that the
plasticity of ocular dominance during the sensitive period is
dependent on the displacement from cells of layer IVc of terminals
carrying reduced activity from the non-deprived eye. Since much of
layer IVc is initially contacted by both sets of axons, the
exclusion of the less active fibres can be viewed as a passive and
irreversible degenerative loss.
in establishing synapses.
REVERSE SUTURING
5112 WEEKS
D
8 WEEKS
D D
9 WEEKS
0 D
L
38 112 WEEKS
0
LEFT----+ RIGHT LEFT-RIGHT
LAYER !Vc NON LAYER !Vc
geniculate
Ocular dominance histograms, arranged as in Fig. 2, are shown
for four monkeys, whose right eyes were initially deprived until
the age indicated and then were re-opened while the left eye was
deprived for at least a further sixteen weeks. (Modified from
Blakemore et al, 1978),
226 C. BLAKEMORE
50 50
.
"'
~
~
"!
0 20 20
>-
0
z
0 10 10
t=
3
0
(j)
UJ
a:
...J
<(
t=
<( Behavioural acuity
"'-
(j)
geniculate
These curves represent limits to the resolution of spatial
detail in normal monkeys of different ages. In each case
resolution (in cycles per deg. of visual angle) is plotted against
age. The filled circles plot estimates of the absolute limit to
resolution imposed by the foveal cone mosaic, based on Hendrickson
and Kupfer's (1976) measurements of the diameter of the rod-free
area in pigtail macaque monkeys, combined with Blakemore and Vital-
Durand's (1986) measurements of the growth of the monkey eye and
hence the increase in retinal magnification. The unfilled
triangles (upright for the LGN; inverted for the striate cortex)
show the maximum spatial frequency of a high-contrast drifting
grating that produced a just-detectable response from the best-
performing neurons. (Data of Blakemore and Vital-Durand, 1983,
1986.) Finally, the filled squares plot behavioural determinations
of visual acuity, based on the preferential looking technique, for
a single pigtail macaque (F2) from the study of Teller et al
(1978). (Unpublished analysis of D.S. Jacobs and C. Blakemore.)
SENSITIVE PERIODS OF THE MONKEY'S VISUAL CORTEX 229
100
50
10
1t
5
0
.~
'--y-
0.5
-- .. ~ 10
"""
geniculate
For the results shown in this diagram individual neurons in
the monkey striate cortex were stimulated with drifting gratings of
different spatial frequency and different contrast to determine a
contrast threshold at each spatial frequency. The ordinate is
contrast sensitivity (the reciprocal of contrast threshold). Each
cu~e is the envelope of a substantial number of individual
contrast sensitivity curves for different cells. The thin upper
curve represents data from a sample of cells from a normal adult
monkey (Adult N). The two lowest, thin curves refer to a normal
45-day-old animal (45 d N) and a 45-day-old animal that was
deprived of vision in both eyes from the day of birth (45 d BD).
The two thick cu~es refer to responses through the right and left
eyes of a monkey that had been deprived of vision in its right eye
until the age of 45 days and then reverse sutured (restoring vision
to the right eye and depriving the left) until more than a year of
age. The results show that contrast sensitivity and spatial
resolution were higher through the eye that had had vision only
until 45 days of age. (Unpublished results of C. Blakemore and F.
Vital-Durand.)
the visual status of the animal and that shifts in ocular dominance
provoked by monocular deprivation or defocus, or by artificial
strabismus, are not necessarily correlated with amblyopia. After
all, even prolonged monocular deprivation around the peak of the
sensitive period still leaves a small population of neurons with
input from the deprived eye: indeed, in layer IVc of the monkey,
the most exaggerated reorganization of ocular dominance stripes yet
described still leaves about 20% of cortical cells dominated by
input from the deprived eye. As far as the behavioural capacity of
the animal is concerned, surely the properties and performance of
neurons through a deprived or deviating eye are at least as
important as the mere fraction of cells still responding through
that eye. Indeed, very recent work by c. Blakemore, I. Vital-
Durand and M.J. Hawken shows that shifts in ocular dominance are
sometimes not correlated with changes in the spatial performance of
cortical cells in the monkey. For instance, brief monocular
deprivation very early in life (1 - 8 days, with subsequent re-
of the deprived eye for more than a year) hardly causes a
openinggeniculate
detectable shift in ocular dominance yet leaves the contrast
sensitivity of cells distinctly lower through the deprived eye than
through the non-deprived eye.
MC V3
MD 14-24 months
j 30
w
u
"- 20
0
"'~
a:
10
::;;
:::>
z
0
1234567 10
0.1 0.5
OCULAR DOMINANCE
SPATIAL FREQUENCY (c/deg)
geniculate
These diagrams show results for a sample of neurons recorded
in the striate cortex of a monkey whose right eye was deprived by
closure of the lids from 14-24 months of age. The ocular dominance
histogram on the left (see Fig. 2) shows a substantial shift in
favour of the contralateral (non-deprived, left) eye. However, the
envelopes of samples of invidual contrast sensitivity curves shown
on the right (of the type shown in Fig. 5) suggest that spatial
performance through the deprived right eye was not severely
affected, certainly at the high spatial-frequency end of the
spectrum, where vision is normally very poor through an amblyopic
eye. The continuous line is the envelope of contrast sensitivity
curves for neurons when stimulated with drifting gratings shown to
the left, non-deprived eye. The interrupted line refers to the
sensitivity of cells when stimulated through the deprived, right-
eye. (Unpublished results of C. Blakemore, M.J. Hawken and F.
Vital-Durand.)
