Adoptive Gay Fathers' Sensitivity and Child Attachment and Behavior Problems
Adoptive Gay Fathers' Sensitivity and Child Attachment and Behavior Problems
Adoptive Gay Fathers' Sensitivity and Child Attachment and Behavior Problems
Éric Alain Feugé, Chantal Cyr, Louise Cossette & Danielle Julien
To cite this article: Éric Alain Feugé, Chantal Cyr, Louise Cossette & Danielle Julien (2018):
Adoptive gay fathers’ sensitivity and child attachment and behavior problems, Attachment & Human
Development, DOI: 10.1080/14616734.2018.1557224
Article views: 26
An increasing number of gay men are now becoming parents through domestic adop-
tion (Jennings, Mellish, Tasker, Lamb, & Golombok, 2014; L’Archevêque & Julien, 2013),
a context in which there is no maternal figure. Adoptive gay fathers face many chal-
lenges. Because they are disrupting traditional gender roles by assuming tasks that are
culturally considered feminine, adoptive gay fathers often elicit strong resistance.
Moreover, as it is the case for other adoptive families, because adopted children have
often been exposed prior to adoption to various forms of maltreatment, neglect, or
prenatal substance abuse, they are likely to show more behavior problems and insecure
attachment than biological children (Juffer & van IJzendoorn, 2009). Recent studies have
focused on adoptive gay fathers’ families (e.g. Farr & Patterson, 2013; Goldberg, Black,
Manley, & Frost, 2017; Golombok et al., 2014), but none of them have specifically
examined child attachment to adoptive gay fathers. During the infancy and preschool
periods, attachment security has been shown to be a reliable predictor of child socio-
emotional functioning and parental sensitivity is a key component of the quality of the
attachment relationship (Sroufe, Egeland, Carlson, & Collins, 2005). The main objective of
this study was to examine the associations between adoptive gay fathers’ sensitivity and
child attachment and behavior problems.
parents, such as avoidance or control of the parental figure (Dozier & Sepulveda,
2004; Dubois-Comtois et al., 2015). Such behaviors are challenging for foster or
adoptive parents, as they may feel rejected by the child or overwhelmed by behavior
problems. Children who were maltreated have also learned to suppress the expres-
sion of attachment needs and foster/adoptive parents may misinterpret such beha-
viors as signs of good adjustment. Given the numerous problems shown by foster
and adopted children, several authors have suggested that a good enough degree of
parental sensitivity is not sufficient to answer an adopted child’s needs nor for this
child to develop a secure attachment to the caregiver (Dozier & Sepulveda, 2004;
Dubois-Comtois, Cyr, Vandal, & Moss, 2013). According to these authors, parents of
foster and adoptive families should be highly sensitive, have a good knowledge of
child development, and possess the skills to modify, rather than consolidate, the
child’s potentially insecure attachment pattern. Adopted children’s problems thus add
to the challenges faced by adoptive gay fathers.
Numerous studies have shown that the socioemotional development of children from
same sex parent families (lesbian and gay parents) do not significantly differ from those
raised by heterosexual parents (e.g. Crowl, Ahn, & Baker, 2008; Julien, 2003; Patterson,
2000, 2017; Short, Riggs, Perlesz, Brown, & Kane, 2007; Tasker, 2005; Vecho & Schneider,
2005). Golombok et al. (2014) even found less externalizing problems among children
raised by adoptive gay fathers than among children from heterosexual parent families
but no differences with children of lesbian mothers. A recent meta-analysis (k = 10) also
found better socioemotional outcomes among children of gay fathers than among
children of heterosexual parents (Miller, Kors, & Macfie, 2017). According to these
authors, the higher socioeconomic status of gay fathers, better preparedness for father-
hood, and more egalitarian parenting roles could explain these positive outcomes.
Although adoptive gay fathers have been found to share parenting roles as equally as
lesbian mothers (Goldberg, Smith, & Perry-Jenkins, 2012), as suggested by Golombok
et al. (2014), the stringent screening process for gay couples who wish to adopt could
lead to higher levels of commitment to parenting among adoptive gay fathers than
among adoptive lesbian mothers or heterosexual parents. Moreover, adoption is less
likely to be a second choice for gay fathers in their quest for a child unlike lesbian
mothers or heterosexual couples who may have experienced infertility and failed fertility
treatments (Golombok et al., 2014).
Regarding specifically children adopted by gay fathers, a number of studies have
found high levels of good adjustment among children adopted by gay fathers (Averett,
Nalavany, & Ryan, 2009; Erich, Leung, & Kindle, 2005; Farr, Forssell, & Patterson, 2010;
Ryan, 2007). Moreover, Golombok et al. (2014) and Miller et al. (2017) reported less child
externalizing problems among families of adoptive gay fathers than among heterosexual
parent families. Golombok et al. (2014) also reported more positive parent–child inter-
action among adoptive gay fathers’ families (e.g. more parental warmth and respon-
siveness, and less disciplinary aggression) than among heterosexual families. As
suggested by the authors, the stringent screening process of adoptive gay couples
may account for these findings. Among families headed by adoptive gay fathers,
associations between fathers’ sensitivity, child attachment security, and psychosocial
adjustment have never been explored but we would expect these variables to be
positively correlated. We may also expect to find high levels of sensitivity among
adoptive gay fathers.
