Unit 1 Introduction To Social Behaviour - Concept, Perceiving and Behaving, Social Interaction
Unit 1 Introduction To Social Behaviour - Concept, Perceiving and Behaving, Social Interaction
Unit 1 Introduction To Social Behaviour - Concept, Perceiving and Behaving, Social Interaction
Structure
1.0 Introduction
1.1 Objectives
1.2 Definition of Person Perception and Impression Formation
1.2.1 Impression Formation
1.2.2 Impression Formation through Stereotyping
1.2.3 Non-Verbal Behavioural Cues
1.2.4 Detecting Deception in the Self-Presentations of Others
1.2.5 Forming Personality Impressions
1.2.6 Central Traits and Forming Impressions
1.3 Biases in Personality Judgment
1.3.1 Implicit Personality Theory
1.3.2 Self-Concept and the False Concept Bias
1.3.3 Positivity Bias
1.3.4 Negativity Bias
1.3.5 Primacy and Recency Effects
1.3.6 Making Attributions
1.4 Heider’s Naïve Psychology
1.4.1 Primary Dimensions of Causal Experience
1.5 Theories of Attributions
1.5.1 Correspondent Inference Theory
1.5.2 Kelly’s Co Variation Theory/Model
1.5.3 Biases in Attribution.
1.5.4 The Role of Perceptual Salience
1.5.5 The Actor – Observer Effect
1.5.6 Social Perceivers being Good or Bad
1.6 Social Interaction
1.6.1 Interdependent Sets of Factors in Social Situations
1.6.2 Factors Related to the Prevailing Relations Among Participants
1.6.3 Set of Factors Pertaining to the Characteristics of the Activity, Task, Problem or
Occasion at Hand
1.6.4 Set of Factors Pertaining to thel Ocation and Facilities
1.6.5 Set of Factors Pertaining to the Individual Participants Relation to the above
3 Sets of Factors
1.6.6 Differential Effects of Social Status
1.6.7 Awareness in “Verbal Conditioning” Experiments
1.6.8 Milgram’s Experiments on Reacrtions to Authority
1.6.9 Actions of Other Persons Like Ourselves
1.6.10 Agreement with Another’s Judgment in Situations Varying in Structure
1.6.11 Moving with the Crowd has Limits
5
Social Behaviour 1.7 Let Us Sum Up
1.8 Unit End Questions
1.9 Suggested Readings
1.0 INTRODUCTION
We are constantly assessing and perceiving the feelings and intentions of other
people and our responses are determined by these perceptions. The term ‘person
perception’ refers to the processes by which we form impressions of other people.
The impressions and evaluations of other people may not be formed through
direct sensory information alone. Our subjective judgments and inferences also
play a role in it. For example when we meet a person briefly, we form an
impression of that individual. Such initial assessments of personality are often
based on the physical appearance, verbal behaviour as well as other expressive
behaviours. In such a situation, even though the available information is limited,
we do form a definite impression about the personality of the person. These
impressions are strong and lasting. Often we assign attributes to a person based
on class or category to which he or she belongs. This phenomenon is known as
‘stereotyping.’ That is, we first categorise a person and place that person in a
class or category and then attribute characteristics and traits as belonging to that
class or category persons. In this unit we will be dealing with how impressions
are formed and created, how do we perceive, how our behaviour is influenced by
our perception. We will also be dealing with social interaction, how we get
attracted to talk toward persons based on certain principles such as proximity,
past experience with similar persons etc.
1.1 OBJECTIVES
After reading this unit, you will be able to:
Define social perception and behaving;
Explain the finer details of interaction in social situations;
Describe the characteristic features of perceiving and behaving;
Explain how we form impressions of others; and
Analyse factors contributing to perception.
How do we “size up” other people during initial encounters? How do we arrive
at conclusions about their character, needs, and abilities? As these two hypothetical
situations illustrate, the process of gathering information about others can be of
the most vital importance to our health and safety. Every day we judge people,
and often react to the based on very little information.
