Female Competition-Causes, Constraints, Content, and Contexts
Female Competition-Causes, Constraints, Content, and Contexts
net/publication/8492669
CITATIONS READS
167 1,955
1 author:
Anne Campbell
Durham University
89 PUBLICATIONS 4,720 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Anne Campbell on 06 February 2017.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted
digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about
JSTOR, please contact [email protected].
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
http://about.jstor.org/terms
Taylor & Francis, Ltd. is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Journal
of Sex Research
This content downloaded from 129.234.252.65 on Thu, 15 Sep 2016 11:47:05 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Female Competition: Causes, Constraints, Content, and Contexts
Anne Campbell
Durham University
Monogamy tends to equalise mate competition between the sexes. However, women show greater restraint in th
direct intrasexual aggression, which, I argue, is a result of their higher parental investment and the consequent
reproductive cost of injury or death. Women usually compete for mates by advertising qualities valued by men (b
sexual exclusiveness) and by using indirect means of denigrating rivals (through gossip and stigmatisation). Howev
well-resourced men are in short supply, women mustfind alternative sources of support or escalate their compe
male partners to physical levels. Data from criminology, psychology, evolutionary biology, and anthropology ar
support these proposals.
In this article I offer an interpretation of female compe- "raging misogyny." As Tanenbaum (2002, p. 47) puts it,
tition from an evolutionary perspective. First, it is useful to "Many women compete over things they think men value,
briefly review prior social science research (not informed such as looking sexy .... The most dangerous outcome of
by such a perspective) to indicate the richness of the qual- this is self hatred; girls and women disparage themselves
itative observations and the alternative positions taken to and dissociate from other females."
their interpretation. The present article sees competition as an inherent part
Despite a recent surge of popular journalistic books (e.g., of our biological status and women's lesser willingness to
Fillion, 1997; Simmons, 2002; Tanenbaum, 2002), academ- escalate competition to direct aggression as arising out of
ic interest in competition among women was almost nonex- their particular biology rather than from conformity to cul-
istent until the 1980s. Initial research (Gilligan, 1982; tural expectations of femininity. Because the vast majority
Goodwin, 1980; Lever, 1976) found that girls tended to of research has been done on young women in the United
avoid competition in favour of tactics that diffuse conflict States and Europe, we lack the data to examine the cultur-
and preserve intelpersonal harmony. When competition is al specificity or generality of female competition.
made inevitable, girls used apologies and excuses to miti- Certainly sex differences in aggression are universal (Daly
gate their behaviour (Hughes, 1.988) or "double voicing" to & Wilson, 1988), but competition can take other forms.
promote their own cases while simultaneously taking into Some work suggests that competition is more direct and
account the positions of their rivals, thereby preserving their physical among poor and minority women than among
relationships (Sheldon, 1992). This attenuation of competi- their middle-class White counterparts (Brown, 1998; Eder,
tion in favour of sustaining positive relationships is thought 1990). However, this could be due to culture-specific gen-
to reflect socialisation into cultural norms against the overt der expectations or greater competition resulting from
expression of conflict among females (Miner & Longino, higher levels of resource scarcity (as I discuss later).
1987; Tracy, 1991) and the greater centrality of intimate Although there is academic agreement on the foci of
friendships to girls than to boys (Brown, 1998). female competition, women's concern with relative attrac-
Research that has examined the focus of female compe- tiveness might result from the internalisation of patriarchal
tition identifies appearance, popularity, and preservation values or from mate competition. Again, cross-cultural
of a "good" sexual reputation as central (Brown, 1998; data are needed. Problematically, men (and women) uni-
Eder, 1985; Merten, 1997; Simmons, 2002; Tanenbaum, versally seem to agree on standards of female facial beau-
2002). These are intimately connected since popularity ty, making it hard to choose between the two accounts
(which consists of "visibility" rather than liking; see Eder, (Langlois et al., 2000). Research certainly suggests that the
1985; Merten, 1997) is associated with physical attractive- current fashion for slimness is not imposed on women by
ness to the opposite sex (often reflected, in the United men because men prefer plumper figures than do women
States, in achieving cheerleader status) but highly selective (Anderson, Crawford, Nadeau, & Lindberg, 1992; Cohn et
sexual availability. Girls, it is argued, come to "ventrilo- al., 1987; Fallon & Rozin, 1985; Furnham & Radley,
quise" patriarchal male attitudes about appropriate female 1989). Women also care more about other women's opin-
appearance and behaviour (Brown, 1998), resulting in ions of attractiveness than those of men (Graziano, Jensen-
Campbell, Shebilske, & Lundgren, 1993), suggesting that
within-sex competition can take on a dynamic of its own.
Address correspondence to Anne Campbell, Psychology Department,
Durham University, South Road, Durham, DH1 3LE, England; e-mail: a.c.camp- Similarly, with regard to sexual conduct and reputation, a
bell @ durham.ac.uk. recent review concluded that women are stronger
The Journal of Sex Research Volume 41, Number 1, February 2004: pp. 16-26 61
This content downloaded from 129.234.252.65 on Thu, 15 Sep 2016 11:47:05 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Campbell
17
enforcers of the double standard than are men (Baumeister family and where male investment is necessary to ensure
& Twenge, 2002), casting doubt on the proposal of inter- child survival. Such conditions are common; in societies
nalisation of male values. where polygyny is legal more than 80% of men marry
I turn now to an evolutionary approach to the under- monogamously (Murdock, 1981). Socially imposed
standing of female-female competition. Conflict can and monogamy is characteristic of large stratified societies
does occur between the sexes; indeed women's rates of (Draper & Harpending, 1988). Because it equalises repro-
aggression (excluding homicide) toward partners equal ductive opportunities among men, it reduces male compe-
those of men (Archer, 2000). Because the theoretical pre- tition (Betzig, 1995; Ridley, 1993; Smuts, 1995).
dictions and the foci of conflict are quite different, I do not Human cultures and individuals show marked variabil-
consider them in the present article. ity in their marriage patterns (Alexander, 1979).
Monogamy tends to benefit the majority of men while
CAUSES OF FEMALE COMPETITION imposing costs on the minority who through wealth
Sex differences in parental investment form the backbone (Betzig, 1986) or genetic quality (Gangestad & Simpson,
of evolutionary accounts of sexual selection (Williams, 2000) could feasibly improve their reproductive success
1966). Parental investment is any investment by the parent by polygyny. Effective polygyny exists where male fitness
in an offspring that increases the chance of its survival at variance exceeds that of females and it can be achieved via
the cost of the parent's ability to invest in other offspring serial monogamy. Serially monogamous men produce
(Trivers, 1972). The higher investing sex becomes the more children then men who remain in a single partner-
resource for which the other sex competes. In 95Wo of ship, but the same is not true for women (Forsberg &
mammals, females provide all the parental care (Clutton- Tullberg, 1995). Men who marry twice are more likely to
Brock, 1991). Consequently, males compete vigorously have children by both wives than are women to have chil-
for status and resources attractive to females. dren by both their husbands (Alexander, 1979). At the
Human sexual dimorphism suggests selection for male- margins of monogamy, then, successful men leave more
male competition congruent with a history of mild polyg- children than women. As Archer and Mehdikahni (in
yny. In common with other polygynous primates, men press) summarise the situation,
compared to women have broader canines (Frayer &
Wolpoff, 1985) and are heavier (McHenry, 1994). Boys ...the sex difference in size in humans is relatively small compared
with that found in primates with a clearly polygynous mating strat-
reach physical maturity later than girls and after puberty
egy. Paternal investment is relatively high in humarls (Geary,
have larger hearts, skeletal muscles, lung capacity, lower 2000), which would make them in some ways nearer to monoga-
resting heart rate and are capable of longer bouts of phys- mous species, while still retaining a tendency towards polygyny.
