Soe Full Birleştirilmiş Ver.
Soe Full Birleştirilmiş Ver.
Soe Full Birleştirilmiş Ver.
COMPLEMENTATION
INTRODUCTION
You have seen in Chapters 28 and 29 how relative constructions can place one clause
within the structure of another larger clause of which it forıns a part. For example, the
vast majority of relative clauses function as modifiers of NPs within a sentence . However,
a'! will become clear in this and the next two chapters, relative constructions are not thc
only possible type of clause-within-clause in English.
This chapter is concemed with English clausal complenıentation. Complements, which
are constituents needed to complete the meaning ofa verb or an adjective, arc often
distinguished from adjtmcts, which are perceived not to be central to the propositional
meaning of the sentencc and which are never required to occur with a verb or adjective:
John believed that }il/ was coming.
(complement)
Thc verb believe must take somc sort of object. This object may be an NP (/olın helier.ıed the
report), or it may be an embedded complement clause as in the fırst example above. By
contrast, the adverbial almost in the second example is not a structurally obligatory
constituent; it is an optional adjunct. Ordinary relative clauses, too, are a type of adjunct:
for example, there is no noun in English that is rcquired in every instance to be followed
by a rclative clause. Relative clauscs arc typically just rıwdijiers of nouns.
1n this chapter we discuss clausal complements that include, at least, a verbal element
with an assumed subject, and often an expresscd subjcct as well. These complements fall
naturally into fıve types:
1. full clausal that-<:omplements with tensed verbs
2. tenseles..'! subjunctive complements
3. infinitivcs
4. gcrunds
5. noun-participle constructions
These fıve types of clausal complements tend to be strictly associated with the occurrence
of certain verbs. Because there are many types of clausal complements and because the
relation of verb to complcmcnt type often seems arbitrary, the grammar of clausal
bn ,<:::::,, THE GRAMMAR BooK
ORDINARY THAT-C0MPLEMENTS
Tensed t/uu-dauses arc one of thc most frcqucnt types of dausal complemcnt:
Scientists daim [that the globe is getting warmer].
We long expected [that nothing worthwhile would come from our effort].
People generally know [that bears don't make good pets].
in cadı case. we havca complete scnt.encc. Noticc that the matcrial inside thc hrnckets
looks cxactly like an ordinary clause cxccpt tlrnt it is prefaccd by tlıat. in cadı bracketcd
clause thcrc is a su ject and a vcrh, and thcrc may be various typcs of additional infonnation
such as one would find in an ordinary scntcncc. The first auxiliary verb may appcar in thc
prescnt or past tense, or it may be a moda! auxiliary. What is dificrent is that these brackcted
clauses camıot standby themsclvt$ as acceptablc scqucnces as long as thcy begin with tluıt.
*That the globe is getting warmer.
*That nothing worthwhile would come from our effort.
*That bears don't make good pets.
A.,, wilh thc adverbial clauses discussed in Chapter 25, such dauses arc callecl depcndcnt"
clauses bccause they dcpend on, or rcquirc tht- presence of, another clause to which they
are attached in some way. Moreovcr, in cadı ful! sentcncc abovc, thc dcpcnclcnl t/uıt
clause seems to stand in a position in which an NP might ordinarily stand. Thc vcrhs daim,
ex/Jert, and know may under other circumstances take simple NPs as objccts:
Scientists often daim [great discoveries].
We expected [nothing else].
Everyone knows [the answer].
This suggcsts that we should rcgard thcse dcpcndcnt clauscs as noun-like in nature and
say that thcy arc eınb(ldd(ld into largcr, inclcpendcnl clauscs, forming an integral parl of
them-in thcse cascs, as dirc ct objects. Additional evidcnce to support thc bdicf that thc
embcdded clauscs are noun-like is thc fact that thcy bchave likc the corresponding NPs:
in all thc cases above, the bracketed matcrial can appcar in a passive scntcncc as thc sulajcct-
that is, in anot.her position whcre NPs occur:
[Great discoveries] are often daimed by scientists.
[That the earth is getting warmer] is daimed by scientists.
[Nothing else] was expected by us.
[That nothing worthwhile would come from our effort] was long expected by us.
[The answer] is known by everyone.
[That bears don't make good pets] is generally known (by people)_I
A third piccc of evidence for thc • ·p stat.us of thcsc clauscs is that cadı t/ınt-dausc above
answers the t>,>ical noun-likc question w/wt?just as docs cadı n,se ofa siınplc NP ol ject:
Question Answer
What do scientists often daim? --+ Great discoveries.
What do scientists often daim? --+ That the earth is getting warmer.
Chapter 31: Complementation ,-.::::,,, fi31
üne way to incorporate this new possibility into our grammar is to rewrite the phrase-
structure rule for the expansion of NPs:
(det)(AP)N (-pl)(PrepP)}
NP pro
{
s
We will now say that it is possible foran S tofunction as an NP. 2
What sort of word is that,and where do we place it in the tree? it should be clear that
this use of that is not the same as the word that in relative clauses (see Chapters 28and 29):
it does not replace a full NP in the same way as relative that.
This is the shop that I told you about. (that = shop)
Scientists daim that the globe is getting warmer. (that = ?)
Nor is that in the above complement clauses identical to the demonstrative determiner that
(see Chapter 16); not onlydoes it not replace an NP, but it also cannot receive emphatic or
contrastive stress in the same way that a demonstrative can:
That's the type of bird I was telling you about.
We should pay attention to this sign, not that one.
?Scientists daim that the globe is getting warmer.
Indeed, it is hard to see what sort of contrast could possibly be intendcd in the last
example. Thus, in the case of complement clauses, it seems we have a third type of that,
one we shall call a "complementizer," a signal ofa complement clause. This complemen-
tizer is neither of the noun/pronoun category nor of any other grammatical category we
have introduced so far in the book. We will abbrcviate it in thc trce as "comp"; we will
placc it under S, in a position adjacent to (and to the lcft of) S
s
SUBJ PRED
1
(\
NP AUX VP
1
T V NP
1 1 1
scientist -pi -pres daim s
comp S'
1
that SUBJ PRED
1P
N -X--------------------VP
AU
/'---._ /'---._
det N T prog cop AP
1
the
1
globc
1
-pres
r---ı._
be get -ing
1
AOJ
1
warmer
h32 THE GRAMMAR BOOK
We also have to cxpand thc options for rewıiting the rule for S to include an optional
complementizer as follows:
S (comp) (sm") S'
Wc must leavc the complcmentizcr optional both becausc simple, single-clausc scntences
do not requirc them and because it is possible for complement clauses like those above to
appear without that.
Scientists often daim the globe is getting warmer.
People generally know bears don't make good pets.
Later in this chapter we will discuss some rcasons why that might be included Of omitted
in such clauses.
Thefe are many verbs that permit or require teıısed that-complements as objects.
Besides believe, daim, expect, and know, among the ınore common are assume, discover,
explain, find, find out, imagine, learn, perceive, point out, promise, prove, see, show, think, and
understand. For cxample,
The lecturer explained that the source of the error was human.
The dealer promised that my car would be fıxed for free.
New evidence shows that broccoli sprouts are more healthful than broccoli.
5UBJUNCTIVE COMPLEMENTS
An additional type of thaklause is supefficially similar to the type above but different in
that the form of the verb in the embedded clause does not vary, regardless of whethef the
subject is fırst, second , or third person Of singulaf/plural, and regardless of time
referencc. Snch clauses are called suhjunctive complements, as in the cascs below:
They insist [that ali the students sign up for counselors].
They insist [that this student sign up fora counselo r].
The customer is demanding [that the store return his money].
The customer demanded [that the store return his money].
The third person singular present tense -sis absent , and past-tcnse forms afe ungrammat-
ical, as the following examples show:
*They insist that this student signs up for a counselor.
*The customer demanded that the store retumed his money.
The fact that the embedded verh is ncver inflected or altered in any other way suggests
that in forming a subjunc tive, one uses only the base form of the verb; the resulting
clauses are then without any soft of tense at all. Furthef evidence fof this daim is that
whefe the vefb in the embeddcd clause is the copula be or thc auxiliary be (in a passive,
progressive, or phrasal moda!) only the hase form is employed aftef insist and demarul:
We insist that he be the one to make the cali.
The customer demanded that his money be returned.
Even strongef evidence comes from negative sentenccs. in subjunctive clauses, a negation
element is always placed directly before the main vefh rather than aftef an auxiliary vefb;
thus, no addition of the do operator is possible:
Chapter 31: Complementatlon ti33
comp S'
1
that SUBJ PRED
1
NP AUX VP
/"'-. 1
det N -imper V NP
1 1 1 /"'-.
the store return det N
1 1
his money
According to the PS rules, modal auxiliaries will not appear in subjunctive clauses; in
general, this holds true in nativ peakef use, although many people do accept shoul.d Of
had to rathef than imperative mood after insist, pefhaps in an attempt to achieve a
softening effect:
We insisted that he (should/had to) take the test again.
The customer demanded that the store (lmust/lhad to) retum his money.
Besides occurring after insist and demand, the subjunctive appears in embedded-clause
vefhs aftef such main-clause verbs of urging and advice such as ask (meaning "request"),
prefer, propose, recommend, request, requ.ire, and suggest:
We ask/request that you please keep a little quieter.
The members prefer that the constitution not be changed this year.
The city proposed that the land be retumed to the fanners.
We recommend that you order the fish.
fi34 THE GitAMMAR 8ooK
INFINITIVE COMPLEMENTS
We h.ave now expanded our notion of "clause" somewhat, to include subject-verb
sequences without tenses. Infinitives (so called because they are not "finite," or tensed)
are an additional, and far more common, type of tenseless clause than subjunctives.
Unlike the other complement clauses we have discussed, they h.ave fi.ve sub-types, each of
which we discuss in turn.
Infinitive Complements
1. believe type: The students believe their professor to be amazing.
2. advise type: We advised them to playbaseball outside.
3. attempt type: I attempted to avoid the ice on the road.
4. want type: I want to clean the house.
(Jor/to infinitive) I want (for) him to clean the house.
S. l,et type: The teacher let the students go home early.
(hare stem)
Belleve-type lnflnitives
The first type of infinitive follows main-clause verbs like believe and imagine. The bracketed
parts of the sentences below seem to show no evidence of tense:
a. The students believed [their professor to be amazing].
b. The citizens lmagined [the politiclan to have led an honest life].
If the above, past-tense main verbs are changed to indicate future time or present-habitual
time,for example, nothing inside the brackets wi11change:
c. The students will belleve [their professor to be amazing].
d. The students always beUeve [th e ir professor to be amazing].
e. The citizens wlll imagine [the politician to have led an honest life].
f. The citlzens currently imagine [the politician to have led an honest life].
In what sense, then, can infinitives be considered clauses? Like other clauses that we h.ave
considered, they seem to possess both subjects and verbs (and in the (b) example above,
a direct object as well). While there may be no marking of tense under the AUX node,
thre e does seem to be a marker-the AUX to-which stands roughly in the position in
which auxiliary elements usually occur. If we place the to under AUX, we will need an
additional expansion option for the AUX rule:
This new expansion suggests that we cannot have infinitive to co-occurring with a moda!
verb but that we can have perfect, progressive, and perfect progressive infinitives. In fa.et,
tb.ese daims are validated by data such as tb.e follo wing. (We cover passive infinitives in
Chapter 32):
*The students believe [their professor to can do almost anything].
The students belleve [their professor to be able to do good research].
The students believe [their professor to be correcting their homewo rk].
The students believe [their professor to have been lecturlng too much lately].
Chapter 31: Complementation ,<=::,> fi35
The next question to answer is what position these infinitives occupy in the structure
of the whole sentence. Are they direct object NPs, as we argued for thaklauses? The
answer seems doubtful if we use the notional test that says that a direct object answers the
question what?about a main verb. The answers below seem odd:
Question Answer
What did the students believel lrrheir professor to be amazing.
What did the citizens lmaglnel ?lThe politician to have led an honest life.
Moreover, if these infinitival clauses are NPs, we might ex.pect them to appear in other NP
positions, such as subject position. Yet they cannot appear as subjects of passive verbs in
the way that "that" complement clauses can:
*[Their professor to be amazing] was believed by ali the students.
*[The polidcian to have led an honest life] was imagined by the citizens.
These facts then suggest that we add an S option to the phrase-structure ex.pansion for VP
to include infinitives (the optional NP before the S will be explained shortly):
VP cop {[J
V { (NP2) (PrepP)}
(NP) S
VPs will now be able to include the option [V+S]. This type ofinfinitive hasa peculiar
characteristic: although we have argued that the infinitive has an overt subject, this
"subject" has some of the characteristics ofa direct object witbin the main clause VP. If we
mak.e the subject of the infinitive a pronoun, it will be of the object type, not the subject
type, perhaps because of the preceding main verb:
The students believe [her to be amazing].
*The students believe [she to be amazing].
The cidzens imagine [him to have led an honest life].
*The citizens imagine [he to have led an honest life].
For example, the sentence "'The students believe slıe to be amazingis ungrammatical. At the
same time, we cannot really view the pronouns above (or the NPs for which they substi-
tute) as true direct objects of the verbs helieve and imagine, in spite of the fact that the
pronouns occur in the objective case. We cannot ask the question,
Who(m) do the students believel
and get as our answer, *Her, to be amazing. Nor can we ask the question,
Who do the cidzens lmaginel
and receive the answer, *Him, to have led an honest life. In fact, it hardly matters whether the
infinitive in the second example appears in the active or the passive voice, for the
meaning comes out approximately the same in either case (though the sentence becomes
stylistically awkward):
The cidzens lmagine [honest lives to have been led by their politiclans].
The fact that verbs like believe tak.e complements that may have object-pronoun subjects
has made the analysis of this type of infinitive diffi.cult for linguists, but we will assume the
amended PS rule as it stands.s The related tree structure is asfollows:
636 THE GRAMMAR BooK
di t
1N P AUX VP
Some other mental cognition verbs of this type include assume,.find, know, perceive, prove,
show, think , and understand:
The players assumed their opponents to have practiced more.
They found the box to contain more money than they had expect ed.
The geologlsts knew the hills to contain gold deposits.
We perceived them to be uninterested in our proposal.
Advlse-type lnfinltives
There is a second similar, though not exactly structurally identical, class of verbs followed
by infinitives. Consider the following sentences:
We advised the children to play baseball o utside .
The police officer ordered myfriend to move his car.
Let us assume as before that the infinitives here have no tense and that the tois to be
analyzed as an element under AUX. in these cases, we get bettcr results when we ask who-
type questions:
Q uestion: Who(m} did we advisel
Answer: The children. (We advised them to play outside.)
Q uestion: Who(m} did the police officer orderl
Answer: My friend. (He ordered him to move his car.}
in these cases, it does seem to matter whether the infinitive is presented in the active or
passive; the sentences below-to thedegree that they are acceptable-show no synonymy
with those above:
lWe advised baseball to be played outside (by the children}.
?The police officer ordered my friend's car to be moved (by him}.
We cannot "advise" or "order" an inanimate object to do anything.
As in the case of the first type of infinitive-the type that occurs in embedded clauses
after verbs like believe--we can substitute an object pronoun by making use of the optional
NP that appears before S in the revised VP rule on p. 635:
We advised them to play baseball outside.
The police officer ordered him to move his car.
Chapter 31: Complementation r-:::,,, fi37
However, since the main clauses here seem to have real objects (whereas the believe- and
expea-clauses did not), then we must ask what the status of the rest of the infinitive is. We
could analyze the remainder of the infinitive as a complement to this object, and reflect the
difference in the PS rules. However, if we wish to say thatall infinitives are clauses, and since
we have presented all clauses in the past as bearing subjects, then we must ask what occupies
the subject position of these infinitives. Following recent generative theory, we will say that
there is an empty subject position whose identity is controlled, or determined, in this case by
the object of the main clause and is identical to that object. We cali this empty element PRO;
it is not a lexical pronoun and doesnot show up on the surface. it merely signals that some
NP in the main clause functions semantically as the subject of the complement clause. The
tree for the sentence Weadvised the childmı to pfay baseball outsideis then:
1N P AUX VP
1 1
pro T V NP s
wc
1 1
-past advise
1
m
the child -pl
SUBJ
1
NP
1
------ -------
AUX
1
to
PRED
VP Advl
1
PRO V
1
play
NP
1
N
X
Prep NP
1 1 1
baseball g N
1
outside
We will then have to allow this PRO element as an option for the expansion of NP in mır
phrase structure rules:
(det)(AP) N (-pl)(PrepP)}
NP pro
{ s
PRO
Other verbs that fit this infinitive pattern are manipulative verbs like allııw, cause,Jorce, help,
permit, persua<k, and u
Our parents allowed us to stay up late.
The wind caused the canopy to fall down.
Bad luck forced us to leave Las Vegas early.
1 helped my sister to find the answer.
Note that the "to"AUX is optional after the verb help.
b38 ,<:::::,., THE GRAMMAR BOOK
Aıtempt-type lnfinltives
A third type of infinitive also involves a type of control. in this case the control comes not
from the object of the main clause but instead from its subjcct lwo main-<:lausc verbs that
require this infinitive type are attempt and tend:
The driver attempted to avoid the ice on the road.
Bears tend to eat voraciously in the spring.
As before, the infinitive seems tenseless, and to is present; in this case, there is clearly no
object at ali. Infinitives following the verbs attempt4. and tend certainly cannot be construed
as direct objects:
Question: What did the driver attempt? Answer: UTo avoid the ice on the road.
