Autism - Oxidative Stress, Inflamation and Immune Abnormalities (Chauhan)
Autism - Oxidative Stress, Inflamation and Immune Abnormalities (Chauhan)
Autism - Oxidative Stress, Inflamation and Immune Abnormalities (Chauhan)
Autism
ǡƪǡ
KWWSERRNVPHGLFRVRUJ
Ǥ
This book contains information obtained from authentic and highly regarded sources. Reasonable efforts
have been made to publish reliable data and information, but the author and publisher cannot assume
responsibility for the validity of all materials or the consequences of their use. The authors and publishers
have attempted to trace the copyright holders of all material reproduced in this publication and apologize to
copyright holders if permission to publish in this form has not been obtained. If any copyright material has
not been acknowledged please write and let us know so we may rectify in any future reprint.
Except as permitted under U.S. Copyright Law, no part of this book may be reprinted, reproduced, transmit-
ted, or utilized in any form by any electronic, mechanical, or other means, now known or hereafter invented,
including photocopying, microfilming, and recording, or in any information storage or retrieval system,
without written permission from the publishers.
For permission to photocopy or use material electronically from this work, please access www.copyright.
com (http://www.copyright.com/) or contact the Copyright Clearance Center, Inc. (CCC), 222 Rosewood
Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, 978-750-8400. CCC is a not-for-profit organization that provides licenses and
registration for a variety of users. For organizations that have been granted a photocopy license by the CCC,
a separate system of payment has been arranged.
Trademark Notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks or registered trademarks, and are used
only for identification and explanation without intent to infringe.
RC553.A88A8726 2010
616.85’882--dc22 2009024353
vii
viii Contents
Chapter 8 Autism and Oxidative Stress: Evidence from an Animal Model ....... 131
Michelle A. Cheh, Alycia K. Halladay, Carrie L. Yochum,
Kenneth R. Reuhl, Marianne Polunas, Xue Ming,
and George C. Wagner
Chapter 17 Autism, Teratogenic Alleles, HLA-DR4, and Immune Function ..... 325
William G. Johnson, Steven Buyske, Edward S. Stenroos,
and George H. Lambert
xi
xii Preface
In Chapter 6, Maria Dronca and Sergiu Paşca present evidence for the involvement
of paraoxonase 1 (PON1) in the pathogenesis of ASD. PON1, an esterase/lactonase
enzyme, plays an important role in hydrolyzing pesticides, protecting against oxida-
tive stress, and modulating the immune/inflammatory response. First, the authors
review the general biochemical properties of PON1, with the recent biochemical and
genetic studies indicating impaired PON1 status in ASD and the literature suggesting
a correlation between pesticide (mainly organophosphates) exposure and neurodeve-
lopmental delays or neuropsychiatric conditions. Second, the authors describe sev-
eral gene × environment models for autism, which include PON1: organophosphates
exposure × reelin × PON1 and organophosphates exposure × acetylcholine receptor
(AchR) × PON1. Finally, they illustrate how PON1 could contribute to the aberrant
immune response and abnormal redox status in autism. They also discuss how these
disturbances could affect the PON1 status and further increase the susceptibility to
various environmental neurotoxic agents during neurodevelopment.
Sulfur metabolism serves a number of critical roles, including the maintenance
of cellular redox status and support for several methylation reactions. Methylation
capacity is reduced during oxidative stress, affecting many processes such as epige-
netic regulation of gene expression, which is critical for development, as well as
dopamine-stimulated phospholipid methylation, which is involved in the synchroni-
zation of neuronal firing. The unique features of sulfur metabolism in the brain make
it highly vulnerable to heavy metals, which bind with high affinity to thiol and sele-
nocysteine oxidoreductases and interfere with redox regulation. Methionine synthase
(folate and vitamin B12–dependent enzyme) plays a key role as a monitor of cellular
redox status and as a regulator of the flux of homocysteine through transsulfuration to
glutathione synthesis. Levels of methionione synthase mRNA are significantly lower
in brain samples from autistic subjects, reflecting an adaptive response to neuroin-
flammation and oxidative stress. These factors allow the formulation of a “redox/
methylation hypothesis of autism,” described by Richard Deth in Chapter 7, which
outlines a molecular mechanism whereby heavy metals promote oxidative stress and
impaired methylation, leading to disrupted development and autism.
In Chapter 8, George Wagner and colleagues describe novel models of autism
in which mice are exposed either pre- or post-natally to toxicants such as valproic
acid and methylmercury. The early toxicant exposure results in neurodevelopmental
deficits in mice that are analogous to deficits observed in humans affected by autism.
Evidence that oxidative stress is involved in autism is provided by the ability of
pretreatment with antioxidants (trolox, a water-soluble vitamin E derivative) to fully
protect the developing mice against the neurobehavioral deficits induced by these
toxicants. These observations are discussed in the context of autism prevention.
In Chapter 9, Woody McGinnis and colleagues discuss an interesting hypothesis
for the unexplained phenomenon of regression in autism. In their model, visceral
dysfunction in autism occurs in conjunction with lost phonation and social function
because of selective toxicant effects on a relatively minute region of the brainstem,
which is known to remain permeable to a broad class of neurotoxins after the closure
of the blood–brain barrier elsewhere. By converging on the same site and sharing
oxidative modes of injury, these toxins may act independently, additively, or sequen-
tially to result in autistic regression.
xiv Preface
In Chapter 10, Ved Chauhan and I review evidence that ASD are associated
with abnormalities in lipid metabolism, membrane-associated proteins, and signal
transduction. Phospholipids and their lipid raft domain play important roles
in cellular signaling, and phosphoinositides are major signaling molecules in
G protein–coupled receptor signaling. We discuss our findings on altered levels
of amino-glycerophospholipids in the membrane, increased peroxidation of lipids,
decreased membrane fluidity, increased activity of phospholipase A 2 (lipid-
metabolizing enzyme), and altered activities of protein kinase C and protein kinase
A in autism, suggesting that membrane signaling may be affected in autism.
We also review the evidence of an association of the phosphatidylinositol 3-kinase
gene in autism, altered brain levels of Bcl2 and p53 (involved in apoptosis), altered
levels of cytokines and inflammation and mutational changes in the proteins
involved in cell signaling such as neuroligins, Pten, SHANK3, Wnt, reelin, and
voltage-dependent calcium channels, all suggesting impairment in signal transduc-
tion in autism. These abnormalities in the signal system may account for some of the
structural changes and cognitive deficits in the brains of individuals with autism.
Mitochondria play a major role in ROS generation and cytosolic calcium seques-
tration, and a primary defect in mitochondrial electron transport and oxidative phos-
phorylation impairs both processes. Conversely, abnormal calcium signaling will
secondarily perturb these mitochondrial functions, as the mitochondria have recently
been shown to participate with the endoplasmic reticulum in this important process
that governs a wide array of cellular functions. In Chapter 11, Jay Gargus presents a
genetic scheme as the basis for the elevated levels of ROS observed in autism, which
integrates earlier observations of genetic and functional mitochondrial dysfunction
in autism with newer observations of defects in calcium signaling in the disease.
Recently, Timothy syndrome, a rare monogenic form of autism, was shown to be
a channelopathy caused by a mutation in a calcium channel. In addition, diseases
comorbid with autism, such as migraine and seizures, share a channelopathy patho-
genesis, strengthening the notion that defects in calcium signaling may be a cardinal
aspect of the disorder that may represent a target for novel therapeutics.
Some parents of autistic children report frequent infection, prolonged illness, or
chronic sinopulmonary symptoms, which are suggestive of immune abnormalities
in autism. Emerging evidence from several independent research groups indicates
the role of the immune system and inflammation in the pathogenesis and pathophys-
iology of ASD. Increased oxidative damage and/or mitochondrial dysfunction can
also lead to inflammation because oxidative stress serves as a major upstream
component in the signaling cascade involved in the activation of redox-sensitive
transcription factors and pro-inflammatory gene expression resulting in an inflam-
matory response.
Recently, it has become evident that the neuroimmune network is crucial for
immune homeostasis and the function of the central nervous system (CNS). In
Chapter 12, Carlos Pardo-Villamizar and Andrew Zimmerman review the findings
on the activation of neuroglia and the neuroimmune system, as evidenced by neu-
roinflammation in brains, reactive astrogliosis, activated microglia, and cytokine
abnormalities. They also discuss the role of the maternal immune environment and
immunogenetic factors in autism.
Preface xv
Innate immunity plays a key role in the neuroimmune network. However, the role
of innate immunity in the onset and progression of ASD is not well understood. In
Chapter 13, Harumi Jyonouchi reviews immune abnormalities reported in children
with ASD, following an overview of innate immunity in the GI tract and the CNS.
Finally, the possible impact of innate immunity on neuroimmune interactions in
autistic children is discussed. In Chapter 14, Paul Ashwood and colleagues review
cell-mediated immune response, autoimmunity, cytokine abnormalities, gut inflam-
mation, and GI dysfunction and suggest a relationship between GI-related immune
dysfunction and autistic behaviors. Abnormal immune responses may predispose
individuals to frequent infections, adverse reactions to benign environmental factors,
and possibly autoimmune conditions, leading to increased oxidative stress.
Oxytocin (OT) is known to be dysregulated in some autistic children. In Chapter 15,
Martha Welch and Benjamin Klein offer the hypothesis that autism arises from
the dysregulation of a unified gut/brain system rather than originating in the brain
alone. They postulate that autism stems from physiological stress, including oxida-
tive stress, which, if unmodulated, triggers a cascade of adverse interrelated auto-
nomic, endocrinological, neurological, and immunological reactions. They review
evidence that dysregulated OT levels and signaling pathways downstream of the
oxytocin receptor combined with oxidative stress in the gut may dysregulate a uni-
fied gut/brain network and be involved in the pathogenesis of a subset of autism.
They also discuss a chain of possible cellular events in gut Paneth cells, involving
ROS, β-catenin, matrix metalloproteinase-7, prodefensin, and defensin, which could
impact various ion channels in enteric neurons and ultimately influence behavior.
A possible mechanism for dysregulation of gut/brain signaling under conditions of
abnormal OT levels during a time window critical for newborn development is dis-
cussed and compared with the same mechanism when modulated by adequate OT
levels in normal newborns. Finally, they discuss possible early therapeutic interven-
tions aimed at the OT-related mechanism postulated in this chapter.
Cytokines play an important role in the regulation of inflammatory responses and
are involved in the regulation of both innate and acquired immunities. They are often
encoded by highly polymorphic genes. Some of these polymorphisms are responsi-
ble for quantitative interindividual differences in cytokine production, thereby influ-
encing the relative strength of immune responses. In the last 10 years, evidence has
accumulated that increased levels of some pro-inflammatory cytokines are present in
the peripheral blood mononuclear cells of children with ASD. In Chapter 16, Fabián
Crespo and colleagues suggest that there are phenotypes of the immune system that
are predisposed to stronger or weaker inflammatory immune responses, and these
phenotypes can manifest from several different combinations of genotypes of differ-
ent cytokine genes with variable expressions. They propose that certain expression
polymorphisms in key cytokine genes may contribute to the etiology or the emergence
of autism by predisposing individuals carrying those genotypes (or their mothers)
to altered immune activation to certain antigens. The authors also suggest that the
maternal immunogenetic makeup may be associated with the fetal pathogenesis of
ASD since cytokines are able to cross the placenta.
In Chapter 17, William Johnson and colleagues discuss alleles of maternal genes
that act in the mother to contribute to the phenotype of their affected offspring. These
xvi Preface
alleles most likely act in the mother during pregnancy to modify the development of
the embryo or fetus, for example, brain development in the affected children. Of the
34 reports so far of these maternal alleles, nearly all were in neurodevelopmental
disorders, including autism. HLA-DR4 was originally suspected of being a maternally
acting gene allele for autism because its allele frequency was increased in individuals
with autism and their mothers, but not their fathers. This has now been confirmed by a
case–parent study design. HLA-DR4 may act in autism by affecting synapse develop-
ment, by a mechanism including oxidative stress, by a combination of these, or by an
as-yet-unknown mechanism.
Chapter 18 is a commentary by Martha Herbert. On the basis of active pathophysi-
ological processes, such as oxidative stress and inflammation in autism, she discusses
that the classical autism model, which frames ASD as a genetically determined devel-
opmental disorder of the brain whose main manifestation is behavioral alterations, does
not predict persistent pathophysiological disturbances in autism. Herbert describes a
pathophysiology-centered model of autism, in which it is argued that ASD is not only
developmental but also a chronic condition based on active pathophysiology; is not
only behavioral but also has somatic and systemic features; is not only genetic but
also environmental; and is not a static encephalopathy but is a dynamic, recalcitrant
encephalopathy.
The history of the treatment of autism has been dominated by a technical
approach mostly highlighted by applied behavior analysis and, to a lesser extent, by
psychopharmacology. In Chapter 19, Eric London proposes the utility of using the
biopsychosocial method elaborated by George Engel as a conceptual way to treat
autism. The implications of these concepts for both research and clinical works are
discussed.
I would like to express my gratitude to my coeditors, Drs. Ved Chauhan and Ted
Brown, for their help in the review process, and to all the contributors for their chap-
ters. My sincere thanks to CRC Press/Taylor & Francis Group, especially Barbara
Norwitz, Patricia Roberson and Jennifer Smith, for their support in compiling and
publishing this book. I hope that it will stimulate hypothesis-driven research and be
a valuable reference source not only to scientists in the laboratory but also to clinicians
and caregivers in the field of autism and related disorders.
Ved Chauhan, PhD, is the head of the Cellular Neurochemistry Laboratory at the
New York State Institute for Basic Research in Developmental Disabilities (IBR),
Staten Island, New York.
Dr. Chauhan received his MS (biochemistry) in 1975 and his PhD (biochemis-
try) in 1980 from the Postgraduate Institute for Medical Education and Research,
Chandigarh, India. After working as a research associate for two years in the
Department of Biochemistry at the University of Southern California, Los Angeles,
he joined IBR as a research scientist in 1983.
Dr. Chauhan has published more than 70 research articles in peer-reviewed
journals. His work includes but is not limited to phospholipid methylation, calcium
traversal across bilayers, the role of phosphoinositides in the activation of protein
kinase C, lipid and amyloid β-protein interactions, hydrophobic domain formation
by fibrillar amyloid β-protein and its regulation by gelsolin, and membrane abnor-
malities and cellular signaling in autism.
xvii
xviii Editors
W. Ted Brown, MD, PhD, is the director of the New York State Institute for Basic
Research in Developmental Disabilities (IBR), the chairperson of IBR’s Department
of Human Genetics, and the director of IBR’s George A. Jervis Clinic. He is a fellow
of the American College of Medical Genetics and an adjunct professor at the State
University of New York-Downstate Medical Center in Brooklyn.
Dr. Brown received his BA in 1967, his MA in 1969 and his PhD in biophysics in
1973 from The Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland. He received his MD
from Harvard Medical School (cum laude) in 1974. He trained in internal medicine
in New York City, undertook a fellowship in clinical genetics, and was appointed
as an assistant professor of medicine at the New York Hospital-Cornell University
Medical Center in 1978. He began research into premature aging syndromes and
Down syndrome while on the Cornell Medical School Faculty, and was an attend-
ing physician at New York Hospital and a faculty member of Rockefeller University.
In 1981, he became the chairperson of the Department of Human Genetics at IBR.
In 1991, he was appointed the director of IBR’s Jervis Clinic, and in 2005, he became
the director of IBR.
Dr. Brown is the author of more than 300 publications. At IBR, his initial research
was on Down syndrome genes. He then focused his research on the fragile X syn-
drome, which was then newly recognized and is now considered the most common
inherited cause of mental retardation. At IBR, he established a DNA diagnostic and
molecular laboratory and developed a screening and prenatal testing program for
fragile X. He was the first to discover a relationship between autism and the fra-
gile X syndrome. His work on fragile X has ranged from clinical studies relating to
phenotype, to family inheritance studies, to mouse model development, and to basic
molecular research. His current research focuses on autism genetics and the fragile
X syndrome. Dr. Brown is also a recognized world authority on progeria, a rare and
tragic disease that afflicts young children with premature aging. He was instrumental
in the discovery of the genetic mutation that causes this disease.
Dr. Brown serves on the editorial board of the American Journal of Intellectual
and Developmental Disability. He has served on the scientific advisory board for
Cure Autism Now, the Progeria Research Foundation, and the National Fragile X
Foundation.
Contributors
Paul Ashwood, PhD Abha Chauhan, PhD
Department of Medical Microbiology Department of Neurochemistry
and Immunology New York State Institute for Basic
and Research in Developmental
Disabilities
M.I.N.D Institute Staten Island, New York
University of California at Davis
Davis, California Ved Chauhan, PhD
Department of Neurochemistry
Tapan Audhya, PhD
New York State Institute for Basic
Division of Endocrinology
Research in Developmental
Department of Medicine
Disabilities
New York University School
Staten Island, New York
of Medicine
New York, New York
Michelle A. Cheh, PhD
and Department of Neuroscience
Vitamin Diagnostics Laboratory Rutgers University
Cliffwood Beach, New Jersey New Brunswick, New Jersey
xix
xx Contributors
CONTENTS
1.1 Introduction .....................................................................................................2
1.2 Clinical, Etiological, and Neuropathological Diversity in Autism .................3
1.2.1 Clinic.................................................................................................. 3
1.2.2 Etiology ..............................................................................................4
1.2.3 Neuropathology..................................................................................5
1
2 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
1.1 INTRODUCTION
The aim of this chapter is to identify the type, topography, and sequelae of neuro-
pathological changes that contribute to the clinical phenotype of autism. Results
of recent magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and postmortem neuropathological
and stereological studies of autism brain suggest a dynamic model of sequen-
tial subdivision of age- and brain-specific structural and functional changes.
Acceleration of brain growth in the first year of life and deceleration in the second
and third years appear to play a pivotal role in the onset of clinical signs of
autism (Courchesne and Pierce, 2005b; Courchesne et al., 2001, 2003; Dawson
et al., 2007; Dementieva et al., 2005; Gillberg and de Souza, 2002; Redcay and
Courchesne, 2005). The range of deviation from the normal trajectory of brain
growth may be a factor determining the severity of the disease (Courchesne et al.,
2003). Developmental heterochronicity (differential rates of growth of various
brain regions compared to controls), resulting in selective overgrowth of some brain
Type, Topography, and Sequelae of Neuropathological Changes 3
1.2.1 CLINIC
In most cases (90%–95%), it is not presently possible to detect a known or specific
etiology. These cases are referred to as idiopathic or nonsyndromic autism (Boddaert
et al., 2009; Gillberg and Coleman, 1996). In 6% (Fombonne, 2003), 5% (Tuchman et al.,
1991), or 10% (Rutter et al., 1994) of cases, autism was diagnosed in association with
other disorders. About 30% of children with idiopathic autism have complex autism,
defined by the presence of dysmorphic features, microcephaly and/or a structural brain
4 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
malformation (Miles et al., 2005). About 70% of children with autism have essential
autism, defined by the absence of physical abnormalities. For most children, the onset
of autism is gradual. However, a multisite study revealed significant regression at ages
of 18 to 33 months (regressive autism) in about 13.8% (Colorado) to 31.6% (Utah) of
autistic subjects (Department of Health and Human Services, 2007). Moreover, the
manifestations of autism vary greatly, depending on developmental level and chrono-
logical age of the affected individual. The majority of patients exhibit serious social
and communicative impairments throughout life but some improve enough to be able
to live relatively independently as adults. In 44.6% of children, autism is associated
with cognitive impairment (defined as having intelligence quotient scores of <70;
Department of Health and Human Services, 2007). Expressive language function in
individuals with autism may vary from mutism to verbal fluency (Rapin, 1996;
Stone et al., 1997; Wetherby et al., 1998). Sensorimotor deficits also show significant
interindividual differences, with more frequent and severe impairments of gross and
fine motor function (motor stereotypes, hypotonia, limbic apraxia) in subjects with
lower IQ (Rogers et al., 1996). Hand mannerisms and body rocking are reported in
37% to 95% of individuals with autism (Lord and Rutter, 1995; Rapin, 1996; Rogers
et al., 1996), whereas preoccupation with sensory features of objects, abnormal
responsiveness to environmental stimuli, or paradoxical responses to sensory stimuli
are seen in 42% to 88% of people with autism (Kientz and Dunn, 1997). Epilepsy is a
comorbid complication, occurring in up to 33% of individuals with autism (Tuchman
and Rapin, 2002).
1.2.2 ETIOLOGY
The clinical diversity of autism reflects the etiologic heterogeneity of this disorder.
Genetic factors; pre-, peri-, and postnatal pathological factors; and concurrent dis-
eases may contribute to autism (Muhle et al., 2004; Newschaffer et al., 2002; Rutter
et al., 1994). About 5% to 10% of cases are associated with several distinct genetic
conditions including fragile X syndrome, tuberous sclerosis, phenylketonuria, Rett
syndrome, and chromosomal anomalies such as Down syndrome (DS) (Folstein
and Rosen-Scheidley, 2001; Fombonne, 2003; Smalley et al., 1988; Yonan et al.,
2003). Autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) in people with DS have been described
in several reports (Ghaziuddin et al., 1992; Howlin et al., 1995; Prasher and Clarke,
1996; Wakabayashi, 1979), and the prevalence of autism in boys with DS was esti-
mated as at least 7% (Kent et al., 1999). The prevalence of autism in the fragile X
syndrome is estimated as 15%–28% (Hagerman, 2002). Cytogenetic abnormalities
(partial duplications, deletions, inversions) in the 15q11-q13 region account for 1% to
4% of autism cases (Cook, 1998; Gillberg, 1998). Several potential candidate genes
have been identified in both autosomes and X chromosomes, including the tuberous
sclerosis gene on chromosomes 9 and 16; serotonin transporter on chromosome 17;
gamma-aminobutyric acid receptor-beta 3 on chromosome 15; neuroligins on the
X chromosome (see Vorstman et al., 2006); and possibly PTEN on chromosome 10
(Butler et al., 2005). Modifications in the tryptophan hydroxylase gene may play a
modest role in autism susceptibility (Coon et al., 2005).
Type, Topography, and Sequelae of Neuropathological Changes 5
1.2.3 NEUROPATHOLOGY
While knowledge of the clinical and genetic factors in autism is based on examination of
thousands of patients, postmortem neuropathological studies are based on reports
of a very small number of brains. A review by Palmen et al. (2004) revealed that
between 1980 and 2003, only 58 brains of individuals with autism have been exam-
ined, and results of only a few neuropathological and stereological studies were pub-
lished. Usually, neuropathological reports and morphometric reports were based on
evaluation of one or several brains. Due to the broad age spectrum and the etiological
and clinical diversity in autism, the pattern of neuropathological changes reported is
incomplete and often inconsistent. As a result, the morphological markers and neu-
ropathological diagnostic criteria of autism have not yet been established (Lord et al.,
2000; Pickett and London, 2005). In the past, the contribution of postmortem studies
to the detection and characterization of neuropathological changes and mechanisms
leading to structural and functional manifestations of autism was limited because
of (a) the deficit of autism brains, resulting in a lack of statistical power, (b) the lack of
efficient mechanisms for sharing the limited tissue resources, (c) the lack of complex
studies of the dynamic of changes during the life span, (d) the infrequent application
of unbiased morphometric methods to detect quantitative differences, and (e) the averag-
ing of results from subjects with different clinical and morphological manifestations
of autism. Heterogeneity within the autism spectrum is the major obstacle to autism
research at all levels (Newschaffer et al., 2002), including neuropathological stud-
ies and attempts at detection of clinicopathological correlations. Recent evidence of
genetic fractionation of social impairment, communication difficulties, and rigid and
repetitive behaviors indicates that heterogeneity in ASD could be an unavoidable con-
sequence of the contribution of nonoverlapping genes. If different features of autism
are caused by different genes associated with different brain regions and related to dif-
ferent core cognitive impairments (Happe et al., 2006), it seems likely that many brain
networks are involved in the pathology of autism. The diversity of neuropathological
findings and the commonly reported inconsistencies in regional findings correspond
to developmental impairments in many interacting brain networks and to expansion
from “local” abnormalities to “nonlocal” effects of the emerging cognitive system.
In spite of these limitations, “localizing” models are still the main approach to the
identification of pathological changes as a component of the structural and functional
abnormalities of the networks (Müller, 2007).
The possibility that autism is associated with neuropathological changes was
explored in the first studies reported between 1980 and 1989 (Bauman and Kemper,
1985; Courchesne et al., 1987, 1988; Damasio et al., 1980; Gaffney et al., 1987;
Hashimoto et al., 1989, 1993; Murakami et al., 1989; Ritvo et al., 1986). Expansion
of these studies through examination of larger cohorts and application of stereology,
functional and structural MRI, and biochemistry resulted in the identification of
several major forms of pathology contributing to the clinical phenotype including
abnormal acceleration of brain growth in early childhood (Redcay and Courchesne,
2005), delay of neuronal growth (Wegiel et al., 2008), changes in brain cytoarchitec-
ture (Bailey et al., 1998; Bauman and Kemper, 1985; Casanova et al., 2002, 2006),
6 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
de Souza, 2002; Redcay and Courchesne, 2005). Using meta-analysis based on eval-
uation of head circumference converted to brain volume, brain volume measured
from MRI, and brain weight from postmortem studies, Redcay and Courchesne
(2005) revealed that brain size increases from 13% smaller than in control subjects
at birth to 10% larger than in control infants at 1 year, but only 2% greater by ado-
lescence. The greater growth rate of head circumference in the first year, and its
return to normal rates thereafter, is not accounted for by an overall growth in stature.
Studies of behavioral development in infants later diagnosed with autism suggest
that the period of acceleration of head growth precedes and overlaps with the onset
of behavioral changes, and that the period of deceleration coincides with a period of
behavioral decline or worsening of symptoms in the second year of life (Dawson
et al., 2007). Coincidence of acceleration of brain growth rate with onset and
worsening of clinical symptoms may indicate that structural developmental changes
critical for a lifelong phenotype occur in early infancy. Acceleration of brain growth
in the first year and deceleration in the second year of life suggest that failure of the
mechanism controlling brain growth in the first year of life plays an essential role in
the onset of clinical features of autism. Identification of these mechanisms may lead
to conceptualization of early preventive treatments.
In the third phase, of 2 to 4 years, the overall rate of brain growth slows but is still
10% more than in normally developing children (Carper et al., 2002; Courchesne
et al., 2001; Hazlett et al., 2005; Sparks et al., 2002). In 4- to 5-year-old autistic
children, MRI-based estimated brain volume is 1350 mL, whereas in normally devel-
oping children, a comparable volume (1360 mL) is reached about 8 years later. In post-
mortem studies, the brain weight of 3- to 5-year-old autistic males was 15% higher
(1451 g) than in control males of this age (1259 g) (Redcay and Courchesne, 2005).
In the fourth phase, the volume of the brain decreases, and this trend extends from
middle/late childhood through adulthood. Head (Aylward et al., 2002) or brain enlarge-
ment (Bailey et al., 1998; Hardan et al., 2001; Lainhart et al., 1997; Piven et al., 1995,
1996) has also been observed in studies of older populations of autistic individuals.
However, by adolescence and adulthood, the average size of the brain is only 1% to 3%
greater in autistic than in control cohorts (Redcay and Courchesne, 2005).
Moreover, the pattern of brain growth reflects the severity of clinical manifestation
of autism (Courchesne et al., 2003). Among infants who have the more severe form of
autism, 71% showed increases during their first year of more than 1.5 S.D., with 59%
showing increases between 2.0 and 4.3 S.D. In children with a less severe form of
autism, PDD-NOS, acceleration of brain growth is observed later, and the increase
is less pronounced. Later onset and slower rate of progression of autism appear to be
associated with a better outcome.
regions that are involved in cognitive, social, and emotional functions as well as lan-
guage development, is synchronized with brain overgrowth in 2- to 4-year-old autistic
children in contrast to a different rate of growth of the occipital cortex (Carper and
Courchesne, 2005; Carper et al., 2002; Courchesne et al., 2001; Hazlett et al., 2005;
Sparks et al., 2002). The reduced size of the body and posterior subregions of the
corpus callosum noted in subjects with autism may indicate disproportions in brain
subregions development (Piven et al., 1997b). The cellular and molecular basis for
transient acceleration of brain growth and enhanced growth of some brain regions
is not known, but Courchesne et al. (2003) proposed that the observed pattern is
associated with an excessive number of neurons, enhanced rate of growth of size of
neurons, and increased number of minicolumns as well as excessive and premature
expansion of the dendritic tree.
gyrus and mamillary body; (b) a congenital decrease in the number of Purkinje
cells in the cerebellum; and (c) age-related differences in cell size and number of neu-
rons in the cerebellar nuclei and in the inferior olivary nucleus. Microdysgenesis is
represented by increased neuronal density in the cortical layer, clustering of cortical
neurons, disorganization of cortical layers, neuron cytomegaly, ectopic neurons, and
nodular heterotopias. A detailed study of serial sections from the brain of a 29-year-old
man with autism revealed reduced neuronal size and increased cell-packing density
(Bauman and Kemper, 1985), both features of an immature brain (Friede, 1975).
Cell-packing density was increased by 66% in the hypothalamus and mamillary
body, and by 54% in the medial septal nucleus, with smaller nerve cells. The reduced
size of neurons and the selective increase in cell-packing density were seen in central
(40%), medial (28%), and cortical nuclei (35%). Atrophy of the neocerebellar cortex,
with marked loss of Purkinje cells and, to a lesser extent, of granule cells, was pres-
ent in gracile, tonsil, and inferior semilunar lobules. Changes were not detected in
the anterior lobe or the vermis. Reduced numbers of cells were noticed in fastiglial,
globose, and emboliform nuclei, and cells were small and pale. The dentate nucleus
was distorted. Retrograde neuronal loss in the inferior olive related to neuronal loss
in cerebellar cortex was not found, but olivary neurons were small and pale. Brain
cytoarchitecture abnormalities were not associated with gliosis. In a 21-year-old
female with autism, Rodier et al. (1996) found that the brain was smaller than a con-
trol brain, and the length of facial nerve nucleus was less than 500 μm as compared
to 2610 μm in the control subject.
One may assume that mechanisms regulating growth of the neuron in early child-
hood are the target of factors that are the cause of autism. The result of deregulation
of these mechanisms could be (a) significantly delayed growth of neuronal body,
nucleus, dendritic tree, spines, and reduced number of synapses and (b) functional
deficits corresponding to these structural developmental delays. These abnormalities
of very early childhood might be the major contributor to clinical deficits that are the
basis for the clinical diagnosis of autism at the age of 3 years.
and accumulation (Brunk et al., 1992; Sohal and Brunk, 1989). The presence of
oxidatively modified proteins and lipids in lipofuscin supports the causative link
between enhanced oxidative stress, autophagocytosis, and deposition of products of
degradation in the lysosomal pathway and lipofuscin (Brunk and Terman, 2002a,b;
Szweda et al., 2003; Terman and Brunk, 2004) and suggests that in autism, abnormal
development is associated with early signs of oxidative stress and enhanced degradation
and, possibly, turnover of cytoplasmic components.
striatal subdivisions, and cerebellum, may contribute to cellular dysfunction and the
clinical expression of autism.
and 10 control subjects revealed a reduced number of neurons in layers III, V, and
VI, and reduced volume of neuronal soma in layers V and VI in the fusiform gyrus. No
alterations in Brodman area 17 in these autistic individuals suggest that the input from
the visual cortex to the fusiform gyrus is intact. These results indicate the underdevel-
opment of connections in the fusiform gyrus that may contribute to abnormal face
perception in autism (van Kooten et al., 2008).
Bailey et al. (1998) noted abnormalities in cytoarchitectonic organization and
neuronal density in the superior frontal cortex and superior temporal gyrus in
autism. Neurons in the superior temporal sulcus are sensitive to the angle of gaze
(Perrett et al., 1985). Neurons that are attuned to particular facial expressions were
found in the inferior and superior temporal lobes (Hasselmo et al., 1989). Cortical
areas responsive to faces, facial expressions, and angle of gaze send direct projec-
tions to the amygdala (Stefanacci and Amaral, 2000). Pathological changes in the
amygdala may play a central role in the dysfunction seen in autism, including
disturbed components of social cognition such as attention to and interpretation of
facial expressions. fMRI studies show that judging from the expression of another
person’s eyes what the other person might be thinking or feeling is associated with
activation in the superior temporal gyrus, frontal cortex, and amygdala, whereas in
subjects with autism, activation appears in the temporal and frontal cortex but not
in the amygdala (Baron-Cohen et al., 1999).
Results of evaluation of the size of the cerebellum using MRI are inconsistent.
In several MRI studies, smaller cerebellar hemispheres (Gaffney et al., 1987;
Murakami et al., 1989) and vermis (Ciesielski et al., 1997; Courchesne et al., 1988;
Hashimoto et al., 1995) were reported. In a study by Piven et al. (1997a), the total
cerebellar volume was found to be greater in subjects with autism than in the control
group, and the increase was proportional to the increased total brain volume. In the
cerebellum, boys with autism had less gray matter, a smaller ratio of gray to white
matter, and smaller lobules VI and VII than did controls. Despite the inconsistency of
reports characterizing topographic autism-associated vermian hypoplasia (Hashimoto
et al., 1993; Kaufmann et al., 2003; Levitt et al., 1999; Piven et al., 1997a; Schaefer et al.,
1996), several reports show associations between the size of the vermis and deficits in
attention-orienting (Harris et al., 1999; Townsend et al., 1999), stereotypic behavior,
and reduced exploration in autism (Pierce and Courchesne, 2001).
The reduced size of the pons, midbrain, and medulla in autism reported by
Hashimoto et al. (1992, 1993, 1995) was not confirmed in other studies (Hsu et al.,
1991; Piven et al., 1992).
Changes in neurons in the deep cerebellar nuclei were noticed by some authors
(Kemper and Bauman, 1998) but not by others (Bailey et al., 1998). Structural
MRI shows variable patterns of changes. Volumetry of the cerebellum may show
no change, hypoplasia, or hyperplasia. Courchesne et al. (1988) reported selective
hypertrophy of lobules VI and VII, but these results were not confirmed in other
subjects. In part, the pattern may correspond to the functional status of subjects. In
highly functioning subjects with autism, hypoplasia of the cerebellum has not been
detected (Holttum et al., 1992).
A decrease in the number of GABAergic Purkinje cells is considered the most
consistent neuropathological finding in autism, as it was detected in at least 50% of
examined cases (Arin et al., 1991; Bailey et al., 1998). Recent studies indicate that
preserved Purkinje cells reveal a 40% decrease in the expression of glutamic acid
decarboxylase 67 (GAD67) mRNA in autistic subjects relative to control patients
(Yip et al., 2007). Moreover, in autism, the basket cells likely provide increased
GABAergic feed-forward inhibition to Purkinje cells. The result may include dis-
ruption in the timing of Purkinje cell firing and altered inhibition of the cerebellar
nuclei, which could directly affect cerebellocortical output, leading to changes in
motor behavior and cognition (Yip et al., 2008).
Repetitive and stereotyped behaviors defined as recurring, nonfunctional activi-
ties, or interests that occur regularly and interfere with daily functioning are a defining
signs of autism. These behaviors include lower-order repetitive motor behavior,
intense circumscribed patterns of interests, and higher-order rituals and compul-
sions (Gabriels et al., 2005). Several studies implicated the role of basal ganglia and
frontostriatal circuitry in the pathophysiology of autism, especially in repetitive and
stereotyped behaviors. Increased volume of the basal ganglia was reported in sev-
eral MRI studies (Herbert et al., 2003; Hollander et al., 2005; Langen et al., 2007;
Sears et al., 1999). Sears et al. (1999) and Hollander et al. (2005) observed a positive
correlation between caudate volumes and repetitive behavior scores. A significant
increase in caudate nucleus volume, disproportional to brain volume, was detected
in MRI studies in two independent samples of medication-naive subjects with autism
Type, Topography, and Sequelae of Neuropathological Changes 17
induces brain alterations that contribute to changes in circuitry, which potentiates the
seizure-genic focus (Armstrong, 2005).
Studies of nonautistic subjects indicate that epilepsy-associated pathology
includes patchy or laminar neuronal loss and gliosis in the cerebral cortex in one or
both hemispheres. In temporal epilepsy, abnormalities were reported in 75% of the
specimens examined, and hippocampal sclerosis was found in 50% (Bruton, 1988).
Loss of hippocampal neurons correlates with the frequency of tonic-cloning seizures
and the total duration of epilepsy (Dam, 1980; Tasch et al., 1999). Loss is accentu-
ated in the CA4 sector and is observed in the granule cell layer in the dentate gyrus.
Dispersion of dentate gyrus granular neurons might be a result of seizure-related,
disturbed migration of neurons (Bengzon et al., 1997), or epilepsy-enhanced neuro-
genesis (Ericksson et al., 1998). Ammon horn sclerosis is a progressive lesion that
can be induced and propagated by seizures (Armstrong, 2005).
In nearly all cases with hippocampal pathology, changes are also observed in
other brain regions. In about 25%, the amygdala, thalamus and mammillary body
are affected with neuronal loss. More severe neuronal loss and gliosis in the hip-
pocampus is paralleled by severe neuronal loss and gliosis in the lateral nucleus
in the amygdala (Bruton, 1988; Hudson et al., 1993; Thom et al., 1999). Ectopias
with more than 8 neurons per 2 sq. mm of white matter occurred in 43% of epileptic
patients but in none of the controls (Hardiman et al., 1988). In 45% of severely affected
epileptics, significant neuronal loss and astrocytosis spreading out into the overlying
molecular layer is observed in the cerebellar cortex. The severity of the cerebellar
damage may range from gross atrophy of most or many folia to the restricted neu-
ronal loss in some folia, especially at their basal portion (Gessaga and Urich, 1985).
Central apnea, asphyxia, and pulmonary edema occurring during a seizure
(Nashef et al., 1996) as well as life-threatening cardiac arrhythmias during seizures
(Earnest et al., 1992; Jallon, 1997; Nashef et al., 1996; Reeves et al., 1996; Saussu
et al., 1998) have been suggested as possible causes of sudden unexpected death in
epilepsy (Thom et al., 1999).
Enhanced electric activity of neurons and/or increased cell synaptic transmis-
sion with enhanced vesicle exocytosis, both in normal and in disease-affected
brains are a common cause of modifications of APP processing and Aβ levels.
Epilepsy is associated with an elevation of APP expression (Sheng et al., 1994) and
occurs in 10 of 11 examined subjects with diffuse nonfibrillar Aβ plaque forma-
tion (mean age 47.9 ± 8.8 years of age) (Mackenzie and Miller, 1994; Mackenzie
et al., 1996).
the appropriate connections survive and form synapses, whereas neurons that fail
to obtain adequate neurotrophins die (Oppenheim, 1991). BDNF is broadly distrib-
uted throughout the human central nervous system (CNS) and provides neurotrophic
support for many neuronal populations in the cortex, amygdala, hippocampus, and
striatum (Murer et al., 2001; Schmidt-Kastner et al., 1996; Tapia-Arancibia et al.,
2004). The hypothalamus is the brain structure that contains the highest BDNF pro-
tein levels (Katoh-Semba et al., 1997; Nawa et al., 1995; Yan et al., 1997) and BDNF
mRNA (Castren et al., 1995; Kawamoto et al., 1996; Yan et al., 1997). In the cerebel-
lum, immunoreactivity was observed in Purkinje cells and the olivary complex of
the nuclei (Kawamoto et al., 1996; Murer et al., 2001).
In the basal forebrain of autistic individuals, the level of BDNF was three times
higher than in controls (Perry et al., 2001). Miyazaki et al. (2004) observed a higher
level of BDNF in the blood samples of young children with autism than in adult
control subjects. The mean BDNF levels in sera of children diagnosed with autism
and childhood disintegrative disorder were about four times higher than in control
children (Connolly et al., 2006). Children with autism and childhood disintegrative
disorder have both elevated BDNF levels and levels of autoantibodies against BDNF.
Serum BDNF has been shown to be increased after seizures (Binder et al., 2001;
Chavko et al., 2002).
system may contribute to structural and functional changes in target brain regions
and structures. Virtually all regions of the brain receive serotonergic afferents from
raphe system neurons. The rostral raphe nuclei form ascending pathways of axons
mainly to the forebrain. The caudal raphe system innervates the lower brain stem
and the spinal cord (Aitken and Törk, 1988; Lidov and Molliver, 1982). The func-
tions of serotonin are mediated by 14 subtypes of 5-HT receptors in the nervous
system (Hoyer et al., 1994; Martin and Humphrey, 1994; Saudou and Hen, 1994a,b).
The serotonin2A (5-HT2A) receptor is known to be one of the major subtypes and is
associated with psychological and mental events (Roth, 1994). The 5-HT2A receptor
plays a role in facilitating the formation and maintenance of synapses (Niitsu et al.,
1995). Staining for 5-HT2A shows the entire somata and dendritic tree of Purkinje
cells in a rat cerebellum (Maeshima et al., 1998). In vitro studies have shown that
that 5-HT inhibits the growth and arborization of Purkinje cell dendrites through
5-HT2A receptors and stimulates them through the 5-HT1A receptor (Kondoh et al.,
2004). 5-HT promotes the formation of synapses in developing and mature brain
and spinal cord (Chen et al., 1997; Niitsu et al., 1995; Okado et al., 1993), and this
process is mediated by the 5-HT2A receptor in the spinal cord (Niitsu et al., 1995).
Biochemical studies support the hypothesis that developmental defects of the raphe
nuclei may make a major contribution to the structural and functional defects of
cortical and subcortical structures. However, raphe nuclei have not yet been examined
in autistic subjects.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This study was supported in part by funds from the New York State Office of
Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities and grants from Autism Speaks
and the Department of Defense Autism Spectrum Disorders Research Program
(AS073234).
Type, Topography, and Sequelae of Neuropathological Changes 21
REFERENCES
Aitken, A. R. and I. Tork (1988). Early development of serotonin-containing neurons and
pathways as seen in wholemount preparations of the fetal rat brain. J. Comp. Neurol.
274:32–47.
Akyol, O., H. Herken, E. Uz, E. Fadillioglu, S. Unal, S. Sogut, H. Ozyurt, and H. A. Savas
(2002). The indices of endogenous oxidative and antioxidative processes in plasma
from schizophrenic patients: The possible role of oxidant/antioxidant imbalance. Prog.
Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 26:995–1005.
American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders DSM-IV-TR. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC.
Arin, D. M., M. L. Bauman, and T. L. Kemper (1991). The distribution of Purkinje cell loss in
the cerebellum in autism. Neurology 41 (suppl.): 307.
Armstrong, D. D. (2005). Epilepsy-induced microarchitectural changes in the brain. Pediatr.
Dev. Pathol. 8:607–614.
Aylward, E. H., N. J. Minshew, K. Field, B. F. Sparks, and N. Singh (2002). Effects of age on
brain volume and head circumference in autism. Neurology 59:175–183.
Aylward, E. H., N. J. Minshew, G. Goldstein, N. A. Honeycutt, A. M. Augustine, K. O. Yates,
P. E. Barta, and G. D. Pearlson (1999). MRI volumes of amygdala and hippocampus in
non-mentally retarded autistic adolescents and adults. Neurology 53:2145–2150.
Bailey, A., C. A. Le, I. Gottesman, P. Bolton, E. Simonoff, E. Yuzda, and M. Rutter (1995).
Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: Evidence from a British twin study. Psychol.
Med. 25:63–77.
Bailey, A., P. Luthert, P. Bolton, C. A. Le, M. Rutter, and B. Harding (1993). Autism and
megalencephaly. Lancet 341:1225–1226.
Bailey, A., P. Luthert, A. Dean, B. Harding, I. Janota, M. Montgomery, M. Rutter, and
P. Lantos (1998). A clinicopathological study of autism. Brain 121 (Pt 5):889–905.
Bailey, A. R., B. N. Giunta, D. Obregon, W. V. Nikolic, J. Tian, C. D. Sanberg, D. T. Sutton,
and J. Tan (2008). Peripheral biomarkers in autism: Secreted amyloid precursor protein-
alpha as a probable key player in early diagnosis. Int. J Clin. Exp. Med. 1:338–344.
Bale, T. L., A. M. Davis, A. P. Auger, D. M. Dorsa, and M. M. McCarthy (2001). CNS region-
specific oxytocin receptor expression: Importance in regulation of anxiety and sex
behavior. J. Neurosci. 21:2546–2552.
Barden, N. (2004). Implication of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in the physiopathology
of depression. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 29:185–193.
Barkovich, A. J., R. I. Kuzniecky, G. D. Jackson, R. Guerrini, and W. B. Dobyns (2005).
A developmental and genetic classification for malformations of cortical development.
Neurology 65:1873–1887.
Baron-Cohen, S. (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of autism. J. Neurol. Neurosurg.
Psychiatry 75:945–948.
Baron-Cohen, S., H. Ring, J. Moriarty, B. Schmitz, D. Costa, and P. Ell (1994). Recognition of
mental state terms. Clinical findings in children with autism and a functional neuroimaging
study of normal adults. Br. J. Psychiatry 165:640–649.
Baron-Cohen, S., H. A. Ring, S. Wheelwright, E. T. Bullmore, M. J. Brammer, A. Simmons,
and S. C. Williams (1999). Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: An fMRI
study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 11:1891–1898.
Bauman, M. L., P. A. Filipek, and T. L. Kemper (1997). Early infantile autism. Int. Rev.
Neurobiol. 41:367–386.
Bauman, M. L. and T. L. Kemper (1985). Histoanatomic observations of the brain in early
infantile autism. Neurology 35:866–867.
Bauman, M. L. and T. L. Kemper (1996). Observation on the Purkinje cells in the cerebellar
vermis in autism. J. Neuropath. Exp. Neurol. 55:613.
22 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Champagne, F., J. Diorio, S. Sharma, and M. J. Meaney (2001). Naturally occurring variations
in maternal behavior in the rat are associated with differences in estrogen-inducible
central oxytocin receptors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 98:12736–12741.
Chandana, S. R., M. E. Behen, C. Juhasz, O. Muzik, R. D. Rothermel, T. J. Mangner,
P. K. Chakraborty, H. T. Chugani, and D. C. Chugani (2005). Significance of abnormali-
ties in developmental trajectory and asymmetry of cortical serotonin synthesis in autism.
Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 23:171–182.
Chauhan, A., V. Chauhan, W. T. Brown, and I. Cohen (2004). Oxidative stress in autism:
Increased lipid peroxidation and reduced serum levels of ceruloplasmin and
transferrin—the antioxidant proteins. Life Sci. 75:2539–2549.
Chauhan, A. and V. Chauhan (2006). Oxidative stress in autism. Pathophysiology 13:171–181.
Chauhan, A., B. Muthaiyah, M. M. Essa, T. W. Brown, J. Wegiel, and V. Chauhan (2009).
Increased lipid peroxidation in cerebellum and temporal cortex in autism. International
Meeting for Autism Research, Chicago, IL, May 8, 2009.
Chavko, M., N. S. Nadi, and D. O. Keyser (2002). Activation of BDNF mRNA and protein after
seizures in hyperbaric oxygen: Implications for sensitization to seizures in re-exposures.
Neurochem. Res. 27:1649–1653.
Chen, L., K. Hamaguchi, S. Hamada, and N. Okado (1997). Regional differences of serotonin-
mediated synaptic plasticity in the chicken spinal cord with development and aging.
J. Neural Transplant. Plast. 6:41–48.
Chow, T. W. and J. L. Cummings (1999). Frontal-subcortical circuits. In B. L. Miller and
J. L. Cummings, (eds.), The Human Frontal Lobes: Functions and Disorders. Guilford
Press, New York, pp. 3–26.
Chugani, D. C., O. Muzik, M. Behen, R. Rothermel, J. J. Janisse, J. Lee, and H. T. Chugani
(1999). Developmental changes in brain serotonin synthesis capacity in autistic and
nonautistic children. Ann. Neurol. 45:287–295.
Ciesielski, K. T., R. J. Harris, B. L. Hart, and H. F. Pabst (1997). Cerebellar hypoplasia
and frontal lobe cognitive deficits in disorders of early childhood. Neuropsychologia
35:643–655.
Cohen, I. L. (2007). A neural network model of autism: Implications for theory and treatment. In
D. Mareschal, S. Sirois, G. Westerman, and M. H. Johnson (eds.), Neuroconstructivism.
Oxford University Press, Oxford.
Cohen, I. L., X. Liu, C. Schutz, B. N. White, E. C. Jenkins, W. T. Brown, and J. J. Holden
(2003). Association of autism severity with a monoamine oxidase A functional polymor-
phism. Clin. Genet. 64:190–197.
Conner, J. M., J. C. Lauterborn, Q. Yan, C. M. Gall, and S. Varon (1997). Distribution of brain-
derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) protein and mRNA in the normal adult rat CNS:
Evidence for anterograde axonal transport. J. Neurosci. 17:2295–2313.
Connolly, A. M., M. Chez, E. M. Streif, R. M. Keeling, P. T. Golumbek, J. M. Kwon,
J. J. Riviello, R. G. Robinson, R. J. Neuman, and R. M. Deuel (2006). Brain-derived neu-
rotrophic factor and autoantibodies to neural antigens in sera of children with autistic spec-
trum disorders, Landau-Kleffner syndrome, and epilepsy. Biol. Psychiatry 59:354–363.
Cook, E. H. Jr. (1998). Genetics of autism. Mental Retardation Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 4:113–120.
Coon, H., D. Dunn, J. Lainhart, J. Miller, C. Hamil, A. Battaglia, R. Tancredi, M. F. Leppert,
R. Weiss, and W. McMahon (2005). Possible association between autism and variants
in the brain-expressed tryptophan hydroxylase gene (TPH2). Am. J. Med. Genet. B
Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 135B:42–46.
Courchesne, E., D. M. Karns, H. R. Davis, R. Ziccardi, R. A. Carper, Z. D. Tigue, H. J. Hisum,
P. Moses, K. Pierce, C. Lord, A. J. Lincoln, S. Pizzo, L. Schreibman, R. H. Haas,
N. A. Akshoomoff, R. Y. Courchesne (2001). Unusual brain growth patterns in early life
in patients with autistic disorder: an MRI study. Neurology 57:245–254.
24 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Courchesne, E., R. Carper, and N. Akshoomoff (2003). Evidence of brain overgrowth in the
first year of life in autism. JAMA 290:337–344.
Courchesne, E., J. R. Hesselink, T. L. Jernigan, and R. Yeung-Courchesne (1987). Abnormal
neuroanatomy in a nonretarded person with autism. Unusual findings with magnetic
resonance imaging. Arch. Neurol. 44:335–341.
Courchesne, E., C. M. Karns, H. R. Davis, R. Ziccardi, R. A. Carper, Z. D. Tigue,
H. J. Chisum, P. Moses, K. Pierce, C. Lord, A. J. Lincoln, S. Pizzo, L. Schreibman,
R. H. Haas, N. A. Akshoomoff, and R. Y. Courchesne, E., R. Yeung-Courchesne,
G. A. Press, J. R. Hesselink, and T. L. Jernigan (1988). Hypoplasia of cerebellar vermal
lobules VI and VII in autism. N. Engl. J. Med. 318:1349–1354.
Courchesne, E. and K. Pierce (2005a). Why the frontal cortex in autism might be talking
only to itself: Local over-connectivity but long-distance disconnection. Curr. Opin.
Neurobiol. 15:225–230.
Courchesne, E. and K. Pierce (2005b). Brain overgrowth in autism during a critical time in
development: Implications for frontal pyramidal neuron and interneuron development
and connectivity. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 23:153–170.
Dalton, K. M., B. M. Nacewicz, T. Johnstone, H. S. Schaefer, M. A. Gernsbacher, H. H.
Goldsmith, A. L. Alexander, and R. J. Davidson (2005). Gaze fixation and the neural
circuitry of face processing in autism. Nat. Neurosci. 8:519–526.
Dam, A. M. (1980). Epilepsy and neuron loss in the hippocampus. Epilepsia 21:617–629.
Damasio, H., R. G. Maurer, A. R. Damasio, and H. C. Chui (1980). Computerized tomographic
scan findings in patients with autistic behavior. Arch. Neurol. 37:504–510.
Davidovitch, M., B. Patterson, and P. Gartside (1996). Head circumference measurements in
children with autism. J. Child Neurol. 11:389–393.
Dawson, G., J. Munson, S. J. Webb, T. Nalty, R. Abbott, and K. Toth (2007). Rate of head
growth decelerates and symptoms worsen in the second year of life in autism. Biol.
Psychiatry 61:458–464.
Dementieva, Y. A., D. D. Vance, S. L. Donnelly, L. A. Elston, C. M. Wolpert, S. A. Ravan,
G. R. DeLong, R. K. Abramson, H. H. Wright, and M. L. Cuccaro (2005). Accelerated head
growth in early development of individuals with autism. Pediatr. Neurol. 32:102–108.
Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
(2007). Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report 56:1–28.
Dissanayake, C., Q. M. Bui, R. Huggins, and D. Z. Loesch (2006). Growth in stature and head
circumference in high-functioning autism and Asperger disorder during the first 3 years
of life. Dev. Psychopathol. 18:381–393.
Earnest, M. P., G. E. Thomas, R. A. Eden, and K. F. Hossack (1992). The sudden unexplained
death syndrome in epilepsy: Demographic, clinical, and postmortem features. Epilepsia
33:310–316.
Ehlert, U., J. Gaab, and M. Heinrichs (2001). Psychoneuroendocrinological contributions to
the etiology of depression, posttraumatic stress disorder, and stress-related bodily disor-
ders: The role of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis. Biol. Psychol. 57:141–152.
Erden-Inal, M., E. Sunal, and G. Kanbak (2002). Age-related changes in the glutathione redox
system. Cell Biochem. Funct. 20:61–66.
Ericksson, P. S., E. Perfilieva, T. Bjork-Eriksson, A. M. Alborn, C. Nordborg, D. A. Peterson,
and F. H. Gage (1998). Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus. Nat. Med.
4:1313–1317.
Evans, T., S. L. Siedlak, L. Lu, X. Fu, Z. Wang, W. R. McGinnis, E. Fakhoury, R. J. Castellani,
S. L. Hazen, W. L. Walsh, A. T. Levis, R. G. Salomon, M. A. Smith, G. Perry, and X. Zhu
(2008). The autistic phenotype exhibits a remarkably localized modification of brain
protein by products of free radical-induced lipid oxidation. Am J Biochem. Biotechnol.,
Special Issue on Autism Spectrum Disorders 4:61–72.
Fairhall, S. L. and A. Ishai (2007). Effective connectivity within the distributed cortical
network for face perception. Cereb. Cortex 17:2400–2406.
Type, Topography, and Sequelae of Neuropathological Changes 25
Fehlow, P., K. Bernstein, A. Tennstedt, and F. Walther (1993). Early infantile autism and
excessive aerophagy with symptomatic megacolon and ileus in a case of Ehlers-Danlos
syndrome. Padiatr. Grenzgeb. 31:259–267.
Ferguson, J. N., L. J. Young, E. F. Hearn, M. M. Matzuk, T. R. Insel, and J. T. Winslow (2000).
Social amnesia in mice lacking the oxytocin gene. Nat. Genet. 25:284–288.
Fidler, D. J., J. N. Bailey, and S. L. Smalley (2000). Macrocephaly in autism and other perva-
sive developmental disorders. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 42:737–740.
Filipek, P. A. (1996). Brief report: Neuroimaging in autism: The state of the science 1995.
J. Autism Dev. Disord. 26:211–215.
Filipek, P. A., P. J. Accardo, G. T. Baranek, E. H. Cook Jr., G. Dawson, B. Gordon, J. S. Gravel,
C. P. Johnson, R. J. Kallen, S. E. Levy, N. J. Minshew, S. Ozonoff, B. M. Prizant,
I. Rapin, S. J. Rogers, W. L. Stone, S. Teplin, R. F. Tuchman, and F. R. Volkmar (1999).
The screening and diagnosis of autistic spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord.
29:439–484.
Folstein, S. E. and B. Rosen-Sheidley (2001). Genetics of autism: Complex aetiology for a
heterogeneous disorder. Nat. Rev. Genet. 2:943–955.
Folstein, S. E. and M. L. Rutter (1988). Autism: Familial aggregation and genetic implications.
J. Autism Dev. Disord. 18:3–30.
Fombonne E. (2003). Epidemiological surveys of autism and other pervasive developmental
disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 33:365–382.
Fombonne, E., B. Roge, J. Claverie, S. Courty, and J. Fremolle (1999). Microcephaly and
macrocephaly in autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 29:113–119.
Friede, R. L. (1975). Developmental Neuropathology. Springer-Verlag, Berlin.
Fuh, J. L. and S. J. Wang (1995). Caudate hemorrhage: Clinical features, neuropsychological
assessments and radiological findings. Clin. Neurol. Neurosurg. 97:296–299.
Gabreels, B. A., D. F. Swaab, D. P. de Kleijn, N. G. Seidah, J. W. Van de Loo, W. J. Van de
Ven, G. J. Martens, and F. W. Van Leeuwen (1998). Attenuation of the polypeptide 7B2,
prohormone convertase PC2, and vasopressin in the hypothalamus of some Prader-Willi
patients: Indications for a processing defect. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 83:591–599.
Gabriels, R. L., M. L. Cuccaro, D. E. Hill, B. J. Ivers, and E. Goldson (2005). Repetitive
behaviors in autism: Relationships with associated clinical features. Res. Dev. Disabil.
26:169–181.
Gaffney, G. R., L. Y. Tsai, S. Kuperman, and S. Minchin (1987). Cerebellar structure in autism.
Am. J. Dis. Child 141:1330–1332.
Gainer, H., M. O. Lively, and M. Morris (1995). Immunological and related techniques for study-
ing neurohypophyseal peptide-processing pathways. Methods Neurosci. 23:195–207.
Garber, H. J. and E. R. Ritvo (1992). Magnetic resonance imaging of the posterior fossa in
autistic adults. Am. J. Psychiatry 149:245–247.
Gessaga, E. C. and H. Urich (1985). The cerebellum of epileptics. Clin. Neuropathol. 4:238–245.
Ghaziuddin, M., L. Y. Tsai, and N. Ghaziuddin (1992). Autism in Down’s syndrome:
Presentation and diagnosis. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 36 (Pt 5):449–456.
Gillberg, C. (1998). Chromosomal disorders and autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 28:415–425.
Gillberg C. and M. Coleman (1996). Autism and medical disorders: A review of the literature.
Dev. Med. Child. Neurol. 38:191–202.
Gillberg, C. and L. de Souza (2002). Head circumference in autism, Asperger syndrome, and
ADHD: A comparative study. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 44:296–300.
Glabe, C. (2001). Intracellular mechanisms of amyloid accumulation and pathogenesis in
Alzheimer’s disease. J. Mol. Neurosci. 17:137–145.
Green, L. A., D. Fein, C. Modahl, C. Feinstein, L. Waterhouse, and M. Morris (2001). Oxytocin
and autistic disorder: Alteration in peptide forms. Biol. Psychiatry 50:609–613.
Grelotti, D., I. Gauthier, and R. T. Shultz (2001). Social interest and the development of
cortical face specialization: What autism teaches us about face processing. Dev.
Psychobiol. 40:213–225.
26 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Mackenzie, I. R. and L. A. Miller (1994). Senile plaques in temporal lobe epilepsy. Acta
Neuropathol. 87:504–510.
Maeshima, T., F. Shutoh, S. Hamada, K. Senzaki, K. Hamaguchi-Hamada, R. Ito, and
N. Okado (1998). Serotonin2A receptor-like immunoreactivity in rat cerebellar Purkinje
cells. Neurosci. Lett. 252:72–74.
Manaye, K. F., D. L. Lei, Y. Tizabi, M. I. vila-Garcia, P. R. Mouton, and P. H. Kelly (2005).
Selective neuron loss in the paraventricular nucleus of hypothalamus in patients suffering
from major depression and bipolar disorder. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 64:224–229.
Martin, G. R. and P. P. Humphrey (1994). Receptors for 5-hydroxytryptamine: Current
perspectives on classification and nomenclature. Neuropharmacology 33:261–273.
Mason-Brothers, A., E. R. Ritvo, C. Pingree, P. B. Petersen, W. R. Jenson, W. M. McMahon,
B. J. Freeman, L. B. Jorde, M. J. Spencer, and A. Mo (1990). The UCLA-University
of Utah epidemiologic survey of autism: Prenatal, perinatal, and postnatal factors.
Pediatrics 86:514–519.
McClelland, J. L. (2000). The basis of hyperspecificity in autism: A preliminary suggestion
based on properties of neural nets. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 30:497–502.
Mendez, M. F., N. L. Adams, and K. S. Lewandowski (1989). Neurobehavioral changes
associated with caudate lesions. Neurology 39:349–354.
Menendez, M. (2005). Down syndrome, Alzheimer’s disease and seizures. Brain Dev
27:246–252.
Miles, J. H., L. L. Hadden, T. N. Takahashi, and R. E. Hillman (2000). Head circumfer-
ence is an independent clinical finding associated with autism. Am. J. Med. Genet.
95:339–350.
Miles, J. H., T. N. Takahashi, S. Bagby, P. K. Sahota, D. F. Vaslow, C. H. Wang, R. E. Hillman,
and J. E. Farmer (2005). Essential versus complex autism: Definition of fundamental
prognostic subtypes. Am. J. Med. Genet. A 135:171–180.
Ming, X., T. P. Stein, M. Brimacombe, W. G. Johnson, G. H. Lambert, and G. C. Wagner
(2005). Increased excretion of a lipid peroxidation biomarker in autism. Prostaglandins
Leukot. Essent. Fatty Acids 73:379–384.
Mitchell, B. F., X. Fang, and S. Wong (1998). Role of carboxy-extended forms of oxytocin in
the rat uterus in the process of parturition. Biol. Reprod. 59:1321–1327.
Miyazaki, K., N. Narita, R. Sakuta, T. Miyahara, H. Naruse, N. Okado, and M. Narita (2004).
Serum neurotrophin concentrations in autism and mental retardation: A pilot study.
Brain Dev. 26:292–295.
Muhle, R., S. V. Trentacoste, and I. Rapin (2004). The genetics of autism. Pediatrics
113:e472–e486.
Muller, R. A. (2007). The study of autism as a distributed disorder. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil.
Res. Rev. 13:85–95.
Murakami, J. W., E. Courchesne, G. A. Press, R. Yeung-Courchesne, and J. R. Hesselink
(1989). Reduced cerebellar hemisphere size and its relationship to vermal hypoplasia in
autism. Arch. Neurol. 46:689–694.
Murer, M. G., Q. Yan, and R. Raisman-Vozari (2001). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor in
the control human brain, and in Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease. Prog.
Neurobiol. 63:71–124.
Narita, N., M. Kato, M. Tazoe, K. Miyazaki, M. Narita, and N. Okado (2002). Increased
monoamine concentration in the brain and blood of fetal thalidomide- and valproic acid-
exposed rat: Putative animal models for autism. Pediatr. Res. 52:576–579.
Nashef, L., F. Walker, P. Allen, J. W. Sander, S. D. Shorvon, and D. R. Fish (1996). Apnoea and
bradycardia during epileptic seizures: Relation to sudden death in epilepsy. J. Neurol.
Neurosurg. Psychiatry 60:297–300.
Nawa, H., J. Carnahan, and C. Gall (1995). BDNF protein measured by a novel enzyme immu-
noassay in normal brain and after seizure: Partial disagreement with mRNA levels. Eur.
J. Neurosci. 7:1527–1535.
30 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Piven, J., K. Saliba, J. Bailey, and S. Arndt (1997a). An MRI study of autism: The cerebellum
revisited. Neurology 49:546–551.
Piven, J., J. Bailey, B. J. Ranson, and S. Arndt (1997b). An MRI study of the corpus callosum
in autism. Am. J. Psychiatry 154:1051–1056.
Piven, J., J. Bailey, B. J. Ranson, and S. Arndt (1998). No difference in hippocampus volume
detected on magnetic resonance imaging in autistic individuals. J. Autism Dev. Disord.
28:105–110.
Poldrack, R. A., V. Prabhakaran, C. A. Seger, and J. D. Gabrieli (1999). Striatal activation
during acquisition of a cognitive skill. Neuropsychology 13:564–574.
Popik, P. and J. M. Van Ree (1992). Long-term facilitation of social recognition in rats by
vasopressin related peptides: A structure–activity study. Life Sci. 50:567–572.
Popik, P., J. Vetulani, and J. M. Van Ree (1992). Low doses of oxytocin facilitate social recog-
nition in rats. Psychopharmacology 106:71–74.
Prasher, V. P. and D. J. Clarke (1996). Case report: Challenging behaviour in a young adult
with Down’s syndrome and autism. Brit. J. Learning Disab. 24:167–169.
Raggenbass, M. 2001. Vasopressin- and oxytocin-induced activity in the central nervous system:
Electrophysiological studies using in-vitro systems. Prog. Neurobiol. 64:307–326.
Rao, V. V., C. Loffler, J. Battey, and I. Hansmann (1992). The human gene for oxytocin-
neurophysin I (OXT) is physically mapped to chromosome 20p13 by in situ hybridization.
Cytogenet. Cell Genet. 61:271–273.
Rapin, I. (1996). Preschool Children with Inadequate Communication: Developmental
Language Disorder, Autism, Low IQ. MacKeith Press, London.
Rathbun, W. and M. J. Druse (1985). Dopamine, serotonin, and acid metabolites in brain
regions from the developing offspring of ethanol-treated rats. J. Neurochem. 44:57–62.
Redcay, E. and E. Courchesne.(2005). When is the brain enlarged in autism? A meta-analysis
of all brain size reports. Biol. Psychiatry 58:1–9.
Reeves, A. L., K. E. Nollet, D. W. Klass, F. W. Sharbrough, and E. L. So (1996). The ictal
bradycardia syndrome. Epilepsia 37:983–987.
Risse, S. C., T. H. Lampe, T. D. Bird, D. Nochlin, S. M. Sumi, T. Keenan, L. Cubberley,
E. Peskind, and M. A. Raskind (1990). Myoclonus, seizures, and paratonia in Alzheimer
disease. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 4:217–225.
Ritvo, E. R., B. J. Freeman, A. B. Scheibel, T. Duong, H. Robinson, D. Guthrie, and A. Ritvo
(1986). Lower Purkinje cell counts in the cerebella of four autistic subjects: Initial findings
of the UCLA-NSAC Autopsy Research Report. Am. J. Psychiatry 143:862–866.
Rodier, P. M., J. L. Ingram, B. Tisdale, S. Nelson, and J. Romano (1996). Embryological
origin for autism: Developmental anomalies of the cranial nerve motor nuclei. J. Comp.
Neurol. 370:247–261.
Rogers, S. J., L. Bennetto, R. McEvoy, and B. F. Pennington (1996). Imitation and panto-
mime in high-functioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Child Dev.
67:2060–2073.
Roth, B. L. (1994). Multiple serotonin receptors: Clinical and experimental aspects. Ann. Clin.
Psychiatry 6:67–78.
Rutter, M., A. Bailey, P. Bolton, and C. A. Le (1994). Autism and known medical conditions:
Myth and substance. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 35:311–322.
Saitoh, O. and E. Courchesne (1998). Magnetic resonance imaging study of the brain in
autism. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 52 Suppl:S219–S222.
Sajdel-Sulkowska, E. M., B. Lipinski, H. Windom, T. Audhya, and W. McGinnis (2008).
Oxidative stress in autism: Cerebellar 3-nitrotyrosine levels. Am. J. Biochem. Biotechnol.
(Special Issue on Autism Spectrum Disorders) 4:73–84.
Sarnat, H. B. and L. Flores-Sarnat (2004). Integrative classification of morphology and
molecular genetics in central nervous system malformations. Am. J. Med. Genet.
A 126A:386–392.
32 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Stromland, K., V. Nordin, M. Miller, B. Akerstrom, and C. Gillberg (1994). Autism in thalido-
mide embryopathy: A population study. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 36:351–356.
Sutcliffe, J. S., R. J. Delahanty, H. C. Prasad, J. L. McCauley, Q. Han, L. Jiang, C. Li, S. E.
Folstein, and R. D. Blakely (2005). Allelic heterogeneity at the serotonin transporter
locus (SLC6A4) confers susceptibility to autism and rigid-compulsive behaviors. Am.
J. Hum. Genet. 77:265–279.
Sutula, T. P. and A. Pitkanen (2001). More evidence for seizure-induced neuron loss: Is
hippocampal sclerosis both cause and effect of epilepsy? Neurology 57:169–170.
Swaab, D. F. (1997). Prader-Willi syndrome and the hypothalamus. Acta Paediatr. Suppl.
423:50–54.
Szatmari, P., M. B. Jones, L. Zwaigenbaum, and J. E. MacLean (1998). Genetics of autism:
Overview and new directions. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 28:351–368.
Szweda, P. A., M. Camouse, K. C. Lundberg, T. D. Oberley, and L. I. Szweda (2003). Aging,
lipofuscin formation, and free radical-mediated inhibition of cellular proteolytic systems.
Ageing Res. Rev. 2:383–405.
Tapia-Arancibia, L., F. Rage, L. Givalois, and S. Arancibia (2004). Physiology of BDNF:
Focus on hypothalamic function. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 25:77–107.
Tasch, E., F. Cendes, L. M. Li, F. Dubeau, F. Andermann, and D. L. Arnold (1999).
Neuroimaging evidence of progressive neuronal loss and dysfunction in temporal lobe
epilepsy. Ann. Neurol. 45:568–576.
Terman, A. and U. T. Brunk (2004). Lipofuscin. Int. J. Biochem. Cell Biol. 36:1400–1404.
Thom, M., B. Griffin, J. W. Sander, and F. Scaravilli (1999). Amygdala sclerosis in sudden and
unexpected death in epilepsy. Epilepsy Res. 37:53–62.
Torsdottir, G., J. Kristinsson, S. Sveinbjornsdottir, J. Snaedal, and T. Johannesson (1999).
Copper, ceruloplasmin, superoxide dismutase and iron parameters in Parkinson’s disease.
Pharmacol. Toxicol. 85:239–243.
Townsend, J., E. Courchesne, J. Covington, M. Westerfield, N. S. Harris, P. Lyden, T. P. Lowry,
and G. A. Press (1999). Spatial attention deficits in patients with acquired or developmental
cerebellar abnormality. J. Neurosci. 19:5632–5643.
Tuchman, R. F., I. Rapin, and S. Shinner (1991). Autistic and dysphasic children II: Epilepsy.
Pediatrics 88:1219–1225.
Tuchman, R. F. and I. Rapin (2002). Epilepsy in autism. Lancet Neurol. 1:352–358.
van Kooten, I., S. J. Palmen, P. von Cappeln, H. W. Steinbusch, H. Korr, H. Heinsen, P. R. Hof,
H. van Engeland, and C. Schmitz (2008). Neurons in the fusiform gyrus are fewer and
smaller in autism. Brain 131:987–999.
Velez, L. and L. M. Selwa (2003). Seizure disorders in the elderly. Am. Fam. Physician
67:325–332.
Voelbel, G. T., M. E. Bates, J. F. Buckman, G. Pandina, and R. L. Hendren (2006). Caudate
nucleus volume and cognitive performance: Are they related in childhood psychopathology?
Biol. Psychiatry 60:942–950.
Volkmar, F. R., A. Carter, S. S. Sparrow, and D. V. Cicchetti (1993). Quantifying social devel-
opment in autism. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 32:627–632.
Vorstman, J. A. S., W. G. Staal, E. van Daalen, H. van Engeland, P. F. R. Hochstenbach,
and L. Franke (2006). Identification of novel autism candidate regions through
analysis of reported cytogenetic abnormalities associated with autism. Mol. Psych.
11:18–28.
Waiter, G. D., J. H. Williams, A. D. Murray, A. Gilchrist, D. I. Perrett, and A. Whiten (2004).
A voxel-based investigation of brain structure in male adolescents with autistic spectrum
disorder. Neuroimage 22:619–625.
Wakabayashi, S. (1979). A case of infantile autism associated with Down’s syndrome. J. Autism
Dev. Disord. 9:31–36.
34 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
CONTENTS
2.1 Oxidative Stress Is a Common Feature during
Neurodegeneration in the Brain .................................................................... 36
2.2 Peripheral Oxidative Stress in Autism .......................................................... 37
2.3 Oxidative Stress in the Autistic Brain ........................................................... 38
2.4 Axon Is the Primary Site for Oxidative Damage in the Autistic Brain:
Possible Mechanisms Underlying Functional Underconnectivity ................40
2.5 Formation of CEP–Adduct Is Not a Postmortem Artifact ............................ 41
2.6 Conclusion ..................................................................................................... 43
Acknowledgments....................................................................................................44
References ................................................................................................................44
35
36 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
enzymes responsible for the removal of oxidative species are also seen in autism.
Plasma concentrations of reduced glutathione (GSH) are lower (4.1 ± 0.5 μmol/L)
for autistics compared with healthy controls (7.6 ± 1.4 μmol/L), p < 0.001 (James
et al., 2004, 2006). Glutathione peroxidase, an antioxidant enzyme, requires
GSH as a cofactor to break down oxidative species and lower levels of glutathione
peroxidase (−44.4%) are found in erythrocytes of autistic individuals compared
with controls (Golse et al., 1978; Yorbik et al., 2002). These lower levels of glu-
tathione peroxidase can lead to a decrease in the ability of the cells to remove
H 2O2 formed by the action of ZnCuSOD. In autistic patients, the combination of
an increase in the formation of oxidative species by SOD and a reduction in the
removal of these species by glutathione peroxidase might result in an overall
increase in oxidative stress.
Along with apparent genetic connections and the presence of systemic oxida-
tive stress indicators, families with a high rate of autoimmune diseases and immune
abnormalities also have higher rates of autism, for example, autoantibodies IgG, IgM,
IgE, and IgA against brain proteins are present in some ASD patients (Jyonouchi
et al., 2001) and proinflammatory cytokines can be produced in excessive amounts
by peripheral blood mononuclear cells from autistic children (Jyonouchi et al., 2001).
Inflammation including autoimmune conditions can cause an increase in oxida-
tive stress since macrophage activity increases during inflammation and causes an
increase in the production of ROS by the macrophages. Autoantibodies can be used
as a marker for an increase in immune responses, and these are found at higher
levels in patients with autism. For example, serum IgG antibrain autoantibodies are
present in 35% of children with ASD compared with 5% from healthy children and
IgM autoantibodies are present in 50% of children with ASD compared with 10%
of controls (Vojdani et al., 2002). In another similar study, sera from 40 healthy
subjects and 40 autistic children was analyzed for the presence of IgG, IgM, and
IgA antibodies against nine neuron-specific antigens and three encephalitogenic and
cross-reactive proteins. For one of these antigens, namely neurofilament, only up to
10% of controls had IgA, IgG, or IgM antibodies against this antigen compared to up
to 57.5% of subjects with autism (Gu et al., 2003).
Our previous studies demonstrated significantly elevated levels of carboxyethyl
pyrrole (CEP) epitopes and the corresponding autoantibodies in the blood of age-
related macular degeneration (AMD) patients compared with healthy age- and
sex-matched controls (Miyagi et al., 2002). However, while we measured levels
of CEP and iso[4]levuglandin E2 [iso[4]LGE2]–protein adducts and levels of CEP
and iso[4]LGE2 –protein autoantibodies in plasma from patients with documented
ASD with age-matched healthy controls, we found no significant difference (Evans
et al., 2008).
lipid peroxides and other products that contribute to loss of membrane function and
cellular damage. Lipids are likely very important in the physiology and function
of the brain, and a decrease in their function and integrity likely plays a role in the
etiology of neurodegenerative disorders. Membrane damage due to lipid peroxida-
tion could lead to a decrease in functional neurons or a decrease in the conductive
ability of axons in the white matter and a decrease in conductivity between different
portions of the brain.
Lipid hydroperoxides and peroxidation products of common cellular lipids are
produced in tissue after death, are unstable and short lived, making analysis of the
levels of these substances in autopsy tissue difficult (Gu et al., 2003). Therefore, to
avoid problems with directly measuring oxidative species, it is possible to identify
other substances that are modified by these oxidative processes. We have recently
developed and characterized a method of detecting CEP adducts using a specific
antibody (Evans et al., 2008). CEP adduct is derived exclusively from free radical–
induced oxidative cleavage of docosahexaenoates, e.g., the docohexaenoic acid
(DHA) ester of 2-lysophosphatidylcholine (DHA-PC), to afford 4-hydroxy-
7-oxohept-5-enoic acid (HOHA) ester of 2-lysophosphatidylcholine (HOHA-PC)
that then reacts with protein to generate CEP modifications of the ε-amino groups
of lysyl residues (Figure 2.1) (Gu et al., 2003). DHA is the most oxidizable fatty acid
in humans, and while a minor lipid in most tissues, it is rich in specific regions of
the brain and retina (Smith et al., 1994; Takeda et al., 2000). Since DHA is present
in the brain at such high levels, CEP adducts can be used as a reliable and sensitive
dosimeter for local oxidative damage (Skinner et al., 1993; Alvarez et al., 1994) and
it provides a reasonable way for detecting oxidative damage in brain tissue in autism.
We also used antibodies against iso[4]LGE2 –protein adducts that arise exclusively
through free radical–induced cyclooxygenation of arachidonates, e.g., the arachi-
donic acid (AA) ester AA-PC, and subsequent adduction of an intermediate iso[4]
LGE2-PC with protein (Figure 2.1). Iso[4]LGE2–protein adducts identify cumulative
damage to cells from oxidative injury, such as that associated with inflammation
(Money et al., 1971; Comi et al., 1999; Poliakov et al., 2003; Sweeten et al., 2003).
In addition to oxidative modification, oxidative stress in biological systems usually
elicits an antioxidant response and therefore some antioxidant enzymes such as heme
oxygenase-1 (HO-1) that are induced by oxidative stress are also widely used as an
acceptable oxidative stress marker in neurodegeneration (Chung et al., 2004).
Using immunocytochemistry, CEP was found localized primarily in the white
matter, often extending well into the gray matter of axons in every case of autism
examined, and was not found in any age-matched or even older control cases that
were analyzed (Evans et al., 2008). Clearly, this pattern of staining is a hallmark of
the autistic brain. The white matter is composed mostly of axons, and staining in
Protein
OH
O2 OHC N (CH2)2COOH
C2H5 4 (CH2)2COOH (CH2)2COOH
Docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) HOHA
CEP
(A) (B)
FIGURE 2.2 In autism, cellular processes contain choline acetyltransferase (A), in a pattern
similar to that seen for CEP modifications (B).
research is warranted on the direct connection between white matter and con-
nectivity in autism, our findings suggested a possible mechanism regarding how
oxidative damage may be involved in underconnectivity and thereby contribute to
the development of disease.
More recently, to characterize the identity of the neurons affected by oxidative
modification, we have explored the association of CEP with various abnormal systems
(i.e., GABAergic, glutamatergic, cholinergic, and serotoninergic systems) in autism
and found that the immunostaining pattern of choline acetyltransferase, a marker
of cholinergic neurons, was very similar to the pattern seen with the CEP antibody
(Figure 2.2), suggesting that cholinergic neurons may be more vulnerable to CEP
modifications in autism.
Consistent with the immunocytochemical detection of CEP primarily in axons,
our further biochemical study suggested that neurofilament heavy (NFH) chain may
be the primary target for CEP modification. Western blot analysis of CEP antibody
revealed a band around 200 kDa, similar to the molecular size of NFH and, coimmu-
noprecipitation experiments using autism brain homogenates confi rmed that NFH is
modified by CEP. Indeed, NFH also demonstrated an immunostaining pattern very
similar to that of CEP in autism tissues and a coimmunostaining assay confirmed the
colocalization of CEP and NFH within the same processes (not shown). This is the
first study implicating a specific protein in autism, which has the potential to advance
our understanding of the mechanisms underlying functional underconnectivity.
(A) (B)
(C) (D)
FIGURE 2.3 In a rat model, CEP is localized to cellular processes within the brain
strikingly similar to the CEP localization in autism. To test for any postmortem changes
in CEP modifications, rat pup brains were fi xed following either 0 h (A), 4 h (B), or 8 h (C)
postmortem delay. All rats displayed similar levels of CEP and following image analysis
showed no significant differences (not shown). Paralleling the fi ndings in human autism
cases, CEP localization overlaps with neurofilament protein accumulation on adjacent sec-
tions (C, CEP and D, neurofi lament protein). Scale bar = 50 μm.
this model, rat pups were kept at room temperature for different interval (0–8 h) prior to
brain dissection to simulate increased PMI. All rat pups from all dams tested accumu-
lated CEP in areas of the brain that overlapped significantly with neurofilaments accu-
mulation as seen using monoclonal antibody SMI-31 (Covance) (Figure 2.3). Image
analysis was performed to measure both the percent area stained and the intensity of
labeling for both CEP and neurofilaments. No correlation was found between either the
amount or intensity of CEP accumulation and PMI. These data strongly argue for neu-
rofilament protein being a specific target for CEP modification (Evans et al., 2008).
To further correlate the rat model data with human disease, the levels of CEP
modifications were compared in autism and control brain sections, which had
varying and disparate PMI. There was no significant correlation between CEP and
PMI, with times ranging from 13 to 39 h (Figure 2.4). Even in the control case with
the longest PMI (36 h), there was no recognizable immunoreactivity to CEP (data
not shown). Densitometric analysis of the staining in these autistic cases clearly
did not show any significant correlation with differences in PMI (Figure 2.4).
Taken together, the human and rat models provide strong evidence that CEP
modifications do not accumulate after death, and therefore may be a direct result
Evidence for Oxidative Damage in the Autistic Brain 43
(A) (B)
35
FIGURE 2.4 In autism, CEP is localized specifically to processes within the brain.
Morphologically, no differences are apparent between cases collected following a relatively
short PMI of 13 h (A), or 24 h (B), or even after a long PMI of 39 h (C). Computer-assisted
image analysis of the intensity of CEP accumulation in nine cases of autism found no signifi-
cant correlation when compared with PMI (D). Scale bar = 50 μm.
of oxidative damage in the living brain. Although this excludes a direct correlation
between postmortem time and staining, this does not account for any differences in
the presence of these substances in the rat brain.
2.6 CONCLUSION
There is ample evidence suggesting systemic oxidative stress in autism patients
and evidence for brain oxidative stress is now beginning to accumulate. Our
immunocytochemical and biochemical studies provide the fi rst experimental evi-
dence demonstrating lipid modification in autistic brain and suggests CEP– and
iso[4]LGE2 –protein adducts, products of lipid peroxidation, as possible hallmark
oxidative stress markers for the autistic brain. Supplementary animal experiments
show that CEP formation is unlikely due to postmortem artifact, supporting CEP
as a specific oxidative stress marker. From a structural perspective, our findings
suggest that axons of cholinergic neurons in the white matter are the primary site
of oxidative damage. At the molecular level, we have identified NFH to be the
major target for CEP modification. Our findings not only support the notion that
brain oxidative stress plays an important role in autism but now warrant future
in-depth mechanistic studies, which have the potential to provide new targets for
therapeutic efforts.
44 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This research was supported by Autism Research Institute. Human tissue was
obtained from the NICHD Brain and Tissue Bank for Developmental Disorders at
the University of Maryland under contracts N01-HD-4-3368 and N01-HD-4-3383.
REFERENCES
Ahlsen, G., Rosengren, L., Belfrage, M., Palm, A., Haglid, K., Hamberger, A., and Gillberg, C.
(1993). Glial fibrillary acidic protein in the cerebrospinal fluid of children with autism
and other neuropsychiatric disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 33:734–743.
Alvarez, R. A., Aguirre, G. D., Acland, G. M., and Anderson, R. E. (1994). Docosapentaenoic
acid is converted to docosahexaenoic acid in the retinas of normal and prcd-affected
miniature poodle dogs. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 35:402–408.
Bailey, A., Le Couteur, A., Gottesman, I., Bolton, P., Simonoff, E., Yuzda, E., and Rutter, M.
(1995). Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: Evidence from a British twin study.
Psychol. Med. 25:63–77.
Bailey, A., Luthert, P., Dean, A., Harding, B., Janota, I., Montgomery, M., Rutter, M., and
Lantos, P. (1998). A clinicopathological study of autism. Brain 121 (Pt 5):889–905.
Chauhan, A. and Chauhan, V. (2006). Oxidative stress in autism. Pathophysiology
13:171–181.
Chauhan, A., Chauhan, V., Brown, W. T., and Cohen, I. (2004). Oxidative stress in autism:
Increased lipid peroxidation and reduced serum levels of ceruloplasmin and transferrin—
the antioxidant proteins. Life Sci. 75:2539–2549.
Chez, M. G., Buchanan, C. P., Aimonovitch, M. C., Becker, M., Schaefer, K., Black, C., and
Komen, J. (2002). Double-blind, placebo-controlled study of l-carnosine supplemen-
tation in children with autistic spectrum disorders. J. Child Neurol. 17:833–837.
Chung, M. K., Dalton, K. M., Alexander, A. L., and Davidson, R. J. (2004). Less white matter
concentration in autism: 2D voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage 23:242–251.
Comi, A. M., Zimmerman, A. W., Frye, V. H., Law, P. A., and Peeden, J. N. (1999). Familial
clustering of autoimmune disorders and evaluation of medical risk factors in autism.
J. Child Neurol. 14:388–394.
Connolly, A. M., Chez, M., Streif, E. M., Keeling, R. M., Golumbek, P. T., Kwon, J. M., Riviello,
J. J., Robinson, R. G., Neuman, R. J., and Deuel, R. M. (2006). Brain-derived neurotrophic
factor and autoantibodies to neural antigens in sera of children with autistic spectrum
disorders, Landau–Kleffner syndrome, and epilepsy. Biol. Psychiatry 59:354–363.
Courchesne, E., Townsend, J., and Saitoh, O. (1994). The brain in infantile autism: Posterior
fossa structures are abnormal. Neurology 44:214–223.
Dolske, M. C., Spollen, J., McKay, S., Lancashire, E., and Tolbert, L. (1993). A preliminary
trial of ascorbic acid as supplemental therapy for autism. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol.
Biol. Psychiatry 17:765–774.
Evans, T., Siedlak, S. L., Lu, L., Fu, X., Wang, Z., McGinnis, W. R., Fakhoury, E., Castellani,
R. J., Hazen, S. L., Walsh, W. L., Lewis, A. T., Salomon, R. G., Smith, M. A., Perry, G.,
and Zhu, X. (2008). The autistic phenotype exhibits a remarkably localized modification
of brain protein by products of free radical-induced lipid oxidation. Am. J. Biochem.
Biotechnol. (Special Issue on Autism Spectrum Disorders) 4:61–72.
Filipek, P. A., Juranek, J., Smith, M., Mays, L. Z., Ramos, E. R., Bocian, M., Masser-Frye, D.,
Laulhere, T. M., Modahl, C., Spence, M. A., and Gargus, J. J. (2003). Mitochondrial dys-
function in autistic patients with 15q inverted duplication. Ann. Neurol. 53:801–804.
Golse, B., Debray-Ritzen, P., Durosay, P., Puget, K., and Michelson, A. M. (1978). Alterations
in two enzymes: Superoxide dismutase and glutathione peroxidase in developmental
infantile psychosis (infantile autism). Rev. Neurol. 134:699–705.
Evidence for Oxidative Damage in the Autistic Brain 45
Grice, D. E. and Buxbaum, J. D. (2006). The genetics of autism spectrum disorders. Neuromol.
Med. 8:451–460.
Gu, X., Meer, S. G., Miyagi, M., Rayborn, M. E., Hollyfield, J. G., Crabb, J. W., and Salomon,
R. G. (2003). Carboxyethylpyrrole protein adducts and autoantibodies, biomarkers for
age-related macular degeneration. J. Biol. Chem. 278:42027–42035.
Halliwell, B., Zhao, K., and Whiteman, M. (1999). Nitric oxide and peroxynitrite. The ugly,
the uglier and the not so good: A personal view of recent controversies. Free Radic. Res.
31:651–669.
Horvath, K. and Perman, J. A. (2002). Autism and gastrointestinal symptoms. Curr.
Gastroenterol. Rep. 4:251–258.
James, S. J., Cutler, P., Melnyk, S., Jernigan, S., Janak, L., Gaylor, D. W., and Neubrander, J. A.
(2004). Metabolic biomarkers of increased oxidative stress and impaired methylation
capacity in children with autism. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 80:1611–1617.
James, S. J., Melnyk, S., Jernigan, S., Cleves, M. A., Halsted, C. H., Wong, D. H., Cutler, P.,
Bock, K., Boris, M., Bradstreet, J. J., Baker, S. M., and Gaylor, D. W. (2006). Metabolic
endophenotype and related genotypes are associated with oxidative stress in children
with autism. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 141B:947–956.
Joosten, E. A. and Houweling, D. A. (2004). Local acute application of BDNF in the lesioned
spinal cord anti-inflammatory and anti-oxidant effects. Neuroreport 15:1163–1166.
Jyonouchi, H., Sun, S., and Le, H. (2001). Proinflammatory and regulatory cytokine produc-
tion associated with innate and adaptive immune responses in children with autism
spectrum disorders and developmental regression. J. Neuroimmunol. 120:170–179.
Keller, F. and Persico, A. M. (2003). The neurobiological context of autism. Mol. Neurobiol.
28:1–22.
Kleijnen, J. and Knipschild, P. (1991). Niacin and vitamin B6 in mental functioning: A review
of controlled trials in humans. Biol. Psychiatry 29:931–941.
Laurence, J. A. and Fatemi, S. H. (2005). Glial fibrillary acidic protein is elevated in
superior frontal, parietal and cerebellar cortices of autistic subjects. Cerebellum
4:206–210.
Lee, H. G., Perry, G., Moreira, P. I., Garrett, M. R., Liu, Q., Zhu, X., Takeda, A., Nunomura,
A., and Smith, M. A. (2005). Tau phosphorylation in Alzheimer’s disease: Pathogen or
protector? Trends Mol. Med. 11:164–169.
McGinnis, W. R. (2004). Oxidative stress in autism. Altern. Ther. Health Med. 10:22–36.
Meagher, E. A. and FitzGerald, G. A. (2000). Indices of lipid peroxidation in vivo: Strengths
and limitations. Free Radic. Biol. Med. 28:1745–1750.
Ming, X., Stein, T. P., Brimacombe, M., Johnson, W. G., Lambert, G. H., and Wagner, G. C.
(2005). Increased excretion of a lipid peroxidation biomarker in autism. Prostaglandins
Leukot. Essent. Fatty Acids 73:379–384.
Miyagi, M., Sakaguchi, H., Darrow, R. M., Yan, L., West, K. A., Aulak, K. S., Stuehr, D. J.,
Hollyfield, J. G., Organisciak, D. T., and Crabb, J. W. (2002). Evidence that light
modulates protein nitration in rat retina. Mol. Cell Proteomics 1:293–303.
Miyazaki, K., Narita, N., Sakuta, R., Miyahara, T., Naruse, H., Okado, N., and Narita, M.
(2004). Serum neurotrophin concentrations in autism and mental retardation: A pilot
study. Brain Dev. 26:292–295.
Money, J., Bobrow, N. A., and Clarke, F. C. (1971). Autism and autoimmune disease: A family
study. J. Autism Child Schizophr. 1:146–160.
Nunomura, A., Perry, G., Aliev, G., Hirai, K., Takeda, A., Balraj, E. K., Jones, P. K., Ghanbari,
H., Wataya, T., Shimohama, S., Chiba, S., Atwood, C. S., Petersen, R. B., and Smith, M. A.