Method
Participants
A sample of 34 families, including 68 gay fathers and their 34 children, were recruited from
across the province of Québec. Families were recruited with the help of the CPS, the LGBT
Family Coalition, the Chaire de recherche sur l’homophobie at UQAM, and with the
collaboration of the participating families. To be eligible, men had to identify as gay,
speak either French or English, and have at least one adopted child between 1 and
6 years of age living with them since at least 6 months. A period of 3 months seems
necessary and sufficient to allow children and parents to establish an attachment relation-
ship with their new parent, whether or not the latter is secure (Dozier et al., 2001). We only
included children whose adoption was completed. None of the participating families had
more than one child for whom the adoption process was completed.
The fathers ranged in age from 30 to 54 years with a mean age of 39.87 years
(SD = 5.43). Table 1 presents descriptive statistics for the sociodemographic variables.
Most fathers were Caucasian (93.3%), from the province of Quebec (66%), and highly
educated (78.2% with a university degree). Most of the fathers (92.8%) were employed
full-time, one father was unemployed, and one on a paternity leave. Fathers’ income was
substantially higher (median annual individual income between $60,000 and $69,999)
than most Quebecers’, whose median income at the time of the study was $39,312
(Institut de la statistique du Québec, 2013). Almost all families (94%) lived in large cities
(Montréal, Québec city, etc.) or in suburban areas close to large cities. Parents had been
together for an average of 12.65 years (SD = 4.19, range = 5 to 25 years) prior to bringing
a child into their family. The majority of the families (51%) had only one child.
As reported in a prior study, adoptive gay fathers reported a rather egalitarian
division of tasks and high levels of involvement in various areas of childcare, but paired
analyses showed a significant difference in involvement between both fathers within
each family (Feugé, Cossette, Cyr, & Julien, 2018). Based on self-assessment on a parental
involvement scale, we determined a primary and a secondary caregiver (see Feugé et al.,
2018). The father with the highest score was referred to as the primary caregiver and his
partner as the secondary caregiver. The mean score of overall involvement of the less
involved fathers (M = 3.9, SD = .32) was more than one standard deviation below that of
the most involved fathers (M = 4.28, SD = .25). The same procedure was used to
determine the primary and the secondary caregivers in the current study.
At time of assessment (see Table 2), children’s mean age was 3.93 years (SD = 1.6,
range = 1.2 to 6.66). They had been living with their adoptive fathers for at least
8 months (M = 2.97 years, SD = 1.45). Their mean age upon arrival in the family was
1.09 years (SD = 1.00) and the number of former placements was 1.32 (SD = .92). There
6 É. A. FEUGÉ ET AL.
were significantly more boys (67%) than girls, χ2(1, N = 34) = 7.67, p < .01. Most of the
children (59.4%) arrived in their adoptive families during their first year of life (28% were
less than 3 months). The majority were Caucasian (64%).
ATTACHMENT & HUMAN DEVELOPMENT 7
Measures
Sociodemographic questionnaire
This questionnaire includes questions about the sociodemographic characteristics of the
families (fathers’ and child’s age, fathers’ level of education, occupational status, income,
etc.). Other questions were related to the child’s adoption (age upon adoption, number
of previous placements, time spent in the adoptive family).
Parental sensitivity
The Maternal Behavior Q-Sort (MBQS; Pederson & Moran, 1995) is a 90-item instrument used
to assess parental sensitivity during parent–child interactions at home. A Q-Sort is a form of
questionnaire with items proposed on a set of cards. Each item is classified by an observer
according to its relevance with the caregiver’s actual behaviors. Items describe the parent’s
tendency to recognize and answer child signals and needs. The sensitivity dimensions
include caregiver’s affects, interactive behaviors with the child, communication skills with
the child, and attention to the child. Each statement has a criterion score. Correlations are
then computed between the observer’s sort and a criterion sort established for
a prototypically sensitive and responsive caregiver by a committee of experts. Final scores
vary from −1.0 (least sensitive/responsive) to 1.0 (prototypically sensitive/responsive). The
MBQS is one of the most valid indicators of maternal sensitivity in relation to attachment
(Pederson, Gleason, Moran, & Bento, 1998; van IJzendoorn, Vereijken, Bakermans-
Kranenburg, & Riksen-Walraven, 2004) and many studies have demonstrated its validity
and fidelity (e.g. Pederson et al., 1998; Tarabulsy, Avgoustis, Phillips, Pederson, & Moran,
1997; Tarabulsy et al., 2003). This instrument has previously been used with preschoolers
and children up to 7 years of age, as well as with internationally and nationally adopted
8 É. A. FEUGÉ ET AL.
children, and foster care children (Bernier, Matte-Gagné, Bélanger, & Whipple, 2014;
Colonnesi et al., 2013; Kim & Kim, 2009; Moss et al., 2011). Atkinson et al. (2000) showed
that the MBQS, compared with several other measures of maternal sensitivity, is the most
strongly associated with attachment security measures.