ical exertion (Tanner, 1990). In consequence they can run
faster, jump further, grip more strongly, and throw faster Monogamy and biparental care reduce fitness variabili-
and further (Thomas & French, 1985). While these sex dif- ty among males. In pure form, they constrain a man's
ferences might reflect division of labour and specialisation reproductive success to that of his partner. Given the heavy
for hunting (Wood & Eagly, 2002), across species they commitment that he will make in their joint progeny, it
appear to be more strongly related to the degree of male- pays a male to be choosy. (Such choosiness does not apply
male competition (Placvan & van Schaik, 1997a, 1997b). to short-term sexual relationships; men are willing to drop
Nonetheless, the majority of men today marry monoga- their standards quite considerably when no investment is
mously. In Western societies at least, both men and women required of them [Kenrick, Sadalla, Groth, & Trost,
value monogamy over short-term affairs (Miller, Putcha- 1990].) Monogamy means that sexual selection acts on
Bhagavatula, & Pedersen, 2002). When Pedersen, Miller, both males and females. The investment costs sustained by
Putcha-Bhagavatula, & Yang (2002) asked participants to both parties support discrimination in partner choice and
indicate the number of partners preferred over the next 30 mean that women must compete with one another to
years, the higher mean for males reported by Buss and secure the best men, just as men vie for the best woman.
Schmitt (1993) was replicated but, crucially, the male dis- "In theory and in practice, the dynamics of human mating
tribution was very heavily skewed. When median values involve female-female competition and male choice, in
were examined, both sexes showed a preference for one addition to male-male competition and female choice"
partner and did not differ in the number of partners they (Geary, 1998, p. 121).
preferred prior to settling down.
There is debate as to the origins of human monogamy. CONSTRAINTS ON FEMALE COMPETITION
Monogamy may have been the result of male-female Why then do women so rarely exhibit the kind of overt,
coevolution of reproductive strategies, initiated by female sometimes lethal physical competitiveness among their
preference for investing males (Geary, 2000) resulting own ranks that men do? The classic explanation of height-
from the protracted period of human infant dependency ened intramale violence is predicated upon polygyny
(Miller & Fishkin, 1997). Ecologically imposed incentive: Males are competing for status and resources
monogamy occurs where harsh conditions prevent many which are associated with the prize of fathering a dispro-
men from acquiring the resources to support more than one portionate number of children (Daly & Wilson, 1988).
This content downloaded from 129.234.252.65 on Thu, 15 Sep 2016 11:47:05 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Female Competition
18
Low levels of female aggression are explained in terms of that the relevant adaptation occurs at an emotional lev
an absence of incentive for competition: Because males (Damasio, 1994; Loewenstein, Weber, Hsee, & Welch,
are willing to inseminate women promiscuously (where 2001). Women's avoidance of danger is mediated by fear
little or no paternal care is required of them) women have and associated behavioural inhibition. This may explain
no need to compete for copulations. why women show higher rates of phobias and anxiety dis-
A complementary view of sex differences in aggression orders than men (Arrindell, Kolk, Pickersgill, & Hageman,
in terms of reproductive costs focuses upon parenting rather 1993), lower levels of sensation-seeking and risk-taking
than mating (Campbell, 1999, 2002). This proposal argues (Zuckerman, 1994), and lower engagement in dangerous
that the chief difference between the sexes lies in the costs sports and occupations (Wilson & Daly, 1985). Women are
rather than the rewards of within-sex aggression. The sex less willing to risk their lives than men.
difference in aggression is found among nonhuman pri- The emotional rather than rational nature of this effect
mates also (Smuts, 1987), despite the fact that there are clear is confirmed by studies of risk. When studies of risk are
advantages to dominant females. They reach sexual maturi- dichotomised into those that are cognitive and abstract
ty earlier, first conceive at an earlier age, produce more off- (e.g., what would the odds of success have to be before
spring, have greater infant survival and live longer (Ellis, you would agree to an operation?) and those that employ
1995; Pusey, Williams, & Goodall, 1997). The crucial fac- immediate, emotion-based behavioural measures (e.g.,
tor is that in these female-bonded species (which include distance of nearest approaching car in relation to willing-
most primates but not humans; Rodseth, Wrangham, ness to make a turn across oncoming traffic), the sex dif-
Harrigan, & Smuts, 1991), maternal rank is inherited rather ferences are more marked in the latter (Byrnes, Miller, &
than fought for. When direct challenge does occur, it does so Schafer, 1999). The emotional basis of the sex difference
under "minimal risk" conditions (Chapais, 1992). appears also in experimental studies of aggression. Women
Competition between women, as between men, is won perceive the danger associated with an act of aggression as
in the currency of inclusive fitness. In men, inclusive fit- higher than men even in the same objective situation, and
ness depends crucially upon sexual access, but in women this perceived danger is a strong negative predictor of
the critical factor is the mother's ability to shepherd her aggression (Bettencourt & Miller, 1996; Eagly & Steffen,
offspring safely through the dangerous selection funnel of 1986). It acts as a "brake" on aggression. In addition, psy-
the juvenile period. In nonhuman primates, 70Wo to 90Wo of chopharmacological studies suggest that men's willing-
young born die before adulthood. In hunter-gatherer soci- ness to engage in risky and dangerous behaviours may be
eties, approximately 50Wo of offspring born survive mediated not by the incentives but by an absence of inhi-
(Kaplan & Lancaster, 2003). This higher survival rate bition. The neurotransmitter serotonin is implicated in
among humans is particular impressive because of the behavioural inhibition, and low levels have been linked
doubling of time spent in the most perilous phase of all, as both to impulsive killings and to suicide (Moore, Scarpa,
juveniles. Central to the survival of the young are the & Raine, 2002). In addition, there is a sex difference favor-
choices and competence of the mother, who delivers the ing women in the availability and uptake of this transmit-
bulk of direct care (Hrdy, 1999). It is not surprising there- ter (Biver et al., 1996; Reisert & Pilgrim, 1991).
