Graham 2018
Graham 2018
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-018-9822-0
123
S. Graham et al.
There are many competing and potentially demanding cognitive actions that writers
must attend to while composing text (Graham, 2006; Kellogg, 1993). Take for
instance the seemingly simple task of writing a sentence (Graham, in press). An idea
must be generated and evaluated for its suitability, crafted into a grammatically
correct sentence, and transcribed into written text with correct spelling, capitaliza-
tion, and punctuation, resulting in a sentence that precisely reflects the author’s
intentions, will be understood by the intended audience, and is appropriate for the
writing community and the audience for which the sentence is created. If the
cognitive actions needed to produce this sentence exceed the capacity of the writer’s
processing system, due to inefficient skills such as slow handwriting or difficulties
with spelling, then cognitive overload is likely (McCutchen, 1988), resulting in a
disruption in one or more aspects of the sentence construction process. This is not
just limited to sentence writing though, as it applies to the larger act of composing
(we refer to this as writing in this article, whereas the term handwriting fluency and
handwriting legibility refers to how quickly and legibly letters of the alphabet are
produced, respectively).
The complexity and difficulty of writing is reflected in the challenges that many
children experience in learning to write. For instance, only 27% of grade 12 students
in the United States (U.S.) performed at or above the “proficient” writing level
(defined as solid academic performance by the National Center for Educational
Statistics, 2012). Unfortunately, such findings are not limited to the U.S., as a
majority of students in countries across the globe perform poorly on writing
assessments (e.g., Department of Education, 2012; Micahelowa, 2001; Research
Center for Psychological and Educational Testing, 2016).
Writing can be especially challenging for young children, as they have not yet
mastered the knowledge, skills, and processes needed to write effectively (Graham,
2006). Not surprisingly, therefore, they typically apply a relatively simple approach
to writing, as they write by telling what they know about a topic (i.e., writing-as-
remembering; Scardamalia & Bereiter, 1986). Such an approach serves an adaptive
function for beginning writers. Writing requires that children transcribe their ideas
onto paper or into digital text. However, the handwriting and keyboarding skills of
beginning writers are slow and laborious, and they cannot spell most words
correctly. Because these text transcription skills require considerable cognitive
effort for young children (Graham & Harris, 2000; McCutchen, 1988), the
knowledge telling approach described above has the advantage of making it possible
to produce text while simultaneously minimizing the use of other demanding
123
The impact of supplemental handwriting and spelling…
Until the transcription skills (e.g., handwriting and spelling) of beginning writers
become more automatic and fluent, they do more than just consume cognitive
resources; they can interfere with other writing processes (Berninger, 1999; Graham
& Harris, 2000). Slow and laborious handwriting or keyboarding, for instance, may
interfere with text generation, as ideas or plans a child is trying to hold in working
memory may be lost before they can be committed to paper or digital text. Likewise,
having to consciously think about how to spell a word while writing may tax
cognitive processing capacity, leading the child to forget ideas in working memory
not yet committed to paper. Further, uncertainty about how to spell a word may lead
to the selection of a different word the child knows how to spell, potentially
undermining the preciseness of the intended message.
Handwriting, keyboarding, and spelling can negatively impact beginning writers
in two other important ways. First, an analysis of the available literature by Graham
and his colleagues (Graham, Harris, & Hebert, 2011) found that readers form
negative judgments about students’ papers when spelling errors are evident or the
text is difficult to read because of issues with handwriting legibility. They also
found that teachers were more likely to judge text that was typed more harshly than
handwritten text when spelling or grammar errors are present, as they expected such
errors to be less evident in typed versus handwritten text.
Second, the impact of poor transcription skills may extend beyond a specific
writing task for young beginning writers. For example, Berninger, Mizokawa, and
Bragg (1991) reported that children who experienced difficulties mastering
handwriting avoided writing whenever possible and developed a mindset that they
could not write. This is not an insignificant issue, as 44% of elementary students
may experience difficulties with handwriting (Graham & Weintraub, 1996).
123
S. Graham et al.
Current study
The current study was designed to answer the following questions: (a) Does a
supplemental handwriting and spelling instructional program improve the hand-
writing and spelling skills of beginning writers who do not acquire these skills as
rapidly as their peers?, and (b) Does supplemental handwriting and spelling
instruction improve these children’s sentence construction fluency and performance
writing compositions? We applied a randomized control trial to answer these
questions. Students were randomly assigned to either the handwriting/spelling
instruction condition or a phonological awareness instruction control condition.
123
The impact of supplemental handwriting and spelling…
(2014) reported that similar forms of instruction enhanced the spelling performance
of poor spellers.