In the present study, the short version of 25 items was used (MBQS-Short). This
version enables to assess sensitivity from short video sequences (Poitras & Tarabulsy,
2017; Tarabulsy et al., 2009; Varela Pulido, 2016). The coding procedure is the same as
the one used for the long version. The short and long French versions of the MBQS have
been validated with heterosexual fathers from normative and high-risk populations, as
well as with heterosexual adoptive fathers (Boisclair, 2000; Colonnesi et al., 2013; Varela
Pulido, 2016). In a recent study based on the short-version, the authors of the MBQS
have set a threshold of .30 for sensitivity predicting security (Pederson, Bailey, Tarabulsy,
Bento, & Moran, 2014). Using the short or long version with primary caregivers recruited
in Canadian cities, the authors of the MBQS and their colleagues reported mean MBQS
sensitivity scores ranging between .34 and .63 for parents from the normative popula-
tion (Pederson et al., 2014; Pederson & Moran, 1996) and between .14 and .27 for at-risk
parents, such as adolescent mothers or maltreating mothers (Moran, Forbes, Evans,
Tarabulsy, & Madigan, 2008; Moss et al., 2011).
For this sample, the video recordings of free play sequences (20 m) at home were
coded by three research assistants. These assistants were blind to other study variables
and none of them coded the two fathers of the same child. Intraclass correlations,
calculated on 20% of the sample, showed excellent inter-judge reliability, ricc = .93.
to other study variables, coded child attachment from the video recordings for reliability
purposes. Inter-rated reliability was computed on 20% of the visits and children, and the
intraclass correlation coefficient was found to be excellent, ricc = .94.
Procedure
Most families were recruited with the help of the Centres Jeunesse du Québec. Social
workers first contacted eligible fathers to tell them about the study. A project coordi-
nator then contacted each interested family to explain the research procedure in details.
If interested, a written consent form was sent to the fathers for their own and child’s
participation. Most of the fathers contacted (82%) have agreed to participate. Other
families were recruited through the LGBT Family Coalition and the Chaire de recherche
sur l’Homophobie at UQAM via Internet forum, e-mail, social media, and with the
collaboration of the participating families. A $40 gift certificate was given to each
participating family. The project was approved by the ethics committee of the Centre
Intégré Universitaire de Santé et de Services Sociaux du Centre-Sud-de-l’île-de-Montréal.
Fathers completed questionnaires sent by mail or e-mail (e.g. sociodemographic
questionnaire). Each father was also observed while interacting with his child at home
in a structured situation based on the London’s protocole (Pederson & Moran, 1996).
According to van IJzendoorn et al. (2004), studies using such an observation protocol in
the child’s natural environment are among the best to predict attachment security
between (heterosexual) parents and their children. During the first visit, the child and
only one of the fathers took part in the activities. The second father was observed with
his child by a different experimenter in a second visit about 2 weeks later. Each visit
lasted approximately 1.5 h and was entirely filmed for reliability purposes. The visit was
divided into six segments: (1) the arrival, (2) a short period of discussion with the father
about child’s history and characteristics (25 m), (3) a father–child semi-structured activity
with age-appropriate toys provided by the research team (20 m), (4) the completion of
the CBCL by the father while the child is playing alone (this sequence also represents
a divided attention task for the father), (5) a father–child snacktime representing a daily
unstructured activity, and (6) the departure. Following the visit, parental sensitivity was
assessed using segment 3. The child-to-father attachment relationship was measured
using all six segments.
Results
Four measures were not normally distributed (primary caregiver’s level of education,
income, number of hours in paid work, and sensitivity for both fathers) and they were
either square rooted or log transformed.
Table 3. Means, standard deviations, and percentages for paternal sensitivity, child attachment, and
behavior problems as a function of primary and secondary caregivers.
Total sample Primary caregiver Secondary caregiver
Variables n = 68 n = 34 n = 34 Paired t-test or McNemar’s
M (SD) or % M (SD) or % M (SD) or % testa
Paternal sensitivity .53 (.37) .56 (.34) .50 (.40) .371
Child attachment security .41 (.14) .40 (.15) .43 (.13) −.948
Child behavior problems
Internalizing 48.68 (10.22) 49.17 (8.87) 48.64 (9.73) 0.32
% children above clinical 5.9%b 2.9% 2.9% 0.00a
level
Externalizing 48.02 (10.32) 48.52 (9.76) 47.58 (8.48) 1.06
% children above clinical 5.9%b 5.9% 2.9% 1.00a
level
a
Coefficient for the McNemar’s test.
b
According to at least one of the two fathers.
significantly different from secondary caregivers’, t(34) = .371, p = .71. For the sample as
a whole, which included all 68 fathers, parental sensitivity mean score was .53 (SD = .37,
range = −.83 to .86). No significant differences were found between our fathers’ mean
scores of sensitivity and the mean scores of sensitivity reported in other studies using
the same instruments (including primary caregivers from the normative population in
major Canadian cities or adoptive/foster care fathers observed during free-play activ-
ities), zs between −.99 and 1.34 (see Table 4). More than 88% of our fathers were above
the threshold of .30 as set by Pederson et al. (2014).