Question: *What do bears tend? Answer: ?ffo eat voraciously in the spring.
How shall we rcpresent this type of infinitive in a tree? in the case of thc first sentence, the
driver is the agent who is both "attempting" and, hopefully, "avoiding." It is not possible
that the agents who avoid or eat in these sentences could be anyone other than the main-
clause subjects. For example, the following make no sense at ali:
*The driver attempted her to avoid the ice on the road.
* Bears tend chlpmunks to eat voraciously in the sprlng.
Following the pattem of the object control infinitive type, we could say that in these cases
the main-<:lause subject controls the identity of the missing infinitival subject, which we
will again call PRO:
The driver attempted to avoid the ice on the road.
s
SUBJ PRED
1 P
N AUX VP
det
t
1
,,,
driver
N
1
T
-past
V
attempt SUBJ
S
PRED
1
NP AUX VP
1 1 /---
PRO to V NP
1
avoid det N PrepP
11
th e icc prep NP
1
on det N
1 1
the road
Our two recently revised PS rules expanding VPand NP will work to permit the genera-
tion of infinitives that are complements to this class of main verb; the class also includes
the verbs begin, continue, decide, fail, forget, manage, offer, proceed, promise, refuse, regret,
remember, start,try, and vow. Some example sentences are:
Chapter 31: Complementation r<:::::,,, ti39
They are nearly all intransitive verbs when they appear with infinitives following them;
however, the verb promise is an exception for many speakers.5
N1 P AUX P
V
1 1
pro T V NP
1 1 1 1
I -pres W'cUlt s
comp S'
1
for S UBJ PRI-:D
1.
NP AUX VP
1 1
pro lo V NP
1 1
hiın rdiııish det N
1 1
the fumiture
With some verbs such as uıant, expect, and lwpe, some native speakers find the Jor-phrase
either overly colloquial, cliale ctal , or bot:h, and simply use ot:her means to express the
same meaning; the factors that bear on this choicc medt further rescarch. Other future-
oriented and affectivc verbs in this class which more readily occur with Jor includ e arrange,
desire, expec,t lıate, hope, intend, like, love, p!,an , and j>refer;for exa mple,
The travel agent arranged (for us) to take another trip.
1 would hate (for you) to be stranded in the cabin this winter.
The doctor would like (for me) to try some new pills.
1 would love (for you) to visit Paris next year.
The tour leaders plan (for us) to visit more museums.
Thc subjcct of the infinitive will, of course, v·ary depending on the presence or absence of
the Jor-complementizer. When the Jor is present, the following NP represen ts the subject
of the infinitive; when there is no Jor-phrase, t:he su ject of the main clause represen ts the
subject of the infinitive, which is represcnted as PRO in thc tree.
Applying the test to see if thc NPs tlıe stmlents and our friend are direct objects of the main-
clause vcrb, we again get odd results:
*Who(m) did the teacher let? Answer: *The students (go home early).
Who(m) did you see? Answer: ?Our friend (leave the station).
As with infinitives that follow instances ofverbs of the let/seecla-,s, we cannot say that bare-
stem infinitivcs are NP objects; we will assume that they arc simply clausal complements to
the main-clause verb. Since they includc no cxpressed to, the AUX position will show the
tcnsclcss [-imper], as it docs in the case of subjunctive complemenL<,. The trce structure
for such sentcnces will be like the following:
We saw our friend leave the station.
s
SUBJ PRED
1
pro AUX VP
1 1
wc T v s
1
1 ------------
-pa<il scc SUBJ PRED
N1 P AUX VP
1
det N -impcr V NP
1 1 1
our friend lcave det N
1 1
the station
Other verbs in this class includc feel, /ıave, lıear, help,6 make, observe, and watcl
We felt the door close behind us.
The teacher had us repeat the exercise.
They heard the bell ring twice.
Please help us move our furniture.
GERUND COMPLEMENTS
While t/ıat-clauses and infinitives cover a largc rangc of verbal complemcnts in English,
there is an additional type of complcment, sometimes callcd the -ing complcmcnt or
geru.ml, which must also be disrussed. Consider, then, an additional pair of sentences:
Fred disliked [making phone calls to John].
Mary preferred [doing other things with her time].
Thc brclcketed parts of the two scııtcnces have somc of thc eannarks of othcr clauses we
have secn-spccifically, subject-control infinitivcs like those that follow the verb attempt.
Wc sccm to have a vcrb-objcct order of elcmcnts, with the logical subjcct of the brackctcd
matcrial being the same as thc subjcct of the whole scntencc:
[(Fred) making phone calls to John] Fred made phone calls to John.
[(Mary) doing other things with her time] Mary did other things with her time.
ti42 lHE GRAMMAR BooK
So it appears that the m ing subject in these clauses is like the PRO-type subject for some
infinitives. Also, we seem to have an auxiliary element in the inflection -ing; though it is
not the -ingof the progressive, it is an affix that attaches itself to a verb stem. it is possible
in fa.et to have both perfect and progressive fonns in sequences like those above:
having made phone calls to John
having been making phone calls to John
However, there are differences between these sequences and normal clauses, just as there
were with infinitives. As with infinitives, there does not seem to be any tense presenL lf we
change the tense of dislike and ,preferin the sentences above, the complement does not
change at all:
Fred will dislike making phone calls to John.
Mary prefers doing other things with her time.
in contrast to infinitives, however, the verbal elements in the sequences-making and
doing-seem to have the characteristics of nouns. With the addition of -ing, verbs can
regularly be made into nouns that can serve as subjects and objects of sentences:
What is most enjoyable for youl Hiking is most enjoyable for me.
What do you enjoyl 1 enjoy swimming.
The "whatrness" of these verbal nouns, called gerund.f, extends to gerund clauses as well:
What did Fred dislikel Making phone calls to John.
What did Mary preferl Doing other things with her time.
We conclude that these gerund clauses are NP objects which happen also to have the
status ofa clausal (S). The relevant tree structure is
s
SUBJ PRED
N1 P AUX VP
1N 1T V--------------------------------------------------------------------------------NP
1 1 1 1
Fred -past dislike s
SUBJ PRED
N1 P -X --------------- VP
AU
1 1
PRO -ing V NP PrepP
rol. (\ prep
1
NP
1
phone cali -pi to N
1
John
Chapter 31: Complementatlon 643
It is also possible to have a gerund phrase that has something that looks li.ke an expressed
subject, one different from the logical PRO su ect exemplified in the sentences above.
We can a1so have sentences like these:
Fred disliked (Susan's making phone calls to John].
Mary prefers [his doing other things wlth his time].
In each pair, we have what looks li.ke an [NP + possessive morpheme ora possessive deter-
miner] before the gerund noun. in both cases, it looks as if we have the basic structure of
clauses with subjects. Thus the tree representation above must be amended to accommo-
date such cases by replacing the PRO in the subject position with Susan or he and positing
a possessive s morpheme as a third type of complementizer. in this case the complemen-
tizer combines with the embedded subject during the mapping rules to yield the surface
possessive form.
Other verbs that take gerund complements include admit, appreciate, avoid, begin,
continue, defend, deny, enjuy, Jeet finish, Jorget, kate, hear, liJıe, love, prefer, quit, reca regret,
rememher, mume, risk, see, smell, start, stop, and try. Some occur only with PRO-NP subjects
(e.g., begin, continu.e), some require NP subjects-preceded by s complementizers that
may be different from the subject of the whole sentence (e.g., appreciate, see), and some
may occur eitherway (e.g., enjuy, remember).
SUMMARY
In this part of the chapter, we have revised our set of phrase-structure rules by adding a
number of options to S, NP, AUX, and VP as follows:
1. S (comp) (sm) S 1
2. comp {f }
(det)(AP) N (-pi)(PrepP)}
3. NP pro
{ s
PRO
Chapter 31: Complementation , fi1:i
; } (perf)(prog)(pass)
4. AUX {
-ıng
-impcr
s. VP
These rcvisions allow us to generale all the t/uıt-dauses, infinitivcs, gerunds, and subject-
participle constructions that we have discusscd in this chapter.
As the prcscntation abovc clcarly shows, there are many clausal coınplementation options
for English verbs. This situation is the sourcc of numcrous ESL/EFL errors. The most
common sourcc of crror is that while stuclent'I have leamed the various fonns of comple-
ment'I, they often couple the wrong complcmcnt with the wrong main verb. They do this
for understandable reasons: a student who generalizes by using some kind of meaning-
analogy may fınd thc errors on the ıight below somewhat puzzling givcn thc grammati-
cally similar models on the lcft:
The general commanded him to leave. *My father demanded me to study harder.
1 like to study history. *1 dislike to study math.
My sister advised me to go abroad. *My friend recommended me to stay home.
The teacher let us go home early. *Our parents allowed us stay up late.
A frequency count in writtcn tcxt by Butoyi (1977) showed that there is considerable
vari.ation in the actual occurrence of the vari.ous types of complemcnt, with tluıHypc comple-
ment clauscs constituting 46 pcrccnt of the total, subject-control infinitivcs (/ want to /,eave, I
tried to /,eave) constituting another 45 pcrccnl, with other complcment types distributed
among the remaining 9 percent. Interestingly, while structures akin to thak:larn,cs and infini-
tives are frequcnt in thc world's languagcs, genmd phrases are far less common. Ali else
being equal, we might expect ESL/EFL lcamers to experience thc greatest diflkulty with
gerund coınplcment use. lndeed, fuıther research by Butoyi (1978) anda study by Anderson
(1976) botlı indicate that thiı; is truc for spcakers ofa vaıiety oflanguagcs acquiring English.
Much does seem idiosyncratic about thc [vcrb + complcıncnt] combinations, and it would
be false to daim that no arbitrnri.ness exist'I in student ma'ltcry in thi'I arca of grammar.
However, there is also fortunatcly some rcgularity within this set!mingly complex array of
possibilities, and to a certain extent teachcrs can exploit these rcgulaıitics pedagogically.
a gerund complement, and make a bare infinitive. in fa.et, a loose correlation docs seem to
exist between the semantics of the main-clause verb and the type of complement it tak.es.
The correlation has been sketched by Giv6n (1980; 1990, Chapter 13).
Verbs Taking That-Complements
The main verbs that take ordinary tensed that-complcments tcnd mostly to denote
mental states or attitudes regarding the truth of thc proposition in the complcment
clause. Thus, to say
think
assume
imagine
know that it will raln today.
understand
say
is to say something about the nature of the particular belief-its relative strength, among
other things (a topic which wc will retum to in the next chapter). Giv6n placcs such vcrbs
in the "cognition-utterance" category and says that a clause in which such a verb occurs is
(1) more "loosely" connected syntactically with the complement clause, and (2) not neces-
sarily well integrated semantically with the complement clause. The fırst quality means, for
instance, that it is easy to detach a that-complement from its main clause, omit the comple-
rnentizer, and be left with a perfectly well-fonned sentence; the second quality means that
it is not at all necessary for the subject of the complement clause to be identical to any
noun in the main clause.
Verbs Taking lnflnitive Complements
We may contrast cognition-utterance verbs with those which Giv6n calls more manipula-
tive; typically, these verbs take infinitival complements. "Manipulative" here means, in
effect, that an agent named in the main clause is in some way related to, or has some
interest in, the real or hypothetical occurrence of the event that is depicted in the comple-
ment clause. Thus, in the sentences, Mary wan'tedjohn to reıum sooneror The ıeacher allowed
the students to leave, Mary and the teacher have an intercst in John's returning or thc
students' leaving. in accordance with Giv6n's criteria, such infinitives tend to be more
integrated syntactically with their main clauses in that an infinitive cannot stand by itself
asa sentencc; it is "more dependent" syntactically on its main clause. The main clause and
infınitivc are also much more likely to share an NP referent in common-as with thc PRO
category, wherc the idcntity of the logical subject of the infinitive (Mary wanıed [PRO] to
go) is controlled by an NP in the main clause. In the case ofbarc-stem infinitives (Mary
madejohn stay,John had the thi,ef arrested), there is even more integration since there is no to
present and since a stronger sort of control or involvement is depicted: indeed, verbs like
let, make, and have together with their complements are often called causative verbs or
constructions since one agcnt is (to one degree or another) "causing" another to act.
Giv6n's correlations are very rough-there are many cxceptional cases-and may
have limited pedagogical value for language teachers who seek to present the English
complement options systematically to their students. However, Giv6n's description is of
some value to teachers who wish to understand why English complement options pattern
as they do. Fora few cases, the schema does make predictions in cases where options exist
in use, and here ESL/EFL leamers might bene.fit from explanation. Consider the
following two sentences:
She helped them to pick cherries.
She helped them plck cherries.
Chapter 31: Complementation A>' ri47
in the first case, the woman mentioned may have provided a Jadder to the cherry-pickers,
while in the second case we are more likely led to believe that she was actually picking the
fruit together with the o thers . Her greater involvement is reflected in the use ofa bare-
stem infinitive. it thus makes sense that a corpus study like that ofLind (1983) would find
bare-stem infinitives appearing six times more frequently with animate subjects than
inaniınate when compared with t<>-infiniti ves.7
INFINITIVE To-COMPLEMENTSVERSUS
GERUND -ING COMPLEMENTS
There is considenıble overlap across the class of verbs that pennit subject-control infıni
tive complements and those that permit gerund-ingcoınplements. The overlap is illus-
tnıted in the italicized forms in the lists below:
Verbs taking infınitives: begin, choose, continu,e, dare, expect, fail, Jorget, lıate,
hope, intend, like, loııe, manage, J>refer, proce e d , promise, refuse, regret, rememlıer,
start, ten ci, try, want, vow
Verbs taking gerund -ing: admit, appreciate, avoid, beg'in, contintte, defend, clcny,
dislike, eıtjoy, fınish, forget, Juıt e, intcnd, like, love, f>refer, q u it, recall, regret,
remember, resuıne, risk, start, stop, try
in many of the cases of overlap, there is a distinct meaning differencc implied, eme which
is worth focusing on here and pointing out to learners. in some cases, the difference is
substantial. Consider the options with Jorget:
He forgot to buy the books.
He forgot buying the books.
in thc case whcre the infinitive is chosen, no books were bought (because of forgetful-
ness), while the action described in thc second sentence above actually happened (but
was subsequently forgotten). Likewise with remeınber.
She remembered to do her homework.
She remembered doing her homework.
in the fırst case, thc meaning may be paraphrased by saying "She did not forget that shc
had to do her homework, so she did it," while in the second, the intention is to say,"Shc
did her homework, and Llıen later she remembered that she had done it."
Some degree of correlation exists between the choice of infinitives with events that
are "hypothetical, future, unfulfilled" and the choicc of gerunds with events that are
"real, vivid, fulfilled," a fact that was first pointed out by Bolinger (1968). in the case of
Chapter 31: Complementation , 649
"forgetting" above, this principle is substantiated. it is also supported in the case of the
verb try. Compare the two sentences below:
Peter tried to go to Oxford.
Peter tried going to Oxford.
In the first instance, native speakers tend to interpret the sentence to mean that Peter
wanted to go to Oxford but did not in fact attend the school; in the second, the prcferred
interpretation is that Peter did go to Oxford but did not complete his studies. Compare
the additional two sentences below:
1 tried to cali you, but there was no phone anywhere.
1 tried calling you, but your line was busy.
in the first instancc, no calling took place; in the second, at least one (unsuccessful)
cali was made.
it is also worthwhile to look at how Bolinger's principle holds up with the extremely
frequent verb like. When someone expresses a desire to do something that he/ she has not
done before, it is common to use the modal constmction woul4 like and follow it with an
infınitive. A gerund is much less acceptable:
1 would like to go bungee-jumping someday.
ll would tike going bungee-jumping so meday.
On the other hand, if someone has already done bungee:iumping and wishcs to express
an affection for that activity, the gerund form is strongly preferred:
l like going bungee-jumping. (1 just did it last week, in fact.)
While the Bolinger principle does not yield such clear distinctions for ali of the overlapping
pairs in the lists above, it does provide teachers with a partial explanation for student.s who
want to understand the meaning differences with verbs taking both types of complements.
We havc argued that infınitives have no tense markings and therefore do not by themselves
cxpress pastvcrsus nonpast time. However, infınitives do express aspectual relationships of
the sort discussed in Chapters 7 and 9. These include the full range of aspectual categories
with the exception that the distinction between habitual (i.e., durative) and punctual (i.e.,
"one time") is Iost in the simple fonn of the verb, as in the case of drink below:
1 want to drink espresso (regularly).
1 want to drink espresso (tonight).
The sense ofa specifıc ongoing activity is expressible with the progressive fonn:
1 would prefer to be drinking espresso (right now).
She expected to be doing homework during the time that her roommate was out.
The sense of the completion of one event prior to the occurrence of another may be
expressed with the perfect form:
1 would prefer not to have drunk so much espresso this evening.
in the above case, the "drinking" occurs prior to the "preferring." Progressive and perfcct
may be combined, just as they mayin ordinary main clauses:
l'm surprised to have been making so much progress these days.
tijO THE GUMMAR BooK
In this case, the ongoing condition, the "making of much progress," is (at least to some
extent) temporallyprior to the "surprise."
For main verbs that express attitudinal states, the sense of future-in-past is conveyed
with a main verb in the past tense plus a simple infinitive:
She wanted to go to Europe the following year.
We didn't expect the weather to change soon.
Where contrary-to-fact conditions exi.st (see Chapter 27), there are two options available.
üne can encode the counterfactuality either in the main verb through a perfect modal or
in the infinitive through use ofa perfect infinitive:
She would have ilkeci to vlsit Munlch on her trip.