Methods
Students
Screening
123
S. Graham et al.
Participating students
Students who met the criteria specified above were randomly assigned by class to
treatment (handwriting and spelling instruction) or control (phonological awareness
instruction). The phonological instruction condition controlled for attention and
maturation effects. The mean chronological age of the 30 students completing the
study was 76.5 months (SD = 3.94). Sixteen of them were boys, 22 of them were
African American (six students were white and two Hispanic), 10 students received
special education services (for speech and language problems or ADHD), three
children were left handed, and English was not the first language for two children
(both in the handwriting and spelling condition). The racial composition of these 30
students was similar to racial composition of the school district and the participating
schools.
There were no statistical differences between students in the two conditions (i.e.,
handwriting and spelling vs. phonological awareness) in terms of gender, race,
students with special needs, and performance on the spelling and handwriting
screening measures (all p’s [ 0.24). With two exceptions, there were also no
statistically significant differences between students in the treatment and control
conditions on all other measures administered before instruction started (all
p’s [ 0.24). These assessments are described in greater detail in the Measures
section. One of the exceptions involved our measure for sentence construction
fluency, the Woodcock Johnson Psychoeducational Battery III Writing Fluency
subtest (WJ Writing Fluency; Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001). The other
exception occurred for a paragraph handwriting fluency task administered at pretest
and posttest. With this measure students copied a short paragraph consisting of two
sentences. Students in the control condition had statistically higher (p = 0.002)
sentence construction fluency scores (M = 95.07; SD = 7.69) than treatment
students (M = 86.33; SD = 6.34). Control students also had statistically faster
123
The impact of supplemental handwriting and spelling…
Although students’ teachers did not deliver either the experimental or control
conditions (this was done by graduate assistants), it is important to describe the
writing instruction students received in their regular class, as it better contextualizes
our investigation (this was based on an interview and two observations of each
teachers’ class). All but three of the participating students 14 teachers indicated they
applied a process plus skills approach to teaching writing. They noted their students
wrote stories, letters, journal entries, paragraphs, responses to reading material, and
informational text. They further indicated they taught spelling and handwriting (this
usually involved the use of commercial materials). Sentence construction skills
were reportedly taught in three of these 11 classes. Two of these 11 teachers
indicated students in their classes spent only 20 min a week writing, with the rest of
the time spent teaching writing skills, whereas the other nine teachers indicated they
balanced time spent on writing and teaching writing skills.
The three teachers who did not apply a process plus skills approach to teaching
writing reported their students did very little writing, as they devoted most of their
“writing time” to teaching handwriting, spelling, and/or sentence construction. Two
of these teachers indicated they used a commercial handwriting and spelling
program.
The 14 teachers varied in how much time they devoted to writing and writing
instruction each day, with four teachers devoting an hour a day. The remaining
teachers indicated they spent 20–40 min a day on writing.
Thus, even though students who participated in this study evidenced a slow rate
of progress in acquiring handwriting and spelling skills, all of their teachers taught
handwriting and spelling. Most but not all students wrote stories as part of regular
classroom instruction, and students in about 40 of the classes received sentence
construction instruction.
Measures
Before instruction began and immediately after it ended, students were administered
tests to measure their handwriting fluency, handwriting legibility, spelling accuracy,
sentence construction fluency, and composition length, vocabulary, and quality. All
tests were administered by trained graduate students who were also involved in
providing instruction. However, assessments were administered to each student by a
different graduate student than the one who acted as their instructor. In addition, the
order in which assessments were administered was randomized for each student.
This occurred at both pretest and posttest. All assessments were independently
scored by two trained raters (their scores were averaged).
123
S. Graham et al.
Compositions
Students’ skills in writing compositions were assed with two different writing tasks.
One writing task asked students to write a story in response to a black-and-white
line drawing (e.g., a frowning turtle sitting in a tree with his arms crossed or a girl
running in a field with a rabbit and a butterfly). A second writing task involved
writing a description (e.g., a favorite place or favorite person in your family).
Prior to the start the study, the story and descriptive prompts were judged to be
appropriate for use with first-grade children by four primary grade teachers. For
stories, the assignment of the two line drawings was counterbalanced so that each
student responded to a different picture at pretest and posttest and an equivalent
number of children wrote about each picture at each testing time. This was also done
for the two descriptive writing prompts.
For both story and descriptive writing, students were directed to write in response
to the assigned prompt, they had as long as they needed to complete the paper, and
the examiner could not provide assistance. For story writing, students were also told
not to describe the picture when writing, but to create a story to go with it.
The resulting compositions were scored for composition length, composition
vocabulary, and composition quality. Composition length was computed by
counting the number of words included in a paper, regardless of the how words
were spelled. Composition vocabulary was the proportion of unique words seven
letters or longer (when the word was spelled correctly) included in a students’ paper.
This was determined by counting the number of letters in each word (when spelled
correctly) in a paper. This was calculated by tallying the number of unique words
seven letters or longer, and dividing this sum by number of different words. This
measure developed by Hammill and Larsen (1996) provided an indication of
vocabulary complexity, as longer words are likely to occur less frequently in young
students’ writing and be more difficult to spell (Graham, Harris, & Loynachan,
1993).