fore that even under biparental care, the death of a mother One way women can compete without risking the* safe-
compromises a child's life more severely than the death of ty or compromising their lives is through acts that ostracise,
a father (Hill & Hurtado, 1996; Sear, Mace, & McGregor, stigmatise, and otherwise exclude others from social interac-
2000; Voland, 1988). A careless attitude to one's own safe- tion without risking direct physical confrontation. Such acts
ty and survival has greater consequences on the reproduc- do not eliminate or physically injure the target, nor do they
tive success of a female than a male. Hence, selection has demonstrate the greater size, strength, or belligerence of the
favored females who avoid danger because of the higher attacker. They do, however, inflict stress and diminish the
fitness costs of risking their lives. opponent's reputation and social support. The target is
Parenthetically, it is worth noting that while human attacked circuitously and the aggressor can therefore remain
females deliver the bulk of parental care, the critical factor unidentified. This set of behaviours is referred to as indirect
in this argument is not sex per se. Allman, Rosin, Kumar, aggression (Bjorkqvist, Lagerspetz, & Kaukiainen, 1992) or
and Hasenstaub (1998) compared ten species of primates relational aggression (Crick & Grotpeter, 1995). Girls
that varied in the amount of paternal or maternal effort exceed boys on measures of indirect aggression by the age of
they contributed to parental care. They plotted the survival 11, and the sex difference continues to be present up to the
curves for each species and found significant sex differ- age of 18. As male physical aggression diminishes with age,
ences in life expectancy favouring whichever sex took the sex difference in indirect aggression lessens and disap-
most parental responsibility. They also ranked the primates pears in adulthood at least among educated middle-class
in terms of which sex lived longer and by how much. The samples (Pellegrini & Archer, in press). These stigmatising
results showed that the amount of parenting was closely and excluding strategies can have devastating effects upon
associated with female-to-male survival ratio. the victim (Ahmad & Smith, 1994; Simmons, 2002).
But evolutionists do not argue that reward-cost deci- An alternative interpretation of these sex differences
sions are calculated rationally or oonsciously. It is likely might be that women's preference for indirect strategies is
This content downloaded from 129.234.252.65 on Thu, 15 Sep 2016 11:47:05 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Campbell ls
a result of gender role prescription. Because women's tance assigned to other attributes (Buss, 2000; Buss &
direct aggression is seen as an aberration from the female Schmitt, 1993; Etcoff, 1999; Feingold, l 990).
stereotype, women might seek alternative means of
Youth and 13eaut
expressing competition that are more acceptable.
Osterman et al. (1994) reasoned that if a form of aggres- A universal feature of men's preferences is youth. After a
sion was socially condemned, then children would be period of adolescent sterility, women become most fertile
more likely to attribute it to their peers and less likely toat the age of 25 from whence their fertility declines until it
admit to it themselves. They asked children to report on reaches zero by the age of about 50. When adult males are
their own and their friends' use of various forms of asked about age preference, they consistently choose
aggression and then computed an Attributional someone who is younger than themselves, and marriage
Discrepancy Index by subtracting self-estimates from patterns indicate that the typical age gap is about 3 years
peer estimates for various forms of aggression. There was (Buss & Schmitt, 1993). But this 3-year gap is not a con-
no sex difference for indirect aggression This suggests stant. Teenage boys rate a woman 5 years older than them-
that stigmatising and rumour spreading are no more selves as the perfect partner (Kenrick, Keefe, Gabrielidis,
socially acceptable for one sex than for the other. Another& Cornelius, 1996). As men age they prefer ever younger
clue to the fact that indirect aggression is not simply the women, and by the age of 60 they prefer women who are
result of human socialisation is that other primate femaleson average 15 years younger than themselves (Kenrick &
use analogous techniques. Females have been observed to Keefe, 1992). Buunk, Dijkstra, Kenrick, and Warntjes
engage in sustained, low-level harassment of other (2001), however, found that men's expressed preference
females; the target is consistently interrupted and dis- may be constrained by realistic pragmatism and the type of
placed as she tries to rest, feed, or mate. The resulting relationship. In line with earlier work they found that 60-
stress suppresses oestrus and can cause abortion (Chapais, year-old men fantasise about and would have casual sex
1992; Hrdy, 1981; Smuts, 1987), diminishing the repro- with women up to their 40s, but would be prepared to
ductive success of the victim. marry a woman up to the age of 55.
While the term indirect aggression emphasises the Men also place a greater premium upon physical attrac-
indirect nature of the attack and relational aggression tiveness than do women, and this is closely bound up with
highlights the manipulation of social re]ationships, both age. Facial features that reflect youth inc]ude shiny hair,
functions come together in the activity of"gossiping," or unwrinkled skin, large eyes, a small nose, and full lips.
social evaluation about a person who is not present (Eder (Etcoff, 1999). A youthful, beautiful appearance is what
& Enke, 1991). Gossiping has been argued variously to women compete with each other to achieve. Historically,
disseminate information, entertain, establish norms, exer- women have used lead, mercury, lemon juice, egg whites,
cise social control, enhance in-group bonding, and act as milk, vinegar, kohl, and dye to enhance their facial fea-
a psychodynamic projective defence (Fine & Rosnow, tures. In the United States, 88% of women over the age of
1978; Leaper & Holliday, 1995; Nevo, Nevo, & Derech- 18 wear makeup designed to correct asymmetries, signal
Zehavi, 1993). But it is also a potent form of indirect com- sexuality, and mimic youth (Etcoff, 1999). Eighty-nine
petition. When we gossip about another person we simul- percent of cosmetic surgical procedures in the United
taneously achieve two goals. Most obviously, we spread States are performed on women, including 91% of face
information that is damaging to the other's reputation and lifts (Etcoff, 1999).
so diminish his or her social standing. But the act of con- Tooke and Camire (1991) asked men and women about
demnation is also an act of self-promotion; one cannot deceptive tactics used to compete with rivals and attract
credibly accuse a rival of behaviours that one engages inthe opposite sex. While men competed with other men by
oneself. If gossip is a form of competition, then women exaggerating superiority, promiscuity, intensity, and popu-
should gossip about the domains about which they are larity, women competed with other women by alterations
to their appearance, such as makeup, nail polish, fake tans,
most competitive and those, in turn, should be those qual-
ities that are important to men in their choice of mates.and tight clothing (see also Buss, 1 988a, 1 988b). Walters
The following discussion considers competition among and Crawford (1994) asked undergraduate subjects about
young women at a time of intense competition for mates. competitive tactics that they used against members of their
Most published material is gained from this age group. own sex. No explicit mention was made of competition for
Little is known about the foci of competition in older mates, but nonetheless women most often nominated, per-
women, although this would be a valuable and fruitful formed, and rated as effective the tactic of attracting atten-
tion to their -appearance. Using diaries to examine the
w .
area oz enqu
everyday experience of competition, Cashdan ( 1998)
THE CONTE
found that while men competed with other men in the
In seeking long-term mates, men and women similarly arena of sports, women competed with one another in
award considerable importance to personal qualities such terms of their appearance. Attractiveness appears to be the
as intelligence and a sense of humour (Buss & Schmitt, currency of female competition even when no mention is
1993). However, men differ from women in the impor- made of what the competition is about.