(2001). Oxidative damage is the earliest event in Alzheimer disease. J. Neuropathol.
Exp. Neurol. 60:759–767.
Nunomura, A., Chiba, S., Lippa, C. F., Cras, P., Kalaria, R. N., Takeda, A., Honda, K., Smith,
M. A., and Perry, G. (2004). Neuronal RNA oxidation is a prominent feature of familial
Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Dis. 17:108–113.
46 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Poliakov, E., Brennan, M. L., Macpherson, J., Zhang, R., Sha, W., Narine, L., Salomon, R. G.,
and Hazen, S. L. (2003). Isolevuglandins, a novel class of isoprostenoid derivatives,
function as integrated sensors of oxidant stress and are generated by myeloperoxidase
in vivo. FASEB J. 17:2209–2220.
Sajdel-Sulkowska, E. M., Lipinski, B., Windom, H., Audhya, T., and McGinnis, W. (2008).
Oxidative stress in autism: Cerebellar 3-nitrotyrosine levels. Am. J. Biochem. Biotechnol.
(Special Issue on Autism Spectrum Disorders) 4:73–84.
Sajdel-Sulkowska, E. M., Ming, X., and Koibuchi, N. (2009). Increase in brain neurotrophin-3
and oxidative stress in autism. Cerebellum [Epub PMID 19357934].
Schumann, C. M. and Amaral, D. G. (2005). Stereological estimation of the number of neurons
in the human amygdaloid complex. J. Comp. Neurol. 491:320–329.
Skinner, E. R., Watt, C., Besson, J. A., and Best, P. V. (1993). Differences in the fatty acid
composition of the grey and white matter of different regions of the brains of patients
with Alzheimer’s disease and control subjects. Brain 116 (Pt 3):717–725.
Smith, M. A., Kutty, R. K., Richey, P. L., Yan, S. D., Stern, D., Chader, G. J., Wiggert, B.,
Petersen, R. B., and Perry, G. (1994). Heme oxygenase-1 is associated with the neurofi-
brillary pathology of Alzheimer’s disease. Am. J. Pathol. 145:42–47.
Sogut, S., Zoroglu, S. S., Ozyurt, H., Yilmaz, H. R., Ozugurlu, F., Sivasli, E., Yetkin, O., Yanik, M.,
Tutkun, H., Savas, H. A., Tarakcioglu, M., and Akyol, O. (2003). Changes in nitric
oxide levels and antioxidant enzyme activities may have a role in the pathophysiological
mechanisms involved in autism. Clin. Chim. Acta 331:111–117.
Sweeten, T. L., Bowyer, S. L., Posey, D. J., Halberstadt, G. M., and McDougle, C. J. (2003).
Increased prevalence of familial autoimmunity in probands with pervasive develop-
mental disorders. Pediatrics 112:420–424.
Sweeten, T. L., Posey, D. J., Shankar, S., and McDougle, C. J. (2004). High nitric oxide
production in autistic disorder: A possible role for interferon-gamma. Biol. Psychiatry
55:434–437.
Takeda, A., Smith, M. A., Avila, J., Nunomura, A., Siedlak, S. L., Zhu, X., Perry, G., and
Sayre, L. M. (2000). In Alzheimer’s disease, heme oxygenase is coincident with
Alz50, an epitope of tau induced by 4-hydroxy-2-nonenal modification. J. Neurochem.
75:1234–1241.
Vojdani, A., Campbell, A. W., Anyanwu, E., Kashanian, A., Bock, K., and Vojdani, E. (2002).
Antibodies to neuron-specific antigens in children with autism: Possible cross-reaction
with encephalitogenic proteins from milk, Chlamydia pneumoniae and Streptococcus
group A. J. Neuroimmunol. 129:168–177.
Yao, Y., Walsh, W. J., McGinnis, W. R., and Pratico, D. (2006). Altered vascular phenotype in
autism: Correlation with oxidative stress. Arch. Neurol. 63:1161–1164.
Yorbik, O., Sayal, A., Akay, C., Akbiyik, D. I., and Sohmen, T. (2002). Investigation of
antioxidant enzymes in children with autistic disorder. Prostaglandins Leukot. Essent.
Fatty Acids 67:341–343.
Zhu, X., Lee, H. G., Casadesus, G., Avila, J., Drew, K., Perry, G., and Smith, M. A. (2005).
Oxidative imbalance in Alzheimer’s disease. Mol. Neurobiol. 31:205–217.
Zoroglu, S. S., Armutcu, F., Ozen, S., Gurel, A., Sivasli, E., Yetkin, O., and Meram, I. (2004).
Increased oxidative stress and altered activities of erythrocyte free radical scavenging
enzymes in autism. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 254:143–147.
3 Oxidative Stress
and Neurotrophin
Signaling in Autism
Elizabeth M. Sajdel-Sulkowska1,2,*
1Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School
and 2Department of Psychiatry, Brigham and
Women’s Hospital, Boston, MA 02115, USA
CONTENTS
3.1 Further Evidence for Involvement of Oxidative Stress in Autism ................ 48
3.2 Neurotrophins and Brain Development: Could the Imbalance
in Brain Neurotrophin Expression Lead to the Abnormalities
Observed in Autism? ..................................................................................... 50
3.2.1 Evidence for Altered Neurotrophin Expression in Autism .............. 51
3.2.2 Possible Mechanisms Involved in Altered Neurotrophin
Signaling in Autism: An Environmental Impact ............................. 52
3.2.3 Neurotrophin Signaling in Autism: Genetic Component ................ 53
3.2.4 Dual Role of Neurotrophins............................................................. 53
3.2.5 Neurotrophin Signaling and Gender ................................................ 54
3.2.6 Possible Role of Neurotrophins as Targets of Future Therapies ...... 55
3.3 Conclusions ................................................................................................... 55
Acknowledgments.................................................................................................... 55
References ................................................................................................................ 55
47
48 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
70
60
50
3-NT (pmol/g)
40
30
20
10
0
Control Autism
children has been reported in autism (Ming et al., 2005). Increased 8-OH-dG levels
have been also observed in lymphocytes (Mecocci et al., 2002), the cerebrospinal
fluid (CSF)-DNA (Lovell et al., 1999), and in the urine of patients with AD (Lee
et al., 2007) and PD (Sato et al., 2005). The children with brain damage showed
increased urinary 8-OH-dG levels (Fukuda et al., 2008). An immunohistochemi-
cal approach showed 10 times higher brain levels of 8-OH-dG in schizophrenia
(Nishioka and Arnold, 2004).
The oxidative stress in autism could result from (1) the exposure to high levels of
environmental pro-oxidants such as pesticides (Abdollahi et al., 2004; D’Amelio et al.,
2005) and mercury (Hg) (Mutter et al., 2005a,b; Palmer et al., 2008; Windham
et al., 2006); (2) inability to metabolize and clear the toxicant, such as heavy metals,
from the system (Bradstreet et al., 2003; McGinnis 2004; Serajee et al., 2004); (3) the
decreased internal antioxidant defense mechanisms (James et al., 2004; Yorbik
et al., 2002, 2006; Zoroglu et al., 2004); or (4) increased sensitivity to oxidative
stress (Buyske et al., 2006; Yang et al., 2008). It is possible that all four mechanisms
may be involved in precipitating autistic pathology.
900
800
700
NT-3 (pg/g) 600
500
400
300
200
100
0
Control Autism
FIGURE 3.2 Increased NT-3 levels in autistic cerebella. (From Sajdel-Sulkowska, E.M.,
Ming, X., and Koibuchi, N. (2009), Cerebellum [Epub PMID 19357934] With permission.)
selected because of its high expression in the developing rat cerebellum (Das et al.,
2001) and its critical role in migration and survival of cerebellar granule (Li et al.,
2004) and Purkinje cells (Kawakami et al., 2000). The data presented in Figure
3.2 shows a statistically significant NT-3 increase (40.3%) in the autistic cerebel-
lum and support a concept of altered brain neurotrophin expression in autism. As
stated above, NT-3 levels in human brain have not been previously quantified; our
data is thus the fi rst on the brain levels of NT-3 in autism. It is intriguing that the
expression of this particular neurotrophin expression appears to decrease with age
in the rodent brain (Das et al., 2001). Consequently, if a similar developmental
pattern is present in human cerebellum, then prolonged and persistent elevation
of NT-3 in autism could upset the balance of neurotrophic factors and affect brain
growth and development. Specifically, overexpression of NT-3 could contribute
to the cerebellar overgrowth observed in some autistic cases (Courchesne et al.,
2001). Furthermore, persistent elevation of NT-3, specifically involved in neuronal
differentiation (Ghosh and Greenberg, 1995), neurite fasciculation (Segal et al.,
1995), and axonal targeting, may have other profound effects such as on synapse
formation.
We also observed a positive correlation between cerebellar NT-3 and cerebellar
3-nitrotyrosine, which suggests an association between increased oxidative stress
and altered brain neurotrophin levels in autism. This association is not perfect
(r = .83), suggesting the presence of a subset of autistic cases with normal neurotro-
phin levels. It is possible that subsets of autistic cases with unique clinical symptoms
are related to specific molecular changes.
Normal oxidative
defense
3-NT
T
T T Normal brain
T T Proteins of development
neurotrophin
signaling cascade
3.3 CONCLUSIONS
The data discussed here support the key role of the following abnormalities in
autism: (1) increased oxidative stress targeting both cerebellar protein and DNA;
(2) genetic predisposition to oxidative stress triggers; (3) altered expression of brain
neurotrophins; and (4) interaction between oxidative stress and neurotrophins. In
particular, developmental elevation in cerebellar NT-3 expression could contribute to
the initial cerebellar overgrowth and a subsequent reduction on cerebellar Purkinje
cell number and/or result in abnormal brain connectivity in a subset of autistic cases.
Chronic elevation in NT-3 in autism could further predispose the autistic individuals
to oxidative damage.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank Autism Research Institute for continuing support. We thank NICHD Brain
and Tissue Bank for Developmental Disorders at the University of Maryland for
providing us with human postmortem brain specimens.
REFERENCES
Abdollahi, M., Ranjbar, A., Shadnia S., Nikfar, S., and Rezaiee, A. (2004). Pesticides and
oxidative stress: A review. Med. Sci. Monit. 10:141–147.
Baird, G., Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Swettenham, J., Wheelwright, S., and Drew, A.
(2000). A screening instrument for autism at 18 months of age: A 6-year follow-up
study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 39:694–702.
Ballerio, R., Ginazza, E., Mussoni, L., Miller, I., Gelosa, P., Guerrini, U., Eberini, I., Gemeiner, M.,
Belcredito, S., Tremoli, E., and Sironi, L. (2007). Gender differences in endothelial
function and inflammatory markers align the occurrence of pathological events in
stroke-prone rats. Exp. Mol. Pathol. 82:33–41.
56 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Bates, B., Hirt, L., Thomas, S. S., Akbarian, S., Le, D., Amin-Hanjani, S., Whalen, M., Jaenisch, R.,
and Moskowitz, M. A. (2002). Neurotrophin-3 promotes cell death induced in cerebel-
lar ischemia, oxygen-glucose deprivation, and oxidative stress: Possible involvement of
oxygen free radicals. Neurobiol. Dis. 9:24–37.
Beal, M. F. (2002). Oxidatively modified proteins in aging and disease. Free Radic. Biol. Med.
32:797–803.
Beckman, J. S. and Koppenol, W. H. (1996). Nitric oxide, superoxide, and peroxynitrite the
good, the bad and the ugly. Am. J. Physiol. 271:c1424–c1437.
Behrens, M. M., Strasser, U., Lobner, D., and Dugan, L. L. (1999). Neurotrophin-mediated
potentiation of neuronal injury. Microsc. Res. Tech. 45:276–284.
Betancourt, A. M., Filipov, N. M., and Carr, L. R. (2007). Alteration of neurotrophins in the
hippocampus and cerebral cortex of young rats exposed to chlorpyrifos and methyl
parathion. Toxicol. Sci. 100:445–455.
Binder, D. K., Croll, S. D., Gall, C. M., and Scharfman, H. E. (2001). BDNF and epilepsy: Too
much of a good thing? Trends Neurosci. 24:318–319.
Bokara, K. K., Brown, E., McCormick, R., Yallapragada, P. R., Rajanna, S., and Bettaiya, R.
(2008). Lead-induced increase in antioxidant enzymes and lipid peroxidation products
in developing rat brain. Biometals 21:9–16.
Bradstreet, J., Geier, D. A., Kartzinel, J. J., Adams, J. B., and Geier, M. R. (2003). A case–
control study of mercury burden in children with autistic disorders. J. Am. Phys. Surg.
8:76–80.
Brown, G. E., Jones, S. D., MacKewn, A. S., and Plank, E. J. (2008). An exploration
of possible pre- and postnatal correlates of autism: A pilot survey. Psychol. Rep.
102:273–282.
Buyske, S., Williams, T. A., Mars, A. E., Stenroos, E. S., Ming, S. X., Wang, R., Sreenath, M.,
Factura, M. F., Reddy, C., Lambert, G. H., and Johnson, W. G. (2006). Analysis of
case–parent trios at a locus with a deletion allele: Association of GSTM1 with autism.
BMC Genet. 7:8.
Cappelletti, G., Maggioni, M. G. Tedeschi, G., and Maci, R. (2003). Protein tyrosine nitration
is triggered by nerve growth factor during neuronal differentiation of PC12 cells. Exp.
Cell. Res. 288:9–20.
Chao, S. L., Moss, J. M., and Harry, G. J. (2007). Lead-induced alterations of apoptosis and
neurotrophic factor mRNA in the developing rat cortex, hippocampus, and cerebellum.
J. Biochem. Mol. Toxicol. 21:265–272.
Chauhan, A. and Chauhan, V. (2006). Oxidative stress in autism. Pathophysiology 13:171–181.
Chauhan, A., Chauhan, V., Brown, W. T., and Cohen, I. (2004). Oxidative stress in autism:
Increased lipid peroxidation and reduced serum levels of ceruloplasmin and
transferrin—the antioxidant proteins. Life Sci. 75:2539–2549.
Chen, W. G., Chang, Q., Lin, Y., Meissner, A., West, A. E., Griffith, E. C., Jaenisch, R., and
Greeberg, M. E. (2003). Derepression of BDNF transcription involves calcium-
dependent phosphorylation of MeCP2. Science 302:885–889.
Connolly, A. M., Chez, M., Streif, E. M., Keeling, R. M., Golumbek, P. T., Kwon, J. M.,
Riviello, J. J., Robinson, R. G., Neuman, R. J., and Deuel, R. M. K. (2006). Brain-
derived neurotrophic factor and autoantibodies to neural antigens in sera of children with
autistic spectrum disorders, Landau–Kleffner syndrome, and epilepsy. Biol. Psychiatry
59:354–363.
Courchesne, E. (1991). Neuroanatomic imaging in autism. Pediatrics 87:781–790.
Courchesne, E., Carper, R., and Akshoomoff, N. (2003). Evidence of brain overgrowth in the
first year life in autism. JAMA 290:393–394.
Courchesne, E., Karns, C. M., Davis, H. R., Ziccardi, R., Carper, R. A., and Tigue, Z. D.
(2001). Unusual brain growth patterns in early life in patients with autistic disorder: An
MRI study. Neurology 57:245–254.
Oxidative Stress and Neurotrophin Signaling in Autism 57
Dam, K., Seidler, F. J., and Slotkin, T. A. (2000). Chlorpyrifos exposure during a critical
neonatal period elicits gender-selective deficits in the development of coordination skill
and locomotor activity. Brain Res. Dev. Brain Res. 121:179–187.
D’Amelio, M., Ricci, I., Sacco, R., Liu, X., D’Agruma, L., Muscarella, L.A., Guarnieri, V.,
Militerni, R., Bravaccio, C., Elia, M., Schneider, C., Melmed, R., Trillo, S., Pascucci, T.,
Puglisi-Allegra, S., Reichelt, K. L., Macciardi, F., Holden, J.J., and Persico, A. M.
(2005). Paraoxonase gene variants are associated with autism in North America, but not
in Italy: Possible regional specificity in gene-environment interactions. Mol. Psychiatry
10:1006–1016.
Das, K. P., Chao, S. L., White, L. D., Haines, W. T., Harry, G. J., Tilson, H. A., and Barone, S.
Jr. (2001). Differential patterns of nerve growth factor, brain-derived neurotrophic
factor and neurotrophin-3 mRNA and protein levels in developing regions of rat brain.
Neuroscience 103:739–761.
de Cabo, C. and Viveros, M. P. (1997). Effects of neonatal naltrexone on neurological and
somatic development in rats of both genders. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 19:499–509.
Egan, M. F., Kojima, M., Callicott, J. H., Goldberg, T. E., Kolachana, B. S., Bertolino, A.,
Zaitsev, E., Gold, B., Goldman, D., Dean. M., Lu, B., and Weinberger, D. R. (2003).
The BDNF val66met polymorphism affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF and
human memory and hippocampal function. Cell 112:257–269.
Enstrom, A., Onore, C., Tarver, A., Hertz-Picciotto, I., Hansen, R., Croen, L., Van de Walter,
J., and Ashwood, P. (2008). Peripheral blood leukocyte production of BDNF following
mitogen stimulation in early onset and regressive autism. Am. J. Biochem. Biotechnol.
(Special Issue on Autism Spectrum Disorders) 4:121–129.
Ferguson, S. A., Paule, M. G., and Holson, R. R. (1996). Functional effects of methylazoxymeth-
anol-induced cerebellar hypoplasia in rats. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 18:529–537.
Fukuda, M., Yamaguchi, H., Yamamoto, H., Aminaka, M., Murakami, H., Kamiyama, N.,
Miyamoto, Y., and Koitabashi, Y. (2008). The evaluation of oxidative damage in children
with brain damage using 8-hydroxydeoxyguanosine levels. Brain Dev. 30:131–136.
Ghosh, A. and Greenberg, M. E. (1995). Distinct roles for bFGF and NT-3 in the regulation of
cortical neurogenesis. Neuron 15:89–103.
Gilmore, J. H., Jarskog, L. F., and Vadlamudi, S. (2003). Maternal infection regulates
BDNF and NGF expression in fetal and neonatal brain and maternal–fetal unit of rat.
J. Neuroimmunol. 138:49–55.
Hashimoto, K., Iwata, Y., Nakamura, K., Tsuji, M., Tsuchiya, K. J., Sekine, Y., Suzuki, K.,
Minabe, Y., Takei, N., Iyo, M., and Mori, N. (2006). Reduced serum levels of brain-derived
neurotrophic factor in adult male patients with autism. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol.
Biol. Psychiatry 30:1529–1531.
James, S. J., Cutler, P., Melnyk, S., Jernigan, S., Janak, L., Gaylor, D. W., and Neubrander, J. A.
(2004). Metabolic biomarkers of increased oxidative stress and impaired methylation
capacity in children with autism. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 80:1611–1617.
James, S. J., Melny, S., Jernigan, S., Cleves, M. A., Halsted, C. H., Wong, D. H., Cutler, P.,
Bock, K., Boris, M., Bradstreet, J. J., Baker, S. M., and Gaylor, D. W. (2006). Metabolic
endotype and related genotypes are associated with oxidative stress in children with
autism. Am. J. Genet. B. Neuropsychiat. Genet. 141:947–956.
Katoh-Semba, R., Wakako, R., Komori, T., Shigemi, H., Miyazaki, N., Ito, H., Kumagai, T.,
Isuzuki, M., Shigemi, K., Yoshida, F., and Nakayama, A. (2007). Age-related changes
in BDNF protein levels in human serum: Differences between autism cases and normal
controls. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 25:367–372.
Kawakami, H., Nitta, A., Matsuyama, Y., Kamiya, M., Satake, K., Sato, K., Kondou, K.,
Iwata, H., and Furukawa, S. (2000). Increase in neurotrophin-3 expression followed by
Purkinje cell degeneration in the adult rat cerebellum after spinal cord transaction.
J. Neurosci. Res. 62:668–674.
58 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Neumann, H., Hazen, L., Weinstein, J., Mehl, R. A., and Chin, J. W. (2008). Genetically encoding
protein oxidative damage. J. Am. Chem. Soc. 130:4028–4033.
Nishimura, K., Nakamura, K., Anitha, A., Yamada, K., Tsuji, M., and Iwayama, Y. (2007).
Genetic analyses of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) gene in autism.
Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 356:200–206.
Nishioka, N. and Arnold, S. E. (2004). Evidence for oxidative DNA damage in the hip-
pocampus of elderly patients with chronic schizophrenia. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry
12:167–175.
Ogier, M., Wang, H., Hong, E., Wang, Q., Greenberg, M. E., and Katz, D. M. (2007). Brain-
derived neurotrophic factor expression and respiratory function improve after ampakine
treatment in a mouse model of Rett syndrome. J. Neurosci. 27:10912–10917.
Palmer, R. F., Blanchard, S., and Wood, R. (2009). Proximity to point sources of environmental
mercury release as a predictor of autism prevalence. Health Place 15:18–24.
Pasarica, D., Gheorghiu, M., Toparceanu, F., Bleotu, C., Ichim, L., and Trandafir, T. (2005).
Neurotrophin-3, TNF-alpha and IL-6 relations in serum and cerebrospinal fluid of
ischemic stroke patients. Roum. Arch. Microbiol. Immunol. 64:27–33.
Perry, E. K., Lee, M. W., Martin-Ruiz, C. M., Court, J. A., Volsen, S. G., Merrit, J., Folly, E.,
Iversen, P. E., Bauman, M. L., Perry, R. H., and Wenk, G. L. (2001). Cholinergic activity
in autism: Abnormalities in the cerebral cortex and basal forebrain. Am. J. Psychiatry
158:1058–1066.
Quartu, M., Lai, M. L., and Del Fiacco, M. (1999). Neurotrophin-like immunoreactivity in the
human hippocampal formation. Brain Res. Bull. 48:375–382.
Reichardt, L. F. (2006). Neurotrophin-regulated signaling pathway. Phil. Trans R. Soc. B
361:1545–1564.
Ren, J. (2007). Influence of gender on oxidative stress, lipid peroxidation, protein damage
and apoptosis in hearts and brains from spontaneously hypertensive rats. Clin. Exp.
Pharmacol. Physiol. 34:432–438.
Rintala, J., Jaatinen, P., Kiianmaa, K., Iikonen, J., Kemppainen, O., Sarviharju, M., and
Hervonen, A. (2001). Dose-dependent decrease in glial fibrillary acidic protein-
immunoreactivity in rat cerebellum after lifelong ethanol consumption. Alcohol
23:1–8.
Rivto, E. R., Freeman, B. J., Scheibel, A. B., Duong, T., Robinson, H., Guthrie, D., and Ritvo, A.
(1986). Lower Purkinje cell counts in the cerebella of four autistic subjects: Initial find-
ings of the UCLA-NSac Autopsy Research Report. Am. J. Psychiatry 143:862–866.
Rout, U. K. and Dhossche, D. M. (2008). A pathogenetic model of autism involving Purkinje
cell loss through anti GAD antibodies. Med. Hypotheses 71:218–221.
Sacksteder, C. A., Qian, W. J., Knyushko, T. V., Wang, H., Chin, M. H., Lacan, G., Melega, W. P.,
Camp, D. G. II, Smith, R. D., Smith, D. J., Squier, T. C., and Bigelow, D. J. (2006).
Endogenously nitrated proteins in mouse brain: Links to neurodegenerative disease.
Biochemistry 45:8009–8022.
Sadakata, T., Washida, M., Iwayama, Y., Shoji, S., Sato, Y., and Ohkura, T. (2007). Autistic-like
phenotypes in Cadps2-knockout mice and aberrant CADPS2 splicing in autistic patients.
J. Clin. Invest. 117:931–943.
Sajdel-Sulkowska, E. M., Lipinski, B., Windom, H., Audhya, T., and McGinnis, W. (2008).
Oxidative stress in autism: Cerebellar 3-nitrotyrosine levels. Am. J. Biochem. Biotechnol.
(Special Issue on Autism Spectrum Disorders) 4:73–84.
Sajdel-Sulkowska, E. M., Ming, X., and Koibuchi, N. (2009). Increase in brain neurotrophin-3
and oxidative stress in autism. Cerebellum [Epub PMID 19357934].
Sato, S., Mizuno, Y., and Hattori, N. (2005). Urinary 8-hydroxydeoxyguanosine levels as a
biomarker for progression of Parkinson disease. Neurology 64:1081–1083.
Scott, F. J., Baron-Cohen, S., Bolton, P., and Brayne, C. (2002). Brief report: Prevalence of
autism spectrum conditions in children aged 5–11 years in Cambridgeshire, UK. Autism
6:231–237.
60 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Segal, R. A., Pomeroy, S. L., and Stiles, C. D. (1995). Axonal growth and fasciculation linked
to differential expression of BDNF and NT-3 receptors in developing granule cells.
J. Neurosci. 15:4970–4981.
Serajee, F. J., Nabi, R., Zhong, H., and Huq, M. (2004). Polymorphism in xenobiotic metabolism
genes and autism. J. Child Neurol. 19:413–417.
Sogut, S., Zoroglu, S. S., Ozyurt, H., Yilmaz, H. R., Ozuğurlu, F., Sivasli, E., Yetkin, O.,
Yanik, M., Tutkun, H., Savaş, H. A., Tarakçioğlu, M., and Akyol, O. (2003). Changes in
nitric oxide levels and antioxidant enzyme activities may have a role in the pathological
mechanisms involved in autism. Clin. Chim. Acta 33:1111–1117.
Sultana, R., Perluigi, M., and Butterfield, D. A. (2006). Protein oxidation, lipid peroxidation in
brain of subjects with Alzheimer’s disease: Insights into mechanism of neurodegenera-
tion from redox proteomics. Antioxid. Redox. Signal. 8:2021–2037.
Sweeten, T. L., Posey, D. J., Shankar, S., and McDougle, C. J. (2004). High nitric oxide
production in autistic disorder: A possible role for interferon-gamma. Biol. Psychiatry
55:434–437.
Tabuchi, A. (2008). Synaptic plasticity-regulated gene expression: A key event in the long-
lasting changes of neuronal function. Biol. Pharm. Bull. 31:327–335.
Tsai, S. J. (2005). Is autism caused by early hyperactivity of brain derived neurotrophic factor?
Med. Hypotheses 65:79–82.
Tsai, S. J. (2007). Semax, an analogue of adrenocorticotropin (4–10), is a potential agent
for the treatment of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and Rett syndrome. Med.
Hypotheses 68:1144–1146.
Vargas, M. R., Pehar, M., Cassina, P., Estavez, A. G., Beckman, J. S., and Barbeito, L. (2004).
Stimulation of nerve growth factor expression in astrocytes by peroxynitrite. In Vivo
18:269–274.
Vicedomini, J. P., Nonneman, A. J., DeKosky, S. T., and Scheff, S. W. (1986). Perinatal
glucocorticoids disrupt learning: A sexually dimorphic response. Physiol. Behav.
36:145–149.
Whitney, E. R., Kemper, T. L., Bauman, M. L., Rosene, D. L., and Blatt, G. J. (2008). Cerebellar
Purkinje cells are reduced in a subpopulation of autistic brains: A stereological experiment
using calbindin-D28k. Cerebellum 7:406–416.
Windham, G. C., Zhang, L., Gunier, R., Croen, L. A., and Grether, J. K. (2006). Autism
spectrum disorders in relation to distribution of hazardous air pollutants in the San
Francisco bay area. Environ. Health Perspect. 114:1438–1444.
Yang, I. A., Fong, K. M., Zimmerman, P. V., Holgate, S. T., and Holloway, J. W. (2008).
Genetic susceptibility to the respiratory effects of air pollution. Thorax 63:555–563.
Yao, Y., Walsh, W. J., McGinnis, W. R., and Pratico, D. (2006). Altered vascular phenotype in
autism: Correlation with oxidative stress. Arch. Neurol. 63:1161–1164.
Yip, J., Soghomonian, J. J., and Blatt, G. J. (2007). Decreased GAD67 mRNA levels in
cerebellar Purkinje cells in autism: Pathophysiological implications. Acta Neuropathol.
113:559–568.
Yorbik, O., Sayal, A., Akay, C., Akbiyik, D. I., and Sohmen, T. (2002). Investigation of
antioxidant enzymes in children with autistic disorder. Prostaglandins Leukot. Essent.
Fatty Acids 67:341–343.
Zhu, S.-W., Pham, T. M., Aberg, E., Brene, S., Winblad, B., Mohammed, A. H., and Baumans, V.
(2006). Neurotrophin levels and behavior in BALB/c mice: Impact of intermittent expo-
sure to individual housing and wheel running. Behav. Brain Res. 167:1–8.
Zoroglu, S. S., Armutcu, F., Ozen, S., Gurel, A., Sivasli, E., Yetkin, O., and Meram, I. (2004).
Increased oxidative stress and altered activities of erythrocyte free radical scavenging
enzymes in autism. Eur. Arch. Psychiatr. Clin. Neurosci. 254:143–147.
4 Genetics of Autism
W. Ted Brown*
Department of Human Genetics, New York State
Institute for Basic Research in Developmental
Disabilities, Staten Island, NY 10314, USA
CONTENTS
4.1 Introduction ................................................................................................... 61
4.2 Epidemiology of Autism ............................................................................... 62
4.3 Heritability of Autism ................................................................................... 63
4.4 Integrative Genetics ...................................................................................... 63
4.5 Single-Gene Mutations Associated with Autism ..........................................64
4.6 Copy Number Variations in Autism.............................................................. 65
4.7 Parental Age and Autism ..............................................................................66
4.8 Prenatal Maternal Stress and Autism............................................................ 67
4.9 Potential Role of Noncoding RNAs in Autism ............................................. 67
4.10 Summary and Conclusions ........................................................................... 68
References ................................................................................................................ 68
4.1 INTRODUCTION
Autism has a strong genetic component. It is highly heritable. Heritability is a genetic
term that provides an estimate of the proportion of phenotypic variation in a popula-
tion that is due to genetic variation. Studies comparing the ratio of concordance of
autism in monozygotic twin pairs (70%–90%) to dizygotic twin pairs (0%–10%) have
provided heritability estimates for autism of >90% (Folstein and Rosen-Sheidley,
2001; Freitag, 2007). The autism heritability estimate of >90% suggests that the
environmental component of autism is <10%, but not zero. The identical twin dis-
cordances may be due to epigenetic changes (Cheung et al., 2008; Kaminsky et al.,
2009), somatic mutations (Bruder et al., 2008), and chorionic environmental influences
(Bohm and Stewart, 2009). The inheritance of autism is not simple: it is rather com-
plex (Abrahams and Geschwind, 2008). Autism is a heterogeneous condition of variable
severity, ranging from severe cognitive impairment with seizures and lack of speech
to the milder Asperger syndrome (AS) with social communication deficits but normal
intelligence. It is now commonly regarded as an autism spectrum disorder (ASD), a
classification that includes autism, pervasive developmental disability disorder—not
otherwise specified (PDD-NOS), and AS (Freitag, 2007).
61
62 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
It is likely that there are subtypes of ASDs. For example, regression is seen in
a certain proportion of cases. One recent study reported that regression was seen
in 41% of a sample of 333 subjects with autism; 26% lost either language or social
skills, while 15% lost both (Hansen et al., 2008). Other domains in which subtypes
are likely include the presence or absence of behavioral inflexibility, macrocephaly,
seizures, and syndromic appearance.
clinical trials (Nelson, 2008; Zhu et al., 2008). Applications of this approach to
neurodevelopmental disorders such as autism will be a challenge because of dif-
ficulties in measuring gene expression levels in the target tissue. Perhaps initially
these difficulties will be addressed with mouse models. A similar approach has
been proposed that employs a probabilistic framework to combine genetic linkage
information with whole-genome molecular-interaction data to predict networks of
interacting genes that contribute to common disorders (Feldman et al., 2008; Goh
et al., 2007; Lossifov et al., 2008). This approach was applied to three disorders—
autism, bipolar disorder, and schizophrenia—and it yielded a number of candidate
genes and predicted gene targets that are likely to be shared among the disorders
(Lossifov et al., 2008).
TABLE 4.1
ASA Gene Candidates
Gene Name Gene Description
RELN Reelin
UBE3A Ubiquitin protein ligase E3A
MECP2 Methyl CpG binding protein 2
GABRB3 Gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) A receptor beta 3
TSC1 Tuberous sclerosis 1
TSC2 Tuberous sclerosis 2
PTEN Phosphatase and tensin homolog
NLGN3 Neuroligin 3
NLGN4 Neuroligin 4
FMR1 Fragile X mental retardation 1
DHCR7 7-Dehydrocholesterol reductase
CADPS2 Ca2+-dependent activator protein for secretion 2
SLC6A4 Solute carrier family 6 (tr-serotonin) member 4
SHANK3 SH3 and multiple ankyrin repeat domains 3
OXTR Oxytocin receptor
MET Met protooncogene
CNTNAP2 Contactin-associated protein-like 2
CACNA1C Calcium channel voltage-dependent L type alpha 1C subunit
SLC25A12 Solute carrier family 25 (mitochondrial, Aralar) member 12
NRXN1 Neurexin 1
GRIK2 Glutamate receptor ionotropic kainate 2 precursor
EN2 Engrailed homeobox 2
AHI1 Abelson helper integration site 1
ITGB3 Integrin beta 3
Source: After Abrahams, B.S. and Geschwind, D.H., Nat. Rev. Genet., 9, 341, 2008; for
original references, see OMIM, Online Mendelian Inheritance in Man.
Note: These genes are found mutated or strongly associated in some families with
autism.
A search for regions of the genome that are highly evolutionarily conserved
among mammals but have rapidly diverged from our last common ancestor, chim-
panzees, led to the discovery of human accelerated region 1 (HAR1) (Pollard et al.,
2006). This 118 bp sequence had accumulated 18 base pair changes, when zero or
one would have been expected to occur by chance. The expression of HAR1 in the
brain overlaps nearly identically with that of the reelin gene, which is a protein that
helps regulate processes of neuronal migration as well as neuronal positioning in the
cortex and the hippocampus, and modulates synaptic plasticity (Niu et al., 2008).
Thus, a rapidly evolving ncRNA gene appears to play an important role in human
brain development. Further, other ncRNAs appear to play key roles in dendritic spine
development (Schratt et al., 2006) and in long-term memory formation (Mercer et al.,
2008). Further research in this exciting new area could help us to better understand
the genetics of autism.
REFERENCES
Abrahams, B.S. and D.H. Geschwind (2008). Advances in autism genetics: On the threshold
of a new neurobiology. Nat. Rev. Genet. 9:341–355.
American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Armengol, L., M. Caceres, A. Brunet, and X. Estivill (2007). Enrichment and variability of
PIWI-interacting RNAs (piRNAs) in segmental duplications and copy number variants
(CNVs) suggest a functional role in the integrity of the genome. Abstract #153 presented
at the Annual Meeting of the American Society of Human Genetics, San Diego, CA.
Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends Cogn. Sci.
6:248–254.
Baron-Cohen, S., R.C. Knickmeyer, and M.K. Belmonte (2005). Sex differences in the brain:
Implications for explaining autism. Science 310:819–823.
Belmonte, M.K. and T. Bourgeron (2006). Fragile X syndrome and autism at the intersection
of genetic and neural networks. Nat. Neurosci. 9:1221–1225.
Genetics of Autism 69
K. Stefansson (2008). Genetics of gene expression and its effect on disease. Nature
452:423–428.
Feldman, I., A. Rzhetsky, and D. Vitkup (2008). Network properties of genes harboring inher-
ited disease mutations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 105:4323–4328.
Folstein, S.E. and B. Rosen-Sheidley (2001). Genetics of autism: Complex aetiology for a
heterogeneous disorder. Nat. Rev. Genet. 2:943–955.
Fombonne, E., R. Zakarian, A. Bennett, L. Meng, and D. McLean-Heywood (2006). Pervasive
developmental disorders in Montreal, Quebec, Canada: Prevalence and links with immu-
nizations. Pediatrics 118:e139–e150.
Freitag, C.M. (2007). The genetics of autistic disorders and its clinical relevance: A review of
the literature. Mol. Psychiatry 12:2–22.
Ghaziuddin, M. (2005). A family history study of Asperger syndrome. J. Autism Dev. Disord.
38:11735:177–182.
Ghildiyal, M. and P.D. Zamore (2009). Small silencing RNAs: An expanding universe. Nat.
Rev. Genet. 10:94–108.
Gillberg, I.C. and C. Gillberg (1989). Asperger syndrome—Some epidemiological consider-
ations: A research note. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 30:631–638.
Goh, K.I., M.E. Cusick, D. Valle, B. Childs, M. Vidal, and A.L. Barabási (2007). The human
disease network. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104:8685–8690.
Guttman, M., I. Amit, M. Garber, C. French, M.F. Lin, D. Feldser, M. Huarte, O. Zuk,
B.W. Carey, J.P. Cassady, M.N. Cabili, R. Jaenisch, T.S. Mikkelsen, T. Jacks, N. Hacohen,
B.E. Bernstein, M. Kellis, A. Regev, J.L. Rinn, and E.S. Lander (2009). Chromatin
signature reveals over a thousand highly conserved large non-coding RNAs in mammals.
Nature 458:223–227.
Hagerman, R.J., E. Berry-Kravis, W.E. Kaufmann, M.Y. Ono, N. Tartaglia, A. Lachiewicz,
R. Kronk, C. Delahunty, D. Hessl, J. Visootsak, J. Picker, L. Gane, and M. Tranfaglia
(2009). Advances in the treatment of fragile X syndrome. Pediatrics 123:378–390.
Hansen, R.L., S. Ozonoff, P. Krakowiak, K. Angkustsiri, C. Jones, L.J. Deprey, D.N. Le,
L.A. Croen, and I. Hertz-Picciotto (2008). Regression in autism: Prevalence and associ-
ated factors in the CHARGE Study. Ambul. Pediatr. 8:25–31.
Harris, S.W., D. Hessl, B. Goodlin-Jones, J. Ferranti, S. Bacalman, I. Barbato, F. Tassone,
P.J. Hagerman, H. Herman, and R.J. Hagerman (2008). Autism profiles of males with
fragile X syndrome. Am. J. Ment. Retard. 113:427–438.
Itsara, A., G.M. Cooper, C. Baker, S. Girirajan, J. Li, D. Absher, R.M. Krauss, R.M. Myers,
P.M. Ridker, D.I. Chasman, H. Mefford, P. Ying, D.A. Nickerson, and E.E. Eichler
(2009). Population analysis of large copy number variants and hotspots of human
genetic disease. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 84:148–161.
Jacquemont, M.L., D. Sanlaville, R. Redon, O. Raoul, V. Cormier-Daire, S. Lyonnet, J. Amiel,
M. Le Merrer, D. Heron, M.C. de Blois, M. Prieur, M. Vekemans, N.P. Carter, A. Munnich,
L. Colleaux, and A. Philippe (2006). Array-based comparative genomic hybridisation
identifies high frequency of cryptic chromosomal rearrangements in patients with syn-
dromic autism spectrum disorders. J. Med. Genet. 43:843–849.
Kaminsky, K.A., T. Tang, S.C. Wang, C. Ptak, G.H. Oh, A.H. Wong, L.A. Feldcamp,
C. Virtanen, J. Halfvarson, C. Tysk, A.F. McRae, P.M. Visscher, G.W. Montgomery,
I.I. Gottesman, N.G. Martin, and A. Petronis (2009). DNA methylation profiles in
monozygotic and dizygotic twins. Nat. Genet. 41:240–245.
Kinney, D.K., A.M. Miller, D.J. Crowley, E. Huang, and E. Gerber (2008a). Autism prevalence
following prenatal exposure to hurricanes and tropical storms in Louisiana. J. Autism
Dev. Disord. 38:481–488.
Kinney, D.K., K.M. Munir, D.J. Crowley, and A.M. Miller (2008b). Prenatal stress and risk for
autism. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32:1519–1532.
Genetics of Autism 71
Knickmeyer, R.C. and S. Baron-Cohen (2006). Fetal testosterone and sex differences in typi-
cal social development and in autism. J. Child. Neurol. 21:825–845.
Kopra, K., L. von Wendt, T. Nieminen-von Wendt, and E.J. Paavonen (2008). Comparison of
diagnostic methods for Asperger syndrome. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38:1567–1573.
Liu, X.Q., A.D. Paterson, and P. Szatmari; Autism Genome Project Consortium (2008).
Genome-wide linkage analyses of quantitative and categorical autism subphenotypes.
Biol. Psychiatry 64:561–570.
Lossifov, I., T. Zheng, M. Baron, T.C. Gilliam, and A. Rzhetsky (2008). Genetic-linkage map-
ping of complex hereditary disorders to a whole-genome molecular-interaction network.
Genome Res. 18:1150–1162.
Lowenthal, R., C.S. Paula, J.S. Schwartzman, D. Brunoni, and M.T. Mercadante (2007).
Prevalence of pervasive developmental disorder in Down’s syndrome. J. Autism Dev.
Disord. 37:1394–1395.
Maher, B. (2008). The case of the missing heritability. Nature 456:18–21.
Marshall, C.R., A. Noor, J.B. Vincent, A.C. Lionel, L. Feuk, J. Skaug, M. Shago, R. Moessner,
D. Pinto, Y. Ren, B. Thiruvahindrapduram, A. Fiebig, S. Schreiber, J. Friedman,
C.E. Ketelaars, Y.J. Vos, C. Ficicioglu, S. Kirkpatrick, R. Nicolson, L. Sloman, A. Summers,
C. A. Gibbons, A. Teebi, D. Chitayat, R. Weksberg, A. Thompson, C. Vardy, V. Crosbie,
S. Luscombe, R. Baatjes, L. Zwaigenbaum, W. Roberts, B. Fernandez, P. Szatmari, and
S.W. Scherer (2008). Structural variation of chromosomes in autism spectrum disorder.
Am. J. Hum. Genet. 82:477–488.
Mattila, M.-L., M. Kielinen, and K. Jussila (2007). An epidemiological and diagnostic study
of Asperger syndrome according to four sets of diagnostic criteria. J. Am. Acad. Child
Adol. Psych. 46:636–646.
Mercer, T.R., M.E. Dinger, J. Mariani, K.S. Kosik, M.F. Mehler, and J.S. Mattick (2008).
Noncoding RNAs in long-term memory formation. Neuroscientist 14:434–445.
Nelson, B. (2008). A disruptive personality, disrupted. Nature 456:26–28.
Niu, S., O. Yabut, and G. D’Arcangelo (2008). The Reelin signaling pathway promotes den-
dritic spine development in hippocampal neurons. J. Neurosci. 28:10339–10348.
Pheasant, M. and J.S. Mattick (2007). Raising the estimate of functional human sequences.
Genome Res. 17:1245–1253.
Pollard, K.S., S.R. Salama, N. Lambert, M.A. Lambot, S. Coppens, J.S. Pedersen,
S. Katzman, B. King, C. Onodera, A. Siepel, A.D. Kern, C. Dehay, H. Igel, Ares, Jr.,
M.P. Vanderhaeghen, and D. Haussler (2006). An RNA gene expressed during cortical
development evolved rapidly in humans. Nature 443:167–172.
Psychiatric GWAS Consortium Steering Committee (2009). A framework for interpreting
genome-wide association studies of psychiatric disorders. Mol. Psychiatry 14:10–17.
Schadt, E.E., J. Lamb, X. Yang, J. Zhu, S. Edwards, D. Guhathakurta, S.K. Sieberts, S. Monks,
M. Reitman, C. Zhang, P.Y. Lum, A. Leonardson, R. Thieringer, J.M. Metzger, L. Yang,
J. Castle, H. Zhu, S.F. Kash, T.A. Drake, A. Sachs, and A.J. Lusis (2005). An integrative
genomics approach to infer causal associations between gene expression and disease.
Nat. Genet. 37:710–717.
Schadt, E.E., C. Molony, E. Chudin, K. Hao, X. Yang, P.Y. Lum, A. Kasarskis, B. Zhang,
S. Wang, C Suver, J. Zhu, J. Millstein, S. Sieberts, J. Lamb, D. GuhaThakurta, J. Derry,
J.D. Storey, I. Avila-Campillo, M.J. Kruger, J.M. Johnson, C.A. Rohl, A. van Nas,
M. Mehrabian, T.A. Drake, A.J. Lusis, R.C. Smith, F.P. Guengerich, S.C. Strom,
E. Schuetz, T.H. Rushmore, and R. Ulrich (2008). Mapping the genetic architecture of
gene expression in human liver. PLoS Biol. 6:e107.
Schratt, G.M., F. Tuebing, E.A. Nigh, C.G. Kane, M.E. Sabatini, M. Kiebler, and M.E. Greenberg
(2006). A brain-specific microRNA regulates dendritic spine development. Nature
439:283–289.
72 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
CONTENTS
5.1 Introduction ................................................................................................... 73
5.2 Assessment Issues and the PDD Behavior Inventory ................................... 74
5.3 Maternal Depression and Autism Severity ................................................... 76
5.4 Monoamine Oxidase-A Gene as a DEP/AUT Gene ..................................... 78
5.5 Association of an MAOA-uVNTR Polymorphism
with Behavior in Autism ............................................................................... 81
5.6 MAOA Gene and Behavior Profiles in Autism, a Maternal Effect............... 82
5.7 Summary and Conclusions ...........................................................................84
References ................................................................................................................ 86
5.1 INTRODUCTION
Autism and the other pervasive developmental disorders (PDD), including childhood
disintegrative disorder, Rett’s disorder, Asperger’s disorder, and PDD—not other-
wise specified, comprise a set of disorders which share a “triad” of impairments
in socialization and communication along with repetitive and ritualistic behaviors
(American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Etiologically, autism is a disorder with
a strong genetic basis (Bailey et al., 1995) but research has yet to identify a single
gene or even a set of genes that account for the vast majority of the identified cases
73
74 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
although progress is being made (Abrahams and Geschwind, 2008). Indeed, Happe
and Ronald (2008) have noted that the triad of impairments characteristic of autism
fractionate when examined in large-scale twin populations and are associated with
nonoverlapping genes, suggesting that it may be fruitless to assume a single cause
is responsible for the disorder. To complicate the matter further, there is strong
heterogeneity not only across diagnostic classes in PDD but within autism itself.
For example, Miles et al. (2005) have suggested two groupings: essential versus
complex autism, with the latter categorized by evidence of significant dysmorphol-
ogy or microcephaly. Such individuals were also more likely to have an identifiable
syndrome. Yet the notion that autism is a syndrome with many different “organic”
etiologies is not new (Coleman, 1976).
These observations suggest that when attempting to understand the puzzle of
autism, it may be more beneficial to consider examining how behavioral compo-
nents of the disorder (rather than the syndrome as a whole) are individually linked
to specific, theoretically relevant, biobehavioral variables. The advantages of this
approach include identifying possible etiologies for behaviors associated with
autism as well with as other overlapping disorders, identifying targets for interven-
tion, and for explaining the wide variation in the severity of autism. The latter fac-
tor impacts development over time, as well as stress within the family. This tactic
has been the focus of our research on autism over the past 25 years, starting with
our initial studies on behaviors that differ in children with autism who have fragile
X syndrome relative to those who do not (Cohen, 1992, 1995, 1996; Cohen et al.,
1989, 1991, 1996).
as well as to most people in the population (since most IQ tests are standardized
with a mean of 100 and an SD of 15). However, raw scores do not provide this type
of information. There is no way to know if raw scores of 10 and 15 in two differ-
ent people on an autism measure such as the ADI-R represent “normal” variation
or an actual meaningful difference in severity. Similar criticism applies to other
autism scales such as the Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) (Schopler et al.,
1980). Related to this, such assessment tools are not age-standardized. This is a
critical issue since children with autism at 2 years of age do not behaviorally look
the same as children with autism at age 12.
Finally, most assessment tools for autism exclusively assess problem behaviors.
They do not measure the person’s abilities in socialization, communication, and
play. There are several problems with this approach. First, there is no way to mea-
sure actual improvement or worsening in skills over time, only a change in problem
behaviors can be assessed. Second, there is no way to differentiate subgroups within
the autism spectrum that differ mainly in their relative assets (e.g., Asperger’s cases
that have intact language but poor social skills). Third, there is no way of identifying
cases that differ on the basis of a relative lack of appropriate skills, and not the pres-
ence of deviant behaviors (e.g., language impaired children).
For the above reasons, we developed the PDD Behavior Inventory (PDDBI)
(Cohen, 2003; Cohen et al., 2003b; Cohen and Sudhalter, 2005), a reliable and
valid informant-based assessment tool standardized on a large, well-diagnosed
sample of children, aged 2−12.5 years with PDD (autism in 86%; PDD-NOS in
12%, and other PDD 2%). Three hundred sixty-nine parents and 277 teachers com-
prised the informant sample. Filling out the PDDBI is relatively straightforward
(items are between the fifth and eighth grade reading level) and can be completed
by most informants in about 30−45 min.
The PDDBI provides age- and norm-referenced (T-scores: mean = 50; SD = 10)
measures of behaviors typically seen in children with autism across two primary
dimensions designated “Approach/Withdrawal Problems” and “Receptive/Expressive
Social Communication Abilities.” The Approach/Withdrawal Problems dimension is
comprised of seven behavioral domains covering a variety of the problem behaviors
seen in children with autism while the Receptive/Expressive Social Communication
Abilities dimension is comprised of three domains capturing social, language, and
memory skills that may be deficient in some children with autism. Domain T-scores
are also used to compute an “Autism Composite” score designed to reflect overall
severity. Each of these domain and composite T-scores are normally distributed
within the reference sample. T-scores from the PDDBI have proven to be sensitive to
relevant independent variables (Chauhan et al., 2004; Cohen et al., 2003a,b; Cohen
and Tsiouris, 2006) and correlate well with diagnosis (Cohen, 2003; Cohen et al.,
2003a,b; Cohen and Sudhalter, 2005).
In this chapter, I will summarize what we have learned, using the PDDBI, about
two related factors influencing the severity of autism: maternal depression and a
polymorphism in the monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) gene. Overall, our data indicate
that maternal depression influences the behavioral profile of autism in a somewhat
paradoxical manner and this effect is remarkably similar to what we see with this
MAOA polymorphism, suggesting a link between mood disorders and autism.
76 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
(Biederman et al., 2001). Thus, the children of mothers with recurrent depression
in our study resembled, to some extent, their non-PDD cohorts. Unlike these chil-
dren, however, their internalizing problems manifested as an increased severity of
certain “autistic” behaviors.
These observations are indeed puzzling. Why would high functioning autism
be linked to a lifetime history of mood disorders in their families, especially their
mothers? In our study, we could not attribute the relatively increased IQ association
to psychosocial factors, selection bias, or other treatments such as applied behavior
analysis (ABA). While it is conceivable that those mothers with a depression
history may be more intelligent and, therefore, their children with autism would be
more likely to be higher functioning, IQs of the children in our study were unre-
lated to maternal educational level, and type of maternal depression was unrelated to
maternal educational level. Further, studies have concluded that IQs of children with
autism do not correlate significantly with IQs of their parents (Szatmari and Jones,
1991; Fombonne et al., 1997).
Genetic factors could be a possible explanation. Smalley et al. (1995) speculated
that the association of familial mood/anxiety disorders with high functioning autism
is due to greater etiological heterogeneity in children with autism who also have intel-
lectual disabilities. That is, in such children, genetic and/or environmental factors
unrelated to mood disorder likely play a role in contributing to causation. DeLong
(2004) has made a similar argument. However, there is no a priori reason for why genes
associated with intellectual disability should not also be involved in depression or
social phobia. Intellectual disability is a common feature in males with fragile X
syndrome. Yet, social anxiety is prominent in these males at all levels of intelligence
(Cohen et al., 1988) and major depression has been reported to be common in their
mothers (Thompson et al., 1996).
In our paper (Cohen and Tsiouris, 2006), we hypothesized a “genetic modifier”
model (cf. Slavotinek and Biesecker, 2003) as an explanation. In this model, “autism
genes” and “depression genes” share common alleles, which interact with one
another (epistatic interactions) to modify the expression of each disorder. In the case
of autism, this would include a relatively “protective” effect of recurrent depression
alleles on IQ in children with autism.
This modifier model is displayed in the Venn diagram (Figure 5.1). Here, the
phenotypes of autism and depression are depicted as separate entities arising from
gene sets that are largely independent. However, the two disorders share some
risk alleles (denoted here as DEP/AUT alleles). When these DEP/AUT alleles are
present, they interact with each other to modify the expression of each disorder
(the shaded regions). Therefore, this model predicts that DEP/AUT alleles should
result in individuals more likely to have a specific autism or depression pheno-
type, depending upon the number and type of these alleles, and that these DEP/
AUT alleles have a protective effect on the deleterious effects on language and
IQ caused by other autism genes, accounting for the increased cognitive ability in
these affected children. If this hypothesis were true, it would have clear implica-
tions for prognosis and treatment. Is there any evidence that genes associated with
mood and anxiety disorders have a protective effect on language and IQ in children
with autism? The answer is yes.
78 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Autism
Autism phenotype
genes
DEP/AUT
Depression
genes Depression
phenotype
FIGURE 5.1 This figure presents a Venn diagram of the hypothesized overlap of risk alleles
(DEP/AUT alleles) that are common to both autism and recurrent mood disorders. The dashed
arrows emerging from the DEP/AUT overlap represent this modifier gene effect. The shaded
regions represent DEP/AUT modified subgroups of autism and depression. (From Cohen, I.L.
and Tsiouris, J.A., J. Aut. Dev. Disord., 36, 1077, 2006. With permission.)
example, in some studies, blood serotonin levels have been found to correlate
inversely with verbal IQ (Cook et al., 1990) and to correlate positively with severity
of autism (Kuperman et al., 1987). Blood serotonin levels have also been found
to be related to mood and anxiety disorders. Cook et al. (1994) reported that
parents of children with autism who had elevated blood serotonin levels also had
increased levels of symptoms of depression and obsessive–compulsive disorder,
relative to parents of children with autism who had normal serotonin levels. More
evidence for a serotonin dysfunction is found in the fact that increased binding
of paroxetine to the serotonin transporter in blood platelets has been observed
in autism (Marazziti et al., 2000), suggesting that a similar mechanism may be
present centrally. Indeed, at the CNS level, Chugani et al. (1999) reported, in a
positron emission tomography (PET) scan study, that brain serotonin synthesis
declines with age in typically developing children; but in children with autism,
brain serotonin synthesis fails to decrease with age.
Many factors affect overall CNS serotonin levels as well as the availability of serotonin
in the synapse. Metabolically, serotonin is synthesized from 5-hydroxytryptophan
(5-HTP) by l-amino acid decarboxylase (l-AADC). 5-HTP is synthesized from tryp-
tophan by tryptophan hydroxylase (TPH), the rate-limiting enzyme in serotonin
synthesis. When released into the synapse, serotonin interacts with a multitude of
pre- and postsynaptic receptor sites. These postsynaptic receptors can induce gene
expression by activating CREB1 (cAMP-responsive element-binding protein), a gene
that has been implicated in recurrent depression in women (Zubenko et al., 2003).
An inhibitory presynaptic autoreceptor (HTR1B/HTR1DB) modulates the release of
serotonin into the synapse. The serotonin reuptake transporter (SERT/SLC6A4/5-HTT)
brings serotonin back into the presynaptic neuron where it is eventually metabolized
by MAO in the mitochondria. Thus, the extent to which serotonin can effect changes
on the postsynaptic neuron is influenced by availability of precursors, enzyme activ-
ity, presynaptic receptor sensitivity and function, genes involved with the cAMP
signaling pathway, and excitatory or inhibitory influences of other neuronal systems.
The density of serotonergic synapses will also influence overall brain serotonin levels.
Understanding how the serotonin system affects behavior in autism will ultimately
require examining the contribution of many of these factors, both singly and in com-
bination. For reasons enumerated below, the gene that codes for MAOA is a reasonable
starting point.
Two forms of MAO exist (A and B). The genes that encode these isoenzymes map
to Xp11.23 ∼ 11.4. MAOA is of interest in this discussion because it preferentially
deaminates serotonin and norepinephrine while MAOB acts on the phenethylamines
and benzylamine (Sabol et al., 1998). Both MAOA and MAOB are present in various
regions of the body including the brain and fibroblasts. Some tissues preferentially
express only one of these isoenzymes. For example, platelets only express MAOB
(Sabol et al., 1998).
A number of different studies indicate that MAOA levels and MAOA gene dif-
ferences are associated with neurotransmitter and behavioral changes. For example,
MAOA knockout mice show large increases in brain serotonin levels and disturbed
CNS development (Cases et al., 1998). Thus, absence of MAOA has marked effects
80 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
65
60
Mean (±95% Cl)
55
50
45
40
35
Soc Appr
LMRL
Sem/Prag Abil
Autism Score
Soc Appr
LMRL
Sem/Prag Abil
Autism Score
Stereotypies
Stereotypies
Parent Teacher
3-repeat 4-repeat
FIGURE 5.2 This figure shows the similarity in parent and teacher profiles for the signifi-
cant PDDBI domain T-scores (mean ± 95% confidence interval) across 3-repeat and 4-repeat
MAOA-uVNTR genotypes (see text). Note that the domain names differ slightly from the
current version of the PDDBI (Cohen and Sudhalter, 2005) and these changes are noted
below. Vertical lines separate the SENSORY (stereotypies) domain (higher scores indicate
increased severity) from the Receptive-Expressive Social Communication Abilities domains
(where higher scores indicate increased ability) and from the overall Autism Score (higher
scores indicate increased severity). Soc Appr, Social Approach domain; LMRL, Learning,
Memory and Receptive Language domain; Sem/Prag Abil is a component of the Expressive
Language domain. (From Cohen, I.L. et al., Clin. Genet., 64, 190, 2003. With permission.)
3-repeat mothers, their data were dropped from the rest of the analyses yielding
three groups: (1) 3_34, i.e., 3-repeat boys from heterozygous mothers; (2) 4_34,
or 4-repeat boys from heterozygous mothers; and (3) 4_44 or 4-repeat boys from
homozygous 4-repeat mothers. If the associations with the MAO-A were maternal
in nature, we would predict that groups 1 and 2 would be similar and both would
differ from group 3. If the associations were strictly due to the alleles inherited by
the boys, then group 1 would differ from groups 2 and 3, with the latter groups
similar to one another. If the boys’ and the mothers’ alleles interacted, then all three
groups would differ from one another.
Data were analyzed with multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVAs) with
domains within each dimension of the PDDBI serving as the dependent variables.
Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to analyze the overall Autism Composite
score. Family status (simplex or multiplex) also served as a predictor to control for
the effects of living with a sibling having autism. Post-hoc analyses were used to
compare groups.
Within the Receptive/Expressive Social Communication Abilities dimension,
higher levels of ability in association with the 4-repeat allele were predicted based
on our previous data. This was confirmed for the Social Approach Behaviors domain
(SOCAPP; a measure of nonvocal social skills such as eye contact, gesture, play, and
empathy), Expressive Language domain (EXPRESS; a measure of phonological,
semantic and pragmatic verbal ability), and the Learning, Memory and Receptive
Language domain (LMRL; a measure of memory and receptive skills) except that
these were indeed maternal in nature. The 3_34, 4_34, and 4_44 groups had mean
SOCAPP (SE) T-scores of 44.9 (2.1), 47.2 (2.3), and 51.1 (1.6). Thus, there was about
a 0.5 SD difference between the 4_44 group and the other two groups (p < 0.022)
who did not differ from each other. Similar effects were present for the other two
domains. Interestingly, the LMRL domain has the highest correlation with IQ
(r (74) = 0.77; Cohen and Sudhalter, 2005) and the difference across groups for this
domain was the largest: the 3_34, 4_34, and 4_44 groups had mean (SE) T-scores
of 47.5 (2.2), 47.3 (2.4), and 54.6 (1.7). Thus, there was almost a 1 SD difference
between the 4_44 group and the other two groups (p < 0.014). These results confirm
our earlier findings but indicate that these differences are maternal in nature.
Within the Approach/Withdrawal Problems dimension of the PDDBI, signifi-
cant differences (p < 0.05) were evident across the three groups for three domains:
Sensory/Perceptual Approach Behaviors (SENSORY), replicating our original
observation; Ritualisms/Resistance to Change (RITUAL; a measure of engaging
in rituals or resisting changes in routines or in the environment); and Specific
Fears (FEARS; a measure of overall anxiety, sensitivity to noises, separation prob-
lems, etc.). The SENSORY effect was not maternal with the 3_34 group showing
higher T-scores (mean (SE) = 52.7 (1.7)) than the 4_34 or 4_44 groups (mean =
(SE) 48.1 (1.8) and 48.4 (1.4), respectively, p < 0.05). All of these scores were in
the autism range but the means differed by about 0.5 SD. However, the RITUAL
and FEARS effects were maternal in nature. The 3_34, 4_34, and 4_44 groups
had mean (SE) RITUAL T-scores of 50.8 (1.5), 47.7 (1.7), and 53.6 (1.3) and mean
(SE) FEARS T-scores of 50.2 (1.6), 48.0 (1.8), and 54.6 (1.4). Thus, again, there
was about a 0.5 SD difference between the 4_44 group and the other two groups
84 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
(p < 0.005). Conceptually, both fears and ritualistic behaviors are related to one
another as “internalizing” behaviors. Indeed, obsessive–compulsive disorder is
characterized in the DSM as an anxiety disorder.
A nonmaternal effect was present, however. Boys with the 3-repeat allele had
higher SENSORY scores, as noted, and higher Autism Composite Scores, a mea-
sure of overall severity, as predicted based on our previous data. The Autism
Composite was worse in boys with the 3-repeat allele by about 0.5 SD (p < 0.028).
The 3_34, 4_34, and 4_44 groups had mean (SE) T-scores of 55.2 (1.6), 49.6 (1.7),
and 50.9 (1.4).
REFERENCES
Abrahams, B. S. and D. H. Geschwind (2008). Advances in autism genetics: On the threshold
of a new neurobiology. Nat. Rev. Genet. 9:341–355.
American Psychiatric Association (1987). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders-III-R. Washington, DC.
American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders Fourth Edition Text Revision. Washington, DC.
Anderson, G. M. (1987). Monoamines in autism: An update of neurochemical research on a
pervasive developmental disorder. Med. Biol. 65:67–74.
Anderson, L. T., M. Campbell, P. Adams, A. M. Small, R. Perry, and J. Shell (1989). The effects
of haloperidol on discrimination learning and behavioral symptoms in autistic children.
J. Autism Dev. Disord. 19:227–239.
Azmitia, E. C. (2001). Neuronal instability: Implications for Rett’s syndrome. Brain Dev. 23,
Suppl 1:S1–S10.
Bailey, A., A. Le Couteur, I. Gottesman, P. Bolton, E. Simonoff, E. Yuzda, and M. Rutter
(1995). Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: Evidence from a British twin study.
Psychol. Med. 25:63–77.
Berument, S. K., M. Rutter, C. Lord, A. Pickles, and A. Bailey (1999). Autism screening
questionnaire: Diagnostic validity. Br. J. Psychiatry 175:144–451.
Biederman, J., S. V. Faraone, D. R. Hirshfeld-Becker, D. Friedman, J. A. Robin, and J. F.
Rosenbaum (2001). Patterns of psychopathology and dysfunction in high-risk children
of parents with panic disorder and major depression. Am. J. Psychiatry 158:49–57.
Bolton, P. F., A. Pickles, M. Murphy, and M. Rutter (1998). Autism, affective and other
psychiatric disorders: Patterns of familial aggregation. Psychol. Med. 28:385–395.
Brunner, H. G., M. Nelen, X. O. Breakefield, H. H. Ropers, and B. A. van Oost (1993).
Abnormal behavior associated with a point mutation in the structural gene for monoamine
oxidase A. Science 262:578–580.
Campbell, M., L. T. Anderson, M. Meier, I. L. Cohen, A.M. Small, C. Samit, and E. J. Sacher
(1978). A comparison of haloperidol and behavior therapy and their interaction in autistic
children. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 7:640–655.
Campbell, M., I. L. Cohen, R. Perry, A. M. Small, and M. Hersen (1988). Psychopharmacological
treatment. In T. H. Ollendick (ed.), Handbook of Child Psychopathology. New York:
Plenum Press.
Cases, O., C. Lebrand, B. Giros, T. Vitalis, E. De Maeyer, M. G. Caron, D. J. Price, P. Gaspar,
and I. Seif (1998). Plasma membrane transporters of serotonin, dopamine, and norepi-
nephrine mediate serotonin accumulation in atypical locations in the developing brain
of monoamine oxidase A knock-outs. J. Neurosci. 18:6914–6927.
Caspi, A., J. McClay, T. E. Moffitt, J. Mill, J. Martin, I. W. Craig, A. Taylor, and R. Poulton
(2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science
297:851–854.
Chauhan, A., V. Chauhan, W. T. Brown, and I. L. Cohen (2004). Oxidative stress in autism:
Increased lipid peroxidation and reduced serum levels of ceruloplasmin and transferrin—
the antioxidant proteins. Life Sci. 75:2539–2549.
Chugani, D. C., O. Muzik, M. Behen, R. Rothermel, J. J. Janisse, J. Lee, and H. T. Chugani
(1999). Developmental changes in brain serotonin synthesis capacity in autistic and
nonautistic children. Ann. Neurol. 45:287–295.
Cohen, I. L., G. S. Fisch, E. G. Wolf Schein, V. Sudhalter, D. Hanson, R. Hagerman, E. C.
Jenkins, and W. T. Brown (1988). Social gaze, social avoidance and repetitive behavior
in fragile X males: A controlled study. Am. J. Ment. Retard. 92:436–446.
Cohen, I. L. (1992). The behavioral phenotype of Fragile X Syndrome and its association with
autism. In R. J. Hagerman (ed.), Third International Fragile X Conference. Denver, CO:
Spectra Publishing.
Autism, Maternal Depression and the MAOA Gene 87
Cohen, I. L. (1995). Behavioral profiles of autistic and non-autistic fragile X males. Dev. Brain
Dysfunc. 8:252–269.
Cohen, I. L. (1996). A theoretical analysis of the role of hyperarousal in the behavior and
learning of fragile X males. Mentl. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 1:286–291.
Cohen, I. L. (2003). Criterion-related validity of the PDD Behavior Inventory. J. Autism Dev.
Disord. 33:47–53.
Cohen, I. L. and V. Sudhalter (2005). The PDD Behavior Inventory. Lutz, FL: Psychological
Assessment Resources, Inc.
Cohen, I. L. and J. Tsiouris (2006). Maternal recurrent mood disorders and high-functioning
autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36:1077–1078.
Cohen, I. L., M. Campbell, D. Posner, A. M. Small, D. Triebel, and L. T. Anderson (1980).
Behavioral effects of haloperidol in young autistic children. An objective analysis using
a within-subjects reversal design. J. Am. Acad. Child Psychiatry 19:665–677.
Cohen, I. L., P. M. Vietze, V. Sudhalter, E. C. Jenkins, and W. T. Brown (1989). Parent–child
dyadic gaze patterns in fragile X males and in non-fragile X males with autistic disorder.
J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 30:845–856.
Cohen, I. L., V. Sudhalter, A. Pfadt, E. C. Jenkins, W. T. Brown, and P. M. Vietze (1991).
Why are autism and the fragile-X syndrome associated? Conceptual and methodological
issues. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 48:195–202.
Cohen, I. L., S. L. Nolin, V. Sudhalter, X. H. Ding, C. S. Dobkin, and W. T. Brown (1996).
Mosaicism for the FMR1 gene influences adaptive skills development in fragile
X-affected males. Am. J. Med. Genet. 64:365–369.
Cohen, I. L., X. Liu, C. Schutz, B. N. White, E. C. Jenkins, W. T. Brown, and J. J. A. Holden
(2003a). Association of autism severity with a monoamine oxidase A functional
polymorphism. Clin. Genet. 64:190–197.
Cohen, I. L., S. Schmidt-Lackner, R. Romanczyk, and V. Sudhalter (2003b). The PDD Behavior
Inventory: A rating scale for assessing response to intervention in children with PDD.
J. Autism Dev. Disord. 33:31–45.
Coleman, M. (1976). The Autistic Syndromes. Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Coleman, M., P. N. Hart, J. Randall, J. Lee, D. Hijada, and C. G. Bratenahl (1977). Serotonin
levels in the blood and central nervous system of a patient with sudanophilic leu-
kodystrophy. Neuropadiatrie 8:459–466.
Collins, F. A., D. L. Murphy, A. L. Reiss, K. B. Sims, J. G. Lewis, L. Freund, F. Karoum,
D. Zhu, I. H. Maumenee, and S. E. Antonarakis (1992). Clinical, biochemical, and
neuropsychiatric evaluation of a patient with a contiguous gene syndrome due to a
microdeletion Xp11.3 including the Norrie disease locus and monoamine oxidase
(MAOA and MAOB) genes. Am. J. Med. Genet. 42:127–134.
Cook, E. H., B. L. Leventhal, W. Heller, J. Metz, M. Wainwright, and D. X. Freedman
(1990). Autistic children and their first-degree relatives: Relationships between sero-
tonin and norepinephrine levels and intelligence. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci.
2:268–274.
Cook, E. H., D. A. Charak, J. Arida, J. A. Spohn, N. J. Roizen, and B. L. Leventhal (1994).
Depressive and obsessive-compulsive symptoms in hyperserotonemic parents of children
with autistic disorder. Psychiatry Res. 52:25–33.
Cote, F., C. Fligny, E. Bayard, J. M. Launay, M. D. Gershon, J. Mallet, and G. Vodjani (2007).
Maternal serotonin is crucial for murine embryonic development. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
USA 104:329–334.
Davis, L. K., H. C. Hazlett, A. L. Librant, P. Nopoulos, V. C. Sheffield, J. Piven, and T. H.
Wassink (2008). Cortical enlargement in autism is associated with a functional VNTR
in the monoamine oxidase A gene. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet.
5:1145–1151.
DeLong, R. (1994). Children with autistic spectrum disorder and a family history of affective
disorder. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 36:674–687.
88 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
DeLong, G. R. (2004). Autism and familial major mood disorder: Are they related?
J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 16:199–213.
DeLong, G. R. and J. T. Dwyer (1988). Correlation of family history with specific autistic
subgroups: Asperger’s syndrome and bipolar affective disease. J. Autism Dev. Disord.
18:593–600.
DeLong, R. and C. Nohria (1994). Psychiatric family history and neurological disease in
autistic spectrum disorders. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 36:441–448.
Denney, R. M., H. Koch, and I. W. Craig (1999). Association between monoamine oxidase A
activity in human male skin fibroblasts and genotype of the MAOA promoter-associated
variable number tandem repeat. Hum. Genet. 105:542–551.
Durkin, M. S., M. J. Maenner, C. J. Newschaffer, L. C. Lee, C. M. Cuniff, J. L. Daniels, R. S.
Kirby, L. Leavitt, L. Miller, W. Zaharodny, and L. A. Schieve (2008). Advanced parental
age and the risk of autism spectrum disorder. Am. J. Epidemiol. 168:1268–1276.
Fombonne, E., P. Bolton, J. Prior, H. Jordan, and M. Rutter (1997). A family study of autism:
Cognitive patterns and levels in parents and siblings. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat.
38:667–683.
Gillberg, C. and M. Coleman (2000). The Biology of the Autistic Syndromes. London: Mac
Keith Press.
Good, C. D., K. Lawrence, N. S. Thomas, C. J. Price, J. Ashburner, K. J. Friston, R. S. J.
Frackowiak, L. Oreland, and D. H. Skuse (2003). Dosage-sensitive X-linked locus
influences the development of amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex, and fear recognition
in humans. Brain 126:1–6.
Happe, F. and A. Ronald (2008). The “Fractionable Autism Triad”: A review of evidence from
behavioural, genetic, cognitive and neural research. Neuropsychol. Rev. 18:287–304.
Holden, J. J. A., X. Liu, M. Floyd, E. C. Jenkins, W. T. Brown, S. Lewis, A. E. Chudley, ASD-
CARC, M. Gonzalez, T. Rovito-Gomez, and I. L. Cohen (2006). Effects of maternal
and child MAOA and serotonin transporter polymorphisms on the behavior patterns
of children on the autism spectrum. Poster presented at the International Meeting for
Autism Research, Montreal.
Hollander, E., A. Phillips, W. Chaplin, K. Zagursky, S. Novotny, S. Wasserman, and R. Iyengar
(2005). A placebo controlled crossover trial of liquid fluoxetine on repetitive behaviors
in childhood and adolescent autism. Neuropsychopharmacology 30:582–589.
Jones, M. B., R. M. Palmour, L. Zwaigenbaum, and P. Szatmari (2004). Modifier effects in
autism at the MAO-A and DBH loci. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet.
126B:58–65.
Karmel, B. Z., J. M. Gardner, L. D. Swensen, I. L. Cohen, E. London, E. M. Lennon, and
M. J. Flory (2008). Early medical and behavioral characteristics of NICU infants later
diagnosed or suspected with autism spectrum disorder. Paper presented at the Pediatric
Academic Societies and Asian Society for Pediatric Research, Honolulu, May 3–6.
Kinney, D. K., A. M. Miller, D. J. Crowley, E. Huang, and E. Gerber (2008). Autism prevalence
following prenatal exposure to hurricanes and tropical storms in Louisiana. J. Autism
Dev. Disord. 38:481–488.
Kolevzon, A., R. Gross, and A. Reichenberg (2007). Prenatal and perinatal risk factors for autism:
A review and integration of findings. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 161:326–333.
Kuperman, S., J. Beeghly, T. Burns, and L. Tsai (1987). Association of serotonin concentration
to behavior and IQ in autistic children. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 17:133–140.
Limperopoulos, C., H. Bassan, N. R. Sullivan, J. S. Soul, R. L. Robertson Jr., M. Moore,
S. A. Ringer, J. J. Volpe, and A. J. du Plessis (2008). Positive screening for autism in
ex-preterm infants: Prevalence and risk factors. Pediatrics 121:758–765.
Lord, C., M. Rutter, and A. Le Couteur (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised:
A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible
pervasive developmental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 24:659–685.
Autism, Maternal Depression and the MAOA Gene 89
Lord, C., M. Rutter, P. C. DiLavore, and S. Risi. (1999). Autism Diagnostic Observation
Schedule (ADOS). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Manuck, S. B., J. D. Flory, R. E. Ferrell, J. J. Mann, and M. F. Muldoon (2000). A regulatory
polymorphism of the monoamine oxidase-A gene may be associated with variabil-
ity in aggression, impulsivity, and central nervous system serotonergic responsivity.
Psychiatry Res. 95:9–23.
Marazziti, D., F. Muratori, A. Cesari, I. Masala, S. Baroni, G. Giannaccini, L. Dell’Osso,
A. Cosenza, P. Pfanner, and G. B. Cassano (2000). Increased density of the platelet
serotonin transporter in autism. Pharmacopsychiatry 33:165–168.
McDougle, C. J., L. Scahill, M. G. Aman, J. T. McCracken, E. Tierney, M. Davies, L. E.
Arnold, D. J. Posey, A. Martin, J. K. Ghuman, B. Shah, S. Z. Chuang, N. B. Swiezy,
N. M. Gonzalez, J. Hollway, K. Koenig, J. J. McGough, L. Ritz, and B. Vitiello (2005).
Risperidone for the core symptom domains of autism: Results from the study by the
autism network of the research units on pediatric psychopharmacology. Am. J. Psychiatry
162:1142–1148.
Meyer, J. H., N. Ginovart, A. Boovariwala, S. Sagrati, D. Hussey, A. Garcia, T. Young,
N. Praschak-Rieder, A. A. Wilson, and S. Houle (2006). Elevated monoamine oxidase A
levels in the brain: An explanation for the monoamine imbalance of major depression.
Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 63:1209–1216.
Miles, J. H., T. N. Takahashi, S. Bagby, P. K. Sahota, D. F. Vaslow, C. H. Wang, R. E. Hillman,
and J. E. Farmer (2005). Essential versus complex autism: Definition of fundamental
prognostic subtypes. Am. J. Med. Genet. 135:171–180.
Pinelli, J. and L. Zwaigenbaum (2008). Chorioamnionitis, gestational age, male sex, birth
weight, and illness severity predicted positive autism screening scores in very-low-birth-
weight preterm infants. Evid. Based Nurs. 11:122.
Piven, J. and P. Palmer (1999). Psychiatric disorder and the broad autism phenotype:
Evidence from a family study of multiple-incidence autism families. Am. J. Psychiatry
156:557–563.
Piven, J., G. A. Chase, R. Landa, M. Wzorek, J. Gayle, D. Cloud, and S. Folstein (1991).
Psychiatric disorders in the parents of autistic individuals. J. Am. Acad. Child Adol.
Psychiat. 30:471–478.
Sabol, S. Z., S. Hu, and D. Hamer (1998). A functional polymorphism in the monoamine
oxidase A gene promoter. Hum. Genet. 103:273–279.
Schendel, D. E. (2008). Autism and prenatal exposure to storms. Evid. Based Ment. Health 11:32.
Schendel, D. and T. K. Bhasin (2008). Birth weight and gestational age characteristics of
children with autism, including a comparison with other developmental disabilities.
Pediatrics 121:1155–1164.
Schopler, E., R. J. Reichler, R. F. Devellis, and K. Daly (1980). Toward objective classification
of childhood autism: Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS). J. Autism Dev. Disord.
10:91–103.
Schulze, T. G., D. J. Muller, H. Krauss, H. Scherk, S. Ohlraun, Y. V. Syagailo, C. Windemuth,
H. Neidt, M. Grassle, A. Papassotiropoulos, R. Heun, M. M. Nothen, W. Maier, K. P.
Lesch, and M. Rietschel (2000). Association between a functional polymorphism in the
monoamine oxidase A gene promoter and major depressive disorder. Am. J. Med. Genet.
96:801–803.
Slavotinek, A. and L. G. Biesecker (2003). Genetic modifiers in human development
and malformation syndromes, including chaperone proteins. Hum. Mol. Genet.
12:R45–R50.
Smalley, S. L., J. McCracken, and P. Tanguay (1995). Autism, affective disorders, and social
phobia. Am. J. Med. Genet. 60:19–26.
Szatmari, P. and M. B. Jones (1991). IQ and the genetics of autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat.
32:897–908.
90 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
CONTENTS
6.1 Introduction ................................................................................................... 91
6.2 Human Serum Paraoxonase 1 .......................................................................92
6.3 PON1 in Autism Spectrum Disorders ...........................................................96
6.4 Environmental Exposures ............................................................................. 98
6.5 Oxidative Stress .......................................................................................... 101
6.6 Inflammation ............................................................................................... 102
6.7 Conclusions ................................................................................................. 104
Acknowledgments.................................................................................................. 104
References .............................................................................................................. 104
6.1 INTRODUCTION
Autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) are a group of clinically and etiologically
heterogeneous disorders, affecting the core domains of communication and social
development and involving abnormal repetitive and restrictive behaviors (Rapin and
Tuchman, 2008). To date, ASDs are considered to be genetically influenced disorders,
91
92 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Centromere
7q21–q22
PON2 PON3 PON1
(a) 140 kb
L55M Q192R
C-909G A-832G A-162G C-126G C-108T Start
T11714A A20352G
(b)
XRE like
XRE
SRE like
NF-Y
NF-1
Sp1
Sp1
Sp1
(c) A-162G C-108T
FIGURE 6.1 Structure of paraoxonases’ genes. (a) PON gene family and location of PON1,
PON2, and PON3 on human chromosome 7, bands q21–q22. (b) The locations of polymor-
phisms in the promoter and coding region of the PON1 gene; substitutions T11714A and
A20352G in the exons 3 and 6 of the coding sequence correspond to L55M and Q192R
variation in the protein sequence respectively. (c) The –162 and –108 polymorphisms and
potential transcription factor binding sites in the 200 bp region of the promoter. SRE, sterol
regulatory element; Sp1, transcription factor; XRE, xenobiotic responsive elements; NF-Y,
nuclear factor-Y.
2001; Mackness, Arrol, and Durrington, 1991; Ng et al., 2005; Rozenberg, Shih, and
Aviram, 2005). PON1 received its name because of the ability to hydrolyze paraoxon,
the microsome-activated form of the insecticide parathion. Besides paraoxon, PON1
has the ability to hydrolyze other OP toxins, arylesters (e.g., phenylacetate), cyclic
carbonates, and lactones (Draganov and La Du, 2004). Recent comprehensive struc-
ture–activity studies with PON1 and PON1 variants generated through directed
evolution have demonstrated that PON1’s native activity is that of a lactonase, while
arylesterase and phosphotriesterase are promiscuous activities (Khersonsky and
Tawfik, 2006).
From a structural perspective, PON1 consists of 354 amino acids residues (355 with
N-terminal methionine) and has a molecular weight that can range from 38 to 45 kDa,
depending on its glycosylation state (Clendenning et al., 1996; Gan et al., 1991). PON1
is unusual in retaining the N-terminal signal sequence, which provides a hydrophobic
anchor for attachment to the HDL particle (Sorenson et al., 1999). Both activity and
stability of PON1 are dramatically dependent on the HDL components (phospholipids
and/or apolipoproteins) (Cabana et al., 2003; Rochu et al., 2007). At present, the exact
structure and catalytic mechanism of the PON1 enzyme are still unknown, although
a gene-shuffled bacterially expressed chimerical PON1 variant suggests that human
PON1 is a six-bladed β-propeller (each blade consisting of four β-pleated sheets) with
a “velcro” closure sealed by a disulfide bond between Cys42 and Cys353 (see Figure 6.2
for more details).
94 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
N
1
D 6 H1
D H2
C
C
2 B
B
H3
A A C
5
C
N
(a) 4 (b)
FIGURE 6.2 (See color insert following page 200.) Overall structure of PON1. (a) View
of the six-bladed-propeller from above. Shown are the N- and C-termini, and the two calcium
atoms in the central tunnel of the propeller (the “catalytic calcium” or Ca1, green (left); the
“structural calcium” or Ca2, red (right)). (b) A side view of the propeller. At the top of the
propeller, there are three helices H1–H3 which determine the PONs’ cell distribution, trans-
location and secretion (H1), and protein–lipid and protein–protein interactions (H2 and H3).
(Reproduced from Harel, M. et al., Nat. Struct. Mol. Biol., 11, 412, 2004. With permission.)
In a given population, plasma PON1 activity can vary up to 40-fold (Davies et al.,
1996; Richter and Furlong, 1999), and PON1 protein levels, within a given genotype
class, can vary up to 13-fold (Jarvik et al., 2000). Although genetic determinants play
an important role in determining the individual PON1 status, the contribution of other
factors in modulating PON1 activity may also be important. Age, gender, ethnicity,
diet (polyphenols, vitamin C and E, unsaturated fats), environmental chemicals
(OP exposure, heavy metals), drugs (statins, fibrates), as well as certain disease con-
ditions (e.g., liver disease, diabetes, inflammatory states), have been found to influence
PON1 activity (Costa et al., 2005; Deakin and James, 2004; Rainwater et al., 2009).
diazoxonase) in children with autism has also been confirmed recently in a bigger
sample size comprising children from Europe and North America (A.M. Persico
et al., unpublished results).
Most studies investigating the association of PON1 with various diseases have
examined only three SNPs: C-108T, L55M, Q192R. However, even if an individual
was genotyped for all known PON1 polymorphisms, this analysis would not
provide the level of plasma PON1 activity or the phase of polymorphisms (which
polymorphisms are on each of an individual’s two chromosomes). A functional
genomic analysis provides a much more informative approach. This is accomplished
through the use of a high-throughput enzyme assay involving two substrates:
paraoxon and phenyl acetate (Eckerson et al., 1983), diazoxon and paraoxon (Richter
and Furlong, 1999), and more recently the non-OP substrates: phenyl acetate and
4-(chloromethyl)-phenyl acetate (Richter et al., 2009). The two-dimensional enzyme
analysis, referred to as the determination of “PON1 status,” is much more useful and
informative than PCR-based genotypes alone for epidemiological studies examining
the role of PON1 in OP or lipid metabolism. Plotting rates of hydrolysis of one sub-
strate against a second substrate provided a clear resolution of the individuals with
low activities (phenotype AA) from individuals with intermediate and high activities
(phenotype AB and BB, respectively). We examined the PON1 phenotype (using
arylesterase and salt-stimulated paraoxonase activities) and found a similar distribution
in the ASD group and the control group (Paşca et al., 2009b). The similar phenotypic
distribution and the fact that there was no difference in the activities’ ratio between
groups suggest that the relationship between the two activities is generally main-
tained in autistics, regardless of a reduction in the hydrolytic protection. On the other
hand, while in the ASD group, the PON1 catalytic activity/Q192R dependency was
more pronounced than in controls, the dependency of the bioavailability of PON1
on the L55M polymorphism was absent in the ASD patients, whereas it reached a
borderline small level in healthy participants. Interestingly, in children with ASD
and first-degree relatives, the PON1R192 allele has been previously associated with a
reduction in PON1 arylesterase activity (Gaita and Persico, 2006). In a recent report,
we also showed, although in a small sample, that the distribution of the PON1 C-108T
genotypes was analogous in autistics and healthy controls (Kaucsár et al., 2009).
Nevertheless, we observed in this study that the PON1 promoter polymorphism had
a lower influence on the arylesterase activity’s variance in autistic patients than controls,
while the paraoxonase activity was not affected.
The intracellular status of PON2 still waits to be investigated in ASD, although
there is circumstantial evidence for a disturbed gene expression in hypothalamic
neurons in a rodent model for Rett syndrome (fold change in MeCP2-Tg versus WT
was 0.327, p = 0.0089) (Chahrour et al., 2008). Although it is expected to find pertur-
bations in PON2 in cells from autistic patients considering the previously reported
alterations in intracellular redox status, the exact contribution of PON2 is worth
exploring in future studies.
It is also worth mentioning that PON1 has a potentially imprinted expression in
mouse placenta (Okita et al., 2003). If confirmed in humans, the imprinted status of
PONs’ genes could have implications for the imprinting hypothesis for the develop-
ment of autism (Badcock and Crespi, 2006).
98 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
children, this effect could be enhanced due to a physiologically precarious PON1 status.
Studies in humans have shown that serum PON1 activity in newborns is fourfold
lower than the mothers’ PON1 levels, and it increases over time to reach a plateau
between 15 and 25 months of age (Cole et al., 2003; Furlong et al., 2005). Moreover,
there is an increased influence of genetic variation on PON1 activity in neonates
(Chen et al., 2003), while low PON1 activity was also reported to be associated
with smaller neonatal head circumference (Berkowitz et al., 2004). Interestingly,
birth outcomes (e.g., birth length, head circumference) have been connected with
PON1 status in mothers (Wolff et al., 2007). In premature babies (33–36 weeks of
gestation), PON1 activity measured using phenyl acetate as a substrate is 24% lower
compared to full-term babies (Ecobichon and Stephens, 1973).
Therefore, the highly reactive intermediates of pesticides might account for the
decreased paraoxonase activity of PON1 in ASD, as these compounds can inactivate
the enzyme (Hernandez et al., 2004). An upregulation of PON1 following chronic
OP exposure was described only in carriers of the PON1192R allele (Browne et al.,
2006), but this allele has been associated with a reduction in PON1 arylesterase
activity in children with ASD and first-degree relatives (Gaita and Persico, 2006).
In addition, it was also reported that carriers of the PON1192R allele, as compared
to individuals with the QQ genotype, exhibited lower acetylcholinesterase activity
(Hernandez et al., 2004). Notably, neonatal exposure to parathion in rats (at doses
straddling the threshold for cholinesterases inhibition) compromises the indices of
acetylcholine synaptic function in adolescence and adulthood (Slotkin, Levin, and
Seidler, 2009). Therefore, an altered PON1 status could also be related, at least con-
ceptually for now, with the previously described alterations in the cholinergic system
in autism (Perry et al., 2001). Differences in the regional expression (e.g., parietal
cortex, cerebellum, and thalamus) of nicotinic acetylcholine receptors’ subunits have
been documented in postmortem brains from autistics and these results are sugges-
tive of a potential enhanced susceptibility to OP exposure (Court et al., 2000; Lee
et al., 2002). Consequently, an impaired catalytic efficacy of PON1 to degrade OP
during an environmental exposure could have far more deleterious consequences in
children displaying subtle alterations in the central cholinergic transmission (Pessah
et al., 2008). This gene (PON1, AchR subunits) × environment (OP exposure, inhibi-
tors of PON1 activity) interaction should be further explored in future prospective
studies in relation to the autism risk.
The data indicating OP exposure as a risk factor for ASD (Eskenazi et al., 2007;
Rauh et al., 2006) and the fact that overall these patients display lower PON1 activi-
ties (Paşca et al., 2006; Paşca et al., 2009b) are congruent with another recently
proposed gene × environment interaction model, which includes PON1, a history
of OP exposure, and a genetic or epigenetic based impairment in Reelin (Persico
and Bourgeron, 2006) (Figure 6.3). Reelin is an extracellular serine protease that
plays a pivotal role in the central nervous system by regulating the processes of neu-
ronal migration and positioning in the developing brain and by modulating synaptic
plasticity in the adult cerebral cortex. Reelin, secreted by Cajal–Retzius cells in the
cortex and external granular layer and cerebellar nuclear neurons in the cerebellum,
is crucial for the configuration of the mature cortical architecture, and exerts its
functions by binding to a variety of receptors (Vldlr, ApoER2, a3b1 integrins) and
100 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
FIGURE 6.3 (See color insert following page 200.) The gene × environment model for
autism. The model involves the Reelin (RELN) and Paraoxonase 1 (PON1) genes, and pre-
natal exposure to organophosphates (OP). RELN variants carrying either “normal” (7−10
repeats) or “long” (≥12 repeats) GGC alleles genetically determine whether levels of reelin
are normal or reduced, respectively. In principle, both conditions are compatible with normal
neurodevelopment. However, prenatal exposure to OP can transiently inhibit the proteolytic
activity of reelin, which might then fall below the threshold required for correct neuronal
migration, also depending on baseline levels of RELN gene expression determined geneti-
cally and epigenetically. In addition, exposure to identical doses of OP can affect reelin to
a different extent depending on the amount and affinity spectrum of the OP- inactivating
enzyme paraoxonase produced by the PON1 alleles of each individual. (Reproduced from
Persico, A.M. and Bourgeron, T., Trends Neurosci., 29, 349, 2006. With permission.)
(i.e., less active PON1 variants × “long” allele of RELN × pre/postnatal OP exposure),
results from our studies indicated that PON1 activities may be impaired in ASD
patients in the absence of an association with less active PON1 gene variants (i.e.,
Q192R, L55M, C-108T) (Kaucsár et al., 2009; Paşca et al., 2009b). Future studies
should better delineate the effects of RELN dosage in the context of a pesticides
exposure and reduced PON1-mediated degradation of toxic agents.
Besides OP, exposure to several other environmental chemicals can have a sub-
stantial impact on serum PON1 activity, and could therefore, be relevant to autism.
For instance, heavy metals cations (cadmium, iron, zinc, and mercury) have been
shown to be, at least in vitro, potent inhibitors of PON1R192 activity, while PON1Q192
appear to be less sensitive to metal inhibition, excepting lead (Cole et al., 2002).
while patients with the less severe form of the disease (i.e., PDD-NOS) pre-
sented only with disturbances in the methionine cycle; in addition, since the lev-
els of both folate and vitamin B12 were within the normal range, we excluded
vitamin deficiencies as a cause for these impairments (Paşca et al., 2009a).