Child attachment
The mean scores of child attachment security were .40 (SD = .15, range = .00 to .60) for
primary caregivers and .43 (SD = .13, range = −.16 to .70) for secondary caregivers.
A paired t-test revealed that attachment security scores to primary and secondary
caregivers were not significantly different, t(34) = −.95, p = .35. For the sample as
a whole, which included all 68 child–father dyads, children’s mean score of attachment
security was .41 (SD = .14, range = .00 to .69). Table 3 presents descriptive statistics for
child attachment security. No significant differences were found between our children’s
AQS scores and scores reported in other studies using the same instrument (including
children between 7 and 42 months of age from the normative population in major
Canadian cities or adopted/foster care children), zs between −.66 and .62 (see Table 4).
Using the attachment security threshold of .32, 75% of our children were above this cut-
off score.
the borderline clinical range. Results of McNemar’s tests show no significant differences
in the number of primary and secondary caregivers reporting internalizing or externaliz-
ing problems in the clinical range, χ2(1) = 0.00, p = 1.00; χ2(1) = 1.00, p = .62.
In order to consider both fathers’ scores in one analysis, children’s behavior problems were
further compared using a three-group variable which we computed in the following way: (1)
a group of children with clinical and borderline problems according to both fathers, (2) a group
of children with clinical or borderline problems according to one of the fathers, and (3) a group
of children with no reports of clinical or borderline problems. Results of one sample χ2
indicated significant differences between fathers’ reports of internalizing, χ2(1,
N = 34) = 26.89,p < .001, and externalizing problems, χ2(1, N = 34) = 36.94, p < .001. Table 3
presents the means and standard deviation of children’s internalizing and externalizing
symptoms as a function of primary and secondary caregivers and the percentages of children
in the clinical range.
Preliminary analyses
Covariates in main analyses
To identify potential covariates, we examined the correlations between sociodemo-
graphic variables and outcome measures (security of attachment, internalizing and
externalizing symptoms). Neither fathers’ age, education, income, nor number of hours
in paid work were significantly associated with child attachment, internalizing, or
externalizing symptoms, rs between −.01 and .20. However, fathers’ age was related to
sensitivity, with older fathers being more sensitive, r(68) = .26, p < .05. Adoption
variables (children’s age upon arrival, number of former placements, time spent in the
adoptive family) and children’s age at assessment and gender were not significantly
related to outcome measures, rs between −.21 and .21.
Main analyses
Subsequent analyses were conducted using multilevel models, which consider data
collected at different levels of analysis (e.g. father and child) that can be studied
without violating assumptions of independence in linear multiple regressions. In this
study, the first-level units were children’s two fathers, for a total of 68 fathers.
The second-level units were the 34 children. In our case, the two fathers’ evaluations
of the same child might not be independent of one another. Multilevel modeling takes
into account these dependencies by estimating variance associated with group (e.g.
the two fathers of the same child) differences in average response (intercepts). This is
accomplished by declaring intercepts to be random effects.
Child attachment
Fathers’ sensitivity and child attachment scores were included in a two-level multilevel
model analysis. Fathers’ age was also included in the model as a control variable. The
relationships between sensitivity and attachment were found to show significant variance in
intercepts across fathers, Z = 4.07, p = .000. Results showed that higher levels of sensitivity
(β = .36, p = .003) were related to higher levels of attachment security (Table 5).
ATTACHMENT & HUMAN DEVELOPMENT 13
Discussion
This study is the first to examine the association between adoptive gay fathers’
parental sensitivity and child socioemotional adjustment. Based on an attachment
framework, we hypothesized that parental sensitivity would be related to child attach-
ment and psychosocial adjustment, as would be expected among heterosexual parent
families. Past research revealed only weak associations between paternal sensitivity
and child attachment. However, more positive parent–child interactions have been
reported among adoptive gay father families than among heterosexual parent families
(Golombok et al., 2014; Miller et al., 2017). Hence, our study was concerned with
documenting both fathers and children’s relational and emotional adjustment
among this understudied, but growing population.
It should be noted that fathers’ sociodemographic characteristics (age, length of
relationship of both parents, education level, ethnicity, and family income) in the
current study were very similar to those found in a study conducted with 230 adoptive
gay fathers in the United States (Tornello, Farr, & Patterson, 2011). This suggests that
our sample is representative of gay fathers and their children in North America. Also, in
accordance with recent studies (Bernier et al., 2014; Cadman et al., 2018), children’s
14 É. A. FEUGÉ ET AL.
attachment security and behavior problems are not different from those found among
normative and non-clinical populations, suggesting that children adopted by gay
fathers are doing well in terms of their socioemotional adjustment and the quality of
their relationship with their fathers.
Attachment
We used observational measures to assess children’s attachment to their gay fathers. To
our knowledge, our study is the first to use these measures with this population.