She would ilke to have visited Munich on her trip.
An exacting attention to tense and aspect here may lead to two different inteıpretatio
thefirst in which the "liking" expressed by the person in question existed prior to the
moment of speaking and may not exist at that moment, and the second in which the
"liking" does exist at the moment of speaking. However, the two forms seem interchange-
able, at least in oral production. To add to the profusion of options, one frequently finds
the same meaning conveyed in colloquial speech through the use ofa doubly-marked
perfect, though this use is frowned on by prescriptive grammarians:
She would have llked to have vlsited Munich on her trlp.
The meaning normally intended in such cases is not one in which one past event or state
(the "visiting") is set temporally prior to another past event or state (the "liking"),
although again that meaning may be conveyed when the fonns are emphatically stressed.
Gerund forms are likewise not marked for tense, but they too may express aspectual
distinctions. When a simple gerund is used, the distinction between perfective, habitual,
and progressive may be lost:9
She remembered drlnking espresso (that night).
She remembered drlnking espresso (durlng her three years in ltaly).
She remembered drinking espresso (while the muslc was playing).
Where the gerund is in the perfect form, however, one may distinguish the progressive
from the perfect through the presence or absence respectively of -ing on the verb in the
complement:
She remembers having been listening to a Grieg concerto (ali e venlng).10
She remembers having listened to a Grieg concerto (at least once in her life).
ünce again, actua1 use may blur the distinction here,just as the distinction between I have
üved hm for ten years and J have beım living hm for ten years lies more in what is implied than
what is necessarily expressed by the form.s themselves.
it may be said with certainty that if tense and aspect in English is difficult for
ESL/EFL students to master in simple clauses, the juxtaposition of tense and aspect in
multi-clausal sentences is even more difficult to accomplish in a nativelike way. As we have
seen, even native speakers exhibit considerable variation in their own production in this
area and are often seen almost struggling to expreM intended tense-aspect relationships,
often rephrasing something that has already been said to make a relationship more
precise. We therefore do not recommend close attention to these relationships in multi-
clausal sentences until learners ofEnglish are well advanced.
Chapter 31: Complementation .-<:::,,> b51
We have said that gerund-type complcments are not well representcd in the world's languages
whilc infınitives arc common. This fact undoubtedly contributcs to the frequency of eıTors
where the (b) sentence pattern abovc is substituted for the (a) pattern. Teachers should
expect frcquent errors of this type and perhaps address the issue directly during class time.
it may be noticed that most of the overlap concems sensory perception verbs. One view
(advanced by Kirsner and Thompson 1976) of the crucial differencc between pairs like
1 saw him break the branch.
1 saw him breaking the branch.
They heard the door close.
They heard the door closing.
They watched the boat disappear over the horizon.
They watched the boat disappearing over the horizon.
h52 ,-=- THE GRAMMAR BooK
reflects a difference between perfective and imperfective aspect: the simple form marks
an event seen as punctual, limited and bounded or perfective; in contrast, the -ing fomı
marks an impcrfective event which is represented as durntive, ongoing, or iterative, scen
without focus on its beginning or end. This account explains why a main verh in the
simple present tense may occur with an -ing form but nota hare infınitive:
Look! 1 see him leaving the building.
*Look! 1 see him leave the building.
Since the simple present is not used to mark discrete, punctual event.s (but instead states
or ongoing or repeated actio ns), it makes sense that it would not take a hare infinitive
incompatible by nature with this viewpoint. The account also explains why a perception
verb like obseroe can appear with a [subject + participle] complement but nota bare-infıni
tive complement even in the simple past tense: part of the in he rent meaning of
"observing" involves the visual perception of something over an extended period of time
without focus on the event asa completed whole; one "observes" an action in progress.
We can "obseıve" the painting ofa picture aı; it progresses, but we would not nonnally
speak of"observing" a sudden, momentary event such as the switching on ofa light. Thus
we are likely to hear sentences like:
We observed them eating dinner at Spago.
but are unlikely to hear ones like:
?We observed them sit down.
Likewise, the capacity for independent action on the part of the su ject waı; also noted by
Kirsner and Thompson to account for pairs such as thesc:
CAUSATIVE CONSTRUCTIONS
We have already spoken of causative constructions as those that depict one agent success-
fully causing another agent to perform an action. Some verbs which enter into these
constructions ( cause, force, get) fail syntactically into the object-control category; others
( make, have) take hare infinitive complements. How do they differ in meaning? Here we
will address have, get, and make, drawing on the work of Martin (1981).
Have. The verb have suggests a routine hiring or selecting in which a relation of authority
is implied, as between customer-businessperson or creditor-debtor:
We had Ray mow the lawn . (He does it every week.)
1 had the barber trim my hair. (it is his profession.)
Fred had John give him fıve dollars. (it was part ofa debt that John owed Fred.)
fHe had a stranger on the street give him directions.
The questioned example above is inappropriate since it suggests a relation of authority
which does not exist between two strangers in a chance meeting. The action performed
must also relate to the specific area of authority.
Get. The verb get often tends to convey the sense that some difficulty was involved;
perhaps the subject of the main clause used persuasion or coercion on the subject of the
embedded clause:
1 got Ray to give me five dollars. (He had refused earlier.)
M.ake. The verb maJu, suggests that the subject of the main clause has coercive power (though
not neceMa.rily authority in any conventional sense) over the subject of the infınitive:
He made a stranger on the street give him five dollars. (Threat was involved.)
Have and get, but not make, may also take passive complements in which these meanings
carry over well:
1 finally had the lawn mowed.
1 finally got the lawn mowed.
*I finally made the lawn mowed.
However, the ge sentence is now ambiguous as to whether the lawn was mowed by the
speaker or by someone else.
Martin (1981), in a discourse analysis of causatives, provides support for the distinc-
tions made above. in one of his native-speaker survey questionnaire items, 20 out of 23
respondents chose get when it was clear that some difficulty was involved:
1 had a lot of trouble finding someone to do it, but I fınally
(a) had the lawn mowed. (= 3) (b) got the lawn mowed. (= 20)
On the other hand, he also found native speakers favoring getfor some fairly routine activ-
ities such as "cashing a check," which may have involved some unexpected difficulties. (1
finally got the check cashed.)
fonnality of discourse: the more forma} the register, the more likely it is for tlıat to be
cxpressed. Another factor concems specific verbs: the greater the relativc frequency of the
main vcrb in discourse, the more likcly it is that the coınplementizcr will be absent:
1. He said he wasn't inte rested .
2. He conceded he wasn't inte rested .
3. ?He snarled he wasn't interested.
A third factor is that the presence of intervening nıaterial between the verb and the
complement tends to increase the likelihood that tlıatwill be present:
He said he wasn't interested.
He said in a recent report he wasn't interested.
?He said in a report released yesterday by UPI he wasn't interested.
in the second and third case above, occurrence of tlıat would resolve any possible
processing problem. A fourth factor mcntioned by Bolinger concems whether tlıe verb in
the main clause "operates" on the verb in the complemcnt clause in a causative sense:
The court decided that wiretapping must stop.
1 decided I was too old.
in the first sentence, the court's power to stop the wiretapping makes tlıe use of that more
likely than in the second case, where no power over events or conditions is apparent.
Underhill (1986) suggests several other interesting factors, including relative degree
of claboratc "speakcr endorsement" of the truth value of the complement clause. The
idea is that the greater the degrec of speaker endorsement, tlıe more likely that the
complementizer wil1 be absent. This may partially contribute to why tlıat is so often absent
where the main clause hegins with / think, as in I tlıink I want to go home:. speakers are
presumahly most certain about their own mental states.
According to recent corpus-based research (Biber 1998), the deletion of the comple-
mentizer that does indeed appear to be sensitive to register differences. For example, that
is omitted most frequently in conversation and least frequently in academic prose, with
fiction and news reportage falling hetwecn the two extremes.
Biber (1998) also provides infonnation on the frequency of tlıat complements according
to register: thcy are most frequent in conversation, next most frequent in fıction, then
news reportage, and least frequent in academic writing . He also reports that the most
coınmon verbs controlling tlu.ıt complements in both conversation and news reports are
think, say, know, see, belie-oe, show, find, and feel. Tlıese are verbs of cognition, perccption,
and specch often related to the speaker's or writer's stance. Such corpus-based infonna-
tion can be of great usc to language teachers for it indicates the verbs to empha.-,ize when
introducing tlıat com ple me n ts.
CONCLUSION
TEACHING SUGGESTIONS
1. Form. Students who wish to leam the information in this chapter simply through a fomı
of rote memorization should be advised that the most effective way to do this is probably not
to make list.s ofwhich verbs take which complements and then to memorize the lists. Ratheı
a more effective self-drill method would be for students to make up their own [verb + comple-
ment] sentences on a verb-class-by-verb-dass basis and-after verifying the grammaticality and
idioınaticity of these sentences-to reread or, betler, recite their sentences orally at home,
attending to their meaning. By making the relevant association ofverb with coınplement type
in this way, student5 will at least approach something resembling actual language in use.
2. Form. At lower class levels, and for review at higher class levels, a useful teaching
technique that will cue students to proper forms and give them practice in using verbs
meaningfully is for the teacher to model questions that incorporate the complement type
in the question itself, and then for the students to practice these forms with verb + verb
fonns written on the board. What follows is an outline of four lesson plans in the spiril of
suggestions made by Rosenzweig (1973).
on 1. Attenıpt-type infinitives (atlempt, tend, fail, pmceed, rııanage, wfuse, prom.ise, offer, decide)
The teacher asks questions that elicit model sentences that can be put on the board:
What will you attempt to do this weekend?
Where have you decided to go on your vacation next year?
The teacher then leads a drill in which students generale serıtences based on pairs of
verbs provided by the teache r, such as refuse/accept:
Student: 1 refuse to accept your offer.
Students can then work in small groups with pairings such as nıanage/finislı, fail/do,
offer/help, refuse/eat, and so on.
Lesson 2. Want-type irıjinitives (want, intend, expect, jn-efer, hate, like, love, lıojJe, desire, love)
The want-class of verbs, which may or may not have objects, make a good focus for
students interviewing other students. Most of these verbs give students ample
opportunity to express their teelings on topics which concern them directly.
The teacher may begin with:
What do you hate/love most to do?
What do you hate/love most for someone else to do?
A drill follows based on pairs provided, such as intend/ study:
Teacher: What do you intend to study?
Student: 1 intend to study engineering.
Teacher: What does your family intend (for) you to study?
Student: My family intends (for) me to study engineering, too.
fi5fi THE GRAMMAR BooK
n 3. Verlıs taking only gerund ohjects (stop, quit, recal dislike, enjoy, avoid, admit, deny, dejend)
The lesson continues to follow essentially the same pattem as above.
Lesson 4. Verbs taking both gerunds and infinitives (forget, remember, try, like, start, begin,
continue, wve)
At this stage, the teacher may find it useful to present Bolinger's principle distin-
guishing gerunds from infınitives, supplying verbs where the distinction is most
clearly exhibited:
Teacher: What do you remember doing yesterday?
What did you remember to do yesterday?
With each student answer, the teacher should make sure to confiım that the student
intended to convey that he or she actually did or did not do the activity mentioned.
The lesson then continues to follow the same pattern as above but with student ques-
tioners cued to elicit alternating pairs, such as jjırgc1+ gerund, + infinitive,
would like + infinitive , liM + gerund or infinitive , and so on.
l . Meaning. For verbs that take both infınitives and gerunds-but with a differencc in
meaning-Bill Gaskill has suggested that explicit time sequences be used to tcach the
differences between infınitives and gerunds with main verbs such as remember and forget.
The teacher introduces these verbs along with a number of situations that can appropri-
ately serve as complemen ts:
call my parents lock the door
tell you the news smoke cigars
Then the notion of time sequence is introduced and an example provided to show that if
the action in the main verb precedes tlıe action in the complement, the infinitive is used:
This happened earlier This happened later
1 remembered 1 called my parents
(main clause) (complement clause)
1 remembered to call my parents.
The teacher must also show the reverse. That is, if the action in the main clause follows the
action in the complement, the gerund is used:
Chapter 31: Complementation 657
4. Meaning. The distinction in use between verbs that altemate between taking hare
infınitives and [subject + participle] complements (see, hear, watch, Jeel, have) can be
modeled to some extent in the classroom by the teacher or by other students. Someone
can close the door slowly and incompletely while asking,
What do you see? (An swer: "I see you closing the doo r: ')
Someone can then close the door quickly and completely and ask,
What did you see? (Answer: "1 saw you close the door.")
Teachers can use their imaginations in coming up with ideas for other verbs. This sort of
activity can , incidentally, be done in conjunction with a review contrasting the simple
present/ present progressive and simple past/past progressive in main clauses ( "'7ıat am l
doing?"What did l do?"What was l doing?and so on).
5. Meaning/Use. A good role-play situation for practicing the causative have is to role-
play a beauty parlor/ barbershop scene. A role-play for three wil1include two customers
and a beautician/ barber. The beautician / barber will ask one customer, "What do want to
have done today?" The customer can then answer in a variety of ways using have: "l want to
have my hair permed," "l want to have my sideburns cut," and so on. This customer can
then ask the other customer what he or she wants to have done, and so on. it has even
been suggested (byJill Rosenheim and Sue Weingarten) that students actually visit a
beauty salon or barbershop as a type of field experience and make an inventory of services
available at the business.
6. Form. Frodesen and Eyring (1997:352) suggest that students role-play the solving ofa
problem to practice the use of subjunctive that-clauses. Two or more students take
opposite sides of an issue. For example:
A young man would like to attend an all-female co llege . The president of the college
wants to maintain the 100-year tradition of an all-female campus.
7. Use. Find a news article containing many cxamples of that complements with several
clauses having the that complementizer present and several others having it absent. Ask your
students to work in groups and underline all the that complements and circle ali the that
complcmentizers. Ask them to come up with generalİ.7..ations about why that appears before
some of complement clauses but not others. If they havc difficulties, give them Bolinger's
(1972) observations and see if they can apply these to the text they are analyzing.
fi51 ,--=:,., THE GRAMMAR Booıt
ExERCISES
BIBLIOGRAPHY
References
Anderson,J. I. (1976). "A Comparison of the Order ofDifficulty ofEnglish Scntential
Complements between Native Speak.ers of Spanish and Native Speakers of Persian."
Paper presented at the Los Angeles Second Language Research Forum, UCLA, 1976.
Biber, D. (1998). Teaching Actual Use in Conversation and Writing. Presentation at
TESOL '98 in Seattle, March, 1998.
Bolinger, D. (1968). "Entailment and the Meaning of Structures," Glossa 2:2, 119-127.
Bolinger, D. (1972). That's That. The Hague: Mouton.
Bolinger, D. (1975). "About Questions." Lecture, UCLALinguistics Colloquium,
February 12, 1975.
Bresnan,J. (1970). "On Complementizers: Towards a Syntactic Theory ofComplement
Types." Foundlıtions of Language6, 297-321.
Butoyi, C. (1977). The Accuracy Order of Sentential Complemımts bıy ESL Learners.
Unpublished M.A. thesis in TESL, UCLA.
Duffiey, Patrick (1992). The English lnfinitive. London: Longman.
Frodesen,J., andj. Eyring (1997). Grammar Dimensions: Farm, Meaning, and Use. Book 4
(2d ed.). Boston: Heinle & Heinle.
Giv6n, T. (1980). "The Binding Hierarchy and the Typology ofComplements." Studies in
Language4:3, 333-377.
Giv6n, T. (1990). Syntax: A Functional-Typologi.callntroduction (vol. 2). Amsterdam:John
Benjamins.
Kirsner, R. and S. Thompson (1976). "The Role of Pragmatic Inference in Semantics: A
Study of Sensory Verb Complements in English." Glossa l 0:2.
Lind, A. (1983). "Thc Variant Forms llelp to/Help 0." English Studies 64:3, 263-273.
Martin, W. (1981). "Causative Verbs in English." Unpublished lndependent Professional
Project, School for Intemational Training, Brattleboro, VL
Quirk, R., S. Grecnbaum, G. Leech, andJ. Svartvik (1985). A Comprehensive Grammar of
Contemporary Engl,ish. London: Longman.
Rosenzweig, F. (1973). "A Strategy for Teaching Gerunds and Infinitives to Advanced
ESL Students." Unpublished English 215 paper, UCIA, Fall 1973.
Underhill, R (1986). "The Discourse Conditions for that-Deletion." Manuscript,
C..alifomia State Univcrsity at San Diego.
Chapter
ÜTHERAsPECTS OF
COMPLEMENTATION AND
EMBEDDED CLAUSES
INTRODUCTION
• complex passives: either the main clause or complcment clause is in the passive voice,
thus making different word orders and structures possible.
[That he has won the toumament] is rumored.
it is rumored [that he has won the toumament].
He is rumored to have won the toumament.
RAISING TO SUBJECT
Every sentence in English must have some ovcrt element in subject position. However, in
certain constructions the subject position is filled with a word that does not fit our
intuitive notion ofwhat a subjcct "should be." We have already seen in Chapter 23 that
there in the there-construction has very general meaning and is placed in subject position
primarily to allow for new information to appear in predicate position. We also saw in
Chaptcr 23 that the word it in the sentence lt is raining has no real-world referenL in this
and the following sections of this chapter, we will look at some additional instances in
which it is used in nonreferential ways.
in Chapter 31, we discussed verb classes that are associated with certain types of comple-
menL One additional type ofverb that takes an obligatory complement is a copular verb,
exemplified here by the verb seem. This verb commonly appears in three fnımes:
The offer seems good.