A disturbed one-carbon metabolism, although (apparently) nonspecific to autism,
could lead to a diminished capacity for methylation, with consequences on gene
expression, neurotransmitter synthesis, and, potentially, on neuronal synchroni-
zation. It remains to be further investigated how much the metabolic profiles in
plasma echo the actual intracellular metabolic dynamics, and whether corrections
of these impairments will lead to genuine clinical improvements.
PON1 was found to efficiently hydrolyze not only lipoprotein-associated perox-
ides (including cholesteryl linoleate hydroperoxides), but also hydrogen peroxide
(H2O2), and may thus play an important role in eliminating potent oxidants involved
in atherogenesis (Aviram et al., 1998). Conversely, PON1 also serves as a target for
lipid peroxidation products, resulting in an inhibition of PON1 activities (arylesterase
and lactonase) and a reduction in PON1 gene expression (Aviram et al., 1999; Van
Lenten et al., 2001). PON1 induces the lysophosphatidylcholine (LPC) formation,
which might act in both direction as pro-oxidant (based on the upregulated effects
on adhesive molecules) and antioxidant (by increasing the expression of extracellular
SOD in monocyte-macrophages) (Rosenblat, Oren, and Aviram, 2006). Moreover,
PON1 can be inactivated by S-glutathionylation, resulting from a reaction between
its free sulfhydryl group at Cys284 and oxidized glutathione (GSSG) (Rozenberg and
Aviram, 2006). Considering these facts, one may assume that in ASD, an altered
PON1 could contribute to the perturbed redox homeostasis, while an enhanced oxi-
dative stress could easily disturb the PON1 status.
6.6 INFLAMMATION
Experimental and clinical evidence accumulated in the last decades have led to
the idea that, at least in a subgroup of patients with autism, immune imbalance or
even autoimmune processes may play a considerable role in the pathophysiology
of the autistic disorder (Ashwood, Wills, and Van de Water, 2006; Pardo, Vargas,
and Zimmerman, 2005). Previous studies have already reported a wide plethora of
immune abnormalities, including T-cell, B-cell, and NK-cell dysfunction; autoanti-
body production; and increased proinflammatory cytokines.
Studies published more than a decade ago indicated that HDL protects against bac-
terial lipopolysaccharide (LPS), the endotoxin found in the outer membrane of Gram-
negative bacteria (Levine et al., 1993). Animal experiments have later showed that
purified PON1 (type Q) is able to protect cells from LPS and prevent or greatly reduce
the release of cytokines (La Du et al., 1999), suggesting a potential role for PON1 in
modulating immune responses. Interestingly, Jyonouchi and coworkers showed that
peripheral blood mononuclear cells (PBMCs) from ASD patients exposed to LPS pro-
duced excessive amounts of tumor necrosis factor-α (TNF-α), interleukin 1 (IL-1),
and/or interleukin 6 (IL-6) compared to controls, indicative of an aberrant innate
immune response in a considerable percentage of children with ASD (Jyonouchi,
Sun, and Le, 2001). Moreover, Sweeten, Posey, and McDougle, (2003) showed that
Paraoxonase 1 Status, Environmental Exposures, and Oxidative Stress 103
the mean absolute monocyte count was significantly higher in the autistic children,
which was accompanied by increased levels of neopterin; these findings were inter-
preted as heightened cell-mediated immunity in ASD. In the context of the maternal
immune activation (MIA) model for developmental disorders, it is worth mentioning
that maternal injections of LPS in rodents result in increased anxiety, deficits in social
interaction in the adult offspring; this suggests that short systemic maternal inflam-
mation has long-lasting consequences on the adult mouse stress and social behavior
(Hava et al., 2006). On the other hand, the cytokine IL-6 was already identified
as a key intermediary contributing to MIA in rodents (Smith et al., 2007). Vargas
et al. (2005) demonstrated in postmortem autistic brain, excessive significantly higher
level of subsets of cytokines (IL-6, IL-10, MCP-3, etc.) in brain regions that have been
previously associated with autism (i.e., anterior cingulated gyrus). In a small sample
of patients with autism displaying regressive features, Chez et al. (2007) reported
elevated levels of TNF-α in cerebrospinal fluid. These and other studies suggest that
at least some autistic children display a subclinical inflammatory state.
During inflammation, particularly during the acute-phase response, there is a
reduction in several proteins important in reverse cholesterol transport and inhibit-
ing oxidation. These include cholesterol ester transfer protein, lecithin cholesterol
acyltransferase, hepatic lipase, apoA-I, and PON1. It is thought that reduction in
these proteins, accompanied by an increase in proteins such as apoJ and serum
amyloid A (SAA), changes HDL from an anti-inflammatory to a proinflammatory
particle (Van Lenten et al., 2006). Several studies provide evidence that hepatic
PON1 mRNA is downregulated in response to inflammatory cytokines (Deakin
and James, 2008). Treatment with LPS and oxidized LDL, known to induce an
immune response, reduced PON1 mRNA in human hepatocyte cells line (Feingold
et al., 1998). A similar effect was observed with administration of TNF-α, IL-1,
and IL-6 (Feingold et al., 1998; Kumon et al., 2002). Therefore, HDL loses its anti-
inflammatory properties during acute infection, and PON1 activity and concentration
are regulated by inflammatory cytokines and endotoxin administration. In this
context, it could be concluded that PON1 is a negative acute-phase protein whose
mRNA is rapidly downregulated (Feingold et al., 1998). In an ongoing study, we did
not identify any statistical correlation between arylesterase or paraoxonase activi-
ties of PON1 and plasma C-reactive protein (CRP) levels in a population of children
with typical autism (unpublished observation).
NO is a unique biological messenger molecule involved in neurodevelopment, but
also a versatile player in the immune system. Recent studies have shown that NO
may also play a role in the pathophysiology of autism. Apparently, autistic children
display increased NO levels in red blood cells (Sogut et al., 2003) and increased levels
of plasma NO metabolites (Sweeten et al., 2004; Zoroglu et al., 2003). A recent arti-
cle reported a nitrite-mediated inactivation of PON1, in a dose- and time-dependent
manner; this inactivation is probably generated through nitration of enzyme phenyl
residues, while tryptophan could be of important value in minimizing this effect
(Abd-Allah and Mariee, 2008). Future research should elucidate in more details the
role of PON1 in modulation of immune responses and, consequently, how is the
altered PON1 status in autism contributing to the aberrant inflammatory responses
in autism.
104 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
6.7 CONCLUSIONS
The biochemical and genetics studies indicate that the PON1 status is impaired in
children with ASD. The current evidence points toward differences in the frequency
of PON1 gene variants in autistic patients between North America and Europe.
Currently, the reasons behind the PON1 status alteration in autism are not known,
nor are all its consequences. Besides the SNPs investigated (i.e., C192R, L55M,
C-108T) to date, other SNPs in the PON1 gene or at distant loci could be associated
with ASD. Chronic OP exposure, increased and prolonged oxidative stress, an altered
hepatic status, environmental heavy metals, or a silent subclinical inflammatory
state are all factors that could contribute to lessening the PON1 bioavailability and/
or catalytic activity. Whatever are the causes, an altered PON1 status would increase
the susceptibility to neurotoxic pesticides (especially in Reelin-deficient subjects
during critical periods) and lead to deficiencies in corticogenesis and cholinergic
neurotransmission, as well as contribute to the perturbation in the redox homeostasis
and immune dysfunctions; and all these despite the relative nonspecificity of the
PON1 alterations in autism.
We suggest that future studies should be performed on bigger, prospec-
tive cohorts of ASD patients, and that they should address the following issues:
(a) investigate for a possible relationship between PON1 and the severity of the
clinical phenotype; (b) elucidate the effect of HDL, inflammation, and oxidative
biomarkers on PON1 in autism; (c) replicate and verify for causality the asso-
ciation between OP exposure and autism; (d) measure the putative physiological
activity of PON1, i.e., lactonase activity; and (e) investigate the effect of other
SNPs or distant genetic loci.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors would like to thank Drs. Anca M. Paşca, Raul C. Muresan, and Carmen
Gherasim for fruitful discussions and pertinent comments on previous versions of
this manuscript.
REFERENCES
Abd-Allah, G. M. and A. D. Mariee (2008). Nitrite-mediated inactivation of human plasma
paraoxonase-1: Possible beneficial effect of aromatic amino acids. Appl. Biochem.
Biotechnol. 150:281–288.
Abrahams, B. S. and D. H. Geschwind (2008). Advances in autism genetics: On the threshold
of a new neurobiology. Nat. Rev. Genet. 9:341–355.
Adams, M., M. Lucock, J. Stuart, S. Fardell, K. Baker, and X. Ng (2007). Preliminary
evidence for involvement of the folate gene polymorphism 19 bp deletion-DHFR in
occurrence of autism. Neurosci. Lett. 422:24–29.
Adkins, S., K. N. Gan, M. Mody, and B. N. La Du (1993). Molecular basis for the polymorphic
forms of human serum paraoxonase/arylesterase: glutamine or arginine at position 191,
for the respective A or B allozymes. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 52:598–608.
Ahmed, Z., A. Ravandi, G. F. Maguire, A. Emili, D. Draganov, B. N. La Du, A. Kuksis, and
P. W. Connelly (2002). Multiple substrates for paraoxonase-1 during oxidation of phos-
phatidylcholine by peroxynitrite. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 290:391–396.
Paraoxonase 1 Status, Environmental Exposures, and Oxidative Stress 105
Ashwood, P., S. Wills, and J. Van de Water (2006). The immune response in autism: A new
frontier for autism research. J. Leukoc. Biol. 80:1–15.
Aviram, M., E. Hardak, J. Vaya, S. Mahmood, S. Milo, A. Hoffman, S. Billicke, D. Draganov,
and M. Rosenblat (2000). Human serum paraoxonases (PON1) Q and R selectively
decrease lipid peroxides in human coronary and carotid atherosclerotic lesions: PON1
esterase and peroxidase-like activities. Circulation 101:2510–2517.
Aviram, M., M. Rosenblat, S. Billecke, J. Erogul, R. Sorenson, C. L. Bisgaier, R. S. Newton,
and B. La Du (1999). Human serum paraoxonase (PON 1) is inactivated by oxi-
dized low density lipoprotein and preserved by antioxidants. Free Radic. Biol. Med.
26:892–904.
Aviram, M., M. Rosenblat, C. L. Bisgaier, R. S. Newton, S. L. Primo-Parmo, and B. N. La Du
(1998). Paraoxonase inhibits high-density lipoprotein oxidation and preserves its func-
tions. A possible peroxidative role for paraoxonase. J. Clin. Invest. 101:1581–1590.
Badcock, C. and B. Crespi (2006). Imbalanced genomic imprinting in brain development: An
evolutionary basis for the aetiology of autism. J. Evol. Biol. 19:1007–1032.
Berkowitz, G. S., J. G. Wetmur, E. Birman-Deych, J. Obel, R. H. Lapinski, J. H. Godbold,
I. R. Holzman, and M. S. Wolff (2004). In utero pesticide exposure, maternal paraoxonase
activity, and head circumference. Environ. Health Perspect. 112:388–391.
Brophy, V. H., M. D. Hastings, J. B. Clendenning, R. J. Richter, G. P. Jarvik, and C. E. Furlong
(2001). Polymorphisms in the human paraoxonase (PON1) promoter. Pharmacogenetics
11:77–84.
Brophy, V. H., G. P. Jarvik, and C. E. Furlong (2002). PON1 polymorphisms. In L. Costa
and C. Furlong (eds.), Paraoxonase (PON1) in Health and Disease: Basic and Clinical
Aspects. Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic Press.
Browne, R. O., L. B. Moyal-Segal, D. Zumsteg, Y. David, O. Kofman, A. Berger, H. Soreq,
and A. Friedman (2006). Coding region paraoxonase polymorphisms dictate accen-
tuated neuronal reactions in chronic, sub-threshold pesticide exposure. FASEB J.
20:1733–1735.
Cabana, V. G., C. A. Reardon, N. Feng, S. Neath, J. Lukens, and G. S. Getz (2003). Serum
paraoxonase: Effect of the apolipoprotein composition of HDL and the acute phase
response. J. Lipid Res. 44:780–792.
Chahrour, M., S. Y. Jung, C. Shaw, X. Zhou, S. T. Wong, J. Qin, and H. Y. Zoghbi (2008).
MeCP2, a key contributor to neurological disease, activates and represses transcription.
Science 320:1224–1229.
Chauhan, A. and V. Chauhan (2006). Oxidative stress in autism. Pathophysiology
13:171–181.
Chauhan, A., V. Chauhan, W. T. Brown, and I. Cohen (2004). Oxidative stress in autism:
Increased lipid peroxidation and reduced serum levels of ceruloplasmin and transferrin—
the antioxidant proteins. Life Sci. 75:2539–2549.
Chen, J., M. Kumar, W. Chan, G. Berkowitz, and J. G. Wetmur (2003). Increased influ-
ence of genetic variation on PON1 activity in neonates. Environ. Health Perspect.
111:1403–1409.
Chez, M. G., T. Dowling, P. B. Patel, P. Khanna, and M. Kominsky (2007). Elevation of
tumor necrosis factor-alpha in cerebrospinal fluid of autistic children. Pediatr. Neurol.
36:361–365.
Clendenning, J. B., R. Humbert, E. D. Green, C. Wood, D. Traver, and C. E. Furlong (1996).
Structural organization of the human PON1 gene. Genomics 35:586–589.
Cole, T. B., R. L. Jampsa, B. J. Walter, T. L. Arndt, R. J. Richter, D. M. Shih, A. Tward, A. J.
Lusis, R. M. Jack, L. G. Costa, and C. E. Furlong (2003). Expression of human paraoxo-
nase (PON1) during development. Pharmacogenetics 13:357–364.
Cole, T. B., W. F. Li, R. J. Richter, C. E. Furlong, and L. G. Costa (2002). Inhibition of
Paraoxonase (PON1) by heavy metals. Toxicol. Sci. 66:312.
106 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Costa, L. G., T. B. Cole, G. P. Jarvik, and C. E. Furlong (2003). Functional genomic of the
paraoxonase (PON1) polymorphisms: Effects on pesticide sensitivity, cardiovascular
disease, and drug metabolism. Annu. Rev. Med. 54:371–392.
Costa, L. G., A. Vitalone, T. B. Cole, and C. E. Furlong (2005). Modulation of paraoxonase
(PON1) activity. Biochem. Pharmacol. 69:541–550.
Court, J. A., C. Martin-Ruiz, A. Graham, and E. Perry (2000). Nicotinic receptors in human
brain: Topography and pathology. J. Chem. Neuroanat. 20:281–298.
D’Amelio, M., I. Ricci, R. Sacco, X. Liu, L. D’Agruma, L. A. Muscarella, V. Guarnieri,
R. Militerni, C. Bravaccio, M. Elia, C. Schneider, R. Melmed, S. Trillo, T. Pascucci,
S. Puglisi-Allegra, K. L. Reichelt, F. Macciardi, J. J. Holden, and A. M. Persico (2005).
Paraoxonase gene variants are associated with autism in North America, but not in
Italy: Possible regional specificity in gene-environment interactions. Mol. Psychiatry
10:1006–1016.
Davies, H. G., R. J. Richter, M. Keifer, C. A. Broomfield, J. Sowalla, and C. E. Furlong (1996).
The effect of the human serum paraoxonase polymorphism is reversed with diazoxon,
soman and sarin. Nat. Genet. 14:334–336.
Deakin, S., I. Leviev, M. C. Brulhart-Meynet, and R. W. James (2003). Paraoxonase-1 promoter
haplotypes and serum paraoxonase: A predominant role for polymorphic position-107,
implicating the Sp1 transcription factor. Biochem. J. 372:643–649.
Deakin, S., I. Leviev, M. Gomaraschi, L. Calabresi, G. Franceschini, and R. W. James (2002).
Enzymatically active paraoxonase-1 is located at the external membrane of producing
cells and released by a high affinity, saturable, desorption mechanism. J. Biol. Chem.
277:4301–4308.
Deakin, S. P. and R. W. James (2004). Genetic and environmental factors modulating serum
concentrations and activities of the antioxidant enzyme paraoxonase-1. Clin. Sci.
107:435–447.
Deakin, S. P. and R. W. James (2008). Transcriptional regulation of the paraoxonase genes. In
B. Mackness, M. Mackness, M. Aviram, and G. Paragh (eds.), The Paraoxonase: Their
Role in Disease Development and Xenobiotic Metabolism. Dordrecht, the Netherlands:
Springer.
Draganov, D. I. and B. N. La Du (2004). Pharmacogenetics of paraoxonases: A brief review.
Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch. Pharmacol. 369:78–88.
Draganov, D. I. and J. F. Teiber (2008). PON’s natural substrates—The key for their physi-
ological roles. In B. Mackness, M. Mackness, M. Aviram, and G. Paragh (eds.), The
Paraoxonase: Their Role in Disease Development and Xenobiotic Metabolism.
Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Springer.
Draganov, D. I., J. F. Teiber, A. Speelman, Y. Osawa, R. Sunahara, and B. N. La Du (2005).
Human paraoxonases (PON1, PON2, and PON3) are lactonases with overlapping and
distinct substrate specificities. J. Lipid Res. 46:1239–1247.
Durrington, P. N., B. Mackness, and M. I. Mackness (2001). Paraoxonase and atherosclerosis.
Arterioscler. Thromb. Vasc. Biol. 21:473–480.
Eckerson, H. W., J. Romson, C. Wyte, and B. N. La Du (1983). The human serum paraoxo-
nase polymorphism: Identification of phenotypes by their response to salts. Am. J. Hum.
Genet. 35:214–227.
Ecobichon, D. J. and D. S. Stephens (1973). Perinatal development of human blood esterases.
Clin. Pharmacol. Ther. 14:41–47.
Engel, S. M., G. S. Berkowitz, D. B. Barr, S. L. Teitelbaum, J. Siskind, S. J. Meisel, J. G.
Wetmur, and M. S. Wolff (2007). Prenatal organophosphate metabolite and organochlo-
rine levels and performance on the Brazelton Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale in
a multiethnic pregnancy cohort. Am. J. Epidemiol. 165:1397–1404.
Eskenazi, B., A. R. Marks, A. Bradman, K. Harley, D. B. Barr, C. Johnson, N. Morga, and
N. P. Jewell (2007). Organophosphate pesticide exposure and neurodevelopment in young
Mexican-American children. Environ. Health Perspect. 115:792–798.
Paraoxonase 1 Status, Environmental Exposures, and Oxidative Stress 107
Laviola, G., W. Adriani, C. Gaudino, R. Marino, and F. Keller (2006). Paradoxical effects
of prenatal acetylcholinesterase blockade on neuro-behavioral development and drug-
induced stereotypies in reeler mutant mice. Psychopharmacology 187:331–344.
Lee, M., C. Martin-Ruiz, A. Graham, J. Court, E. Jaros, R. Perry, P. Iversen, M. Bauman, and
E. Perry (2002). Nicotinic receptor abnormalities in the cerebellar cortex in autism.
Brain 125:1483–1495.
Levine, D. M., T. S. Parker, T. M. Donnelly, A. Walsh, and A. L. Rubin (1993). In vivo
protection against endotoxin by plasma high density lipoprotein. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
USA 90:12040–12044.
Lintas, C. and A. M. Persico. (2008). Reelin gene polymorphisms in autistic disorder. In S. H.
Fatemi (ed.), Reelin Glycoprotein: Structure, Biology and Roles in Health and Disease.
New York: Springer.
Mackness, B., M. I. Mackness, S. Arrol, W. Turkie, and P. N. Durrington (1998). Effect of the
human serum paraoxonase 55 and 192 genetic polymorphisms on the protection by high
density lipoprotein against low density lipoprotein oxidative modification. FEBS Lett.
423:57–60.
Mackness, M. I., S. Arrol, C. Abbott, and P. N. Durrington (1993). Protection of low-density
lipoprotein against oxidative modification by high-density lipoprotein associated
paraoxonase. Atherosclerosis 104:129–135.
Mackness, M. I., S. Arrol, and P. N. Durrington (1991). Paraoxonase prevents accumulation of
lipoperoxides in low-density lipoprotein. FEBS Lett. 286:152–154.
Mochizuki, H., S. W. Scherer, T. Xi, D. C. Nickle, M. Majer, J. J. Huizenga, L. C. Tsui, and
M. Prochazka (1998). Human PON2 gene at 7q21.3: Cloning, multiple mRNA forms,
and missense polymorphisms in the coding sequence. Gene 213:149–157.
Ng, C. J., D. M. Shih, S. Y. Hama, N. Villa, M. Navab, and S. T. Reddy (2005). The paraoxonase
gene family and atherosclerosis. Free Radic. Biol. Med. 38:153–163.
Ng, C. J., D. J. Wadleigh, A. Gangopadhyay, S. Hama, V. R. Grijalva, M. Navab, A. M.
Fogelman, and S. T. Reddy (2001). Paraoxonase-2 is a ubiquitously expressed protein
with antioxidant properties and is capable of preventing cell-mediated oxidative modifi-
cation of low density lipoprotein. J. Biol. Chem. 276:44444–44449.
Okita, C., M. Meguro, H. Hoshiya, M. Haruta, Y. K. Sakamoto, and M. Oshimura (2003).
A new imprinted cluster on the human chromosome 7q21-q31, identified by human–
mouse monochromosomal hybrids. Genomics 81:556–559.
Pardo, C. A., D. L. Vargas, and A. W. Zimmerman (2005). Immunity, neuroglia and neuroin-
flammation in autism. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 17:485–495.
Paşca, S. P., E. Dronca, T. Kaucsár, E. C. Craciun, E. Endreffy, B. Ferenz, F. Iftene, I. Benga,
R. Cornean, R. Banerjee, and M. Dronca (2009a). One carbon metabolism disturbances
and the C677T MTHFR gene polymorphism in children with autism spectrum disorders.
J. Cell. Mol. Med. (in press).
Paşca, S. P., E. Dronca, B. Nemeş, T. Kaucsár, E. Endreffy, F. Iftene, I. Benga, R. Cornean, and
M. Dronca (2009b). Paraoxonase 1 activities and polymorphisms in autism spectrum
disorders. J. Cell. Mol. Med. (in press).
Paşca, S. P., B. Nemes, L. Vlase, C. E. Gagyi, E. Dronca, A. C. Miu, and M. Dronca (2006).
High levels of homocysteine and low serum paraoxonase 1 arylesterase activity in children
with autism. Life Sci. 78:2244–2248.
Perry, E. K., M. L. Lee, C. M. Martin-Ruiz, J. A. Court, S. G. Volsen, J. Merrit, E. Folly,
P. E. Iversen, M. L. Bauman, R. H. Perry, and G. L. Wenk (2001). Cholinergic activity in
autism: abnormalities in the cerebral cortex and basal forebrain. Am. J. Psychiatry
158:1058–1066.
Perry, S. W., J. P. Norman, A. Litzburg, and H. A. Gelbard (2004). Antioxidants are required
during the early critical period, but not later, for neuronal survival. J. Neurosci. Res.
78:485–492.
110 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Persico, A. M. and T. Bourgeron (2006). Searching for ways out of the autism maze: Genetic,
epigenetic and environmental clues. Trends Neurosci. 29:349–358.
Persico, A. M., L. D’Agruma, N. Maiorano, A. Totaro, R. Militerni, C. Bravaccio, T. H.
Wassink, C. Schneider, R. Melmed, S. Trillo, F. Montecchi, M. Palermo, T. Pascucci,
S. Puglisi-Allegra, K. L. Reichelt, M. Conciatori, R. Marino, C. C. Quattrocchi, A. Baldi,
L. Zelante, P. Gasparini, and F. Keller (2001). Reelin gene alleles and haplotypes as a
factor predisposing to autistic disorder. Mol. Psychiatry 6:150–159.
Pessah, I. N., R. F. Seegal, P. J. Lein, J. LaSalle, B. K. Yee, J. Van De Water, and R. F.
Berman (2008). Immunologic and neurodevelopmental susceptibilities of autism.
Neurotoxicology 29:532–545.
Primo-Parmo, S. L., R. C. Sorenson, J. Teiber, and B. N. La Du (1996). The human serum
paraoxonase/arylesterase gene (PON1) is one member of a multigene family. Genomics
33:498–507.
Rainwater, D. L., S. Rutherford, T. D. Dyer, E. D. Rainwater, S. A. Cole, J. L. Vandeberg,
L. Almasy, J. Blangero, J. W. Maccluer, and M. C. Mahaney (2009). Determinants of varia-
tion in human serum paraoxonase activity. Heredity 102:147–154.
Rapin, I. and R. F. Tuchman (2008). Autism: Definition, neurobiology, screening, diagnosis.
Pediatr. Clin. North Am. 55:1129–1146.
Rauh, V. A., R. Garfinkel, F. P. Perera, H. F. Andrews, L. Hoepner, D. B. Barr, R. Whitehead,
D. Tang, and R. W. Whyatt (2006). Impact of prenatal chlorpyrifos exposure on
neurodevelopment in the first 3 years of life among inner-city children. Pediatrics
118:e1845–1859.
Reddy, S. T., D. J. Wadleigh, V. Grijalva, C. Ng, S. Hama, A. Gangopadhyay, D. M. Shih,
A. J. Lusis, M. Navab, and A. M. Fogelman (2001). Human paraoxonase-3 is an HDL-
associated enzyme with biological activity similar to paraoxonase-1 protein but is not
regulated by oxidized lipids. Arterioscler. Thromb. Vasc. Biol. 21:542–547.
Richter, R. J. and C. E. Furlong (1999). Determination of paraoxonase (PON1) status requires
more than genotyping. Pharmacogenetics 9:745–753.
Richter, R. J., G. P. Jarvik, and C. E. Furlong (2009). Paraoxonase 1 (PON1) status and substrate
hydrolysis. Toxicol. Appl. Pharmacol. 235:1–9.
Roberts, E. M., P. B. English, J. K. Grether, G. C. Windham, L. Somberg, and C. Wolff
(2007). Maternal residence near agricultural pesticide applications and autism spectrum
disorders among children in the California Central Valley. Environ. Health Perspect.
115:1482–1489.
Rochu, D., E. Chabriere, F. Renault, M. Elias, C. Clery-Barraud, and P. Masson (2007).
Stabilization of the active form(s) of human paraoxonase by human phosphate-binding
protein. Biochem. Soc. Trans. 35:1616–1620.
Rodrigo, L., B. Mackness, P. N. Durrington, A. Hernandez, and M. I. Mackness (2001). Hydrolysis
of platelet-activating factor by human serum paraoxonase. Biochem. J. 354:1–7.
Roest, M., T. M. van Himbergen, A. B. Barendrecht, P. H. Peeters, Y. T. van der Schouw, and
H. A. Voorbij (2007). Genetic and environmental determinants of the PON-1 phenotype.
Eur. J. Clin. Invest. 37:187–196.
Rosas, L. G. and B. Eskenazi (2008). Pesticides and child neurodevelopment. Curr. Opin.
Pediatr. 20:191–197.
Rosenblat, M., R. Oren, and M. Aviram (2006). Lysophosphatidylcholine (LPC) attenuates mac-
rophage-mediated oxidation of LDL. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 344:1271–1277.
Rosenblat, M., J. Vaya, D. Shih, and M. Aviram (2005). Paraoxonase 1 (PON1) enhances
HDL-mediated macrophage cholesterol efflux via the ABCA1 transporter in association
with increased HDL binding to the cells: a possible role for lysophosphatidylcholine.
Atherosclerosis 179:69–77.
Rozenberg, O. and M. Aviram (2006). S-Glutathionylation regulates HDL-associated paraoxo-
nase 1 (PON1) activity. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 351:492–498.
Paraoxonase 1 Status, Environmental Exposures, and Oxidative Stress 111
Rozenberg, O., D. M. Shih, and M. Aviram (2005). Paraoxonase 1 (PON1) attenuates mac-
rophage oxidative status: Studies in PON1 transfected cells and in PON1 transgenic
mice. Atherosclerosis 181:9–18.
Santanam, N. and S. Parthasarathy (2007). Aspirin is a substrate for paraoxonase-like activity:
Implications in atherosclerosis. Atherosclerosis 191:272–275.
Serajee, F. J., R. Nabi, H. Zhong, and M. Huq (2004). Polymorphisms in xenobiotic metabo-
lism genes and autism. J. Child Neurol. 19:413–417.
Slotkin, T. A., E. D. Levin, and F. J. Seidler (2009). Developmental neurotoxicity of parathion:
Progressive effects on serotonergic systems in adolescence and adulthood. Neurotoxicol.
Teratol. 31:11–17.
Smith, S. E., J. Li, K. Garbett, K. Mirnics, and P. H. Patterson (2007). Maternal immune activation
alters fetal brain development through interleukin-6. J. Neurosci. 27:10695–10702.
Sogut, S., S. S. Zoroglu, H. Ozyurt, H. R. Yilmaz, F. Ozugurlu, E. Sivasli, O. Yetkin,
M. Yanik, H. Tutkun, H. A. Savas, M. Tarakcioglu, and O. Akyol (2003). Changes in
nitric oxide levels and antioxidant enzyme activities may have a role in the pathophysi-
ological mechanisms involved in autism. Clin. Chim. Acta 331:111–117.
Sorenson, R. C., M. Aviram, C. L. Bisgaier, S. Billecke, C. Hsu, and B. N. La Du (1999).
Properties of the retained N-terminal hydrophobic leader sequence in human serum
paraoxonase/arylesterase. Chem. Biol. Interact. 119–120:243–249.
Sozmen, E. Y., B. Mackness, B. Sozmen, P. Durrington, F. K. Girgin, L. Aslan, and M. Mackness
(2002). Effect of organophosphate intoxication on human serum paraoxonase. Hum.
Exp. Toxicol. 21:247–252.
Suh, J. H., W. J. Walsh, W. R. McGinnis, A. Lewis, and B. N. Ames (2008). Altered sulfur amino
acid metabolism in immune cells of children diagnosed with autism. Am. J. Biochem.
Biotechnol. (Special Issue on Autism Spectrum Disorders) 4:105–113.
Sweeten, T. L., D. J. Posey, and C. J. McDougle (2003). High blood monocyte counts and
neopterin levels in children with autistic disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 160:1691–1693.
Sweeten, T. L., D. J. Posey, S. Shankar, and C. J. McDougle (2004). High nitric oxide production
in autistic disorder: A possible role for interferon-gamma. Biol. Psychiatry 55:434–437.
Van Lenten, B. J., S. T. Reddy, M. Navab, and A. M. Fogelman (2006). Understanding changes
in high density lipoproteins during the acute phase response. Arterioscler. Thromb. Vasc.
Biol. 26:1687–1688.
Van Lenten, B. J., A. C. Wagner, M. Navab, and A. M. Fogelman (2001). Oxidized phospholip-
ids induce changes in hepatic paraoxonase and ApoJ but not monocyte chemoattractant
protein-1 via interleukin-6. J. Biol. Chem. 276:1923–1929.
Vargas, D. L., C. Nascimbene, C. Krishnan, A. W. Zimmerman, and C. A. Pardo (2005).
Neuroglial activation and neuroinflammation in the brain of patients with autism. Ann.
Neurol. 57:67–81.
Whyatt, R. M., V. Rauh, D. B. Barr, D. E. Camann, H. F. Andrews, R. Garfinkel, L. A. Hoepner,
D. Diaz, J. Dietrich, A. Reyes, D. Tang, P. L. Kinney, and F. P. Perera (2004). Prenatal
insecticide exposures and birth weight and length among an urban minority cohort.
Environ. Health Perspect. 112:1125–1132.
Wolff, M. S., S. Engel, G. Berkowitz, S. Teitelbaum, J. Siskind, D. B. Barr, and J. Wetmur
(2007). Prenatal pesticide and PCB exposures and birth outcomes. Pediatr. Res.
61:243–250.
Worth, J. 2002. Paraoxonase polymorphisms and organophosphates. Lancet 360:802–803.
Yorbik, O., A. Sayal, C. Akay, D. I. Akbiyik, and T. Sohmen (2002). Investigation of antioxidant
enzymes in children with autistic disorder. Prostaglandins Leukot. Essent. Fatty Acids
67:341–343.
Young, J. G., B. Eskenazi, E. A. Gladstone, A. Bradman, L. Pedersen, C. Johnson, D. B. Barr,
C. E. Furlong, and N. T. Holland (2005). Association between in utero organophosphate
pesticide exposure and abnormal reflexes in neonates. Neurotoxicology 26:199–209.
112 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Zoroglu, S. S., F. Armutcu, S. Ozen, A. Gurel, E. Sivasli, O. Yetkin, and I. Meram (2004).
Increased oxidative stress and altered activities of erythrocyte free radical scavenging
enzymes in autism. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 254:143–147.
Zoroglu, S. S., M. Yurekli, I. Meram, S. Sogut, H. Tutkun, O. Yetkin, E. Sivasli, H. A. Savas,
M. Yanik, H. Herken, and O. Akyol (2003). Pathophysiological role of nitric oxide and
adrenomedullin in autism. Cell Biochem. Funct. 21:55–60.
7 The Redox/Methylation
Hypothesis of Autism:
A Molecular Mechanism
for Heavy Metal-Induced
Neurotoxicity
Richard C. Deth* and Christina R. Muratore
Department of Pharmaceutical Sciences,
Northeastern University, Boston, MA 02115, USA
CONTENTS
7.1 Introduction ................................................................................................. 113
7.2 Redox: An Evolutionary Perspective .......................................................... 114
7.3 Redox Metabolism in Human Brain ........................................................... 115
7.4 Redox and Methylation ............................................................................... 117
7.5 Heavy Metals and Redox Status ................................................................. 118
7.6 Oxidative Stress and Neuroinflammation in Autism .................................. 120
7.7 A Molecular Mechanism for Heavy Metal-Induced Autism ...................... 122
7.7.1 Preexisting Genetic Risk Factors ................................................... 122
7.7.2 Heavy Metal Exposure................................................................... 123
7.7.3 Molecular Targets .......................................................................... 124
7.7.4 Cellular Consequences................................................................... 125
7.8 Summary ..................................................................................................... 126
References .............................................................................................................. 127
7.1 INTRODUCTION
The rapid rise of autism rates over the past 25 years suggests a potential toxic effect
from one or more environmentally encountered substances on the normal trajectory
of neurological development, although increased diagnosis may also contribute. Two
major autism categories can be identified, based upon whether or not individuals
did or did not initially exhibit normal cognitive function and language acquisition.
113
114 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
(1) Classical autism, described prior to 1980, corresponds to the latter pattern where
infants fail to achieve standard developmental milestones from birth. (2) Regressive
autism, which is more typical of the contemporary condition, reflects a loss of pre-
viously acquired abilities that can occur over a short period of time (i.e., days to
weeks). Anecdotal reports from some parents link regression to the time period after
vaccination, raising the possibility that vaccine components, including mercury and/
or aluminum and the accompanying inflammatory activation they promote, might
contribute to autism. While this proposal is both frightening and controversial, it
requires serious consideration and investigation. Autistic children exhibit signs of
oxidative stress and neuroinflammation, which may be attributed in part to heavy
metal toxicity. This chapter reviews clinical observations and relevant molecular
mechanisms, which bear on this critical public health question.
Dopamine
Neuron
FIGURE 7.1 Pathways of thiol metabolism in human neuronal cells. In human neurons,
transsulfuration is impaired and cysteine for synthesis of GSH is provided primarily by
uptake via EAAT3, which is dependent upon GSH released from astrocytes. GSH is required
for synthesis of methylcobalamin (MeCbl) and for activity of methionine synthase. During
oxidative stress, low levels of GSH and MeCbl cause a decrease in a large number of SAM-
dependent methylation reactions, and a decrease in dopamine-stimulated, D4 dopamine
receptor-mediated phospholipid methylation.
for maintaining cellular redox status. (3) Agents affecting EAAT3 activity will exert
a more powerful influence on redox status. (4) The limited available pool of GSH
may be more dynamically utilized. (5) Human brain function is more vulnerable to
toxic substances, which impair redox metabolism.
ability to cross the blood–brain barrier than organomercurials, but significant levels
in brain can be detected after vaccination (Flarend et al., 1997).
Within the brain compartment, mercury and other metals affect thiol metabolism
in different cell types, including pluripotent stem cells, neurons, astrocytes, micro-
glia, and oligodendrocytes. Under mild oxidative stress conditions, an increased
proportion of pluripotent stem cells become astrocytes, whereas mild reducing con-
ditions increase the proportion of neuronal cells (Prozorovski et al., 2008). Since
astrocytes serve as reservoirs of GSH and provide cysteine for neurons (Figure
7.1), this mode of regulation appears to adjust cell fate in response to prevailing
redox conditions, and heavy metal-induced oxidative stress would reduce neuronal
development. Neuronal stem cells are particularly sensitive to mercury, and low
nanomolar concentrations of methylmercury activate caspase-dependent apopto-
sis (Tamm et al., 2006). Heavy metal-induced oxidative stress in oligodendrocytes
can lead to impaired myelination (Crang and Jacobson, 1982), while it can lead to
activation and the release of proinflammatory cytokines in microglia (Kim and de
Vellis, 2005). Importantly, each of these conditions has been observed in the brain
of children with autism.
In 2004, our group first described the potent inhibitory effects of mercury,
thimerosal, aluminum, and lead on methylation and methionine synthase activity
in SH-SY5Y human neuronal cells (Waly et al., 2004). Subsequently, we determined
that inhibition reflected the ability of these heavy metals to lower GSH levels
(M. Waly et al., unpublished observation), resulting in decreased synthesis of meth-
ylcobalamin (methylB12), which is required for methionine synthase activity in
these neuronal cells, as illustrated in Figure 7.1. These heavy metals potently inhibit
EAAT3-mediated uptake of cysteine, which accounts for their ability to decrease
GSH, methylB12, and methionine synthase activity. Together, these studies illustrate
the critical role of EAAT3 in regulating redox status and methylation activity in
human neuronal cells, as well as their vulnerability to heavy metals.
An important breakthrough in understanding the molecular mechanism of mer-
cury toxicity was provided from studies carried out by Holmgren and colleagues
at the Karolinska Institute in Sweden (Carvalho et al., 2008). They compared the
potency of inorganic mercury and methylmercury to inhibit several enzymes, each
of which promote a reduced intracellular redox state, including thioredoxin, thiore-
doxin reductase, glutathione reductase, and glutaredoxin. Among these, thioredoxin
and thioredoxin reductase showed exceptionally high sensitivity to both mercury
compounds, strongly suggesting that they are primary targets for mercury-induced
neurotoxicity. Thioredoxin has multiple activities, including the ability to release
GSH from glutathionylated proteins (i.e., proteins with a thiol-bound GSH), while
thioredoxin reductase, a selenoprotein, serves to reactivate thioredoxin after it has
carried out deglutathionylation (Figure 7.2). The extent of protein glutathionylation
reflects the level of cellular oxidative stress, and mercury inhibition of the thiore-
doxin system will promote the accumulation of glutathionylated proteins, producing
and sustaining a state of high oxidative stress. The ultrahigh affinity of mercury for
selenoproteins has long been recognized, and selenium supplementation has been
suggested as a treatment for mercury toxicity.
120 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Oxidation GSH
Thioredoxin
GSH or
Hg2+
glutaredoxin
2.0
Control
Autistic
1.5
Methionine synthase
mRNA (fold change)
1.0
**
0.5
*
0.0
CAP COB
FIGURE 7.3 Methionine synthase mRNA levels in human cortex are reduced in autism.
RNA samples from autistic and nonautistic subjects were probed using qRT-PCR with spe-
cific primers to the CAP and COB domains of methionine synthase. n = 11 for each group
(∗ = p < 0.05 compared to control for the CAP primer set; ∗∗ = p < 0.05 compared to control
for the COB primer set).
122 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Intramolecular disulfide bonds provide potential binding sites for mercury, par-
ticularly if they undergo cycles of oxidation and reduction. Vicinal (i.e., consecutive)
cysteine residues provide a special case, and the oxidized and reduced proteins have
significantly different conformations and activities. In such cases, the disulfide bond
serves as a redox sensor, and the binding of mercury stabilizes a conformation,
which mimics the oxidized condition. Another structural motif with high mercury
binding affinity is the thioredoxin fold, named after the ubiquitous protein in which
it was first characterized. In the thioredoxin fold, two cysteines are separated by two
residues (e.g., glycine and proline), which allow the formation of a redox-sensitive
disulfide bond that binds inorganic mercury with high affinity. Since thioredoxin
catalyzes removal of thiols such as GSH or cysteine from proteins, mercury inhi-
bition results in the accumulation of these thiol-modified proteins. Under normal
circumstances, thiolation of proteins is increased when GSSG and cystine levels are
high, so the net effect of mercury is to shift cells to an oxidative stress condition.
Mercury binds potently to thiols in their unprotonated thiolate state, but the
prevalence of this state is much higher for selenocysteine, resulting in a millionfold
higher affinity for mercury, as compared to cysteine, with a binding constant of 1045
(Dyrssen and Wedborg, 1991). This property is the basis for the unique redox capa-
bilities of selenoproteins, but also makes them highly vulnerable to mercury inhibition.
Moreover, redistribution of mercury away from thiols of lower affinity results in its
increasing association with selenoproteins. Thus the targets of mercury, which are
most likely to contribute to autism are selenoproteins, which include thioredoxin
reductases, glutathione peroxidases, thyroid hormone deiodinases, selenoprotein P,
and other proteins whose function remains to be established. Selenoproteins are par-
ticularly important in brain, and under deficiency conditions, other tissues release
their selenium, which is taken up by the brain (Behne and Kyriakopoulos, 2001).
Since both thioredoxin and thioredoxin reductase possess molecular features,
which render them highly sensitive to inorganic mercury, their combined inhibition will
result in a highly amplified accumulation of thiol-modified proteins (Figure 7.2).
When combined with the unique pattern of sulfur metabolism in human neuronal
cells (Figure 7.1), these actions may account for the extremely potent reduction in
GSH levels caused by mercury. Importantly, autistic subjects have a significantly
lower level of selenium, which may increase their sensitivity to mercury (Jory and
McGinnis, 2008).
7.8 SUMMARY
The ability to counteract oxidation is a critical role for sulfur metabolism in all cells,
but human neuronal cells are particularly vulnerable to oxidative stress. As illustrated
in Figure 7.4, mercury and other heavy metals impair GSH synthesis and inhibit
methylation activity by binding with high affinity to thiols and selenoproteins. Studies
indicate that the concentration of inorganic mercury delivered to the brain following
vaccination is sufficient to inhibit the activity of selenoproteins such as thioredoxin
reductase. This inhibition creates a state of oxidative stress, which disrupts methy-
lation activities including epigenetic regulation of gene expression and dopamine-
stimulated PLM. Together, these observations provide a coherent and compelling
molecular mechanism by which mercury and other heavy metals can contribute to
the etiology of autism. Additional studies are needed to confi rm specific aspects
of this molecular hypothesis.
Heavy metals
(mercury)
Impaired methylation
FIGURE 7.4 Molecular hypothesis for heavy metal-induced autism. Heavy metals such as
mercury bind with high affinity to protein thiols and to selenoproteins, disrupting their role in
redox regulation. The resulting oxidative stress inhibits methionine synthase activity, causing
a decrease in DNA methylation and a decrease in D4 dopamine receptor-mediated phos-
pholipid methylation. In a genetically vulnerable subpopulation, these neurotoxic effects are
significant factors in the etiology of autism.
The Redox/Methylation Hypothesis of Autism 127
REFERENCES
Aoyama, K., Suh, S.W., Hamby, A.M., Liu, J., Chan, W.Y., Chen, Y., and Swanson, R.A.
(2006). Neuronal glutathione deficiency and age-dependent neurodegeneration in the
EAAC1 deficient mouse. Nat. Neurosci. 9:119–126.
Banner, S.J., Fray, A.E., Ince, P.G., Steward, M., Cookson, M.R., and Shaw, P.J. (2002). The
expression of the glutamate re-uptake transporter excitatory amino acid transporter 1
(EAAT1) in the normal human CNS and in motor neurone disease: An immunohis-
tochemical study. Neuroscience 109:27–44.
Behne, D. and Kyriakopoulos, A. (2001). Mammalian selenium-containing proteins. Annu.
Rev. Nutr. 21:453–473.
Benzie, I.F. (2000). Evolution of antioxidant defence mechanisms. Eur. J. Nutr. 39:53–61.
Burbacher, T.M., Shen, D.D., Liberato, N., Grant, K.S., Cernichiari, E., and Clarkson, T.
(2005). Comparison of blood and brain mercury levels in infant monkeys exposed
to methylmercury or vaccines containing thimerosal. Environ. Health Perspect.
113:1015–1021.
Campbell, A. (2006). The role of aluminum and copper on neuroinflammation and Alzheimer’s
disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 10:165–172.
Carvalho, C.M., Chew, E.H., Hashemy, S.I., Lu, J., and Holmgren, A. (2008). Inhibition of the
human thioredoxin system. A molecular mechanism of mercury toxicity. J. Biol. Chem.
283:11913–11923.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (1999). Notice to Readers: Thimerosal in Vaccines:
A Joint Statement of the American Academy of Pediatrics and the Public Health Service.
Morb. Mort. Wkly. Rep. 48:563–565.
Charleston, J.S., Bolender, R.P., Mottet, N.K., Body, R.L., Vahter, M.E., and Burbacher, T.M.
(1994). Increases in the number of reactive glia in the visual cortex of Macaca fascicu-
laris following subclinical long-term methyl mercury exposure. Toxicol. Appl. Pharmacol.
129:196–206.
Charleston, J.S., Body, R.L., Mottet, N.K., Vahter, M.E., and Burbacher, T.M. (1995).
Autometallographic determination of inorganic mercury distribution in the cortex
of the calcarine sulcus of the monkey Macaca fascicularis following long-term sub-
clinical exposure to methylmercury and mercuric chloride. Toxicol. Appl. Pharmacol.
132:325–333.
Chauhan, A. and Chauhan, V. (2006). Oxidative stress in autism. Pathophysiology 13:171–181.
Chauhan, A., Chauhan, V., Brown, W.T., and Cohen, I. (2004). Oxidative stress in autism:
Increased lipid peroxidation and reduced serum levels of ceruloplasmin and transferrin—
The antioxidant proteins. Life Sci. 75:2539–2549.
Clark, P.D., Dowling, N.I., and Huang, M. (1998). Comments on the role of H2S in the chemistry
of Earth’s early atmosphere and in prebiotic synthesis. J. Mol. Evol. 47:127–132.
Crang, A.J. and Jacobson, W. (1982). The relationship of myelin basic protein (arginine) meth-
yltransferase to myelination in mouse spinal cord. J. Neurochem. 39:244–247.
Deth, R., Muratore, C., Benzecry, J., Power-Charnitsky, V.A., and Waly, M. (2008). How
environmental and genetic factors combine to cause autism: A redox/methylation
hypothesis. Neurotoxicology 29:190–201.
Do, S.H., Fang, H.Y., Ham, B.M., and Zuo, Z. (2002). The effects of lidocaine on the activity
of glutamate transporter EAAT3: The role of protein kinase C and phosphatidylinositol
3-kinase. Anesth. Analg. 95:1263–1268.
Domingo, J.L. (1995). Reproductive and developmental toxicity of aluminum: A review.
Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 17:515–521.
Dyrssen, D. and Wedborg, M. (1991). The sulfur–mercury(II) system in natural waters. Water
Air Soil Pollut. 56:507–519.
Falkowski, P.G. (2006). Evolution. Tracing oxygen’s imprint on earth’s metabolic evolution.
Science 311:1724–1725.
128 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Liptak, M.D. and Brunold, T.C. (2006). Spectroscopic and computational studies of Co1 +
cobalamin: Spectral and electronic properties of the “superreduced” B12 cofactor.
J. Am. Chem. Soc. 128:9144–9156.
McConkie-Rosell, A., Lachiewicz, A.M., Spiridigliozzi, G.A., Tarleton, J., Schoenwald, S.,
Phelan, M.C., Goonewardena, P., Ding, X., and Brown, W.T. (1993). Evidence that
methylation of the FMR-I locus is responsible for variable phenotypic expression of the
fragile X syndrome. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 53:800–809.
McGinnis, W.R. (2004). Oxidative stress in autism. Altern. Ther. Health Med. 10:22–36.
Ming, X., Stein, T.P., Brimacombe, M., Johnson, W.G., Lambert, G.H., and Wagner, G.C.
(2005). Increased excretion of a lipid peroxidation biomarker in autism. Prostaglandins
Leukot. Essent. Fatty Acids 73:379–384.
Miranda, T.B. and Jones, P.A. (2007). DNA methylation: The nuts and bolts of repression.
J. Cell. Physiol. 213:384–390.
NRC (National Research Council) (2000). Toxicological Effects of Methylmercury. Washington,
DC: National Academy Press.
Oliveira, G., Diogo, L., Grazina, M., Garcia, P., Ataíde, A., Marques, C., Miguel, T., Borges, L.,
Vicente, A.M., and Oliveira, C.R. (2005). Mitochondrial dysfunction in autism spectrum
disorders: A population-based study. Dev. Med. Child. Neurol. 47:185–189.
Oteiza, P.I., Mackenzie, G.G., and Verstraeten, S.V. (2004). Metals in neurodegeneration:
Involvement of oxidants and oxidant-sensitive transcription factors. Mol. Aspects Med.
25:103–115.
Paşca, S.P., Nemes, B., Vlase, L., Gagyi, C.E., Dronca, E., Miu, A.C., and Dronca, M. (2006).
High levels of homocysteine and low serum paraoxonase 1 arylesterase activity in children
with autism. Life Sci. 78:2244–2248.
Pichichero, M.E., Gentile, A., Giglio, N., Umido, V, Clarkson, T., Cernichiari, E., Zareba, G.,
Gotelli, C., Gotelli, M., Yan, L., and Treanor, J. (2008). Mercury levels in newborns and
infants after receipt of thimerosal-containing vaccines. Pediatrics 121:e208–e214.
Poling, J.S., Frye, R.E., Shoffner, J., and Zimmerman, A.W. (2006). Developmental regression
and mitochondrial dysfunction in a child with autism. J. Child Neurol. 21:170–172.
Prozorovski, T., Schulze-Topphoff, U., Glumm, R., Baumgart, J., Schröter, F., Ninnemann, O.,
Siegert, E., Bendix, I., Brüstle,O., Nitsch, R., Zipp, F., and Aktas, O. (2008). Sirt1
contributes critically to the redox-dependent fate of neural progenitors. Nat. Cell Biol.
10:385–394.
Quig, D. (1998). Cysteine metabolism and metal toxicity. Altern. Med. Rev. 3:262–270.
Ralston, N.V., Blackwell, J.L. III, and Raymond, L.J. (2007). Importance of molar ratios in
selenium-dependent protection against methylmercury toxicity. Biol. Trace Elem. Res.
119:255–268.
Raymond, J. and Segrè, D. (2006). The effect of oxygen on biochemical networks and the
evolution of complex life. Science 311:1764–1767.
Schafer, F.Q. and Buettner, G.R. (2001). Redox environment of the cell as viewed through
the redox state of the glutathione disulfide/glutathione couple. Free Radic. Biol. Med.
30:1191–1212.
Schechter, R. and Grether, G.K. (2008). Continuing increases in autism reported to California’s
developmental services system: Mercury in retrograde. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 65:19–24.
Sharma, P. and Mishra, K.P. (2006). Aluminum-induced maternal and developmental toxic-
ity and oxidative stress in rat brain: Response to combined administration of tiron and
glutathione. Reprod. Toxicol. 21:313–321.
Smalley, S.L., Asarnow, R.F., and Spence, M.A. (1988). Autism and genetics. A decade of
research. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 45:953–961.
Steuerwald, U., Weihe, P., Jørgensen, P.J., Bjerve, K., Brock, J., Heinzow, B., Budtz-Jørgensen, E.,
and Grandjean, P. (2000). Maternal seafood diet, methylmercury exposure, and neonatal
neurologic function. J. Pediatr. 136:599–605.
130 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Tallan, H.H., Moore, S., and Stein, W.H. (1958). l-Cystathionine in human brain. J. Biol.
Chem. 230:707–716.
Tamm, C., Duckworth, J., Hermanson, O., and Ceccatelli, S. (2006). High susceptibility of
neural stem cells to methylmercury toxicity: Effects on cell survival and neuronal
differentiation. J. Neurochem. 97:69–78.
Thatcher, K.N., Peddada, S., Yasui, D.H., and Lasalle, J.M. (2005). Homologous pairing of
15q11–13 imprinted domains in brain is developmentally regulated but deficient in Rett
and autism samples. Hum. Mol. Genet. 14:785–797.
U.S. EPA. (2001). Methylmercury Reference Dose for Chronic Oral Exposure. U.S. Environ-
mental Protection Agency, Integrated Risk Information System (IRIS). Washington, DC:
U.S. Environmental Protection Agency: http://www.epa.gov/NCEA/iris/subst/0073.htm
Vargas, D.L., Nascimbene, C., Krishnan, C., Zimmerman, A.W., and Pardo, C.A. (2005).
Neuroglial activation and neuroinflammation in the brain of patients with autism. Ann.
Neurol. 57:67–81.
Verstraeten, S.V., Aimo, L., and Oteiza, P.I. (2008). Aluminium and lead: Molecular mechanisms
of brain toxicity. Arch. Toxicol. 82:789–802.
Vitvitsky, V., Thomas, M., Ghorpade, A., Gendelman, H.E., and Banerjee, R. (2006). A functional
transsulfuration pathway in the brain links to glutathione homeostasis. J. Biol. Chem.
281:35785–35793.
Waly, M., Olteanu, H., Banerjee, R., Choi, S.W., Mason, J.B., Parker, B.S., Sukumar, S.,
Shim, S., Sharma, A., Benzecry, J.M., Power-Charnitsky, V.A., and Deth, R.C. (2004).
Activation of methionine synthase by insulin-like growth factor-1 and dopamine: A target
for neurodevelopmental toxins and thimerosal. Mol. Psychiatry 9:358–370.
Wan, M., Lee, S.S., Zhang, X., Houwink-Manville, I., Song, H.R., Amir, R.E., Budden, S.,
Naidu, S., Pereira, J.L., Lo, I.F., Zoghbi, H.Y., Schanen, N.C., and Francke, U. (1999).
Rett syndrome and beyond: Recurrent spontaneous and familial MECP2 mutations at
CpG hotspots. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 65:1520–1529.
Watson, W.P., Munter, T., and Golding, B.T. (2004). A new role for glutathione: Protection of
vitamin B12 from depletion by xenobiotics. Chem. Res. Toxicol. 17:1562–1567.
Yao, Y., Walsh, W.J., McGinnis, W.R., and Pratico, D. (2006). Altered vascular phenotype in
autism: Correlation with oxidative stress. Arch. Neurol. 63:1161–1164.
Zoroglu, S.S., Armutcu, F., Ozen, S., Gurel, A., Sivasli, E., Yetkin, O., and Meram, I. (2004).
Increased oxidative stress and altered activities of erythrocyte free radical scavenging
enzymes in autism. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 254:143–147.
8 Autism and Oxidative
Stress: Evidence from
an Animal Model
Michelle A. Cheh,1 Alycia K. Halladay,2,3 Carrie
L. Yochum,2 Kenneth R. Reuhl,3 Marianne
Polunas,3 Xue Ming,4 and George C. Wagner2,*
Departments of 1Neuroscience, 2Psychology,
and 3Pharmacology and Toxicology, Rutgers
University, New Brunswick, NJ 08854, USA
4Department of Neuroscience, University of Medicine
and Dentistry of New Jersey, Newark, NJ 07103, USA
CONTENTS
8.1 Introduction: An Overview of Autism ........................................................ 132
8.2 Etiology of Autism ...................................................................................... 133
8.3 Animal Model of Autism ............................................................................ 135
8.4 Oxidative Stress and Autism ....................................................................... 136
8.5 Valproic Acid, Autism, and Oxidative Stress.............................................. 137
8.6 Mercury Exposure and Autism ................................................................... 138
8.7 Trolox Protects Mice against Early Exposure to MeHg ............................. 139
8.7.1 Body Weight .................................................................................. 140
8.7.2 Reflex Development ....................................................................... 140
8.7.2.1 Midair Righting .............................................................. 140
8.7.2.2 Negative Geotaxis .......................................................... 140
8.7.2.3 Hanging Wire ................................................................. 140
8.7.3 Cognitive Development .................................................................. 141
8.7.3.1 Hidden Platform Water Maze......................................... 141
8.7.3.2 Reversal Learning .......................................................... 143
8.7.3.3 Visible Platform.............................................................. 144
8.7.4 Social Development ....................................................................... 144
8.7.4.1 Sniffing ........................................................................... 144
8.7.4.2 Attacks............................................................................ 145
131
132 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
recent reports continuing to emphasize the safety of the MMR vaccine (Demicheli
et al., 2005; Honig et al., 2008). Nonetheless, one consequence of the MMR contro-
versy is that there has been increased awareness that early exposure to environmental
toxicants may place certain individuals at risk for autism.
As noted, about one-third of those diagnosed with autism experience autistic
regression, having apparently normal development interrupted at some point by a
dramatic setback with a loss of acquired skills. Yet even in these cases, as well as
for the remaining two-thirds of the cases where autism appears to be the result
of a neurodevelopmental defect present at birth (Kanner, 1943b), it remains true that
the etiology of autism remains unknown. Evidence for a genetic contribution for the
disorder is clear, but no single gene has been identified; rather, a constellation of gene
polymorphisms appears to increase the risk for individuals. The controversy surround-
ing the link between autism and the MMR vaccination suggested the involvement of
an environmental toxicant exposure as a second factor, at least for autistic regression.
Other studies have linked autism to prenatal exposure to toxicants such as thalidomide
or valproic acid (VPA) (Rodier et al., 1997). Based upon these observations, together
with the fact that autism fails to reach 100% concordance rates in monozygotic
twins, a hypothesis has been developed that the etiology of autism may arise as the
result of a gene by environment interaction with a gene polymorphism(s) enhancing
the sensitivity of individuals to the deleterious effects of environmental toxicants
(London and Etzel, 2000). Based upon our observations in humans (Ming et al.,
2005, 2008a) and mice (Wagner et al., 2006; Ming et al., 2008b), we have advanced
this hypothesis with the proposal that autism is the result of a gene by environ-
ment interaction where the environmental toxicant triggers oxidative stress while the
genetic deficiency affects the ability of the individual to respond effectively to the
deleterious effects of oxidative stress (Ming et al., 2008b). Toward this end, we have
developed an animal model of autism and have used this model to assess the effects
of gene alterations (Cheh et al., 2006) as well as early exposure to toxicants such as
VPA (Wagner et al., 2006; Yochum et al., 2008) and mercury (Wagner et al., 2007)
on neurobehavioral development.
glutathione, and GSH/GSSG and cysteine levels (James et al., 2004) were found in
autistic individuals compared to controls. Likewise, levels of exogenous antioxi-
dants were also found to be reduced in autism, including vitamin C, vitamin E, and
vitamin A in plasma, and zinc and selenium in erythrocytes (James et al., 2004).
Finally, evidence of altered oxidative stress in autism is derived from evidence of
impaired energy metabolism. Magnetic resonance spectroscopic study of the brains
of individuals with autism showed reduced synthesis of ATP (Minshew et al., 1993).
In addition, higher lactate (Coleman and Blass, 1985; Chugani et al., 1999) and pyru-
vate (Moreno et al., 1992) levels in autism may suggest mitochondrial dysfunction in
autism (Filipek et al., 2003); one major cause of mitochondria dysfunction is a result
of oxidative injury (Packer, 1984). Taken together, these lines of evidence support the
hypothesis that at least some children with autism exhibit enhanced oxidative stress.
As noted, we have used our animal model to examine the role of oxidative stress in
causing the neurobehavioral deficits following early exposure to VPA (Ming et al.,
2008b) and mercury.
MeHg exposure during neural development leads to more severe toxicity than
comparable exposure in adults. Specifically, early exposure results in a more dif-
fuse neuropathology and can affect the organization of the cerebellum, cerebral
cortex, and striatum (Chang et al., 1977; Chang and Annau, 1984; Sager et al.,
1984; Sakamoto et al., 2002). Commonly reported neurobehavioral deficits in
rodents following early MeHg exposure include deficits in reflex development
and locomotor activity and coordination as well as cognitive deficits in learning
and memory (Olson and Boush, 1975; Chang and Annau, 1984; Vorhees, 1985;
Doré et al., 2001; Sakamoto et al., 2002; Stringari et al., 2006). In general, the
severity of the behavioral deficits following MeHg increases with dose and earlier
exposure times.
Antioxidants have been shown to confer significant protection against MeHg tox-
icity in vitro and in vivo, reducing the levels of ROS thereby attenuating the neuro-
pathological damage (Chang et al., 1978; Andersen and Andersen, 1993; Yee and
Choi, 1994; Usuki et al., 2001; Shanker and Aschner, 2003; Beyrouty and Chan,
2006). One recent study reported that pretreatment with vitamin E attenuated MeHg-
induced behavioral deficits in the acoustic startle response of rats (Beyrouty and
Chan, 2006). However, the protective effect of antioxidants on persistent MeHg-
induced neurobehavioral disruption is not well studied. Using our animal model
of autism, we have previously shown that early exposure to MeHg sensitizes the
developing mouse to the later appearance of stereotypic and self-injurious intrusive
behavior (Wagner et al., 2007).
and reflexes including midair righting, negative geotaxis, and hanging wire strength.
Adolescent mice were then evaluated in the water maze. Finally, pairs of similarly
treated adult mice were tested in a resident-intruder paradigm. Details of the behav-
ioral test procedures may be found in Wagner et al. (2006, 2007).
We observed that MeHg-induced deficits in reflex development and spatial learning
and memory as well as in adult social interactions, and that Trolox pretreatment pro-
tected the mice against these MeHg-induced neurodevelopmental deficits. Specific
observations were as follows.
2 * *
*
*
1
*
* *
0
13 14 15 16 17 18 19
(a) Postnatal day
30
28
26
24
22
Second to turn 180°
20
18
16
14
12
10
8
6
4
2
0
13 14 15 16 17 18 19
(b) Postnatal day
FIGURE 8.1 (a) The number of times out of three that mice were successful at midair righting
across postnatal development is illustrated. * Indicates significantly different from control using a
distribution free nonparametric post-hoc test. (b) Latency to perform the negative geotaxis response
across postnatal development is depicted. Error bars indicate SEM.
water maze performance than controls (F(4,42) = 5.9, p = 0.001). Post-hoc analysis
revealed that the escape latency for MeHg-exposed mice was significantly greater
than controls for the average escape latency on day 1, as well as on trials 9, 10, 13, 14,
15 on day 2. This deficit was attenuated by high-dose of Trolox on day 2 of testing,
but not by the lower dose. As an additional measure, escape latency was converted
to the proportion of time spent in the quadrant where the platform was located. This
60
Average escape latency (s)
50
*
40
30
20
10
0
PBS MeHg 4 Tro 2.5 + Tro 1.0 +
MeHg MeHg
(a) Average escape latency, day 1
60 *
* * *
50 + * *
+ *
40 +
+ +
+
30
20
10
0
9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16
(b) Escape latency by trial, day 2
PBS Trolox 2.5 mg/kg + MeHg
MeHg 4 mg/kg Trolox 1.0 mg/kg + MeHg
FIGURE 8.2 (a) Escape latency averaged for eight trials of hidden-platform water maze test-
ing on day one and (b) escape latency across eight trials of hidden-platform water maze testing
on day two is shown.
Autism and Oxidative Stress: Evidence from an Animal Model 143
Reversal learning
60
*
50
Escape latency (s)
*+ * *
40 *
30 +
20
10
0
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
(c)
Proportion of escape latency
0.6
0.4 *
* *
0.2
0.0
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
(d) Trial number
FIGURE 8.2 (continued) (c) Latency to escape to the hidden-platform when moved to the
opposite quadrant across eight trials, and (d) the proportion of time spent in the quadrant
where the platform was previously located across eight trials is depicted. * Indicates MeHg
significantly different from control. + Indicates significantly different than MeHg-treated.
Error bars indicate SEM.
proportion of time spent in the escape quadrant increased across trials for all mice
(F(15,630) = 2, p = 0.013). MeHg-treated mice showed a smaller proportion of time in
the escape quadrant compared to controls (F(4,42) = 3.5, p = 0.015), further indicating
that they did not learn as well. Trolox pretreatment partially improved this measure,
with both doses showing similar efficacy during the later trials (data not shown). Finally,
in separate group of mice, high-dose Trolox was administered in the absence of MeHg
and no effect on escape latency was observed (data not shown), indicating that Trolox
itself does not affect spatial learning, but is effective at attenuating the MeHg-induced
deficit in hidden-platform water maze performance.
8.7.4.1 Sniffing
There was a significant effect of day on the number of times resident mice sniffed
an intruder (F(1,26) = 5.9, p < 0.001). In addition, a significant effect of treatment
(F(1,26) = 6.68, p < 0.004) and a significant treatment-by-day interaction were found
(F(2,26) = 9.0, p < 0.001). As shown in Figure 8.3a, MeHg-exposed resident mice
made significantly fewer sniffs of the intruder on day 1 compared to control mice.
Pretreatment with Trolox resulted in complete reversal of the deficit in sniffing observed
in MeHg-treated mice. Furthermore, both control and Trolox-pretreated mice engaged in
a greater number of sniffing episodes on day one than on day 2. MeHg-exposed mice
Autism and Oxidative Stress: Evidence from an Animal Model 145
Resident-intruder paradigm
35 10
30 *
8 *
Number of attacks
Sniffing episodes
25
+ 6
20
+
15
*
4
10
2
5
0 0
(a) Day 1 Day 2 (b) Day 1 Day 2
FIGURE 8.3 (a) The number of times a pair of mice sniffed one another and (b) the number
of attacks resident mice made to the intruder in the resident–intruder paradigm on two consecu-
tive days is illustrated. * Indicates significantly different than control. + Indicates significantly
different than on day one of testing. Error bars indicate SEM.
engaged in a similar level of sniffing on both test days that was reduced compared to
the control and Trolox-pretreated mice.
8.7.4.2 Attacks
A significant effect of treatment on the number of attacks was also observed in the
resident–intruder paradigm (F(1,26) = 6.68, p < 0.004). MeHg-exposed resident mice
made many more attacks than controls. This increased aggression was completely
absent in Trolox-pretreated mice, such that none of the resident mice pretreated
with Trolox attacked the intruder (see Figure 8.3b). All three groups maintained a
similar level of attack behavior on the first and second day of testing. Results of the
resident–intruder paradigm indicate that less social investigation and greatly enhanced
aggressive behavior occur in adult mice that were exposed to MeHg during postnatal
development. This behavioral pattern was completely protected by high-dose Trolox
pretreatment.
8.8 CONCLUSIONS
Methylmercury is a developmental neurotoxicant that causes damage, in part, through
the generation of ROS. A number of studies have reported that MeHg exposure dur-
ing development leads to impaired cognitive and motor behavior. The postnatal
MeHg exposure regimen used here resulted in behavioral impairments consistent
with those reported by others including impaired reflex development and deficits in
a spatial learning task. In addition, the present study also revealed that early MeHg
exposure results in abnormal social and aggressive interactions behavior much later
in life. Pretreatment with Trolox, a water-soluble vitamin E derivative, resulted in a
robust protective effect against these MeHg-induced behavioral deficits. Trolox was
particularly effective at abolishing the MeHg-induced deficits in midair righting and
146 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
the increased aggressive behaviors of the adults. These data suggest that free-radical
scavenging antioxidants might be an effective means of reducing the potential health
risks of early MeHg exposure.
Dramatically increased aggressive behavior together with reduced social investiga-
tion was observed in MeHg-treated mice compared to controls. When control mice are
paired in a small chamber with a target intruder mouse for the first time, they quickly
develop a “social” memory (File and Seth, 2003). That is, they frequently approach and
investigate the intruder on the first test day but this approach behavior decreases when
they are exposed to the same intruder on subsequent days. MeHg-treated mice, how-
ever, approached the intruder significantly fewer times on the first test day, but main-
tained a similar level of investigation upon the second exposure to the same intruder.
Moreover, MeHg-exposed mice attacked the intruder much more frequently than con-
trols on both test days. This indicates that developmental MeHg exposure altered the
social behavior expressed in adulthood, leading to increased aggression with reduced
social investigation. We recently reported that mice treated with the same MeHg regi-
men exhibited significant increases in amphetamine-induced self-injurious behaviors
as adults (Wagner et al., 2007). In the present study, Trolox pretreatment protected
mice against the MeHg-induced alterations in social behavior observed in the resident–
intruder paradigm, completely preventing increase in intrusive attack behavior.
Learning and memory deficits were observed in MeHg-treated mice tested in the
water maze during adolescence. We found an increase in their escape latency in
the hidden platform water maze, as well as in the reversal-learning task. Sensorimotor
problems did not appear to contribute significantly to the MeHg-induced deficits,
since no difference in escape latency was observed between groups in the visible
platform version of the task. Trolox elicited a partial, dose-related protective effect
on hidden-platform and reversal-learning performance.
The proportion of time that mice spent in the previous escape quadrant was
recorded during the reversal-learning task. This measure functioned as a probe trial
to assess memory for the former escape quadrant. The control mice spent more time
in the former escape location on early trials. This decreased after just a few trials
and the controls were able to quickly adapt their strategy to learn the new plat-
form location. In contrast, mice exposed to MeHg showed a decreased proportion of
time spent in the former escape location, reflecting their overall poor performance
at finding the hidden-platform. However, Trolox pretreatment did not improve this
measure. The lack of protection on this measure suggests that subtle abnormalities
of MeHg exposure may still be present. Future studies determining the effect of
Trolox in other learning and memory tasks may lead to a clearer understanding of the
degree of protection conferred on MeHg-induced learning deficits.
Finally, the maturation of the mid-air righting reflex over the first 3 weeks of post-
natal life is mediated, in part, by continuing cerebellar maturation. MeHg exposure
resulted in a significant disruption in the maturation of the midair righting reflex and,
as above, Trolox pretreatment effectively attenuated this deficit.
It appears likely that pretreatment with the free radical scavenging antioxidant,
Trolox, protected mice against the behavioral deficits by decreasing the amount of
oxidative damage to neurons induced by the MeHg. Gutierrez et al. (2006) recently
reported that rats exposed to mercuric chloride displayed reduced levels of total
Autism and Oxidative Stress: Evidence from an Animal Model 147
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Supported by: NIEHS grants ES05022, ES07148, ES11256, as well as the NJ Governor’s
Council on Autism (GCW) and Autism Speaks (GCW).
REFERENCES
Ardinger, H.H., Atkin, J.F., Blackston, R.D., Elsas, L.J, Clarren, S.K., Livingstone, S.,
Flannery, D.B., Pellock, J.M., Harrod, M.J., and Lammer, E.J. (1988). Verification of
the fetal valproate syndrome phenotype. Am. J. Med. Genet. 29:171–185.
Andersen, H.R. and Andersen, O. (1993). Effects of dietary alpha-tocopherol and beta-carotene
on lipid peroxidation induced by methyl mercuric chloride in mice. Pharmacol. Toxicol.
4:192–201.
148 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Aylward, E.H., Minshew, N.J., Field, K., Sparks, B.F., and Singh, N. (2002). Effects of age on
brain volume and head circumference in autism. Neurology 59:175–183.
Bailey, A., Phillips, W., and Rutter, M. (1996). Autism: Towards an integration of clinical, genetic,
neuropsychological, and neurobiological perspectives. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry
37:89–126.
Ballaban-Gil, K. and Tuchman, R. (2000). Epilepsy and epileptiform EEF: Association with
autism and language disorders. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 6:300–308.
Bartak, L. and Rutter, M. (1976). Differences between mentally retarded and normally intelligent
autistic children. J. Autism Child Schizophr. 6:109–120.
Bauman, M. and Kemper, T.L. (1985). Histoanatomic observations of the brain in early infantile
autism. Neurology 35:866–874.
Beyrouty, P. and Chan, H.M. (2006). Co-consumption of selenium and vitamin E altered the repro-
ductive and developmental toxicity of methylmercury in rats. Neurotox. Teratol. 28:49–58.
Castoldi, A.F., Barni, S., Turin, I., Gandini, C., and Manzo, L. (2000). Early acute necrosis,
delayed apoptosis and cytoskeletal breakdown in cultured cerebellar granule neurons
exposed to methylmercury. J. Neurosci. Res. 59:775–787.
Chang, L.W. and Annau, Z. (1984). Developmental neuropathology and behavioral teratology
of methylmercury. Neurobehav. Teratol. 18:405–432.
Chang, L.W., Gilbert, M., and Sprecher, J. (1978). Modification of methylmercury neurotoxicity
by vitamin E. Environ. Res. 17:356–366.
Chang, L.W., Reuhl, K.R., and Lee, G.W. (1977). Degenerative changes in the developing nervous
system as a result of in utero exposure to methylmercury. Environ. Res. 14:414–423.
Chauhan, A. and Chauhan, V. (2006). Oxidative stress in autism. Pathophysiology 13:171–181.
Chauhan, A., Chauham, V., Brown, W.T., and Cohen, I. (2004). Oxidative stress in autism:
Increased lipid peroxidation and reduced serum levels of ceruloplasmin and transferring
the antioxidant proteins. Life Sci. 75:2539–2549.
Cheh, M.A., Millonig, J.H., Roselli, L.M., Ming, X., Jacobsen, E., Kamdar, S., and Wagner, G.C.
(2006). En2 mutant mice display neurobehavioral and neurochemical alterations relevant
to disorder. Brain Res. 1116:166–176.
Chugani, D.C., Sundram, B.S., Behen, M., Lee, M.L., and Moore, G.J. (1999). Evidence of altered
energy metabolism in autistic children. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry
23:635–641.
Clarkson, T.W., Magos, L., and Myers, G.J. (2003). The toxicology of mercury—Current
exposures and clinical manifestations. N. Engl. J. Med. 349:1731–1737.
Coleman, M. and Blass, J.P. (1985). Autism and lactic acidosis. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 15:1–8.
Costa, L.G., Aschner, M., Vitalone, A., Syversen, T., and Soldin, O.P. (2004). Developmental
neuropathology of environmental agents. Annu. Rev. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 44:87–110.
Courchesne, E., Karns, C.M., Davis, H.R., Ziccardi, R., Carper, R.A., Tigue, Z.D., Chisum, H.J.,
Moses, P., Pierce, K., Lord, C., Lincoln, A.J., Pizzo, S., Schreibman, L., Haas, R.H.,
Akshoofoff, N.A., and Courchesne, R.Y. (2001). Unusual brain growth patterns in early
life in patients with autistic disorder: An MRI study. Neurology 57:245–254.
Dales, L., Hammer, S.J., and Smith, N.J. (2001). Time trends in autism and in MMR immuni-
zation coverage in California. JAMA 285:1183–1185.
Davidson, P.W., Myers, G.J., and Weiss, B. (2004). Mercury exposure and child development
outcomes. Pediatrics 113:1023–1029.
Demicheli, V., Jefferson, T., Rivetti, A., and Price, D. (2005). Vaccines for measles, mumps
and rubella in children. The Cochrane Library 4:CD004407.
Doré, F.Y., Goulet, S., Gallagher, A., Harvey, P.O., Cantin, J.F., Aigle, T.D., and Mirault, M.E.
(2001). Neurobehavioral changes in mice treated with methylmercury at two different
stages of fetal development. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 23:463–472.
Edelson, S.B. and Cantor, D.S. (1998). Autism: Xenobiotic influences. Toxicol. Ind. Health
14:799–811.
Autism and Oxidative Stress: Evidence from an Animal Model 149
Edelson, S.B. and Cantor, D.S. (2000). The neurotoxic etiology of the autistic spectrum disorder:
A replicative study. Toxicol. Ind. Health 16:239–247.
File, S.E. and Seth, P. (2003). A review of 25 years of the social interaction test. Eur. J. Pharmacol.
463:35–53.
Filipek, P.A., Juranek,J., Smith, M., Mays, L.Z., Ramos, E.R., Bocian, M., Masser-Frye, D.,
Laulhere, T.M., Modahl, C., Spence, M.A., and Gargus, J.J. (2003). Mitochondrial dys-
function in autistic patients with 15q inverted duplication. Ann. Neurol. 53:801–804.
Fombonne, E., Roge, B., Claverie, J., Courty, S., and Fremolle, J. (1999). Microcephaly and
macrocephaly in autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 29:113–119.
Gutierrez, L.L., Mazzotti, N.G., Araujo, A.S., Klipel, R.B., Fernandes, T.R., Llesuy, S.F., and
Bello-Klein, A. (2006). Peripheral markers of oxidative stress in chronic mercuric chloride
intoxication. Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. 39:767–772.
Hazlett, H.C., Poe, M., Gerig, G., Smith, R.G., Provenzale, J., Ross, A., Gilmore, J., and Piven, J.
(2005). Magnetic resonance imaging and head circumference study of brain size in autism:
Birth through age 2 years. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62:1366–1376.
Honig, M., Briese, T., Buie, T., Bauman, M.L., Lauwers, G., Siemetzki, U., Hummel, K., Rota, P.A.,
Bellini, W.J., O’Leary, J.J., Sheils, O., Alden, E., Pickering, L., and Lipkin, W.I. (2008).
Lack of association between measles virus vaccine and autism with enteropathy: A
case–control study. PLoS One 3:3140.
Howard, M.A., Cowell, P.E., Boucher, J., Broks, P., Mayes, A., Farrant, A., and Roberts, N.
(2000). Convergent neuroanatomical and behavioural evidence of an amygdala hypoth-
esis of autism. Neuroreport 11:2931–2935.
Ingram, J.L., Peckharm, S.M., Tisdale, B., and Rodier, P. (2000). Prenatal exposure of rats to
valproic acid reproduces the cerebellar anomalies associated with autism. Neurotoxicol.
Teratol. 22:319–324.
James, S.J., Cutler, P., Melnyk, S., Jernigan, S., Janak, L., Gaylor, D.W., and Neubrander, J.A.
(2004). Metabolic biomarkers of increased oxidative stress and impaired methylation
capacity in children with autism. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 80:1611–1617.
Jurima-Romet, M., Abbott, F.S., Tang, W., Huang, H.S., and Whitehouse, L.W. (1996).
Cytotoxicity of unsaturated metabolites of valproic acid and protection by vitamins C
and E in glutathione-depleted rat hepatocytes. Toxicology 112:69–85.
Kanner, L. (1943a). Co-editor’s introduction. Nervous Child 2:216.
Kanner, L. (1943b). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child 2:217–250.
Kemper, T.L. and Bauman, M.L. (1998). Neuropathology of infantile autism. J. Neuropath.
Exp. Neurology 57:645–652.
London, E. and Etzel, R.A. (2000). The environment as an etiologic factor in autism: A new
direction for research. Environ. Health Perspect. 108:401–404.
Madsen, K.M., Hviid, A., Vestergaard, M.., Schendel, D., Wohlfahrt, J., Thorsen, P., Olsen, J.,
and Melbye, M. (2002). A population-based study of measles, mumps, and rubella vac-
cination and autism. N. Eng. J. Med. 347:1477–1482.
Mendola, P., Selevan, S.G., Gutter, S., and Rice, D. (2002). Environmental factors associated
with a spectrum of neurodevelopmental deficits. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev.
8:188–197.
Ming, X., Brimacombe, M., Malek, J.H., Jani, N., and Wagner, G.C. (2008a). Autism spectrum
disorders and identified toxic land fills: Co-occurrence across States. Environ. Health
Insights 2:55–59.
Ming, X., Cheh, M.A., Yochum, C.L., Halladay, A.K., and Wagner, G.C. (2008b). Evidence
of oxidative stress in autism derived from animal models. Am. J. Biochem. Biotechnol.
(Special Issue on Autism Spectrum Disorders) 4:218–225.
Ming, X., Stein, T.P., Johnson, W.G., Lambert, G.H., and Wagner, G.C. (2005). Increased
excretion of a lipid peroxidation biomarker in autism. Prostaglandins Leukot. Essent.
Fatty Acids 73:379–384.
150 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Minshew, N.J., Goldstein, G., and Dombrowski, S.M. (1993). A preliminary 31P MRS study
of autism: Evidence for undersynthesis and increased degradation of brain membranes.
Biol. Psychiatry 33:762–773.
Moreno, H., Borjas, L., Arrieta, A., Sáez, L., Prassad, A., Estévez, J., and Bonilla, E. (1992).
Clinical heterogeneity of the autistic syndrome: A study of 60 families. Invest. Clin.
33:13–31.
Nagashima, K. (1997). A review of experimental methylmercury toxicity in rats: Neuro-
pathology and evidence for apoptosis. Toxicol. Pathol. 25:624–631.
Olson, K. and Boush, M. (1975). Decreased learning capacity in rats exposed prenatally and
postnatally to low doses of mercury. Bull Environ. Contam. Toxicol. 13:73–79.
Piven, J., Arndt, S., Bailey, J., Havercamp, S., Andreasen, N.C., and Palmer, P. (1995). An MRI
study of brain size in autism. Am. J. Psychiatry 152:1145–1149.
Packer, L. (1984). Oxygen radicals in biological systems. In L. Packer (ed.), Methods in
Enzymology 105. New York: Academic Press.
Rapin, I. (1997). Autism. N. Eng. J. Med. 337:97–104.
Ritvo, E.R., Freeman, B.J., Scheibel, A.B., Duong, T., Robinson, H., Guthrie, D., and Ritvo, A.
(1986). Lower Purkinje cell counts in the cerebella of four autistic subjects: Initial find-
ings of the UCLA-NSAC autopsy research report. Am. J. Psychiatry 143:862–866.
Rodier, P.M., Ingram, J.L., Tisdale, B., and Croog, V.J. (1997). Linking etiologies in humans
and animal models: Studies of Autism. Repro. Toxicol. 11:417–422.
Rodier, P.M. and Hyman, S.L. (1998). Early environmental factors in autism. Ment. Retard.
Dev. Dis. Res. Rev. 4:121–128.
Rutter, M. (1979). Language, cognition and autism. In R. Katzman (ed.), Congenital and
Acquired Cognitive Disorders, New York, Raven Press, pp. 247–264.
Sager, P.R., Aschner, M., and Rodier, P.M. (1984). Persistent, differential alterations in develop-
ing cerebellar cortex of male and female mice after methylmercury exposure. Brain Res.
314:1–11.
Saitoh, O., Courchesne, E., Egaas, B., Lincoln, A.J., and Schreibman, L. (1995). Cross-sectional
area of the posterior hippocampus in autistic patients with cerebellar and corpus callosum
abnormalities. Neurology 45:317–324.
Sakamoto, M., Kakita, A., Wakabayashi, K., Takahashi, H., Nakano, A., and Akagi, H. (2002).
Evaluation of changes in methylmercury accumulation in the developing rat brain and
its effects: A study with consecutive and moderate dose exposure throughout gestation
and lactation periods. Brain Res. 949:51–59.
Schain, R.J. and Freedman, D.X. (1961). Studies on 5-hydroxyindole metabolism in autistic
and mentally retarded children. J. Pediatrics 58:315–320.
Schneider, T. and Przewlocki, R. (2005). Behavioral alterations in rats prenatally exposed to
valproic acid: Animal model of autism. Neuropsychopharmacology 30:80–89.
Shanker, G. and Aschner, M. (2003). Methylmercury-induced reactive oxygen species forma-
tion in neonatal cerebral astrocytic cultures is attenuated by antioxidants. Mol. Brain Res.
110:85–91.
Shanker, G., Syversen, T., Aschner, J.L., and Aschner, M. (2005). Modulatory effect of glu-
tathione status and antioxidants on methylmercury-induced free radical formation in
primary cultures of cerebral astrocytes. Mol. Brain Res. 137:11–22.
Simon, G., Moog, C., and Obert, G. (1994). Valproic acid reduces the intracellular level of
glutathione and stimulates human immunodeficiency virus. Chem. Biol. Interact.
91:111–21.
Sobaniec-Lotowweska, M.E. (2001). Ultrastructure of Purkinje cell perikarya and their den-
dritic processes in the rat cerebellar cortex in experimental encephalopathy induced by
chronic application of valproate. Int. J. Exp. Pathol. 82:337–348.
Autism and Oxidative Stress: Evidence from an Animal Model 151
Sögüt, S., Zoroglu, S.S., Ozyurt, H., Yilmaz, H.R., Ozugurlu, F., Sivasli, E., Yetkin, O., Yanik, M.,
Tutkun, H., Savas, H.A., Tarakçioglu. M., and Akyol, O. (2003). Changes in nitric oxide
levels and antioxidant enzyme activities may have a role in the pathophysiological mecha-
nisms involved in autism. Clin. Chim. Acta 331:111–117.
Soto-Ares, G., Joyes, B., Lemaitire, M.P., Vallee, L., and Pruvo, J.P. (2003). MRI in children
with mental retardation. Pediatr. Radiol. 33:334–345.
Sparks, B.F., Friedman, S.D., Shaw, D.W., Aylward, E.H., Echelard, D., Artru, A.A.,
Maravilla, K.R., Giedd, J.N., Munson, J., Dawson, G., and Dager, S.R. (2002). Brain
structural abnormalities in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Neurology
59:184–192.
Stringari, J., Meotti, F.C., Souza, D.O., Santos, A.R.S., and Farina, M. (2006). Postnatal methyl-
mercury exposure induces hyperlocomotor activity and cerebellar oxidative stress in
mice: Dependence on the neurodevelopmental period. Neurochem. Res. 31:563–569.
Tabatabaei, A.R. and Abbott, F.S. (1999). Assessing the mechanism of metabolism-dependent
valproic acid-induced in vitro cytotoxicity. Chem. Res. Toxicol. 12:323–330.
Taylor, B., Miller, E., Farrington, C.P., Petropoulous, M-C., Favot-Mayaud, I., Li, J., and
Waight, P.A. (1999). Autism and measles, mumps, and rubella vaccine: No epidemio-
logical evidence for a causal association. Lancet 353:2026–2029.
Tuchman, R.F. and Rapin, I. (1997). Regression in pervasive developmental disorders: Seizures
and epileptiform electroencephalogram correlates. Pediatrics 99:560–566.
Usuki, F., Yasutake, A., Umehara, F., Tokunaga, H., Matsumoto, M., Eto, K., Ishiura, S., and
Higuchi, I. (2001). In vivo protection of a water-soluble derivative of vitamin E, Trolox,
against methylmercury-intoxication in the rat. Neurosci. Lett. 304:199–203.
Vicente, É., Boer, M., Netto, C., Fochesatto, C., Dalmaz, C., Siqueira, I.R., and Gonçalves, C.A.
(2004). Hippocampal antioxidant system in neonates from methylmercury-intoxicated
rats. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 26:817–823.
Volkmar, F.R. and Nelson, D.S. (1990). Seizure disorders in autism. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc.
Psychiatry 29:127–129.
Vorhees, C.V. (1985). Behavioral effects of prenatal methylmercury in rats: A parallel trial to
the collaborative behavioral teratology study. Neurobehav. Toxicol. Teratol. 7:717–725.
Wagner, G.C., Avena, N., Kita T., Nakashima T., Fisher H., and Halladay, A.K. (2004). Risperidone
attenuation of amphetamine-induced self-injurious behavior in mice. Neuropharmacology
46:700–708.
Wagner, G.C., Reuhl, K.R., Cheh, M., McRae, P., and Halladay, A.K. (2006). A new neurobehav-
ioral model of autism in mice: Pre- and postnatal exposure to sodium valproate. J. Autism
Dev. Disord. 36:779–793.
Wagner, G.C., Reuhl, K.R., Ming, X., and Halladay, A.K. (2007). Behavioral and neurochemical
sensitization to amphetamine following early postnatal administration of methylmercury.
NeuroToxicology 28:59–66.
Wakefield, A.J., Murch, S.H., Anthony, A., Linnell, J., Casson, D.M., Maiik, M., Berelowitz, M.,
Dhillon, A.P., Thompson, M.A., Harvey, P., Valentine, A., Davies, S.E., and Walker-
Smith, J.A. (1998). Ileal-lymphoid-nodular hyperplasia, non-specific colitis, and perva-
sive developmental disorder in children. Lancet 351:637–641.
Williams, G., King, J., Cunningham, M., Stephan, M., Kerr, B., and Hersh, J.H. (2001). Fetal
valproate syndrome and autism: Additional evidence of an association. Dev. Med. Child
Neurol. 43:202–206.
Yee, S. and Choi, B.H. (1994). Methylmercury poisoning induces oxidative stress in the mouse
brain. Exp. Mol. Pathol. 60:188–196.
Yee, S. and Choi, B.H. (1996). Oxidative stress in neurotoxic effects of methylmercury
poisoning. NeuroToxicology 17:17–26.
152 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Yochum, C.L., Dowling, P., Reuhl, K.R., Wagner, G.C., and Ming, X. (2008). VPA-induced
apoptosis and behavioral deficits in neonatal mice. Brain Res. 1203:126–132.
Yorbik, O., Sayalm A., Akay, C., Akbiyik, D.I., and Sohmen, T. (2002). Investigation of anti-
oxidant enzymes in children with autistic disorder. Prostaglandins Leukot. Essent. Fatty
Acids 67:341–343.
Zoroglu, S.S., Armutcu, F., Ozen, S., Gurel, A., Sivasli, E., Yetkin, O., and Meram, I. (2004).
Increased oxidative stress and altered activities of erythrocyte free radical scavenging
enzymes in autism. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 254:143–147.
Zoroglu, S.S., Yürekli, M., Meram, I., Sögüt, S., Tutkun, H., Yetkin, O., Sivasli, E., Savas,
H.A., Yanik, M., Herken, H., and Akyol, O. (2003). Pathophysiological role of nitric
oxide and adrenomedullin in autism. Cell. Biochem. Function 21:55–60.
9 Neurotoxic Brainstem
Impairment as Proposed
Threshold Event in
Autistic Regression
Woody R. McGinnis,1,* Veronica M. Miller,2
Tapan Audhya,3,4 and Stephen M. Edelson5
1
Autism House of Auckland, Autism New Zealand, Inc.,
Auckland, New Zealand
2
Wadsworth Center for Laboratories and Research,
New York State Department of Health, Albany,
NY 12201, USA
3
Vitamin Diagnostics Laboratory,
Cliffwood Beach, NJ 07735, USA
4
Division of Endocrinology, Department of Medicine,
New York University School of Medicine,
New York, NY 10016, USA
5 Autism Research Institute, San Diego, CA 92116, USA
CONTENTS
9.1 Introduction ................................................................................................. 154
9.2 Somatobehavioral Regression in Autism .................................................... 154
9.3 Toxicants as Potential Triggers for Regression in Autism .......................... 155
9.4 Toxicant Accumulation and Injury in CVO ................................................ 157
9.5 Findings Consistent with Brainstem Pathology in Autism ......................... 160
9.5.1 Clinical and Electrophysiological Brainstem
Findings in Autism......................................................................... 160
9.5.2 Abnormal Brainstem Morphology ................................................. 161
9.5.3 Biochemical Abnormalities in Autism .......................................... 161
9.5.4 Features of Regression in Autism .................................................. 162
153
154 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
9.1 INTRODUCTION
In this chapter, we propose a model for autistic regression resulting from passage
of toxicants into regions of brain unprotected by blood–brain barrier (BBB). These
portals surround the primitive brainstem, a reportedly abnormal brain region in
autistic subjects. We propose that toxicant-induced impairment of a specific struc-
ture, the dorsal vagal complex (DVC) of the medulla, sufficiently accounts for the
primary neurophysiological changes of autistic regression. Toxicant effects on other
unprotected sites may explain associated abnormalities in autism, including altered
melatonin and oxytocin production. In autistic regression, it is possible that brain-
stem is primary target for toxicant-induced inflammation, which ramifies to higher
structures in a pattern resembling the known stages of Parkinson disease (PD).