This finding indicates that in forming first impression of others, people initially
tend to rely upon the more superficial aspects of their personal qualities. Some
of the most universally salient physical features are those based on race, sex,
age, and attractiveness, Because categorising others by these physical features is
done so frequently that it becomes habitual and automatic, often occurring without
conscious thought or effort.
Social categorisation does not typically end with merely grouping people into
different categories. Within these social categories, there are certain beliefs about 7
Social Behaviour the individuals’ personalities, abilities, and motives. These are often learned from
others, and like other cognitive frameworks, stereotype significantly influences
how we process social information.
That is, once a stereotype is activated, we have a tendency to see people within
that social category as possessing the traits or characteristics associated with the
stereotyped group. In this sense, stereotypes are fixed ways of thinking about
people that puts them into categories and doesn’t allow for individual variation.
These social judgments are arrived at solely on the basis of perceiving static
cues and not based on anything else about the individuals. Yet once a person is
categorised into a particular social grouping, people tend to think they know a
good deal more about that individual.
If people believed that every member of a particular social group possessed all
the attributes stereotypically associated with it, this sort of all-or-none thinking
could easily be considered irrational. Going back to our hypothetical situation of
seeking help from heavily armed police officers, very few people believe that all
police officers are helpful and fair-minded in their service to the community.
Those who would insist that this is the case could justifiably be thought of as
being out of touch with reality.
One of the essential functions of stereotyped thinking is that it is fast and gives
us a basis for immediate action in uncertain circumstances. In a way stereotypes
are “shortcuts to thinking” that provide us with rich and distinctive information
about individuals we do not personally know. Stereotyping also appears to “free
up” cognition for other tasks. Stereotyped thinking allows people to cognitively
engage in other necessary activities. Stereotyping has a resource preserving effect.
Since stereotyping helps spend less energy in cognitive resource, the perceiver
has more energy to redirect to more pressing issues.
Facial Expression
Facial expressions reveal a person’s true feelings. Facial expressions not only
play an important role in communication, but certain emotional expressions are
innate and thus are understood throughout the world.
Paul Ekman and his colleagues (1987) asked participants from ten different
Western and non-Western cultures to identify the six primary emotions displayed
by White men and women in a series of photograph. Across cultures there was a
high percentage of agreement.
Being able to read the emotions of others through facial expressions allows people
to predict the other person’s behavioural intentions. For example, “Do they mean
to harm me?”
Reading facial expressions also helps understand how others are interpreting the
world (“Why are they afraid? Are we all in danger in this situation?”) At the
8
same time it must be remembered that all people do not attend equally to all Introduction to Social
Behaviour – Concept
facial expressions, but rather exhibit the most sensitivity to those that would Perceiving and Behaving,
give them the best chances of survival. Social Interaction
Eye Contact
People who are in love and who are in competition tend to lover or opponent
respectively. This latter form of eye contact, known as staring, does not indicate
amorous intentions. Rather, it is recognised as a sign of dominance seeking and
aggression. Most people become tense and nervous if others stare at them, and
they will withdraw from the situation in order to escape the attention. If withdrawal
is not possible, people will often avoid eye contact in an apparent attempt to
reduce potential conflict. This gaze aversion is often interpreted by others as a
submissive gesture.
Body Movements
People who walk with a good deal of hip sway, knee bending, loose jointedness,
and body bounce are perceived to be younger and more powerful than those who
walk with less pronounced gaits. A number of studies have demonstrated that
when people are emotionally aroused, they tend to engage in a greater number of
body movements than when they are calm.