232 C. BLAKEMORE
Acknowledgements
References
INTRODUCTION
235
236 R.S. HARWERTH and EARLL. SMITH III
Several studies have shown that there is not a unitary critical period
for the whole visual system, but rather, there are different critical periods
of development for various levels of the visual pathway (Blakemore et al.,
1978; LeVay et al., 1980; Jones et al., 1984) and, at a given level, different
response characteristics of visual neurons may have different critical
periods (Berman and Daw, 1977). On the basis of these physiological
findings, it might logically be predicted that psychophysical measures of
visual system function would exhibit different critical periods for
processing various types of visual stimuli. Therefore, we have conducted a
psychophysical assessment of critical periods of development in monkeys
through measurements of the alterations produced by monocular form
deprivation on four visual functions (scotopic spectral sensitivity, photopic
increment-threshold spectral sensitivity, spatial contrast sensitivity, and
binocular summation). The age of onset of form deprivation was varied
between animals, but its duration was held constant at 18 months.
1A 18
'\
-~---------------
~ ....____________________ _
4 6 8 10 12 14 16 2 14 16 18 20 22
IIG~ AT TIMf' MONOCULAR DC PRIVATION !MONTHS AGE AT TIME- MONOCULAR DEPRIVATION !MONTHS'•
1C
~ .,.*':--~;\
11.0 \
g
0.0 ---------~~--~---;:=:--:::--:::--:=:--~--~--~--~--=--""--
6 e •o '' ,. 1c •s zo 22 ?•
AGE AT TIMf MONOCUI AH DEPRIVATION 1MONTHS1
2.0 . - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - , 2.0
1.8 2A 28
0 1.6
>=
1.4 <(
a: 1.2
>-
f-
1.0 > 0.8
>=
Ci5
z 0.4
06 Ul
rn
(!)
0.0
-0.4
-0.2 -<---,-,---,---.-,-----.---.------,---r'
.12 .25 .50 1.0 2.0 4.0 8.0 16.0 32.0 .12 .25 .50 1.0 2.0 4.0 8.016.0 32.0
SPATIAL FREQUENCY (c/deg) SPATIAL FREQUENCY (c/deg)
s -0.1 \. •
X
•
MD-2
MD-8
presented in Fig. 2A. In this figure the logarithm of the ratio of the
sensitivity of the eye with higher sensitivity to that of the eye with lower
sensitivity was plotted as a function of the spatial frequency of the test
stimulus. For comparison purposes, the 95% confidence limits for the
population variability of interocular sensitivity differences for normal
control animals is shown by the dashed lines. The data demonstrate that
only one subject, the subject treated for six weeks (BD-6), had interocular
sensitivity ratios within the range of control subjects. The sensitivity
ratios of all of the other subjects were outside the normal range, but the
magnitudes of the deviations were not ordered with respect to the duration
of form deprivation. For example, the sensitivity ratios for subject BD-18,
the subject treated for 18 weeks, were closer to the normal range than
those of the subject treated for 13 weeks (BD-13). However, in every case,
242 R.S. HARWERTH and EARLL. SMITH III
Of the bilaterally deprived subjects, the data for subject BD-2 show
the largest deviations from the normal range and require special
consideration. Subject BD-2 was form deprived for two weeks, which was
the shortest treatment period used in the study, and based on the data for
subject BD-6, BD-2 would have been expected to have approximately equal
and normal sensitivity functions. However, at the end of the treatment
period, when the lids were parted, the animal developed an eyelid infection
of the left eye and he voluntarily held his left eyelids closed for about five
days while the infection was treated. Although, in a sense, this subject was
compromised for the bilateral form deprivation experiment, his data are
presented to demonstrate the clinically relevant effects of a short-term,
incomplete, monocular form deprivation during a relatively early portion of
the critical period of development. On the other hand, it is appropriate to
include BD-2 in the study since the monocular form deprivation followed
the completion of the bilateral-form-deprivation treatment and the direct-
deprivation effects of the left, unifected eye are still important to the
purposes of the present experiments. In fact, the evaluation of direct
deprivation effects was restricted to the eyes with higher sensitivities for
all of the subjects, because all but one of them developed bilateraly
asymmetric losses of contrast sensitivity.
CONCLUSIONS
REFERENCES
Bedell, H.E., Flom, M.C. and Barbeito, R. (1985). Spatial aberrations and
acuity in strabismus and amblyopia. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci.,~ 909-
916.
Berman, N. and Daw, N.W. (1977). Comparison of the critical periods for
monocular and directional deprivation in cats. J. Physiol., 265, 249-259.
Harwerth, R.S., Crawford, M.L.J., Smith, E.L. and Boltz, R.L. (1981).
Behavioral studies of stimulus deprivation amblyopia in monkeys. Vision
Res., 1!_, 779-789.
Harwerth, R.S., Smith, E.L., Duncan, G.C., Crawford, M.L.J. and von
Noorden, G.K. (1986). Multiple sensitive periods in the development of the
primate visual system. Science, 232, 235-238.
Harwerth, R.S., Smith, E.L., Paul, A.D., Crawford, M.L.J. and von Noorden,
G.K. (1987). Psychophysical studies of binocular deprivation in monkeys.
Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. (suppl), ~ 318.
Bubel, D.H. and Wiesel, T.N. (1970). The period of susceptibility to the
physiological effects of unilateral eye closure in kittens. J. Physiol., 206,
419-436.
LeVay, S., Wiesel, T.N. and Hubel, D.H. (1980). The development of ocular
dominance columns in normal and visually deprived monkeys. J. Comp.
Neurol., 191, 1-51.
Levi, D.M. and Klein, S.A. (1982). Differences in vernier discrimination for
gratings between strabismic and anisometropic amblyopes. Invest.
Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci.,~ 398-407.
Smith, E.L., Harwerth, R.S., Duncan, G.C. and Crawford, M.L.J. (1986). A
comparison of the spectral sensitivities of monkeys with anisometropic and
stimulus deprivation amblyopia. Behav. Brain Res.,~ 13-24.
Wiesel, T.N. (1982). Postnatal development of the visual cortex and the
influence of environment. Nature, 299, 583-591.
246 R.S. HARWERTH and EARL L. SMITH III
Wiesel, T.N. and Rubel, D.H. (197 4). Ordered arrangement of orientatio
columns in monkeys lacking visual experience. J. Comp. Neurol., 158, 307
318.
20
NORMAL VISUAL DEVELOPMENT AND ITS DEVIATIONS
RICHARD HELD
247
248 R.HELD
Measurement
increase over the first six months. A review of this literature can
be found in Banks and Dannemiller (1987). Measurements of contrast
threshold can provide sensitive indices of amblyopia. However,
psychophysical methods require large numbers of trials which make
their application to infants rather difficult. Perhaps new VEP
methods may make these measurements more accessible (Norcia and
Tyler, 1985).
Application to Amblyopia
VERNIER ACUITY
Two criticisms of this model have been made. First, the pro-
cess of segregation of the columns takes a considerable period of
time, one that varies with species. Can such a protracted process
account for the apparently sudden onset of stereopsis? To this we
can only answer that, like the onset of many other behaviors, an
underlying neuronal process which develops gradually must at some
point exceed an internal threshold thereby allowing the expression
of behavior. The second criticism says that some species, like the
tree shrew, have stereopsis but do not have segregated ocular domi-
nance columns. To this one can only reply that different species
252 R.HELD
AMBLYOPIA
Vergence movements are not well coordinated for the first few
months after birth although newborns are said to converge appropri-
ately for targets at 25 and 50 ern (Slater and Findlay, 1975).
Aslin (1977) recorded vergence movements in 1-, 2-, and 3-rnonth olds
as they followed a target that continuously moved from a 50 ern to a
15 ern distance. Only the 3-month olds showed consistent convergent
following of the target. Moreover, following was better on slower
moving targets. On the other hand when convergence demand was in-
creased suddenly and stepwise by the application of base-out wedge
prisms, vergence following was effective only after an age between
4.5 and 6.0 months. Several possible explanations have been pro-
posed for this difference in results (Aslin, 1987). I should like
to propose a new one in line with the developmental model for binoc-
ularity discussed above.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
REFERENCES
Bauer, J.; Shimojo, S.; Gwiazda, J.; and Held, R. (1986). Sex
differences in the development of binocularity in human infants.
Investigative Ophthalmol. Visual Sci.,~. 265.
Birch, E.; Gwiazda, J.; Bauer, J.; Naegele, J.; and Held, R. (1983A).
Visual acuity and its meridional variation in children aged 7 to 60
months. Vision Research, ~. 1019-1024.
Fox, R.; Aslin, R.; Shea, S.; and Dumais, S. (1980). Stereopsis in
human infants. Science, 207, 323-324.
Gwiazda, J.; Bauer, J.; Thorn, F.; and Held, R. (1986). Meridional
amblyopia does result from astigmatism in early childhood. Clinical
Vision Sciences, l, 145-152.
Gwiazda, J.; Brill, S.; Mohindra, I.; and Held, R. (1980). Prefer-
ential looking acuity in infants from two to fifty-eight ~•eeks of
age. Amer. J. of Optometry and Physiol. Optics, 2l• 428-432.
Huttenlocher, P.; de Courten, C.; Garey, L.; and van der Loos, H.
(1982). Synaptogenesis in human visual cortex- evidence for synapse
elimination during normal development. Neuroscience, ]2, 247-252.
Mohindra, I.; Jacobson, S.; Zwaan, J.; and Held, R. (1983). Psycho-
physical assessment of visual acuity in infants with visual disor-
ders. Behavioral Brain Research, ~, 51-58.
Petrig, B.; Julesz, B.; Kropfl, W.; Baumgartner, G.; and Anliker, M.
(1981). Development of stereopsis and cortical binocularity in human
infants: electrophysiological evidence. Science, 213, 1402-1404.
Shimojo, S.; Bauer, J.; O'Connell, K.; and Held, R. (1986). Pre-
stereoptic binocular vision in infants. Vision Research, ~, 501-510.
Shil'lojo, S.; Birch, E.; Gwiazda, J.; and Held, R. (1984). Develop-
ment of vernier acuity in infants. Vision Research, ~, 721-728.
Sokol, S.; Hansen, V.; Moskowitz, A.; Greenfield, P.; and Towle, V.
(1983). Evoked potential and preferential looking estimates of vis-
ual acuity in pediatric patients. Ophthalmology, 90, 552-562.
WOLF SINGER
INTRODUCTION
Activity-dependent changes of neuronal connectivity are an
ubiquitous phenomenon during early ontogeny and a constituent
factor in the self-organization of the brain. During postnatal
development, these shaping processes become increasingly subtle and
dependent on a growing number of variables including sensory
experience.
In the following paragraph I shall concentrate on
activity-dependent modifications of cortical connections that are
required for the development of binocular receptive fields.
However, similar processes are likely to promote the sharpening of
orientation selectivity and the generation of selectively coupled
neuron ensembles subserving stereopsis and higher visual functions.