Over the past decade, it has become clear that a variety of gastrointestinal
problems—constipation, diarrhea, abdominal pain, and distension—commonly
associate with autistic behavior. Over half of a mixed cohort of autistic children
had radiographic fecal loading or megacolon (Afzal et al., 2003), as did 100% of a
regressive cohort, with or without history of intercurrent diarrhea (Torrente et al.,
2002). Endoscopy of regressed cohorts demonstrated very high incidence of entero-
colitis (Wakefield et al., 1998, 2002) and reflux esophagitis (Horvath et al., 1999).
Case reports suggest that gastrointestinal symptoms and behavioral regression
occur at about the same time (Madsen and Vestergaard, 2004). On the basis of hun-
dreds of clinical interviews oriented to somatic as well as behavioral changes, we
(McGinnis, Audhya, and Edelson) concur with Goldberg’s published observation:
“Parents often report their children as having gastrointestinal symptoms that started
about the same time autistic symptoms appeared” (Goldberg, 2004). Refined data
for onset of gastrointestinal symptoms are not sufficiently evolved, but the collective
clinical knowledge strongly suggests a triad of changes in autistic regression: (1) lost
vocalization, (2) lost social skills, AND (3) gastrointestinal impairment.
Subfornical
organ
Organum
vasculosum
Pineal
Median
eminence
Posterior
Area pituitary
postrema
FIGURE 9.1 The CVO, in proximity to primitive brainstem (shaded), remain unprotected
by BBB after its maturation in humans by about 12 months of age.
a central site of action for circulating angiotensin II (Kim et al., 2008). A rich network
of axons emerges from the AP, including serotonergic connections to the pons.
AP-lesioned animals consume remarkable amounts of water (Curtis et al., 1996)
or concentrated salt water (Johnson and Edwards, 1991)—an interesting observation
in light of published reports of increased water consumption in autistic children
(Terai et al., 1999) and an 8% (2,090/25,637) incidence of salt-craving in autistic
children surveyed by ARI. Food aversions and cravings for carbohydrates and bland
diets result from ablation of AP (Edwards et al., 1997), and although inadequately
quantified in autism, are commonly reported by parents. Flavor aversion after cad-
mium exposure is reversed by dimercaptosuccinic acid (Peele et al., 1988), a chelating
agent which does not cross the BBB, but which reportedly improved behavior in a
number of children with autism (Geier and Geier, 2006).
AP is proximate to the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus (DMV) and the nucleus
tractus solitarius (NTS). The three structures comprise the DVC, which mediates
autonomic function of the cervical, thoracic, and abdominal viscera (Figure 9.2).
Ablation of the DMV blocks viscerosecretory as well as visceromotor function.
The NTS receives abundant viscerosensory input via cranial nerves VII, IX, and X,
integrates peripheral and central signals, and sends both sympathetic and parasym
pathetic efferents to the viscera. Conceivably, reported rapid improvement in
children with autism after secretin infusion (Horvath et al., 1998) relates to NTS,
where binding of intravenous secretin is most prominent (Yang et al., 2004).
DMV is the consistent site of initial pathology in PD, which ascends in stages
to outlying brainstem and distal structures including neocortex (Figure 9.3). Not
surprisingly, digestive symptoms are frequent in PD and occasionally dominate
the clinical picture (Spellman and Warner, 1977). Disorders of gastrointestinal
motility are prominent in PD (Cersosimo and Benarroch, 2008). The gastroe-
sophageal sphincter is lax, and 61% of patients with PD complained of esophageal
Neurotoxic Brainstem Impairment as Proposed Threshold Event 157
AP
IV DMV
NTS
FIGURE 9.2 Cross section of luxol-blue stained medulla at level of area postrema (AP),
which adjoins the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus (DMV) and the NTS in the floor of the
fourth ventricle (IV). AP lacks tight BBB and tight CSF barrier and is envisioned as key por-
tal for neurotoxicants in autistic regression.
symptoms (Bassotti et al., 1998). Many environmental risk factors have been
identified for PD (Onyango, 2008).
Years after autistic regression, reported physical changes in brain are evident in
widespread areas not limited to brainstem. Plausible mechanisms exist within our
model for ramifying changes after initial brainstem injury. Brainstem injury itself
may disturb development of higher structures (Geva and Feldman, 2008; Tanguay and
Edwards, 1982). The spread of pathology from brainstem CVO conceivably involves
diffusion of toxicants or reactive inflammatory cytokines. Elevated cytokine levels
in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) of subjects with autism (Vargas et al., 2005) plausibly
result from toxicant-induced production in CVO, two of which—AP and median
eminence (ME)—lack tight junctions with CSF (Broadwell et al., 1983).
Experimentally, inflammatory cytokine diffuses from lateral ventricle along
white matter nerve bundles of the corpus callosum, external capsule, and striatum
all the way to amygdala (Vitkovic et al., 2000). Cytokine injected in striatum exac-
erbates excitoxicity in cortex (Lawrence et al., 1998). The pattern of inflammatory
cytokine diffusion along nerve bundles suggests an anisotropic diffusion pathway in
small channels located outside myelinated axons (Agnati et al., 1995).
Neocortex,
Presymptomatic Symptomatic primary,
phase phase secondary
Neocortex,
high order
association
Mesocortex,
thalamus
Threshold Substantia
nigra,
amygdala
Gain setting
nuclei
Dorsal
motor ×
nucleus
1 2 3 4 5 6 Stages of the
PD-related
(A) path, process
(B)
FIGURE 9.3 (See color insert following page 200.) The ascending pathology of Parkinson’s
disease occurs in six recognizable stages, beginning in the DMV of the medulla. Progressive
shading in table (A) corresponds to the like-shaded anatomic regions represented in diagram (B).
(From Braak, H. et al., Cell. Tissue Res., 31, 121, 2004. With permission.)
concentrates in CVO, and also that administration of mercury in the organic form—
which is commonly understood to traverse the BBB—results in accumulation of
inorganic mercury in CVO, with long-term residence after remote conversion and
redistribution via blood. A review of the literature will make it clear that oxidative
mechanisms of cytotoxicity are prominent for each of the aforementioned toxicants.
Cadmium injected intravenously into adult rats accumulated only in regions out-
side the BBB, including AP and pineal, but did not appear elsewhere in the brain
(Arvidson, 1986; Arvidson and Tjalve, 1986). Cadmium induces oxidative stress,
Neurotoxic Brainstem Impairment as Proposed Threshold Event 159
of rats concentrated in the motor nuclei of brainstem and deep nuclei of cerebellum
(Moller-Madsen and Danscher, 1986).
Organic mercury, as found in fish and as preservative in some vaccines (Poling
2008), passes the BBB readily. Most organic mercury is converted to inorganic mer-
cury for excretion in feces, but some recirculates to increase inorganic mercury con-
centration in blood (Havarinasab et al., 2007). Once in the brain, inorganic mercury
persists for years (Vahter et al., 1994). It is the inorganic form which associates
with immune stimulation (Havarinasab et al., 2007) and increased microglia in brain
(Geier and Geier, 2007). Notably, amalgam removal decreased plasma and red-cell
inorganic mercury levels by 73% (Halbach et al., 2008).
The absence of BBB increases accumulation of inorganic mercury during and
after chronic methyl mercury (organic) exposure. Methyl mercury was administered
orally for 18 months to adult female primates, and mercury levels were measured in
six regions of brain, including pituitary, a CVO lacking BBB. Inorganic mercury
concentrations increased on average in the six areas 30-fold at 6 months and 60-fold
at 18 months, but by far the highest concentrations of mercury—largely inorganic—
were achieved in pituitary. In animals in which methyl mercury was discontinued
at 6 months, inorganic levels continued to climb in the pituitary—doubling between
6 and 12 months—but not in regions with BBB. Mercury in control animals was
undetectable in most regions, but appeared higher in pituitary. One or two primates
in each exposure group had distinctly higher or lower fractions of inorganic mercury
across brain regions, assumed secondary to individual variations in demethylation
(Vahter et al., 1994).
… medulla oblongata was large but the pyramids were relatively small. There was mild
widening of the sulci of the superior cerebellar vermis…apparent reduplication of the
medial accessory olive, and multiple small bilateral groups of ectopic neurons lay lateral
to the olives in the inferior cerebellar peduncles. An aberrant tract was present in both
sides of the pontine tegmentum…midbrain was unusually small and the periaqueductal
grey matter and raphe nuclei appeared disproportionately large… (Bailey et al., 1998).
Two subjects with autistic behaviors had numerous swollen axon terminals
(“spheroids”) in various motor and sensory nuclei and reticular formation of the
medulla, as well as hypothalamus, dorsomedial thalamus, hippocampus, and cere-
bral cortex (Weidenheim et al., 2001). Abnormal morphology was evident in neurons
of the superior olivary nucleus of the caudal pons in five autistic subjects (Kulesza
and Mangunay, 2008).
(A) (B)
FIGURE 9.4 (See color insert following page 200.) Enlargement of Paneth’s cells, as
indicated by dark staining of secretory granules, was a frequent finding in a series of duodenal
biopsies from children with regressed autism (A), as compared to non-autistic controls
(B). (Photographs courtesy of Karoly Horvath MD, PhD, Professor of Pediatrics, Thomas
Jefferson University, Philadelphia, PA.)
TABLE 9.1
Reported Findings in Autism, Which Are Consistent with Brainstem
Abnormality or Consistent with Abnormality of CVO
Found within or Proximate to the Brainstem
Studies References
Clinical observations
Excessive thirst Terai et al. (1999)
Salt-craving and flavor-aversion ARI parent survey
Tone-of-voice alterations Paul et al. (2005)
Talk → whisper ARI parent survey
Electrical speech generation Thunber et al. (2007)
Secretin effect Horvath et al. (1998)
Altered postrotatory response Ornitz (1983)
Cardiac baroreflex Ming et al. (2005)
Heart rate, respiratory and vascular Althaus et al. (2004), Bonvallet
and Allen (1963)
Electrical studies
Auditory brainstem response Klin (1993), Kwon et al. (2007)
Centrencephalic electroencephalograms Gilberg and Schaumann (1983)
Decreased cardiac parasympathetic tone Ming et al. (2005)
Vagal nerve stimulation Murphy et al. (2000)
Radiographic studies
Fecal loading in 100% of regressed cohort Torrente et al. (2002)
Fecal loading in >50% of mixed cohort Afzal et al. (2003)
Endoscopic studies
Esophageal reflux 67% of regressed cohort Horvath et al. (1999)
Microscopic studies
Neuronal morphology/clustering inf. olives Kemper and Bauman (1993)
Neuronal morphology superior olives Kulesza and Mangunay (2008)
Ectopic neurons and aberrant tracts Bailey et al. (1998)
Swollen axon terminals in medullary nuclei Weidenheim et al. (2001)
Paneth’s cells enlarged with granules Horvath et al. (1999)
Biochemical studies
Abnormal oxytocin production Green et al. (2001), Modahl et al.
(1998)
Abnormal melatonin production Kulman et al. (2000), Nir et al. (1995),
Tordjman et al. (2005)
166 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
McGinnis, 2004; Suh et al., 2008; Yao et al., 2006) and greater oxidative modification
of hippocampus (Evans et al., 2008), cerebral cortex (Evans et al., 2008; Lopez-
Hurtado and Prieto, 2008), and cerebellum (Sajdel-Sulkowska et al., 2008) exists in
children already diagnosed with autism. Gastrointestinal inflammation, emotional
stress associated with noncommunication, sleep disturbance, and lower production
of melatonin and oxytocin may contribute variably to greater oxidative stress after
regression.
There is some support—elevated brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in
cord blood, greater maternal stress during gestation, more perinatal complications—
for the suggestion that oxidative stress is heightened prior to regression (McGinnis,
2004, 2007). If oxidative stress were higher in brain prior to regression, then cor-
respondingly lower antioxidant protection in brain would be expected to influence
thresholds for brainstem injury by toxicants. Greater peripheral oxidative stress alone
plausibly lowers the threshold for CVO-mediated regression, because the cholinergic—
especially, muscarinic—nervous system is sensitive to oxidative stress (McGinnis,
2004), and extracranial vagal parasympathetics are exclusively cholinergic.
Cholinergic receptors are inhibited preferentially by oxidants (De Sarno and
Pope, 1998), and numbers of cholinergic receptors are reduced under conditions of
oxidative stress (Gajewski et al., 1988). Cholinergic receptor activity measured in
basal forebrain and parietal cortex was lower in autism (Perry et al., 2001), and it
has been suggested that cerebral hypoperfusion in autism results from muscarinic
inhibition (McGinnis, 2004). The cholinergic system is essential for brain develop-
ment, acting as modulator of neuronal proliferation, migration, and differentiation
(Roda et al., 2008).
We retrospectively compared acetylcholine (ACh) levels in plasma and CSF from
children aged 3–15 who were identifiable as autistic or nonautistic. Mean plasma
ACh was much lower in autistic children (192 ± 36 ng/L; n = 94) than in nonautistic
children (464 ± 85 ng/L; n = 66). Mean ACh in CSF also was lower in an autistic
cohort (941 ± 202 ng/L; n = 16) than a nonautistic cohort (1558 ± 510 ng/L; n = 8).
It is not known whether these levels reflect the state of neurological transmission
in autism. Chronic exposure of animals to cadmium or methyl mercury is noted to
lower ACh in brain (Hrdina et al., 1976), but existence of any such relationship in
autism is speculative.
and altered calcium response to glutamate (Ammori et al., 2008). Besides endogenous
inflammatory factors, viruses also easily may enter at CVO (Banks, 2000).
REFERENCES
Adams, J.B., Romdalvik, J., Ramanujam, V.M., and Legator, M.S. 2007. Mercury, lead, and
zinc in baby teeth of children with autism versus controls. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health
A. 70:1046–1051.
Afzal, N., Murch, S., Thirrupathy, K., Berger, L., Fagbemi, A., and Heuschkel, R.
2003. Constipation with acquired megarectum in children with autism. Pediatrics
112:939–942.
Agnati, L.F., Zoli, M., Stromberg, I., and Fuxe, K. 1995. Intercellular communication in the
brain: Wiring versus volume transmission. Neuroscience 69:711–726.
Allegra, M., Reiter, R.J., Tan, D.X., Gentile, C., Tesoriere, L., and Livrea, M.A. 2003. The
chemistry of melatonin’s interaction with reactive species. J. Pineal Res. 34:1–10.
Neurotoxic Brainstem Impairment as Proposed Threshold Event 169
Althaus, M., Van Roon, S.M., Lambertus, Mulder, L.J.M., Mulder, G., Aarnoudse, C.C., and
Minderaa, R.B. (2004). Autonomic response patterns observed during the performance
of an attention-demanding task in two groups of children with autistic-type difficulties
in social adjustment. Psychophysiology 41:893–904.
Ammori, J.B., Zhang, W.Z., Li, J.Y., Chai, B.X., and Mulholland, M.W. 2008. Effect of
intestinal inflammation on neuronal survival and function in the dorsal motor nucleus
of the vagus. Surgery 144:149–158.
Arvidson, B. 1986. Autoradiographic localization of cadmium in the rat brain. Neurotoxicology
7:89–96.
Arvidson, B. 1992. Accumulation of inorganic mercury in lower motoneurons of mice.
Neurotoxicology 13:277–80.
Arvidson, B. and Tjalve, H. 1986. Distribution of 109Cd in the nervous system of rats after
intravenous injection. Acta Neuropathol. 69:111–116.
Bailey, A., Luthert, P., Dean, A., Harding, B., Janota, I., Montgomery, M., Rutter, M., and
Lantos, P. 1998. A clinicopathological study of autism. Brain 121:889–905.
Bandyopadhyay, D., Biswas, K., Bhattacharyya, M., Reiter, R.J., and Banerjee, R.K. 2002.
Involvement of reactive oxygen species in gastric ulceration: Protection by melatonin.
Indian J. Exp. Biol. 2002:693–705.
Banks, W.A. 2000. Protector, prey and perpetrator: The pathophysiology of the blood–brain
barrier in neuroAIDS. NeuroAIDS 3:1–5.
Bassotti, G., Germani, U., Pagliaricci, S., Plesa, A., Giulietti, O., Mannarino, E., and Morelli,
A. 1998. Esophageal manometric abnormalities in Parkinson’s disease. Dysphagia
13:28–31.
Basu, N., Scheuhammer, A.M., Sonne, C., Letcher, R.J., Born, E.W., and Dietz, R. 2009.
Is dietary mercury of toxicological concern to wild polar bears (Ursus maritimus)?
Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 28:133–140.
Bauman, M. and Kemper, T.L. 1985. Histoanatomic observations of the brain in early
infantile autism. Neurology 35:866–874.
Bawari, M., Babu, G.N., Ali, M.M., and Misra, U.K. 1995. Effect of neonatal monosodium
glutamate on lipid peroxidation in adult rat brain. Neuroreport 6:650–652.
Baydas, G., Nedzvetsky, V.S., Nerush, P.A., Krichenko, S.V., Demchenko, H.M., and Reiter,
R.J. 2002. A novel role for melatonin: Regulation of the expression of cell adhesion
molecules in the rat hippocampus and cortex. Neurosci. Lett. 326:109–112.
Bernard, S., Enayati, A., Redwood, L., Roger, H., and Binstock, T. 2001. Autism: A novel
form of mercury poisoning. Med. Hypotheses 56:462–471.
Bonvallet, M. and Allen, M.B. 1963. Prolonged spontaneous and evoked reticular activation
following discreet bulbar lesions. Electroencephal. Clin. Neurophysiol. 15:969–988.
Braak, H., Ghebremedhin, E., Rub, U., Bratzke, H., and Del Tredici, K., 2004. Stages in the
development of Parkinson’s related pathology. Cell. Tissue Res. 31:121–134.
Breder, C.D., Hazuka, C., Ghayur, T., Klug, C., Huginin, M., Yasuda, K., Teng, M., and Saper,
C.B. 1994. Regional induction of tumor necrosis factor alpha expression in the mouse
brain after systemic lipopolysaccharide administration. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
91:11393–11397.
Broadwell, R.D., Balin, B.J., Salcman, M., and Kaplan, R.S. 1983. Brain–blood barrier? Yes
and no. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 80:7352–7356.
Carter, C.S., Williams, J.R., Witt, D.M., and Insel, T.R. 1992. Oxytocin and social bonding.
Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 652:204–211.
Cersosimo, M.G. and Benarroch, E.E. 2008. Neural control of the gastrointestinal tract:
Implications for Parkinson disease. Mov. Disord. 23:1065–1075.
Chaparro-Huerta, V., Rivera-Cervantes, M.C., Torres-Mendoza, B.M., and Beas-Zarate, C.
2002. Neuronal death and tumor necrosis factor-alpha response to glutamate-induced
excitotoxicity in the cerebral cortex of neonatal rats. Neurosci. Lett. 333:95–98.
170 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Charleston, J.S., Bolender, R.P., Mottet, N.K., Body, R.L., Vahter, M.E., and Burbacher, T.M.
1994. Increases in the number of reactive glia in the visual cortex of Macaca fascicularis
following subclinical long-term methylmercury exposure. Toxicol. Appl. Pharmacol.
129:196–206.
Chauhan, A. and Chauhan, V. 2006. Oxidative stress in autism. Pathophysiology 13:171–181.
Chauhan, A., Sheikh, A.M., and Chauhan, V. 2008. Increased copper-mediated oxidation of
membrane phosphatidylethanolamine in autism. Am. J. Biochem. Biotechnol. (Special
Issue on Autism Spectrum Disorders) 4:101–104.
Chen, W., Body, R.L., and Mottet, N.K. 1983. Biochemical and morphological studies of
monkeys chronically exposed to methylmercury. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health 12:407–416.
Chez, M.G., Dowling, T., Patel, P.B., Khanna, P., and Kominsky, M. 2007. Elevation of
tumor necrosis factor-alpha in cerebrospinal fluid of autistic children. Pediatr. Neurol.
36:361–365.
Cordes, S.P. 2005. Molecular genetics of the early development of hindbrain serotonergic
neurons. Clin. Genet. 68:487–494.
Courchesne, E. 1997. Brainstem, cerebellar and limbic neuroanatomical abnormalities in
autism. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 7:269–278.
Curtis, K.S., Verbalis, J.G., and Stricker, E.M. 1996. Area postrema lesions in rats appear to
disrupt rapid feedback inhibition of fluid intake. Brain Res. 726:31–38.
Davidovitch, M., Glick, L., Holtzman, G., Tirosh, E., and Safir, M.P. 2000. Developmental
regression in autism: Maternal perception. J Autism Dev. Disord. 30:113–119.
De Sarno, P. and Pope, R.S. 1998. Phosphoinositide hydrolysis activated by muscarinic or
glutamatergic, but not adrenergic, receptors is impaired in ApoE-deficient mice and by
hydrogen peroxide and peroxynitrite. Exp. Neurol. 152:123–128.
Dufault, R., LeBlanc, B., Schnoll, R., Cornett, C., Schweitzer, L., Wallinga, D., Hightower, J.,
Patrick, L., and Lukiw, W.J. (2009). Mercury from chlor-alkali plants: Measured
concentrations in food product sugar. Environ. Health 8:2.
Edwards, G.L., White, B.D., He, B., Dean, R.G., and Martin, R.J. 1997. Elevated hypotha-
lamic neuropeptide Y levels in rats with dorsomedial hindbrain lesions. Brain Res.
755:84–90.
Eletskii, I., Kulikov, O.V., and Tsibulevskii, A. 1984. Reactions of Paneth cells of the rat
jejunum to section of the vagus nerves (ultrastructural analysis). Arkh. Anat. Gistol.
Embriol. 86:73–79.
Erroi, A., Bianchi, M., and Ghezzi, P. 1992. The pneumotoxicant paraquat potentiates IL-1 and
TNF production by human mononuclear cells. Agents Actions 36:66–69.
Evans, T.A., Siedlak, S.L., Lu, L., Fu, X., Wang, Z., McGinnis, W.R., Fakhoury, E., Castellani, R.J.,
Hazen, S.L., Walsh, W.J., Lewis, A.T., Salomon, R.G., Smith, M.A., Perry, G., and
Zhu, X. 2008. The autistic phenotype exhibits a remarkably localized modification
of brain protein by products of free radical-induced lipid oxidation. Am. J. Biochem.
Biotechnol. (Special Issue on Autism Spectrum Disorders) 4:61–72.
Fatemi, S.H., Halt, A.R., Stary, J.M., Kanodia, R., Schulz, S.C., and Realmuto, G.R. 2002.
Glutamic acid decarboxylase 65 and 67 kDa proteins are reduced in autistic parietal and
cerebellar cortices. Biol. Psychiatry 52:805–810.
Ferguson, J.N., Young, L.J., Hearn, E.F., Matzuk, M.M., Insel, T.R., and Winslow, J.T. 2000.
Social amnesia in mice lacking the oxytocin gene. Nat. Genet. 25:284–288.
Gajewski, M., Laskowska-Bozek, H., Orlewski, P., Maslinski, S., and Ryzewski, J. 1988.
Influence of lipid peroxidation and hydrogen peroxide on muscarinic cholinergic
receptors and ATP level in rat myocytes and lymphocytes. Int. J. Tissue React.
10:281–290.
Geier, D.A. and Geier, M.R. 2007. A case series of children with apparent mercury toxic
encephalopathies manifesting with clinical symptoms of regressive autistic disorders. J.
Toxicol. Environ. Health A. 70:837–851.
Neurotoxic Brainstem Impairment as Proposed Threshold Event 171
Geier, D.A., Kern, J.K., Garver, C.R., Adams, J.B., Audhya, T., Nataf, R., and Geier, M.R.
(2008). Biomarkers of environmental toxicity and susceptibility in autism. J. Neurol.
Sci. 280:101–118.
Geva, R. and Feldman, R. 2008. A neurobiological model for the effects of early brainstem func-
tioning on the development of behavior and emotion regulation in infants: Implications
for prenatal and perinatal risk. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 49:1031–1041.
Gilberg, C. and Schaumann, H. 1983. Epilepsy presenting as infantile autism? Two case studies.
Neuropediatrics 14:206–212.
Goldberg, E.A. 2004. The link between gastroenterology and autism. Gastroenterol. Nurs.
27:16–19.
Goldberg, W.A., Osann, K., Filipek, P.A., Laulhere, T., Jarvis, K., Modahl, C., Flodman, P.,
and Spence, M.A. 2003. Language and other regression: Assessment and timing.
J. Autism Dev. Disord. 33:607–616.
Grandjean, P. and Landrigan, P.J. 2006. Developmental neurotoxicity of industrial chemicals.
Lancet 368:2167–2178.
Grandjean, P., Murata, K., Budtz-Jorgensen, E., and Weihe, P. 2004. Cardiac autonomic activity
in methylmercury neurotoxicity: 14-year follow-up of a Faroese birth cohort. J. Pediatr.
144:169–176.
Green, L., Fein, D., Modahl, C., Feinstein, C., Waterhouse, L., and Morris, M. 2001. Oxytocin
and autistic disorder: Alterations in peptide forms. Biol. Psychiatry 50:609–613.
Gross, P.M., Wall, K.M., Pang, J.J., Shaver, S.W., and Wainman, D.S. 1990. Microvascular
specializations promoting rapid interstitial solute dispersion in nucleus tractus solitarius.
Am. J. Physiol. 259:1131–1138.
Gupta, Y.K., Gupta, M., and Kohli, K. 2003. Neuroprotective role of melatonin in oxidative
stress vulnerable brain. Indian J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 47:373–386.
Halbach, S., Vogt, S., Kohler, W., Felgenhauer, N., Weizl, G., Kremers, L., Silker, T., and
Melchart, D. 2008. Blood and urine mercury levels in adult amalgam patients of a
randomized controlled trial: Interaction of Hg species in erythrocytes. Environ. Res.
107:69–78.
Hansen, R.L., Ozonoff, S., Krakowiak, P., Angkustsiri, K., Jones, C., Deprey, L.J., Le, D.N.,
Croen, L.A., and Hertz-Picciotto, I. 2008. Regression in autism: Prevalence and associ-
ated factors in the CHARGE Study. Ambul. Pediatr. 8:25–31.
Hashimoto, T., Tayama, M., Miyazaki, M., Murakawa, K., Shimakawa, S., Yoneda, Y., and
Kuroda, Y. 1993. Brainstem involvement in high functioning autistic children. Acta
Neurol. Scand. 88:123–128.
Havarinasab, X., Bjorn, E., Nielsen, J.B., and Hultman, P. 2007. Mercury species in lym-
phoid and non-lymphoid tissues after exposure to methyl mercury: Correlation with
autoimmune parameters during and after treatment in susceptible mice. Toxicol. Appl.
Pharmacol. 221:21–28.
Hermann, G.E. and Rogers, R.C. 2008. TNFalpha: A trigger of autonomic dysfunction.
Neuroscientist 14:53–67.
Hollander, E., Novotny, S., Hanratty, M., Yaffe, R., DeCaria, C.M., Aronowitz, B.R., and
Mosovich, S. 2003. Oxytocin infusion reduces repetitive behaviors in adults with
autistic and Asperger’s disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 28:193–198.
Horvath, K., Papadimitriou, J.C., Rabsztyn, A., Drachenberg, C., and Tildon, J.T. 1999.
Gastrointestinal abnormalities in children with autistic disorder. J. Pediatr. 135:559–563.
Horvath, K., Stefanatos, G., Sokolski, K.N., Wachtel, R., Nabors, L., and Tildon, J.T. 1998.
Improved social and language skills after secretin administration in patients with autism
spectrum disorders. J. Assoc. Acad. Minor. Phys. 9:9–15.
Hrdina, P.D., Peters, D.A., and Singhai, R.L. 1976. Effects of chronic exposure to cadmium, lead
and mercury of brain biogenic amines in rat. Res. Commun. Chem. Pathol. Pharmacol.
15:483–493.
172 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Insel, T.R. 1997. A neurobiological basis of social attachment. Am. J. Psychiatry 154:726–735.
Insel, T.R. and Hulihan, T.J. 1995. A gender-specific mechanism for pair bonding: Oxytocin
and partner preference. Behav. Neurosci. 109:782–789.
Insel, T.R., O’Brien, D.J., and Leckman, J.F. 1999. Oxytocin, vasopressin, and autism: Is there
a connection? Biol. Psychiatry 45:145–157.
Institute of Medicine (2004). Immunization safety review committee: Vaccines and Autism.
Washington, DC. http:/www.iom.edu/report.asp?id = 20155
Johnson, A.K. and Edwards, G.L. (1991). Central projections of osmotic and hypervolaemic
signals in homeostatic thirst. In Thirst—Physiological and Psychological Aspects, eds.
D.J. Ramsay and D.A. Booth. New York: Springer-Verlag, pp. 149–175.
Johnson, A.K. and Gross, P.M. 1993. Sensory circumventricular organs and brain homeostatic
pathways. FASEB J. 7:678–686.
Jou, R.J., Minshew, N.J., Melhem, N.M., Keshavan, M.S., and Hardan, A.Y. 2008. Brainstem
volumetric alterations in children with autism. Psychol. Med. 24:1–8.
Jyonouchi, H., Sun, S., and Le, H. 2001. Proinflammatory and regulatory cytokine production
associated with innate and adaptive immune responses in children with autism spectrum
disorders and developmental regression. J. Neuroimmunol. 120:170–179.
Kanner, L. 1943. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child. 2:217–250.
Karcsu, S., Jancso, G., Kreutzberg, G.W., Toth, L., Kiraly, E., Bacsy, E., and Laszlo, F.A.
1985. A glutamate-sensitive neuronal system originating from the area postrema
terminates in and transports acetylcholinesterase to the nucleus of the solitary tract.
J. Neurocytol. 14:563–578.
Kemper, T.L. and Bauman, M.L. 1993. The contribution of neuropathological studies to the
understanding of autism. Neurol. Clin. 11:175–187.
Kern, J.K. and Jones, A.M. 2006. Evidence of toxicity, oxidative stress, and neuronal insult in
autism. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health 9:485–499.
Kim, S.J., Jeong, H.J., Myung, N.Y., Kim, M.C., Lee, J.H., So, H.S., Park, R.K.,Kim, H.J.,
Um, J.Y., and Hong, S.H. 2008. The protective mechanism of antioxidants in cadmium-
induced ototoxicity in vitro and in vivo. Environ. Health Perspect. 116:854–862.
Klin, A. 1993. Auditory brainstem responses in autism: Brainstem dysfunction or peripheral
hearing loss? J. Autism Dev. Disord. 23:15–35.
Kotler, M., Rodriguez, C., Sainz, R.M., Antolin, I., and Mendendez-Pelaez, A. 1998. Melatonin
increases gene expression for antioxidant enzymes in rat brain cortex. J. Pineal Res.
24:83–89.
Kulesza, R.J. and Mangunay, K. 2008. Morphological features of the medial superior olive in
autism. Brain Res. 1200:132–137.
Kulman, G., Lissoni, P., Rovelli, F., Roselli, M.G., Brivio, F., and Sequeri, P. 2000. Evidence of
pineal endocrine hypofunction in autistic children. Neurol. Endocrinol. Lett. 21:31–34.
Kwon, S., Kim, J., Choe, B.H., Ko, C., and Park, S. 2007. Electrophysiological assessment
of central auditory processing by auditory brainstem responses in children with autism
spectrum disorders. J. Korean Med. Sci. 22:656–659.
Larsen, P.J., Moller, M., and Mikkelsen, J.D. 1991. Efferent projections from the periven-
tricular and medial parvicellular subnuclei of the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus
to circumventricular organs of the rat: A Phaseolus vulgaris-leucoagglutinin (PHA-L)
tracing study. J. Comp. Neurol. 306:462–479.
Lathe, R. 2008. Environmental factors and limbic vulnerability in childhood autism. Am.
J. Biochem. Biotechnol. (Special Issue on Autism Spectrum Disorders) 4:183–197.
Lawrence, C.B., Allan, S.M., and Rothwell, N.J. (1998). Interleukin-1beta and the interleukin-1
receptor antagonist act in the striatum to modify excitotoxic brain damage in the rat. Eur.
J. Neurosci. 10:1188–1195.
Lopez-Hurtado, E. and Prieto, J.J. 2008. A microscopic study of language-related cortex in autism.
Am. J. Biochem. Biotechnol. (Special Issue on Autism Spectrum Disorders) 4:121–129.
Neurotoxic Brainstem Impairment as Proposed Threshold Event 173
Lord, C., Schulman, C., and DiLavore, P. 2004. Regression and word loss in autistic spectrum
disorders. J Child Psychol. Psychiatry 45:936–955.
Madsen, K.M. and Vestergaard, M. 2004. MMR vaccination and autism: What is the evidence
for a causal association? Drug Safety 27:831–840.
Mangano, E.N. and Hayley, S. (2009). Inflammatory priming of the substantia nigra influences
the impact of later paraquat exposure: Neuroimmune sensitization of neurodegeneration.
Neurobiol. Aging 30:1361–1378.
Marvel, F.A., Chen, C.C., Badr, N., Gaykema, R.P., and Goehler, L.E. 2004. Reversible inacti-
vation of the dorsal vagal complex blocks lipopolysaccharide-induced social withdrawal
and c-Fos expression in central autonomic nuclei. Brain Behav. Immun. 18:123–134.
McCoy, M.K., Martinez, T.N., Ruhn, K.A., Szymkowski, D.E., Smith, C.G., Botterman, B.R.,
Tansey, K.E., and Tansey, M.G. 2006. Blocking soluble tumor necrosis factor signaling
with dominant-negative tumor necrosis factor inhibitor attenuates loss of dopaminergic
neurons in models of Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurosci. 26:9365–9375.
McGinnis, W.R. 2001. Mercury and autistic gut disease. Environ Health Perspect. 109:A303–304.
McGinnis, W.R. 2004. Oxidative stress in autism. Altern. Ther. Health Med. 10:22–36.
McGinnis, W.R. 2007. Could oxidative stress from psychosocial stress affect neurodevel-
opment in autism? J Autism Dev. Disord. 37:993–994.
Meister, B., Ceccatelli, S., Hokfelt, T., Anden, N.E., Anden, M., and Theodorsson, E. 1989.
Neurotransmitters, neuropeptides and binding sites in the rat mediobasal hypothalamus:
Effects of monosodium glutamate (MSG) lesions. Neurochem. Int. 76:343–368.
Mendez-Armenta, M., Villeda-Hernandez, J., Barroso-Moguel, R., Nava-Ruiz, C.,
Jimenez-Capdeville, M.E., and Rios, C. 2003. Brain regional lipid peroxidation and
metallothionein levels of developing rats exposed to cadmium and dexamethasone.
Toxicol. Lett. 144:151–157.
Ming, X., Brimacombe, M., Malek, J.H., Jani, N., and Wagner, G.C. 2008. Autism spectrum
disorders and identified toxic land fills: Co-occurrence across states. Environ. Health
Insights 2:55–59.
Ming, X., Julu, P.O.O., Brimacombe, M., Connor, S., and Daniels, M.L. 2005. Reduced
cardiac parasympathetic activity in children with autism. Brain Dev. 27:509–516.
Modahl, C., Green, L., Fein, D., Morris, M., Waterhouse, L., Feinstein, C., and Levin, H. 1998.
Plasma oxytocin levels in autistic children. Biol. Psychiatry 43:270–277.
Moller-Madsen, B. and Danscher, G. 1986. Localization of mercury in CNS of the rat.
I. Mercuric chloride (HgCl2) per os. Environ. Res. 1:29–43.
Moosmann, B. and Behl, C. 2002. Secretory peptide hormones are biochemical antioxidants:
Structure–activity relationship. Mol. Pharmacol. 61:260–268.
Murphy, J.V., Wheless, J.W., and Schmoll, C.M. 2000. Left vagal nerve stimulation in six
patients with hypothalamic hamartomas. Pediatr. Neurol. 23:167–168.
Nadeau, S. and Rivest, S. 1999. Regulation of the gene encoding tumor necrosis factor alpha
(TNF-alpha) in the rat brain and pituitary in response in different models of systemic
immune challenge. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 58:61–77.
Naylor, J.L., Widdowson, P.S., Simpson, M.G., Farnworth, M., Ellis, M.K., and Lock, E.A.
1995. Further evidence that the blood/brain barrier impedes paraquat entry into the
brain. Hum. Exp. Toxicol. 14:587–594.
Nelson, K.B. and Bauman, M.L. 2003. Thimerosal and autism? Pediatrics 111:674–679.
Nemeroff, C.B., Mayberg, H.S., Krahl, S.E., McNamara, J., Frazer, A., Henry, T.R.,
George, M.S., Charney, D.S., and Brannan, S.K. 2006. VNS therapy in treatment-
resistant depression: Clinical evidence and putative neurobiological mechanisms.
Neuropsychopharmacology 31:1345–1355.
Nir, I., Meir, D., Zilber, N., Knowlber, H., Hadjez, J., and Lerner, Y. 1995. Brief report:
Circadian melatonin, thyroid-stimulating hormone, prolactin, and cortisol levels in
serum of young adults with autism. J Autism Dev. Disord. 25:641–654.
174 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Nordberg, G.F. and Sereniu, F. 1969. Distribution of inorganic mercury in the guinea pig brain.
Acta Pharmacol. Toxicol. 27:269–283.
Onyango, I.G. 2008. Mitochondrial dysfunction and oxidative stress in Parkinson’s disease.
Neurochem. Res. 33:589–597.
Opitz, H., Schweinsberg, F., Grossmann, T., Wendt-Gallitelli, M.F., and Meyermann, R. 1996.
Demonstration of mercury in the human brain and other organs 17 years after metallic
mercury exposure. Clin. Neuropathol. 15:139–144.
Ornitz, E.M. 1983. The functional neuroanatomy of infantile autism. Int. J. Neurosci. 19:85–124.
Palmeira, C.M., Moreno, A.J., and Madeira, V.M. 1995. Thiols metabolism is altered by the
herbicides paraquat, dinoseb and 2,4-D: A study in isolated hepatocytes. Toxicol. Lett.
81:115–123.
Palmer, R.F., Blanchard, S., and Wood, R. 2009. Proximity to point sources of environmental
mercury release as a predictor of autism prevalence. Health Place 15:18–24.
Paul, R., Augustyn, A., Klin, A., and Volkmar, F.R. 2005. Perception and production of prosody
by speakers with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 35:205–220.
Peele, D.B., Farmer, J.D., and MacPhail, R.C. 1988. Behavioral consequences of chelator
administration in acute cadmium toxicity. Fundam. Appl. Toxicol. 11:416–428.
Peng, J., Peng, L., Stevenson, F.F., Doctorow, W.R., and Anderson, J.K. 2007. Iron and paraquat
as synergistic environmental risk factors in sporadic Parkinson’s disease accelerate
age-related neurodegeneration. J. Neurosci. 27:6914–6922.
Perry, E.K., Lee, M.L, Martin-Ruiz, C.M., Court, J.A., Volsen, S.G., Merrit, J., Folly, E.,
Iversen, P.E., Bauman, M.L., Perry, R.H., and Wenk, G.L. 2001. Cholinergic activity
in autism: Abnormalities in the cerebral cortex and basal fore brain. Am. J. Psychiatry
158:1058–1066.
Peruzzo, B., Pastor, F.E., Blazquez, J.L., Schobitz, K., Pelaez, B., Amat, P., and Rodriguez,
E.M. 2000. A second look at the barriers of the medial basal hypothalamus. Exp. Brain
Res. 132:10–26.
Poling, J.S. 2008. Vaccines and autism revisited. N. Engl. J. Med. 359:655.
Porges, W.W., Doussard-Roosevelt, J.A., Portales, A.L., and Greenspan, S.I. 1996. Infant
regulation of the vagal “brake” predicts child behavior problems: A psychobiological
model of social behavior. Dev. Psychobiol. 8:697–712.
Rascher, K. and Mestres, P. 1980. Reaction of the hypothalamic ventricular lining following
systemic administration of MSG. Scan Electron Microsc. 3:457–464.
Reiter, R.J., Guerrero, J.M., Garcia, J.J., and Acuna-Castroviejo, D. 1998. Reactive oxygen
intermediate, molecular damage, and aging. Relation to melatonin. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci.
854:41–424.
Reiter, R.J., Tan, D.X., Mayo, J.C., Sainz, R.M., and Lopez-Burillo, S. 2002. Melatonin,
longevity and health in the aged: An assessment. Free Radic. Res. 36:1323–1329.
Rimland, B. 1964. Infantile Autism, New York: Meredith Publishing Company.
Rimland, B. and McGinnis, W. 2002. Vaccines and autism. Lab. Med. 33:708–716.
Roda, E., Coccini, T., Acerbi, D., Castoldi, A., Bernocchi, G., and Manzo, L. 2008. Cerebellum
cholinergic muscarinic receptor (subtype-2 and -3) and cytoarchitecture after devel-
opmental exposure to methylmercury: An immunohistological study in rat. J. Chem.
Neuroanat. 35:285–294.
Rodier, P.M., Ingram, J.L., Tisdale, B., and Croog, V.J. 1997. Linking etiologies in humans and
animal models: Studies of autism. Reprod. Toxicol. 11:417–422.
Saeed, R.W., Varma, S., Nemeroff, T.P., Sherry, B., Balakhaneh, D., Huston, J., Tracey, K.J.,
Al-Abed, Y., and Metz, C.N. 2005. Cholinergic stimulation blocks endothe-
lial cell activation and leukocyte recruitment during inflammation. J. Exp. Med.
201:1113–1123.
Sajdel-Sulkowska, E.M., Lipinksi, B., Windom, H., Audhya, T., and McGinnis, W. 2008.
Oxidative stress in autism: Elevated cerebellar 3-nitrotyrosine levels. Am. J. Biochem.
Biotechnol. (Special Issue on Autism Spectrum Disorders) 4:73–84.
Neurotoxic Brainstem Impairment as Proposed Threshold Event 175
Santos, F.W., Zeni, G., Rocha, J.B., Weis, S.N., Fachinetto, J.M., Favero, A.M., and Noguiera,
C.W. 2005. Diphenyl diselenide reverses cadmium-induced oxidative damage on mice
tissues. Chem. Biol. Interact. 151:159–165.
Shaffer, M.J., Jackson, C.E., Szabo, C.A., and Simpson, C.B. 2005. Vagal nerve stimulation:
Clinical and electrophysiological effects on vocal fold function. Ann. Otol. Rhinol.
Laryngol. 114:7–14.
Singh, P., Mann, K.A., Mangat, H.K., and Kaur, G. 2003. Prolonged glutamate excitotoxicity:
Effects on mitochondrial antioxidants and antioxidant enzymes. Mol. Cell. Biochem.
243:139–145.
Souza, V., Escobar Md Mdel, C., Gomez-Quiroz, L., Bucio, L., Hernandez, E., Cossio, E.C.,
and Gutierrez-Ruiz, M.C. 2004. Acute cadmium exposure enhances AP-1 DNA binding
and induces cytokines expression and heat shock protein in HepG2 cells. Toxicology
197:213–228.
Spellman, S.J. and Warner, H.A. 1977. Parkinsonism and the gut. Prim. Care 4:447–462.
Suh, J.H., Walsh, W.J., McGinnis, W.R., Lewis, A., and Ames, B.N. 2008. Altered sulfur amino
acid metabolism in immune cells of children diagnosed with autism. Am. J. Biochem.
Biotechnol. (Special Issue on Autism Spectrum Disorders) 4:105–113.
Tanguay, P.E. and Edwards, R.M. 1982. Electrophysiological studies of autism: The whisper
of the bang. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 12:177–184.
Terai, K., Munesue, T., and Hiratani, M. 1999. Excessive water drinking behavior in autism.
Brain Dev. 21:103–106.
Thunberg, G., Ahlsen, E., and Sandberg, A.D. 2007. Children with autistic spectrum disorders
and speech-generating devices: Communication in different activities at home. Clin.
Linguist Phonology 21:457–479.
Tordjman, S., Anderson, G.M., Pichard, N., Charbuy, H., and Touitou, Y. 2005. Noctural excre-
tion of 6-sulphatoxymelatonin in children and adolescents with autistic disorder. Biol.
Psychiatry 57:134–138.
Torrente, F., Ashwood, R.D., Day, R., Machado, N., Furlano, R.I., Anthony, A., Davies, S.E.,
Wakefield, A.J., Thomson, M.A., Walker-Smith, J.A., and Murch, S.H. 2002. Small
intestinal enteropathy with epithelial IgG and complement deposition in children with
regressive autism. Mol. Psychiatry 7:375–382.
Tuchman, R.F. and Rapin, I. 1997. Regression in pervasive developmental disorders: Seizures
and epileptiform electroencephalogram correlates. Pediatrics 99:560–566.
Urena-Guerrero, M.E., Lopez-Perez, S.J., and Beas-Zarate, C. 2003. Neonatal monosodium
glutamate treatment modifies glutamic acid decarboxylase activity during rat brain
postnatal development. Neurochem. Int. 42:269–276.
Vahter, M., Mottet, N.K., Friberg, L., Lind, B., Shen, D.D., and Burbacher, T. 1994. Speciation
of mercury in the primate blood and brain following long-term exposure to methyl
mercury. Toxicol. Appl. Pharmacol. 124:221–229.
Vargas, D.L., Nascimbene, C., Krishnan, C., Zimmerman, A.W., and Pardo, C.A. 2005.
Neuroglial activation and neuroinflammation in the brain of patients with autism. Ann.
Neurol. 57:67–81.
Vitkovic, L., Konsman, J.P., Bockaert, J., Dantzer, R., Homburger, V., and Jacque, C. 2000.
Cytokine signals propagate through the brain. Mol. Pscyhiatry 5:604–615.
Wakefield, A.J., Murch, S.H., Anthony, A., Linnell, J., Casson, D.M., Malik, M., Berelowitz,
W., Dhillon, A.P., Thomson, M.A., Harvey, P., Valentine, A., Davies, S.E., and
Walker-Smith, J.A. 1998. Illeal-lymphoid-nodular hyperplasia, non-specific colitis,
and pervasive developmental disorder in children. Lancet 351:637–641.
Wakefield, A.J., Thomson, M.A., Walker-Smith, J.A., and Murch, S.H. 2002. Small intestinal
enteropathy with epithelial IgG and complement deposition in children with regressive
autism. Mol. Psychiatry 7:375–382.
Walker, R. and Lupien, J.R. (2000). The safety evaluation of monosodium glutamate. J. Nutr.
130:1049S–10452S.
176 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Wang, Y., Fang, J., Leonard, S.S., and Rao, K.M. 2004. Cadmium inhibits the electron transfer
chain and induces reactive oxygen species. Free Radic. Biol. Med. 36:1434–1443.
Weidenheim, K.M., Goodman, L., Dickson, D.W., Gillberg, C., Rastam, M., and Rapin, I.
2001. Etiology and pathophysiology of autistic behavior: Clues from two cases with an
unusual variant of neuroaxonal dystrophy. J. Child Neurol. 16:809–819.
Weissman, J.R., Kelly, R.I., Bauman, M.L., Cohen, B.H., Murray, K.F., Mitchell, R.L., Kern,
R.L., and Natowicz, M.R. 2008. Mitochondrial disease in autism spectrum disorder
patients: A cohort analysis. Public Library of Science (PloS ONE) 3:1–6.
Werner, E. and Dawson, G. 2005. Validation of the phenomenon of autistic regression using
home videotapes. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62:889–895.
Windham, G.C., Zhang, L., Gunier, R., Croen, L.A., and Grether, J.K. 2006. Autism spectrum
disorders in relation to distribution of hazardous air pollutants in the San Francisco Bay
area. Environ. Health Perspect. 114:1438–1444.
Wislocki, G.B. and Putnam, T.J. 1920. Note on the anatomy of the areae postremae. Anat. Rec.
19:281–287.
Wu, X.F., Block, M.L., Zhang, W., Qin, L., Wilson, B., Zhang, W.Q., Veronesi, B., and Hong,
J.S. 2005. The role of microglia in paraquat-induced dopaminergic neurotoxicity.
Antioxid. Redox Signal. 7:654–661.
Wyllie, W.G. and Stern, R.O. 1931. Pink disease: Its morbid anatomy, with a note on treatment.
Arch. Dis. Child 6:137–156.
Yang, B., Goulet, M., Boismenu, R., and Ferguson, A.V. 2004. Secretin depolarizes nucleus
tractus solitarius neurons through activation of a nonselective cationic conductance.
Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Comp. Physiol. 286:927–934.
Yao, W., Walsh, W.J., McGinnis, W.R., and Pratico, D. 2006. Altered vascular phenotype in
autism: Correlation with oxidative stress. Arch. Neurol. 63:1161–1164.
Yun, A.J., Bazar, K.A., and Lee, P.Y. 2004. Pineal attrition, loss of cognitive plasticity, and
onset of puberty during the teen years: Is it a modern maladaptation exposed by
evolutionary displacement? Med. Hypotheses 63:939–950.
Zimmerman, A.W., Jyonouchi, H., Comi, A.M., Connors, S.L., Milstien, S., Varsou, A., and
Heyes, M.P. 2005. Cerebrospinal fluid and serum markers of inflammation in autism.
Pediatr. Neurol. 33:195–201.
10 Abnormalities in
Membrane Lipids,
Membrane-Associated
Proteins, and Signal
Transduction in Autism
Ved Chauhan* and Abha Chauhan
Department of Neurochemistry, New York State
Institute for Basic Research in Developmental
Disabilities, Staten Island, NY 10314, USA
CONTENTS
10.1 Introduction ................................................................................................. 178
10.2 Abnormalities in Membrane Amino-Glycerophospholipids
in Autism ..................................................................................................... 179
10.3 Membrane Fluidity and Role of Fatty Acids in Autism .............................. 179
10.4 Phospholipase A2 and Autism ..................................................................... 181
10.5 Polyphosphoinositides and Autism ............................................................. 182
10.6 Protein Kinases and Autism........................................................................ 183
10.7 Lipoprotein, Cholesterol, and Autism ......................................................... 183
10.8 Calcium and Calcium Ion Channels in Autism........................................... 184
10.9 Role of Membrane-Associated Proteins Such as Pten, Neuroligins,
SHANK3, Reelin, and Serotonin Receptors in Autism .............................. 185
10.9.1 Potential Role of Pten and PI3K/Akt Pathway in Autism .............. 188
10.9.2 Neuroligins and Autism ................................................................. 189
10.9.3 SHANK3 and Autism .................................................................... 189
10.9.4 Reelin and Autism.......................................................................... 190
10.9.5 Serotogenic Receptors and Autism ................................................ 190
10.9.6 Evidence of Altered Levels of Bcl2 and p53
(Markers of Apoptosis) in the Brains of Autism Subjects ............. 191
177
178 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Membrane lipids play an important role in the control of cellular functions. In this
chapter, we present evidence that autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are associated
with abnormalities in lipid metabolism, membrane-associated proteins, and signal
transduction. Altered levels of amino-glycerophospholipids (AGP) in the membrane,
increased peroxidation of lipids, and decreased membrane fluidity in autism suggest
that membrane signaling may be affected in autism. Increased phospholipase A2
(PLA2) activity in the blood and lymphoblasts of autistic subjects suggests that this
lipid-metabolizing enzyme may be affected in autism. If membrane lipids abnormal-
ities occur in autism, then lipid rafts, membrane domains with a strong affinity for
signaling molecules, may also be involved in the etiology of autism. An association
of phosphatidylinositol 3-kinase (PI3K) gene in autism; decreased activity of protein
kinase C (PKC); increased activity of protein kinase A (PKA) in the lymphoblasts
from autistic subjects; altered brain levels of Bcl2 and p53 involved in apoptosis;
inflammation and altered levels of cytokines; and mutational changes in the pro-
teins involved in cell signaling such as neuroligins, Pten, SHANK3, Wnt, reelin, and
voltage-dependent calcium channels suggest impairment in signal transduction in
autism. These abnormalities in the signal system may account for some of the struc-
tural changes and cognitive deficits in the brains of individuals with autism.
10.1 INTRODUCTION
Autism spectrum disorders (ASD), also known as pervasive developmental disor-
ders (PDDs), cause severe and pervasive impairment in language, cognition, and
socialization. These disorders include autism (severe form), pervasive develop-
mental disorder—not otherwise specified (PDD-NOS), and a much milder form,
Asperger syndrome. They also include two rare disorders: Rett syndrome and
childhood disintegrative disorder. Autism is a heterogeneous disorder, both etio-
logically and phenotypically. Within this autism spectrum, there are variations in
the severity of the disorder, the level of cognitive functioning, the presence or
absence of associated medical conditions such as seizures or other neurological
disorders, and whether or not there is a history of regression from apparent normal
development. Etiologically, autism is generally considered to be a polygenetic dis-
order resulting from interactions of alleles at several loci (Sutcliffe, 2008).
The genetic predisposition of autism may be obvious, but there is limited knowl-
edge of causative or secondary abnormalities in the biochemical pathways in autism.
The concept of membrane abnormalities as a cause of neurodevelopmental disorders
was suggested by Horrobin (1999). Phospholipids make up the bulk of all internal
and external neuronal membranes. Alteration in membrane lipids can result in defec-
tive membrane functions and therefore may have a wide impact on learning and
Membrane and Signal Transduction Abnormalities 179
0.35
0.30
0.20
0.15
0.10
0.05
0.00
Autism Siblings
FIGURE 10.1 Increased AGP content in the plasma of autism subjects as compared to
typically developing sibling controls. Total lipids extracted from the plasma samples of
14 children with autism (䊏) and their 14 nonautistic siblings (Δ) were labeled with trinitroben-
zene sulfonic acid (TNBS), and absorbance of the samples read at 410 nm. (Reproduced from
Chauhan, V. et al., Life Sci., 74, 1635, 2004b. With permission.)
and have lower melting points. We have observed that fluidity of the erythrocyte
membrane from children with autism is significantly lower than that of normal sib-
lings (Chauhan et al, 2005). These results suggest that membranes are more rigid
in autism. Biomembranes are generally in the fluid liquid-crystalline phase and
maintenance of optimal membrane fluidity is critical to biological functions. It has a
marked effect on membrane properties, modulating the activity of membrane-bound
enzymes and other membrane-associated molecules such as ion channels and recep-
tors (Houslay and Stanley, 1982). The activities of integral membrane proteins are
markedly affected by the physical state of the lipids in which they are embedded.
For example, fluid membranes have a greater number of insulin receptors and more
responsive receptors, resulting in heightened sensitivity to insulin (Neufeld and
Corbo, 1984). Rigid membrane adversely affects the function of tissues throughout
the body, including the brain. In autistic subjects with developmental regression,
Bu et al. (2006) observed increased levels of ω-9 fatty acids, i.e., eicosenoic acid
(20:1n9) and erucic acid (22:1n9) when compared with typically developing controls.
In addition, an increase in eicosadienoic acid (20:2n6) and a decrease in palmi-
etaidic acid (trans,16:1n7t) were observed in autistic children with clinical regression
Membrane and Signal Transduction Abnormalities 181
compared to those with early onset autism. The above evidence on decreased fluid-
ity of the erythrocyte membrane, and alterations in the fatty acid composition in
autism suggest that abnormalities in fatty acid metabolism may be involved in this
disorder.
The composition of fatty acids in the membrane phospholipids can be influenced
by the dietary intake of polyunsaturated fatty acids, the status of oxidative stress,
and/or the action of phospholipase A2 (PLA2), an enzyme that removes the sn-2 fatty
acids of phospholipids. It is known that dietary ω-3 fatty acids affect osmotic fragility
and membrane fluidity (Hagve, Lie, and Gronn, 1993). Such effects may be respon-
sible for the decreased blood viscosity and improved microcirculation observed after
feeding ω-3 fatty acids to animals (Young and Conquer, 2005).
Long-chain ω-3 fatty acids such as eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA) and docosa-
hexaenoic acid (DHA) have important structural roles as components of cellular
membranes. Both DHA and EPA are linked with many aspects of neural function
including neurotransmission, membrane fluidity, ion channel, enzyme regulation,
and gene expression (Young and Conquer, 2005). If unsaturated fatty acids are more
in our diet, the cell membrane becomes more fluid. DHA is the most unsaturated of
all fatty acids, and it helps in fluid cell membranes. The incorporation of EPA and
DHA into membrane phospholipids increases after ingestion of large amounts of ω-3
fatty acids.
Epidemiological studies suggest that dietary consumption of EPA and DHA,
commonly found in fish or fish oil, may decrease the risk for certain neuropsychiatric
disorders. Decreased blood levels of ω-3 fatty acids have been reported in several
neuropsychiatric conditions, including attention deficit (hyperactivity) disorder,
Alzheimer’s disease, schizophrenia, and depression (Young and Conquer, 2005).
Supplementation studies, using individual or a combination of ω-3 fatty acids, sug-
gest that certain symptoms associated with some of these disorders may improve
with ω-3 fatty acid supplementation (Young and Conquer, 2005). Recently, Amminger
et al. (2007) reported an advantage of dietary supplementation of ω-3 fatty acids for
the hyperactivity and stereotypy of individuals with behavioral disorders.
assessed between primary autism and ApoE alleles in 223 complete trios, from 119
simplex Italian families, and 44 simplex/29 multiplex Caucasian-American families.
Statistically significant disequilibrium favored the transmission of epsilon2 alleles
to autistic offspring, over epsilon3 and epsilon4 (Persico et al., 2004). However, the
study by Raiford et al. (2004) did not find an association between apoE and autism.
In blood, lipids are transported by lipoproteins. Corbett et al. (2007) reported
a potential role for dyslipidemia in the pathogenesis of some forms of ASD. Using
two-dimensional electrophoresis and analyzing 6348 peptide components of serum,
apolipoprotein (apo) B-100 had an effect size >0.99, with a p < 0.05 and a Mascot
identification score of 30 or greater, for autism compared to controls. In addition, apo
B-100 and apo A-IV were higher in children with high-functioning autism compared
to low-functioning autism.
Some alterations of lipids in autism may also be linked to secondary abnormalities.
For example, Smith-Lemli-Opitz syndrome (SLOS), a genetic condition of impaired
cholesterol biosynthesis, is associated with autism (Tierney et al., 2001). It has been
suggested that in addition to SLOS, there may be other disorders of sterol metabolism
or homeostasis associated with ASD (Tierney et al., 2006). However, the incidence
of SLOS and other sterol disorders among individuals with ASD is not known.
Cellular
stress
PI
PI 4,5 - P2 PI3K PKB/Akt Dab-1
3, 4, 5 – P3
Ca2+
Pten mTOR
p53
GSK-3β
Bcl2
Tau
FIGURE 10.2 (See color insert following page 200.) Participation of growth factor
receptors, reelin, voltage-gated calcium channels, p53, and Bcl2 in neuronal migration,
proliferation, and apoptosis. Growth factor binds to its receptor and activated phosphati-
dylinositol 3-kinase (PI3K), which converts phosphatidylinositol 4,5-bisphosphate (PI 4,
5-P2) to phopsphatidylinositol 3, 4, 5-trisphosphate (PI 3, 4, 5-P3). PI 3, 4, 5-P3 is an activa-
tor of protein kinase B (also known as Akt) that triggers proliferation pathways by activating
the mammalian target of rapamycin (mTOR). Akt also activates phosphatase and tensin
homolog on chromosome 10 (Pten), which dephosphorylates PI 3, 4, 5-P 3 to PI 4, 5-P2. Pten
activates glycogen synthase kinase-3β (GSK-3β), which phosphorylates tau protein involved
in neuronal migration. Reelin binds to very low-density lipoprotein receptor (VLDLR) and
apolipoprotein E receptor 2 (ApoER2), which activate Disabled-1 protein (Dab-1). Dab-1
activates the Akt involved in neuronal migration and proliferation. Cellular stress, on the
other hand, affects the activities of p53 and Bcl2, proteins involved in apoptosis.
which converts PI 4,5-P2 (PIP2) to PI 3,4,5-P3 (PIP3). PIP3 is an activator of PKB (also
known as Akt) that triggers proliferation of the cell. Akt also activates phosphatase
and tensin homolog on chromosome 10 (Pten), which dephosphorylates PIP3 to PIP2 and
may initiate apoptotic signaling. Pten activates glycogen synthase kinase-3β (GSK-3β),
which phosphorylates tau protein, a cytoskeletal protein. Phosphorylation of tau protein
is important for neuronal migration. Reelin binds to VLDL receptor (VLDLR) and
apolipoprotein E receptor 2 (apoER2), and it activates Disabled-1 protein (Dab1) that
also activates PKB. Calcium channels are important for controlled and fast requirement
of intracellular calcium, which is vital for the biological activities of numerous proteins.
Cellular stress initiates pathways leading to apoptosis by decreasing the levels of p53 (a
proapoptotic protein) and decreasing the levels of Bcl2 (an antiapoptotic protein).
In Figure 10.3, the role of neuroligins (Nlgn), neurexins (Nrxns), as well as SH3
and multiple ankyrin repeat domains 3 (SHANK3) proteins in cellular signaling are
Membrane and Signal Transduction Abnormalities 187
Presynaptic vesicle
Neurexin
Ca2+ Ca2+
channel channel
Neuroligin
SHANK3/ProSAP
PSD-95 PSD-95
Foderin Cortactin
Actin filament
Postsynaptic vesicle
FIGURE 10.3 (See color insert following page 200.) Interaction of neurexin and
neuroligins. Neurexins participate in synaptic transmission by affecting calcium channels.
They also interact with neuroligins of postsynaptic vesicles. Neuroligin controls the actin
assembly by interacting with postsynaptic density protein 95 (PSD-95), which further inter-
acts with SHANK3. SHANK3/PSD-95 interact with foderin and cortactin, which bind
to actin filament.
Serotonin
Serotonin reuptake
transporter Synaptic gap
Serotonin receptors
FIGURE 10.4 (See color insert following page 200.) Serotonin and its receptors, and
serotonin reuptake transporter. Serotonin molecules from the presynaptic vesicles are released
into the synaptic gap, where they are internalized by serotonin receptors into the postsynaptic
vesicles. Serotonin molecules are also retaken from synaptic gaps to the presynaptic vesicle
by the serotonin reuptake transporters. An imbalance of the uptake and reuptake system in
serotonin causes brain dysfunctions.
with TSC mutations exhibiting central nervous system disorders, including autism
(Wiznitzer, 2004). The PI3K/Akt pathway has also been linked to fear conditioning
in rats (Lin et al., 2001). Bcl2, an antiapoptotic protein, is one of the downstream tar-
gets modulated by Akt. Activated Akt stimulates changes in Bcl2 expression, result-
ing in antiapoptotic effects in hippocampal neurons (Matsuzaki et al., 1999).
in vivo, and a role for SHANK1 in specific cognitive processes, a feature that may be
relevant to ASD in humans. Considering the involvement of SHANK3 in dendritic
spine formation and its binding to Nlgn, it is possible that SHANK3 may play an
important role in the etiology of autism.
and metabolism. Serotonin has broad activities in the brain, and genetic varia-
tions in serotonin receptors and the serotonin transporter, which facilitates
reuptake of serotonin into presynapses, have been implicated in neurological dis-
eases. Increased platelet serotonin levels have been consistently found in a cohort
of autistic subjects. Suggested mechanisms for hyperserotonemia in autism are
increased synthesis of serotonin by tryptophan hydroxylase, increased uptake
into platelets through 5HT transporter (5HTt), diminished release from platelets
through 5HT2A receptor (5HT2Ar), and decreased metabolism by monoamine
oxidase (MAOA) (Hranilovic et al., 2008). Recent studies suggest that sero-
tonin transporter (SERT)–binding capacity measured by single-photon emission
computed tomography (SPECT) is disturbed in autism (Makkonen et al., 2008;
McDougle, 2008).
The serotogenic system is also affected in prenatal viral infection, which also
has an association with neurodevelopmental disorders such as schizophrenia and
autism. Winter et al. (2008) reported disruption of serotonergic system after prenatal
viral infection that may be attributed to potential modeling disruptions occurring
in patients with schizophrenia and autism. Hranilovic et al. (2007) have reported a
significant negative relationship between platelets serotonin levels and speech devel-
opment, indicating the relationship between the peripheral 5HT concentrations and
verbal abilities in autistic subjects. However, no association of polymorphic variants
of the serotonin transporter (5-HTT) gene-linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR)
with ASD was observed (Zhong et al., 1999). Further research is warranted to con-
firm the role of serotonin in the etiology of autism.
Other studies also indicated increased levels of other lipid peroxidation and
protein oxidation markers in autism, thus confirming increased oxidative stress in
autism. Zoroglu et al. (2004) reported increased TBA-reactive substances in the
erythrocytes of autism subjects as compared to normal controls. Ming et al. (2005)
reported increased excretion of 8-isoprostane-F2alpha in the urine of children with
autism. Isoprostanes are produced from the free radical oxidation of arachidonic
acid through nonenzymatic oxidation of cell membrane lipids. Evans et al. (2008)
reported increased levels of lipid-derived oxidative protein modification, i.e., car-
boxyethyl pyrrole and iso[4]levuglandin E2–protein adducts, in the autistic brain,
primarily in the white matter. Sajdel-Sulkowska et al. (2008) reported increased
levels of 3-nitrotyrosine (a specific marker for oxidative damage of protein) in the
cerebella of autistic subjects. Lipofuscin, a matrix of oxidized lipid and cross-linked
protein, forms as a result of oxidative injury in the tissues. Density of lipofuscin was
observed to be greater in cortical brain areas concerned with social behavior and
communication in autism (Lopez-Hurtado and Prieto, 2008).
Several studies have suggested alterations in the enzymes that play a vital role
in the defense mechanism against damage by ROS in autism. In autism, decreased
activity of glutathione peroxidase (GPx) in plasma (Yorbik et al., 2002) and in eryth-
rocytes (Pasca et al., 2006; Yorbik et al., 2002), reduced levels of total glutathione
and lower redox ratio of reduced glutathione (GSH) to oxidized glutathione (GSSG)
in plasma (James et al., 2004), decreased catalase activity in red blood cells (Zoroglu
et al., 2004), and decreased superoxide dismutase (SOD) activity in plasma, all point
toward oxidative stress. On the contrary, Sogut et al. (2003) reported unchanged
plasma SOD activity and increased GPx activity in autism.
Ceruloplasmin (a copper-transporting protein) and transferrin (an iron-transporting
protein) are major antioxidant proteins that are synthesized in several tissues includ-
ing brain (Arnaud, Gianazza, and Miribel, 1988; Loeffler et al., 1995). Ceruloplasmin
inhibits the peroxidation of membrane lipids catalyzed by metal ions such as Fe and
Cu (Gutteridge, Richmond, and Halliwell, 1979). It also acts as ferroxidase and SOD,
and it protects polyunsaturated fatty acids in red blood cell membranes from active
oxygen radicals (Arnaud, Gianazza, and Miribel, 1988). We reported reduced levels
of serum ceruloplasmin and transferrin in children with autism as compared to their
unaffected siblings (Chauhan et al., 2004a). The transferrin levels were observed to
be lower in 16 of 19 (84%) children with autism as compared to their unaffected sib-
lings, while ceruloplasmin levels were lower in 13 of 19 (68%) children with autism
as compared to their developmentally normal siblings (Chauhan et al., 2004a). It
was of particular interest to observe that the levels of ceruloplasmin and transferrin
were reduced more markedly in children with autism who had lost acquired lan-
guage skills (Chauhan et al., 2004a). Children who had not lost language skills had
levels similar to that seen in the typically developing siblings. These results suggest
that there is an altered regulation of transferrin and ceruloplasmin in children with
regressive autism. Such alterations may lead to abnormal iron and copper metabo-
lism in autism, which may have a pathological role in autism. Some preliminary
studies have suggested altered serum Cu/Zn ratios in autism (McGinnis, 2004).
We recently reported that there might be a link between decreased levels of PE
in the membrane and copper-mediated oxidation of lipids (Chauhan, Sheikh, and
Chauhan, 2008). We studied the effect of copper on the oxidation of liposomes
194 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
composed of mouse brain lipids, and also on the lymphoblasts from autism and
control subjects. Among the various metal cations (copper, iron, calcium, cadmium,
and zinc), only copper was found to oxidize PE, while having no effect on other
phospholipids. The action of copper on PE oxidation was time-dependent. Copper
oxidized PE in a concentration-dependent manner. No difference was observed
between copper-mediated oxidation of diacyl-PE and alkenyl-PE (plasmalogen).
Copper-mediated oxidation of PE in autistic lymphoblasts was higher than in con-
trol lymphoblasts. Taken together, our studies suggest that ceruloplasmin and cop-
per may be involved in the oxidative stress, and in reducing the levels of membrane
PE in autism.
10.12 CONCLUSIONS
In this chapter, we have gathered evidence on how membrane lipids and proteins
may be involved in the development of autism. Genetic and environmental factors
are the most important factors in the etiology of autism. If we look into the down
196 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
stream target of these factors, i.e., membrane and proteins function, it becomes
apparent that membrane is the intermediate link between genetic/environmental
factors and the functions of proteins. Other signaling molecules that are directly or
indirectly connected to lipids such as reelin, SHANK3, Wnt, neuroligins, Bcl2 and
Pten can also contribute significantly to the etiology of autism.
REFERENCES
Aamodt, S. M. and M. Constantine-Paton (1999). The role of neural activity in synaptic
development and its implications for adult brain function. Adv. Neurol. 79:133–144.
Abate, C., L. Patel, F. J. Rauscher III, and T. Curran (1990). Redox regulation of fos and jun
DNA-binding activity in vitro. Science 249:1157–1161.
Abrahams, B. S. and D. H. Geschwind (2008). Advances in autism genetics: On the threshold
of a new neurobiology. Nat. Rev. Genet. 9:341–355.
Allen, J. A., R. A. Halverson-Tamboli, and M. M. Rasenick (2007). Lipid raft microdomains
and neurotransmitter signaling. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8:128–140.
Amminger, G. P., G. E. Berger, M. R. Schafer, C. Klier, M. H. Friedrich, and M. Feucht
(2007). Omega-3 fatty acids supplementation in children with autism: a double-blind
randomized, placebo-controlled pilot study. Biol. Psychiatry 61:551–553.
Araghi-Niknam, M. and S. H. Fatemi (2003). Levels of Bcl-2 and P53 are altered in
superior frontal and cerebellar cortices of autistic subjects. Cell Mol. Neurobiol.
23:945–952.
Araki, N., T. Morimasa, T. Sakai, H. Tokuoh, S. Yunoue, M. Kamo, K. Miyazaki, K. Abe,
H. Saya, and A. Tsugita (2000). Comparative analysis of brain proteins from p53-deficient
mice by two-dimensional electrophoresis. Electrophoresis 21:1880–1889.
Arnaud, P., E. Gianazza, and L. Miribel (1988). Ceruloplasmin. Methods Enzymol. 163:441–452.
Ashwood, P. and A. J. Wakefield (2006). Immune activation of peripheral blood and mucosal
CD3 + lymphocyte cytokine profiles in children with autism and gastrointestinal
symptoms. J. Neuroimmunol. 173:126–134.
Barnby, G., A. Abbott, N. Sykes, A. Morris, D. E. Weeks, R. Mott, J. Lamb, A. J. Bailey,
and A. P. Monaco (2005). Candidate-gene screening and association analysis at the
autism-susceptibility locus on chromosome 16p: Evidence of association at GRIN2A
and ABAT. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 76:950–966.
Barrett, S., J. C. Beck, R. Bernier, E. Bisson, T. A. Braun, T. L. Casavant, D. Childress,
S. E. Folstein, M. Garcia, M. B. Gardiner, S. Gilman, J. L. Haines, K. Hopkins, R. Landa,
N. H. Meyer, J. A. Mullane, D. Y. Nishimura, P. Palmer, J. Piven, J. Purdy, S. L. Santangelo,
C. Searby, V. Sheffield, J. Singleton, and S. Slager et al. (1999). An autosomal genomic
screen for autism. Collaborative linkage study of autism. Am. J. Med. Genet. 88:609–615.
Bauman, M. L. and T. L. Kemper (2005). Neuroanatomic observations of the brain in autism:
A review and future directions. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 23:183–187.
Bell, J. G., E. E. Mackinlay, J. R. Dick, D. J. MacDonald, R. M. Boyle, and A. C. Glen (2004).
Essential fatty acids and phospholipase A2 in autistic spectrum disorders. Prostaglandins
Leukot. Essent. Fatty Acids 71:201–204.
Bu, B., P. Ashwood, D. Harvey, I. B. King, J. V. Water, and L. W. Jin (2006). Fatty acid
compositions of red blood cell phospholipids in children with autism. Prostaglandins
Leukot. Essent. Fatty Acids 74:215–221.
Bucki, R., P. A. Janmey, R. Vegners, F. Giraud, and J. C. Sulpice (2001). Involvement of
phosphatidylinositol 4,5-bisphosphate in phosphatidylserine exposure in platelets: Use
of a permeant phosphoinositide-binding peptide. Biochemistry 40:15752–15761.
Burger, K., G. Gimpl, and F. Fahrenholz (2000). Regulation of receptor function by cholesterol.
Cell Mol. Life Sci. 57:1577–1592.
Membrane and Signal Transduction Abnormalities 197
Horton, A. A. and S. Fairhurst (1987). Lipid peroxidation and mechanisms of toxicity. Crit.
Rev. Toxicol. 18:27–79.
Houslay, M. D. and K. K. Stanley (1982). Dynamics of Biological Membranes: Influence on
Synthesis, Structure and Function. Chichester and New York: Wiley.
Hranilovic, D., Z. Bujas-Petkovic, R. Vragovic, T. Vuk, K. Hock, and B. Jernej (2007).
Hyperserotonemia in adults with autistic disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37:1934–1940.
Hranilovic, D., R. Novak, M. Babic, M. Novokmet, Z. Bujas-Petkovic, and B. Jernej (2008).
Hyperserotonemia in autism: The potential role of 5HT-related gene variants. Coll.
Antropol. 32, Suppl 1:75–80.
Hung, A. Y., K. Futai, C. Sala, J. G. Valtschanoff, J. Ryu, M. A. Woodworth, F. L. Kidd,
C. C. Sung, T. Miyakawa, M. F. Bear, R. J. Weinberg, and M. Sheng (2008). Smaller
dendritic spines, weaker synaptic transmission, but enhanced spatial learning in mice
lacking Shank1. J. Neurosci. 28:1697–1708.
Ikonomov, O. C., D. Sbrissa, T. Yoshimori, T. L. Cover, and A. Shisheva (2002). PIKfyve
Kinase and SKD1 AAA ATPase define distinct endocytic compartments. Only PIKfyve
expression inhibits the cell-vacoulating activity of Helicobacter pylori VacA toxin.
J. Biol. Chem. 277:46785–46790.
International Molecular Genetic Study of Autism Consortium (1998). A full genome screen
for autism with evidence for linkage to a region on chromosome 7q. Hum. Mol. Genet.
7:571–578.
Jain, S. K. (1984). The accumulation of malonyldialdehyde, a product of fatty acid peroxida-
tion, can disturb aminophospholipid organization in the membrane bilayer of human
erythrocytes. J. Biol. Chem. 259:3391–3394.
Jamain, S., C. Betancur, H. Quach, A. Philippe, M. Fellous, B. Giros, C. Gillberg, M. Leboyer,
and T. Bourgeron (2002). Linkage and association of the glutamate receptor 6 gene with
autism. Mol. Psychiatry 7:302–310.
Jamain, S., H. Quach, C. Betancur, M. Rastam, C. Colineaux, I. C. Gillberg, H. Soderstrom,
B. Giros, M. Leboyer, C. Gillberg, and T. Bourgeron (2003). Mutations of the X-linked
genes encoding neuroligins NLGN3 and NLGN4 are associated with autism. Nat.
Genet. 34:27–29.
James, S. J., P. Cutler, S. Melnyk, S. Jernigan, L. Janak, D. W. Gaylor, and J. A. Neubrander
(2004). Metabolic biomarkers of increased oxidative stress and impaired methylation
capacity in children with autism. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 80:1611–1617.
Jarskog, L. F., J. H. Gilmore, E. S. Selinger, and J. A. Lieberman (2000). Cortical bcl-2 protein
expression and apoptotic regulation in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 48:641–650.
Jeffries, A. R., S. Curran, F. Elmslie, A. Sharma, S. Wenger, M. Hummel, and J. Powell (2005).
Molecular and phenotypic characterization of ring chromosome 22. Am. J. Med. Genet.
A 137:139–147.
Jyonouchi, H., L. Geng, A. Ruby, and B. Zimmerman-Bier (2005). Dysregulated innate
immune responses in young children with autism spectrum disorders: Their relationship
to gastrointestinal symptoms and dietary intervention. Neuropsychobiology 51:77–85.
Kharbanda, S., R. Ren, P. Pandey, T. D. Shafman, S. M. Feller, R. R. Weichselbaum, and
D. W. Kufe (1995). Activation of the c-Abl tyrosine kinase in the stress response to
DNA-damaging agents. Nature 376:785–788.
Kim, J., S. Ghosh, H. Liu, M. Tateyama, R. S. Kass, and G. S. Pitt (2004). Calmodulin
mediates Ca2+ sensitivity of sodium channels. J. Biol. Chem. 279:45004–45012.
Kisseleva, M. V., L. Cao, and P. W. Majerus (2002). Phosphoinositide-specific inositol
polyphosphate 5-phosphatase IV inhibits Akt/protein kinase B phosphorylation and
leads to apoptotic cell death. J. Biol. Chem. 277:6266–6272.
Komuro, H. and P. Rakic (1998). Orchestration of neuronal migration by activity of ion channels,
neurotransmitter receptors, and intracellular Ca2+ fluctuations. J. Neurobiol. 37:110–130.
Membrane and Signal Transduction Abnormalities 201
Korvatska, E., Water J. Van de, T. F. Anders, and M. E. Gershwin (2002). Genetic and
immunologic considerations in autism. Neurobiol. Dis. 9:107–125.
Krebs, M. O., C. Betancur, S. Leroy, M. C. Bourdel, C. Gillberg, and M. Leboyer (2002).
Absence of association between a polymorphic GGC repeat in the 5′ untranslated region
of the reelin gene and autism. Mol. Psychiatry 7:801–804.
Kwon, C. H., B. W. Luikart, C. M. Powell, J. Zhou, S. A. Matheny, W. Zhang, Y. Li, S. J. Baker,
and L. F. Parada (2006). Pten regulates neuronal arborization and social interaction
in mice. Neuron 50:377–388.
Lamb, J. A., J. Moore, A. Bailey, and A. P. Monaco (2000). Autism: Recent molecular genetic
advances. Hum. Mol. Genet. 9:861–868.
Lander, H. M., A. J. Milbank, J. M. Tauras, D. P. Hajjar, B. L. Hempstead, G. D. Schwartz,
R. T. Kraemer, U. A. Mirza, B. T. Chait, S. C. Burk, and L. A. Quilliam (1996). Redox
regulation of cell signaling. Nature 381:380–381.
Laumonnier, F., S. Roger, P. Guerin, F. Molinari, R. M’rad, D. Cahard, A. Belhadj, M. Halayem,
A. M. Persico, M. Elia, V. Romano, S. Holbert, C. Andres, H. Chaabouni, L. Colleaux,
J. Constant, J. Y. Le Guennec, and S. Briault (2006). Association of a functional deficit
of the BKCa channel, a synaptic regulator of neuronal excitability, with autism and
mental retardation. Am. J. Psychiatry 163:1622–1629.
Lee, M. H. and R. M. Bell (1991). Mechanism of protein kinase C activation by phosphati-
dylinositol 4,5-bisphosphate. Biochemistry 30:1041–1049.
Li, X., Chauhan, A., A.M. Sheikh, S. Patil, V. Chauhan, X-M. Li, L. Ji, T. Brown, and M. Malik
(2009). Elevated immune response in the brain of autistic patients. J. Neuroimmunol.
207:111–116.
Lin, C. H., S. H. Yeh, C. H. Lin, K. T. Lu, T. H. Leu, W. C. Chang, and P. W. Gean (2001).
A role for the PI-3 kinase signaling pathway in fear conditioning and synaptic plasticity
in the amygdala. Neuron 31:841–851.
Lin, E. Y., A. Orlofsky, H. G. Wang, J. C. Reed, and M. B. Prystowsky (1996). A1, a Bcl-2
family member, prolongs cell survival and permits myeloid differentiation. Blood
87:983–992.
Lintas, C., R. Sacco, K. Garbett, K. Mirnics, R. Militerni, C. Bravaccio, P. Curatolo,
B. Manzi, C. Schneider, R. Melmed, M. Elia, T. Pascucci, S. Puglisi-Allegra, K. L.
Reichelt, and A. M. Persico (2008). Involvement of the PRKCB1 gene in autistic dis-
order: Significant genetic association and reduced neocortical gene expression. Mol.
Psychiatry. 13: 1–14.
Loeffler, D. A., J. R. Connor, P. L. Juneau, B. S. Snyder, L. Kanaley, A. J. DeMaggio, H. Nguyen,
C. M. Brickman, and P. A. LeWitt (1995). Transferrin and iron in normal, Alzheimer’s
disease, and Parkinson’s disease brain regions. J. Neurochem. 65:710–724.
Lopez-Hurtado, E. and Prieto J.J. (2008). A microscopic study of language-related cortex
in autism. Am. J. Biochem. Biotechnol. (Special Issue on Autism Spectrum Disorders)
4:130–145.
Luo, J., B. D. Manning, and L. C. Cantley (2003). Targeting the PI3K-Akt pathway in human
cancer: Rationale and promise. Cancer Cell 4:257–262.
MacDonell, L. E., F. K. Skinner, P. E. Ward, A. I. Glen, A. C. Glen, D. J. Macdonald, R. M.
Boyle, and D. F. Horrobin (2000). Increased levels of cytosolic phospholipase A2 in
dyslexics. Prostaglandins Leukot. Essent. Fatty Acids 63:37–39.
Maehama, T. and J. E. Dixon (1998). The tumor suppressor, PTEN/MMAC1, dephospho-
rylates the lipid second messenger, phosphatidylinositol 3,4,5-trisphosphate. J. Biol.
Chem. 273:13375–13378.
Makkonen, I., R. Riikonen, H. Kokki, M. M. Airaksinen, and J. T. Kuikka (2008). Serotonin
and dopamine transporter binding in children with autism determined by SPECT. Dev.
Med. Child Neurol. 50:593–597.
202 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Mao, Y., G. Song, Q. Cai, M. Liu, H. Luo, M. Shi, G. Ouyang, and S. Bao (2006). Hydrogen
peroxide-induced apoptosis in human gastric carcinoma MGC803 cells. Cell Biol. Int.
30:332–337.
Matsura, T., A. Togawa, M. Kai, T. Nishida, J. Nakada, Y. Ishibe, S. Kojo, Y. Yamamoto, and
K. Yamada (2005). The presence of oxidized phosphatidylserine on Fas-mediated
apoptotic cell surface. Biochim. Biophys. Acta 1736:181–188.
Matsuzaki, H., M. Tamatani, N. Mitsuda, K. Namikawa, H. Kiyama, S. Miyake, and
M. Tohyama (1999). Activation of Akt kinase inhibits apoptosis and changes in Bcl-2 and
Bax expression induced by nitric oxide in primary hippocampal neurons. J. Neurochem.
73:2037–2046.
McDougle, C. J. (2008). Serotonin and dopamine transporter binding in children with autism.
Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 50:565.
McGinnis, W. R. (2004). Oxidative stress in autism. Altern. Ther. Health Med. 10:22–36.
Menkes, D. B., M. M. Rasenick, M. A. Wheeler, and M. W. Bitensky (1983). Guanosine
triphosphate activation of brain adenylate cyclase: Enhancement by long-term antide-
pressant treatment. Science 219:65–67.
Ming, X., T. P. Stein, M. Brimacombe, W. G. Johnson, G. H. Lambert, and G. C. Wagner
(2005). Increased excretion of a lipid peroxidation biomarker in autism. Prostaglandins
Leukot. Essent. Fatty Acids 73:379–384.
Minshew, N. J., G. Goldstein, S. M. Dombrowski, K. Panchalingam, and J. W. Pettegrew
(1993). A preliminary 31P MRS study of autism: Evidence for undersynthesis and
increased degradation of brain membranes. Biol. Psychiatry 33:762–773.
Missler, M., W. Zhang, A. Rohlmann, G. Kattenstroth, R. E. Hammer, K. Gottmann, and
T. C. Sudhof (2003). Alpha-neurexins couple Ca2+ channels to synaptic vesicle exocytosis.
Nature 423:939–948.
Moody, W. J. and M. M. Bosma (2005). Ion channel development, spontaneous activ-
ity, and activity-dependent development in nerve and muscle cells. Physiol. Rev.
85:883–941.
Morris, D. W., D. Ivanov, L. Robinson, N. Williams, J. Stevenson, M. J. Owen, J. Williams,
and M. C. O’Donovan (2004). Association analysis of two candidate phospholipase
genes that map to the chromosome 15q15.1–15.3 region associated with reading
disability. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 129B:97–103.
Neubig, R. R. (1994). Membrane organization in G-protein mechanisms. FASEB J.
8:939–946.
Neufeld, N. D. and L. M. Corbo (1984). Membrane fluid properties of cord blood mononuclear
leucocytes: Association with increased insulin receptors. Pediatr. Res. 18:773–778.
Nicolau, D. V., Jr., K. Burrage, R. G. Parton, and J. F. Hancock (2006). Identifying optimal
lipid raft characteristics required to promote nanoscale protein–protein interactions on
the plasma membrane. Mol. Cell. Biol. 26:313–323.
Niu, S., A. Renfro, C. C. Quattrocchi, M. Sheldon, and G. D’Arcangelo (2004). Reelin
promotes hippocampal dendrite development through the VLDLR/ApoER2-Dab1
pathway. Neuron 41:71–84.
Niu, S., O. Yabut, and G. D’Arcangelo (2008). The Reelin signaling pathway promotes
dendritic spine development in hippocampal neurons. J. Neurosci. 28:10339–10348.
Pardo, C. A., D. L. Vargas, and A. W. Zimmerman (2005). Immunity, neuroglia and neuroin-
flammation in autism. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 17:485–495.
Parola, M., G. Bellomo, G. Robino, G. Barrera, and M. U. Dianzani (1999). 4-Hydroxynonenal
as a biological signal: Molecular basis and pathophysiological implications. Antioxid.
Redox. Signal. 1:255–284.
Pasca, S. P., B. Nemes, L. Vlase, C. E. Gagyi, E. Dronca, A. C. Miu, and M. Dronca (2006).
High levels of homocysteine and low serum paraoxonase 1 arylesterase activity in
children with autism. Life Sci. 78:2244–2248.
Membrane and Signal Transduction Abnormalities 203
Singh, V. K., R. P. Warren, J. D. Odell, and P. Cole (1991). Changes of soluble interleukin-2,
interleukin-2 receptor, T8 antigen, and interleukin-1 in the serum of autistic children.
Clin. Immunol. Immunopathol. 61:448–455.
Skaar, D. A., Y. Shao, J. L. Haines, J. E. Stenger, J. Jaworski, E. R. Martin, G. R. DeLong, J. H.
Moore, J. L. McCauley, J. S. Sutcliffe, A. E. Ashley-Koch, M. L. Cuccaro, S. E. Folstein,
J. R. Gilbert, and M. A. Pericak-Vance (2005). Analysis of the RELN gene as a genetic
risk factor for autism. Mol. Psychiatry 10:563–571.
Sogut, S., S. S. Zoroglu, H. Ozyurt, H. R. Yilmaz, F. Ozugurlu, E. Sivasli, O. Yetkin,
M. Yanik, H. Tutkun, H. A. Savas, M. Tarakcioglu, and O. Akyol (2003). Changes in nitric
oxide levels and antioxidant enzyme activities may have a role in the pathophysiological
mechanisms involved in autism. Clin. Chim. Acta 331:111–117.
Sooksawate, T. and M. A. Simmonds (2001). Effects of membrane cholesterol on the sensitivity
of the GABA(A) receptor to GABA in acutely dissociated rat hippocampal neurones.
Neuropharmacology 40:178–184.
Spitzer, N. C., C. M. Root, and L. N. Borodinsky (2004). Orchestrating neuronal differentiation:
Patterns of Ca2+ spikes specify transmitter choice. Trends Neurosci. 27:415–421.
Splawski, I., K. W. Timothy, N. Decher, P. Kumar, F. B. Sachse, A. H. Beggs, M. C. Sanguinetti,
and M. T. Keating (2005). Severe arrhythmia disorder caused by cardiac L-type calcium
channel mutations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102:8089–8096.
Splawski, I., K. W. Timothy, L. M. Sharpe, N. Decher, P. Kumar, R. Bloise, C. Napolitano,
P. J. Schwartz, R. M. Joseph, K. Condouris, H. Tager-Flusberg, S. G. Priori,
M. C. Sanguinetti, and M. T. Keating (2004). Ca(V)1.2 calcium channel dysfunction
causes a multisystem disorder including arrhythmia and autism. Cell 119:19–31.
Splawski, I., D. S. Yoo, S. C. Stotz, A. Cherry, D. E. Clapham, and M. T. Keating (2006).
CACNA1H mutations in autism spectrum disorders. J. Biol. Chem. 281:22085–22091.
Staal, F. J., M. Roederer, L. A. Herzenberg, and L. A. Herzenberg (1990). Intracellular thiols
regulate activation of nuclear factor kappa B and transcription of human immunodefi-
ciency virus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 87:9943–9947.
Stromme, P., M. E. Mangelsdorf, M. A. Shaw, K. M. Lower, S. M. Lewis, H. Bruyere,
V. Lutcherath, A. K. Gedeon, R. H. Wallace, I. E. Scheffer, G. Turner, M. Partington,
S. G. Frints, J. P. Fryns, G. R. Sutherland, J. C. Mulley, and J. Gecz (2002). Mutations
in the human ortholog of Aristaless cause X-linked mental retardation and epilepsy. Nat.
Genet. 30:441–445.
Sutcliffe, J. S. (2008). Genetics. Insights into the pathogenesis of autism. Science 321:208–209.
Szatmari, P., A. D. Paterson, L. Zwaigenbaum, W. Roberts, J. Brian, X. Q. Liu, J. B. Vincent,
J. L. Skaug, A. P. Thompson, L. Senman, L. Feuk, C. Qian, S. E. Bryson, M. B. Jones,
C. R. Marshall, S. W. Scherer, V. J. Vieland, C. Bartlett, L. V. Mangin, R. Goedken,
A. Segre, M. A. Pericak-Vance, M. L. Cuccaro, J. R. Gilbert, H. H. Wright, R. K. Abramson,
C. Betancur, T. Bourgeron, C. Gillberg, M. Leboyer, J. D. Buxbaum, K. L. Davis,
E. Hollander, J. M. Silverman, J. Hallmayer, L. Lotspeich, J. S. Sutcliffe, J. L. Haines,
S. E. Folstein, J. Piven, T. H. Wassink, V. Sheffield, D. H. Geschwind, M. Bucan, and
W. T. Brown et al. (2007). Mapping autism risk loci using genetic linkage and chromo-
somal rearrangements. Nat. Genet. 39:319–328.
Talebizadeh, Z., D. Y. Lam, M. F. Theodoro, D. C. Bittel, G. H. Lushington, and M. G. Butler
(2006). Novel splice isoforms for NLGN3 and NLGN4 with possible implications in
autism. J. Med. Genet. 43(5):e21.
Tappel, A. L. (1973). Lipid peroxidation damage to cell components. Fed. Proc. 32:1870–1874.