People often infer underlying emotional states by reading the body movements
during social interaction. For example, in a creative analysis of dance characters
in classical ballet, the researchers found that the body and arm displays of the
threatening characters were more diagonal or angular, whereas those of the warm
characters were more rounded. In subsequent studies, college students who were
asked to evaluate various geometric shapes judged those with diagonal shape to
be more bad, powerful, and active than those that were rounded. These findings
suggest that people do analyse the shape of large-scale body movements to betterin
order to determine another person’s behavioural intentions. It appears then that
body movements, in addition to facial gestures and eye contact, convey quite a
wide variety of information to others that may well have a significant impact on
the impression formation process.
Despite these attempts at self-control, the face can reveal the lie for the acutely
attentive perceiver. Before people can monitor and mask their facial expressions
following an emotion provoking event, they emit micro expressions. These
expressions are fleeting facial signals lasting only a few tenths of a second, and
are difficult to suppress. Due to the difficulty in masking this observable
expression of emotion, micro expressions can be quite revealing about one’s
actual state of mind. The eyes can also reveal the lie. When individuals avoid the
gaze of others, or blink frequently, this may be a signal of deception.
Besides the face, we also rely on the sound of another’s voice and the subtle
movements of the body. When people deceive, the pitch in their voice often rises
slightly, and their speech is filled with many pauses and other sentence hesitations.
Deception is also signaled by fidgety movements of the hands and feet, and
restless shifts in body posture.
In his classic study. Asch (1946) told participants they would hear a list of discrete
traits that belonged to a particular person and that they should try to form an
impression based on this information. For some participants, the following traits
were then presented: intelligent skillful-industrious, warm determined, practical,
cautious. For other participants, the trait warm was replaced with trait cold, but
10
otherwise everything else was identical. Those who had been told that the Introduction to Social
Behaviour – Concept
hypothetical person was warm rated him as significantly more generous, Perceiving and Behaving,
humorous, sociable, and popular than those who had been told that he was cold. Social Interaction
Based on these results, Asch concluded that warm and cold are central traits that
significantly influence overall impression formation. In a warm and caring
individual, being industrious and determined would likely carry positive
connotations, but in a cold and heart-less individual these same traits carry very
different, more negative, connotations.
In and elaboration of this research, Harold Kelley (1950) attempted to determine
how these same traits might influence impression formation in a real-life situation.
When students arrived at their college psychology class, they were greeted by a
representative to the instructor who told them they were to have a guest lecturer
that day because the regular instructor was out of town. The representative led
some students to believe that the soon-to-arrive lecturer was a rather warm person,
and other students were led to expect a rather cold individual. The lecture then
appeared and led the class in a 20-minute discussion. Results indicated the those
given the “warm” pre information not only rated the lecturer positively but also
the lecture as good . On then other hand those who were told the lecturer will be
cold rated the lecture also not interesting and rated then lecturer somewhat
negatively.
This tendency to direct attention to negatively evaluated stimuli, like the tendency
to notice fearful and angry faces in a crowd, is believed to have survival value
for human beings.
Why does early information figure more prominently than later information in
our impression of others? One possible explanation is that the early bits of
information we learn about another provide a cognitive schema or mental
“outline,” This we use to process later information. If the later information
contradicts this schema, we are likely to ignore it. Research suggests that the
primacy effect is particularly strong when people are given little time to make
judgments and are not under a great deal of pressure to be correct.
Although the primacy effect regularly occurs in social perception, it can sometimes
be reversed if people are warned against making hasty judgments or told that
they will be asked to justify their impressions of a target person. In such
circumstances, the last bits of information learned may be given greater weight
than earlier information. This is known as the recency effect.
Thus, if you have a shy friend whom you’d like to introduce to a potential romantic
partner, you might guard against the primacy effect in the following manner.
You could inform this person that your friend is somewhat shy and that her true
personality doesn’t always shine through in first meetings. If this person takes
your advice, he will ignore some of the early awkwardness and social fumbling
in the friend and pay more attention to what he learns about her after she feels
more comfortable with him.
In the example of trash that was given in the earlier paragraphs, the man’s actions
were attributed by me to external factors. brought by adverse weather.