Children in our sample showed an average AQS score of .42. Moreover, 75% of our
children were above the average observer AQS security score of .32 found in a meta-
analysis (van IJzendoorn et al., 2004). Knowing that there is an overrepresentation of
insecure-disorganized attachment in foster and adopted children (van den Dries et al.,
2009), these results are very encouraging, especially given that attachment has an
important predictive value for the child’s emotional, cognitive, and social abilities
(Weinfield, Sroufe, Egeland, & Carlson, 2008). Therefore, our findings suggest that
adoptive families headed by two gay fathers provide their children with good care.
However, because the Attachment Q-Sort does not assess child attachment disorganiza-
tion and because disorganized attachment behaviors have been frequently reported in
adopted children (see van den Dries et al., 2009 for a meta-analysis), insecure attach-
ment classifications or dimensional styles should be examined among children of
adoptive gay fathers.
adopted children should consider using different procedures to assess child attachment.
Finally, although no correlation was found between fathers’ sensitivity and child age,
another limitation stems from the fact that parental sensitivity was measured with
children of a wide age range.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by a grant from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council
(SSHRC) and the Institut universitaire Jeunes en difficulté, CIUSSS Centre-Sud-de-L’Île-de-Montréal.
We thank the participating families and our collaborators, les Centres Jeunesse de Montréal,
Québec, Lanaudière, Outaouais et Laurentides, the LBGT Family Coalition, and the Chaire sur
l’homophobie at UQAM for their invaluable assistance in this research project.
Funding
This work was supported by the Institut universitaire Jeunes en difficulté, CIUSSS Centre-Sud-de
-L’Île-de-Montréal; and the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada. Research
support was also provided to the second by the Canada Research Chairs Program.
References
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2000). Manual for the ASEBA preschool forms & profiles.
Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.
Amato, P. R., & Rivera, F. (1999). Paternal involvement and children’s behavior problems. Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 61(2), 375–384.
Atkinson, L., Niccols, A., Paglia, A., Coolbear, J., Parker, K. C. H., Poulton, L., … Sitarenios, G. (2000). A
meta-analysis of time between maternal sensitivity and attachment assessments: Implications
for internal working models in infancy/toddlerhood. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships,
17(6), 791–810.
18 É. A. FEUGÉ ET AL.
Averett, P., Nalavany, B., & Ryan, S. (2009). An evaluation of gay/lesbian and heterosexual adoption.
Adoption Quarterly, 12(3–4), 129–151.
Barber, J. G., Delfabbro, P. H., & Cooper, L. (2001). The predictors of unsuccessful transition to foster
care. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42(6), 785–790.
Barth, R. P., & Needell, B. (1996). Outcomes for drug-exposed children four years post-adoption.
Children and Youth Services Review, 18(1–2), 37–56.
Bernier, A., Matte-Gagné, C., Bélanger, M. È., & Whipple, N. (2014). Taking stock of two decades of
attachment transmission gap: Broadening the assessment of maternal behavior. Child
Development, 85(5), 1852–1865.
Boisclair, A. (2000). Validation du tri-de-cartes des comportements maternels chez une population de
pères (Unpublished master’s thesis). Université Laval.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss, vol. 2: Separation. New York: Basic Books.
Evan B. Donaldson Adoption Institute; Brodzinsky, D. (2011). Expanding resources for children:
Research-based best practices in adoption by gays and lesbians. New York: Author.
Brown, G. L., McBride, B. A., Shin, N., & Bost, K. K. (2007). Parenting predictors of father-child
attachment. Fathering, 5, 197–219.
Cabrera, N. J., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bradley, R. H., Hofferth, S., & Lamb, M. E. (2000). Fatherhood in
the twenty-first century. Child Development, 71, 127–136.
Cadman, T., Diamond, P. R., & Fearon, R. P. (2018). Reassessing the validity of the attachment
Q-sort: An updated meta-analysis. Infant and Child Development, 27, 1–54.
Caldera, Y. M. (2004). Paternal involvement and infant-father attachment: A Q-set study. Fathering,
2, 191–210.
Colonnesi, C., Wissink, I. B., Noom, M. J., Asscher, J. J., Hoeve, M., Stams, G. J. J. M., & Kellaert-
Knol, M. G. (2013). Basic trust: An attachment-oriented intervention based on mind-mindedness
in adoptive families. Research on Social Work Practice, 23, 179–188.
Crowl, A., Ahn, S., & Baker, J. (2008). A meta-analysis of developmental outcomes for children of
same-sex and heterosexual parents. Journal of GLBT Family Studies, 4(3), 385–407.
Dozier, M., & Sepulveda, S. (2004). Foster mother state of mind and treatment use: Different
challenges for different people. Infant Mental Health, 25(4), 368–378.
Dozier, M, Stovall, K. C, Albus, K. E, & Bates, B. (2001). Attachment for infants in foster care: the role
of caregiver state of mind. Child Development, 72, 1467–1477.
Dubois-Comtois, K., Cyr, C., Vandal, C., & Moss, E. (2013). Le placement en famille d’accueil:
Vulnérabilité socioaffective de l’enfant et modèle d’intervention relationnelle. In C. Dufresne,
M. Provost, J. Lemelin, & A. Plamondon (Eds.), Le développement social de l’enfant (pp. 29–50).
Sainte-Foy: Les Presses de l’Université du Québec.