Composition quality was assessed using a traditional holistic writing measure.
Raters were asked to read each story (or description) attentively, but not laboriously,
to obtain a general impression of overall quality. Examiners were told that
imagination, organization, aptness of word choice, grammar, and sentence structure
should all be taken into account when forming a judgement about story quality and
that no one factor should receive undue weight. Students’ papers were scored on a
9-point scale, with higher scores representing higher quality of writing. These same
directions were given to raters scoring descriptions, except the word “imagination”
was replaced with “ideation.”
To guide the composition quality scoring process, raters were provided with a
representative paper for a score of 2, 4, 6, and 8. One set of representative papers
was provided for scoring stories and another for descriptions. These representative
papers or anchor points were obtained from students in four first-grade classes not
participating in the study, but in the same school district. Students in these classes
wrote a story and description following the same procedures applied in this
investigation. After reading through all of the stories and descriptions written in
these classes, two former elementary school teachers selected anchor papers for the
123
The impact of supplemental handwriting and spelling…
scoring points identified above for each type of writing separately. Their ordering of
these anchor points were confirmed independently by two first grade teachers.
Before any pre- or post-test story and descriptive paper was scored, it was typed
(removing all identifying information) and corrected for spelling, punctuation, and
capitalization. Students’ handwriting, spelling, and usage errors can bias raters’
perceptions of writing quality (Graham et al., 2011). Scoring occurred after all data
had been collected. Interrater scoring reliability (based on correlations between the
scores assigned by the two reviewers) for composition length, composition
vocabulary, and composition quality was 0.99 (0.99 for both stories and
descriptions), 0.95 (0.96 for stories and 0.95 for descriptions), and 0.84 (0.86 for
stories and 0.81 for descriptions), respectively.
The WJ Writing Fluency subtest (Woodcock et al., 2001) was used to assess
sentence construction fluency. This is a timed measure where students compose
sentences from three written words that go with a picture. A sentence is scored as
correct if all three words are used without change in a grammatically complete
sentence. The test–retest reliability for primary grade students reported in the test
manual is greater than 0.80, and scoring reliability in this study was 0.97.
Handwriting fluency
Two timed tasks were used to assess handwriting fluency. For each task, students
used a pencil without an eraser and primary-lined paper. They were told to cross out
and rewrite if they made mistakes. One of the tasks, alphabet handwriting fluency,
was developed by Abbott and Berninger (1993), and asked students to print the
entire lowercase alphabet as quickly as possible from memory without making any
mistakes. After 15 s, a slash was made after the last letter completed. Students were
then asked to finish the task. The score for this task was the number of letters written
correctly during the 15-s interval. Reversals, substitutions, omissions, additions, and
letters written in the wrong case (i.e., upper-case) were not counted as correctly
written letters. Interrater scoring reliability was 0.96.
The second task, paragraph handwriting fluency, was used previously by Graham
et al. (2000) and involved copying a two sentence paragraph. This paragraph
included the sentence used on the handwriting fluency screening measure (A little
boy lived with his father in a large forest.), but it was followed by a second sentence
(Every day the father went out to cut wood.). This paragraph was displayed at the
top of a page of primary-lined paper, and students copied the paragraph directly
below the display. The directions were identical to the directions for the handwriting
fluency screening measure. The score for this task was the number of letters copied
correctly. Again, reversals, substitutions, additions, and omissions were not counted
as correct letters. Interrater scoring reliability was 0.98.
123
S. Graham et al.
Handwriting legibility
Students’ stories, descriptive compositions, and texts produced via the paragraph
handwriting fluency task were scored for handwriting legibility. They were scored
using the scale from the Test of Handwriting (Larsen & Hammill, 1989). The
reported test–retest reliabilities exceed 0.80 in the test manual. This legibility scale
provides scored samples of students’ handwriting that illustrate more to less legible
handwriting (higher scores represent more legible handwriting). The examiner
matches the sample of text produced by the student to the scored samples on the
scale to derive a legibility score. Interrater scoring reliability was 0.70.
Spelling
Instruction was delivered to students in the treatment and control conditions by six
graduate education majors blind to the purpose and hypotheses for the present study.
All instruction was delivered individually, with each instructor teaching students in
both conditions. Instruction was delivered as a supplement to the regular literacy
program.
Before the start of the study, instructors were taught how to implement both the
treatment and control conditions. For each condition, they were provided with a
notebook containing detailed directions for implementing all activities. The
materials for each condition were printed in different colors. Training took place
over a 2 week period (1 week for treatment and 1 week for control in that order). In
both conditions, training involved the first author providing an overview of the
material and stressing the value of instruction for helping students become better
123
The impact of supplemental handwriting and spelling…
writers and readers. It also involved the first author modeling lessons, and
instructors practicing them with each other until they could implement them
correctly. As they practiced lessons, they had a checklist for the elements of each
lesson, checking them as they were completed.
