2019 - Van Aert - Publication Bias Examined in Meta-Analyses From Psychology and Medicine - A Meta-Meta-Analysis

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 32

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Publication bias examined in meta-analyses


from psychology and medicine: A meta-meta-
analysis
Robbie C. M. van Aert ID1*, Jelte M. Wicherts1, Marcel A. L. M. van Assen1,2

1 Department of Methodology and Statistics, Tilburg University, Tilburg, the Netherlands, 2 Department of
Sociology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands

a1111111111 * [email protected]
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111 Abstract
a1111111111
Publication bias is a substantial problem for the credibility of research in general and of
meta-analyses in particular, as it yields overestimated effects and may suggest the exis-
tence of non-existing effects. Although there is consensus that publication bias exists, how
OPEN ACCESS strongly it affects different scientific literatures is currently less well-known. We examined
evidence of publication bias in a large-scale data set of primary studies that were included in
Citation: van Aert RCM, Wicherts JM, van Assen
MALM (2019) Publication bias examined in meta- 83 meta-analyses published in Psychological Bulletin (representing meta-analyses from
analyses from psychology and medicine: A meta- psychology) and 499 systematic reviews from the Cochrane Database of Systematic Re-
meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 14(4): e0215052. https://
views (CDSR; representing meta-analyses from medicine). Publication bias was assessed
doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052
on all homogeneous subsets (3.8% of all subsets of meta-analyses published in Psychologi-
Editor: Malcolm R. Macleod, University of
cal Bulletin) of primary studies included in meta-analyses, because publication bias meth-
Edinburgh, UNITED KINGDOM
ods do not have good statistical properties if the true effect size is heterogeneous.
Received: August 1, 2018
Publication bias tests did not reveal evidence for bias in the homogeneous subsets. Overes-
Accepted: March 26, 2019 timation was minimal but statistically significant, providing evidence of publication bias that
Published: April 12, 2019 appeared to be similar in both fields. However, a Monte-Carlo simulation study revealed that
the creation of homogeneous subsets resulted in challenging conditions for publication bias
Copyright: © 2019 van Aert et al. This is an open
access article distributed under the terms of the methods since the number of effect sizes in a subset was rather small (median number of
Creative Commons Attribution License, which effect sizes equaled 6). Our findings are in line with, in its most extreme case, publication
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
bias ranging from no bias until only 5% statistically nonsignificant effect sizes being pub-
reproduction in any medium, provided the original
author and source are credited. lished. These and other findings, in combination with the small percentages of statistically
significant primary effect sizes (28.9% and 18.9% for subsets published in Psychological
Data Availability Statement: Data of the meta-
analyses are available via https://osf.io/dc9e8/ and Bulletin and CDSR), led to the conclusion that evidence for publication bias in the studied
https://osf.io/3gbfk/ for the meta-analyses obtained homogeneous subsets is weak, but suggestive of mild publication bias in both psychology
from Psychological Bulletin and the Cochrane and medicine.
Database of Systematic Reviews, respectively. The
vast majority of data for the primary studies of the
meta-analyses published in Psychological Bulletin
is available via https://osf.io/tqvue/ (i.e., data of
86.7% of the included meta-analyses), because
these data were reported in the published meta- Introduction
analysis. If data of the primary studies was not
included in the published meta-analysis, the Meta-analysis is the standard technique for synthesizing different studies on the same topic,
authors sent an email to the corresponding author and is defined as “the statistical analysis of a large collection of analysis results from individual

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 1 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