Bernard Weiner and his colleagues expanded Heider’s primary distinction between
the internal and external locus of causality to include questions about stability
and controllability. Stable causes are permanent and lasting, whereas unstable
causes are temporary and fluctuating. This stable/unstable dimension is
independent of the direction of causality. Some causes, called dispositional, are
both internal and stable. (“She insulted me because she is rude”). Other cause is
considered to be internal but unstable (“She insulted me because she has a
headache”). Likewise, some causes are seen as external and stable (“She insulted
me because I, the external factor, rub people the wrong way”), whereas others
are perceived as external and unstable (“She insulted me because the weather
conditions that day made her job very difficult”).
On the other hand how much one chooses to study depends on internal, unstable
and controllable factors. Sometimes a controllable factor like effort will only get
you so far in academic achievement. Then we must consider internal factors that
are seen as uncontrollable, such as innate intellectual ability (stable) and one’s
mood during exam time (unstable).
External factors that are considered controllable might be rather stable, such as
knowing that the person’s teacher looks for specific definitions of terms and use
of examples in test answer. Or they might also be unstable, such as others deciding
to help the individual prepare for an exam (this help is presumably under their
control). Finally, the difficulty of tests given by the teacher would be perceived
as being external, stable, and uncontrollable, whereas luck is an external, unstable
factor typically perceived as uncontrollable.
Since Heider’s initial formulations, other social psychologist have expanded upon
his insights and developed formal attributions theories.
However social behaviour being ambiguous, and the causes not being so obvious,
one cannot make attributions with much confidence. Therefore, to guide them in
their attempts to inter personal characteristics from behaviour, Jones and Davis
stated that people use several logical rules of thumb.
Taking these rules into account, according to Jones and Davis’ theory, people are
most likely to conclude that other people’s actions reflect underlying dispositional
traits (that is, they are likely to make correspondent inferences) when the actions
are perceived to (1) be low in social desirability, (2) be freely chosen, and (3)
result in unique, noncommon effects.
16
One theory that specifically attempts to explain attributions derived from multiple Introduction to Social
Behaviour – Concept
observational points and details the processes for making external as well as Perceiving and Behaving,
internal attributions is Harold Kelley’s (1967) co variation model. Social Interaction
Kelley was also of the view that human beings are rational and logical observers,
acting much like naïve scientists in the manner in which they tested their
hypotheses about the behaviour of others. According to Kelley, people make
attributions by using the co variation principle. This principle states that for
something to be the cause of a particular behaviour, it must be present when the
behaviour occurs and must be absent when the behaviour does not occur. In
other words, the cause and observed effect must “co vary.”
Whenever there are several possible causal explanations for a particular event,
we tend to be much less likely to attribute the effect to any particular cause.
In assessing co variation, Kelley stated that people rely upon three basic kinds of
information. (i) Consensus information. This deals with the extent to which others
react in the same way to some stimulus or entity as the person whose actions we
are attempting to explain. (ii) Consistency information concerns the extent to
which the person reacts to this stimulus or entity in the same way on other
occasions, (iii) The distinctiveness information refers to the extent to which the
person reacts in the same way to other, different stimuli or entities.
Suppose while lecturing the teacher finds one of the students fast asleep. The
teacher wonders why the student slept, was it the lecture was boring? Or was it
that the students had late night and did not sleep? The co variation model prefects
that the teacher would seek an attribution by gathering consensus, consistency,
and distinctiveness information. For consensus, he would look at the behaviour
of his other students, that if all others also sleep. To find out the consistency
factor, whether this student slept in the class also in the past. To decide about the
distinctiveness, whether the he student’s behaviour in other professors’ classes
also was the same (sleeping). Or does he fall asleep only in this teacher’s class.