They seem to have succeeded.
it seems that they have succeeded .
in the first case, we have a sentence that we can already generale by the base rule for VP,
one of whose possible expansions is [cop + AP]. The other sentences cannot be so easily
generated. We follow a standard analysis by which the second type of scntence is
generated through a rule called raisingt-o-subject, which gcnerates the subject of the main
clause in the embedded sentence and then moves it forward and up into the main clause
subject position:
[ ] seem [they to have succeeded] roising
They seem [[ ] to have succeeded].
in one way, the verb seem is like the verb hope in cases where the latter is followed by an
infinitive. Wherever the vcrb seem is used with an infinitive following, the infinit:ive has as
its logical subject the subject of the main clause. We can not have, for example:
*They seem their friend to have succeeded .
However, the similarity cnds there. Onc reason for the raising analysis is that this type of
structure is not quite like that for verbs likc hope, where thc subject of the scntence
controls a PRO in the dependent infinitive clause:
They hope [PRO to win the game]
(PRO = they)
The two structures are differcnt because, among other things, it is possible to have the
seınantically vague there in subject position in the main clause if the main verb is seem, but
not if the main verb is hope:
Chapter 32: Other Aspects of Complementation and Embedded Clauses ,<::::::,.> 665
Upto now, all of the clausal complements we have presented have been complemcnt'i to
verbs. However, these are not the only types of complements: both nouns and adjectives
may take complements as well.
COMPLEX-NP COMPLEMENTS
We have seen many cases in C...1ıaptcr 31 of that-clauses that function as embedded complement
objects of main verbs. Thaı-clauses often function as complements ofNPs as well, especially in
conjunction with certain NPs that represent a cognitive stance or indicatc the presumed truth
value of the proposition expressed in the complement clause. Such NPs are often callcd
comp!,ex NPs. Some example sentences include those below, with the head noun in italics:
The be/ief(that petroleum reserves are infinite] is again common nowadays.
1 don't agree with the notion [that there is no future in electric cars].
Do you believe the claim [that Sam is really a bigamist]l
Her interest in Menet lies in the fact [that he was a French painter].
The news [that a hurricane was coming our way] made us worry.
lt's time [to give up and go to bed].
This is a good occasion [to open a bottle of champagne].
Each complex NP includes a simple NP plus a complement cla1L'ie. Note carefully that
although the that-clause examples look similar to relative clauses, thcy are grammatically
distinct. For instance, the that at the beginning of the complement clause cannot be identi-
fied with any NP in tlıc clause (as can the word that in a clause like Mary saw the coat [that she
wanted to buy [ ]], where tlıat is moved forward from object position and ili identical to coat).
Our phrase structure rule for NP must be adapted again to permit an expansion of this type:
NP NP S
S comp S 1
Chapter 32: Other Aspects of Complementation and Embedded Clauses .-<::::,ı tih7
s
SUBJ PRED
1
NP AUX VP
NP s T1 cop AP
-- 1 1
1
1N
P -X---------------- VP
AU
i
AD] rcserve -pi
·f
-pres
cl i
be AD]
1 1
petroleum infinitc
Some types of noun complement with the phrase theJact, where the NP is a preposi-
tional object, have become part of complex prepositional-phrase chunks which, when
spoken, may be uttered so quickly that they seem like single words:
1 turned off the projector because of the fact that no one liked my movie.
Parrots don't understand what they say due to the fact that they aren't smart.
She was angry on account ofthe fact that he hadn't called her back.
They left town in spite of the fact that they loved the place.
The formulaic nature of these sorts of sequences may partially be accounted for by the
inability ofa full that-clause to appear as the object ofa preposition and the need for a
grammatical means to serve the same function-a need that is fulfılled by NPs like theJact.
Teachers will probably see the following sentence type produced by ESL/ EFL students
whoare unaware of this restriction:
* My friend drove me home in my car because of I was too tired to drive .
* T he principal scolded me in spite of I hadn't done anything w ro ng.
it is tlıerefore worthwhile for a teacher to make students aware of exp ressions involving
thefact and to provide them with examples in use, especially in writtcn prose.3
As a final note, if NPs of urging or recommendation take that-clause complements,
the verbs in these complement.s will appear in the subjunctive,just as they do for verbs of
urging or recommendation taking subjunctive clauses:
Ordinary subjunctive verb complements:
We rccommended that he take an carly plane.
The govemment demanded that truck drivers bespecially testcd.
Sentenu,s wi.th complex NP subjunctive complements:
The recommendation that he take an carly plane was a wise one.
Thc government's demand that truck drivers hespecially tested was excessivc.
titi8 ,..;::::,., TllE GRAMMAR BooK
ADJECTIVE COMPLEMENTS
Adjectives, like nouns and verbs, may also take clausal complements; tht..--y may be infinitival,
or they may be tensed that-dauses, depending on the adjective. 0/Ve mentioned some su unc-
tive cases in Chapter 31.) You will recognize the idiomatic pairings in the sentences below:
Susan is eager [to know your birthday].
She was fascinated [to learn the truth].
We're happy [that you could be here with us tonight].
The students are disappointed [that they have to sit and watch another movie in class].
Such clauses can be said to branch from the AP node and will necessitate a revision of the
AP phrase structure rule to read:
Theresulting tree structure for the sentence Susan is eager to know your birthday is:
Susan is eager to know your birthday.
s
SUBJ PRED
1 P
N -X---------------------VP
AU
1 1
N T cop AP
1 1 1
Susan -pres be ADJ S
1
cager SUBJ PRED
,,
1
NP
PRO
AUX
to V
VP
NP
,
know det
1
your
N
1
birthday
As with earlier infınitives, the analysis makes use of PRO, which suggests that some adjec-
tives, like many verbs, incorporate a "control" type meaning. Other similar adjectives
include unwilling, willing, disappointed, inclined, anxious, and uverjayed; most express emotional
states. Most are not sources of many ESL/EFL errors, except for the occasional confusion of
which type of complement-that-clause or infinitive-goes with which adjective.
There are instances other than seem-type sentences where it appcars in subject position
andan embedded clause follows the main verb. However, they are distinctly different in
that in these cases, the eınbedded clause could replace the it in subject position and yicld
an acccptable paraphrase. For instance, we cannot have sentences like
Chapter 32: Other Aspects of Complementation and Embedded Clauses 669
s
SUBJ PRED
1
NP AUX VP
1 1
s T cop AP
1 1 1
comp S' -pres be AD]
1
1t
t SUBJ -------------------------PRF.D unfortunate
1P
N -X---------------- VP
AU
1 1
pro T V PrepP
1 1 1
he -past leave Prep NP
1
without det N
1 1
his money
------77--.S.:.:..::1.:e:xtr:apo
== sition (from SUBJ)
SUBJ PRED s
1
NP AUX VP comp S'
1 1 /"--...
Extraposition, in these cases, shifts an entire subject clause" to thc end ofa sentence,
where complements usually occur.
Chapter 32: Other Aspects of Complementation and Embedcled Clawıes fi7 l
The same sort of movement seems to be taking place in sentences such as the
following, where extraposition occurs from an object position. In these cases, the first
alternative is clearly interpretable but may be awkward:
ll doubt [that she has the answer] very much. Note: the optional
1 doubt (it) very much [that she has the answer]. it in panmtheses Is
a pronomlnal reflex
lWe appreclate [that you had the dme to help us] greatly. for the extraposed
We appreciate (it) greatly [that you had the time to help us]. that clause.
We want [to go back to our dorrnitory] very much.
We want very much [to go back to our dormltory].
An extraposition rule has been claimed to operate elsewhere as well. Sometimes just
one part ofa subject, such as a relative clause or a complement, may detach and move to
sentence-final position; such movement in structures other than complements is most
common in colloquial usage but is not considered good forma! written style:
Extraposed NP complement:
The fact [that you have won the lottery] is irrelevant.
The fact is irrelevant [that you have won the lottery].
&traposed re'tative cl.awes:
The plan [which the president proposed] has been approved t.oday.
The plan has been approved t.oday [which the president proposed].
The problem [that I told you about] has been diagnosed.
The problem has been diagnosed [that I told you about].
&araposed participial phrase:
Any questions [regarding the contract] should be dealt with im.med.iately.
Any questions should be dealt with im.mediately [regarding the contract].
Such movement is sometimes called extraposition from NP.6
Since NPs can in principle be of indefinite length, it should therefore not be surprising
that a rule like extraposition regularly operates in English.
Extraposition may serve an additional function. As we have mentioned in earlier
chapters, English tends to place old, given information in subject position and new infor-
mation in the predicate. Since old information is typically encoded in brief form relative
to the rest of the sentence (because known or assumed information requires no elabora-
tion), a sentence like the following tends to sound better with an extraposed subject:
Unextraposed form:
That the govemor will fonnally announce the new sales tax bill is likely.
Extraposed form:
It is likely that the governor will formally announce the new sales tax bill.
If the bili in question is already famHiar to hearers/readers and is mcntioned within the
subject, it would be far more likely to appear in shorter form, approximately as follows:
The govemor's new sales tax bill is likely to be announced today.
Thus, the given/new distinction may play a considerable role in an utterer's decision to
extrapose.
Conversely, we might ask why someone chooses not to extrapose--why a full clause
might be retained in subject position. üne fairly clear case is that in which the pred.icate
of thc main clause itself contains a clause. The operation of extraposition in such cases
may present serimıs problems for sentence processing:
That he always selects the correct answer shows that he has studied the material.
llt shows that he has studied the material that he always selects the correct answer.
That you brought this matter to our attentlon helps us to see the real problem.
lft helps us to see the real problem that you brought the matter to our attentlon.
in fact, a careful discourse analysis of the use of clausal subjects would be useful since it
might confirm that subject complements occur most frequently in cases exactly like these.
In terms ofwhere we find such structures, Lisovsky (1988) shows that formal writing hasa
significantly higher number of clausal that subjects than spoken or infomıal written genres.
5TRUCTURALISSUES
The easy-to-please structure depends on the presence of certain adjectives denoting
personaljudgments about something: easy, hard, challenging, difftcult, annaying, important,
(im')possible, safa, dangerous, nice, boring, interesting, and fun:
lt's easy to please my friend John = My friend John is easy to please.
lt's fun to play this game. = This game is fun to play.
lt's impossible to solve the problem. = The problem is impossible to solve.
lt's not safe to eat those mushrooms. = Those mushrooms are not safe to eat.
The alternation may occur in embedded structures as well, as in the following bare-infinitive
complement:
Chapter 32: Other Aspects of Complementation and Embedded Clauses ,.-=::::,,., 673
The basic structure is then one in which the embedded clause originates as the comple-
ment to an adjective like easy, difficult or impossihle. The relevant part of the tree is illus-
trated as follows:
it is impossible to solve the problem.
(The problem is impossible to solve.)
s
SUBJ PRED
1
NP AUX VP
1 1
[l_.] T cop AP
\ 1 1
-pres be ADJ S
If the object NP in the complement is raised to fili the empty slot in the main clause, it
insertion does not take place: The problem is impossible to solve. Alternatively, if the
embedded object NP is not moved out of the infinitive to serve as the higher subject,
nonreferential itfills thesubject slot: [it] is impossihle to solve this problem.
fi74 THE GRAMMAR 8ooK
*I am easy to be pleased.
Such an error is readily understood in view of the overall meaning of thc sentcnce, but a
passive infinitive is not acceptable here: the unnamed PRO makes the sentence active. A
third source of error arises from a lack of awareness of the fact that with raising an NP is
moved to the left in a manner similar to relative pronoun fronting, where there can be no
pronominal reflex in the original NP position; thus the ungrammaticality of the under-
lined pronouns:
*1 am easy to please me.
*The movie is worth watching n.
Teachers working at intermediate to advanced levels would do well to help students sort
out thc dilliculties in these structures, whose similarities of syntax and meaning make them
highly confusing. Though they are syntactically marked constructions, they arc frequcnt in
everyday spccch and writing, and it makes sense to devotc somc attention to them.
COMPLEX PASSIVES
At this point it is uscful to rcturn to a structure that was covered in Chaptcr 18 on thc
passive voicc and to see how the structure and its uscs integrate into the grammar of
multiclausal sentences. As you rccall from Chapter 18, a passive structure hasa patient or
recipient in thc su jcct position of the sentence and places thc agcnt of the scntence into
a sccondary position, appearing (if at all) in a by-phr.:ıse in the VP.
We will treat reportcd spccch and writing in the next chapter; however, it is worth
mentioning at tlıis point that passives frequently appear in reports wherc the agent-that
is, thc reporter-is of no particular intcrcst, and thc report, or message, is the focus.
Rather than saying Peopl,e believe that ... with a clatıse following, it is more common to sce
sentences likc thosc below:
it is believed that weather patterns are changing worldwide.
it is not known for ce rta.in who Homer was.
it is rumored that Dick has been fired.
A"ı you will recall from thc discussion in Chapter 18, the passive construction is a mcans to
topicalizc the recciver of an action or the patient of tlıc scntcncc rather than the agent. in
the case of the sentences abovc there are no real "actions," hence no real agents.
However, thc "logical" subjects of believe/know/rumorarc unstated, possibly because they
are unknown or not being divulged but also because they seem relatively irrelevant to the
main message, which is the information in the embedded clauses. As we noted in Chapter
18, the vcrb rumored does not even have an active countcrpart, as in
*People have rumored that Dick has been fired.
in these sentences extraposition of the subject clauscs also seems to have taken place, as
elsewhere seemingly for tlıe reasons (a) that heavier, lengthier constituents tend to be
placed at the end of the sentence and (b) that new information tends to fall within the
prcdicate. in these commonly employed ways of reporting other•s utterances, then, we see
thc functional interaction of two grammatical structures.
Passivcs ınay occur witlıin emhedded clauses as well and arc prcsumably subject to the
same conditions of use in discourse. Howcvcr, thc syntactic options are somewhat more
limited, depending on which type of complement among those outlined in Chapter 31 is
present. We will mention some of the possibilities here. Passives may occur with cognition-
utterance verbs taking tensed or infınitival complements:
b7ti r<-:::::,1 THE GRAMMAR BooK
Mary believed (that) her son had been given the grand prize.
Mary thought her son to have been treated unfairly.
The choice of the passive in the second scntence abovc seems motivated by a desire to
focus on the recciver/paticnt (i.e., her son) rathcr than the agcnt. Note that while passives
may occur in infınitival clanses as we ll, the forms of bl' must be untcnsed in such cases:
be/bP.en/beingoccur, but am/is/are/was/werl' do nol. For want-typc vcrbs, for cxample, wc
mayhave:
Mary wanted her son to be given the grand prize.
Again, the passive seems prcferable to its active counterpart. in the case of allow-type vcrbs
(allow, cause, order; persuade), thc choice of passive may sometimes be required, since the
active would have a completcly different meaning:
Mary persuaded her son to be examined by a podiatrist.
Mary persuaded a podiatrist to examine her son.
in thc case ofbare--infinitive or NP + participle cornplements, passives may occur, bearing in
mind the usual conditions of use. Recall that these complement types follow verbs of
perception (see, hem; waıch, notice) and manipulation (make, /.et, have). While thc ı,yntax of the
following two pairs of sentences is idcntical, thcy differ decidedly in temıs of naturalness:
We saw a squirrel eating a nut.
?We saw a nut being eaten by a squirrel.
We saw a bear chasing a man.
We saw a man being chased by a bear.
The reason is that we would normally find it odd for a speaker obscrving the event
depicted in the first pair of sentenccs to focus on the nut; however, wc would not find it at
all strange in the second pair to focus eithcr on the bcar or on thc man, i.c. the animacy
of the noun phrase is higlıly relevaut here.
in this and the last chapter we havc talked about complcments in tcrms of syntactic types,
verbal frames, and thc semantic classes ofverbs that pattern with one or another of thesc
frames. üne dimension we have not explored involves the issue of truth versus falsity of
complements.
Kiparsky and Kiparsky ( l968) distinguished verbs and adjectival predicates according to
whetlıer they carry presuppositions, or inherent assumptions, regarding thc factual status of
their complcments. Some exaınples are listcd below:
Factive Nonfactive
coınprchend believe
regret daim
bearin mind maintain
be signifkant seem
bcodd be likely
be clear be possible
Chapter 32: Other Aspects of Complementatlon and Embedded Clauses fi77
A fa.eti.ve complement clauseis a clause that is true regardless of whether the higher clause
is affirmati.ve, negati.ve, or interrogative:
John regrets that he eommitted the erime.
John does not regret that he eommitted the erime.
Does John regret that he eommitted the erime?
in a1l three examples, it rcmains a presumed truth thatJohn committed the erime. in a
courtroom trial, the defense attomey would likely object if the questi.on above were posed
directly to the defendant as "Do you regret that you committed the erime?" Whether the
question is answered "yes" or "no ," the response amounts to an admission of guilt. The
same quality holds with the other words and expressions on the above list of factives:
Whether thc sentence Tt is odd that our.friend disappeared is negated, questioned, or left in
thc affirmative, it remains true that our friend disappeared.
Compare now a nonfactive eomplcment, which yields different results:
The pollee maintain that John eommitted the erime.
The police do not maintain that John eommitted the erime.
Do the poliee maintain that John eommitted the erimel
in these sentences, there is no presumption about whetherJohn did or did not eommit
the erime, regardless of whether the police have strong beliefs about John's guilt or
innoeence. in a similar way, to say it is likely that the Yankees won the game does not presume
that the Yankees won or did not win; nor is this presumed if the sentenee is made negative
or cast as a question.