of the meta-analysis to request for these data. They studies for the purpose of integrating the findings” [1]. One of the greatest threats to the valid-
sent a reminder to the corresponding author if he/ ity of meta-analytic results is publication bias, meaning that the publication of studies depends
she did not respond within two weeks. The authors
on the direction and statistical significance of the results [2]. Publication bias generally leads to
are not allowed to share data of primary studies of
the included meta-analyses (i.e., data of 13.3% of effect sizes being overestimated and the dissemination of false-positive results (e.g., [3, 4]).
the included meta-analyses) if these data were Hence, publication bias results in false impressions about the magnitude and existence of an
obtained after contacting the corresponding author. effect [5] and is considered one of the key problems in contemporary science [6].
The authors of this study promised to not share Indications for the presence of publication bias are present in various research fields. The
these data with others which was often a
main hypothesis tested in the psychology and psychiatry literature is statistically significant in
requirement by the corresponding author before
he/she was willing to share the data. Nevertheless,
approximately 90% of the cases[7, 8], which is not in line with the on average low statistical
they decided to also include data from these meta- power of about 50% or less in, for instance, psychology [9, 10] and may be caused by publica-
analyses in this study at the expense of not being tion bias. Franco, Malhotra, and Simonovits [11] examined publication bias in studies that
able to share these data to base their conclusions received a grant within the social sciences and found that 64.6% of the studies where most or
on the largest number of meta-analyses. The all results did not support the alternative hypotheses was not written up compared to 4.4% of
authors believe that authors of these meta-
the studies where most or all the alternative hypotheses were supported (cf. [12, 13]). In a
analyses will also be willing to share data with
other researchers since they were also willing to highly similar project within the psychological literature, Franco, Malhotra, and Simonovits
share the data with them. A list with references of [14] showed that 70% of the included outcomes in a study were not reported, and that this
these meta-analyses are provided in a supporting selective reporting depended on statistical significance of the outcomes. Although these find-
information file (S1 File). Due to copyright ings suggest that publication bias is present in numerous research fields, mixed results were
restrictions, the authors are not allowed to share
observed when analyzing the distribution of p-values [15–21] where a difference between p-
the data of the primary studies for the systematic
reviews from the Cochrane Database of Systematic
values just above and below α = .05 may be interpreted as evidence for publication bias.
Reviews. However, they provide R code (https:// Compared to the social sciences, more attention has been paid to publication bias in medi-
osf.io/x6yca/) that can be used in combination with cine [22]. Medicine has a longer history in registering clinical trials before conducting the
the Cochrane scraper (https://github.com/ research (e.g., [23, 24]). As of 2007, the US Food and Drug Administration Act (FDA) even
DASpringate/Cochrane_scraper) to web scrape the requires US researchers to make the results of different types of clinical trials publicly available
same systematic reviews as they included in this
independent of whether the results have been published or not [25]. With registers like clinical-
study. This enables other researchers to get the
same data from the primary studies as the authors trials.gov, it is easier for meta-analysts to search for unpublished research, and to include it in
used in this study. their meta-analysis. Furthermore, it is straightforward to study publication bias by comparing
the reported results in registers with the reported results in publications. Studies comparing
Funding: RvA received Grant number: 406-13-050
from The Netherlands Organization for Scientific the reported results in registers and publications show that statistically significant outcomes
Research (NWO), URL funder website: www.nwo. are more likely to be reported, and clinical trials with statistically significant results have a
nl. The funders had no role in study design, data higher probability of getting published [26–28].
collection and analysis, decision to publish, or A number of methods exist to test for publication bias in a meta-analysis and to estimate a
preparation of the manuscript. RvA also received
meta-analytic effect size corrected for publication bias. However, publication bias is often not
funding from Berkeley Initiative for Transparency in
the Social Sciences and the Laura and John Arnold
routinely assessed in meta-analyses [29–31] or analyzed with suboptimal methods that lack
Foundation, URL funder website: www.bitss.org. statistical power to detect it [32, 33]. It has been suggested to reexamine publication bias in
The funders had no role in study design, data published meta-analyses [30, 34] by applying recently developed methods to better understand
collection and analysis, decision to publish, or the severity and prevalence of publication bias in different fields. These novel methods have
preparation of the manuscript. JW received Grant better statistical properties than existing publication bias tests and methods developed earlier
number: 726361 (IMPROVE) fromThe European
to correct effect sizes for publication bias. Moreover, several authors have recommended to
Research Council, URL funder website: www.erc.
europa.eu. The funders had no role in study design, not rely on a single method for examining publication bias in a meta-analysis, but rather to use
data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or and report a set of different publication bias methods [35, 36]. This so-called triangulation
preparation of the manuscript. should take into account that some methods do not perform well in some conditions and that
Competing interests: I have read the journal’s none of the publication bias methods outperforms all the other methods under each and every
policy and the authors of this manuscript have the condition; one method can signal publication bias in a meta-analysis whereas another one
following competing interests: Jelte M. Wicherts is does not. Using a set of methods to assess the prevalence and severity of publication bias may
a PLOS ONE Editorial Board member. This does yield a more balanced conclusion.
not alter the authors’ adherence to PLOS ONE
We set out to answer three research questions in this paper. The first research question con-
Editorial policies and criteria.
cerned the prevalence of publication bias: “What is the prevalence of publication bias within
published meta-analyses in psychological and medical research?” (1a), and “Is publication bias