Both correspondent inference theory and the co variation model have significantly
advanced the original insights of Heider by attempting to better understand how
people make inferences about the causes of behaviour. In its original form
correspondent inference theory dealt primarily with assigning meaning to single
instances of behaviour, whereas the co variation model was designed to finds
out how meaning is assigned to a sequence of behaviour over time. Both theories
assume that people are rational and logical observers, acting like naïve scientists
by testing hypotheses about the location of causality for social events. Yet how
logical are we really in our daily attributions?
Consider one example of this particular cognitive bias in operation. Ross and his
colleagues (1977) devised a simulated TV quiz game in which students were
randomly assigned to serve the role of “quizmaster” or “ contestants .” the
17
Social Behaviour quizmasters were told to think up ten challenging but fair question, and the
contestants were told to answer as many of them as they could. Under such
conditions the contestants answered only four of the ten questions correctly.
Despite the fact that the quizmaster role gave students playing that part a decided
advantage, the contestants failed to discount or take this external factor into
account in assigning a causal explanation for the quiz show’s results. Contestants
saw the quizmasters as far more knowledgeable than themselves, even though
they knew the quizmasters had enjoyed the great advantage of controlling what
questions were asked in the quiz. Observers who watched the game, but were
not directly involved in the outcome, also rated the quizmasters as more
knowledgeable than the contestants.
Even if our response is one of sympathetic caring for unfortunate others, the
assignment of dispositional blame will influence the type of solutions we as a
society implement for these people. That is, if we attribute the difficulties of
unfortunate others to personal defects rather than to their circumstances, it is
likely that the treatment programs will focus on changing individuals and not on
improving the conditions of their social environment. Yet if the individuals in
these treatment programs are members of particular social groups in which failure
is often due to discrimination rather than to personal defects (like ethnic minorities
and women), our attempted interventions may prove to be psychologically
damaging.
The basic argument here is that people generally expect to succeed and therefore
are more willing to accept responsibility when it occurs. Based on Kelley’s
covariation model, this explanation contends that when people do succeed, the
success is low in distinctiveness and high in consistency. Therefore, people will
make an internal attribution.
In summary, then people tend to assume more credit for success than responsibility
for failure, and the most common explanation for this effect is motivational, that
is a desire to enhance and protect self-esteem.
Then there is the perspective of Tversky and Kahneman that we are cognitively
“lazy” and susceptible to a number of biases that distort our view of social reality.
Whether it is in first impressions, one-shot attributions, or attributions derived
from multiple observations over time, there are many points in the social judgment
process at which problems can arise. Overall, it appears as though human beings
are not as rational in their information processing as once thought by attribution
theorists.
In a complex and ever changing world, given that we are so predisposed to make
such a wide variety of errors in social perception, the question arises as to how
do humans manage. One possible answer is that our social world is much more
flexible and dynamic than the static and artificial laboratory conditions that
characterize social psychological research.
In a laboratory study, once a research participant makes a judgment error, it
becomes a “data point,” frozen in time, but in the course of everyday life, people
are constantly revising their social assessments due to feedback from the
environment.
As a result of this flexibility, many of the social perception errors committed in
the “real world” are corrected through normal interaction with others. For
example, someone may meet you and, based on that limited encounter, form a
certain impression of you. Another person, upon hearing of that impression, may
provide new meaningful information that “redefines” you in the first person’s
eyes. This evolution of social reality is ongoing and can be extremely forgiving
of individual judgments errors, so that others can arrive at an “efficient definition”
of you that can be used in the social world. Consistent
19
Social Behaviour Another perspective on cognitive errors that has recently been given greater
credence is that these “mistakes” are actually often beneficial to the person’s
health and welfare. According to this view, an important reason for these errors
is that unlike computers, people have an investment in their own self-beliefs and
their beliefs about others, making motivational biases likely in social perception.
Through such biases we often can justify our self-concepts and our world view,
making it possible for us to more confidently engage in social interaction and
meet daily challenges. These cognitive biases may often create a healthier
psychological buffer from some of life’s harsher realities than a more rational
mode might.