Taylor, D. R. and N. M. Hooper (2006). The prion protein and lipid rafts. Mol. Membr. Biol.
23:89–99.
Tierney, E., I. Bukelis, R. E. Thompson, K. Ahmed, A. Aneja, L. Kratz, and R. I. Kelley
(2006). Abnormalities of cholesterol metabolism in autism spectrum disorders. Am.
J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 141B:666–668.
Membrane and Signal Transduction Abnormalities 205
CONTENTS
11.1 Introduction .................................................................................................208
11.2 Mitochondrial DNA Mutations in Autism ..................................................209
11.3 Functional Mitochondrial Defects in Autism ............................................. 210
11.4 Mitochondria are Central Participants in Calcium Homeostasis................ 211
11.5 Timothy Syndrome Causes Autism via a Defect in Calcium
Channel Function ........................................................................................ 212
11.6 Other Defects in the Calcium Signaling Pathway in Autism ...................... 214
11.7 Calcium Signaling Dysfunction Causes Defects in Neurosecretion ........... 215
11.8 Family of Autism-Related Diseases with Defective Neuronal
Calcium Signaling ....................................................................................... 216
11.9 Pharmacogenetic Calcium Signaling Abnormalities .................................. 218
11.10 Conclusion ................................................................................................... 218
Acknowledgment ................................................................................................... 219
References .............................................................................................................. 219
There are several suggestions in the literature that oxidative stress and mitochondrial
function are abnormal in autism. However, these defects produce such global
perturbations of cellular homeostasis that it is difficult to discern the critical
pathway leading to the disease phenotype. Resolving this pathway is important
207
208 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
since it provides the most promising target for novel drug development. Since
most reactive oxygen species (ROS) arise as by-products of electron transport
and oxidative phosphorylation, primary mitochondrial defects are a likely source
of ROS. Certainly, genetic defects in mitochondrial function do impose a mas-
sive oxidative stress. In the same fashion in which there have been many hints of
abnormal ROS levels in autism, many disparate clues have recently come to sug-
gest that abnormal neuronal calcium signaling also plays a role in autism. This is
a process recently recognized to be under mitochondrial regulation and capable
of disrupting neuronal synaptic function and hence behavior. Therefore, a body
of evidence now suggests that calcium signaling abnormalities are a fundamental
pathway perturbed in autism, with many lesions arising from primary defects in
mitochondrial function, but with other lesions primarily perturbing other com-
ponents of the calcium signaling pathway and only secondarily impairing mito-
chondrial function.
11.1 INTRODUCTION
The autistic spectrum disorders (ASD) all share the same characteristic core
deficits in social interaction, communication, and behavior; however, they remain
a group of developmental disorders that are only behaviorally, not yet pathophysi-
ologically, defined. The high heritability of ASD is assurance that genes and the
biochemical pathways they subserve underlie the phenotype, but to date these
pathways remain elusive. There are several suggestions in the literature that oxi-
dative stress and mitochondrial dysfunction are abnormal in autism, likely in
turn impacting a variety of downstream processes (Chauhan and Chauhan, 2006;
Gargus and Imtiaz, 2008; James et al., 2006). But since these impacts on cellular
homeostasis are so broad, it remains difficult to discern those pathways most
directly connected to the critical phenotypes of autism, and therefore those that
would appear the most promising targets for novel drug development. Oxidative
stress produces oxidative damage in a cell, tissue, or organ caused by the reac-
tive oxygen species (ROS), such as free radicals and peroxide, interacting with
an array of critical endogenous biomolecules. Is this ROS damage etiologically
important in the neurological phenotype, or does it merely serve the role of a use-
ful correlative biomarker? Since most ROS arise as by-products from essential
metabolic reactions, with electron transport and oxidative phosphorylation of the
mitochondria accounting for the vast majority, primary mitochondrial defects are
a likely source. Certainly genetic defects in mitochondrial function do impose
a huge endogenous oxidative stress (Esposito et al., 1999; Subramaniam et al.,
2008). But while they cause an increase in ROS, they also impose a dramatic
deficiency in the cell’s essential ATP energy supply and in the other essential cel-
lular processes subserved by the mitochondria, such as maintenance of the mem-
brane potential and calcium homeostasis. Just as there have been many hints of
abnormal ROS levels in autism, many genetic and biophysical clues have recently
come to suggest that abnormal neuronal calcium signaling may also play a role
in autism (Gargus, 2009). While calcium homeostasis and signaling are intensely
studied biophysical phenomena that are well recognized to play a central role
Mitochondrial Component of Calcium Signaling Abnormality in Autism 209
in many aspects of excitable cell biology and synaptic physiology (Flavell and
Greenberg, 2008), their role in disease processes is only beginning to emerge
(Bezprozvanny and Gargus, 2008). Likewise the coordinating role mitochon-
dria play in these processes has only recently been recognized (Szabadkai and
Duchen, 2008). In this chapter, evidence will be presented that calcium signaling
abnormalities are a critical and fundamental pathway perturbed in autism, with
many lesions arising from primary defects in mitochondrial function, but other
lesions perturbing this calcium signaling being independent of a direct impact on
the mitochondria.
whose name arises from the fact that it is the target of rapamycin (Chen et al., 2008;
Liu and Butow, 2006). While magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) has been
shown to detect elevated central nervous system (CNS) lactate in this disease (Yapici
et al., 2007), peripheral biochemical markers of mitochondrial energy deficiency
have yet to be reported. In addition, linkage and association studies have revealed
autism susceptibility loci at 1p, 2q, 3q, 5p, 7q, 9q, 11p, 15q, 16p, 17q, and Xq; how-
ever, none makes a major contribution to the common disease risk, each accounting
for <1%, and while many hypothetical candidates have been proposed, no specific
susceptibility genes in the loci have been proven (Abrahams and Geschwind, 2008).
Recently, linkage and association studies in nonsyndromic autism have focused
attention on mitochondrial dysfunction caused by variation in SLC25A12. This gene
on 2q24 encodes the brain-specific isoform of the mitochondrial calcium-regulated
aspartate/glutamate carrier. In a large study, nine candidate genes in this region
were scanned for autism-associated single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) and
two SNPs, located in introns 3 and 16 of SLC25A12, were found associated with the
disease (Ramoz et al., 2004). The same risk haplotype at these two SNPs was then
confirmed to be linked and associated with the disease in 197 families (Ramoz et al.,
2004). Subsequent studies confirmed that autism was associated with other SNPs
within the locus, although none appeared to be functional (Segurado et al., 2005).
More recently, a study of postmortem brain tissue from six patients with autism and
matched controls showed significantly increased transport activity by the product of
the SLC25A12 gene in autism. However, no mutations or polymorphisms were found
associated with the disease (Palmieri et al., 2008). Furthermore, all of the excess
enzyme activity found in brain samples from patients with autism was calcium-
dependent and was found to be associated with elevated cytosolic calcium levels in
tissue from subjects with autism (Palmieri et al., 2008). They found that controlling
for the calcium levels, transport activity was identical in isolated mitochondria from
patients and controls. They therefore concluded that the critical link to this altered
mitochondrial metabolism observed in the brains of patients with autism was in fact
caused by altered calcium homeostasis, although it was never directly studied.
syndrome suggests that excess current is conducted by the mutant channel. This is
supported by the finding that a loss-of-function allele at this locus causes the short
QT Brugada syndrome (Antzelevitch et al., 2007) and by pharmacology, since the
channel blocker verapamil is used to treat TS, and the channel opener Bay K 8644
can mimic the TS arrhythmia (Jacobs et al., 2006; Sicouri et al., 2007). The mutant
and normal wild-type (WT) versions of the channel have been expressed in vitro
and kinetic analysis has been applied to dissecting the molecular defect biophysi-
cally (Antzelevitch et al., 2007; Barrett and Tsein, 2008). It is clear that the major
effect of the TS mutation is to alter the speed with which the opened conducting
channel returns to a nonconducting conformation, a process called channel inacti-
vation. The channel inactivation arising from changes in the membrane potential
are slowed, as would be predicted from the cardiac findings, but a separate mecha-
nism of the inactivation regulated by the calcium signal itself is greatly accelerated.
The net result of the mutant is a very rapid inactivation of 50% of the current, and
then a very slow inactivation of the remainder (Barrett and Tsein, 2008). Therefore,
it remains to be determined exactly which aspect is key to neuronal dysfunction and
how downstream signaling is perturbed by this altered neuronal mechanism to pro-
duce the characteristic phenotype of autism. Nonetheless, these biophysical findings
greatly extend the pathophysiology of autism and begin to render it a neurobiologi-
cal rather than strictly behavioral phenotype. This brightens the prospect that new
molecular targets can be discovered against which new generations of drugs can be
developed in this disease.
serve as links to the release of ER calcium stores. Therefore, each syndrome involves
a sodium and calcium channel plus a link to release of the ER calcium stores. Finally,
this pathway seems to extend to the mitochondria since recent studies have directly
shown that for murine CACNA1A seizure alleles in the FHM1 locus, pathogenesis is
through a pathway that involves disruptions in calcium signaling leading to induced
mitochondrial dysfunction that is ultimately capable of causing apoptosis (Bawa and
Abbott, 2008).
There are no simple monogenic forms of BPD, however, an illustration of how
calcium signaling pathogenesis extends into behavioral neuropsychiatric syndromes
is revealed by the recent discovery that the same CACNA1C locus mutated in TS/
LQT8 was found to be the sole replicated significant association found in two large
genome-wide association studies (GWAS) of BPD (Sklar et al., 2008). The SNP
markers that were found to be associated with BPD were all in intron 3 of the gene,
but their effect has yet to be functionally characterized. Similarly, SNP variants in
BCL2, an autosomal gene encoding an antiapoptotic integral mitochondrial mem-
brane protein that controls calcium signaling and contributes to the modulation of
many cellular functions including gene expression and synaptic plasticity, was found
to be associated with BPD and additionally to cause functional defects in calcium
signaling in vitro and in mouse models (Du et al., 2008; Einat et al., 2005). The
BCL2 variant associated with increased risk for BPD decreased BCL2 protein levels,
increased baseline cytosolic calcium levels, and elevated IP3R agonist-stimulated
cytosolic calcium release and apoptosis.
Finally, calcium signaling abnormalities have been revealed in the monogenic
mitochondrial migraine syndromes, monogenic neurodegenerative diseases, such
as Huntington disease, and in multigenic neurodegenerative diseases such as
Alzheimer and Parkinson disease. Migraine is a common symptom of mitochondrial
encephalomyopathy (Finsterer, 2006) and these mitochondrial migraine syndromes
are the only migraine syndromes recognized to be caused by major-effect loci other
than FHM. These mitochondrial syndromes include disease caused by the classic
mtDNA mutations, such as the point mutations underlying mitochondrial encepha-
lomyopathy, lactic acidosis, and strokes (MELAS), or the more recently resolved
autosomal nDNA mutations, such as those causing cerebral autosomal dominant
arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoencephalopathy (CADASIL) or in
POLG, the autosomal gene encoding mitochondrial DNA polymerase γ (Finsterer,
2006; Gladstone and Dodick, 2005; Hudson and Chinnery, 2006; Porter et al., 2005).
Huntington disease is caused by dominant polyglutamine-expansion mutations in
HTT, encoding huntingtin. Evaluation of striatal GABAergic neurons from mouse
models expressing full-length human huntingtin revealed that the toxic polyglu-
tamine-expanded huntingtin increased mitochondrial depolarizations in response
to N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) receptor calcium signals, leading to apoptosis
(Fernandes et al., 2007; Zhang et al., 2008). An analysis of calcium signaling abnor-
malities in brain slices of Alzheimer disease transgenic mouse models highlight the
critical roles of calcium signaling in the neuronal pathophysiology, with some stud-
ies pointing to direct presenilin-induced ER calcium release (Nelson et al., 2007;
Tu et al., 2006), others to presenilin-linked disruptions in RyR calcium signaling
(Stutzmann et al., 2007), and still others to presenilin-linked disruptions in IP3R
218 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
calcium signaling (Cheung et al., 2008). The presenilin mutations are rare monogenic
causes of the disease, however, mitochondrial dysfunction secondary to oxidative
stress seems to be an important contributor to common forms of the disease as well
as to Parkinson disease (Yang et al., 2008). In the case of Parkinson disease, the
importance of the TS-related calcium channel CACNA1D in pacemaker activity of
the substantia nigra dopaminergic neurons that are vulnerable in this disease seems
critical. The demands the calcium signals driven by this pacemaker channel place
on mitochondria create the age-dependent vulnerability characteristic of the dis-
ease, and blockers of this channel spare the mitochondria and are neuroprotective
in a murine model (Surmeier, 2007).
11.10 CONCLUSION
Just as clues began to accumulate several years ago that mitochondrial function and
ROS production are abnormal in autism, many new disparate clues have come to
suggest that abnormal neuronal calcium signaling may serve as the scaffold that
unites these and newly recognized genetic lesions into a consensus pathophysiology
of autism with a more mechanistic neurobiological underpinning. This evidence now
suggests that calcium signaling abnormalities are a fundamental pathway per-
turbed in autism, with the multigenic disease architecture including primary defects
in mitochondrial function, but also other lesions perturbing this calcium signaling
pathway independent of a direct impact on the mitochondria. Aspects of this same
Mitochondrial Component of Calcium Signaling Abnormality in Autism 219
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
Supported in part by grants to J.J.G. from the National Institutes of Health, the Doris
Duke Charitable Foundation and National Alliance for Autism Research/Autism
Speaks. The author has no conflicting financial interests.
REFERENCES
Abrahams, B.S. and Geschwind, D.H. (2008). Advances in autism genetics: On the threshold
of a new neurobiology. Nat. Rev. Genet. 9: 341–355.
Antzelevitch, C., Pollevick, G.D., Cordeiro, J.M., Casis, O., Sanguinetti, M.C., Aizawa, Y.,
Guerchicoff, A., Pfeiffer, R., Oliva, A., Wollnik, B., Gelber, P., Bonaros, E.P. Jr,
Burashnikov, E., Wu, Y., Sargent, J.D., Schickel, S., Oberheiden, R., Bhatia, A., Hsu, L.F.,
Haïssaguerre, M., Schimpf, R., Borggrefe, M., and Wolpert, C. (2007). Loss-of-function
mutations in the cardiac calcium channel underlie a new clinical entity characterized
by ST-segment elevation, short QT intervals, and sudden cardiac death. Circulation 115:
442–449.
Arking, D.E., Cutler, D.J., Brune, C.W., Teslovich, T.M., West, K., Ikeda, M., Rea, A., Guy,
M., Lin, S., Cook, E.H., and Chakravarti, A. (2008). A common genetic variant in the
neurexin superfamily member CNTNAP2 increases familial risk of autism. Am. J. Hum.
Genet. 82: 160–164.
Arndt, T.L., Stodgell, C.J., and Rodier P.M. (2005). The teratology of autism. Int. J. Dev.
Neurosci. 23: 189–199.
Bannwarth, S., Procaccio, V., Rouzier, C., Fragaki, K., Poole, J., Chabrol, B., Desnuelle,
C., Pouget, J., Azulay, J.P., Attarian, S., Pellissier, J.F., Gargus, J.J., Abdenur, J.E.,
Mozaffar, T., Calvas, P., Labauge, P., Pages, M., Wallace, D.C., Lambert, J.C., and
Paquis-Flucklinger, V. (2008). Rapid identification of mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA)
mutations in neuromuscular disorders by using surveyor strategy. Mitochondrion 8:
136–145.
Barrett, C.F., Cao, Y.Q., and Tsien, R.W. (2005). Gating deficiency in a familial hemiplegic
migraine type 1 mutant P/Q-type calcium channel. J. Biol. Chem. 280: 24064–24071.
Barrett, C.F. and Tsien, R.W. (2008). The Timothy syndrome mutation differentially affects
voltage- and calcium-dependent inactivation of CaV1.2 L-type calcium channels. Proc.
Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105: 2157–2162.
Bawa, B. and Abbott, L.C. (2008). Analysis of calcium ion homeostasis and mitochondrial
function in cerebellar granule cells of adult CaV 2.1 calcium ion channel mutant mice.
Neurotox. Res. 13: 1–18.
Berridge, M.J., Lipp, P., and Bootman, M.D. (2000). The versatility and universality of calcium
signalling. Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 1: 11–21.
Bezprozvanny, I. and Gargus, J.J. (2008). Calcium signaling disease. J. Gen. Physiol. 132:
1a–32a.
Cao, Y.Q., Piedras-Rentería, E.S., Smith, G.B., Chen, G., Harata, N.C., and Tsien, R.W.
(2004). Presynaptic Ca2+ channels compete for channel type-preferring slots in altered
neurotransmission arising from Ca2+ channelopathy. Neuron 43: 387–400.
220 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Cao, Y.Q. and Tsien, R.W. (2005). Effects of familial hemiplegic migraine type 1 mutations
on neuronal P/Q-type Ca2+ channel activity and inhibitory synaptic transmission. Proc.
Natl. Acad. Sci U.S.A. 102: 2590–2595.
Chauhan, A. and Chauhan, V. (2006). Oxidative stress in autism. Pathophysiology 13:
171–181.
Chen, C., Liu, Y., Liu, R., Ikenoue, T., Guan, K.L., Liu, Y., and Zheng, P. (2008). TSC-mTOR
maintains quiescence and function of hematopoietic stem cells by repressing mito-
chondrial biogenesis and reactive oxygen species. J. Exp. Med. 205: 2397–2408.
Cheung, K.H., Shineman, D., Müller, M., Cárdenas, C., Mei, L., Yang, J., Tomita, T., Iwatsubo, T.,
Lee, V.M., and Foskett, J.K. (2008). Mechanism of Ca2+ disruption in Alzheimer’s
disease by presenilin regulation of InsP3 receptor channel gating. Neuron 58: 871–883.
Cook, E.H. Jr, Lindgren, V., Leventhal, B.L., Courchesne, R., Lincoln, A., Shulman, C., Lord, C.,
and Courchesne, E. (1997). Autism or atypical autism in maternally but not paternally
derived proximal 15q duplication. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 60: 928–934.
Danial, N.N., Gramm, C.F., Scorrano, L., Zhang, C.Y., Krauss, S., Ranger, A.M., Datta, S.R.,
Greenberg, M.E., Licklider, L.J., Lowell, B.B., Gygi, S.P., and Korsmeyer, S.J. (2003).
BAD and glucokinase reside in a mitochondrial complex that integrates glycolysis and
apoptosis. Nature 424: 952–956.
DeVivo, D., Bohan, T., Coulter, D., Dreifuss, F., Greenwood, R., Nordli, D. J., Shields, W.,
Stafstrom, C., and Tein, I. (1998). l-Carnitine supplementation in childhood epilepsy:
Current perspectives. Epilepsia 39: 1216–1225.
Dichgans, M., Freilinger, T., Eckstein, G., Babini, E., Lorenz-Depiereux, B., Biskup, S.,
Ferrari, M.D., Herzog, J., van den Maagdenberg, A.M., Pusch, M., and Strom, T.M.
(2005). Mutation in the neuronal voltage-gated sodium channel SCN1A in familial
hemiplegic migraine. Lancet 366: 371–377.
Dodick, D.W. and Gargus, J.J. (2008). Why migraines strike. Sci. Am. 299: 56–63.
Du, J., Marchado-Vierira, R., Pivovarova, N.B., Chen, G., Andrews, S.B., and Manji, H.K.
(2008). Calcium dysregulation in bipolar disorder: A critical role for Bcl-2 gene
polymorphisms in human subjects. J. Gen. Physiol. 132: 32a.
Durand, C.M., Betancur, C., Boeckers, T.M., Bockmann, J., Chaste, P., Fauchereau, F.,
Nygren, G., Rastam, M., Gillberg, I.C., Anckarsäter, H., Sponheim, E., Goubran-Botros,
H., Delorme, R., Chabane, N., Mouren-Simeoni, M.C., de Mas, P., Bieth, E., Rogé, B.,
Héron, D., Burglen, L., Gillberg, C., Leboyer, M., and Bourgeron, T. (2007). Mutations
in the gene encoding the synaptic scaffolding protein SHANK3 are associated with
autism spectrum disorders. Nat. Genet. 39: 25–27.
Eeg-Olofsson, O., al-Zuhair, A.G., Teebi, A.S., Daoud, A.S., Zaki, M., Besisso, M.S., and
Al-Essa, M.M. (1990). Rett syndrome: A mitochondrial disease? J. Child Neurol. 5:
210–214.
Einat, H., Yuan, P., and Manji, H.K. (2005). Increased anxiety-like behaviors and mitochon-
drial dysfunction in mice with targeted mutation of the Bcl-2 gene: Further support for
the involvement of mitochondrial function in anxiety disorders. Behav. Brain Res. 165:
172–180.
Esposito, L.A., Melov, S., Panov, A., Cottrell, B.A., and Wallace, D.C. (1999). Mitochondrial
disease in mouse results in increased oxidative stress. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96:
4820–4825.
Fernandes, H.B., Baimbridge, K.G., Church, J., Hayden, M.R., and Raymond, L.A. (2007).
Mitochondrial sensitivity and altered calcium handling underlie enhanced NMDA-
induced apoptosis in YAC128 model of Huntington’s disease. J. Neurosci. 27:
13614–13623.
Filipek, P.A., Juranek, J., Nguyen, M.T., Cummings, C., and Gargus, J.J. (2004). Relative
carnitine deficiency in autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34: 615–623.
Filipek, P.A., Juranek, J., Smith, M., Mays, L.Z., Ramos, E.R., Bocian, M., Masser-Frye,
D., Laulhere, T.M., Modahl, C., Spence, M.A., and Gargus, J.J. (2003). Mitochondrial dys-
function in autistic patients with 15q inverted duplication. Ann. Neurol. 53: 801–804.
Mitochondrial Component of Calcium Signaling Abnormality in Autism 221
Fillano, J.J., Goldenthal, M.J., Rhodes, C.H., and Marín-García, J. (2002). Mitochondrial
dysfunction in patients with hypotonia, epilepsy, autism and developmental delay:
HEADD syndrome. J. Child Neurol. 17: 435–439.
Finsterer, J. (2006). Central nervous system manifestations of mitochondrial disorders. Acta
Neurol. Scand. 114: 217–238.
Flavell, S.W. and Greenberg, M.E. (2008). Signaling mechanisms linking neuronal activity to
gene expression and plasticity of the nervous system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 31: 563–590.
Gargus, J.J. (2003). Unraveling monogenic channelopathies and their implications for complex
polygenic disease. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 72: 785–803.
Gargus, J.J. (2005). Receptor, transporter and ion channel diseases. In: Encyclopedia of
Molecular Cell Biology and Molecular Medicine. R.A. Meyers, eds. Wiley-VCH,
Weinheim, Vol. 11, pp. 637–711.
Gargus, J.J. (2006). Ion channel functional candidate genes in multigenic neuropsychiatric
disease. Biol. Psychiatry 60: 177–185.
Gargus, J.J. (2008a). Receptor, transporter and ion channel diseases. In: Neurobiology.
R.A. Meyers, eds. Wiley-VCH, Weinheim, Vol. 2, pp. 669–742.
Gargus, J.J. (2008b). Calcium signaling abnormalities in familial hemiplegic migraine, type 2
(FHM2), a window into autism and polygenic neuropsychiatric channelopathies. J. Gen.
Physiol. 132: 19a–20a.
Gargus, J.J. (2009). Genetic calcium signaling abnormalities in the CNS: Seizures, migraine
and autism. The Year in Human & Medical Genetics 2009. Annals of the New York
Academy of Sciences 1151: 133–156.
Gargus, J.J. and Imtiaz, F. (2008). Mitochondrial energy-deficient endophenotype in autism.
Am. J. Biochem. Biotechnol. (Special Issue on Autism Spectrum Disorders) 4: 198–207.
Gargus, J.J. and Shih, C.H. (2007). Monogenic migraine syndromes highlight novel drug targets.
Drug Dev. Res. 68: 432–440.
Gargus, J.J. and Tournay, A. (2007). Novel mutation confirms seizure locus SCN1A is also
FHM3 migraine locus. Ped. Neurol. 37: 407–410.
Gladstone, J.P. and Dodick, D.W. (2005). Migraine and cerebral white matter lesions: When to
suspect cerebral autosomal dominant arteriopathy with subcortical infarcts and leukoen-
cephalopathy (CADASIL). Neurologist 11: 19–29.
Graf, W.D., Marin-Garcia, J., Gao, H.G., Pizzo, S., Naviaux, R.K., Markusic, D., Barshop, B.A.,
Courchesne, E., and Haas, R.H. (2000). Autism associated with the mitochondrial DNA
G8363A transfer RNALys mutation. J. Child Neurol. 15: 357–361.
Hayashi, T. and Su, T.P. (2007). Sigma-1 receptor chaperones at the ER-mitochondrion
interface regulate Ca2+ signaling and cell survival. Cell 131: 596–610.
Hudson, G. and Chinnery, P.F. (2006). Mitochondrial DNA polymerase-gamma and human
disease. Hum. Mol. Genet. 15: R244–R252.
Jacobs, A., Knight, B.P., McDonald, K.T., and Burke, M.C. (2006). Verapamil decreases ven-
tricular tachyarrhythmias in a patient with Timothy syndrome (LQT8). Heart Rhythm
3: 967–970.
Jamain, S., Quach, H., Betancur, C., Råstam, M., Colineaux, C., Gillberg, I.C., Soderstrom,
H., Giros, B., Leboyer, M., Gillberg, C., and Bourgeron, T., and Paris Autism Research
International Sibpair Study (2003). Mutations of the X-linked genes encoding neuroli-
gins NLGN3 and NLGN4 are associated with autism. Nat. Genet. 34: 27–29.
James, S.J., Melnyk, S., Jernigan, S., Cleves, M.A., Halsted, C.H., Wong, D.H., Cutler, P.,
Bock, K., Boris, M., Bradstreet, J.J., Baker, S.M., and Gaylor, D.W. (2006). Metabolic
endophenotype and related genotypes are associated with oxidative stress in children
with autism. Am. J. Med. Genet. B. Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 141B: 947–956.
Kaja, S., van de Ven, R.C., Broos, L.A., Veldman, H., van Dijk, J.G., Verschuuren, J.J., Frants,
R.R., Ferrari, M.D., van den Maagdenberg, A.M., and Plomp J.J. (2005). Gene dosage-
dependent transmitter release changes at neuromuscular synapses of CACNA1A R192Q
knockin mice are non-progressive and do not lead to morphological changes or muscle
weakness. Neuroscience 135: 81–95.
222 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Kaplin, A.I., Ferris, C.D., Voglmaier, S.M., and Snyder, S.H. (1994). Purified reconstituted
inositol 1,4,5-trisphosphate receptors. Thiol reagents act directly on receptor protein.
J. Biol. Chem. 269: 28972–28978.
Kriaucionis, S., Paterson, A., Curtis, J., Guy, J., Macleod, N., and Bird, A. (2006). Gene expres-
sion analysis exposes mitochondrial abnormalities in a mouse model of Rett syndrome.
Mol. Cell Biol. 26: 5033–5042.
Liu, Z. and Butow, R.A. (2006). Mitochondrial retrograde signaling. Annu. Rev. Genet. 40:
159–185.
Ma, S. and Gargus, J.J. (2007). The genetics of neuronal channelopathies. In: Encyclopedia of
Life Sciences. Wiley, Chichester.
McElroy, S.L. (2004). Diagnosing and treating comorbid (complicated) bipolar disorder.
J. Clin. Psychiatry 65: Suppl 15: 35–44.
Moretti, P., Bouwknecht, J.A., Teague, R., Paylor, R., and Zoghbi, H.Y. (2005). Abnormalities
of social interactions and home-cage behavior in a mouse model of Rett syndrome.
Hum. Mol. Genet. 14: 205–220.
Morrow, E.M., Yoo, S.Y., Flavell, S.W., Kim, T.K., Lin, Y., Hill, R.S., Mukaddes, N.M., Balkhy, S.,
Gascon, G., Hashmi, A., Al-Saad, S., Ware, J., Joseph, R.M., Greenblatt, R., Gleason, D.,
Ertelt, J.A., Apse, K.A., Bodell, A., Partlow, J.N., Barry, B., Yao, H., Markianos, K.,
Ferland, R.J., Greenberg, M.E., and Walsh, C.A. (2008). Identifying autism loci and
genes by tracing recent shared ancestry. Science 321: 218–223.
Nelson, E.D., Kavalali, E.T., and Monteggia, L.M. (2006). MeCP2-dependent transcriptional
repression regulates excitatory neurotransmission. Curr. Biol. 16: 710–716.
Nelson, O., Tu, H., Lei, T., Bentahir, M., de Strooper, B., and Bezprozvanny, I. (2007). Familial
Alzheimer disease-linked mutations specifically disrupt Ca2+ leak function of presenilin
1. J. Clin. Invest. 117: 1230–1239.
Oliveira, G., Ataíde, A., Marques, C., Miguel, T.S., Coutinho, A.M., Mota-Vieira, L.,
Gonçalves, E., Lopes, N.M., Rodrigues, V., Carmona da Mota, H., and Vicente, A.M.
(2007). Epidemiology of autism spectrum disorder in Portugal: Prevalence, clinical
characterization, and medical conditions. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 49: 726–733.
Oliveira, G., Diogo, L., Grazina, M., Garcia, P., Ataíde, A., Marques, C., Miguel, T., Borges, L.,
Vicente, A.M., and Oliveira, C.R. (2005). Mitochondrial dysfunction in autism spectrum
disorders: A population-based study. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 47: 185–189.
Palmieri, L., Papaleo, V., Porcelli, V., Scarcia, P., Gaita, L., Sacco, R., Hager, J., Rousseau, F.,
Curatolo, P., Manzi, B., Militerni, R., Bravaccio, C., Trillo, S., Schneider, C., Melmed, R.,
Elia, M., Lenti, C., Saccani, M., Pascucci, T., Puglisi-Allegra, S., Reichelt, K.L.,
and Persico, A.M. (2008). Altered calcium homeostasis in autism-spectrum disorders:
evidence from biochemical and genetic studies of the mitochondrial aspartate/glutamate
carrier AGC1. Mol. Psychiatry 2008 Jul 8 [Epub ahead of print].
Patterson, R.L., van Rossum, D.B., Kaplin, A.I., Barrow, R.K., and Snyder, S.H. (2005).
Inositol 1,4,5-trisphosphate receptor/GAPDH complex augments Ca2+ release via
locally derived NADH. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102: 1357–1359.
Pellock, J.M. (2004). Understanding co-morbidities affecting children with epilepsy. Neurology
62 (5 Suppl. 2):S17–S23.
Plummer, N.W. and Meisler, M.H. (1999). Evolution and diversity of mammalian sodium
channel genes. Genomics 57: 323–331.
Pons, R., Andreu, A.L., Checcarelli, N., Vilà, M.R., Engelstad, K., Sue, C.M., Shungu, D.,
Haggerty, R., de Vivo, D.C., and DiMauro, S. (2004). Mitochondrial DNA abnormalities
and autistic spectrum disorders. J. Pediatr. 144: 81–85.
Porter, A., Gladstone, J.P., and Dodick, D.W. (2005). Migraine and white matter hyperintensities.
Curr. Pain Headache Rep. 9: 289–293.
Priori, S.G. and Napolitano, C. (2006). Role of genetic analyses in cardiology. Part I: Mendelian
diseases: Cardiac channelopathies. Circulation 113: 1130–1135.
Mitochondrial Component of Calcium Signaling Abnormality in Autism 223
Ramoz, N., Reichert, J.G., Smith, C.J., Silverman, J.M., Bespalova, I.N., Davis, K.L., and
Buxbaum, J.D. (2004). Linkage and association of the mitochondrial aspartate/glutamate
carrier SLC25A12 gene with autism. Am. J. Psychiatry 161: 662–669.
Saleh, S., Yeung, S.Y., Prestwich, S., Pucovsky, V., and Greenwood, I. (2005). Electrophys-
iological and molecular identification of voltage-gated sodium channels in murine
vascular myocytes. J. Physiol. 568(Pt. 1): 155–169.
Schanen, N.C. (2006). Epigenetics of autism spectrum disorders. Hum. Mol. Genet. 15: Spec
No 2: R138–R150.
Schneider, T. and Przewlocki, R. (2005). Behavioral alterations in rats prenatally exposed to
valproic acid: Animal model of autism. Neuropsychopharmacology 30: 80–89.
Segall, L., Scanzano, R., Kaunisto, M.A., Wessman, M., Palotie, A. Gargus, J.J., and Blostein,
R. (2004). Kinetic alterations due to a missense mutation in the Na,K-ATPase α2 subunit
cause familial hemiplegic migraine type 2. J. Biol. Chem. 279: 43692–43696.
Segall, L., Mezzetti, A., Scanzano, R., Gargus, J.J., Purisima, E., and Blostein, R. (2005).
Alterations in the α2 isoform of the Na,K-ATPase associated with familial hemiplegic
migraine type 2. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102: 11106–11111.
Segurado, R., Conroy, J., Meally, E., Fitzgerald, M., Gill, M., and Gallagher, L. (2005).
Confirmation of association between autism and the mitochondrial aspartate/
glutamate carrier SLC25A12 gene on chromosome 2q31. Am. J. Psychiatry 162:
2182–2184.
Sicouri, S., Timothy, K.W., Zygmunt, A.C., Glass, A., Goodrow, R.J., Belardinelli, L., and
Antzelevitch, C. (2007). Cellular basis for the electrocardiographic and arrhyth-
mic manifestations of Timothy syndrome: effects of ranolazine. Heart Rhythm 4:
638–647.
Sklar, P., Smoller, J.W., Fan, J., Ferreira, M.A., Perlis, R.H., Chambert, K., Nimgaonkar, V.L.,
McQueen, M.B., Faraone, S.V., Kirby, A., de Bakker, P.I., Ogdie, M.N., Thase, M.E.,
Sachs, G.S., Todd-Brown, K., Gabriel, S.B., Sougnez, C., Gates, C., Blumenstiel, B.,
Defelice, M., Ardlie, K.G., Franklin, J., Muir, W.J., McGhee, K.A., MacIntyre, D.J.,
McLean, A., VanBeck, M., McQuillin, A., Bass, N.J., Robinson, M., Lawrence, J.,
Anjorin, A., Curtis, D., Scolnick, E.M., Daly, M.J., Blackwood, D.H., Gurling, H.M.,
and Purcell, S.M. (2008). Whole-genome association study of bipolar disorder. Mol.
Psychiatry 13: 558–569.
Smith, I.F., Shih, C, Blostein, R, Parker, I., and Gargus, J.J. (2009). Role of Na/K pump
in regulation of IP3R calcium channel demonstrated by familial hemiplegic migraine
(FHM2) mutations. The Royal Society Meeting on Membrane Transport in Flux: The
Ambiguous Interface Between Channels and Pumps. London, May 19, 20, 2008. Phil.
Trans. Royal Soc. B. (in press).
Spät, A., Szanda, G., Csordás, G., and Hajnóczky, G. (2008). High- and low-calcium-dependent
mechanisms of mitochondrial calcium signalling. Cell Calcium 44: 51–63.
Splawski, I., Timothy, K.W., Decher, N., Kumar, P., Sachse, F.B., Beggs, A.H., Sanguinetti,
M.C., and Keating, M.T. (2005). Severe arrhythmia disorder caused by cardiac L-type
calcium channel mutations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102: 8089–8096.
Splawski, I., Timothy, K.W., Sharpe, L.M., Decher, N., Kumar, P., Bloise, R., Napolitano,
C., Schwartz, P.J., Joseph, R.M., Condouris, K., Tager-Flusberg, H., Priori, S.G.,
Sanguinetti, M.C., and Keating, M.T. (2004). Ca(V)1.2 calcium channel dysfunction
causes a multisystem disorder including arrhythmia and autism. Cell 119: 19–31.
Splawski, I., Yoo, D.S., Stotz, S.C., Cherry, A., Clapham, D.E., and Keating, M.T.
(2006). CACNA1H mutations in autism spectrum disorders. J. Biol. Chem. 281:
22085–22091.
Stutzmann, G.E., Smith, I., Caccamo, A., Oddo, S., Laferla, F.M., and Parker, I. (2006).
Enhanced ryanodine receptor recruitment contributes to Ca2+ disruptions in young, adult,
and aged Alzheimer’s disease mice. J. Neurosci. 26: 5180–5189.
224 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Stutzmann, G.E., Smith, I., Caccamo, A., Oddo, S., Parker, I., and Laferla, F. (2007). Enhanced
ryanodine-mediated calcium release in mutant PS1-expressing Alzheimer’s mouse
models. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1097: 265–277.
Subramaniam, V., Golik, P., Murdock, D.G., Levy, S., Kerstann, K.W., Coskun, P.E., Melkonian,
G.A., and Wallace, D.C. (2008). MITOCHIP assessment of differential gene expression
in the skeletal muscle of Ant1 knockout mice: Coordinate regulation of OXPHOS,
antioxidant, and apoptotic genes. Biochim. Biophys. Acta 1777: 666–675.
Südhof, T.C. (2008). Neuroligins and neurexins link synaptic function to cognitive disease.
Nature 455: 903–911.
Surmeier, D.J. (2007). Calcium, ageing, and neuronal vulnerability in Parkinson’s disease.
Lancet Neurol. 6: 933–938.
Szabadkai, G. and Duchen, M.R. (2008). Mitochondria: The hub of cellular Ca2+ signaling.
Physiology 23: 84–94.
Tabuchi, K., Blundell, J., Etherton, M.R., Hammer, R.E., Liu, X., Powell, C.M., and Südhof,
T.C. (2007). Neuroligin-3 mutation implicated in autism increases inhibitory synaptic
transmission in mice. Science 318: 71–76.
Tao, J. and Cao, Y.Q. (2008). Voltage-gated Ca2+ channels in trigeminal nociceptive processing.
J. Gen. Physiol. 132: 22a.
Tottene, A., Fellin, T., Pagnutti, S., Luvisetto, S., Striessnig, J., Fletcher, C., and Pietrobon,
D. (2002). Familial hemiplegic migraine mutations increase Ca(2+) influx through single
human CaV2.1 channels and decrease maximal CaV2.1 current density in neurons.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99: 13284–13289.
Tu, H., Nelson, O., Bezprozvanny, A., Wang, Z., Lee, S.F., Hao, Y.H., Serneels, L., De
Strooper, B., Yu, G., and Bezprozvanny, I. (2006). Presenilins form ER Ca2+ leak chan-
nels, a function disrupted by familial Alzheimer’s disease-linked mutations. Cell 126:
981–993.
Wagner, G.C., Reuhl, K.R., Cheh, M., McRae, P., and Halladay, A.K. (2006). A new neurobe-
havioral model of autism in mice: Pre- and postnatal exposure to sodium valproate.
J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36: 779–793.
Weiss, L.A., Escayg, A., Kearney, J.A., Trudeau, M., MacDonald, B.T., Mori, M., Reichert, J.,
Buxbaum, J.D., and Meisler, M.H. (2003). Sodium channels SCN1A, SCN2A and
SCN3A in familial autism. Mol. Psychiatry 8: 186–194.
Yan, J., Noltner, K., Feng, J., Li, W., Schroer, R., Skinner, C., Zeng, W., Schwartz, C.E.,
and Sommer, S.S. (2008). Neurexin 1alpha structural variants associated with autism.
Neurosci. Lett. 438: 368–370.
Yang, J.L., Weissman, L., Bohr, V.A., and Mattson, M.P. (2008). Mitochondrial DNA damage
and repair in neurodegenerative disorders. DNA Repair 7: 1110–1120.
Yapici, Z., Dörtcan, N., Baykan, B.B., Okan, F., Dinçer, A., Baykal, C., Eraksoy, M., and
Roach, S. (2007). Neurological aspects of tuberous sclerosis in relation to MRI/MR
spectroscopy findings in children with epilepsy. Neurol. Res. 29: 449–454.
Zhang, H., Li, Q., Graham, R.K., Slow, E., Hayden, M.R., and Bezprozvanny, I. (2008). Full
length mutant huntingtin is required for altered Ca2+ signaling and apoptosis of striatal
neurons in the YAC mouse model of Huntington’s disease. Neurobiol. Dis. 31: 80–88.
12 Inflammation and
Neuroimmunity in the
Pathogenesis of Autism:
Neural and Immune
Network Interactions
Carlos A. Pardo-Villamizar1,* and
Andrew W. Zimmerman2,3,4
1Division of Neuroimmunology and Neuroinfectious
Disorders, Department of Neurology and
Departments of 2Neurology and 3Psychiatry and
Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins University
School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21287, USA
4Department of Neurology and Developmental Medicine,
Kennedy Krieger Institute, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA
CONTENTS
12.1 Introduction ............................................................................................... 226
12.2 Concept of Inflammation .......................................................................... 227
12.2.1 Immunological Responses in the CNS....................................... 228
12.2.2 What Is Neuroinflammation? ..................................................... 228
12.3 How Do Immune Factors and Inflammation Influence ASD? .................. 229
12.3.1 Influence of the Immune System on Brain Development
and the Pathogenesis of ASD ..................................................... 229
12.3.2 Cytokines and Chemokines in Brain Development ................... 230
12.3.3 Maternal Environment and Effects of Immune Responses
on Neurodevelopment in the Pathogenesis of ASD ................... 231
12.3.4 Neuroglia and Neuroinflammatory Responses
during Adulthood ....................................................................... 233
12.3.5 Neuroglia, Neuroinflammation, and Synaptic Plasticity ........... 234
12.3.6 Chronic Neuroimmune Reactions in ASD ................................. 235
225
226 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
The role of the immune system and inflammation in the pathogenesis and
pathophysiology of autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) is controversial. It is very
clear now that the interaction of the immune system with the central nervous sys-
tem (CNS) is critical for normal neurological and behavioral functions. Recent
studies support the view that immune responses are involved in the modeling of
the CNS during prenatal and postnatal stages, and that neuroimmune activity may
disrupt normal neurodevelopment and contribute to the neuropathological abnor-
malities found in ASDs. This review focuses on the most recent research that links
immunological factors, inflammation, and neuroimmune responses with autism.
The findings include maternal autoantibodies against fetal neural epitopes, the
activation of neuroglia and neuroimmune pathways and abnormalities in systemic
immune responses in children with autism. These immunological factors influence
two important stages of the CNS function: early brain development and neuronal
organization, and later neuronal and synaptic physiology. A better understanding
of the role of immunity and neuroinflammation in the pathogenesis of autism may
have important clinical and therapeutic implications. Future studies should focus
on the actions of neuroimmune factors during brain development in the pathogen-
esis of autism.
12.1 INTRODUCTION
Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are neurodevelopmental disorders with neurobe-
havioral manifestations that involve communication, social interaction, and
behavioral abnormalities (Rapin and Tuchman, 2008). It is increasingly clear that
immunological factors and immune dysregulation are important in the pathogenesis
and persistence of neurobehavioral abnormalities in ASD (Ashwood, Wills, and Van
de, 2006; Pardo, Vargas, and Zimmerman, 2005). In addition, evidence from the
effects of maternal viral infections during pregnancy (Chess, Fernandez, and Korn,
1978), an excess of autoimmune disorders in mothers of subjects with ASD or their
families (Comi et al., 1999), and the effects of environmental factors on the immune
system supports the view that various types of disturbances of immune function
are important in the pathogenesis of ASD and in the perpetuation of their associ-
ated behavioral and neurological abnormalities. Growing evidence of immunologi-
cal abnormalities in patients with ASD (Ashwood, Wills, and Van de, 2006) also
indicates that, in addition to the spectrum of neurological and behavioral problems
exhibited by patients with ASD, other systems, including the immune system and
gastrointestinal tract, are also affected. The causes and effects of these immune system
Inflammation and Neuroimmunity in the Pathogenesis of Autism 227
abnormalities in autism are unknown but could be critical for maintaining, if not
also initiating, some of the abnormalities in central nervous system (CNS) function.
These abnormalities likely have both polygenic and environmental bases that will
have important clinical and therapeutic implications. Current evidence suggests that
neurobiological abnormalities in ASD are associated with changes in cytoarchitec-
tural and neuronal organization that may be determined by genetic, environmental,
immunological, and toxic factors (DiCicco-Bloom et al., 2006; Pardo and Eberhart,
2007). Since neuroimmune pathways and CNS cell populations, such as the neuroglia
(astroglia and microglia), play central roles during brain development, in cortical
organization, neuronal function, and the modulation of immune responses, it is quite
possible that these factors, acting in concert with host immunogenetic factors, con-
tribute to the pathogenesis of ASD.
activation, along with increased expression by CNS cells, neurons, neuroglia, and
elements of the BBB; and cytokines, chemokines, and other neuroimmune mediators
(e.g., metalloproteinases, Toll-like receptors [TLR], and microRNA) (Baltimore et al.,
2008; Cardona et al., 2008; Charo and Ransohoff, 2006; Page-McCaw, Ewald, and
Werb, 2007; Ransohoff, Liu, and Cardona, 2007). Innate neuroimmune responses
occur in neurodegenerative diseases with abnormalities of CNS homeostasis such
as those that occur in metabolic and seizure disorders. Frequently, both innate and
adaptive neuroimmune responses occur concomitantly; however, they also may have
distinctive pathogenic roles at different stages of neurological dysfunction. There is
a growing controversy about whether the roles of neuroinflammatory responses are
deleterious or protective in the setting of neurological dysfunction or injury, and it
is now evident from different experimental approaches that neuroinflammation may
play dual roles, both in providing neuroprotection as well as producing injury in the
CNS (Griffiths, Neal, and Gasque, 2007; Skaper, 2007; Tilleux and Hermans, 2007).
factors and neurotrophins); as well as other classical immune pathways such as those
associated with the major histocompatibility complex (e.g., MHC class I) and com-
plement—participate in mechanisms of neurodevelopment during intrauterine and
postnatal stages. Therefore, a dual role, developmental and immunological, is now a
well-recognized feature of all elements of neuroimmune responses (Boulanger and
Shatz, 2004; Tonelli, Postolache, and Sternberg, 2005).
Because the disorganization of cortical neurons, abnormalities in mini-columnar
organization and subcortical white matter, as well as brain growth abnormalities
are the dominant features of the neuropathology of ASD (Bauman and Kemper,
2005; Casanova, 2007), it is likely that the critical period of pathogenesis in autism
occurs during fetal brain development and the first year of life (Pardo, 2008; Pardo
and Eberhart, 2007). During this critical pathogenic period, neurodevelopmental
processes such as neuronal migration, cortical lamination, synaptic and dendritic
modeling, and the establishment of neuronal and cortical networks are influenced
not only by genetic factors, but also by important neuroimmune mechanisms that
involve astrocytes and microglia; interactions of cytokines, chemokines, and their
receptors; the developmental expression of TLRs; and complement activation (Bauer,
Kerr, and Patterson, 2007; Lagercrantz and Ringstedt, 2001; Stevens et al., 2007).
These neurobiological processes, along with the influence of the neuroimmune sys-
tem, are crucial for the development of neurological and behavioral trajectories, such
as social cognition, language and communication, and motor development (Pardo
and Eberhart, 2007). Disturbances of specific neurobiological trajectories triggered
by abnormalities in the maternal environment or by genetic influences may eventually
translate into abnormal patterns of neurodevelopment and behaviors that characterize
ASD. In addition to their actions during the period of pathogenesis, neuroimmune
responses also may be activated during post-pathogenic stages during which neuro-
logical regression, abnormal neuronal activity (e.g., seizures), and aberrant neural
networks may trigger such responses as the part of deleterious responses or even as
the part of neuroprotective pathways. It is unclear at present, whether neuroimmune
responses or neuroinflammation are common occurrences in the brain of patients
with ASD during postnatal stages or adulthood; however, our neuroimmunopatho-
logical studies suggest that a chronic stage of immune activation or neuroinflamma-
tion occurs at least in subsets of patients with ASD. The presence of these changes is
not determined by the age or duration of the disorder, and in the postmortem brain
tissues we examined, it appears to be an ongoing, long-term neuropathological
process (Vargas et al., 2005).
There is growing evidence that the maternal immune environment affects the devel-
oping fetal CNS and determines specific patterns of inflammation-mediated brain
and behavioral pathology (Hagberg and Mallard, 2005; Meyer, Yee, and Feldon,
2007; Meyer et al., 2006). The most exciting line of research that links maternal
immunological factors in the pathogenesis of ASD comes from the demonstration
of maternal autoantibodies in the serum of some mothers of patients with autism
(Braunschweig et al., 2007; Singer et al., 2008; Zimmerman et al., 2007) and their
presence correlates with a history of early developmental regression in their off-
spring (Braunschweig et al., 2007). Two different groups of researchers have dem-
onstrated the presence of antibodies that cross-react with fetal, but not adult, neural
antigens. These findings suggest that, at least in a subset of patients with autism, the
potential passive transfer of maternal antibodies during fetal life may have played a
pathogenic role in the presence of the disorder. It is unclear, however, what the specific
232 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
been exposed as a critical part of the cascade of molecular and cellular events lead-
ing to either the dysfunction of specific neuronal populations, such as nigral cells
in PD, or the processing and degradation of amyloid in AD (Blasko and Grubeck-
Loebenstein, 2003; McGeer and McGeer, 2002; Nagatsu and Sawada, 2005). Several
studies have shown that microglia are activated and increased in number in schizo-
phrenia, both in postmortem tissue and on positron emission tomography, which
may result from several possible underlying metabolic disturbances (Radewicz et al.,
2000; van Berckel et al., 2008; Wierzba-Bobrowicz et al., 2005). Clinical similarities
with schizophrenia raise the possibility of using analogous approaches to diagnostic
imaging in autism. Because of the central importance of neuroinflammatory path-
ways and neuroglia in response to neuronal dysfunction and their pathogenic roles in
diverse neurological disorders, we have hypothesized that neuroimmune responses
and neuroinflammation are associated with the pathogenic mechanisms involved in
cortical and neuronal dysfunction observed in ASD. Neuroinflammation in autism
may lend itself to treatment trials using anti-inflammatory drugs, which have shown
some benefit in PD and AD, as well as schizophrenia (Muller et al., 2002; Vlad
et al., 2008; Wahner et al., 2007).
efficacy and long-term synaptic plasticity (Montana et al., 2006; Panatier et al.,
2006; Pascual et al., 2005). Among these factors, cytokines such as TNF-α appear
to be involved in mechanisms of glia-mediated homeostasis during the activity-
dependent remodeling of the developing and established neuronal circuits that
follow brain injury (Beattie et al., 2002; Stellwagen and Malenka, 2006). These
observations support the important role of the gliotransmitter environment in the
modulation of neuronal function.
A critical issue in the natural history of a disease is the influence that the immune
system and immunogenetic host factors may have on its pathogenesis. The patho-
genic mechanisms of many neurological diseases, including neurodegenerative and
neuroimmunological disorders, are influenced by the spectrum and functions of the
proteins associated with immunological pathways. The expression of proteins such
as MHC or its HLA, cytokines, chemokines, and integrins is closely associated with
the function of these immunological pathways and may determine the patterns of sus-
ceptibility and severity of disease. Allelic variations in regulatory regions of cytokine
236 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
genes, point mutations, or single nucleotide substitutions have been shown to affect
gene transcription and levels of cytokine expression and to produce interindividual
variation in cytokine production (Meenagh et al., 2002). Single nucleotide polymor-
phisms (SNPs) and haplotypes of cytokine and chemokine genes produce genetic
differences in cytokine expression that predispose to disease or confer resistance in
immune-mediated disorders by influencing the strength and duration of the immune
response (Bidwell et al., 1999, 2001; Haukim et al., 2002). The clinical outcomes of
autoimmune, inflammatory, and neurodegenerative disorders appear to be influenced
by the balance between pro-inflammatory and anti-inflammatory pathways, modu-
lated by cytokines and chemokines. This relationship has been supported by numer-
ous reports of the association of some cytokine alleles and expression phenotypes
with immune-mediated or autoimmune disorders (Bidwell et al., 1999, 2001; Haukim
et al., 2002). The association of SNPs of genes associated with inflammation has been
investigated in inflammatory and neurodegenerative disorders such as AD and PD, as
well as neuroinflammatory disorders such as multiple sclerosis and HIV-associated
dementia (Crusius et al., 1995; Epplen et al., 1997; Gonzalez et al., 2002; Hakansson
et al., 2005a,b; Mycko et al., 1998). These polymorphisms may modify the natural
history of the disease by producing increased or decreased susceptibility to immune-
mediated responses or other pathogenic factors. For example, one polymorphism of
the MCP-1 gene (A-2518G) is associated with an increased risk of developing HIV
dementia (Gonzalez et al., 2002), early onset of PD (Nishimura et al., 2003), increased
resistance to antipsychotic therapy in schizophrenic patients (Mundo et al., 2005), and
increased levels of MCP-1 in serum in AD patients (Fenoglio et al., 2004; Pola et al.,
2004). In other disorders, SNPs of cytokine genes have been shown to be protective
against the onset or increased severity of disease. For example, a polymorphism in
position 1082 of the IL-10 gene promoter has been shown to be protective against
severe forms of multiple sclerosis, with the effect increasing over the years (Luomala
et al., 2003). More recently, the potential association of a SNP of IL-6 has been
associated with the development of PD (Hakansson et al., 2005a,b).
studies from different areas (Comi et al., 1999; Molloy et al., 2006; Mouridsen et al.,
2007; Sweeten et al., 2003). Recent studies of HLA in sets of parents, grandparents,
and children with autism have demonstrated the selective transmission of HLA DR4
from maternal grandparents to the mother, with greater frequency than from the
mother to her child with autism (Johnson et al., 2008). This enlarges our concept of
a specific type of susceptibility in which the mother becomes the “genetic patient”
in an immune interaction with her fetus. This is one possible immunogenetic mecha-
nism for autism that requires further investigation. Future studies of other potential
immunogenetic factors should also focus on defining the presence of specific SNPs
and haplotypes in cytokines, chemokines, their receptors, and other immune factors,
such as complement, integrins, and matrix metalloproteinases.
12.5 CONCLUSION
Inflammation and immune factors may influence the pathogenesis and perpetuation
of pathophysiological events that lead to ASD by their effect on brain development
as well as CNS function during adulthood. Because specific immune challenges
may occur during neurodevelopment, immunological and neuroimmune responses
that follow such challenges, whether environmental, maternal, or neurogenetic,
may constitute critical pathogenic factors in the development of ASD. The role
of immune factors and neuroinflammation are still uncertain but could be critical
in maintaining, if not also in initiating, some of the abnormalities present in the
CNS in ASD. A better understanding of the role of the immune system and neu-
roimmune reactions in the pathogenesis of ASD may have important clinical and
therapeutic implications.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Dr. Pardo is supported by the Peter Emch Fund for Autism Research, the Bart A.
McLean Fund for Neuroimmunology Research, Cure Autism Now, and NIH-NIDA
(K08-DA16160). Dr. Zimmerman is supported by the Hussman Foundation.
REFERENCES
Abbott, N. J., L. Ronnback, and E. Hansson. 2006. Astrocyte-endothelial interactions at the
blood-brain barrier. Nat. Rev. 7:41–53.
Aloisi, F. 2001. Immune function of microglia. Glia 36:165–179.
Ambrosini, E. and F. Aloisi. 2004. Chemokines and glial cells: A complex network in the
central nervous system. Neurochem. Res. 29:1017–1038.
Ashwood, P., S. Wills, and J. Van de Water. 2006. The immune response in autism: A new
frontier for autism research. J. Leukoc. Biol. 80:1–15.
Bach, J. P., B. Rinn, B. Meyer, R. Dodel, and M. Bacher. 2008. Role of MIF in inflammation
and tumorigenesis. Oncology 75:127–133.
Baltimore, D., M. P. Boldin, R. M. O’Connell, D. S. Rao, and K. D. Taganov. 2008. MicroRNAs:
New regulators of immune cell development and function. Nat. Immunol. 9:839–845.
Barton, G. M. 2008. A calculated response: Control of inflammation by the innate immune
system. J. Clin. Invest. 118:413–420.
238 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Bauer, J., H. Rauschka, and H. Lassmann. 2001. Inflammation in the nervous system: The human
perspective. Glia 36:235–243.
Bauer, S., B. J. Kerr, and P. H. Patterson. 2007. The neuropoietic cytokine family in development,
plasticity, disease and injury. Nat. Rev. 8:221–232.
Bauman, M. L. and T. L. Kemper. 2005. Structural brain automy in autism: What is the evidence?
In The Neurobiology of Autism, M. L. Bauman and T. L. Kemper (Eds.). Baltimore, MD:
The Johns Hopkins University Press, pp. 136–149.
Beattie, E. C., D. Stellwagen, W. Morishita, J. C. Bresnahan, B. K. Ha, M. Von Zastrow,
M. S. Beattie, and R. C. Malenka. 2002. Control of synaptic strength by glial
TNFalpha. Science 295:2282–2285.
Benn, T., C. Halfpenny, and N. Scolding. 2001. Glial cells as targets for cytotoxic immune
mediators. Glia 36:200–211.
Bessis, A., C. Bechade, D. Bernard, and A. Roumier. 2007. Microglial control of neuronal
death and synaptic properties. Glia 55:233–238.
Bidwell, J., L. Keen, G. Gallagher, R. Kimberly, T. Huizinga, M. F. McDermott, J. Oksenberg,
J. McNicholl, F. Pociot, C. Hardt, and S. D’Alfonso. 2001. Cytokine gene polymorphism
in human disease: On-line databases, supplement 1. Genes Immun. 2:61–70.
Bidwell, J. L., N. A. Wood, H. R. Morse, O. O. Olomolaiye, L. J. Keen, and G. J. Laundy.
1999. Human cytokine gene nucleotide sequence alignments: Supplement 1. Eur. J.
Immunogenet. 26:135–223.
Blanco, P., A. K. Palucka, V. Pascual, and J. Banchereau. 2008. Dendritic cells and cytok-
ines in human inflammatory and autoimmune diseases. Cytokine Growth Factor Rev.
19:41–52.
Blasko, I. and B. Grubeck-Loebenstein. 2003. Role of the immune system in the pathogenesis,
prevention and treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. Drugs Aging 20:101–113.
Boulanger, L. M. and C. J. Shatz. 2004. Immune signalling in neural development, synaptic
plasticity and disease. Nat. Rev. 5:521–531.
Braunschweig, D., P. Ashwood, P. Krakowiak, I. Hertz-Picciotto, R. Hansen, L. A. Croen,
I. N. Pessah, and J. Van de Water. 2007. Autism: Maternally derived antibodies specific
for fetal brain proteins. Neurotoxicology 29:226–231.
Bushong, E. A., M. E. Martone, Y. Z. Jones, and M. H. Ellisman. 2002. Protoplasmic astro-
cytes in CA1 stratum radiatum occupy separate anatomical domains. J. Neurosci.
22:183–192.
Cardona, A. E., M. Li, L. Liu, C. Savarin, and R. M. Ransohoff. 2008. Chemokines in and out
of the central nervous system: Much more than chemotaxis and inflammation. J. Leukoc.
Biol. 84:587–594.
Casanova, M. F. 2007. The neuropathology of autism. Brain Pathol. 17:422–433.
Charo, I. F. and R. M. Ransohoff. 2006. The many roles of chemokines and chemokine receptors
in inflammation. N. Engl. J. Med. 354:610–621.
Chess, S., P. Fernandez, and S. Korn. 1978. Behavioral consequences of congenital rubella.
J. Pediatr. 93:699–703.
Christiaens, I., D. B. Zaragoza, L. Guilbert, S. A. Robertson, B. F. Mitchell, and D. M. Olson. 2008.
Inflammatory processes in preterm and term parturition. J. Reprod. Immunol. 79:50–57.
Comi, A. M., A. W. Zimmerman, V. H. Frye, P. A. Law, and J. N. Peeden. 1999. Familial
clustering of autoimmune disorders and evaluation of medical risk factors in autism.
J. Child Neurol. 14:388–394.
Croen, L. A., J. K. Grether, C. K. Yoshida, R. Odouli, and J. Van de Water. 2005. Maternal
autoimmune diseases, asthma and allergies, and childhood autism spectrum disorders:
A case-control study. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 159:151–157.
Crusius, J. B., A. S. Pena, B. W. van Oosten, G. Bioque, A. Garcia, C. D. Dijkstra, and
C. H. Polman. 1995. Interleukin-1 receptor antagonist gene polymorphism and mul-
tiple sclerosis. Lancet 346:979.
Inflammation and Neuroimmunity in the Pathogenesis of Autism 239
Dalton, P., R. Deacon, A. Blamire, M. Pike, I. McKinlay, J. Stein, P. Styles, and A. Vincent. 2003.
Maternal neuronal antibodies associated with autism and a language disorder. Ann.
Neurol. 53:533–537.
Daniels, W. W., R. P. Warren, J. D. Odell, A. Maciulis, R. A. Burger, W. L. Warren, and A. R. Torres.
1995. Increased frequency of the extended or ancestral haplotype B44-SC30-DR4 in
autism. Neuropsychobiology 32:120–123.
DiCicco-Bloom, E., C. Lord, L. Zwaigenbaum, E. Courchesne, S. R. Dager, C. Schmitz,
R. T. Schultz, J. Crawley, and L. J. Young. 2006. The developmental neurobiology of
autism spectrum disorder. J. Neurosci. 26:6897–6906.
Dietert, R. R. and J. M. Dietert. 2008. Potential for early-life immune insult including devel-
opmental immunotoxicity in autism and autism spectrum disorders: Focus on critical
windows of immune vulnerability. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health B Crit. Rev. 11:660–680.
Dong, Y. and E. N. Benveniste. 2001. Immune function of astrocytes. Glia 36:180–190.
Engelhardt, B. and R. M. Ransohoff. 2005. The ins and outs of T-lymphocyte trafficking to the
CNS: Anatomical sites and molecular mechanisms. Trends Immunol. 26:485–495.
Epplen, C., S. Jackel, E. J. Santos, M. D’Souza, D. Poehlau, B. Dotzauer, E. Sindern,
M. Haupts, K. P. Rude, F. Weber, J. Stover, S. Poser, W. Gehler, J. P. Malin, H. Przuntek,
and J. T. Epplen. 1997. Genetic predisposition to multiple sclerosis as revealed by
immunoprinting. Ann. Neurol. 41:341–352.
Fatemi, S. H., A. V. Snow, J. M. Stary, M. Araghi-Niknam, T. J. Reutiman, S. Lee, A. I. Brooks, and
D. A. Pearce. 2005. Reelin signaling is impaired in autism. Biol. Psychiatry 57:777–787.
Fenoglio, C., D. Galimberti, C. Lovati, I. Guidi, A. Gatti, S. Fogliarino, M. Tiriticco, C. Mariani,
G. Forloni, C. Pettenati, P. Baron, G. Conti, N. Bresolin, and E. Scarpini. 2004. MCP-1
in Alzheimer’s disease patients: A-2518G polymorphism and serum levels. Neurobiol.
Aging 25:1169–1173.
Fields, R. D. and B. Stevens-Graham. 2002. New insights into neuron-glia communication.
Science 298:556–562.
Golde, T. E. 2002. Inflammation takes on Alzheimer disease. Nat. Med. 8:936–938.
Gomes, F. C., V. O. Sousa, and L. Romao. 2005. Emerging roles for TGF-beta1 in nervous
system development. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 23:413–424.
Gonzalez, E., B. H. Rovin, L. Sen, G. Cooke, R. Dhanda, S. Mummidi, H. Kulkarni, M. J. Bamshad,
V. Telles, S. A. Anderson, E. A. Walter, K. T. Stephan, M. Deucher, A. Mangano, R. Bologna,
S. S. Ahuja, M. J. Dolan, and S. K. Ahuja. 2002. HIV-1 infection and AIDS dementia are
influenced by a mutant MCP-1 allele linked to increased monocyte infiltration of tissues
and MCP-1 levels. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99:13795–13800.
Graeber, M. B., K. Bise, and P. Mehraein. 1993. Synaptic stripping in the human facial nucleus.
Acta Neuropathol. 86:179–181.
Griffiths, M., J. W. Neal, and P. Gasque. 2007. Innate immunity and protective neuroinflammation:
New emphasis on the role of neuroimmune regulatory proteins. Int. Rev. Neurobiol.
82:29–55.
Grosche, J., V. Matyash, T. Moller, A. Verkhratsky, A. Reichenbach, and H. Kettenmann. 1999.
Microdomains for neuron-glia interaction: Parallel fiber signaling to Bergmann glial
cells. Nat. Neurosci. 2:139–143.
Hagberg, H. and C. Mallard. 2005. Effect of inflammation on central nervous system develop-
ment and vulnerability. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 18:117–123.
Hakansson, A., L. Westberg, S. Nilsson, S. Buervenich, A. Carmine, B. Holmberg, O. Sydow,
L. Olson, B. Johnels, E. Eriksson, and H. Nissbrandt. 2005a. Interaction of polymor-
phisms in the genes encoding interleukin-6 and estrogen receptor beta on the suscepti-
bility to Parkinson’s disease. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 133:88–92.
Hakansson, A., L. Westberg, S. Nilsson, S. Buervenich, A. Carmine, B. Holmberg, O. Sydow,
L. Olson, B. Johnels, E. Eriksson, and H. Nissbrandt. 2005b. Investigation of genes cod-
ing for inflammatory components in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 20:569–573.
240 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Halassa, M. M., T. Fellin, H. Takano, J. H. Dong, and P. G. Haydon. 2007. Synaptic islands
defined by the territory of a single astrocyte. J. Neurosci. 27:6473–6477.
Haukim, N., J. L. Bidwell, A. J. Smith, L. J. Keen, G. Gallagher, R. Kimberly, T. Huizinga,
M. F. McDermott, J. Oksenberg, J. McNicholl, F. Pociot, C. Hardt, and S. D’Alfonso.
2002. Cytokine gene polymorphism in human disease: On-line databases, supplement
2. Genes Immun. 3:313–330.
Infante-Duarte, C., S. Waiczies, J. Wuerfel, and F. Zipp. 2008. New developments in under-
standing and treating neuroinflammation. J. Mol. Med. 86:975–985.
Johnson, W. G., M. Sreenath, S. Buyske, and E. S. Stenroos. 2008. Teratogenic alleles in autism
and other neurodevelopmental disorders. In Autism: Current Theories and Evidence,
A. W. Zimmerman (Ed.). Totowa, NJ: Humana Press, pp. 41–68.
Jonakait, G. M. 2007. The effects of maternal inflammation on neuronal development: Possible
mechanisms. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 25:415–425.
Kaul, M., G. A. Garden, and S. A. Lipton. 2001. Pathways to neuronal injury and apoptosis in
HIV-associated dementia. Nature 410:988–994.
Kim, J. H., J. H. Kim, J. A. Park, S. W. Lee, W. J. Kim, Y. S. Yu, and K. W. Kim. 2006. Blood-
neural barrier: Intercellular communication at glio-vascular interface. J. Biochem. Mol. Biol.
39:339–345.
Kirov, S. A. and K. M. Harris. 1999. Dendrites are more spiny on mature hippocampal neurons
when synapses are inactivated. Nat. Neurosci. 2:878–883.
Lagercrantz, H. and T. Ringstedt. 2001. Organization of the neuronal circuits in the central
nervous system during development. Acta Paediatr. 90:707–715.
Lazarini, F., T. N. Tham, P. Casanova, F. Arenzana-Seisdedos, and M. Dubois-Dalcq. 2003.
Role of the alpha-chemokine stromal cell-derived factor (SDF-1) in the developing and
mature central nervous system. Glia 42:139–148.
Lee, L. C., A. A. Zachary, M. S. Leffell, C. J. Newschaffer, K. J. Matteson, J. D. Tyler, and
A. W. Zimmerman. 2006. HLA-DR4 in families with autism. Pediatr. Neurol. 35:303–307.
Luomala, M., T. Lehtimaki, H. Huhtala, M. Ukkonen, T. Koivula, M. Hurme, and I. Elovaara.
2003. Promoter polymorphism of IL-10 and severity of multiple sclerosis. Acta Neurol.
Scand. 108:396–400.
Ma, Q., D. Jones, P. R. Borghesani, R. A. Segal, T. Nagasawa, T. Kishimoto, R. T. Bronson,
and T. A. Springer. 1998. Impaired B-lymphopoiesis, myelopoiesis, and derailed cerebel-
lar neuron migration in C. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95:9448–9453.
Martin, L. A., P. Ashwood, D. Braunschweig, M. Cabanlit, J. Van de Water, and D. G. Amaral.
2008. Stereotypies and hyperactivity in rhesus monkeys exposed to IgG from mothers
of children with autism. Brain Behav. Immun. 22:806–816.
McGeer, P. L. and E. G. McGeer. 2002. Local neuroinflammation and the progression of
Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurovirol. 8:529–538.
Medzhitov, R. 2008. Origin and physiological roles of inflammation. Nature 454:428–435.
Meenagh, A., F. Williams, O. A. Ross, C. Patterson, C. Gorodezky, M. Hammond, W. A. Leheny,
and D. Middleton. 2002. Frequency of cytokine polymorphisms in populations from western
Europe, Africa, Asia, the Middle East and South America. Hum. Immunol. 63:1055–1061.
Meyer, U., M. Nyffeler, A. Engler, A. Urwyler, M. Schedlowski, I. Knuesel, B. K. Yee, and
J. Feldon. 2006. The time of prenatal immune challenge determines the specificity of
inflammation-mediated brain and behavioral pathology. J. Neurosci. 26:4752–4762.
Meyer, U., B. K. Yee, and J. Feldon. 2007. The neurodevelopmental impact of prenatal infections
at different times of pregnancy: The earlier the worse? Neuroscientist 13:241–256.
Molloy, C. A., A. L. Morrow, J. Meinzen-Derr, G. Dawson, R. Bernier, M. Dunn, S. L. Hyman,
W. M. McMahon, J. Goudie-Nice, S. Hepburn, N. Minshew, S. Rogers, M. Sigman,
M. A. Spence, H. Tager-Flusberg, F. R. Volkmar, and C. Lord. 2006. Familial autoimmune
thyroid disease as a risk factor for regression in children with autism spectrum disorder:
A CPEA study. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36:317–324.
Inflammation and Neuroimmunity in the Pathogenesis of Autism 241
Montana, V., E. B. Malarkey, C. Verderio, M. Matteoli, and V. Parpura. 2006. Vesicular transmit-
ter release from astrocytes. Glia 54:700–715.
Morris, C. M., M. Pletnikov, A. W. Zimmerman, and H. S. Singer. 2008. Maternal antibodies
and the placental-fetal IgG transfer theory. In Autism: Current Theories and Evidence,
A. W. Zimmerman (Ed.). Totowa, NJ: Humana Press, pp. 309–328.
Mouridsen, S. E., B. Rich, T. Isager, and N. J. Nedergaard. 2007. Autoimmune diseases in
parents of children with infantile autism: A case-control study. Dev. Med. Child Neurol.
49:429–432.
Muller, N., M. Riedel, C. Scheppach, B. Brandstatter, S. Sokullu, K. Krampe, M. Ulmschneider,
R. R. Engel, H. J. Moller, and M. J. Schwarz. 2002. Beneficial antipsychotic effects
of celecoxib add-on therapy compared to risperidone alone in schizophrenia. Am.
J. Psychiatry 159:1029–1034.
Mundo, E., A. C. Altamura, S. Vismara, R. Zanardini, S. Bignotti, R. Randazzo, C. Montresor,
and M. Gennarelli. 2005. MCP-1 gene (SCYA2) and schizophrenia: A case-control
association study. Am. J. Med. Genet. 132B(1):1–4.
Munoz-Fernandez, M. A. and M. Fresno. 1998. The role of tumour necrosis factor, interleukin
6, interferon-gamma and inducible nitric oxide synthase in the development and pathol-
ogy of the nervous system. Prog. Neurobiol. 56:307–340.
Murai, K. K. and D. J. Van Meyel. 2007. Neuron glial communication at synapses: Insights
from vertebrates and invertebrates. Neuroscientist 13:657–666.
Mycko, M., W. Kowalski, M. Kwinkowski, A. C. Buenafe, B. Szymanska, E. Tronczynska,
A. Plucienniczak, and K. Selmaj. 1998. Multiple sclerosis: The frequency of allelic forms
of tumor necrosis factor and lymphotoxin-alpha. J. Neuroimmunol. 84:198–206.
Nagatsu, T. and M. Sawada. 2005. Inflammatory process in Parkinson’s disease: Role for
cytokines. Curr. Pharm. Des. 11:999–1016.
Nakashima, K. and T. Taga. 2002. Mechanisms underlying cytokine-mediated cell-fate regulation
in the nervous system. Mol. Neurobiol. 25:233–244.
Nedergaard, M., B. Ransom, and S. A. Goldman. 2003. New roles for astrocytes: Redefining
the functional architecture of the brain. Trends Neurosci. 26:523–530.
Nedergaard, M., T. Takano, and A. J. Hansen. 2002. Beyond the role of glutamate as a neu-
rotransmitter. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3:748–755.
Neumann, H. 2001. Control of glial immune function by neurons. Glia 36:191–199.
Nishimura, M., S. Kuno, I. Mizuta, M. Ohta, H. Maruyama, R. Kaji, and H. Kawakami. 2003.
Influence of monocyte chemoattractant protein 1 gene polymorphism on age at onset of
sporadic Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 18:953–955.
Page-McCaw, A., A. J. Ewald, and Z. Werb. 2007. Matrix metalloproteinases and the regulation
of tissue remodelling. Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 8:221–233.
Panatier, A., D. T. Theodosis, J. P. Mothet, B. Touquet, L. Pollegioni, D. A. Poulain, and
S. H. Oliet. 2006. Glia-derived D-serine controls NMDA receptor activity and synaptic
memory. Cell 125:775–784.
Pardo C. A. 2008. Can neuroinflammation influence the development of autism spectrum
disorders? In Autism: Current Theories and Evidence, A. W. Zimmerman (Ed.).
Totowa, NJ: Humana Press, pp. 329–346.
Pardo, C. A. and C. G. Eberhart. 2007. The neurobiology of autism. Brain Pathol. 17:434–447.
Pardo, C. A., D. L. Vargas, and A. W. Zimmerman. 2005. Immunity, neuroglia and neuroin-
flammation in autism. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 17:485–495.
Pardo, C. A., E. P. Vining, L. Guo, R. L. Skolasky, B. S. Carson, and J. M. Freeman. 2004.
The pathology of Rasmussen syndrome: Stages of cortical involvement and neuro-
pathological studies in 45 hemispherectomies. Epilepsia 45:516–526.
Paredes, M. F., G. Li, O. Berger, S. C. Baraban, and S. J. Pleasure. 2006. Stromal-derived
factor-1 (CXCL12) regulates laminar position of Cajal-Retzius cells in normal and dys-
plastic brains. J. Neurosci. 26:9404–9412.
242 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Smith, S. E., J. Li, K. Garbett, K. Mirnics, and P. H. Patterson. 2007. Maternal immune activation
alters fetal brain development through interleukin-6. J. Neurosci. 27:10695–10702.
Stellwagen, D. and R. C. Malenka. 2006. Synaptic scaling mediated by glial TNF-alpha.
Nature 440:1054–1059.
Stevens, B., N. J. Allen, L. E. Vazquez, G. R. Howell, K. S. Christopherson, N. Nouri,
K. D. Micheva, A. K. Mehalow, A. D. Huberman, B. Stafford, A. Sher, A. M. Litke,
J. D. Lambris, S. J. Smith, S. W. John, and B. A. Barres. 2007. The classical complement
cascade mediates CNS synapse elimination. Cell 131:1164–1178.
Stumm, R. and V. Hollt. 2007. CXC chemokine receptor 4 regulates neuronal migration and
axonal pathfinding in the developing nervous system: Implications for neuronal regen-
eration in the adult brain. J. Mol. Endocrinol. 38:377–382.
Sweeten, T. L., S. L. Bowyer, D. J. Posey, G. M. Halberstadt, and C. J. McDougle. 2003.
Increased prevalence of familial autoimmunity in probands with pervasive developmental
disorders. Pediatrics 112:e420.
Taga, T. and S. Fukuda. 2005. Role of IL-6 in the neural stem cell differentiation. Clin. Rev.
Allergy Immunol. 28:249–256.
Tilleux, S. and E. Hermans. 2007. Neuroinflammation and regulation of glial glutamate uptake
in neurological disorders. J. Neurosci. Res. 85:2059–2070.
Tonelli, L. H., T. T. Postolache, and E. M. Sternberg. 2005. Inflammatory genes and neural
activity: Involvement of immune genes in synaptic function and behavior. Front. Biosci.
10:675–680.
Torres, A. R., A. Maciulis, E. G. Stubbs, A. Cutler, and D. Odell. 2002. The transmission
disequilibrium test suggests that HLA-DR4 and DR13 are linked to autism spectrum
disorder. Hum. Immunol. 63:311–316.
Torres, A. R., T. L. Sweeten, A. Cutler, B. J. Bedke, M. Fillmore, E. G. Stubbs, and D. Odell. 2006.
The association and linkage of the HLA-A2 class I allele with autism. Hum. Immunol.
67:346–351.
Trapp, B. D., J. R. Wujek, G. A. Criste, W. Jalabi, X. Yin, G. J. Kidd, S. Stohlman, and R. Ransohoff.
2007. Evidence for synaptic stripping by cortical microglia. Glia 55:360–368.
van Berckel, B. N., M. G. Bossong, R. Boellaard, R. Kloet, A. Schuitemaker, E. Caspers,
G. Luurtsema, A. D. Windhorst, W. Cahn, A. A. Lammertsma, and R. S. Kahn. 2008.
Microglia activation in recent-onset schizophrenia: A quantitative (R)-[11C]PK11195
positron emission tomography study. Biol. Psychiatry 64:820–822.
Vargas, D. L., C. Nascimbene, C. Krishnan, A. W. Zimmerman, and C. A. Pardo. 2005. Neuroglial
activation and neuroinflammation in the brain of patients with autism. Ann. Neurol.
57:67–81.
Vlad, S. C., D. R. Miller, N. W. Kowall, and D. T. Felson. 2008. Protective effects of NSAIDs
on the development of Alzheimer disease. Neurology 70:1672–1677.
Volterra, A. and J. Meldolesi. 2005. Astrocytes, from brain glue to communication elements:
The revolution continues. Nat. Rev. 6:626–640.
Wahner, A. D., J. M. Bronstein, Y. M. Bordelon, and B. Ritz. 2007. Nonsteroidal anti-
inflammatory drugs may protect against Parkinson disease. Neurology 69:1836–1842.
Wierzba-Bobrowicz, T., E. Lewandowska, W. Lechowicz, T. Stepien, and E. Pasennik. 2005.
Quantitative analysis of activated microglia, ramified and damage of processes in the
frontal and temporal lobes of chronic schizophrenics. Folia Neuropathol. 43:81–89.
Williams, K. C. and W. F. Hickey. 2002. Central nervous system damage, monocytes and
macrophages, and neurological disorders in AIDS. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 25:537–562.
Zerrate, M. C., M. Pletnikov, S. L. Connors, D. L. Vargas, F. J. Seidler, A. W. Zimmerman,
T. A. Slotkin, and C. A. Pardo. 2007. Neuroinflammation and behavioral abnormalities
after neonatal terbutaline treatment in rats: Implications for autism. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther.
322:16–22.
244 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
CONTENTS
13.1 Introduction ...............................................................................................246
13.2 Innate Immunity: An Overview ................................................................246
13.2.1 Innate Immunity in the Gut Mucosa .......................................... 247
13.2.1.1 The Epithelium .......................................................... 247
13.2.1.2 GALT .........................................................................248
13.2.1.3 Effector T-Cell Subsets..............................................248
13.2.1.4 Treg Cells................................................................... 249
13.2.1.5 Effects of Innate Immunity ....................................... 250
13.2.2 Innate Immunity in the CNS ...................................................... 251
13.2.2.1 Innate Immune Cells in the CNS .............................. 252
13.2.2.2 Role of Treg Cells in the CNS Homeostasis.............. 253
13.2.2.3 Neuro-Immune Network ........................................... 253
13.3 Evidence of Innate Immune Abnormalities in Autism ............................. 254
13.3.1 GI System ................................................................................... 254
13.3.1.1 Intestinal Permeability .............................................. 255
13.3.1.2 Dysbiosis.................................................................... 255
13.3.1.3 Autism Colitis ............................................................ 255
13.3.1.4 Food Allergy.............................................................. 256
13.3.2 CNS ............................................................................................ 258
13.3.2.1 CNS Inflammation..................................................... 258
13.3.2.2 Mechanisms of Chronic CNS Inflammation ............. 259
13.4 Possible Impact of Innate Immunity on Neuro-Immune
Interactions in Autism ............................................................................... 259
13.4.1 Impaired Signaling of Neurotransmitters ..................................260
13.4.1.1 Cholinergic Neurotransmission .................................260
13.4.1.2 GABA Neurotransmission.........................................260
13.4.1.3 Ca2+ Signaling............................................................ 261
13.4.1.4 β2 Adrenergic Receptor............................................. 261
245
246 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
13.1 INTRODUCTION
Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are complex developmental disorders with largely
unknown etiology. Although recent progress in genetics have defined various disor-
ders that manifest autistic features, this may account for up to 10%–15% of ASD,
usually manifested as severe classical autism. For the remaining ASD patients, the
diagnosis of ASD is solely based on subjective behavioral symptoms, which can
vary markedly time to time during development. The presence of comorbidities also
affects their behavioral symptoms. Gastrointestinal (GI) inflammation, one of the
most common comorbidities in ASD, likely affects mood, irritability, and concentra-
tion simply because of abdominal discomfort. Recent studies indicate the presence
of more complex mechanisms affecting the brain via gut neuro-immune network,
as detailed later.
Apart from GI symptoms, a subset of ASD children presents with frequent child-
hood infections including otitis media, rhinosinusitis, and upper respiratory-tract
infection. However, conventional immune workup is often unrevealing in such ASD
children. Subtle immune abnormalities have been described in ASD children, although
the effects of such abnormalities are unclear. Nevertheless, recent studies may indi-
cate the possible impact of innate immunity on the onset and/or progress of ASD in
some but not all ASD children.
In this chapter, the overview of innate immunity in the GI tract and in the central
nervous system (CNS) is discussed first, and then immune abnormalities reported in
the GI tract and in the CNS in autism are discussed. Lastly, the possible impact of
innate immunity on neuro-immune interactions in autism is discussed.
the first family of PRRs characterized (Zedler and Faist, 2006). PRRs can also sense
molecular patterns derived from injured tissue (the so-called danger-signals) and
can initiate immune reaction, a process which appears to be vital for wound healing
(Zedler and Faist, 2006). It is now well recognized that the innate immunity bridges
subsequent adaptive immune responses that then reciprocally affect innate immune
responses. Namely, PRR-mediated signaling activates antigen (Ag)-presenting cells
(APCs) by up-regulating the expression of major histocompatibility complex (MHC)/
co-stimulatory molecules, augmenting the Ag processing, and facilitating the migra-
tion of APCs to the lymphoid organs (Teitelbaum and Allan Walker, 2005).
The innate immunity is exerted in an Ag-independent manner and components
of innate immunity are genetically predetermined. Thus, the innate immunity can
function at birth and may play more significant roles in the first years of life before
Ag-dependent adaptive immunity fully develops. However, such characteristics of
innate immunity may be accompanied by a possibility of being more vulnerable to
subtle genetic variation than adaptive immunity.
13.2.1.2 GALT
The LP located beneath the intestinal epithelium serves as a meshwork of connective
tissue containing plasma blasts, T cells, and various innate immune cells including
dendritic cells (DCs), mast cells, macrophages, and granulocytes (neutrophils and
eosinophils). The presence of abundant mast cells in the LP during infancy and early
childhood is thought to be important for mucosal immune defense against extracel-
lular parasites. In addition, mast cells are also indicated to have a role in tolerance
induction against macronutrients (Lu et al., 2006). However, the excessive mast-
cell activation can cause various abdominal symptoms by the release of mediators
including histamine.
PPs are lymphoid aggregates composed of B-cell follicles, surrounding inter-
follicular CD4+ and CD8+ T cells and Ag-presenting innate immune cells (DCs,
macrophages, etc.) intervening beneath the follicle-associated epithelium. DCs
composed of various subsets are abundant in the subepithelial dome (Wershil and
Furuta, 2008). Activated DCs can migrate to mesenteric lymph nodes or LPs and can
exert stimulatory and modulatory actions, bridging innate immunity and adaptive
immunity (Coombes et al., 2007). The production of tumor necrosis factor (TNF)-α
and the expression of TNF-α receptors are important for the development of PPs
(Fu and Chaplin, 1999).
The presentation of luminal Ags is affected by the dose of Ag and the presence
or absence of adjuvant effects. Pathogenic bacteria produce microbial byproducts,
which can stimulate innate immune cells including gut APCs and serve as the major
source of adjuvant (Abreu et al., 2005; Franchi et al., 2006). In contrast, a commensal
flora often down-regulate APC activation (Smith and Nagler-Anderson, 2005).
the fact that LPS enhances the suppressive activity of TLR4 +CD4+CD45RBlowCD25+
Treg cells (Caramalho et al., 2003). A prolonged exposure to LPS or TLR2 stimulants
also leads to tolerance and cross-tolerance to other PAMPs in intestinal epithelial cell
lines (Otte et al., 2004). Likewise, other TLR agonists derived from the commensal
flora also appear to be important in maintaining proper communication between the
gut mucosal immune system and the commensal intestinal flora (Abreu et al., 2005;
Kelly et al., 2005; Lee et al., 2007, 2008).
In addition to anti-inflammatory effects mediated by the host-immune system, the
commensal flora can exert direct anti-inflammatory actions. Bacteroides thetaiotao-
micron was shown to restrict the signaling induced by flagellin, a TLR5 agonist, and
flagellated pathogens. These microbes block downstream signaling associated with
NF-κB activation by promoting the nuclear export of transcriptionally active RelA
(Kelly et al., 2004). This effect of B. thetaiotanomicron may partly explain why
the gut tolerates large amounts of flagellated, potentially inflammatory commensal
bacteria. In addition, other commensal bacteria were shown to block the activation
of NF-κB by inhibiting IκB-α ubiquitination (Neish et al., 2000).
13.2.2.3.1 Cytokines
In addition to PAMPs, numerous studies report the effects of cytokines on the CNS.
Cytokine produced outside the brain can affect the CNS; neuronal tissues are one
of the target organs. They may directly affect the brain at the level of brain paren-
chyma crossing the BBB or entering the brain area lacking the BBB (Goncharova
254 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
and Tarakanov, 2007). They may also indirectly affect the brain via peripheral nervous
systems and also via secondary messengers induced by cytokines (Goncharova and
Tarakanov, 2007). For example, IL-2 can penetrate the intact BBB and affect cell
growth and survival, the release of multiple mediators, and neuronal activities via
IL-2 receptor expressed on neuronal and glial cells (Goncharova and Tarakanov,
2007). Proinflammatory cytokines released by innate immune responses (IL-1β,
IL-6, and TNF-α) are known to cause “sickness behaviors” affecting multiple
aspects of the CNS functions (Goncharova and Tarakanov, 2007; Gosselin and
Rivest, 2007). A part of their actions may be attributed to their regulation of the
activity of the serotonin transporter via p38 mitogen-activated protein-kinase path-
way (Zhu et al., 2006). IL-1 is also implicated in long-term memory (Goeb et al.,
2006; Pickering and O’Connor, 2007). Type 1 IFNs are also known to affect multiple
CNS functions. It is well known that a high dose of exogenous type I IFNs can cause
depression (Goeb et al., 2006).
These cytokines are also produced by the brain-resident innate immune cells as
described in Section 13.2.2.1. TNF-α and IL-1β have been implicated in neurotoxic
effects observed in neuronal damages (Gosselin and Rivest, 2007). IL-1β induces
the production of reactive oxygen/nitrogen species, modulate activities of metallo-
proteinases, and increase the synaptic release of nitric oxide. TNF-α triggers the
recruitment of neutrophils and monocytes to the site of inflammation and augments
their phagocytic functions to eliminate pathogens and tissue debris. Such effects can
not only be neuroprotective but also be neurotoxic.
Reciprocally, the CNS affects cytokine network by releasing various mediators such
as GCs. For example, acute stress responses via hypophysis-pituitary-adrenal (HPA)
axis limit excessive immune responses and restore immune homeostasis. However,
chronic stress responses lead to the chronic production of GCs and catecholamines
(CAs), which can dysregulate immune functions. Evidence indicates that both GCs and
CAs render Th2-skewed responses by suppressing the production of IL-12 by APC;
IL-12 is the key cytokine for Th1 differentiation (Blotta et al., 1997; Elenkov, 2008).
GCs are also reported to up-regulate the production of IL-4, IL-10, and IL-13 by Th2
cells (Blotta et al., 1997; Elenkov, 2008). Both GCs and CAs affect the production of
proinflammatory cytokines by macrophage-monocyte lineage cells (Elenkov, 2008).
The cholinergic signaling provides anti-inflammatory action to leukocytes via cholin-
ergic receptors (Franco et al., 2007). Evidence indicates that anti-inflammatory signal-
ing via HPA axis and cholinergic nervous system is dysregulated in the IBD patients
as evidenced by markedly reduced 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HT) production
(Franco et al., 2007).
in the onset and progress of autism. Proposed abnormalities may be more or less asso-
ciated with “presumed,” impaired gut mucosal innate immunity in autistic children.
13.3.1.2 Dysbiosis
Intestinal commensal flora also plays a role in maintaining intestinal homeostasis
as discussed in Section 13.2.1.5.2. There have been anecdotal reports of the onset of
autism following the administration of antibiotics and the subsequent appearance of
GI symptoms. It was hypothesized that antimicrobial administration led to the dis-
ruption of commensal flora and the colonization of bacteria producing neurotoxin. In
open-label trial, the administration of oral vancomycin in 10 children with regressive
autism resulted in short-term improvement in their behavioral symptoms (Sandler
et al., 2000). Two studies that examined the constitution of gut microflora in ASD
children reported differences from normal controls. One study examined 7 children
with regression autism and documented GI symptoms as well as 4 control children
and their results revealed significant difference in the upper and the lower intestinal
floras between autistic and control children (Finegold et al., 2002). Another study
examined 58 ASD children, 15 normal siblings, and 10 unrelated healthy children.
The authors reported a higher incidence of Clostridium histolyticum group in ASD
children; most of ASD children had the history of multiple antibiosis and significant
GI symptoms (Parracho et al., 2005). In these studies, it is possible that other factors
such as previous frequent antibiosis and the restricted diet on which many ASD chil-
dren were already placed at the time of the study entry may have affected the results.
The careful selection of the study subjects with appropriate case controls may shed a
light whether dysbiosis has any role in the pathogenesis of autism.
mild GI mucosal inflammation have been reported in ASD children (Furlano et al.,
2001; Wakefield et al., 2000, 2005). Immunohistochemical studies of biopsy speci-
men from ASD children revealed higher numbers of CD3+CD8+ T cells in the epi-
thelium as well as higher numbers of CD3+ T cells and CD19+ B cells in the LP as
compared to those from normal controls (Ashwood et al., 2003). In children with
regressive autism, Torrente et al. (2002) reported lymphocytic colitis that is char-
acterized with epithelial IgG and complement deposition. LNH can be observed in
normal children and the significance of “autism colitis” appears still controversial.
However, one study indicated significantly higher prevalence of LNH in the ileum
and colon in ASD children than controls; LNH appears to be not affected by the diet
or age at the time of colonoscopy (Wakefield et al., 2005).
