1) A series of design-based research studies were conducted on an online discussion tool called SpeakEasy to form, apply, refine, and test a theory of socially relevant representations for learning.
2) The initial goal was to create a tool that allowed collaborative knowledge building and taking advantage of multimedia in a more intuitive interface than existing tools.
3) Early designs drew on examples of video bulletin boards and CSILE, emphasizing scaffolding, topical discussions, and semantic labels to support productive discourse. Studies of these early designs helped develop and refine the theory of socially relevant representations.
1) A series of design-based research studies were conducted on an online discussion tool called SpeakEasy to form, apply, refine, and test a theory of socially relevant representations for learning.
2) The initial goal was to create a tool that allowed collaborative knowledge building and taking advantage of multimedia in a more intuitive interface than existing tools.
3) Early designs drew on examples of video bulletin boards and CSILE, emphasizing scaffolding, topical discussions, and semantic labels to support productive discourse. Studies of these early designs helped develop and refine the theory of socially relevant representations.
1) A series of design-based research studies were conducted on an online discussion tool called SpeakEasy to form, apply, refine, and test a theory of socially relevant representations for learning.
2) The initial goal was to create a tool that allowed collaborative knowledge building and taking advantage of multimedia in a more intuitive interface than existing tools.
3) Early designs drew on examples of video bulletin boards and CSILE, emphasizing scaffolding, topical discussions, and semantic labels to support productive discourse. Studies of these early designs helped develop and refine the theory of socially relevant representations.
1) A series of design-based research studies were conducted on an online discussion tool called SpeakEasy to form, apply, refine, and test a theory of socially relevant representations for learning.
2) The initial goal was to create a tool that allowed collaborative knowledge building and taking advantage of multimedia in a more intuitive interface than existing tools.
3) Early designs drew on examples of video bulletin boards and CSILE, emphasizing scaffolding, topical discussions, and semantic labels to support productive discourse. Studies of these early designs helped develop and refine the theory of socially relevant representations.
:his story i s that the design-based research paradigm aermitted not only evaluative research and hypothesis Research Methods and formation to take place, but also allowed careful refinement and testing of a new psychological theory, Theory Building: the theory of socially relevant representations (SRRs)for learning (Hoadley, 1999a; Hoadley & Kirby, 2004). A Case Study of In the sections below, I describe some of the initial design goals, early designs of the predecessor to the Research with SpeakEasy software and associated educational activities, and the theories used to construct these SpeakEasy designs.* Then, I describe h o w early studies led to development and refinement of the theory of SRRs. Finally, I discuss how later DBR studies allowed testing of the theory and delineate some of the implications of Christo her M. Hoadley 7 The Pennsy vania State University this research trajectory for DBRM in general.
SpeakEasy and Its Predecessor, the
Multimedia Forum Kiosk In 1992, the Web was still an idea more than a Design-based research methods are an emerging reality. Multimedia was novel and expanding onto the research paradigm that blends empirical research with computing scene. Educational CD-ROMs were design and implementation. This article discusses how becoming more important, and video online was no a design-based research trajectory can help not only longer restricted to specialized videodisc or high-end suggest and refine theories, but also permit their falsification. In a series of design-based research studies workstations for the first time. Though the Web was on an online discussion tool (called SpeakEasy), the basically unknown, the Internet was not; e-mail and author shows how a theory of socially relevant Usenet newsgroups were beginning to ascend i n representations for learning was formed, applied, popularity not only with academic computer scientists, refined, and ultimately tested. but also with college students, businesspeople, and home users through services like Prodigy and AOL. At ,As described in the introductory article in this special that time, Sherry Hsi, Christina Schwarz, and I began issue, design-based research methods (DBRM) i n work on an online multimedia discussion tool called education are an exciting, productive set of research the Multimedia Forum Kiosk. We had a simple core methods that blend a desion stance with empirical design goal: To create a tool that