Measuring Affective Learning
Measuring Affective Learning
Abstract
Agricultural educators are familiar with the three domains of learning: 1) cognitive, 2) affective,
and 3) psychomotor. When teaching agricultural content, the instructional and assessment
strategies are typically focused on the cognitive domain of learning because of the difficulty in
measuring gains in the affective domain. The purpose of this study was to measure affective
learning after viewing an asynchronously delivered simulation, reflecting (metacognition), and
writing about the experience. Research in agricultural education is devoid of writing as an
assessment tool to measure learning in the affective domain. Content analysis of 83 reflective
writing samples was used to analyze affective learning at the levels of receiving, responding,
valuing, organization, and characterization. It was evident in the reflective writing that all
students participated at the receiving and responding levels. It is much more difficult to assess
students at the higher levels of the affective domain (valuing, organizing, and characterizing).
From the analysis of reflective writing, the researchers recognized and determined that some
students expressed affective learning at higher levels of the affective taxonomy and increased
their level of reflective writing in the process.
learning through the whole-brain processing and internal states that drive us in a
of doing, depicting, and symbolizing (Emig, particular direction (McLeod, 1991). Within
1988). When a learner reflects upon their attribution (motivational) theory, Dweck
thoughts and emotions as a result of an (1989) proposes a framework made up of
instructional sequence, then the nature of the beliefs and values. This framework consists
learning process helps the learner to of students’ beliefs about the nature of
construct meaning from information and competence and the level of their own
experiences. In order to fully understand competence, variables that influence
this study, it is important to review the outcomes, and their ability to achieve those
taxonomy of the affective domain, as well as outcomes. Therefore, students’ beliefs
the use of reflective writing as an about their competence and ability will
assessment tool. impact their learning, emphasizing the
relationship between affect and cognition.
Theoretical Framework The Taxonomy of the Affective Domain
was first developed by Krathwohl et al.,
When we think about beliefs and 1964. This original taxonomy contains five
attitudes, we are on a less trodden path levels, from lowest to highest: 1) receiving
called the affective domain of learning. when the learner is aware and attending the
Researchers studying human behavior instructional event; 2) responding when the
recognize the need to consider both learner reacts to the instructional event or
cognitive and affective dimensions of content; 3) valuing when the learner
learning, and their interrelationships demonstrates a voluntary commitment to the
(McKeachie, 1976; McLeod, 1991; instructional event or content; 4)
Vygotsky, 1962). Perhaps the first step on organization when the learner demonstrates
this path is to operationally define these internalization of a value system; and 5)
terms. “Affect” is often described with characterization when the learner
words such as feelings, emotions, consistently acts within the value system.
motivations, attitudes, and beliefs. Studies This taxonomy was applied to the students’
have noted relationships exist between reflective writing to assess the level of
moods and information storage and retrieval changes in affective learning.
(Bower, 1981). There is evidence that affect Topics presented within an affective
can directly influence cognition. Several framework become very real and relevant in
studies describe relationships between a students’ lives (Rompelman, 2002).
student’s mood and learning, as well as Reflection-in-action is a form of
relationships between moods and metacognition in which the student
information storage and retrieval (Bastick, questions both the unexpected event and the
1982; Bower; Bower & Cohen, 1982; knowledge-in-action that brought on the
Kuiken, 1991; Morris, 1989). unexpected event. Surprises cause us to
Attitude can be defined as “a mental or reflect on the cause of that surprise
neural state of readiness, organized through (Bruning, Schraw, & Ronning, 1999).
experience, exerting a directive or dynamic Reflection encourages students to
influence upon the individual’s response to integrate theory with practice. Boud and
all objects and situations with which it is Walker (1985) define reflection as “those
related” (Allport, 1935, p. 810). Attitude intellectual and affective activities in which
theory contends that the expression of individuals engage to explore their
attitude is a social phenomenon and fits experiences in order to lead to new
within the social constructivist paradigm of understandings and appreciations” (p. 19).
learning (McLeod, 1991). Reflection may include the sharing of
Beliefs can be defined as “judgments of feelings, observations, ideas, and reactions
the credibility of conceptualization” regarding a learning activity. While
(McLeod, 1991, p. 7). Values on the other reflection can take many forms,
hand are representations of either positive or reflective writing is often used to get
negative notions (McLeod, 1991; Rokeach, students to delve into subject matter at a
1986). Motivation can denote both external deeper level.
Journal of Agricultural Education 25 Volume 47, Number 3, 2006
Boyd, Dooley, & Felton Measuring Learning in the…
can be confirmed or made more or less participate at these lower levels. It is much
plausible than others using a fairly more difficult to assess students at the
standard range of internal and external higher levels of the affective domain
(social criteria). (p. 401) (valuing, organizing, and characterizing).
Valuing is the worth that the learner
Therefore, this study cannot be separated gives to the content. Action verbs
from its context and the descriptive describing this level include differentiate,
examples in the findings allow the reader to propose, appreciation for, and concern for.
draw their own inferences of transferability. One respondent expressed:
families, children won’t start working (world hunger). I now see how important
until the age of sixteen or later. Even it would be to have a job in international
later in life, your parents are still there agricultural development to help people
for you and would do anything for you. around the world that work so hard just
In these third world countries, you start to get by. (14)
working from the moment that you can
walk. It’s not that your parents are being Some students were moved to action by the
cruel, but because they had to do it to activity, “This really encouraged me to get
survive. (13) involved in any way I can even while still in
school to try and alleviate this problem in
Other students noted that “…other our world” (5). As the students reached this
people around the world are struggling affective level, their reflective writing
just to have enough to eat, while we gained more depth. These students
throw away millions of pounds of food used dialogic reflection (Hatton & Smith,
everyday” (29), and 1995).
discernment of whether others share their positioned for inclusion to join in this line of
level of awareness. inquiry.