Instructors taught each of their assigned students three times a week for 20 min a
session. Instruction was delivered in a quite area in the school (e.g., library,
lunchroom).
The program included eight units. In each unit, handwriting instruction focused on
two alphabet letters, whereas spelling instruction centered on two or more spelling
patterns (see below). There were six 20 min lessons in each unit. Each unit was
delivered over a 2 week period, with three lessons each week. Both handwriting and
spelling were taught in the first five lessons in a unit. Lesson six only involved
spelling instruction
Handwriting was taught in the first five lessons in each unit using the following
procedures. Each child was initially asked to write each lower-case letter of the
alphabet. Based on their performance on this assessment, the student was taught
how to form letters he or she produced incorrectly, inefficiently, or both. In each
unit, the student worked on two of these letters (e.g., i, t). During lessons one and
two in a unit, students were provided with a card for each letter that included
numbered arrows visually showing how to form the letter. After watching the
instructor trace and describe how to form each letter on the card, the student then
traced it and described how to form it. The student then discussed with the instructor
how the two letters were similar and different. After the student practiced tracing,
copying, and writing the two letters, the child circled his or her best-formed letters.
During lessons three and four, the student continued to practice tracing, copying,
and writing the two letters, but the child now practiced copying words that
contained each target letter (e.g., adding for the letter d). In lesson five, practice was
changed from copying words to copying “hinky-pinkys” that contained the target
letter (e.g., fuddy-duddy for the letter d). In lessons three to five, students circled
their best-formed word or hinky-pinky.
Each unit also included instruction on two or more spelling patterns involving
short vowel sounds, long vowel sounds, or both (the only exception was unit 1,
which just focused on short /a/). The sequence of skills taught was as follows: short /
a/ (unit 1); short /o/ and /e/ (unit 2); short /i/ and /u/ (unit 3); short and long /a/ (unit
4); short and long /i/ (unit 5); short and long /o/ (unit 6); short and long /e/ (unit 7),
and long /a/ in words like made, may, and maid (unit 8).
The first five lessons in each unit followed a set pattern that included four
different activities: phonics warm-up, word building, word study, and writing. The
sixth lesson in each unit involved a word sorting activity.
Phonics warm-up was designed to improve students’ skills in identifying letters
that correspond to sounds for consonants, blends, digraphs, and short vowels. Each
of the 46 target sound and letter combination(s) was depicted on a card with a
picture on one side of the card (e.g., a picture of a hat representing the sound /h/ at
123
S. Graham et al.
the start of the word) and a corresponding letter (e.g., the letter h) on the other side
of the card. Students practiced identifying which letters made the sound represented
on the card (the target sound could be represented at the beginning, middle, or end
of the word), receiving feedback and additional practice when their response was
incorrect.
The word building activity was designed to improve spelling knowledge about
onset and rimes in words (a rime is the string of letters following the onset). In
lesson one, a rime (e.g., ate) that corresponded to one of the spelling patterns
emphasized in that unit (i.e., long /a/) was introduced on a card. After hearing the
sound made by the rime and observing how to combine the rime card with onset
cards containing a consonant (e.g., letter l) or a blend (e.g., Letters pl) to form
words, the student practiced forming words by combining the rime card with various
onset cards. In lesson two, the student repeated this process with the same rime, but
now built words on a worksheet instead of with cards. Lessons three and four were
identical to the first two lessons, except a new rime was introduced for the other
spelling pattern emphasized in that unit. During lesson 5, the student reviewed the
rimes introduced in the first four lessons, by using these rimes to build words on a
worksheet.
The purpose of the word study activity was to ensure students could correctly
spell words they were likely to use when writing. The words a student practiced
were either misspelled on the pretest measure, spelling common words, or
misspelled in the writing activity they completed as part of instruction (described
below). At the start of the study, each student was given five words to study. Each
word was written on a card and they were attached together with a ring. During
lessons one to five in each unit, the child studied the words attached to the ring by
saying it, studying its letters, writing it from memory, and checking whether it was
spelled correctly (and correcting it, if it was not). After a student spelled a word
correctly six times in a row over two lessons, the word was removed from the ring
and another previously misspelled word was added to the ring. Periodically,
students spelling of the words removed from the ring were retested, and if a word
was missed the student restudied it as needed.
With the writing activity, students completed writing tasks that encouraged them
to use words that fit the spelling patterns they were studying as they wrote a short
composition. For example, in unit 1 where the short vowel sound for /a/ was
emphasized, students were asked to write a paper about “A fat cat who is trying to
catch Mat the rat.” After writing the story, the child with help from the instructor
placed a star next to any word in the paper that fit the targeted spelling patterns. The
primary instructional purposes of the writing activity were to promote handwriting
fluency and provide an opportunity to apply spelling skills learned in context.