would allow research in context to advance our theoretical collaborative knowledge building while taking knowledge of learning, while simultaneously producing advantage of multimedia. We wanted something that valued educational outcomes for learners in real would be "better than Netnews" (our unofficial mantra) settings 'Design-Based Research Collective, 2003; by supporting higher quality, more coherent discourse Kelly, 2003). In this article, 1 describe the development through a more intuitive graphical user interface. We of an online discussion tool for learning, called also identified as one of our use-case scenarios the SpeakEasy {Hoadlev, Hsi, & Berman, 1995a), and collaborative qualitative analysis or discussion of video discuss how a long trajectory of design-based research data. Implicitly, our dissatisfaction w i t h Usenet not only allowed theory to inform design, but also newsgroups hinted at a tacit goal we shared: building a enabled clesiqn to inform psychological theory system that w o u l d promote greater social connectedness rather than disconnection. Our initial system, built i n Hypercard, drew on some examples we were familiar with, namely a video bulletin board that Sherry had helped develop at the Apple Multimedia Lab (for a much later write-up of this design's evolution, see Bellamy, Woolsey, & Kerns, Christopher M. Hoadley designs, builds, and studies ways for technolop to enhance collaboration and learning. He is 1995), and Scardamalia and Bereiter's CSILE (Com- currently Assistant Proiessor of Instructional Systems and of Information Sciences and Technology at Penn State University. He was previously president of the International *More information about the design of the SpeakEasy and its Society for the Learning Sciences. He co-rounded and leads predecessor, the Multimedia Forum Kiosk, is available the Spencer-tunded Design-Based Research Collective (e- elsewhere (Hoadley, 1999a, 2002, in press; Hoadley & Hsi, mail: EdTechDBRCbS'toplie.~iet). 1993; Hoadley et a/., 1995a).
42 EDUCATIONAL TECHNOLOGY/january-February 2005
puter-Supported Intentional Learning Environments) two more theories were significant influences. We software (Scardamalia, Bereiter, McLean, Swallow, & came across work on small-group interaction that Woodruff, 1989). We admired the Apple project's use framed the types of discursive moves typically taken in of the brand-new QuickTime multimedia technology, collaboration; this supported and extended our although it depended on an expensive, specialized semantic labels (Bales, 1969). We also followed Pea's hardware setup to allow video capture to make more specific theories of the mechanisms for learning postings. We also were influenced by Scardamalia and through collaborative discussion (Pea, 1993a, 19930). Bereiter's use of scaffolding to improve the quality of This theory helped us focus on process support, instead discussion in their tool. Taking both strategies to heart, of merely focusing on usability of the system; we we created a kiosk-based bulletin board system. Unlike emphasized developing social supports that worked in Netnews, which was subject-based, our tool was conjunction with the software to produce certain kinds topically based, with all participation centered on a of interaction that constituted "productive discourse." question or discussion point posed by a topic author. As is typical, these theories did not completely The Multimedia Forum Kiosk system had two main specify what types of design decisions we should interfaces. The opinion area provided a welcome to the undertake, either in the interface or in the surrounding topic; and, in addition to presenting the topic author's activities. Should we permit anonymity? What types of motivating question, it supplied a representation of all group size would best help learning? What types of of the participants in that topic's discussion and their questions, topics, or adult moderation might best points of view (revisable over time, but only one engender "productive discussion"? None of these opinion per person), showing a community gestalt. The questions could be answered from first principles with discussion area was more similar to what we now are the theories at hand, but we drew on our own and accustomed to as threaded discussion. It provided a expert intuitions (including expert designers at Apple, chance for people to post comments and respond to Tal igent, Interval Research, and elsewhere) for one another, with a graphical tree structure (rather than guidance and used iterative refinement to develop a outline-based view, this was truly tree-like). Similar to workable educational interventions using prototypes of CSILEfs "think types," each comment subtitled with a the tool. semantic label (such as "and," "but," "or," "?" and so At this point, we used both formative evaluation and on) that identified how the comment related to prior iterative refinement just to get something working in ones. practice. We tried our tool