Before visual experience becomes effective in influencing the
ocularity of cortical neurons, most cells in the visual cortex of
cats and monkeys respond to stimulation of both eyes. This
condition is maintained with normal experience but also with dark
rearing. With monocular deprivation, however, the large majority of
cortical cells lose the ability to respond to the deprived eye
(Blakemore et al., 1978; Wiesel and Hubel, 1965a). The cells which
continue to respond to the deprived eye are located preferentially
within layer IV in the center of the shrunken termination fields of
the deprived eye (Shatz and Stryker, 1978). This suggests that only
those cells continue to respond to the deprived eye which have been
monocularly driven by this eye right from the beginning. With
reverse suture, if it occurs early enough, the previously closed
eye may recover control over most cortical neurons while the
previously open eye in turn becomes ineffective (Blakemore et
al., 1978; Wiesel and Hubel, 1965b).
259
260 W. SINGER
CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS
The adaptive mechanisms disclosed by monocular deprivation and
the resulting neuronal changes can be related directly to the
pathophysiology of deprivation amblyopia. Most likely,
however,theye are relevant also for other developmental
disturbances of binocular functions. No further assumptions need to
be made to account for the disruption of binocular cooperativity in
the (ase of alternating strabismus. Due to the squint the activity
pattern conveyed by the afferents from the two eyes show no
systematic correlation. Hence, afferents from one eye stabilize
always on the expense of the respective afferents from the other
268 W. SINGER
REFERENCES
Altmann, L., Luhmann, H.J., Greuel, J.M. and Singer, W. (1987)
Functional and neuronal binocularity in kittens raised with rapidly
alternating monocular occlusion. J. Neurophysiol., in print.
Artola, A. and Singer, W. (1987) Long-term potentiation and NMDA
receptors in rat visual cortex. Nature, submitted.
Bear, M.F. and Daniels, I.D. (1983) The plastic response to
monocular deprivation persists in kitten visual cortex after
chronic depletion of norepinephrine. J. Neurosci. l· 407-416.
Bear, M.F. and Singer, W.(1986) Modulation of visual cortical
plasticity by acetylcholine and noradrenaline. Nature 320, 172-176.
Blakemore, C. and Van Sluyters, R.C. (1974) Reversal of the
physiological effects of monocular deprivation in kittens:
Further evidence for a sensitive period. J. Physiol. 237, 195-216.
Blakemore, C., Garey, L.J. and Vital-Durand, F. (1978) The
physiological effects of monocular deprivation and their reversal
in the monkey's visual cortex. J. Physiol. 238, 223-262.
Bode-Greuel, K.M. an~ Singer, W. (1987) The distribution of binding
sites for organic Ca +-channel blockers in the visual cortex of
kittens and adult cats. Exp. Brain Res., submitted.
Buisseret, P., Gary-Bobo, E. and Imbert, M. (1978) Ocular motility
and recovery of orientational properties of visual cortical
neurones in dark-reared kittens. Nature 272, 816-817.
Changeux, J.P. and Danchin, A. (1976) Selective stabilization of
developing synapse as a mechanism for the specification of neuronal
networks. Nature 264, 705-712.
Changeux, J.P., Heidmann, T. and Patter, P. (1984) Learning by
selection. In The Biology of Learning, Dahlem workshop. (eds. P.
and H.S. Terrace) Springer-Verlag, Berlin, pp. 115-133.
Crewther, S.G., Crewther, D.P., Peck, C.K. and Pettigrew, J.D.
(1980) Visual cortical effects of rearing cats with monocular or
binocular cyclotorsion. J. Neurophysiol. 44, 97-118.
Cynader, M. and Mitchell, D.E. (1977) Monocular astigmatism effects
on kitten visual cortex development. Nature 270, 177-178.
Daw, N.W., Robertson, T.W., Rader, R.K., Videen, T.O. and Cosica,
C.J. (1984) Substantial reduction of noradrenaline by lesions of
adrenergic pathway does not prevent effects of monocular
deprivation. J. Neurosci. i· 1354-1360.
272 W. SINGER
Daw, N.W., Videen, T.O., Parkinson, D. and Rader, R.K. (1985} DSP-4
depletes noradrenaline in kitten visual cortex without altering the
effects of monocular deprivation. J. Neurosci. ~. 1925-1933.
Freeman, R.D. and Bonds, A.B. (1979) Cortical plasticity in
monocularly deprived immobilized kittens depends on eye movement.
Science 206, 1093-1095.
Freeman, R.D. and Olson, C. (1982) Brief periods of monocular
deprivation in kittens: Effects of delay prior to physiologcial
study. J. Neurophysiol. 47, 139-150.
Geiger, H. and Singer, W. (1986) A possible role of calcium
currents in developmental plasticity. Exp. Brain Res. Ser. 1!·
256-270.
Greuel, J.M., Luhmann, H.J. and Singer, W. (1987) Persistent
changes of single cell responses in kitten striate cortex produced
by pairing sensory stimulation with ionophoretic application of
neurotransmitters and neuromodulators. In Cellular Mechanisms of
Conditioning and Behavioral Plasticity. (eds. C. Woody et al.)
Plenum Press, in print.
Hebb, D.O. (1949) The organization of behavior. John Wiley &Sons,
New York.
Kasamatsu, T. and Pettigrew, J.D. (1979) Preservation of
binocularity after monocular deprivation in the striate cortex of
kittens treated with 6-hydroxydopamine. J. Camp. Neurol. 185,
163-162.
Kasamatsu, T., Pettigrew, J.D. and Ary, M.-L. (1979) Restoration of
visual cortical plasticity by local microperfusion of
norepinephrine. J. Comp. Neurol. 185, 163-182.
Kleinschmidt, M., Bear, M.F. and Singer, W. (1987) Evidence for an
involvement of NMDA receptor mechanisms in experience-dependent
plasticity of kitten striate cortex. Science, in print.
