The Neural Correlates of Trauma Related Autobiographical Memory in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: A Meta Analysis
The Neural Correlates of Trauma Related Autobiographical Memory in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: A Meta Analysis
The Neural Correlates of Trauma Related Autobiographical Memory in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: A Meta Analysis
DOI: 10.1002/da.22977
REVIEW
1
Department of Psychiatry, Western
University, London, Ontario, Canada Abstract
2
Department of Theoretical Neuroscience, Background: Autobiographical memory (AM) refers to memories of events that are
Central Institute of Mental Health Mannheim,
personally relevant and are remembered from one’s own past. The AM network is a
Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg
University, Heidelberg, Germany distributed brain network comprised largely by prefrontal medial and posteromedial
3
Department of Neuroscience, Western cortical brain regions, which together facilitate AM. Autobiographical memories with
University, London, Ontario, Canada
4
high arousal and negatively valenced emotional states are thought to be retrieved
Mood Disorders Program, St. Joseph’s
Healthcare, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada more readily and re‐experienced more vividly. This is critical in the case of trauma‐
5
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural related AMs, which are related to altered phenomenological experiences as well as
Neurosciences, McMaster University,
Hamilton, Ontario, Canada
aberrations to the underlying neural systems in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD).
6
Homewood Research Institute, Guelph, Critically, these alterations to the AM network have not been explored recently and
Ontario, Canada have never been analyzed with consideration to the different processes of AM, them
Correspondence being retrieval and re‐experiencing.
Ruth A. Lanius, Department of Psychiatry, Methods: We conducted a series of effect‐size signed differential mapping meta‐
Western University, University Hospital
(Room C3‐103), 339 Windermere Rd., London, analyses across twenty‐eight studies investigating the neural correlates of trauma‐
ON., Canada N6A 5A5. related AMs in participants with PTSD as compared with controls. Studies included
Email: [email protected]
either trauma‐related scripts or trauma‐related materials (i.e., sounds, images,
Funding information pictures) implemented to evoke the recollection of a trauma‐related memory.
Canadian Institutes of Health Research,
Grant/Award Numbers: 137150, 97914; Results: The meta‐analyses revealed that control and PTSD participants displayed
Mitacs; CIMVHR greater common brain activation of prefrontal medial and posteromedial cortices,
respectively. Whereby the prefrontal medial cortices are suggested to facilitate
retrieval monitoring, the posteromedial cortices are thought to enable the visual
imagery processes of AM.
Conclusions: Taken together, reduced common activation of prefrontal cortices may
be interpreted as a bias toward greater re‐experiencing, where the more salient
elements of the traumatic memory are relived as opposed to retrieved in a controlled
manner in PTSD.
KEYWORDS
autobiographical memory, autobiographical memory network, functional magnetic resonance
imaging, meta‐analysis, posttraumatic stress disorder, trauma
1 | INTRODUCTION
Staniloiu, 2011). The information is encoded within multiple memory project the perceived self into past experiences to examine and to
systems and is retrieved when the information holds value to the hypothesize our own actions, intentions, and emotions at the time of
present moment (Corkin, 1968; Ferbinteanu, 2018; Penfield & recollection (Markowitsch & Staniloiu, 2011; Wheeler, Stuss, &
Milner, 1958; Scoville & Milner, 1957; Squire, 2004). The memory Tulving, 1997).
literature differentiates between two classes of memory on the basis To engage autonoetic consciousness, AM relies on a distributed
of their conscious awareness during retrieval. Whereby explicit brain network, which includes the hippocampus and parahippocam-
memory is consciously retrieved, implicit memory does not require pal cortices, medial and lateral prefrontal cortices, posterior parietal
conscious awareness during retrieval (Schacter & Tulving, 1994; and posteromedial cortices, and visual processing regions (Buckner &
Squire & Dede, 2015; Tulving, 1987, 2007). Explicit memories Carroll, 2007; Cabeza & St Jacques, 2007; Svoboda, McKinnon, &
represent personal events that are associated with specific spatial Levine, 2006). Notably, the AM network is hypothesized to be
and temporal characteristics (i.e., episodic memory) as well as our predominantly left‐lateralized (Svoboda et al., 2006), and overlaps
general knowledge of facts, concepts, and ideas that are learned over partially with the default‐mode network (DMN). The DMN is an
the lifetime (i.e., semantic memory). By contrast, implicit memories intrinsic connectivity network composed of midline prefrontal and
represent learned reactions to the external world (i.e., learned posterior parietal cortices, recruited during AM, self‐referential
contingency of events, procedural patterns, priming) and do not have processing, and internal cognition (Menon, 2011). The DMN, and
generally an available mental representation that can be verbalized related AM network, are comprised largely by two cortical
consciously (Kandel, Dudai, & Mayford, 2014; Kandel, Schwartz, subsystems that contribute to the expression of an AM. Whereas
Jessell, Siegelbaum, & Hudspeth, 2012; Squire & Dede, 2015; Tulving, the dorsal‐medial subsystem (i.e., anterior, lateral temporal, orbito-
1987). Implicit memory systems are engaged rapidly and are thought frontal, prefrontal cortices) is thought to underlie the processing and
to rely on more evolutionarily conserved neural structures to elicit the maintenance of abstract representations of cognitive and of
their learning and memory effects (Squire, 2004), whereas explicit schematic information, the medial‐temporal subsystem (i.e., para-
memory systems are expressed more in large‐scale, phylogenetically hippocampal gyrus, restrosplenial, and posterior parietal cortices)
recent cortical networks (Kim, 2012; Rovee‐Collier, Hayne, & contributes more to perceptual and visual imagery features of
Colombo, 2001; Spreng & Grady, 2010). The harmonious interaction self‐related information as expressed in AMs (Cabeza & St Jacques,
of explicit and of implicit memory systems is thought to facilitate the 2007; St Jacques, Kragel, & Rubin, 2011; Svoboda et al., 2006).
development of our emotional, cognitive, and self‐related processing These dissociable cortical subsystems are also thought to
systems, which critically seem to be affected by traumatic events contribute disproportionately to retrieval and re‐experiencing
(Brewin, 2011, 2014; Ehlers & Clark, 2000; Schacter & Tulving, 1994; processes during AM (Araujo, Kaplan, & Damasio, 2013; Buckner &
Squire, 1987; Squire & Dede, 2015; Tulving, 2002; Van der Kolk & Carroll, 2007; Cabeza & St Jacques, 2007; Svoboda et al., 2006).
Fisler, 1995). Here, the AM literature distinguishes between retrieval phases
(i.e., the active and the controlled process of constructing an AM) and
re‐experiencing phases of AM (i.e., the elaboration of the perceptual,
1.1 | Autobiographical memory (AM)
sensory, and emotional elements of a memory by which reliving
AM is a form of explicit memory involving events that are personally sensations are experienced; Addis, Wong, & Schacter, 2007; Botzung,
relevant and are remembered from one’s own past (Brewer, 1986; Denkova, Ciuciu, Scheiber, & Manning, 2008; Cabeza & St Jacques,
Squire & Dede, 2015). AM incorporates both episodic and semantic 2007; St Jacques, Botzung, Miles, & Rubin, 2011). The former and the
components of explicit memory. Specifically, episodic components of latter phase are hypothesized to be expressed predominantly within
AM involve recollections of experiences specific in time and in place, the dorsal‐medial and the medial‐temporal subsystem, respectively
with accompanying mental and emotional features, including a (Buckner & Carroll, 2007; Cabeza & St Jacques, 2007; Svoboda et al.,
subjective sense of personal continuity across time (Levine, Svoboda, 2006). Re‐experiencing follows generally the retrieval of an AM;
Hay, Winocur, & Moscovitch, 2002; Tulving, 1987). By contrast, however, memories of very high arousal can often engage re‐
semantic components involve information about the world and experiencing processes rapidly—seemingly without the controlled
oneself that are independent of time and of place, such as self‐ retrieval (Ehlers, Hackmann, & Michael, 2004). Although the
relevant facts, traits, and general knowledge (Willoughby, Desrocher, distinction between these phases is important, there are often
Levine, & Rovet, 2012). The integration of these explicit memory inconsistencies during data generation and reporting of the neural
components is thought to allow for AMs to be expressed in narrative correlates associated with AM, which make it difficult to tease apart
form, where self‐relevant information and events associate to these processes (Botzung et al., 2008; Piolino, Desgranges, &
produce a sense of self (Conway & Pleydell‐Pearce, 2000; Levine Eustache, 2009). Oftentimes, the delineation between experimental
et al., 2002). This emergent sense of self is synonymous with, or phases is made by way of an arbitrary cut‐off time that denotes when
related to, the function of autonoetic consciousness (Piolino et al., a participant should have retrieved the memory and is now re‐
2006; Prebble, Addis, & Tippett, 2013; Tulving, 1985)—emerging only experiencing its more salient elements, whereas other studies are
when a child has developed a sense of self (Fivush, 2011; Reese, self‐paced (i.e., ask participants to press a button to signify they are
2002). Here, autonoetic consciousness refers to the human ability to now re‐experiencing the AM). The improved standardization of these
THOME ET AL. | 3
AM paradigms can facilitate a stronger understanding of the 1.2 | Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and the
processes related to AMs in healthy and in disordered populations AM network
(Addis et al., 2007; Botzung et al., 2008; Piolino et al., 2009; St
PTSD is a disorder characterized by hypervigilance, hyperarousal,
Jacques et al., 2011).
and, in some cases, dissociative symptoms following a traumatic
The ease of retrieval of an AM is thought to be mediated by the
experience (APA, 2013). A hallmark feature of the disorder is the
(emotional) significance of the memory (Talarico, LaBar, & Rubin,
intrusive re‐experiencing of the trauma memory, often following
2004; Williams et al., 2007), with arousal, valence, and consequenti-
exposure to trauma‐related stimuli (APA, 2013). Re‐experiencing
ality serving as three factors that contribute to its (emotional)
symptoms are thought to be evoked through the reestablishment of
significance (Finkenauer et al., 1998). Whereas arousal is thought to
the physiological, psychological, and emotional state of the individual
reflect the degree of physiological response generated during
that occurred during the original encoding (Brewin, 2018).
retrieval, valence considers the positivity or the negativity of the
Experimentally, AMs of trauma are studied through the exposure
event (Conway, 2013; Conway et al., 1994; Kensinger & Corkin,
of often personalized and trauma‐related stimuli to participants with
2004). The arousal and valence dimensions are often considered
PTSD (Bremner, Narayan, et al., 1999; Britton, Phan, Taylor, Fig, &
perpendicular, where different combinations of these two factors
Liberzon, 2005; Dahlgren et al., 2018; Lanius et al., 2002; Liberzon
associate to produce an array of different emotions (Bliss‐Moreau,
et al., 1999; Rauch et al., 1996; Shin et al., 1997). The stimuli used
Williams, & Santistevan, 2019; Lang, Greenwald, Bradley, & Hamm,
vary, but are generally in the form of trauma‐related images (Cwik,
1993; Russell & Mehrabian, 1977; see also Kensinger & Corkin, 2004;
Sartory, Nuyken, Schurholt, & Seitz, 2017; Morey et al., 2009; Zhang
Russell, 1980). These factors are thought to contribute to memory
et al., 2011), trauma‐related sounds (Liberzon et al., 1999; Pissiota
enhancement via dissociable neural systems, with arousal and
et al., 2002), or script‐driven imagery, which involves presenting a
valence dependent primarily on amygdalar‐hippocampal systems
short, verbal description of an individual’s trauma story to facilitate
and amygdalar‐prefrontal systems, respectively (Dolcos, LaBar, &
the retrieval of the fuller memory (Hopper, Frewen, van der Kolk, &
Cabeza, 2004; Kensinger & Corkin, 2004). On the other hand,
Lanius, 2007; Ke et al., 2015; Lanius et al., 2002; Rauch et al., 1996).
consequentiality is a factor concerned with the self‐relevance and the
During the presentation of these stimuli, it is crucial to consider
personal significance of the memory (Conway et al., 1994; McGaugh,
whether brain function is measured during or following the exposure
2004), where AMs with greater personal significance are retrieved
to trauma‐related stimuli. Whereas the former is thought to relate
more quickly and with greater detail (Brown & Kulik, 1977; Kelley
more to retrieval processes, the latter is associated more strongly
et al., 2002; Pezdek, 2003; Rogers, 1977). Generally, AMs with very
with the re‐experiencing processes of AM (see, e.g., St Jacques et al.,
high arousal and either a strong negative or a positive valence are
2011). Notably, trauma‐related AMs are thought to display greater
associated with increased reports of vividness during retrieval as well
altered retrieval and re‐experiencing processes as compared with
(Kelley et al., 2002; Pezdek, 2003; Quevedo et al., 2003). Historically,
nontraumatic, neutral memory recollection, which is reflected in the
highly arousing AMs of this nature are often referred to as “flashbulb
abnormal recruitment of the underlying memory systems in PTSD
memories,” on account of their vivid and, in some cases, intrusive
(McKinnon et al., 2015). These differences are to be discussed, both
retrieval (Brown & Kulik, 1977; Conway, 2013; Conway et al., 1994).
phenomenologically and in terms of the underlying neural systems, to
Despite increased reports of vividness, highly arousing and valenced
follow.
memories do not aid indiscriminately in the retrieval of an AM
With respect to altered AM characteristics, PTSD is often related
(Finn & Roediger, 2011; Sharot & Phelps, 2004). For instance,
to greater re‐experiencing of trauma‐related AMs as compared with
amygdala activation, a reliable indicator of the highly arousing and
trauma‐exposed or healthy control subjects (Brewin, 2015, 2018;
the negatively valenced fear state, is shown to boost the retrieval of
Ehlers & Clark, 2000; Ehlers et al., 2004; van der Kolk & Fisler, 1995).
certain memory characteristics. In particular, increased amygdala
As mentioned, re‐experiencing is identified by strong sensory and
activity is associated with enhanced reality‐monitoring accuracy as
emotional features, which appear to lack the perspective and the
well as reduced memory distortion of emotional as compared with
surrounding context of the trauma memory (Ehlers & Clark, 2000).
neutral stimuli (Kensinger & Schacter, 2005, 2006). Although
Here, Elbert and Schauer (2002) make a distinction between the hot
amygdala activity is thought to enhance the retrieval of emotional
(i.e., sensory, emotional) and the cold (i.e., cognitive, contextual)
stimuli, this enhancement couples strongly with a reduction in the
features of an AM and suggest that trauma memories are the result
retrieval of stimuli presented before the emotional cue (Holland &
of a disconnect, or a fragmentation of these components (Berntsen,
Kensinger, 2010; Strange, Hurlemann, & Dolan, 2003). This observa-
Willert, & Rubin, 2003; Brewin, 2015). In turn, some trauma‐related
tion is in keeping with the claim that negatively valenced arousal
AMs in PTSD are thought to remain in a state‐dependent,
increases the attentional processing of emotional, item‐specific
emotionally charged form that exhibits strong priming to trauma‐
stimuli, while also decreasing the processing of neutral, contextual‐
related cues (Arntz, de Groot, & Kindt, 2005; Grey & Holmes, 2008;
based stimuli presented concurrently (Phelps, Ling, & Carrasco, 2006;
Kleim, Ehring, & Ehlers, 2012; Michael, Ehlers, & Halligan, 2005).