In subsequent studies, these authors report the up-regulation of proinflammatory
cytokines in the intestinal mucosa. It was reported that LP CD3+ T cells in the duo-
denum express higher IL-2, TNF-α, and IFN-γ, but less IL-10 and also the higher
expression of TNF-α and IFN-γ by LP CD3+ T cells in the colon in ASD children
with GI symptoms (Ashwood and Wakefield, 2006; Ashwood et al., 2004). In 18
ASD children with GI symptoms, authors reported increased expression of TNF-α
and IFN-γ and less expression of IL-10 by CD3+ T cells in both intestinal mucosa and
peripheral blood as compared to normal controls (Ashwood and Wakefield, 2006).
On the other hand, other studies failed to reveal any difference between ASD
children and normal controls in the concentration of proinflammatory cytokines
(IL-6, IL-8, and IL-1β) in the GI mucosa (DeFelice et al., 2003) or in the stool
concentration of calprotectin or rectal nitric oxide: these are nonspecific markers
of GI inflammation. Such conflicting results may be partly attributed to the small
numbers of study subjects, although most of the studies focus on ASD children
with GI symptoms. It still remains to be seen whether “autism colitis” is present or
such GI inflammation may be associated with other immune abnormalities such as FA.
Unfortunately, in all these studies, the presence or absence of IgE-mediated FA or
NFA has not been addressed.
How is the GI inflammation documented in ASD children associated with
innate immune abnormalities? Innate immunity is crucial for maintaining immune
homeostasis. If there exists aberrant innate immune responses in the gut mucosa
as indicated in the peripheral blood of a subset of ASD children (Jyonouchi et al.,
2002, 2005b), these ASD children may be vulnerable to GI inflammation triggered
by immune insults, developing chronic inflammation as postulated in patients with
Crohn’s disease.
TABLE 13.1
Prevalence of Atopic Disorders
Study Group Atopic Disorders AR + AC AD Atopic Asthma
ASD children 34/123 (27.6%) 34/123 (27.6%) 10/123 (8.1%) 6/123 (4.9%)
a
ROM/CRS 3/25 (12.0%) 3/25 (12.0%) 0/25 (0%) 3/25 (12.0%)2
FA 6/27 (22.2%) 4/27 (14.8%) 4/27 (14.8%) 3/27 (11.1%)
Control 10/46 (21.7%) 10/46 (21.7%) 3/46 (6.5%) 7/46 (15.2%)2
Note: AC, allergic conjunctivitis; AD, allergic dermatitis; AR, allergic rhinitis.
a Among ROM/CRS patients and controls, 16 and 2 children were diagnosed with non-atopic asthma,
respectively.
One study examining 30 autistic children reports the higher frequency of skin prick
test reactivity in their study population, but it is unclear whether these children dem-
onstrated corresponding clinical features; authors report normal IgE levels and no
asthma symptoms in these autistic children (Bakkaloglu, 2007). In 123 ASD children
evaluated in the Pediatric Allergy/Immunology clinic at our institution, we found
no evidence of the high prevalence of atopy (Table 13.1). These ASD children were
evaluated by standard diagnostic measures for atopic disorders including the mea-
surement of allergen-specific IgE and skin prick testing. Likewise, we did not find the
high frequency of IgE-mediated FA in ASD children evaluated in our clinic.
Various dietary intervention measures have been tried on the basis of anec-
dotal reports despite the lack of evidence of IgE-mediated FA. Among such dietary
intervention measures, a casein-free, gluten-free (cf/gf) diet appears most popular
with frequent beneficial effects per parental reports. However, prospective studies
addressing the effects of the cf/gf diet revealed conflicting results (Elder et al., 2006;
Knivsberg et al., 2002). This may be partly attributed to the random selection of the
study subject without proper workup for FA. These studies may have been formu-
lated to test the “leaky gut hypothesis.”
In our previous studies, we hypothesized that GI symptoms frequently seen in
ASD children can be partly explained by NFA and immune reactivity to food pro-
teins can be detected by measuring the production of TNF-α and other inflammatory
cytokines by peripheral blood mononuclear cells (PBMCs) as reported in NFA chil-
dren (Benlounes et al., 1999). Our results revealed the presence of cellular immune
reactivity to common dietary proteins (mainly milk protein) in young ASD children
with GI symptoms (Jyonouchi et al., 2005a). We also found that such immune reac-
tivity to food protein was correlated with aberrant responses to LPS (TLR4 agonist)
by PBMCs (Jyonouchi et al., 2005b). However, NFA may be playing a lesser role
in GI symptoms in older ASD children, since most children likely outgrow NFA
condition with the maturation of the gut immune system and the establishment of
oral tolerance. We also observed the less prevalence of NFA in older (>6 years) ASD
children (unpublished observation).
Given our findings, it is possible that aberrant innate immune responses may
provoke undesired immune reactivity against commensal flora in ASD children as
258 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
observed in IBD patients. In the studies of children who underwent the elimination
diet, we found the decline of the cellular reactivity to milk proteins but persistent
reactivity to candida Ag, a representative microbial luminal Ag (Jyonouchi et al.,
2007). Taken together, our data indicate a role of NFA in GI symptoms in some ASD
children in association with aberrant innate immune responses.
In summary, there exist convincing data supporting the presence of chronic GI
inflammation in ASD children, but the etiology of GI inflammation, which is likely
affected by multiple genetic and environmental factors, is still not well understood.
NFA can partly explain the GI symptoms and beneficial effects of dietary interven-
tions in some ASD children. However, aberrant innate immune responses, if present
as suggested by our data, likely affect immune reactivity not only to food protein but
also to commensal flora. Apparent effects of oral vancomycin and altered commensal
flora reported in ASD children may be explained partly by aberrant innate immune
responses. Further studies will be required to understand a role of innate immunity in
the GI symptoms observed in ASD children.
13.3.2 CNS
Autism has been considered as a heterogeneous neuro-developmental disorder
defined by behavioral symptoms and in a majority of patients, the development of
autism is likely affected by multiple genetic and possibly environmental factors
starting from fetal life. A role of the immune system in the brain pathology has been
controversial secondary to the lack of clear evidence of immune reactions unlike
other neurodegenerative diseases. However, recent studies indicate the evidence of
chronic mild inflammation in the CNS in autism patients.
of GABRγ1 and other genes located on chromosome 15q11–13 are reported to be risk
factors for autism (Ashley-Koch et al., 2006; Vincent et al., 2006). On the other hand,
Tochigi et al. (2007) reported no association between GABR genes in 15q11–13 and
autism in a Japanese population. In the periphery, GABA also serves as a neurotrans-
mitter as well as a hormone like factor in both peripheral nervous system and endocrine
organs. For example, GABA increases insulin secretion (Gladkevich et al., 2006).
In type 1 diabetes, a T-cell-mediated organ-specific autoimmune disease, GABA
expression is down-regulated with the presence of autoantibodies against GABA and
glutamic acid decarboxylate (GAD) 65 that catalyzes glutamate to GABA and carbon
dioxide by decarboxylation (Gladkevich et al., 2006). GAD65 autoantibody is also
known to be present in patients with stiff person’s syndrome (SPS), a rare autoimmune
neurological disorder. In SPS patients, others report the presence of autoantibody against
GABAA-receptor (Raju et al., 2006). Innate immune cells do also express GABAA
receptors and selective GABAA receptor agonist was shown to activate macrophages
independent of TLRs in vitro and in vivo (Lubick et al., 2007). Environmental expo-
sures to chemicals that inhibit GABA neurotransmission can thus potentially cause
various symptoms affecting nervous, endocrine, and immune systems in genetically
vulnerable individuals. However, it is not known how such environmental exposure has
a role in the development of autism.
agonist, infection with influenza in the late second trimester revealed significant
changes in the expression of genes implicated with autism and schizophrenia in the
brain in addition to brain atrophy (Fatemi et al., 2008). Interestingly, authors also
reported altered levels of serotonin, 5-HT, and taurine in this model; they report
decreased levels of serotonin in the cerebella of offspring at postnatal day 14 but not
at day 56 (Winter et al., 2008). Elevated levels of serum serotonin has been reported
in a subset of autistic children (Burgess et al., 2006), while low plasma serotonin lev-
els in mothers of autistic children was indicated as a risk factor for autism (Connors
et al., 2006). These results indicate that altered serotonin levels may be associated
with prenatal immune insult.
The pitfalls of the above-described rodent models are that the growth pattern of
the brain differs in humans and rodents. The timing of in utero exposure in these
animal models is considered to be still in the early stage of gestation in humans even
if in utero challenge occurs in the late pregnancy in rodents. It will be necessary to
collect data in pregnant women and outcomes of neuropsychiatric functions in off-
spring prospectively.
In studying the gene expression of whole peripheral blood from children with
autism or ASD, the up-regulation of genes associated with innate immunity has
been reported, albeit up-regulation is modest (about twofold) (Gregg et al., 2008).
These genes are those expressed by natural killer (NK) cells and many of them
belong to the NK cytotoxicity pathways (Gregg et al., 2008). NK cells are one of the
major effector cells in innate immunity at the time of viral syndrome. Such findings
may support a role of innate immunity in the development of autism. However, such
changes were found in both ASD and autism patients (Gregg et al., 2008) and it is
unclear whether such changes are more significantly observed in a subset of ASD
children with apparent susceptibility to recurrent infection or regression triggered
by microbial infection.
Previously, we retrospectively reviewed ASD subjects seen in our clinic for the past
3 years with regard to infection-induced exacerbation. We defined such exacerbation
as significant exacerbation in behavioral symptoms as well as the loss of once-acquired
cognitive skills documented by caretakers/teachers/therapists independent of parents,
excluding initial regression. With such definition, up to 10% of ASD children falls
into this category and this does not seem to be associated with atopy or autism sub-
types (autism, PDD-NOS, or ASD) (Table 13.2). The diagnosis of primary immunode-
ficiency such as specific polysaccharide antibody deficiency is not associated with this
subset of ASD children. Our findings indicate a more potent role of innate immunity in
TABLE 13.2
Prevalence of Microbial Infection–Induced
Exacerbationb in ASD Children Evaluated
in the Pediatric Allergy/Immunology Clinic
Infection-Induced Exacerbation
ASD without atopya
Autisma 9/107 (8.4%)
PDD-NOS 6/84 (7.1%)
ASD 2/32 (6.3%)
ASD with atopy
Autism 4/44 (9.1%)
PDD-NOS 1/43 (2.3%)
ASD 1/11 (9.1%)
ASD children with the above-described clinical features and in such children, genetic
susceptibility may be defined by altered innate immune responses.
It is also of note that when we tested the effects of the elimination diet in ASD
children with NFA (mainly to milk protein), we observed the decline of cellular
reactivity to offending food protein, but persistent altered responses to LPS, a TLR4
agonist, in ASD/NFA children but not in non-ASD/NFA children. Such findings may
also indicate the presence of aberrant innate immune responses affecting responses
to immune insult after birth.
13.5 CONCLUSIONS
No definite evidence exits that the innate immunity affects the development of autism.
However, the clinical and laboratory findings described in this chapter appear suf-
ficient to support a role of innate immunity in some autistic children. Further studies
in a well-defined subset of ASD children indicative of innate immune abnormali-
ties will help in defining the role of innate immunity in the development/progress
of autism.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
The author is thankful to Dr. L. Huguenin for critically reviewing this chapter.
ABBREVIATIONS
5-HT 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid
ASD autism spectrum disorders
Ag antigen
APC Ag-presenting cells
BBB blood brain barrier
BMDM bone marrow–derived microglial
CAs catecholamines
CD Crohn’s disease
cf/gf casein-free, gluten-free
CNS central nervous system
CSF cerebrospinal fluid
DCs dendritic cells
FA food allergy
GABA gamma-aminobutyric acid
GABR GABA receptors
GAD glutamic acid decarboxylate
GALT gut-associated lymphoid tissue
GCs glucocorticoids
GI gastrointestinal
HPA hypophysis-pituitary-adrenal
IBD inflammatory bowel disease
Ig immunoglobulin
266 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
IFN interferon
IL interleukin
LNH lymphoid nodular hyperplasia
LP lamina propria
LPS lipopolysaccharide
MCP-1 macrophage chemoattractant protein-1
MHC major histocompatibility complex
MS multiple sclerosis
nAChR nicotinic acetylcholine receptor
NFA non-IgE-mediated food allergy
NK natural killer
Nod2 nucleotide oligomerizing domain 2
PAMPs pathogen associated molecular patterns
PBMCs peripheral blood mononuclear cells
PGN proteoglycans
PP Peyer’s patches
PRRs pattern recognition receptors
SPS stiff person’s syndrome
TGF transforming growth factor
Th T-helper
TLR Toll-like receptors
TNF tumor necrosis factor
Treg regulatory T cells
aTreg adaptive Treg
REFERENCES
Abbott, N.J., Ronnback, L., and Hansson, E. 2006. Astrocyte-endothelial interactions at the
blood-brain barrier. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7:41–53.
Abreu, M.T., Fukata, M., and Arditi, M. 2005. TLR signaling in the gut in health and disease.
J. Immunol. 174:4453–4460.
Acosta-Rodriguez, E.V., Rivino, L., Geginat, J., Jarrossay, D., Gattorno, M., Lanzavecchia, A.,
Sallusto, F., and Napolitani, G. 2007. Surface phenotype and antigenic specificity of
human interleukin 17-producing T helper memory cells. Nat. Immunol. 8:639–646.
Arndt, T.L., Stodgell, C.J., and Rodier, P.M. 2005. The teratology of autism. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci.
23:189–199.
Ashley-Koch, A.E., Mei, H., Jaworski, J., Ma, D.Q., Ritchie, M.D., Menold, M.M., Delong, G.R.,
Abramson, R.K., Wright, H.H., Hussman, J.P., Cuccaro, M.L., Gilbert J.R., Martin E.R.,
and Pericak-Vance M.A. 2006. An analysis paradigm for investigating multi-locus effects
in complex disease: Examination of three GABA receptor subunit genes on 15q11-q13 as
risk factors for autistic disorder. Ann. Hum. Genet. 70:281–292.
Ashwood, P., Anthony, A., Pellicer, A.A., Torrente, F., Walker-Smith, J.A., and Wakefield, A.J.
2003. Intestinal lymphocyte populations in children with regressive autism: Evidence
for extensive mucosal immunopathology. J. Clin. Immunol. 23:504–517.
Ashwood, P., Anthony, A., Torrente, F., and Wakefield, A.J. 2004. Spontaneous mucosal
lymphocyte cytokine profiles in children with autism and gastrointestinal symptoms:
Mucosal immune activation and reduced counter regulatory interleukin-10. J. Clin.
Immunol. 24:664–673.
Possible Impact of Innate Immunity on Autism 267
Ashwood, P. and Wakefield, A.J. 2006. Immune activation of peripheral blood and mucosal
CD3+ lymphocyte cytokine profiles in children with autism and gastrointestinal symp-
toms. J. Neuroimmunol. 173:126–134.
Ashwood, P., Wills, S., and Van de Water, J. 2006. The immune response in autism: A new
frontier for autism research. J. Leukoc. Biol. 80:1–15.
Ashwood, P., Kwong, C., Hansen, R., Hertz-Picciotto, I., Croen, L., Krakowiak, P., Walker, W.,
Pessah, I.N., and Van de Water, J. 2008. Brief report: Plasma leptin levels are elevated in
autism: Association with early onset phenotype? J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38:169–175.
Astier, A.L. and Hafler, D.A. 2007. Abnormal Tr1 differentiation in multiple sclerosis.
J. Neuroimmunol. 191:70–78.
Bacchetta, R., Bigler, M., Touraine, J.L., Parkman, R., Tovo, P.A., Abrams, J., de Waal Malefyt, R.,
de Vries, J.E., and Roncarolo, M.G. 1994. High levels of interleukin 10 production in
vivo are associated with tolerance in SCID patients transplanted with HLA mismatched
hematopoietic stem cells. J. Exp. Med. 179:493–502.
Bakkaloglu, B., Anlar, A.B., Anlar, F.Y., Oktem, F., Pehlivantürk, B., Unal, F., Ozbesler, C.,
and Gökler, B. 2007. Atopic features in early childhood autism. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol.
12:476–479.
Bashir, M.E., Louie, S., Shi, H.N., and Nagler-Anderson, C. 2004. Toll-like receptor 4 sig-
naling by intestinal microbes influences susceptibility to food allergy. J. Immunol.
172:6978–6987.
Batten, M., Li, J., Yi, S., Kljavin, N.M., Danilenko, D.M., Lucas, S., Lee, J., de Sauvage, F.J.,
and Ghilardi, N. 2006. Interleukin 27 limits autoimmune encephalomyelitis by
suppressing the development of interleukin 17-producing T cells. Nat. Immunol.
7:929–936.
Benlounes, N., Candalh, C., Matarazzo, P., Dupont, C., and Heyman, M. 1999. The time-
course of milk antigen-induced TNF-alpha secretion differs according to the clinical
symptoms in children with cow’s milk allergy. J. Allergy Clin. Immunol. 104:863–869.
Bettelli, E., Korn, T., and Kuchroo, V.K. 2007. Th17: The third member of the effector T cell
trilogy. Curr. Opin. Immunol. 19:652–657.
Blotta, M.H., DeKruyff, R.H., and Umetsu, D.T. 1997. Corticosteroids inhibit IL-12 production
in human monocytes and enhance their capacity to induce IL-4 synthesis in CD4+
lymphocytes. J. Immunol. 158:5589–5595.
Bode, L., Salvestrini, C., Park, P.W., Li, J.P., Esko, J.D., Yamaguchi, Y., Murch, S., and Freeze, H.H.
2008. Heparan sulfate and syndecan-1 are essential in maintaining murine and human
intestinal epithelial barrier function. J. Clin. Invest. 118:229–238.
Burgess, N.K., Sweeten, T.L., McMahon, W.M., and Fujinami, R.S. 2006. Hyperserotoninemia
and altered immunity in autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36:697–704.
Caramalho, I., Lopes-Carvalho, T., Ostler, D., Zelenay, S., Haury, M., and Demengeot, J. 2003.
Regulatory T cells selectively express toll-like receptors and are activated by lipopoly-
saccharide. J. Exp. Med. 197:403–411.
Chatila, T.A. 2005. Role of regulatory T cells in human diseases. J. Allergy Clin. Immunol.
116:949–959.
Chatila, T.A., Li, N., Garcia-Lloret, M., Kim, H.J., and Nel, A.E. 2008. T-cell effector pathways
in allergic diseases: Transcriptional mechanisms and therapeutic targets. J. Allergy Clin.
Immunol. 121:812–823.
Chauhan, A. and Chauhan, V. 2006. Oxidative stress in autism. Pathophysiology 13:171–181.
Chauhan, A., Chauhan, V., Brown, W.T., and Cohen, I. 2004. Oxidative stress in autism:
Increased lipid peroxidation and reduced serum levels of ceruloplasmin and transferrin—
the antioxidant proteins. Life Sci. 75:2539–2549.
Cheslack-Postava, K., Fallin, M.D., Avramopoulos, D., Connors, S.L., Zimmerman, A.W.,
Eberhart, C.G., and Newschaffer, C.J. 2007. Beta2-Adrenergic receptor gene variants
and risk for autism in the AGRE cohort. Mol. Psychiatry 12:283–291.
268 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Chez, M.G., Dowling, T., Patel, P.B., Khanna, P., and Kominsky, M. 2007. Elevation of
tumor necrosis factor-alpha in cerebrospinal fluid of autistic children. Pediatr. Neurol.
36:361–365.
Connors, S.L., Matteson, K.J., Sega, G.A., Lozzio, C.B., Carroll, R.C., and Zimmerman, A.W.
2006. Plasma serotonin in autism. Pediatr. Neurol. 35:182–186.
Coombes, J.L., Siddiqui, K.R., Arancibia-Carcamo, C.V., Hall, J., Sun, C.M., Belkaid, Y.,
and Powrie, F. 2007. A functionally specialized population of mucosal CD103+ DCs
induces Foxp3+ regulatory T cells via a TGF-beta and retinoic acid-dependent mechanism.
J. Exp. Med. 204:1757–1764.
Corbett, B.A., Mendoza, S., Wegelin, J.A., Carmean, V., and Levine, S. 2008. Variable cortisol
circadian rhythms in children with autism and anticipatory stress. J. Psychiatry Neurosci.
33:227–234.
Crack, P.J. and Bray, P.J. 2007. Toll-like receptors in the brain and their potential roles in
neuropathology. Immunol. Cell. Biol. 85:476–480.
Croen, L.A., Braunschweig, D., Haapanen, L., Yoshida, C.K., Fireman, B., Grether, J.K.,
Kharrazi, M., Hansen, R.L., Ashwood, P., and Van de Water, J. 2008. Maternal mid-
pregnancy autoantibodies to fetal brain protein: The early markers for autism study.
Biol. Psychiatry 64:583–588.
Dai, D., Nanthkumar, N.N., Newburg, D.S., and Walker, W.A. 2000. Role of oligosaccha-
rides and glycoconjugates in intestinal host defense. J. Pediar. Gastroenterol. Nutr.
30(Suppl. 2):S23–S33.
Davoust, N., Vuaillat, C., Androdias, G., and Nataf, S. 2008. From bone marrow to microglia:
Barriers and avenues. Trends Immunol. 29:227–234.
de Jonge, W.J. and Ulloa, L. 2007. The alpha7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor as a pharma-
cological target for inflammation. Br. J. Pharmacol. 151:915–929.
de Vries, E., de Bruin-Versteeg, S., Comans-Bitter, W.M., de Groot, R., Hop, W.C., Boerma, G.J.,
Lotgering, F.K., and van Dongen, J.J. 2000. Longitudinal survey of lymphocyte sub-
populations in the first year of life. Pediatr. Res. 47:528–537.
Deaglio, S., Dwyer, K.M., Gao, W., Friedman, D., Usheva, A., Erat, A., Chen, J.F., Enjyoji, K.,
Linden, J., Oukka, M., Kuchroo, V.K., Strom, T.B., and Robson, S.C. 2007. Adenosine
generation catalyzed by CD39 and CD73 expressed on regulatory T cells mediates
immune suppression. J. Exp. Med. 204:1257–1265.
DeFelice, M.L., Ruchelli, E.D., Markowitz, J.E., Strogatz, M., Reddy, K.P., Kadivar, K.,
Mulberg, A.E., and Brown, K.A. 2003. Intestinal cytokines in children with pervasive
developmental disorders. Am. J. Gastroenterol. 98:1777–1782.
Denning, T.L., Wang, Y.C., Patel, S.R., Williams, I.R., and Pulendran, B. 2007. Lamina pro-
pria macrophages and dendritic cells differentially induce regulatory and interleukin
17-producing T cell responses. Nat. Immunol. 8:1086–1094.
Deth, R., Muratore, C., Benzecry, J., Power-Charnitsky, V.A., and Waly, M. 2008. How envi-
ronmental and genetic factors combine to cause autism: A redox/methylation hypothesis.
Neurotoxicology 29:190–201.
Di Cristo, G. 2007. Development of cortical GABAergic circuits and its implications for neu-
rodevelopmental disorders. Clin. Genet. 72:1–8.
Djukic, M., Mildner, A., Schmidt, H., Czesnik, D., Bruck, W., Priller, J., Nau, R., and Prinz, M.
2006. Circulating monocytes engraft in the brain, differentiate into microglia and con-
tribute to the pathology following meningitis in mice. Brain 129:2394–2403.
Dupont, C. and Heyman, M. 2000. Food protein-induced enterocolitis syndrome: Laboratory
perspectives. J. Pediatr. Gastroenterol. Nutr. 30:S50–S57.
Elder, J.H., Shankar, M., Shuster, J., Theriaque, D., Burns, S., and Sherrill, L. 2006. The gluten-
free, casein-free diet in autism: Results of a preliminary double blind clinical trial. J. Autism
Dev. Disord. 36:413–420.
Possible Impact of Innate Immunity on Autism 269
Hausmann, M., Kiessling, S., Mestermann, S., Webb, G., Spottl, T., Andus, T., Scholmerich, J.,
Herfarth, H., Ray, K., Falk, W., and Rogler, G. 2002. Toll-like receptors 2 and 4 are
up-regulated during intestinal inflammation. Gastroenterology 122:1987–2000.
Hornef, M.W., Frisan, T., Vandewalle, A., Normark, S., and Richter-Dahlfors, A. 2002. Toll-like
receptor 4 resides in the Golgi apparatus and colocalizes with internalized lipopolysac-
charide in intestinal epithelial cells. J. Exp. Med. 195:559–570.
Hornef, M.W., Normark, B.H., Vandewalle, A., and Normark, S. 2003. Intracellular recognition
of lipopolysaccharide by toll-like receptor 4 in intestinal epithelial cells. J. Exp. Med.
198:1225–1235.
Horvath, K., Papadimitriou, J.C., Rabsztyn, A., Drachenberg, C., and Tildon, J.T. 1999. Gastro-
intestinal abnormalities in children with autistic disorder. J. Pediatr. 135:559–563.
Inohara, N., Ogura, Y., Fontalba, A., Gutierrez, O., Pons, F., Crespo, J., Fukase, K., Inamura, S.,
Kusumoto, S., Hashimoto, M., Foster, S.L, Moran A.P., Fernandez-Luna, J.L., and
Nurez, G. 2003. Host recognition of bacterial muramyl dipeptide mediated through
NOD2. Implications for Crohn’s disease. J. Biol. Chem. 278:5509–5512.
Iwata, M., Hirakiyama, A., Eshima, Y., Kagechika, H., Kato, C., and Song, S.Y. 2004. Retinoic
acid imprints gut-homing specificity on T cells. Immunity 21:527–538.
James, S.J., Melnyk, S., Jernigan, S., Cleves, M.A., Halsted, C.H., Wong, D.H., Cutler, P.,
Bock, K., Boris, M., Bradstreet, J.J., Baker, S.M., and Gaylor D.W. 2006. Metabolic
endophenotype and related genotypes are associated with oxidative stress in children
with autism. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 141B:947–956.
Jyonouchi H, Geng, L., Ruby, A., and Reddy, C. 2007. Suboptimal responses to dietary
intervention in children with autism spectrum disorders and non-IgE mediated food
allergy. In Autism Research Advances, ed. L. B. Zhao, pp. 169–184. Nova Science
Publishers, Inc, Hauppauge, NY.
Jyonouchi, H., Geng, L., Ruby, A., Reddy, C., and Zimmerman-Bier, B. 2005a. Evaluation of an
association between gastrointestinal symptoms and cytokine production against common
dietary proteins in children with autism spectrum disorders. J. Pediatr. 146:605–610.
Jyonouchi, H., Geng, L., Ruby, A., and Zimmerman-Bier, B. 2005b. Dysregulated innate
immune responses in young children with autism spectrum disorders: Their relationship
to gastrointestinal symptoms and dietary intervention. Neuropsychobiology 51:77–85.
Jyonouchi, H., Sun, S., and Itokazu, N. 2002. Innate immunity associated with inflammatory
responses and cytokine production against common dietary proteins in patients with
autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychobiology 46:76–84.
Kang, S.G., Lim, H.W., Andrisani, O.M., Broxmeyer, H.E., and Kim, C.H. 2007. Vitamin A
metabolites induce gut-homing FoxP3+ regulatory T cells. J. Immunol. 179:3724–3733.
Karlsson, M.R., Rugtveit, J., and Brandtzaeg, P. 2004. Allergen-responsive CD4+ CD25+
regulatory T cells in children who have outgrown cow’s milk allergy. J. Exp. Med.
199:1679–1688.
Kelley, E., Paul, J.J., Fein, D., and Naigles, L.R. 2006. Residual language deficits in optimal
outcome children with a history of autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36:807–828.
Kelly, D., Campbell, J.I., King, T.P., Grant, G., Jansson, E.A., Coutts, A.G., Pettersson, S.,
and Conway, S. 2004. Commensal anaerobic gut bacteria attenuate inflammation by
regulating nuclear-cytoplasmic shuttling of PPAR-gamma and RelA. Nat. Immunol.
5:104–112.
Kelly, D., Conway, S., and Aminov, R. 2005. Commensal gut bacteria: Mechanisms of immune
modulation. Trends Immunol. 26:326–333.
Kleinman, J.M., Ventola, P.E., Pandey, J., Verbalis, A.D., Barton, M., Hodgson, S., Green, J.,
Dumont-Mathieu, T., Robins, D.L., and Fein, D. 2008. Diagnostic stability in very
young children with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38:606–615.
Knivsberg, A.M., Reichelt, K.L., Hoien, T., and Nodland, M. 2002. A randomised, controlled
study of dietary intervention in autistic syndromes. Nutr. Neurosci. 5:251–261.
Possible Impact of Innate Immunity on Autism 271
Kohm, A.P., McMahon, J.S., Podojil, J.R., Begolka, W.S., DeGutes, M., Kasprowicz, D.J.,
Ziegler, S.F., and Miller, S.D. 2006. Cutting edge: Anti-CD25 monoclonal antibody
injection results in the functional inactivation, not depletion, of CD4+ CD25+ T regula-
tory cells. J. Immunol. 176:3301–3305.
Korn, T., Reddy, J., Gao, W., Bettelli, E., Awasthi, A., Petersen, T.R., Backstrom, B.T.,
Sobel, R.A., Wucherpfennig, K.W., Strom, T.B., Oukka M., and Kuchroo V.K. 2007.
Myelin-specific regulatory T cells accumulate in the CNS but fail to control autoim-
mune inflammation. Nat. Med. 13:423–431.
Laflamme, N. and Rivest, S. 2001. Toll-like receptor 4: The missing link of the cerebral innate
immune response triggered by circulating gram-negative bacterial cell wall components.
FASEB J. 15:155–163.
Lainhart, J.E., Ozonoff, S., Coon, H., Krasny, L., Dinh, E., Nice, J., and McMahon, W. 2002.
Autism, regression, and the broader autism phenotype. Am. J. Med. Genet. 113:231–237.
Lala, S., Ogura, Y., Osborne, C., Hor, S.Y., Bromfield, A., Davies, S., Ogunbiyi, O., Nunez, G.,
and Keshav, S. 2003. Crohn’s disease and the NOD2 gene: A role for paneth cells.
Gastroenterology 125:47–57.
Lee, J., Gonzales-Navajas, J.M., and Raz, E. 2008. The “polarizing-tolerizing” mechanism of
intestinal epithelium: Its relevance to colonic homeostasis. Semin. Immunopathol. 30:3–9.
Lee, J., Mo, J.H., Shen, C., Rucker, A.N., and Raz, E. 2007. Toll-like receptor signaling in
intestinal epithelial cells contributes to colonic homoeostasis. Curr. Opin. Gastroenterol.
23, 27–31.
Levy, S.E. and Hyman, S.L. 2005. Novel treatments for autistic spectrum disorders. Ment.
Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 11:131–142.
Liu, Y., Teige, I., Birnir, B., and Issazadeh-Navikas, S. 2006. Neuron-mediated generation of
regulatory T cells from encephalitogenic T cells suppresses EAE. Nat. Med. 12:518–525.
Lu, L.F., Lind, E.F., Gondek, D.C., Bennett, K.A., Gleeson, M.W., Pino-Lagos, K., Scott, Z.A.,
Coyle, A.J., Reed, J.L., and Van Snick, J. 2006. Mast cells are essential intermediaries in
regulatory T-cell tolerance. Nature 442:997–1002.
Lubick, K., Radke, M., and Jutila, M. 2007. Securinine, a GABAA receptor antagonist,
enhances macrophage clearance of phase II C. burnetii: Comparison with TLR agonists.
J. Leukoc. Biol. 82:1062–1069.
Manel, N., Unutmaz, D., and Littman, D.R. 2008. The differentiation of human T(H)-17
cells requires transforming growth factor-beta and induction of the nuclear receptor
RORgammat. Nat. Immunol. 9:641–649.
Marin-Teva, J.L., Dusart, I., Colin, C., Gervais, A., van Rooijen, N., and Mallat, M. (2004).
Microglia promote the death of developing Purkinje cells. Neuron 41:535–547.
Martin-Ruiz, C.M., Lee, M., Perry, R.H., Baumann, M., Court, J.A., and Perry, E.K. 2004.
Molecular analysis of nicotinic receptor expression in autism. Brain Res. Mol. Brain
Res. 123:81–90.
McGeachy, M.J. and Cua, D.J. 2008. Th17 cell differentiation: The long and winding road.
Immunity 28:445–453.
Meyer, U., Nyffeler, M., Schwendener, S., Knuesel, I., Yee, B.K., and Feldon, J. 2008a. Relative
prenatal and postnatal maternal contributions to schizophrenia-related neurochemical
dysfunction after in utero immune challenge. Neuropsychopharmacology 33:441–456.
Meyer, U., Nyffeler, M., Yee, B.K., Knuesel, I., and Feldon, J. 2008b. Adult brain and behav-
ioral pathological markers of prenatal immune challenge during early/middle and late
fetal development in mice. Brain Behav. Immun. 22:469–486.
Meylan, E., Tschopp, J., and Karin, M. 2006. Intracellular pattern recognition receptors in the
host response. Nature 442:39–44.
Molloy, C.A., Morrow, A.L., Meinzen-Derr, J., Schleifer, K., Dienger, K., Manning-Courtney, P.,
Altaye, M., and Wills-Karp, M. 2006. Elevated cytokine levels in children with autism
spectrum disorder. J. Neuroimmunol. 172:198–205.
272 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Mouridsen, S.E., Rich, B., Isager, T., and Nedergaard, N.J. 2007. Autoimmune diseases in
parents of children with infantile autism: A case-control study. Dev. Med. Child Neurol.
49:429–432.
Mucida, D., Park, Y., Kim, G., Turovskaya, O., Scott, I., Kronenberg, M., and Cheroutre, H.
2007. Reciprocal TH17 and regulatory T cell differentiation mediated by retinoic acid.
Science 317:256–260.
Murch, S.H., Winyard, P.J., Koletzko, S., Wehner, B., Cheema, H.A., Risdon, R.A., Phillips, A.D.,
Meadows, N., Klein, N.J., and Walker-Smith, J.A. 1996. Congenital enterocyte heparan
sulphate deficiency with massive albumin loss, secretory diarrhoea, and malnutrition.
Lancet 347:1299–1301.
Neish, A.S., Gewirtz, A.T., Zeng, H., Young, A.N., Hobert, M.E., Karmali, V., Rao, A.S.,
and Madara, J.L. 2000. Prokaryotic regulation of epithelial responses by inhibition of
IkappaB-alpha ubiquitination. Science 289:1560–1563.
Ni, L., Acevedo, G., Muralidharan, B., Padala, N., To, J., and Jonakait, G.M. 2007. Toll-like
receptor ligands and CD154 stimulate microglia to produce a factor(s) that promotes
excess cholinergic differentiation in the developing rat basal forebrain: Implications for
neurodevelopmental disorders. Pediatr. Res. 61:15–20.
O’Connor, R.A. and Anderton, S.M. 2008. Foxp3+ regulatory T cells in the control of experi-
mental CNS autoimmune disease. J. Neuroimmunol. 193:1–11.
O’Connor, R.A., Malpass, K.H., and Anderton, S.M. 2007. The inflamed central nervous sys-
tem drives the activation and rapid proliferation of Foxp3+ regulatory T cells. J. Immunol.
179:958–966.
Ogura, Y., Bonen, D.K., Inohara, N., Nicolae, D.L., Chen, F.F., Ramos, R., Britton, H., Moran, T.,
Karaliuskas, R., Duerr, R.H., Achkar, J.P., Brant, S.P., Hanauer, S.P., Nunez, G.,
and Cho, J.H. 2001. A frameshift mutation in NOD2 associated with susceptibility to
Crohn’s disease. Nature 411:603–606.
Oliveira, G., Ataide, A., Marques, C., Miguel, T.S., Coutinho, A.M., Mota-Vieira, L.,
Goncalves, E., Lopes, N.M., Rodrigues, V., Carmona da Mota, H., and Vincente, A.M.
2007. Epidemiology of autism spectrum disorder in Portugal: Prevalence, clinical
characterization, and medical conditions. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 49:726–733.
Olson, J.K. and Miller, S.D. 2004. Microglia initiate central nervous system innate and
adaptive immune responses through multiple TLRs. J. Immunol. 173:3916–3924.
Otte, J.M., Cario, E., and Podolsky, D.K. 2004. Mechanisms of cross hyporesponsiveness
to Toll-like receptor bacterial ligands in intestinal epithelial cells. Gastroenterology
126:1054–1070.
Pardo, C.A. and Eberhart, C.G. 2007. The neurobiology of autism. Brain Pathol. 17:434–447.
Pardo, C.A., Vargas, D.L., and Zimmerman, A.W. 2005. Immunity, neuroglia and neuroinflam-
mation in autism. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 17:485–495.
Parracho, H.M., Bingham, M.O., Gibson, G.R., and McCartney, A.L. 2005. Differences
between the gut microflora of children with autistic spectrum disorders and that of
healthy children. J. Med. Microbiol. 54:987–991.
Pessah, I.N., Seegal, R.F., Lein, P.J., LaSalle, J., Yee, B.K., Van De Water, J., and Berman, R.F.
2008. Immunologic and neurodevelopmental susceptibilities of autism. Neurotoxicology
29:532–545.
Pickering, M. and O’Connor, J.J. 2007. Pro-inflammatory cytokines and their effects in the
dentate gyrus. Prog. Brain Res. 163:339–354.
Poling, J.S., Frye, R.E., Shoffner, J., and Zimmerman, A.W. 2006. Developmental regression
and mitochondrial dysfunction in a child with autism. J. Child Neurol. 21:170–172.
Porter, E.M., Bevins, C.L., Ghosh, D., and Ganz, T. 2002. The multifaceted Paneth cell.
Cell. Mol. Life Sci. 59:156–170.
Raju, R., Rakocevic, G., Chen, Z., Hoehn, G., Semino-Mora, C., Shi, W., Olsen, R., and
Dalakas, M.C. 2006. Autoimmunity to GABAA-receptor-associated protein in stiff-
person syndrome. Brain 129:3270–3276.
Possible Impact of Innate Immunity on Autism 273
Robertson, M.A., Sigalet, D.L., Holst, J.J., Meddings, J.B., Wood, J., and Sharkey, K.A. 2008.
Intestinal permeability and glucagon-like peptide-2 in children with autism: A controlled
pilot study. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38:1066–1071.
Rodriguez, M., Alvarez-Erviti, L., Blesa, F.J., Rodriguez-Oroz, M.C., Arina, A., Melero, I.,
Ramos, L.I., and Obeso, J.A. 2007. Bone-marrow-derived cell differentiation into
microglia: A study in a progressive mouse model of Parkinson’s disease. Neurobiol.
Dis. 28:316–325.
Rodriguez-Bores, L., Fonseca, G.C., Villeda, M.A., and Yamamoto-Furusho, J.K. 2007.
Novel genetic markers in inflammatory bowel disease. World J. Gastroenterol.
13:5560–5570.
Rosenstiel, P., Fantini, M., Brautigam, K., Kuhbacher, T., Waetzig, G.H., Seegert, D.,
and Schreiber, S. 2003. TNF-alpha and IFN-gamma regulate the expression of
the NOD2 (CARD15) gene in human intestinal epithelial cells. Gastroenterology
124:1001–1009.
Roumier, A., Bechade, C., Poncer, J.C., Smalla, K.H., Tomasello, E., Vivier, E., Gundelfinger, E.D.,
Triller, A., and Bessis, A. 2004. Impaired synaptic function in the microglial KARAP/
DAP12-deficient mouse. J. Neurosci. 24:11421–11428.
Sajdel-Sulkowska, E.M., Lipinski, L.B., Windom, H., Audhya, T., and McGinnis, W. 2008.
Oxidative stress in autism: Elevated cerebellar 3-nitrotyrosine levels. Am. J. Biochem.
Biotechnol. (Special Issue on Autism Spectrum Disorders) 4:73–84.
Sandler, R.H., Finegold, S.M., Bolte, E.R., Buchanan, C.P., Maxwell, A.P., Vaisanen, M.L.,
Nelson, M.N., and Wexler, H.M. 2000. Short-term benefit from oral vancomycin treat-
ment of regressive-onset autism. J. Child Neurol. 15:429–435.
Schmidt-Weber, C.B., Akdis, M., and Akdis, C.A. 2007. TH17 cells in the big picture of
immunology. J. Allergy Clin. Immunol. 120:247–254.
Shimizu, H., Watanabe, E., Hiyama, T.Y., Nagakura, A., Fujikawa, A., Okado, H., Yanagawa, Y.,
Obata, K., and Noda, M. 2007. Glial Nax channels control lactate signaling to neurons
for brain [Na+] sensing. Neuron 54:59–72.
Shultz, S.R., MacFabe, D.F., Ossenkopp, K.P., Scratch, S., Whelan, J., Taylor, R., and Cain, D.P.
2008. Intracerebroventricular injection of propionic acid, an enteric bacterial metabolic
end-product, impairs social behavior in the rat: Implications for an animal model of
autism. Neuropharmacology 54:901–911.
Simard, A.R., Soulet, D., Gowing, G., Julien, J.P., and Rivest, S. 2006. Bone marrow-derived
microglia play a critical role in restricting senile plaque formation in Alzheimer’s disease.
Neuron 49:489–502.
Singer, H.S., Morris, C.M., Gause, C.D., Gillin, P.K., Crawford, S., and Zimmerman, A.W. 2008.
Antibodies against fetal brain in sera of mothers with autistic children. J. Neuroimmunol.
194:165–172.
Singer, H.S., Morris, C.M., Williams, P.N., Yoon, D.Y., Hong, J.J., and Zimmerman, A.W.
2006. Antibrain antibodies in children with autism and their unaffected siblings.
J. Neuroimmunol. 178:149–155.
Sitkovsky, M., Lukashev, D., Deaglio, S., Dwyer, K., Robson, S.C., and Ohta, A. 2008.
Adenosine A2A receptor antagonists: Blockade of adenosinergic effects and T regulatory
cells. Br. J. Pharmacol. 153(Suppl. 1):S457–S464.
Smith, D.W. and Nagler-Anderson, C. 2005. Preventing intolerance: The induction of non-
responsiveness to dietary and microbial antigens in the intestinal mucosa. J. Immunol.
174:3851–3857.
Smith, P.D., Smythies, L.E., Mosteller-Barnum, M., Sibley, D.A., Russell, M.W., Merger, M.,
Sellers, M.T., Orenstein, J.M., Shimada, T., Graham, M.F., and Kubagawa, H. 2001.
Intestinal macrophages lack CD14 and CD89 and consequently are down-regulated for
LPS- and IgA-mediated activities. J. Immunol. 167:2651–2656.
Smith, S.E., Li, J., Garbett, K., Mirnics, K., and Patterson, P.H. 2007. Maternal immune activation
alters fetal brain development through interleukin-6. J. Neurosci. 27:10695–10702.
274 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Splawski, I., Timothy, K.W., Sharpe, L.M., Decher, N., Kumar, P., Bloise, R., Napolitano, C.,
Schwartz, P.J., Joseph, R.M., Condouris, K., Tager-Flusberg, H., Driori, S.G., Sanguinetti,
M.D., and Keating, M.T. 2004. Ca(V)1.2 calcium channel dysfunction causes a multi-
system disorder including arrhythmia and autism. Cell 119:19–31.
Streit, W.J., Walter, S.A., and Pennell, N.A. 1999. Reactive microgliosis. Prog. Neurobiol.
57:563–581.
Stumhofer, J.S., Laurence, A., Wilson, E.H., Huang, E., Tato, C.M., Johnson, L.M., Villarino, A.V.,
Huang, Q., Yoshimura, A., Sehy, D., Saris, C.J., O’Shea, J.J., Henninghausen, L., Ernst,
M., and Hunter, C.A. 2006. Interleukin 27 negatively regulates the development of inter-
leukin 17-producing T helper cells during chronic inflammation of the central nervous
system. Nat. Immunol. 7:937–945.
Sun, C.M., Hall, J.A., Blank, R.B., Bouladoux, N., Oukka, M., Mora, J.R., and Belkaid, Y.
2007. Small intestine lamina propria dendritic cells promote de novo generation of
Foxp3 T reg cells via retinoic acid. J. Exp. Med. 204:1775–1785.
Sutera, S., Pandey, J., Esser, E.L., Rosenthal, M.A., Wilson, L.B., Barton, M., Green, J.,
Hodgson, S., Robins, D.L., Dumont-Mathieu, T., and Fein, D. 2007. Predictors of
optimal outcome in toddlers diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev.
Disord. 37:98–107.
Sweeten, T.L., Bowyer, S.L., Posey, D.J., Halberstadt, G.M., and McDougle, C.J. 2003a.
Increased prevalence of familial autoimmunity in probands with pervasive developmen-
tal disorders. Pediatrics 112:e420.
Sweeten, T.L., Posey, D.J., and McDougle, C.J. 2003b. High blood monocyte counts and neop-
terin levels in children with autistic disorder. Am. J Psychiatry 160:1691–1693.
Sweeten, T.L., Posey, D.J., Shankar, S., and McDougle, C.J. 2004. High nitric oxide pro-
duction in autistic disorder: A possible role for interferon-gamma. Biol. Psychiatry
55:434–437.
Teitelbaum, J.E. and Allan Walker, W. 2005. The development of mucosal immunity. Eur.
J. Gastroenterol. Hepatol. 17:1273–1278.
Thompson, W.W., Price, C., Goodson, B., Shay, D.K., Benson, P., Hinrichsen, V.L., Lewis, E.,
Eriksen, E., Ray, P., Marcy, S.M., Dunn, J., Jackson, L.A., Lieu, T.A., Black, S.,
Weintraub, E.S., Davis, R.L., and DeStefano, F. 2007. Early thimerosal exposure and
neuropsychological outcomes at 7 to 10 years. N. Engl. J. Med. 357:1281–1292.
Tochigi, M., Kato, C., Koishi, S., Kawakubo, Y., Yamamoto, K., Matsumoto, H., Hashimoto, O.,
Kim, S.Y., Watanabe, K., Kano, Y., Nanba, E., Kato, N., and Sasaki, T. 2007. No evidence
for significant association between GABA receptor genes in chromosome 15q11-q13
and autism in a Japanese population. J. Hum. Genet. 52:985–989.
Torgerson, T.R. and Ochs, H.D. 2007. Immune dysregulation, polyendocrinopathy, enteropathy,
X-linked: Forkhead box protein 3 mutations and lack of regulatory T cells. J. Allergy Clin.
Immunol. 120:744–750.
Torrente, F., Ashwood, P., Day, R., Machado, N., Furlano, R.I., Anthony, A., Davies, S.E.,
Wakefield, A.J., Thomson, M.A., Walker-Smith, J.A., and Murch, S.H. 2002. Small
intestinal enteropathy with epithelial IgG and complement deposition in children with
regressive autism. Mol. Psychiatry 7:375–382.
Vargas, D.L., Nascimbene, C., Krishnan, C., Zimmerman, A.W., and Pardo, C.A. 2005.
Neuroglial activation and neuroinflammation in the brain of patients with autism. Ann.
Neurol. 57:67–81.
Venken, K., Hellings, N., Broekmans, T., Hensen, K., Rummens, J.L., and Stinissen, P. 2008.
Natural naive CD4+ CD25+ CD127low regulatory T cell (Treg) development and func-
tion are disturbed in multiple sclerosis patients: recovery of memory Treg homeostasis
during disease progression. J. Immunol. 180:6411–6420.
Vesce, S., Rossi, D., Brambilla, L., and Volterra, A. 2007. Glutamate release from astrocytes in
physiological conditions and in neurodegenerative disorders characterized by neuroin-
flammation. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 82:57–71.
Possible Impact of Innate Immunity on Autism 275
Vincent, J.B., Horike, S.I., Choufani, S., Paterson, A.D., Roberts, W., Szatmari, P., Weksberg, R.,
Fernandez, B., and Scherer, S.W. 2006. An inversion inv(4)(p12-p15.3) in autistic
siblings implicates the 4p GABA receptor gene cluster. J. Med. Genet. 43:429–434.
Wakefield, A.J., Anthony, A., Murch, S.H., Thomson, M., Montgomery, S.M., Davies, S.,
O’Leary, J.J., Berelowitz, M., and Walker-Smith, J.A. 2000. Enterocolitis in children
with developmental disorders. Am. J. Gastroenterol. 95:2285–2295.
Wakefield, A.J., Ashwood, P., Limb, K., and Anthony, A. 2005. The significance of ileo-
colonic lymphoid nodular hyperplasia in children with autistic spectrum disorder. Eur.
J. Gastroenterol. Hepatol. 17:827–836.
Wannemuehler, M.J., Kiyono, H., Babb, J.L., Michalek, S.M., and McGhee, J.R. 1982.
Lipopolysaccharide (LPS) regulation of the immune response: LPS converts germfree
mice to sensitivity to oral tolerance induction. J. Immunol. 129:959–965.
Wark, P.A., Johnston, S.L., Bucchieri, F., Powell, R., Puddicombe, S., Laza-Stanca, V., Holgate,
S.T., and Davies, D.E. 2005. Asthmatic bronchial epithelial cells have a deficient innate
immune response to infection with rhinovirus. J. Exp. Med. 201:937–947.
Wershil, B.K. and Furuta, G.T. 2008. Gastrointestinal mucosal immunity. J. Allergy Clin.
Immunol. 121:S380–S383.
Westphal, V., Murch, S., Kim, S., Srikrishna, G., Winchester, B., Day, R., and Freeze, H.H.
2000. Reduced heparan sulfate accumulation in enterocytes contributes to protein-losing
enteropathy in a congenital disorder of glycosylation. Am. J. Pathol. 157: 1917–1925.
White, J.F. 2003. Intestinal pathophysiology in autism. Exp. Biol. Med. (Maywood)
228:639–649.
Wildin, R.S., Ramsdell, F., Peake, J., Faravelli, F., Casanova, J.L., Buist, N., Levy-Lahad, E.,
Mazzella, M., Goulet, O., Perroni, L., Bricarelli, F.D., Byrne, G., McRuen, M., Proll, S.,
Appleby, H., and Brunkow, M.E. 2001. X-linked neonatal diabetes mellitus, enteropathy
and endocrinopathy syndrome is the human equivalent of mouse scurfy. Nat. Genet.
27:18–20.
Winter, C., Reutiman, T.J., Folsom, T.D., Sohr, R., Wolf, R.J., Juckel, G., and Fatemi, S.H. 2008.
Dopamine and serotonin levels following prenatal viral infection in mouse-Implications
for psychiatric disorders such as schizophrenia and autism. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol.
18:712–716.
Zedler, S. and Faist, E. 2006. The impact of endogenous triggers on trauma-associated inflam-
mation. Curr. Opin. Crit. Care 12:595–601.
Zerrate, M.C., Pletnikov, M., Connors, S.L., Vargas, D.L., Seidler, F.J., Zimmerman, A.W.,
Slotkin, T.A., and Pardo, C.A. 2007. Neuroinflammation and behavioral abnormalities
after neonatal terbutaline treatment in rats: Implications for autism. J. Pharmacol. Exp.
Ther. 322:16–22.
Zhu, C.B., Blakely, R.D., and Hewlett, W.A. 2006. The proinflammatory cytokines
interleukin-1beta and tumor necrosis factor-alpha activate serotonin transporters.
Neuropsychopharmacology 31:2121–2131.
Zimmerman, A.W., Connors, S.L., Matteson, K.J., Lee, L.C., Singer, H.S., Castaneda, J.A.,
and Pearce, D.A. 2007. Maternal antibrain antibodies in autism. Brain Behav. Immun.
21:351–357.
Zimmerman, A.W., Jyonouchi, H., Comi, A.M., Connors, S.L., Milstien, S., Varsou, A., and
Heyes, M.P. 2005. Cerebrospinal fluid and serum markers of inflammation in autism.
Pediatr. Neurol. 33:195–201.
Zlokovic, B.V. 2008. The blood-brain barrier in health and chronic neurodegenerative disorders.
Neuron 57:178–201.
14 Autism, Gastrointestinal
Disturbance, and
Immune Dysfunction:
What Is the Link?
Paul Ashwood,1,3,* Amanda Enstrom,1,3
and Judy Van de Water2,3
1Department of Medical Microbiology and
Immunology, 2Division of Rheumatology, Allergy
and Clinical Immunology, Department of Internal
Medicine, and 3M.I.N.D. Institute, University of
California at Davis, Davis, CA 95616, USA
CONTENTS
14.1 Introduction ............................................................................................... 278
14.2 Autism and Immune Dysfunction ............................................................. 279
14.2.1 Autoimmunity and Autism ......................................................... 279
14.2.2 Cell-Mediated Immune Response in Autism ............................. 281
14.2.2.1 Cytokines in Autism .................................................. 282
14.3 Gastrointestinal Dysfunction in Autism ................................................... 282
14.4 Discussion: Possible Links between Immune Dysfunction,
Gastrointestinal Dysfunction and Autism ................................................. 285
References .............................................................................................................. 291
277
278 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
immunohistochemical analyses of the GI tract in children with ASD who present with
GI symptoms have revealed chronic inflammatory changes throughout the gut, includ-
ing extensive immune cell infiltration of the gut wall and increased pro-inflammatory
responses in mucosal T-lymphocyte cells. In addition, some of the changes observed
suggest the potential involvement of an autoimmune response that is directed toward
the epithelial barrier surrounding the gut lumen. Here, we review evidence supporting
a potential role of GI dysfunction in some cases of ASD, the potential link between
immune dysfunction and gut inflammation, and the hypotheses regarding the rela-
tionship between autistic behaviors and GI-related immune dysfunction.
14.1 INTRODUCTION
Autism, or autistic disorder, is now recognized in the DSM-IV-TR as a heterogeneous
syndrome, belonging to a group of neurodevelopmental disorders known as the
autism spectrum disorders (ASD) (APA, 2000) that include other specific diagnostic
subtypes such as Asperger’s disorder and Rett’s disorder.
The etiology of autism is largely unknown, although ASD are highly heritable.
The evidence for a genetic link came initially from twin studies, where the concor-
dance rate for autism was 60%–90% in monozygotic twins compared with 3%–5%
in dizygotic twins (Bailey et al., 1995; Folstein and Rutter, 1977). More recent stud-
ies have also produced evidence that twinning is a risk factor for developing autism
(Taniai et al., 2008). Betancur, Leboyer, and Gillberg (2002) reported the proportion
of twins with autistic sibling pairs was 10%, compared to the expected twinning
rate based on the normal population of 2.4%. There is also an indication of fourfold
increased autism prevalence among dizygotic twins (Greenberg et al., 2001). Autism
has also been associated with single-gene defects such as tuberous sclerosis com-
plex, 15 q duplications, and fragile X syndrome, but these account for only a small
percentage of cases (Muhle, Trentacoste, and Rapin, 2004). While these studies
yield evidence that there are genes that strongly impact the likelihood of developing
autism, no definitive pattern of genes has been identified and a multitude of different
and varied candidate genes have been implicated in autism (Muhle, Trentacoste, and
Rapin, 2004). Moreover, the replication of these data has been inconsistent, prob-
ably due in part to the heterogeneity of the phenotypes within the autism spectrum.
However, several studies have linked autism with immune-based genes, such as
human leukocyte antigen (HLA)-DRB1*04, interleukin (IL)-4 receptor, and comple-
ment C4B null allele (Lee et al., 2006; Torres et al., 2006; Warren et al., 1992, 1995).
These data are supported by findings showing the higher prevalence of autoimmune
disease in the primary and secondary family members of autistic children when
compared to families with no history of autism (Comi et al., 1999; Money, Bobrow,
and Clarke, 1971; Mouridsen et al., 2007). These data suggest that alterations in the
genes that control the immune system/immune function may act as susceptibility
factors for the development of autism. The role of autoimmunity and immune dys-
function in autism is discussed in detail in Section 14.2.1.
It is most likely that the vast majority of the cases of autism are caused by the
combination of environmental factors and multiple susceptibility genes (Cederlund
and Gillberg, 2004; Glasson et al., 2004). Environmental factors such as congenital
Autism, Gastrointestinal Disturbance, and Immune Dysfunction 279
to serum from the test mother compared with those mice exposed in utero to serum
from the control mothers. Recently, purified IgG isolated from sera of mothers of
children with autism and from sera of control mothers with typically developing
children were injected into pregnant rhesus macaques. The offspring of macaques
injected with IgG from mothers of children with autism displayed significant whole
body stereotypies and behavioral changes that were strikingly reminiscent of autism
and were not present in the offspring of macaques treated with IgG from mothers of
typically developing children (Martin et al., 2008).
In addition to potentially increased familial autoimmunity in relatives of subjects
with ASD, there are an increasing number of reports that show direct indications
of autoimmunity in children with ASD (Cabanlit et al., 2007; Connolly et al., 1999,
2006; Singer et al., 2006; Todd et al., 1988; Weizman et al., 1982; Wills et al., 2007).
The presence of self-reactive antibodies has been extensively reported in autistic
children. In one study, 27% of ASD children had self-reactive IgG autoantibodies
compared with 2% of control children; 36% had self-reactive IgM, compared with
none in the control group (Connolly et al., 1999). Specifically of interest, autoanti-
bodies have been detected toward many components of the central nervous system
(CNS) in some individuals with autism. These include antibodies toward neuron-axon
filament proteins (Singh et al., 1997), myelin basic protein (MBP; Singh et al., 1993),
serotonin receptors (Singh, Singh, and Warren, 1997), brain-derived neurotrophic
factor (Connolly et al., 2006), nerve growth factor (Kozlovskaia et al., 2000), and
brain endothelial cells (Connolly et al., 1999). Although these autoantibodies are not
detectable in all individuals with autism, the number of individuals with autism who
have one or more of these antibodies is significant. In one study of 68 children with
autism, 49% had autoantibodies that were reactive against the caudate nucleus and
18% had antibodies against the cerebral cortex (Singh and Rivas, 2004). In separate
studies, Singh et al. (1993, 1997) found 55%–70% of sera collected from children with
autism were positive for antibodies against MBP and brain-derived neurofilament
protein. However, it is unclear whether these autoantibodies are directly involved in
the induction of autistic behavior or secondary to the innate CNS pathology.
subsets, there have been reports of partial activation, evinced by an increase in cell
surface HLA-DR without the expression of the IL-2 receptor (Plioplys et al., 1994;
Warren et al., 1995). NK cells from autistic children also have reduced lytic activity
compared to controls (Enstrom et al., 2009; Warren, Foster, and Margaretten, 1987).
These studies have provided valuable information on the immune status of autistic
children and implicate several cellular subsets as potential antagonists. However,
many of these studies were conducted under steady state or nonspecific mitogenic
conditions at singular time points in the child’s development. At a steady state,
i.e., non-challenged milieu, immune cells may only slightly differ from neurotypi-
cal controls, but when challenged there may be an altered or inappropriate immune
response and corresponding cytokine profile in autism compared with the controls.
dysfunction in children with ASD reported by different studies varies greatly, with
existing data putting the overall rate anywhere between 18% and 91% (Afzal et al.,
2003; Horvath and Perman, 2002b; Horvath et al., 1999; Lightdale et al., 2001; Ming
et al., 2008; Molloy and Manning-Courtney, 2003; Niehus and Lord, 2006; Parracho
et al., 2005; Valicenti-McDermott et al., 2006). Although it has been suggested that
GI dysfunction is a more common complaint in children with ASD compared with
typically developing children, few studies have utilized adequate controls, making
it challenging to come to a definitive conclusion as to the true extent of GI dysfunc-
tion in ASD (Erickson et al., 2005; Kuddo and Nelson, 2003; Levy et al., 2007).
Despite this, however, it is clear that a substantial proportion of children with autism
present with abnormal GI function. Symptoms of GI dysfunction include persistent
diarrhea, chronic constipation, alternating bowel habits, vomiting, gaseousness and
bloating, abdominal distention, and pain, with the latter symptom often being severe.
In a retrospective evaluation of non-referred children with autism or ASD (n = 137),
24% had ≥1 GI symptom; diarrhea was the most commonly reported symptom
(12%, n = 17) (Molloy and Manning-Courtney, 2003). In another study, the incidence
of GI symptoms in 385 children with pervasive developmental disorders (PDD) was
compared with the incidence in 48 non-ASD siblings and 102 unrelated controls
(Melmed et al., 2000). Chronic GI symptoms were reported in 46% of children with
PDD: 19% had chronic diarrhea, 19% had chronic constipation, and the remaining 8%
alternated between diarrhea and constipation. In contrast, only 8% of healthy
siblings and 5% of unrelated controls had chronic diarrhea, and 10% of healthy sib-
lings and 5% of controls had chronic constipation. In a recent study of 150 children,
the lifetime prevalence of GI symptoms in ASD was 70% compared with 42% of
children with developmental delay but not ASD, and compared with 28% of typically
developing children (Valicenti-McDermott et al., 2006). The high rates of GI symp-
toms reported in this group may likely be due to a recording of both previous and
current GI symptoms. However, the analysis of more specific symptoms reported
was similar to those seen for other studies. For instance, 22% of children with ASD
and 8% of typically developing children reported chronic constipation, which were
similar to the frequency seen in the study by Melmed et al. (2000).
Children with ASD and GI symptoms are more likely to experience sleep distur-
bance, sudden irritability, unexplained crying, and aggressive behavior than children
with ASD but no GI symptoms (Horvath and Perman, 2002a). In their study, Horvath
and colleagues reported disturbed sleep and night-time wake up in 51% of children
with ASD who had GI symptoms, compared with 14% of children with autism but
no GI symptoms, and compared with only 7% of healthy siblings. The same research
team reported that 15.5% of children with ASD had reflux esophagitis, and that nearly
two-thirds of these children experienced sleep disturbances (Horvath and Perman,
2002b). They also reported that 43% of children who had abnormal esophageal histol-
ogy had episodes of unexplained daytime irritability compared with 13% of children
with ASD who had normal esophageal histology. Observations that GI symptoms may
peak before and are relieved following bowel movements suggest that some autism
behaviors may be exacerbated due to GI dysfunction (Ashwood et al., 2003; Wakefield,
2002). However, the significance of the relationship between more severe autistic
behaviors and the presence of GI symptoms in ASD requires further investigation.
284 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
IBD patients. A correlation between high serum 5-HT and decreased 5-HT recep-
tor sensitivity with the extended HLA haplotype B44-DR4, as well as complement
C4B null allele, has been reported in ASD (Warren, 1996). Furthermore self-reactive
antibodies to 5-HT receptors were identified in the serum of ASD patients (Cook
and Leventhal, 1996; Todd and Ciaranello, 1985). This is consistent with findings
of 80% loss of 5-HT binding in mice upon the addition of sera from ASD patients
(Singh, Singh, and Warren, 1997). Genetic disequilibrium at the 5-HT transporter
gene (SLC6A4) has also been reported in ASD individuals in two separate studies
(Cook et al., 1990; Kim et al., 2002). In healthy individuals, approximately 95%
of peripheral 5-HT is produced in the GI tract with the rest produced in the brain.
Increased levels of 5-HT in the brain have been associated with increased levels of
destructive and aggressive behaviors, while increased levels in the GI tract are
associated with IBD. Furthermore, the usage of 5-HT reuptake inhibitors risperi-
done, clomipramine, and fluvoxamine has been shown to decrease certain aberrant
behaviors, such as anger, compulsive behavior, and ritualized behavior, in ASD indi-
viduals (Gordon et al., 1993; Hellings et al., 1996, 2006; McDougle et al., 1998).
The possible links between GI tract inflammation resulting in, or being caused by,
increased local 5-HT are compelling based on the connection between serotonin, the
immune system, and the nervous system. However, 5-HT in ASD with GI symptoms
has not been extensively studied.
How might increased GI permeability, abnormal immune response, and GI
inflammation trigger behaviors associated with ASD? Perhaps the most plausible
factor may be that of cytokine action. Cytokines and their receptors are distrib-
uted throughout the brain and are known to influence neural development, synaptic
transmission, and behavioral traits, and have been implicated in autism as well as
schizophrenia (Dunn, 2006; Larson, 2002; Licinio, Kling, and Hauser, 1998; Meyer
et al., 2006; Muller and Ackenheil, 1998; Nawa, Takahashi, and Patterson, 2000;
Smith et al., 2007; Tohmi et al., 2004; Vargas et al., 2005; Vereker, O’Donnell, and
Lynch, 2000; Yamada et al., 2000). Importantly, the immunocytochemical analysis
of brain tissue from patients with autism showed the marked activation of micro-
glia and astroglia, most notably in the cerebellum. Cytokine profiling indicated that
MCP-1 and TGF-β1, derived from neuroglia, were the most prevalent cytokines in
brain tissues (Vargas et al., 2005). The authors suggested that the activation of the
CNS innate immune system leading to cytokine release may play a role in the patho-
genesis of the disease. It is possible that altered levels of peripheral cytokines occur
as a result of immune dysregulation in the GI tract. These altered peripheral cytokine
levels may subsequently act directly on neurons within the CNS or may trigger a
CNS-mediated inflammatory response via glial cells. These actions could in turn
affect neurodevelopment and/or elicit behaviors associated with ASD.
Autoimmune responses may also be important in impairing GI function in ASD
and may be another mechanism relevant to ASD pathophysiology. It is possible that
antibodies toward neuronal tissue seen in some patients with ASD may be generated
as a result of secondary inflammation in the CNS. However, it is not known whether
these autoantibodies are pathogenic and are involved in the generation of behaviors
associated with ASD. Vojdani et al. (2002) demonstrated that antibodies against
many neuronal proteins in ASD individuals also cross-react with butyrophilin,
290 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Proinflammatory
cytokines (e.g., TNF-α
Molecular mimicry
and IFN-γ)
leading to cross-reactive
Ab΄s and/or T cells Regulatory cytokines
(e.g., IL-10)
FIGURE 14.1 (See color insert following page 200.) Possible mechanism by which GI
dysfunction may trigger autistic behaviors in children with autism and GI disturbance.
REFERENCES
Afzal, N., S. Murch, K. Thirrupathy, L. Berger, A. Fagbemi, and R. Heuschkel. 2003.
Constipation with acquired megarectum in children with autism. Pediatrics 112:939–942.
Anderson, G. M. 1987. Monoamines in autism: An update of neurochemical research on a
pervasive developmental disorder. Med. Biol. 65:67–74.
APA. 2000. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition, text revision
(DMS-IV-TR), Ed. APA. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association Publishing, Inc.
Ashwood, P. and A. J. Wakefield. 2006. Immune activation of peripheral blood and mucosal
CD3+ lymphocyte cytokine profiles in children with autism and gastrointestinal symp-
toms. J. Neuroimmunol. 173:126–134.
Ashwood, P., A. Anthony, A. A. Pellicer, F. Torrente, J. A. Walker-Smith, and A. J. Wakefield.
2003. Intestinal lymphocyte populations in children with regressive autism: Evidence
for extensive mucosal immunopathology. J. Clin. Immunol. 23:504–517.
292 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Ashwood, P., A. Anthony, F. Torrente, and A. J. Wakefield. 2004. Spontaneous mucosal lym-
phocyte cytokine profiles in children with autism and gastrointestinal symptoms: mucosal
immune activation and reduced counter regulatory interleukin-10. J. Clin. Immunol.
24:664–673.
Asperger, H. 1961. Psychopathology of children with coeliac disease. Ann. Paediatr.
197:346–351.
Bailey, A., A. Le Couteur, I. Gottesman, P. Bolton, E. Simonoff, E. Yuzda, and M. Rutter. 1995.
Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: Evidence from a British twin study. Psychol. Med.
25:63–77.
Baird, G., H. Cass, and V. Slonims. 2003. Diagnosis of autism. BMJ 327:488–493.
Betancur, C., M. Leboyer, and C. Gillberg. 2002. Increased rate of twins among affected
sibling pairs with autism. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 70:1381–1383.
Bjarnason, I., T. J. Peters, and N. Veall. 1983. Intestinal permeability defect in coeliac disease.
Lancet 1:1284–1285.
Braunschweig, D., P. Ashwood, P. Krakowiak, I. Hertz-Picciotto, R. Hansen, L. A. Croen,
I. N. Pessah, and J. Van de Water. 2008. Autism: Maternally derived antibodies specific
for fetal brain proteins. Neurotoxicology 29:226–231.
Cabanlit, M., S. Wills, P. Goines, P. Ashwood, and J. Van de Water. 2007. Brain-specific autoan-
tibodies in the plasma of subjects with autistic spectrum disorder. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci.
1107:92–103.
Cederlund, M. and C. Gillberg. 2004. One hundred males with Asperger syndrome: A clinical
study of background and associated factors. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 46:652–660.
Chess, S. 1971. Autism in children with congenital rubella. J. Autism Child Schizophr.
1:33–47.
Christison, G. W. and K. Ivany. 2006. Elimination diets in autism spectrum disorders: Any
wheat amidst the chaff? J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 27:S162–171.
Comi, A. M., A. W. Zimmerman, V. H. Frye, P. A. Law, and J. N. Peeden. 1999. Familial
clustering of autoimmune disorders and evaluation of medical risk factors in autism.
J. Child Neurol. 14:388–394.
Connolly, A. M., M. G. Chez, A. Pestronk, S. T. Arnold, S. Mehta, and R. K. Deuel. 1999.
Serum autoantibodies to brain in Landau-Kleffner variant, autism, and other neurologic
disorders. J. Pediatr. 134:607–613.
Connolly, A. M., M. Chez, E. M. Streif, R. M. Keeling, P. T. Golumbek, J. M. Kwon, J. J. Riviello,
R. G. Robinson, R. J. Neuman, and R. M. Deuel. 2006. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor
and autoantibodies to neural antigens in sera of children with autistic spectrum disorders,
Landau-Kleffner syndrome, and epilepsy. Biol. Psychiatry 59:354–363.
Cook, E. H. and B. L. Leventhal. 1996. The serotonin system in autism. Curr. Opin. Pediatr.
8:348–354.
Cook, E. H., Jr., B. L. Leventhal, W. Heller, J. Metz, M. Wainwright, and D. X. Freedman.
1990. Autistic children and their first-degree relatives: Relationships between sero-
tonin and norepinephrine levels and intelligence. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci.
2:268–274.
Cooke, W. T. and W. T. Smith. 1966. Neurological disorders associated with adult coeliac disease.
Brain 89:683–722.
Croen, L. A., J. K. Grether, C. K. Yoshida, R. Odouli, and J. Van de Water. 2005. Maternal
autoimmune diseases, asthma and allergies, and childhood autism spectrum disorders:
A case-control study. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 159:151–157.
Croonenberghs, J., E. Bosmans, D. Deboutte, G. Kenis, and M. Maes. 2002. Activation of the
inflammatory response system in autism. Neuropsychobiology 45:1–6.
Cummins, A. G., I. A. Penttila, J. T. Labrooy, T. A. Robb, and G. P. Davidson. 1991. Recovery
of the small intestine in coeliac disease on a gluten-free diet: Changes in intestinal
permeability, small bowel morphology and T-cell activity. J. Gastroenterol. Hepatol.
6:53–57.
Autism, Gastrointestinal Disturbance, and Immune Dysfunction 293
Graus, F., B. Lang, P. Pozo-Rosich, A. Saiz, R. Casamitjana, and A. Vincent. 2002. P/Q type
calcium-channel antibodies in paraneoplastic cerebellar degeneration with lung cancer.
Neurology 59:764–766.
Greenberg, D. A., S. E. Hodge, J. Sowinski, and D. Nicoll. 2001. Excess of twins among
affected sibling pairs with autism: Implications for the etiology of autism. Am. J. Hum.
Genet. 69:1062–1067.
Guggenmos, J., A. S. Schubart, S. Ogg, M. Andersson, T. Olsson, I. H. Mather, and C. Linington.
2004. Antibody cross-reactivity between myelin oligodendrocyte glycoprotein and the
milk protein butyrophilin in multiple sclerosis. J. Immunol. 172:661–668.
Gupta, S., S. Aggarwal, B. Rashanravan, and T. Lee. 1998. Th1- and Th2-like cytokines in
CD4+ and CD8+ T cells in autism. J. Neuroimmunol. 85:106–109.
Hadjivassiliou, M., R. A. Grunewald, and G. A. Davies-Jones. 2002. Gluten sensitivity as a
neurological illness. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 72:560–563.
Hallert, C. and T. Derefeldt. 1982. Psychic disturbances in adult coeliac disease. I. Clinical
observations. Scand. J. Gastroenterol. 17:17–19.
Hellings, J. A., L. A. Kelley, W. F. Gabrielli, E. Kilgore, and P. Shah. 1996. Sertraline response
in adults with mental retardation and autistic disorder. J. Clin. Psychiatry 57:333–336.
Hellings, J. A., J. R. Zarcone, R. M. Reese, M. G. Valdovinos, J. G. Marquis, K. K. Fleming,
and S. R. Schroeder. 2006. A crossover study of risperidone in children, adolescents and
adults with mental retardation. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36:401–411.
Hoekstra, P. J. and R. B. Minderaa. 2005. Tic disorders and obsessive-compulsive disorder:
Is autoimmunity involved? Int. Rev. Psychiatry 17:497–502.
Horvath, K. and J. A. Perman. 2002a. Autism and gastrointestinal symptoms. Curr. Gastroenterol.
Rep. 4:251–258.
Horvath, K. and J. A. Perman. 2002b. Autistic disorder and gastrointestinal disease. Curr. Opin.
Pediatr. 14:583–587.
Horvath, K., J. C. Papadimitriou, A. Rabsztyn, C. Drachenberg, and J. T. Tildon. 1999.
Gastrointestinal abnormalities in children with autistic disorder. J. Pediatr. 135:559–563.
Hyman, S. L. and S. E. Levy. 2005. Introduction: Novel therapies in developmental disabilities—
hope, reason, and evidence. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 11:107–109.
Jyonouchi, H., S. Sun, and H. Le. 2001. Proinflammatory and regulatory cytokine production
associated with innate and adaptive immune responses in children with autism spectrum
disorders and developmental regression. J. Neuroimmunol. 120:170–179.
Jyonouchi, H., L. Geng, A. Ruby, and B. Zimmerman-Bier. 2005. Dysregulated innate immune
responses in young children with autism spectrum disorders: Their relationship to gas-
trointestinal symptoms and dietary intervention. Neuropsychobiology 51:77–85.
Kalat, J. W. 1978. Speculations on similarities between autism and opiate addiction. J. Autism
Child Schizophr. 8:477–479.
Kim, S. J., N. Cox, R. Courchesne, C. Lord, C. Corsello, N. Akshoomoff, S. Guter,
B. L. Leventhal, E. Courchesne, and E. H. Cook, Jr. 2002. Transmission disequilibrium
mapping at the serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4) region in autistic disorder. Mol.
Psychiatry 7:278–288.
Knivsberg, A. M., K. L. Reichelt, and M. Nodland. 2001. Reports on dietary intervention in
autistic disorders. Nutr. Neurosci. 4:25–37.
Kozlovskaia, G. V., T. P. Kliushnik, A. V. Goriunova, I. L. Turkova, M. A. Kalinina, and
N. S. Sergienko. 2000. Nerve growth factor auto-antibodies in children with various
forms of mental dysontogenesis and in schizophrenia high risk group. Zh. Nevrol.
Psikhiatr. Im. S. S. Korsakova 100:50–52.
Kuddo, T. and K. B. Nelson. 2003. How common are gastrointestinal disorders in children
with autism? Curr. Opin. Pediatr. 15:339–343.
Lang, B., R. C. Dale, and A. Vincent. 2003. New autoantibody mediated disorders of the central
nervous system. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 16:351–357.
Autism, Gastrointestinal Disturbance, and Immune Dysfunction 295
Taniai, H., T. Nishiyama, T. Miyachi, M. Imaeda, and S. Sumi. 2008. Genetic influences on
the broad spectrum of autism: Study of proband-ascertained twins. Am. J. Med. Genet.
B. Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 147B:844–849.
Todd, R. D. and R. D. Ciaranello. 1985. Demonstration of inter- and intraspecies differences
in serotonin binding sites by antibodies from an autistic child. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
U.S.A. 82:612–616.
Todd, R. D., J. M. Hickok, G. M. Anderson, and D. J. Cohen. 1988. Antibrain antibodies in
infantile autism. Biol. Psychiatry 23:644–647.
Tohmi, M., N. Tsuda, Y. Watanabe, A. Kakita, and H. Nawa. 2004. Perinatal inflammatory
cytokine challenge results in distinct neurobehavioral alterations in rats: Implication in
psychiatric disorders of developmental origin. Neurosci. Res. 50:67–75.
Torrente, F., P. Ashwood, R. Day, N. Machado, R. I. Furlano, A. Anthony, S. E. Davies,
A. J. Wakefield, M. A. Thomson, J. A. Walker-Smith, and S. H. Murch. 2002. Small
intestinal enteropathy with epithelial IgG and complement deposition in children with
regressive autism. Mol. Psychiatry 7:375–382, 334.
Torrente, F., A. Anthony, R. B. Heuschkel, M. A. Thomson, P. Ashwood, and S. H. Murch.
2004. Focal-enhanced gastritis in regressive autism with features distinct from Crohn’s
and Helicobacter pylori gastritis. Am. J. Gastroenterol. 99:598–605.
Torres, A. R., T. L. Sweeten, A. Cutler, B. J. Bedke, M. Fillmore, E. G. Stubbs, and D. Odell.
2006. The association and linkage of the HLA-A2 class I allele with autism. Hum.
Immunol. 67:346–351.
Valicenti-McDermott, M., K. McVicar, I. Rapin, B. K. Wershil, H. Cohen, and S. Shinnar.
2006. Frequency of gastrointestinal symptoms in children with autistic spectrum disor-
ders and association with family history of autoimmune disease. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr.
27:S128–S136.
Vargas, D. L., C. Nascimbene, C. Krishnan, A. W. Zimmerman, and C. A. Pardo. 2005.
Neuroglial activation and neuroinflammation in the brain of patients with autism. Ann.
Neurol. 57:67–81.
Vereker, E., E. O’Donnell, and M. A. Lynch. 2000. The inhibitory effect of interleukin-1beta
on long-term potentiation is coupled with increased activity of stress-activated protein
kinases. J. Neurosci. 20:6811–6819.
Vojdani, A., A. W. Campbell, E. Anyanwu, A. Kashanian, K. Bock, and E. Vojdani. 2002.
Antibodies to neuron-specific antigens in children with autism: Possible cross-reaction
with encephalitogenic proteins from milk, Chlamydia pneumoniae and Streptococcus
group A. J. Neuroimmunol. 129:168–177.
Wakefield, A. J. 2002. The gut-brain axis in childhood developmental disorders. J. Pediatr.
Gastroenterol. Nutr. 34(Suppl. 1):S14–S17.
Wakefield, A. J., A. Anthony, S. H. Murch, M. Thomson, S. M. Montgomery, S. Davies,
J. J. O’Leary, M. Berelowitz, and J. A. Walker-Smith. 2000. Enterocolitis in children
with developmental disorders. Am. J. Gastroenterol. 95:2285–2295.
Wakefield, A.J., P. Ashwood, K. Limb, and A. Anthony. 2005. The significance of ileo-colonic
lymphoid nodular hyperplasia in children with autistic spectrum disorder. Eur. J.
Gastroenterol. 17:827–836.
Walker-Smith J. and J. Andrews. 1972. Alpha-1-antitrypsin, autism, and coeliac disease.
Lancet 2:883–884.
Wang, W., S. Uzzau, S. E. Goldblum, and A. Fasano. 2000. Human zonulin, a potential modu-
lator of intestinal tight junctions. J. Cell. Sci. 113 (Pt. 24):4435–4440.
Warren, R. P., A. Foster, and N. C. Margaretten. 1987. Reduced natural killer cell activity in
autism. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 26:333–335.
Warren, R. P., J. D. Odell, W. L. Warren, R. A. Burger, A. Maciulius, W. W. Daniels, and
A. R. Torres. 1996. Strong association of the third hypervariable region of HLA-DR
beta 1 with autism. J. Neuroimmunol. 67:259–274.
298 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
CONTENTS
15.1 Introduction .................................................................................................300
15.2 OT Synthesis ...............................................................................................300
15.3 Expression and Activation of OTR ............................................................. 301
15.4 Oxytocin Levels and Oxytocin Receptor in Autism ...................................302
15.5 Autism and Oxidative Stress ....................................................................... 303
15.6 Possible Contribution of Oxidative Stress to GI Aspects of Autism...........304
15.7 Proposed Cellular Mechanism in Gut/Brain Signaling .............................. 305
15.8 Proposed Role of Low Oxytocin in the Generation
of an Autism Phenotype ..............................................................................307
15.9 Possible Implications for the Treatment of Autism
and Developmental Disorders .....................................................................308
References ..............................................................................................................309
299
300 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
15.1 INTRODUCTION
The working hypothesis of our laboratory is that developmental disorders such as
autism arise from the dysregulation of a unified gut/brain system, rather than origi-
nating in the brain alone. Indeed, 70% of autistic children suffer from at least one
chronic gastrointestinal (GI) symptom (Horvath et al., 1999), potentially sorting this
group as a subset of autism.
The question of oxytocin (OT) deficiency as a contributing factor in autism was
raised over 15 years ago (Modahl et al., 1992). Recent findings have established OT’s
importance in modulating stress and predict its importance to gut/brain develop-
ment. OT’s antistress (Uvnas-Moberg and Petersson, 2005) and anti-inflammatory
properties (Iseri et al., 2005; Petersson et al., 2001) are well known. OT is present in
mother’s milk. There is an early exposure of the GI tract to this peptide (Stefanidis
et al., 2008). OT antagonists alter gut motility (Monstein et al., 2004). To understand
how OT deficiency might contribute to the development of autism, one must almost
certainly examine stress physiology in the gut. At the cellular level, this means
oxidative stress.
Oxidative stress is defined as an imbalance between the generation of reactive
oxygen species (ROS) and the decreased antioxidant defense systems. We hypoth-
esize that autism stems from physiological stress, including oxidative stress, which if
unmodulated triggers a cascade of adverse interrelated autonomic, endocrinological,
neurological, and immunological reactions.
This chapter examines how the deficiency of OT combined with oxidative stress
in the gut might dysregulate a unified gut/brain network. It reviews evidence that OT
levels and signaling pathways downstream of the oxytocin receptor (OTR) may be
involved in the excitatory drive of gut/brain signaling and thereby in the pathogen-
esis of a subset of autism. It discusses the role of oxidative stress in autism and the
possible contribution of differential responses to hypoxia in preterm and full-term
infants. Finally, it presents a theoretical framework and cellular mechanism for the
modulation of gut/brain signaling by OT and discusses possible implications for
the treatment of autism and developmental disorders, specifically those with comor-
bid GI symptoms and/or inflammation.
15.2 OT SYNTHESIS
Initially, the site of OT synthesis was considered to be restricted to the hypothalamus,
where it is synthesized as a preprohormone (Brownstein, 1983). The preprohormone
is subsequently cleaved into two bioactive peptides: OT close to the N-terminus and
neurophysin from the C-terminus (Gainer et al., 1977a). The OT and neurophysin
precursors are converted into bioactive peptides during axonal translocation to
the neurohypophysis (Gainer et al., 1977b). Like several other neuropeptides, OT
becomes bioactive and resistant to degradation by having its C-terminus amidated
(Prigge et al., 1997). Additional tissues have been reported as sites of synthesis and
secretion of OT. The mRNA and OT peptide were found in vasculature (Jankowski
et al., 2000) and cardiac myocytes consistent with its involvement in the control of
vessel tonus and the secretion of atrial natriuretic peptide (Gutkowska et al., 2000).
Possible Mechanism Involving Intestinal Oxytocin, Oxidative Stress 301
OT induces growth in trophoblasts (Cassoni et al., 2001b) and small lung cancer
cells (Pequeux et al., 2004). The growth inhibition mediated by OTR was shown to
be transduced via cyclic AMP and protein kinase A, whereas growth stimulation
was transduced via Ca2+ flux and protein kinase C (Cassoni et al., 2001a). This dual
activity of OT could also be due to the differential activation of OTR at varying
concentrations of OT.
OT concentrations in breast milk increase above those in serum several days
postpartum (Leake et al., 1981). Therefore, one would expect the upper GI tract
epithelium of the normal breastfed neonate would be exposed to exogenous OT
and/or its fragments. If the contact between GI epithelium luminal surface and
OT has a physiological significance, then OTR could be expressed somewhere
along the GI tract, at least during the neonatal period if not during the entire
breastfeeding period.
Indeed, our laboratory has recently shown that OTR is expressed in the epithelium
of colon, in the small intestinal villi, in the muscularis mucosa, and in the myenteric
neuronal cells in rat newborns (Welch et al., 2009). We have also shown that OTR
expression is developmentally regulated. In fact, transcripts of both OT and OTR
mRNA peak at postnatal day 7 in rat pups. This pattern of OTR expression in the GI
tract could be induced either by endogenous OT from the gut or other sources and/or
by exogenous OT from breast milk.
We hypothesize from our experiments to date that the pattern of OTR expression
in the mucosal smooth muscles and the autonomous neural plexus implies develop-
mental induction. This expression may also imply the conditioning of gut/brain
sensory networks and later functional maintenance, for example, of peristaltic intes-
tinal movements. However, the biological functional advantage of OTR expression
in the absorptive enterocytes (villus epithelium) is not clear and its elucidation will
require further experiments.
The temporal changes of OTR in the villi suggest that OT/OTR is required for a
brief time window, perhaps to regulate the villus maturation process. In rodents, villi
develop during the first 2 weeks postpartum (Porter et al., 2002). Our data show that
after postnatal day 14, OTR gut expression in rat pups appeared in the crypts and
at the crypt–villus junctions where the immature precursors of the crypts develop
into functioning villus absorptive cells (Welch et al., 2009). Although this distribu-
tion is compatible with the possibility that OT/OTR signaling affects or regulates
epithelial maturation, other crypt cells that might also be regulated include Paneth
cells and secretory cells.
et al., 2002). This finding raises the possibility that the hyper-connectivity between
neurons in the cortex associated with cell packing, together with reduced blood
perfusion, may result in hyperactive neurons on one hand and lower oxygenation
on the other. Such partial hypoxia could induce oxidative stress, forming ROS in
mitochondria. This sequence could exemplify a nonspecific marker resulting from
an autism-specific mini-column phenomenon.
A possible anomaly that could connect autism to oxidative stress is a disruption
of the secretion of OT hormone by the newborn hypophysis during labor. Areas
in the newborn brain undergo aglycemia and hypoxia during labor, which can
lead to neuronal death (Carbillon, 2007). Normally, neuronal death is inhibited by
OT-induced switch-off of neuronal firing by gamma aminobutyric acid (GABA) in
the brain (Cossart et al., 2006; Tyzio et al., 2006). However, a shortage of OT or the
dysfunction of its receptor might contribute to oxidative stress. Of interest to our
laboratory is whether the OT/OTR signaling system has a comparable cell-survival
function in peripheral organs, particularly the gut, which might be compromised
during episodes of perinatal hypoxia when blood flow may be preferentially distrib-
uted to the brain (Dyess et al., 1998). Also of great interest is the origin of OT regu-
lating the brain GABA switch. OT was shown to be of maternal origin (Khazipov
et al., 2008; Tyzio et al., 2008), but evidence suggests that endogenous fetal OT may
be induced by perinatal hypoxia (Carbillon, 2007).
It is logical to assume that the excitatory-to-inhibitory switch in GABA receptor
responses to stimulation may not occur by the time of birth in premature infants,
inasmuch as this process is normally completed near the time of full-term delivery
(Tyzio et al., 2008). Given the effects of OT on mechanisms regulating concentra-
tions of intracellular chloride, which in turn moves the GABA receptor bias toward
inhibition (Khazipov et al., 2008), we speculate that the up-regulation of the
premature infant OT system through high nurture may promote the maturation of
this important neurophysiologic process. If so, we would expect to find this evidence
in markers of neural function that would include the normalization of patterns of
electroencephalogram development as well as behavior.
baseline after hypoxia in full-term neonates, perfusion returns to only 50% in preterm
piglets; this differential gut response to hypoxia in full-term versus preterm animals
suggests a mechanism for increased risk of intestinal ischemic disease in preterm
human neonates (Dyess et al., 1998). Perinatal hypoxia also suggests a mechanism
that could contribute to the increased risk of autism with or without diagnosed GI
involvement in a vulnerable preterm population (Limperopoulos et al., 2008).
In addition to premature birth, inflammatory bowel disease can cause hypoxia
to the intestine (Hauser et al., 1988). Since OT has a protective effect against
hypoxia in the brain (Carbillon, 2007), one could reasonably predict its protective
effect against hypoxia in the gut. In such cases, the abnormally low levels of gut
OT could lead to chronic inflammation in gut colitis. Indeed, evidence supporting
this hypothesis may be provided in an organ system that synthesizes OT and OTR.
In a rat model of renal ischemia/reperfusion injury, OT protects against oxidative
stress (Biyikli et al., 2006).
Further evidence supporting the role of OT in colitis is provided by findings in our
laboratory. We have shown that experimental induction of colitis (oxidative stress)
by means of trinitrobenzene sulfonic acid elicited inflammation-related early gene
responses in central nervous system nuclei (Welch et al., 2005). Gut/brain signal-
ing measured by c-fos activation was inhibited by combined OT/secretin treatment
(Welch, unpublished results) in brain areas known to be abnormal in autism (Bauman
and Kemper, 1985, 2003; Buchsbaum et al., 2001; Chugani et al., 1997; Rumsey and
Ernst, 2000; Sparks et al., 2002; Vargas et al., 2005). Since intestinal inflammation
is associated with ROS production in the gut (Ardite et al., 2000), these findings
suggest that abnormal gut/brain interactions might be involved in the mechanisms
underlying autism.
that sodium currents are attenuated by defensin interaction with the channel protein
(Plakhova et al., 2002). During chronic gut inflammation and associated oxidative
stress, one can postulate that the augmented firing of ganglionic afferent currents could
strongly stimulate nuclei in the brain. If such stimuli occur immediately after birth under
conditions that inhibit the attenuation of neuronal sodium currents, noxious stimuli may
persist chronically and greatly affect neuronal centers in the brain. The “readout” or
imprinting of such chronic and unattenuated intense stimuli could induce epigenetic
patterns of gene expression that result in an autistic epigenetically induced phenotype.
s
nel
4 MMP-7 Defensin c han
Ion Na+ Brain
5 K+
Pro-defensin Enteric
Nucleus neuron
MMP-7
Paneth
cell
Hypoxia
3
1
β-Catenin
2 ROS
FIGURE 15.1 Possible mechanism in Paneth cell for the modulation of gut inflammation
by OT in normal newborns. Hypoxia from gut inflammation stimulates the production of
ROS within Paneth cell (1). OT inhibits ROS and/or its effect (2), enabling a Wnt-dependent
β-catenin pathway (3). β-catenin in the nucleus transcribes MMP-7, which is then translo-
cated to the cytosol where it catalyzes the cleavage of prodefensin to activate α-defensin (4).
The active α-defensin theoretically permeates to enteric neurons to attenuate currents through
various ion channels (Na+ and K+), thus dampening signaling of hypoxia-induced noxious
stimuli to the brain (5).
308 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
s
nel
c han Brain
Ion Na+
K+
4
Pro-defensin Enteric
MMP-7 5 neuron
Nucleus
3
Paneth me
cell te a so
Pro 1
in
redox Hypoxia
β-Catenin e uro
N 2 ROS
FIGURE 15.2 Possible mechanism in Paneth cell for the dysregulation of gut/brain signal-
ing under the condition of low OT levels in newborns at risk for autism. Hypoxia from gut
inflammation stimulates the production of ROS within Paneth cell (1). If OT level is inad-
equate to counter ROS production, ROS induces neuroredoxin, which in turn inhibits Wnt
pathway (2). As a result, β-catenin transcription factor becomes tagged by phosphorylation
(small red dot) and degraded by the proteasome (3). Without β-catenin in nucleus, MMP-7
gene is not expressed and defensin precursor peptide granulations (large stars) accumulate
within the cell (4). Without the attenuation provided by defensin, hypoxia-induced excitation
of ion channels in enteric neurons may drive noxious gut/brain signaling pathways (5).