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 2 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

more prevalent in psychology than in medicine after controlling for the number of studies in a
meta-analysis?” (1b). Medicine was selected to be compared to psychology, because more
attention has been paid to publication bias in general [22] and study registration in particular
(e.g., [23, 24]) within medicine. We also evaluated the amount of agreement between different
publication bias methods. In the second research question, we examined whether effect size
estimates of traditional meta-analysis and corrected for publication bias by the p-uniform
method can be predicted by characteristics of a meta-analysis: “What are predictors of the
meta-analytic estimates of traditional meta-analysis and p-uniform?”. Our third research ques-
tion also consisted of two parts and is about overestimation of effect size caused by publication
bias: “How much is effect size overestimated by publication bias in meta-analyses in psychol-
ogy and medical research?” (3a), and “What are predictors of the overestimation in effect size
caused by publication bias in meta-analyses in psychology and medical research?” (3b). The
aim of this paper is to shed light on the prevalence of publication bias and the overestimation
that it causes by answering the above stated research questions. As we focus on homogeneous
(subsets of) meta-analyses (i.e., with no or small heterogeneity), we examine these questions
for the population of homogeneous subsets. A large-scale dataset will be used containing 83
meta-analyses published in the psychological literature and 499 systematic reviews in the med-
ical literature making this paper a thorough and extensive assessment of publication bias in
psychological and medical research.
The hypotheses as well as our planned analyses were preregistered (see https://osf.io/8y5ep/)
meaning that hypotheses, analysis plan, and code of the data analyses were specified in detail
before the data were analyzed. Some additional analyses were conducted that were not included
in the pre-analysis plan. We will explicate which analyses were exploratory when describing
these analyses and their results. The paper continues by providing an overview of publication
bias methods. Next, we describe the criteria for a meta-analysis to be included in our study.
Then we describe how the data of meta-analyses were extracted and analyzed, and list our
hypotheses. Subsequently, we provide the results of our analyses and conclude with a discussion.

Publication bias methods


Methods for examining publication bias can be divided into two groups: methods that assess
or test the presence of publication bias, and methods that estimate effect sizes corrected for
publication bias. Methods that correct effect sizes for publication bias usually also provide a
confidence interval and test the null hypothesis of no effect corrected for publication bias.
Table 1 summarizes the methods together with their characteristics and recommendations on
when to use each method. The last column of the table lists whether the method is included in
our analyses. Readers that are not interested in the details regarding the publication bias meth-
ods can focus on the summary in Table 1.

Assessing or testing publication bias


The most often used method for assessing publication bias is fail-safe N [34, 37]. This method
estimates how many effect sizes with a zero effect size have to be added to a meta-analysis for
changing a statistically significant summary effect size in a meta-analysis to a nonsignificant
result [38]. Applying the method is discouraged, because it makes the unrealistic assumption
that all nonsignificant effect sizes are equal to zero, does not take study sample size into
account, and focuses on statistical significance and not on the magnitude of an effect that is of
substantial importance [39, 40].
Another popular method is the funnel plot [41]. In a funnel plot, the effect size estimates of
the included studies in a meta-analysis are presented on the x-axis and some measure of the

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 3 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