Storbeck & Clore, 2008). The association between arousal, valence,
Increasing evidence assessing the characteristics of these AMs have
and memory is of critical interest, particularly in the case of a trauma‐
revealed that participants with PTSD report more vivid recollections
related memory.
4 | THOME ET AL.
of their traumatic memory in terms of more intense sensory and reports of functional activation gathered during the retrieval and
emotional modalities as compared with trauma‐exposed and healthy the re‐experiencing phases of trauma‐related AM in participants
control subjects (Berntsen et al., 2003; Crespo & Fernandez‐Lansac, with PTSD.
2016; O’Kearney & Perrott, 2006). Moreover, the likelihood to
engage in strong re‐experiencing during trauma‐related AM is found
2 | M A T E R I A L S AN D M E T H O D S
to correlate positively with PTSD symptom severity (Berntsen et al.,
2003). Interestingly, participants with the highest PTSD symptom
2.1 | Study selection
severity also report that their trauma memory had become a part of
their identity (Berntsen & Rubin, 2007; Berntsen et al., 2003). These The present meta‐analysis examines the neural correlates of trauma‐
results have since been replicated in a sample of veterans with PTSD related AM, where studies are considered that investigate the
as well as a sample of adult survivors of childhood sexual abuse who retrieval as well as the re‐experiencing of trauma‐related AMs in
would go on to develop PTSD (Brown, Antonius, Kramer, Root, & participants with PTSD as well as in control subjects. Of note, we
Hirst, 2010; Robinaugh & McNally, 2011). Taken together, these conducted separate meta‐analyses, including a meta‐analysis across
findings support the claim of the centrality of trauma to personal all identified studies investigating trauma‐related AM, as well as
identity in participants with PTSD, which is likely to be mediated by analyses that investigate neural activation during the retrieval as well
the AM network. as the re‐experiencing of trauma‐related AMs, independently.
These altered phenomenological characteristics of trauma‐ However, this distinction has to be interpreted with caution, since
related AMs are reflected, in part, by the underlying neural the categorization is based mainly on the experimental time course
systems recruited during retrieval in PTSD. To this point, (i.e., neural response during as compared with after exposure of the
Sartory et al. (2013) conducted a meta‐analysis where they trauma‐related AM material, which has been related to the retrieval
compiled reports of brain activation within trauma‐related phase (i.e., construction) and the re‐experiencing phase (i.e., elabora-
symptom provocation paradigms across nineteen studies that tion), respectively), without a qualitative assessment of the indivi-
included participants with PTSD as well as trauma‐exposed dual’s subjective experience per se.
controls. For studies included in the meta‐analysis, however, Primary research articles were identified until January of 2019
Sartory et al. did not differentiate between those that scanned (first publication appeared in 1996). Search databases included
during (i.e., retrieval) or following (i.e., re‐experiencing) trauma‐ PubMed, Web of Science, PsycINFO, and Medline. A combination of
related stimulus exposure. In discussing the results, Sartory et al. three keywords were used to refine the results generated. The first
highlight that participants with PTSD showed significantly more of the keywords included one of the following: “traumatic memory
common brain activation in the precuneus, restrosplenial cortex, reexperiencing,” “traumatic memory retrieval,” “traumatic memory
caudate nucleus, and anterior cingulate as compared with controls. reliving,” “autobiographical memory recall,” “autobiographical reex-
By contrast, trauma‐exposed controls displayed more consistent periencing,” “autobiographical memory,” “autobiographical memory
activation in the superior temporal, middle occipital, and post- testing,” “script‐driven imagery,” “symptom provocation,” or “mental
central gyri as compared with PTSD. Notably, participants with imagery.” These keywords were then combined with either “post-
PTSD showed more common activation of regions often implicated traumatic stress disorder” or “PTSD.” The last keyword included
in re‐experiencing symptoms (i.e., precuneus, restrosplenial cor- either “functional magnetic resonance imaging” or “fMRI” (for the
tex), which is corroborative with the altered trauma‐related AM combination breakdown, see Supporting Information Materials S1).
features. Taken together, these findings support the notion that The literature search and the research abstract screening was
PTSD is associated with abnormal functioning of the AM network conducted by author J. T. Full‐text articles were assessed for
during trauma‐related AM retrieval. eligibility by authors B. T. and J. T., independently. Decision for the
The research reviewed here points strongly to the role of AM in inclusion or the exclusion of the identified studies was based on
the development of a sense of self that emerges in response to the further screening and discussion by authors R. A. L., B. T., and J. T.
dynamic interplay of episodic and of semantic memory components. Studies were included if they met the following inclusion criteria: (a)
The present study builds on the earlier findings of Sartory et al. the PTSD patient group met the diagnostic criteria as defined by the
(2013) by conducting an updated meta‐analysis examining the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) (DSM‐
differences in brain activation during trauma‐related AM in a larger III or later); (b) participants were exposed to a paradigm that was
sample of participants with PTSD and control subjects. Moreover, associated with a trauma‐related memory (i.e., explicit exposure to a
our meta‐analysis is novel in that we conduct symptom correlations traumatic script or a trauma‐related stimulus); (c) the experimental
with reports of brain activation across the included studies. In contrast included trauma‐related AM recollection or provocation of
addition, we employ two meta‐analytic methods (i.e., activation trauma‐related AMs as compared with another condition (i.e.,
likelihood estimation, effect‐size signed differential mapping) to neutral/negative script, neutral/negative words, neutral/negative
assess convergence across the meta‐analytic approaches and per- sounds/images), or to a baseline condition; (d) the contrasts were
form sensitivity estimations to determine the robustness of the reported as either within‐group or between‐group comparisons. The
findings. Finally, in the present meta‐analysis, we distinguish between between‐group comparisons had to include either a healthy,
THOME ET AL. | 5
F I G U R E 1 Flowchart of fMRI study inclusion and exclusion process for the meta‐analysis of trauma‐related AM retrieval and/or
re‐experiencing in posttraumatic stress disorder. Abbreviations: AM, autobiographical memory; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging
nontrauma‐exposed or a healthy, trauma‐exposed control sample. emphasize the aspect that their study design operationalizes
Additionally, studies were included if within‐group symptom correla- re‐experiencing (e.g., Bremner, Narayan, et al., 1999; Dahlgren
tions within the PTSD sample were reported; and (e) whole‐brain or et al., 2018; Lanius et al., 2001; Shin et al., 2004) (see also,
region‐of‐interest (ROI) results were reported with associated St Jacques et al., 2011). Hence, studies examining the response
stereotactic coordinates. In case of publications reporting analyses during AM stimulus presentation are thought to capture mainly the
on a similar or an overlapping sample, only the most recent retrieval of a given AM (Botzung et al., 2008; St Jacques et al., 2011).
publication was included (exclusion: Cisler et al., 2014; Hou et al., However, the distinction is not always confirmed by further analysis
2007; Ke et al., 2015). Included publications were then cross‐checked (i.e., subjective experiences).
for additional references that may have been missed. Moreover, we Nineteen of these studies reported within‐group findings in their
compared the identified studies to a previous meta‐analysis on PTSD sample (retrieval: N = 11; re‐experiencing: N = 8), and eight
trauma‐related AM in PTSD as conducted by Sartory et al. (2013). studies reported within‐group findings in their control sample
Studies that were reported by Sartory et al. but not identified based (retrieval: N = 5; re‐experiencing: N = 3). Twelve studies contributed
on our search criteria were included to the meta‐analyses (i.e., between‐group analyses of PTSD as compared with a control group
Bremner, Narayan, et al., 1999; Bremner, Staib, et al., 1999; Driessen (PTSD > Controls: retrieval: N = 9; re‐experiencing: N = 3; controls >
et al., 2004; Flatten et al., 2004; Liberzon et al., 1999; Morey et al., PTSD: retrieval: N = 6; re‐experiencing: N = 6). Notably, four of the
2009; Pissiota et al., 2002; Rauch et al., 1996; Shin et al., 1997, 1999). between‐group comparisons included healthy, nontrauma‐exposed
In total, twenty‐eight studies met the inclusion criteria for the controls as opposed to trauma‐exposed control subjects. The
meta‐analysis (see Tables 1A and Figure 1). The assignment of between‐group meta‐analyses were thus re‐run after removal of
studies to the category of “retrieval” or “reexperiencing” was based the four studies reporting the between‐group comparisons with
on whether the study focused on the neural response during or after healthy, nontrauma‐exposed controls and are included in the
trauma‐related stimulus exposure. Here, the rational is rooted in the Supporting Information (Tables S2 and S4; PTSD > trauma‐exposed
fact that studies focusing on the response post‐AM cue exposure did controls: retrieval: N = 5; trauma‐exposed controls > PTSD: retrieval:
6
|
PTSD Controls
Studies Year N N Type Paradigm Stimulus duration Stimulus order Time course of interest Contrast of interest
Bremner et al. 1999 10 12 TEC Script‐driven imagery 60 s Fixed (neutral first) During and 20 s after stimulus Trauma script > neutral
presentation
Bremner et al. 1999 10 10 TEC Trauma‐related/neutral Four pictures in 120 s Fixed (neutral first) During stimulus presentation Trauma > neutral
pictures
Britton et al. 2005 16 15 TEC Script‐driven imagery 40 s Randomized After stim/25 s overlap Trauma script > neutral
Cwik et al. 2017 19 None TEC Trauma‐related/neutral 3–5 s n.a. During stimulus presentation Trauma > neutral pictures
pictures
Dahlgren et al. 2018 12 14 TEC Script‐driven imagery 50 s n.a. After stimpres/duration 30 s Trauma script > neutral
Driessen et al. 2004 6 6 TEC Script‐driven imagery n.a. Randomized After stimpres/duration 30 s Trauma script > neutral
Hopper et al. 2007 27 None None Script‐driven imagery 30 s n.a. After stimpres/duration 30 s Trauma script > neutral
Ke et al. 2016 12 14 TEC Trauma‐related/baseline 6s Fixed (neutral first) During stimulus presentation Trauma script > baseline
pictures
Landre et al. 2012 17 17 HC Trauma‐related/neutral 2s Randomized During stimulus presentation Trauma > neutral words
words
Lanius et al. 2001 9 9 TEC Script‐driven imagery 30 s Fixed After stimpres/duration 30 s Trauma script > baseline
Lanius et al. 2002 7 10 TEC Script‐driven imagery 30 s Fixed After stimpres/duration 30 s Trauma script > baseline
Lanius et al. 2003 10 10 TEC Script‐driven imagery 30 s Fixed (neutral first) After stimpres/duration 30 s Trauma script > baseline
Lanius et al. 2007 14 16 TEC Script‐driven imagery 30 s Fixed After stimpres/duration 30 s Trauma script > baseline
Liberzon et al. 1999 14 11 TEC Trauma‐related/neutral 180 s Fixed During stimulus presentation Trauma > neutral sounds
sounds
Ludäscher et al. 2010 15 None none Script‐driven imagery 25–32 s Fixed (neutral first) During stimulus presentation Trauma script > neutral
Mickleborough et al. 2011 17 26 TEC Pain during script‐driven 30 s Fixed (neutral first) After stimpres Trauma script > neutral
imagery
Morey et al. 2008 39 None None Trauma‐related/neutral 2s Fixed (neutral first) During stimulus presentation Trauma > neutral pictures
pictures
Piefke et al. 2008 6 None None Script‐driven imagery 90 s Fixed (stress,trauma,neutral) During stimulus presentation Trauma script > baseline
Pissiota et al. 2002 7 None None Trauma‐related/neutral n.a. Fixed (neutral first) During stimulus presentation Trauma > neutral sounds
sounds
Protopopescu et al. 2005 9 14 HC Trauma‐related/panic‐ 2s Randomized During stimulus presentation Trauma > neutral words
related/
Positive/neutral words
Rabellino et al. 2016 26 20 HC Subliminal and 16 ms Fixed During stimulus presentation Trauma > neutral words
supraliminal
Trauma‐related/neutral
words +
Fearful/neutral faces
THOME
Rauch et al. 1996 8 None None Script‐driven imagery n.a. Randomized After stimpres/duration 90 s Trauma script > neutral
ET AL.
(Continues)
THOME ET AL. | 7
Abbreviations: fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; HC, nontrauma‐exposed, healthy controls; PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder; stimpres, stimulus presentation; TEC, trauma‐exposed, healthy
Trauma > negative scenes
nontrauma‐exposed control group). Seven studies contributed
duration self‐paced
60 s
Randomized
Randomized
n.a.
n.a.
n.a.
6s
Trauma‐related/negative
Trauma‐related/flower
Script‐driven imagery
Script‐driven imagery
Script‐driven imagery
pictures
pictures
pictures
none
TEC
TEC
TEC
TEC
HC
each category namely: (a) across all identified studies (i.e., retrieval
Controls
19
24
26
N
16
17
10
38
N
presenting more than one contrast that matched inclusion criteria: (a)
2005
1997
1999
2004
2013
2011
Year
within PTSD, (b) within controls, (c) PTSD > control, (d) control >
PTSD, and (e) within PTSD symptom correlations. For a meta‐analysis
(Continued)
Whalley et al.
more than one contrast that matched our inclusion criteria (i.e.,
TABLE 1A
Zhang et al.