Dubois-Comtois, K., Bernier, A., Tarabulsy, G. M., Cyr, C., St-Laurent, D., Lanctôt, A.-S., & Béliveau, M.-
J. (2015). Behavior problems of children in foster care: Associations with foster mothers’
representations, commitment, and the quality of mother–Child interaction. Child Abuse &
Neglect, 48, 119–130.
Erich, S., Leung, P., & Kindle, P. (2005). A comparative analysis of adoptive family functioning with
gay, lesbian, and heterosexual parents and their children. Journal of GLBT Family Studies, 1,
43–60.
Farr, R. H., Forssell, S. L., & Patterson, C. J. (2010). Parenting and child development in adoptive
families: Does parental sexual orientation matter? Applied Developmental Science, 14,
164–178.
Farr, R. H., & Patterson, C. J. (2013). Coparenting among lesbian, gay, and heterosexual couples:
Associations with adopted children’s outcomes. Child Development, 84(4), 1226–1240.
Fearon, R. P., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Lapsley, A., & Roisman, G. I.
(2010). The significance of insecure attachment and disorganization in the development of
children’s externalizing behavior: A meta-analytic study. Child Development, 81, 435–456.
Feugé, É. A., Cossette, L., Cyr, C., & Julien, D. (2018). Parental involvement among adoptive gay
fathers: Associations with resources, time constraints, gender role, and child adjustment.
Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity, Advance online publication.
doi:10.1037/sgd0000299
ATTACHMENT & HUMAN DEVELOPMENT 19
Goldberg, A. E., Black, K. A., Manley, M. H., & Frost, R. (2017). “We told them that we are both really
involved parents”: Sexual minority and heterosexual adoptive parents’ engagement in school
communities. Gender and Education, 29(5), 614–631.
Goldberg, A. E., Smith, J. Z., & Perry-Jenkins, M. (2012). The division of labor in lesbian, gay, and
heterosexual new adoptive parents. Journal of Marriage and Family, 74, 812–828.
Golombok, S., Mellish, L., Jennings, S., Casey, P., Tasker, F., & Lamb, M. E. (2014). Adoptive gay
father families: Parent-child relationships and children’s psychological adjustment. Child
Development, 85(2), 456–468.
Goodsell, T. L., & Meldrum, J. T. (2010). Nurturing fathers: A qualitative examination of child-father
attachment. Early Child Development & Care, 180, 249–262.
Howe, D. (1997). Parent-reported problems in 211 adopted children: Some risk and protective
factors. Journal of Child Psychology Psychiatry, 38(4), 401–411.
Howes, C., Rodning, C., Galluzzo, D. C., & Myers, L. (1990). Attachment and child care: Relationships
with mother and caregiver. In N. Fox & G. G. Fein (Eds.), Infant day care: The current debate (pp.
169-182). Westport, CT, US: Ablex Publishing.
Hussey, D. L., & Guo, S. (2005). Characteristics and trajectories of treatment foster care youth. Child
Welfare, 84(4), 485–506.
Institut de la statistique du Québec. (2013). Québec handy numbers. Retrieved from http://www.
stat.gouv.qc.ca/quebec-chiffre-main/pdf/qcm2017_an.pdf
Jennings, S., Mellish, L., Tasker, F., Lamb, M. E., & Golombok, S. (2014). Why adoption? Gay, lesbian,
and heterosexual adoptive parents’ reproductive experiences and reasons for adoption.
Adoption Quarterly, 17(3), 205–226.
Juffer, F., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2009). International adoption comes of age: Development of
international adoptees from a longitudinal and meta-analytical perspective. In G. M. Wrobel &
E. Neil (Eds.), International advances in adoption research for practice (pp. 169–192). London:
John Wiley and Sons.
Julien, D. (2003). « L’adaptation des jeunes gais, lesbiennes ou personnes bisexuelles et de leurs
parents en contexte urbain et régional ». In D. Julien & J. J. Lévy (Eds.), Homosexualités –
Variations régionales (pp. 141–159). Québec: Presses de l’Université du Québec.
Kaniuk, J., Steele, M., & Hodges, J. (2004). Report on a longitudinal research project, exploring the
development of attachments between older, hard-to-place children and their adopters over the
first two years of placement. Adoption & Fostering, 28(2), 61–67.
Kerig, P. K. (Ed.). (2001). Introduction and overviews: Conceptual issues in family observational
research. London, UK: Erlbaum.
Kim, E. S., & Kim, B. S. (2009). The structural relationships of social support, mother’s psychological
status, and maternal sensitivity to attachment security in children with disabilities. Asia Pacific
Education Review, 10, 561–573.
L’Archevêque, A., & Julien, D. (2013). Intégration des identités homosexuelle et paternelle chez les
pères gays. Revue Canadienne Des Sciences Du Comportement, 45(1), 72–84.
Lamb, M. E. (1997). Fathers and child development: An introductory overview and guide. In
M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development (3rd ed., pp. 1–18). New York:
John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Lawrence, R. C., Carlson, A. E., & Egeland, B. (2006). The impact of foster care on development.