Factivity carries over to NP complements as wcll. C...omplements of the NP the jact are,
obviously, facti.ve:
the fact that the President is elected every four years
lthe fact that the President is elected every month
Complements of the daim, the supposition, the beliej, and other nouns are not fa.eti.ve:
the bel ief that the earth is round
the daim that the earth is flat
in Chapter 31 we spoke of the Bolingcr principle, which says that gerunds tend to
depict events which are "vivid, real, fulfilled" while infinitives tend to depict events which
arc "hypothetieal, futurc, and unfulfilled." As it turns out, gerunds-especially those with
possessive subjects-and infinitives pattem to a large extent along the faetive-nonfaetive
dimension as well. The use of gerunds in subjeet position, for example, involves a
presuınption that the event asserted in the gerund is true; if the event is asserted not (yet)
to have oeeurred, the sentenee is slightly odd:
Do hn('s) doing that] annoyed me.
ljohn's doing that would annoy me.
If the event has not (yet) oecurred, the far more idiomatie construetion would be an
infinitive:
For John to do that would annoy me.
The same holds for gerunds in object position. If a gerund is used with hate, we expeet
that the event is fulfılled and thus find use of the infinitive less idiomatic:
1 hate his having talked like that.
ll hate for him to have talked like that.
fi78 lııE GRAMMAR IOOK
With the conditional w<ndd, indicating an unfulfilled condition, we get the opposite results:
1 would hate for hlm to say that.
ll would hate his saying that.
The same distinctions may be observed with the verbs likeand l.ove.
IMPLICATIVE VERBS
Karttunen (1971) identified a further distinction among verbs in which the factuality or
nonfactuality of the complement depends both on the verb itself and on whether the
main clause asserts or denies the complement. These verbs can be "positive implicatives"
or "nega tive implicatives" as follows:
Posilive implicative Negative implicative
manage fail
succeed neglect
remember forget
lf wesay, Mary managed to finish herwoik, we assume thatMaıy did in factfinish it. lf we say,
Mary failed ıo fmish her work, we assume that she did not. The same holds for the other
verbs on the list. When implicatives are negated, however, the situation reverses:
Mary did not manage to finlsh her work. (She dld not finlsh it.)
Mary did not fail to flnlsh her work. (She flnished it.)
In the first case (a negated positive implicative)Mary did not finish her work, while in the
second (a negated negative implicative) she did.
Though the above examples are infinitival, it should be pointed out that the majority
of verbs taking infinitives are not of the impli.cative type. Most tend simply to be ordinary
nonfactives: the use of allaw, believe, prefer, kate, or imaginewith the infinitives following
them presupposing nothing about the truth of the event expressed in the complement,
regardless of whether the sentence is affirmed or denied. Factive verbs like regret and
factive adjectival predicates such as be odd tend to take that-clause complements, though a
great many nonfactive verbs take that-clause com plemen ts as well.
CONCLUSION
in this chapter we have focused most strongly on a nuınber of structures in which stylistic
variation comes into the picture-that is, where one has the choice of usingone syntactic form
or another when the options produce roughly synonymous sentences but when discourse
considerations tend to dictatea certain formfor certain functions. These structures include:
a. Raising to subject in a seem-type clause or usinga tensed seem-complement
b. Extraposing or not extraposing
c. Moving or not moving an easy-to-pkase object into main clause subject position
d. Using a complex passive or using the roughly synonymous active form
The fact of rough synonymy here means that issues of use as well as form come to the fore
as elements of ESL/ EFL teaching. High-intermediate as well as advanced writing students
may benefit tremendously from a knowledge of the options available to them once they
master the more basic synt.actic structures of the language.
in the next chapter you will see how com plex sentences are put to use in reported
speech and writing. We also carry the discussion of factive versus nonfactiveverbsfurther to
seewhat bears on choosing one or the other in the course of reporting others' utterances.
Chapter 32: Other Aspects of Complementatlon and Embedded Clauses ,c:::::,., fi79
TEACHING SUGGESTIONS
1. Form/ Use. Learning the rules for raising, extraposition, easy-to-pkase, and complex
passives help students to become aware ofa variety of syntacti.c fonns thatshare essenti.ally
the same meaning. Because the variations are roughly synonymous, there is some value
simply in presenting students with one form and having them transform sentences into
roughly synonymous variants, as for example:
That Seattle is so rainy does not bother me.
(>it does not bother me that Seattle is so rainy.)
To work with clay is interesting.
( > lt's interesting to work with clay. / Clay is interesting to work with.)
People believe that electric cars may soon be popular.
(>it is believed that electric cars may soon be popular.)
The fact thatJohn passed the exam is not surprising.
( > John's passing the exam is not surprising.)
However, it is also of immense value where more complex constructions are concerned
for students to see how the constructions pattem in naturally occurring discourse. After
covering the constructions and their functions in class, teachers should present whole
paragraphs from written texts they have collected and ask students why a writer may have
chosen one structure over the other. (in many cases, of course, no definitive answer may
be obvious.) Another option would be for students to collect their own exaınple para
graphs containing one or more of these structures from the opinion section of the daily
newspaper (where syntax tends to be somewhat more complex) and to present them to
the class, offering their own explanations ofwhy the writer chose to write thc sentences in
the way that they appeared.
Studcnts can then work in groups, taking turns asking each other such questions while
using the adjectives on the blackboard as options.
3. Form. A clause<ombining cxercise may help students integrate some of the constnıc
tions in this chaptcr and the last in such a way as to allow them not only to master the
formal aspects but also the stylistic ones. it is important to point out the usefulness of
expressions like the fact (that . . . ) in clause combining in view of the impossibility of tW
clauses occurring after prepositions. With these things in mind, teachers can set scntences
like those below side by side and have students turn them into all the possible grammat-
ical sentenccs they can think of, allowing them to bring in other clause types they know
(such as relative clauses and adverbial clauses, for example). The resulting sentences may
or may not express identical ideas:
a. My brother sleeps late.
That bothers me.
I can't play ıny music.
That is a fa.et.
1) it bothers me that my brother sleeps late because of the fa.et that I can't play
mymusic.
2) The fact that my brother slceps late bothers me because I can't play my music.
3) My brother's slceping late means that I can't play my music, and that fact
bothers me.
b. It's unfortunate.
You won't be able to join me for lunch.
My friend is in town.
You wanted to meet my friend.
1) (The fact) that you won't be able to join me for lunch is unfortunate because
my friend who(m) you wanted to meet is in town.
2) It's unfortunate that you won't be able to join mc for lunch becausc my friend
is in town who(m) you wanted to mcet.
c. Somcthing is (not) likely.
My brother will remember something.
Next Friday is my bir thday.
My parents will call my brothcr.
My parents will remind my brother.
1) it is not likely that my brother will remember my birthday, which is next Friday,
evcn though my parents will call him to teli him thaL
2) it is likely that my parents will call my brother to remind him that next Friday
is my birthday, but my brother will not rcmember.
One advantage of this kind of activity is that teachers have the opportunity to guidc
studcnts away from the less fclicitous combinations that result and toward more natural-
sounding ones. At the same time, however, students often come up with quite sensible
combinations that the teacher would never have thought of!
There is an altemative, more contextualized way of doing this sort of cxercise, but it
will somewhat constrain the possible combinations. With some integration, individual
combincd clausc sets may be sequenced in such a way as to form a coherent story or other
text, provided the right combinations are chosen. Tcachers choosing this option may
sclect paragraphs from newspapcrs or magazines and adapt them for the purpose of the
exercise, creating threc- and fouHentcnce sets of short clauses to combine.
Chapter 32: Other Aspects of Complementation and Embeclded Clauses ,-.::::,,., fiil
4. Use. The form of raising structures, as well as their use, can be practiced with the
help ofa series of pictures that depict a progression of events (see, for example, the
Longman Picture Series, which provides ready-made materials for this sort of activity). The
teacher can teli students that they cannot be absolutely certain of what is happening in
the pictures, so they will have to hedge their descriptions with verbs like seem, appear,
and look like. This can be a good opportunity for students to practice relatively focused
raising structures and relatively unfocused it-structures. The teacher can ask a general
question :first:
What is happening in this picturel
Appropriate responses might be,
it seems that it is a very windy day.
it looks like it is a very windy day in the mountains.
The teacher can then zero in on ind.ividuals in the picture and ask,
What is the man doingl
An appropriate response which focuses on the man would be,
The man seems to be holding onto his hat.
This sort of questioning can continue for the other pictures, until the last picture:
Teacher: How did the story tum outl
Sıudenr it tumed out that everyone was safe inside a cabin.
Teacher: Who did the man turn out to bel
Studenr He tumed out to be a very farnous movie star.
The task is also easily adaptable as a written exercise involving narration and description.
S. Form/Meaning. Complex-NP complement structures like the fact that, the recommenda-
tion that, and so on can be practiced by providing students with a list of possible NPs like
the following:
the fact the news
the claim the belief
the idea the announcement/report
the possibility/ chance therecommendation/proposal
the likelihood/ probability the demand
the suggestion the argument
Students are then given sentences to be tumed into complements of suitable NPs chosen
from the list; these may include NPs that take subjunctive complements. The sentences
represent controversial statements likely to elicit inspired responses of d.ifferent types:
a. it has just been announced on the news that engineering students wil1 pay higher
college tuition because they will make higher salaries in the future.
Possible responses:
1) The idea that engineering students should pay higher tuition is ridiculous
because they will not necessarily make higher salaries.
2) There is no possibility that engineering students wil1 pay higher tuition.
3) I anı surprised to hear the news that engineering students will pay higher
tuition, because it does not seem fair.
682 r-:::,,., THE GRAMMAR BooK
ExERCISES
5. One way has been mentioned in this chapter for distinguishingbetween relative clauses
with that and noun complements with that: the word that at the beginning of the
complement clause cannot be identified with an NP in the clause the way the relative
pronoun that can. Can you think of another test that can serve to distinguish them?
Chapter 32: Other Aspects of Complementation and Embedded Clauses ,-c;;::,., bHl
6. in this chapter we havc presented the categories "factive" and "nonfactivc" to classify
verbs. How wou1d you classify the verbs see and understand based on the following
examp1e sentcnces?
1 see that you've bought a new car.
The drunken sailor saw snakes crawling on the floor.
1 now understand that there is a faster route to Houston.
1 understood Mary to say that she was from Houston.
Exp1ain your answcr based on the criteria presented in the chapter.
BIBLIOGRAPHY
References
Akmajian, A., and F. Hcny (1975). An lntroduction to the Princip!,es of1ransf<mna1,ional
Syntax. Cambridge: MiT Press.
Baker, C. L. (1989). Englislı Syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Emonds,J. E. (1976). A Transfarmationol Approach toEnglish Syntax. New York: Academic Press.
Giv6n, T. (1990). Syntax: A Functional-Typol<>gical Introduction (vol. 2). Philadelphia:
John Benjamins.
Karttunen, L. (1971). "lmplicative Verbs," l.,anguage47:2, 340-358.
Kipa rsky, P., and C. Kiparsky (1970). "Fact." in M. Bierwisch and K. Heidolph (eds.),
Progress in, l i n guistics. The Hague: Mouton.
Kostcr,J. (1978). "Why Subject Sentcnces Don't Exist." in S.J. Keyser (ed.), Recent
Transformational Studi.es in European Languages, Cambridge: MIT Press, 53-64.
Lisovsky, K. (1988) . A Discourse Analysis of Nominal Th,u-dauses in Eng1ish.
Unpublished Master's thcsis, UCLA.
Santos, T. (undated). "The Frequency and Usage of Raised and Non-Raised Structurcs in
English Discourse." Unpublished paper, UClA.
PHONOLOGICAL FORM
Consider the following sentence:
John cooked the dinner.
When produced in a normal, unmarked style, the speaker stresses the accented syllable of
the !ast content word and uses this syllable as a pivot for the rising-falling intonation
contour that normally accompanies a declarative English sentence:
John cooked ttıej di
However, a nonemphatic sentence such as this one may contain an emphatic constituent
ifthe speaker assigns special stress or prominence to one ofthe constituents (see also mır
earlier discussion ofstress-focused yes/no questions in Chapter 11):
J cooked the dlnner. (not someone else)
English also has several morphological and lexical means for expressing special emph asis.
fi12 ,<:::,, THE GIIAMMAR 8ooK
Emphatic Do
An entire sentence reccives grcater emphasis if the auxiliary isstressed. Dois introduced
whcn no auxiliary verb is present to carry emphatic strcss:
That w6uld be nice!' it does taste nice!
Do occurs as a marker of emphasis in affırmative declarative sentences (like the second
one above) that have no becopula or auxiliary verb to serveas the stress bearing operator.
It also occurs in the two following constructions:
• Affirmative wh-<jues tio ns that ask about the subject (other auxiliary verhs may occur
with stress here too)
However, it isalso possible to postpose the reflexive pronoun emphasizing the subject and
havc it at thc end of the sentence:
1 saw the president myself.
This use of reflexives is different from thcir referential use, which was discussed in
Chapter 16 (e.g., John cut himsclf) in that they do not rcplace a coreferential noun
phrase but occur after a noun phrase and refeı·back to it to mak.e it more emphatic. They
arc al<ıo different from the adverbial use of reflexives that means "all alonc; without assis-
tance." When used with the lattcr meaning, the rcflexive pronoun is often preceded by
the prepo sition by:
T he owner built thls house (by) himself.
Emphatic Own
Possessivc determiners (and somctimes nouns with a possessive intlection) that modify a
head noun can be madc emphatic by the additioo of own, which in turn can be intensified
by the addition of vcry:
(After having accepted Chomsky's analysis of comparatlve sentences for several years),
1 later developed my own analysis for such se nte nces.
Is that Johnny'svery own Ferraril (1 d idn't know he had the money to buy such a car.)
Adverbials of position
in both of these cases the (a) version, which could in fact be direclly generated by our
phrase stnıcture nıles, seems to signal discourse emphasis or contrast of the initial
adverbial element, whereas the (b) version of the two sentences appears to give focus to
the delayed subject of the sentence. it is the inverted (b) versions that are of particular
interest to us here and will be discussed again later in the use section.
lt-Clefts
An ikleft isa specially marked construction that puts some constituent, typically an NP,
into focus. The constnıction implies contrast. (Note that contrastive stress alone without
the cleft. transformation could signal the same meaning as the cleft.)
Neutral: John wants acar.
Cleft It's acar thatjohn wants (nota house).
Neutral: The manager mows the lawn.
Cleft: lt's the manager who mows the lawıı (nota gardener).
It is difficult to formulate a rulc that wouJd generate all iklefts since many different
constituent5 such as subject NPs, object NPs, and even prepositional phrases and adverbial
clauses can be put into the focus slot that follows it+ be:
lt's the teacher who corrects the papers (not the aide).
lt's power that the president wants (not money).
lt's in the kitchen that I study (not in the den).
it was because he was jealous that Bobby lied about his sister's prize. (not for some
other reason).
in each case, not only can the negative presupposition be expressed overtly, but if it is, it
can be moved forward; for example, It's the teacher, not the aide, who corrf>,ets the papers. Note
that a oegated vel'8ion of this construction yields sentcnces like the following (i.e., if the
focus is negative, the affirmative presuppo sitioo. is contrastive and is often expressed in a
phrase marked by but):
lt's not the gardener who mows the lawn (but the manager).
Again, the contrastive presupposition may be moved forward if it is expressed overtly:
lt's not the gardener, but the manager, who mows the lawn.
fi16 THE GRAMMAR Booıc
S lt + AUX + be + (Not) + D. + { ; } + S
focused (minus focused
constituent constituent)
in particular, our use of the "AUX" ı.-ymbol in the cleft fonnula is a gross approximation.
it has been suggested to us that only the ''tense" constituent of the auxiliary be u ed in
the It + be segment of the rule rather than the entire auxiliary. The logic behind this
suggestion is that in Standard orth American English the it+ be segment of the cleft can
never take a phrasal modal, a have . . . -en (perfectivc), ora be . . . -ing (progressive):
lt's in the kitchen that I am able to study.
* it is able to study that I amin the kitchen.
lt's these books that Peter has written.
*lt's have written these books that Peter has do ne.
lt's the teacher who was correcting the papers.
* lt's correcting the papers that the teacher was (doing).
(in other dialects of English some of the st.an·ed scntences are possible).
it is, however, possible for the it+ be segınent ofa cleft to contain a modal auxiliary
(logical use) in standard North American:
it might be Marty who stole the money.
it must be the butler who killed Mr. Smith.
Also, it has been claimed that the tense of the it+ he segınent is merely a copy of the tense
used in the main sentence. This daim is not always true, however, because defts such as the
following occur, where different tenses are used in the il+ be segmenı and the main sentence:
So, it's the butler who killed Mr. Smith (not his wife).
pres past
Thus, unril ali thcse comple x factors have been resolved, we will continue to usc the
symboE "AUX" in Lhis rule-with the added condition lhat ehe AUX may cont.ain only a
tense or a logical (i.e., epistemic) moda!. Furlhermore, if a moda) and nol are both
present in the il + be segment, the not will follow the modal and precede the be:.
it can't be Peter who wrote this book. (someone else did)
Note that who may come between the focused constituent and the S only if the former
refers to a person. Otherwise, lhat is used to refer to objects and prepositiona1 phra.ses as
well as to persons when the register is infonnal.