Shin et al.
Shin et al.
Shin et al.
trauma vs. neutral and trauma vs. baseline condition), we selected the
Studies
controls.
Contrast reported
Within Between
Studies Analysis Type p PTSD Controls Symptoms PTSD > Controls >
Bremner WB uncorr .001 1 1 0 1 1
Bremner WB uncorr .001 1 1 0 1 1
Britton WB uncorr .005 1 1 0 0 0
Cwik WB uncorr .005 0 0 1† 0 0
Dahlgren WB corr .001 0 0 0 0 1
Driessen WB corr .05 1 0 0 0 0
a
Hopper ROI corr .05 1 0 1 0 0
†
Ke WB corr .05 0 0 1 1 1
Landre WB corr .005 1 1 0 1 0
Lanius WB uncorr .001 0 0 0 0 1
Lanius WB corr .05 0 0 0 1 1
Lanius WB corr .05 0 0 0 0 1
Lanius SVC corr .05 1 1 0 1 1
Liberzon WB corr .05 1 1 0 0 0
Ludäscher WB uncorr .001 1 0 0 0 0
†,a
Mickleborough ROI corr .001 1 0 1 1 0
†
Morey WB corr .05 1 0 1 0 0
Piefke ROI corr .01 1 0 0 0 0
Pissiota WB corr .01 1 0 0 0 0
Protopopescu SVC uncorr .001 0 0 1† 1 0
Rabellino ROI corr .001 1 0 1† 1 0
Rauch WB uncorr .001 1 0 0 0 0
a
Sakamoto WB uncorr .001 1 1 1 1 1
Shin WB uncorr .001 1 1 0 0 0
Shin WB uncorr .001 1 0 0 0 0
Shin WB uncorr .001 1^ 0 1† 1^ 1
Whalley WB uncorr .001 1 0 0 1 1
Zhang WB uncorr .001 0 0 0 1 1
Abbreviations: 1, yes; 0, no; corr, α‐level adjusted for multiple comparisons (FWE or FDR); fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging;
PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder; ROI, region‐of‐interest analysis; SVC, small volume correction; uncorr, α‐level not adjusted for multiple comparisons;
WB, whole‐brain analysis.
†
Symptom correlations.
^
Contrast reported could not be included in present analyses, as analyses has been conducted across PTSD and control subjects.
a
Descriptive presentation can be found in Tables S5–S7 and Figures S1 and S2.
condition. This decision was based on the fact that a neutral paper performed five separate meta‐analyses for each of: (a) common
condition (e.g., neutral picture, word, or sound) was more comparable neural activation for trauma‐related AM in general, (b) studies
to the trauma condition in terms of the stimulus modality (e.g., visual focusing on retrieval of trauma‐related AM, and (c) studies focusing
or auditory stimulus complexity), hence the stimuli differed only in on re‐experiencing of trauma‐related AM.
regard to the content or affective nature of the material (i.e., trauma‐ The meta‐analyses included: (a) a within‐group analysis of
related vs. neutral). individuals with PTSD, (b) a within‐group analysis of control subjects
with and without trauma exposure, (c) a between‐group analysis of
brain regions displaying greater common activation in PTSD as
2.3 | Statistical analyses
compared with control subjects (i.e., PTSD > Controls), (d) a between‐
To investigate common neural activation during trauma‐related AM group analysis of brain regions displaying greater common
in participants with PTSD as well as control subjects, the present activation in controls as compared with participants with PTSD
THOME ET AL. | 9
(i.e., Control > PTSD), and (e) a within‐group positive correlation of event that an effect size was not reported in the primary study, the
PTSD symptom severity scores (i.e., CAPS, DTS, PTSS) and common direction of the finding (i.e., activation or deactivation) was used to
brain activation as reported in the PTSD patient sample across the calculate the minimum effect size. Coordinates reported in
included studies. Talairach space were converted to Montreal Neurological Institute
As mentioned, four studies reported analyses on negative (MNI) space via Lancaster transformation (incorporated within the
correlations of PTSD symptom severity, while two studies performed ES‐SDM software). Preprocessing of the incorporated peaks (i.e.,
correlations with state dissociation during trauma‐related AM. activation foci) involved smoothing as described before (anisotropic
However, due to the small number of these reported studies, we kernel of 20 mm full‐with at half maximum; Radua et al., 2014). The
did not perform a quantitative meta‐analysis on negative correlations created study maps were subsequently used to calculate random‐
of PTSD symptom severity or state dissociation and brain activation effects models, taking the sample size and intra‐ as well as
during trauma‐related AM. Nevertheless, we provide descriptive interstudy variance into account. Statistical significance was
information on these correlations within the Supporting Information determined using the randomization test (50 permutations). Results
(see Tables S5–S7 and Figures S1 and S2; negative correlation PTSD are reported that survived a standard thresholding of an SDM Z
symptom severity: Cwik et al., 2017; Ke et al., 2016; Mickleborough value ≥ 1, a voxel height threshold of 1, a peak probability of
et al., 2011; Rabellino, Densmore, Frewen, Theberge, & Lanius, 2016; p ≤ .005, with a cluster extent threshold of ≥20 (Radua et al., 2012;
other symptoms: Hopper et al., 2007; Sakamoto et al., 2005). see also Hart, Radua, Mataix‐Cols, & Rubia, 2012; Sartory et al.,
To test the replicability of the meta‐analysis conducted by 2013; Schulze, Schmahl, & Niedtfeld, 2016; Schulze, Schulze,
Sartory et al. (2013), we performed a coordinate‐based meta‐analysis Renneberg, Schmahl, & Niedtfeld, 2019). Robustness of the findings
with the effect‐size signed differential mapping software (ES‐SDM was analyzed by a jackknife analysis, i.e., data from one contrast (i.e.,
instead; version 5.142; http://www.sdmproject/com; Radua et al., study) is removed systematically from the analysis to determine if
2012, 2014). In addition, meta‐analyses were also repeated through the results remain significant. The process is repeated for all
application of the widely applied activation‐likelihood estimation possible combinations to estimate the robustness of the findings as
(ALE) technique (Eickhoff et al., 2009; Turkeltaub, Eden, Jones, & derived from the persistence of significant thresholds met, while
Zeffiro, 2002), as implemented using version 2.3.6 of the GingerALE also permitting authors to determine if certain results are driven
software (http://www.brainmap.org/ale). Both methods have in predominantly by a small set of studies.
common that they convolve a kernel with each included peak
coordinate (i.e., activation foci) (for a description see Radua et al.,
2.3.2 | Activation‐likelihood estimation
2012, 2014). Here, “kernel” means that an effect size (effect‐size
signed differential mapping [ES‐SDM]) or a likelihood (ALE) is The ALE method involves extracting the activation foci from
assigned to each voxel, roughly based on its Euclidean distance to individual studies, converted into Talairach space (spm MNI to
its closest peak (i.e., activation foci) by means of an (un)normalized TAL function), which are, in turn, smoothed as previously described,
Gaussian kernel, respectively (e.g., Radua et al., 2012, 2014). First taking the full‐width at half maximum threshold for each distribu-
versions of the ES‐SDM algorithms further multiplied the kernel with tion, while taking its sample size into account (i.e., likelihood map for
the effect size of a given voxel (in case of more than one assigned each peak). The ALE method considers the threshold of each
peak, these values were averaged weighting by the square of the distribution as well as the sample size of the study to produce a
distance to the closest peaks; Radua et al., 2012). The new ES‐SDM likelihood map. Likelihood maps are generated for each study and
method contrarily is based on the idea that voxels close to each other then combined to estimate the probability of union in the meta‐
are more likely to correlate higher, meaning that higher correlated analysis. Results are reported that survive a threshold of un-
voxels are assumed to lie closer together. Based heron, the distance corrected p ≤ .001
between voxels is “rearranged,” bringing voxels closer together that
are highly correlated, while uncorrelated voxels are moved further
away. The Gaussian kernel is then applied to this new deformed 3 | RESULTS
space and subsequently, voxels are restored in their original space
(Radua et al., 2014). In total, twenty‐eight study samples were included that investigated
414 participants with PTSD as well as 296 control subjects (trauma‐
exposed: N = 229; healthy, nontrauma‐exposed: N = 67). The follow-
2.3.1 | Effect‐size signed differential mapping
ing results pertain to common neural activation across these groups
In general, the ES‐SDM technique considers the activation peaks’ during trauma‐related AM recollection, as estimated by the ES‐SDM
effect sizes, while also weighting the studies by intrastudy variance technique. Findings were reported that survived a thresholding
and interstudy heterogeneity (Radua et al., 2012). For all criterion of an SDM Z value ≥ 1, a voxel height threshold of 1, a peak
coordinate‐based results, peaks, and their associated Z or T values probability of p ≤ .005, with a cluster extent threshold of ≥20.
were extracted from whole‐brain and ROI‐related reports by Findings for each particular analysis are listed in order of descending
authors B. T. and J. T., independently and cross‐checked. In the robustness, as defined by the jackknife analysis described above.
10 | THOME ET AL.
T A B L E 2 Brain regions recruited most commonly within studies on trauma‐related AM: (a) retrieval and re‐experiencing, (b) retrieval, and (c)
re‐experiencing in participants with PTSD and control subjects (ES‐SDM meta‐analysis)
Peak
TABLE 2 (Continued)
Peak
Findings of the ALE methods are reported in the Supporting (extending into the orbitofrontal and the perigenual anterior
Information (for within‐ and between‐group differences, see Tables cingulate) (BA 10, 11, 24, 25), the left midline nucleus of the
S1 and S2; positive PTSD symptom correlations, see Table S3; Figure 1). thalamus (extending into the medial and the lateral dorsal nuclei),
and the left angular gyrus (BA 39) (see Table 2a and Figure 3). The
3.1 | Trauma‐related AM recollection (i.e., retrieval results did not change after excluding studies comparing nontrauma‐
and re‐experiencing) exposed controls to participants with PTSD (see Table S4).
F I G U R E 2 Within‐group findings of common neural activation for: (a/b) control subjects and (c/d) participants with PTSD (ES‐SDM
meta‐analysis). (a/c) Meta‐analytic findings across trauma‐related AM retrieval and re‐experiencing. (b/d) Meta‐analytic findings on AM retrieval (lighter
color), and re‐experiencing (darker color). Results are thresholded at: SDM Z value ≥ 1, a voxel height threshold of 1, a peak probability of p ≤ .005, with a
cluster extent threshold of ≥20. Abbreviations: dACC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex; dm, dorsomedial; inf, inferior; mid, middle; PFC, prefrontal cortex;
pHG, parahippocampal gyrus; PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder; STG, superior temporal gyrus; vm, ventromedial
3.2 | Trauma‐related AM retrieval cluster centered on the right ventromedial prefrontal cortex
(extending into the dorsomedial prefrontal and the perigenual
3.2.1 | Controls: Within‐group trauma‐related AM
anterior cingulate cortex) (BA 9, 10, 24, 32). Common neural
retrieval
activation was also observed in the left lingual gyrus (BA 18,19)
In controls, a meta‐analysis on trauma‐related AM retrieval (N = 5) and the right posterior cerebellum lobule VI in control subjects (see
revealed common neural activation with the most robust finding in a Table 2b and Figure 2).
THOME ET AL. | 13
F I G U R E 3 Between‐group findings of common neural activation between participants with PTSD as compared with control subjects
(ES‐SDM meta‐analysis). (a) Meta‐analytic findings across all identified studies. (b) Meta‐analytic findings on trauma‐related AM retrieval.
(c) Meta‐analytic findings on trauma‐related AM re‐experiencing. Results are thresholded at SDM Z value ≥ 1, a voxel height threshold of
1, a peak probability of p ≤ .005, with a cluster extent threshold of ≥20. Abbreviations: dACC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex; MidNuc,
midline nucleus (thalamus); MiTG, middle temporal gyrus; (i)OFC, (inferior)orbitofrontal cortex; pACC, perigenual anterior cingulate
cortex; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex; pHG, parahippocampal gyrus; PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder; vmPFC, ventromedial
prefrontal cortex
3.2.2 | PTSD: Within‐group trauma‐related AM 3.2.3 | Control > PTSD: Between‐group comparison
retrieval of trauma‐related AM retrieval
In participants with PTSD, a meta‐analysis on trauma‐related AM In comparing neural activation during trauma‐related AM retrieval
retrieval (N = 11) revealed common neural activation with the between groups (N = 6), as compared with PTSD, controls exhibited
most robust findings associated with clusters centered on the significantly more common neural activation in the left middle
left ventromedial prefrontal cortex (extending into the perigen- temporal gyrus (BA 39) as well as the right medial orbitofrontal
ual anterior cingulate cortex) (BA 9, 10, 32), the left precentral cortex (extending into the perigenual anterior cingulate cortex) (BA
gyrus (BA 6), the left supramarginal gyrus (BA 49), and the left 11, 25) (see Table 2b and Figure 3). The results did not change after
caudate nucleus of the dorsal striatum (see Table 2b and excluding studies comparing nontrauma‐exposed control subjects to
Figure 2). participants with PTSD (see Table S4).
14 | THOME ET AL.
T A B L E 3 Common brain regions associated positively with PTSD symptom severity during trauma‐related AM across studies on: (a) retrieval
and re‐experiencing, and (b) retrieval in participants with PTSD (ES‐SDM meta‐analysis)
Peak Cluster
MNI coordinates
3.2.4 | PTSD > Control: Between‐Group comparison 3.2.5 | PTSD symptom severity and its relation to
of trauma‐related AM retrieval trauma‐related AM retrieval
In comparing neural activation during trauma‐related AM retrieval A meta‐analysis on PTSD symptom severity (i.e., CAPS, DTS, PSS
between groups (N = 9), as compared with controls, PTSD displayed scores; N = 7) revealed that PTSD symptom scores were correlated
more common activation in clusters centered on the left posterior positively with common neural activation during trauma‐related AM
cingulate (extending into the precuneus and the mid‐cingulate gyrus) retrieval in clusters centered on the left superior frontal gyrus
(BA 7, 23, 24, 30, 31, 32), the right parahippocampal gyrus (BA 28, 30, (extending into the medial orbitofrontal gyrus) (BA 10), the left insula
35), and the left precentral gyrus (BA 4, 6) (see Table 2b and Figure 3). (extending into the lenticular nucleus, the putamen, the hippocampus,
The results did not change after excluding studies comparing and the amygdala) (BA 48), the right middle frontal gyrus (BA 45), and
nontrauma‐exposed control subjects to participants with PTSD (see the right caudate nucleus (extending into the anterior nucleus of the
Table S4). thalamus) (see Table 3b and Figure 4).
F I G U R E 4 Positive association between PTSD symptom severity and brain activation during trauma‐related AM in PTSD (ES‐SDM meta‐analysis).
Figure displays meta‐analytic findings across all studies. Results did not change when conducting the meta‐analysis on retrieval studies solely. A meta‐
analysis on studies focusing on re‐experiencing solely was not conducted due to the small number of studies available. Results are thresholded at
SDM Z value ≥ 1, a voxel height threshold of 1, a peak probability of p ≤ .005, with a cluster extent threshold f ≥ 20. Abbreviations: AntNuc, anterior
nucleus (thalamus); lenNuc, lentiform nucleus; MiFG, middle frontal gyrus; pACC, perigenual anterior cingulate cortex; PCC, posterior cingulate
cortex; PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder; vmPFC, ventromedial prefrontal cortex
THOME ET AL. | 15
frontal gyrus (BA 47), and the right precuneus (BA 19). Moreover, in
controls as compared with PTSD, more common activation was observed
in the dorsal anterior cingulate, where this finding is less robust (see
Table 2c and Figure 3). Since only healthy, trauma‐exposed subjects have
been compared with PTSD across all the included studies, these analyses
were not repeated.
are in large part keeping with the results generated by Sartory et al. trauma‐related AM, where the memory is recollected more as a
(2013); however, we provide a more recent and in‐depth look into relived experience and less as a controlled retrieval (Brewin, 2015;
the neural differences expressed by the network during trauma‐ Ehlers, 2010; Ehlers & Clark, 2000). Interestingly, our meta‐analysis
related AM recollection. revealed a positive correlation between PTSD symptom severity and
brain activation of the medial prefrontal cortices. At first glance, this
may seem antithetical, as individuals with PTSD displaying greater
4.2 | AM network: Control > PTSD
activation of the medial prefrontal cortices also tend to have the
The meta‐analyses revealed several brain regions exhibiting more greatest symptoms. However, it is well‐established that individuals
consistent activation in control subjects as compared with with PTSD exhibiting the greatest symptom severity are more likely
individuals with PTSD during trauma‐related AM recollection, to meet the criteria for the dissociative subtype (Frewen, Zhu, &
where the term recollection is to denote analyses that collapse Lanius, 2019; Karam et al., 2014; Lanius et al., 2014; Stein et al.,
across retrieval and re‐experiencing processes of trauma‐related 2013), identified by the presence of supplementary dissociative
AM. In particular, the control group demonstrated significantly symptoms (i.e., depersonalization, derealization; APA, 2013).
more common activation of the right medial prefrontal cortices Lanius et al. (2007) have posited that the dissociative subtype
(i.e., ventromedial, dorsomedial), the right perigenual anterior displays an overmodulation of the medial prefrontal cortices directed
cingulate, the left thalamus (midline, medial, and lateral nuclei), toward the limbic system. This is in contrast to the undermodulation
and the left angular gyrus as compared with individuals with associated more strongly with the traditional symptom pattern of
PTSD during trauma‐related AM recollection. In the meta‐ PTSD (Fenster, Lebois, Ressler, & Suh, 2018; Lanius et al., 2010,
analyses specific to retrieval processes, the control group 2014, 2018). Taken together, the positive correlation between PTSD
displayed greater common activation of the middle temporal symptom severity and activation of the medial prefrontal cortices
gyrus and the medial orbitofrontal gyrus as compared with PTSD. may be driven, in part, by the presence of dissociative symptomatol-
For analyses specific to re‐experiencing, control subjects re- ogy in these participants as an attempt to detach from the
vealed greater common activation of the medial prefrontal overwhelming emotional experience associated with their trauma
cortices, the inferior frontal gyrus, the thalamus (midline, medial, memory. To this point, two studies included in the meta‐analysis
and lateral dorsal nuclei), the precuneus, and the dorsal anterior reported dissociative symptom severity scores and, of those, one
cingulate as compared with the PTSD group. study revealed a positive association between dissociative scores and
The medial prefrontal cortices are thought to facilitate the activation of the medial prefrontal cortices in participants with PTSD
constructive processes of AM, such as memory search and controlled (Hopper et al., 2007; see Table S5 and Figure S2A).
retrieval (Cabeza & St Jacques, 2007; St Jacques et al., 2011; St. In addition to the medial prefrontal cortices, the thalamus, centered
Jacques, 2012; Svoboda et al., 2006). Specifically, the ventro‐ and the on the midline nucleus and encompassing the medial and lateral dorsal
dorsomedial prefrontal cortices are proposed to monitor the process nuclei, also revealed greater activation in controls as compared with
of AM retrieval, in particular the self‐referential aspects (Amodio & PTSD. The thalamus is a subcortical structure that relays information
Frith, 2006; Gilboa, 2004). The prefrontal cortices that underlie this between different processing regions, both subcortical and cortical
monitoring are thought to differ regionally from cortices that (Posner & Petersen, 1990; Steriade & Llinás, 1988). Given its centralized
monitor the retrieval of less personal, experimental tests of memory location and ability to facilitate communication between disparate
(i.e., paired associations, item list tasks; Moscovitch & Winocur, cortices, the thalamus serves a role in high‐level functions of arousal,
2002). Experimental memory tests reveal greater activation in lateral attention, and consciousness (Posner, 1994; Schiff, 2008; Sturm et al.,
prefrontal cortices during retrieval, whereas AM elicits greater 1999; Timofeev & Steriade, 2004). These functions are engaged during
activation of medial prefrontal cortices during retrieval processes AM recollection and rely largely on the communication between the
(Gilboa, 2004; Moscovitch & Winocur, 2002). Here, Moscovitch and prefrontal and the parietal cortices, as modulated by the thalamus (Crone
Winocur (2002) suggest AM relies on a quick, intuitive, and et al., 2014; de Bourbon‐Teles et al., 2014; Scolari, Seidl‐Rathkopf, &
preconscious form of retrieval monitoring, coined as the “feeling‐of‐ Kastner, 2015). The impaired thalamic ability to transmit information
rightness.” In line with this proposition, medial prefrontal damage between these cortices is related to disorders of attentional and of
often spares the ability to retrieve AMs; however, these memories conscious processing (Carrera & Bogousslavsky, 2006; Crone et al., 2014;
are prone to monitoring impairments (Gilboa et al., 2006). Located in van Der Werf, Jolles, Witter, & Uylings, 2003). Notably, the cluster of
close proximity to the medial prefrontal cortices, the perigenual activation revealed by the meta‐analysis was specific to midline and
anterior cingulate has been linked to emotional conflict evaluation dorsal thalamic nuclei, which are associated most strongly with salient
and is suggested to serve a role in emotion regulation via appraisal‐ and emotional stimulus processing (LeDoux, Farb, & Romanski, 1991;
based, top‐down strategies (Meyer‐Lindenberg & Tost, 2012). Metzger et al., 2010; Seeley et al., 2007; for review see Metzger, van der
In turn, the results revealed by our meta‐analyses for controls Werf, & Walter, 2013). In PTSD, the thalamus is evidenced to display
may be interpreted as an intact monitoring process during trauma‐ reduced activation during script‐driven symptom provocation as well as
related AM. In PTSD, compromised memory search and retrieval during rest (Frewen, Pain, Dozois, & Lanius, 2006; Kim et al., 2007; Lanius
monitoring may suggest a tendency to be overwhelmed by the et al., 2001, 2003; Liberzon, Taylor, Fig, & Koeppe, 1996; Yan et al., 2013;
THOME ET AL. | 17
Zhu et al., 2017). Moreover, the thalamus is demonstrated to exhibit in a state‐dependent, emotionally charged form that exhibits strong
reduced resting‐state functional connectivity with the medial prefrontal perceptual priming to trauma‐related material (Arntz et al., 2005;
and the anterior cingulate cortices in PTSD (Lanius et al., 2005; Yin et al., Brewin, 2014; Michael et al., 2005; van der Kolk & Fisler, 1995). Hence,
2011). Other studies also report decreased thalamic functional con- trauma‐related AMs in PTSD are more likely to be re‐experienced
nectivity, specifically the pulvinar nuclei, with more posteromedial vividly, as opposed to retrieved in a controlled manner (Berntsen et al.,
cortices in PTSD as compared with controls (Terpou et al., 2018; Yin 2003; Brewin, 2014; Ehlers et al., 2004). Northoff and Bermpohl (2004)
et al., 2011). These aberrant characteristics may restrict, in part, the relay have proposed a system where anterior midline structures (i.e., medial
of information between medial prefrontal and posteromedial cortices prefrontal, anterior cingulate) of the AM network engage the
during AM processes in PTSD. In turn, impaired thalamic modulation may monitoring, evaluation, and representation of AMs, whereas posterior
produce a disconnect between the medial prefrontal (i.e., retrieval midline structures (i.e., precuneus, posterior cingulate) engage self‐
monitoring processes) and the posteromedial (i.e., reliving and visual referential and visual imagery aspects of AMs (Spreng & Grady, 2010;
imagery processes) cortices during trauma‐related AM that is required to Svoboda et al., 2006). As a result, increased activation of posterior
engage into a controlled reliving experience (i.e., mental time traveling parietal structures may promote greater vividness during recollection
involving the reestablishment of sensory and of emotional components of of the sensory and the emotional features of the AM (Berntsen et al.,
the AM, while simultaneously activating the knowledge that this 2003; Ehlers et al., 2004). Notably, the PTSD group also showed an
experience is related to the past). increase in activation of the dorsal anterior cingulate, a robust finding
as it was generated by both meta‐analyses. The dorsal anterior
cingulate is associated with negatively valenced emotion processing, in
4.3 | AM network: PTSD > Control
particular the regulation of visceral sensations (Lieberman & Eisenber-
The meta‐analyses revealed several brain regions exhibiting more ger, 2015; Rainville, 2002). In turn, increased activation of the dorsal
common activation in participants with PTSD as compared with anterior cingulate may contribute to the vivid retrieval (i.e., bodily
trauma‐ and nontrauma‐exposed control subjects. Namely, the sensations) of trauma‐related AMs in participants with PTSD. These
precuneus, the posterior cingulate, the middle cingulate, and the findings are also consistent with the centrality of trauma to personal
parahippocampal gyrus were reported to display greater activation identity in PTSD, where re‐experiencing of the trauma, as mediated by
in the PTSD group during trauma‐related AM recollection as posterior parietal activation (e.g., posterior cingulate, precuneus), may
compared with controls. These findings corroborate reports by integrate with an individual’s sense of self (Berntsen & Rubin, 2007;
Sartory et al. (2013), where the posterior parietal and the poster- Berntsen et al., 2003).
omedial cortices were activated more commonly in their PTSD In addition to the posterior parietal and the posteromedial
sample as well. Interestingly, the posterior parietal and the cortices, the parahippocampal gyrus also demonstrated more consis-
parahippocampal gyrus activation revealed across the recollection tent activation during trauma‐related AM recollection in the PTSD
analyses was also found in analyses specific to trauma‐related AM group as compared with controls. The parahippocampal gyrus is part
retrieval. Given that these regions are thought to underlie the self‐ of the limbic system and plays a role in memory encoding as well as
referential and the visual imagery aspects of AM (Spreng & Grady, emotional and anxiety‐related processing (Kilpatrick & Cahill, 2003;
2010; Svoboda et al., 2006), neural activation of these structures Nordahl et al., 1990; Reiman, Fusselman, Fox, & Raichle, 1989; Reiman,
during retrieval phases provides evidence for rapid re‐experiencing Raichle, Butler, Herscovitch, & Robins, 1984). During a resting‐state
of trauma‐related AMs following exposure to trauma‐related stimuli functional connectivity analysis conducted by Ward et al. (2014) with
in persons with PTSD (Ehlers et al., 2004). Moreover, the meta‐ healthy participants, the parahippocampal gyrus was shown to display
analyses on re‐experiencing processes revealed greater common the strongest connectivity of multiple medial temporal lobe regions
activation of the ventromedial prefrontal and the dorsal anterior with the DMN. The authors suggested that the parahippocampal gyrus
cingulate in the PTSD group as compared with controls. Although may be the primary hub mediating the relationship between the
these analyses are based on a small study sample (N = 6), they may medial temporal lobe and the DMN (Ward et al., 2014). Interestingly,
provide evidence for the delayed recruitment of memory search and the parahippocampal gyrus is shown to exhibit a decrease in functional
retrieval monitoring processes (i.e., medial prefrontal cortices) in connectivity with the central node of the DMN, the posterior
PTSD, a seemingly opposite trend to that of healthy control cingulate, in individuals with PTSD during rest (Bluhm et al., 2009).
subjects. In line with the present meta‐analysis, emerging evidence suggests
Autobiographical memories in PTSD are thought to be distinct in that the DMN may be recruited to a greater extent during trauma‐
form from AMs recollected in controls; however, it is important to note related processing in PTSD as compared with controls (Daniels,
that this interpretation does not refer to a specific feature of a study Frewen, McKinnon, & Lanius, 2011; Nicholson et al., 2018; Terpou
design. In participants with PTSD, some traumatic memories are said to et al., 2019). Here, Terpou et al. (2019) have suggested that
remain in an unprocessed state, where sensory, emotional, and participants with PTSD may experience greater self‐related proces-
cognitive components of the AM are fragmented, or are dissociated sing, as indicated by DMN recruitment, during trauma‐related stimulus
(Berntsen et al., 2003; McKinnon, Brewer, Cameron, & Nixon, 2017; St exposure as compared with during resting‐state. In closing, the
Jacques, Kragel, & Rubin, 2013). These AMs are proposed to be stored increased activation of the parahippocampal gyrus may reflect an
18 | THOME ET AL.
increased engagement of the DMN during trauma‐related AM systems, which are shown to be associated with greater PTSD
recollection in participants with PTSD. symptom severity.
4.5 | Limitations
4.4 | AM and implicit memory systems
There are several limitations to consider. The presented meta‐
Interestingly, the within‐group meta‐analysis of PTSD, but not analyses revealed altered common activation in high‐order cortical
the control group, revealed activation of the caudate nucleus, a regions during trauma‐related AM recollection in PTSD as compared
region more characteristic of implicit memory systems, during with controls, namely enhanced common activation of posteromedial
trauma‐related AM recollection. The caudate is part of the dorsal cortices, with concomitant reduced recruitment of medial prefrontal
striatum, which functions during procedural, associative, and emo- brain activation. Despite a more common activation of the
tional learning processes (McDonald & White, 1993; Packard, Cahill, parahippocampal gyrus in PTSD as compared with controls, the
& McGaugh, 1994; Setlow, 1997). Procedural memory is thought to present meta‐analyses did not reveal more common activation of the
be acquired after many repetitions of a certain behaviour, after amygdala, or deeper‐layer brain regions (e.g., periaqueductal gray)
which the behaviour transitions from an explicit to an implicit underlying affective processing in general, which has been discussed
representation (Kandel et al., 2012; Maddox & Ashby, 2004; Poldrack frequently as contributing to the emotional intensity during trauma‐
& Packard, 2003). The implicit representation of a procedural related AMs in PTSD (e.g., Brewin, 2018; Maddox et al., 2019).
memory relies on the ventral and the dorsal striatum, the substantia Generally, deep‐layer brain regions are smaller in size, which
nigra, the subthalamic nuclei as well as the precentral gyri of the increases the difficulty to demarcate these structures with meta‐
cortex (Henke, 2010; Kandel et al., 2012). Here, activation of the analytical techniques assuming a strong overlap. Second, many
caudate and the precentral gyri may be interpreted as an increase in studies included in the meta‐analyses investigated rather small data
procedural‐based memory storage of a patient’s trauma memory. sets (with some studies including only six subjects in one experi-
Given the intrusive and the vivid nature of trauma‐related AMs mental group; e.g., Driessen et al., 2004), which result in increased
(Berntsen et al., 2003; St Jacques et al., 2013), frequent re‐ between‐study variance, while at the same time, reducing statistical
experiencing of the trauma memory may promote a transition to power (Button et al., 2013). Third, it is important to mention that the
greater implicit storage of the procedural component. Although little identified studies did apply different experimental paradigms in
is known on the differences expressed by the procedural memory general (i.e., script‐driven imagery, trauma‐related stimulus presenta-
system in PTSD, it is well‐established that individuals with PTSD tion), as well as different stimulus modalities in particular (i.e., visual,
show a physiological hypersensitivity to threat stimuli (Blechert, auditory), which is likely to be related to different neural substrates.
Michael, & Wilhelm, 2019; Niles et al., 2018; Rabellino et al., 2017; However, given the number of identified studies, creating further
Seligowski et al., 2019; Young et al., 2018). In particular, trauma‐ subcategories would have resulted in subgroups that were too small
related stimuli generate an increased expression of the sympathetic to conduct meaningful analyses by virtue of the increased difficulty
nervous system, which is identified behaviourally by altered startle to observe common activation. Lastly, it is important to note that
responses in persons with PTSD (D’Andrea, Pole, DePierro, Freed, & PTSD has been recognized as a heterogenous disorder, ranging from
Wallace, 2013; Grillon & Morgan, 1999; McTeague et al., 2010; presentations with predominant states of hypervigilance to states of
Thome et al., 2018), as well as upregulated defensive responses (i.e. detachment (i.e., derealization, depersonalization), with the latter
fight, flight, freeze) in animal models of PTSD (Dean, 2011; being more often associated to the recently formulated dissociative
Hagenaars, Oitzl, & Roelofs, 2014; Vianna, Szapiro, McGaugh, subtype (Fenster et al., 2018; Lanius et al., 2018). In addition, PTSD is
Medina, & Izquierdo, 2001). The enhanced sympathetic reactivity often associated with several comorbidities, like, for example, major
may include a motor component, to be represented by the procedural depressive disorder or substance use disorders (Driessen, Schulte
memory system. These sympathetic‐mediated alterations may also et al., 2008; Nichter, Norman, Haller, & Pietrzak, 2019; Pietrzak,
relate to associative memory processes (LeDoux, 2003). In line with Goldstein, Southwick, & Grant, 2011). However, due to the limited
this proposition, both the caudate and a cluster encompassing the number of identified studies, creating sufficiently sized subgroups for
amygdala, the hippocampus, and the insula revealed common each of the possible combinations was not possible. Future work is
activation correlating positively with PTSD symptom severity in the needed urgently to disentangle potential confounding or moderating
PTSD group. The amygdala and the hippocampus are implicated in variables and their influence on trauma‐related AM recollection.
the learning and the memory of associative and of nonassociative
fear responses involved during trauma‐related AM processing
4.6 | Conclusion
(Golier, Yehuda, Lupien, & Harvey, 2003; Lambert & McLaughlin,
2019; Maddox, Hartmann, Ross, & Ressler, 2019; van der Kolk, 1994; These meta‐analyses investigate the differences in common activation
Werner et al., 2009). Taken together, activation of the caudate, the of the AM network during retrieval and re‐experiencing of trauma‐
amygdala, and the hippocampus during trauma‐related AM may related AMs in participants with PTSD as compared with controls. The
suggest a greater storage of trauma material toward implicit memory AM network is shown to demonstrate broadly group‐specific
THOME ET AL. | 19
asymmetries in the engagement of the prefrontal and the posterior Amodio, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Meeting of minds: The medial frontal
parietal cortices during trauma‐related AMs. In particular, control cortex and social cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(4),
268–277. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1884
subjects and participants with PTSD revealed more common activation
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual
of the medial prefrontal and the posteromedial cortices, respectively. of mental disorders (5 ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
The medial prefrontal cortices are thought to underlie the constructive Publishing.
and the retrieval processes of AM, whereas the posteromedial cortices Araujo, H. F., Kaplan, J., & Damasio, A. (2013). Cortical midline structures
and autobiographical‐self processes: An activation‐likelihood estima-
engage the re‐experiencing features by facilitating self‐referential and
tion meta‐analysis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 548. https://doi.
visual imagery processes. The predominance of the latter can explain, org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00548
in part, the phenomenological differences of trauma‐related AMs in Arntz, A., de Groot, C., & Kindt, M. (2005). Emotional memory is
PTSD. These findings highlight the role of the medial prefrontal perceptual. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry,
36(1), 19–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2004.11.003
cortices toward monitoring the retrieval of trauma‐related AMs, which
Baddeley, A. (1992). What is autobiographical memory? In M. A. Conway, D.
may be diminished in persons with PTSD. Notably, the analyses also C. Rubin, H. Spinnler & W. A. Wagenaar (Eds.), Theoretical perspectives on
revealed a dissociation within the anterior cingulate, where anterior autobiographical memory (pp. 13–29). Dordrecht: Springer.
(i.e., perigenual anterior), and posterior (i.e. dorsal anterior) cingulate Berntsen, D., & Rubin, D. C. (2007). When a trauma becomes a key to
cortices showed more common activation within controls and identity: Enhanced integration of trauma memories predicts post-
traumatic stress disorder symptoms. Applied Cognitive Psychology,
individuals with PTSD, respectively. Future research is urged to
21(4), 417–431. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1290
investigate the efficacy of adjunctive treatments (i.e., neurofeedback) Berntsen, D., Willert, M., & Rubin, D. C. (2003). Splintered memories or
that may promote greater recruitment of the medial prefrontal vivid landmarks? Qualities and organization of traumatic memories
cortices during trauma‐related AM recollection, as this may reduce re‐ with and without PTSD. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17(6), 675–693.
https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.894
experiencing processes in favor of a more controlled retrieval in
Blake, D., Weathers, F., Nagy, L., Kaloupek, D., Klauminzer, G.,
individuals with PTSD. Charney, D., & Keane, T. (1990). Clinician‐administered PTSD scale
(CAPS). Boston, 7.
Blechert, J., Michael, T., & Wilhelm, F. H. (2019). Video‐based analysis of
A C K N O W L E D GM E N T S bodily startle and subsequent emotional facial expression in post-
traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology,
This study was funded by grants provided by the Canadian Institutes 4(4), 435–447. https://doi.org/10.5127/jep.030712
of Health Research [Grant Numbers: 137150, 97914]. Ruth A. Lanius Bliss‐Moreau, E., Williams, L. A., & Santistevan, A. C. (2019). The
is supported by the Harris‐Woodman Chair in Psyche and Soma; immutability of valence and arousal in the foundation of emotion.
Emotion, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000606
Margaret C. McKinnon is supported by the Homewood Chair in
Bluhm, R. L., Williamson, P. C., Osuch, E. A., Frewen, P. A., Stevens, T. K.,
Mental Health and Trauma. Boksman, K., … Lanius, R. A. (2009). Alterations in default network
connectivity in posttraumatic stress disorder related to early‐life
trauma. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 34(3), 187–194.
CO NFLICT OF I NTERE STS
PMCID:PMC2674971
Botzung, A., Denkova, E., Ciuciu, P., Scheiber, C., & Manning, L. (2008). The
The authors declare that there are no conflict of interests.
neural bases of the constructive nature of autobiographical memories
studied with a self‐paced fMRI design. Memory, 16(4), 351–363.
https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210801931222
D A T A A V A I L AB I L I T Y S T A TE M E N T
de Bourbon‐Teles, J., Bentley, P., Koshino, S., Shah, K., Dutta, A., Malhotra,
P., … Soto, D. (2014). Thalamic control of human attention driven by
The data that support the findings of this study are openly available
memory and learning. Current Biology, 24(9), 993–999. https://doi.org/
on OSF via the following link https://osf.io/9pdx2/?view_only= 10.1016/j.cub.2014.03.024
2656e9f28d404d4a8127090e1475d847. Brain maps of all analyses Bremner, J. D., Narayan, M., Staib, L. H., Southwick, S. M., McGlashan, T., &
are available at https://neurovault.org/collections/UOWUSAMV/. Charney, D. S. (1999). Neural correlates of memories of childhood
sexual abuse in women with and without posttraumatic stress
disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156(11), 1787–1795.
OR CID https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.156.11.1787
Bremner, J. D., Staib, L. H., Kaloupek, D., Southwick, S. M., Soufer, R., &
Janine Thome http://orcid.org/0000-0002-0215-2097 Charney, D. S. (1999). Neural correlates of exposure to traumatic
Braeden A. Terpou http://orcid.org/0000-0002-6770-2715 pictures and sound in Vietnam combat veterans with and without
posttraumatic stress disorder: A positron emission tomography study.
Biological Psychiatry, 45(7), 806–816.
Brewer, W. F. (1986). What is autobiographical memory? In D. C. Rubin (Ed.),
REFERENC ES Autobiographical memory (pp. 25–49). New York, NY: Cambridge
University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511558313.006
Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). Remembering the past Brewin, C. R. (2011). The nature and significance of memory disturbance
and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates in posttraumatic stress disorder. In S. Nolen‐Hoeksema, D. Cannon
during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia, 45(7), & T. Widiger (Eds.), Annual review of clinical psychology (7,
1363–1377. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.016 pp. 203–227).
20 | THOME ET AL.
Brewin, C. R. (2014). Episodic memory, perceptual memory, and their International Journal of Psychophysiology, 90(1), 80–89. https://doi.
interaction: Foundations for a theory of posttraumatic stress disorder. org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.07.008
Psychological Bulletin, 140(1), 69–97. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033722 Dahlgren, M. K., Laifer, L. M., VanElzakker, M. B., Offringa, R., Hughes, K.
Brewin, C. R. (2015). Re‐experiencing traumatic events in PTSD: New avenues C., Staples‐Bradley, L. K., … Shin, L. M. (2018). Diminished medial
in research on intrusive memories and flashbacks. European Journal of prefrontal cortex activation during the recollection of stressful events
Psychotraumatology, 6, 27180. https://doi.org/10.3402/ejpt.v6.27180 is an acquired characteristic of PTSD. Psychological Medicine, 48(7),
Brewin, C. R. (2018). Memory and forgetting. Current Psychiatry Reports, 1128–1138. https://doi.org/10.1017/s003329171700263x
20(10), 87. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920‐018‐0950‐7 Daniels, J. K., Frewen, P., McKinnon, M. C., & Lanius, R. A. (2011). Default
Britton, J. C., Phan, K. L., Taylor, S. F., Fig, L. M., & Liberzon, I. (2005). mode alterations in posttraumatic stress disorder related to early‐life
Corticolimbic blood flow in posttraumatic stress disorder during trauma: A developmental perspective. Journal of Psychiatry and
script‐driven imagery. Biological Psychiatry, 57(8), 832–840. https:// Neuroscience, 36(1), 56–59. https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.100050
doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.025 Davidson, J. R., Tharwani, H. M., & Connor, K. M. (2002). Davidson trauma
Brown, A. D., Antonius, D., Kramer, M., Root, J. C., & Hirst, W. (2010). scale (DTS): Normative scores in the general population and effect
Trauma centrality and PTSD in veterans returning from Iraq and sizes in placebo‐controlled SSRI trials. Depression and Anxiety, 15(2),
Afghanistan. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 23(4), 496–499. https://doi. 75–78. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.10021
org/10.1002/jts.20547 Dean, C. (2011). Endocannabinoid modulation of sympathetic and
Brown, R., & Kulik, J. (1977). Flashbulb memories. Cognition, 5(1), 73–99. cardiovascular responses to acute stress in the periaqueductal gray
https://doi.org/10.1016/0010‐0277(77)90018‐X of the rat. American Journal of Physiology‐Regulatory, Integrative and
Buckner, R. L., & Carroll, D. C. (2007). Self‐projection and the brain. Trends Comparative Physiology, 300(3), R771–R779. https://doi.org/10.1152/
in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 49–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006. ajpregu.00391.2010
11.004 Dolcos, F., LaBar, K. S., & Cabeza, R. (2004). Dissociable effects of arousal
Button, K. S., Ioannidis, J. P., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, B. A., Flint, J., Robinson, and valence on prefrontal activity indexing emotional evaluation and
E. S., & Munafò, M. R. (2013). Power failure: Why small sample size subsequent memory: An event‐related fMRI study. NeuroImage, 23(1),
undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neu- 64–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.05.015
roscience, 14(5), 365–376. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3475 Driessen, M., Beblo, T., Mertens, M., Piefke, M., Rullkoetter, N., Silva‐
Cabeza, R., & St Jacques, P. (2007). Functional neuroimaging of autobio- Saavedra, A., … Woermann, F. G. (2004). Posttraumatic stress disorder
graphical memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(5), 219–227. https://doi. and fMRI activation patterns of traumatic memory in patients with
org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.02.005 borderline personality disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 55(6), 603–611.
Carrera, E., & Bogousslavsky, J. (2006). The thalamus and behavior: Effects https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.08.018
of anatomically distinct strokes. Neurology, 66(12), 1817–1823. https:// Driessen, M., Schulte, S., Luedecke, C., Schaefer, I., Sutmann, F., Ohlmeier,
doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000219679.95223.4c M., … Broese, T. (2008). Trauma and PTSD in patients with alcohol,
Cisler, J. M., Steele, J. S., Lenow, J. K., Smitherman, S., Everett, B., Messias, E., drug, or dual dependence: A multi‐center study. Alcoholism: Clinical
& Kilts, C. D. (2014). Functional reorganization of neural networks during and Experimental Research, 32(3), 481–488. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.
repeated exposure to the traumatic memory in posttraumatic stress 1530‐0277.2007.00591.x
disorder: An exploratory fMRI study. Journal of Psychiatric Research, Ehlers, A. (2010). Understanding and treating unwanted trauma memories
48(1), 47–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.09.013 in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Psychology, 218(2),
Conway, M. (2013). Flashbulb memories. London: Psychology Press. 141–145. https://doi.org/10.1027/0044‐3409/a000021
https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203775820 Ehlers, A., & Clark, D. M. (2000). A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress
Conway, M. A., Anderson, S. J., Larsen, S. F., Donnelly, C. M., McDaniel, M. disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38(4), 319–345. https://doi.
A., McClelland, A. G., … Logie, R. H. (1994). The formation of flashbulb org/10.1016/S0005‐7967(99)00123‐0
memories. Memory & Cognition, 22(3), 326–343. Ehlers, A., Hackmann, A., & Michael, T. (2004). Intrusive re‐experiencing in
Conway, M. A., & Pleydell‐Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of post‐traumatic stress disorder: Phenomenology, theory, and therapy.
autobiographical memories in the self‐memory system. Psychological Memory, 12(4), 403–415. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210444000025
Review, 107(2), 261–288. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033‐295X.107.2.261. Eickhoff, S. B., Laird, A. R., Grefkes, C., Wang, L. E., Zilles, K., & Fox, P. T.
Corkin, S. (1968). Acquisition of motor skill after bilateral medial (2009). Coordinate‐based activation likelihood estimation meta‐
temporal‐lobe excision. Neuropsychologia, 6(3), 255–265. https://doi. analysis of neuroimaging data: A random‐effects approach based on
org/10.1016/0028‐3932(68)90024‐9 empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. Human Brain Mapping,
Crespo, M., & Fernandez‐Lansac, V. (2016). Memory and narrative of 30(9), 2907–2926. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20718
traumatic events: A literature review. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Elbert, T., & Schauer, M. (2002). Psychological trauma: Burnt into memory.
Research, Practice, and Policy, 8(2), 149–156. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Nature, 419(6910), 883.
tra0000041. Falsetti, S. A., Resnick, H. S., Resick, P. A., & Kilpatrick, D. G. (1993). The
Crone, J. S., Soddu, A., Höller, Y., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Schurz, M., Bergmann, J., modified PTSD symptom scale: A brief self‐report measure of
… Kronbichler, M. (2014). Altered network properties of the fronto‐ posttraumatic stress disorder. The Behavior Therapist, 16, 161–162.
parietal network and the thalamus in impaired consciousness. NeuroImage, Fenster, R. J., Lebois, L. A. M., Ressler, K. J., & Suh, J. (2018). Brain circuit
4, 240–248. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2013.12.005 dysfunction in post‐traumatic stress disorder: From mouse to man.
Cwik, J. C., Sartory, G., Nuyken, M., Schurholt, B., & Seitz, R. J. (2017). Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 19(9), 535–551. https://doi.org/10.1038/
Posterior and prefrontal contributions to the development posttrau- s41583‐018‐0039‐7
matic stress disorder symptom severity: An fMRI study of symptom Ferbinteanu, J. (2018). Memory systems 2018–towards a new paradigm.
provocation in acute stress disorder. European Archives of Psychiatry Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 157, 61–78. https://doi.org/10.
and Clinical Neuroscience, 267(6), 495–505. https://doi.org/10.1007/ 1016/j.nlm.2018.11.005
s00406‐016‐0713‐6 Finkenauer, C., Luminet, O., Gisle, L., El‐Ahmadi, A., Van Der Linden, M., &
D’Andrea, W., Pole, N., DePierro, J., Freed, S., & Wallace, D. B. (2013). Philippot, P. (1998). Flashbulb memories and the underlying mechan-
Heterogeneity of defensive responses after exposure to trauma: isms of their formation: Toward an emotional‐integrative model.
Blunted autonomic reactivity in response to startling sounds. Memory and Cognition, 26(3), 516–531.
THOME ET AL. | 21
Finn, B., & Roediger, H. L., III (2011). Enhancing retention through Karam, E. G., Friedman, M. J., Hill, E. D., Kessler, R. C., McLaughlin, K. A.,
reconsolidation: Negative emotional arousal following retrieval Petukhova, M., … Bromet, E. J. (2014). Cumulative traumas and risk
enhances later recall. Psychological Science, 22(6), 781–786. https:// thresholds: 12‐month ptsd in the world mental health (WMH)
doi.org/10.1177/0956797611407932 surveys: Cumulative trauma and PTSD. Depression and Anxiety,
Fivush, R. (2011). The development of autobiographical memory. Annual 31(2), 130–142. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22169
Review of Psychology, 62, 559–582. Ke, J., Zhang, L., Qi, R., Xu, Q., Li, W., Hou, C., … Lu, G. (2015). Altered
Flatten, G., Perlitz, V., Pestinger, M., Arin, T., Kohl, B., Kastrau, F., … Erli, H. blood oxygen level‐dependent signal variability in chronic post‐
J. (2004). Neural processing of traumatic events in subjects suffering traumatic stress disorder during symptom provocation. Neuropsychia-
PTSD: A case study of two surgical patients with severe accident tric Disease and Treatment, 11, 1805–1815. https://doi.org/10.2147/
trauma. GMS Psycho‐Social‐Medicine, 1, 1–13. NDT.S87332
Frewen, P., Zhu, J., & Lanius, R. (2019). Lifetime traumatic stressors and Ke, J., Zhang, L., Qi, R. F., Li, W. H., Hou, C. L., Zhong, Y., … Lu, G. M. (2016).
adverse childhood experiences uniquely predict concurrent PTSD, A longitudinal fMRI investigation in acute post‐traumatic stress
complex PTSD, and dissociative subtype of PTSD symptoms whereas disorder (PTSD). Acta Radiologica, 57(11), 1387–1395. https://doi.org/
recent adult non‐traumatic stressors do not: Results from an online 10.1177/0284185115585848
survey study. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 10(1), 1606625. Kelley, W. M., Macrae, C. N., Wyland, C. L., Caglar, S., Inati, S., &
https://doi.org/10.1080/20008198.2019.1606625 Heatherton, T. F. (2002). Finding the self? An event‐related fMRI
Frewen, P. A., Pain, C., Dozois, D. J., & Lanius, R. A. (2006). Alexithymia in study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(5), 785–794. https://doi.
PTSD: Psychometric and FMRI studies. Annals of the New York org/10.1162/08989290260138672
Academy of Sciences, 1071(1), 397–400. https://doi.org/10.1196/ Kensinger, E. A., & Corkin, S. (2004). Two routes to emotional memory:
annals.1364.029 Distinct neural processes for valence and arousal. Proceedings of the
Gilboa, A. (2004). Autobiographical and episodic memory—one and the National Academy of Sciences, 101(9), 3310–3315. https://doi.org/10.
same? Neuropsychologia, 42(10), 1336–1349. https://doi.org/10.1016/ 1073/pnas.0306408101
j.neuropsychologia.2004.02.014 Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2005). Retrieving accurate and
Gilboa, A., Alain, C., Stuss, D. T., Melo, B., Miller, S., & Moscovitch, M. distorted memories: Neuroimaging evidence for effects of emotion.
(2006). Mechanisms of spontaneous confabulations: A strategic NeuroImage, 27(1), 167–177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.
retrieval account. Brain, 129(6), 1399–1414. https://doi.org/10. 2005.03.038
1093/brain/awl093 Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2006). Amygdala activity is associated
Golier, J. A., Yehuda, R., Lupien, S. J., & Harvey, P. D. (2003). Memory for with the successful encoding of item, but not source, information for
trauma‐related information in holocaust survivors with PTSD. positive and negative stimuli. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(9),
Psychiatry Research, 121(2), 133–143. 2564–2570. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5241‐05.2006
Grey, N., & Holmes, E. A. (2008). “Hotspots” in trauma memories in the Kilpatrick, L., & Cahill, L. (2003). Amygdala modulation of parahippocam-
treatment of post‐traumatic stress disorder: A replication. Memory, pal and frontal regions during emotionally influenced memory
16(7), 788–796. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210802266446 storage. NeuroImage, 20(4), 2091–2099. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Grillon, C., & Morgan, C. A., 3rd. (1999). Fear‐potentiated startle neuroimage.2003.08.006
conditioning to explicit and contextual cues in Gulf War veterans Kim, H. (2012). A dual‐subsystem model of the brain’s default network:
with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Self‐referential processing, memory retrieval processes, and autobio-
108(1), 134–142. graphical memory retrieval. NeuroImage, 61(4), 966–977. https://doi.
Hagenaars, M. A., Oitzl, M., & Roelofs, K. (2014). Updating freeze: Aligning org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.025
animal and human research. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 47, Kim, S. J., Lyoo, I. K., Lee, Y. S., Kim, J., Sim, M. E., Bae, S. J., … Jeong, D. U.
165–176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.07.021 (2007). Decreased cerebral blood flow of thalamus in PTSD patients
Hart, H., Radua, J., Mataix‐Cols, D., & Rubia, K. (2012). Meta‐analysis of as a strategy to reduce re‐experience symptoms. Acta Psychiatrica
fMRI studies of timing in attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder Scandinavica, 116(2), 145–153. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600‐0447.
(ADHD). Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(10), 2248–2256. 2006.00952.x
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.08.003 Kleim, B., Ehring, T., & Ehlers, A. (2012). Perceptual processing advantages
Henke, K. (2010). A model for memory systems based on processing for trauma‐related visual cues in post‐traumatic stress disorder.
modes rather than consciousness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(7), Psychological Medicine, 42(1), 173–181. https://doi.org/10.1017/
523–532. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2850 s0033291711001048
Holland, A. C., & Kensinger, E. A. (2010). Emotion and autobiographical Lambert, H. K., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2019). Impaired hippocampus‐
memory. Physics of Life Reviews, 7(1), 88–131. https://doi.org/10.1016/ dependent associative learning as a mechanism underlying PTSD: A
j.plrev.2010.01.006 meta‐analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 107, 729–749.
Hopper, J. W., Frewen, P. A., van der Kolk, B. A., & Lanius, R. A. (2007). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiorev.2019.09.024
Neural correlates of reexperiencing, avoidance, and dissociation in Landré, L., Destrieux, C., Andersson, F., Barantin, L., Quidé, Y., Tapia, G., … El‐
PTSD: Symptom dimensions and emotion dysregulation in responses Hage, W. (2012). Working memory processing of traumatic material in
to script‐driven trauma imagery. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 20(5), women with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Psychiatry and
713–725. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.20284 Neuroscience, 37(2), 87–94. https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.100167
Hou, C., Liu, J., Wang, K., Li, L., Liang, M., He, Z., … Jiang, T. (2007). Brain Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., & Hamm, A. O. (1993).
responses to symptom provocation and trauma‐related short‐term Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral
memory recall in coal mining accident survivors with acute severe reactions. Psychophysiology, 30(3), 261–273. https://doi.org/10.1111/
PTSD. Brain Research, 1144, 165–174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. j.1469‐8986.1993.tb03352.x
brainres.2007.01.089 Lanius, R. A., Boyd, J. E., McKinnon, M. C., Nicholson, A. A., Frewen, P.,
Kandel, E. R., Dudai, Y., & Mayford, M. R. (2014). The molecular and Vermetten, E., … Spiegel, D. (2018). A review of the neurobiological
systems biology of memory. Cell, 157(1), 163–186. https://doi.org/10. basis of trauma‐related dissociation and its relation to cannabinoid‐
1016/j.cell.2014.03.001 and opioid‐mediated stress response: A transdiagnostic, translational
Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. H., Jessell, T. M., Siegelbaum, S. A., & Hudspeth, A. J. approach. Current Psychiatry Reports, 20(12), 118. https://doi.org/10.
(2012). Principles of neural science (5th Edition). McGraw‐Hill Education. 1007/s11920‐018‐0983‐y
22 | THOME ET AL.
Lanius, R. A., Frewen, P. A., Girotti, M., Neufeld, R. W. J., Stevens, T. K., & memory encoding. Neuron, 102(1), 60–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Densmore, M. (2007). Neural correlates of trauma script‐imagery in neuron.2019.03.017
posttraumatic stress disorder with and without comorbid major depres- Maddox, W. T., & Ashby, F. G. (2004). Dissociating explicit and procedural‐
sion: A functional MRI investigation. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, learning based systems of perceptual category learning. Behavioural
155(1), 45–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2006.11.006 Processes, 66(3), 309–332. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2004.03.011
Lanius, R. A., Vermetten, E., Loewenstein, R. J., Brand, B., Schmahl, C., Markowitsch, H. J., & Staniloiu, A. (2011). Memory, autonoetic conscious-
Bremner, J. D., & Spiegel, D. (2010). Emotion modulation in PTSD: ness, and the self. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(1), 16–39. https://
Clinical and neurobiological evidence for a dissociative subtype. doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.09.005
American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(6), 640–647. https://doi.org/10. McDonald, R. J., & White, N. M. (1993). A triple dissociation of memory
1176/appi.ajp.2009.09081168 systems: Hippocampus, amygdala, and dorsal striatum. Behavioral
Lanius, R. A., Williamson, P. C., Bluhm, R. L., Densmore, M., Boksman, K., Neuroscience, 107(1), 3–22.
Neufeld, R. W. J., … Menon, R. S. (2005). Functional connectivity of McGaugh, J. L. (2004). The amygdala modulates the consolidation of
dissociative responses in posttraumatic stress disorder: A functional memories of emotionally arousing experiences. Annual Review of
magnetic resonance imaging investigation. Biological Psychiatry, 57(8), Neuroscience, 27, 1–28. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.
873–884. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.011 070203.144157
Lanius, R. A., Williamson, P. C., Boksman, K., Densmore, M., Gupta, M., McKinnon, A., Brewer, N., Cameron, K., & Nixon, R. D. V. (2017). The
Neufeld, R. W. J., … Menon, R. S. (2002). Brain activation during relationship between processing style, trauma memory processes, and
script‐driven imagery induced dissociative responses in PTSD: A the development of posttraumatic stress symptoms in children and
functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation. Biological adolescents. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry,
Psychiatry, 52(4), 305–311. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006‐3223(02) 57, 135–142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2017.04.004
01367‐7 McKinnon, M. C., Palombo, D. J., Nazarov, A., Kumar, N., Khuu, W., &
Lanius, R. A., Williamson, P. C., Densmore, M., Boksman, K., Gupta, M. A., Levine, B. (2015). Threat of death and autobiographical memory: A
Neufeld, R. W., … Menon, R. S. (2001). Neural correlates of traumatic study of passengers from Flight AT236. Clinical Psychological Science,
memories in posttraumatic stress disorder: A functional MRI 3(4), 487–502. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702614542280
investigation. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(11), 1920–1922. McTeague, L. M., Lang, P. J., Laplante, M. C., Cuthbert, B. N., Shumen, J. R.,
https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1920 & Bradley, M. M. (2010). Aversive imagery in posttraumatic stress
Lanius, R. A., Williamson, P. C., Hopper, J., Densmore, M., Boksman, K., Gupta, disorder: Trauma recurrence, comorbidity, and physiological reactiv-
M. A., … Menon, R. S. (2003). Recall of emotional states in posttraumatic ity. Biological Psychiatry, 67(4), 346–356. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
stress disorder: An fMRI investigation. Biological Psychiatry, 53(3), biopsych.2009.08.023
204–210. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006‐3223(02)01466‐X Menon, V. (2011). Large‐scale brain networks and psychopathology: A
Lanius, R. A., Wolf, E. J., Miller, M. W., Frewen, P. A., Vermetten, E., Brand, unifying triple network model. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(10),
B., & Spiegel, D. (2014). The dissociative subtype of PTSD. In M. J. 483–506. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.003
Friedman, T. M. Keane & P. A. Resick (Eds.), Handbook of PTSD: Science Metzger, C. D., Eckert, U., Steiner, J., Sartorius, A., Buchmann, J. E.,
and practice (pp. 234–250). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Stadler, J., … Abler, B. (2010). High field FMRI reveals thalamocortical
LeDoux, J. (2003). The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala. Cellular integration of segregated cognitive and emotional processing in
and Molecular Neurobiology, 23(4‐5), 727–738. mediodorsal and intralaminar thalamic nuclei. Frontiers in Neuroanat-
LeDoux, J. E., Farb, C. R., & Romanski, L. M. (1991). Overlapping omy, 4, 138. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnana.2010.00138
projections to the amygdala and striatum from auditory processing Metzger, C. D., van der Werf, Y. D., & Walter, M. (2013). Functional
areas of the thalamus and cortex. Neuroscience Letters, 134(1), mapping of thalamic nuclei and their integration into cortico‐striatal‐
139–144. https://doi.org/10.1016/0304‐3940(91)90526‐Y thalamo‐cortical loops via ultra‐high resolution imaging—from animal
Levine, B., Svoboda, E., Hay, J. F., Winocur, G., & Moscovitch, M. (2002). anatomy to in vivo imaging in humans. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7, 24.
Aging and autobiographical memory: Dissociating episodic from https://doi.org/10.3389/fnana.2010.00138
semantic retrieval. Psychology and Aging, 17(4), 677–689. https://doi. Meyer‐Lindenberg, A., & Tost, H. (2012). Neural mechanisms of social risk
org/10.1037/0882‐7974.17.4.677 for psychiatric disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 15(5), 663–668. https://
Liberzon, I., Taylor, S. F., Amdur, R., Jung, T. D., Chamberlain, K. R., doi.org/10.1038/nn.3083
Minoshima, S., … Fig, L. M. (1999). Brain activation in PTSD in Michael, T., Ehlers, A., & Halligan, S. L. (2005). Enhanced priming for
response to trauma‐related stimuli. Biological Psychiatry, 45(7), trauma‐related material in posttraumatic stress disorder. Emotion,
817–826. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006‐3223(98)00246‐7 5(1), 103–112. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528‐3542.5.1.103
Liberzon, I., Taylor, S. F., Fig, L. M., & Koeppe, R. A. (1996). Alteration of Mickleborough, M. J., Daniels, J. K., Coupland, N. J., Kao, R., Williamson, P.
corticothalamic perfusion ratios during a PTSD flashback. Depression C., Lanius, U. F., … Lanius, R. A. (2011). Effects of trauma‐related cues
and Anxiety, 4(3), 146–150. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1520‐ on pain processing in posttraumatic stress disorder: An fMRI
6394(1996)4:3<146::AID‐DA9>3.0.CO;2‐E investigation. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 36(1), 6–14.
Lieberman, M. D., & Eisenberger, N. I. (2015). The dorsal anterior https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.080188
cingulate cortex is selective for pain: Results from large‐scale reverse Morey, R. A., Dolcos, F., Petty, C. M., Cooper, D. A., Hayes, J. P., LaBar, K.
inference. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United S., & McCarthy, G. (2009). The role of trauma‐related distractors on
States of America, 112(49), 15250–15255. https://doi.org/10.1073/ neural systems for working memory and emotion processing in
pnas.1515083112 posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43(8),
Ludäscher, P., Valerius, G., Stiglmayr, C., Mauchnik, J., Lanius, R. A., Bohus, 809–817. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.10.014
M., & Schmahl, C. (2010). Pain sensitivity and neural processing during Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G. (2002). The frontal cortex and working
dissociative states in patients with borderline personality disorder with memory. Principles of frontal lobe function, 188, 188–209.
with and without comorbid posttraumatic stress disorder: A pilot Nicholson, A. A., Rabellino, D., Densmore, M., Frewen, P. A., Paret, C.,
study. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 35(3), 177–184. https:// Kluetsch, R., … Neufeld, R. W. (2018). Intrinsic connectivity network
doi.org/10.1503/jpn.090022 dynamics in PTSD during amygdala downregulation using real‐time
Maddox, S. A., Hartmann, J., Ross, R. A., & Ressler, K. J. (2019). fMRI neurofeedback: A preliminary analysis. Human Brain Mapping,
Deconstructing the gestalt: Mechanisms of fear, threat, and trauma 39(11), 4258–4275. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24244
THOME ET AL. | 23
Nichter, B., Norman, S., Haller, M., & Pietrzak, R. H. (2019). Physical health Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human
burden of PTSD, depression, and their comorbidity in the US veteran brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13(1), 25–42.
population: Morbidity, functioning, and disability. Journal of Psychoso- Prebble, S. C., Addis, D. R., & Tippett, L. J. (2013). Autobiographical
matic Research, 124, 109744. memory and sense of self. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 815–840.
Niles, A. N., Luxenberg, A., Neylan, T. C., Inslicht, S. S., Richards, A., https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030146
Metzler, T. J., … O’Donovan, A. (2018). Effects of threat context, Protopopescu, X., Pan, H., Tuescher, O., Cloitre, M., Goldstein, M.,
trauma history, and posttraumatic stress disorder status on physio- Engelien, W., … Stern, E. (2005). Differential time courses and
logical startle reactivity in gulf war veterans. Journal of Traumatic specificity of amygdala activity in posttraumatic stress disorder
Stress, 31(4), 579–590. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.22302 subjects and normal control subjects. Biological Psychiatry, 57(5),
Nordahl, T. E., Semple, W. E., Gross, M., Mellman, T. A., Stein, M. B., Goyer, 464–473. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.026
P., … Cohen, R. M. (1990). Cerebral glucose metabolic differences in Quevedo, J., Sant’Anna, M. K., Madruga, M., Lovato, I., de‐Paris, F.,
patients with panic disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology, 3, 261–272. Kapczinski, F., … Cahill, L. (2003). Differential effects of emotional
Northoff, G., & Bermpohl, F. (2004). Cortical midline structures and the arousal in short‐and long‐term memory in healthy adults. Neurobiology
self. Trends in cognitive sciences, 8(3), 102–107. https://doi.org/10. of Learning and Memory, 79(2), 132–135. https://doi.org/10.1016/
1016/j.tics.2004.01.004 S1074‐7427(02)00034‐5
O’Kearney, R., & Perrott, K. (2006). Trauma narratives in posttraumatic Rabellino, D., D’Andrea, W., Siegle, G., Frewen, P. A., Minshew, R.,
stress disorder: A review. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 19(1), 81–93. Densmore, M., … Lanius, R. A. (2017). Neural correlates of heart rate
https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.20099 variability in PTSD during sub‐ and supraliminal processing of trauma‐
Packard, M. G., Cahill, L., & McGaugh, J. L. (1994). Amygdala modulation of related cues. Human Brain Mapping, 38(10), 4898–4907. https://doi.
hippocampal‐dependent and caudate nucleus‐dependent memory org/10.1002/hbm.23702
processes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the Rabellino, D., Densmore, M., Frewen, P. A., Theberge, J., & Lanius, R. A.
United States of America, 91(18), 8477–8481. https://doi.org/10.1073/ (2016). The innate alarm circuit in post‐traumatic stress disorder:
pnas.91.18.8477 Conscious and subconscious processing of fear‐ and trauma‐related
Penfield, W., & Milner, B. (1958). Memory deficit produced by bilateral lesions cues. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 248, 142–150. https://doi.org/
in the hippocampal zone. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 79(5), 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.12.005
475–497. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneurpsyc.1958.02340050003001 Radua, J., Grau, M., Van Den Heuvel, O. A., De Schotten, M. T., Stein, D. J.,
Pezdek, K. (2003). Event memory and autobiographical memory for the Canales‐Rodríguez, E. J., … Mataix‐Cols, D. (2014). Multimodal voxel‐
events of September 11, 2001. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17(9), based meta‐analysis of white matter abnormalities in
1033–1045. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.984 obsessive–compulsive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology, 39(7),
Phelps, E. A., Ling, S., & Carrasco, M. (2006). Emotion facilitates 1547–1557. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2014.5
perception and potentiates the perceptual benefits of attention. Radua, J., Mataix‐Cols, D., Phillips, M. L., El‐Hage, W., Kronhaus, D. M.,
Psychological Science, 17(4), 292–299. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467‐ Cardoner, N., & Surguladze, S. (2012). A new meta‐analytic method
9280.2006.01701.x for neuroimaging studies that combines reported peak coordinates
Piefke, M., Pestinger, M., Arin, T., Kohl, B., Kastrau, F., Schnitker, R., … and statistical parametric maps. European Psychiatry, 27(8), 605–611.
Flatten, G. (2007). The neurofunctional mechanisms of traumatic and https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2011.04.001
non‐traumatic memory in patients with acute PTSD following Rainville, P. (2002). Brain mechanisms of pain affect and pain modulation.
accident trauma. Neurocase, 13(5), 342–357. https://doi.org/10. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 12(2), 195–204. https://doi.org/10.
1080/13554790701851494 1016/S0959‐4388(02)00313‐6
Pietrzak, R. H., Goldstein, R. B., Southwick, S. M., & Grant, B. F. (2011). Rauch, S. L., Van Der Kolk, B. A., Fisler, R. E., Alpert, N. M.,
Prevalence and axis I comorbidity of full and partial posttraumatic Orr, S. P., Savage, C. R., … Pitman, R. K. (1996). A symptom
stress disorder in the United States: Results from Wave 2 of the provocation study of posttraumatic stress disorder using positron
National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. emission tomography and script‐driven imagery. Archives of General
Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 25(3), 456–465. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Psychiatry, 53(5), 380–387. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1996.
janxdis.2010.11.010 01830050014003
Piolino, P., Desgranges, B., Clarys, D., Guillery‐Girard, B., Taconnat, L., Reese, E. (2002). Social factors in the development of autobiographical
Isingrini, M., & Eustache, F. (2006). Autobiographical memory, autonoetic memory: The state of the art. Social Development, 11(1), 124–142.
consciousness, and self‐perspective in aging. Psychology and Aging, 21(3), https://doi.org/10.1111/1467‐9507.00190
510–525. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882‐7974.21.3.510 Reiman, E. M., Fusselman, M. J., Fox, P. T., & Raichle, M. E. (1989).
Piolino, P., Desgranges, B., & Eustache, F. (2009). Episodic autobiographi- Neuroanatomical correlates of anticipatory anxiety. Science,
cal memories over the course of time: Cognitive, neuropsychological 243(4894), 1071–1074. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.2784226
and neuroimaging findings. Neuropsychologia, 47(11), 2314–2329. Reiman, E. M., Raichle, M. E., Butler, F. K., Herscovitch, P., & Robins, E.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.01.020 (1984). A focal brain abnormality in panic disorder, a severe form of
Pissiota, A., Frans, Ö., Fernandez, M., von Knorring, L., Fischer, H., & anxiety. Nature, 310(5979), 683–685.
Fredrikson, M. (2002). Neurofunctional correlates of posttraumatic Robinaugh, D. J., & McNally, R. J. (2011). Trauma centrality and PTSD
stress disorder: A PET symptom provocation study. European Archives symptom severity in adult survivors of childhood sexual abuse. Journal
of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 252(2), 68–75. https://doi.org/ of Traumatic Stress, 24(4), 483–486. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.20656
10.1007/s004060200014 Rogers, T. B. (1977). Self‐reference in memory: Recognition of personality
Poldrack, R. A., & Packard, M. G. (2003). Competition among multiple items. Journal of Research in Personality, 11(3), 295–305. https://doi.
memory systems: Converging evidence from animal and human brain org/10.1016/0092‐6566(77)90038‐1
studies. Neuropsychologia, 41(3), 245–251. https://doi.org/10.1016/ Rovee‐Collier, C. K., Hayne, H., & Colombo, M. (2001). The development of
S0028‐3932(02)00157‐4 implicit and explicit memory (24). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John
Posner, M. I. (1994). Attention: The mechanisms of consciousness. Benjamins Publishing.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of Rubin, D. C. (2005). A Basic‐Systems Approach to Autobiographical
America, 91(16), 7398–7403. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.91.16. Memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(2), 79–83.
7398 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963‐7214.2005.00339.x
24 | THOME ET AL.
Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and medial prefrontalcortex during traumatic imagery in male and female
and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1161–1178. https://doi.org/10.1037/ vietnam veterans with ptsd. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61(2), 168–176.
h0077714 https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.61.2.168
Russell, J. A., & Mehrabian, A. (1977). Evidence for a three‐factor theory Spreng, R. N., & Grady, C. L. (2010). Patterns of brain activity supporting
of emotions. Journal of Research in Personality, 11(3), 273–294. https:// autobiographical memory, prospection, and theory of mind, and their
doi.org/10.1016/0092‐6566(77)90037‐X relationship to the default mode network. Journal of Cognitive
Sakamoto, H., Fukuda, R., Okuaki, T., Rogers, M., Kasai, K., Machida, T., … Neuroscience, 22(6), 1112–1123. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.
Kato, N. (2005). Parahippocampal activation evoked by masked 21282
traumatic images in posttraumatic stress disorder: A functional MRI Squire, L. R. (1987). The organization and neural substrates of human
study. NeuroImage, 26(3), 813–821. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. memory. International Journal of Neurology, 21‐22, 218–222.
neuroimage.2005.02.032 Squire, L. R. (2004). Memory systems of the brain: A brief history and
Sartory, G., Cwik, J., Knuppertz, H., Schurholt, B., Lebens, M., Seitz, R. J., & current perspective. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 82(3),
Schulze, R. (2013). In search of the trauma memory: A meta‐analysis 171–177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2004.06.005
of functional neuroimaging studies of symptom provocation in Squire, L. R., & Dede, A. J. (2015). Conscious and unconscious memory
posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). PLoS One, 8(3):e58150. systems. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology, 7(3), a021667.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0058150 https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a021667
Schacter, D. L., & Tulving, E. (1994). Memory systems. Cambridge, MA: MIT St Jacques, P. L. (2012). Functional neuroimaging of autobiographical
Press. memory. In D. Berntsen & D. C. Rubin (Eds.), Understanding
Schiff, N. D. (2008). Central thalamic contributions to arousal regulation autobiographical memory: theories and approaches (pp. 114–138).
and neurological disorders of consciousness. Annals of the New York Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Academy of Sciences, 1129(1), 105–118. https://doi.org/10.1196/ St Jacques, P. L., Botzung, A., Miles, A., & Rubin, D. C. (2011).
annals.1417.029 Functional neuroimaging of emotionally intense autobiographical
Schulze, L., Schmahl, C., & Niedtfeld, I. (2016). Neural correlates of memories in post‐traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Psychiatric
disturbed emotion processing in borderline personality disorder: A Research, 45(5), 630–637. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.
multimodal meta‐analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 79(2), 97–106. https:// 10.011
doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.03.027 St Jacques, P. L., Kragel, P. A., & Rubin, D. C. (2011). Dynamic neural
Schulze, L., Schulze, A., Renneberg, B., Schmahl, C., & Niedtfeld, I. (2019). networks supporting memory retrieval. NeuroImage, 57(2), 608–616.
Neural correlates of affective disturbances: A comparative meta‐ https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.039
analysis of negative affect processing in borderline personality St Jacques, P. L., Kragel, P. A., & Rubin, D. C. (2013). Neural networks
disorder, major depressive disorder, and posttraumatic stress supporting autobiographical memory retrieval in posttraumatic stress
disorder. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, disorder. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 13(3),
4(3), 220–232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.11.004 554–566. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415‐013‐0157‐7
Scolari, M., Seidl‐Rathkopf, K. N., & Kastner, S. (2015). Functions of the Stein, D. J., Koenen, K. C., Friedman, M. J., Hill, E., McLaughlin, K. A.,
human frontoparietal attention network: Evidence from neuroima- Petukhova, M., … Borges, G. (2013). Dissociation in posttraumatic
ging. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 1, 32–39. https://doi.org/ stress disorder: Evidence from the world mental health surveys.
10.1016/j.cobeha.2014.08.003 Biological Psychiatry, 73(4), 302–312. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral biopsych.2012.08.022
hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, Steriade, M., & Llinás, R. R. (1988). The functional states of the thalamus
20(1), 11–21. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11 and the associated neuronal interplay. Physiological Reviews, 68(3),
Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H., Kenna, 649–742. https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.1988.68.3.649
H., … Greicius, M. D. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks Storbeck, J., & Clore, G. L. (2008). Affective arousal as information: How
for salience processing and executive control. Journal of Neuroscience, affective arousal influences judgments, learning, and memory. Social
27(9), 2349–2356. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587‐06.2007 and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(5), 1824–1843. https://doi.org/
Seligowski, A. V., Lebois, L. A. M., Hill, S. B., Kahhale, I., Wolff, J. D., 10.1111/j.1751‐9004.2008.00138.x
Jovanovic, T., … Ressler, K. J. (2019). Autonomic responses to fear Strange, B. A., Hurlemann, R., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). An emotion‐induced
conditioning among women with PTSD and dissociation. Depression retrograde amnesia in humans is amygdala‐ and ‐adrenergic‐
and Anxiety, 36, 625–634. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22903 dependent. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
Setlow, B. (1997). The nucleus accumbens and learning and memory. United States of America, 100(23), 13626–13631. https://doi.org/10.
Journal of Neuroscience Research, 49(5), 515–521. https://doi.org/10. 1073/pnas.1635116100
1002/(SICI)1097‐4547(19970901)49:5<515::AID‐JNR1>3.0.CO;2‐E Sturm, W., De Simone, A., Krause, B., Specht, K., Hesselmann, V.,
Sharot, T., & Phelps, E. A. (2004). How arousal modulates memory: Radermacher, I., … Willmes, K. (1999). Functional anatomy of intrinsic
Disentangling the effects of attention and retention. Cognitive, alertness: Evidencefor a fronto‐parietal‐thalamic‐brainstem network
Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4(3), 294–306. in theright hemisphere. Neuropsychologia, 37(7), 797–805. https://doi.
Shin, L. M., Kosslyn, S. M., McNally, R. J., Alpert, N. M., Thompson, W. L., org/10.1016/S0028‐3932(98)00141‐9
Rauch, S. L., … Pitman, R. K. (1997). Visual imagery and perception in Svoboda, E., McKinnon, M. C., & Levine, B. (2006). The functional
posttraumatic stress disorder: A positron emission tomographic neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: A meta‐analysis. Neuropsy-
investigation. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54(3), 233–241. https:// chologia, 44(12), 2189–2208. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.
doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830150057010 2006.05.023
Shin, L. M., McNally, R. J., Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., Rauch, S. L., Talarico, J. M., LaBar, K. S., & Rubin, D. C. (2004). Emotional intensity
Alpert, N. M., … Pitman, R. K. (1999). Regional cerebral blood flow predicts autobiographical memory experience. Memory and Cognition,
during script‐driven imagery in childhood sexual abuse‐related PTSD: 32(7), 1118–1132.
A PET investigation. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156(4), 575–584. Terpou, B. A., Densmore, M., Theberge, J., Frewen, P., McKinnon, M. C., &
https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.156.4.575 Lanius, R. A. (2018). Resting‐state pulvinar‐posterior parietal decou-
Shin, L. M., Orr, S. P., Carson, M. A., Rauch, S. L., Macklin, M. L., Lasko, N. B., … pling in PTSD and its dissociative subtype. Human Brain Mapping, 39,
Cannistraro, P. A. (2004). Regional cerebral blood flow in the amygdala 4228–4240. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24242
THOME ET AL. | 25
Terpou, B. A., Densmore, M., Thome, J., Frewen, P., McKinnon, M. C., & Wheeler, M. A., Stuss, D. T., & Tulving, E. (1997). Toward a theory of
Lanius, R. A. (2019). The Innate Alarm System and Subliminal Threat episodic memory: The frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness.
Presentation in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Neuroimaging of the Psychological Bulletin, 121(3), 331–354. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033‐
Midbrain and Cerebellum. Chronic Stress, 3, 247054701882149. 2909.121.3.331
https://doi.org/10.1177/2470547018821496 Williams, J. M. G., Barnhofer, T., Crane, C., Hermans, D., Raes, F., Watkins,
Thome, J., Hauschild, S., Koppe, G., Liebke, L., Rausch, S., Herzog, J. I., … E., & Dalgleish, T. (2007). Autobiographical memory specificity and
Hermans, D. (2018). Generalisation of fear in PTSD related to prolonged emotional disorder. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 122–148. https://
childhood maltreatment: An experimental study. Psychological Medicine, doi.org/10.1037/0033‐2909.133.1.122
48(13), 2223–2234. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717003713 Willoughby, K. A., Desrocher, M., Levine, B., & Rovet, J. F. (2012). Episodic
Timofeev, I., & Steriade, M. (2004). Neocortical seizures: Initiation, and semantic autobiographical memory and everyday memory during
development and cessation. Neuroscience, 123(2), 299–336. https:// late childhood and early adolescence. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 53.
doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.08.051 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00053
Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology, 26(1), Yan, X., Brown, A. D., Lazar, M., Cressman, V. L., Henn‐Haase, C., Neylan, T.
1–12. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0080017 C., & Yehuda, R. (2013). Spontaneous brain activity in combat related
Tulving, E. (1987). Multiple memory systems and consciousness. Human PTSD. Neuroscience Letters, 547, 1–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.
Neurobiology, 6(2), 67–80. 2013.04.032
Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Yin, Y., Jin, C., Hu, X., Duan, L., Li, Z., Song, M., … Jin, H. (2011). Altered
Psychology, 53, 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53. resting‐state functional connectivity of thalamus in earthquake‐induced
100901.135114 posttraumatic stress disorder: A functional magnetic resonance
Tulving, E. (2007). Are there 256 different kinds of memory. In J. S. Nairne imaging study. Brain Research, 1411, 98–107. https://doi.org/10.1016/
(Ed.), The foundations of remembering: Essays in honor of Henry L. j.brainres.2011.07.016
Roediger (3, pp. 39–52). New York, NY: Psychology Press. Young, D. A., Chao, L., Neylan, T. C., O’Donovan, A., Metzler, T. J., &
Turkeltaub, P. E., Eden, G. F., Jones, K. M., & Zeffiro, T. A. (2002). Meta‐ Inslicht, S. S. (2018). Association among anterior cingulate cortex
analysis of the functional neuroanatomy of single‐word reading: volume, psychophysiological response, and PTSD diagnosis in a
Method and validation. NeuroImage, 16(3), 765–780. Veteran sample. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 155,
van Der Kolk, B. A. (1994). The body keeps the score: Memory and the 189–196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2018.08.006
evolving psychobiology of posttraumatic stress. Harvard Review of Zhang, L., Zheng, H. R., Li, L. J., Li, W. H., Zhang, Y., Hou, C. L., … Zhang, Z. J.
Psychiatry, 1(5), 253–265. (2011). Course‐dependent response of brain functional alterations in
van der Kolk, B. A., & Fisler, R. (1995). Dissociation and the fragmentary men with acute and chronic post‐traumatic stress disorder: A follow‐
nature of traumatic memories: overviewOverview and exploratory up functional magnetic imaging study. Asia‐Pacific Psychiatry, 3(4),
study. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 8(4), 505–525. 192–203. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1758‐5872.2011.00152.x
van Der Werf, Y. D., Jolles, J., Witter, M. P., & Uylings, H. B. (2003). Zhu, X., Helpman, L., Papini, S., Schneier, F., Markowitz, J. C., Van Meter, P. E.,
Contributions of thalamic nuclei to declarative memory functioning. … Neria, Y. (2017). Altered resting state functional connectivity of fear
Cortex, 39(4‐5), 1047–1062. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010‐9452(08) and reward circuitry in comorbid PTSD and major depression. Depression
70877‐3 and Anxiety, 34(7), 641–650. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22594
Vianna, M. R., Szapiro, G., McGaugh, J. L., Medina, J. H., & Izquierdo, I.
(2001). Retrieval of memory for fear‐motivated training initiates
extinction requiring protein synthesis in the rat hippocampus. Proceed-
ings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, SUPPO RTING IN F ORMATION
98(21), 12251–12254. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.211433298
Ward, A. M., Schultz, A. P., Huijbers, W., Van Dijk, K. R., Hedden, T., & Additional supporting information may be found online in the
Sperling, R. A. (2014). The parahippocampal gyrus links the default‐mode Supporting Information section.
cortical network with the medial temporal lobe memory system. Human
Brain Mapping, 35(3), 1061–1073. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22234
Werner, N. S., Meindl, T., Engel, R. R., Rosner, R., Riedel, M., Reiser, M., & Fast,
K. (2009). Hippocampal function during associative learning in patients How to cite this article: Thome J, Terpou BA, McKinnon MC,
with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43(3), Lanius RA. The neural correlates of trauma‐related
309–318. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.03.011 autobiographical memory in posttraumatic stress disorder:
Whalley, M. G., Kroes, M. C. W., Huntley, Z., Rugg, M. D., Davis, S. W., &
A meta‐analysis. Depress Anxiety. 2019;1–25.
Brewin, C. R. (2013). An fMRI investigation of posttraumatic flash-
backs. Brain and Cognition, 81(1), 151–159. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22977
bandc.2012.10.002