Llinas, R. (1979) The role of calcium in neuronal function. In The
Neurosciences Fourth Stud Pro ram, 4th. (eds. F. 0. Schmitt ana
F.G. Worden. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 555-571.
Llinas, R. and Sugimori, M. (1979) Calcium conductances in Purkinje
cell dendrites: their role in development and integration. Progr.
Brain Res. ~. 323-334.
Mioche, L. and Singer, W. (1987) Long-term recordings and receptive
field measurements from single units of the visual cortex of awake
unrestrained kittens. in prep.
Rauschecker, J.P. and Hahn, S. (1987) Ketamine-xylazine anaesthesia
MONOCULAR DEPRIVATION 273
275
276 G.K. von NOORDEN
References
1. Aulhorn, E. (1967). Die gegenseitige Beeinflussung
abbildungsgleicher Netzhautstellen bei normalem und
gest8rten Binocularsehen. Doc. Ophthalmol., 23,
26-61. -
2. Awaya, S. and von Noorden, G.K. (1972). Visual
acuity of amblyopic eyes under monocular and
binocular conditions: further observations. In
Orthoptics. (eds. J.J.M. Bierlaagh and T.E.A.
Brummelkamp-Dons). Excerpta Medica, Amsterdam.
ABNORMAL BINOCULAR INTERACTION 283
Held: I'm not sure you want to make that claim because
there are kittens that show natural strabismus as well as
monkeys. On the other hand I would agree that the lengthy
immaturity may make the visual system vulnerable.
285
286 G.K. von NOORDEN
Garey: When you compare humans with Old and New World
monkeys it is surprising that there are so few
differences in normal maturation, especially when you
remember that the structure of the visual cortex in New
World species lacks typical ocular dominance columns.
320
!J N =8 (Crawford,
NORMAl MONKEYS
1987)
280 IZl N= 7 PRISM REARED MONKEYS
240
N~
~ 200
:Iw
0
~
180
-Hi 'AI
120
80
1 2
MAG. PARVO.
LGN LAYERS
Harwerth: The data for the 1:4 ratio for the relative
rates of development of monkeys and humans comes from
comparisons of the relative rates of development of
visual acuity in the two species*. The generalizability
of the ratio would be strengthened if other functions
that were compared for monkeys and humans resulted in the
same ratio. One other function that has been compared and
found to develop at about the same 1:4 rate is
accommodation**· In addition, studies of the development
of the ocular components of cynomolgus monkeys have shown
an approximate ratio of 1:3 for monkey to human
development***· Therefore, the results of several studies
seem to converge and I do not think that the 1:4 ratio is
very far off.
Held: That may be true but the important question is: are
there disparity mechanisms other than cortical?
304 G.K. von NOORDEN
EMILIO C. CAMPOS
311
312 E.C. CAMPOS
strabismus. Because of ocular deviation, corresponding
points in space are projected onto non-corresponding
retinal elements. In the case of ARC some patients
acquire binocular integration, as though the spatial
"map" of the deviated eye has shifted so that
anatomically disparate retinal positions gain functional
correspondence with the spatial coordinates of the
fellow eye. Thus, the patient may be capable of
binocular visual localization and in certain cases even
of coarse stereopsis: (Mehdorn, 1982; Schor et al,
1983). This adaptive modification of binocular
correspondence is referred to as anomalous binocular
vision (Bagolini, 1967). In this situation, there is
usually no suppression of the image of the deviated eye.
AMBLYOPIA
REFERENCES
INTRODUCTION
319
320 L. MAFFEI and A. FIORENTINI
ELECTROPHYSIOLOGICAL EXPERIMENTS
The OB of the Vth nerve was cut in kittens 5-6 weeks old and
the animals were then raised in a normal visual environment until
the 4th month of age, when recording from the striate visual
cortex was performed. The proportion of cortical neurones that
could be activated by a stimulus in either eye was found to be
much lower in these animals than in normal controls. Sixty percent
of the neurons were found to respond to one eye only (Maffei,
1979). These findings have been confirmed and extended by Trotter
et al., (1983).
BEHAVIOURAL EXPERIMENTS
Orienting Behaviour
COMMENTS
References
Rose, P.K. and Abrahams, V.C. (1975). The effect of passive eye
movement on unit discharge in the superior colliculus of the
cat. Brain Res., 97, 95-106.
327
328 M.J. STEINBACH, M.A. MUSARELLA and B.L. GALLIE
TABLE 1
AGE AT VISION
PATIENT SURGERY DIAGNOSIS LE RE
TABLE 2
Pre- and Post-Operative Measures of Precision for Enucleated and Strabismic Surgical
Patients (Averaged Standard Deviations of the Open-Loop Pointing Responses Given in
Degrees)
2 2 3 4
Enucleates 1.3 (12)* 1.8 (12) 1.8 (12) 1.5 (11) 1.0 (5)
6
4
2
0
-2
-4
Pre P1 P2 P7
visible target
Pre P1 P2 P3 p 12
Fig. 1. Average constant
8
6
4
2
0'-r-~-..--,---,,..---,---,.---
Pre 1 Pre 2 P 1 P 2 P 3 P 6 P 13 Pre P1 P3
TEST SESSION
Fig. 1. Average constant error of the 13 surgical patients in pointing to a visible target
without sight of hand. All were enucleated, except for patient 13, who had an evisceration.
PSYCHOPHYSICS OF PROPRIOCEPTION 333
J~_./ /
CS1 CS2
~
sa: /--
0
CI:I-
~
=iJ Pre 1
I I I I I I
-4
CI:I Pre 2 Pre 3 P1 P2 P3
I I I I I I
=n--- v-
UJC) Pre 1 Pre 2 Pre 3 P1 P2 P3
I--
~~
..____ .___...