in a wide variety of In our system, we used multimedia in various contexts, including graduate courses in engineering, an innovative ways. Topics could include short video clips informal lounge in an education department, science as a conversation starter. Topics, opinions, and museums, a self-paced study center, and (in what comments were individually authored; and picture would eventually become our primary research icons were used to highlight this authorship. Two context) a middle school physics classroom. In this design decisions that were byproducts of our context, we ended up supporting an inquiry-based technological constraints were the fact that the system science curriculum through students' discussing topics was a same-place, different-time system (due to limited that asked them to explain science phenomena, networks for sharing large files); and that users were presented through multimedia (Bell, Davis, & Linn, permitted to log in anonymously. Because of the photo 1995; Hoadley & Bell, 1996). We eventually arrived at icons and the relative difficulty of obtaining and a very stable set of activities based on this particular digitizing images of users, the kiosk had some guest learning context. accounts to allow use of the system by people who had Some of our early findings centered on the not been previously photographed and set up with importance of the context of use. We began with basic accounts. usability studies-our software fit the HCI "ten minute rule" (meaning that novices could walk up and use the system without any training, uncovering all its features Designing from Theory and in ten minutes or less). But usability is only one Our Early Evaluation Work component of the success of such a system, and we Initially our designs were based upon theories of began to understand how important the activities and collaboration and learning. Our emphasis on context of use were for us. For example, when we used collaboration came from Vygotskian notions of co- the system with engineering students to help assess construction and collaboration (Newman, Griffin, & learning climate issues, we discovered through Cole, 1989; Wertsch, 1979, 1985), but more comparison of different courses that physical access to specifically we used ideas about knowledge-building the system was important, but more important was the communities (Scardamalia & Bereiter, 1991, 1993) and degree to which the course instructor showed evidence communities of practice (Lave & Wenger, 1991) to of incorporating feedback provided by students. One guide our vision of successful use of the system. Later, particularly interesting example involved a non-native
EDUCATIONAL TECHNOLOGYfjanuary-February 2005
speaker of English; this student reported that the MFK description). We also used a variety of informal data made a big difference in his own learning climate, sources, such as our own experiences in the context as because-unlike in face-to-face interaction in the participant observers or the input of expert designers, class-he could compose and edit contributions to and used these to help guide not only the design, but class discussions on his own time (Hsi & Hoadley, also the more formal research activities such as 1995). We later demonstrated with the middle-school treatment design and data collection. Constructing science students that identity, gender, anonymity, and theories, design hypotheses, and sometimes more online participation played an important role not only fragile understandings of how our treatments played out in students' participation in the discussion, but also in in context were the primary goals. Falsification was an their learning (Hsi & Hoadley, 1997). important part of our work; in some cases, we were As the next stage of our design-based research and able to falsify null hypotheses, and in many others, we building on this work, prior to the introduction of the were able to falsify incomplete or incorrect under- first Netscape browser, we developed the second ever standings of the learning taking place. Web-based threaded discussion tool,* called SpeakEasy Through our design work, we stumbled on an area (Hoadley eta/., 1995a). SpeakEasy retained most of the that appeared to be central to our success-how Multimedia Forum Kiosk interface; but, because it was students construed the social space they were entering Web-based, this application no longer depended on when they participated in online discussion. Intuitively, limited access to a single kiosk (different-place, we felt that there was a great deal of social different-time communication, instead of same-place, connectedness and motivation wrapped up in our tool, different-time communication). Our tree-based layout in part due to the nature of the representations in the was a casualty of the limits of HTML and was replaced interface. We began to develop a theory of how social by the indented outline-style interface. The opinion cues, such as the face icons and our semantic labels, area