In the final analysis, our judgments of other should not be expected to be any
more accurate or efficient those are our judgments of ourselves. Just as we have
a need for consistency when assessing our own self-beliefs, we also express that
need in social perception. When we are faced with contradictory information,
our inclination is to distort or explain away the contradictions.
Have you ever observed a person who thought that he was completely alone? He
may act very differently than he does when at an office, school, or social gathering.
The problem may be defined operationally for study purposes. We shall define
the differential effect of a social situation as the difference between behaviour
when an individual is in a social situation and when he is alone. This operational
definition can be put into a simple formula:
D.E. = Bs – Ba
Where Bs refers to behaviour in a social situation and Ba to behaviour alone.
D.E. = Differential Effects
The difference defines the differential effects (D.E.) of a social situation.
You will note that the differential effect has a positive sign if the social situation
produces an increase in intensity, quantity, or quality (improvement). Conversely,
it may have a negative sign under opposite situations.
The differential effects of two or more social situations may also be compared as
given below
D.E. = Bs1 – Bs2
What produces differential effects in a social situation? We learned as a general
principle that psychological patterning, hence behaviour, is always a product of
interacting influences from the environment and from the participating person.
They may be fellow members of the same group (family, chums, club)., members
of different groups, or
They may form varying combinations in terms of group membership. They may
be participating in a collective interaction situation.
Such tasks lack objective structure in varying degrees. Included in this set of
factors are communications or instructions relative to the problem or task.
At times, the person’s cultural background defines the task or problem for him.
At others, there are also explicit instructions from other persons as to how to he
have or to interpret the problem. In experiments, instructions by the experimenter
serve this purpose.
21
Social Behaviour
1.6.4 Set of Factors Pertaining to the Location and Facilities
Interaction does not take place in a vacuum. Material culture provides many
different locations and facilities, some of them specific to certain activities,
Cultural norms define appropriate places for interaction and task performance,
There are appropriate and inappropriate settings for making love, for worshipping,
for learning, and for working. There are tasks and problems that cannot be solved
unless certain facilities are available. Some locations are crowded by other people
not participating in the interaction. Others are so noisy that conversation is
impossible. As we shall see, a psychological laboratory with equipment, recording
instruments, one-way mirrors, and assistants in white coats produces a distinctive
atmosphere that unmistakably affects the interaction of research subject and
experimenter.
These four sets of factors provide a scheme for analysing the frame of reference
in any social situation. Each of the four sets of factors has demonstrable effects
on behaviour, but variations in any one factor need not produce one-to–one
changes in behaviour. The factors in a social situation are interrelated, hence
function interdependently. The social situation forms a pattern of stimulation
through the selective processing of an individual with a cultural background,
with motives and attitudes relevant to the situation.
Acting “In the name of Science….” Milgram studied the conditions in which Ss
would agree or refuse to administer increasingly severe electric shocks to another
person when that person erred in learning a list of paired words. The learner was
actually in league with E, having instructed to give wrong answers and to demand
with increasing insistence as the shock increased that the experiment be stopped.
The shock generator (which actually only delivered a sample shock to convince
S of its authenticity) was marked from 15 (“slight shock”) to 450 volts (“danger:
severe shock”). S was instructed to drill the learner and to increases the shock on
each error. At 300 volts, the learner refused to answer and demanded to be freed.
E commanded them to continue. S’s score was assigned on the basis of the
maximum intensity he delivered, a score of 30 representing the highest voltage
and 0 representing unwillingness to administer any shock. Ss were mature males
in samples stratified by age and socio-economic rank.
The first condition studied was proximity of the learner. The decrease in the
average intensity of shock delivered when the “victim” was in another room (but
pounded on the wall,) when only his voice could be heard, when he was 1½ feet
away, and when S was ordered to force the victim’s hand on the shock plate after
he had removed it. In the latter variation, 70 percent of the Ss refused to continue
the experiment, as compared with 34 percent when the victim was in the next
room.