This content downloaded from 129.234.252.65 on Thu, 15 Sep 2016 11:47:05 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
20 Female Competition
This content downloaded from 129.234.252.65 on Thu, 15 Sep 2016 11:47:05 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Campbell 21
placed the girl in danger of developing a sexual reputation. the falsity of the accusation. If she publicly confronts the
The girls themselves were vocal in enforcing this code. boy she is alleged to have had sex with, he has every rea-
"The girls regulated their contact with other girls who son to lie and it is his word against hers. If she refuses to
were known as 'lays' in order to preserve their own repu- rise to the bait and respond to the remark, she is taken to
tations. In fact they openly ridiculed the girls referring to have tacitly admitted the truth of the accusation. If she
them as 'whores"' (Wilson, 1978, p. 70). Fifteen years admits the act but repudiates the double standard used
after this study, teenagers were still alert to the distinction against her, what others view as her "free and easy" atti-
between nice girls and tarts and avoided friendships with tude to sex may further damage her reputation. The best
sexually available girls for fear of reputation-by-associa- she can do is to forcefully repel anyone who labels her as
tion (Lees, 1993). Boys continued to make the same dis- a tart and so minimise the likelihood of such a reputation
tinction between prospective wives ("Not someone who's attack being repeated.
been out with people I know"; Lees, 1993, p. 139) and
slags ("You wouldn't go out with her, you would just THE CONTEXTS OF FEMALE COMPETITION
knock her ofr'; Duncan, 1999, p. 54). Evolutionary accounts are sensitive to the fact that any
Indeed, girls themselves actively collude in enforcing evolved mechanism interacts with environmental factors.
the double standard not only through distancing them- With respect to mate competition, many variables affect a
selves from "easy" girls but through gossip and rumour young woman's strategy, including her family structure
spreading (Coleman, 1961; Du Bois-Reymond & and circumstances (Belsky, Steinberg, & Draper, 1991),
Ravesloot, 1996). Buss and Dedden's (1990) study found age, sexual maturity, and mate value (Campbell, 1995).
that young women were judged more likely than men to Here I briefly consider how one ecological factor (resource
question a rival's fidelity and to draw attention to her availability) can alter reproductive timing and the intensi-
promiscuity. Nor is such gossip about sexual reputation ty of female competition.
confined to "nice" middle-class girls (Millhausen & As we have noted, women require assistance in raising
Herold, 1999). Members of street gangs in deprived inner- children and all the more so in contemporary Western
city areas show the same concern (Campbell, 1984; nations where an artificial barrier has been erected
Hanna, 1999). between women's roles as mothers and as workers. As
Hrdy (1999) points out, women in traditional hunter-gath-
[T]he girls have very distinct notions and expectations of other
erer societies are able to forage locally and to care for their
female members' appearance and conduct that are clearly tied to
their sexual reputation.... At times, they can be more judgemen-
children at the same time. The requirement that women
tal regarding other girls' respectability than their male counter- now abandon their children and travel to a child-free site
parts...we find gang girls spending a great deal of energy 'bitch- to work for 8 hours a day has created a situation that ben-
ing' or casting doubt on others' reputations. This cross-cultural efits neither mothers nor children. The problem has result-
process operates not only as a mechanism of social control, but
ed in a bifurcation of women's reproductive strategies.
also of distancing and confirming one's own reputation. (Joe
Laidler & Hunt, 2001, p. 668)
One route is to delay childbirth, a choice often made by
those whose income is high enough to guarantee future
Feminists have struggled to understand the continuing advantages for their offspring (private schooling, tertiary
potency of accusations of sexual accessibility. With the education). But among women facing minimum wage
inequity of the sexual double standard acknowledged, "slut" employment or survival on state benefits, delayed repro-
should have lost its power long ago. But even today, young duction promises no economic benefits for their children.
women are deeply offended by such accusations, and eman- Early childbearing has positive advantages (Geronimus,
cipated attempts to turn the same accusation of excessive 1996). Adult mortality is one of the strongest predictors of
sexual experience on men simply provoke laughter (Duncan, reproductive timing (Low, 2000), and in poor Black popu-
1999, p. 53). Unlike young women, sexual conquests lations in the United States, mortality and morbidity are
enhance rather than detract from a young man's reputation. high. With age, poor health increases the difficulties of
When physical fights do occur among young women, both conceiving and raising a child. A woman who gives
they are frequently a response to gossip spread about a birth at a younger age also improves the likelihood that her
girl's sexual reputation (Campbell, 1982; Duncan, 1999; own mother will be alive to provide assistance. By the age
Marsh & Paton, 1986; Owens et al., 2000). As Lees (1993, of 20, the probability of a Black woman's mother being
p. 267) notes alive drops to only 40%, compared to 75% during the
teenage years (see Low, 2000).
...a girl reacts by denying the accusation rather than by objecting But why do so many young women in poor communi-
to the use of the category. For them what is important is to prove
ties rely on maternal assistance? First, there is a severe
that you are not a slag: what they unquestioningly accept is the
legitimacy of the category of slag. In other words, the category
shortage of men. In the 20 to 29 age group there are 85
has uncontested status. Black men for every 100 women, compared to 99 among
Whites (U.S. Bureau of the Census, l995). The ratio has
Such accusations place girls in a strategically awkward sit- been worsening since the 1920s, and the current situation
uation. It is virtually impossible for a girl to demonstrate is a confluence of high mortality and incarceration rates
This content downloaded from 129.234.252.65 on Thu, 15 Sep 2016 11:47:05 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
22 Female Competition
(Tucker & Mitchell-Kernan, 1995). Second, young women see, I am just with him because that is the environment that I am
in, and I am trying to get out of this environment. I would like to
are understandably reluctant to marry men who have few
have a man. Not no nigger that wants to beat me and makes me
material benefits to offer (Cherlin, 1981). Between 1970 make money all the time and give it to him. I want somebody you
and 1990, the proportion of Black women who had ever know that cares about me and loves me, and loves my kids and
married dropped from 83% to 63%. The high proportion of helps me raise them, and give them a good family." (Joe Laidler
births to Black single mothers (which rose from 42% to & Hunt, 2001, p. 674)
response to such situations (e.g., Anderson, 1981; Secord, 1983). Cross culturally teenage girls are more like-
Glasgow, 1981; Wilson, 1987). Unable to effectively con- ly to become pregnant where there is a shortage of men
tribute in the home and viewing it as a female preserve, the (Barber, 2000). Although women may succeed in extract-
men pass their days in the streets talking, drinking, and ing short-term resources in return for sex with "high
attempting to salvage some pride if only in their ability rollers" (Taylor, 1993), these men are unlikely to remain
to attract women despite their conspicuous absence of and assist with child-rearing, forcing women back to their
resources. Divorce rates among Black Americans are high- mothers for practical and financial assistance.