and discussion area remained. might be influencing student cognition and might -- facilitate sensemaking and learning (Hoadley, 1998; Hoadley & Hsi, 1996; Hoadley, Hsi, & Berman, 1995b; Building a Theory of Hsi & Hoadley, 1994). As a design strategy, we began Socially Relevant Representations explicitly manipulating and studying various social How did we use this design-based research to representations i n the interface, such as identity (as investigate theory? While there is debate about what conveyed through the face icons) (Hsi, 1997; Hsi & precisely are the characteristics of design-based Hoadley, 1997). We also began hypothesizing research, I believe our example to be relatively mechanisms by which social cues might aid learning. prototypical. Our work involved a theory-driven, One method we used to aid in development of this iterative design of an artifact and associated activities theory was to look longitudinally over our design that constituted a learning environment over a long trajectory. As soon as we began to suspect that social period of time (eight years, to be exact). We tested our cues were facilitating participation and learning, we intervention in a variety of learning contexts; each began to manipulate our design accordingly. In some iteration was implemented and evaluated in some cases, this took the form of following success; we tried authentic learning setting. Our design work was in embedding additional social cues in our SpeakEasy some cases connected to intuitive or informal interventions, such as linking to descriptive personal knowledge, but more often was the result of embedded homepages, adding short usernames to the comment investigations over the course of the development icons, and moving some of what had been placed in trajectory. We altered our intervention on a daily basis seed comments into introductory videos of actors if needed to support its success in context, but we also voicing those seed comments. created planned comparisons between successive Embedded within our design-based research versions of the intervention, or between versions trajectory was a chance to run traditional treatment- administered simultaneously to different populations in control studies, in which some participants received a similar or the same setting. Our data collection was one version of the intervention and others received principled, but wide ranging; we collected some data another. We could sometimes compare an improved to help orient us to the phenomena at hand, but also version against baseline data (one semester of students collected targeted data related to the planned compared against another), but in addition we could comparisons (experiments) embedded in the work, and randomly assign students to conditions, some of which gathered still other data to help elaborate or explore were designed to gather information by introducing poorly understood aspects of the phenomena (rich changes we thought might be worse, to test our budding theories. For instance, we compared students in discussions who were forced to make all comments *We were beaten to the punch by Hypernews by a few anonymously with students who were forced to identify months. themselves on all comments.
44 EDUCATIONAL TECHNOLOGY/January-February 2005
More than simply analyzing the dependent variables were able to determine that some students in these cases, we collected rich descriptive data about approximately half) use social cues i n navigating how the students participated with the system and used SpeakEasy, that some (but not all) students encode and these to further inform our theory. By trying out various ,emember these cues in their understanding of the topic representations, collecting data from not only af discussion, and that on average providing students participants but from the literature on interfaces, and with social cues led to better learning outcomes in iterating, we improved our understanding of how a SpeakEasy discussions (Hoadley, 1999a, 1999b; particular treatment corresponded to a theoretical Hoadley & Linn, 2000). entity. Elsewhere (Hoadley, 2002) I describe how our notion of anonymity changed as a result of this kind of Theory Building and Testing iteration-initially we saw anonymity as evidence of Our design-based research methods helped us build social inclusiveness because it improved participants' theory in four ways: induction, orientation, constraints, perceptions of social safety, but later we realized that and manipulation. social safety was independent of the use of the anonymity function and that in fact this feature Induction. Induction i s perhaps the most obvious threatened inclusivity. form of research, yet i s often overlooked. We collected Sometimes design-based research gave us a chance a great deal of data on the use of our tool in various to refine the ways we operationalized our theory, contexts. While some have argued that over-collection helping to increase the alignment between our of data may be seen as