The word-sorting activity (based on Graham et al., 1996) was designed to help
students to learn the rule for each spelling pattern emphasized in the unit. For
instance, in unit 3 (short /i/ and /u/), the student participated in a word sort where
were placed into two categories: CVC-type words containing the short vowel sounds
of /i/ (e.g., sit) and /u/ (e.g., fun).
The word sorting activity began with the student observing the instructor placing
master word cards for each spelling pattern next to each other (e.g., sit and fun).
123
The impact of supplemental handwriting and spelling…
Each master word was then pronounced twice, with emphasis placed on the target
spelling feature in the word (e.g., short vowel sound for /i/ in sit and /u/ in fun).
Next, the student considered how the master words were similar and different. The
student’s attention was focused on critical features in both words, such as how the
letter “i” is pronounced in each master word and the combination of consonants and
vowels in each master word. The word sorting activity continued with the student
working with the instructor to decide under which master word they should place an
additional 12 words, with the idea of figuring out the rule for why the letter “i”
makes the short /i/ sound in sit, and the letter “u” makes the short /u/ sound in fun.
This continued until all words were sorted. The child (with help from the instructor
if needed) generate a rule for each spelling pattern emphasized in that word sort.
The student then generated words of their own, writing them on blank word cards
and placing them into the appropriate category.
The phonological awareness control condition also involved eight units. There were
also six 20 min lessons in each unit. All phonological awareness activities were
taken from the Ladders to Literacy Program (O’Connor, Notari-Syverson, &
Vadasy, 1998) or based on Rosner’s (1979) Auditory Analysis Program. The only
exception was the Reading activity which involved common folktales. Each lesson
in a unit involved five activities: Guess What Word, Sound Play, Rhyming Triplets,
Sound Songs, and Reading. Starting in lesson 34, Magic Squares took the place of
Rhyming Triplets.
With Guess What Word, the student listened as a target word was spoken aloud
in the following three ways: (1) stretching the syllable (e.g., hat as haaat), (2) saying
the onset and the rime separately (e.g., /h /at/), and (3) saying each phoneme
separately (/h a t/). The child then identified the word (from a list of four words).
Next, the student was directed to say the word, phoneme-by-phoneme.
During Sound Play, the student played four sound games. Sound games in the
first two units involved syllables. In Find the Hidden, the student was asked if a
specific syllable was hidden in a word (e.g., “Is the word ant hidden in the word
anthill?”). With Say the Missing, the child figured out which syllable a word was
missing (e.g., “Say Carelessly; now say lessly: what is missing?”). For Say the Word
Without, the child repeated a word without one of its syllables (e.g., “Say
friendliness; now say it without ness.”). With Substitute, one syllable was
substituted for another in a word (e.g., “Say party; now don’t say it with y say it
with ner.”).
Sound Play in the remaining six units applied the same four tasks, but focused on
phonemes, including finding where specific phonemes were hidden (e.g., “Does the
word big begin with a b sound?”), figuring out which sound was missing from a
word (e.g., “Say tin; now say in; what is missing?”), repeating a word without one
of its sounds (e.g., “Say tin; now say it without /t/.”), and substituting one phoneme
for another in a word (e.g., “Say tin; now don’t say it with /t/ say it with /k/.”).
With Rhyming Triplets, a pair of rhyming words were spoken aloud (e.g., cake-
snake) and the child was asked to repeat them and identify other words that rhymed
123
S. Graham et al.
with them. Starting in lesson unit six, Rhyming Triplets was replaced by Magic
Squares. With this new activity, a word was said aloud (e.g., dog), and then said
phoneme by phoneme (e.g., /d /o/ /g/). The student then said the word phoneme by
phoneme with the instructor and identified how many sounds were in the word. The
student and instructor then said the sounds in the words again, with the student
sequentially touching a square in an Elkonian box as each sound was produced.
With Sound Songs, students substituted the letter sound of the day (from the
second activity) with one of the sounds in well-known songs (i.e., Old MacDonald,
Jimmy Cracked Corn, and the Birthday Song).
With the final activity, Reading, the student listened as a folktale was read aloud.
The reading activity was interactive as the student was asked questions about the
cover of the book (e.g., What do you see on the cover? What do you think the book
will be about? Why do you think so?) and material presented within it. Once the
folktale had been read, the student described what they liked best about the book
and if they would recommend it to a child their age.
It should be noted that none of the activities in the phonological awareness
condition involved presenting written letters or words. This would have moved the
control condition into the realm of spelling instruction. The spelling activities,
however, were likely to promote phonological awareness, as they involved
matching letters to the sounds in words, likely making students more aware that
words are composed of separate sounds. Spelling instruction did enhance
phonological awareness in Graham and Santangelo (2014).