CS3
=!~
CS4
1-1-
(f)(/)
z-:-
0 ·-
ut
wUJ
I I I I I I I I I I I I
~~
:il ..___....
css CS6
Jl~
UJ(f)
------
>-
<( +
~
I I I I I I -4 I I I I I I
Pre 1 Pre 2 Pre 3 p 1 P2 P3 Pre 1 Pre 2 Pre 3 P 1 P2 P3
TEST SESSION TEST SESSION
Fig. 2. Average constant error for six normal control subjects who wore a patch for 48 hours
over one eye. The localizing responses made using the unpatched eye are shown for the
three days preceding the occlusion and the three days following the occlusion.
REFERENCES
EMILIO C. CAMPOS
INTRODUCTION
337
338 E.C. CAMPOS
CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS
REFERENCES
M.L.J. CRAWFORD
345
346 M.L.J. CRAWFORD
A B
] 80 " '
RE
D D
f TIOled Excitatory
j
i
TIOled - o r y
<1------~~~~------i----~~~==~----
Dopth
EUGENE M. HELVESTON
Screening
359
360 E.M. HELVESTON
The TNO also utilizes the Julesz principle but with red
and blue-green random dots. This test requires red and
blue-green anaglyph spectacles to produce the awareness of a
red and green embedded target which is seen in depth. The
STEREOPSIS AND STRABISMUS 361
Prediction
In our clinic, the Randot and TNO plates are used less
frequently. While the TNO produces fewer monocular clues,
it is significantly more difficult, in our experience, for a
younger observer to appreciate. The Randot is certainly a
satisfactory device and one which deserves attention. The
Lang, or Frisbee test for screening may be less useful in an
office setting where testing of higher grades of stereo
acuity is required. Projected stereo charts using the AO
projector at distance are used for detecting malingerers who
will report seeing a letter out of one eye or the other
without knowing it.
364 E.M. HELVESTON
References
INTRODUCTION
369
370 D.M. LEVI and S.A. KLEIN
1000
300
0w
"'a:
0
&
~ & '
...
> @
5
0 & &
< '•
z @
&
0
;::: 30 ,, &
iii
0 ' '/ ' /
a..
@'
&
~~
~~~
"ot:! • p
.s 10 20 so
Fig. 1. The relationship between Vernier acuity (in arc sec) and Snellen
acuity (in arc min). The data are from several studies in our
lab. Data of normal control observers are shown by the open
circles. Preferred eyes of strabismic amblyopes are shown by
the P's in boxes, those of anisometropic amblyopes shown by the
P's. Data of the amblyopic eyes are shown by A's, S's and B's for
anisometropic and strabismic amblyopes and those with both.
Also included are data from Rentschler and Hilz (1985). The
open triangle is the mean data of their normal observers, the
filled triangle, the mean of the preferred eye of their
amblyopes. Letter in triangles show the amblyopic eyes of all
but 1 of their observers.
form perception. It is not surprising then that in amblyopia, which is
considered to be an abnormality of the "form" sense (Wald and Burian,
1944), that hyperacuity is markedly degraded. What is both interesting, and
surprising, is that in hyperacuity tasks, several lines of evidence now
suggest that the neural losses of strabismic and anisometropic amblyopes
are fundamentally different. The purpose of this chapter is to review
recent studies of the spatial sense and to point to mechanisms which may
underly the psychophysical and clinical anomalies of the amblyopic visual
system.
8 400
{j]
() Ill
••
A 300
@ w
200 Ul 200
(,)
> a:
I-
5 s /
0 100 _,
Q 100 /
I
< /
/ 0 /
a:
w~
/ ::t
60
ii'l /
/
- 50
z 0w ~/
Ul
w /
a: Ul
w @ a: /
> 0 0/ ::t 40 /
/
1-
o"' /o z
~·t•
//®
wS 20 0
"
z
< ~
j::
(,)
20 ~/
~/
...-'z w
,/
/ /
10 / Ul /
iii
:I
p _, 10
tl
5 4(
:I ®
j::
0.. 5
.5 5 10 0 .5 2 3 5 10
GRATING ACUITY GRATING ACUITY (MINUTES)
(MINUTES)
similar relationship exists between bisection acuity and Snellen acuity (Levi
and Klein, 1983; 1987). Specifically, the slope of the line drawn through
the data of Figure 1 is unity, suggesting that Snellen acuity and position
acuity are affected in the same way by the amblyopic process.
Figure 2 plots vernier acuity (Figure 2A) and acuity for another
position discrimination task, viz. bisection (Figure 2B), as a function of
grating acuity. In each of these figures, the data of nonstrabismic
anisometropic amblyopes are shown by A's while those of strabismic
amblyopes, and those with both constant strabismus and anisometropia are
shown by S's and B's respectively. For both tasks the A's fall along the
straight line with unity slope, showing that grating resolution and positional
acuity are similarly affected by the amblyopic process. However, the
strabismic amblyopes show grater losses in the accuracy of positional
acuity than for grating resolution.
because Vernier acuity is often much more degraded than grating acuity,
easily resolved targets may be subject to strong spatial interference, since
the zone of interference is tied to the Vernier acuity rather than the
grating acuity. Thus "crowding" occurs at spatial frequencies well below
the cutoff in strabismic amblyopes. As will be discussed below, we think
that the interference effects which occur for Vernier acuity, are closely
linked to the crowding effect for Snellen acuity, which is well known to
clinicians.