Ex.cept for the presence of contrastive stTess in cleft sentences, they sometimes
resemble relative clauses. For example, note the ambiguity of the following sen te nce
dted by Schachter (1973) in his discussion of cleft sentcnces a focus constıuctions that
sometimes resemblc relative clauses:
Wh-Clefts
Wh-clefts (also referred to as "pseudoclefts") are tlıe other important focus constnıction in
English. Consider the following examplcs;
What he is, is a complete fool.
Where we found the key was in the flowerpot.
What I said wa.s that we shouldn't go there.
What he does is get himself in t ro uble. 3
Now compare these wh-clefts with their neutral counterparts:
He is a complete fool.
We found the key in the flowerpot.
1 said that we shouldn't go there.
He gets himself in trouble.
The wh<left sentences give special emphasis to the constituent(s) fo11owing some form of
the copula be:
What he is a complete fool.
presumed shared knowtedge: ..he is something'' (element recetving focus, emphasis)
As the examples above demonstrate, the constinıent receiving special focus can be a noun
phrase, a verb phrase, a prepositional phrase, oran embedded noun clause (see Chapters
31 and 33). lf there are two forms of the verb be presen t, the second one is the pivotal be
structure.
Wh-clefts sbould not be confused with the less emphatic free relative clausc construc-
tion discussed in the preceding chapter.
WTı,cleft:
What I said was that we shouldn't go there.
Free relative:
What he said doesn't concem me.
in the free relative, the what = the thing that, and there is no pivotal form of be. Also, in the
free relative, what he said.functions as the suiject of the sentence. in the wh<.left, thcre is a
pivotal form of be (i.e., was), and the initial wh-clause, what I said, get.s elaborated and put
into focus in the material tlıat comes after the pivot, that we shotılıln'l go f.here.
Free relatives can occur wherever NPs normally occur. so they can occur as objects of
verbs or prepositions as easily as thcy can occur as subjects:
l' m not lnterested in what he said. (free relative)
Wh-clefts can be reversed, although they occur primarily in initial position, as our examples
above have shown:
We shouldn't go there was what I sald.
A complete fool is what he is.
EMPHATIC Use OF Do
Emphatic dois used in at least five context.s, according Lo Frdllk (1993:94): 1
1. Affımıative contrc:1.dictions of negative statements (often used with but ):
My teacher daims that I didn't turn in my paper but I did turn it in.
18 ,c::,,., THE GRAMMAR 8ooK
2. Emphasis ofa verb used with a preverbal adverb (see Chapter 25):
The horse he bet on always did win.
The guest we were waiting for never did arrive.
3. Emphasis ofa positive outcome after some initial doubt.
l'm relieved to know that he does ilke beef stroganoff.
(because that's what we're having for dlnner)
◄. Strong concession bordering on contrast:
Even though I dislike most nonclassical muslc, 1 do find myself drawn to Dixieland jazz.
5. Emphasis ofa whole sentence (de often occurs with an in tensifying adverb such as
r.ertainly, really, <kfinitely):
1 certainly do like that color on you.
They definltely did win the game.
Of course, when sentences contain auxiliary verbs that can be used as operators, the
auxiliary verbs can be stressed in the same contexts as those identified above for emphatic
use of do:
1. You predict we won't win the match, but wewiH win. You wait and see!
2. Aaron never can figure out problems like that.
3. l'm happy to hear you are plannlng to be there.
4. Although he hasn't made a donadon this year; he has contributed generously in the past.
5. We certalnly will support your elecdon to the board.
Ali the sequenccs above occur only in somewhal nonstandard colloquial usage; howcver,
the (b) sentences reprcsent a more acceptable variant than the (a) ones do.
What is the motivation for fronting adverbials of direction or position in sentences where
subject-verb inversion also occurs?
lnto the house ran Jo hn.
in the garden stands an elm tree.
A pilot study by C':ıary (1974) suggests that the speaker/ writer has selected the subject NP-
now in final position- to sutprise the speaker/ reader, create suspense, and spccifically to
go counter to the expectati.ons of lhe listener/ reader. For example, using tcxts such as the
following, Gary claims that the counter-Lo-expectation function of the (b) version of the text-
final sentencc carries a special presupposition of counterexpectancy and that this contrasts
with the neutral, noninverted (a) version, which has no special prcsuppositions:
Keith Sebastlan had given me detailed instrUctions on how to find his house; he
was to meet me there with the money. 1 drove up the driveway and got out of my car.
Just as the car door dosed, 1 heard the main door to the house open.
Keith Sebastian}
a. Dan Carlyle stepped out of the ho use.
{
The Sheriff
#Keith Sebastian}
b. Out of the house stepped Dan Cariyle .
{ the Sherlff
(Note: #=not acceptable given the discourse context)
hı the (b) vcrsion, Keith Sebastian is not acceptable as the postposed subject, according
to Cary, because there is no countcrexpectancy; that is, thc reader would normally expect
Keüh Sebas-ıian to be the subject just as he is in the first option of thc (a) version, but given
the use of the (b) construction, which signals counterexpectancy, the readcr is invited to
be swprised when somcone else is the postposed subjecL
Gary convincinglydemonstrates that concepts such as definitcncss versus indefiniten
oroew infonnation versus given infonnation do not adequately explain this example-and
the olher exaınples he cites-as effectively as does the notion of counterexpectancy.
Gary provides evidence that use of present and past participle fronting may also
signal sutprise or count.crexpectancy to the listener/reader in the samc way that fronting
of adverbials of directi.on and position do:6
Sitting in the front seat of the car was my cousin Joe.
Hidden at the back of my father's wine cellar was an old bottle of Chateau d'Yquem.
ti20 ,-=:::::., THE Gıwowt 8ooK
On the other hand, Gary does not feel that the fronting of negative or of degree or cxtent
constituenL accomplishes the same discourse function:
Never have I seen such a mess!
So absurd was his pro position that no one believed him.
We agrec with Gary and feci that these !ast two cases of fronting express an emphatic
cxclamatory meaning. Use of negative or degree/extent/comparison fronting reflects
cxclamatory speaker/ writcr stance rather than signaling the possibility of surprise or
countercxpectation on the part of the listcner/reader.
Gary (1974) uscd fiction (i.e., paperback novels) as his source for examples. Yang
( 1989) uscd a largcr and more diversified database that induded transcribed specch,
short stories, academic writing, and journ alism. One interesting finding she made is that
markcd word order with inversion did not occur at all in her oral corpus (about 24,500
words), which had only onc instance of direct object fronting:john I tike. In her written
<.·orpus (about 70,000 words drawn from fıction,journalism, and academic writing), Yang
found 29 instances of inverted word order, only five of which (17 pcrcent) cxpressed
contrast or counter expectation. However, she found 16 cases (55 percent) whcre the
marked word order with inversion servcd to maintain thematic continuity and eight cases
(28 percent) where it established or resumed a topic.7
In tlıe following example, thematic continuity is maintained through use of marked
word orcler and inversion:
(A short LA Times artide describes Mother Teresa's visit to South Africa. Having
reported what Mother Teresa said upon arrival at the airport, the writer continues:)
Accompanylng her (Mother Teresa) were four nuns who will run the new mission....
(Yang 1989:23)
Here thc present participle fronting and inversion serve to keep Mother Teresa as the
initial noun phrase in the clause. Thc same goal could have been achieved, of course, by
using passive voice (a focus construction we have discussed previouslyin Chapter 18).
She was accompanied by four nuns who ...
The following text begins a paragraph and is an example of using marked word order
with invcrsion to introduce or present a character or participant:
Across the aisle was an elderfy woman, dressed resolutely in black--dress, scarf,
stoc kings, shoes. [paragraph continues to describe the old woman]
(Yang 1989 : 15)
Since the preccding paragraph had described anotl1er character in thc short story, one
might say that this construction is being used to express contrast in addition to intro-
ducing a new participant or topic. In fact, Yang acknowledges that the three discourse
functions she idcnıifıed can overlap and that often two occur in any one instance of
marked and inve rted word order.
Thcre was at lcast one clear case of contrast (or countcrexpectation) that Yang found
in academic writing dealing with language acquisition:
These dause types are followed by relative clauses modifying common nouns in object
positio n. Stil/ missing at age 4 are relative dauses built on sentence subjects.
(Yang, 1989:20)
Chapter 30: Focus and Emphasis . h21
The article is clearly more about Colc than Duffy, since she is the topic of four of thc para-
graphs, including this one:
itwas Cole who chose Fargo as the microcosm for the debate on federal benevolence
and intrusion. Says Duffy, who wrote the story, "She saw it as a fascinıating mix of
frontler and front page. Then she dissected the town until she knew more about it than
Fargoans. Late last week, needlng an anecdote, she ran down to a local bowllng alley,
dld three lntervlews and delivered a freshly minted kicker for the story inside an hour:'
The placement of Gole in the infonnation focus of the paragraph initial, stress focus üdeft
sentence signals that Cole will be the topic of the paragraph. it alsa provides stylistic
variation in that the othcr three paragraphs about Cole begin: OJ/ıı luıs . _ . , Wendy luıs... ,
CoJe/011:nd . . . .
Use of Wh lefts in Conversation
Kim ( I995) has carried out one of the most completc snıdies of w/tclefts in English conver-
sation. He found 76 instances of wh-clefts in naturally occurring conversations and found
that ali but seven of thc tokens had four types of verbs in the w/,clausc (Kim 1995:251):
1. Verbs expressing a speaker-internal statc (29 tokens): realiz.e, want, fr.e think, nıjoy,
know, objecı to, ete.
2. Verbs expressing a metalinguistic dimension (22 tokens): ll'U'.an, say. ask,ete.
3. Verbs marking meta-events (12 tokens): lıaf>pf.n (c.g., W1ıat do,s lıappen is (that) thm'.r a
lerulency to forget. tlıis.)
4. Verbs marking ıneta-actions (6 tokens): do (e .g., What l'tn doing righl 1W'W is the
vacuuming.)
Therefore, the types ofverbs used in the wh-clause are highly constrained and give usa
good general sense of the interactional functions of wh-clefts, which according to Kim
(1995:253) are:
1. Discours<."-<>rgani1.ation uses, such as marking a topic shift or cxpressing the gist of
prior talk:
(Context: The therapist, Dan, marks the gist of exchanges between two teenagers,
Ken and Louise, about how parents never get over treating offspring as children):
K: My father's 45 or 43, an' he'II go over to my grandmother's house, and insteada my
grandmother offering him a drink of beer, she'II say
L: Wanna glassa milkl
[minor digression by K and L)
K: Wouldja like a peanut butter an' jelly sandwicM
D: So, in a way, what you're saying is you'II never get through that
(adapte<! from Kim, 1995:256)
2. Intcractional managcment uses such as cxpressing or dcaling with disagreement or
initiating repair (see Schegloff, 1979):
(context: A is Brad Crandall, a talk show host, and 8 Is the caller. They are having a
disagreement about the caller's Medicaid eligibility)
A: They won't be taking you off Medicaid.
8: They will!
Chapter 30: Focus and Emphasls 623
CONCLUSION
There are problems from the outset in trying to define focus and empbasis in English
becausc they overlap with other topics such a. exclamation and intensification. in addition,
it ınust be recognized that emphasis is essentially a semantic notion, and we have shown
that it may be signaled in many ways, including usc of special stress and intonation
patterns, choicc of words, choice of grammatical markcrs, and so on. Focus, on the other
hand, seems to be a discoursc-functional notion, related to wbat the speak.er/writer wants
to draw the listener/reader's attention to in the ongoing discourse or texL As we have
tried to point out, such attention can be activated through the use of marked word order
and special focus constructions. We make no daim to having provided an exhaustive
treatment of focus and emphasis in English, but wc hope to have drawn attention to this
iınportant arca.
TEACHING SUGGESTIONS
J. Form/Meaning. For teaching it.cleft sentences, the teacher should give several
cxamples showiııg how ik:lefts embody certain presuppositions and differ from their
nonnal aflimıative and negative stateınent counterparts. For examplc:
Sam studies physics. John doesn't drive a Ford.
(Cue: He doesn't study chemistry.) (Cue: He drives a 8uick.)
lt's physic.s that Sam studies, not lt's not a Ford that John drives, but a
chemistry. Buick.
-or- -or-
lt's physics, not chemistry, that lt's not a Ford, but a Buick, that
Sam studies. John drives.
in groups of three, students should be given one 3 x 5 card, cach with a false affimıative
or negative statement asa cue. The statements will be about their fellow students. They
should write a mini-dialog that makes natura} use of an ikleft construction.
24 .-<:::::,, THE GIWIMAR Booıc
On cue card: Kim comes from Hong Kong. (It's a false statement.)8
Sample student-generated dialog:
A: Is anyone in this class from Hong Kong!
8: Yes, Kim comes from Hong Kong.
C: No, he doesn't. { lt's Lee who comes from Hong Kong. not Kim.
lt's Korea that Kim comes from, not Hong Kong.
These dialogues can then be preseııted to the c asa whole for evaluatioıı and correction.
2. Meaning. You might want to ı.ry the following sequence of activitics for teaching
emphatic reflexives.
a. Show the class sentences with emphatic reflexives:
Subjr.ct r.mphasis:
1. The owner himself built the house. l. Did you yourself have a good time?
Object emphasis (all objects):
3. 1 met Troy Aikman himself.
4. We gave the President himself a copy of the proclamation.
5. Susan did her term paper on Einstein himself.
b. Read these sentences and have the class repeat. Ask them to paraphrase thc
sentences and to describe the function of the reflexive pronoun.
c. Point out the position of the reflexivc and ask them to explain what is emphasized
in each sentence.
d. Show the class several picture cards with sentcnces and have studcnts supply
correct reflexive pronouns to emphasize the subjeccs, For exaınple:
Cue: Mary made the dress.
Si: Mary herselfmade thc dress.
e. Then a student selects a card, and someone else in the class asks a yes/no
question based on the card. The studcnt who selected the card should give a
ıneaningful response. For example:
Sl: Did you yourself make that dress!
S2: Yes, 1 did. / No, 1didn't. My mother did.
4. Use. Fronting directional adverbs. R.ead a short paragraph or anecdote to your dass.
For example:
Everyone but Harry had arrived on time for the meeting. We waited 15 mlnutes.
There stlll was no sign of Harry. We had just decided to proceed wlthout hlm, when
into the corıference room dashed Harry!
Ask them about the word order of the Jast clause. Why does the adverbial come first? See
if they can explain the fact that the others had decided Harry wouldn't come, and when
he did, they were surprised because their expectation that Hany would not come tumed
out to be false.
Divide the class into small groups and give each group a sentence involving some
class member(s) with the directional adverb fronted (e.g., Out of the house came Maria
and Rosa). Have each group write a paragraph that uses the inverted cue sentence as the
Jast sentence. Groups will then excbange paragraphs so that each group canjudge the
appropriateness and accuracy of another group's story.
6. Use. Show intermediate or advanced students authentic ex:cerpts from written texts
containing ik:lefts. Ask tbem to decide in pairs or groups what tbe iı-defts are doing in
tbe written textıı. Have each pair or group compose their own passage with an il-cleft.
ExEilCISES
BIBUOGRAPHY
References
Bimer, B.J. (1994). "lnformation Status and Word Order: An Analysis ofEnglish
Inversion. "Language 70:2, 232-259.
Bresnan,J. (1994). "Locative Inversion and the Architecture of Universal Grammar."
La 70:l, 71-131.
Celce-Murcia, M. (1996). "Describing and Teaching English Grammar with Reference to
Written Discourse." TheJoumal of 1ESOL France 3:1, 5-18.
Frank, M. (1993). Modem English: A Practical Re/erence Gııide. (2d ed). Englewood Cliffs,
NJ.: Prentice-Hall Regents.
Gary, N. (1974). "A Discourse Analysis ofCertain Root Transformations in English."
Unpublished paper. Department ofLinguistics, UCI.A (Repmduced and distrib-
uted by the Indiana University Linguistics Club.)
Halliday, M.• and R. Hasan (1976). ('.,o/ıesion in Englislı. London: Longman.
Coordinating Conjunction
Conjunction (coordination) is the process of combining two constituents of the same type to produce another, larger
constituent of the same type.
Other than and, the traditionally labeled simple coordinating conjunctions include: or, nor, so, but, yet, and for.
He is friendly but/yet vain.
I will have coffee or tea.
Coordinating Conjunction
Syntactic Options for Simple and Complex Conjunctions
The easiest way to begin a discussion of conjunction is by mentioning the most common signal used to conjoin: the
coordinating conjunction «and»:
Coordinating Conjunction
Syntactic Options for Simple and Complex Conjunctions
A conjunction of two noun phrases seems to behave syntactically exactly as a single, simple noun phrase: it can
figure as subject, direct object, indirect object, object of preposition, and so on.
Coordinating Conjunction
Syntactic Options for Simple and Complex Conjunctions
Now let us examine the following tree diagrams:
Coordinating Conjunction
* Ellipsis (deletion): occurs frequently in English to eliminate the redundancy.
Birds can fly and I can fly too. = Birds can fly and I can too. (uninverted)
Birds can fly and so can I. (inverted)
She hasn’t left and he hasn’t left either. = She hasn’t left and he hasn’t either. (uninverted)
She hasn’t left and neither has he. (inverted)
* Gapping: may occur provided the conjoined sentences have (a) nonidentical subjects and (b) at least
one nonidentical predicate constituent apart from the verb.
Jack trimmed the tree and Ellen the hedge.
The wind was brisk, the sun bright, and the ocean calm.
Coordinating Conjunction
* Correlative Conjunction: two-part correlative structure where one part precedes the first conjunct
and the other precedes the second.