To ensure both conditions were delivered as planned, the following safeguards were
implemented. One, instructors received intensive practice applying each condition
(as noted). Two, instructors met with the first author weekly to discuss any glitches
that occurred during implementation. Reported glitches were rare, and when they
did occur usually involved an inadvertent mistake (e.g., skipping a step of
instruction). Three, instructors checked each step of a lesson on a recording sheet as
it was completed. For handwriting and spelling instruction 99.1% of steps were
checked (ranging from 98.0 to 99.7% across instructors), whereas 99.8% of steps
were checked for phonological awareness instruction (ranging from 99 to 100%
across instructors). Four, in both the treatment and control condition 8 lessons (17%
of lessons) were tape recorded for each child. A trained graduate student listed to all
taped sessions (240 sessions) and checked which steps of instruction had been
completed (using the same checklist instructors applied during instruction). A
second graduate student independently scored 60 sessions (25%). Interrater
reliability was 97%. The percentage of steps completed on the tape recorded
lessons for handwriting and spelling was 97.4% (93.2–100%) across instructors. For
phonological awareness, percentage of steps completed was 98.1% (93.8–100%
across instructors).
123
The impact of supplemental handwriting and spelling…
Results
Handwriting
123
S. Graham et al.
Table 1 Means and standard deviations for measures by condition and time
Measures Pretest Posttest Gain
The spelling achievement and sentence construction fluency measures involved norm-referenced tests and
standard scores are reported; spelling in context gain score represents improvement in reduced proportion
of spelling errors
Spelling
Treatment students made greater gains than control students on two of the three
spelling measures. Students who received supplemental handwriting and spelling
instruction made statistically greater pretest to posttest gains (M = 13.60; SD = 4.39)
than control students in the phonological awareness control condition (M = 8.93;
SD = 7.05) on spelling achievement measures (i.e., the TWS), F(1,28) = 5.76,
p = 0.038, d = 0.75. Further, treatment students learned the correct spelling of more
words (M = 23.40; SD = 10.83) from pretest to posttest than control students
123
The impact of supplemental handwriting and spelling…
Discussion
123
S. Graham et al.
Students participating in this study all scored at or below the bottom 25th percentile
on screening measures of spelling and handwriting fluency competence. Each
student in the experimental condition received one-on-one instruction from a trained
tutor using instructional procedures for teaching handwriting and spelling that were
successful in previous studies with beginning writers (e.g., Graham, 1999; Graham
et al., 2000, 2002). This supplemental instruction was in addition to the handwriting
and spelling instruction provided by their regular classroom teachers, which
generally involved the use of commercial programs to teach these skills.
The combined supplemental handwriting and spelling instruction these children
received had a positive impact on their handwriting and spelling. In comparison to
the control condition where students received one-on-one phonological awareness
instruction, students in the experimental treatment condition made greater gains on
two handwriting fluency measures (i.e., measures of alphabet and paragraph
handwriting fluency), a measure of handwriting legibility (although these gains may
have mainly been a consequence in the decline of control students’ scores), and two
measures of spelling accuracy (i.e., spelling achievement as measured by a norm-
referenced measures and spelling common words). The observed gains were
relatively large, with effect sizes of 0.81, 0.95, and 0.72 for paragraph handwriting
fluency, alphabet handwriting fluency, and handwriting legibility, respectively.
Similar effects were obtained for the two statistically significant spelling accuracy
measures, with and effect size of 0.75 and 0.82 for the spelling achievement and
spelling common words measures, respectively.
These spelling and handwriting gains compared favorably to gains made by
students who were taught handwriting separately or spelling separately in recent
meta-analyses. Santangelo and Graham (2016) found handwriting instruction
produced an average ES of 0.63 for handwriting fluency and 0.59 for handwriting
legibility. Graham and Santangelo (2014) reported spelling instruction produced an
ES of 0.54 for spelling. While caution must be exercised in comparing the results of
a single study such as ours to findings from a meta-analysis (e.g., studies may differ
in terms of types of students or how handwriting and spelling are taught), the
findings from the two published meta-analyses provide a general benchmark for
thinking about the effectiveness of our study.
It is important to note that statistically greater gains were not obtained for the
handwriting and spelling treatment on one of the spelling measures: spelling in
context. Treatment students did not make greater reductions in proportion of
spelling errors made when writing stories and descriptions when compared to
phonological awareness controls. One possible reason for this is that the average
length of students’ compositions were generally short at both pretest and posttest
123
The impact of supplemental handwriting and spelling…
(28 and 55 words, respectively), and the number of different words was even less
(fewer than 10 and 20 different words, respectively). As a result, the opportunity to
showcase newly acquired spelling skills was limited. We obtained some support for
this explanation by conducting a post hoc analysis (reported here), where we found
that treatment students had lower spelling error scores than controls (p = 0.045),
once pretest spelling errors and number of unique words at posttest were statistically
controlled.
A second possible explanation for why treatment students did not make greater
statistical gains in reducing spelling errors is that phonological awareness
instruction improved spelling skills (Graham et al., 2017). This was borne out in
this investigation, as control students evidenced improved scores on all spelling
measures from pretest to posttest. Consequently, they may have made enough
spelling improvement to blunt any possible statistical difference with treatment
students with regards to spelling in context.