This research demonstrated that
descriptive reflection was an appropriate References
assessment tool for affective learning. In
addition, it was noted that as students Abrahamson, M. (1983). Social research
increased their level of the affective domain, methods. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice
they also reflected at a deeper level. Some Hall.
students’ writings at the characterization
level moved from descriptive to dialogic Allport, G. W. (1935). Attitudes. In C.
reflection, and perhaps a bit to critical Murchison (Ed.), Handbook of
reflection. While not a specific purpose of social psychology, (pp. 798-844).
this study, the findings imply that a Worcester, MA: Clark University
relationship may exist between student’s Press.
level of reflective writing and the higher
levels of the affective domain. At the Bastick, T. (1982). Intuition: How we
higher levels of affect, some students’ think and act. New York: John Wiley and
reflections were more conversations with Sons.
themselves (dialogic) while others
were moved to question why society is Berg, B. L. (2001). Qualitative research
not doing more to improve the condition methods for the social sciences. Needham
of those in need (critical reflection). Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Further study is needed to examine the
relationship between levels of reflection and Bloom, B., & Krathwohl, D. (1956).
affect. Taxonomy of educational objectives: The
Instructional implications may exist for classification of educational goals, by a
prompting higher levels of reflective writing committee of college and university
in agricultural educations courses. examiners. Handbook I: Cognitive Domain.
Instructors should consider the use of New York: Longman, Green.
reflective writing, both as an instructional
tool to improve learners’ cognitive models, Bohlin, R. M. (1998). The affective
as well as an assessment tool to measure domain: A model of learner-instruction
changes in attitudes, beliefs, values, and interaction. Proceedings of Selected
motivations. Research and Development Presentations.
Affective learning and assessment is an The National Convention of the Association
under represented area of research in for Educational Communications and
agricultural education. There is a need for Technology (AECT).
additional research examining the
relationship between all three domains Borg, W. R., & Gall, M. D. (1989).
of learning: cognition, affect, and Educational research: An introduction (5th
psychomotor. Implications for our Ed.). White Plains, NY: Longman.
profession exist in terms of instructional
strategies and content delivery. Retention Boud, D., & Walker, D. (1985). Barriers
and transfer of agricultural content to reflection on experience. In D. Boud, R.
could be enhanced with the inclusion of Keough, and D. Walker (Eds.), Reflection:
affective and psychomotor dimensions. Turning experience into learning (pp. 18-
Furthermore, Krathwohl et al., (1964) 40). London: Kogan Page.
admitted difficulty in placing learning
into specific levels within the affective Bower, G. (1981). Mood and memory.
domain. As a result, other theorists American Psychologist, 36, 129-148.
have proposed the need for a new taxonomy
(McLeod, 1991; Bohlin, 1998). Bower, G., & Cohen, P. R. (1982).
Further research is needed in these Emotional influences in memory and
areas and agricultural educators are thinking: Data and theory. In M. S. Clark &
Journal of Agricultural Education 30 Volume 47, Number 3, 2006
Boyd, Dooley, & Felton Measuring Learning in the…
S. T. Fiske (Eds.), Affect and Cognition (pp. developing behavioral objectives. New
291-331). Hillsdale N.J.: Erlbaum. York: McKay.
Bruning, R. H., Schraw, G. J., & Hatton, N., & Smith, D. (1995).
Ronning, R. R. (1999). Cognitive Reflection in teacher education: Towards
psychology and instruction. Upper Saddle definition and implementation. The
River, NJ: Merrill Publishing. University of Sydney: School of Teaching
and Curriculum Studies. Retrieved January
Cano, J. (1990). The relationship 23, 2005 from http://alex.edfac.usyd.
between instruction and student performance edu.au/LocalResource/study1/hattonart.html
at the various levels of cognition among
selected Ohio production agriculture Hodder, I. (1994). The interpretation of
programs. Journal of Agricultural documents and material culture (pp. 393-
Education, 31(2), 74-80. 402). In N. K. Denzin, & Y. S. Lincoln,
(Eds.) Handbook of qualitative research.
Cano, J., & Newcomb, L. H. (1990). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Cognitive level of instruction and student
performance among selected Ohio Huba, M. E., & Freed, J. E. (2000).
production agriculture programs. Journal of Learner-centered assessment on college
Agricultural Education, 31(1), 46-51. campuses: Shifting the focus from teaching
to learning. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Clark, M. S., & Fiske, S. T. (1982).
Affect and cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Kember, D. (1999). Determining the
Erlbaum. level of reflective thinking from students'
written journals using a coding scheme
Day, T. M., Raven, M. R., & Newman, based on the work of Mezirow.
M. E. (1998). The effects of world wide International Journal of Lifelong Education,
web instruction and traditional instruction 18(1), 18-30.
and learning styles on achievement and
changes in student attitudes in a technical Krathwohl, D., Bloom, B., & Masia, B.
writing in an agricommunications course. (1964). Taxonomy of educational objectives:
Journal of Agricultural Education, 39(4), The classification of educational goals.
65-75. Handbook II: Affective domain. New York:
David McKay Co.
Dweck, C. S. (1989). Motivation, In A.
Lesgold & R. Glaser, (Eds.) Foundations for Kuiken, D. Ed. (1991). Mood and
a psychology of education, (pp. 87-136). memory: Theory, research, and application.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.