Either Jack or Mike played football. / Neither Jack nor Mike played football.
Ellen is either funny or energetic. / Ellen is neither funny nor energetic.
Sam did his homework either quickly or efficiently.
Sam did his homework neither quickly nor efficiently.
Coordinating Conjunction
The Meaning and Use of Coordinating Conjunctions
A straightforward account of the meaning of the coordinating conjunctions might look like this:
Coordinating Conjunction
The Meaning and Use of Coordinating Conjunctions
1. «and»as logical operator: Stu is a cook and Fred is a waiter.
2. «and» as marker of many meanings: Ann is in the kitchen and she is making doughnuts.
3. «and» as inferential connective: John fell down and hurt his knee.
4. «and» as marker of speaker continuation: … and I told him.
5. «but/yet» in denials of expectations: She is friendly but/yet introverted.
6. «but/yet» as marker of semantic contrast: Winter is warm in Miami but cold in Moscow.
7. «but» as marker of speaker return: … but that was not true!
8. Inclusive «or»: We will serve fish or chicken.
9. Exclusive «or»: You can study or you can play.
10. «or» as warning: Do it or …
11. «or» in paraphrases: This is a matsutakeor pine mushroom.
12. «or» as self-correction device: You are a joy to be around. Or, to put it another way, I love you.
13. «so»: The rope broke so the box fell down.
Coordinating Conjunction
Complete the sentences with correct conjunctions.
1. I'm going to go shopping on Sunday …….. buy some new clothes.
2. I've just eaten dinner ….. I'm not hungry .
3. Why don't you ring Sue …… find out what time she's coming over tonight?
4. Don't tell john about his birthday party ……. you'll spoil the surprise .
5. I have been saving my money this year ……. next year I plan to take a long holiday in Europe.
6. Secretary to boss: Do you want anything else ….. can I go home now?
7. I love to travel ….. I hate travelling by bus .
8. I'm bored! Let's go out to dinner …. see a movie.
9. I like living in the city …… my brother prefers living in the country.
10. Betty's just got a promotion at work ……. she's very happy.
11. You're thirty now. Don't you think it's time that you settled down …… got married?
12. Tom got a great Christmas bonus from work this year ….. he and his family can have a good vacation this year .
Coordinating Conjunction
13. It's late. You should go to bed now ….. you'll be tired tomorrow.
14. The taxi stopped at the train station …… two men got out of it.
15. I was in the area …… I thought I'd drop in and say hello.
16. I really hate to have to sell my car ……. I need the money.
17. My friend fell down the stairs …… sprained his ankle.
18. I won't be home for Christmas ….. I will be there for New Year's.
19. Julia was very angry with Tom …… she went for a long walk to cool down.
20. Can you stop at the shop …… get some milk on your way home from work?
21. Are you busy this weekend ….. do you have some free time? I need some help moving to my new house.
22. You've been working hard in the garden all day. Why don't you sit down …….. I'll bring you a nice cold drink .
23. Nobody was home when I rang Jenny ….. I left a message for her.
24. I've been dieting ……. I'm not losing any weight.
Adverbials
• Adverbials are the most diverse grammatical structures in English
morphologically and syntactically.
• Direction occurs before position: I last saw Jack walking across the
street in Atlanta.
• Manner occurs before time or frequency: Mr. Lee worked very
efficiently yesterday.
• Position or direction before time or frequency: Tourists travel to the
Caribbean every December.
• Goal before time or frequency: The train arrived at the station before
dinner.
• Reason or purpose come last: Sam studies hard to get good grades.
Adverbials
Form of Sentence-Initial Adverbials
• maybe vs perhaps
• maybe tends to cluster with other tokens of maybe, emphasizing
the speaker’s uncertainty as well as the different alternatives
possible: So maybe just maybe I could work my way.
• maybe is weaker in terms of probability than perhaps: Maybe
there was no divinity in this whole thing at all.
• perhaps rarely clusters and tends to collocate with positive
superlatives: Perhaps the most hopeful sign for the future is…
Adverbials
Use of Sentence-Initial Adverbials
• sometimes
• sometimes presents the topic and provides a transition with what preceded
when used paragraph-initially: Sometimes the truth itself is better than
fiction.
• sometimes elaborates upon or restates the preceding argument when used
paragraph- or episode-medially: Well, there are some aspects that overlap.
Sometimes, for instance, the matter of point of view will come up with a
poem.
• sometimes is used to sum up the preceding arguments to show the result of a
process or some change or to offer a final alternative when used paragraph-
or episode-finally: I feel comfortable again in moments of repose. Sometimes
I feel very happy.
Adverbials
Use of Sentence-Initial Adverbials
• of course vs obviously
• of course has a persuasive rhetorical function and presumes the
listener shares the opinion expressed by the speaker.
• obviously tends to reinforce a negative or self-effacing perspective and
it does not presume that the listener agrees with the speaker.
Adverbials
Use of Sentence-Initial Adverbials
• fortunately vs unfortunately
• unfortunately tends to co-occur with other grammatical and lexical
items indicative of a negative or problematic situation as the
speaker/writer signals negative assessment and regret about
circumstances.
• fortunately indicates the speaker’s conviction that the reality of things
is in fact less negative and more positive than prior information might
lead one to expect.
Adverbials
Reduced Forms of Adverbial Clauses
Some adverbial clauses of time may appear in subordinate clauses in which the subject and
sometimes the auxiliary verb seem to have been deleted:
Until I came to this city, I did not know what excitement was.
Until coming to this city, I did not know what excitement was.
Although he was rather late to the party, he was still able to get some food.
Although rather late to the party, he was still able to get some food.
Adverbials
Participles Functioning as Adverbials
Turning the corner, John was surprised to see Alice walking toward him.
• PAFs are used to express approximately how many times a habitual action or
condition is repeated.
• Sentence-final PAFs: I brush my teeth every day.
• Sentence-initial PAFs: Every now and then you should write to your parents.
• PAFs in the middle of a sentence: She is always late for the meeting.
• PAFs modify the entire sentence in which they occur: Sandra never smiles at
strangers.
• Negative PAF (never) may not co-occur with the particle not: Jim is not never
on time.
Practice
Rewrite the complete sentence using the adverb in brackets in its usual position.
1.I must see a dentist. (also)
2.I was joking. (only)
3.Did you enjoy the film? (both)
4.Jane watches TV. (hardly) (ever)
5.She drives her car. (carefully)
6.The men are digging a hole. (in the garden)
7.I go to the library. (often)
8.Sean fell off the chair. (almost)
9.I will go to Spain for a holiday. (probably)
10.My sister is getting married. (definitely)
Logical Connectors
Conjunctive Adverbials
They do not subordinate a clause; rather, they
connect independent clauses.
Jack should leave; however, Harry will object.
----- for mimicking teen-idols is personal insecurity. Preteens are in between child and
adult stages. They are no longer children, ----- the ways they behaved in the past are no
longer appropriate. ----- they are not yet adults; ----- they do not know the ways of the
adult world. This conflict can ----- feelings of insecurity. ----- when they were younger, they
could whine and cry to get attention from their parents and other children. ----- that kind
of behavior would be "uncool" around their teenage peers. Often, ----- the preteen does
not know how to act his or her age. ----- preteens do no know what to do, they often turn
to copy-cat behavior as a way to fit in and be more secure.
Logical Connectors
A second cause is that preteens feel they need to be popular ----- be more secure within
their own age group. -----, they turn toward models of popularity - teen idols - and start
dressing like them. Unfortunately, many of their idols dress and behave in ways that are
not age-appropriate. Teens need better role models than Hollywood currently has to
offer. ----- they interpret "dressing up" as wearing provocative, sexy clothing, rather than
clothing that makes them look good and feel comfortable and secure. ----- of their need to
be popular, young girls start dressing and acting as if they were much older than they really
are.
----- for young girls mimicking teen idols is peer pressure. They often see the adults closest
to them, their parents and teachers, as "uncool", ----- as enemies. ----- they turn to peers
who pressure each other to look, act, and dress exactly alike. This peer group can exert ---
-- pressure ----- often preteens do things as a group that they would not normally do. One
of these things is spending ridiculous amounts of money on idol clothing-lines. That's
right! Young Hollywood celebrities are making money off their twelve-year old "peers"
who give in to peer pressure. The peer pressure here is ----- great ----- most normal girls
will succumb. ---- we can see that personal insecurity, desire to be popular and peer
pressure can cause a young girl to look like a little "tart" standing outside of a mini-mart.
Conditional Sentences
A conditional sentence is a complex sentence that consists of a main clause
and a subordinate clause; the latter typically begins with the adverbial
subordinator if:
Had I known that, I would not have said anything. (subject/operator inversion)
Should the guests arrive early, no one will be here to greet them.
(subject/operator inversion)
Would you like to make a class presentation? If so, volunteer. If not, you don’t
have to. (conditional clause pro-forms)
Conditional Sentences
English conditional sentences express three different kinds of semantic
relationships:
1. Factual conditional relationships
timeless
generic
habitual
time-bound
implicit inference
explicit inference
2. Predictive conditional relationships
strong condition and result
degrees of weakened condition and result
Conditional Sentences
English conditional sentences express three different kinds of semantic
relationships:
3. Imaginative conditional relationships
hypothetical
present
future
counterfactual
present
past
Conditional Sentences
Factual Conditional Sentences
Generic factual conditionals: express relationships that are true and
unchanging.
If you boil water, it vaporizes.
Because of their unchanging truth value, these conditionals normally
take a simple present tense in both clauses.
Habitual factual conditionals: express relationships that are based on
habits instead of physical law.
If he washes the dishes, she dries them.
Both clauses usually have the same tense. Simple present in both clauses
if the habitual relationship refers to extended present time. Simple past
in both clauses if the sentence refers to a past habit.
Conditional Sentences
Implicit inference conditionals: express inferences about
specific time-bound relationships.
If it’s Friday, it’s Sam’s birthday.
These conditionals tend to maintain the same tense and
aspect or the same modal in both clauses.
Explicit inference conditionals: is the only case where
there is no strict parallelism of tense, aspect or modal in
both clauses because the conditional is used as the basis
for making an explicit inference and the result clause
contains an inferential modal.
If anyone has the answer, it should be Gandalf.
Conditional Sentences
Predictive Conditional Sentences
Strong condition and result: express future plans or contingencies.
If it rains, we will stay home.
The normal pattern for this type of conditional is simple present tense in the if clause and
some explicit indication of future time (will or be going to) in the result clause.
Subject NP: The book that is on the table was written by Wallace.
Direct object NP: The authors that she mentioned are well known.
Indirect object NP: The girl (to) whom I gave the message is not
here.
Oblique object NP: The child from whom you took the candy is crying.
Genitive NP: The man whose name you wanted to know is Anthony.
4. She didn’t see the snake …….. was lying on the ground.
1) February, which is the second month of the year, is the month ---- many
of my colleagues take vacation for skiing.
2) The dress ---- the movie star is wearing weighs about fifty pounds.
3) Playing fetch, our dogs, ran after the tennis ball ---- bounced across the
kitchen floor.
4) The old words you see on the board, most ---- were becoming obsolete,
are now trending again.
5) The family ---- house was decorated the most beautifully for the holidays
will receive a trophy.
Relative Clauses
Write relative clauses without using the relative pronoun.
• I gave you a book. It had many pictures.
→ The book
WEEK 1
Grammar: the mental system
that allows people to speak and
understand a language
Question: What is the
purpose of learning/teaching
grammar? Using language
grammatically or being able to
communicate?
Approaches to Teaching
Language
O Focus on language use (by
communicating)
O Sentential (syntactic)
She was walking home from school.
O Suprasentential (discourse)
She has never been so lucky as she ran into a friend when she was
walking home from school.
Grammar Rules?
O airtight formulations?
O fuzzy boundaries?
O rules or reasoning?
Three Dimensions
Grammatical Metalanguage
O Subsentential terminology
O Sentential terminology
O Suprasentential terminology
Subsentential Terminology
O Parts of Speech
O Aspect
denotes whether or not the event has
occurred earlier (perfect aspect) or is still
in progress (progressive aspect)
Adjectives
adjective: describes or denotes the qualities of
something
-able (likeable)
-ish (childish)
-ful (thoughtful)
-y (lazy)
O Main moods:
O declarative /indicative
O interrogative
O imperative
O Minor moods:
O exclamatory
O subjunctive
Sentence Moods
medical genre
literary genre
news genre
Next Week:
The Lexicon
The Copula and Subject-Verb
Agreement
Introduction to Phrase
Structure
THE LEXICON
«mental inventory of words and productive word derivational
processes»
We treat lexical units at three levels:
1. that of the individual word and its
components
2. that of word compounds and co-
occurrences
3. that of conventionalized multiword
phrases
What does it mean to know a word?
■ spelling (orthography)
■ phonetic representation (pronunciation, syllabification,
and stress)
■ morphological irregularity (if any)
■ syntactic features and restrictions (including part of
speech)
■ common derivations and collocations
■ semantic features and restrictions
■ pragmatic features and restrictions
Example: «child»
■ c-h-i-l-d
■ /čayld/
■ irregular plural: children
■ common countable noun
■ common derivatives: childlike, childish, childhood &
common collocations: child’s play, child labor, child
psychology
■ includes the concept «human», neutral regarding gender,
contrasts with «infant, baby, adult, adolescent»
■ My kids are home from school. (acceptable)
My kid is home from school. (questionable)
The Form of Words: Morphological
Affixation
Morphemes can be divided into two basic categories:
Morphological Affixation
■ Verb-Noun Restrictions
• transitive-intransitive verbs
The stain disappeared.
The man brought a gift.
• prepositional verbs require an adverbial of location,
direction, or a recipient
The child lay on the bed.
The boy headed home.
I handed the note to Alex.
Syntactically Relevant Lexical Features
■ Compounding
house (n): household, housemate, houseboat,
housebound, housefly, housekeeper,
houselights, housewife, housework, etc.
■ Derivational Affixation
• combine with stem/base forms to derive
new words
• can be prefixes that often change the
meaning (expatriate, unrepentant)
• can be suffixes that usually change the
part of speech of the word stem
(washable, childish)
Productive Lexical Processes
■ Derivational Affixation
• A word stem can have both a derivational prefix
and suffix (unthinkable) or more than one suffix
(governmental)
• most common and useful derivational prefixes are:
anti-, bi-, inter-, intra-, pre-, un- & suffixes are: -able,
-er, -ism, -ist, -less, -ness
• which words take which affixes is not always
predictable (e.g., suggestment or resent)
• when both a derivational and an inflectional suffix
are fixed to the same word, the inflectional suffix
occurs last: weaknesses vs. weaksness
Productive Lexical Processes
Literal Metaphoric
The wind blew. The wind whispered.
Put it into the basket. Put it into words.
He is in the garage. He is in love.
They take the plural verb inflection when more than one entity is
implied:
2 dollars are on the table.
3 years are missing from this file.
10 miles are to be run.
Rules for Persistently Troublesome
Cases
■ Arithmetical operations (add, subtract, multiply, and
divide) take the singular:
S= sm (sentence modifier) + S’
NP= pronoun
PHRASE STRUCTURE RULES
The Tense-Aspect-Modality
System in Discourse
Negation
Yes/No Questions
Week 4
Present Progressive
Activityin progress: He is playing piano now.
Extended present: She is studying medicine at the University
of New York.
A temporary situation: He is living with his parents.
Repetition or iteration in a series of ongoing actions: Alex is
kicking the soccer ball around at the backyard.
Expresses future (when event is planned; usually with a future-
time adverbial): They are coming tomorrow.
Emotional comment on present habit: She’s always reaching
out to people in need.
A change in progress: Ela is becoming more and more like her
mother.
Meaning in the English Tense-Aspect
System: Progressive Aspect
Past Progressive
An action in progress at a specific point of time in the
past: She was walking to school at 7.30 this morning.
Past action simultaneous with some other event that is
usually stated in the simple past: While he was
sleeping, someone broke into his apartment.
Repetition or iteration of some ongoing past action:
The baby was coughing all night long.
Social distancing: I was hoping you could lend me your
car.
Meaning in the English Tense-Aspect
System: Progressive Aspect
Future Progressive
An action that will be in progress at a specific
time in the future: She will be flying to Chicago
at 9a.m. tomorrow.
Duration of some specific future action: Alex
will be working on this project for the next two
years.
Meaning in the English Tense-Aspect
System: Perfect Progressive Aspect
In English, modals are derived from verbs that did carry tense
and take agreement markers during much earlier stage of the language.
However, English no longer inflects modals for tense and
number.
The logical meanings of modals typically deal with the speaker making an inference or
prediction. What increases is the degree of certainty regarding our inference:
John: Someone’s knocking.
Jake: That must be William.
That will be William.
That should be William.
That may be William.
That could/might be William.
Making Requests:
Historical past forms of modals are considered more
polite and less presumptuous than the historical
present forms.
Could you help me with this math problem?
Can I leave the room now?
When asking for permission, the selection of may or
can is socially significant.
May I talk to you for a minute?
Can I talk to you for a minute?
Social Functions of Modals
Giving Advice
Modals can be ordered according to the speaker’s degree of
authority or urgency of the advice.
You must see a doctor.
You should/ought to see a doctor.
You might/could see a doctor.
be able to is much less frequent and seems more formal than can.
shall is usually found in requests for a decision or for advice from the
addressee.
be going to is more informal and interpersonal than will which is
more formal and neutral as an expression of future time.
will conveys a sense of promise or commitment in the statement but be
going to conveys a plan or intention.
should expresses someone’s internal moral judgment or a higher
moral authority.
ought to is a marker of some general external moral or social
standard.
be supposed to refers to explicit, externally agreed-upon standards,
schedules, or expectations that may be restricted to a certain group.