a weakness of design-based theoretical notions (in this case, "representations of research (Dede, 2004), i t also permits hypothesis social cues in interfaces") and how those notions were generation from a large body of knowledge and is embodied i n the research setting and their effects especially effective when deduction from known first measured. Sandoval has termed this type of exploration principles is not possible. Additionally, because the the testing of "embodied hypotheses" (Sandoval, 2002, design-based researcher has intimate knowledge of the in press) We had a lot of exploring to do in terms of research setting, the researcher can draw educated what constituted a more or less socially relevant guesses about which explanations for the observed representation. In one very promising early study, we phenomena might be most parsimonious. It is difficult compared the standard version of SpeakEasy that was to apply "Occam's razor" from afar. designed to "feel like a conversation" with an interface that used similar screen layouts, but that was altered to Orientation. Learning in context i s a complex sys- feel more like posting to a database. We used visual tem, with multitudinous interacting factors. Which icons that were not faces to represent contributors, and matter most for success? This is an important design changed the discussion area from a discursive question that poses challenges for research. By focusing organization (semantic labels and a response-oriented on the design of the learning environment and the thread structure) to a topical organization (labels such improvement of that design over time, the design-based as "pro" and "con" and a conceptually oriented thread research process is forced to converge quickly on what structure). Our results were somewhat surprising; works through refinement and hill-climbing within a students participated equivalently in both formats (same design space. The net result is to help orient the number of comments, same comment quality), but had researcher quickly to those factors that are most design- different outcomes. On the positive side, more students relevant in a given context with a given set of tacit changed their ideas based on the activity in the socially design assumptions. relevant condition, but frequently they changed their ideas for the worse! By doing post hoc analysis of the Constraints. An infinite number of theories can be comments themselves, we were able to trace this back generated to explain any finite set of data. However, to some of the particular seed comments we provided. much as sampling may be used to test a model created This led to continued refinement of our notion of with one subset of data by attempting prediction on mechanisms for socially relevant representations (SRRs) another subset of the same data, design-based research to support learning and also led to changes in the allows theories to be falsified by systematic additional activity structure that were irrelevant to our ideas about analysis of existing data. Comprehensive data how SRRs worked, but that were necessary (but not collection in design-based research permits not only sufficient) for learning and conceptual progress. this type of model validation based on subsampling, The research trajectory culminated in a series of but also allows additional post hoc analysis in which studies that suggested that SRRs can enhance learning models, hypotheses, and in some cases theories can be outcomes, but indicated there are significant individual put up for falsification by making predictions about differences in how these representations are used. data already collected and then examining that data. Through a combination of interviews, surveys related to Since creation of rich, contextualized interventions is social orientation, and learning and recall tasks, we enormously expensive, and orientation to relevant
EDUCATIONAL TECHNOLOGY/January-February 2005
factors in these interventions requires intimacy with the more general theories of psycholoe;~or human- research setting, this type of post hoc analysis and/or machine interaction (Carroll & Rosson, 1992)], and the theory constraint through prior data can be invaluable context might all be incompletely understood, for testing ideas in an authentic way. Often, the choice faced by a researcher is whether it is more valuable to do messy research on big, Manipulation. Manipulation is the heart of experi- complicated questions using design-based research, or mentation. In traditional controlled laboratory whether i t i s more valuable to perform tightly experimentation, we manipulate the world in precisely controlled studies while limiting oneself to questions controlled ways to test our theories' ability to predict and interventions that can be tightly controlled. Or, put the outcomes. But what can one do when theory another way, "The contingency of artificial phenomena incompletely predicts 3n outcome, or when variables has always created doubts as