REFERENCES
Ardite E., M. Sans, J. Panes, F. J. Romero, J. M. Pique, and J. C. Fernandez-Checa. 2000.
Replenishment of glutathione levels improves mucosal function in experimental acute
colitis. Lab. Invest. 80:735–744.
Bale T. L. and D. M. Dorsa. 1998. NGF, cyclic AMP, and phorbol esters regulate oxytocin
receptor gene transcription in SK-N-SH and MCF7 cells. Brain Res. Mol. Brain Res.
53:130–137.
Bartz J. A. and E. Hollander. 2008. Oxytocin and experimental therapeutics in autism
spectrum disorders. Prog. Brain Res. 170:451–462.
Bauman M. L. and T. L. Kemper. 1985. Histoanatomic observations of the brain in early infan-
tile autism. Neurology 35:866–874.
Bauman M. L. and T. L. Kemper. 2003. The neuropathology of the autism spectrum disorders:
What have we learned? Novartis Found. Symp. 251:112–122.
Biyikli N. K., H. Tugtepe, G. Sener, A. Velioglu-Ogunc, S. Cetinel, S. Midillioglu, N. Gedik,
and B. C. Yegen. 2006. Oxytocin alleviates oxidative renal injury in pyelonephritic rats
via a neutrophil-dependent mechanism. Peptides 27:2249–2257.
310 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Blankenfeldt W., K. Nokihara, S. Naruse, U. Lessel, D. Schomburg, and V. Wray. 1996. NMR
spectroscopic evidence that helodermin, unlike other members of the secretin/VIP
family of peptides, is substantially structured in water. Biochemistry 35:5955–5962.
Brabletz T., A. Jung, S. Dag, F. Hlubek, and T. Kirchner. 1999. Beta-catenin regulates the
expression of the matrix metalloproteinase-7 in human colorectal cancer. Am. J. Pathol.
155:1033–1038.
Brownstein M. J. 1983. Biosynthesis of vasopressin and oxytocin. Annu. Rev. Physiol.
45:129–135.
Buchsbaum M. S., E. Hollander, M. M. Haznedar, C. Tang, J. Spiegel-Cohen, T. C. Wei,
A. Solimando, B. R. Buchsbaum, D. Robins, C. Bienstock, C. Cartwright, and
S. Mosovich. 2001. Effect of fluoxetine on regional cerebral metabolism in autistic
spectrum disorders: A pilot study. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 4:119–125.
Carbillon L. 2007. Comment on Maternal oxytocin triggers a transient inhibitory switch in
GABA signaling in the fetal brain during delivery. Science 317:197.
Casanova M. F., I. A. van Kooten, A. E. Switala, H. van Engeland, H. Heinsen, H. W.
Steinbusch, P. R. Hof, J. Trippe, J. Stone, and C. Schmitz. 2006. Minicolumnar abnor-
malities in autism. Acta Neuropathol. 112:287–303.
Cassoni P., T. Marrocco, S. Deaglio, A. Sapino, and G. Bussolati. 2001a. Biological relevance
of oxytocin and oxytocin receptors in cancer cells and primary tumors. Ann. Oncol.
12(Suppl. 2):S37–S39.
Cassoni P., T. Marrocco, A. Sapino, E. Allia, and G. Bussolati. 2006. Oxytocin synthesis within
the normal and neoplastic breast: First evidence. Int. J. Oncol. 28:1263–1268.
Cassoni P., A. Sapino, L. Munaron, S. Deaglio, B. Chini, A. Graziani, A. Ahmed, and G. Bussolati.
2001b. Activation of functional oxytocin receptors stimulates cell proliferation in human
trophoblast and choriocarcinoma cell lines. Endocrinology 142:1130–1136.
Chalazonitis A., T. D. Pham, Z. Li, D. Roman, U. Guha, W. Gomes, L. Kan, J. A. Kessler, and
M. D. Gershon. 2008. Bone morphogenetic protein regulation of enteric neuronal pheno-
typic diversity: Relationship to timing of cell cycle exit. J. Comp. Neurol. 509:474–492.
Champagne F. A. and J. P. Curley. 2009. Epigenetic mechanisms mediating the long-term
effects of maternal care on development. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33:593–600.
Champagne F. A., I. C. Weaver, J. Diorio, S. Dymov, M. Szyf, and M. J. Meaney. 2006.
Maternal care associated with methylation of the estrogen receptor-alpha1b promoter
and estrogen receptor-alpha expression in the medial preoptic area of female offspring.
Endocrinology 147:2909–2915.
Chauhan A. and V. Chauhan. 2006. Oxidative stress in autism. Pathophysiology 13:171–181.
Chibbar R., F. D. Miller, and B. F. Mitchell. 1993. Synthesis of oxytocin in amnion, chorion, and
decidua may influence the timing of human parturition. J. Clin. Invest. 91:185–192.
Chugani D. C., O. Muzik, R. Rothermel, M. Behen, P. Chakraborty, T. Mangner, E. A. da
Silva, and H. T. Chugani. 1997. Altered serotonin synthesis in the dentatothalamocorti-
cal pathway in autistic boys. Ann. Neurol. 42:666–669.
Cossart R., Z. Petanjek, D. Dumitriu, J. C. Hirsch, Y. Ben-Ari, M. Esclapez, and C. Bernard.
2006. Interneurons targeting similar layers receive synaptic inputs with similar kinetics.
Hippocampus 16:408–420.
Courchesne E., R. Carper, and N. Akshoomoff. 2003. Evidence of brain overgrowth in the first
year of life in autism. JAMA 290:337–344.
Dyess D. L., D. P. Christenberry, G. L. Peeples, J. N. Collins, J. L. Ardell, W. S. Roberts,
E. J. Tacchi, and R. W. Powell. 1998. Organ blood flow redistribution in response to
hypoxemia in neonatal piglets. J. Invest. Surg. 11:381–392.
Faure C., A. Chalazonitis, C. Rheaume, G. Bouchard, S. G. Sampathkumar, K. J. Yarema,
and M. D. Gershon. 2007. Gangliogenesis in the enteric nervous system: Roles of the
polysialylation of the neural cell adhesion molecule and its regulation by bone morpho-
genetic protein-4. Dev. Dyn. 236:44–59.
Possible Mechanism Involving Intestinal Oxytocin, Oxidative Stress 311
Klimova T. and N. S. Chandel. 2008. Mitochondrial complex III regulates hypoxic activation
of HIF. Cell Death Differ. 15:660–666.
Krehbiel D., P. Poindron, F. Levy, and M. J. Prud’Homme. 1987. Peridural anesthesia disturbs
maternal behavior in primiparous and multiparous parturient ewes. Physiol. Behav.
40:463–472.
Kusui C., T. Kimura, K. Ogita, H. Nakamura, Y. Matsumura, M. Koyama, C. Azuma, and
Y. Murata. 2001. DNA methylation of the human oxytocin receptor gene promoter regu-
lates tissue-specific gene suppression. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 289:681–686.
Leake R. D., R. E. Weitzman, and D. A. Fisher. 1981. Oxytocin concentrations during the
neonatal period. Biol. Neonate 39:127–131.
Limperopoulos C., H. Bassan, N. R. Sullivan, J. S. Soul, R. L. Robertson Jr., M. Moore,
S. A. Ringer, J. J. Volpe, and A. J. du Plessis. 2008. Positive screening for autism in
ex-preterm infants: prevalence and risk factors. Pediatrics 121:758–765.
Lollivier V., P. G. Marnet, S. Delpal, D. Rainteau, C. Achard, A. Rabot, and M. Ollivier-
Bousquet. 2006. Oxytocin stimulates secretory processes in lactating rabbit mammary
epithelial cells. J. Physiol. 570:125–140.
Lombard J. 1998. Autism: A mitochondrial disorder? Med. Hypotheses 50:497–500.
Lu Q., B. Jiang, C. Lin, and T. Shan. 2008. Dark aberrant crypt foci with activated Wnt pathway
are related to tumorigenesis in the colon of AOM-treated rat. J. Exp. Clin. Cancer Res.
27:26.
Marchini G., H. Lagercrantz, J. Winberg, and K. Uvnas-Moberg. 1988. Fetal and maternal
plasma levels of gastrin, somatostatin and oxytocin. Early Hum. Dev. 18:73–79.
Matthiesen A. S., A. B. Ransjo-Arvidson, E. Nissen, and K. Uvnas-Moberg. 2001. Postpartum
maternal oxytocin release by newborns: Effects of infant hand massage and sucking.
Birth 28:13–19.
McGinnis W. R. 2004. Oxidative stress in autism. Altern. Ther. Health Med. 10:22–36.
Mitchell B. F., X. Fang, and S. Wong. 1998. Role of carboxy-extended forms of oxytocin in the
rat uterus in the process of parturition. Biol. Reprod. 59:1321–1327.
Modahl C., D. Fein, L. Waterhouse, and N. Newton. 1992. Does oxytocin deficiency mediate
social deficits in autism? J. Autism Dev. Disord. 22:449–451.
Monstein H. J., N. Grahn, M. Truedsson, and B. Ohlsson. 2004. Oxytocin and oxytocin-receptor
mRNA expression in the human. Regul. Pept. 119:39–44.
Ohlsson B., M. Truedsson, P. Djerf, and F. Sundler. 2006. Oxytocin is expressed throughout
the human gastrointestinal tract. Regul. Pept. 135:7–11.
Ohnishi T., H. Matsuda, T. Hashimoto, T. Kunihiro, M. Nishikawa, T. Uema, and M. Sasaki. 2000.
Abnormal regional cerebral blood flow in childhood autism. Brain 123:1838–1844.
Oliveira G., L. Diogo, M. Grazina, P. Garcia, A. Ataide, C. Marques, T. Miguel, L. Borges,
A. M. Vicente, and C. R. Oliveira. 2005. Mitochondrial dysfunction in autism spectrum
disorders: A population-based study. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 47:185–189.
Ougolkov A. V., K. Yamashita, M. Mai, and T. Minamoto. 2002. Oncogenic beta-catenin and
MMP-7 (matrilysin) cosegregate in late-stage clinical colon cancer. Gastroenterology
122:60–71.
Pequeux C., B. P. Keegan, M. T. Hagelstein, V. Geenen, J. J. Legros, and W. G. North.
2004. Oxytocin- and vasopressin-induced growth of human small-cell lung cancer
is mediated by the mitogen-activated protein kinase pathway. Endocr. Relat. Cancer
11:871–885.
Petersson M., U. Wiberg, T. Lundeberg, and K. Uvnas-Moberg. 2001. Oxytocin decreases
carrageenan induced inflammation in rats. Peptides 22:1479–1484.
Pinto D. and H. Clevers. 2005a. Wnt control of stem cells and differentiation in the intestinal
epithelium. Exp. Cell Res. 306:357–363.
Pinto D. and H. Clevers. 2005b. Wnt, stem cells and cancer in the intestine. Biol. Cell
97:185–196.
Possible Mechanism Involving Intestinal Oxytocin, Oxidative Stress 313
CONTENTS
16.1 Cytokine Biology: A General Introduction............................................... 315
16.2 Cytokine Polymorphisms and Cytokine Expression ................................ 317
16.3 Cytokines and the Brain ........................................................................... 318
16.4 Cytokines and Autism ............................................................................... 319
16.5 Why Study Cytokine Polymorphisms in Autism?
The Underlying Genetic Variability Hypothesis....................................... 320
16.6 Why Study Maternal Cytokine Polymorphisms? ..................................... 320
16.7 Discussion ................................................................................................. 321
16.8 Future Directions ...................................................................................... 322
References .............................................................................................................. 322
315
316 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Cytokines are usually very complex in their activities. Most cytokines are highly
pleiotropic and act on many different cell types exerting different effects. Different
cytokines can have redundant effects, or their activities can be either synergistic or
antagonistic depending on the target cells. Normally, cytokines do not act alone, but
are produced in a cascade fashion, and it is their combined effect that determines the
type of response. The responsiveness of a cell to a particular cytokine is determined
by the expression of specific receptors for that cytokine. The binding of the cytokine to
its receptor then triggers the activation of a variety of signaling mechanisms that lead
to changes at the level of gene expression, resulting in transient- or chronic-altered
cellular proliferation, differentiation, and/or function.
Cytokines play an essential role in the regulation of inflammatory responses and
are involved in the regulation of both innate (natural) and acquired immunities.
The cytokines that function in innate immunity and inflammation are normally
produced by cells of the monocitic and myeloid lineages and several other types of
cells in response to microbial, chemical, physical, and other types of inflammatory
stimuli. The main goal of the cytokines in these responses is the localization and
elimination of the instigating insult, orchestrating the recruitment of both cells and
molecules (accomplished through increased vascular permeability and leukocyte
infiltration) at the local site and a variety of systemic responses that include fever
and acute-phase protein synthesis, and the mobilization of leukocytes from the bone
marrow. Among the main cytokines involved in these responses are tumor necrosis
factor α (TNFα), interleukin (IL)-1, IL-6, IL-12, type I interferons (IFN; IFNα and
IFNβ), and chemokines, a family of cytokines that function to mobilize and attract
different types of leukocytes to inflammatory sites. These cytokines are said to
have a “proinflammatory” activity. If cytokines are secreted in excess as a result
of an overwhelming infection, or in cases where the insult cannot be easily elimi-
nated or the stimulus for cytokine secretion persists, leading to chronic inflammation,
these same cytokines can have pathologic effects leading to the damage of healthy
cells and tissues. Because of its potentially serious consequences, the immune sys-
tem has mechanisms to prevent excessive inflammation, including cytokines with
“anti-inflammatory” activity. These cytokines include transforming growth factor
β (TGFβ) and IL-10, which antagonize many of the effects of the “proinflamma-
tory” cytokines mentioned above. It should be kept in mind, however, that certain
cytokines can have both pro- and anti-inflammatory effects depending on different
factors such as the cell and tissue type and the kinetics of release.
In the case of specific immunity, cytokines play an equally important role. In general,
the type of cytokines produced during an immune response determines the effector
mechanisms that predominate and major expression patterns have been character-
ized (Mosmann and Coffman, 1989). For example, different subsets of helper (CD4+)
T cells that differ in both their cytokine secretion patterns and the effector mechanisms
that they induce have been identified. Cells belonging to the Th1 subset secrete IFNγ,
IL-2, and TNFα/β and are primarily involved in cellular immunity mechanisms and
delayed-type hypersensitivity reactions; cells of the Th2 subset secrete IL-4, IL-5,
IL-10, and IL-13 and are primarily involved in humoral mechanisms and allergic-
type reactions; and cells of the newly described Th17 subset secrete IL-17, IL-22, and
a variety of other proinflammatory cytokines (Harrington et al., 2006). Th17 cells
are thought to be involved not only in immune responses to extracellular bacteria
Cytokine Polymorphisms in Autism: Their Role in Immune Alterations 317
but also in autoimmune diseases. Th1 and Th2 cells affect one another: Th1 cells
trigger macrophage activation using IFNγ, which inhibits the proliferation of Th2
cells, and Th2 cells secrete IL-10, which inhibits the secretion of IFNγ by Th1 cells.
In keeping with the need for balance in the immune system, a different subset of
T cells exists, namely T-regulatory cells (Treg), which act as negative regulators
of the activities of other subsets. These cells act, in part, through the secretion of the
“anti-inflammatory” cytokines TGFβ and IL-10 (Bettelli et al., 2006).
Both in inflammation/innate immunity as well as in specific immunity, the
maintenance of a balance between pro- and anti-inflammatory cytokines or among
the different CD4+ T-cell subsets and their cytokines is essential for homeostasis
and the proper function of the immune system. Disruptions in this balance can
have pathologic implications resulting in excessive inflammation and tissue dam-
age, increased susceptibility to infectious agents, and/or the emergence of autoimmune
conditions (Ollier, 2004). For example, abnormal levels of different cytokines have
been described in many diseases, such as autoimmune hepatitis, rheumatoid arthritis,
asthma, systemic lupus erythematosus, inflammatory bowel disease, and some brain
disorders like schizophrenia and Alzheimer’s disease (Kronfol and Remick, 2000;
Theoharides et al., 2004; Vitkovic et al., 2000). Given that cytokines are key components
in the homeostatic mechanisms regulating the immune system, it is not surprising
that variations in their structure at the genetic or protein level (qualitative) or their
production level (quantitative) have been found to be associated with disease pro-
cesses and/or susceptibility to infections.
also regulated in cascades through feedback loops, and cytokine receptors have been
detected in the brain (Kronfol and Remick, 2000). Besides providing communica-
tion between neural cells, specific cytokines have a significant role in signaling the
brain to produce neurochemical, neuroendocrine, neuroimmune, and behavioral
changes (Maes et al., 1995). There is suggestive evidence that this signaling is a part
of the comprehensive mechanism to mobilize resources to combat physical and phys-
iological stress in an attempt to maintain relative homeostasis. Because cytokines
are associated with central neurotransmitters and cytokine regulation is affected by
stress, many studies have investigated the possible role for cytokines in psychiatric
disorders. These studies have demonstrated the role of abnormal levels of cytokines in
major depression, Alzheimer’s disease, and schizophrenia (Hanson and Gottesman,
2005; Hopkins, 2007; Maes et al., 1995; McGeer and McGeer, 2001a,b).
Juul-Dam et al., 2001; Maimburg and Vaeth, 2006; Patterson, 2002). The immune
activation of pregnant mice or rats using either lipopolysaccharide (a proxy for bacte-
rial infection) or polyriboinosinic polyribocytidylic acid (a proxy for viral infection)
results in modified cytokine expression in the maternal–fetal tandem pair (Gilmore
et al., 2005; Urakubo et al., 2001). Furthermore, the timing of the insult during ges-
tation is important with regard to the ultimate neurological and behavioral impact
(Smith et al., 2007).
This may have to do with the nature of the link between maternal and fetal immune
systems, and when the fetal immune system is capable of mounting an appropriate
response. It may be inferred that the timing and etiological origin of immune activa-
tion potentiates different neurological and behavioral fetal impacts. The variability
of the effect in both the timing of immune activation and time since activation is
likely linked both to the innate development of the fetal immune system and to an
altered Th1/Th2 balance in the mother. To our mind, variability due to cytokine
expression polymorphisms could either exacerbate or attenuate the degree of both
maternal and fetal immune activations.
16.7 DISCUSSION
Several studies demonstrate that an abnormal cytokine production is present in some
autistic populations, suggesting a more complex scenario where an abnormal immune
response is another component in the patient phenotype. If specific cytokine geno-
types associated with abnormal cytokine expression are linked to autism, this could
be understood as a genetic predisposition in the patient population. However, we must
be careful when trying to understand the meaning of “genetic predisposition” in the
development of this complex disease. Beyond the “cause or effect” paradox applied
to immune alterations in autism, we propose that specific genetic cytokine makeup
could be understood as a risk factor that will exacerbate any immune response that was
triggered by the multiple factors leading to the oxidative stress present in this disease
(Chauhan and Chauhan, 2006). Moreover, if the abnormal cytokine genetic makeup
is also present on the mother, since cytokines can cross the placenta, the convergence
of both phenotypes (mother and child) leading to an abnormal immune response could
significantly increase the risk—during pregnancy—for the development of autism.
Recent findings indicate that during the development of cortical and neuronal
organization, unknown factors influence neuronal and neuroglial cell populations,
disturbing neurodevelopment and producing the characteristic neuroanatomical
changes seen in autism (Vitkovic et al., 2000). This abnormality is best exemplified
in the significantly more narrow cortical minicolumns present in autistic individuals
when compared to controls (Casanova et al., 2006). As an effect of heterochronic
modifications to their developmental program, many autistic subjects have aug-
mented prefrontal cortices. This over-exuberant expansion of the prefrontal cortex
is by the addition of minicolumns. Clearly, there is population variability in cortical
size and minicolumnar density, and there are normal individuals with large brains
and densely packed minicolumns (and indeed autism has a familial component);
so why are some of these individuals autistic and others not? The manifestation of
emergent properties is dynamic, and their expression is a function of sufficient meta-
bolic supplies and active controls (Casanova and Tillquist, 2008).
322 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Here, we argue that the crossing of the threshold leading a disruption of active
controls and impinging upon long-range neural networks, leading to autism, is mater-
nal and/or fetal immune activation. This may have been a single activation, but we
would like to propose the novel idea that maternal and/or fetal immune activation may
permanently alter the fetal Th1/Th2/Th17 balance, predisposing the fetus to a lifetime
of chronic inflammation (or susceptibility to inflammation) or autoimmunity issues.
A major underlying genetic contribution to the balance of Th1/Th2/Th17 populations
must be related to the expression of different cytokines involved in the immune cas-
cades generated during immune activation. Given expression polymorphisms in certain
key cytokine genes, some overall immune phenotypes will be predisposed for stronger
or weaker immune activation, contributing to the etiology or emergence of autism.
REFERENCES
Ashwood, P., Wills, S., and Van de Water, J. 2006. The immune response in autism: A new
frontier for autism research. J. Leukoc. Biol. 80:1–15.
Bauer, J., Rauschka, H., and Lassmann, H. 2001. Inflammation in the nervous system: The human
perspective. Glia 36:235–243.
Bettelli, E., Carrier, Y., Gao, W., Korn, T., Strom, T.B., Oukka, M., Weiner, H.L., and Kuchroo, V.K.
2006. Reciprocal developmental pathways for the generation of pathogenic effector TH17
and regulatory T cells. Nature 441:235–238.
Borish, L.C. and Steinke, J.W. 2003. Cytokines and chemokines. J. Allergy Clin. Immunol.
111:S460–S475.
Casanova, M. and Tillquist, C. 2008. Encephalization, emergent properties, and psychiatry:
A minicolumnar perspective. Neuroscientist 14:101–118.
Casanova, M., van Kooten, I.A., Switala, A.E., van Engeland, H., Heinsen, H., Steinbusch, H.W.,
Hof, P.R., Trippe, J., Stone, J., and Schmitz, C. 2006. Minicolumnar abnormalities in
autism. Acta Neuropathol. 112:287–303.
Cytokine Polymorphisms in Autism: Their Role in Immune Alterations 323
Maes, M., Meltzer, H.Y., Bosmans, E., Bergmans, R., Vandoolaeghe, E., Ranjan, R., and
Desnyder, R. 1995. Increased plasma concentrations of interleukin-6, soluble interleukin-6
receptor, soluble interleukin-2 receptor and transferring receptor in major depression.
J. Affect. Disord. 34:301–309.
Maimburg, R. and Vaeth, M. 2006. Perinatal risk factors and infantile autism. Acta Psychiatr.
Scand. 114:257–264.
McGeer, P. and McGeer, E. 2001a. Polymorphisms in inflammatory genes and the risk of
Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 58:1790–1792.
McGeer, P. and McGeer, E. 2001b. Inflammation, autotoxicity and Alzheimer disease.
Neurobiol. Aging 22:799–809.
Molloy, C.A., Morrow, A.L, Meizen-Derr, J., Schleifer, K., Dienger, K., Manning-Courtney, P.,
Altaye, M., and Wills-Karp, M. 2006. Elevated cytokine levels in children with autism
spectrum disorder. J. Neuroimmunol. 172:198–205.
Mosmann, T. and Coffman, R. 1989. TH1 and TH2 cells: Difference patterns of lymphokine
secretion lead to different functional properties. Annu. Rev. Immunol. 7:145–173.
Ollier, W. 2004. Cytokines genes and disease susceptibility. Cytokine 28:174–178.
Patterson, P. 2002. Maternal infection: Window on neuroimmune interactions in fetal brain
development and mental illness. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 12:115–118.
Shi, L., Fatemi, S.H., Sidwell, R.W., and Patterson, P.H. 2003. Maternal influenza infection
causes marked behavioral and pharmacological changes in the offspring. J. Neurosci.
23:297–302.
Singh, V. 1996. Plasma increase of interleukin-12 and interferon-γ. Pathological significance
in autism. J. Neuroimmunol. 66:143–145.
Smith, S.E., Li, J., Garbett, K., Mirnics, K., and Patterson, P.H. 2007. Maternal immune activation
alters fetal brain development through interleukin-6. J. Neurosci. 27:10695–10702.
Stubbs, E. and Crawford, M. 1977. Depressed lymphocyte responsiveness in autistic children.
J. Autism Child Schizophr. 7:49–55.
Sweeten, T.L., Posey, D.J., and McDougle, C.J. 2003. High blood monocyte counts and neopterin
levels in children with autistic disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 160:1691–1693.
Sweeten, T.L., Posey, D.J., Shanker, S., and McDougle, C.J. 2004. High nitric oxide production
in autistic disorder: Possible role for interferon-γ. Biol. Psychiatry 55:434–437.
Theoharides, T., Weinkauf, C., and Conti, P. 2004. Brain cytokines and neuropsychiatric
disorders. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 24:577–581.
Urakubo, A., Jarskog, L.F., Lieberman, J.A., and Gilmore, J.H. 2001. Prenatal exposure to
maternal infection alters cytokine expression in the placenta, amniotic fluid, and fetal
brain. Schizophr. Res. 47:27–36.
Vargas, D.L., Nascimbene, C., Krishnan, C., Zimmerman, A.W., and Pardo, C.A. 2005.
Neuroglial activation and neuroinflammation in the brain of patients with autism. Ann.
Neurol. 57:67–81.
Vitkovic, L., Bockaert, J., and Jacque, C. 2000. Inflammatory cytokines: Neuromodulators in
normal brain? J. Neurochem. 74:457–471.
Vojdani, A., Pangborn, J.B., Vojdani, E., and Cooper, E.L. 2003. Infections, toxic chemicals
and dietary peptides binding to lymphocyte receptors and tissue enzymes are major
instigators of autoimmunity in autism. Int. J. Immunopathol. Pharmacol. 16:189–199.
Warlé, M.C., Farhan, A., Metselaar, H.J., Hop, W.C., Perrey, C., Zondervan, P.E., Kap, M.,
Kwekkeboom, J., Ijzermans, J.N., Tilanus, H.W., Pravica, V., Hutchinson, I.V., and
Bouma, G.J. 2003. Are cytokine gene polymorphisms related to in vitro cytokine
production profiles? Liver Transpl. 9:170–181.
Wills, S., Cabanlit, M., Bennett, J., Ashwood, P., Amaral, D., and Van de Water, J. 2007.
Autoantibodies in autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1107:79–91.
Zaretsky, M.V., Alexander, J.M., Byrd, W., and Bawdon, R.E. 2004. Transfer of inflammatory
cytokines across the placenta. Obstet. Gynecol. 103:546–550.
17 Autism, Teratogenic
Alleles, HLA-DR4,
and Immune Function
William G. Johnson,1,3,* Steven Buyske,4,5
Edward S. Stenroos,1,3 and George H. Lambert2,3
Departments of 1Neurology, 2Pediatrics, 3Center
for Childhood Neurotoxicology and Exposure
Assessment, Robert Wood Johnson Medical
School, University of Medicine and Dentistry
of New Jersey, Piscataway, NJ 08854, USA
Departments of 4Statistics and 5Genetics, Rutgers University,
New Brunswick, NJ 08854, USA
CONTENTS
17.1 Teratogenic Alleles in Neurodevelopmental Disorders............................. 326
17.2 Study Designs that Suggest or Document Action
of a Teratogenic Allele .............................................................................. 329
17.3 Early Examples of Teratogenic Alleles ..................................................... 330
17.3.1 Rh Incompatibility ..................................................................... 330
17.3.2 Maternal Phenylketonuria .......................................................... 331
17.4 A Rationale for Finding Teratogenic Alleles in Autism ........................... 332
17.5 HLA-DR4 and Autism ............................................................................... 332
17.5.1 Major Histocompatibility Complex ............................................ 332
17.5.2 Case–Control Studies of HLA-DR and Autism ......................... 332
17.5.3 Case–Parent Study of Autism .................................................... 334
17.6 Possible Modes of Action by which HLA-DR4
Might Contribute to Autism ...................................................................... 334
17.6.1 Oxidative Stress.......................................................................... 335
17.6.2 Synaptic Pruning and the MHC ................................................. 336
17.6.3 HLA-DR4 and High Relative Birthweight ................................. 337
17.6.4 Maternal Antibodies and Autism ............................................... 337
17.7 Future Studies of HLA-DR4 in Autism ..................................................... 337
References .............................................................................................................. 337
325
326 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
TABLE 17.1
Reports of Teratogenic Alleles
Teratogenic Allele
Report Disease Analysis By References
Maternal TDT
1. MTR*2756G NTD/spina bifida Mat TDT, log-linear Doolin et al. (2002)
2. MTRR*66G NTD/spina bifida Mat TDT, log-linear Doolin et al. (2002)
3. GSTP1*val105 Autism Mat TDT van Beynum et al.
(2006)
Maternal TDT-equivalent
4. Rh d Erythroblastosis Mat TDT equivalent Westgren et al. (1995)
fetalis
5. PAH mutations Maternal phe- Mat TDT equivalent Rouse and Azen (2004)
nylketonuria
6. Rh d Schizophrenia Mat TDT equivalent, Hollister, Laing, and
log-linear Mednick (1996),
Palmer et al. (2002)
Regression analysis
11. APOE*E2 Lower LDL-C, apoB, Forward stepwise Descamps et al. (2004)
higher HDL-C, and regression analysis
apoA1 in newborns
12. APOC3*S2 Lower newborn Forward stepwise Descamps et al. (2004)
LDL-C, apoB, regression analysis
HDL-C, and apoA1
13. LPL*S447X Lower newborn Forward stepwise Descamps et al. (2004)
LDL-C, apoB, regression analysis
and TG
14. GSTP1*Val105, Asthma Multiple linear Carroll et al. (2005)
*Val114 regression
Case–control
19. C4B*0 Autism Case–control Warren et al. (1991)
20. HLA-DR4 Autism Case–control Warren et al. (1996)
21. GSTP1–1b Recurrent early Case–control Zusterzeel et al. (2000)
pregnancy loss
22. MTHFR*1298C NTD/spina bifida Case–control De Marco et al. (2001)
23. MTRR*66G *GG Down syndrome Case–control O’Leary et al. (2002)
24. MTHFR*677T/ Down syndrome Case–control O’Leary et al. (2002)
MTRR*66GG
25. GSTT1*0 Oral-facial clefting Case–control van Rooij et al. (2001)
26. MTHFR*1298C NTD/spina bifida Case–control Gonzalez-Herrera et al.
(2002)
27. GSTM1*0 Recurrent pregnancy Case–control Sata et al. (2003)
loss
28. DHFR*19bp Spina bifida Case–control Johnson et al. (2004)
deletion
29. CYP1A1*2A Recurrent pregnancy Case–control Suryanarayana,
loss Deenadayal, and
Singh (2004)
(continued)
328 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Source: Adapted and reprinted from Johnson, W.G. et al., Teratogenic alleles in autism and other
neurodevelopmental disorders, in Autism: Current Theories and Evidence, ed. Zimmerman,
A., Humana Press, Totowa, NJ, 2008. With permission.
Notes: Reports of teratogenic alleles are listed: (1) according to the method of analysis used and
(2) according to the date of the report beginning with the earliest. The specific teratogenic allele
is listed on the left, next the specific disease or disorder studied, next the study design, and
finally, the literature reference. The specific teratogenic allele is given in the nomenclature: gene
symbol*allele designation. The names of the genes corresponding to the gene symbol are given
in the text and correspond to the designation in Online Mendelian Inheritance in Man. Gene
symbols are given in uppercase letters and italicized while the corresponding protein symbol is
given in the same uppercase letters but not italicized. Sometimes a genotype is given, as in #31
and 32, where “MTHFR*1298C” refers to the C-allele at position 1298 of the gene “MTHFR”
and CC, the genotype, refers to a double dose of the C-allele, i.e., homozygosity. Human genes are
given in uppercase italics, corresponding proteins in the same uppercase letters that are not itali-
cized. MTR, methionine synthase gene; MTRR, methionine synthase reductase gene; GSTP1,
glutathione S-transferase P1 gene; NTD, neural tube defect; PAH, phenylalanine hydroxylase
gene; Rh, Rhesus factor, a protein that is either present or absent on the surface of human red
blood cells; MTHFD1, methylenetetrahydrofolate dehydrogenase gene; CYP1A1, cytochrome
P450 1A1 gene; NAT1, N-acetyltransferase gene; CCL, monocyte chemoattractant protein gene;
APOE, apolipoprotein E; APOC3, apolipoprotein C3; LPL, lipoprotein lipase; MTHFR, methyl-
enetetrahydrofolate reductase gene; HLA, human leukocyte antigen system; OFC, oral-facial
clefting; CHD, congenital heart defect; C4B, complement component 4B, a blood group antigen;
GSTT1, glutathione S-transferase T1 gene; GSTM1, glutathione S-transferase M1 gene; DHFR,
dihydrofolate reductase gene; and RFC1, reduced folate carrier 1 gene.
living maternal grandparents can more readily be found. This approach is direct and
strongly supports the action of a teratogenic allele, but it does not address the interac-
tion between maternal and fetal genes.
A second approach is to use the case–parent log-linear method (Starr, Hsu, and
Schwartz, 2005; Weinberg, 1999; Weinberg and Wilcox, 1999; Weinberg et al., 1998;
Wilcox, Weinberg, and Lie, 1998). Since this method requires only the trio consisting
of the individual with the neurodevelopmental disorder and the two parents, it may
be the most suitable approach if living maternal grandparents are difficult to find. It
also addresses the question of the interaction between maternal and fetal genes. With
this method, the data is stratified by parental mating type and a modeling term is
included for maternal genotype. For example, a mother with AA genotype and father
with aa genotype represent the same mating type as a mother with aa and father with
AA, but the observation that the former pair occurs more often in parents of affected
offspring constitutes evidence that the AA genotype in mothers is a risk genotype for
offspring. There are reasons to believe that the case–parent log-linear method may
have less power than maternal TDT for the same number of families studied based
upon the one report that used both methods on the same dataset (Doolin et al., 2002)
(Table 17.1, #1 and 2).
A third approach uses “pents,” i.e., families with neurodevelopmental proband, par-
ents, and maternal grandparents (five individuals per family) (Mitchell and Weinberg,
2005). The pent approach has the advantage of estimating both maternal and offspring
genetic effects, and offers increased power, per proband, compared with the log-linear
approaches. Since DNA from maternal grandparents is required, as a practical matter,
it will work best for early onset disorders.
These three analytical approaches are not the only possible ones. Other approaches
have also been presented (Mitchell et al., 2005). Interestingly, some of the genes
identified in affected individuals by conventional approaches, especially allelic asso-
ciation studies, and thought to act in affected individuals may in fact be teratogenic
alleles. This is because these affected individuals frequently receive the teratogenic
alleles from their mothers simply by descent. These affected individuals will thus
themselves have an increased frequency of the teratogenic allele, as discussed ear-
lier, despite the fact that the allele acts in the mothers not in the affected individuals
themselves to contribute the phenotype of the affected individuals.
17.3.1 RH INCOMPATIBILITY
For Rh incompatibility to occur (Table 17.1, #4), the mother must lack the RhD
antigen on the surface of her erythrocytes and thus be Rh negative (RhD negative);
i.e., she must be a homozygote for the Rh d allele with the d/d genotype. Also the
Autism, Teratogenic Alleles, HLA-DR4, and Immune Function 331
fetus must carry an Rh D allele that can only be of paternal origin (father is RhD
positive). During the pregnancy, the Rh d/d mother is exposed to the RhD antigen
produced by the fetus. Since the mother lacks this antigen, she makes antibodies
to RhD antigen as the pregnancy progresses. During a subsequent pregnancy with an
RhD positive fetus, maternal antibodies are again produced but in greater amount and
in an accelerated fashion. With further RhD positive fetuses, the mother mounts an
immunological attack upon the fetus who may develop erythroblastosis fetalis and a
developmental disorder. This developmental disorder consists of three clinical syn-
dromes in the fetus and neonate: anemia of the newborn; neonatal jaundice that can
lead to the severe fetal encephalopathy of kernicterus; and the generalized neonatal
edema of hydrops fetalis with massive anasarca, pleural effusions, and ascites. The
teratogenic allele here is Rh d, for which the mother is a homozygote. Each of her
parents carries an Rh d allele and has transmitted an Rh d allele to her. Thus, trans-
mission disequilibrium is present. The mechanisms of fetal damage are unclear, but
cytokine abnormalities have been observed (Westgren et al., 1995).
Rh incompatibility has also been recently linked to another neurodevelopmental
disorder, schizophrenia (Table 17.1, #6). An increased incidence of schizophrenia
has been found in the offspring of pregnancies with Rh incompatibility (Hollister,
Laing, and Mednick, 1996) compared to control pregnancies. This effect was not dem-
onstrated for autism in a similar study (Zandi et al., 2006).
(maternal PKU, mothers, and their parents), it would show transmission disequi-
librium. However, since the maternal PKU mothers have PKU and are thus known
to be homozygotes for PAH mutations, such a TDT is now unnecessary. Thus, this
documentation of the action of a teratogenic allele in maternal PKU is, in a sense, the
equivalent of a maternal TDT.
The finding of the increased frequency of a polymorphic allele in mothers but not
fathers of affected individuals is an interesting one because it may help identify a factor
that contributes to the disease. There are several possible explanations of this pattern
of increased allele frequency. The known possible reasons for the increased frequency
of an allele observed in mothers of affected individuals include the following:
levels have been reported in children with ASD (Molloy et al., 2006). Interestingly
increased HLA-DR homozygosity both in pre-eclamptic women and in their part-
ners has been strongly associated with pre-eclampsia (de Luca Brunori et al.,
2000), a disorder in which oxidative stress plays a role. A number of possible
mechanisms exist by which maternal inflammation during pregnancy can affect
neuronal development (Jonakait, 2007). The neuropoietic cytokine family appears
to play an important role in nervous system development as well as in affecting
neuronal plasticity (Bauer, Kerr, and Patterson, 2007). In a rat model using lipopoly-
saccharide (LPS) to initiate maternal inflammation and induce maternal infection
during pregnancy, it has been demonstrated that cytokines may mediate effects of
prenatal infection on the fetus, a finding that has implications for neurodevelop-
mental disorder such as schizophrenia (Ashdown et al., 2006) and autism. Another
approach to inducing maternal infection used maternal poly I:C exposure to cause
maternal inflammation without an infectious agent and found that this regulates
tumor necrosis factor-α, brain-derived neurotrophic factor, and nerve growth factor
expressions in neonatal brain and the maternal–fetal unit of the rat (Gilmore, Jarskog,
and Vadlamudi, 2005). In a rat model, the inflammatory/cytokine response from the
maternal injection of LPS affected both placenta and fetal brain (Bell, Hallenbeck,
and Gallo, 2004). In another report using a rat model, prenatal exposure to mater-
nal inflammation induced by LPS mimicking maternal infection, altered cytokine
expression in placenta, amniotic fluid, and fetal brain (Urakubo et al., 2001).
REFERENCES
Abadie, V., E. Depondt, J. P. Farriaux, J. Lepercq, S. Lyonnet, N. Maurin, H. Ogier de Baulny,
and M. Vidailhet. 1996. Pregnancy and the child of a mother with phenylketonuria.
Arch. Pediatr. 3:489–486.
Allen, R. J., J. Brunberg, E. Schwartz, A. M. Schaefer, and G. Jackson. 1994. MRI character-
ization of cerebral dysgenesis in maternal PKU. Acta Paediatr. Suppl. 407:83–85.
American Psychiatric Association. 1994. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, 4th ed., Washington D.C.: American Psychiatric Association.
Ashdown, H., Y. Dumont, M. Ng, S. Poole, P. Boksa, and G. N. Luheshi. 2006. The role of
cytokines in mediating effects of prenatal infection on the fetus: Implications for schizo-
phrenia. Mol. Psychiatry 11:47–55.
338 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Bauer, S., B. J. Kerr, and P. H. Patterson. 2007. The neuropoietic cytokine family in development,
plasticity, disease and injury. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8:221–232.
Bell, M. J., J. M. Hallenbeck, and V. Gallo. 2004. Determining the fetal inflammatory response
in an experimental model of intrauterine inflammation in rats. Pediatr. Res. 56:541–546.
Braunschweig, D., P. Ashwood, P. Krakowiak, I. Hertz-Picciotto, R. Hansen, L. A. Croen,
I. N. Pessah, and J. Van de Water. 2008. Autism: Maternally derived antibodies specific
for fetal brain proteins. Neurotoxicology 29:226–231.
Brody, L. C., M. Conley, C. Cox, P. N. Kirke, M. P. McKeever, J. L. Mills, A. M. Molloy,
V. B. O’Leary, A. Parle-McDermott, J. M. Scott, and D. A. Swanson. 2002. A polymor-
phism, R653Q, in the trifunctional enzyme methylenetetrahydrofolate dehydrogenase/
methenyltetrahydrofolate cyclohydrolase/formyltetrahydrofolate synthetase is a mater-
nal genetic risk factor for neural tube defects: Report of the Birth Defects Research
Group. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 71:1207–1215.
Buyske, S., T. A. Williams, A. E. Mars, E. S. Stenroos, S. X. Ming, R. Wang, M. Sreenath, M. F.
Factura, C. Reddy, G. H. Lambert, and W. G. Johnson. 2006. Analysis of case-parent trios
at a locus with a deletion allele: Association of GSTM1 with autism. BMC Genet. 7:8.
Carroll, W. D., W. Lenney, F. Child, R. C. Strange, P. W. Jones, and A. A. Fryer. 2005. Maternal
glutathione S-transferase GSTP1 genotype is a specific predictor of phenotype in children
with asthma. Pediatr. Allergy Immunol. 16:32–39.
Casanova, M. F., D. Buxhoeveden, and J. Gomez. 2003. Disruption in the inhibitory architec-
ture of the cell minicolumn: Implications for autism. Neuroscientist 9:496–507.
Casanova, M. F., D. P. Buxhoeveden, A. E. Switala, and E. Roy. 2002. Minicolumnar pathology
in autism. Neurology 58:428–432.
Chauhan, A. and V. Chauhan. 2006. Oxidative stress in autism. Pathophysiology 13:171–181.
Chauhan, A., V. Chauhan, W. T. Brown, and I. Cohen. 2004. Oxidative stress in autism:
Increased lipid peroxidation and reduced serum levels of ceruloplasmin and transferrin—
the antioxidant proteins. Life Sci. 75:2539–2549.
Chen, D., Y. Hu, F. Yang, Z. Li, B. Wu, Z. Fang, J. Li, and L. Wang. 2005. Cytochrome P450
gene polymorphisms and risk of low birth weight. Genet. Epidemiol. 28:368–375.
Comi, A. M., A. W. Zimmerman, V. H. Frye, P. A. Law, and J. N. Peeden. 1999. Familial
clustering of autoimmune disorders and evaluation of medical risk factors in autism.
J. Child Neurol. 14:388–394.
Daniels, W. W., R. P. Warren, J. D. Odell, A. Maciulis, R. A. Burger, W. L. Warren, and
A. R. Torres. 1995. Increased frequency of the extended or ancestral haplotype
B44-SC30-DR4 in autism. Neuropsychobiology 32:120–123.
de Luca Brunori, I., L. Battini, M. Simonelli, F. Clemente, E. Brunori, M. L. Mariotti, and
A. R. Genazzani. 2000. Increased HLA-DR homozygosity associated with pre-eclamp-
sia. Hum. Reprod. 15:1807–1812.
De Marco, P., M. G. Calevo, A. Moroni, L. Arata, E. Merello, A. Cama, R. H. Finnell,
L. Andreussi, and V. Capra. 2001. Polymorphisms in genes involved in folate metabo-
lism as risk factors for NTDs. Eur. J. Pediatr. Surg. 11(Suppl. 1):S14–S17.
Descamps, O. S., M. Bruniaux, P. F. Guilmot, R. Tonglet, and F. R. Heller. 2004. Lipoprotein
concentrations in newborns are associated with allelic variations in their mothers.
Atherosclerosis 172:287–298.
Doolin, M. T., S. Barbaux, M. McDonnell, K. Hoess, A. S. Whitehead, and L. E. Mitchell.
2002. Maternal genetic effects, exerted by genes involved in homocysteine remethyla-
tion, influence the risk of spina bifida. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 71:1222–1226.
Fatemi, S. H., J. M. Stary, and E. A. Egan. 2002. Reduced blood levels of reelin as a
vulnerability factor in pathophysiology of autistic disorder. Cell Mol. Neurobiol.
22:139–152.
Fatemi, S. H., J. M. Stary, A. R. Halt, and G. R. Realmuto. 2001. Dysregulation of reelin and
Bcl-2 proteins in autistic cerebellum. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 31:529–535.
Autism, Teratogenic Alleles, HLA-DR4, and Immune Function 339
Gilmore, J. H., L. F. Jarskog, and S. Vadlamudi. 2005. Maternal poly I:C exposure during
pregnancy regulates TNF alpha, BDNF, and NGF expression in neonatal brain and the
maternal-fetal unit of the rat. J. Neuroimmunol. 159:106–112.
Goddard, C. A., D. A. Butts, and C. J. Shatz. 2007. Regulation of CNS synapses by neuronal
MHC class I. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104:6828–6833.
Gonzalez-Herrera, L. J., M. P. Flores-Machado, I. C. Castillo-Zapata, M. G. Garcia-Escalante,
D. Pinto-Escalante, and A. Gonzalez-Del Angel. 2002. Interaction of C677T and
A1298C polymorphisms in the MTHFR gene in association with neural tube defects in
the State of Yucatan, Mexico (abst). Am. J. Hum. Genet. 71:367.
Heuer, L, P. Ashwood, and J. Van de Water. 2008. The immune system in autism. Is there a
connection? In Autism: Current Theories and Evidence, ed. A. W. Zimmerman, Totowa,
NJ: Humana Press.
Holladay, S. D., L. Sharova, B. J. Smith, R. M. Gogal Jr., D. L. Ward, and B. L. Blaylock.
2000. Nonspecific stimulation of the maternal immune system. I. Effects on teratogen-
induced fetal malformations. Teratology 62:413–419.
Holladay, S. D., L. V. Sharova, K. Punareewattana, T. C. Hrubec, R. M. Gogal, Jr., M. R. Prater,
and A. A. Sharov. 2002. Maternal immune stimulation in mice decreases fetal malforma-
tions caused by teratogens. Int. Immunopharmacol. 2:325–332.
Hollister, J. M., P. Laing, and S. A. Mednick. 1996. Rhesus incompatibility as a risk factor for
schizophrenia in male adults. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 53:19–24.
Hummel, M., S. Marienfeld, M. Huppmann, A. Knopff, M. Voigt, E. Bonifacio, and A. G. Ziegler.
2007. Fetal growth is increased by maternal type 1 diabetes and HLA DR4-related gene
interactions. Diabetologia 50:850–858.
James, S. J., P. Cutler, S. Melnyk, S. Jernigan, L. Janak, D. W. Gaylor, and J. A. Neubrander.
2004. Metabolic biomarkers of increased oxidative stress and impaired methylation
capacity in children with autism. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 80:1611–1617.
Jensen, L. E., A. J. Etheredge, K. S. Brown, L. E. Mitchell, and A. S. Whitehead. 2006a.
Maternal genotype for the monocyte chemoattractant protein 1 A(-2518)G promoter
polymorphism is associated with the risk of spina bifida in offspring. Am. J. Med. Genet. A
140:1114–1118.
Jensen, L. E., K. Hoess, L. E. Mitchell, and A. S. Whitehead. 2006b. Loss of function poly-
morphisms in NAT1 protect against spina bifida. Hum. Genet. 120:52–57.
Johnson, W. G. 1999. The DNA polymorphism-diet-cofactor-development hypothesis and the
gene-teratogen model for schizophrenia and other developmental disorders. Am. J. Med.
Genet. 88:311–323.
Johnson, W. G. 2003. Teratogenic alleles and neurodevelopmental disorders. Bioessays
25:464–477.
Johnson, W. G., E. S. Stenroos, J. Spychala, S. Buyske, S. Chatkupt, and X. Ming. 2004.
A new 19 bp deletion polymorphism in intron-1 of dihydrofolate reductase (DHFR):
A risk factor for spina bifida acting in mothers during pregnancy? Am. J. Med. Genet.
124A: 339–345.
Johnson, W. G., T. O. Scholl, J. R. Spychala, S. Buyske, E. S. Stenroos, and X. Chen. 2005.
Common dihydrofolate reductase 19bp deletion allele: A novel risk factor for preterm
delivery. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 81:664–668.
Johnson, W. G., M. Sreenath, S. Buyske, and E. S. Stenroos. 2008. Teratogenic alleles in autism
and other neurodevelopmental disorders. In Autism: Current Theories and Evidence, ed.
A. Zimmerman. Totowa, NJ: Humana Press.
Johnson, W. G., S. Buyske, A. E. Mars, M. Sreenath, E. S. Stenroos, T. A. Williams, R. Stein,
and G. H. Lambert. 2009. HLA-DR4 as a risk allele for autism, acting in mothers of
probands possibly during pregnancy. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 163:542–546.
Jonakait, G. M. 2007. The effects of maternal inflammation on neuronal development: Possible
mechanisms. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 25:415–425.
340 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Piven, J. and D. O’Leary. 1999. Neuroimaging in autism. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am.
6:305–323.
Rai, A. K., S. Singh, S. Mehta, A. Kumar, L. K. Pandey, and R. Raman. 2006. MTHFR C677T
and A1298C polymorphisms are risk factors for Down’s syndrome in Indian mothers.
J. Hum. Genet. 51:278–283.
Rapin, I. 1997. Autism. N. Engl. J. Med. 337:97–104.
Rodier, P. M., J. L. Ingram, B. Tisdale, S. Nelson, and J. Romano. 1996. Embryological origin
for autism: Developmental anomalies of the cranial nerve motor nuclei. J. Comp. Neurol.
370:247–261.
Rouse, B. and C. Azen. 2004. Effect of high maternal blood phenylalanine on offspring
congenital anomalies and developmental outcome at ages 4 and 6 years: The impor-
tance of strict dietary control preconception and throughout pregnancy. J. Pediatr.
144:235–239.
Sadeharju, K., M. Knip, M. Hiltunen, H. K. Akerblom, and H. Hyoty. 2003. The HLA-DR phe-
notype modulates the humoral immune response to enterovirus antigens. Diabetologia
46:1100–1105.
Sata, F., H. Yamada, T. Kondo, Y. Gong, S. Tozaki, G. Kobashi, E. H. Kato, S. Fujimoto, and
R. Kishi. 2003. Glutathione S-transferase M1 and T1 polymorphisms and the risk of
recurrent pregnancy loss. Mol. Hum. Reprod. 9:165–169.
Scala, I., B. Granese, M. Sellitto, S. Salome, A. Sammartino, A. Pepe, P. Mastroiacovo,
G. Sebastio, and G. Andria. 2006. Analysis of seven maternal polymorphisms of genes
involved in homocysteine/folate metabolism and risk of Down syndrome offspring.
Genet. Med. 8:409–416.
Serajee, F. J., H. Zhong, and A. H. M. Huq. 2006. Association of Reelin gene polymorphisms
with autism. Genomics 87:75–83.
Serajee, F. J., R. Nabi, H. Zhong, and M. Huq. 2004. Polymorphisms in xenobiotic metabolism
genes and autism. J. Child Neurol. 19:413–417.
Sharova, L., P. Sura, B. J. Smith, R. M. Gogal, Jr., A. A. Sharov, D. L. Ward, and S. D. Holladay.
2000. Nonspecific stimulation of the maternal immune system. II. Effects on gene
expression in the fetus. Teratology 62:420–428.
Skaar, D. A., Y. Shao, J. L. Haines, J. E. Stenger, J. Jaworski, E. R. Martin, G. R. DeLong,
J. H. Moore, J. L. McCauley, J. S. Sutcliffe, A. E. Ashley-Koch, M. L. Cuccaro, S. E. Folstein,
J. R. Gilbert, and M. A. Pericak-Vance. 2005. Analysis of the RELN gene as a genetic
risk factor for autism. Mol. Psychiatry 10:563–571.
Starr, J. R., L. Hsu, and S. M. Schwartz. 2005. Assessing maternal genetic associations:
A comparison of the log-linear approach to case-parent triad data and a case-control
approach. Epidemiology 16:294–303.
Stevens, B., N. J. Allen, L. E. Vazquez, G. R. Howell, K. S. Christopherson, N. Nouri,
K. D. Micheva, A. K. Mehalow, A. D. Huberman, B. Stafford, A. Sher, A. M. Litke,
J. D. Lambris, S. J. Smith, S. W. John, and B. A. Barres. 2007. The classical complement
cascade mediates CNS synapse elimination. Cell 131:1164–1178.
Stromland, K., V. Nordin, M. Miller, B. Akerstrom, and C. Gillberg. 1994. Autism in thalidomide
embryopathy: A population study. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 36:351–356.
Suryanarayana, V., M. Deenadayal, and L. Singh. 2004. Association of CYP1A1 gene poly-
morphism with recurrent pregnancy loss in the South Indian population. Hum. Reprod.
19:2648–2652.
Szatmari, P. 2003. The causes of autism spectrum disorders. Brit. Med. J. 326:173–174.
Thams, S., A. Oliveira, and S. Cullheim. 2008. MHC class I expression and synaptic plasticity
after nerve lesion. Brain Res. Rev. 57:265–269.
Torres, A. R., A. Maciulis, E. G. Stubbs, A. Cutler, and D. Odell. 2002. The transmission
disequilibrium test suggests that HLA-DR4 and DR13 are linked to autism spectrum
disorder. Hum. Immunol. 63:311–316.
342 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
CONTENTS
18.1 Introduction ................................................................................................. 345
18.1.1 Classical Model: Behavioral Syndrome Deriving from
Genetically Determined Static Encephalopathy ............................ 345
18.1.2 Emerging Understanding of Active Persistent Pathophysiology .....346
18.1.3 Does Active, Ongoing Pathophysiology Actively Impact
Functions Central to ASD? ............................................................ 347
18.1.4 Evidence for the Potential for Plasticity and Its Pertinence ........... 349
18.1.5 Rethinking Basic Assumptions ...................................................... 350
18.2 Interrogation of Earlier Assumptions and Prior Findings from Newer
Vantage Point .............................................................................................. 352
18.2.1 Is Autism Purely a Developmental Disorder?
Or Are Its Active and Persistent Pathophysiological
Features Centrally Important? ....................................................... 352
18.2.1.1 Reassessing What We Know .......................................... 353
18.2.1.2 Probable Centrality of Glial Cells in ASD ..................... 358
18.2.1.3 From “Developmental Disorder” to “Chronic
Dynamic Encephalopathy” ............................................ 359
18.2.1.4 Sample Scenario of Pathophysiology-Based
Narrative of Autistic Regression ....................................360
343
344 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
The purpose of this chapter is to reflect upon the implications of the identification
of active pathophysiological processes in autism spectrum disorders (ASD), and to
reflect back upon prior findings and formulations in the light of these recent dis-
coveries. This chapter articulates challenges posed by these discoveries to deeply
held assumptions about the ASD. These assumptions are embodied in a classi-
cal model framing the ASD as a problem of genes, brain, and behavior, i.e., as a
genetically determined developmental disorder of the brain whose main manifesta-
tion is behavioral alterations based on an indelible static encephalopathy; this model
would not have predicted the growing documentation of pathophysiological distur-
bances. This chapter also describes an emerging pathophysiology-centered model of
autism that can subsume genes, brain, and behavior but also includes much more.
Prior findings and models are reevaluated to support the framing of the ASD as
(1) not only a developmental but also a chronic condition based on active pathophysi-
ology, (2) not only having behavioral but also having somatic and systemic features
that are not secondary but rather intrinsic consequences of underlying mechanisms,
(3) not only genetic but also environmental, (4) not a static encephalopathy but a
dynamic, recalcitrant encephalopathy, and (5) not a set of discrete behavioral fea-
tures neatly mapping to specific genetic mechanisms but a set of emergent properties
dynamically arising from pathophysiological systems whose parameters have been
dramatically and interactively perturbed. It is argued that a research program based
on this approach will incorporate the strengths of the classical model, will encourage
many more routes to investigations with practical and treatment applications, and may
be a much more rapid path to provide much-needed help to affected individuals and
their families.
Autism: The Centrality of Active Pathophysiology 345
18.1 INTRODUCTION
While autism spectrum disorders (ASD) can involve exquisite gifts and unusual
qualities of perception and thought, they can also involve a great deal of suffering,
for the individual on the spectrum as well as family and community. On this account,
a core question in autism work needs to be how to help the most people in the most
effective ways as quickly as possible. The goal of making sense of autism and its
mechanisms needs to be deeply harnessed to this core purpose. Our aim should be to
relieve suffering at multiple levels—from aversive sensory overload, sleep disruption,
recurrent infections, and gastrointestinal (GI) troubles to overcoming obstacles to
communication; to misunderstanding by the non-autistic people of the experiences
of people with autism; to aggression and self-injurious behavior; to the burden of
allocating scarce resources to deliver therapies that may not be optimally designed,
targeted, or implemented; and to acrimonious debate and fiscal drain. Last but hardly
the least, if any part of the impairment of optimal functioning in new cases of autism
is not purely genetically determined, the suffering and severity that is therefore neither
inevitable nor necessary should be prevented or ameliorated.
If we are to help most quickly and with the broadest and greatest effectiveness,
then how do we do so, and how much can we really help? What can we realisti-
cally expect to accomplish? The answers we give to these questions are greatly
conditioned by what we understand autism to be. The main goal of this chapter is
to explain and compare two models of autism that lead to greatly different expec-
tations: (1) a classical model of autism as a genetically determined developmental
brain disorder and static encephalopathy and (2) an emerging model of autism
centered around active systemic environmentally as well as genetically influenced
pathophysiological processes beginning early in life and leading to an chronic
persistent encephalopathy with dynamic features. This comparison will show that
the emerging dynamic pathophysiological model not only includes the strengths
of the classical static model, but also takes account of emerging data that are incom-
mensurable with the older formulations. It will also give support to the argument
that the emerging more inclusive model offers more opportunities for constructive
investigation and intervention.
some critical aberrant pathways have any chance of unlocking neural functioning,
but these pathways have yet to be discovered and the molecules to target them are yet
to be invented. Therefore, to identify targets and develop effective and safe interven-
tions, an extensive, expensive, and long-term research strategy is necessary in order
even to begin to relieve suffering in any serious way.
The recent framing of autism as heterogeneous, or “autisms” (plural), modifies
this model by suggesting that “autism” is really a collection of different “autisms,”
each with its own mechanism and perhaps even its own gene(s). The research
program derived from this framing would still look for distinctive mechanisms, but
now may implicitly propose multiple parallel searches for mechanisms. If this is
not accompanied by seeking final common pathways that may bridge across these
distinct “autisms” and provide routes of intervention that could be beneficial more
broadly than to any one small subgroup, then the road ahead is even longer.
which generally occurs somewhere around the middle of the second year of life. Even
if “regression” is preceded by a variety of subtle signs of dysfunction, it is occurring
far beyond the most critical periods of brain development and deserves investigation
as a new event and in particular, as a shift in functional/metabolic/neurodynamic state
and not just as an inevitable playing out of early hard-wired brain alterations.
With chronic-active pathophysiology identified systemically and in brain tissue
from individuals with autism, with this active pathophysiology having potential
neuromodulatory effects, and with functional changes such as regression needing
mechanistic explanation, it becomes necessary to consider the possibility that the
biological basis of the autism behavioral phenotype may not be determined “archi-
tecturally” once and for all in utero, but rather may be actively sustained, possibly
even caused or at least substantially aggravated by persistently active pathophysiology
(Anderson, Hooker, and Herbert 2008, Zimmerman 2008).
We can imagine a number of possibilities: (1) inputs (e.g., genes and environmental
factors) create an indelible alteration in prenatal or early postnatal brain development;
1b) these indelible in utero impacts of genes and environmental factors are mediated
by pathophysiological processes such as inflammation or oxidative stress; (2) some
inputs (e.g., genes, teratogens, infections, or immune responses to infections) increase
vulnerability to other inputs that alter early prenatal or early postnatal brain devel-
opment; (3) some inputs increase vulnerability to other inputs (e.g., excitotoxins) or
pathophysiological states (e.g., immune triggers and oxidative stress) that alter neu-
ral function postnatally; and (4) chronic, persistent alteration in neural function (e.g.,
cumulative toxic body burden and/or chronic neuroinflammation having a persistent
excitotoxic impact) can in turn lead to changes in brain tissue (e.g., mitochondrial
damage → cellular dysfunction → cell death), which in turn may feed back to further
affect function.
Once these additional dimensions beyond the genetic determination of altered
brain development join the parameters of concern, how do we assess what the type
of influence and relative weight may be of each class of contributor? How far can this
be pushed? For example, if the excitotoxic modulation of synaptic function is chronic
(i.e., from ongoing exposure or chronic inflammation) and/or persistent (i.e., with
semipermanent effects from even a transient exposure), can we consider whether it
could contribute to a chronic encephalopathy? And could such a chronic encephal-
opathy potentially in some cases not simply modulate the autism but actually be the
autism? Could genetic vulnerability and genetic impacts turn into autism (or more
severe autism) with the onset of these pathophysiological processes? We obviously
do not know the answer, but this chapter reflects on the question.
Insofar as pathophysiological mechanisms can be affected by environmental
input, it is also important to consider potential positive impacts. If there is a for-
mative role for pathophysiology, this suggests that factors like diet, sleep quality,
stress, exercise, autonomic arousal, environmental exposures, and medications all
could be having substantial short-term impacts on symptom severity and the qual-
ity of life. It also suggests that such factors, which include both health-promoting
and health-destroying variants, can have substantial effects over time on the level
of function and the quality of life. On the scale of years, the “ongoing” nature of
pathophysiological activity means that some interventions might be able to provide
major long-term benefits as well.
Autism: The Centrality of Active Pathophysiology 349
18.1.4 EVIDENCE FOR THE POTENTIAL FOR PLASTICITY AND ITS PERTINENCE
To make a plausible argument that active, persistent pathophysiology might strongly
modulate or even create core features of autism, there would need to be evidence
of some kind of intraindividual variabilities in the phenotype that occurred in
relationship to pathophysiologically pertinent processes. Such variability (e.g.,
symptom onset, marked worsening, or marked improvement) would suggest that
fluctuations in modulatory processes might have significant impact. As it happens,
such evidence exists.
The idea that physiological modulation could contribute more than marginally
is becoming less far-fetched in the light of published reports of short-term marked
improvements in core features of autism. Investigators recently pursued suggestions
from clinical case reports that behaviors and core capacities in autism may improve
markedly in the setting of fever (Curran et al. 2007)—clinicians were fairly com-
monly hearing from parents that their affected children could relate better, make more
eye contact, and sometimes even talk transiently in the setting of fever—one mother
poignantly described her experience during her child’s fever to the author of the pres-
ent review as “visiting with my son.” A prospective study was thus performed utiliz-
ing the Aberrant Behavior Checklist to rate behavior changes; the study found that
fewer aberrant behaviors were recorded for febrile patients on the subscales of irrita-
bility, hyperactivity, stereotypy, and inappropriate speech compared with control sub-
jects in a fashion that was not associated with the severity of illness. While lethargy
scores were greater during fevers, and all improvements were transient, the behavioral
improvements could not be attributed to the lethargy and the results instead suggested
a genuine improvement in core functions. An earlier paper investigated 11 children
with the history common in ASD of a period of often recurrent infection and antibi-
otic exposure followed by the development of chronic persistent diarrhea and then the
onset of autistic features, or “regression” (Sandler et al. 2000). This common phenom-
enon has spawned research demonstrating abnormal variants of clostridial bacteria
in ASD (Finegold et al. 2002, Parracho et al. 2005, Song, Liu, and Finegold 2004)
and animal models showing nervous system and behavioral impacts of propionic
acid, a metabolic product of clostridia (MacFabe et al., 2007, Shultz et al. 2008a,b)
that are part of a larger ferment of research on the influence of intestinal microecology
(the “microbiome”) on medical and psychiatric health (Alverdy and Chang 2008,
Li et al. 2008, Nicholson, Holmes, and Wilson 2005). This study investigated impact
on the behavior of oral vancomycin, which is a potent antibiotic normally given intrave-
nously and minimally absorbed from the intestine but that devastates intestinal micro-
organisms. They noted significant short-term improvement using multiple pre- and
post-therapy evaluations coded by a blinded clinical psychologist, with the transiency
presumably due to the regrowth of pathogenic intestinal microorganisms after the ces-
sation of antibiotic dosing. In both of these cases, the improvement was notable, rapid
in onset, and short in duration suggesting that the maladaptive physiological setpoint
was insufficiently challenged by fever or transiently altered intestinal microbiota to
shift to a different semi-stable state.
Some challenges prior to conceptions of developmental disorders have also
emerged on the laboratory front. Symptom reversal has recently been reported in
mouse models of developmental disorders—fragile X syndrome (Hayashi et al. 2007),
350 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Rett syndrome (Guy et al. 2007), and tuberous sclerosis (Ehninger et al. 2008), all
considered genetic and incurable—through molecular intervention, including in
older animals. This is striking because it forever undermines the basis for simply
taking for granted that neurodevelopmental disorders are incurable or have only a
narrow critical window after which intervention is pointless. At the other end of the
lifecourse, rapid though transient reduction of Alzheimer’s disease symptoms within
minutes of the administration of perispinal etanercept suggests that chronic-active
and potentially reversible pathophysiology may also contribute to the encephalopathy
in this devastatingly progressive disorder (Tobinick and Gross 2008a,b).
With regard to the autism clinical papers discussed above, it is critical to note
that fever does not create a permanent alteration of immunologic or neurobiological
pathways, and oral vancomycin does not permanently alter intestinal flora, consis-
tent with the changes not being persistent. But it is also critical to note that these
supposedly lifelong core features of autism could be altered even in the short term,
which itself is inconsistent with a “static encephalopathy” model. All of this chal-
lenges us to think outside of the box of irretrievable brain damage in relation to the
encephalopathy of ASD (and other conditions as well). The potential mechanisms
that come to mind are in the domain of active, dynamic pathophysiology (including
but hardly limited to altered gene expression) rather than genetic predetermination,
as the genetic mechanisms causing an in utero disturbance of brain development
would not explain such short-term fluctuations. In the Curran et al. (2007) paper on
improvement with fever, the authors speculated that the phenomenon was driven by
some mechanisms related to immunologic and neurobiological pathways, intracel-
lular signaling, and synaptic plasticity; in the Sandler et al. (2000) paper, the authors
speculated that the oral antibiotic transiently suppressed an enteric microorganism
and its production of a neurotoxin-like substance.
If such marked short-term changes are possible, the idea that the encephalopathy
in ASD is a dynamic (albeit recalcitrant) “state” rather than a wired-in static “trait”
becomes conceivable, and the possibility of identifying the mechanism for and
extending the duration of such changes can be framed as a worthwhile and important
goal for research.
The implication of this is major: it means that we must consider the possibility
that the functional impairments we observe in individuals with autism may be prod-
ucts not so much of innate “deficits” as of the active (and obstinate) pathophysiological
obstruction of capacities for which brain substrate is still at least partly present.
Moreover, given that these processes are known to progressively assault and damage
cellular integrity, and given the evidence suggesting progressive changes in brain
tissue (cellular changes (Bauman and Kemper 2005) and volume loss (Aylward et al.
2002)), the importance of finding ways to medically intervene to slow or stop this
degeneration as early as possible comes into clear focus.
findings for fresh interpretations. The goal of this chapter is thus to spell out
how emerging fi ndings are revealing limitations in the assumptions of the classi-
cal view, and to outline some core features of a newer more inclusive view. These
emerging findings are elucidating mechanisms suggesting that autism is more than
a developmental disorder, that more than genes are etiologically contributory, and
that the encephalopathy has dynamic features so that it is not strictly static.
We will develop the argument by posing the following questions, and explaining
our rationale for the following responses.
Questions:
Responses:
need to target features specific to autism, but that therapies targeting underly-
ing physiological features that are contributory but not unique to ASD could
lead to altered emergent properties including altered behaviors.
The idea that ASD is a developmental disorder seems self-evident. ASD begins in
early childhood, with abnormalities in responsiveness sometimes even evident at
birth. Brain abnormalities have been documented at the neuropathological level con-
sistent with changes occurring in utero. The high heritability and high recurrence
rate also support this framing. The characteristic clustering of behavioral features in
the ASD phenotype suggests some kind of specific causes.
There are other ways of interpreting the above cluster of phenomena. These fea-
tures of early onset, neuropathology changes suggestive of in utero onset, specific
behavioral configuration, and high heritability/high recurrence suggestive of genetic
cause can be at least partly decoupled from the inferences with which they have been
associated. Certainly important events occur at these early stages of development.
The problem arises at the level of drawing implications from these observations about
underlying mechanisms. If one assumes a priori that this is a “developmental disor-
der” in the neurobiological or neurogenetic sense, clinical and research observations
may be given interpretations consistent with the implications of this assumption,
while other potentially valid interpretations consistent with a more chronic model
may be neglected.
The notion of a “developmental disorder” has a number of different connota-
tions. From a developmental psychology point of view, it can connote simply that
because function and capability change with development, a disorder in childhood
will manifest differently at different ages. This is unquestionably true. However,
there are other perspectives carrying more severe connotations. From a medical
and neurobiological vantage point, “developmental disorder” commonly connotes
at least the following four characteristics: (1) that there is a profound, if poten-
tially subtle, alteration in the developmental trajectory of the brain, (2) that the
ensuing developmental brain alterations are primary core targets of the etiological
agent rather than incidental or secondary, (3) that these alterations directly cause
the behavioral phenotype, and (4) that these brain features and the accompanying
encephalopathy are indelibly unchangeable. This “developmental disorder” model
is derived from observations in neurogenetic syndromes and syndromes of brain
malformation where there are clearly observable and classifiable alterations in brain
development based upon a fault in some neurochemical or regulatory processes that
lead to fairly predictable consequences in affected individuals.
While this framing of developmental disorders is most commonly associated with
syndromes having genetic etiologies, the fields of developmental neurotoxicology
and teratology have shown that exogenous substances can target early developmental
Autism: The Centrality of Active Pathophysiology 353
since the brains lack major dysmorphology, they are unlikely to have
suffered significant insult prior to the late gestational or early postnatal
period (Ciaranello, VandenBerg, and Anders 1982, Coleman et al. 1985,
Raymond, Bauman, and Kemper 1996). The observation has been made
that there is a striking disconnection between the almost indiscernible
white matter tract as well as general structural abnormalities and the
dramatic functional impairments (Conturo et al. 2008).
b. Suggestions that neurodevelopmental disorders can be triggered by
events during the fetal period are supported by a growing body of
literature (Connors et al. 2008, Fatemi et al. 2002, Patterson 2002, Shi
et al. 2003, Smith et al. 2007). There is a huge literature on developmen-
tal neurotoxicity (Slikker and Chang 1998) as well as developmental
immune injury (Dietert and Dietert 2008, Hertz-Picciotto et al. 2008).
However, while these exposures can now be said to increase the poten-
tial for neurodevelopmental disorders, there is no support at this time
for going further—i.e., such exposures have by no means been shown to
be sufficient on their own to cause postnatally emerging developmental
disorders or ASD in particular. Nor have developmental disorders or
ASD in particular have been shown to be necessarily or in all cases
preceded by such events.
c. The model of autism derived from the connectivity literature related
to connectivity impairments underlying impairments in complex pro-
cessing (Just et al. 2004, 2007, Muller 2007) is synchronic—i.e., it can
be marshaled to explain the apparent selective impairment of complex
processing in individuals with fully developed autism at a particular
point in time. It is not a diachronic model—i.e., it does not help at all in
explaining the phenomenon of the development of autism, and particu-
larly the phenomenon of regression into autism. We do not understand
what changes so that a child who was producing behaviors closely con-
sistent with normal developmental milestones either falters, plateaus,
shifts tracks, or in some other ways shifts to slow and/or alter develop-
ment. If one assumes that autism is inborn, then it is possible to construct
a narrative stating that the connectivity problem is innate or prenatal in
origin, but does not show itself until critical processes kick in (or fail to
occur) postnatally at which point the innately altered wiring becomes
a problem. An alternative narrative with a slightly later developmental
timepoint is the idea that there is the “failure of the pruning” of excess
neural processes. We have no direct evidence to prove either narrative,
and in fact imaging evidence as noted in point d below goes against the
idea that there has been a pruning failure.
2. Alternative Interpretations of Prior Findings
d. The brain findings to date contain many suggestions of prenatal events,
but it must be remembered that explaining findings in a fully devel-
oped brain of a child past toddlerhood and particularly of an adult is
an “archaeological” exercise in reading a developmental history from
a snapshot—i.e., it is highly interpretive. At least some of the findings
Autism: The Centrality of Active Pathophysiology 355
The point of all of these examples is to give a taste of various ways that the intro-
duction of pathophysiological variables can point to rather different interpretations
of existing brain findings. It also serves to illustrate how much of what we think
we know about the brain in autism is actually a morass of fragments of data being
extrapolated to support inferences based on a priori assumptions. By showing that
when we augment the conceptual input parameters to include chronic pathophysiol-
ogy and not just genetics and brain development, we get as output a substantially
different set of interpretations, I hope I have at least begun to demonstrate how tenu-
ous are the established interpretations. On this basis, I would argue that we have no
solid grounds for excluding or dismissing a research program based on a different
set of assumptions than “developmental disorder of prenatal onset.” On the contrary,
there are many reasons for arguing that it is very important that we pursue a research
program based on these different assumptions, as well as communication and synergy
across specialized silos, and do so aggressively.
spread rapidly and interact with neuronal activity; and they are centrally involved
in regulating neurovascular coupling (Koehler, Gebremedhin, and Harder 2006).
It is increasingly appreciated that astrocytes are influenced by inflammation in a
variety of disease states (Kielian 2008); the activation of astroglial and microglial
cells have a wide variety of effects that are arguably consistent with many observed
features of ASD. Microglia activation is associated with vasogenic and cytotoxic
edema associated with hypoperfusion; activated microglia release glutamate that
induces astrocyte edema (Han et al. 2004, Liang et al. 2008). Microglial activation
can occur rapidly in response to insults; when it persists, its neurotoxic impact
progressively increases over time. The astroglial support of neuron chemistry
and the secretion of neuromodulators are altered in the activated state (Aschner,
Sonnewald, and Tan 2002). Astroglia maintain the redox potential including through
the production of glutathione, which they transfer to neurons. In their resting
state, astrocyte networks prevent glutamate excitotoxicity in the brain (Schousboe
and Waagepetersen 2005). In the setting of acute inflammation, these functions are
compromised, leading to increased neuronal vulnerability (Orellana et al. 2009,
Tilleux and Hermans 2007). This might lead to a runaway self-reinforcing vicious
cycle, with microglial activation releasing glutamate and activating astroglia, and the
activation of astroglia reducing their ability to perform their multiple metabolic and
signaling functions. In summary, the activation of these classes of glial cells leads
to a series of pathophysiological phenomena that can be self-perpetuating and also
progressively more excitotoxic and neurotoxic.
Given how insensitive existing in vivo imaging is to neuroinflammation and how
few clinical measures collected to date pertain to these processes, we have no way of
knowing how pervasive these processes are among people with ASD, how they inter-
act with contributory genes, whether the above cascade of cellular and molecular
changes is either sufficient or necessary to produce ASD, or whether genetic vul-
nerability is required. But all of the above raises the possibility that dysfunction in
these cells could be central to ASD pathophysiology and functional impairment,
prominently triggered by noxious environmental influences, and only subordinately
related to genetic influence. These mechanisms suggest that there are substantial
complexities beyond the boundaries implied by the assumption that ASD is simply a
“developmental disorder.”
∗ Table 2 in Herbert and Anderson (2008) schematically lays out other possibilities.
Autism: The Centrality of Active Pathophysiology 361
While the details of this scenario could be modified at various places along the
way, and while many linkages have not been tested, the starting points and subse-
quent features for each step of the narrative are taken from existing literature. The
point of presenting this sample narrative is to show that aberrant pathophysiology,
with or perhaps even without genetic vulnerability, could lead to a systems shift in
state that would cause altered brain function that could plausibly produce outputs
including autistic behaviors.
Another very important point is that much of what has been identified in autism
neuropathology and imaging could potentially be caused by rather than the cause
of this cascade. Purkinje cell loss or dysfunction could be due to excitotoxicity
(Blaylock and Strunecka 2009, Kern 2003, Yip, Soghomonian, and Blatt 2008).
White matter enlargement could be due to inflammation (Hendry et al. 2006, Pardo
et al. 2008, Dager et al. 2008). Limbic structure enlargement could also be due to
inflammatory processes particularly given some evidences that these structures have
greater immune sensitivity and vulnerability(Buller and Day 2002, Churchill et al.
2006, Kim et al. 2000). Altered connectivity could be due to an interaction of fac-
tors including reduced perfusion, gap junction closure, mitochondrial dysfunction,
362 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
and for the last few decades with the accumulation of evidence of brain and ner-
vous system abnormalities as a neuropsychiatric, neurodevelopmental syndrome
(Tuchman and Rapin 2006). At the same time, there has long been a more whole-
body physiological strand in autism research and treatment. Although several early
scattered papers appeared describing measurable changes in somatic and systemic
physiological features, these insights have not been integrated or assimilated into the
dominant model of autism.
There are several reasons for this lack of integration of physiological and behav-
ioral understanding:
1. Many of the physiological studies have been weak: small sample size, meth-
odological problems, and inconsistency of results between studies have
contributed to keeping these findings marginalized.
2. The immaturity of methods of investigation has limited the strength of such
findings and hindered their ability to engender serious interest.
3. The behavioral definition of ASD has made it seem necessary or at least
important to map physiological findings to specific behavioral features of
this syndrome in order to support their significance to the condition, but
attempts to do this have produced weak results, probably because the
systems pathophysiology is unlikely to lead to this kind of specific mecha-
nism-to-behavior mapping.
4. The heterogeneity of ASD is only recently being appreciated, so that most
studies to date have not been designed to tease out distinctive subgroups.
The problem of subgroups is particularly pertinent here: a pathophysiology-
centered approach would emphasize that subgroups may be effectively
distinguished at the physiological level; but at the same time, there is no
guarantee of discerning any one specific measure at the metabolic or
immune level that is present in the majority of a cohort. Thus, physiological
insights, particularly those that could be pertinent to such subgroups, have
not been clearly identified.
In recent years, multisystem and systemic features of autism have been getting
more attention, in part because of research (Herbert 2005a) and also because of the
experiences of patients and the insistence of many such patients and their families
that these are major issues and should not be ignored. Most commonly appreciated
at this point are the GI symptoms (such as chronic constipation, diarrhea, and gas-
troesophageal reflux) (Afzal et al. 2003, Torrente et al. 2002, Valicenti-McDermott
et al. 2006) and the immune abnormalities (such as recurrent infections and chronic
allergies) (Ashwood and Van de Water 2004a,b, Ashwood, Wills, and Van de Water
2006), both of which appear to have high prevalence in individuals with ASD and
sometimes in their family members (Croen et al. 2005). Less widely known but sup-
ported by a growing body of literature are the underlying abnormalities in oxidative
metabolism and sulfur metabolism already discussed above (Chauhan and Chauhan
2006, James et al. 2006). There are also various nervous system manifestations that
are highly prevalent but that fall outside the triad of behaviors that define autism;
these include sensory abnormalities (present in as many as 95% of individuals with
364 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
autism) (Tomchek and Dunn 2007), sleep disturbances (Malow 2004, Malow et al.
2006), abnormal autonomic reactivity (Goodwin et al. 2006, Groden et al. 2005,
Ming et al. 2004), epilepsy (Canitano 2007), and various motor and neuromuscu-
lar abnormalities. In parallel with these developments in the ASD literature, there
are analogous developments in other neuropsychiatric fields where the interest is
expanding beyond behaviors to include pathophysiology and systemic biomarkers
(Schwarz and Bahn 2008).
framing of autism as not only specifically neurobiological but also nothing more than
a genetically caused brain-based syndrome is actually the best framing? Do we have
enough multidisciplinary systems biological phenomic research to prove that there
are really cases of ‘pure autism’ with absolutely no features other than the three core
behaviors? Where are the systematic studies conducting sufficient appropriately sen-
sitive measures in people with apparently nonsystemic, non-somatic presentations
to exclude all implicated dysregulated physiology? Can anyone point to a literature
reporting the systematic investigation and exclusion of the possibility that there may
indeed be a relationship between brain and body features in affected individuals?”
In fact, from the vantage point of a pathophysiology-centered approach to autism,
there are many reasons to expect that there is a vital linkage between body and brain,
and strong reason to disagree with the idea that the somatic and systemic features
are simply secondary to “the autism.” In truth, as mentioned in the introduction,
there are many mechanisms by which brain and body may very well be related in
autism (and in many other settings), and in particular, by which body may signifi-
cantly influence brain, and there are many papers in the non-autism peer-reviewed
literature showing immune–brain and gut–brain relationships via mechanisms that
may very well also be operating in autism. We do not need to remind people that the
notions that such mechanisms are irrelevant or of minimal effect because the brain
is immune-privileged and/or the blood-brain barrier is fully protective are obsolete
(Carson et al. 2006). Particularly pertinent are that both brain and body are known
to be affected by oxidative stress, mitochondrial abnormalities, and inflammation,
mechanisms that growing evidence implicates in autism.
1. To develop study designs that have the capacity to concretely address and
elucidate brain–body–systemic relationships in autism itself, and not merely
by inference from other domains
366 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Addressing the above research questions will lay the foundation for incorporating
somatic/systemic features into phenotyping and defining autism spectrum disorders,
and help us develop a coordinated research and clinical approach that integrates
somatic and systemic features with brain, behavior, and genetic factors.
There is nothing in the formal DSM-IV (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, 4th
edition) definition of autism relating to etiology except for one thing: to qualify for a
diagnosis of childhood autism, the disturbance cannot be better accounted for by
Rett’s syndrome or childhood disintegrative disorder. Beyond this, there is no exclu-
sion for any specific genetic etiology or for that matter, for any biological etiology
whatsoever. The disorder is defined simply by a constellation of behavioral symp-
toms. This clustering of behavioral symptoms into a diagnosis not unique to ASD but
is standard procedure in the DSM (American Psychiatric Association 1994).
In the early literature, some papers noted that a high proportion of parents of
individuals with autism had occupations that would expose them to potentially toxic
chemicals (Coleman 1979, Felicetti 1981, Rosenberger-Debiesse and Coleman 1986).
But for decades, the emphasis in thinking about etiology has been on genetics. More
recently, there has been increasing openness to considering environmental influences
and gene—environment interactions (Campbell et al. 2006, 2008, D’Amelio et al.
2005, Newschaffer et al. 2007, Persico and Bourgeron 2006, Tsuang et al. 2004). By
now, there are fewer who would maintain that autism is purely genetic, but still many
who would expect that genetic influence is primary and greater, while environmental
influence is lesser and of much smaller effect.
While a variety of genes have been implicated as associated with autism, no gene
identified to date has both high impact and high prevalence. Even developmental or
neurogenetic disorders associated with high rates of ASD, such as fragile X or tuber-
ous sclerosis, do not have anything near a 100% prevalence of ASD amongst affected
Autism: The Centrality of Active Pathophysiology 369
individuals (Belmonte and Bourgeron 2006), suggesting that the genetic alterations
underlying these conditions would be better construed as conferring high risk, rather
than being called causal.
The basis for privileging genetics is largely inference from indirect evidence
rather than a solid knowledge of which specific genes are implicated and in what
ways they lead to what we call ASD, since such knowledge does not exist. As with
the discussion of prior points, the issue becomes examining whether the indirect
evidence makes a strong case for a uniquely primary genetic contribution, or whether
it is also consistent with a significant contribution from nongenetic factors.
A full discussion of these issues is beyond the scope of this chapter, and good
coverage of much of this is available elsewhere (Corrales and Herbert in press). The
discussion here focuses on what is most pertinent in the setting of articulating a
pathophysiology-centered approach to autism.
Two key pieces of indirect evidence for a strong or primary role for genetics are
the high monozygotic-twin concordance rates and the high sibling-recurrence rates.
However, a number of factors could contribute to at least somewhat altered interpre-
tations of these numbers:
The G×E findings raise the possibility that the mistake has been to assume that all
genetic effects are “main” effects independent of the environment. The truth may be
that there is much more gene–environment interdependence than has been appreciated
up until now (see Caspi and Moffitt 2006, Rutter 2007). Also, it is very striking that
the G×E effects that have been found are of moderate size and by no means are as
small as the main effects of single genes considered independently of the environment
(Rutter 2008).
The chronic features of autism, the fact that environmental triggers are known more
broadly to be associated with much of the pathophysiology identified in ASD (even
if specific linkages have not yet been established in this context) as well as the other
above arguments all point toward the need for a framework that includes environ-
ment as well as genes.
This dismissive attitude has not made recovery stories go away. A substantial num-
ber of anecdotal reports are circulating that describe transient improvement under
conditions of stress, intense emotion, clear fluid diet in preparation for colonoscopy/
endoscopy, and after anesthesia. In addition, the Internet and YouTube abound in
narrative and video documentations and parent testimonials about recovered autis-
tic children. But for decades, there has also been a small amount of the academic
documentation of improvement, the loss of diagnosis, and recovery. Early reports of
improved outcome include the Case #1 in the 1943 paper by Kanner in which autism
was first described and named. This individual appeared severely affected through
childhood—his parents were in fact told that there was nothing to be done for him
and were advised to let him live with a caring family elsewhere and get on with their
lives. In late adolescence after a severe illness diagnosed as juvenile rheumatoid
arthritis, for which gold salts treatment was administered, he experienced a remis-
sion not only of the arthritis but also of the autistic symptoms, and went on to earn
a bachelor’s degree, live independently, hold down a job, and travel widely (Kanner
1968, 1971, Olmsted 2005). Early documentation of improvement and recovery also
includes papers coauthored by Rutter, Greenfeld, and Lockyer (1967), Rutter and
Lockyer (1967), Gajzago and Prior (1974), DeMyer, Hingtgen, and Jackson (1981),
and Lovaas (1987). Fein and colleagues have produced a further review of outcome
studies and the notion of autism recovery (Helt et al. 2008). Recent reports of the
loss of diagnosis in children rigorously diagnosed with autism according to cur-
rent diagnostic standards suggest that the loss of diagnosis is likely to be accompa-
nied by residual neurodevelopmental impairments such as attention deficit disorder
or language impairment, and that good motor functioning is predictive of optimal
outcome (Fein et al. 2005, Kelley et al. 2006, Sutera et al. 2007). In addition to
recoveries, there are also the transient improvements (e.g., fever or oral antibiotic
associated) and animal model reversals of developmental disorders already reviewed
in the introduction.
The loss of diagnosis and the cases of transient improvement in core features—
as well as the fairly common short-term fluctuation between more lucid days and
days of being more severely “zoned out” that parents can find maddening—raise
intriguing questions about what kinds of underlying neurobiological basic features
and changes could enable such variability and improvement to occur (Herbert and
Anderson 2008). The transient improvements add further intrigue by suggesting
that changes occurring over a very short timescale can lead to significant observable
improvements in the level of functioning, further honing the questions posed by
these phenomena—what kinds of neurobiological mechanisms could be amenable
to such rapid change?
An interesting potentially related phenomenon is an increasing recognition of an
often substantial discrepancy between expressive and receptive language impair-
ments. Many clinicians and parents are observing signs of receptive language abilities
in some individuals with autism far in advance of their expressive language capa-
bilities. Some such individuals test extremely well on IQ tests and can read and use
keyboards to express themselves (sometimes showing great creativity and nuance),
but not produce speech. Some attribute this discrepancy to oro-motor apraxia and
others focus on sensory-processing issues; increasing research attention is being paid
Autism: The Centrality of Active Pathophysiology 373
neural system that can affect relatively rapid changes in the functional
activity of widespread neural networks involved in the core features of ASD
(Mehler and Purpura 2009). The authors specifically suggest that fever
might transiently restore normal function to a dysregulated locus ceruleus–
noradrenergic system (LC/NA). This system is capable of facilitating rapid
and widespread neural network remodeling to behavioral adaptations
to environmental challenges. The authors note that their hypothesis is in
keeping with studies that have failed to find substantive neuropathologi-
cal lesions in the cerebral cortex and other brain sites. Both their specific
hypothesis about LC/NA and the more general notion that a mechanism
performing the rapid regulation of functional remodeling is likely to be
operative are intriguing and will undoubtedly trigger further research on
mechanisms of plasticity and interventions for enhancing it.
Each of these examples suggests in its own way that if some of the pathophysi-
ology or dysfunction can be reduced, there is a potential for clinical improvement
that would not be predicted in the classical “developmental disorder” framing of
ASD. These insights come from a pathophysiology-centered approach to autism
which, not being bound by the model of static encephalopathy, can orient us to
a range of possible mechanisms that could contribute to transient and sustained
improvement. Such an orientation is better suited than a central focus on genetic
alterations of brain development for studying these changeable features that are
more suggestive of dynamic than static encephalopathy. The possibility that we are
not dealing with a developmentally based deficit but with potentially partly or fully
functional domains whose dysfunction is not only or even necessarily structurally
base but also has a significant contribution from obstruction by pathophysiologi-
cal dysfunction can be considered in this fresh framework; this can open a greater
range of approaches to potentially helpful treatments. Moreover even if there is a
developmentally based alteration of brain development, that in itself is not sufficient
reason to exclude the possibilities of (1) brain plasticity and (2) clinically significant
improvement via the amelioration of exacerbating contributors such as metabolic
and energetic dysfunctions.
Finally, investigating dynamic features of autism could contribute to our
understanding of underlying mechanisms. The treatment of pathophysiological
maladaptation (e.g., remediation of inflammation or oxidative stress) could be an
interesting cognitive neuroscience probe. If these treatments have any efficacy
in improving behavioral function, it would be most interesting to document the
nature and distribution of the functional and structural neural systems impacts,
and ask some important questions: Does functional improvement occur one
domain at a time or in an across the board fashion? Might some treatments (e.g.,
the treatment of disrupted gut microbiology) more specifically target repetitive
behaviors, obsessions, and stereotypies while other treatments (e.g., the reduction
of oxidative stress) have more general effects? The answers would tell us a lot
about underlying brain mechanisms; but to get these answers, a real partnership
in research and treatment between pathophysiology and cognitive neuroscience
is necessary.
376 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
REFERENCES
Afzal, N., S. Murch, K. Thirrupathy, L. Berger, A. Fagbemi, and R. Heuschkel. 2003. Consti-
pation with acquired megarectum in children with autism. Pediatrics 112:939–942.
Alverdy, J. C. and E. B. Chang. 2008. The re-emerging role of the intestinal microflora in
critical illness and inflammation: Why the gut hypothesis of sepsis syndrome will not go
away. J. Leukoc. Biol. 83:461–466.
American Psychiatric Association. 1994. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, 4th ed. (DSM IV). Washington DC: APA.
Anderson, M. P., B. S. Hooker, and M. R. Herbert. 2008. Bridging from cells to cogni-
tion in autism pathophysiology: Biological pathways to defective brain function and
plasticity. Am. J. Biochem. Biotechnol. (Special Issue on Autism Spectrum Disorders)
4:167–176.