er than among other ethnic groups (Norton & Glick, 1979). A second consequence is that intensified female compe-
In 1990, there were 358 divorces per thousand among tition, usually managed by display and indirect aggression,
Black women compared to 166 among U.S. women as a can escalate to physical levels. O'Brien (1988) generated
whole (Tucker & Mitchell-Kernan, 1997). The lower the predictions about the expected distribution of male-on-male
rate of male employment, the higher the rate of female- and female-on-female crime based on the proportion of each
headed households with children (Sampson, 1995). sex in the population and the sex of violent offenders. He
Anxiety about the ability to provide has been found to be found that women commit simple and aggravated assault
a strong contributor to marital instability among African against other women more often than expected by demo-
Americans (Hatchett, Veroff, & Douvan, 1995). However,
graphic predictions, especially in instances of simple
when the effects of poverty and family size are controlled,
assault. An examination of juvenile female crime in the
Black partners are less likely to separate than Whites
United States found that the majority of victims of violence
(Hampton, 1975). This underlines the central importance
(70 - 75%) were other females predominantly of similar age
of economic rather than cultural factors in mating tactics.
to their attackers (Bureau of Justice Statistics, 1999; Federal
Black men earn less, are unemployed more often, and
Bureau of Investigation, n.d.), and this is equally true in
occupy low status positions all of which make them eco-
Britain (Home Office Research and Planning Unit, 1995, p.
nomically dispensable (Wilson, 1987). With economically
8). A number of studies (Campbell, 1986a; George, 1999;
supportive males thin on the ground, young women often
Home Office Research and Planning Unit, 1993; Home
seek support from female kin (Apfel & Seitz, 1996;
Leadbeater, Way, & Raden, 1996). Office Statistical Bulletin, 1996) concur that female-female
But this strategy seems to be a second-best choice. fights are usually between young similarly aged women
Tucker and Mitchell-Kernan (1997, p. 2) note that the (15-24 years old) who are acquainted. They occur chiefly in
"striking decline in marriage entry among Blacks has not drinking establishments or in the streets and involve non-
been accompanied by a devaluing of marriage as an insti- weapon, hand-to-hand tactics such as pushing, shoving,
tution, but rather recognition of constraints on the ability grabbing, tripping, slapping, kicking, and punching.
to marry." Ethnographic studies of young underclass But what are they fighting about? Campbell (1986a)
women affirm that their ideal situation is a solvent and sta- found that the most common category (accounting for 46%
ble husband (Campbell, 1984; Miller, 1986). Street hus- of fights) was an attack on the girl's personal integrity
tlers and gang members, far from rejecting traditional which included instances where there had been allegations
roles, continue to aspire to a situation where they can raise about the girl's promiscuity, false accusations, or pejora-
their children in economic and emotional security. tive gossiping behind her back. The next most common
category was loyalty, in which the girl fought to defend the
We are looking for working men basically. Men that want to work
name of a friend or relative who had been the butt of an
and are going to be responsible. Like if they get us pregnant we
want them to stay with us. Whereas the men that we are left with integrity attack. The third most common category was
are the street niggers that wear gold rings, wear gerry curls. You Jealousy about a romantic partner (12%).
This content downloaded from 129.234.252.65 on Thu, 15 Sep 2016 11:47:05 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
23
Campbell
Violence occurs disproportionately among the under- appearance and guarding their sexual reputation, since this
. . . . .
twin questions that have vexed traditional psychology are Allman, J., Rosin, A., Kumar, R., & Hasenstaub, A. ( 1998). Parenting and
survival in anthropoid primates: Caretakers live longer. Proceedings of
how to explain women's lesser aggression relative to men the National Academy of Science, 95, 6866-6869.
and how to offer an account of the circumstances under Anderson, E. (1981). A place on the corner. London: Chicago University
which women can and do use aggression. I have argued for Press.
a single model that can explain both effects. Women Anderson, J. L., Crawford, C. B., Nadeau, J., & Lindberg, T. (1992). Was
the Duchess of Windsor right? A cross-cultural review of the socioecol-
aggress less frequently than men because their threshold ogy of ideals of female body shape. Ethology and Sociobiolo,gn 13,
for the expression of competition as overt aggression is set 1 97-227.
higher, and this is an evolved adaptation that has served to Apfel, N., & Seitz, H. (1996). African American adolescent mothers, their
families and their daughters: A longitudinal perspective over twelve
increase women's reproductive success. Women can and
years. ln B. J. R. Leadbeater & N. Way (Eds.), Urban girls: Resisting
do use physical aggression where the competition for stereot>pes, creating identities (pp. 149-170). London: New York
reproductively relevant resources becomes extreme. This University Press.
model directs attention to the specifically female costs and Archer, J. (2000). Sex differences in aggression between heterosexual part-
ners: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 651-680.
benefits of "crossing the line" into overt aggression, and in
Archer, J., & Mehdikhani, M. (in press). Variability in sexually selected
doing so sets out predictions about the triggers that are attributes. Review of General Psychology.
salient to women but not men. Men more than women Arrindell, W. A., Kolk, A. M., Pickersgill, M. J., & Hageman, W. J. J. M.
should be concerned with publicly visible dominance rank (1993). Biological sex, sex-role orientation, masculine sex role stress,
dissimulation and self-reported fears. Advances in Behaviour Research
(and threats to it through affronts and humiliation), since
and Therapy, 15, 103-146.
this has considerable implications for their mating strategy Barber, N. (2000). On the relationship between country sex ratios and teen
and is a trait that affects female choice. Women more than pregnancy rates: A replication. Cross-Cultural Research, 34, 26-37.
men should be concerned with enhancing their physical Baumeister, R. F., & Twenge, J. M. (2002). Cultural suppression of female
This content downloaded from 129.234.252.65 on Thu, 15 Sep 2016 11:47:05 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
24 Female Competition
sexuality. Review of General Psychology, 6, 16S203. Coleman, J. S. (1961). The adolescent society. New York: Free Press.
Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, inter- Connor-Greene, P. A., Striegel-Moore, R. H., & Cronan, S. (1994).
personal development, and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary theo- Perceived social climate and weight preoccupation in women. Eating
ry of socialization. Child Development, 62, 647470. Disorders, 2, 12S134.
Bettencourt, B. A., & Miller, N. (1996). Gender differences in aggression as Crick, N. R., & Grotpeter, J. K. (1995). Relational aggression, gender and
a function of provocation: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 119, social-psychological adjustment. Child Development, 66, 71s722.
422447. Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1988). Homicide. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Betzig, L. (1986). Despotism and differential reproduction: A Darwinian Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes' error: Emotion, reason and the human
view of history. New York: Aldine de Gruyter. brain. New York: Putnam.
Betzig, L. (1995). Medieval monogamy. Journal of Family History, 20, Dijkstra, P., & Buunk, B. P. (2001). Sex differences in the jealousy-evoking
181-216. nature of a rival's body build. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22,
Bischoping, K. (1993). Gender differences in conversation topics, 335-341.