Table 1. Summary of publication bias methods to assess publication bias and estimate effect sizes corrected for publication bias. The penultimate column lists prin-
cipal references of the different methods and the final column indicates whether a method is included in the analyses of this paper.
Method Description Characteristics/Recommendations Included in
analyses
Assessing publication bias
Fail-safe N Estimates number of effect sizes in the file-drawer Method is discouraged to be used, because it, for instance, No
assumes that all nonsignificant effect sizes are equal to zero and
focuses on statistical instead of practical significance [39, 40].
Funnel plot Graphical representation of small-study effects where funnel Publication bias is not the only cause of funnel plot asymmetry No
plot asymmetry is an indicator of small-study effects [43]. Eyeballing a funnel plot for asymmetry is subjective [46], so
recommendation is to use a statistical test (i.e., Egger’s [43] or
rank-correlation test [47]).
Egger’s and rank- Statistical tests for testing funnel plot symmetry Publication bias is not the only cause of funnel plot asymmetry Yes
correlation test [43]. Methods are recommended to be applied when there are 10
or more effect sizes [48] otherwise the methods have low
statistical power [47, 49].
Test of Excess Computes whether observed and expected number of Do not apply the method in case of heterogeneity in true effect Yes
Significance statistically significant results are in agreement size [50]. Method is known to be conservative [51].
p-uniform’s Examines whether statistically significant p-values are Method does not use information of nonsignificant effect sizes Yes
publication bias uniformly distributed at the estimate of the fixed-effect model and, assumes homogeneous true effect size [5, 52].
test
Correcting effect size for publication bias
Trim and fill Method corrects for funnel plot asymmetry by trimming Method is discouraged to be used because it falsely imputes No
method most extreme effect sizes and filling these effect sizes to effect sizes when none are missing and other methods have
obtain funnel plot symmetry shown to outperform trim and fill [5, 53, 54]. Moreover, funnel
plot asymmetry is not only caused by publication bias [43], and
the method does also not perform well if heterogeneity in true
effect size is present [5, 55].
PET-PEESE Extension of Egger’s test where the corrected estimate is the Method becomes biased if it is based on less than 10 effect sizes, No
intercept of a regression line fitted through the effect sizes in the between-study variance in true effect size is large, and the
a funnel plot sample size of primary studies included in a meta-analysis is
rather similar [56–59].
p-uniform/p- Estimate is the effect size for which the distribution of Method does not use information of nonsignificant effect sizes Yes
curve conditional p-values is uniformly distributed and assumes homogeneous true effect size [5, 52, 53].
Selection model Method makes assumptions on the distribution of effect sizes User has to make sophisticated assumptions and choices [39]. No
approach (effect size model) and mechanism of observing effect sizes Large number of effect sizes (more than 100) are needed to avoid
(selection model). Estimation is performed by combining convergence problems [55, 60], but recent research showed that
these two models. convergence problems of the approach by Iyengar and
Greenhouse [61, 62] were only severe if there was no or extreme
publication bias in combination with no or a small amount of
heterogeneity in true effect size.
10% most precise Only the 10% most precise effect sizes are used for estimation 90% of the available effect sizes is discarded and bias in estimates Yes
effect sizes with a random-effects model increases as a function of heterogeneity in true effect size [63].
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052.t001

effect sizes’ precision is displayed on the y-axis. The left panel in Fig 1 shows a funnel plot for a
meta-analysis in the systematic review by Jürgens and Graudal [42] studying the effect of
sodium intake on different health outcomes. Solid circles in the funnel plot indicate studies’
Hedges’ g effect sizes (y-axis) and their standard errors (x-axis). A funnel plot illustrates
whether small-study effects are present. That is, whether there is a relationship between effect
size and its precision. The funnel plot should be symmetric and resemble an inverted funnel in
the absence of small-study effects, whereas a gap in the funnel indicates that small-study effects
exist. Publication bias is one of the causes of small-study effects [43], but funnel plot asymme-
try is often interpreted as evidence for publication bias in a meta-analysis. Small-study effects
can also be caused by, for instance, researchers basing their sample size on statistical power
analyses in combination with heterogeneity in true effect size (see supplemental materials of

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 4 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

Fig 1. Funnel plot showing the relationship between the observed effect size (Hedges’ g; solid circles) and its standard error in a meta-analysis by Jürgens
and Graudal [42] on the effect of sodium intake on Noradrenaline (left panel). The funnel plot in the right panel also includes the Hedges’ g effect sizes that
are imputed by the trim and fill method (open circles).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052.g001

