Outside The Box - Rethinking ADD - ADHD in Children and Adults - A Practical Guide I
Outside The Box - Rethinking ADD - ADHD in Children and Adults - A Practical Guide I
Outside The Box - Rethinking ADD - ADHD in Children and Adults - A Practical Guide I
Laden with compelling case examples that humanize this condition, the
writing is both authoritative and readable. Overall, the book masterfully blends
Outside the Box
understanding of the disorder, its complexity, and its causes, but also a number
of insightful cases and evidence-based recommendations for its management.” A PR ACTIC AL GUIDE
A PR ACTICAL GUIDE
Russell A. Barkley, Ph.D., Clinical Professor of
Psychiatry, Medical University of South Carolina
Rethinking ADD/ADHD
in CHILDREN and ADULTS
A PRACTICAL GUIDE
“This down-to-earth book shows what ADHD really is and what it isn’t.
Brown gives us a true ‘feel’ for ADHD and the impact of symptoms on peo
ple’s lives. He defines executive functions in clear examples just as his pa
tients described to him. For those who want an in-depth understanding of
ADHD as it occurs in children, youth and adults, this book is a must read.”
Michael J. Manos, Ph.D., Head of Pediatric Behavioral Health,
Cleveland Clinic
“This book is essential reading for anyone who wants to better under
stand ADHD across the life span. Brown gathers together some of the
best thinking and research from a variety of fields to answer some of the
most crucial questions about this still vexing and all-too-common con
dition. He has delivered a clear, comprehensive work that is both engag
ing and original. A rewarding, useful, and accessible read.”
F. Xavier Castellanos, M.D., The Child Study Center at NYU Langone
Medical Center, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
“Outside the Box offers an accessible update on the latest research about
ADHD and answers questions and doubts that many struggle with
about this disorder, which is very prevalent in children and adults all
over the world.”
Sandra Kooij, M.D., Ph.D., Chair, European Network Adult ADHD
“Tom Brown’s clearly written evidence-based update on attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder is a must read for professionals working with peo
ple struggling with ADHD as well as for adolescents and adults with
ADHD who are seeking a deeper understanding of the disorder. The many
clinical vignettes add interest and remind us of the person behind the diag
nosis. Sections debunking common misunderstandings and summarizing
important facts about ADHD are gems!”
Mina K. Dulcan, M.D., Head, Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Ann and
Robert H. Lurie Children’s Hospital of Chicago and Professor of Psychiatry
and Behavioral Sciences and Pediatrics, Northwestern University Feinberg
School of Medicine
“Comprehensive, compassionate, and clear, Brown’s authoritative survey
on attention-deficit disorders integrates rich clinical experience with cut
ting-edge neuroscience and epidemiology. Brown’s own ‘out of the box’
views on such topics as measurement bias and nonpharmaceutical adjunc
tive treatment are compelling. This rigorous yet approachable text is ideal
for clinicians, academic researchers, parents, and patients alike.”
Ronald C. Kessler, Ph.D., McNeil Family Professor, Department of
Health Care Policy, Harvard Medical School
“Well written, clear, and concise, Dr. Brown’s Outside the Box explodes
many of the myths and misunderstandings surrounding ADHD. In his
calm, reassuring style, he provides the information families and young
adults with ADHD need not only to understand this disorder but also to
seek out proper treatment. This is a much-needed book amidst all the
confusion that abounds today.”
Patricia O. Quinn, M.D., Developmental Pediatrician, Washington, DC,
and author of 100 Questions and Answers About Attention-Deficit
Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) in Women and Girls
“Dr. Brown, a leading international expert in ADD and related conditions,
provides a superb, nuanced introduction to this multifaceted condition and
a rich, personal, compassionate exploration of the challenges faced by at
least 5% of the world’s population. His advice and guidance on how to an
ticipate and deal with the realities of ADD make this a must read for teach
ers, psychologists, and medical professionals, as well as for the parents,
teens, adults, and partners whose lives are directly touched, and potentially
derailed, by ADD. Dr. Brown’s guidance is especially important in today’s
world with its vast array of information, much of which is incomplete, con
tradictory, unscientific, outdated, and at times simply wrong.”
James F. Leckman, M.D., Ph.D., Neison Harris Professor of Child Psy
chiatry, Psychiatry, Pediatrics and Psychology, Yale University
Outside the Box
Rethinking ADD/ADHD
in CHILDREN and ADULTS
A PRACTICAL GUIDE
Outside the Box
Rethinking ADD/ADHD
in CHILDREN and ADULTS
A PRACTICAL GUIDE
by
But even at its early stages, the whole business of the matter of
Gerald M. Edelman
Bright Air, Brilliant Fire: On the Matter of the Mind
Preface. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .xiii
Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . xv
Chapter 1
Why can persons with ADHD focus well on a few tasks or activities
but not on many others that they know are important?
Chapter 2
What are executive functions, and how are they different in ADHD?
Chapter 3
How can really smart people, especially those who are not hyper
active, have ADHD?
Chapter 4
How does ADHD change as you get older, and does it always start
in childhood?
Chapter 5
How ADHD Impacts “Brain Googling”
for Motivations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 61
Chapter 6
How ADHD Develops, Sometimes Gets Worse,
and Sometimes Improves . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79
How and why does ADHD sometimes get better and sometimes
get worse as one gets older?
Chapter 7
How and Why Other Disorders Often
Co-occur With ADHD . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 101
Why do individuals with ADHD tend to have additional learning or
psychiatric problems more often than most people without ADHD?
Chapter 8
Assessing Children, Teenagers, and Adults
for ADHD. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 129
Why can’t ADHD be diagnosed accurately with objective tests?
What is an adequate way to diagnose ADHD, and who is quali
fied to do it?
Chapter 9
Emotional Dynamics in Individuals, Couples,
and Families Coping With ADHD . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 151
How and why are emotions so complicated in relationships be
tween persons with ADHD and their family, friends, partners, and
children?
Chapter 10
What medication treatments are safe and effective for adults, ad
olescents, and children with ADHD? What are possible risks or
side effects?
Chapter 11
Chapter 12
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 257
Index . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 279
About the
Author
Dr. Brown has indicated a financial interest in or other affiliation with a com
mercial supporter, a manufacturer of a commercial product, a provider of a
commercial service, a nongovernmental organization, and/or a government
agency as follows: Publication royalties, American Psychiatric Press, The Psycho
logical Corporation, and Yale University Press; Consultant, research support,
speaker: Eli Lilly Co., and Shire, Inc.; Speaker: Jannsen Pharm; and Consultant:
Novartis and Shionoga Pharma.
xi
Preface
xv
xvi OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
One example is a 19-year-old young man who came with his parents
to consult with me over the Christmas break of his first year of college.
Let’s call him Jake.
ever work for him, but he had decided to check out this consultation and
maybe give one more approach a try, even though he continued to have
serious doubts about whether it would actually work.
cient evidence for its largely biological underpinnings and major asso
ciated impairments.... Stimulants work far too well for individuals with
ADHD...for this diagnosis and such medications to quickly leave the
scene. (Hinshaw and Scheffler 2014, p. 161)
Mystery of ADHD
1
2 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
All of these factors are influential, yet the most fundamental reason
for the escalating diagnoses and medication treatment of ADHD boils
down to two simple and related facts:
Persons with ADHD, even those who are very bright, tend to expe
rience a wide variety of impairments in their ability to learn and to man
age themselves in multiple activities of daily life: in their schooling,
family interactions, and social relationships. For some, these impair
ments are extremely costly to them and their families, at least during
childhood and/or adolescence and often also into adult years as they
encounter challenges of employment, driving a motor vehicle, manag
ing a household and finances, establishing and maintaining close rela
tionships, and, for some, parenting. Medications for ADHD, adequately
administered, do not cure, but often, although not always, they help to
alleviate these impairments and can improve quality of life. Increasing
numbers of persons in the United States and elsewhere around the
world are discovering that untreated ADHD is damaging and that, for
many, medication treatment for ADHD often works.
To fully appreciate why increasing numbers of individuals now
want to seek treatment for ADHD, it is helpful to have an updated un
derstanding of how scientific research has dramatically changed our
understanding of this disorder. Research studies over the past 30 years
have demonstrated that the disorder now known as ADHD is not essen
tially a disorder of misbehavior as it was understood in 1902. It is a com
plicated problem with the unfolding development and functioning of
the brain’s management system, its executive functions. It was not until
1980 that the term attention deficit was introduced into the name of the
disorder now known as ADHD. And it was not until 1980 that psychi
atric diagnostic criteria for ADHD began to include the possibility that
for some children with these attentional difficulties, symptoms may
persist into adulthood. Since that time, understanding of this complex
disorder has rapidly expanded.
4 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
Scientific research has taught us much more about the nature, causes,
course, and treatment of ADHD. Here is a summary of 12 important ba
sic findings that are explained further in later chapters.
dividuals with ADHD tend to have a few tasks or situations where they
demonstrate impressive or, at least, quite adequate competence in exer
cising various cognitive management skills that they are unable to exer
cise with consistency in most other activities of daily life, even though
they see the importance of doing those tasks and very much want to per
form them successfully.
Symptoms of ADHD are chronic, but in each person, they appear
with notable exceptions, usually in situations where the person has
strong personal interest in a particular task or activity or when they be
lieve that something very unpleasant for them is likely to occur very
quickly if they do not attend to this specific activity right here, right
now. Clinical observations and empirical research have consistently
demonstrated that ADHD symptoms are situationally variable and that
there is much intra-individual variability in the symptoms of this disor
der. This is the central mystery of ADHD.
A classic example of this puzzling paradox of ADHD is the situation
of Larry, a sturdy, sandy-haired high school junior who was the goalie
for his school’s ice hockey team. It happened that the day before his
evaluation, Larry had helped his team win the state championship in
hockey by blocking many shots on goal. He was an extraordinarily fine
goalie, and he was also a very bright student who scored in the very su
perior range on IQ tests. He wanted to get good grades because he was
hoping eventually to go to medical school. Yet he was chronically in
trouble with his teachers. Often they said to him, “Once in a while, you
make very perceptive comments in class that show how smart you are,
but most of the time you’re out to lunch—looking out the window or
staring at the ceiling. Occasionally, you turn in a really good homework
paper, but most of the time you don’t even know what the homework is
supposed to be.” The teachers kept asking Larry, “If you can pay atten
tion so well when you’re playing hockey, why can’t you pay attention
when you are in class? If you can work so hard to practice and stay in
shape for hockey, why can’t you show some consistent effort for your
schoolwork?”
After hearing his parents tell me about these recurrent complaints
from his teachers, Larry quietly responded,
I don’t know why this keeps happening. I’m just as frustrated and even
more worried about this than you are.... I know what I need to do and I
really want to do it because I know how important it is for all the rest of
my life.... I know I should be able to do it; I just can’t! I just can’t make
myself pay steady attention to my work for school anywhere near the
way I pay attention for hockey.
8 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
I’ve got a sexual example for you to show what it’s like to have ADHD.
It’s like having erectile dysfunction of the mind. If the task you are faced
with is something that turns you on, something that is really interesting
for you, you’re “up for it” and you can perform. But if the task is not
something that’s intrinsically interesting to you, if it doesn’t turn you on,
you can’t get up for it and you can’t perform. It doesn’t matter how
much you tell yourself, ‘I need to, I ought to.’ It’s just not a willpower
kind of thing. (Brown 2005a)
Our thoughts and actions are routinely guided by System 1 and generally
are on the mark. One of the marvels is the rich and detailed model of our
world that is maintained in associative memory....(System 1) holds the vast
repertory of skills we have acquired in a lifetime of practice, which automat
ically produces adequate solutions to challenges as they arise. (p. 416)
are based on the specific individual’s memories and priorities; these are
shaped by previous life experiences and that individual’s current situa
tion as he or she perceives it. More information about how ADHD im
pacts these operations that shape the brain’s “googling” for motivation
is provided in Chapter 5 (“How ADHD Impacts ‘Brain Googling’ for
Motivations”).
In Chapter 2 (“A New Model of ADHD”), I provide more informa
tion about the nature, causes, and various ADHD-related impairments
over the course of the life span.
Summary
Despite controversy and considerable misinformation about ADHD
in the media, the rate of ADHD diagnosis and use of medication for this
disorder has significantly increased in the United States and interna
tionally. The most fundamental reasons for this are that 1) ADHD can
cause considerable chronic impairment and suffering for affected indi
viduals and families and 2) although medication treatments do not cure
ADHD, if appropriately administered, they can safely reduce impair
ment and improve functioning.
This chapter states 12 important science-based findings about ADHD
as complex impairment of the brain’s self-management system; these
findings are more fully explained in subsequent chapters. The chapter
also highlights the central mystery of ADHD—that those with ADHD
are able to function quite effectively when engaged in a few specific ac
tivities or tasks that interest them, utilizing those same self-management
functions that are chronically impaired for most other activities. This is
explained in terms of the brain’s “automatic system,” which is not deter
mined by “willpower.”
2
A New Model of ADHD
11
12 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
Executive functions
(work together in various combinations)
1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6.
Activation Focus Effort Emotion Memory Action
All my life, I’ve had trouble getting started on my work when I have to
work by myself. I have no trouble talking with clients or working with
our secretaries or other attorneys, but when I have paperwork to do, I
have great difficulty getting started.
Twice each week, I set aside several hours to do paperwork that I
want to get done and need to get done because I’m not going to get paid
until it’s completed. I’m in my office with the door closed. Nobody is
bothering me, and the secretary can cover the phone. I have all the stuff
I need right in front of me, and I don’t touch it. I turn on my computer
and check my e-mails. Then I write a few notes to various people. Then
I get on a couple of news sites to see what’s going on in the world. Then,
magically, a video game appears on my screen, and I spend several
hours playing that. Then I go home, and my work is still not done. I get
home, get something to eat, watch a little TV, and then, about 10 P.M., it
occurs to me, “Oh, my God. I’ve got that report to get done tonight. If I
don’t get that in by 8 A.M., I’m going to be in very serious trouble at work
tomorrow morning.” Then I finally get started on my home computer,
work consistently until 2 or 3 A.M., and produce an excellent report. But
that’s a hell of a way to live.
corner. While doing all this, we may be thinking about what we are go
ing to buy when we get to the grocery store.
When focusing on our driving, we are shifting the focus of our vision
and attention across multiple aspects of what is around us as we move
along the street, keeping in mind what we notice as potentially impor
tant and ignoring distractions that are not currently significant. If we
notice any objects or movements that are unusual or may create a prob
lem, our attention focuses instantly to reassess that situation while we
still keep in mind the larger shifting picture of where we are and what
we are doing as we drive the car down the street.
Often, I stay up a lot longer than I really want to or should because I’ve
found that if I try to go to bed before I’m really exhausted, I can’t shut my
head off; I just keeping thinking about stuff. So I stay up late reading or
watching TV or using the computer until I’m fully exhausted, and then I
can fall asleep without much trouble.
Yet these same people often complain that once they fall asleep, they
tend to sleep like dead people and have much difficulty in waking up.
Often, they report that they are unable to hear and respond to an alarm
clock or that they hit the snooze button repeatedly or simply turn off the
alarm and go back to sleep unless they have someone who is available
and willing to help them get up and out of bed. Without such help, they
are quite likely to show up late or completely miss school, work, or any
other commitments that they actually want to and need to be present for.
During the day, they are usually OK so long as they are walking
around or talking a lot. But if they have to sit still for a long time to read
or listen to a lecture or attend a class or meeting, their eyelids tend to get
heavy, and they feel drowsy as they fight off falling asleep.
Another struggle for many with ADHD is illustrated by a university
student who was a runner on the track team:
My mind is a great sprinter, but it’s a lousy distance runner. If the task I
have to do is something where you can go all out and finish it in one
chunk, I’m fine. But if it’s a longer-term project, something you can’t
complete in one quick chunk, something you have to do a bit at a time,
day after day, that’s much more difficult for me. Typically, either I rush to
get the damn thing done as quickly as possible or I just put it aside, say
ing I’ll get to it when it becomes more of an emergency.
18 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
There weren’t many people in the diner; it was late afternoon. I was in a
good mood eating my soup, when a guy sitting in the booth behind me
began chewing his sandwich very loudly, “chomp, chomp, chomp!”
There was something about that noise that was driving me nuts. It in
vaded my mind like a computer virus invades a hard drive—taking up
all of the space. All I could think of was that noise! My fist clenched, and
I was seriously thinking about smacking this guy in the mouth. I didn’t
do it; I didn’t want to get arrested. Then after a few minutes, he was still
making the same noise, but at that point it didn’t bother me anymore.
That sort of thing happens to me often. Some little frustration that on
a scale of 0 to 10 most people would rate as a 0 or a 1 can hit me like a 7
or an 8 or a 9! I feel like punching somebody or breaking something.
Then, in just a couple of minutes, it usually goes away.
It’s not always like that. Today I was walking down the hall at work, and
a friend of mine who works in another department came around the cor
ner and was walking toward me while he was reading some papers. I
hadn’t seen him for a long time, so I stopped and said, “Hi, how’ve you
been?” He looked up, said, “Hi,” and then put his head down and kept
on walking. Most people would blow that off in a second. They’d just
say, “He’s probably in a hurry to get to a meeting or something. We can
talk later.” Not me! That happened at lunchtime and I didn’t get any
thing done all afternoon. I spent the whole afternoon thinking, “Did I do
something to piss him off?” Or maybe I offended somebody in his de
partment, and they’re all mad at me. Or maybe I’m just a person nobody
likes, and nobody will tell me about it.
Other people with ADHD do not have problems with emotions, but
they do have problems when they get an idea about something they want
to get, something they want to buy, or something they want to do. They get
the feeling “I have to have it now!” And they do everything they possibly
can to get it now. It does not matter to them how much the item costs, or
how much it is going to make a problem for them or for someone else, or
whether they are using time or money now for this item when they know
they need to use the time or money for something else that is more impor
tant tomorrow. They just do everything they can to get whatever it is they
feel they must have now. And they keep that up until either they get it or
20 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
they hit a brick wall. But even if they get it, often they are not satisfied be
cause soon they are on to something else they want.
Some others do not have problems with wanting something imme
diately, but they worry a lot. One woman described an experience driv
ing on an expressway:
I was in the left lane driving along next to the Jersey barrier, and there was
an 18-wheel truck cruising in the lane beside me. He started to move over a
little toward me; he didn’t get into my lane, but it got me to thinking—what
would happen if he didn’t see me and pulled into my lane and squished me
and my car? Soon I wasn’t just thinking about it; I was running a very vivid
movie in my head. I pictured how he could smash into my car and squish
me against the Jersey barrier. I thought of how the car would be crumpled
and sharp pieces of metal would be sticking into me and I would be bleed
ing to death. And the truck would be dragging the car along against the bar
rier, and then it would jackknife, and we’d be getting hit repeatedly by other
cars and trucks. And there would be a massive traffic jam, and it would take
a long time for the rescue squad to get there to cut me out of the car. And by
the time they got me out, I would have bled to death; they would have to
call my family and tell them I was dead. All this was running in my head
while I was trying to drive my car 65 miles an hour down the expressway!
That sort of thing happens to me a lot, where everything is going along fine
and I start thinking, “What if this happened? Or what if that happened?”
It is not that everyone with ADHD has all of these problems with emo
tions, but many have at least one or two of them. Whether it is getting really
irritated about some little thing, or thinking too much about having had
one’s feelings hurt by someone, or getting that intense feeling of “I’ve got to
have it now!” or wondering “What if this or that might happen?”—in each
case, that emotion can gobble up all the space in the person’s thoughts like
a computer virus can gobble up all the space on a hard drive. And it is very
hard for those people to put the emotion into perspective, into the back of
their minds, and move on with whatever they need to do. For a while, the
problematic emotion just floods their brain.
This flooding with an uncomfortable feeling is not the only way
emotions are problematic for persons with ADHD, but managing such
emotions is one aspect of executive functions that tends to present
chronic difficulties for many with ADHD. Other ways emotions are in
volved in ADHD are discussed in Chapter 5 (“How ADHD Impacts
‘Brain Googling’ for Motivations”).
member things that no one else in the family can recall. They may be
able to tell in detail the entire story line of a movie they saw just once
more than 10 years ago. Or they may recall almost every play that was
run when they watched the Super Bowl 2 years ago. They may be able to
recall all the music and all the words of every verse of hundreds of songs
that were popular many years ago. Yet despite their having excellent re
call of some things from a long time ago, often they are unable to recall
something that happened just a few minutes ago.
The problem with memory in ADHD is generally not with long-term
storage memory. The difficulty is usually with short-term working
memory. Working memory is what helps us to keep one thing in mind
while we are doing something else. This is the aspect of memory that
makes it difficult when you call to get a phone listing and do not have
paper or pencil to write it down. In that situation, many persons with
ADHD tend to mix up the numbers, unable to keep them in mind long
enough to dial the call correctly. Working memory is what fails us when
we go into another room to get something and then cannot remember
what we came to get. It is what causes a problem when we go down
stairs to get something needed to do a project and then see something
down there that is interesting or that needs doing, and soon we get in
volved in project number two, completely forgetting that we had been
working on project number one upstairs and needed to get it done.
Working memory failure is involved when a student raises his or her
hand to give an answer to a question the teacher has asked the class and
then has to wait because the teacher calls on someone else first. Mo
ments later, when the teacher asks, “Yes, what were you going to say?”
the student has to say, “Sorry, I forgot what I was going to say, and now
I’ve even forgotten what you were asking us. Could you repeat the
question, please?” This is the same problem that may afflict a person
who is thinking of five things he or she needs to take along when he or
she goes out and then half an hour later is able to remember only one of
the five items and cannot recall the other four, even to save his or her life.
Many individuals with ADHD have memory problems with read
ing, especially if what they are reading is not particularly interesting to
them. They can read pages of text and understand every word as they
read it, yet a few minutes later they do not have the foggiest idea of what
they have just read.
But there is also another way in which working memory often fails
persons with ADHD. Working memory is the search engine for the
brain, instantaneously pulling up various thoughts, memories, and im
ages that may be relevant to what we are noticing or thinking about or
doing. Often, students with ADHD will study the night before an exam
22 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
and then have someone quiz them on what they have studied. They may
know all the answers quite well and go to class the next day expecting to
get a very good grade on the exam. But as they are taking the exam, a lot
of what they knew so well the night before just evaporates and they can
not pull it out of their head when they need it for the exam. However, a
few hours or a few days later, something may jog their memory, and all
that they were unable to recall earlier comes back without any further
studying. It is as though their brain’s search engine was simply unable to
recall what was needed when it was needed. The problem is not that they
failed to learn the information. They learned the information but were
unable to retrieve it when they wanted it.
Dynamic Interactions
of Executive Functions
Although the model of executive functions depicted in Figure 2–1
shows six separate boxes, each with its own label, these functions are
not separate unitary functions. They are not like height, weight, or
blood pressure, each of which is a unitary variable. Each of these clus
ters should be thought of as a collection of related cognitive functions
24 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
Summary
In this chapter, I describe a six-factor model of the brain’s self-management
system, its executive functions. These factors are 1) activation, organiz
ing, prioritizing, and activating to work; 2) focus, focusing, sustaining,
and shifting attention to tasks; 3) effort, regulating alertness, sustaining
effort, and adjusting processing speed; 4) emotion, managing frustration
and modulating emotions; 5) memory, utilizing working memory and
accessing recall; and 6) action, monitoring and self-regulating action.
A New Model of ADHD 25
27
28 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
Differences in Severity
of Impairment
Even within the range of impairment sufficient to warrant diagnosis,
ADHD comes in small, medium, and large. Persons with this disorder
vary in terms of how much their ADHD problems interfere with or re
duce the quality of their daily life—how they are able to work in school
or do their job or get along with their family and other people.
For some individuals, their problems with ADHD symptoms are rel
atively mild, clearly more problematic than for most persons of similar
age yet not very noticeable except in certain situations or only to the
people closely involved with them on a daily basis. For example, these
individuals may perform well in most aspects of their jobs, yet their per
sonal finances may be disorganized because of chronic inability to keep
track of their bills and their taxes and a tendency to make many impul
sive purchases that exceed their financial means. A person who is a pro
ductive worker in his business may be a careless and impulsive driver
who speeds excessively and routinely violates basic rules of the road,
endangering himself and others.
For others, impairments of ADHD are moderate, often causing sig
nificant frustrations and difficulties in their studies or employment and
disrupting their interactions with others, yet they still function in many
situations without being severely incapacitated. A student may earn
high grades in a few classes in which she has strong interest while ig
Differences Among Persons With ADHD 29
Differences in Intensity of
Hyperactivity and/or Impulsiveness
For many decades, the syndrome now identified as ADHD was under
stood simply as a chronic problem with disruptive behavior, mostly in
little boys who could not sit still, would not behave, and were far more
careless and annoying to parents and teachers than most other boys of
the same age. Common stereotypes of those with ADHD characterize
them as constantly restless and in perpetual motion and quick to speak
or to act impulsively, without enough attention to potential conse
quences. Early descriptions of the disorder did not say much about it as
a problem with attention. In fact, it was not until 1980 that the Diagnostic
and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) introduced the term
30 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
“attention deficit” into its name for this disorder and recognized that it
can occur with or without excessive hyperactive behavior (American
Psychiatric Association 1980).
More recent research-based understandings of ADHD recognize
that many of the individuals diagnosed with ADHD have never had
any significant behavior problems and have never been hyperactive. On
the contrary, a large number of those with ADHD tend to be more slug
gish and struggle to mobilize themselves to do what needs to be done.
Some researchers have suggested that those whose attentional prob
lems are characterized by considerable sluggishness ought to be seen as
having a disorder distinct from ADHD (Barkley 2015). However, per
sons in this more sluggish group seem to be just a more extreme variant
of the predominantly inattentive type of ADHD.
Even individuals who are quite impulsive and hyperactive during
early childhood usually tend to become much less “hyper” and impul
sive as they enter later childhood or early adolescence. Typically, how
ever, their attentional difficulties persist or sometimes increase as they
encounter the increasing demands for self-management that emerge
and multiply from mid-adolescence and throughout adulthood. How
ever, impulsiveness is not limited to quick movements and actions.
Many individuals with ADHD who are not too quick to speak or act re
port that they are often impulsive in the way they think, that they tend
to be too quick to jump to conclusions.
been found to have ADHD when they were evaluated at age 11 years
(Caye et al. 2016). Here again, the researchers argued that the data sup
ported the notion that ADHD does not always begin in childhood, that
it could arise for the first time in young adulthood.
A third birth cohort study also yielded findings that challenged the
assumption that ADHD always has onset during childhood. This study
of 2,040 children was done in England and Wales starting in 1995.
Agnew-Blais et al. (2016) found that 247 met diagnostic criteria for
ADHD in childhood; of those, only 54 (21.9%) continued to meet ADHD
diagnostic criteria when they were 18 years old. Yet there were 112 in the
sample who had not met diagnostic criteria for ADHD in childhood but
did meet full diagnostic criteria for ADHD when they were adults at age
18 years. Thus, three recent longitudinal studies have independently
demonstrated that a significant number of individuals who do not ap
pear to have ADHD in childhood may have experienced initial onset of
ADHD-related impairments in their late teens or early adulthood.