Until handwriting and spelling are mastered, they can theoretically lead to cognitive
overload and interference when writing (Berninger, 1999; Graham, in press). For
young writers who are just beginning to learn these skills, this can result in the
development of a restricted approach to writing that minimizes the use of other
attention demanding writing processes (McCutchen, 1988). It can also lead to
students restricting the words they use in writing to ones they already know how to
spell correctly. Problems with cognitive overload, interference, and restriction may
be especially problematic for beginning writers who do not master handwriting and
spelling as quickly as their peers, leading to difficulties with sentence construction
as well as shorter compositions, with less diverse vocabulary, and poorer overall
writing quality.
The results of the current study provided partial support for these theoretical
propositions. When compared to peers who received phonological awareness
instruction, treatment students made greater gains in sentence construction fluency
(as measured by the WJ—Writing Fluency subtest) and composition vocabulary (i.e.,
proportion of words seven letters or longer in their compositions). The observed
gains were relatively large, with effect sizes of 0.77 and 0.91 for sentence
construction fluency and composition vocabulary, respectively. The transfer gain for
sentence construction fluency compares favorably with gains reported in prior
studies; Santangelo and Graham (2016) reported in their meta-analysis that
handwriting instruction produced an ES of 0.48 for writing fluency. No previous
studies have examined the impact of teaching handwriting or spelling on
composition vocabulary, so comparable effects from the literature for this variable
were not available.
We did not find statistically significant transfer effects from handwriting and
spelling instruction to composition length or composition quality, contrary to our
predictions. In contrast, Graham and Santangelo (2014) reported that handwriting
instruction enhanced composition length (ES = 1.33) and composition quality
123
S. Graham et al.
(ES = 0.84). So why did handwriting and spelling instruction not enhance these two
aspects of writing in this study? One possible reason is that students in this study
differed from students in prior studies where positive results were obtained. This
seems unlikely, as prior studies by Berninger et al. (1997) and Graham et al. (2000)
worked with similar students and obtained stronger results. A more likely
explanation is that transfer effects to composition length and composition quality
are due mostly to handwriting and not spelling instruction. Spelling instruction did
not improve writing performance in prior studies in the meta-analysis conducted by
Graham and Santangelo (2014). Thus, the inclusion of spelling instruction in this
study may have weakened transfer effects to these two measures of writing
performance, as it reduced the amount of handwriting instruction provided.
123
The impact of supplemental handwriting and spelling…
includes a relatively small sample of students, limiting the power of the study to
detect possible statistical differences between conditions, especially for transfer
measures to writing. The small sample size may have also limited the power of
randomization, as we found that performance on the paragraph handwriting fluency
measure and the sentence construction fluency measure favored students in the
control condition at the start of the study. It is not clear how these differences
affected the outcomes obtained. Further, the study did not provide equal time to
handwriting and spelling instruction. This should be corrected in future studies.
Lastly, it is not clear if the effects of handwriting and spelling instruction linearly
impacted the outcome measures. This determination is dependent on future research
in this area.
References
Abbott, R., & Berninger, V. (1993). Structural equation modeling of relationships among developmental
skills and writing skills in primary and intermediate-grade writers. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 85, 478–508.
Alves, R., Limpo, T., Fidalgo, R., Carvalhais, L., Pererira, L., & Carlio, S. (2016). The impact of
promoting transcription on early text production: Effects on bursts and pauses, levels of written
language, and writing performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108, 665–679.
Berninger, V. (1999). Coordinating transcription and text generation in working memory during
composing: Automatic and constructive processes. Learning Disability Quarterly, 22, 99–112.
Berninger, V., Mizokawa, D., & Bragg, R. (1991). Scientific practitioner: Theory-based diagnosis and
remediation of writing disabilities. Journal of School Psychology, 29, 57–79.
Berninger, V. W., Vaughan, K. B., Abbott, R. D., Abbott, S. P., Rogan, L. W., Brooks, A., et al. (1997).
Treatment of handwriting problems in beginning writers: Transfer from handwriting to composition.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 652–666.
Berninger, V. W., Vaughan, K., Abbott, R. D., Brooks, A., Abbott, S., Rogan, L., et al. (1998). Early
intervention for spelling problems: Teaching functional spelling units of varying sizes with a
multiple-connection framework. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 587–605.
Berninger, V., Vaughan, K., Abbott, R., Begay, K., Byrd, K., Curtain, G., Minnich, J., & Graham, S.
(2002). Teaching spelling and composition alone and together: Implications for the simple view of
writing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 291–304.
Case-Smith, J., Holland, T., & White, S. (2014). Effectiveness of a co-taught handwriting-writing
program for first grade students. Physical & Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, 34, 30–43.
Department for Education. (2012). The research evidence on writing. London, England: Department for
Education. Retrieved From https://www.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_.