Barlow (1979) suggested the intriguing notion that the processing zone
required for optimal hyperacuity has its anatomical basis In the modular
organization of the visual cortex. The finding that the dimensions of this
zone grow in peripheral vision, in proportion to the cortical magnification
factor (Levi et al., 1985; Yap et al., 1987) lend credence to this notion, and
suggest that spatial interference may reveal information about these
modules in human vision. In the normal fovea, the psychophysical spatial
processing module is 4-5 minutes of arc. This is approximately the spatial
extent of a normal human ocular dominance column, which is about 1mm
(Hitchcock and Hickey, 1980). It is Interesting to note that this is also
about the overall dimension of a threshold level Snellen letter (20 /20 letter
= 5').
374 D.M. LEVI and S.A. KLEIN
ECCENTRICITY (OEG)
0 5
25
i
::::E
- 20
IIJ
0
z
..:
.. 15
1/)
0
~ 10
0
;:::::
a:
0 •
0
account for the high precision of foveal positional acuity on the basis of the
spatial filtering and spatial sampling properties of the visual system (e.g.
Watt and Morgan, 1985; Klein and Levi, 1985; Wilson, 1986). In the
following section we consider several factors which could account for the
losses which occur in the amblyopic visual system, and their implications.
blur per se, but rather abnormal binocular interaction due to monocular
image degradation, which results in the loss of sensitivity in the
anisometropic amblyope.
2. Spatial sampling density
REFERENCES
Barlow, H.B. (1979). Reconstructing the visual image in space and time.
Nature, 279, 189-190.
Bedell, H.E. and Flom, !VI.C. (1983). Normal and abpormal space
perception. Am. J. Optom. Physiol. Opt. 60, 426-435.
Hess, R.F. and Jacobs, R.J. (1979). A preliminary report of acuity and
contour interactions across the amblyopes visual field. Vision Res. 19,
1403-1408.
Hess, R.F., Campbell, F.W. and Greenhalgh, T. (1978). On the nature of the
neural abnormality in human amblyopia: Neural aberrations and neural
sensitivity loss. Pflugers Arch. Ges. Physiol. 377, 201-207.
Levi, D.M. and Klein, S.A. (1982a). Hyperacuity and amblyopia. Nature,
298, 268-270.
Levi, D.M. and Klein, S.A. (1982b). Differences in Vernier acuity between
strabismic and anisometropic amblyopes. Invest. Ophthal. Visual Sci., 23,
398-407.
Levi, D.M. and Klein, S.A. (1983). Spatial localization in normal and
amblyopic vision. Vision Res. 23, 1005-1017.
Levi, D.M. and Klein, S.A. (1985). Vernier acuity, crowding and
amblyopia. Vision Res. 25, 979-991.
Levi, D.M. and Klein, S.A. (1986). Sampling in Spatial Vision. Nature, 320,
360-362.
Levi, D.M., Klein, S.A., and Aitsebaomo, P.A. (1985). Vision Res. 25, 963-
977.
Levi, D.M., Yap, Y.L. and Klein, S.A. (1987). Positional uncertainty in
peripheral and amblyopic vision. Vision Res. 27, 581-598.
Levi, D.M. and McKee, S.P. (1986) Dichoptic Hyperacuity: the precision of
Nonius alignment. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A. 13, 12.
Sakkit, B. and Barlow, H.B. (1982). A model for the economical encoding of
the visual image in the cerebral cortex. Biol. Cibernet. 43, 97-108.
Wald, G. and Burian, H.M. (1944). The dissociation of form vision and light
perception in strabismic amblyopia. Am. Ophthalmol. 27, 950-963.
Watt, R.J. and Hess, R.F. (1987). Spatial information and uncertainty in
anisometropic amblyopia. Vision Res. 27, 661-67 4.
Watt, R.J. and Morgan, M.J. (1985). A theory of the primitive spatial code
in human vision. Vision Res. 25, 1661-1674.
Yap, Y.L., Levi, D.M. and Klein, S.A. (1987). Peripheral Hyperacuity: 3-
dot bisection scales to a single factor from 0 to 10 degrees. J. Opt. Soc.
Am. A. In Press.
31
AMBLYOPIA: CLINICAL ASPECTS
W. HAASE
381
382 W. HAASE
II I
3' gratings of 3°, 1° field size
and a very small field size
0. ., G)).·, ~
1 ,
~ ,.
P
equivalent to the outer dia-
meter of a Landolt ring. The
L------~~--~=---~~--~~-----
LR 0LR ,. 3"
spatial f5equences 0 varied bet-
ween 3 c/ and 60/ . Landolt
rings were also used from
Fig. 1
AMBL YOPIA:CLINICAL ASPECTS 383
--
Vilual KUity min/arc
-----
2.0
1.6
1.25
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4 Fig. 5
Anisometropia + Squint
(mean difference = 2.4 D)
Hypermetropic anisorr. 'Myopic anisom.
(mean d. 1.9 D) (mean d.= 3.75 D)
49 n = 17
n = 26
0.6 s
L
1
LR
-
1.5 D anisom.
21 ( 75%)
n 2 L 1
2
- 1.5 D anisom.
4.3 Strabismus-Amblyopia
According to the reports in the literature the rate of
improved cases ceases the more the older the patients
are at onset oftreatment (Sattler 1927, Oppel 1964,
Dayson 1968). We assume that the amblyopia inducing
factor (s) continue to detoriate the amblyopic eye even
above the lOth year of life, because the difference of
the 2 acuities between the dominant and the amblyopic
eye increases with the age of the patients (Fig.6).
~88 W. HAASE
IWplofeoultylllnel
.._._,"'""'
Acuity clomlnent .,.
mlnue .aulty ~;•.,.
