The Natural Approach
The Natural Approach
-In this period,teacher should be as silent as possible in the classroom in order to encourage
the learner.
-subconscious
-emphasizing on meaning
-conscious
-forced speech
f) Even though the instruction is in the target language, the students first language may
be used
yes
no
The Natural Approach
First, the teacher provides a constant flow of comprehensible input in the target language
and provides non-linguistic clues.
Second, the teacher has to create a harmonious classroom atmosphere that fosters a low
affective filter. Third, the teacher decides on the classroom activities and tasks regarding
group sizes, content, contexts, and materials.
Finally, the teacher must, "communicate clearly and compellingly to students the
assumptions, organizations, and expectations of the method." (Richards & Rodgers 2001:
188)
Monitor hypothesis.
This hypothesis asserts that a learner's learned system acts as a monitor to what they are
producing. In other words, while only acquired system is able to produce spontaneous
speech, the learned system is used to check what is being spoken. The hypothesis says that
when we produce utterance in a Second Language, the utterance is initiated by the acquired
system, and our conscious learning only comes into play later, so we can use the monitor to
make changes in our utterances only after the utterances has been generated by the
acquired system. This may happen before we speak or write because before the learner
produces an utterance, he or she internally scans it for errors, and uses the learned system
to make corrections. Self- correction occurs when the learners used the Monitor to correct a
sentence after it is uttered. According to the hypothesis, such self -monitoring and
self-correction/repair are the only function of conscious language learning. Figure one
represents the Monitor Model for adult second language performance.
1) The performer has to have enough time: this is very difficult condition to meet because in
rapid conversation, taking time to think about rules, such as the subjunctive or subject-verb
agreement, may disrupt communication. Using the monitor requires the speaker to slow
down and focus on form.
3) The performer has to know the rule: This is a very difficult condition to meet because it
means that the speaker must have had explicit instructions on the language form that he or
she is trying to produce.
A very important point about the monitor hypothesis is that it does not say that acquisition is
unavailable for self-correction. We often self- correct or edit, using acquisition in both first
and in second languages. What the Monitor hypothesis claims is that conscious learning has
only this function, that it is not used to initiate production in a second language. Also, the
monitor does a better job with some parts of grammar than with others. Specifically, it seems
to do better with rules that can be characterized as simple in two different ways. First, simple
rules do not require elaborate or complex movements of permutation. An easy rule in this
sense is the English third person singular, which only requires the attachment of a
morpheme-s to the end of certain verbs.
However, monitor hypothesis has three types of users.
Monitor over-users. These language learners are too concerned and focused on correctness
that they cannot speak with any real fluency. Some characteristics of monitor over-users are:
1) They know many of the rule of the English language.
2) They are not able to communicate in speech.
3) Their written English might be quite accurate.
4) They do not have speaking fluency because they are not to concerned with being
grammatically correct.
Monitor under-users: These language learners are not focused on correctness because they
have not consciously learned the rules or because they have decided not to use their
conscious knowledge of the target language. Some examples of these are:
1) These learners are not able to correct their own errors in written English.
2) These students might not like grammar.
3) They believe that grammar rules are important but hardly use when they speak.
4) These students are not embarrassed to make mistakes.
Optimal monitor-users: These language learner are able to keep a balance between
self-correction and fluency so error correction is not an obstacle in their quest of
communication. These learners use their knowledge appropriately. Some examples if these
students are:
Finally, there are many difficulties with the use of the monitor, making the monitor rather
weak as a language tool.
1) Knowing the rule: this is a difficult condition to meet, because even the best students do
not learn every rule that is taught, cannot remember every rule they have learned, and can’t
always correctly apply the rules they do remember. Furthermore, every rule of a language is
not always included in a text nor taught by the teacher.
2) Having time to use the monitor: there is a price that is paid for the use of the monitor- the
speaker is then focused on form rather than meaning, resulting in the production and
exchange of less information, thus slowing the flow of conversation. Some speakers
over-monitor to the point that the conversation is painfully slow and sometimes difficult to
listen to.
3) The rules of language make up only a small portion of our language competence:
Acquisition does not provide 100% language competence. There is often a small portion of
grammar, punctuation, and spelling that even the most proficient native speakers may not
acquire. While it is important to learn these aspects of language, since writing is the only
form that requires 100% competence, these aspects of language make up only a small
portion of our language competence.