Graham, S. (1999). Handwriting and spelling instruction for students with learning disabilities: A review.
Learning Disability Quarterly, 22, 78–98. https://doi.org/10.2307/1511268.
Graham, S. (2006). Writing. In P. Alexander & P. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology
(pp. 457–478). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Graham, S. (in press). A writer(s) within community model of writing. In C. Bazerman, V. Berninger, D.
Brandt, S. Graham, J. Langer, S. Murphy, P. Matsuda, D. Rowe, & M. Schleppegrell (Eds.), The
lifespan development of writing. Urbana, IL: National Council of English.
Graham, S., & Harris, K. (2000). The role of self-regulation and transcription skills in writing and writing
development. Educational Psychologist, 35, 3–12.
Graham, S., Harris, K. R., & Fink, B. (2000). Is handwriting causally related to learning to write?
Treatment of handwriting problems in beginning writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92,
620–633.
Graham, S., Harris, K. R., & Fink-Chorzempa, B. (2002). Contributions of spelling instruction to the
spelling, writing, and reading of poor spellers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 669–686.
123
S. Graham et al.
Graham, S., Harris, K. R., & Hebert, M. (2011). It is more than just the message: Presentation effects in
scoring writing. Focus on Exceptional Children, 44(4), 1–12.
Graham, S., Harris, K. R., & Loynachan, C. (1993). The basic spelling vocabulary. Journal of
Educational Research, 86, 363–368.
Graham, S., Harris, K. R., & Loynachan, C. (1996). The directed spelling thinking activity: Application
with high frequency words. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 11, 34–40.
Graham, S., Hebert, M., Sandbank, M., & Harris, K. R. (2016). Credibly assessing the writing
achievement of young struggling writers: Application of generalizability theory. Learning Disability
Quarterly, 39, 72–82.
Graham, S., Liu, K., Bartlett, B., Ng, C., Harris, K. R., Aitken, A., et al. (2017). Reading for writing: A
meta-analysis of the impact of reading and reading instruction on writing. Review of Educational
Research. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654317746927.
Graham, S., & Santangelo, T. (2014). Does spelling instruction make students better spellers, readers, and
writers? A meta-analytic review. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 27, 1703–1743.
Graham, S., & Weintraub, N. (1996). A review of handwriting research: Progress and prospect from l980
to l994. Educational Psychology Review, 8, 7–87.
Hammill, D., & Larsen, S. (1996). Test of written language—3. Austin, TX: Pro-ED.
Kellogg, R. (1993). The psychology of writing. New York: Oxford University Press.
Larsen, S., & Hammill, D. (1989). Test of legible handwriting. Austin, TX: Pro-ED.
Larsen, S., & Hammill, D. (1999). Test of written spelling—4. Austin, TX: Pro-ED.
McCutchen, D. (1988). “Functional automaticity” in children’s writing: A problem of metacognitive
control. Written Communication, 5, 306–324.
Micahelowa, K. (2001). Primary education quality in Francophone Sub-Saharan Africa: Determinants of
learning achievement and efficiency considerations. World Development, 29, 1699–1716.
National Center for Education Statistics. (2012). The Nation’s Report Card: Writing 2011 (NCES 2012–
470). Washington, D.C.: Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.
O’Connor, R., Notari-Syverson, A., & Vadasy, P. (1998). Ladders to literacy: A kindergarten activity
book. Baltimore: Brookes.
Research Center for Psychological and Educational Testing (RCPET). (2016). 105年國中教育會考各科
計分與閱卷結果說明 [2016 comprehensive assessment program for junior high school students:
Explanation of grading results]. Retrieved from http://www.cap.ntnu.edu.tw/exam/105/
Pressrelease1050603.pdf.
Rieben, L., Ntamakiliro, L., Gonthier, B., & Fayol, M. (2005). Effects of various early writing practices
on reading and spelling. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 145–166.
Rosner, J. (1979). Helping children overcome learning disabilities (2nd ed.). New York: Walker.
Santangelo, T., & Graham, S. (2016). A comprehensive meta-analysis of handwriting instruction.
Educational Psychology Review, 28, 225–265.
Scardamalia, M., & Bereiter, C. (1986). Written composition. In M. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook of research
on teaching (3rd ed., pp. 778–803). New York: MacMillan.
Uhry, J. K., & Sheperd, M. J. (1993). Segmentation/spelling instruction as part of a first-grade reading
program: Effects on several measures of reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 28, 219–233.
Wechsler, D. (2009). Wechsler individual achievement test—III. San Antonio, TX: Psychological
Corporation.
Weintraub, N., Yinon, M., Hirsch, I., & Parush, S. (2009). Effectiveness of sensorimotor and task-
oriented handwriting intervention in elementary school-aged students with handwriting difficulties.
OTJR: Occupation Participation, and Health, 29, 125–134.
Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III tests of achievement.
Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
123