Development and Psychopathology, 18(1), 57–76.
Lucassen, N., Tharner, A., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Volling, B. L.,
Verhulst, F. C., … Tiemeier, H. (2011). The association between paternal sensitivity and
infant-father attachment security: A meta-analysis of three decades of research. Journal of
Family Psychology, 25(6), 986–992.
MacLean, K, & Gunion, M. (2003). Learning with care: The education of children looked after away
from home by local authorities in scotland. Adoption & Fostering Journal, 27(2), 20–31.
Madigan, S., Atkinson, L., Laurin, K., & Benoit, D. (2013). Attachment and internalizing behavior in
early childhood: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 49(4), 672–689.
20 É. A. FEUGÉ ET AL.
Miller, B. G., Kors, S., & Macfie, J. (2017). No differences? Meta-analytic comparisons of psycholo-
gical adjustment in children of gay fathers and heterosexual parents. Psychology of Sexual
Orientation and Gender Diversity, Advance online publication. doi:10.1037/sgd0000203
Miner, J. L., & Clarke-Stewart, A. (2008). Trajectories of externalizing behaviour from age 2 to age 9:
Relations with gender, temperament, ethnicity, parenting, and rater. Developmental Psychology,
44, 771–786.
Moran, G., Forbes, L., Evans, E., Tarabulsy, G. M., & Madigan, S. (2008). Both maternal sensitivity and
atypical maternal behavior independently predict attachment security and disorganization in
adolescent mother–Infant relationships. Infant Behavior and Development, 31(2), 321–325.
Moss, E., Cyr, C., Bureau, J.-F., Tarabulsy, G. M., & Dubois-Comtois, K. (2005). Stability of attachment
during the preschool period. Developmental Psychology, 41, 773–783.
Moss, E., Dubois-Comtois, K., Cyr, C., Tarabulsy, G. M., St.-Laurent, D., & Bernier, A. (2011). Efficacy of
a home-visiting intervention aimed at improving maternal sensitivity, child attachment, and
behavioral outcomes for maltreated children: A randomized control trial. Development and
Psychopathology, 23, 195–210.
Moss, E., Smolla, N., Cyr, C., Dubois-Comtois, K., Mazzarello, T., & Berthiaume, C. (2006). Attachment
and behavior problems in middle childhood as reported by adult and child informants.
Development and Psychopathology, 18(2), 425–444.
Newton, R, Litrownik, A. J, & Landsverk, J. A. (2000). Children and youth in foster care: disentan-
gling the relationship between problem behaviors and number of placements. Child Abuse &
Neglect, 24(10), 1363–1374.
Owens, E. B., & Shaw, D. S. (2003). Predicting growth curves of externalizing behavior across the
preschool years. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 575–590.
Pallanca, D. (2008). Les caractéristiques des mères d’accueil et leur niveau de sensibilité maternelle
dans le développement d’une nouvelle relation d’attachement chez les enfants placés
(Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Université du Québec à Montréal, Montréal, Canada.
Patterson, C. J. (2000). Family relationships of lesbian and gay men. Journal of Marriage and Family,
62(4), 1052–1069.
Patterson, C. J. (2017). Parents’ sexual orientation and children’s development. Child Development
Perspectives, 11, 45–49.
Pederson, D. R., Bailey, H. N., Tarabulsy, G. M., Bento, S., & Moran, G. (2014). Understanding
sensitivity: Lessons learned from the legacy of Mary Ainsworth. Attachment & Human
Development, 16(3), 261–270.
Pederson, D. R., Gleason, K., Moran, G., & Bento, S. (1998). Maternal attachment representations,
maternal sensitivity, and the infant-mother attachment relationship. Developmental Psychology,
34, 925–933.
Pederson, D. R., & Moran, G. (1995). A categorical description of infant-mother relationships in the
home and its relation to Q-sort measures of infant-mother interaction. Monographs of the
Society for Research in Child Development, 60(3). doi:10.2307/1166174
Pederson, D. R., & Moran, G. (1996). Expressions of the attachment relationship outside of the
strange situation. Child Development, 67(3), 915–927.
Petitclerc, A., & Tremblay, R. E. (2009). Childhood disruptive behaviour disorders: Review of their
origin, development, and prevention. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 54(4), 222–231.
Pierrhumbert, B., Muhlemann, I., Antonietti, J.-P., Sieye, A., & Halfon, O. (1995). Étude de validation
d’une version francophone du « Q-sort » d’attachement de Waters et Deane. Enfance, 48,
293–315.
Poitras, K, & Tarabulsy, G. (2017). Les contacts parent-enfant suite au placement en famille
substitut: liens avec la stabilité du placement. Enfances Familles Générations, 28, 1–26.
Poitras, K., Tarabulsy, G. M., St-Pierre, A., & Varela-Pulido, N. (2015, March). Contact with biological
parents following placement in foster care: Associations with internalization and externalization
symptoms. Poster presented at the Conference of the Society for Research in Child
Development, Philadelphia, PA.
Ryan, S. (2007). Parent-child interaction styles between gay and lesbian parents and their adopted
children. Journal of GLBT Family Studies, 3, 105–132.