to whether they tall cannot be controlled? Some w o u l d argue that properly within the compass of science. Sometimes inferencine must not take place in these circumstances, these doubts are directed at the ...difficulty of but Dewey would argue otherwise: "The conjunction of disentangling prescription from description. This seems problematic and determinate characters i n nature to me not to be the real difficulty. The genuine problem renders every existence, as well as every idea and is to show how empirical propositions can be made at human act, an experiment in fact, even though not in all about systems that, given different circumstances, design. To be intelligently experimental i s but to be might be quite other than they are" (Simon, 1969). conscious of this intersection of natural conditions so as Design-based research can be one way to build to profit by it instead of being at its mercy" (Dewey, theoretical, empirical propositions about learning with 1925). technology. 0 The very nature of design (as opposed to problem- solving or deduction) is reasoning under uncertainty, in underconstrained problem spaces, where the outcomes of actions cannot be fully specified. In design-based References research, w e can take advantage of this type of reasoning, by incrementally increasing our under- Bales, R. F. (1969). Personality and interpersonal behavior. standing of a particular designed intervention i n a New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston. particular context over time. We poke it, prod it, and Bell, P., Davis, E. A., & Linn, M. C. (1995). The knowledge integration environment: Theory and design. In S. continuously monitor the results. Much as calculus Goldman & J. Greeno (Eds.), Computer supported suggests approximating the change in a function's output by applying smaller and smaller changes to its collaborative learning '95 (pp. 14-2.1). Mahwah. NJ: . Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. input and taking the limit, we add incremental "deltas" Bellamy, R., Woolsey, K., & Kerns, C. (1995). D e s i g n to our interventions in design-based research and view experiments with media-rich messaging. Paper presented the results. This provides not only a means to get to a at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational particular outcome (i.e., formative evaluation), but also Research Association, San Francisco. provides a way to systematically explore the design Carroll, J. M., & Rosson, M. B. (1992). Getting around the space through hill climbing. As the relationship task-artifact cycle: How to make claims and design by between interventions and outputs becomes better scenario. Transactions o n Information Systems, 70(2), understood in one context, it provides the opportunity 181-212. Dede, C. (2004). If design-based research i s the answer, what to attempt generalization to other contexts. is the question? Journal of the Learning Sciences, 73(1), 105-1 14. Conclusion Design-Based Research Collective. (2003). Design-based Design-based research boils down to trying to research: An emerging paradigm for educational inquiry. understand the world by trying to change it. This article Educational Researcher, 32(1), 5-8. focuses on some of the theory-building aspects of one Dewey, J. (1925). Volume I: 1925. In J. A. Boydston (Ed.), trajectory of design-based research. Certainly, there are John Dewey, the later works (Electronic ed.). Carbondale, many ways to test theories, and many more ways to IL: Southern Illinois University Press. build or propose them. Design-based research i s only Hoadley, C. (1998). Shaping social interactions for one way to do these. It i s not recommended in knowledge integration through technology. In B. K. situations where other, less demanding methods (such Nichols, A. C. Kemp, & D. Jackson (Eds.), 71st NARSJ Annual Meeting (p. 166). San Diego: National Association as good guesswork and a simple experiment) will do. for Research in Science Teaching. However, DBR can be useful for creating and testing Hoadley, C. (1 999a). Scaffolding scientific discussion using theories in situations, such as complex educational socially relevant representations in networked multimedia. technology innovations, where little is fixed: the Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, University of California, interventions, the theories [both what Reigeluth terms Berkeley. instructional-design theories (Reigeluth, 1999) and Hoadley, C. (1999b). Social text: Learning in online peer
46 EDUCATIONAL TECHNOLOGY/january-February 2005