Arnold, S. E. 2001. Contributions of neuropathology to understanding schizophrenia in late
life. Harv. Rev. Psychiatry 9:69–76.
Aschner, M., U. Sonnewald, and K. H. Tan. 2002. Astrocyte modulation of neurotoxic injury.
Brain Pathol. 12:475–481.
Aschner, M., J. W. Allen, H. K. Kimelberg, R. M. LoPachin, and W. J. Streit. 1999. Glial cells
in neurotoxicity development. Annu. Rev. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 39:151–173.
Ashwood, P. and J. Van de Water. 2004a. A review of autism and the immune response. Clin.
Dev. Immunol. 11:165–174.
378 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Ashwood, P. and J. Van de Water. 2004b. Is autism an autoimmune disease? Autoimmun. Rev.
3:557–562.
Ashwood, P., S. Wills, and J. Van de Water. 2006. The immune response in autism: A new
frontier for autism research. J. Leukoc. Biol. 80:1–15.
Aylward, E. H., N. J. Minshew, K. Field, B. F. Sparks, and N. Singh. 2002. Effects of age on
brain volume and head circumference in autism. Neurology 59:175–183.
Bauman, M. 2006. Beyond behavior: Biomedical diagnoses in autism spectrum disorders.
Autism Advocate 45:27–29.
Bauman, M. L. and T. L. Kemper. 2005. Neuroanatomic observations of the brain in autism:
A review and future directions. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 23:183–187.
Belmonte, M. K. and T. Bourgeron. 2006. Fragile X syndrome and autism at the intersection
of genetic and neural networks. Nat. Neurosci. 9:1221–1225.
Blaylock, R. L. and A. Strunecka. 2009. Immune-glutamatergic dysfunction as a central
mechanism of the autism spectrum disorders. Curr. Med. Chem. 16:157–170.
Braunschweig, D., P. Ashwood, P. Krakowiak, I. Hertz-Picciotto, R. Hansen, L. A. Croen,
I. N. Pessah, and J. Van de Water. 2008. Autism: Maternally derived antibodies specific
for fetal brain proteins. Neurotoxicology 29:226–231.
Bu, B., P. Ashwood, D. Harvey, I. B. King, J. V. Water, and L. W. Jin. 2006. Fatty acid
compositions of red blood cell phospholipids in children with autism. Prostaglandins
Leukot. Essent. Fatty Acids 74:215–221.
Buller, K. M. and T. A. Day. 2002. Systemic administration of interleukin-1beta activates
select populations of central amygdala afferents. J. Comp. Neurol. 452:288–296.
Buller, K. M., A. S. Hamlin, and P. B. Osborne. 2005. Dissection of peripheral and central
endogenous opioid modulation of systemic interleukin-1beta responses using c-fos
expression in the rat brain. Neuropharmacology 49:230–242.
Calderon-Garciduenas, L., A. Mora-Tiscareno, E. Ontiveros, G. Gomez-Garza, G. Barragan-Mejia,
J. Broadway, S. Chapman, G. Valencia-Salazar, V. Jewells, R. R. Maronpot, C. Henriquez-
Roldan, B. Perez-Guille, R. Torres-Jardon, L. Herrit, D. Brooks, N. Osnaya-Brizuela,
M. E. Monroy, A. Gonzalez-Maciel, R. Reynoso-Robles, R. Villarreal-Calderon, A. C. Solt,
and R. W. Engle. 2008a. Air pollution, cognitive deficits and brain abnormalities: A pilot
study with children and dogs. Brain Cogn. 68:117–127.
Calderon-Garciduenas, L., A. C. Solt, C. Henriquez-Roldan, R. Torres-Jardon, B. Nuse,
L. Herritt, R. Villarreal-Calderon, N. Osnaya, I. Stone, R. Garcia, D. M. Brooks,
A. Gonzalez-Maciel, R. Reynoso-Robles, R. Delgado-Chavez, and W. Reed. 2008b.
Long-term air pollution exposure is associated with neuroinflammation, an altered innate
immune response, disruption of the blood-brain barrier, ultrafine particulate deposition,
and accumulation of amyloid beta-42 and alpha-synuclein in children and young adults.
Toxicol. Pathol. 36:289–310.
Campbell, D. B., J. S. Sutcliffe, P. J. Ebert, R. Militerni, C. Bravaccio, S. Trillo, M. Elia, C.
Schneider, R. Melmed, R. Sacco, A. M. Persico, and P. Levitt. 2006. A genetic variant
that disrupts MET transcription is associated with autism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
103:16834–16839.
Campbell, D. B., C. Li, J. S. Sutcliffe, A. M. Persico, and P. Levitt. 2008. Genetic evidence
implicating multiple genes in the MET receptor tyrosine kinase pathway in autism
spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 1:159–168.
Canitano, R. 2007. Epilepsy in autism spectrum disorders. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry
16:61–66.
Carson, M. J., J. M. Doose, B. Melchior, C. D. Schmid, and C. C. Ploix. 2006. CNS immune
privilege: Hiding in plain sight. Immunol. Rev. 213:48–65.
Caspi, A. and T. E. Moffitt. 2006. Gene-environment interactions in psychiatry: Joining forces
with neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7:583–590.
Autism: The Centrality of Active Pathophysiology 379
Caviness, V. S., Jr, N. T. Lange, N. Makris, M. R. Herbert, and D. N. Kennedy. 1999. MRI-
based brain volumetrics: Emergence of a developmental brain science. Brain Dev.
21:289–295.
Chauhan, A. and V. Chauhan. 2006. Oxidative stress in autism. Pathophysiology 13:171–181.
Chauhan, V., A. Chauhan, I. L. Cohen, W. T. Brown, and A. Sheikh. 2004. Alteration in
amino-glycerophospholipids levels in the plasma of children with autism: A potential
biochemical diagnostic marker. Life Sci. 74:1635–1643.
Chugani, D. C., B. S. Sundram, M. Behen, M. L. Lee, and G. J. Moore. 1999. Evidence
of altered energy metabolism in autistic children. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol.
Psychiatry 23:635–641.
Churchill, L., P. Taishi, M. Wang, J. Brandt, C. Cearley, A. Rehman, and J. M. Krueger. 2006.
Brain distribution of cytokine mRNA induced by systemic administration of interleukin-
1beta or tumor necrosis factor alpha. Brain Res. 1120:64–73.
Ciaranello, R. D., S. R. VandenBerg, and T. F. Anders. 1982. Intrinsic and extrinsic
determinants of neuronal development: Relation to infantile autism. J. Autism Dev.
Disord. 12:115–145.
Coleman, M. 1979. Studies of the autistic syndromes. In Congenital and Acquired Cognitive
Disorders, ed. R. Katzman. New York: Raven Press, pp. 265–275.
Coleman, N. 1989. Autism: Nondrug biological treatments. In Diagnosis and Treatment of
Autism, ed. C. Gillberg. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 219–235.
Coleman, P. D., J. Romano, L. Lapham, and W. Simon. 1985. Cell counts in cerebral cortex of
an autistic patient. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 15:245–255.
Connors, S. L., P. Levitt, S. G. Matthews, T. A. Slotkin, M. V. Johnston, H. C. Kinney, W. G.
Johnson, R. M. Dailey, and A. W. Zimmerman. 2008. Fetal mechanisms in neurodevel-
opmental disorders. Pediatr. Neurol. 38:163–176.
Conturo, T. E., D. L. Williams, C. D. Smith, E. Gultepe, E. Akbudak, and N. J. Minshew.
2008. Neuronal fiber pathway abnormalities in autism: An initial MRI diffusion
tensor tracking study of hippocampo-fusiform and amygdalo-fusiform pathways. J. Int.
Neuropsychol. Soc. 14:933–946.
Corrales, M. and M. Herbert. Autism and environmental genomics: Synergistic systems
approaches to autism complexity. In Autism Spectrum Disorders, eds. D. Amaral,
G, Dawson, and D. Geschwind. New York: Oxford University Press. (in press).
Correia, C., A. M. Coutinho, L. Diogo, M. Grazina, C. Marques, T. Miguel, A. Ataide,
J. Almeida, L. Borges, C. Oliveira, G. Oliveira, and A. M. Vicente. 2006. Brief report:
High frequency of biochemical markers for mitochondrial dysfunction in autism:
No association with the mitochondrial aspartate/glutamate carrier SLC25A12 gene.
J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36:1137–1140.
Courchesne, E. and K. Pierce. 2005. Why the frontal cortex in autism might be talking only to
itself: Local over-connectivity but long-distance disconnection. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.
15:225–230.
Coyle, J. T. and R. Schwarcz. 2000. Mind glue: implications of glial cell biology for psychiatry.
Arch: Gen. Psychiatry 57:90–93.
Croen, L. A., J. K. Grether, C. K. Yoshida, R. Odouli, and J. Van de Water. 2005. Maternal
autoimmune diseases, asthma and allergies, and childhood autism spectrum disorders:
A case-control study. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 159:151–157.
Curran, L. K., C. J. Newschaffer, L. C. Lee, S. O. Crawford, M. V. Johnston, and A. W.
Zimmerman. 2007. Behaviors associated with fever in children with autism spectrum
disorders. Pediatrics 120:e1386–e1392.
D’Amelio, M., I. Ricci, R. Sacco, X. Liu, L. D’Agruma, L. A. Muscarella, V. Guarnieri,
R. Militerni, C. Bravaccio, M. Elia, C. Schneider, R. Melmed, S. Trillo, T. Pascucci,
S. Puglisi-Allegra, K. L. Reichelt, F. Macciardi, J. J. Holden, and A. M. Persico.
380 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
2005. Paraoxonase gene variants are associated with autism in North America, but
not in Italy: Possible regional specificity in gene-environment interactions. Mol.
Psychiatry 10:1006–1016.
Dager, S. R., S. D. Friedman, H. Pegropoulos, and D. W. W. Shaw. 2008. Imaging evidence
for pathological brain development in autism spectrum disorders. In Autism: Current
Theories and Evidence, ed. A. Zimmerman. Totowa, NJ: Humana Press.
Davis, J. O., J. A. Phelps, and H. S. Bracha. 1995. Prenatal development of monozygotic twins
and concordance for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 21:357–366.
DeMyer, M. K., J. N. Hingtgen, and R. K. Jackson. 1981. Infantile autism reviewed: A decade
of research. Schizophr. Bull. 7:388–451.
DeVito, T. J., D. J. Drost, R. W. Neufeld, N. Rajakumar, W. Pavlosky, P. Williamson, and
R. Nicolson. 2007. Evidence for cortical dysfunction in autism: A proton magnetic
resonance spectroscopic imaging study. Biol. Psychiatry 61:465–473.
Dietert, R. R. and J. M. Dietert. 2008. Potential for early-life immune insult including
developmental immunotoxicity in autism and autism spectrum disorders: Focus on
critical windows of immune vulnerability. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health B. Crit. Rev.
11:660–680.
Ehninger, D., S. Han, C. Shilyansky, Y. Zhou, W. Li, D. J. Kwiatkowski, V. Ramesh, and
A. J. Silva. 2008. Reversal of learning deficits in a Tsc2+/− mouse model of tuberous
sclerosis. Nat. Med. 14:843–848.
Endo, T., T. Shioiri, H. Kitamura, T. Kimura, S. Endo, N. Masuzawa, and T. Someya. 2007.
Altered chemical metabolites in the amygdala-hippocampus region contribute to autistic
symptoms of autism spectrum disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 62:1030–1037.
Evans, T. A., S. L. Siedlak, Lian Lu, X. Fu, Z. Wang, W. R. McGinnis, E. Fakhoury,
R. J. Castellanio, S. L. Hazen, W. H. Walsh, A. T. Lewis, R. G. Salomon, M. A. Smith,
and G. Zhu X. Perry. 2008. The autistic phenotype exhibits a remarkably localized
modification of brain protein by products of free radical-induced lipid oxidation. Am. J.
Biochem. Biotechnol. (Special Issue on Autism Spectrum Disorders) 4:61–72.
Fatemi, S. H., J. Earle, R. Kanodia, D. Kist, E. S. Emamian, P. H. Patterson, L. Shi, and
R. Sidwell. 2002. Prenatal viral infection leads to pyramidal cell atrophy and macro-
cephaly in adulthood: Implications for genesis of autism and schizophrenia. Cell. Mol.
Neurobiol. 22:25–33.
Fein, D., P. Dixon, J. Paul, and H. Levin. 2005. Pervasive developmental disorder can evolve
into ADHD: Case illustrations. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 35:525–534.
Felicetti, T. 1981. Parents of autistic children: Some notes on the chemical connection. Milieu
Ther. 1:13–16.
Fernell, E., Y. Watanabe, I. Adolfsson, Y. Tani, M. Bergstrom, P. Hartvig, A. Lilja, A. L. von
Knorring, C. Gillberg, and B. Langstrom. 1997. Possible effects of tetrahydrobiopterin
treatment in six children with autism—clinical and positron emission tomography data:
A pilot study. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 39:313–318.
Fields, R. D. 2006. Advances in understanding neuron-glia interactions. Neuron Glia Biol.
2:23–26.
Fields, R. D. 2008. Oligodendrocytes changing the rules: Action potentials in glia and
oligodendrocytes controlling action potentials. Neuroscientist 14:540–543.
Filipek, P. A., J. Juranek, M. T. Nguyen, C. Cummings, and J. J. Gargus. 2004. Relative carnitine
deficiency in autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34:615–623.
Finegold, S. M., D. Molitoris, Y. Song, C. Liu, M. L. Vaisanen, E. Bolte, M. McTeague,
R. Sandler, H. Wexler, E. M. Marlowe, M. D. Collins, P. A. Lawson, P. Summanen,
M. Baysallar, T. J. Tomzynski, E. Read, E. Johnson, R. Rolfe, P. Nasir, H. Shah,
D. A. Haake, P. Manning, and A. Kaul. 2002. Gastrointestinal microflora studies in late-
onset autism. Clin. Infect. Dis. 35:S6–S16.
Autism: The Centrality of Active Pathophysiology 381
Herbert, M. R. 2002. Genetics finding its place in larger living schemes. Crit. Pub. Health
12:221–236.
Herbert, M. R. 2005a. Autism: A brain disorder or a disorder that affects the brain? Clin.
Neuropsychiatry 2:354–379.
Herbert, M. R. 2005b. Large brains in autism: The challenge of pervasive abnormality.
Neuroscientist 11:417–440.
Herbert, M. R. and M. P. Anderson. 2008. An expanding spectrum of autism models:
From fixed developmental defects to reversible functional impairments. In Autism:
Current Theories and Evidence, ed. A. Zimmerman. Totowa, NJ: Humana Press,
pp. 429–463.
Herbert, M. R. and V. S. Caviness. 2006. Neuroanatomy and imaging studies. In Autism:
A Neurobiological Disorder of Early Brain Development, eds. R. Tuchman and I. Rapin.
London: Mac Keith Press, 115–140.
Hertz-Picciotto, I. and L. Delwiche. 2009. The rise in autism and the role of age at diagnosis.
Epidemiology 20:84–90.
Hertz-Picciotto, I., H. Y. Park, M. Dostal, A. Kocan, T. Trnovec, and R. Sram. 2008. Prenatal
exposures to persistent and non-persistent organic compounds and effects on immune
system development. Basic Clin. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 102:146–154.
Holtzman, D. 2008. Autistic spectrum disorders and mitochondrial encephalopathies. Acta
Paediatr. 97:859–860.
Hugg, J. W., R. I. Kuzniecky, F. G. Gilliam, R. B. Morawetz, R. E. Fraught, and H. P.
Hetherington. 1996. Normalization of contralateral metabolic function following tem-
poral lobectomy demonstrated by 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging. Ann.
Neurol. 40:236–239.
James, S. J., S. Melnyk, S. Jernigan, M. A. Cleves, C. H. Halsted, D. H. Wong, P. Cutler,
K. Bock, M. Boris, J. J. Bradstreet, S. M. Baker, and D. W. Gaylor. 2006. Metabolic
endophenotype and related genotypes are associated with oxidative stress in children
with autism. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 141:947–956.
James, S. J., S. Melnyk, S. Jernigan, A. Hubanks, S. Rose, and D. W. Gaylor. 2008. Abnormal
transmethylation/transsulfuration metabolism and DNA hypomethylation among
parents of children with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38:1966–1975.
Jones, D. S. 2005. Textbook of Functional Medicine. Gig Harbor, WA: Institute for Functional
Medicine.
Just, M. A., V. L. Cherkassky, T. A. Keller, and N. J. Minshew. 2004. Cortical activation and
synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: Evidence
of underconnectivity. Brain 127:1811–1821.
Just, M. A., V. L. Cherkassky, T. A. Keller, R. K. Kana, and N. J. Minshew. 2007. Functional
and anatomical cortical underconnectivity in autism: Evidence from an FMRI study of an
executive function task and corpus callosum morphometry. Cereb. Cortex 17:951–961.
Kanner, L. 1943. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nerv. Child 10:217–250.
Kanner, L. 1968. Early infantile autism revisited. Psychiatry Dig. 29:17–28.
Kanner, L. 1971. Follow-up study of eleven autistic children originally reported in 1943.
J. Autism Child Schizophr. 1:119–145.
Kelley, E., J. J. Paul, D. Fein, and L. R. Naigles. 2006. Residual language deficits in optimal
outcome children with a history of autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36:807–828.
Kern, J. K. 2003. Purkinje cell vulnerability and autism: A possible etiological connection.
Brain Dev. 25:377–382.
Kielian, T. 2008. Glial connexins and gap junctions in CNS inflammation and disease.
J. Neurochem. 106:1000–1016.
Kim, W. G., R. P. Mohney, B. Wilson, G. H. Jeohn, B. Liu, and J. S. Hong. 2000. Regional
difference in susceptibility to lipopolysaccharide-induced neurotoxicity in the rat brain:
Role of microglia. J. Neurosci. 20:6309–6316.
Autism: The Centrality of Active Pathophysiology 383
Mehler, M. F. and D. P. Purpura. 2009. Autism, fever, epigenetics and the locus coeruleus.
Brain Res. Rev. 59:388–392.
Miller, A. H., V. Maletic, and C. L. Raison. 2009. Inflammation and its discontents: the role of
cytokines in the pathophysiology of major depression. Biol. Psychiatry 65:732–741.
Ming, X., P. O. Julu, J. Wark, F. Apartopoulos, and S. Hansen. 2004. Discordant mental and
physical efforts in an autistic patient. Brain Dev. 26:519–524.
Minshew, N. J., G. Goldstein, S. M. Dombrowski, K. Panchalingam, and J. W. Pettegrew. 1993.
A preliminary 31P MRS study of autism: Evidence for undersynthesis and increased
degradation of brain membranes. Biol. Psychiatry 33:762–773.
Moretti, P., T. Sahoo, K. Hyland, T. Bottiglieri, S. Peters, D. del Gaudio, B. Roa, S. Curry,
H. Zhu, R. H. Finnell, J. L. Neul, V. T. Ramaekers, N. Blau, C. A. Bacino, G. Miller,
and F. Scaglia. 2005. Cerebral folate deficiency with developmental delay, autism, and
response to folinic acid. Neurology 64:1088–1090.
Morrow, E. M., S. Y. Yoo, S. W. Flavell, T. K. Kim, Y. Lin, R. S. Hill, N. M. Mukaddes,
S. Balkhy, G. Gascon, A. Hashmi, S. Al-Saad, J. Ware, R. M. Joseph, R. Greenblatt,
D. Gleason, J. A. Ertelt, K. A. Apse, A. Bodell, J. N. Partlow, B. Barry, H. Yao,
K. Markianos, R. J. Ferland, M. E. Greenberg, and C. A. Walsh. 2008. Identifying autism
loci and genes by tracing recent shared ancestry. Science 321:218–223.
Morton, J. and U. Frith. 1995. Causal modelling: A structural approach to developmental
psychopathology. In Manual of Developmental Psychopathology, eds. D. Cicchetti and
D. J. Cohen. New York: John Wiley, pp. 357–390.
Muller, R. A. 2007. The study of autism as a distributed disorder. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil.
Res. Rev. 13:85–95.
Natowicz, M. 2004. Personal communication.
Nelson, K. B., J. K. Grether, L. A. Croen, J. M. Dambrosia, B. F. Dickens, L. L. Jelliffe, R. L.
Hansen, and T. M. Phillips. 2001. Neuropeptides and neurotrophins in neonatal blood of
children with autism or mental retardation. Ann. Neurol.49:597–606.
Newschaffer, C. J., L. A. Croen, J. Daniels, E. Giarelli, J. K. Grether, S. E. Levy, D. S. Mandell,
L. A. Miller, and J. Pinto-Martin. 2007. The epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders.
Annu. Rev. Pub. Health 28:235–256.
Nicholson, J. K., E. Holmes, and I. D. Wilson. 2005. Gut microorganisms, mammalian
metabolism and personalized health care. Nat. Rev. Microbiol. 3:431–438.
Noble, D. 2008. The Music of Life. New York: Oxford University Press.
Nyffeler, M., U. Meyer, B. K. Yee, J. Feldon, and I. Knuesel. 2006. Maternal immune activation
during pregnancy increases limbic GABAA receptor immunoreactivity in the adult
offspring: Implications for schizophrenia. Neuroscience 143:51–62.
Oliveira, G., L. Diogo, M. Grazina, P. Garcia, A. Ataide, C. Marques, T. Miguel,
L. Borges, A. M. Vicente, and C. R. Oliveira. 2005. Mitochondrial dysfunction
in autism spectrum disorders: A population-based study. Dev. Med. Child Neurol.
47:185–189.
Olmsted, D. 2005. The age of autism: Case 1 revisited. American Chronicle. Available online
at http://www.americanchronicle.com/articles/view/1872
Opler, M. G. and E. S. Susser. 2005. Fetal environment and schizophrenia. Environ. Health
Perspect. 113:1239–1242.
Orellana, J. A., P. J. Saez, K. F. Shoji, K. A. Schalper, N. Palacios-Prado, V. Velarde, C. Giaume,
M. V. Bennett, and J. C. Saez. 2009. Modulation of brain hemichannels and gap junc-
tion channels by pro-inflammatory agents and their possible role in neurodegeneration.
Antioxid. Redox Signal. 11:369–399.
Page, T. 2000. Metabolic approaches to the treatment of autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism
Dev. Disord. 30:463–469.
Page, T. and C. Moseley. 2002. Metabolic treatment of hyperuricosuric autism. Prog.
Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol Psychiatry 26:397–400.
Autism: The Centrality of Active Pathophysiology 385
Shi, L., S. H. Fatemi, R. W. Sidwell, and P. H. Patterson. 2003. Maternal influenza infection
causes marked behavioral and pharmacological changes in the offspring. J. Neurosci.
23:297–302.
Shultz, S. R., D. F. Macfabe, S. Martin, J. Jackson, R. Taylor, F. Boon, K. P. Ossenkopp, and
D. P. Cain. 2009. Intracerebroventricular injections of the enteric bacterial metabolic
product propionic acid impair cognition and sensorimotor ability in the Long-Evans rat:
Further development of a rodent model of autism. Behav. Brain Res. 200: 33–41.
Shultz, S. R., D. F. MacFabe, K. P. Ossenkopp, S. Scratch, J. Whelan, R. Taylor, and D. P. Cain.
2008b. Intracerebroventricular injection of propionic acid, an enteric bacterial metabolic
end-product, impairs social behavior in the rat: Implications for an animal model of
autism. Neuropharmacology 54:901–911.
Slikker, W. and L. W. Chang. 1998. Handbook of Developmental Neurotoxicology. San Diego,
CA: Academic Press.
Smith, S. E., J. Li, K. Garbett, K. Mirnics, and P. H. Patterson. 2007. Maternal immune activation
alters fetal brain development through interleukin-6. J. Neurosci. 27:10695–106702.
Song, Y., C. Liu, and S. M. Finegold. 2004. Real-time PCR quantitation of clostridia in feces
of autistic children. Appl. Environ. Microbiol. 70:6459–6465.
Standridge, J. B. 2006. Vicious cycles within the neuropathophysiologic mechanisms of
Alzheimer’s disease. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 3:95–108.
Sundaram, S. K., A. Kumar, M. I. Makki, M. E. Behen, H. T. Chugani, and D. C. Chugani.
2008. Diffusion tensor imaging of frontal lobe in autism spectrum disorder. Cereb.
Cortex 18:2659–2665.
Sutera, S., J. Pandey, E. L. Esser, M. A. Rosenthal, L. B. Wilson, M. Barton, J. Green, S. Hodgson,
D. L. Robins, T. Dumont-Mathieu, and D. Fein. 2007. Predictors of optimal outcome in
toddlers diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37:98–107.
Theis, M., G. Sohl, J. Eiberger, and K. Willecke. 2005. Emerging complexities in identity and
function of glial connexins. Trends Neurosci. 28:188–195.
Thevarkunnel, S., M. A. Martchek, T. L. Kemper, M. B. Bauman, and G. J. Blatt. 2004.
A neuroanatomical study of the brainstem nuclei in autism. Society for Neuroscience
Abstract 1028.14.
Tilleux, S. and E. Hermans. 2007. Neuroinflammation and regulation of glial glutamate uptake
in neurological disorders. J. Neurosci. Res. 85:2059–2070.
Tobinick, E. L. and H. Gross. 2008a. Rapid cognitive improvement in Alzheimer’s disease
following perispinal etanercept administration. J. Neuroinflammation 5:2.
Tobinick, E. L. and H. Gross. 2008b. Rapid improvement in verbal fluency and aphasia following
perispinal etanercept in Alzheimer’s disease. BMC Neurol. 8:27.
Tomchek, S. D. and W. Dunn. 2007. Sensory processing in children with and without autism:
A comparative study using the short sensory profile. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 61:190–200.
Torrente, F., P. Ashwood, R. Day, N. Machado, R. I. Furlano, A. Anthony, S. E. Davies,
A. J. Wakefield, M. A. Thomson, J. A. Walker-Smith, and S. H. Murch. 2002. Small
intestinal enteropathy with epithelial IgG and complement deposition in children with
regressive autism. Mol. Psychiatry 7: 334, 375–382.
Tsuang, M. T., J. L. Bar, W. S. Stone, and S. V. Faraone. 2004. Gene-environment interactions
in mental disorders. World Psychiatry 3:73–83.
Tuchman, R. and I. Rapin. 2006. Autism: A Neurobiological Disorder of Early Brain
Development. London, U.K.: Mac Keith Press.
Valicenti-McDermott, M., K. McVicar, I. Rapin, B. K. Wershil, H. Cohen, and S. Shinnar.
2006. Frequency of gastrointestinal symptoms in children with autistic spectrum
disorders and association with family history of autoimmune disease. J. Dev. Behav.
Pediatr. 27:S128–S136.
Vancassel, S., G. Durand, C. Barthelemy, B. Lejeune, J. Martineau, D. Guilloteau, C. Andres,
and S. Chalon. 2001. Plasma fatty acid levels in autistic children. Prostaglandins Leukot.
Essent. Fatty Acids 65:1–7.
Autism: The Centrality of Active Pathophysiology 387
CONTENTS
19.1 Introduction ............................................................................................... 389
19.2 Biopsychosocial Model ............................................................................. 391
19.3 Scope of the Term “Autism” ..................................................................... 393
19.4 Treatment of Autism ................................................................................. 394
19.5 Recommendations for Autism Treatment ................................................. 398
19.5.1 Primary Care Autism Professional ............................................ 399
19.5.2 Assessment and Treatment Centers ............................................ 401
19.5.3 Funding, Government, and Collaboration ..................................403
19.5.4 Positive Psychology ....................................................................405
19.6 Autism, Anxiety, and Autonomic Regulation ...........................................408
19.7 Treating the Environment ......................................................................... 410
19.8 Conclusion ................................................................................................. 413
References .............................................................................................................. 413
19.1 INTRODUCTION
An anecdote (take 1): My 21-year-old adult son with autism was taken to a klezmer
music concert. The staff of his treatment program reports that it was a wonderful
success. He loved the music, so much so that he got up and jumped up and down
and waved his arms ecstatically. His joy was infectious and made many of the other
389
390 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
concert-goers loosen up and enjoy the concert. His treatment team will look for other
concerts, which might also make him that happy.
Anecdote (take 2): My 21-year-old son was taken to a klezmer music concert.
The staff of his treatment program reported that this was not a successful outing.
He became so excited by the music that he could not sit in his seat; he waved his
arms and jumped, which must have disturbed others at the concert. Redirection was
ineffective as he was so excited. The plan will be to take him to more concerts but
perhaps not as stimulating for him so that he can learn appropriate behavior in a
concert setting; however, for the foreseeable future, klezmer music will not be part
of his plan.
These two versions of events are composites of a host of true stories that go on
every day in the world of autism. Aside from being a tactical decision as to whether
or not the treatment team should enforce “appropriate” behavior, the question also
speaks to a whole concept of how society and those with the disease of autism should
interface with each other. To explore this topic will raise a host of questions as to how
the presently established systems, which interface with autism, might be redirected
toward revised roles and relationships with each other.
Autism and its related disorders are among the most severe of the chronic child-
hood onset illnesses. A diagnosis given to a two-year-old will surely be a life-altering
experience. That family will never be the same. Nearly any measure of morbidity
can document the severity of this disease and for that reason, there is a pressing need
for treatment. In the past 20 years, we have seen great progress in the development
of some treatments for autism. These treatments have grown out of existing tech-
nologies such as applied behavior analysis (ABA) and psychopharmacology. These
techniques have been applied to autism, sometimes with a great deal of success and
sometimes not. Looking at the situation broadly however, Howlin et al. (2004) pres-
ent a bleak picture. Only 12% of autistic adults were rated as having a very good
outcome with 46% poor and another 12% were termed very poor outcomes. Some
of the measures used in this report were cognitive, language, social, communication
measures, as well as behavioral problems. Few of those individuals lived alone, had
close friends, or permanent employment. In another report studying a cohort born
in the 1960s through the 1980s, Billstedt et al. (2005) overall outcome was poor in
78% of the cases. It is likely that with more developed and more generally available
treatments, the numbers would be better if measured now; however, the overall con-
clusion must be that a very significant number of autistic individuals have and will
continue to have negative outcomes, which dwarf that of most other diseases.
The great need for treatments has directed the discussion, thinking, and plan-
ning about autism to simple questions as to whether these treatments are effective
and or whether they have side effects that outweigh their benefits. While even these
questions are difficult to answer, there exists little discussion in the literature or at
professional meetings as to what the more overarching goals of autism treatment
ought to be.
The science of the treatment of autism (and the other developmental disabilities)
can reasonably be characterized as being in its infancy. Although the state of the art
is disappointing to those who are in dire need of relief, the more optimistic view is
that we have an opportunity to conceptualize treatment possibilities in a fresh way.
A Reevaluation of the State of Autism Treatment 391
We can learn from some of the mistakes made in other areas of the medical psycho-
logical and social treatments for various diseases and conditions.
The goal of this chapter is to elucidate some of the issues, which have been either
totally ignored or received less than adequate attention by the professional and the
advocacy community. As this is a broad task, many topics will only be alluded to
rather than exhaustively explored. Clearly, there are no simple answers as to the best
way to approach the treatment of autism and with a spirit of humility, we must strive
to understand our predicament. In doing so, we must evaluate our conceptual models
with an attitude of inquiry. Quick fixes and sound bites will only hamper the effort to
provide benefit to those who most need it. I will attempt to challenge some prevailing
concepts around autism treatment and make a list of action items, which might be
used in moving toward a more effective and rational treatment portfolio for autism.
These action items will include the creating of a primary care model for autism treat-
ment, the creation of specialized assessment and treatment centers, the formation of
collaborations to cross disciplines including both the voluntary sector as well as the
government, the recognition and the more explicit treatment of autonomic dysregu-
lation, and the shift in focus of intervening in the environmental factors that have
large effects on the outcomes in autism. The central concept that unifies all of these
modifications is the application of the biopsychosocial model of treatment delivery.
medicine (except for some of the neater technologic fixes such as vaccinations and
plumbing to produce clean water) was left in the dust.
In reality, another societal force, the introduction of managed health care and cost
containment, helped the family practitioners to prosper at first with the concept of
replacing higher priced specialists with lower cost generalists. Perhaps for the same
reason, they became the next target of cost containment. In 1998, the number of
family practice residencies positions filled by U.S. medical graduates began to fall.
This appears not to be inconsistent with a move back to “expert” technically oriented
care or in other words, back to what has been called the medical model. Preventive
medicine and a more holistic practice continue to be only a minor part of the
physician’s role.
Engle, in describing his theory of biopsychosocial medicine, draws from general
systems theory and outlines a number of interacting “systems.” He describes these
starting with subatomic particles, and progressing to atoms, molecules, organelles,
cells, tissues, organs/organ systems, nervous system, person, two person, family,
community, culture/subculture, society/nation, and biosphere. Looking at the above
list of systems and conceptualizing them one a linear chart, one can subcategorize that
the first few systems from subatomic particles to nervous system are what we usually
term basic sciences. These are the foci of the medical model. The next level of person
up to the family is usually under the prevue of psychology while the systems of com-
munity up to biosphere are generally thought of as social. In our specialized world
of medical care, the physician handles medical model, the psychologist handles the
psychological issues, and the social worker deals with the social issues. If health
problems were to neatly fit into these systems there may be no reason to even do
this level of analysis; however, this is not the case. Each of these systems maintains
a separate existence and separate boundaries only up to a point. There is always a
dynamic interplay between the systems. A molecule, for example, retinoic acid, if
ingested, can lead to a depression, which can lead to family problems, or even rise to
the level of societal problem if the protagonist is in a central enough position. In the
other direction, wars cause emotional trauma, which we now know can alter levels
of the molecules made in the brain. Systems interact with each other not only on
this vertical level, but also a two-person system can interact with other two-person
system. The relationship between an employer and his employee can influence the
relationship between that employee and his wife.
The application of a systems model would change the content and certainly the
emphasis of the scientific literature associated with clinical issues. Scientific method
is inherently reductionist. The more complex the system, the less elegant the science
will be to study it. It is no accident that despite a desire to be scientific, there remains
very little “good science” to prove the benefits of psychotherapy. The evidence that
does exist is largely limited to behavioral therapy and cognitive therapies. Both of
these two therapies lend themselves more easily to manualization and therefore stan-
dardization. Therapies that call for more spontaneity on the part of the therapist are
much more difficult to standardize and study (Frank, 1979). Those therapies with
more concrete outcomes are more easily measured. For example, it is easier to mea-
sure a behavior than an insight. The more factors one includes in their analysis (as is
inevitable in a systems-based approach), the less amenable the therapy becomes to
A Reevaluation of the State of Autism Treatment 393
being researched. There is now a general appreciation that often, randomized con-
trolled clinical trials give results that differ from “real-life” population-based studies.
This is to say that the studies that take into account the larger or more systemic
issues, while being less specific, may offer results closer to the realities of clinical
problems. Effectiveness studies, also known as naturalistic, pragmatic, practical, or
real-life studies attempt to mimic daily clinical practice but often at the cost of
scientific reducibility. Therefore, one could say that clinical studies may suffer if sci-
entific rigor is elevated at the expense of a systemic understanding. It is only recently
that “real-life” studies are appearing in the literature and that funding sources are
acknowledging their necessity.
What is true for research is all the more of an issue in clinical practice. The patient,
who typically identifies a problem (be it on a biological or psychological or social
level), generally choose to go to one practitioner. If that practitioner is limited in his
or her scope, which nearly all practitioners are, this can lead to inferior care and often
to a lack of success in treatment. Despite the wide spread acknowledgment of Engel’s
work (up until 2002, there were 1419 citations of his science article), it appears that
the biopsychosocial model remains on the margins of medicine. Lindau et al. (2003)
believes that the biopsychosocial model has failed to replace the biomedical model
for two major reasons both of which are conceptual. One is because of our being
wedded to a particular model of fighting disease (rather than promoting health) and
the second is our existing institutions are historically rooted in the medical model.
I would like to add that the failure of this model to be more accepted could also be
explained by systems theory itself. Physicians live in multiple systems. Physicians
are also humans who desire to make money to support their families. Managed care
discourages this model as each physician spends only a few minutes with each patient.
If physicians were to delve into the psychological and social spheres with the same zest
that they approach the biological issues, it would take much more time. This would be
financed directly by reducing the physicians’ own income. This is a huge disincentive
to broaden ones view of the patient. Medical schools despite giving lip service to the
biopsychosocial model, I would suspect, continue to prefer the biomedical model, and
therefore, reward students who are more interested in that model. A brief glance at
the National Institute of Health funding patterns should convince anyone that the road
to an academic career (except in rare cases) is not paved through the biopsychosocial
model. Further, with other disciplines staking out their “turf,” it seems safer to fall
back on the skill that makes the practitioner unique, which for the MD is biologi-
cal. Despite the psychiatric establishment being sympathetic to the biopsychosocial
model, a huge political effort was aroused when psychologists tried to get prescribing
privileges thus potentially usurping their biologic role. Despite the ongoing calls of
prestigious institutions, the National Research Council and the Institute for Medicine,
to embrace this model, it appears that this will require much more effort than one can
reasonably expect from our currently empowered institutions.
controversy that autism does at this time. There could be a host of reasons for this;
however, I believe that the heterogeneity of the illness is a major factor. Most clinicians
and investigators are in agreement that autism is the syndromic endpoint of an etiologic
heterogeneous group of diseases (London, 2007). Further, there is a huge heterogene-
ity of the phenotype, which may or may not correlate with the etiologic heterogeneity
(Happe et al., 2006). Autism is present with a huge range of core symptom and severi-
ties. In addition, there is a wide range of associated symptoms with varying severities.
Even in a given individual, one can note an ever-changing set of symptoms, which vary
and sometimes even disappear with age and development. Nosologic considerations
dictate that we continue to use the word “autism” (Miller et al., 2006), yet this leads
to a situation characterized by the elephant and the blind-man paradigm, that is, the
vantage point that one is looking from and even the time frame of when you look will
determine what you see.
With this level of heterogeneity, one might easily understand that the concept of
“treatment” for “autism” is a very broad and often a confusing topic. Many treat-
ments, from many theoretical origins, practiced by many different practitioners; with
a dearth of literature support are the norm at this point. Despite all of these difficul-
ties, it appears to me that autism could be much better treated, and managed with the
application of biopsychosocial principles. Some of the ways we can improve treat-
ment would be by creating specificity about what we are treating, and clearly defining
the treatment goals. Through the inclusion more disciplines as part of a broader
treatment team and through an optimized treatment delivery system, the lives of
autistic individuals and their families can be greatly improved.
similar to ABA, there was often a lack of quality studies or adequately sized studies.
Their overall conclusion was that based on the lack of hard evidence for ABA as well
as other treatments that interventions be guided by individual needs and the avail-
ability of resources. In a similar vein, Reichow et al. (2008) motivated by the lack of
any treatment (for young autistic individuals) that meets the criterion for evidence-
based practice in autism, suggest better methods of evaluating research so that the
practitioner can have more informed guidance for their clinical work.
It is clear that ABA has much to offer the autistic individual. However, the quan-
titation of how much it offers remains less clear. The argument often invoked by
behaviorists that unproved treatments could crowd out the better—proven behavioral
treatment can be looked upon in a different way. Although the leading treatment at
the moment, the history of medicine also shows that conventional treatments eventu-
ally yield to other types or treatments. We must be vigilant not to exclude other forms
of treatment, which currently have less or no evidence of benefit solely due to a lack
of research to document benefit. These facts call for the necessity of the rigorous
but also creative study of many types of treatments. A blind adherence to orthodoxy
can be almost as dangerous as blind faith in unproven treatments. A case that I had
some personal experience with involved an orthodox ABA school that when con-
fronted with an increase in previously well-treated behaviors, resumed behavioral
procedures to extinguish these behaviors. A few days later the young man died of
a ruptured appendix. The staff was just unaware that not all behaviors should be
treated with ABA and only ABA.
Perhaps an even more confusing situation exists for the use of psychopharma-
ceutical agents in autism. Mandell et al. (2008) in a survey of Medicaid enrolled
children with the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders found that 56% were on
at least 1 psychotropic medication and 20% were on 3 or more medications. The use
of psychotropics was common even in the <2 age group. These numbers are not out of
line with previous estimates (Aman et al., 2005). Similar to behavioral interventions,
it seems that there can be little serious doubt that psychopharmacology has much to
offer to the autistic population. The specifics, sadly, are again not that clear.
The most established medication for use in autism is risperidone, which has
obtained Food and Drug Administration approval for the indication of irritability
associated with autism (Parikh et al., 2008). Risperidone and virtually all other
medications used in autism target associated rather than the core symptoms of
autism. In contrast, there are no drugs that are commonly used or proven effective in
the treatment of the core social and communication impairments that are hallmarks
of the pervasive developmental disorders (PDDs) (Posey et al., 2008). The lack of a
clear understanding of the pathophysiology of the core symptoms of autism makes
it difficult to identify drug targets for autism.
Therefore, current medications are targeted at symptoms rather than the disease.
It would appear that the attempt to show that a medication can treat as heterogeneous
disease as autism might be a conceptual error. The literature almost invariably frames the
benefit of the medicine in the context of the disease although nearly all medications
in psychiatry cross diagnostic boundaries in their efficacies. Medications that may be
very effective at treating one or more associated symptoms in some individuals may be
rather ineffective at treating the broad group of patients diagnosed with autism.
396 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Another related conceptual problem is the use of the “dual diagnosis” concept,
which has been developed over the years for mental retardation. The concept is
that intellectual disability is not the reason for a behavioral syndrome. Therefore,
there must be a second diagnosis to explain the behavior. This is based on many
assumptions, which are problematic. One is the fact that mental retardation or intel-
lectual disabilities are gross descriptors of a very heterogeneous group. It seems
almost certain that some forms of intellectual disability can explain behavior prob-
lems on a biological level. A second problem is the imposition of Diagnostic and
Statistical Manuel of Disorders (DSM) diagnoses which were formulated and vali-
dated on more “neurotypical” individuals and applying these diagnoses to those who
have various types of brain development abnormalities. Is the repetitive behavior
observed in autism the same (on a neurobiological level) as obsessive symptoms seen
in obsessive compulsive disorder? Are the social difficulties seen in autism the same
as that seen in social anxiety disorder? Although there is likely an overlap between
a developmentally disabled and a more neurotypical persons with these symptoms,
it is also very likely that when the neurobiology is clarified, it will show many funda-
mental differences despite having similar behavioral endpoints. Again, medications
are more likely to be efficiently targeted to specific symptoms, which will cross
diagnostic boundaries; however, in many cases, medications may be more specific if
the underlying pathophysiology is treated rather than the symptom.
Clinicians treating autism, due to a lack of literature support, generally have to
form their own practice styles and call on their clinical experience (which may be of
great value or highly skewed). An example of the problems facing the pharmacologi-
cally oriented clinician, trying to practice evidence-based medicine is highlighted
by a recent publication. Tyrer et al. (2008) compared haloperidol, risperidone, and
placebo in 86 nonpsychotic intellectually disabled subjects having aggressive chal-
lenging behaviors. Remarkably, the placebo group showed the most benefit in reduc-
ing the aggression scale score, with placebo, risperidone, and haloperidol showing
79%, 58%, and 65% improvement, respectively. The author’s conclusion was that
“antipsychotic drugs should no longer be regarded as an acceptable routine treatment
for aggressive challenging behavior in people with intellectual disability.” What was
meant by an acceptable routine treatment is not clear to me. An interpretation of this
conclusion could be that they are contending that this type of patient is being inad-
equately treated on a non-pharmacologic basis although they do not state that explic-
itly. In the same issue of Lancet (Matson and Wilkens, 2008), there is a comment on
that article. These authors point out that in most cases of aggression in people with
intellectual disability, there is an environmental component such as escape from
demands, attracting one’s caregiver, or gaining access to preferred items. They are
taking a more biopsychosocial perspective and suggesting that perhaps a behavioral
intervention or a combination of treatments would be more effective. Scahill et al.
(2008), in response to this study, point out a couple of methodological flaws to the
Tyrer article. These include the use of two active comparators with a small sample
size and the selection criterion used.
Tyrer et al. (2008) themselves also note that their findings were not concordant
with other studies in the literature, which have shown antipsychotics valuable for this
population. It seems that Tyrer et al. (2008) as well as Matson and Wilkins (2008)
A Reevaluation of the State of Autism Treatment 397
were tying to put forth the argument that a psychosocial intervention should be tried
in aggressive persons with intellectual disabilities. The data presented had little to do
with that conclusion. Rather instead of clearly promoting a biopsychosocial approach,
the impression was left that antipsychotics are no better than placebo in this group
that strains credulity. It is striking that a 79% improvement on placebo was not even
commented on by the authors. A 79% improvement on placebo, if taken literally,
might necessitate that all intellectually impaired subjects with aggression problems
be given a trial on placebo despite the ethical issues that might be raised. Another
and perhaps the most important lesson from the above discussion might be acknowl-
edging the enormous complexity in conceptualizing benefit in this population.
The lack of a biopsychosocial model and a lack of attention to the limitations of
reductionist assumptions that permeate the clinical research field continue to make
evidence-based understandings difficult if not impossible for the clinician interested
in autism and developmental disorders. A much more interesting and informative
publication would have been an analysis of why this study was able to document
such a robust placebo effect. The psychopharmacologic literature rarely, if ever, con-
trols or even comments on psychosocial treatments, which the subjects are invari-
ably exposed too (even if only in school). This is true also for the behavioral and
educational literature making little comment on the medications being used by their
subjects. My own experience includes being an MD prescribing medications to autis-
tic children who were in behavioral (ABA) studies, where the medications were not
mentioned in the publications documenting the benefits of ABA. I suppose that I am
not alone in having that experience.
It is estimated that as many as 50%–75% of children with autism may be treated
with complimentary and alternative medicine (CAM) treatments (Levy and Hyman,
2008). Levy et al. (2003) reported that almost one-third of children referred for evalu-
ation were already being treated with dietary therapies by their parents even before
the confirmation of the diagnosis. This should not be surprising in that the National
Center for Complimentary and Alternative Medicine reports that over three-fourths of
American adults use CAM treatments for disease or to maintain health. It is estimated
that 2%–50% of children in the United States are given CAM therapies (Davis and
Darden, 2003), and this is likely to be an underestimate. Children with chronic illness
such as cancer asthma, rheumatoid arthritis, and developmental disorders are even
more likely to be using CAM therapies. Although there are some CAM treatments
with evidence to support its use (such as melatonin for sleep), many other treatments
have been clearly disproved (e.g., secretin), or are disproportionately dangerous
(e.g., chelating agents). The majority of CAM treatments, however, have not been stud-
ied and there is scant evidence to support or deny their use. Levy and Hyman (2008)
comment on the issue of families even reporting CAM use to their clinicians. Although
there may be many reasons for this, I would contend that the lack of a biopsychosocial
perspective on the part of the clinician leads the families to disqualify the practitioners
as consultants on this topic. The assumption is made that the physician is constricted in
their thinking and unable to see past what is an inadequate literature. Often this drives
the families into a counterculture of practitioners who are resistant to examining the
evidence at all and tout their own treatments. These practitioners sometimes make
rather fantastic profits on treatments, which have no medical evidence. A few years
398 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
ago when the hormone secretin was reputed to be beneficial for autism, there were
practitioners charging five figure amounts per injection.
I would also hypothesize that a large part of the reason for the sizable anti-medical
and antiscience movements, which have grown around the families of autistic indi-
viduals, is related to the lack of the application of the biopsychosocial model.
An important article “Needs of parents of mentally retarded children” was written
by Murray (1959), a chairperson of the Virginia Association for Mentally Retarded
Children (an organization now know as ARC). In this article, she outlined six basic
problems that the parents faced. One was “coping with inept, inaccurate, or ill-timed
professional advice.” Rather than this being rejecting of need for professionals, she
goes on to say “The greatest single need of parents of mentally retarded (MR) chil-
dren is constructive professional counseling at various stages in the child’s life which
will enable the parents to find the answers to their own individual problems.”
There is a feeling of disconnection that the families feel from their practitioners.
Parents perceive that practitioners tailor treatments to their own theoretical predis-
positions, tout their brand of therapy to the exclusion of others, dismiss other forms
of therapy, and have little understanding or interest in how the families perceive the
problems. Into that vacuum enter other parent advocates, often with little under-
standing of medicine, psychology, or science but with a great deal of understanding
of the problems (often systemic) that they are encountering on a daily basis. The
response has been to take the solutions to these problems into their own hands and
become their own case managers. There is no shortage of “alternative” practitioners
who are willing to lead this overtly angry charge against “conventional” practitio-
ners. A plausible way of framing the issue is that the treatment community, by isolating
themselves from the families issues (bio, psychological, and social) have not only
facilitated the propagation of an industry of dubious practices, but also the spin-off
has been the creation of a dangerous anti-vaccine lobby that is currently threatening
the public health both in America and worldwide (Offit, 2008).
in Alzheimer’s disease, the burden of care giving falls in nontraditional settings, that
is, the families and the school systems. Schools have the legal obligation to provide
an “appropriate education,” a vague term if there ever was one. The educational
system has de facto become the major treatment entity for autism. This role has not
been fondly embraced. Evidence for this is the microscopic funding that the federal
Department of Education spends on autism treatment research, which is at odds
with an astronomical budget that school districts pay for the special education of
autistic children.
Each of the recommendations made involve major changes conceptualizing how
we treat autism. Each one involves multiple systems to be involved in the changes.
Again I would hope that these recommendations are looked upon as conceptual
directions and that further work will need to go into refining the details of how to
facilitate real change and hopefully improvement.
in these cases, will be related to autism symptoms but many will not be. The PCAP
must have the expertise to make that differentiation. Many problems may cross over.
An example might be self-abusive behavior. Although wounds must be tended to on
a medical level, the behavior must also be controlled. The PCAP must also have to
have a working knowledge of behavioral treatments, including ABA, psychopharma-
cology, as well as other emerging treatment modalities. The practitioner would need
to have the ability to institute some treatments and also a good working understand-
ing of places to refer. The practitioner would also need to be conversant with the edu-
cational system and be able to function as a case manager for obtaining the right fit
for an educational placement. As the autistic individual ages toward adulthood, the
PCAP will help council the family as to their options and help to supervise the transi-
tional planning toward the work world. The PCAP would also function in the role that
social workers usually take in geriatrics. That is facilitating the process of obtaining
support systems such as respite. The PCAP, by virtue of being “in the middle” of
these issues, would be able to perform an advocacy role. They would be fully aware
of the services that are not available but badly needed in that community.
People with autism grow up and live roughly normal life spans. It is not a child-
hood disease when the subject becomes an adult. The practitioner should be equally
conversant with the problems encountered in adulthood. The existence of a disci-
pline such as PCAP is more than just a bureaucratic nicety. As the disease of autism
is not likely to be eliminated any time in the near future, a host of treatment-based
decisions must be made. Bergsma and Engel (1988) discuss this issue in reference
to medical decisions, which must be made around the concept of the quality of life
(QOL). Some of these decisions entail when and how much palliative care should
be instituted especially at the end of life. They outline three different models of the
doctor–patient relationship. One is the model of the doctor being authoritative. That
is he or she will basically make the decision due to their knowledge and experience.
A second option is the one where the patients make their own decision, and the phy-
sician’s opinion is only of minor importance. Other sources of information can be used
such as getting second opinions, consulting with friends, relatives, and, nowadays,
the Internet. This leads to “doctor shopping” or going to different doctors until one is
found who takes the position already formulated. Unfortunately, this type of doctor–
patient relationship is not uncommon now in those families afflicted with autism. With
only specialists to consult with (the behaviorist, the occupational therapist, the psy-
chopharmacologist, etc.) families find themselves with professionals who have only
one treatment. Often each one touts his/her own treatment, sometimes to the exclusion
of the other. More perniciously, there are many practitioners who predict dire conse-
quences if their treatment is not done exclusively and around the clock. It is no wonder
that the families at this point regroup and decide that only they can make the choices.
This is often done during times of crisis and under extreme emotional pressure (not
unlike the end of life decisions discussed above). Most families are going through this
issue for the first time and have little of their own experience to fall back on. In mak-
ing these decisions, they can be more influenced by those with the slickest message
promising the best outcome. Parents bond together using the Internet and advise each
other; however, the highly emotionally charged nature of these interactions can readily
be seen. All of this often leads to poor choices or worse dangerous ones. The danger
A Reevaluation of the State of Autism Treatment 401
appears now to spilling over from the autism cases themselves to the community at
large (Offit, 2008).
The third kind of relationship is one characterized by openness and mutual
respect. In this relationship, it is possible to have a dialogue and to arrive at a conclu-
sion together. This type of relationship is unfortunately becoming quite rare in our
society. It calls for trust, which is formed through a personal relationship. In most
cases, this can only be formed through long-term relationships.
Decisions are often multifactorial and often there is no absolute right answer.
Sandler and Hulgus (1992) analyze the clinical application of the biopsychosocial
model and its applicability in clinical situations. They outline three components to
the decision-making process in medicine. The first can be described as having the
scientific knowledge and the experience that the clinician can offer in their role.
The second is the ethical aspect. In this sphere, decisions need to be made based on
values. At times, the values held by the patient or the family might be different than
that of the doctor, that is, “I would rather die than have my legs amputated.” The
biopsychosocially competent practitioner must be able to represent their own values
but at the same time be respectful of the patient’s values. A dialogue about these val-
ues might help the patient (or the doctor) revise their thinking. The third component
is the pragmatic aspect. For example, if a behavioral program is recommended and
is not available or the finances to pay for it are not available, what should be done?
Perhaps a legal consultation is needed or perhaps, an alternative type of therapy
should be explored first. Again the clinician might have a great deal to offer in helping
the family make the very hard decisions of how to proceed.
I would propose the PCAP could serve in these roles for autism. As one who
is knowledgeable about the field as well as the patient and their family, decisions
can be informed by the longitudinal history developed over years. The PCAP must
be trained in systems theory, to understand that these decisions are often not one-
dimensional. If the family needs to hire an attorney to fight for behavioral therapy,
will that mean that the sibling cannot go to summer camp that year? This might
be the real issue, which the family is facing. How often is a family able to express
problem to a clinician? How often does the clinician actually help the family address
the issue? Systems theory might suppose that the stress on the family would lead
to stress on the autistic individual, which could lead to behavioral acting out. This
would lead the psychopharmacologist to suggest a medication. This may have side
effects such as sedation or gastrointestinal problems, which could lead to further
consultations. This cycle could go on and the only way to get a handle on it is for
someone (at this point it is generally the parent) to see the whole chain of events and
work with the system to maximize benefits.
of the medical world and the educational world, there would be a synergistic effect of
improving outcome.
Up to this point, this chapter has focused mostly on issues related to service delivery
and the systemic roles of the players. There are specific treatment-oriented items in
the literature, which have received very little attention, and which would have a large
influence on the way autism treatment could be approached.
Dykens (2006) reviews the history of treatment in mental retardation with its empha-
sis on “external measures of success and how happiness has been de-emphasized.”
She reviews four major movements. These movements include the QOL movement,
the dual diagnosis movement, the personality motivation movement, and family
research.
There have been some efforts to quantify the “QOL” in developmental disabili-
ties, autism, and Aspergers disorder (Fresher-Samways et al., 2003; Gerber et al.,
2008; Jennes-Coussens et al., 2006) and in their families (Allik et al., 2006; Mugno
et al., 2007). The concept of QOL is in itself an elusive concept (Fresher-Samways et al.,
2003) with 44 different definitions found in 47 studies on the topic. The questions
asked are highly subjective. With a nonverbal population, the studies have had
to rely on proxies to survey (Gerber et al., 2008) with the finding that staff and
families have different opinions and the scores are considerably different. QOL
includes the following dimensions: (1) emotional well-being, (2) interpersonal rela-
tions, (3) material well-being, (4) personal development, (5) physical well-being,
(6) self-determination, (7) social inclusion, and (8) rights. Only 1% of the questions
in QOL surveys related directly to the concepts of happiness, contentment, enjoy-
ment, and zest for life. It is true that QOL items found in the surveys are likely to be
indirectly correlated to happiness; however, caution should be exercised in that we
are not projecting a “neurotypical” slant on what makes an autistic person happy.
Direct measurement of these factors would be very difficult; nevertheless, strategies
need to be developed to accomplish this task. A possible solution will be discussed
below under the topic of autonomic functioning.
The issue of psychiatric problems or the “dual diagnosis” movement is Dykens’
second area of focus. The 40% of MR population has clinically significant psychi-
atric problems (Einfeld and Tongue, 1997) and 52% of autistic individuals are on
psychotropic medications. The most common issues include self-injury, depression,
aggression, anxiety, and stereotypies. It would seem that these behaviors or symp-
toms would have a profound influence on happiness. There remain questions of how
many of these problems are related to biologic or brain-based abnormalities (to be
treated largely with medications and biologic treatments) and how many of them are
related to environmental issues (to be treated with various psychologically based
treatments). A third outlook on the problem and one rarely taken is that the symp-
toms are a response to a lack of happiness and can be treated by the focus and devel-
opment on those factors that will increase happiness such as working toward creating
a sense of pleasure in ones work.
The third movement is described as the personality-motivation movement. This
describes the phenomenon, which aside from the low IQ or brain lesion is causing
problems. These secondary problems may be very significant. Often the MR popu-
lation has a sense of low expectations for success, which leads to a low motivation
to master challenge and ultimately a learned helplessness (which has been tied to
depression). This issue of motivation might be both in the realm of psychology, that
is, learned helplessness, and there is also a biologic overlap, which appears large
in autism. There have been studies on “intrinsic motivation” a concept that due to
a lack of attention, which might be a very profound deficiency in our understand-
ing of autism. The basis of behavioral treatments in autism is to provide artificial
A Reevaluation of the State of Autism Treatment 407
motivators (such as edibles) for children who typically receive intrinsic motivation.
An example is in playing with toys. Autistic children in behavioral programs are
given edibles to induce them to increase the time they might spend playing with a toy
in contrast to the typical child who cannot be kept away from a new toy or in other
words have a powerful intrinsic motivation. While the behaviorist focuses on the
endpoint of the length of time spent in playing, there has been little or no attention
paid to the phenomenon of the intrinsic reward or lack of it, and whether there are
ways to improve the intrinsic rewards.
The last movement described is the family research movement. Systemic fam-
ily therapy has long viewed the system or in this case, the family as the “identified
patient.” With developmental disabilities and perhaps more so autism, there has been
a reluctance to view the family functioning as an object of treatment. The reason for
this is likely due to the early history of autism treatment in which Bettelheim (1967)
and the psychoanalytic movement introduce the concept of the “refrigerator mother”
who was too emotionally cold to raise a healthy child. Autistic children were taken
away from their families and placed in inpatient settings. Much of the early advocacy
in autism was aimed at reversing this notion. In reality, some families do function
better than others and there are correctable systemic issues, which would make the
family happier and most likely the individual also. This could be framed in a positive
psychology format. The clear message should be that the families functioning did not
create the autism but rather the family has the ability to use their discretion to set up
systems to problem solve. Research to examine this is just beginning.
Dykens asks us to ponder the applicability of these formulations to mental retar-
dation. She notes that based on the research, little can be said; as there has not been
“a shred of research” in mental retardation that speaks to the issues that makes peo-
ple thrive and be happy. She then proposes that the same features that help people in
general to thrive and be happy—positive emotions, gratifications, flow, virtues and
strengths—also operate in the context of mental retardation. I agree with Dykins
on this concept. It would seem very improbable if there were not a set of properties
that would be concordant with producing happiness in the developmentally disabled.
It would be hard to argue that producing happiness is not a worthwhile goal. The ques-
tion that appears to be the more controversial might be whether the field of autism
treatment will consciously adopt the happiness of the individual and his systems as
a treatment target to be spoken about, planned for, measured, and researched in order
to improve the technology for that goal. There is no doubt that behaviorists as well as
psychopharmacologists consciously or unconsciously target their treatments toward
creating happiness. Many of the competencies built through behaviorism have been
shown to be a powerful part of the arsenal of the positive psychologist.
Perhaps a more productive way of framing the debate would be about the bal-
ancing of the goals. Referring back to the opening paragraphs of this chapter, let
us review the initial question. Should ecstatic jumping and arm waving be encour-
aged as an integral part of achieving happiness during the music concert, or is the
competency of being able to sit appropriately in the concert hall and be an accepted
part of the crowd more important. Dykens call to research on this topic is more
than an academic exercise. Rather it might speak to the heart of treatment programs
for autism.
408 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
A new phenomenon that is going to be present in the next few years will be the
emergence of an unprecedented numbers of autistic adults. This new demographic
group might color the thinking related to these questions. The field has long been
dominated by a rehabilitative, learning model to correct what is “wrong” with the
autistic individual. I suspect that this will continue to be the model adopted and
supported by the parents of young children who are hopeful that the autism will be
“cured.” As the child ages, an alternative viewpoint would be created by the tacit
acknowledgment that these adults are largely going to be who they are now and that
the amount of biologic or internal capacity improvement in older teens or adults is
likely to be minimal. Instead, the most beneficial treatments of these older groups
will be to organize optimal systems around them. The question of how to optimize
these systems is first dependent on our clearly prioritizing our goals. If we do clarify
our priorities, the development of the systems to optimally create happiness will still
be very challenging. I would like to suggest two specific directions that would be
important for this goal, although many more ideas need to be created.
temporal cortex, cerebellum, and the brain stem can support a central nervous
system hypothesis of autonomic dysregulation (Courchesne, 1997). Abnormalities
in the oxytocin systems of the brain in autism (Green et al., 2001; Modahl et al.,
1998) and some evidence of symptom improvement with oxytocin (Hollander
et al., 2003) would also fit an autonomic dysregulation hypothesis. Oxytocin has
been shown to reduce the fear-inducing activation of the amygdala, which via a
brain-stem pathway regulates autonomic functioning (Kirsch et al., 2005). These
findings are consistent with the polyvagal theory (Porges, 2006), which predicts
that there will be pathologic findings in a social engagement system in which vagal
functioning is dysregulated.
Studies have shown low cortisol levels in autism or normal levels despite the auto-
nomic hyperactivity and or high adrencorticotropic hormone (ACTH) levels (Curin
et al., 2003; Jansen et al., 2003; Tani et al., 2005) with a slow response to ACTH
(Marinovic-Curin et al., 2008) leading to the speculation that some of the symp-
toms seen in autism are a result of the lack of a normal hypothalamic–pituitary–
adrenal axis and autonomic physiologic functioning that helps the organism cope
with stress.
Despite the substantial amount of evidence that these types of symptoms are very
common in autism, and that they likely have a profound impact on behavior, the
ability to learn and to adjust to various situations and settings, there has been very
little systematic study of the treatment of these problems. Pharmaceuticals have been
studied (Findling, 2005) and no doubt have an important role; however, safety and
tolerability considerations make this option less than optimal. One successful study
of cognitive behavioral therapy with high functioning autism or Aspergers syndrome
has been published (Chalfant et al., 2007). The question of how generalized this is
to the lower functioning group as well as how many of even the higher functioning
group will cooperate with this is unknown. Alternative methods such as relaxation
and yoga have been suggested; however, there is little in the literature to document
its practicality as a treatment.
A concerted effort would need to be made to monitor the extent of this above
discussed phenomena and to prove that treatments are available that could lower the
anxiety, with the likely outcome of improving behavior, improving cognition, and
perhaps even enhancing happiness.
be a place where some with their own personal problems may find work sometimes
leading to scandalous examples of abuse (Kittay, 2001). Family caregivers likewise
face enormous stress; by 2004, there were an estimated 711,000 Americans with
intellectual disability living with caregivers over the age of 60 (Pollack, 2007).
The goal of public health (Talley and Crews, 2007) is to promote healthy indi-
viduals living in healthy communities, pursuing QOL rather than just the absence of
disease. Caregiving has become an issue that affects the QOL for millions of indi-
viduals. Scientists and practitioners rarely thought of caregiving as a public health
matter. The authors call for the public health systems to understand more about the
caregivers themselves and then to design and implement evidence-based interven-
tions to address the identified needs. Research needs to be done to uncover the pos-
sible risk factors associated with the endless types of caregiving situations. Of all
diseases, progress in this area seems most critical for autism.
19.8 CONCLUSION
To many in society, and even to those in the health professions, autism seems to
have placed itself in our consciousness, as if sneaking in from nowhere. This may
be because the prevalence is actually on the rise or the more likely explanation is
that it is due to redefinition, and the awareness of these symptoms in the population.
In practical terms, there is no debate that there must be a new and improved set of
interventions for this very hard to treat group. Defying easy classification, autism
straddles many systems (i.e., educational and medical) diagnostic entities (psychi-
atric or intellectual disability) and, to some extent, has led the way to an intensified
interest in brain development and early childhood development. It is also true that
those searching for answers do have many bases of knowledge to start from. Many
medications developed for psychiatric indications work fairly well for autism. Much
of the progress in the conceptualization and integration of intellectual disability can
be transferred to autism. Models of care, which have been developed and held out
much hope such as milieu therapy and the biopsychosocial model of medicine, need
to be dusted off and applied more consciously to autism. The new advances in
neuroscience hold out great hope for our understanding, leading to treatments and
prevention for autism and if added to enhance clinical delivery systems, there is great
promise for improvement in the lives of these affected individuals.
REFERENCES
Allik, H., J.O. Larsson, and H. Smedje. 2006. Health-related quality of life in parents of
school-age children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Health Qual.
Life Outcomes 4: 1, http://www.hqlo.com/content/4/1/1
Althous, M., A.M. Van Roon, L.J. Mulderm, G. Mulder, C.C. Aarnoudse, and R.B. Minderaa.
2004. Autonomic response patterns observed during performance of an attention-
demanding task in two groups of children with autistic-type difficulties in social adjustment.
Psychophysiology 41: 893–904.
Aman, M.G., K.S. Lam, and M.E. Van Bourgndien. 2005. Medication patterns in patients
with autism: Temporal, regional, and demographic influences. J. Child Adolesc.
Psychopharmacol. 15: 116–126.
414 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Amaral, D.G. and B.A. Corbett. 2003. The amygdala, autism and anxiety. Novartis Found. Symp.
251: 177–187.
American Psychiatric Association (1980) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, 3rd edition, Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association Press.
American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, 4th edition, Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association Press.
Axelrod, F.B., G.C. Chelimsky, and D.E. Weese-Mayer. 2006. Pediatric autonomic disorders.
Pediatrics 118: 309–321.
Baron-Cohen, S., H.A. Ring, E.T. Bullmore, S. Wheelwright, C. Ashwin, and S.C. Williams.
2000. The amygdala theory of autism. Neruosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24: 355–364.
Bergsma, J. and G.L. Engel. 1988. Quality of life: Does measurement help? Health Policy 10:
267–279.
Bettelheim, B. 1967. “The Empty Fortress” Infantile Autism and the Birth of the Self, New York:
The Free Press.
Billstedt, E., I.C. Gillberg, and C. Gillberg. 2005. Autism after adolescence: Population-based
13-22 year follow-up study of 120 individuals with autism diagnosed in childhood.
J. Autism Dev. Disord. 33: 351–360.
Bodfish, J.W., F.J. Symons, D.E. Parker, and M.H. Lewis. 2000. Varieties of repetitive behavior
in autism: Comparisons to mental retardation. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 30: 237–243.
Burnette, C.P., P.C. Mundy, J.A. Meyer, S.K. Sutton, A.E. Vaughan, and D. Charak. 2005. Weak
central coherence and its relations to theory of mind and anxiety in autism. J. Autism Dev.
Disord. 35: 63–73.
Cassidy, J. 2001. Truth lies and intimacy: An attachment perspective. Attach. Hum. Dev.
3: 121–155.
Chalfant, A.M., R. Rapee, and L. Carroll. 2007. Treating anxiety disorders in children with
high functioning autism spectrum disorders: A controlled trial. J. Autism Dev. Disord.
37: 1842–1857.
Chamberlin, B., C. Kasar, and E. Rotheram-Fuller. 2007. Involvement or isolation? The
social networks of children with autism in regular classrooms. J. Autism Dev. Disord.
37: 230–242.
Courchesne, E. 1997. Brainstem, cerebellar and limbic neuroanatomic abnormalities in autism.
Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 7: 269–278.
Curin. J.M., J. Terzic, Z.B. Petkovic, L. Zekan, I.M. Terzic, and I.M. Susnjara. 2003. Lower
cortisol and higher ACTH levels in individuals with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord.
33: 443–448.
Czura, C.J. and K.J. Tracey. 2005. Autonomic neural regulation of immunity. J. Intern. Med.
257: 156–166.
Davis, M.P. and P.M. Darden. 2003. Use of complementary and alternative medicine by children
in the United States. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 157: 393–396.
Dunne, W., B.S. Myles, and S. Orr. 2002. Sensory processing issues associated with Asperger
syndrome: A preliminary investigation. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 56: 97–102.
Dykens, E.M. 2006. Toward a positive psychology of mental retardation. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry
76: 185–193.
Einfeld, S. and B. Tonge. 1997. Population prevalence of psychopathology in children and
adolescents with intellectual disability II: Epidemiologic findings. J. Intellect. Disabil.
Res. 40: 90–109.
Engel, G.L. 1977. The need for a new medical model: A challenge for biomedicine. Science
196: 129–135.
Engel, G.L. 1980. The clinical application of the biopsychosocial model. Am. J. Psychiatry
137: 535–544.
Engel, G.L. 1992. How much longer must medicine’s science be bound by a seventeenth century
world view? Psychother. Psychosom. 57: 3–16.
A Reevaluation of the State of Autism Treatment 415
Engel, G.L. 1997. From biomedical to biopsychosocial. Psychother. Psychosom. 66: 57–62.
Esch, T. and G.B. Stefano. 2005. The neurobiology of love. Neuroendocrinol. Lett. 26:
175–192.
Findling, R. 2005. Pharmacologic treatment of behavioral symptoms in autism and pervasive
developmental disorders. J. Clin. Psychiatry 66(Suppl. 10): 26–31.
Foxx, R.M. 2008. Applied behavior analysis treatment of autism: The state of the art. Child
Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 17: 821–834.
Frank, J.D. 1979. The present status of outcome studies. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 47: 310–316.
Fresher-Samways, K., S.E. Roush, K. Choi, Y. Desrosiers, and G. Steel. 2003. Perceived
quality of life of adults with developmental and other significant disabilities. Disabil.
Rehabil. 25: 1097–1105.
Gadow, K.D., C.J. DeVincent, J. Pomeroy, and A. Azizian. 2004. Psychiatric symptoms in
preschool children with PDD, clinical and comparisons samples. J. Autism Dev. Disord.
34: 379–393.
Gerber, F., M.A. Baud, M. Geroud, and G. Galli Carninati. 2008. Quality of life of adults with
pervasive developmental disorders and intellectual disabilities. J. Autism Dev. Disord.
38: 1654–1665.
Grandin, T. 1991. Emergence: Labeled Autistic, Novato, CA: Arena Press.
Graveling, R.A. and J.D. Brooke. 1978. Hormonal and cardiac response of autistic children to
changes in environmental stimulation. J. Autism Child Schizophr. 8: 441–455.
Green, G. 1996. Early behavioral intervention for autism: What does research tell us?
In Behavioral Intervention for Young Children with Autism: A Manual for Parents and
Professionals, eds. C. Maurice, G. Green, and S.C. Luce, Austin, TX: Pro-Ed, pp. 29–44.
Green, L., D. Fein, C. Modhal, C. Feinstein, L. Waterhouse, and M. Morris. 2001. Oxytocin
and autistic disorder: Alterations in peptide forms. Biol. Psychiatry 50: 609–613.
Happe, F., A. Ronald, and R. Plomin. 2006. Time to give up on a single explanation for Autism.
Nat. Neurosci. 9: 1218–1220.
Hirstein, W., P. Iversen, and V.S. Ramachandran. 2001. Autonomic responses of autistic children
to people and objects. Proc. Biol. Sci. 268: 1883–1888.
Hollander, E., S. Novotny, M. Hanratty, R. Yaffe, C.M. DeCaria, B.R. Aronowitz, and S. Mosovich.
2003. Oxytocin infusion reduces repetitive behaviors in adults with autistic and Aspergers
disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 28: 193–198.
Howlin, P., S. Goode, J. Hutton, and M. Rutter. 2004. Adult outcome for children with autism.
J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 54: 212–229.
Hutt, C., S.J. Forrest, and J. Richer. 1975. Cardiac arrhythmia and behaviour in autistic children.
Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 51: 361–372.
Jansen, L., C.C. Gispen-de Weid, R.J. van der Gaag, and H. van Engeland. 2003. Differentiation
between autism and multiple complex developmental disorder in response to psycho-
logical stress. Neuropsychopharmacology 28: 582–590.
Jansen, L., C.C. Gispen-de Wied, V.M. Wiegant, H.G. Westenberg, B.E. Lahuis, and
R.J. van Engeland. 2006. Autonomic and neuroendocrine responses to a psycho-
social stressor in adults with autistic spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36:
891–899.
Jennes-Coussens, M., J. Magill-Evans, and C. Koning. 2006. The quality of life of young men
with Asperger syndrome: A brief report. Autism 10: 403–414.
Juranek, J., P.A. Fillpek, G.R. Berenji, C. Modahl, K. Osann, and M.A. Spence. 2006.
Association between amygdala volume and anxiety level: Magnetic resonance imaging
study in autistic children. J. Child Neurol. 21: 1051–1058.
Kanner, L. 1943. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child 2: 217–250.
Karst, T.O. 1991. Historical rational of the therapeutic community. In Autistic Adults at
Bittersweet Farms, eds. N.S., Giddan and J.J. Giddan, Binghampton, NY: Haworth Press,
pp. 30–52.
416 Autism: Oxidative Stress, Inflammation, and Immune Abnormalities
Kirsch, P., C. Esslinger, Q. Chen, D. Mier, S. Lis, S. Siddhanti, and H. Gruppe. 2005. Oxytocin
modulates neuronal circuitry for social cognition and fear in humans. J. Neurosci.
25: 11489–11493.
Kittay, E.F. 2001. When caring is just and justice is caring: Justice and mental retardation.
Public Cult. 13: 557–580.
Kittay, J. 2008. The sound surround exploring how one might design the everyday soundscape
for the truly captive audience. Nord. J. Music Ther. 17: 41–54.
Levy, S.E. and S.L. Hyman. 2008. Complementary and alternative medicine treatments for chil-
dren with autism spectrum disorders. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 17: 803–820.
Levy, S.E., D.S. Mandell, S. Merhar, R.F. Ittenbach, and J.A. Pinto-Martin. 2003. Use of
complementary and alternative medicine among children recently diagnosed with autistic
spectrum disorder. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 24: 418–423.
Lindau, S.T., E.O. Laumann, W. Levinson, and L.J. Waite. 2003. Synthesis of scientific disci-
plines in pursuit of health: The interactive biopsychosocial model. Perspect. Biol. Med.
46(Suppl. 3): S74–S86.
London, E. 2007. The role of the neurobiologist in redefining the diagnosis of autism. Brain
Pathol. 17: 408–411.
Mandell, D.S., K.H. Morales, S.C. Marcus, A.C. Stahmer, J. Doshi, and D.E. Polsky. 2008.
Psychotropic medication use among medicaid enrolled children with autism spectrum
disorders. Pediatrics 121: e441–e448.
Marinovic-Curin, J, I. Marinovic-Terzicet, Z. Bujas-Petkovic, L. Zekan, V. Skrabic, Z. Dogas,
and J. Terzic. 2008. Slower cortisol response during ACTH stimulation test in autistic
children. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 17: 39–43.
Matson, J.S. and J. Wilkins. 2008. Antipsychotic drugs for aggression in intellectual disability.
Comment. Lancet 371: 9–10.
Miller, F.A., M.E. Begbie, M. Giacomini, C. Ahern, and E.A. Harvey. 2006. Redefining disease?
The nosologic implications of molecular genetic knowledge. Perspect. Biol. Med. 49:
99–114.
Ming, X., P.O. Julu, M. Brimacombe, S. Connor, and M.L. Daniels. 2005. Reduced cardiac
parasympathetic activity in children with autism. Brain Dev. 27: 509–516.
Modahl, C., L. Green, D. Fein, M. Morris, L. Waterhouse, C. Feinstein, and H. Levin. 1998.
Plasma oxytocin levels in autistic children. Biol. Psychiatry 43: 270–277.
Mugno, D., L. Ruta, V.G. D’Arrigo, and L. Mazzone. 2007. Impairment of quality of life in
parents of children and adolescents with pervasive developmental disorder. Health Qual.
Life Outcomes 5: 22, http://www.hqlo.com/contents/5/1/22
Muris, P., P. Steerneman, H. Meckelbach, I. Holdrinet, and C. Meesters.1998. Comorbid
anxiety symptoms in children with pervasive developmental disorders. J. Anxiety
Disord. 12: 387–393.
Murray, M.A. 1959. Needs of parents of mentally retarded children. Am. J. Ment. Defic. 63:
1078–1088.
Offit, P.A. 2008. Autism’s False Prophets: Bad Science, Risky Medicine, and the Search for a
Cure, New York: Columbia University Press.
Ospina, M.B., J. Krebs Seida, B. Clark, M. Karkhaneh, L. Hartling, L. Tjosvold, B. Vandermeer,
and V. Smith. 2008. Behavioural and developmental interventions for autism spectrum
disorder: A clinical systematic review. PLOS ONE 3: e3755.
Parikh, M.S., A. Kolevzon, and E. Hollander. 2008. Psychopharmacology of aggression in
children and adolescents with autism: A critical review of efficacy and tolerability.
J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 18: 157–178.
Pollack, H. 2007. Learning to walk slow: America’s partial policy success in the arena of
intellectual disability. J. Policy Hist. 19: 95–112.
Porges, S.W. 2006. The polyvagal perspective. Biol. Psychol. 74: 116–143.
A Reevaluation of the State of Autism Treatment 417
Posey, D.J., C.A. Erickson, and C.J. McDougle. 2008. Developing drugs for core social
and communication impairment in autism. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 17:
787–801.
Powley, T.L. 2000. Vagal input to the enteric nervous system. Gut 47(Suppl. iv): 30–32.
Reichow, B., F.R. Volkmar, and D.V. Ciccetti. 2008. Development of the evaluative method for
evaluating and determining evidence-based practices in autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord.
38: 1311–1319.
Sandler, J.Z. and Y.F. Hulgus. 1992. Clinical problem solving and the biopsychosocial model.
Am. J. Psychiatry 149: 1315–1323.
Scahill, L., M.G. Aman, J.T. McCracken, C.J. McDougle, and B. Vitiello. 2008. Beware of
over-interpreting negative trials. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38: 1609–1610.
Seligman, M. 2002. Positive psychology, positive prevention and positive therapy. In Handbook
of Positive Psychology, eds. C.R. Snyder and S.J. Lopez, New York: Oxford University
Press, pp. 3–13.
Shogren, K.A., M.L. Wehmeyer, C.L. Buchanan, and S.J. Lopez. 2006. The application of posi-
tive psychology and self-determination to research in intellectual disability: A content
analysis of 30 years of literature. Res. Pract. Pers. Severe Disabil. 31: 338–345.
Sukhodolsky, D.G., L. Scahill, K.D. Gadow, L.E. Arnold, M.G. Aman, C.J. McDougle,
J.T. McCracken, E. Tierney, S. Williams White, L. Lecavalier, and B. Viteillo. 2008.
Parent rated anxiety symptoms in children with pervasive developmental disorders:
Frequency and association with core autism symptoms and cognitive functioning.
J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 36: 117–128.
Swiezy, N., M. Stuart, and P. Korzekwa. 2008. Bridging for success in Autism: Training and
collaboration across medical, educational and community systems. Child Adolesc.
Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 17: 907–922.
Talley, R.C. and J.E. Crews. 2007. Framing the public health of caregiving. Am. J. Public
Health 97: 224–228.
Tani, P., N. Lindberg, V. Matto, B. Appelberg, T. Nieminen-von Wendt, L. von Wendt, and
T. Porka-Heiskanen. 2005. Higher plasma ACTH levels in adults with Aspergers syndrome.
J. Psychsom. Res. 58: 533–536.
Toichi, M. and T. Kamio. 2003. Paradoxical autonomic response to mental tasks in autism.
J. Autism Dev. Disord. 33: 417–426.
Tyrer, P., P.C. Oliver-Africano, Z. Ahmed, N. Bouras, S. Cooray, S. Deb, D. Murphy, M. Hare,
M. Meade, B. Reece, K. Kramo, S. Bhaumik, D. Harley, A. Regan, D. Thomas, B. Rao,
B. North, J. Eliahoo, S. Kartela, A. Soni, and M. Crawford. 2008. Risperidone, haloperi-
dol and placebo in the treatment of aggressive challenging behaviour in patients with
intellectual disability: A randomized controlled trial. Lancet 371: 57–63.
Volkmar, F., E.H. Cook, J. Pomeroy, G. Realmuto, and P. Tanguay. 1999. Practice parameters
for the assessment of and treatment of children, adolescents and adults with autism
and other pervasive developmental disorders. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry
38 (Suppl. 12): 32–54.
Zahn, T.P., J.M. Rumsey, and D.P. Van Kammen. 1987. Autonomic nervous system activity
in autistic schizophrenic and normal men: Effects of stimulus significance. J. Abnorm.
Psychol. 96: 135–144.
Index
A public health, 413
safe, 410–411
ABA, see Applied behavior analysis tantrum, 411
Active pathophysiology; see also Chronic haloperidol, risperidone and placebo, 396
dynamic encephalopathy illness heterogeneity, 393–394
functions to ASD, 347–348 intellectual disabilities, 397
persistent, 346–347 medical model, 398–399
potential for plasticity, 349–350 model
Age related macular degeneration (AMD), 38 application, 392–393
AGP, see Amino-glycerophospholipids basic concepts, 391
AGRE, see Autism genetic resource exchange biomedical model, 393
Amino-glycerophospholipids (AGP) medicine theory, 392
blood plasma, 179 New York autism consortium,
content, 180 404–405
Animal model, 135–136 PCAP (see Primary care autism professional)
Applied behavior analysis (ABA) positive psychology
benefit, 394 biologic/internal capacity, 408
orthodox, 395 family therapy, 407
PCAP, 400 mental retardation, 405
Asperger syndrome (AS), 62 personality-motivation movement,
Autism colitis 406–407
gastrointestinal (GI) mucosa, 255–256 quality of life (QOL) and dual
proinflammatory cytokines, 256 diagnosis, 406
Autism genetic resource exchange (AGRE), 333 psychopharmaceutical agents, 395
Autistic cerebella 3-nitrotyrosine levels, 49 quagmire, 404
speech therapy, 403–404
B Bipolar disease (BPD)
comorbid conditions, 216
BBB, see Blood–brain barrier SNP markers, 217
BDNF, see Brain-derived neurotrophic factor Blood–brain barrier (BBB)
Biopsychosocial treatment cadmium, 158–159
ABA, 394–395 CVO, 155, 156
anti-medical and antiscience movements, 398 integrity, 233
antipsychotics, 396–397 melatonin, 162
anxiety and autonomic regulation neuroglial cell populations, 228
causes and significance, 409 neurotoxicants, 157–158
cortisol levels, 410 NTS, 164
description, 408 organic mercury, 160
dysregulation, 409–410 paraquat, 159
types, 408–409 TNF, 167
assessment and treatment centers BPD, see Bipolar disease
and diagnosis, 402 Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF)
psychiatric units, modalities, antioxidant factor, 40
402–403 cord blood, 166
university-based programs, hippocampal, 50
401–402 immunoaffinity chromatography, 51
CAM, 397–398 and neurotrophins
diagnosis, 403 levels, 19
environment NT-3 and NT-4/5, 18–19
children, 411–412 polymorphism, 53
intimacy bonds, 413–414 Rett syndrome, 55
419
420 Index
definition, 315 F
polymorphism
and expression, 317–318 Familial hemiplegic migraine (FHM)
genetic variability hypothesis, 320 and autism alleles, 215
maternal, 320–321 mutational lesions, 216
Th1/Th2/Th17 responses, FHM, see Familial hemiplegic migraine
319–320
G
D
GABA, see Gamma-aminobutyric acid
Developmental disorders GALT, see Gut-associated lymphoid tissue
connotations, 352 Gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA)
knowledge gaps, 353 adult brain, 260
reassessment type 1 diabetes, 261
prior findings, alternative interpretations, Gastrointestinal (GI) dysfunction
354–356 children, 282–283
restrictive impact, 356–358 vs. immune dysfunction
weak spots, 353–354 anxiety and depression, 287
treatment mechanism, 308–309 cognitive function, 285–286
working hypothesis and OT cytokines, 286
deficiency, 300 disorders, 287–288
DHA, see Docohexaenoic acid immunocytochemical analysis, 289
Diagnostic and statistical manuel of disorders mechanism, 291
(DSM), 396 molecular mimicry, 290
DMV, see Dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus opioids, 285
Docohexaenoic acid (DHA) serotonin, 288–289
description, 39 tight junctions, 288
and EPA, 181 infiltration, 284–285
Dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus (DMV) LNH, 284
area postrema (AP), 156 symptoms, 283
NTS, 164, 168 Genetics
PD, 156–157 CNVs
phonation, 163 family types, 66
TNF, 167 frequency of, 65–66
visceromotor fibers, 162 epidemiology, 62
heritability, 63
E integrative, 63–64
noncoding RNA role, 67–68
Eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA) parental age, 66–67
dietary consumption, 181 prenatal maternal stress and, 67
PLA2 concentrations, 182 single-gene mutations
Environmental exposures candidate genes, 64–65
organophosphates (OP), 98 fragile X syndrome overlapping, 64
paraoxonase activity, 99 Glial cells, probable centrality
reelin, 99–100 activation, 358
RELN, 100–101 microglia, 358–359
Enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA) in vivo imaging, 359
analysis, 51 Glutathione (GSH)
EPA, see Eicosapentaenoic acid astrocytes, 119
Epilepsy cellular, reduction, 125–126
lesions, 17–18 cysteine residue, 115
pathology, 18 and GSSG ratios, 120
Etiology HCY, 117
autistic regression, 135 human neuronal cells, 124
gastrointestinal (GI) distress, plasma concentration, 38
133–134 synthesis, 115–116
MMR vaccination, 134–135 and transsulfuration, 121
422 Index
KWWSERRNVPHGLFRVRUJ
1 N
D 6
H1
D
C H2
C
2 B
B
A C H3
A
N
4
(a) (b)
FIGURE 6.2 Overall structure of PON1. (a) View of the six-bladed-propeller from above.
Shown are the N- and C-termini, and the two calcium atoms in the central tunnel of the
propeller (the “catalytic calcium” or Ca1, green (left); the “structural calcium” or Ca2, red
(right)). (b) A side view of the propeller. At the top of the propeller, there are three helices
H1–H3 which determine the PONs’ cell distribution, translocation and secretion (H1), and
protein–lipid and protein–protein interactions (H2 and H3). (Reproduced from Harel, M.
et al., Nat. Struct. Mol. Biol., 11, 412, 2004. With permission.)
FIGURE 6.3 The gene × environment model for autism. The model involves the Reelin
(RELN) and Paraoxonase 1 (PON1) genes, and prenatal exposure to organophosphates (OP).
RELN variants carrying either “normal” (7−10 repeats) or “long” (≥12 repeats) GGC alleles
genetically determine whether levels of reelin are normal or reduced, respectively. In prin-
ciple, both conditions are compatible with normal neurodevelopment. However, prenatal
exposure to OP can transiently inhibit the proteolytic activity of reelin, which might then fall
below the threshold required for correct neuronal migration, also depending on baseline levels
of RELN gene expression determined genetically and epigenetically. In addition, exposure
to identical doses of OP can affect reelin to a different extent depending on the amount and
affinity spectrum of the OP- inactivating enzyme paraoxonase produced by the PON1 alleles
of each individual. (Reproduced from Persico, A.M. and Bourgeron, T., Trends Neurosci., 29,
349, 2006. With permission.)
Neocortex,
Presymptomatic Symptomatic primary,
phase phase secondary
Neocortex,
high order
association
Mesocortex,
thalamus
Threshold Substantia
nigra,
amygdala
Gain setting
nuclei
Dorsal
motor ×
nucleus
1 2 3 4 5 6 Stages of the
PD-related
(A) path, process
(B)
FIGURE 9.3 The ascending pathology of Parkinson’s disease occurs in six recognizable
stages, beginning in the DMV of the medulla. Progressive shading in table (A) corresponds
to the like-shaded anatomic regions represented in diagram (B). (From Braak, H. et al., Cell.
Tissue Res., 31, 121, 2004. With permission.)
(A) (B)
FIGURE 9.4 Enlargement of Paneth’s cells, as indicated by dark staining of secretory gran-
ules, was a frequent finding in a series of duodenal biopsies from children with regressed
autism (A), as compared to non-autistic controls (B). (Photographs courtesy of Karoly
Horvath MD, PhD, Professor of Pediatrics, Thomas Jefferson University, Philadelphia, PA.)
Growth factor Reelin
Ca2+ Ca2+
Voltage-gated
Growth factor receptor calcium channels
VLDLR ApoER2
Cellular
PI stress
PI 4,5 - P2 PI3K PKB/Akt Dab-1
3, 4, 5 – P3
Ca2+
Pten mTOR
p53
GSK-3β
Bcl2
Tau
FIGURE 10.2 Participation of growth factor receptors, reelin, voltage-gated calcium chan-
nels, p53, and Bcl2 in neuronal migration, proliferation, and apoptosis. Growth factor binds
to its receptor and activated phosphatidylinositol 3-kinase (PI3K), which converts phosphati-
dylinositol 4,5-bisphosphate (PI 4, 5-P2) to phopsphatidylinositol 3, 4, 5-trisphosphate (PI 3, 4,
5-P3). PI 3, 4, 5-P3 is an activator of protein kinase B (also known as Akt) that triggers prolifera-
tion pathways by activating the mammalian target of rapamycin (mTOR). Akt also activates
phosphatase and tensin homolog on chromosome 10 (Pten), which dephosphorylates PI 3, 4,
5-P3 to PI 4, 5-P2. Pten activates glycogen synthase kinase-3β (GSK-3β), which phosphorylates
tau protein involved in neuronal migration. Reelin binds to very low-density lipoprotein recep-
tor (VLDLR) and apolipoprotein E receptor 2 (ApoER2), which activate Disabled-1 protein
(Dab-1). Dab-1 activates the Akt involved in neuronal migration and proliferation. Cellular
stress, on the other hand, affects the activities of p53 and Bcl2, proteins involved in apoptosis.
Presynaptic vesicle
Neurexin
2+ 2+
Ca Ca
channel channel
Neuroligin
SHANK3/ProSAP
PSD-95 PSD-95
Foderin Cortactin
Actin filament
Postsynaptic vesicle
Serotonin reuptake
transporter Synaptic gap
Serotonin receptors
FIGURE 10.4 Serotonin and its receptors, and serotonin reuptake transporter. Serotonin
molecules from the presynaptic vesicles are released into the synaptic gap, where they are
internalized by serotonin receptors into the postsynaptic vesicles. Serotonin molecules are
also retaken from synaptic gaps to the presynaptic vesicle by the serotonin reuptake transport-
ers. An imbalance of the uptake and reuptake system in serotonin causes brain dysfunctions.
Proinflammatory
cytokines (e.g., TNF-α
Molecular mimicry
and IFN-γ)
leading to cross-reactive
Ab΄s and/or T cells Regulatory cytokines
(e.g., IL-10)
FIGURE 14.1 Possible mechanism by which GI dysfunction may trigger autistic behaviors
in children with autism and GI disturbance.