ATTACHMENT & HUMAN DEVELOPMENT 21
Short, E., Riggs, W., Perlesz, A., Brown, R., & Kane, G. (2007). Lesbian, gay, bisexual and transgender
(LGBT) parented families: A literature review prepared for the Australian Psychological Society (pp.
29). Melbourne: Australian Psychological Society.
Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E. A., & Collins, W. A. (2005). The development of the person: The
Minnesota study of risk and adaptation from birth to adulthood. New York: Guilford.
Steele, M., Hodges, J., Kaniuk, J., Steele, H., Hillman, S., & Asquith, K. (2008). Forecasting outcomes
in previously maltreated children: The use of the AAI in a longitudinal adoption study. In
H. Steele & M. Steele (Eds.), Clinical applications of the adult attachment interview (pp.
427–451). New York: Guilford Press.
Stovall, K. C., & Dozier, M. (2000). The development of attachment in new relationships: Single
subject analyses for ten foster infants. Development and Psychopathology, 12(2), 133–156.
Tarabulsy, G., Provost, M., Deslandes, L., St-Laurent, D., Moss, E., Lemelin, J.-P., & Dassylva, J.-F.
(2003). Individual differences in infant still face response at 6 months. Infant Behavior and
Development, 26, 421–438.
Tarabulsy, G. M., Avgoustis, E., Phillips, J., Pederson, D. R., & Moran, G. (1997). Similarities and
differences in mothers’ and observers’ descriptions of attachment behaviors. International
Journal of Behavioral Development, 21, 599–619.
Tarabulsy, G. M., Provost, M. A., Bordeleau, S., Trudel-Fitzgerald, C., Moran, G., Pederson, D. R., …
Pierce, T. (2009). Validation of a short version of the maternal behavior Q-set applied to a brief
video record of mother-infant interaction. Infant Behavior and Development, 32(1), 132–136.
Tarren-Sweeney, M. (2008). Retrospective and concurrent predictors of the mental health of
children in care. Children and Youth Services Review, 30(1), 1–25.
Tasker, F. (2005). Lesbian mothers, gay fathers, and their children: A review. Journal of
Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 26, 224–240.
Tornello, S. L., Farr, R. H., & Patterson, C. J. (2011). Predictors of parenting stress among gay
adoptive fathers in the United States. Journal of Family Psychology, 25(4), 591–600.
Trautmann-Villalba, P., Gschwendt, M., Schmidt, M. H., & Laucht, M. (2006). Father-infant interac-
tion patterns as pre- cursors of children’s later externalizing behavior problems: A longitudinal
study over 11 years. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 256(6), 344–349.
Van Bakel, H.J.A, & Riksen-Walraven, J.M.A. (2004). AQS security scores; what do they represent? A
study in construct validation. Infant Mental Health Journal, 25, 175–193.
van den Dries, L., Juffer, F., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2009). Fostering
security? A meta-analysis of attachment in adopted children. Children and Youth Services Review,
31, 410–421.
van IJzendnorn, M. H., & Sagi, A. (1999). Cross-cultural patterns of attachment: Universal and
contextual dimensions. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory,
research, and clinical applications (pp. 713–734). New York: Guilford Press.
van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Juffer, F. (2006). The Emmanuel Miller memorial lecture 2006. Adoption as
intervention. Meta-analytic evidence for massive catch-up and plasticity in physical,
socio-emotional, and cognitive development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47,
1228–1245.
van IJzendoorn, M. H., Sagi, A., & Lambermon, M. W. E. (1992). The multiple caretaker paradox:
Data from Holland and Israel. New Directions for Child Development, 57, 5–24.
van IJzendoorn, M. H., Vereijken, C. M. J. L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Riksen-Walraven, J. M.
(2004). Assessing attachment security with the attachment Q Sort: Meta-analytic evidence for
the validity of the observer AQS. Child Development, 75(4), 1188–1213.
Varela Pulido, N. (2016). Expérience de la position kangourou par rapport au stress et la sensibilité
ultérieure des pères de bébés prématurés (Doctoral dissertation). Université du Québec à
Montréal, Montréal, Canada.
Vecho, O., & Schneider, B. (2005). Homoparentalité et développement de l’enfant: Bilan de 30 ans
de publication. Psychiatrie de l’enfant, 68(1), 271–328.
Waters, E., & Deane, K. E. (1985). Defining and assessing individual differences in attachment
relationships: Q-methodology and the organization of behavior in infancy and early childhood.
22 É. A. FEUGÉ ET AL.
In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points of attachment theory and research. Monographs
of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50 (1–2, Serial No. 209)
Weinfield, N. S., Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., & Carlson, E. (2008). Individual differences in infant-
caregiver attachment: Conceptual and empirical aspects of security. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver
(Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 78–101).
New York: Guilford Press.
Zeegers, M. A. J., Colonnesi, C., Stams, G.-J.-J. M., & Meins, E. (2017). Mind matters: A meta-analysis
on parental mentalization and sensitivity as predictors of infant–Parent attachment.
Psychological Bulletin, 143(12), 1245–1272.