One possible explanation for these findings may be found in the dis
cussion in the section “Differences in Age at Onset of ADHD Symp
toms”—some impairments of ADHD do not become apparent until the
individual meets the increased challenges of late adolescence or adult
hood, at times when parental support has been more or less withdrawn.
Faraone and Biederman (2016) have argued this view.
Yet another possibility was raised by an earlier Swedish genetic
study of 1,480 twin pairs who were studied for attentional problems at
ages 8/9, 13/14, 16/17, and 19/20. This study used both parental rat
ings and self-ratings of attentional problems and found that genetic ef
fects operating at 8/9 years tended to persist, explaining 41%, 34%, and
24% of the variance over the three later age groups, respectively. How
ever, researchers also found that new sets of genetic risk factors emerged
at ages 13/14, 16/17, and 19/20 years (Chang et al. 2013). This finding
suggests that attention problems are a developmentally complex phe
notype that may involve both continuity and emerging genetic change
across the life span.
For some individuals, their ADHD impairments do not become very
noticeable until they encounter the challenges of adolescence or adult
hood. They function quite adequately or even very well in comparison
with their age-mates during preschool and elementary school. There may
be no reason for parents, teachers, or anyone else to assume that these
children have ADHD—until they reach middle school, where they no
longer have one teacher for most of the day and they need to move from
class to class. Students now must accommodate to the different rules and
expectations of various teachers with differing teaching styles and differ
34 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
Differences in Persistence or
Remission of ADHD Symptoms
For most of the twentieth century, it was assumed that the disorder now
known as ADHD occurred only in young children and that it was out
grown in later childhood or early to middle adolescence. These assump
tions were based on observations that hyperactive children usually
became much less “hyper” sometime during that period. During those
decades, ADHD was defined primarily by hyperactivity and misbehav
ior; attentional problems were not recognized as a primary component
of the disorder.
In many countries, ADHD is still thought of as a childhood disorder
that is simply outgrown during adolescence. A study by the World
Health Organization has challenged that view. Using data from a survey
in 10 different countries as diverse as France, Germany, Italy, Lebanon,
Mexico, Colombia, and the United States, Lara et al. (2009) found that an
Differences Among Persons With ADHD 35
Differences in Gender
Early studies reported that six boys were diagnosed with ADHD for ev
ery one girl identified with the disorder. This ratio was based on chil
dren brought into clinical settings such as hospitals or child guidance
clinics. Epidemiological studies that made inquiries in community sam
ples, not counting only those brought in for treatment, have reported ra
tios closer to 3:1. However, when adults are assessed, the ratio of males
to females with ADHD is closer to 1:1. This suggests that there are many
girls with ADHD-related impairments who are not identified until they
become adults and are able to come in for evaluation themselves. Per
haps this occurs because during early and middle childhood most girls
with ADHD are not as disruptive with teachers and parents as are boys.
Comparisons of samples of boys diagnosed with ADHD with girls
diagnosed with ADHD have shown no significant differences between
the genders in the symptoms they experience, in their cognitive or psy
chosocial functioning, or in the pattern of other psychological problems
associated with their ADHD (Biederman et al. 2010c, 2012; Hinshaw
2002, 2009; Hinshaw et al. 2012).
Although those studies of children included follow-up into late teenage
years, there is little systematic research to describe the impact of ADHD on
affected adult women. There is evidence that estrogen has a substantial im
pact on release of dopamine in the female brain (McEwen 1983; Thompson
and Moss 1994). There are also clinical data suggesting that varying lev
els of estrogen due to variations in different phases of the menstrual cy
cle or diminution of estrogen due to menopause may cause more
variability in levels of impairment among females than is generally seen
in males. Some adolescent and adult women report that their ADHD
impairments tend to worsen and become less responsive to medication
treatment regimens at those points in their menstrual cycle where estro
gen levels tend to be lowest. A more recent line of research has also dem
38 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
Differences in Underlying
Temperament
Temperament is a term that describes a collection of various personality
characteristics that are biologically “built in” in each individual, some of
which can be observed in early infancy and many of which often persist
in subtle and/or blatant patterns into later years. Developmental psy
chologist Jerome Kagan described and emphasized how
Differences in Environmental
Challenges and Supports
Although the primary factors in causing an individual to have ADHD
seem to be genetic, that is not to say that environment is uninvolved.
42 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
Genes provide blueprints for our growth and development, but en
vironment has a powerful influence on how and when potential strengths
and weaknesses programmed in our genes do or do not activate in ways
that may help one to flourish or to experience continuing struggles. Two
types of environmental factors are especially important: the intrauterine/
perinatal environment during pregnancy and birth and the external en
vironment of people and living conditions from birth onward.
Children whose mothers abused alcohol during pregnancy have
double the risk of having ADHD, and those whose mothers were alco
hol-dependent during pregnancy were found to be three times more
likely to have ADHD (Banerjee et al. 2007). Some pregnancy-related
complications, such as toxemia or eclampsia, poor maternal health, ex
ceptionally long labor, umbilical cord around the neck, or low birth
weight (≤ 2,500 g), may also increase risk of developing ADHD. One
population-based study of adults with ADHD showed that low birth
weight, preterm birth, and low Apgar scores increase the risk of ADHD,
which persists up to 40 years after birth (Halmøy et al. 2012).
As a child is growing up, some family, school, and community envi
ronments provide fairly consistent support, which helps the boy or girl
to feel safe and encouraged to learn, to fit in with others, and to cope
with the various stresses encountered in growing up. In other families
and schools, lack of basic economic resources for food, shelter, clothing,
and health care; chronic tensions between family members; or physical
or mental health problems of caretakers may provide stress, which
makes it far more difficult for the child with ADHD to cope and ade
quately develop. A classic study by child psychiatrist Michael Rutter
(Rutter and Quinton 1977) demonstrated that six factors within a family
correlated significantly with various psychiatric problems in children:
1) severe marital discord, 2) low social class, 3) large family size, 4) pater
nal criminality, 5) mental illness of the mother, and 6) foster placement.
He found that no one of these factors alone specifically impaired devel
opment, but each one added to the negative effect on the children.
The impact of adversity is often seen in clinical practice, but adver
sity at home or school is especially problematic for those children who
suffer from ADHD. Some children live with parents who are so preoc
cupied in their own struggles with finances, medical illness, chronic
marital conflict, depression, anxiety, addiction, and so forth that they
are unable to provide much consistent attention or support to their chil
dren, especially when a child has special needs beyond the needs of
most other children of the same age. Some mothers and fathers are
chronically engaged in intense conflicts with one another over their chil
dren, one harshly criticizing and punishing the child while the other
Differences Among Persons With ADHD 43
takes the opposing side, blindly defending the child against all criti
cism; both leave the child stressed and confused, caught in the paralyz
ing guilt of loyalty conflicts.
In some families with a child who has ADHD, parental conflict po
larizes over the issue of whether the child does or does not suffer from
impairments of ADHD. One parent may see treatment for ADHD as es
sential to the child’s future growth, whereas the other is steadfastly op
posed to such a diagnosis and to any accommodations or treatment for
ADHD. It is very difficult when one parent tells the child with ADHD,
“It’s really important for you to take your ADHD medication every
day,” while the other parent feels the medicine is likely to be harmful
and begs the child to avoid taking it.
In some other families, parents are consistently engaged in support
ing their child with ADHD. They encourage and support the child in his
or her schoolwork and in peer relationships; they keep in touch with the
child’s teachers and monitor the child’s homework on a daily basis.
They are realistic in recognizing areas in which the child needs extra
support or remedial tutoring, and they work hard to provide what is
needed, while also recognizing and encouraging the child’s strengths in
whatever academic or social domains they appear. Any child with
ADHD who has such support on a reasonably consistent basis is likely
to suffer much less frustration, stress, and shame compared with the
child with ADHD whose impairments are compounded by chronic
stress and lack of consistent support at home.
School experience can be a powerful source of stress or a resource of
support for the child with ADHD. Some teachers understand very well
that the student with ADHD may be suffering from dramatic inconsis
tency in academic performance or in classroom behavior not because of
willfulness or laziness but because that child, at that point in his or her
development, does not have adequate control. Faced with this ongoing
disappointment and frustration in their efforts to help that child while
also attending to the needs of their other students, some teachers are
quite patient, doing all they can to help such students.
Other teachers become chronically angry and confrontational with the
student who has ADHD, often shaming or punishing the student while
continually complaining to the child and his or her parents about the need
for improvement of these shortcomings, which the student may be unable
to control effectively. Encountering subtle indications or overt confronta
tions day after day from the teacher who is frequently disappointed, frus
trated, and annoyed with the student’s ADHD-related impairments can
seriously sabotage that child’s self-esteem as well as his or her motivation
for sustaining effort in school and hope for eventual success.
44 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
There are many types of environmental stress that can impact any
child or adult. Thus far, there have been few studies of how stressors
such as unemployment, change of residence, disruption of a close
friendship or marital relationship, excessive debt, incarceration, serious
illness, or death of a close friend or family member might exacerbate im
pairments of persons with ADHD. One study of adults with ADHD
found that those whose ADHD symptoms are more severe tend to also
experience a greater frequency of negative life events than do those with
less severe ADHD, regardless of whether they also have a comorbid
psychiatric disorder (Garcia et al. 2012). In some cases, the individual’s
ADHD-related impairments may have contributed substantially to the
negative life events (e.g., disruption of relationships, family conflict,
loss of employment). In other instances, it may be that individuals ex
perienced serious worsening of their ADHD because they were more se
verely deprived of adequate family security and support at critical
points in their growing up.
Summary
All individuals have difficulty with impairments characteristic of
ADHD sometimes, but that does not mean that everyone has ADHD.
The diagnosis is appropriately used only for those children, adoles
cents, or adults who persistently experience much more chronic and sig
nificant impairment in their executive functions than do most others of
the same age group. Persons with ADHD have these significant impair
ments in common. Nevertheless, there are many differences among
those who are appropriately diagnosed with ADHD.
This variety includes differences in severity of impairment, intensity
of hyperactivity and/or impulsiveness, age at onset, persistence or re
mission of symptoms, gender, underlying temperament, cognitive abil
ities, and environmental challenges and supports. The main point of
this chapter is that those with ADHD are not all alike in their strengths
or in their difficulties.
To understand and respond adequately to a person with ADHD, the
clinician needs to take into account the unique strengths and vulnera
bilities that emerge from that patient’s life history and current life situ
ation. In Chapter 4, I discuss how ADHD impacts persons at different
ages and stages of development, and in Chapter 7, I describe the ways in
which ADHD is often complicated by additional learning and/or psy
chiatric or medical disorders.
4
Ways ADHD Can
Impair Functioning at
ATTENTION-DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER
(ADHD) is now understood as essentially developmental impairment
of the brain’s self-management system, its executive functions. The
brain structures that support executive functions are not fully devel
oped in early childhood; they are among the slowest developing as
pects of the human brain. Early foundations for executive functions
begin to emerge quite gradually beginning in the second year of life, but
these complex systems for self-management do not fully mature until
the late teen years, early 20s, or later.
The model of executive functions introduced in Chapter 2, “A New
Model of ADHD,” includes six clusters (Brown 2001):
45
46 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
As each child grows older, adaptive skills such as those listed above
are expected to become stronger and to be more consistently demon
strated in a wider range of activities and settings without constant need
for adult step-by-step reminders or intervention. Being careful may ex
pand to riding a bike carefully in the street; doing tasks without need for
excessive supervision may apply to homework or household chores. As
the child reaches elementary school, expectations gradually develop for
additional adaptive skills.
lating daily routines undermine their success and may disrupt their
schooling and limit their career.
For some individuals who experience ADHD during childhood
and early adolescence, their ADHD-related impairments of executive
function gradually diminish as they reach their late teens and early
20s. Reasons for this are described in Chapter 3, “Differences Among
Persons With ADHD.” Those fortunate persons who experience con
siderable remission of their ADHD impairments may deal with the
additional expectations of young adulthood as effectively as most of
their peers (Sayal et al. 2015 ). For those whose ADHD impairments
persist, either for lack of diagnosis and adequate treatment or be
cause attempted treatments have been ineffective, the increased ex
pectations encountered during early adulthood may be extremely
challenging, placing them in situations where they are not able to
keep up with the levels of accomplishment attained by most of their
peers. For those affected, this may lead to much disappointment and
frustration, particularly if they become alienated from their more
successful peers and remain more obviously dependent on their par
ents for a variety of financial and practical supports than do most of
their age-mates. Yet even for those whose ADHD-related impair
ments become less severe as they get older, the following expecta
tions of young adulthood can be quite challenging.
ments need to be made. For those with persisting ADHD, caring for their
own health also involves obtaining and sustaining appropriate medical
and, if needed, mental health treatment for their ADHD impairments
and related disorders. Population studies have shown that those with
persisting ADHD have increased risk of premature death, usually due to
accidents (Dalsgaard et al. 2015b). One population study found sig
nificantly elevated risk of suicide or attempted suicide among per
sons with ADHD (Ljung et al. 2014).
• Contribute emotional support and necessary care to aging parents
and extended family. In addition to coping with the adaptive respon
sibilities described above, as adults get older, other concerns and
challenges often arise from aging parents and possibly other ex
tended family members—grandparents, siblings, aunts, uncles,
cousins, or others—who may need help with managing emerging
health problems or other life stresses. Loyalties to these family mem
bers, particularly as they enter their later years, often cause adults to
struggle with multiple demands and desires to provide emotional,
practical, and/or financial support, small or large, short term or lon
ger term. This involves time for staying in touch and helping out in
addition to the ongoing responsibilities or pressures from their im
mediate family, work, and other interests.
These responsibilities to parents and extended family are some
times quite limited and may be shared by numerous other family
members. Sometimes, however, such responsibilities may be ex
tremely burdensome and long-lasting in ways that would be over
whelming for any adult. For adults with persisting ADHD, managing
such multiple tasks and expectations may be extremely difficult, es
pecially when they arise as the adult himself or herself is dealing with
the various demands of daily life and also approaching a time when
more detailed planning for his or her own aging and retirement may
also need increasing consideration.
have lower income, had three times greater odds of being divorced or
never married, had a more limited network of social contacts, and re
ported higher levels of emotional loneliness than a comparison group of
adults of comparable age without ADHD (Michielsen et al. 2015a, 2015b).
It should be noted, however, that these are group data, and not all partic
ipants with ADHD experienced the reported impairments.
Summary
Executive functions depend upon an infrastructure composed of the
slowest developing structures of the human brain. Neuroscience data
demonstrate that although primordial foundations of those functions
begin to emerge in preschool years, the brain’s management system
does not approach maturity until an individual’s late teen years, early
twenties, or even later. In this chapter, I describe specific skills and adap
tive behaviors usually expected from individuals at various stages of
development from preschool years to adulthood and the ways in which
individuals with ADHD often struggle with those challenges over the
course of development.
The chapter does not include all the additional challenges often faced
by many in the general population as well as individuals with ADHD.
Examples include job losses, disruptive relationships in marriages and
parenting, persisting poverty, severe and chronic physical or mental ill
ness in key family members, untimely deaths of friends or family mem
bers, and any of the wide variety of misfortunes and bad luck that may
occur throughout life. What often is amazing is the way that many chil
dren, adolescents, and adults—with and without ADHD—often cope
extremely well with such misfortunes. However, among those with
ADHD, such additional stressors can become overwhelming.
5
How ADHD Impacts
“Brain Googling”
for Motivations
61
62 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
Google has collected and continues to update and index more than
100 million gigabytes of visual images and verbal content. When pre
sented with a query, Google rapidly searches its index to find relevant
content and seeks out additional information linked to that content.
Google then prioritizes that selected content according to its apparent de
gree of relevance to the immediate query and the frequency with which
each piece of content has been utilized in previous similar searches by
others. Google presents the results of all this searching and prioritizing
within seconds or milliseconds.
At present, there is no way to quantify how many units of visual and
verbal information are stored in the human brain, nor is there any measure
to quantify the rate and ways in which this information expands in the
brain from infancy across the years of an individual’s life span. Neverthe
less, most people would agree that any given individual’s unique store of
images and verbal content rapidly expands to massive proportions over
the course of life experiences. The unique differences between one person’s
information base and another’s and the expression of these differences are
major factors in what makes us puzzling and interesting to one another.
The primary difference between Google searches and any given in
dividual’s motivations, beyond the obvious differences in the size of the
information database, is the process by which relevance and prioritizing
of information are determined. Google prioritizes based on the rele
vance of manifest content, which words and synonyms match, and on
How ADHD Impacts “Brain Googling” for Motivations 63
Unconscious Prioritizing
by Emotions
Many people think of emotions as involving only conscious feelings,
limited to sensations of sadness, anger, pleasure, worry, and so on, that a
person is fully aware of and generally able to identify. Neuroscience has
shown that conscious feelings are only a tiny part of the rich and varie
gated range of emotions that operates within each person to motivate
executive functions. Neuroscientist Joaquin Fuster (2003) emphasized,
“Whereas we may be fully conscious of a retrieved memory—sometimes
How ADHD Impacts “Brain Googling” for Motivations 65
Many of the things we do, including the appraisal of the emotional sig
nificance of events in our lives and the expression of emotional behav
iors in response to those appraisals, do not depend on consciousness, or
even on processes that we necessarily have conscious access to. (p. 65)
[T]he emotional signal can operate entirely under the radar of conscious
ness. It can produce alterations in working memory, attention and reason
ing so that the decision-making process is biased toward selecting the action most
66 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
likely to lead to the best possible outcome, given prior experience. The individual
may not even be aware of this covert operation. (p. 148, emphasis added)
It was a difficult exam, and Jim was having a lot of trouble answering most
of the questions, possibly because he had not yet read even half of the chap
ters assigned for the test. Jim had put off doing any work on the exam for
several days. He had been preoccupied since an e-mail 3 days earlier from
his girlfriend back home. She wrote that she wanted to break up because he
was now too far away and she had gotten involved with someone else.
About 2 A.M., after struggling with the exam for several hours, Jim
decided to take a nap for a couple of hours and then try to finish the
exam. He set his alarm for 4 A.M. When the alarm buzzed, Jim woke up
for a few moments, turned the alarm off, and went back to sleep. He did
not wake up until 5 hours later. When he realized he had slept through
the deadline, Jim panicked. The professor had announced that he would
not accept any late exams. Recognizing that he would certainly get an F
on the midterm, Jim impulsively decided he was not ready to be in col
lege. Without discussing his decision with anyone, he packed his suit
case and left to go home, planning to stay there until the following fall,
when he would try again to go to college.
In talking with me back home about this a week later, Jim initially said
that dropping out of college was the best thing for him at that point. He
said he had been excited to go off to college, but the work seemed too hard
for him, he had not yet made any real friends, and he had really been
missing his girlfriend and his parents. He also claimed that getting an F
How ADHD Impacts “Brain Googling” for Motivations 67
on the midterm would have meant failing that course, so it made no sense
for him to continue any of his courses that semester. He could see no other
way to deal with that situation. He also mentioned that in coming home
he had hoped he could win back his girlfriend’s affections. As it turned
out, she was not at all interested in getting reinvolved with him.
Jim had quit many activities before. After successfully playing Little
League for many years, he had gone out for his high school baseball team
and then quit before the first game. He had started two different ad
vanced placement courses and then dropped each of them before the final
exam. He was quick to feel unsure of himself and quick to get himself out
of any situation where he was afraid he might not do well. Given his prior
experiences, quickly escaping from anything stressful felt like the best
possible outcome for him. He was biased toward early escape from stress.
Only after several months of psychotherapy was Jim able to see that
his “accidental” going back to sleep that morning, his failure to even dis
cuss his situation with his college advisor, and his assumption that he
faced inevitable failure were not actually the best choices for him. Those
decisions were all examples of his long-standing pattern of quickly jump
ing to conclusions and fearfully choosing to run away from any stressful
situation. Only gradually could he see the various ways he might have
been able to work things out so he could have made a success of that first
semester, despite his girlfriend’s decision to break up and despite his fail
ing that exam. He returned to college the next semester after gradually
finding ways to cope more effectively with his fears.
Jim’s situation is just one of many possible examples of how people of
ten bias their decisions. They do not realize how much their actions are
based on memories of past experiences, in which, for example, they learned
to run away too quickly from what they feared. Sometimes, people give up
too soon when they feel frustrated or discouraged, or they become de
manding and aggressive to try to get what they want from others. Often,
memories and associated emotions involved in such situations remain to
tally unconscious, and the person mistakenly thinks it is obvious that what
they choose is the best action. Looking back, it is possible to discern these
processes repeatedly operating in the patterns of a lifetime of relationships.
our present are very much like one or another person in our past. Some
times, people in our current lives may be quite similar in one or many
ways to others we have known, but at other times, we may assume they
are similar when, in fact, they are not.
John Bowlby (1978), pioneer developer of attachment theory, wrote
about the persistence of early experience in choices and relationships we
make throughout our lifetime. He used the term representational models
to refer to individuals’ persisting personal views of self and others—
their aggregated expectations, complex and only partly conscious, of
persons to whom they have been attached. These models or memories
include our image of who a person is and what that person wants, in
tends, and will do. They also include our picture of how the person
thinks about us, how much that person may be interested in us, how
much he or she likes or does not like us, and how much he or she will ap
prove or disapprove of us and our actions and attitudes.
Bowlby suggested that we tend to rely heavily on our representa
tional models of the persons we have been attached to during childhood
and adolescence, particularly our parents, siblings, and other family
members. These attachment figures tend to be a continuing source of ex
pectations about others whom we meet throughout our lives. He also
emphasized that each of us develops representational models of our
selves from our earlier patterns of interactions with family, classmates,
and friends we are close to. He claims that we tend to hang on to the pic
tures of ourselves that we have developed over the years of our growing
up, and we often tend to keep on using those old pictures of what kind
of person we are and how others will probably think of us and deal with
us. We are often unaware of how much we cluelessly bring our past into
our present. Here is the way Bowlby (1978) described this process:
The mother explained that after each time her husband scolded their
sons so harshly, he would later calm down and then mumble an apology
to the boys. She said, “I know he loves them both and would give his life
for either one of them, but when he gets wound up in one of those
Wednesday night episodes, he gets so enraged that he seems to forget
that those are his sons whom he loves and wants to protect. He really
doesn’t want to hurt them or have them believe they’re destined for fail
ure. All he knows in that moment is that he is furious with both of them
for not having done that chore.”
Any parent can lose his or her temper with a child occasionally, but
most parents, most of the time, can express their frustration to the child
without such an intense verbal attack. They can usually keep in mind
that this is their child whom they love and want to nurture and protect,
despite the frustrations of the moment. Their memory of that larger con
text allows them to avoid an excessively harsh display of rage, even
though they are feeling intense frustration and anger. Their working
memory allows them to hold in mind their love even while their anger is
taking up a lot of space in their head.
Unfortunately, individuals with ADHD often suffer from significant
impairments of working memory that frequently impact not only their
ability to remember where they have left their keys, what time they
promised to meet someone, what they have just read or heard, and what
they were about to say or do. For many, those working memory impair
ments also impact their ability to remember their love for someone
while they are angry at him or her or to keep in mind their hopes for that
person when they feel disappointed by him or her. Flooded with one
emotion in that moment, they are unable to keep in mind other thoughts
and emotions that are important and relevant for guiding their actions.
ADHD researchers have repeatedly demonstrated impairments in
working memory as a central feature of this disorder (Alderson et al.
2013; Fried et al. 2016; Kennedy et al. 2016; Martinussen et al. 2005;
Quinlan and Brown 2003), but not much has been written to clarify how
this memory works or possible causes of working memory impairments
commonly seen in ADHD.
How ADHD Impacts “Brain Googling” for Motivations 71
aware that the task should be done and may even feel that they want to
get it started, but they simply do not do it. They may report that they feel
urgency for the task they are avoiding, but they persist in avoiding it.
Despite their being consciously aware of the urgency, they tend not to
feel sufficient urgency until the deadline is imminent.
Some researchers have used the concept of delay aversion to describe
the tendency of many with ADHD, more than their peers, to have a neg
ative reaction to having to wait, a tendency to want to take a smaller
payoff received immediately rather than to wait for a bigger payoff that
will be received only after a delay (Sonuga-Barke 2005). A related con
cept is delay discounting, a tendency of some persons to think of a reward
or punishment as less appealing (rewards) or less frightening or intim
idating (unpleasant consequences), depending on how much farther
down the road it is likely to be encountered (Da Matta et al. 2012). In
both cases, a potential payoff or punishment is seen by the individual as
being not so much to be concerned about in comparison with whatever
payoff is likely to be available more immediately, in the here and now.
Having ADHD is like having erectile dysfunction of the brain. If the task
you are faced with is something that strongly interests you, if it really
turns you on, you’re “up” for it and you can perform. But if the task is
not something that really interests you, then you can’t get it up and you
can’t perform. In that situation, it doesn’t matter how much you may say
to yourself, “I need to, I ought to, I should.” You can’t make it happen.
It’s just not a willpower kind of thing. (Brown 2005a)
long and busy day. A protracted argument ensued; it was almost 3 A.M.
before they were able to resolve this situation and get to sleep.
When the husband’s alarm clock buzzed at 6 A.M., he awakened for a
moment and thought about how he felt exhausted and needed to get more
sleep before going to work. He turned the alarm off and went back to sleep,
intending to get up after another half hour. He awoke 2 hours later and
rushed to shower, dress, and get to his office. He arrived at the office 2 hours
late, just as the mandatory weekly staff meeting was concluding. Only then
did he remember that on the previous day his boss had warned him that his
job would be in jeopardy if he did not stop his frequent habit of arriving late
to work. He had not even thought about that very recent warning when he
turned off his alarm clock at 6 A.M. to get more sleep. He was too immersed
in his strong wish for more sleep and did not remember that he had recently
been warned that he might lose his job if he continued his pattern of being
chronically late to work. He lost sight of the threat to his job because his
working memory failed him. He was unable to retrieve the memory of that
important warning he had received the preceding day. He remained too en
grossed in the emotion of the moment.
Another way in which context influences motivation is illustrated in
the following example. My wife and I were on a transatlantic flight, for
tunate enough to ride in business class where mid flight the flight crew
baked chocolate chip cookies to serve only to passengers in business
seats. The fragrance of the cookies baking wafted through the cabin, and
I really wanted one. However, at that time I was on a diet trying to lose a
few pounds, and my wife was serving as the food police, reminding me to
avoid eating too many sweets. I resolved to decline the cookies and felt
proud of myself for abstaining until I noticed that my wife, sitting next to
me, was asleep...This put me in considerable conflict between my wish to
maintain my diet and my wish to eat one of those delicious cookies. I
looked again to be sure she was asleep, and she was. At that moment, the
flight attendant held out a tray of cookies and asked if I wanted one, so I
said, “Yes, please” and also asked if she would give me a second one for
my wife, who was asleep. As soon as I got the cookies, I ate both of them
and then hustled down the galley to dispose of the evidence. I didn’t
bother my wife with the facts until later in the trip, and we laughed about
it. However, I can honestly report that if she had been awake, I would
have declined the cookie, feeling somewhat deprived but also proud of
myself for sticking to my diet. This is an example of how one can have
conflicting motives and be swayed sharply toward one or the other de
pending on the circumstances of where one is and who else is present.