. , Processing Conference, Jackson Hole, Wyoming. zone: Working for cognitive change i n school. New York: Hoadley, C. (2002). Creating context: Design-based research Cambridge University Press. in creating and understanding CSCL. In C. Stahl (Ed.), "ea, R. D. (1993a). Learning scientific concepts through Computer support for collaborative learning 2002 (pp. material and social activities: Conversational analysis 453-462). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. meets conceptual change. Educational Psychologist, 28131, Hoadley, C. (in press). Methodological alignment in design- 265-277. based research. Educational Psychologist. Pea, R. D . (1993b). Seeing what we b u i l d together: Hoadley, C., & Bell, P. (1996, Sept.). Web for your head: The Distributed multimedia learning environments for design of digital resources to enhance lifelong learning. D- transtormative communications. journal o f the Learning Ub Magazine. Sciences, 3 ( 3 ) ,285-299. Hoadley, C., & Hsi, 5. (1993). A multimedia interface for Reigeluth, C. M. (1 999). What is instructional-design theory knowledge building and collaborative learning. In Adjunct and how is it changing? In C. M. Reioeluth iEd.i, Proceedings of the International Computer Human Instructional-design theories and models: A new paradign7 Interaction Conference (InterCHI) '93 (pp. 103-1 04). o f instructional theory (Vol. II, pp. 5-29!. ~Mahwah,Nj: Amsterdam, The Netherlands: ACM Press. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Hoadley, C., & Hsi, S. (1996, April). Towards a theory o f Sandoval, W. A. (2002, April). Learning from designs: collaborative networking i n the science classroon7. Paper Learning environments as embodied hypotheses. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting o f the American presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New York. Educational Research Association, New Orleans. Hoadley, C., Hsi, S., & Berman, B. P. (1995a). The Sandoval, W. A. (in press). Developing learning theory by Multimedia Forum Kiosk and SpeakEasy. i n P. Zeilweger refining conjectures embodied i n educational designs. (Ed.), Proceedings o f the Third ACM International Educational Psychologist. Conference on Multimedia (pp. 363-364). San Francisco: Scardamalia, M., & Bereiter, C. (1991). Higher levels of ACM Press. agency for children in knowledge building: A challenge Hoadley, C., Hsi, S., & Berman, B. P. (1995b, April). for the design of new knowledge media. Journal o f the N e t w o r k e d multimedia f o r c o m m u n i c a t i o n a n d Learning Sciences, 7 (1 ), 37-68. collaboration. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of Scardamalia, M., & Bereiter, C. (1993). Computer support tor the American Educational Research Association, San knowledge-building communities. Journal o f the Learning Francisco. Sciences, 3(3), 265-283. Hoadley, C., & Kirby, j. (2004). Socially relevant Scardamalia, M., Bereiter, C., McLean, R. S., Swallow, I., & representations in interfaces for learning. In Y. B. Katai, W. Woodruff, E. (1 989). Computer-supported intentional A. Sandoval, N. Envedy, A. Scott Nixon, & F. Herrera learning environments. journal o f Educational Computing (Eds.), International Conference o f the Learning Sciences Research, 6(1), 55-68. (ICLS) 2004 (pp. 262-269). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Simon, H . A. (1969). The sciences o r the artificial. Erlbaum Associates. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Hoadley, C., & Linn, M. C. (2000). Teaching science through Wertsch, I . (1985). Vvgotsky and the social formation of on-line, peer discussions: SpeakEasy i n the Knowledge mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Integration Environment. International Journal of Science Wertsch, J. V. (1979). From social interaction to higher Education, 22(8), 839-858. psychological processes: A clarification and application of Hsi, S. H . (1997). Facilitating knowledge integration in Vygotsky's theory. Human Development, 2211 ), 1-22. science through electronic discussion: The Multimedia Forum Kiosk. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, Berkeley. Hsi, S., & Hoadley, C. (1994, April). An interactive multimedia kiosk as a tool for collaborative discourse, reflection, and assessment. Paper presented at the Annual Features on Web Site Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans. Visitors to the Web Site maintained for this magazine Hsi, S., & Hoadley, C. (1995). Assessing curricular innovation will find the following features: in engineering: Using the Multimedia Forum Kiosk. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting o f the American See all of these features at: BooksToRead.com/etp Educational Research Association, San Francisco. Hsi, S., & Hoadley, C. (1997). Productive discussion in science: Gender equity through electronic discourse. Sample Articles. At least two recently published Journal o f Science Education and Technology, 1 0(1), 23- articles from this magazine are always available at 36. the site. Kelly, A. E. (2003). Research as design. Educational Contributing Editors. The complete list of our Researcher, 32(1), 3-5. regular contributors is available at the site. Lave, I., & Wenger, E. (1991 ). Situated learning: Legitimate Author Guidelines. Prospective authors of articles peripheral participation. New York: Cambridge University for the magazine are encouraged to read these Press. guidelines.