23
Social Behaviour By moving the experiment from Yale University to an office building in another
town, Milgram hoped to assess the effect of the experiment’s location. The
experiment was presented at the work of a firm called “Research Associates”
(which probably is a prestigeful title.). At yale, 65 percent delivered the maximum
shock while 48 percent did so in the office building.
In the early stages, Milgram was sufficiently surprised at the extent of compliance
with demands to increase the shock that he made a check to see whether other
competent persons would have anticipated the results. He described the
experimental setup and procedures to 40 psychiatrists, asking them to predict
the percentage who would increase the shock to each level. The predictions
underestimated considerably the amount of shock actually administered. The
psychiatrists predicted that only about one-tenth of a percent would deliver the
maximum shock, while an average of 62 percent actually did.
Blake reported that one S agreeing to serve when the assistant refused said that
he was interested in psychology and had been hoping to have the opportunity to
serve as subject. The example of prior attitude reminds us of the importance of
internal factors in the frame of reference. If the experiment had been performed
during final examination week, the assistant’s compliance or refusal might have
had very little effect.
Another factor that can affect whether S will be influenced by the spoken
judgments of another person in a highly unstructured situation is his feeling of
certainty-uncertainly or confidence-lack-of-confidence in his judgments. In
general and unstructured situation arouses uncertainly in some degree-unease
and even anxiety in extreme cases. Measures that reduce the uncertainty also
reduce the likelihood that S will adopt the standard provided by another person.
For example, when S is told either that his judgments are correct and/or that the
other person is unreliable in performing such tasks, he is unlikely to be influenced
by the judgments of the other person.
25
Social Behaviour Asch’s question was: what would happen when social consensus contradicted
clear-cut perceptual evidence? He found that 32 percent of the total estimates on
the critical trials (when the majority erred) were errors in the direction of the
majority, However, individual differences were great, of 50 naïve Ss, 13 did not
err at all, 15 made one to three errors in the majority direction, 7 made four to
five, 11 made six to nine errors, and only 4 Ss made 10 or 11 errors out of a
possible total of 12.
Of those who went along with the majority. Asch found very few who were not
aware that their estimates erred toward the majority. The remainder squared their
experience with their actions by deciding that there must be some illusion or
inadequacy in their own perception, or that it was better to be wrong than appear
“different” in the situation. Most of those who did not err reported considerable
discomfiture and conflict at the experience, despite their conviction that they
were correct.
In short, Asch’s experiments show that, despite severe jars to the person’s
confidence, there are limits to the extent of influence that can be exerted by a
majority in a transitory social situation. These limits are determined by (1) the
objective differences between the stimuli (smaller differences being more
unstructured); (2) the extent of the discrepancy between the correct choice and
the choice of the majority; and (3) the presence or absence of person’s giving
erroneous judgments and their number.
Self Assessment Questions
1) Describe correspondent inference theory.
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
2) Explain role of perceptual salience.
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
3) What is social interaction?
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
...............................................................................................................
26
Introduction to Social
1.7 LET US SUM UP Behaviour – Concept
Perceiving and Behaving,
Social Interaction
The process of perception enables us to understand the physical world and respond
to it in a meaningful way. However, besides the physical environment, perceptual
processes are also involved in our understanding of people, which in turn shape
our social interactions in various situations in life. Impression Formation is the
process by which one integrates various source of information about another
into an overall judgment. The process of forming impressions is viewed by social
psychologists as a dynamic one with judgments being continually updated in
response to new information. It is analogous to building a “working model” of a
person and then using this as a guideline in your actions toward him or her. The
model works if your mental representation of the person accurately predicts his
or her behaviour.
Muzafer Sherif and Carolyn W. Sherif (1969), Social Psychology, Harper & Row,
Publishers New York, Evanson.
28