1922-1990. Sex Roles, 28, 1-18. Draper, P., & Harpending, H. (1988). A sociobiological perspective on the
Biver, F., Lotstra, F., Monclus, M., Wikler, D., Damhaut, P., Mendlewicz, J., development of human reproductive strategies. In K. B. MacDonald
et al. (1996). Sex difference in SHT(2) receptor in the living human (Ed.), Sociobiological perspectives on human development (pp.
brain. Neuroscience Letters, 204, 1-2. 34s372). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Bjorkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K., & Kaukiainen, A. (1992). Do girls manipu- Dubois-Reymond, M., & Ravesloot, J. (1996). The roles of parents and
late and boys fight? Developmental trends in regard to direct and indi- peers in the sexual and relational socialization of adolescents. In K.
rect aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 18, 117-127. Hurrelmann & S. Hamilton (Eds.), Social problems and social contexts
Brown, L. M. (1998). Raising their voices: The politics of girls' anger. in adolescence (pp. 175-197). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
London: Harvard University Press. Duncan, N. (1999). Sexual bullying: Gender conJ7ict and pupil culture in
Brownfield, D. (1986). Social class and violent behaviour. Criminology, 24, secondary schools. London: Routledge.
421439. Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. (1986). Gender and aggressive behavior: A meta-
Burbank, V. (1987). Female aggression in cross-cultural perspective. analytic review of the social psychological literature. Psychological
Behavioral Science Research, 21, 70-100. Bulletin, 100, 309-330.
Bureau of Justice Statistics. (1999). Special report: Women offenders. Eckert, P. (1990). Cooperative competition in adolescent "girl talk."
Retrieved 2003 from http://www.ojp.usdoj.gov/bjs/pub/pdf/wo.pdf Discourse Processes, 13, 91-122.
Buss, D. M. (1988a). The evolution of human intrasexual competition: Eder, D. (1985). Cycles of popularity: Interpersonal relations among female
Tactics of mate attraction. Journal of Personality and Social adolescents. Sociology of Education, 58, 15>165.
Psychology, 54, 61 S628. Eder, D. (1990). Serious and playful disputes: Variations in conflict talk
Buss, D. M. (1988b). From vigilance to violence: Tactics of mate retention among female adolescents. In A. D. Grimshaw (Ed.), Conflict talk:
in American undergraduates. Ethology and Sociobiology, 9, 291-317. Sociolinguistic investigations of arguments in conversations (pp.
Buss, D. M. (2000) The dangerous passion. London: Bloomsbury. 67-84). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Buss, D. M., & Dedden, L. A. (1990). Derogation of competitors. Journal Eder, D., & Enke, J. L. (1991). The structure of gossip: Opportunities and
of Personal and Social Relationships, 7, 395v22. constraints on collective expression among adolescents. American
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evolution- Sociological Review, 56, 49>508.
ary perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100, 20$232. Ellis, L. (1995). Dominance and reproductive success among nonhuman
Buunk, B. P., Dijkstra, P., Kenrick, D. T., & Warntjes, A. (2001). Age pref- animals: A cross-species comparison. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16,
erences for mates as related to gender, own age, and involvement level. 257-333.
Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 241-250. Etcoff, N. (1999). Sarvival of the prettiest. London: Little, Brown and
Byrnes, J. P., Miller, D. C., & Schafer, W. D. (1999). Gender differences in Company.
risk taking: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 367-383. Fallon, A. E., & Rozin, P. (1985). Sex differences in perceptions of desir-
Campbell, A. (1982). Female aggression. In P. Marsh & A. Campbell (Eds.), able body shape. Journal of Abnortnal Psychology, 94, 102-105.
Aggression and violence (pp. 137-150). Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Farnworth, M., Thornberry, T. P., Krohn, M. D., & Lizotte, A. J. (1994).
Campbell, A. (1984). The girls in the gang. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Measurement in the study of class and delinquency: Integrating theory
Campbell, A. (1986a). Self report of fighting by females. British Journal of and research. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 31, 3241.
Criminology, 26, 2846. Federal Bureau of Investigation. (n.d.). Unifortn crime reports for the
Campbell, A. (1986b). The streets and violence. In A. Campbell & J. Gibbs United States 1997. Section V: Juvenile female crime: A special study.
(Eds.), Violent transactions: The limits of personality (pp. 115-132). Retrieved 2003 from http://www.fbi.gov/ucr/Cius_97/97crime/
Oxford, UK: Blackwell. 97crimeS.pdf
Campbell, A. (1995). A few good men: Evolutionary psychology and female Feingold, A. (1990). Gender differences in effects of physical attractiveness
adolescent aggression. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16, 99-123. on romantic attraction: A comparison across five paradigms. Journal of
Campbell, A. (1999). Staying alive: Evolution, culture and women's intra- Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 981-993.
sexual aggression. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 203-252. Fillion, K. (1997). Lip service: Challenging the sexual roles of the modern
Campbell, A. (2002). A mind of her own: The evolutionary psychology of woman. London: Pandora.
women. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Fine, G. A., & Rosnow, R. L. (1978). Gossip, gossipers, gossiping.
Campbell, A., Muncer, S., & Bibel, D. (1998). Female-female criminal Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4, 161-168.
assault: An evolutionary perspective. Journal of Research in Crime and Forsberg, A. J. L., & Tullberg, B. S. (1995). The relationship between cumu-
Delinquency, 35, 413428. lative number of cohabiting partners and number of children for men and
Cashdan, E. (1998). Are men more competitive than women? British women in modern Sweden. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16, 221-232.
Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 213-229. Frayer, D. W., & Wolpoff, M. H. (1985). Sexual dimorphism. Annual
Chapais, B. ( 1992). The role of alliances in social inheritance of rank among Review of Anthropology, 14, 429-473.
female primates. In A. Harcourt & F.B.M. de Waal (Eds.), Coalitions Furnham, A., & Radley, S. (1989). Sex differences in the perception of male
and alliances in humans and other animals (pp. 29-59). Oxford, UK: and female body shapes. Personality and Individual Differences, 10,
Oxford University Press. 653-662.
Cherlin, A. (1981). Marriage, divorce, remarriage. Cambridge, MA: Gangestad, S., & Simpson, J. (2000). The evolution of human mating:
Harvard University Press. Trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23,
Clutton-Brock, T. H. (1991). The evolution of parental care. Princeton, NJ: 573-644.
Princeton University Press. Geary, D. (1998). Male, female: The evolution of human sex differences.
Cohn, J., Adler, N. E., Irwin, C. E., Millstein, S. G., Kegeles, S. M., & Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Stone, G. (1987). Body-figure preference in male and female adoles- Geary, D. (2000). Evolution and proximate expression of human paternal
cents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 27S279. investment. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 55-77.
This content downloaded from 129.234.252.65 on Thu, 15 Sep 2016 11:47:05 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Campbell 25
George, M. J. (1999). A victimization survey of female-perpetrated assaults Lees, S. (1993). Sugar and spice: Sexuality and adolescent girls. London:
in the United Kingdom. Aggressive Behavior, 25, 67-79. Penguin
Geronimus, A. T. (1996). What teen mothers know. Human Nature, 7, Lever, J. (1976). Sex differences in the games children play. Social
323-352. Problems, 23,478-487.
Gilligan, C. (1982). In a diffierent voice: Psychological theory and women's Levin, P., & Arluke, A. ( 1985). An exploratory analysis of sex differences in
development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. gossip. Sex Roles, 12, 281-286.