Contexts of Meaning and Use for Some
Modals and Phrasal Forms
2. Will you help me with this problem? / Would you help me with this
problem?
3. I was able to go to the library last night. / I could have gone to the
library last night.
4. The ground is wet. It may have rained last night. / The ground is
wet. It must have rained last night.
Affirmative Negative
Sméagol likes to fish. Sméagol doesn’t like to fish.
Tolkien is a professor. Tolkien is not a professor.
Ramsey can cook well. Ramsey cannot cook well.
The Negation System:
The Lexical Level
The way to make many adjectives and adverbs negative is to add a
negative derivational prefix to the word:
happy---unhappy happily---unhappily
appropriate---inappropriate appropriately---inappropriately
possible---impossible possibly---impossibly
logical---illogical logically-illogically
relevant---irrelevant relevantly---irrelevantly
ordered---disordered orderly---disorderly
typical---atypical typically---atypically
The Negation System:
The Lexical Level
«in-», «dis-», and «un-» tend to be pejoratively evaluative of the stems to which
they attach while «non-» and «a-» prefixes are more descriptive and objective:
Pejorative/Evaluative Descriptive/Objective
irrational nonrational
disbeliever nonbeliever
disfunctional nonfunctional
unprofessional nonprofessional
untheoretical atheoretical
immoral amoral
The Negation System:
The Lexical Level
• «-less» can be used to negate nouns by expressing their absence or
nonexistence, thus forming adjectives such as : hopeless, penniless, speechless,
lifeless, shameless.
• A similar suffix is «-free» with a more positive connotation: carefree, smoke-free,
fat-free.
• Certain indefinite pronouns and an adverb beginning with «no-» can also be
used to give a negative meaning: nothing, nobody, nowhere.
• Negative adverb of frequency: never
• Negative coordinating conjunction: nor
• Negative correlative conjunctions: neither …. nor
• Quantifiers: little, few
The Negation System:
The Lexical Level
• Negative adverbial subordinator of conditionals: unless
• Negative adverb of time: yet
• Negative intensifier: too
• Negative adverbs of frequency: seldom, rarely, scarcely, hardly.
• Negative content words: fail, forget, absent, lack, exclude, etc.
The Negation System:
The Phrase Level
• «no» can function as a negative determiner in a noun phrase:
I am surprised that no alternative was proposed.
No plans have been made.
No way! / No wonder! / No sweat!
No kidding! / No running. / No smoking.
• Before infinitive verbs in infinitive phrases, «not» is used to make the
phrase negative:
She decided not to go.
The Negation System:
The Sentence Level
• «not» is the main sentence-level negator:
Statement: I am not surprised.
Question: Are you not going?
Command: Don’t move!
Exclamation: Isn’t that grand!
• «no» can also make a sentence negative by negating the subject:
No one was home to welcome him.
The Negation System:
The Sentence Level
• «no» can be a substitute for an entire sentence:
A: Are you going to the party?
B: No.
English articles:
• definite the
• indefinite a/an
• unstressed some
• no article
ARTICLES
Form-based information about English articles
depends on the English noun classification system:
• Noncount nouns are singular in number for purposes of
subject-verb agreement but cannot take the indefinite
article and the plural inflection: a water, some waters, a
luggage, some luggages
• Noncount nouns and plural count nouns take the zero
article or indefinite some: water, some water, suitcase,
some suitcases
• Proper nouns are a special case: they are like common
count nouns because they are countable but they are
different because they are inherently definite: Mr. Brown,
America
• When proper nouns take the plural inflection, they
require the definite article to retain proper noun status:
the Browns, the Americas
ARTICLES
1. Personal Reference
2. Demonstrative Reference
3. Comparative Reference
Reference
1. Personal Reference: The personal pronouns in their various
permutations constitute the personal reference system in English.
• Subject pronouns: function as subject NPs (I, you, he/she/it,
we, you, they)
• Object pronouns: function as direct, indirect or prepositional
objects (me, you, him/her/it, us, you, them)
• Possessive pronouns: perform two syntactic functions
(determiner function=my, your, his/her/its, our, your, their;
pronominal function=mine, yours, hers/his, ours, yours, theirs)
This is her book. (possessive determiner)
This book is hers. (possessive pronoun)
• Others: reflexive pronouns that replace NP objects (myself,
yourself, herself/himself/itself, ourselves, yourselves,
themselves) and reciprocal pronouns that replace NP objects
when the subject is conjoined or plural (each other, one
another)
Reference
2. Demonstrative Reference
• Demonstrative determiners vary along two
dimensions: proximity and number (this, that,
these, those)
• They can also function as pronouns and represent
an entire subject or object NP:
Please fill this form/these forms out. (determiner
function)
Please fill this/these out. (pronominal function)
• They are core determiners that can occur with a
pre-determiner and a post determiner:
all these other issues
Reference
3. Comparative Reference
• Identity: are used mainly as determiners (same, self-same)
The same/self-same young man had been there the day
before.
• General similarity: have different grammatical functions
(such, so, likewise)
Such argumentation tends to bore me. (precedes noncount
noun)
Such lectures bore me. (precedes plural count noun)
Such a lecture tends to bore me. (followed by a/an when it
modifies a singular count noun)
Our table is so long.
He made a generous donation. We were hoping you would do
likewise. (refers to a previously occurring verb phrase)
Reference
3. Comparative Reference
• Difference: refers to some target item other than
the antecedent (other, (the) others, another, else)
I couldn’t find Mike, so I looked for someone else.
• Particular comparison: can be used like
pronouns or adverbs to refer to something in prior
discourse (more, less, better, worse, etc.)
I finished my coffee. He offered me more.
A: How are you feeling?
B: Better.
Possessive Forms
In addition to the possessive determiners and
possessive pronouns, there are two other major ways of
signaling possession in English:
1. In writing, the first is by inflecting regular singular nouns
and irregular pronouns not ending in s with ‘s as in: the
women’s grace
or by adding an apostrophe after the s ending of regular plural
nouns and singular forms that already end in the sound s: the
boys’ trip, Kansas’ farmlands
NP
det det N
my neighbor’s son
Possessive Forms
det N PrepP
of det N
my neighbor
The Scope of Referential Forms
• The basic difference between the reflexive and reciprocal pronouns
and all the other personal pronouns: the antecedent for reflexive
and reciprocal pronouns must be in the same sentence or clause:
Mike can take care of himself.
Ryan and Sam can’t stand each other.
• For possessive pronouns: the antecedent can be either within the
same clause/sentence or in an earlier clause:
Shez loves his dog.
Do you know Shez? I walk his dog.
• Subject and object personal pronouns typically refer to an
antecedent in a preceding clause:
Do you know Jack? He’s just moved to Atlanta.
Do you know Jack? Yes, I have known him for years.
Interaction of Demonstratives,
Personal Pronouns, and Articles
• Demonstratives give the referent more emphasis than use of an
article or pronoun:
I heard a speaker on campus this afternoon.
1. The speaker was talking about the dangers of nuclear power.
(emphasis is on the topic of nuclear power)
2. This speaker was the best I’ve heard regarding the nuclear
power issue. (emphasis is on the speaker)
All our many hopes were kept alive by her encouraging words.
These next two weeks before the school starts will be hectic.
Assignment
Explain the ungrammaticality or semantic problems in
each of the following sentences:
1. A lot people were at the garage sale.
2. Although he had few close friends, he was very lonely.
3. She has a deal of energy today.
4. Almost people like Mexican food.
5. I still have much problem in learning English.
6. Larry bought a dozen of eggs.
7. We have much homework tonight.
8. Mike needs to get some informations.
9. She needs a couple a minutes.
10. Some of books on the table may be yours.
Assignment
Rewrite the paragraph with definite article and/or indefinite articles.
Ability to use language requires complex of knowledge and skills that is automatically
available to everyone when they acquire L1 as child. However, comparable level is
seldom achieved in L2, even if learners expend great deal of time and effort on learning
task. Different linguistic approaches have explored basic questions about SLA with
either internal or external focus of attention. Views on what is being acquired range
from underlying knowledge of highly abstract linguistic principles and constraints, to
ability to structure and convey information in second language; views on how SLA
takes place differ in their emphasis on continued innate UG capacity for language
learning or on requirements of communicative processing; views on why some learners
are more or less successful range from factors which are largely internal to language and
mind, to explanations which involve communicative need and opportunity. Purely
linguistic approaches, though, have largely excluded psychological and social factors.
To gain in-depth, “stereoscopic” understanding of L2 acquisition, we unquestionably
need to view process through more than one lens. Still-fuzzy nature of present picture
reflects need for more refined theoretical models and additional research.
NEXT WEEK
QUESTION FORMS
WEEK 7
• Wh- questions
• Other question structures
Wh- Questions
• get-passive
• get does not function as a true auxiliary in questions and
negatives the way that be does:
A: Was Bruno arrested? A: Did Bruno get arrested?
B: No. He wasn’t even caught. B: No. He didn’t even get caught.
• have-passive
Mary had her purse snatched. (experiential-beyond control)
Mary had her purse snatched. (causative)
• be in complex passives
It is rumored that he will get the job.
That he will get the job has been decided.
John is thought to be intelligent.
The Passive Voice
• The more definite the subject is, the more acceptable the
sentence in the passive form is:
This poem was written by Nazım Hikmet.
The Passive Voice
• With stative verbs, the more definite the object in the by phrase
is, the more likely it is to be acceptable in its passive form:
The movie was seen by everybody in town.
Adjective or Passive?
conversation
fiction
journalism
science
Decide whether the words in bold are subjects, direct objects or indirect
objects:
1. She sent her friend an email .
2. Sally will help you with your housework.
3. Where did you put the keys?
4. He gave them a bag full of money.
5. Alice wrote a book on the French revolution .
6. I need your help.
7. He offered her a flower.
8. My father bought us a new TV set.
9. John sold me his car.
Respond to the questions. Use the words in brackets and prepositions if necessary.
Example: Did she give Mary the book?
No, she didn't. She .................... . (it, Sam)
Rewrite these sentences putting the indirect object first after the verb. Note
that the new sentence often sounds more natural to the English ear.
Note: drop «to» and «for».
1. The doctor has given a pill to Robert.
2. John told the news to peter.
3. Please fetch the newspaper for your father.
4. The waiter offered chicken and fried potatoes to the guests.
5. Will you please bring back a pot of jam for me?
6. My father wrote a long letter to me when I was on holiday.
7. We will send a telegram to your parents at once.
8. Some students give a lot of their time to English lessons.
9. I'll fetch a cushion for you to sit on.
10. A baker makes bread for us to eat.
Sentences with Indirect Objects
• Adjectives do not have any typical form but there are some derivational
affixes that are commonly associated with adjectives: -al (mental, special), -
able (commendable, possible), -ar (particular, popular), -full/-less (hopeful,
hopeless), -ic (scientific, basic), -ive (attractive, expensive), -ous
(dangerous, delicious), and –y (pretty, dirty).
Attributive Adjectives
4. Adjectives that identify the reference of the noun itself (that may occur
after copula be): a medical doctor
5. Adjectives that qualify the time reference of the noun: the future queen
6. Adjectives that qualify the geographical reference of the noun: the urban
crisis
7. Adjectives that intensify or emphasize the head noun: a total stranger
8. Adjectives that show the uniqueness of the head noun: the only survivor
Adjectives
Saying that the adjectives are prenominal simply means that they
occur before a noun. Thus, wherever nouns can occur, so can adjectives:
Predicative Adjectives
Certain adjectives can only be predicative. There are three such
categories:
Some adjectives look as if they were past participles: the one-legged man,
the naked truth
Verb + Prep: to rely on, to detract from, to consist of, to substitute for, to part with
Adj + Prep: to be dependent on, to be free of/from, to be afraid of, to be sorry for, to be content
with
* Sometimes the same verb with two different prepositions will have significantly
different meanings:
* Sometimes noun phrases are both preceded and followed by prepositions to form
multiword clusters: with respect to, at odds with, in return for.
* about denotes spatial movement in any direction: He walked about the room searching for where he
left the keys.
* under denotes a trajector that is at a lower point than a landmark: Don’t sit under the stairs.
* over denotes a trajector that is at a higher point than a landmark: They peered over the fence.
Prepositions
Write the correct preposition in each of the following:
9. "In accordance _____ the wishes of my people," said the president, "I'm retiring from public life."
to/of/with
10. I was so afraid _____ missing the train that I took a taxi to the station.
for/about/of
11. It never occurred _____ me that I was living with a liar.
with/for/to
12. She made a point _____ coming late so that everyone would look at her.
for/of/about
13. My neighbor takes care of my little boys in the afternoon. She's so good _____ children.
about/to/with
14. He has worked _____ this company for 20 years, but now they're going to dismiss him.
with/for/in
15. In fairy tails, stepmothers are always unkind _____ their stepchildren.
for/of/to
16. This regulation doesn't apply _____ us. We don't live here.
about/to/for
Phrasal Verbs
Consider the following sentences and explain the function of «up» in each sentence:
Phrasal verbs are made up of two (or more) parts that function as a single verb.
PV= V «prt»
PV= turn out
Phrasal Verbs
* Like single-word verbs, phrasal verbs can be transitive (e.g., turn on, pass out, do over, look over, fill
out, find out): I called off the meeting.
* Phrasal verbs can also be intransitive (e.g., take off, play around, come back, come over, make up): His
car broke down.
* Many phrasal verbs take a specific preposition. In that case, the phrasal verb and preposition
must be learned as a unit:
put up with get along with cut down on
look in on check out of break up with
get back to stand up for run up against
(p. 427)
Phrasal Verbs
* The only thing that can be added to such a string is an adverb or adverbial phrase between the particle and
the preposition:
I haven’t kept up fully with the work.
He has cut down almost completely on his smoking.
* There is one syntactic characteristic peculiar to transitive phrasal verbs: sometimes the particle can be
separated from the verb by the direct object and sometimes it cannot. Separation is obligatory when the
direct object is a pronoun:
Mark threw away the ball. Mark threw it away.
Mark threw the ball away. Mark threw away it.
Others: look up, take up, leave out, pass out, bring back, turn down, etc.
* There are inseparable phrasal verbs where the particle cannot be separated fro m its verb:
I came across an interesting article last night. I came across it last night.
I came an interesting article across last night. I came it across last night.
Others: get over, run into, go over, look into, go for, etc.
Phrasal Verbs
* A few phrasal verbs seem to occur only with the verb and particle separated:
How can I get the message through to him? (get … through: convey, transmit)
* Avoid the ambiguity with the inseparable phrasal verbs that have the same form but a
different meaning:
Consider the following examples and adopt the following hierarchy of tests:
1. Can you put the object noun between the verb and P?
Jack looked up the new word. = Yes: separable PV (look up)
Jack looked at the newspaper. = No: Inseparable PV or V + Prep
Jack looked into the matter. = No: Inseparable PV or V + Prep
* Literal: Since the particle retains its prepositional meaning, the result is a phrasal verb whose meaning
is fully compositional: sit down, stand up, hand out, take down, throw away, climb up, fall down, etc.
* Aspectual: The meaning is not as transparent and the verbs to which certain particles contribute
consistent aspectual meaning: take off, set out, start up, run on, work away, mess around, sing through,
think over, burn down, catch up, etc.
* Idiomatic: Phrasal verbs whose meaning seems difficult, if not possible, to figure out by combining the
separate meanings of its parts: chew out, tune out, put off, etc.
Phrasal Verbs
Decide whether the phrasal verbs are separable or inseparable. Choose the correct sentences.
1. turn on → He turned on the light. / He turned the light on.
2. hand in → Hand in your homework on time. / Hand your homework in on time.
3. get on → They got on the bus. / They got the bus on.
4. look up → Look up the words in a dictionary. / Look the words up in a dictionary.
5. grow up → He grew up in north Carolina. / He grew in north Carolina up.
6. check in → We checked in at our hotel. / We checked at our hotel in.
7. give up → They gave up smoking. / They gave smoking up.
8. ask out → Henry asked Jenny out. /Henry asked out Jenny.
9. mix up → We mixed up the numbers. / We mixed the numbers up.
10. look after → Dennis looked after his brother. / Dennis looked his brother after.
Phrasal Verbs
Use the following verbs(believe, fill, get, look, put, switch, take, throw, turn, try) and the prepositions(away, down,
for, in, off, on, out) and form meaningful sentences:
1. Quick! ……. the bus. It's ready to leave.
2. I don't know where my book is. I have to ……. it.
3. It's dark inside. Can you ……. the light, please?
4. ……. the form, please.
5. I need some new clothes. Why don't you ……. these jeans?
6. It's warm inside. ……. your coat.
7. This pencil is really old. You can ……. it ……...
8. It's so loud here. Can you ……. the radio a little?
9. The firemen were able to ……… the fire in church street.
10. Does your little brother ……. ghosts?
Phrasal Verbs
1. Quick! Get on the bus. It's ready to leave.
2. I don't know where my book is. I have to look for it.
3. It's dark inside. Can you switch on the light, please?
4. Fill in the form, please.
5. I need some new clothes. Why don't you try on these jeans?
6. It's warm inside. Take off your coat.
7. This pencil is really old. You can throw it away.
8. It's so loud here. Can you turn down the radio a little?
9. The firemen were able to put out the fire in church street.
10. Does your little brother believe in ghosts?
NEXT WEEK:
Coordinating Conjunctions
Adverbials
Logical Connectors