78 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
Summary
This chapter delves further into the “central mystery of ADHD,” which
is the puzzling fact that persons with ADHD suffer chronic impair
ments in their ability to exercise adequate executive functions in many
activities of daily life, yet they are able to exercise those same executive
functions quite well for a few specific tasks or activities that arouse
strong interest or strong fear. This mystery involves situational specific
ity of unconscious motivations.
In this chapter, I describe the importance and complexity of uncon
scious emotions in motivation for all people. I then propose that those
with ADHD tend to suffer chronic deficits in motivation for many tasks
of daily life because they suffer from chronic impairments of working
memory. The more limited bandwidth of their working memory re
stricts their ability to keep in mind the larger context, important aspects
of the situation, and the future beyond the present moment. This often
leads to inadequate motivation for tasks or activities for which their
emotions are not strong enough to stimulate heightened release of do
pamine at the involved neuronal junctions.
6
How ADHD
Develops, Sometimes
Sometimes Improves
79
80 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
There are three major ways in which research thus far has found brain
development and functioning to be different in individuals with ADHD:
Maturation of Functional
Connectedness Within the Brain
The cortical imaging and white matter studies discussed in the previous
section all reported on structural differences in brain development of
persons with ADHD versus those without. Other studies have used
functional MRI (fMRI) to study the interactive connectedness of two dif
ferent regions of the cortex in adults who had been carefully diagnosed
with ADHD in childhood (Mattfeld et al. 2014; Sporns 2011, 2012; Sri
pada et al. 2014). These studies assessed not how well those structures of
the brain’s management system were built but how effectively they in
teracted for mutual communication.
Research has demonstrated that the human brain is organized into
large functional networks over which patterns of communication from
one region of the brain change rapidly depending on what is currently
going on within the brain in response to what is happening in the im
mediate environment. Scientists study these networks with fMRI to see
How ADHD Develops, May Get Worse, or May Improve 85
Dynamics of Electrochemical
Communications in the Brain
The third area where research has demonstrated differences in brains of
those with ADHD is the process of communication between groups of
neurons that compose the networks of the brain. To grasp how ADHD
can impair electrochemical communication in the brain, it is necessary
to understand the basic principles of how neurons communicate (more
information about this process is presented in Chapter 10, “Practical As
pects of Medication Treatments for ADHD”).
The building blocks of the human cortex are neurons, tiny cells that are
only one-millionth of an inch across. Each neuron has a cell body on
which develop thousands of tiny branches called dendrites; these den
drites receive information from other neurons. Each neuron also has one
extension, called an axon, for sending information out to other cells; axons
can range in length from less than a millimeter to more than a meter long.
Neurons make contact with other neurons at junctions called synapses.
Every neuron establishes 1,000 or more synapses with surrounding neu
rons. Neurons are not actually wired together; they do not touch each
other.
At each synapse, there is a tiny gap between the back end of one neu
ron and the front end of the next. The whole network system works on
low-voltage electrical impulses. Each of the messages carried between
neurons is a tiny electrical charge of about 0.1 volt. As this moving
charge reaches each synapse, it has to jump the gap like the spark of a
spark plug. This jump across each synaptic gap depends on release of
microdots of chemicals manufactured in each neuron.
Most neurons release just one specific transmitter chemical, although
some make and release more than one. Each time a message-carrying
How ADHD Develops, May Get Worse, or May Improve 87
improvement is huge. For others, it is substantial but not huge. And for
about 20% of those with ADHD, current medications for ADHD are not
helpful or cause side effects that require stopping use of the medication.
Several different types of research have demonstrated how stimu
lant medications affect brain functioning in persons with ADHD. Sev
eral fMRI studies of children and adults have shown that stimulant
medication can alleviate excessive distractibility typical of many with
ADHD by reducing excessive drifting into default mode and strength
ening task-focused networks (Peterson et al. 2009). Other fMRI studies
of children with ADHD have shown that stimulant medication normal
izes the attentional network and improves motivational networks for
tasks (Rubia et al. 2009, 2011; Wong and Stevens 2012).
Positron emission tomography (PET) studies have demonstrated
that stimulant medications can facilitate engagement of external task fo
cus and deactivate the default mode network to reduce task disruption
by excessive distractibility (Volkow et al. 2008, 2012). Other PET studies
have shown that stimulant medications, when active, can enhance mo
tivation for tasks that otherwise are uninteresting (Volkow et al. 2004).
PET studies have also shown that methylphenidate and amphetamine
in clinically appropriate doses can increase the amount of neurotrans
mitters such as dopamine and norepinephrine released into the synapse
to improve signaling between neurons in specific circuits (Villemagne et
al. 1999; Volkow et al. 2009).
Up to this point in this chapter, I have focused on what has been
learned thus far about development of the brain, patterns of functional
connectivity in the brain of individuals with ADHD, and electrochemi
cal communication between neurons. In the remainder of this chapter, I
focus on questions of when and how ADHD tends to get worse and/or
better.
they are being too pushy or too demanding. They feel chronically be
wildered about how to respond to classmates’ teasing or how to get
others to let them join in a conversation or a game. They may repeat
edly be too bossy and be excluded by playmates, or they may simply
retreat into solitary activities, avoiding the risk of peer rejection by
immersing themselves in playing video games.
Some children with ADHD need much more support from parents
or teachers for doing their schoolwork and/or managing social inter
actions. This need for extra support may emerge early in preschool
years, or it may not become noticeable until the child enters middle
school or high school, when more independent self-management is
expected. For some, the need for extra support does not emerge no
ticeably until the adolescent is preparing to move away from home to
go to college. For those who need such support and do not receive it
or who receive too much support and do not have ample chances to
learn to manage for themselves, such activities at various stages of
development may become almost as perilous as trying to cross a busy
street before they have learned how to do it.
porary, relief from these painful emotions. The problem is that occa
sional use can readily lead to more chronic use, which can rapidly lead
to a persistent cycle of addiction from which it may be extremely diffi
cult to recover. Addiction to these substances can result in worsening
of ADHD impairments in multiple aspects of schooling, employment,
social relationships, and other areas of daily life.
decade or two of life; they do not encompass the full range of devel
opment across the life span. Yet the few studies that have explored
ADHD during adulthood, especially those that have looked at
midlife and beyond, clearly indicate that for those individuals whose
ADHD persists into middle adulthood and beyond, significant im
pairments tend to remain and sometimes worsen.
One population-based study of more than 2,000 men and women
ages 47–54 years found that 6.2% reported significant symptoms of
ADHD; no difference in ADHD symptoms was found between men
and women in this sample (Das et al. 2012). Evaluation of those who
reported significant ADHD impairments found that those individu
als were less likely to be employed full-time, struggled more with
physical health problems, and reported more problems in personal
relationships and in their personal finances as well as lower quality
of social life and well-being.
With or without ADHD, there are a number of physical changes
associated with aging in both males and females. Imaging studies
have demonstrated age-related decline in various elements of the
brain that provide infrastructure for executive functioning, even for
healthy adults without ADHD (Backman and Farde 2005; Gazzaley
and D'Esposito 2005; Raz 2005). Volkow et al. (1996) found a 6.6% de
crease per decade of life in availability of dopamine transporters in
healthy volunteers. Age-related decline in brain dopamine activity
even in healthy volunteers has also been documented in several other
regions of brain important for executive functions.
White matter decreases in the brain are also associated with aging
in the general population. One study found that the total length of
white matter fibers decreases by 10% per decade of life in the general
population, up to a total decline of about 45% by age 80 years, with
about a 16% greater average decline in females (Marner et al. 2003). It
should be noted, however, that these percentages of decline in the
general population are based on averages that may mask consider
able variability among various individuals.
Very little research has assessed ADHD in the geriatric population.
Many health care practitioners tend to assume that any attentional
difficulties experienced by elderly individuals are due simply to the
slow degenerative processes of aging or, possibly, to the early stages
of dementia. Adult patients sometimes fear that their ADHD predis
poses them to onset of Alzheimer’s or some other variety of demen
tia. Currently, there is no evidence to support that assumption.
ADHD in the older population may be mistakenly diagnosed as
mild neurocognitive disorder, a disorder that involves some cogni
94 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
tive decline that does not interfere with the capacity for independence
in everyday activities. Mild neurocognitive disorder is sometimes,
but certainly not always, a prelude to onset of dementia. Screening for
ADHD in any elderly person who presents with symptoms of mild
neurocognitive disorder may be helpful not only for increasing un
derstanding of possible relationships between these two disorders
but also for identifying adults whose cognitive impairments may be
due to lifelong problems with ADHD rather than to geriatric deterio
ration (Ivanchak et al. 2012).
Changes associated with menopause are an aspect of aging that is
associated with cognitive impairments similar to ADHD. Women
with no childhood history of ADHD, many of them well-educated
and high-functioning businesswomen and professional women, re
port onset of ADHD-like impairments of working memory, organiza
tional skills, and ability to sustain focus that appear coincident with
their decline of estrogen and cessation of their menses. This associa
tion makes sense in that estrogen is one of the primary modulators of
the release of dopamine in the female brain. Insufficiency of dopa
mine in the brain networks that manage executive functions is one of
the major problems associated with ADHD. Studies published by
groups at Yale and the University of Pennsylvania have demonstrated
that medications used for treatment of ADHD may help to alleviate
these midlife-onset impairments of executive functions (Epperson et
al. 2011, 2015; Shanmugan et al. 2017).
ally, the first few years of recovery are the most difficult, as the indi
vidual struggles to cope with a variety of emotional problems that
may have been the initial precipitants of the excessive substance use.
In some cases, recovery is helped substantially by the individual’s
finding productive work, which provides satisfaction and financial
rewards that give the person in recovery motivation to continue to
sustain their recovery.
Another factor that helps some persons who have substance use
disorders, persistent depression, or excessive anxiety is finding
someone with whom they are able to develop a close friendship or
perhaps even a life partnership. Finding and sustaining a mutually
beneficial relationship with someone can be a strong factor in helping
individuals make important changes in their previous maladaptive
lifestyle.
Summary
This chapter begins with an explanation of the primary cause of ADHD:
it is genetic. This disorder is highly heritable; however, what is inherited
is not the disorder itself but vulnerability to the disorder. Environmental
factors play a significant role, directly and indirectly influencing brain
development and functioning. Several major ways in which research
has demonstrated that brain development and functioning in individu
als with ADHD differs from those who do not have ADHD are also de
scribed: 1) brain structure and maturation; 2) maturation of functional
neural connectedness within the brain; and 3) dynamics of electrochem
ical communications in the brain.
ADHD is not a static condition. Across situations and over the
course of a lifetime, sometimes it gets worse and sometimes it gets bet
ter. This chapter outlines six factors that may cause ADHD impairments
to worsen and seven factors that may help to improve the functioning of
those with ADHD in ways that significantly enhance their functioning
and reduce their impairments in daily life.
7
How and Why Other
Disorders Often
101
102 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
odds ratio for each disorder. An odds ratio of 1.0 indicates that preva
lence of the disorder is the same for persons with ADHD as for persons
of the same age in the general population; 3.0 indicates three times the
base rate of persons of the same age in the general population.
Data from these three studies (Collaborative Multisite Multimodal
Treatment Study of Children With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disor
der, National Survey of Children’s Health, and National Comorbidity Sur
vey Replication) highlight the fact that children and adults with ADHD
tend to have significantly more learning or psychiatric disorders than those
without ADHD. This raises an important question: Why is it that children
and adults with ADHD are so much more likely to have multiple learning
or psychiatric problems? Are they just especially unlucky?
One possible explanation for the high rates of co-occurring disorders
associated with ADHD is that ADHD is not just one more among other
psychiatric disorders. It is a foundational disorder in that executive
function impairments of ADHD also underlie many other disorders.
Neuroscientist Joaquin Fuster (2003) emphasizes the critical role of at
tention, broadly defined, for all aspects of cognitive activity:
Thus, the critical requirement for automaticity is for the reader to encode
the relevant items in memory and to retrieve them on a subsequent en
counter, and for both encoding and retrieval, attention is central. (Shay
witz and Shaywitz 2008, p. 1332)
are not so important for decoding and pronouncing single words, but
they are critical for comprehension of sentences and paragraphs. This
was described by Sesma and colleagues (2009):
sentences; when doing math, one starts with a stated problem. How
ever, when translating thoughts into sentences and paragraphs on a
blank screen or piece of paper, we must focus in order to plan and orga
nize our thoughts in coherent sequences, deciding what to say first, se
lecting appropriate words and images, and deciding how much to
elaborate and what to delete so we say enough to communicate each
thought and not so much that the writing is boring or redundant. We
need to maintain connections within each paragraph while also keeping
in mind links with what we have written in preceding paragraphs and
also what will follow. Then, we need to review and revise to clarify and
shape our intended message. All of these functions require integrated
operation of our executive functions.
Another aspect of executive functions essential to reading, mathe
matics, and written expression is activation—alertness and motivation
to perform the tasks. If a person is not motivated to read a text or to try
to solve a math problem or to write sentences and paragraphs, those
tasks are not likely to be adequately done, even if the person has the req
uisite skills. A study of junior high students who were asked to read so
cial studies texts found that the importance a student attributes to
reading a particular text is a more important determinant of whether or
not the student will comprehend a text than is the individual’s cognitive
capacity to read well.
If I’m reading some text that doesn’t really interest me, it’s as though I’m
just licking the words and not chewing them. My eyes go over each
word and I understand what all of it means as I’m reading it, but I’m just
not engaged enough to get the information to stick inside my head.
(Brown 2013, p. 140)
Anxiety Disorders
How Is ADHD Related to Anxiety Disorders?
Anxiety disorders are much more common among children and adults
with ADHD than in the general population. In samples of children with
ADHD, the incidence of anxiety disorders ranges from 9.6% to 34% (Jen
sen et al. 2001; Yoshimasu et al. 2012). Among adults with ADHD, rates
range from 27.9% to 47.1% (Kessler et al. 2005; Van Ameringen et al.
2011). A meta-analysis found that across multiple studies, the average
rate of anxiety disorders in children with ADHD was three times the
rate among children in the general population. A nationwide study of
adults with ADHD found the rate of anxiety to be 3.7 times as high as
among the general population of adults in the United States.
Anxiety disorders come in many different forms. Some individuals
have just one anxiety disorder, whereas others are burdened by several
types of anxiety disorders. Many children and some adults have phobic
fears. For example, they may get very tense, worried, and upset when they
get near to or expect very soon to get too close to a dog. Suddenly, their
heart beats a lot faster, they imagine themselves being attacked by the
dog, they feel panicky, and they want to get away from the dog as quickly
as possible. They then want to continue to avoid that dog and, perhaps,
any other dog. As soon as they can avoid the dog and safely stay away
from it, the fear usually diminishes quickly. Other examples of phobia in
clude intense fear of snakes, bees, thunder and lightning, being in high
places, getting an injection, seeing blood, or flying in an airplane.
Many children and some adults have one or more such fears that are
stronger, more persistent, and more problematic than is usual for their
108 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
peers. Usually, fears are not diagnosed as phobic unless they are mark
edly more intense, more frequent, and significantly out of proportion to
any actual danger posed by the specific object or situation.
Social anxiety is a type of phobia in which a person is almost always in
tensely fearful of getting into social situations with others whom they do
not already know and trust. They worry that other people will see them
as unappealing, boring, ridiculous, incompetent, socially awkward, ugly,
immature, offensive, stupid, or in some other way not fitting in with oth
ers in the situation. To warrant diagnosis, this anxiety must be way out of
proportion to any realistic threat in the situation. For children, this diag
nosis is fitting only if the social anxiety occurs not only with adults but
also with peers.
Sometimes social phobia appears only when the person enters into
an unfamiliar group or situation where he or she will be expected to in
teract with others. For others with social phobia, their fear is just as in
tense even if they are simply eating in a restaurant or doing some other
activity among strangers where they may be observed or expect to be
noticed by others.
The primary way that those with social phobia usually try to avoid
feeling too anxious is to stay away from unfamiliar situations where they
believe they are likely to have to interact or to be observed in a negative
way. Often, their fear is not only that they will be overtly treated in a neg
ative way; they are often intensely intimidated by what they assume oth
ers are thinking about them, even if other people are not speaking to or
even actively noticing the worried person. Typically, the social anxiety
does not occur when the person is with family or others whom he or she
knows well and trusts to not be exaggeratedly critical or rejecting.
Another type of anxiety disorder is panic disorder: brief but recurrent
episodes of sudden, overwhelming fear. Typically, a panic attack in
volves sudden onset of dizziness, sweating, trembling, shortness of
breath, chest pain, and rapid heart rate, sometimes with pounding heart
sensations that cause the person to feel that he or she is suffering a heart
attack and is about to die. For some people, these terrifying sensations
come without any warning and are not associated with a known fear;
they usually subside within 10 or 15 minutes. For others, a panic attack
may be expected to occur because they are approaching a setting or sit
uation in which a panic attack has been experienced previously. Panic
attacks may occur once or twice a week for months at a time, or they
may be experienced in frequent batches (e.g., once or more often every
day for a week or two) and then not for many months. Often, the most
problematic aspect of panic disorder is the individual’s worrying in
tensely about when and where another panic attack might occur.
How and Why Other Disorders Often Co-occur With ADHD 109
Depressive Disorders
How Is ADHD Related to Depressive Disorders?
Children and adults with ADHD have considerably greater likelihood
of having a depressive disorder sometime in their lifetime than do per
sons without ADHD. Rates for depression in children with ADHD
range from 9.6% to 34%, much higher than the 1.4%–7.2% in the general
population of children. A meta-analysis of 21 epidemiological studies
found that the odds of a youth diagnosed with ADHD also being diag
nosed with depression are more than fivefold higher than for youth
without ADHD (Angold et al. 1999).
The importance of depression in children with ADHD is highlighted
by research that demonstrated that children with ADHD and comorbid
mood disorders are three times more likely to complete suicide than
those diagnosed with either ADHD or mood disorder alone (James et al.
2004). Rate of depression for adults with ADHD is approximately 31%,
considerably above the 17% rate for adults in the general population
(Kessler et al. 2006).
One of the two major types of depression is persistent depressive dis
order (formerly dysthymia). This diagnosis is not usually made unless the
person has suffered from this syndrome for at least 2 years (1 year for
children or adolescents). One of my young adult patients described his
experience with persistent depressive disorder as follows:
How and Why Other Disorders Often Co-occur With ADHD 111
For the past couple of years, I’ve been walking around feeling bummed
out. I can still keep doing most of what I need to do each day, like getting
to work and picking up groceries once in a while. But I almost never enjoy
any of it, including things I really used to enjoy doing, like seeing my
friends and going out. I don’t have much energy, and I tend to get down
on myself quite a bit. Occasionally, something interesting will happen
and I can get into that for a while, but as soon as the interesting part is
over, I go right back to that same dark feeling: “Everything sucks, every
thing always sucks, everything always will suck. That’s just the way it is.”
It’s been nearly 2 months now, and almost every day I feel completely
hopeless. Often, I feel dead tired but just can’t get to sleep, or I fall asleep
for a few hours and then I’m wide awake and can’t get back to sleep even
though it’s the middle of the night. There have been a few times when
I’ve stayed in bed for a couple of days, half sleeping or half awake, just
getting up once in a while to get a drink of water or use the bathroom,
not able to get myself to go to work or do anything productive. I don’t
feel like talking with anybody and I don’t feel like eating much of any
thing. When I’m awake, I just keep thinking about how much of a hope
less failure I’ve always been and how everyone would probably be
better off if I weren’t around anymore.
Episodes of major depressive disorder may last for just a few months
or for a year or more. Some people have just one such episode in their
lifetime. Others may have recurrent episodes of major depression inter
spersed with periods of no depression. Still others suffer from persistent
depressive disorder for much of their life, with episodes of major de
pression occurring from time to time.
sion (Young et al. 2016). Findings showed that both those currently de
pressed and those who were depressed but whose depression had
remitted demonstrated less activation of the amygdala and related cir
cuits when recalling positive memories from their life and greater brain
activation when recalling negative autobiographical memories. Their
brain activity was significantly biased for recall of negative experiences
of the past. The recall bias problem demonstrated in these patients with
depression but not ADHD is likely to be even stronger in those with
ADHD who experience depression or have experienced depression,
given the weakness of working memory usually found in those with
ADHD.
Another perspective on the linkages between depression and ADHD is
provided by data from genetic studies. A major study of genetic linkages
between psychiatric disorders found significant moderate correlation
between ADHD and major depressive disorder based on genome-wide
single-nucleotide polymorphism data (Lee et al. 2013).
Bipolar Disorders
How Is ADHD Related to Bipolar Disorders?
For many years, the diagnosis of bipolar disorder was classified with
depressive disorders as though they were all just variations of the same
processes. More recent research has highlighted differences that make it
important to conceptualize depression and bipolar disorder as two sep
arate categories of significant problems with mood. Although incidence
rates of bipolar disorder in the general population are quite low, about
3.9% in adults (Kessler et al. 2005), those with this disorder have ex
tremely high rates of substance use disorders and premature death due
to suicide and associated medical conditions, as well as substantial role
impairments, e.g., ability to work, and elevated rates of hospitalization.
When the full spectrum of bipolar syndromes, including subthreshold
cases, is considered, incidence in the general population increases to al
most 6% (Merikangas et al. 2007).
Reported rates of bipolar disorder among adults diagnosed with
ADHD are about 17% (Millstein et al. 1997). Among children with
ADHD, estimated incidence of bipolar disorder ranges from 2.4% to
21%; the wide differences in those rates depend on whether or not re
searchers required that bipolar symptoms occurred in episodes, as stip
ulated by DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association 1994). Including
those who tend to have consistently irritable mood without episodes of
nonagitated, euthymic mood inflates the estimated incidence of bipolar
114 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
Obsessive-Compulsive
and Related Disorders
How Is ADHD Related to Obsessive-Compulsive
and Related Disorders?
Obsessive-compulsive and related disorders are a grouping of disorders that
involve either or both of two aspects: 1) recurrent thoughts, images, or
urges that persist, even though the individual wishes to avoid or forget
them, and 2) repetitive behaviors or mental actions that the person feels
driven to do and that result in anxiety if the behavior is not done, even
though the person may recognize that these actions do not make much
sense or provide much realistic benefit.
Prevalence of obsessive-compulsive disorder is reported to be about
2%–4% in the general population of children and adolescents; preva
lence in adults is about 1.5%–3% in the general population. Among chil
dren and adolescents diagnosed with ADHD, the incidence ranges from
6% to 33% (Geller and Brown 2009; Storch et al. 2008). There is great va
riety in the way obsessive-compulsive and related disorders present; a
few examples are provided below.
not do it and probably never would ever attend church again. In psycho
therapy, he disclosed that about 6 weeks earlier, while in church, he had
the thought “What would happen if I stood up during this quiet prayer
time and said ‘Fuck you!’ out loud?” He thought of how embarrassing it
would be for him and his parents. He told himself that he would never
actually do such a thing, but he kept thinking about it every day and
could not get it out of his mind. Every Sunday for the next month he
struggled in church, terrified that he would suddenly lose control, get
up and do it. Finally, to avoid the risk, he stubbornly refused to go to
church, although he felt unable to explain his reason to his parents.
This boy’s sudden refusal to attend church services was the result of
his being terrified of his unacceptable urge to embarrass himself in a
public place. He was puzzled and frightened by this persisting thought,
terrified that he might act on it. Gradually, in psychotherapy it became
clear that this was related to his feeling increasing guilty about mastur
bating to erotic fantasies.
a pedestrian
destrians were waiting at the curbside to cross, he had to check his rear
view mirror to make certain that there was no pedestrian lying injured in
the crosswalk. If he could not see that all were safe, he felt compelled to
drive around the block and drive once again through that intersection to
verify that he had not injured anyone.
mind and loses the ability to prioritize which objects are worth keeping and
which ones need to be released or discarded.
volving more than 4,000 persons with ADHD and 6,000 persons without
ADHD assessed at an average age of 18.9 years. Numbers shown repre
sent pooled odds ratios in which 1.0 indicates no difference from the non-
ADHD sample and 2.0 indicates twice the likelihood of the non-ADHD
sample (Lee et al. 2011).
These data from multiple studies indicate that overall substance use
disorder is more than two and a half times as frequent among young
adults with ADHD than in the general population of similar age (Lee et
al. 2011). The negative impact of substance use disorders combined with
ADHD is substantial, particularly their impact on education. A massive
study of 29,000 adults in the United States found that failure to graduate
from high school by the usual age of 18 years was significantly more
common among persons with ADHD in combination with a substance
use disorder than among persons with any other psychiatric disorder
combined with substance use disorder (Breslau et al. 2011). Often sub
stance use disorders become sufficiently impairing that the individual
seeks treatment. A meta-analysis of 29 studies found that one out of ev
ery four patients seeking treatment for substance use disorder also had
ADHD (van Emmerik-van Oortmerssen et al. 2012).
attention problems assessed at the time of school entry are associated with
lower academic achievement, as measured by standardized tests, at the end
of primary school...and at the end of high school....This increased burden
in students with ADHD to perform the tasks that underlie academic perfor
mance (i.e. working memory, processing speed, organization of informa
tion) may have cumulative negative effects. (Breslau et al. 2011, p. 299)
Odds ratio
Nicotine 2.82
Alcohol 1.7
Marijuana 2.29
Cocaine 2.05
Note. Odds ratio of 1.0 indicates no difference between persons with ADHD and per
sons without ADHD; odds ratio of 2.0 indicates that persons with ADHD have twice
Clinical experience supports this view that social context often plays
an important role in substance use disorders. Individuals who use
drugs of abuse excessively often withdraw from their previous social re
lationships and tend to spend more time with others with whom they
can join comfortably in more excessive drug use. This can provide com
panionship with others less likely to be judgmental of their drug use; it
may also provide connections where they can more easily procure
drugs not legally available to them.
124 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
with ADHD and 996 children with ASD; a significant number of genetic
variants in each group also appear in the other. Di Martino et al. (2013)
used fMRI to identify overlapping functional networks in both cortical
and subcortical areas of the brain in 151 children: 56 diagnosed with
ASD, 45 diagnosed with ADHD, and 50 typical children; their study
also identified distinctive areas of the brain that differentiated ASD
from ADHD.
It is important to note that although genetic influences are related to
problems with social interaction, language difficulties, and social anxi
ety in individuals with ASD, familial studies also identify strengths of
persons with ASD. Simon Baron-Cohen (2000; Baron-Cohen et al. 1998)
found that parents of children with ASD or Asperger’s disorder were
more likely to be engineers, physicists, or mathematicians. Ozonoff et al.
(2002) observed that genetics associated with ASD brings a certain dis
tinctive style of thinking, relating, and reacting to the world that has
both limitations and strengths.
Individuals with ADHD tend to be more fully engaged in and more
motivated for interactions with other persons than most individuals
with ASD or Asperger’s disorder, yet both share limitations that impact
their social interactions in varying degrees and various contexts. Nij
meijer et al. (2008) pointed out the following:
These difficulties often persist in adults with ADHD just as they of
ten persist in adults with ASD, but in both groups the difficulties in so
cial interactions depend considerably on the specifics of the social
situation.
was controversial and remains so, pending further research. Many re
searchers and clinicians consider having a separate diagnosis for As
perger’s disorder to be more useful because of significant differences
between it and other disorders classified on the autism spectrum (Volk
mar et al. 2014). These differences are described briefly in Chapter 12,
“Treatment Adaptations for ADHD With Various Complications.”