Glasgow, D. G. (1981). The black underclass. New York: Vintage. Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, N. (2001). Risk as
Goodwin, M. (1980). He-said-she-said: Formal cultural procedures for the feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 267-286.
construction of a gossip dispute activity. American Ethnologist, 7, Low, B. S. (2000). Why sex matters: A Darwinian look at human behaviour
674-694. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Graziano, W. G., Jensen-Campbell, L. A., Shebilske, L. J., & Lundgren, S. Marsh, P., & Paton, R. (1986). Gender, social class and conceptual schemas
R. (1993). Social influence, sex differences and judgements of beauty: of aggression. In A. Campbell & J. Gibbs (Eds.), Violent transactions:
Putting the interpersonal back into interpersonal attraction. Journal of The limits of personality (pp. 59-85). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 522-531. Martin, R. (1997). "Girls don't talk about garages!": Perceptions of conver-
Guttentag, M., & Secord, P. (1983). Too many women? Beverly Hills, CA: sation in same-sex and cross-sex friendships. Personal Relationships, 4,
Sage. 115-130.
Hall, K. (2002). Who do men and women gossip about and what is discussed McHenry, H. M. (1994). Behavioral ecological implications of early
about them? Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Durham University, hominid body size. Journal of Human Evolution, 27, 77-87.
England. Mealey, L. (2000). Anorexia: A losing strategy? Human Nature, 11,
Hampton, R. L. (1975). Marital disruption: Some social and economic con- 105-116.
sequences. In G. J. Duncan & J. N. Morgan (Eds.), Five thousand Merten, D. (1997). The meaning of meanness: Popularity, competition and
American families: Patterns of economic progress (pp. 163-188). Ann conflict among junior high school girls. Sociology of Education, 70,
Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research. 175-191.
Hanna, C. (1999). Ganging up on girls: Young women and their emerging Miller, E. M. (1986). Street woman. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
violence. Arizona Law Review, 41, 93-141. Miller, L. C., & Fishkin, S. A. (1997). The dynamics of human bonding and
Hatchett, S., Veroff, J., & Douvan, E. (1995). Marital instability among reproductive success: Seeking a window on the adapted-for-human-
Black and White couples in early marriage. In M. B. Tucker & C. environment interface. In J.A. Simpson & D.T. Kenrick (Eds.),
Mitchell-Kiernan (Eds.), The decline in marriage among Afro- Evolutionary social psychology (pp.197-235). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Americans: Causes, consequences and policy implications (pp. Miller, L. C., Putcha-Bhagavatula, A., & Pedersen, W. C. (2002). Men's and
177-218). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. women's mating preferences: Distinct evolutionary mechanisms?
Hill, K., & Hurtado, A. M. (1996). Ache life history: The ecology and Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 88-93.
demography of a foraging people. New York: Aldine de Gruyter. Millhausen, R. R., & Herold, E. S. (1999). Does the sexual double standard
Home Office Research and Planning Unit. (1993). The 1992 British Crime still exist? Perceptions of university women. The Journal of Sex
Survey. London: Her Majesty's Stationary Office. Research, 36,361-368.
Home OfElce Research and Planning Unit. (1995). Young people, victimisa- Miner, V., & Longino, H. (Eds.). (1987). Competition: A feminist taboo?
tion and the police: British Crime Survey findings on the experiences and New York: Feminist Press.
attitudes of 12 to 15 year olds. London: Her Majesty's Stationary Office. Moore, T. M., Scarpa, A., & Raine, A. (2002). A meta-analysis of serotonin
Home Office Statistical Bulletin. (1996). The 1996 British Crime Survey metabolite 5-HIAA and antisocial behavior. Aggressive Behavior, 28,
England and Wales. London: Government Statistical Service. 299-3 16.
Hrdy, S. B. (1981). The woman that never evolved. Cambridge, MA: Murdock, G. P. (1981). Atlas of world cultures. Pittsburgh, PA: University
Harvard University Press. of Pittsburgh Press.
Hrdy, S. B. (1999). Mother Nature: Natural selection and the female of the Nevo, O., Nevo, B., & Derech-Zehavi, A. (1993). The development of the
species. London: Chatto and Windus. Tendency to Gossip Questionnaire: Construct and concurrent validity
Hughes, L. (1988). "But that's not really mean": Competing in a coopera- for a sample of Israeli college students. Educational and Psychological
tive mode. Sex Roles, 19, 669487. Measurement, 53, 973-981.
Joe Laidler, K., & Hunt, G. (2001). Accomplishing femininity among the Norton, A. J., & Glick, P. C. (1979). Marital stability in America: Past, pre-
girls in the gang. British Journal of Criminology, 41, 656-678. sent and future. In G. Levinger & O. C. Moles (Eds.), Divorce and sep-
Kaplan, H. S., & Lancaster, J. B. (2003). An evolutionary and ecological aration: Context, causes and consequences (pp. S19). New York: Basic
analysis of human fertility, mating patterns and parental investment. In Books.
K. W. Wachter & R. A. Bulatao (Eds.), Offspring: Human fertility in O'Brien, R. (1988). Exploring the intersexual nature of violent crimes.
biodemographic perspective (pp. 170-223). Washington, DC: National Criminology, 26, 151-170.
Academies Press. Osterman, K., Bjorkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K., Kaukiainen, A., Huesmann,
Kenrick, D. T., & Keefe, R. C. (1992). Age preferences in mates reflect sex L. R., & Fraczek, A. (1994). Peer and self-estimated aggression and vic-
differences in human reproductive strategies. Behavioral and Brain timisation in 8-year-old children from five ethnic groups. Aggressive
Sciences, 15, 75-133. Behavio; 20, 411A28.
Kenxick, D. T., Keefe, R. C., Gabrielidis, C., & Cornelius, J. S. (1996). Owens, L., Shute, R., & Slee, P. (2000). "Guess what I just heard!": Indirect
Adolescents' age preferences for dating partners: Support for an evolu- aggression among teenage girls in Australia. Aggressive Behaviog 26,
tionary model of life-history strategies. Child Development, 67, 67-83.
1499-1511. Pederson, W. C., Miller, L. C., Putcha-Bhagavatula, A. D., & Yang, Y. J.
Kenrick, D. T., Sadalla, E. K., Groth, G., & Trost, M. R. (1990). Evolution, (2002). Evolved sex differences in the number of partners desired? The
traits and the stages of human courtship: Qualifying the parental invest- long and short of it. Psychological Science, 13, 157-161.
ment model. Journal of Personality, 58, 97-116. Pellegrini, A. D., & Archer, J. (in press). Sex differences in competitive and
Langlois, J. H., Kalakanis, L., Rubenstein, A. J., Larson, A., Hallam, M., & aggressive behavior: A view from sexual selection theory. In B. J. Ellis
Smoot, M. (2000). Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and the- & D. F. Bjorklund (Eds.), Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary psy-
oretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 390923. chology and child development. New York: Guildford.