Fig. 6
-1 2-3 4·1 Adultl .. 27y.old
.n tl .. 111. t.r
10 )'L'IIIIr• an • ~<•'''''
1.0=- -
"'ho "'•• urdrr centro! b<IC'ilv•r
tJt llqlllrt
1.211
..........
_IT.,
-·IT
-
Oo
~~ , todooc.IILI" ~--1
01 eoc.fb. r
I
I
I
Ool
Ool
e t ~ n 12 a_.,,..,.
Fig. 7
Fjg. 8 a
390 W . HAASE
SPATIAL. DISJOR!IO!r:f
I beiOte t~ment l
"'
Legend for Fig. 8 a, b, c
The triangles (Fig. 8 a)
were constantly presented
monocularly to the obser-
ver. The light bar has
been flashed for 130 msec
randomi zed 10 times at
each displacement 1 - 5
(= 6' until 30' of arc).
The number of wrong answers
were counted for each dis-
placement.
b = before, c = after
treatment.
"'
Fig. 8 b
SPATIAL DISTORTION
l•tt•tr~)
Str.oilmk~.n•'t!
"'
"'
Fig. 8 c
AMBL YOPIA:CLINICA L ASPECTS 391
References
Beller, R.; Hoyt, C.S.; Marg,E.and Odom, J.V. (1981).
Good Visual Function After Neonatal Surgery for Congeni-
tal Monocular Cataracts. Am.J.Ophthal.,~, 559-565.
Dayson, A. (1968). 5 Year Survey of the Use of Occlusion
in the TreatmeDt of Eccentric Fixation. Brit.orthop .J.,
.?2_,66-74.
Florn ,M.C.; Kahnemann, C.and Weymouth,F.W . (1963) .Visual
Resolution and Contour Interaction. J.Optical Soc.Amer.,
2}, 1026-1037.
393
394 E.C. CAMPOS
GERALD WESTHEIMER
413
414 G. WESTHEIMER
confluence of signals from the two eyes to the cortex was taken up
on several occasions in the history of neuro-ophthalmology. Both
from anatomy and from neural injury, expecially after the first
world war, it was known what happens to vision in various parts of
the visual field when there are gross lesions of various parts of
the cortex. But the microcircuitry was left open. The first
electrophysiological recordings from the cat cortex by Hubel and
Wiesel showed many units that responded to stimuli from both eyes.
Anatomists still did not rise to the bait and it required these
workers another decade to demonstrate ocular dominance columns. Now
we know, of course, that the two eyes' signals are kept apart
through the geniculate and, in the monkey, into a layer of mono-
cular, center/surrounds cells in layer IVc. Electrophysiologists,
psychophysicists and clinician-scientists did rise to the bait,
however, and proceeded to draw a great number of conclusions, many
of them quite unwarranted. That lots of single units respond to
stimuli to both eyes, does not necessarily mean that the substrate
of single binocular vision and stereopsis resides in these units.
For disparity processing, for example, the contrary can be argued,
for a single monocular stimulus can be paired with one to the left
of it coming from the other eye for the purposes of detection of
crossed disparity, and another to the right, for the purposes of
uncrossed disparity. And these can be many degrees apart and with
quite a different orientation. Yet I know of no better indication
of good binocularity than the detection of disparity for the
purposes of fusional movements and stereopsis.
what the strength of the claims are and what their possible
application in the clinic. Usually one cannot hurry laboratory
research, particularly in days of reduced funding, opposition from
animal rights groups, and brain drain to highly appealing subjects
such as molecular biology and artificial intelligence. But
clinicians, particularly, should insist on recognition, by these
scientists who claim to care, of the validity of their observations
on patients. This is one of the most important talking points
between clinicians and scientists. Clinical observations can be
strengthened by clear measurements, by statistics, by proper double-
blind protocols and so on. Much needs to be done to improve and
standardize such measurements as eye position, eye movement, image
quality, e.r.g., visual evoked potentials and so on. But once made
convincing, such clinical results cannot be ignored in our global
picture. No claim can be made about the universality of a
formulation that does not include such clinical findings. A
sterling example of what I mean is anomalous retinal correspondence.
This syndrome was seen as important early on in the history of
binocular vision, for example in Tschermak's voluminous writings
(though it should be added that he himself had this condition). But
little has been heard of anomalous retinal correspondence since,
even in discussions of the development of cortical connections.
417
418 G. WESTHEIMER
B. JULESZ
427
428 B. JULESZ
Fender and Julesz (1967) did not specify whether the observed
extension of Panum' s fusional area was due to an increase of
fusional interaction or to the workings of the matching process
that eliminates false targets. In the intervening years Tyler and
Julesz (1980) found stereopsis in briefly flashed RDS in excess of
2 deg arc binocular disparities if the target was adequately wide
and the background was aligned. Obviously, in these brief flashes
convergence eye-movements could not be initiated and, therefore,
the binocular disparity of 2 deg arc matched the values found by
Fender and Julesz (1967) after slow pulling. It was still not
ruled out in the Tyler and Julesz (1980) experiments that in the
brief presentations the cyclopean target (center rectangle) did not
reach its final depth, but only appeared to jump towards the front
or the back. Therefore, Schumer and Julesz (1984) modified the
previous study by superposing a corrugated depth grating over the
cyclopean rectangle (that served as a depth pedestal). Observers
had to report the presence or absence of this corrugation, and were
able to do so for one deg arc binocular disparities.
REFERENCES
Hyson, M., Julesz, B. and Fender, D.H. (1963). Eye Movements and
Neural Remapping during Fusion of Misaligned Random-Dot Stereograms.
J. Opt. Soc. Am., 73, No 12.
431
432 INDEX