According to DSM-5, ASD is characterized by two major impair
ments: 1) “persistent deficits in social communication and social inter
action across multiple contexts” and 2) “restricted, repetitive patterns of
behavior, interests, or activities” (American Psychiatric Association
2013, p. 50). These may occur with or without accompanying intellec
tual impairment and with or without accompanying language impair
ment; ASD is diagnosed in one of three classes depending on the level of
support needed by the individual.
Executive functions play a central role in social-emotional function
ing, just as they do in many other aspects of daily life. Social and emo
tional functioning depend on executive functions described in the
model of ADHD presented in Chapter 2. These functions include the
following:
Summary
There is strong evidence that children, adolescents, and adults with
ADHD tend to have other learning or psychiatric disorders at some point
in their lives more often than do those without ADHD. In this chapter, I
describe research on the frequency of specific learning disorders, anxiety
disorders, depressive disorders, bipolar disorders, oppositional defiant
and conduct disorders, obsessive-compulsive and related disorders, sub
stance use disorders, and autism spectrum disorder.
How and Why Other Disorders Often Co-occur With ADHD 127
Teenagers, and
129
130 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
now I want to go to sleep,” how long does it usually take for you to
actually fall asleep? How many nights in a typical week does it take
you more than half an hour to get to sleep? Once you fall asleep, do
you sleep through the night, or do you get up to use the bathroom or
get a drink? If you wake up during the night, how long does it usu
ally take you to get back to sleep? How difficult is it for you to get up
and out of bed in the morning when you must go to school or work?
Can you get up by yourself with just an alarm clock, or do you need
another person to wake you up? Do you snore when you sleep? If
you do snore, do you sometimes hold your breath while you’re
sleeping, so someone nearby might think you’ve stopped breathing?
How often are you late to work or school when it is your fault? If you
are late, how late are you? During the daytime, do you find that you
get drowsy and have a hard time keeping yourself awake? Do you
take naps? If so, how often and usually for how long?
12. Appetite and physical development. Ask about the patient’s
physical development and concerns. How is your appetite? Are
you a big eater or not such a big eater? How much do you weigh?
Has your weight been stable over the past year or has it gone up or
down a lot? Are you satisfied with your current weight or do you
think a lot about how you would like to weigh less or more? How
tall are you? (For a child or adolescent, pose the following ques
tions: How big are you compared with most other kids your age?
When was your fastest growing time over the past few years? For
a female of appropriate age, inquire about the following: Are your
menstrual periods regular? Any significant changes in your per
iods over the past year? Do you tend to have a lot of moodiness or
discomfort during or a few days before your period?)
13. Current moods. Explore the patient’s state of mind. What has been
your mood most of the time in recent months? Do you think you
feel sad or unhappy any more than most others your age? Do you
think you worry any more than most others your age? Do you
think you have any more trouble getting along with others than
most others your age? Do you think you get frustrated or irritable
or lose your temper any more than most others your age?
Administration of ADHD
Rating Scales
After obtaining a careful history, I administer a normed rating scale for
ADHD appropriate for the age of the patient. Among the possible
choices are the Barkley Rating Scales (Barkley 2011, 2012a), the Brown
ADD Rating Scales (Brown 1996, 2001), the Brown Executive Function/
Attention Rating Scales (Brown, in press), and the Conners ADHD Rat
ing Scales (Conners 2008a; Conners et al. 1999), all of which are available
in versions normed for age groups across the life span. First, I hand out
3-inch×8-inch cards on which are written the patient’s possible response
numbers; each card has one to three words defining what the number
means. For the Brown scales, the objective of the card system is to deter
mine not only how often a problem occurs but also how much trouble
that problem makes for the person (Table 8–1). The clinician is seeking
information about both the frequency and the intensity of the problem.
I then explain to the patient and collateral that I will be reading a list of
problems that people sometimes have; after each problem is read, I will
ask the patient to say the number that best indicates how applicable the
problem is for him or her (not over his or her whole life but just over the
past 6 months). I suggest rounding up if the patient is stuck between two
numbers. I request that if the patient is unsure about what I am asking, he
or she tells me; in that case, I try to rephrase the item to make it more un
derstandable. I prefer to ask the patient to respond first and then to have
the collateral reply to the same item, immediately after the patient has re
sponded. I ask the collateral to select responses based on integrating what
he or she has seen with his or her own eyes or heard in talking with the pa
tient or in the opinions of others (e.g., family members, teachers), assum
ing those opinions are considered reasonable. I emphasize that the
respondents do not need to agree in their responses to each item. The cli
nician is interested in how each individual sees the situation from his or
her own point of view. It is also important to keep participants from trying
to persuade one another that they should change an answer already given.
Severity Frequency
needed. “Yes” means that the patient has much more difficulty with that
problem than most others of the same age; “no” means that the patient
may have the problem but not in a way that is worse than in most anyone
else of the same age. After verifying that all respondents understand the
directions, I read aloud each of the nine ADHD symptoms of inattention
and each of the nine ADHD symptoms of hyperactivity/impulsivity. I
elicit and record a response from the patient and collateral, noting any
item for which, on the basis of all available data, I would assign a differ
ent response.
Wechsler Memory Scale, 3rd Edition (Wechsler 1997); they are identi
fied as the Logical Memory test. This measure consists of two stories,
each about one paragraph long with 25 word units, and a good scoring
system with norms for various age groups from 16 to 89 years. My pro
cedure is to say to the patient, “I’m going to read you two short stories;
each is just a paragraph. Please listen carefully. After I read the first story
to you, I’ll stop and ask you to repeat that story back to me, with as close
to the same words as possible, without making any changes. I don’t ex
pect you to remember every word of the story—there are too many
words for that—but I am asking you to pretend you are a tape recorder
and say it back to me with as close to the same words as you can.” Im
mediately after reading each story, I stop and ask the patient to repeat it
back to me, without my giving any prompting except to ask, when the
patient has said what he or she can recall, “Anything else? Even if it’s
out of order, that doesn’t matter.” As the patient responds, I check off
each word unit on the scoring sheet. The whole process is then repeated
with the second story. More details on this measure are described by
Kennedy et al. (2016). For children under 16 years, I use the Children’s
Memory Scale (Cohen 1997), which has stories and scoring systems for
three different age groups as well as items for a test of digit span for
ward and backward with age-based norms for each age group.
After the two stories have both been repeated back to me, I admin
ister a standard digit span test. I say to the patient, “Now I’m going read
you some numbers and ask you to say them back to me in the same or
der that I say them to you.” I then read the various strings of numbers,
allowing about 1 second for each number and trying to avoid any
chunking of the numbers that might give an unfair advantage. This is
continued with each pair of number strings from 2 to 9 digits each until
the patient misses two strings in one set or completes all the examples. I
then say, “Now I’m going to say some more numbers, and this time I’m
going to ask you to say the numbers back to me in reverse order. For ex
ample, if I say ‘1, 2’ you would say ‘2, 1.’ ” When we have completed
both the digits forward and digits backward, I thank the patient and
then go on to take some additional history. After about 15–20 minutes, I
stop and say, “Now I’d like to ask you to go back for a minute and tell me
again anything you can still remember from that first story I read to you
a few minutes ago.” After listening and scoring the patient’s response, I
then ask him or her to tell me what he or she can recall from the second
story I read. The response is then scored and set aside while we go on to
the next segment of the clinical interview. I do not give the scores on
these measures to the patient until the remaining sections of the evalu
ation are completed and it is time to report the diagnosis.
Assessing Children, Teenagers, and Adults for ADHD 141
or does not have ADHD. I say that there is no one test that can say for
certain whether a person has ADHD or not. I explain that we look at all
the information we have regarding the person’s current functioning and
earlier history to see what is easy for him or her and what is difficult; we
see how the patient does on remembering the stories and the numbers;
we look at what he or she and the collaterals say on the rating scales; we
consider whether there are any other problems that are complicating
things; and then we try to put the pieces together.
I then briefly summarize what I have learned about the patient’s
strengths and current difficulties, also mentioning relevant stresses and
supports. In that context, I offer my impression that the patient does or
does not meet diagnostic criteria for ADHD and/or any related disorders.
I cite the data that support my diagnostic impression. I show the summary
scores on the listing of DSM-5 diagnostic criteria, the obtained scores on
the story memory task and the digit span test, and the summary scores on
the ADHD rating scale that he or she and the collaterals helped to com
plete. If other disorders are occurring concurrently, I also mention those
disorders. Often in this discussion, I utilize the circles inside squares
graphic organizer described in Brown (2005b), which is available on my
Web site (www.DrThomasEBrown.com). At that point, I usually inquire as
to whether what I have just said makes sense to the patient and collaterals.
I also invite and respond to any comments or questions they raise.
Following that discussion, I ask the patient and collaterals to consider
what they want to do about the problems represented in this diagnosis.
One option, of course, is to do nothing at present, to simply wait a while
and see how things work out. Another option is to consider the possibility
of further testing, for example, psychoeducational testing to assess for pos
sible learning disorders or to ascertain eligibility for accommodations in
school or work under Section 504 of the Rehabilitation Act of 1973, the In
dividuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act, or the Americans
with Disabilities Act. Another option is to undertake a trial of treatment
with medication to try to alleviate the patient’s ADHD symptoms. Possible
benefits, costs, and potential risks of each of these options can then be dis
cussed. More information about various treatment options is offered in
Chapters 10 (“Practical Aspects of Medication Treatments for ADHD”), 11
(“Practical Aspects of Nonmedication Interventions for ADHD”), and 12.
Modifications of Assessment
for Younger Children
When younger children are brought by their parents for assessment of
possible ADHD, modifications need to be made to address the specific
Assessing Children, Teenagers, and Adults for ADHD 145
needs of the child. For very young children, I usually begin by reassur
ing the child that this doctor visit is not going to involve getting any
shots. Many children assume that anytime they are brought to see a doc
tor, they will very likely be getting an injection. It can be helpful to re
move this fear at the outset.
In the beginning of the interview, I usually try to engage the child in
some conversation at whatever level he or she can participate, depend
ing on the age, verbal abilities, and willingness of the child to interact
with me. I usually explain that I will need to be asking lots of questions:
“Some questions will be for you, some will be for your parents, and oth
ers will be for all of you.”
If you do not structure the interaction very soon after everyone is in
the room, it is likely that the parents will begin a lengthy description of
problems they see in their child, possibly embarrassing the child so
much that he or she will be unwilling to respond directly to any ques
tions from the clinician.
Usually, I begin with simple questions that the child is likely to be
able to answer easily, for example, What is the name of your school?
What grade are you in? What is your teacher’s name? Do you like your
teacher? Do you think he or she likes you? How are the other children in
your school—are they usually friendly or are some of them mean to
you? Of all the things you do at school, what do you like the most?
Which things are the most difficult for you? What do you like to do for
fun when you’re not in school? Do you have one or two kids you play
with a lot, or a whole big bunch of kids you play with, or do you do a lot
of stuff by yourself?
After eliciting such information, I usually ask the young child to draw a
picture for me. I have the child sit at a small table where he or she can work
in the same room with his or her parents and me. I offer either crayons or
markers with some drawing paper. I ask the child to draw a picture that has
three things in it: a house, a tree, and a person. I suggest that he or she can
put in anything else they choose, but I ask the child to remember to put in at
least those three items. I then turn my attention to the parents, asking them
what they are hoping we can accomplish together in this consultation. I
then begin raising the questions described in the paragraphs above while
also keeping an eye on the child, noticing level of engagement in the task
and skill in doing the drawing. When the child is finished with the picture,
depending on the child’s age, I may ask the child to print his or her name
and to write the alphabet and a series of numbers or simple math problems
suitable for the child’s age. Once these tasks are completed, I usually offer
the child a limited number of choices for play: a box of Legos, some models
of animals and action figures, or paper for drawings of anything he or she
146 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
chooses. Some children prefer to play with toys they brought with them.
Usually, I allow the child to stay in the room while the parents and I are talk
ing so the child can hear what is happening and, if he or she wishes, join in
the conversation, so long as the child does not disrupt it. Meanwhile, I also
am observing how the parents are or are not interacting with their child.
Teacher Evaluations
For children who are in prekindergarten through elementary school, it is
often quite helpful to obtain information from the teachers who spend the
most time with them. Often teachers will provide useful information in
notes or on teacher forms for rating ADHD symptoms. This is most true
when the child is in one class with the same teacher most of the day. For
older students (e.g., in middle school or high school) who are changing
classes throughout the day, most teacher reports may not be as helpful be
cause the teachers do not have as much sustained opportunity to get to
know individual children in so many aspects of their school activities.
and difficulties, but such measures are not necessary for making a diag
nosis of ADHD. Some individuals seeking evaluation may already have
had such testing recently or much earlier. If one or more reports of such
evaluations are available, it may enhance the clinician’s understanding
of the patient’s cognitive abilities. Commonly used measures for such
assessment include the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fifth
Edition (WISC-V; Wechsler 2014), the Wechsler Adult Intelligence
Scale—Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV; Wechsler 2008) IQ tests, and the
Woodcock-Johnson IV Tests of Cognitive Abilities (Woodcock-Johnson
2016a). Achievement tests include the Wechsler Individual Achieve
ment Test-III (Wechsler 2009) and the Woodcock-Johnson Achievement
Tests-IV. Often students diagnosed with ADHD and seeking educa
tional accommodations such as extended time for taking exams are re
quired to submit results of such measures of ability and achievement.
Another measure often helpful for assessing need for extra-time accom
modations is the Nelson-Denny Reading Test (Brown et al. 1993). This
measure for adolescents and adults allows assessment of the individ
ual’s vocabulary and reading comprehension abilities under both stan
dard and extended-time conditions.
For students who have significant difficulties in learning a foreign
language and who are seeking a waiver from foreign language require
ments, the Modern Language Aptitude Test may be a useful assessment
measure (Carroll and Sapon 2002).
school-age form for ages 4–18 years, and both observer and self-report
forms for adults age 19 years and up (Constantino and Gruber 2012).
Goethe’s famous comment that dissecting a fly and studying its parts
will not tell you how it flies could almost have been intended for the
neuropsychology of executive function. . . .[E]xecutive functioning is
called into play only when the activities of the cognitive architecture
must be coordinated....Thus, if a methodology is used where a task is
broken down into its component parts, no deficit will be discovered in
dysexecutive patients. (pp. 99–102)
R.A. Barkley (1997, 2012b, 2015) and I (Brown 2000, 2005a, 2006, 2013)
claim that all children and adults with ADHD suffer from significant ex
ecutive function impairments relative to others of similar age and that
developmental impairments of these executive functions are the es
sence of ADHD. We assert that these impairments can be seen much
more clearly and validly in assessments of how individuals with ADHD
perform over time in a wide variety of tasks for which they need to man
age themselves than in any battery of neuropsychological tests.
Summary
Given that all persons have some of the symptoms of ADHD sometimes,
it is important to have an effective way to determine which individuals
experience sufficient chronic impairment from ADHD symptoms to
warrant diagnosis and treatment. There is no single test for ADHD—no
laboratory test, genetic test, computer test, imaging test, or battery of
neuropsychological tests that can adequately determine which individ
uals meet diagnostic criteria for ADHD and which do not.
The complexity and situational specificity of ADHD symptoms re
quires information about how the individual functions in a wide variety
of situations and tasks of daily life relative to others of the same age. The
most effective tool for diagnosis of ADHD is a well-conducted clinical
interview with the patient (and, if possible, one or two people who
know the patient well) by a clinician who is familiar with ADHD and
with other medical or psychological disorders that might produce sim
ilar symptoms in a person of comparable age.
This chapter includes a thorough outline of what a comprehensive
clinical evaluation for ADHD should include for various age groups. It
also describes some instruments advertised for use in clinical evalua
tions that are less helpful for assessment of ADHD.
9
Emotional Dynamics
in Individuals,
Couples, and Families
Coping With ADHD
THERE IS NO SINGLE PROFILE OF EMOTIONS
common to all individuals with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor
der (ADHD). There is much diversity due to differences in age, temper
ament, personality style, family life, cultural background, and many
other variables. Yet there are some ADHD characteristics and some sit
uations often experienced by many with ADHD (and those involved
with them) that cause particular patterns of emotional dynamics to
emerge more frequently among these individuals. In this chapter, I de
scribe some emotional dynamics often reported by children, adoles
cents, or adults with ADHD and those who interact with them.
The palette of human emotions is rich and variegated. It includes
happiness, enthusiasm, interest, disinterest, boredom, delight, worry,
fearfulness, panic, terror, frustration, annoyance, anger, rage, pride,
envy, embarrassment, shame, guilt, jealousy, disappointment, discour
agement, grief, hopelessness, sadness, depression, longing, trust, opti
mism, expectancy, determination, affection, passion, love, hope, and
many other emotions.
151
152 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
Emotions are dynamic in that they often change and interact, some
times in an instant, sometimes over hours, weeks, or years. Often they
change in response to specific circumstances of a situation, what some
one else says or does and how individuals perceive and react to one an
other in given moments and over time. Sometimes emotions are quite
transient, a flash of anger or a moment of jealousy, pride, or affection
that may quickly be modified or replaced by other emotions that may be
quite contradictory. Emotions may also be persistent over much of a life
time, absorbed into the fabric of one’s personality across differing set
tings.
Emotions vary not only in type but also in intensity. Sometimes emo
tions arise with fierce or crushing intensity; at other times, that same
emotion may be scarcely noticeable. Emotions also vary in level of con
sciousness. Sometimes a person is fully aware of a particular emotion in
a given moment, yet at another time, that person may be totally un
aware of an emotion that others readily recognize and respond to.
In all persons, emotions tend to arise in multiple mixes and blends.
Sometimes the blend is subtle and convergent—affection and longing,
pride and hope. In other instances, emotions strongly conflict with one
another—interest and fear, pleasure and guilt, pride and resentment,
love and hate. Sometimes the conflict is immediate; in other instances,
one emotion may be followed quickly or gradually with another, or a
person may experience rapid alternation between one emotion and an
other. Examples of emotions described in this chapter may be experi
enced by various individuals in many different ways, only some of
which are included here.
picked on by adults
The parents of 8-year-old Michael explained, “He’s very polite and well
behaved 90% of the time, but several times a day, like when we have to
tell him to do a simple thing like turn off a video game he’s playing so he
can start getting ready for bed, he often, but not always, will fly into a
rage, swear at us, and head-butt us, and then keep kicking against a
door. This goes on for about 10 or 20 minutes, and then he starts crying
and says, ‘I can’t move, I’m stuck, come help me.’ He wants one of us to
154 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
come hold him quietly for a minute or two, then he tells us he’s very
sorry for being so bad, and then it’s over, and he’s all good again until
the next time.” Michael’s mother shook her head and said, “We’ve tried
systems to reward him for any day without these meltdowns, but that
didn’t help at all.” His father said, “When he does that stuff, it makes me
so mad that I start screaming at him, even though I know that does no
good at all and probably makes it worse.” Michael’s parents also re
ported, “He’s had a few episodes at school where he had meltdowns and
hit other kids; he got suspended twice, but those are rare. Mostly, this
just happens at home.”
tensely and were committed to one another, but they were feeling in
creasingly frustrated with him, embarrassed that they could not control
his outbursts or their own, and hopeless about how to help him and one
another.
Eleven-year-old Sandy was the best goalie on her travel soccer team. She
was well liked by her teammates and often praised by her coaches for
her skills and consistent effort. Yet she hated school. She got along all
right with her classmates and usually got passing grades, but she was
seen by her teachers as stubborn and temperamental. Now in sixth
grade, she was having increasingly frequent incidents of what the teach
ers called “going on strike.” When the class was asked to write para
graphs or brief essays, Sandy often wrote nothing. When the teacher
asked what was wrong, Sandy just stared ahead and did not respond.
When the class was given a timed challenge test for math problems,
Sandy often started with the others and then suddenly stopped, tore up
her paper, refused to talk, and began repetitively kicking the desk in
front of her until the teacher sent her to the principal, who told Sandy to
complete the work at home and return with a better attitude. Sandy’s
parents reported that it often took them 5 or 10 minutes to explain the
writing assignment to Sandy and help her get started, but then she was
able to complete the task, producing results that the teacher said were
fully satisfactory.
When I first met with Sandy and her parents, she was initially un
willing to answer any of my questions, but as I continued to talk with her
parents, she gradually warmed up and began to respond, first with just
facial expressions and nods or head shaking, then gradually with words.
Her mother told me that Sandy had been slow to speak as a young child,
producing no words until she was 3 years old, but at that point she began
suddenly to speak in sentences. I also learned that both Sandy and her
mother had been diagnosed with ADHD several years earlier and that
both were taking stimulant medication that they found helpful.
Over a series of conversations together, I found that Sandy readily
spoke with me about how her soccer team was doing, yet she was un
willing to discuss any incidents in school where the teacher had com
plained to her parents about her behavior. When her parents told me
how teachers were complaining about her being angry, being stubborn,
and going on strike, Sandy kept her head down and stared at the floor as
her eyes began to fill up with tears.
Gradually, it became clear that Sandy’s teachers were mistaken
when they interpreted her “on strike” behavior as anger and stubborn
156 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
ness. That behavior was covering intense feelings of shame and fear.
Sandy had very high standards for herself, especially for expository
writing and for math. She also had ADHD-related problems with work
ing memory and processing speed. Her working memory problems of
ten caused her to get confused about oral directions given for writing
assignments, so she did not understand and remember what she was
being asked to do. Her slow processing speed made it very difficult for
her to keep up with her classmates in doing tightly timed math chal
lenges. When she saw her classmates working much faster on the timed
math quizzes, she felt embarrassed and gave up. When she felt confused
about how to start her writing assignments, she froze in shame and was
unable to respond to the teacher’s offers of help. What appeared as op
positional behavior was, in fact, a diversionary maneuver that served to
distract her, her classmates, and her teacher from what Sandy saw as hu
miliating failure.
I asked Sandy’s pediatrician to add a selective serotonin reuptake in
hibitor (SSRI) to the stimulant medication Sandy was taking for her
ADHD; gradually that helped to reduce her chronic anxiety. I also tried
to help Sandy and her parents to understand the puzzling intensity of
her reactions to confusion and perceived failure. Her mother then re
ported that both her sister (Sandy’s aunt) and Sandy’s maternal grand
mother also had long-standing reputations in their family for quickly
getting angry and then pulling into their shell when they felt anxious,
especially when stress occurred in a social situation.
We had a meeting with Sandy’s team of teachers, who readily agreed
to give written directions for writing assignments and to provide extra
help for Sandy to learn how to get herself started on writing assign
ments. Her pediatrician, her parents, and I also arranged to make some
adjustments to Sandy’s ADHD medications to provide more help for
her problems with working memory and processing speed.
by her classmates
vited but I didn’t. They all hate me! I can tell. They tried to keep this
sleepover birthday party a secret, but I heard them talking about it when
I was in the bathroom and they didn’t know I was there. I heard Shelly
say, ‘You can’t tell Jessica about this, I’ve invited all of you except her.
She just doesn’t fit in.’ And they all said, ‘Yeah, nobody else will want
her there either. It’ll be a lot better without her.’”
This sort of rejection by peers would be difficult for any child, but chil
dren (and some adults) with ADHD tend to get overwhelmed in such sit
uations. Sometimes this is because the child with ADHD is especially
sensitive to rejection. Yet for many with ADHD, the problem is that rejec
tion from peers occurs more quickly and more often because the child with
ADHD may actually be too impatient, too immature, too demanding, too
sensitive to criticism, or too bossy with other children and not as willing to
compromise in working out interpersonal conflicts as most others of simi
lar age. One study obtained peer ratings for 165 children with ADHD ages
7–9 years and did follow-up peer ratings for 6 years. They found that 52% of
those children were rated in the rejected range by their peers, whereas only
1% were rated as having popular status (Hoza et al. 2005).
It can be extremely difficult for parents to find an effective way to
help their child in peer group conflicts without making the situation
worse. They may intervene and talk with the parents of the other chil
dren involved or with the teacher to try to get peer interactions at school
changed. Yet if the other children are confronted by their own parents or
their teacher and punished for rejecting the outcast child or urged to be
friend him or her, those children may comply superficially while subtly
intensifying their rejection or bullying of the outcast, who is now seen as
the tattler who got them into trouble.
then refused to start taking it. He reported that his mother and her new
husband had told him that he did not need medication for ADHD, and
they warned him that ADHD medication would stunt his growth.
scars from several small cuts she had made on her arm with a razor
blade. Her teachers said that Cindy, a girl new to this school, often
seemed tired and somewhat inattentive in class. Her homework was al
ways done well and handed in on time, but her test scores were often
much lower than would be expected from someone who performed so
well on homework assignments. A few teachers wondered if Cindy had
been getting too much help from her mother when doing her home
work. When I met with Cindy’s mother, she told me that Cindy was to
tally independent in doing her homework and worked 3 or 4 hours most
evenings to get it done. She also said that Cindy got very upset when she
got low test grades because she was trying to show her father, who lived
out of state, that she was a smart, hard worker. Her mother was unaware
that Cindy had been cutting herself.
When I met with Cindy and her mother together, I learned that the
self-cutting had begun shortly after Cindy came to this school and usu
ally occurred after Cindy received a low grade on a test. Cindy said that
she had heard about self-cutting from a TV show and decided to try it
one day when she came home from school after receiving an almost fail
ing grade on a test for which she had spent a lot of time studying the
night before. She said that seeing a few drops of her blood after making
a small cut on her arm seemed to take away some of her feelings of sad
ness and shame about doing poorly on the test. Since then, she had re
peated the self-cutting on several other days when she received a low
test score.
As Cindy talked about her difficulties in remembering material she
had read and studied to prepare for tests, it soon became clear that she
was suffering from undiagnosed ADHD, which had not been recog
nized at her previous school. Like many other bright students with
ADHD, she had done very well in elementary school, where most of her
class time was spent with the same teacher. Now, in middle school,
where she had to deal not only with a new school setting but also with
changing classes, multiple teachers, a more challenging curriculum, and
heavier homework demands, her underlying impairments of executive
functions had become more noticeable.
Cindy felt there was a lot at stake with her grades. Her parents had
recently divorced, and her father had moved to take a job a thousand
miles away. He kept in touch with her daily by phone, but much of his
talk with her involved asking her about what homework she was doing
and what grades she was getting. He praised her a lot for good grades
and kept encouraging her to work harder when she got low grades. She
feared that he would be disappointed and, perhaps, lose interest in her if
she did not maintain high grades. That motivated her intense home
work efforts and led to her fearful shame that prompted the self-cutting.
160 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
Don, a 15-year-old high school freshman, had many friends and was es
pecially popular because he was one of the best athletes on the freshman
football team. However, he was not well liked by his teachers. Most of
them found him annoying because he was often argumentative and dis
rupted class with wise guy comments. He tended to score high on tests
but often did not do his homework.
After he got warnings of possible failure in two courses, his mother
asked the teachers for weekly reports and told Don that she would give
him double allowance for each week with a good report. The following
4 weeks brought no improvement in the weekly reports. Don com
plained that the teachers simply disliked him and were always looking
for something to complain about.
At that point, Don’s father called the football coach and insisted he
was taking Don off the football team for the rest of the season. He also
told Don that he would be grounded: no after-school or weekend activ
ities until he produced good reports for at least 4 successive weeks.
Don’s mother protested that his father’s actions were too harsh and
would make things worse. She wanted to take Don to be evaluated for
ADHD because a friend had suggested that as a possible reason for his
problems. His father argued that Don had no problem except for insuf
ficient self-discipline; he feared that ADHD might be used as an excuse
for Don’s laziness.
struggle with executive functions, as those with ADHD do, the transi
tion into middle school and then into high school is usually much more
difficult.
The high school transition is often problematic for parents too be
cause communication between parents and teachers tends to be more
limited in that setting. Unless the school provides and maintains an ex
ceptionally effective Web site monitoring system, parents are unable to
monitor what assignments have been given in each subject, what the
specific requirements are, when each assignment is due, and whether
their son or daughter is getting them done adequately and in on time.
Many high school students quickly learn to manage their work ade
quately, but, like Joanne, many with ADHD struggle considerably, es
pecially during their freshman year.
Joanne clearly cared a lot about doing a good job with her school
work, but the loss of her mother’s daily support for organizing and
completing homework presented her with tasks for which she was not
prepared. She was accustomed to meeting with her mother each after
noon for more than an hour to review what homework needed to be
done and to work under her mother’s close supervision and encourag
ing support. Her mother’s unavailability during those important hours
was a significant loss.
Joanne was proud of being chosen to be a cheerleader for her school,
and she enjoyed attending the practice sessions every day after school.
However, this meant that Joanne now needed to do her homework after
dinner rather than immediately after school each day. One effect of this
change in routine was that the ADHD medication Joanne had found
helpful for many years wore off before she got home to do her home
work. We arranged for her to get a short-acting booster dose of her
ADHD medication to take in late afternoon so it would provide cover
age for doing homework after dinner but not interfere with her getting
to sleep in the evening. This helped to improve her focus, but the med
ication could not replace the help Joanne had previously been receiving
from her mother.
when it was time to pack up his stuff and move into the dorm on the uni
versity campus 2 hours away from his home. He was quite shy, except
with family and a few friends he had known since elementary school.
When he arrived on campus, Alan attended his classes but mostly kept
to himself in his room, with the exception of getting meals and using the
bathroom. He struggled with writing papers, but he was too fearful to
seek help from the writing clinic. He got poor grades on many of his tests
because he did not feel comfortable asking any other students to study
with him and was terrified of going to open office hours where his pro
fessors were available for individual consultations with students. Alan
was ashamed of his declining grades and kept telling his parents that
college was difficult but he was doing OK. He did not let them know
how much difficulty he was actually having until his first semester was
over and he received failing grades in four of his five courses.
Alan came with his parents for a consultation with me shortly after he
received his semester grades. It quickly became clear that he was an ex
tremely bright young man who continued to suffer from ADHD and also
from severe social anxiety and some characteristics of Asperger’s syn
drome. After considering alternatives, Alan and his parents decided that
he would request an immediate medical leave from the university and
return to living at home while he began a medication trial, engaged in
psychotherapy with me, and enrolled in a couple of engineering-related
courses in the local community college.
I had several joint meetings with Alan and his parents to address
Alan’s intense shame over his failure at college, his guilt over having
kept his difficulties secret from his parents until it was too late to salvage
his semester, and his intense fears about whether he would ever be able
to return to the university and be successful. His parents felt guilty that
they had not been more active in keeping track of how Alan was doing;
they also resented the fact that they had paid a very sizable tuition for a
semester that yielded no academic credits. Another difficult issue was
how Alan and his parents would explain to neighbors, extended family,
and friends why he was now living at home and going to the commu
nity college when all of them had understood that he had been enjoying
a successful first semester of his freshman year at the university.
Alan’s treatment included SSRI medication to address his intense so
cial anxiety, stimulant medication to alleviate his ADHD symptoms,
and cognitive-behavioral treatment to learn to develop the social skills
needed to interact with faculty, staff, and other students at the commu
nity college. He needed coaching to plan how to approach classmates to
form small study groups for course work review and projects in both of
his classes. We discussed how to choose students he might feel comfort
able approaching and what words he could use to initiate conversation,
164 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
how to learn their names, and how to get contact information. With that
support, he gradually discovered that he was able to make successful
contact with a few students in each of his classes to work together on
problem sets, to review for tests and exams, and sometimes to have a
good time with. Alan continued at the community college for the sum
mer program and then decided to take an additional year at the com
munity college. He did well in that year, both academically and socially;
this led to successfully resuming his studies and living in the dorm at
the university campus.
sponded in a scolding tone, “You know exactly what I’m talking about—
I tell you about this same stuff all the time. You’re always late when
you’re supposed to meet me, and there’ve been some times when you’ve
totally forgotten what we planned to do. Lots of times when I’m trying
to talk with you, you’re not really paying any attention, you’re just mess
ing with your phone. Sometimes you go for days without even sending
me a text, you get too busy with your schoolwork or just hanging out
with your guy friends. And when I try to tell you what I need, you just
try to change the subject or go do something else. You don’t really an
swer or have a decent conversation to resolve things.”
Debbie then turned to me and asked, “Can’t you change his medi
cine so he can be better about these things?” George answered before I
could respond. He said, “What do you think Doc is going to do, get me
some medicine that will totally get rid of my ADHD problems and make
me some kind of robot that will do everything the way you want me to?
Does it count at all that I love you? Is the way I am now really that bad?
You’re not exactly perfect all the time yourself.”
perwork. I’m always late for everything. I spend a lot of time on the
computer just surfing, and I can’t stop myself to get to the grocery store to
get something to cook for dinner and have ready when the boys get home
from school and team practice. And in all this, I’m a perfectionist. I feel
stuck, especially over the past few years, unable to do what I need to do to
take care of my kids and myself. I’m ashamed of my house, of my life, and
of my parenting. It’s never been this bad before.”
enough getting back. I feel like I’m raising not just our two children, but
three! My husband, Greg, does a good enough job at work, and he’s a
good provider, but at home he’s just like one of the kids. I have to struggle
to get him up and off to work every morning, and I have to remind him to
get off the computer every night so he won’t stay up playing video games
until 2:00 or 3:00 A.M. I’ve given up on his taking care of our monthly bills
because he never pays them on time. We were getting threatening letters
and calls from creditors because he forgot to pay. There was enough
money in the bank, and he said he would take care of it.
“Once in a while he’ll start a project at home, but then he never fin
ishes it. For 2 years, we’ve had bare two-by-fours in our bedroom where
he says he’s going to put in new plasterboard. Mostly when he’s home,
he’s on the computer or watching sports on TV. He says he’s going to
come to the kids’ sports events, but then he forgets to show up. When I
try to talk with him, he listens for maybe a minute or two, then he’s drift
ing off talking about something else. He’s not a heavy drinker and he’s
not abusive, but he is neglectful. I’ve been married to him for 16 years,
but all that time I’ve been neglected by this man. He seems to need a
mother to take care of him day by day more than he needs or wants a
wife.”
I expected that Greg would respond to his wife’s complaints with
anger, but he didn’t. He cried. He acknowledged his frequent forgetful
ness and frequent neglect of tasks, events, and concerns that were im
portant to his wife and to his children. He then said, “You’re right! I
make a lot of promises to you and the kids that I don’t follow through on.
And I do depend on you to keep me organized and to remind me about
what needs to be done and when to do it. I’ve never been good at stuff
like that. When I was a kid, all the way through high school, my mother
had to hassle me every day to get me up in the morning. If she hadn’t, I
would have slept through school. I’ve never been able to get myself up
with an alarm clock. And she had to keep pressuring and reminding me
to get my homework and chores done. Even though I wanted to, I just
couldn’t manage that stuff myself. You help me with so much, and
you’re not even mean about it. I just don’t know if I can change the way
I’ve been for so many years. I don’t know if it’s something that can be
changed.”
Greg clearly had ADHD that had never been diagnosed or treated.
He willingly agreed to try a regimen of medication to alleviate his
chronic ADHD problems. After a couple of dosage adjustments, it was
quite helpful to him. But that did not fix all the problems in their mar
riage. We spent several months of weekly sessions together to try to
identify specific changes that this couple needed to make to protect their
marriage and improve the quality of life for themselves and their chil
dren.
The problems of this couple were not limited to Martha’s complaints.
Gradually, Greg began to air some of his frustrations with her. He re
Emotional Dynamics of ADHD 169
Summary
The new model of ADHD described in this book emphasizes two ways in
which emotions are often problematic for persons with ADHD: 1) exec
utive function impairments often impair their ability to modulate emo
tional expression and 2) working memory impairments often bias their
motivations in ways that lead to maladaptive decision making and prob
lematic behaviors as a result of not considering the larger context or likely
future consequences.
Examples in this chapter illustrate just a small number of the many
ways emotions are entwined in the daily life experiences of not only in
dividuals with ADHD but also other members of their family, class
mates, friends, persons they date, persons they marry, and children they
care for. Additional examples are provided and discussed in my earlier
book, Smart but Stuck: Emotions in Teens and Adults with ADHD (Brown
2014). It should be noted, however, that many of these emotional dy
namics are not consciously recognized unless those affected are pro
vided assistance in examining the complexity of their emotions, which
are often quite subtle and interactive.
10
Practical Aspects
of Medication
ATTENTION-DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER
(ADHD) is essentially a problem in the chemical dynamics of the brain,
and the most effective treatment for ADHD is usually medication.
However, medication for ADHD cures nothing. It is not like an antibi
otic that may cure a strep throat. It is more like my eyeglasses. I have a
problem with my eyes that makes it hard for me to read typewriter-size
print; those letters look totally blurry to me. When I put my eyeglasses
on, I can read those letters as well as almost anyone else. When I take
my glasses off, I’m right back where I started—unable to read the blurry
letters. The eyeglasses do not cure my vision problem, and medications
do not cure ADHD.
Although medications do not cure ADHD, for about 8 of 10 people
with ADHD, approved medications, properly fine-tuned, can signifi
cantly improve their ADHD impairments. For some, the improvement
is huge; for others, improvement is not huge, but it is substantial. For
some, it helps a little but not very much, and for 2 of 10, the medications
customarily used for ADHD either don’t work at all or cause side effects
that the person should not or does not want to put up with.
171
172 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
they are not able to mobilize themselves to focus and function effectively.
Medications for ADHD, when they work, help those with ADHD to ac
tivate themselves and manage themselves more effectively during that
part of the day when the medication is active in their body. While active,
medication for ADHD can help to improve communication between
neurons in ways that can significantly improve the person’s abilities for
self-management on a more consistent basis, even for tasks that are not
especially interesting or threatening.
Medications approved by the U.S. Food and Drug Administration
(FDA) for treatment of ADHD include stimulant medications such as
methylphenidate and amphetamine and some other medications that
are not stimulants. Methylphenidate has been used for ADHD since the
mid-1950s. The ADHD medication best known to most people is Ritalin,
which is one brand name for methylphenidate. Other brand names for
methylphenidate include Metadate, Concerta, Focalin, and Daytrana.
Amphetamines have been available since 1937; they currently include
Dexedrine, Adderall, and Vyvanse. Hundreds of peer-reviewed re
search studies on the use of stimulant medications have been published.
Tables 10–1, 10–2, and 10–3 list names and characteristics of currently
approved medications for treatment of ADHD and show which are
available in immediate-release and/or longer-acting formulations.
Nonstimulant medications include atomoxetine, guanfacine, and
clonidine. They are different from the stimulant medications in that they
do not act primarily on the dopamine system of the brain. Atomoxetine
(brand name Strattera), like the stimulants, is considered a first-line
treatment for ADHD. It acts primarily on the norepinephrine system
and indirectly on the dopamine system. Atomoxetine is not usually as
powerful as the stimulants in improving inattention symptoms, but it
sometimes works when stimulant medications are ineffective or cause
too many side effects. More than 30 research studies have demonstrated
the usefulness of atomoxetine for children, adolescents, and adults not
only for ADHD but also for ADHD accompanied by anxiety, tics, and
some other comorbid problems. Atomoxetine, unlike the stimulants, is
dosed according to the individual’s weight, starting with a stipulated
small dose and then gradually increasing over several weeks. Often it is
4–6 weeks before it is possible to determine how effective the medica
tion will be for that person.
Other nonstimulant medications currently approved for treatment
of ADHD are guanfacine (Intuniv) and clonidine (Kapvay); both are α2
adrenergic agonists that act primarily on the norepinephrine system
and have been used to reduce excessively high blood pressure in adults.
These nonstimulants may be helpful not only for ADHD symptoms but
TABLE 10–1. Amphetamines approved for the treatment of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder
(ADHD)
Trade name, Approximate Ages
(year FDA approved), Usual dosage range duration of action approved for
Duration of action formulation Available strengths (mg) (start–maximum) (mg) (hours) ADHD
Shorter-acting Adderall [G] 5, 7.5, 10, 12.5, 15, 20, 30 3–5 years: 2.5 qam to 20 bid 6–8 3+
medications (1960) 6–17 years: 5 qam to 20 bid
Tablet 18+: 5 qam to 20 bid
Dexedrine 5, 10 3–5 years: 2.5 qam to 20 bid 3–5 3–16
(1976) 6–16 years: 5 qam to 20 bid
Tablet
Evekeo 5, 10 3–5 years: 2.5 qam to 20 bid 3–5 6–17
(2012) 6–16 years: 5 qam to 20 bid
Tablet
ProCentra [G] 5 mg/5 mL 5–20 bid 3–5 3–16
(2008)
Oral solution
Zenzedi 2.5, 5, 7.5, 10, 15, 20, 30 3–5 years: 2.5 qam to 20 bid 3–5 3–16
Practical Aspects of Medication Treatments for ADHD
Adzenys XR-ODT 3.1, 6.3, 9.4, 12.5, 15.7, 6–12 years: 6.3–18.8 qam 11–14 6+
(2016) 18.8 13–17 years: 6.3–12.5 qam
Tablet 18+: 12.5–18.8 qam
Dexedrine Spansule [G] 5, 10, 15 5 qam to 20 bid 10–14 6+
(1976)
Capsule
Dyanavel XR 2.5 mg/1 mL to 6–17 years: 2.5–20 qam 10–14 6+
(2015) 20 mg/8 mL
Oral solution
Vyvanse 10, 20, 30, 40, 50, 60, 70 30–70 qam 8–12 6–17, adults
(2007)
Capsule
Note. FDA=U.S. Food and Drug Administration; G=generic; ODT=orally disintegrating tablet; XR=extended release.
Source. Adapted from Puzantian T, Carlat D: Medication Fact Book, 3rd Edition. Newburyport, MA, Carlat Publishing, 2016. Used with permission.
(2001)
Methylin CT [G]
5, 10, 20 2.5 bid to 20 tid 3–4 6–17, adults
(2003)
Chewable tablet
Methylin [G]
5 mg/5 mL; 10 mg/5 mL 2.5 bid to 20 tid 3–4 6–17, adults
(2002)
Liquid
Ritalin [G]
5, 10, 20 2.5 bid to 20 tid 3–4 6–17, adults
(1955)
IR tablet
Longer-acting Aptensio XR
10, 15, 20, 30, 40, 50, 60 10–60 qam 8–12 6+
medications (2015)
177
TABLE 10–2. Methylphenidates approved for the treatment of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder
(ADHD) (continued)
Trade name, Usual dosage range Approximate Ages
(year FDA approved), (start–maximum) duration of action approved for
Duration of action formulation Available strengths (mg) (mg) (hours) ADHD
Longer-acting Concerta [G] 18, 27, 36, 54 18–72 qam 10–16 6–12, adults
medications (2000)
(continued) Capsule (22% IR, 78% DR)
Daytrana Patch 10, 15, 20, 30 10–30 qam & remove 8–12 6+, adults
CR transdermal patch
Focalin XR [G] 5, 10, 15, 20, 25, 30, 35, 40 6–17 years: 8–12 6–17, adults
Metadate CD [G] 10, 20, 30, 40, 50, 60 20–60 qam 8–12 6+
(2001)
(2000)
(2012)
Ritalin LA [G] 10, 20, 30, 40, 50, 60 20–60 qam 8–12 6+
(2002)
Note. CR= controlled release; CT=chewable tablet; DR=delayed release; ER=extended release; FDA=U.S. Food and Drug Administration; G=generic;
IR=immediate release; LA=long acting; MPH=methylphenidate.
Source. Adapted from Puzantian T, Carlat D: Medication Fact Book, 3rd Edition. Newburyport, MA, Carlat Publishing, 2016. Used with permission.
Practical Aspects of Medication Treatments for ADHD
179
TABLE 10–3. Nonstimulants approved for the treatment of
attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)
Trade name Approximate
(year FDA approved) Available Usual dosage range duration of action Ages
Class Formulation strengths (mg) (start–maximum) (hours) approved for ADHD
Source. Adapted from Puzantian T, Carlat D: Medication Fact Book, 3rd Edition. Newburyport, MA, Carlat Publishing, 2016. Used with permission.
people who respond best to very small doses of stimulants, doses simi
lar to what would usually be given to a preschooler.
Because age and weight are not an adequate guide to what dose of
stimulant works best for someone with ADHD, it is usually best to be
gin with a very minimal dose and then, if there are no significant bene
fits and no adverse effects, to increase every 3–5 days until a dose is
found that is effective without causing undesirable side effects.
This process of adjusting the dose of a stimulant medication is essen
tially a search for that “sweet spot” between too much and too little. If a
person is taking too little medication for ADHD, it is useless; you might
just as well be taking breath mints. If you are taking too much stimulant
medication you are likely to be feeling too “wired” or jittery, as though
you’ve consumed way too much coffee. Or you may be feeling too irri
table, much more likely to get angry over little frustrations that would
not usually bother you (Arnsten and Li 2005; Arnsten and Pliszka 2011).
Becoming too serious is one additional problem that may be caused
by a dose of stimulant that is too high for an individual. Individuals may
lose their “sparkle,” their spontaneity, their sense of humor. They may
be able to focus well for tasks, but they do not feel much like interacting
with others. They do not feel like their regular self.
These side effects that can come from taking a dose of stimulant that
is too high for the person are usually very temporary; they generally dis
appear as the medication wears off later in the day.
attention was improved compared with 10 years ago, and they tended
to be employed, whereas those who had never taken medication or who
had stopped taking medication did not report improved attention and
were less likely to be employed. Those who continued taking medica
tion also reported better self-efficacy—better ability to manage their life
than those who did not continue medication treatment.
The mean dosages of medications used were 54.1 mg/day for meth
ylphenidate and 29.5 mg/day for amphetamines. A review by Torgersen
et al. (2016) concluded that many patients age 50 years or more who had
ADHD-related impairments experienced beneficial effects from treat
ment with ADHD medications. However, Torgersen et al. (2016) em
phasized the importance of an adequate physical examination prior to
starting such medications in this older population and urged starting
with low doses and very gradual titration. It is also important to be alert
to possible interactions between ADHD medications and other medica
tions an older patient may be taking for various medical or psychiatric
difficulties.
bedside table and set an alarm clock to awaken them an hour before
they need to get up; they set a different alarm clock across the room to
awaken them at the time they actually need to get up. However, many
individuals with ADHD have chronic difficulty remembering to make
these preparations at bedtime.
Many people find that longer-acting stimulants, medications like
Vyvanse, Concerta, Adderall XR, Focalin XR, and generic equivalents,
work well for them during the day once they kick in. These longer-lasting
formulations reduce the need to take multiple doses over the course of
the day, and they often have fewer ups and downs in their coverage.
However, there is a lot of variability from one person to another in how
long the longer-lasting stimulants actually work.
Published estimates claim that some particular long-lasting medica
tions work effectively for 12–14 hours; for other long-lasting medica
tions, estimates are 8–10 hours or 6–8 hours of effectiveness. These
estimates are all averages based on trials with carefully selected sam
ples. For some patients, the medication lasts significantly longer than
the reported average. For other patients, that same medication may lose
effectiveness in less than half of the reported average time.
These differences in duration of action can make a big difference in
planning adequate coverage of medication for any given individual pa
tient. For example, our group found that 60% of adolescents and adults
being treated with a long-acting stimulant with a reported 10- to 16-hour
duration of action needed a booster dose of short-acting stimulant in
midafternoon to late afternoon to cover additional hours to successfully
manage tasks in late afternoon and early evening.
When considering addition of short-acting medications to extend
the duration of action of longer-acting stimulants, it is important for the
prescriber to keep in mind that for most longer-acting medications, the
potency of the extended dose at any time over the course of the medi
cation’s action is only one-half of the face value of the longer-acting for
mulation. For example, a 20-mg dose of Adderall XR does not provide
20 mg of coverage throughout its duration of action. It provides 10 mg of
coverage for the first few hours and then another 10 mg of coverage for
the following few hours. In contrast, the immediate-release version of
Adderall 10 mg releases the full 10 mg at the outset. This needs to be
taken into account when planning combined use of longer-acting and
shorter-acting stimulant medications.
Another problem with fitting stimulant medications to an individ
ual’s daily schedule is time of onset. Some short-acting stimulants kick in
within about 30 minutes after the pill is swallowed. Some longer-acting
stimulants produce very few benefits until 60–90 minutes after ingestion.
186 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
hours they are engaged in practice or games. About 50% like to take their
ADHD medications when playing their sport. It helps them improve their
ability to sustain focus and planning while they are playing so they do not
experience mind wandering or poor recall of the coach’s directions dur
ing the game. The remaining 50% prefer not to take their ADHD medica
tion when playing their sport because they feel it causes them to think too
much, to hesitate in making their moves as spontaneously as may be
needed. The only way I know to decide who should or should not take
their medication while playing their sport is to have them try it both ways
and then let me know which way seems to work better for them.
functions and then soon loses that beneficial response. If that is the case,
the most likely explanation is that the dose used during that time was al
most—but not quite—at the optimal point for that person. It is as
though the novelty of the drug’s effect on the brain wore off, and those
networks returned to their usual level of functioning. Often a slight in
crease in dose will restore the previously noticed effectiveness.
If a person has been taking an ADHD medication for a longer time—
months or years—and then notices that the medication has gradually lost
its earlier effectiveness, there may be a need to increase the dose a bit to
see if that will restore effectiveness. This may become necessary because
the person is under more stress than usual, is encountering more chal
lenges than previously, is not getting enough sleep, is in a growth spurt, is
fighting an infection, or is taking an additional drug that interferes with
the ADHD medication (e.g., using too much marijuana), or other changes
in the body or daily life may be altering the threshold of efficacy. How
ever, it is important to inquire into possible factors that may be producing
the change before escalating the dose. Some other intervention may be
more helpful. However, it is also true that occasionally, for some patients,
a medication may simply lose its effectiveness after a period of prolonged
benefit for no identifiable reason. This type of unexplained “poop out”
can occur with almost any type of medication, including antidepressants,
antihistamines, and many other medications unrelated to ADHD. In such
situations, it is sometimes necessary to try a different ADHD medication,
which might work more effectively. If after a “vacation” from the earlier
medication it is restarted, effectiveness may return.
It should also be noted, however, that sometimes an ADHD medica
tion that has been effective for a fairly long time at a stable dose may be
gin to cause the person to feel uncomfortably restless or much too
focused or too irritable over many days without any apparent reason.
Sometimes what is needed in that situation is a slight reduction of the
dose. This is not uncommon among children or adolescents, who some
times need a smaller dose of medication as they get older.
lant medication while drinking alcohol because they feel it will keep
them more alert while partying. The problem with this is that the stim
ulant medication is likely to make it more difficult for a person to rec
ognize when he or she has had too much alcohol or is getting too tired
and needs to stop. This can lead to excessive drinking, which can put a
person at risk of not only an intense hangover but also more serious
problems due to impaired judgment or high-risk behavior or possible
alcohol poisoning, which can be fatal.
Many who take stimulant medications early in the day will refrain
from taking any more stimulant in the afternoon or evening; they will
drink only a moderate amount of alcohol later in the evening when the
level of stimulant in their bloodstream is reduced. However, given the
wide variations in how quickly or slowly various individuals clear stim
ulants out of their bloodstream, anyone using ADHD medication who
wants to drink alcohol should discuss this with his or her clinician to get
personalized advice that also includes consideration of any other med
ications the individual may be taking that could complicate his or her
reaction to alcohol.
Many different kinds of medications can significantly alter how al
cohol affects a person’s body. While in the bloodstream, medications
used for anxiety, depression, sleep, pain, and other problems can poten
tiate the effects of drinking alcohol. Depending on the level of the spe
cific medication in the bloodstream at the time, one drink can double or
triple the bodily effects of any alcohol-containing drink without the
drinker realizing what is happening. Many medications when com
bined with alcohol can also cause cardiovascular difficulties, which can
range from mild to serious. The National Institute on Drug Abuse has
published a booklet that describes what types of medications can cause
such problems (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism
2003).
Such reactions are extremely rare and generally occur only in individu
als whose health is seriously impaired, but they do occasionally occur.
However, when medications are administered within accepted guide
lines to basically healthy individuals, adverse effects from medications
approved for treatment of ADHD involve relatively few risks.
The active ingredients of most medications currently approved to
treat ADHD have been in use for decades by very large numbers of chil
dren, adolescents, and adults. Amphetamine has been available since
the 1930s, and methylphenidate was introduced in the 1950s. The first
reports of the beneficial effects of amphetamine on learning and behav
ior of children were provided in 1937 by Bradley (1937), who discovered
these benefits accidentally in his research with emotionally and behav
iorally disturbed children.
Initially, these medications were widely used by adults as a treatment
for depression and to induce weight loss. They were officially issued to and
used by the American, British, German, and Japanese soldiers, sailors, and
airplane crews during the Second World War and are still officially issued
to some pilots in the U.S. Air Force to help them stay alert on long flights.
Both methylphenidate and amphetamines are controlled by the U.S.
government and most other governments in the same way opiate pain
medications are controlled; this is because, when misused, they have the
potential for abuse when administered in excessive doses or when ad
ministered in other than oral modalities. In the 1940s, there were numer
ous reports of stimulants being abused as treatments for weight loss.
That is no longer considered an appropriate use of stimulants.
The common side effects of stimulants are not persistent or danger
ous. It is not unusual for patients starting these medications to report
some difficulty with headache, stomachache, reduced appetite, and/or
falling asleep (Aagaard and Hansen 2011). Generally, such reactions are
not severe, tend to be quite transient, and usually respond to adjustment
of dose and/or timing. Some adverse reactions occurred because the
starting dose prescribed was too high for that individual. As mentioned
in the section “What Influences the Effectiveness of ADHD Medica
tion?” there is wide variation among patients in all age groups regard
ing what dose is likely to be the most effective.
Those considering use of ADHD medication for their children or
themselves are often concerned about the potential for more serious ad
verse effects such as heart attack or stroke, growth delays, or the possibil
ity of the child eventually becoming addicted to the ADHD medication or
later getting addicted to some illicit drug. Research has addressed each of
those concerns.
Practical Aspects of Medication Treatments for ADHD 199
will never become addicted to alcohol or another drug, but it tends to re
duce that risk to a level comparable to that for children without ADHD.