Leadbeater, B. J. R., Way, N., & Raden, A. (1996). Why not marry your Placvan, J. M., & van Schaik, C. P. (1997a). Interpreting hominid behavior
baby's father? Answers from African American and Hispanic mothers. In on the basis of sexual dimorphism. Journal of Human Evolution, 32,
B. J. R. Leadbeater & N. Way (Eds.), Urban girls: Resisting stereotypes, 345-374.
creating identities (pp. 193-209). London: New York University Press. Placvan, J. M., & van Schaik, C. P. (1997b). Intrasexual competition and
Leaper, C., & Holliday, H. (1995). Gossip in same-gender and cross-gender body weight dimorphism in anthropoid primates. American Journal of
friends' conversations. Personal Relationships, 2, 237-246. Physical Anthropology, 103, 37-68.
This content downloaded from 129.234.252.65 on Thu, 15 Sep 2016 11:47:05 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
26 Female Competition
Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (2002). Causes of eating disorders. Annual Thomas, J. R., & French, K. E. (1985). Gender differences across age in
Review of Psychology, 53, 187-213. motor performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 9S,
Pusey, A., Williams, J., & Goodall, J. (1997). The influence of dominance 26(}282.
rank on the reproductive success of female chimpanzees. Science, 277, Tooke, W., & Camire, L. (1991). Patterns of deception in intersexual and
823-83 1. intrasexual mating strategies. Ethology and Sociobiology, 12, 345-364.
Reisert, I., & Pilgrim, C. (1991). Sexual differentiation of monoaminergic Townsend, J. M. (1995). Sex without emotional involvement: An evolu-
neurons genetic or epigenetic? Trends in Neural Sciences, 14, tionary interpretation of sex differences. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
468A73. 24, 173-206.
Ridley, M. (1993). The red queen: Sex and the evolution of human nature. Tracy, L. (1991). The secret between us: Competition among women.
London: Penguin. Boston: Little, Brown.
Rodseth, L., Wrangham, R. W., Harrigan, A. M., & Smuts, B. B. ( 1991). The Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B.
human community as a primate society. Current Anthropology, 32, Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selection and the descent of man 1871-1971.
221-254. Chicago: Aldine.
Sampson, R. (1995). Unemployment and imbalanced sex ratios: Race-spe- Tucker, M. B., & Mitchell-Kernan, C. (Eds.). (1995). The decline in mar-
CifilC consequences for family structure and crime. In M. B. Tucker & C. riage among African Americans: Causes, consequences and policy
Mitchell-Kiernan (Eds.), The decline in marriage among Afro- implications. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Americans: Causes, consequences and policy implications (pp. Tucker, M. B ., & Mitchell-Kernan, C. ( 1997). Understanding marital
229-254). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. decline among African Americans. Perspectives, 3, 1-5.
Schuster, I. (1985). Female aggression and resource scarcity: A cross-cul- United States Bureau of the Census. (1992). Households, families and chil-
tural perspective. In M. Haug, D. Benton, P. Brain, B. Oliver, & J. Mos dren: A 30 year perspective (Current Population Reports Series P23,
(Eds.), The aggressive female (pp. 185-208). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Publication No. 181). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing
CIP-Gegevens Koninklijke Bibioteheek. Office.
Sear, R., Mace, R., & McGregor, I. A. (2000). Maternal grandmothers United States Bureau of the Census. ( 1995). Retrived 2003 from
improve nutritional status and survival of children in rural Gambia. http://www.census.gov/population/socdemo/race/black/tabs95/tabO 1 .txt
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B, 267, 1641-1647. Voland, E. (1988). Differential infant and child mortality in evolutionary
Sheldon, A. (1992). Conflict talk: Sociolinguistic challenges to self assertion perspective: Data from late 17th to l9th century Ostfrieland (Germany).
and how young girls meet them. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 38, 95-117. In L. Betzig, M. Borgerhoff Mulder, & P. Turke (Eds.), Human repro-
Simmons, R. (2002). Odd girl out: The hidden culture of aggression in girls. ductive behavior: A Darwinian perspective (pp. 253-262). Cambridge,
London: Harcourt.
UK: Cambridge University Press.
Singh, D. (1993). Adaptive significance of female physical attractiveness: Walters, S., & Crawford, C. B. (1994). The importance of mate attraction
Role of waist to hip ratio. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
for intrasexual competition in men and women. Ethology and
65,293-307.
Sociobiology, 15, 5-30.
Smith, T. (1994). Attitudes toward sexual permissiveness: Trends, correlates
Webber, J. (2003). An investigation into the sexual competition hypothesis
and behavioral connections. In A. S. Rossi (Ed.), Sexuality across the life
of eating disorders. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Durham
course (pp.63-97). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
University, England.
Smuts, B. B. (1987). Sexual competition and mate choice. In B. B. Smuts,
Weinraub, M., & Gringlas, M. B. ( 1995). Single parenthood. In M.
D. L. Cheney, R. M. Seyfarth, R. W. Wrangham, & T. T. Struhsaker
Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting, Volume 3: Status and social
(Eds.), Primate societies (pp.385-399). Chicago: University of Chicago
conditions of parenting (pp. 65-87). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Press.
VVilliams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and natural selection: A critique of
Smuts, B. B. (1995). The evolutionary origins of patriarchy. Human Nature,
some current evolutionary thought. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University
6,1-32.
Press.
Steffensmeier, D., & Allan, E. (1996). Gender and crime: Toward a gendered
Wilson, D. (1978). Sexual codes and conduct: A study of teenage girls. In
theory of female offending. Annual Review of Sociology, 22,459 4 87.
C. Smart and B. Smart (Eds.), Women, sexuality and social control (pp.
Streeter, S. A., & McBurney, D. H. (2003). Waist-hip ratio and attractive-
65-73). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
ness: New evidence and a critique of "a critical test." Evolution and
Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (1985). Competitiveness, risk-taking and violence:
Human Behavior, 24,88-98.
The young male syndrome. Ethology and Sociobiology, 6, 59-73.
Striegel-Moore, R. H., Connor-Greene, P. A., & Shime, B. S. (1991). School
Wilson, W. J. (1987). The truly disadvantaged. Chicago: University of
milieu characteristics and disordered eating in high school graduates.
Chicago Press.
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 10, 187-192.
Wolf, N. (1998). Promiscuities: The secret struggle for womanhood. New
Striegel-Moore, R. H., Silberstein, L. R., Grunberg, N. E., & Rodin, J.
York: Ballantine.
(1990). Competing on all fronts: Achievement orientation and disor-
Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2002). A cross-cultural analysis of behavior of
dered eating. Sex Roles, 23,697-702.
Tanenbaum, L. (1999). Slut: Growing up female with a bad reputation. New women and men: Implications for the origins of sex differences.
Tanenbaum, L. (2002). CaMight: Women and competition. New York: Seven Zaadstra, B. M., Seidell, J. C., Van Noord, P. A., te Velde, E. R., Habbema,
Stories. J. D., Vrieswijk, B., et al. (1993). Fat and female fecundity: Prospective
Tanner, J. M. (1990). Foetus into man: Physical growth from conception to study of body fat distribution on conception rates. British Medical
This content downloaded from 129.234.252.65 on Thu, 15 Sep 2016 11:47:05 UTC
View publication stats
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms