Rhetoric and Stylistics: Some Basic Issues in The Analysis of Discourse
Rhetoric and Stylistics: Some Basic Issues in The Analysis of Discourse
Rhetoric and Stylistics: Some Basic Issues in The Analysis of Discourse
net/publication/276191228
CITATIONS READS
3 2,568
1 author:
Leon James
University of Hawaiʻi at Mānoa
173 PUBLICATIONS 791 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Leon James on 10 February 2016.
INTRODUCTION
UTTERANCES AS INDEXICAL EXPRESSIONS
THE NATURE OF STYLISTIC DESCRIPTION
THE ANALYSIS OF THE QUALITY OF DISCOURSE
EPILOGUE: THE MOST BASIC ISSUE OF ALL
REFERENCES
FOOTNOTE
INTRODUCTION
1
the will of the hand that holds it. Do words refer to things, do
utterances denote propositions, or is it that the speaker
himself, using words, refers to this or that, and the writer,
composing phrases and sentences, denotes perceptions and
cognitions?
2
The juxtaposition of sentences in discourse implies relations
between their propositions that are not overtly expressed in
linguistic form. Consider:
(4) You can forget about the golf game. It's raining. How
about bridge? I'll call John and Mary to see they're
interested.
3
while it is possible to define all the words in a language, it is
impossible to specify all the meanings of a single word.
4
pompousness of the expression in (6b) is not mitigated by
any knowledge one might have about the utterer (say, that he
is actually a modest and humble person).
5
use of an elliptical process whose success and effectiveness
depends on a number of factors: the indexical feature of
words whose communicative possibilities are specified by
dictionary-type definitions, the inferential capacities of the
listener, and the presuppositions (background expectations)
which both speaker and listener hold in common about the
speech act--the intentions for which a speaker is held
responsible (accountable) when performing a particular
speech act. In this theoretical context, the nature of stylistic
description (variation) pertains to an explicit account of
these factors as they have been enumerated.
6
purple plum of Syrian origin." A semantic feature analysis
might give the following description for the same word:
"Concrete object; inanimate; edible; fruit." A componential
analysis of the concept category "fuit" might describe a
"damson" as follows: "Arboreal; smooth-skinned; juicy;
sweet; drupaceous." None of the features or components
listed are mentioned directly in the dictionary definition,
although they could be discovered by successively looking
up the entries for the words used in the definition of
"damson" and thereafter. "Plum" yields "edible" and "fruit";
the latter yields "drupaceous," "juicy" and "product"; the
latter yields "thing" which in turn leads to "inanimate," etc.).
But no such mechanical search could yield information as to
the meaning of "damson" in the following sentences:
7
possibilities of words.
8
the listener" (Jakobovits, 1968, p. 12):
9
psychologist and sociologist, and the quality of the
psychological and sociological theories that he has
developed for himself will determine the quality and
effectiveness of his communication act. the psycholinguistic
and sociolinguistic enterprise, as formal disciplines are at
the present time of their development at a stage far less
advanced than the comparable personal subjective
enterprise attained by the average individual who has
developed communicative competence.
10
creativity in the organization and inventiveness of an essay.
Command over the manipulation of complex syntactic
constructions may not be related to the ability to choose
words whose indexical features facilitate the reception of the
intended thought.
11
conventionalized symbols and expressions: fad words, code
words, technical words, proper names, acronyms, greetings,
trite phrases, idioms, proverbs etc. The "difficulty" of words
is not simply a function of familiarity. Thorndike and
Garrettson (1968) report that high school pupils find abstract
words more difficult to understand than concrete words, and
root words are more difficult than compound words. These
differences in difficulty were independent of word-count
frequency. Words differ also in the intensity of their
connotation. Some words like "snake," crime," "death"
evoke intense negative affective responses; others like
"stick," "February," "five" are almost neutral in connotation
(Center for comparative Psycholinguistics, 1969). Some
words have definitions which allow for diffuse and
unspecific reference (e.g. "wonderful," "feathery," "thing,"
"anywhere") while others are restrictive and specific (e.g.
"screwdriver," "melting point," "naturalized").
12
information in their encyclopedias or review it in depth. A
particular poem can be either rich or poor in meaning
depending on the extent to which the reader explores the
indexical possibilities it contains. On the other hand,
discourse in a restricted code contains little indexical
potential, in which case the reader's inferential reasoning
capacities will be limited value.
13
listener to expand or restrict the applicability of the original
declarative sentence; (d) Elaboration-Summarization: to ask
a question which requires elaboration (more wordiness) or
summarization (less wordiness) of the original declarative
sentence; (e) Predicting: formulating a declarative sentence
which anticipates questions and provides answers for them
before they occur.
14
these counts (see the review by DeVito, 1967b). This
approach is sometimes referred to as "stylometrics." It is
generally recognized by users of this approach that type
frequencies are merely indicators of style rather than
inherent features of it. By developing style indicators that
correlate with inherent features that have more face validity,
but are also more subjective and less reliable, the
proponents of this approach hope to increase the objectivity
and reliability of essay grading. For example, Slotnick and
knapp (1967) developed the MOMSR computer program
(Machine Oriented Measurement of Student Writing) which
utilizes some 40 mechanical indicators based on Page (1967)
and Bernstein (1964). In a recent application of the MOMSR
program, Slotnick (personal communication) obtained a
multiple correlation of .67 between teacher evaluations of
style and four of the mechanical measures. The data was
based on 40 essays written by college freshmen on an
assigned topic (literary criticism of a character in a novel).
The four measures were: number of rare words (an indicator
of "vocabulary richness"), number of temporal prepositions
(negative correlation), total number of sentences, and total
number of quotations.
15
traditional measure of sentence length by the "T-unit"
("minimal terminable units"), which is defined as a main or
independent clause together with its subordinate elements.
Hunt found that the length of the T-unit in the compositions
of school children gradually increases as their writing skill
improves. He argues, however, that the maturing of style
does not consist merely of lengthening the T-units but in the
way in which the T-unit is made longer. Hunt concludes that
the significant factor is the development of "sentence
embedding transformations" whereby adjectival clauses are
lengthened by the use of noun clauses and phrases (in place
of nouns and pronouns) and modifiers embedded before and
after nouns. Christensen (1968) questions this conclusion
and argues instead that the more relevant indicator of a
"mature style" is the extensive use (frequency and length) of
free modifiers, especially in the final position, while the base
clause is kept relatively short.
It is clear even from this brief and selective review that the
analysis of discourse can be approached from a large
number of vantage points. In fact, it would appear that
discourse analysis is an open-ended enterprise, in which
relevant features for study can be invented without limit in
number. This is hardly surprising given that the variability of
language is infinite and that new stylistic devices can be
created which have never existed before. These remarks
apply to the approaches developed and used thus far, all of
which are descriptive. It is obvious that no taxonomic
approach can give a complete description of an open-ended
system. Until more powerful theoretical devices are invented,
the analysis of discourse must perforce remain incomplete,
descriptive, merely enumerative.
16
a Hemingway and a James is not of the same sort as the
difference between a high school senior and David
Halberstam.
17
It would seem that the problem of developing evaluation
criteria is the problem of determining what questions the
evaluator wishes to ask about the discourse. Heinberg,
Harms, and Yamada (1969), concerned as they are with
speech communication (see above), asks the question of
whether the speaker can get the listener to respond in a
predetermined way. If he can accomplish this within the
restriction imposed upon the communication situation (time,
number of questions asked, etc.), then his style is
"satisfactory," "effective," etc. for this particular purpose. If
the literary critic decides (by any means whatsoever) that the
intent of a writer in a particular passage is "obviously
intended to suggest an atmosphere of terror and foreboding"
(Hughes and Duhamel, 1966, p. 182), then the first paragraph
of Edgar Allan Poe's The fall of the House of Usher is
"effective," for indeed the trained reader (and possibly the
untrained as well) does "get the message." If the critic
decides that the purpose of metaphors and similes is to
suggest to the reader novel recombinations of perceptions,
then Shakespeare's writings are replete with schemes of this
sort that are supremely effective, certainly insofar as the
trained reader is concerned, and possibly even for the
untrained reader. A professor's lecture might be entertaining
but uninstructive, instructive but boring, or both entertaining
and instructive; then, the lecture is "effective,"
"satisfactory," "creative," "original" depending on what the
critic decides the purpose of the lecture is or ought to be.
18
problem of evaluating any human endeavor. The principles
are the same whether the object is art, a teaching method, a
piece of machinery, or a scientific theory. But, whereas the
decision of the function of a piece of machinery easily
commands common consensus among its users, such
consensus is more difficult to atttain about the decision of
the function of a work of art. This is because the indexical
possibilities are so much richer for a work of art than for a
machine.
19
mathematician and developer of computers, John von
Neumann, has characterized two kinds of "games" in just
terms discussed here for the distinction between mechanical
and creative processes. He defines an "interesting" game as
one whose solution presents some unsolved problems. An
"uninteresting" game is one whose solution is completely
known. Thus, checkers ceased to be an interesting game (in
the mathematical, not psychological sense) when all the
problems connected with playing it were solved. A computer
can now play a perfect game of checkers. The simulation of a
process via computer is an ipso facto demonstration of its
interesting status. The program which controls the
computer's game is the algorithm of the process. It has
become mechanical.
20
"rules" one finds in "how to" type cookbooks of rhetoric and
English composition: the "do's" and "don't's" of
punctuation, of sentence length and subordination, of theme
organization, and so on. the result of such teaching, when it
is successful, can never in and of itself produce anything but
stale writing. The importance of such teaching, when it is
successful, can never in and of itself produce anything but
stale writing. The importance of such teaching should not,
however, be minimized. There are two reasons for this. In the
first place, stale writing of this sort is often more effective for
communication than the undisciplined composition of the
untrained, insensitive individual. In the second place,
teaching of this sort may not impart merely the knowledge of
the algorithms involved.3 The human mind can be of a
staunchly independent spirit: it can resist indoctrination and
control by algorithms. The question whether the teaching of
rhetorical algorithms facilitates or inhibits such
independence or creativeness is one for which we do not
now have the answer. On it hinges the value of our
educational system.
REFERENCES
21
Thought. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins Press, 1965.
22
Levels. Champaign, Ill.: The National Council of Teachers of
English, 1965. (Research Report No. 3.)
23
University of Illinois, Urbana, 1969. (Mimeo.)
FOOTNOTES
JOURNAL ARTICLES
24
STUDENTS ARTICLES
An Evaluation of People's Attitudes Toward Technostress and Techniques on How to Overcome it.
To Fear or not to Fear the Computer
Home
25