In considering any medical treatment for any disorder, the impor
tant question is, Are the risks of using this treatment for the diagnosed
problem greater or less than the risks of not using the treatment? As de
scribed in Chapter 4 (“Ways ADHD Can Impair Functioning at Various
Age Levels”) and other parts of this book, the risks associated with ADHD
are often quite substantial and may increase with age:
Summary
This chapter begins with the assertion that ADHD is essentially a prob
lem of the chemical dynamics of the brain and that the most effective
treatment for ADHD is usually medication. A description of how these
medications work in the brain is provided. It is noted that medication
does not cure ADHD, although for about 80% of children, adolescents,
and adults, approved medication, properly fine-tuned, can alleviate
their ADHD impairments during the hours it is active. Tables list char
acteristics and usual dosages of medications currently approved by the
FDA for treatment of ADHD.
The bulk of the chapter then presents practical information on fac
tors that may influence the effectiveness of medications for ADHD for
various individuals: how a medication regimen can be tailored to a
given individual, how various side effects can be addressed, how to tell
if a dose is too high, how to assess whether a medication is working for
a specific person, and how a clinician should respond if a medication
seems to lose its effectiveness. Answers to a number of other questions
that are frequently asked by clinicians and patients are also provided.
The chapter concludes with a summary of scientific research on the risks
and benefits of medications for ADHD.
11
Practical Aspects
of Nonmedication
Interventions
for ADHD
203
204 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
tient. This approach is designed not only to describe the new model of
ADHD but also to function as a way to gather more information about
the degree of fit between current difficulties of this particular patient
and the current scientific understanding of ADHD and/or other disor
ders.
In concluding an initial consultation, we generally offer the patient
and family a brief oral summary of our impressions from what we have
learned. This includes an indication of whether the patient’s presenta
tion actually does or does not meet diagnostic criteria for ADHD and/
or any other co-occurring problems that seem important. We also try to
highlight the specific strengths of the patient that may have helped him
or her to cope with the ADHD impairments. We also acknowledge any
specific stressors in past and/or present that have complicated or im
peded the patient’s efforts to deal with ADHD. In doing this, we often
use the circles inside squares graphic organizer (Brown 2005b), which is
available on my Web site (www.DrThomasEBrown.com). We then in
vite questions from the patient and any family members. This leads to a
brief description of “where to from here” options for follow-up; these
may include treatment, further evaluation, or neither. We also provide
an opportunity for the patient and family to raise and get answers to any
additional questions or concerns they may have.
Education about ADHD should not be limited to the initial consulta
tion. We usually provide each new patient with a packet of written infor
mation to take home. The packet includes some fact sheets and articles
about ADHD and its treatment. We encourage patients to get back in
touch with us if they have additional questions or concerns after com
pleting the interview. Often patients will want to schedule a follow-up
appointment as we conclude the initial consultation.
Some patients come to their initial consultation after having tried
some medications for ADHD. If so, during the clinical interview we will
discuss with them what they see as the benefits and/or problems asso
ciated with medications they are currently taking or have taken in the
past. This discussion may lead to our making suggestions for them to
take back to their prescriber or an offer to be available to consult with
their prescriber if they wish. To facilitate this process, we provide the pa
tients with a packet of written materials about ADHD and their evalu
ation data to take to their prescriber for follow-up.
If the patient has ADHD and is not currently taking any prescribed
medication for ADHD and is interested in a trial of medication, we usu
ally offer to provide a brief note to be taken to the patient’s primary care
provider or referring clinician. That note briefly summarizes the evalu
ation findings and suggests that the patient be given a physical exami
206 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
nation to identify or rule out any medical problems that might impact
the choice of treatment plan, particularly the possible use of medication.
If the patient wishes, we also offer a recommendation for a specific
ADHD medication that seems likely to be most helpful. The patient can
give this suggestion to his or her referring clinician or to his or her pri
mary care provider along with our offer to collaborate with that clini
cian in developing and monitoring a treatment plan for the patient’s
ADHD if that is agreeable to the clinician and the patient.
Most of our patients diagnosed with ADHD choose to return to us to
collaborate in designing and monitoring their treatment for ADHD. For
some, this involves just a few additional visits, after which they are fol
lowed by other providers with the option to return to the clinic for fur
ther consultation if needed. Others engage in more intensive and
sustained treatment in our clinic for their ADHD and any related disor
ders that concern them.
Some other patients, for various reasons, do not want to engage in
any kind of treatment for their ADHD in response to their initial consul
tation. In such cases, we offer the patients written information and as
sure them that they are welcome to return at any time if they have
further questions or want to reopen discussion of possible treatment op
tions.
• Is medication for ADHD safe for our child, and if so, what medica
tion is likely to work best? What side effects may occur, and how can
you tell if the medication is working? What monitoring do we need
to do, and how often should we meet with the clinician?
Usually, though not always, a student being evaluated for ADHD has
been having significant difficulties in school. Frequently, the ADHD
evaluation has been, in large part, sparked by those difficulties.
Sometimes the problems are behavioral, sometimes they are aca
demic, and often they are both. For students in elementary or second
ary school, the evaluation usually involves seeking information from
teachers about the strengths and difficulties of the student in school
functioning. Once an evaluation has been completed, if the student
has been found to have ADHD, it is usually a good idea for the par
ents to discuss the diagnosis with the student’s primary teacher. It is
important for parents to be aware that increasing numbers of teach
ers are well informed about ADHD and can offer valuable help in
planning ways to help students with ADHD to improve their func
tioning in school. However, it is also important for parents to know
that although all experienced teachers have had experience in deal
ing with students with ADHD, many are not aware of current under
standings of ADHD, and some, lacking adequate training in this area,
hold negative attitudes about the diagnosis and are unaware of best
practices for dealing with those students.
After learning that their child has been diagnosed with ADHD,
parents should inquire of the teacher or principal about what the
school’s usual policies are for supporting and accommodating stu
dents with ADHD. Information about possible supports and accom
210 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
modations for ADHD in school can be found later in this chapter. The
main point here is that parents should find out about the policies and
attitudes about ADHD held by teachers and administrators in their
child’s school and then seek to collaborate with school staff to plan
for minimizing negative effects and optimizing their child’s learning
and behavior in light of the ADHD diagnosis. It is also important for
parents to keep in mind that there are necessary limits to how much
any given teacher and school can or should accommodate to the
needs and desires of any one student and his family. If parents find
that some of their child’s teachers do not have up-to-date information
about ADHD, they may want to print and provide copies of the arti
cle “ADHD: From Stereotype to Science,” a piece written by the au
thor of this book that was published in Educational Leadership (Brown
2016), a national magazine for teachers. A copy can be found at my
Web site (www.DrThomasEBrown.com).
• One of us parents probably has ADHD and has never been treated for
it. Should we get evaluation and treatment for this? Would that help
our child?
• How can we provide the help that our child with ADHD needs at
home without being unfair to our other sons and daughters?
• Does our child need some kind of counseling or therapy to deal with
his or her ADHD and/or related problems? If so, where can we find
the right help?
When parents discover that their child has a significantly impairing dis
order, especially if they have overlooked or misinterpreted that prob
lem for some time, they are likely to want to do everything possible to
address that problem immediately in multiple ways. That may be a
mistake. First, it is usually not easy to find adequate professional help
for ADHD, particularly if it is complicated by anxiety, depression, sub
stance abuse, a specific learning disorder, or other co-occurring prob
lems. Second, suddenly pushing a child (or anyone else) to see and be
evaluated and treated by a variety of different clinicians for any prob
lem is likely to generate a “something very serious is wrong with me”
feeling. Deciding on a treatment approach usually works best if there is
one clinician who serves as case manager to help parents identify what
interventions are likely to be helpful to their child and to figure out
what is actually necessary and available. For some persons with
ADHD, particularly adolescents and adults, it may be very helpful to
have some conversations or psychotherapy with a knowledgeable and
empathic clinician about how their ADHD has impacted them and
what practical steps they might take to improve some of their difficul
ties. For others, particularly younger children, it may be best for the
consultations to be provided primarily to the parents so they can be an
ongoing source of support as their child is learning to deal with ADHD.
212 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
• Is our child’s ADHD likely to get better or worse as time goes on?
What does this mean for his or her future education, relationships,
and career?
ous tasks and types of learning. They watch how students interact with
one another and notice subtle changes in the dynamics of friendships
and alliances and conflicts between subgroups. Some teachers function
as trusted, empathic advisors for particular students, whereas they may
be seen by other students as harsh critics or overbearing managers or
menacing adversaries.
There are also differences among teachers in their willingness to share
information with inquiring parents. In some schools, there is an adminis
trative climate that urges teachers to be very cautious in telling parents any
thing about their son’s or daughter’s performance that might be taken by
the parents as overly harsh or critical. Those teachers may be instructed to
limit their reports to parents to just factual recitation of grades on assign
ments or very limited description of behavior, without any elaboration as to
what the teacher has noticed about the student’s attitude, social interac
tions, or more subtle aspects of his or her functioning in school.
Most school districts have a policy that forbids teachers from sug
gesting that a student may have ADHD, a learning disability, or any
other specific problem. Such policies have been put in place because
some teachers in the past have told parents that their child has ADHD
and insisted that the child be prescribed medication, seriously overstep
ping the boundaries of their training and competence. Yet, in some
schools, that caution against suggesting a diagnosis has been inter
preted as a requirement to refrain from making even such statements as
“Your child seems to have much more difficulty in focusing than most
others of similar age. You might want to consider asking your pediatri
cian about that.”
The usual way for most clinicians to solicit information about the
school functioning of a student being evaluated for ADHD or related
problems is to obtain copies of recent report cards from the parents and
to provide parents with rating scales they can take to teachers to request
specific information relevant to the evaluation. Many pediatricians use
the Vanderbilt ADHD Diagnostic Parent Rating Scale (National Insti
tute for Children’s Health Quality and American Academy of Pediatrics
2011) for this purpose. Schools and specialist clinicians more often use
other scales for teachers such as the Behavior Assessment System for
Children—Third Edition (BASC-3; Reynolds and Kamphaus 2015), Be
havior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (Gioia et al. 2000), Brown
Attention Deficit Disorder Scales (Brown 2001), Comprehensive Execu
tive Function Inventory (Naglieri and Goldstein 2013), or Conners
ADHD Rating Scales—3 (Conners 2008a); these rating scales are normed
and elicit more comprehensive information relevant to current under
standing of ADHD.
Practical Aspects of Nonmedication Interventions for ADHD 215
When a parent requests services for their child with a disability such
as ADHD or a specific learning disability, the school is required to con
vene within 45 days a meeting that usually involves the parents, one or
more of the student’s current teachers, the school psychologist, a special
education teacher, the school nurse, and the principal or another desig
nated administrator of that school. At that meeting, the parent’s concerns
are heard and the school staff members present updated information on
the student’s educational history, current functioning, and other relevant
background information. At the meeting, it is determined what further
action may be needed; this may include arranging for psychoeducational
testing or other specialist assessments.
Among the actions might be development of a 504 plan. This plan
provides supports and accommodations for a student with an identified
disability (e.g., ADHD, learning disability) that can be implemented
216 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
extra time, ordinarily 1.5 of the usual time, for tests and exams given in
the local school system, including state-mandated exams. However, a
504 plan or individualized education plan (IEP) does not, in itself, pro
vide accommodations for students taking national exams used for col
lege applications such as the SAT or the American College Testing
(ACT) exam.
Accommodations such as extended time for the SAT and/or ACT are
not obtained simply by having a 504 plan or an IEP. These national exams
require a detailed report documenting the nature of the student’s disabil
ity with evidence from school reports, standardized testing (usually in
cluding a full IQ test), academic achievement testing, and a clinical
evaluation done by a qualified professional such as a clinical psychologist
or neuropsychologist who is familiar with requirements for such accom
modations. This report has to be submitted with a specific application for
accommodations, in addition to the usual application for taking the
exam.
An application for accommodations must be submitted to the SAT
and/or ACT office at least 6 weeks prior to the date the student hopes to
take the exam. Representatives of the SAT or ACT then review the re
port to see if it meets requirements that are published on their Web site.
If so, extended time is granted. If not, the reviewer will deny the request
and notify the student as to what additional information would be re
quired for accommodations to be granted. If a request for accommoda
tions is denied, the student may get the decision reversed if additional
assessment data or other information required by the ACT or SAT re
viewers is provided in an appeal submission.
For students with identified disabilities whose needs cannot be met
adequately with just a 504 plan, the school is required by federal law to
convene a planning and placement team (PPT) meeting, where it will be
initially determined what additional assessments are needed for ade
quate planning. Once those data are obtained, the school develops an IEP,
which stipulates the nature of the student’s disability and provides guid
ance for needed services, such as remedial instruction, speech/language
or occupational therapy services, and/or accommodations such as ex
tended time for tests. The IEP identifies specific objectives and goals for
intervention, identifies professionals to provide needed interventions,
and specifies the frequency and amount of time for each intervention to
be provided.
Usually, those accommodations and interventions can be provided
by special education teachers or other specialists located within the
school; if not, visits from additional specialists needed may be arranged.
It is required by law that these plans be reviewed at least annually and
218 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
Accommodations in College
and Postgraduate Settings
For students with ADHD or related difficulties who enroll in postsec
ondary schooling, the college or university has no obligation to provide
accommodations or support services unless the student notifies the
school and provides appropriate documentation. Many schools will ac
cept a high school 504 plan or IEP, or the reports used to obtain accom
modations on the SAT or ACT, as sufficient evidence of the student’s
need for accommodations. However, some students who demonstrated
no need for accommodations in high school find they do need accom
modations to deal with the more challenging requirements of college.
Those accommodations may include extended time for exams, a mini
mally distracting setting for taking tests, or a peer note-taker whose
notes from a lecture can help to compensate for the student’s difficulty
in rapidly taking notes during class. If the student has had no accom
modations in elementary or high school, usually the college will require
a report similar to what is usually required for the SAT or ACT.
Some college students find that they need accommodations for
ADHD in the latter years of their undergraduate studies even though
they did not apply for or feel a need for any accommodations previ
ously. This may be due to not having recognized and been diagnosed
with ADHD earlier, or they may feel need for accommodations because
of increased demands for executive functions in meeting requirements
for more advanced courses. Some other university students with
ADHD may discover that they need accommodations for examinations
such as the GMAT, LSAT, or MCAT to gain admission to graduate-level
programs. Graduate students with ADHD may need accommodations
for taking exams such as the United States Medical Licensing Examina
tion, law boards, or other exams required for entrance into practice of
their profession.
For those who are applying for test-taking accommodations for the
first time relatively late in their education, it is important to ensure that
the person administering the required standardized tests also includes
in the report an adequate explanation of why accommodations are be
ing sought now when they were not sought in earlier years of schooling.
Some of the organizations offering such accommodations are exces
220 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
students and some parents have found helpful. Detailed legal informa
tion pertaining to higher education is provided in Peter S. Latham and
Patricia H. Latham’s (2007) book, Learning Disabilities/ADHD and the
Law in Higher Education and Employment.
Supportive Psychotherapy
for Young, Middle-Aged,
and Older Adults
After receiving a diagnosis of ADHD, many young and older adults
have three concurrent feelings. One important feeling is relief that there
is a name and a reason for many of the problems they have struggled
with and felt ashamed of for a long time. Another significant feeling is
hope that available treatments will be helpful in alleviating many of
those problems. A third common feeling is anger and resentment that
these problems were not recognized and treated long ago. They wonder
how different—how much better—their lives might have been if they
had been diagnosed and treated much earlier.
222 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
from the U.S. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. In addition to
its Web site, the center offers a free newsletter about ADHD and a tele
phone helpline to answer questions with science-based information
about the disorder. Adults may also find useful information from look
ing at Attention or ADDitude, two magazines that often provide helpful,
updated information for and about persons with ADHD. Books that
may prove useful include the following:
Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy
Although medication is usually the most effective treatment for ADHD,
many individuals continue to struggle with executive function impair
ments in daily life that are not adequately alleviated by medication
alone. For many of these individuals, especially adolescents and adults,
cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) can be very effective, especially
when used in conjunction with adequately fine-tuned medication treat
ment and when provided by a clinician who has learned to adapt tradi
tional cognitive-behavioral methods to treatment for ADHD.
CBT is quite different from many traditional forms of psychotherapy.
It focuses on specific assumptions or thoughts that lead to and reinforce
problematic behaviors, but it is concerned with not only those assump
tions and thoughts but also the resulting problematic behaviors. Its focus
is cognitive and behavioral—how one thinks about and interprets events
of daily life—and how those often-unrecognized thoughts and interpre
tations can interfere with managing many aspects of daily life.
Practical Aspects of Nonmedication Interventions for ADHD 225
Additional Supports
For students who have significant difficulty in a particular subject
area(s) and need more remedial tutoring than their school can provide,
many have found the Khan Academy (www.khanacademy.org/) to be a
valuable resource. This site provides a wide range of concise presenta
tions on countless topics for students from elementary school through
college. It is free and is available 24/7.
Some parents arrange private tutoring for their son or daughter. Of
ten school administrators or other parents can recommend a local tutor
230 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
Treatment interventions d
fractions to show how much effect a treatment has in comparison to another treatment
Summary
Medications alone are not usually adequate for effective treatment of
ADHD. In this chapter, a wide variety of nonmedication interventions
for ADHD that are often helpful for patients with ADHD and their fam
ilies are described. The most important aspect of treatment is for the cli
nician to provide accurate, science-based information about the nature
of ADHD, how it may change across the life cycle, and how it can most
effectively be treated. Questions frequently asked by parents concerned
about their child with ADHD are presented with suggestions for appro
priate guidance to be adapted and offered by the clinician.
When the patient is a student in elementary or high school, it may be
necessary and helpful for the diagnostic process for the clinician to col
laborate not only with the patient and family but also with the school
staff to establish ongoing communication and arrange possible interven
tions or accommodations to be provided by the school. Practical infor
mation and suggestions about such arrangements are offered in this
chapter along with information about other useful resources. Informa
tion about resources and supports for college and postgraduate students
is also included.
The chapter also furnishes suggestions about the possible usefulness
of supportive psychotherapy, cognitive-behavioral therapy, couple ther
apy, and coaching for ADHD. The chapter concludes with research
based information evaluating various alternative treatments.
12
Treatment
Adaptations for
Complications
233
234 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
cused on fear and worst-case possibilities “from the gut” without ade
quate awareness of the fuller context and of the ways the situation could
be addressed.
At present, ADHD medications are not approved for use in alleviat
ing excessively intense emotion, but a significant number of patients re
port that their ADHD medication helps them to remain more calm and
to be less easily agitated or upset by experiences or thoughts that made
them quite anxious before they were taking the ADHD medication. In
strengthening patients’ other executive functions, the ADHD medica
tion may also be strengthening their ability to modulate their anxiety or
other emotions that prior to medication use were excessively intense or
persistent.
In past years, and sometimes even today, clinicians were oftentimes
reluctant to prescribe ADHD stimulant medications for patients with
serious anxiety because they feared that stimulants would increase
these patients’ anxiety. A recent meta-analysis of stimulant medication
treatment in children with ADHD has challenged the assumption that
stimulants are likely to cause or exacerbate anxiety.
After analyzing 23 studies involving almost 3,000 children with
ADHD, Coughlin et al. (2015) found that treatment with stimulant med
ications for ADHD significantly reduced the risk of anxiety when com
pared with placebo treatment. These data led Coughlin et al. to recognize
that some children do experience anxiety when treated with stimulants,
but significantly more children with ADHD experience a reduction of
anxiety with stimulant treatment. These findings have not yet been rep
licated in adult samples, but clinical experience suggests that patterns
are similar in adult patients with ADHD.
If a patient is reporting seriously disabling anxiety problems, such as
acute panic attacks with persistent worry about having another panic at
tack, or if the anxiety is seriously and frequently disrupting the patient’s
eating or sleeping to the point where the patient is not able to function
adequately during the day, primary consideration should be given to
reducing the acute anxiety before any medication for ADHD is started.
A trial of low dosing of a benzodiazepine may be needed to facilitate
sleep or to provide immediate support for an imminent threat of a panic
attack. Meanwhile, the patient is likely to benefit from some cognitive
behavioral therapy and/or psychotherapy targeted toward reducing the
acute anxiety problems. An SSRI may also be started to reduce the pa
tient’s baseline anxiety level.
However, if a patient is diagnosed with ADHD and reports chronic
anxiety that is not acutely disabling, it may be preferable to discuss with
the patient the possibility of starting an ADHD medication first and sta
236 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
bilizing the dose to see if that medication can be tolerated and can be ti
trated to a dose that will significantly improve the ADHD symptoms
while perhaps also helping to reduce the chronic anxiety by improving
top-down executive control. Usually, a longer-acting stimulant medica
tion is less likely to exacerbate anxiety than immediate-release formula
tions. If the patient is willing to do this and the ADHD medication
regimen is adequately stabilized and if there is still persisting difficulty
with anxiety symptoms, the possibility of adding an SSRI to provide
further reduction of anxiety symptoms can be considered.
If a patient with ADHD has considerable difficulty with anxiety in
addition to ADHD impairments, he or she may already be taking med
ications for anxiety that were prescribed prior to his or her seeking eval
uation for ADHD. In that case, it is usually wise to consult with the
clinician who prescribed the antianxiety medication to seek agreement
to have the patient continue with the present antianxiety regimen when
beginning a trial of medication for ADHD. Stopping the antianxiety
medication and then immediately starting an ADHD medication would
make it very difficult to tell whether emerging problems are due to stop
ping the one medication or starting the other.
In any case, the clinician should advise the patient that some people
find ADHD medication makes them a bit more anxious for a time but
the majority of patients find that ADHD medication does not cause in
creased anxiety and that it may actually help to reduce anxiety. The pa
tient should be encouraged to contact the clinician before their next
scheduled visit if there are any significant difficulties or questions.
Medication follow-up with a patient experiencing anxiety should in
clude careful attention to any adverse effects and to the time and cir
cumstances of their occurrence so that the clinician can help the patient
sort out whether the adverse effects experienced result from the medi
cation or from the patient’s anticipation and expectation of such diffi
culties. If any adverse effects seem due to the medication, consideration
should be given to the possibility of reducing the dose versus remaining
at the starting dose for a bit longer to see if the patient’s body becomes
better able to tolerate the medication.
Many patients struggling with anxiety and ADHD also benefit from
cognitive-behavioral treatments. Ramsay (2010) and Ramsay and Ros
tain (2015a, 2015b) have described how cognitive-behavioral techniques
can be helpfully adapted to adults with ADHD and various complica
tions. Szigethy et al. (2012) provide strategies for cognitive-behavioral
treatment of children and adolescents, whereas Alfano and Beidel
(2011) have compiled guidance for treatment of social anxiety in chil
dren and adolescents and young adults in a variety of situations. Al
Treatment Adaptations for ADHD With Various Complications 237
and these mood problems seemed to have their initial onset with the use
of that medication, it may be that the ADHD medication itself is contrib
uting to or precipitating the mood problems. In inquiring about this
possibility, it is important to query about the time of day and the situa
tions in which the mood problems are emerging. If the problems occur
shortly after the ADHD medication is ingested and then improve when
the medication is expected to be wearing off, it may be that the dose of
the ADHD medication is too high for that patient or that the patient is
simply not able to tolerate that drug.
However, if the patient is not reporting mood problems throughout
the time the medication is active and does experience excessive moodi
ness around the time the medication is wearing off, that may be a sign of
rebound, in which the moodiness is a reaction to an excessively rapid
downward trajectory of the medication’s effects such that the person is
“crashing.” In that case, the mood problem may be resolved by adding
a small booster dose to slow down the excessively rapid declining tra
jectory of the medication’s effects. Sometimes rebound moodiness is
characterized by heightened irritability, sometimes by anxious restless
ness, and sometimes by depressive mood symptoms.
If the patient is presenting with a history of mood problems that did
not start in response to ADHD medication, then diagnostic inquiry
should focus on what kind of mood problems occur, how often and for
how long these mood problems have been occurring, and what circum
stances make them better or worse. It may also be helpful to inquire as to
whether any blood-related relatives have shown a similar pattern of
mood problems so that both genetic and environmental etiological fac
tors can be taken into account.
If the patient with ADHD has a long history of excessive irritability,
low frustration tolerance, frequent or protracted temper outbursts over
relatively trivial matters, or excessively aggressive verbal or physical ac
tions that are apparently not due to excessive drinking of alcohol, other
drug use, or environmental provocations (e.g., household members or
others deliberately provoking), a trial of stimulant medication may be a
reasonable first step. Both stimulant medication and atomoxetine have
been shown to be effective in alleviating core symptoms of ADHD but
also often have beneficial effects on emotional dysregulation. Shaw et al.
(2014) have reviewed the evidence and recommend these ADHD med
ications as first-line treatment for emotional dysregulation, possibly
with accompanying behavioral treatments. Another option with some
empirical support is guanfacine in combination with a stimulant. This
combination has been approved by the U.S. Food and Drug Adminis
tration (FDA) for such difficulties.
242 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
For patients with ADHD and depression who have prominent emo
tional dysregulation, Shaw et al. (2014) suggest that stimulant medica
tion combined with an SSRI may be a reasonable choice. However, there
is reason to be cautious in use of antidepressant medications with child,
adolescent, or young adult patients who manifest more severe mood
symptoms, such as signs of major depression and anxiety, and episodic
manic symptoms, such as sleep disturbances, anger or irritability, in
creased energy, or rapid mood fluctuations. Rios et al. (2015), in their
study of early stages of bipolar disorder, noted an emerging body of ev
idence that treatment with antidepressants, tricyclic antidepressants or
SSRIs, may precipitate or exacerbate suicidality and manic symptoms,
possibly reducing age at onset of mania. This risk warrants careful mon
itoring of patient response when an antidepressant is being started, es
pecially in the early weeks.
Rios et al. (2015) also noted that acute treatment with stimulants may
stimulate mania in persons in the prodromal stage of bipolar disorder.
However, Carlson and Meyer (2009) emphasized the importance of
treating ADHD impairments in patients with significant mood prob
lems. They suggested that if a patient clearly seems to have mania or a
bipolar disorder, treatment should begin with a mood stabilizer and/or
an atypical antipsychotic medication, with medication treatment for
ADHD added after the mood problems are stabilized if the patient re
mains impaired by ADHD symptoms. In situations where a mood dis
order diagnosis is less clear-cut, Carlson and Meyer described beginning
with ADHD medication treatment as acceptable so long as the clinician
is alert to possible disinhibition resulting from stimulant medication.
Joshi and Wozniak (2015) emphasize that medication treatment of
ADHD should not be addressed in cases of bipolar disorder until after
the bipolar symptoms have been stabilized. However, they also recog
nize that in a mood-stabilized youth with bipolar disorder, ADHD
symptoms with their impact on social, academic, and emotional func
tioning often become the next most severe complaint presented by the
patient.
For many children and some adults, it is often difficult to stop activ
ities of the day at a reasonable time. Some children with ADHD are
chronically oppositional and tend to resist directions to prepare for bed
much as they resist many other directions from their parents. Many chil
dren and adults with ADHD find it very difficult to stop their evening
activities and get settled into bed at a reasonable time even when they
recognize the need to do so. Sometimes the problem with older children,
adolescents, and adults is staying up late to finish homework or reports
they are expected to hand in the next morning at school or work. More
often, delays in getting into bed are due to their reluctance to stop en
gaging in favored activities such as watching television, playing video
games, texting or interacting on social media with friends, surfing the
Internet, or pleasure reading.
In many instances, the individual may get into bed but continue to
engage in these activities that interest him or her. Sometimes adoles
cents or young adults will get into bed, fall asleep, and then be awak
ened by friends who text them at late hours and whom they want to
talk with or feel obliged to interact with until very late. Many of those
who are chronically delayed in getting to sleep report, “I really want to
get to sleep earlier, but most nights I just can’t shut my head off, so I
have to busy myself with something else until I’m exhausted. I can fall
asleep easily at that point, but often that doesn’t leave me many hours
to sleep.”
Some individuals with ADHD report that they struggle on many
nights with getting to sleep because of thinking about things that sadden
or worry them; they are unable to dismiss those disturbing thoughts and
associated feelings when trying to get to sleep. At such times, they may
be flooded by sadness, worry, or guilt about something that has recently
happened or something they expect to happen soon.
When onset of sleep has been delayed, regardless of the reason, total
sleep time is reduced. For those who are required to get up so they can
be on time for school or work, this can create significant problems, es
pecially if sleep delays tend to be long and chronic.
A common result of chronic difficulty in getting to sleep is chronic
difficulty in awakening on schedule. Some persons with ADHD report
chronic difficulty in awakening even when they have had an ample
quantity of decent sleep. Many with ADHD report that every day be
244 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
with street terms for various drugs is Buzzed: The Straight Facts About the
Most Used and Abused Drugs From Alcohol to Ecstasy (Kuhn et al. 2008).
If a patient diagnosed with ADHD is found to have a recent or cur
rent problem with alcohol or drug use, it is extremely important to take
this into account in assessment and treatment planning. Persons with
substance use problems are not all alike, and they do not all have the
same needs for particular types of treatment. The so-called addictive
personality is a myth. Individuals who become involved in substance
use that reaches the level of a disorder vary considerably in their age,
race, ethnic background, social class, and personality characteristics.
Maia Szalavitz (2016), who herself experienced and recovered from
serious problems with drug addiction, has provided, in Unbroken Brain:
A Revolutionary New Way of Understanding Addiction, an articulate de
scription of the complex diversity among persons suffering from sub
stance use disorders and their struggles in treatment and recovery:
Any efforts to provide help and support for persons with a sub
stance use disorder should be guided by recognition that substance use
disorders are not fundamentally fueled by a quest for pleasure but by
desperate efforts to find relief from pain and stress that the individual
has not been able to cope with adequately. This is important because it
makes clear why curtailing a substance use disorder initially brings not
just relief but also intensification of whatever emotional burdens that
person has been trying to keep at bay by excessive use of the problem
atic substance. Initial pride and pleasure in stopping problematic use is
often quickly challenged by fears of being overwhelmed by hidden vul
nerabilities.
Many treatment programs for persons with substance use disorders
are based on the assumption that persons with substance use disorders
must “hit bottom” before they can recover—that they will be motivated to
stop their addictive behavior only when they have suffered painful loss of
virtually all that is valuable to them. This view often encourages parents
and other family members to adhere to a “tough love” approach in deal
252 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
ing with the addicted person. This model, often promulgated in 12-step
programs such as Alcoholics Anonymous (AA) and Narcotics Anony
mous, typically includes the notion that the goal of treatment must be
achieving and sustaining total abstinence from the problematic sub
stance. Another premise of such programs is that recovery can begin only
when one admits to being totally powerless to overcome addiction and to
needing to invoke the help of a higher power, which may be prayer and/
or continuing attendance at AA or a similar support group. This approach
has helped a very large number of people to find and sustain recovery
from a substance use disorder, but there are also many for whom it does
not work effectively and for whom it is a barrier to recovery.
An alternative approach to helping persons with a substance use
disorder is harm reduction, as summarized in Harm Reduction: Pragmatic
Strategies for Managing High-Risk Behaviors, edited by G. Alan Marlatt
(1998). This approach acknowledges total abstinence as a worthy goal, but
it accepts alternatives that reduce harm. It is a gradual “step-down” ap
proach that encourages those engaged in high-risk behaviors to take it “one
step at a time” to reduce the harmful consequences of their substance use.
Harm reduction approaches do not wait for an individual to “hit
bottom”; they try to encourage individuals to reduce their problematic
use to less destructive levels so they can protect whatever resources they
have. Szalavitz (2016) emphasizes the potential benefit of this approach:
[P]eople are actually more likely to recover when they still have jobs,
family, and greater ties to mainstream society, not less. Indeed, the more
“social capital” someone has—friends, education, employment, job con
tacts and other knowledge that promotes links to the conventional
world—the more likely recovery is. (p. 184)
Over the past 20 years, there have been many changes in scientific
understanding of addiction and its treatment. Carroll and Rounsaville
(2006) have described how and why inpatient treatment for substance
use disorders is now much less frequently used. It has been replaced by
outpatient and intensive day treatment approaches that seek to help pa
tients to learn to cope with their addiction in the context of their own
community. It is now known that brief behavioral therapies with a mo
tivational approach are more effective for most individuals with sub
stance use disorders. These approaches typically include
If a clinician is evaluating a patient with ADHD and finds that the pa
tient has a current substance use disorder that is significantly impairing
functioning, the clinician needs to determine with the patient whether
the substance use disorder is at a point where involvement in specialized
treatment such as motivational behavioral therapy or intensive outpa
tient treatment may be needed. For more acute problems, detoxification
or residential treatment may be needed. Medication treatment for
ADHD is not likely to be effective in such circumstances and may, in fact,
exacerbate the problem, although it may be quite helpful once the acute
phase is resolved.
If a patient with significant impairment from ADHD has a pattern of
occasional use of alcohol or marijuana that is not significantly disrupt
ing his or her functioning, a cautious trial of medication treatment for
ADHD may be quite appropriate. In such situations, longer-acting for
mulations of stimulants or nonstimulant medications for ADHD are less
likely to be misused and more likely to be effective than are preparations
with medium duration of action or immediate-release preparations
(Wilens and Morrison 2015).
If a clinician agrees to undertake such a treatment plan, it is impor
tant to provide close monitoring of both medication use and the pattern
of alcohol or marijuana use, including not only frequencies but also
quantities. The clinician, in such situations, needs to avoid the role of
“enforcer” for reducing alcohol or marijuana use. Techniques of moti
vational interviewing described by Miller and Rollnick (2013) can be
helpful in respecting the patient’s ambivalence and autonomy.
With such support, many patients with ADHD are able to maintain a
helpful regimen of medication treatment for their ADHD while holding
their use of marijuana to low doses a couple of times a week and their
consumption of alcohol to a reasonable level. However, for some pa
tients, it is extremely difficult to avoid resuming excessive use, in which
case the medication for ADHD is likely to become ineffective and they
are likely to return to a situation where they have insufficient motiva
tion to do tasks important for their success.
their learning and with their being able to sustain reciprocal conversa
tion (Volkmar et al. 2014).
Often persons whose ADHD is combined with AS are brought for
treatment during elementary, middle, or high school because of inatten
tion problems and/or problems with social interaction with peers; often
the social problems include persistent difficulties with being bullied. For
example, a 13-year-old boy was brought by his mother for evaluation:
Fred was in eighth grade and was in special education classes; he had
been diagnosed with ADHD in fourth grade. He had been prescribed
two different ADHD medications but in each instance had to stop the
medication because of adverse effects, primarily increased anxiety. At
the time of initial evaluation, he was taking 50 mg of sertraline but had
become increasingly resistant to doing schoolwork, often sitting in
classes with his head on his desk, refusing to do classwork or home
work, and missing many days of school with complaints such as stom
achache and headache. At home, he spent most of his time in his room
playing video games. When I asked him to describe himself, he said, “I
hate school, and I have no real friends.” His major complaint was that he
hated being in special education classes: “The teachers keep telling me
that I’m very smart, but they treat me like a fourth grader. I’m not going
to do what they tell me until they start treating me a lot better.”
In our psychotherapy sessions, Fred initially tried to press his argu
ment that his teachers needed to change their behavior before he would
change his. After a few sessions, he responded to my confronting him with
how he would need to change his oppositional behavior at school before
anyone would change anything else for him. I administered an IQ test, and
Fred scored in the superior range. Seeing those results, he said, “OK, I
know I’m smart, but I really am broken.” We then began to talk about how
he would soon be starting high school, where he could be placed in main
stream classes if he could show that he would do his job as a student for the
remaining months of the school year. I also told him that if he wanted to
improve his interactions with classmates, he would need to use his strong
intellect to learn skills for dealing with adults and other kids, skills that
most kids pick up intuitively. Low doses of stimulant medication were pre
scribed for Fred’s ADHD, which he was able to tolerate as the dosage was
very gradually increased. Over the final quarter of the semester, his behav
ior and work output at school improved markedly, and he was able to start
ninth grade in mainstream classes. This combination of having his strong
abilities confirmed while addressing his weaknesses in social skills, com
bined with gradual introduction of ADHD medication, helped to motivate
Fred to make the necessary changes to achieve his goal.
Summary
For the many individuals whose ADHD is complicated by various other
learning, psychiatric, or medical problems, it is very important for the
clinician to adapt treatment plans for the specific needs of the patient.
This is crucial not only for those who fully meet diagnostic criteria for
another disorder but also for patients who are affected by additional
symptoms, even if those symptoms do not fully meet official diagnostic
criteria.
The discussions in this chapter provide some brief guidance for cli
nicians who are treating children or adults whose ADHD is complicated
by additional difficulties. More detailed guidance for such complicated
situations is included in ADHD Comorbidities: Handbook for ADHD Com
plications in Children and Adults (Brown 2009). That edited volume offers,
for example, full chapters on ADHD with anxiety disorders, mood dis
orders, sleep disorders, substance use disorders, oppositional and ag
gressive disorders, and learning disorders in multiple age groups, each
chapter written by experts who are specialists in that field.
This chapter moves beyond the information in Chapter 7 about var
ious co-occurring disorders. It provides information about how ADHD
assessment and treatment may need to be adapted for patients who also
suffer from anxiety, depression, specific learning problems, emotional
regulation and mood problems, sleep and arousal problems, opposi
tionality and/or aggression, obsessional and/or compulsive problems,
substance use problems, Asperger’s syndrome and other autism spec
trum difficulties, or any combination of these.
References
Aagaard L, Hansen EH: The occurrence of adverse drug reactions reported for
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) medications in the pediat
ric population: a qualitative review of empirical studies. Neuropsychiatr
Dis Treat 7:729–744, 2011 22247615
Abikoff H, Gittelman R: The normalizing effects of methylphenidate on the
classroom behavior of ADDH children. J Abnorm Child Psychol 13(1):33–
44, 1985 3973251
Achenbach T: Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18 and 1991 Profile,
Burlington, VT, University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry, 1991
Agnew-Blais JC, Polanczyk GV, Danese A, et al: Evaluation of the persistence,
remission, and emergence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in
young adulthood. JAMA Psychiatry 73(7):713–720, 2016 27192174
Alderson RM, Kasper LJ, Hudec KL, et al: Attention-deficit/hyperactivity dis
order (ADHD) and working memory in adults: a meta-analytic review.
Neuropsychology 27(3):287–302, 2013 23688211
Alfano CA, Beidel DC: Social Anxiety in Adolescents and Young Adults: Trans
lating Developmental Science Into Practice. Washington, DC, American
Psychological Association, 2011
Alloway TP, Elliott J, Place M: Investigating the relationship between attention
and working memory in clinical and community samples. Child Neuro
psychol 16(3):242–254, 2010 20221932
American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, 3rd Edition. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Associa
tion, 1980
American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, 4th Edition. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association,
1994
American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, 5th Edition. Arlington, VA, American Psychiatric Association,
2013
Americans with Disabilities Act of 1990, Pub. L. 101-366
Angold A, Costello EJ, Erkanli A: Comorbidity. J Child Psychol Psychiatry
40(1):57–87, 1999 10102726
Anmarkrud Ø, Braten I: Motivation for reading comprehension. Learn Individ
Differ 19:252–256, 2009
Antshel KM, Faraone SV, Stallone K, et al: Is attention deficit hyperactivity dis
order a valid diagnosis in the presence of high IQ? Results from the MGH
Longitudinal Family Studies of ADHD. J Child Psychol Psychiatry
48(7):687–694, 2007 17593149
Arcos-Burgos M, Vélez JI, Solomon BD, et al: A common genetic network under
lies substance use disorders and disruptive or externalizing disorders.
Hum Genet 131(6):917–929, 2012 22492058
257
258 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
Blair JR, Leibenluft E, Pine DS: Conduct disorder and callous-unemotional traits
in youth. N Engl J Med 371(23):2207–2216, 2014
Bowlby J: Attachment theory and its therapeutic implications. Adolesc Psychi
atry 6:5–33, 1978 742687
Bradley C: Behavior of children receiving Benzedrine. Am J Psychiatry 94(11):577–
585, 1937
Breslau J, Miller E, Joanie Chung WJ, et al: Childhood and adolescent onset psy
chiatric disorders, substance use, and failure to graduate high school on
time. J Psychiatr Res 45(3):295–301, 2011 20638079
Britton JC, Lissek S, Grillon C, et al: Development of anxiety: the role of threat
appraisal and fear learning. Depress Anxiety 28(1):5–17, 2011 20734364
Brown TE: Brown Attention Deficit Disorder Scales for Adolescents and Adults.
San Antonio, TX, The Psychological Corporation, 1996
Brown TE: Attention-deficit Disorders and Comorbidities in Children, Adoles
cents, and Adults. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 2000
Brown TE: Brown Attention-Deficit Disorder Scales for Children and Adoles
cents. San Antonio, TX, The Psychological Corporation, 2001
Brown TE: Atomoxetine and stimulants in combination for treatment of atten
tion deficit hyperactivity disorder: four case reports. J Child Adolesc Psy
chopharmacol 14(1):129–136, 2004 15142400
Brown TE: Attention Deficit Disorder: The Unfocused Mind in Children and
Adults. New Haven, CT, Yale University Press, 2005a
Brown TE: Circles inside squares: a graphic organizer to focus diagnostic for
mulations. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 44(12):1309–1312, 2005b
16292124
Brown TE: Executive function and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: impica
tions for two conflicting views. Intl J Disabil Dev Educ 53(1):35–46, 2006
Brown TE: Developmental complexities of attentional disorders, in ADHD Co
morbidities: Handbook for ADHD Complications in Children and Adults.
Edited by Brown TE. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Publishing,
2009, pp 3–22
Brown TE: A New Understanding of ADHD in Children and Adults: Executive
Function Impairments. New York, Routledge, 2013
Brown TE: Smart but Stuck: Emotions in Teens and Adults With ADHD. New
York, Jossey-Bass, 2014
Brown TE: ADHD: from stereotype to science. February 2016. Available at:
http://www.drthomasebrown.com/adhd-from-stereotype-to-science/.
Accessed October 6, 2016.
Brown TE: Brown Executive Function/Attention Rating Scales, New York,
Pearson (in press)
Brown TE, McMullen WJ: Attention deficit disorders and sleep/arousal distur
bances. Ann N Y Acad Sci 931:271–286, 2001 11462746
Brown TE, Fishco VV, Hanna G: Nelson-Denny Reading Test: Manual for Scor
ing and Interpretation. Itasca, IL, Riverside, 1993
Brown TE, Reichel PC, Quinlan DM: Executive function impairments in high IQ
adults with ADHD. J Atten Disord 13(2):161–167, 2009 19420282
Brown TE, Reichel PC, Quinlan DM: Impairments of written expression in 13- to
25-year-old students with ADHD. Presented at the annual meeting of the
American Psychological Association, San Diego, CA, August 2010
References 261
Brown TE, Reichel PC, Quinlan DM: Executive function impairments in high IQ
children and adolescents with ADHD. Open J Psychiatr 1:56–65, 2011a
Brown TE, Reichel PC, Quinlan DM: Extended time improves reading compre
hension test scores for adolescents with ADHD. Open J Psychiatr 1:79–87,
2011b
Buchanan TW, Tranel D, Adolphs R: The human amygdala in social function, in
The Human Amygdala. Edited by Whalen PJ, Phelps EA. New York, Guil
ford, 2009, pp 289–318
Burgess PW: Theory and methodology in executive function research, in P. Rab
bit (Ed.), Methodology of Frontal and Executive Function. Edited by Rabbit
P. East Sussex, UK, Psychology Press, 1997, pp 81–116
Burke JD, Waldman I, Lahey BB: Predictive validity of childhood oppositional
defiant disorder and conduct disorder: implications for the DSM-V. J Ab
norm Psychol 119(4):739–751, 2010 20853919
Carlson CL, Pelham WE Jr, Swanson JM, et al: A divided attention analysis of the
effects of methylphenidate on the arithmetic performance of children with at
tention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 32(3):463–
471, 1991 2061366
Carlson GA, Meyer SE: ADHD with mood disorders, in ADHD Comorbidities:
Handbook for ADHD Complications in Children and Adults. Edited by
Brown TE. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Publishing, 2009, pp 97–
130
Carretti B, Borella E, Cornoldi C, et al: Role of working memory in explaining
the performance of individuals with specific reading comprehension diffi
culties: a meta-analysis. Learn Individ Differ 19(2):246–251, 2009
Carroll JB, Sapon S: Modern Language Aptitude Test: Manual 2002 Edition.
Bethesda, MD, Second Language Testing, 2002
Carroll KM, Rounsaville BJ: Behavior therapies: the glass would be half full if
only we had a glass, in Rethinking Substance Abuse: What the Science
Shows, and What We Should Do About It. Edited by Miller WR, Carroll
KM. New York, Guilford, 2006, pp 223–239
Caye A, Rocha TBM, Anselmi L, et al: Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder
trajectories from childhood to young adulthood: evidence from a birth co
hort supporting a late-onset syndrome. JAMA Psychiatry 73(7):705–712,
2016 27192050
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention: National marriage and divorce trends
rates; Provisional number of marriages and marriage rate: United States, 2000–
2014. Atlanta, GA, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Cen
ter for Health Statistics, National Vital Statistics System, 2015
Chacko A, Feirsen N, Bedard AC, et al: Cogmed Working Memory Training for
youth with ADHD: a closer examination of efficacy utilizing evidence
based criteria. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 42(6):769–783, 2013 23668397
Chamberlain SR, Blackwell AD, Feinberg NA, et al: The neuropsychology of ob
sessive compulsive disorder: the importance of failures in cognitive and be
havioral inhibition as candidate endophenotypic markers. Neurosci
Biobehav Rev 29:399–419, 2005 15820546
Chang Z, Lichtenstein P, Asherson PJ, Larsson H: Developmental twin study of
attention problems: high heritabilities throughout development. JAMA
Psychiatry 70(3):311–318, 2013 23303526
262 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
Kessler RC, Green JG, Adler LA, et al: Structure and diagnosis of adult atten
tion-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: analysis of expanded symptom criteria
from the Adult ADHD Clinical Diagnostic Scale. Arch Gen Psychiatry
67(11):1168–1178, 2010 21041618
Klass P, Costello E: Quirky Kids: Understanding and Helping Your Child Who
Doesn’t Fit In—When to Worry and When Not to Worry. New York, Ballan
tine Books, 2003
Koegel LK, Koegel RL, Fredeen RM, et al: Naturalistic behavioral approaches to
treatment, in Autism Spectrum Disorders in Infants and Toddlers: Diagno
sis, Assessment, and Treatment. Edited by Chawarska K, Klin A, Volkmar
FR, New York, Guilford, 2008, pp 207–242
Konrad K, Neufang S, Fink GR, et al: Long-term effects of methylphenidate on
neural networks associated with executive attention in children with
ADHD: results from a longitudinal functional MRI study. J Am Acad Child
Adolesc Psychiatry 46(12):1633–1641, 2007 18030085
Koob GF, Volkow ND: Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacol
ogy 35(1):217–238, 2010 19710631
Kuhn C, Swartwelder S, Wilson W: Buzzed: The Straight Facts About the Most
Used and Abused Drugs From Alcohol to Ecstasy. New York, WW Norton,
2008
Kuriyan AB, Pelham WE Jr, Molina BSG, et al: Young adult educational and vo
cational outcomes of children diagnosed with ADHD. J Abnorm Child Psy
chol 4(1):27–41, 2013 22752720
Lara C, Fayyad J, de Graaf R, et al: Childhood predictors of adult attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder: results from the World Health Organization World
Mental Health Survey Initiative. Biol Psychiatry 65(1):46–54, 2009 19006789
Larson K, Russ SA, Kahn RS, et al: Patterns of comorbidity, functioning, and ser
vice use for US children with ADHD, 2007. Pediatrics 127(3):462–470, 2011
21300675
Latham PS, Latham PH: Learning Disabilities/ADHD and the Law in Higher
Education and Employment. Washington, DC, JKL Communications, 2007
LeDoux JE: The Emotional Brain. New York, Simon & Schuster, 1996
Lee SH, Ripke S, Neale BM, et al; Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Ge
nomics Consortium; International Inflammatory Bowel Disease Genetics
Consortium (IIBDGC): Genetic relationship between five psychiatric disor
ders estimated from genome-wide SNPs. Nat Genet 45(9):984–994, 2013
23933821
Lee SS, Humphreys KL, Flory K, et al: Prospective association of childhood at
tention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and substance use and
abuse/dependence: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev 31(3):328–
341, 2011 21382538
Lensing MB, Zeiner P, Sandvik L, Opjordsmoen S: Psychopharmacological
treatment of ADHD in adults aged 50+: an empirical study. J Atten Disord
19(5):380–389, 2015 24681898
Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW: Neuropsychological Assessment, 4th
Edition. New York, Oxford University Press, 2004
Ljung T, Chen Q, Lichtenstein P, Larsson H: Common etiological factors of at
tention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and suicidal behavior: a population
based study in Sweden. JAMA Psychiatry 71(8):958–964, 2014 24964928
268 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
Rief S: How to Reach and Teach Children With ADD/ADHD: Practical Tech
niques, Strategies, and Interventions, 2nd Edition. San Francisco, CA,
Jossey-Bass, 2005
Rios AC, Noto MN, Rizzo LB, et al: Early stages of bipolar disorder: character
ization and strategies for early intervention. Rev Bras Psiquiatr 37(4):343–
349, 2015 26692432
Rubia K, Halari R, Cubillo A, et al: Methylphenidate normalises activation and
functional connectivity deficits in attention and motivation networks in
medication-naïve children with ADHD during a rewarded continuous per
formance task. Neuropharmacology 57(7–8):640–652, 2009 19715709
Rubia K, Halari R, Cubillo A, et al: Methylphenidate normalizes fronto-striatal
underactivation during interference inhibition in medication-naïve boys
with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology
36(8):1575–1586, 2011 21451498
Rutter M, Quinton D: Psychiatric disorder: ecological factors and concepts of
causation, in Ecological Factors in Human Development. Edited by Mc-
Gurk H. Amsterdam, The Netherlands, North-Holland Publishing, 1977,
pp 173-187
Safer J: The Normal One: Life With a Difficult or Damaged Sibling. New York,
Bantam Dell, 2003
Sayal K, Washbrook E, Propper C: Childhood behavior problems and academic
outcomes in adolescence: longitudinal population-based study. J Am Acad
Child Adolesc Psychiatry 54(5):360–368, 2015 25901772
Schmeichel BJ, Volokhov RN, Demaree HA: Working memory capacity and the
self-regulation of emotional expression and experience. J Pers Soc Psychol
95(6):1526–1540, 2008 19025300
Schwarz A: Risky rise of the good-grade pill. The New York Times, June 9, 2012
Schwarz A: The selling of attention deficit disorder. The New York Times, De
cember 15, 2013
Schwebel DC, Speltz ML, Jones K, et al: Unintentional injury in preschool boys
with and without early onset of disruptive behavior. J Pediatr Psychol
27(8):727–737, 2002 12403863
Schweren LJ, Hartman CA, Heslenfeld DJ, et al: Thinner medial temporal cortex in
adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and the effects of
stimulants. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 54(8):660–667, 2015 26210335
Section 504 of the Rehabilitation Act of 1973, 34 C.F.R. Part 104
Sesma HW, Mahone EM, Levine T, et al: The contribution of executive skills to
reading comprehension. Child Neuropsychol 15(3):232–246, 2009 18629674
Seymour KE, Chronis-Tuscano A, Halldorsdottir T, et al: Emotion regulation
mediates the relationship between ADHD and depressive symptoms in
youth. J Abnorm Child Psychol 40(4):595–606, 2012 22113705
Shallice T: Specific impairments of planning. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci
298(1089):199–209, 1982 6125971
Shanmugan S, Loughead J, Nanga RP, et al: Lisdexamfetamine effects on executive
activation and neurochemistry in menopausal women with executive function
difficulties. Neuropsychopharmacology 42(2):437–445, 2017 27550732
Shaw P, Eckstrand K, Sharp W, et al: Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
104(49):19649–19654, 2007 18024590
References 273
Shaw P, Malek M, Watson B, et al: Development of cortical surface area and gyri
fication in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 72(3):191–
197, 2012 22418014
Shaw P, Malek M, Watson B, et al: Trajectories of cerebral cortical development
in childhood and adolescence and adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder. Biol Psychiatry 74(8):599–606, 2013 23726514
Shaw P, Stringaris A, Nigg J, et al: Emotion dysregulation in attention deficit hy
peractivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry 171(3):276–293, 2014 24480998
Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA: Paying attention to reading: the neurobiology of
reading and dyslexia. Dev Psychopathol 20(4):1329–1349, 2008 18838044
Shechner T, Britton JC, Pérez-Edgar K, et al: Attention biases, anxiety, and devel
opment: toward or away from threats or rewards? Depress Anxiety 29(4):282–
294, 2012 22170764
Sidlauskaite J, Sonuga-Barke E, Roeyers H, Wiersema JR: Default mode net
work abnormalities during state switching in attention deficit hyperactiv
ity disorder. Psychol Med 46(3):519–528, 2016 26456561
Silvetti M, Wiersema JR, Sonuga-Barke E, et al: Deficient reinforcement learning
in medial frontal cortex as a model of dopamine-related motivational defi
cits in ADHD. Neural Netw 46:199–209, 2013 23811383
Sinita E, Coghill D: The use of stimulant medications for non-core aspects of
ADHD and in other disorders. Neuropharmacology 87:161–172, 2014
24951855
Sobanski E, Banaschewski T, Asherson P, et al: Emotional lability in children
and adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD):
clinical correlates and familial prevalence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry
51(8):915–923, 2010 20132417
Solanto MV: Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy for Adult ADHD: Targeting Execu
tive Dysfunction. New York, Guilford, 2013
Sonuga-Barke EJ: Causal models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder:
from common simple deficits to multiple developmental pathways. Biol
Psychiatry 57(11):1231–1238, 2005 15949993
Sonuga-Barke EJ, Brandeis D, Cortese S, et al; European ADHD Guidelines
Group: Nonpharmacological interventions for ADHD: systematic review
and meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials of dietary and psycho
logical treatments. Am J Psychiatry 170(3):275–289, 2013 23360949
Sparrow EP, Erhardt D: Essentials of ADHD Assessment for Children and Ad
olescents. Hoboken, NJ, Wiley, 2014
Sparrow SS, Cicchetti DV, Saulnier CA: Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales
Manual, Third edition (Vineland-3). Bloomington, MN, Pearson, 2016
Sporns O: Networks of the Brain. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press, 2011
Sporns O: Discovering the Human Connectome. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press, 2012
Sripada CS, Kessler D, Angstadt M: Lag in maturation of the brain’s intrinsic
functional architecture in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Proc
Natl Acad Sci USA 111(39):14259–14264, 2014 25225387
Stahl SM, Mignon L: Stahl’s Illustrated Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disor
der. New York, Cambridge University Press, 2011
Stark R, Bauer E, Merz CJ, et al: ADHD related behaviors are associated with
brain activation in the reward system. Neuropsychologia 49(3):426–434,
2011 21163276
274 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
Stavro GM, Ettenhofer ML, Nigg JT: Executive functions and adaptive function
ing in young adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Int Neuro
psychol Soc 13(2):324–334, 2007 17286889
Storch EA, Merlo LJ, Larson MJ, et al: Impact of comorbidity on cognitive
behavioral therapy response in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. J
Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 47(5):583–592, 2008 18356759
Stroop JR: Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol
18(6):643–662, 1935
Surman CB, Biederman J, Spencer T, et al: Understanding deficit emotional self
regulation in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a con
trolled study. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord 5(3):273–281, 2013 23413201
Swanson JM, Elliott GR, Greenhill LL, et al: Effects of stimulant medication on
growth rates across 3 years in the MTA follow-up. J Am Acad Child Ado
lesc Psychiatry 46(8):1015–1027, 2007a 17667480
Swanson JM, Hinshaw SP, Arnold LE, et al: Secondary evaluations of MTA 36
month outcomes: propensity score and growth mixture model analyses. J
Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 46(8):1003–1014, 2007b 17667479
Swanson J, Baler RD, Volkow ND: Understanding the effects of stimulant med
ications on cognition in individuals with attention-deficit hyperactivity
disorder: a decade of progress. Neuropsychopharmacology 36(1):207–226,
2011 20881946
Szalavitz M: Unbroken Brain: A Revolutionary New Way of Understanding Ad
diction. New York, St Martin’s Press, 2016
Szigethy E, Weisz JR, Findling RL: Cognitive-Behavior Therapy for Children
and Adolescents. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Publishing, 2012
Thomas K: Generic ADHD drug, not equivalent to the brand, is in use anyway.
The New York Times, June 16, 2015
Thompson AL, Molina BS, Pelham W Jr, et al: Risky driving in adolescents and
young adults with childhood ADHD. J Pediatr Psychol 32(7):745–759, 2007
17442694
Thompson TL, Moss RL: Estrogen regulation of dopamine release in the nu
cleus accumbens: genomic- and nongenomic-mediated effects. J Neuro
chem 62(5):1750–1756, 1994 8158125
Toll SW, Van der Ven SH, Kroesbergen EH, et al: Executive functions as predic
tors of math learning disabilities. J Learn Disabil 44(6):521–532, 2011
21177978
Torgersen T, Gjervan B, Lensing MB, Rasmussen K: Optimal management of
ADHD in older adults. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 12:79–87, 2016 26811680
Torrente F, Lischinsky A, Torralva T, et al: Not always hyperactive? Elevated
apathy scores in adolescents and adults with ADHD. J Atten Disord
15(7):545–556, 2011 20207850
Tripp G, Wickens JR: Research review: dopamine transfer deficit: a neurobiolog
ical theory of altered reinforcement mechanisms in ADHD. J Child Psychol
Psychiatry 49(7):691–704, 2008 18081766
Vaa T: ADHD and relative risk of accidents in road traffic: a meta-analysis. Ac
cid Anal Prev 62:415–425, 2014, 24238842
Van Ameringen M, Mancini C, Simpson W, et al: Adult attention deficit hyper
activity disorder in an anxiety disorders population. CNS Neurosci Ther
17(4):221–226, 2011 20406249
References 275
van der Meer JMJ, Oerlemans AM, van Steijn DJ, et al: Are autism spectrum dis
order and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder different manifestations
of one overarching disorder? Cognitive and symptom evidence from a clin
ical and population-based sample. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
51(11):1160.e3–1172.e3, 2012 23101742
van Emmerik-van Oortmerssen K, van de Glind G, van den Brink W, et al: Prev
alence of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in substance use disorder
patients: a meta-analysis and meta-regression analysis. Drug Alcohol De
pend 122(1–2):11–19, 2012 22209385
Vidal-Ribas P, Brotman MA, Valdivieso I, et al: The status of irritability in psychi
atry: a conceptual and quantitative review. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psy
chiatry 55(7):556–570, 2016 27343883
Villemagne VL, Wong DF, Yokoi F, et al: GBR12909 attenuates amphetamine
induced striatal dopamine release as measured by [(11)C]raclopride con
tinuous infusion PET scans. Synapse 33(4):268–273, 1999 10421707
Visser SN, Danielson ML, Bitsko RH, et al: Trends in the parent-report of health
care provider-diagnosed and medicated attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder: United States, 2003–2011. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
53(1):34.e2–46.e2, 2014 24342384
Volkmar FR, Chawarska K, Klin A: Autism spectrum disorders in infants and
toddlers: an introduction, in Autism Spectrum Disorders in Infants and
Toddlers: Diagnosis, Assessment, and Treatment. Edited by Chawarska K,
Klin A, Volkmar FR. New York, Guilford, 2008, pp 1–22
Volkmar FR, Klin A, McPartland JC: Asperger syndrome: an overview, in As
perger Syndrome: Assessing and Treating High-Functioning Autism Spec
trum Disorders, 2nd Edition. Edited by McPartland JC, Klin A, Volkmar
FR. New York, Guilford, 2014, pp 1–42
Volkow ND, Ding YS, Fowler JS, et al: Dopamine transporters decrease with
age. J Nucl Med 37(4):554–559, 1996 8691238
Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, et al: Evidence that methylphenidate enhances
the saliency of a mathematical task by increasing dopamine in the human
brain. Am J Psychiatry 161(7):1173–1180, 2004 15229048
Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ, et al: Methylphenidate decreased the amount
of glucose needed by the brain to perform a cognitive task. PLoS One
3(4):e2017, 2008 18414677
Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Kollins SH, et al: Evaluating dopamine reward pathway
in ADHD: clinical implications. JAMA 302(10):1084–1091, 2009 19738093
Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Newcorn JH, et al: Motivation deficit in ADHD is asso
ciated with dysfunction of the dopamine reward pathway. Mol Psychiatry
16(11):1147–1154, 2011 20856250
Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Tomasi D, et al: Methylphenidate-elicited dopamine in
creases in ventral striatum are associated with long-term symptom improve
ment in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Neurosci
32(3):841–849, 2012 22262882
Vullumier P: The role of the human amygdala in perception and attention, in
The Human Amygdala. Edited by Whalen PJ, Phelps EA. New York, Guil
ford, 2009, pp 220–249
Waber DP, Holmes JM: Assessing children’s copy productions of the Rey-Oster
rieth Complex Figure. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 7(3):264–280, 1985 3998091
276 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
Walcott CM, Landau S: The relation between disinhibition and emotion regula
tion in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Clin Child Ad
olesc Psychol 33(4):772–782, 2004 15498744
Wallace GL, Eisenberg IW, Robustelli B, et al: Longitudinal cortical develop
ment during adolescence and young adulthood in autism spectrum disor
der: increased cortical thinning but comparable surface area changes. J Am
Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 54(6):464–469, 2015 26004661
Wechsler D: Wechsler Memory Scale, 3rd Edition. San Antonio, TX, The Psycho
logical Corporation, 1997
Wechsler D: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 4th Edition. Bloomington, MN,
Pearson, 2008
Wechsler D: Wechsler Individual Achievement Test, 3rd Edition. Bloomington,
MN, Pearson, 2009
Wechsler D: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 5th Edition. Bloomington,
MN, Pearson, 2014
Wehman P, Smith MD, Schall C: Autism & the Transition to Adulthood: Success
Beyond the Classroom. Baltimore, MD, Paul H Brooks, 2009
Wilens TE, Hammerness PG: Straight Talk About Psychiatric Medications for
Kids, 4th Edition. New York, Guilford, 2016
Wilens TE, Morrison NR: Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and the sub
stance use disorders in ADHD, in Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder
in Adults and Children. Edited by Adler LA, Spencer TJ, Wilens TE. Cam
bridge, UK, Cambridge University Press, 2015, pp 111–122
Wilens TE, Adler LA, Adams J, et al: Misuse and diversion of stimulants pre
scribed for ADHD: a systematic review of the literature. J Am Acad Child
Adolesc Psychiatry 47(1):21–31, 2008 18174822
Wilens TE, Martelon M, Joshi G, et al: Does ADHD predict substance-use disor
ders? A 10-year follow-up study of young adults with ADHD. J Am Acad
Child Adolesc Psychiatry 50(6):543–553, 2011 21621138
Wilens TE, Robertson B, Sikirica V, et al: A randomized, placebo-controlled trial
of guanfacine extended release in adolescents with attention-deficit/hy
peractivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 52(11):916–925,
2015 26506582
Willer J: Could It Be Adult ADHD? A Clinician’s Guide to Recognition, Assess
ment, and Treatment. New York, Oxford University Press 2017
Wong CG, Stevens MC: The effects of stimulant medication on working mem
ory functional connectivity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Biol Psychiatry 71(5):458–466, 2012 22209640
Woodcock-Johnson: Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Ability, 4th Edition.
Boston, MA, Houghton Mifflin Harcourt, 2016a
Woodcock-Johnson: Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Achievement, 4th Edition. Bos
ton, MA, Houghton Mifflin Harcourt, 2016b
Yoshimasu K, Barbaresi WJ, Colligan RC, et al: Written-language disorder among
children with and without ADHD in a population-based birth cohort. Pedi
atrics 128(3):e605–e612, 2011 21859915
Yoshimasu K, Barbaresi WJ, Colligan RC, et al: Childhood ADHD is strongly as
sociated with a broad range of psychiatric disorders during adolescence: a
population-based birth cohort study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 53(10):1036–
1043, 2012 22647074
References 277
Young KD, Siegle GJ, Bodurka J, et al: Amygdala activity during autobiograph
ical memory recall in depressed and vulnerable individuals: association
with symptom severity and autobiographical overgenerality. Am J Psychi
atry 173(1):78–89, 2016 26541813
Zeigler Dendy CA: Teenagers With ADD, ADHD & Executive Function Deficits:
A Guide for Parents and Professionals, Third Edition. Bethesda, MD,
Woodbine House, in press
Zeigler Dendy CA, Zeigler A: A Bird’s-Eye View of Life With ADHD and
EFD...Ten Years Later: Advice From Young Survivors, 3rd Edition. Cedar
Bluff, AL, Cherish the Children, 2010
Index
ing disorders
The ADHD Explosion, xxvi–xxvii
of adolescents, 51
The ADHD Guide to Career Success:
50
Your Challenges, 221
tions
adaptive skills usually expected
tings, 219–221
ADHD medications for, 201
3, 7, 78, 173–174
attachment figures of, 68
Activity level, 22. See also Hyperac autism spectrum disorder in, 124,
tivity
254–255
ders
13-year-old who cuts herself
279
impairments, 45–60
Amphetamines, 87, 174, 181, 189,
adolescents, 50–53
198, 246
adults, 56–60
dosing of, 175–176, 183
47, 49–50, 80
173
181, 246
child who has outbursts of rage
245–247
of parents, 70, 154, 161
Aging
of preschoolers, 49
and, xx
Antidepressants, 167, 192, 207, 237
Agoraphobia, 109
mania induced by, 242
by adolescents, 52
Antipsychotics, atypical, 242, 246
nancy, 42
elevated risk for, 102, 107, 137
as self-medication, 91–92
and, 109–110
282 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
235
146, 209, 213–214
256
report cards or school or col
Apathy, 51
lege transcripts, 146, 214
Apgar scores, 42
scale for autism spectrum
253–255, 256
Attachment theory, 68
253–255, 256
Adults, 224
ory, 139–140
Attention magazine, 224
132
also Focusing attention
148–149
Aull E, 254
Index 283
and, 125–127
suicide and, 113, 114, 242
Axons, 86
Body dysmorphic disorder, 119
Bowlby J, 68
Baron-Cohen S, 125
amygdala, 39, 63–64, 83, 113, 173
214
default mode network of, 85–86,
147, 214
and, 62–63, 173
in schools, 216
System 1 and System 2 of, 8–9
252–253
working memory as a network
Mania
cortical thickening, 81–83
114
ADHD, xxvi, 3, 4–5, 81–88, 99
84–86
Children and Adults with Attention
environmental influences on, 99
Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder
genes and, 81
(CHADD), 141, 223
195
Circles inside squares graphic orga
in persons with remitting vs. per nizer, 144, 205
36–37, 82
empathy of, xxvi, 153, 211, 252
45–46
training for ADHD diagnosis, xx,
214
Clonidine, 174, 180, 181, 245
103, 137
147
disorders, 117–121, 247–249
specificity of motivation
249–253
Index 285
227, 231
prevalence of, 116
academic, 220
Conners’ ADHD Rating Scales, 138,
Coghill D, 188
Conners’ Comprehensive Behavior
program, 229
Conners’ CPT-II, 148
254, 255
Cortese S, 244
Edition, 225
Creativity, xxiv, 39
function, 225
child’s complaints about,
Compensatory strategies, 98
self-criticism, 166, 167
Inventory, 214
Cultural knowledge, 51
228–229, 230
Damasio A, 65–66, 68
Dating relationships, 52, 55, 57, 169, substance use disorders and, 121
223. See also Partner relation suicide and, 110, 111, 237
ships treatment-resistant, 188
22-year-old in conflict with his working memory problems and,
girlfriend and her reaction to 112–113
his ADHD, 164–165 Developmental delay, 13, 25, 46
couple therapy, 225–226 Dexedrine; Dexedrine Spansule
Dawson P, 208 (dextroamphetamine), 87, 175,
Daytrana (methylphenidate), 174, 176
178 Diagnosis of ADHD, xvi, xix
Deadlines and time management, 6, arguments about overdiagnosis,
16, 23, 34, 40, 47, 66, 73–74, 120 xxvi
Death, xxii, 59 assessment for, xix, 129–149
Delay discounting, 74 inadequacy of DSM-5 diagnostic
Dementia, xx, 13, 93, 94 criteria, xxi
Dendrites, 86 increased impairment due to
Depressive disorders, xix, xxii, 4, 27, inadequacy of, 94
29, 59, 91, 94, 98, 110–113, 126, professional training for, xx, xxvii
151, 165, 166, 167, 188, 233, 234, related to severity of impairments,
237, 256 4
ADHD and, 110–111, 137
rising rates of, xxvi–xxvii, 2–3
genetic studies of, 113
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
treatment of, 237
Mental Disorders (previous edi
antidepressants for, 242 tions)
asking patient about, 132, 134 ADHD relation to bipolar disor
bipolar disorder and, 114 der, 113
college support services for, 220 age at onset criterion for ADHD, 30
counseling/psychotherapy for, introduction of term “attention
211, 222 deficit,” 29–30
elevated risk for, 102, 110, 137 persistence of ADHD symptoms
with emotional dysregulation, meeting DSM-IV diagnostic
111–112, 240, 242 criteria, 35
rebound moodiness when Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
medication is wearing off, Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition
241 (DSM-5)
executive function impairments ADHD diagnostic criteria in, xxi,
and, 111–113 xxviii
major depressive disorder, 111 for adults, xxi, xxiii
obsessive-compulsive disorder age at onset criterion, 31, 46
and, 248 hyperactivity criterion, xvi,
oppositional defiant disorder xxiii, 4, 30
and, 116 lack of emotional problems cri
of parents, 42, 154 terion, 187–188
persistent depressive disorder limitations and suggestions for
(dysthymia), 110–111, 237 improvement of, xxviii
Index 287
noninclusion of executive
effects of ADHD medications on,
xxiii
estrogen-modulated release of,
patient/parent, 144
motivation, 75, 78
247
focusing attention for, xvi, 16–17
115
Drug addiction. See Substance use
specific learning disorder in,
disorders
103–104
DSM. See Diagnostic and Statistical
Dietary treatments, 228, 230
Manual of Mental Disorders
Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), 84
DTI (diffusion tensor imaging), 84
Diphenhydramine, 245
Dyscalculia (mathematics disorder),
regulation
Distractibility, 17, 40, 41, 126, 130. See
Eating and nutrition, 47, 77, 131
driving and, 52
189
of preschool children, 48
depression and, 111
165, 167
Education about ADHD for patient/
Dodge K, 63
family, 132, 141–144, 203–206
ity of, 93
additional supports for, 229–230
288 OUTSIDE THE BOX: RETHINKING ADD/ADHD IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS
ation, 133
215–218
case examples of
skills usually expected of adoles
7-year-old who feels picked on
cents, 51
by adults, 152–153
skills usually expected of elemen
11-year-old who “goes on
tary school children, 49–50
write, 155–156
215–216, 217, 254, 255
mates, 156–157
213, 217, 219, 239
semester of college,
high school, 161–162
162–164
transition to employment from,
212–219, 231
school or college transcripts,
53–54
what to tell the school about
on, 122–123
Electrochemical communication in
217–218, 219
explaining to patient/family,
21–22
173
53–54
80, 159
200
case examples of
Index 289
213
Aggressive behavior; Irritability
Emotion(s), 151–152
ADHD medications and, 240–241
regulation, xviii, 5
conduct disorder and, 116
64–66, 72
depression and, 111–112, 240, 241,
ents disagree about his treat of clinician, xxvi, 153, 211, 252
Employment (continued)
activation: organizing, prioritiz
difficulties with, xxi, xxii, xxiii, 2,
ing, and activating to work,
3, 6, 29, 35, 36
14–15, 45
education/training for, 53
age at onset of, xvii, 5, 12, 30–34, 46
nancy complications, 42
anxiety disorders and, 109–110
Erhardt D, 145
125–127
Essentials of ADHD Assessment for bipolar disorders and, 113–114
Children and Adolescents, 145
as characteristics rather than
Estrogen level, xx, 37–38, 94, 167
cause of ADHD, 12
119, 247
228–229, 230
Executive function(s)
depressive disorders and, 111–113
8–10
effort: regulating alertness, sus
brain development supporting,
taining effort, and adjusting
intelligence and, 41
attention when needed,
persistence of, xx
gender and, 37
of, 13–14, 46
95–99, 212
(EFDD), xxiii, 11
10, 61, 75
Index 291
94, 166–167
6, 13, 15, 16–17, 24, 25, 45, 61, 79,
and, 116–117
for driving, xvi, 16–17
161, 173–174
marijuana effects on, 194
104–107
and, 120
122–124
situational specificity of, xvii, xxii,
Exercise, 7, 51
106, 149, 161, 173–174
with ADHD
Fodor J, 41
132
Freud S, 65
141–144, 203–206
169
factors associated with psychiat of adolescents, 50, 52, 160, 243, 255
of origin, 134
death of a friend, 44, 60
54–55, 200
peer rejection, 50, 90, 156–157,
fMRI. See Functional magnetic reso substance use disorders and, 98,
nance imaging
121, 124
Friendships (continued)
executive function impairments
220
in persistence of impairments, 35
168–169
environmental factors and, 41–42,
driving and, 52
142, 210
obsessive-compulsive disorder
113
and, 120
linking ADHD and substance use
of preschool children, 49
twin studies, 4, 33, 79–80
of teachers, 43
Graphic organizer, 144, 205
241
GRE exam, 133
125
Guanfacine, 174, 180, 181, 189, 241,
of functional connectedness in
246
brain, 84–86
Guare R, 208
of memory impairments in
Guidance for parents, 206–212
depression, 112–113
tioning, 88
247
Gardner H, 41
Harm reduction approach to sub
Gender differences, 37–38
stance use disorders, 252
Index 293
Impulsivity (continued)
rebound moodiness and, 241
obsessive-compulsive disorder
115, 245, 246
and, 120
of preschool children, 12
and, 116
190, 191, 192
in sexual relationships, 52
Is It You, Me, or Adult A.D.D.? Stop
stimulant medications for allevia ping the Roller Coaster When
temperament and, 38
IVA-2 (Integrated Visual and Audi
Individualized education plan (IEP),
tory 2), 148
217–218, 219
218
Kessler RC, 137
problems
Klass P, 146
(IVA-2), 148
Larson K, 137
254, 255
Latham PH, 221
Irritability, 240
Learning disorders, xxii, xxiii, 3, 6,
on awakening, 184
102, 103–107, 200. See also Math
109
skills
medications for
treatment of, 238–240
Index 295
executive function impairments Marijuana use, xxii, 91, 97, 123, 249,
215–218, 240
and, 194
2006, 215
Marriage in an Age of Interrup
Individuals with Disabilities Edu tion, 226
221
mathematics disorder (dyscalcu
Leisure-time activities, 133
lia), 104, 107, 215
Levy S, 249
treatment of ADHD with, 238,
Lezak M, 13
239
Lithium, 246
MCAT exam, 133, 219
establishment of a home, 57
1–2, 10
Love relationships, 35
197–200
ers, 218
87–88, 143, 171, 191, 201
disorders
“crashing” when medication is
antidepressant-induced, 242
wearing off, 190, 241
195–196
methylphenidate, 174, 176–179
187, 198
for, 189–190
172–174
(methylphenidate), 177
185–186
mechanism of action of, 87, 88, 173
regulation, 241–242
147, 239
ders, 240
bipolar disorders, 113–115
disorders, 253
elevated risk for, 103, 137
Index 297
tinued)
(ODD), 115–117
225–226
treatment of, 245–247
203–206
and, 116–117
adults, 221–224
Organizational problems and skills,
Obsessive-compulsive disorder
employment and, 97
117–121, 247–248
in obsessive-compulsive disorder,
pedestrian, 118–119
writing tasks, 18, 106
and, 120–121
with agoraphobia, 109
Obstetrical factors, 42
Parent to Parent Program of CHADD,
order
57–58, 154, 210
ODD. See Oppositional defiant dis The Parent’s Guide to the Medical
order
World of Autism: A Physician
182–183
Parents of child with ADHD. See also
Index 299
132
Philipsen A, 244
family, 211
P.L. 93-112 (Rehabilitation Act of
255
of preschoolers, 48
Pera G, 226
sports, 6, 50, 67, 133, 168, 186,
249
video games, 6, 15, 40, 90, 153,
248
Population studies, xxii, 59
symptoms
PPT (planning and placement team)
36–37, 82
Pregnancy, maternal substance
53
adaptive skills usually expected
gender and, 35
of, 13, 48–49, 60, 89
Personality traits, 38
collaboration with school, 212
144–145
146–147, 215, 238–239
116, 246
for ADHD with autism spectrum
200
224–225
147
supportive, for adults with
Preterm birth, 42
by parents, 42, 160–161, 211
worldwide, xxi
Quality of life, 3, 168, 233
obsessive-compulsive disorder
Rating scales for ADHD, xix, 112,
and, 120–121
130, 132, 138, 139, 144, 147
xxvii
treatment of ADHD with, 238–240
Index 301
221
Self-discipline, 160, 161
212, 255
Self-image, 133–134, 153
Persistence of impairments
Severity of impairments, 4, 28–29
85
Shame, xxvi, 43, 73, 91, 151, 156, 159,
in depression, 113
165, 166, 221
116
about obsessive-compulsive dis
Report cards, 146, 214
order, 248
Representational models, 68
Shaw P, 111
129, 148
Shyness, 38, 109, 133, 163
Rief S, 218
Siblings of child with ADHD, 12, 46,
Ritalin; Ritalin LA (methylpheni 90, 92, 96, 134, 142, 166, 211, 220
Rollnick S, 253
xvii, xxii, xxiv, 6–8, 61, 74–78,
Rutter M, 42
reading and, 106
Scheffler R, xxvi–xxvii, 2
119, 247
Scientific evidence for ADHD diag xv, 4, 15, 17, 24, 41, 45, 242–244
218, 221
242–245, 256
242
college support services for, 220
der, 248
excessive daytime sleepiness, 17,
Self-control, 3, 115
244–245
109
See also Learning disorders
of preschool children, 12
217, 219, 239
106
Stimulant medications, 174, 181
with, 242–245
alcohol use and, 193–194, 253
234, 243–244
175–176
Sleep diary, 245
assessing impact on anxiety and
224
duration of treatment with, 195–196
Social anxiety, 108, 125, 163, 236, 256
effectiveness of, 191, 207
43, 56, 92, 124. See also Friend for generic formulations,
ships
192–193
autism spectrum disorder and,
loss of, 191–192
125, 126, 254–255
magnitude of treatment
older memories and shaping of
effects, 229, 230
212, 255
187, 198
123–124
186–187
supportive, 95–96
fitting to patient’s schedule,
Edition, 147–148
mechanism of action of, 75, 87–88,
Solanto M, 225
172–174
Solden S, 224
methylphenidate preparations,
Index 303
regulation, 241–242
Supportive psychotherapy for
in patients with obsessive adults, 221–224
compulsive disorder, 248
Supportive relationships, 95–96
in patients with oppositionality,
Sustaining attention and effort, xv, 4,
246
6, 13, 15, 16–18, 24, 25, 43, 45. See
disorders, 253
Synaptic transmission, 74, 75, 86–87,
207
System 1 and System 2 of brain, 8–9
189, 241
Szigethy E, 236
and, 122–124
136, 241
during pregnancy, 42
Test taking
Attention), 148
146
Understanding Girls With ADHD:
Trazodone, 245
How They Feel and Why They Do
Treatment of ADHD
What They Do, 2nd Edition, 208
effective, 98–99
Understood (online resource), 230
inadequacy of, 94
Examination, 219
229, 230
Vanderbilt ADHD Diagnostic Parent
medications, 171–201
Rating Scale, 214
nonmedication interventions,
Variability of ADHD impairments, 5,
203–231
7, 27–44
parental disagreement about, 43,
age at symptom onset, 30–34
157–158
cognitive abilities and, 39–41
in patients with co-occurring dis environmental challenges and
orders, 233–256 supports, 41–44
anxiety, 234–237 gender and, 37–38
Asperger syndrome/autism how ADHD sometimes becomes
spectrum disorder, 253–256
more problematic, 88–94, 99,
depression, 237
212
242–245
95–99, 212
249–253
treatment, 98–99
Index 305
catches up, 95
for Children—Fifth Edition),
compensatory strategies, 98
147, 238, 239
impulsiveness, 29–30, 95
Test-III, 147
161, 173–174
as aspect of attention system, 103
185, 195
marijuana effects on, 194
238
motivation, 76–77
147, 238
xviii, 4, 6, 15, 20–22, 24, 45, 61,
140
computer training programs for,
Wehman P, 256
228–229, 230
147, 238
156, 238, 239
Willer J, 146
oppositional defiant disorder
61, 75
substance use disorders and, 122
A PR ACTICAL GUIDE
Russell A. Barkley, Ph.D., Clinical Professor of
Psychiatry, Medical University of South Carolina