[44] and [45]). In this case, larger true effect sizes are associated with studies using smaller
sample sizes, resulting in funnel plot asymmetry.
Evaluating whether small-study effects exist by eyeballing a funnel plot is rather subjective
[46]. Hence, Egger’s regression test [43] and the rank-correlation test [47] were developed to
test whether small-study effects are present in a meta-analysis. Egger’s regression test uses lin-
ear regression with the observed effect sizes as dependent variable and a measure of primary
studies’ precision as predictor. Evidence for small-study effects is obtained if the slope of this
regression line is significantly different from zero. The rank-correlation test computes the
rank correlation (Kendall’s τ) between the study’s effect sizes and their precision to test for
small-study effects. Drawback of these two tests is that statistical power to detect publication
bias is low especially if there are few effect sizes in a meta-analysis [47, 49]. Hence, these meth-
ods are recommended to be only applied to meta-analyses with ten or more effect sizes [48].
The test of excess significance (TES) compares the number of statistically significant effect
sizes in a meta-analysis with the expected number of statistically significant effect sizes [50].
The expected number of statistically significant effect sizes is computed by summing the statis-
tical power of each primary study in a meta-analysis. More statistically significant results than
expected indicate that some effect sizes are (possibly because of publication bias) missing from
the meta-analysis. Ioannidis and Trikalinos [50] recommend to not apply the method if het-
erogeneity in true effect size is present. Moreover, the TES is known to be conservative [5, 51].
Another more recently developed method for examining publication bias is the p-uniform
method [5, 52]. This method is based on the statistical principle that the distribution of p-val-
ues at the true effect size is uniform. For example, the distribution of p-values under the null
hypothesis is uniform. Since in the presence of publication bias not all statistically nonsignifi-
cant effect sizes get published, p-uniform discards nonsignificant effect sizes and computes p-
values conditional on being statistically significant. These conditional p-values should be

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 5 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

uniformly distributed at the (fixed-effect) meta-analytic effect size estimate based on the signif-
icant and nonsignificant effect sizes, and deviations from the uniform distribution signals pub-
lication bias. P-uniform’s publication bias test was compared to the TES in a Monte-Carlo
simulation study [5], and statistical power of p-uniform was in general larger than the TES
except for conditions with a true effect size of zero in combination with statistically nonsignifi-
cant studies included in a meta-analysis. This simulation study also showed that Type-I error
rate of p-uniform’s publication bias test was too low if the true effect size was of medium size.
Limitations of p-uniform’s publication bias test are that it assumes that the true effect size is
homogeneous (which is not very common, see for instance [64–66]), and that the method may
inefficiently use the available information by discarding statistically nonsignificant effect sizes
in a meta-analysis.

Correcting effect sizes for publication bias


Publication bias tests provide evidence about the presence of publication bias in a meta-analy-
sis. However, statistical power of publication bias tests is often low in practice [54], because the
number of effect sizes in a meta-analysis is often small. For instance, the median number of
effect sizes in meta-analyses published in the Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews was
equal to 3 [67, 68]. Furthermore, the magnitude of the effect after correcting for publication
bias is more of interest from the perspective of an applied researcher.
The most popular method to correct for publication bias in a meta-analysis is trim and fill
[69, 70]. This method corrects for funnel plot asymmetry by trimming the most extreme effect
sizes from one side of the funnel plot and filling these effect sizes in the other side of the funnel
plot to obtain funnel plot symmetry. The corrected effect size estimate is obtained by comput-
ing the meta-analytic estimate based on the observed and imputed effect sizes. Trim and fill
can also be used to create a confidence interval and test the null hypothesis of no effect after
adjusting for funnel plot asymmetry. The procedure of trim and fill is illustrated in the right
panel of Fig 1. The most extreme effect sizes from the right-hand side of the funnel plot are
trimmed and imputed in the left-hand side of the funnel plot (open circles in the right panel of
Fig 1). A drawback of trim and fill, which it shares with other methods based on the funnel
plot, is that it corrects for small-study effects that are not necessarily caused by publication
bias. Furthermore, the method cannot accurately correct for publication bias when the true
effect size is heterogeneous (e.g., [5, 55]). Simulation studies have also shown that results of
trim and fill cannot be trusted because it incorrectly adds studies when none are missing [55,
71, 72]. Hence, trim and fill is discouraged because of its misleading results [5, 53, 54].
The PET-PEESE method [65] is an extension of Egger’s regression test to estimate an effect
size in a meta-analysis corrected for small-study effects. PET-PEESE is based on a regression
analysis where the observed effect sizes are regressed on their standard errors by means of a
weighted least squares regression with the inverse of the effect sizes’ sampling variances as
weights. If the intercept is not significantly different from zero, the estimate of the intercept is
interpreted as the effect size estimate corrected for publication bias. The estimate of the inter-
cept reflects the effect size estimate in a study with a standard error of zero (i.e., a study with
an infinite sample size). However, this estimator is biased if the intercept is significantly differ-
ent from zero [65]. Hence, in case the intercept is significantly different from zero, the inter-
cept of another weighted least squares regression analysis (with the inverse sampling variances
as weights) is interpreted as the effect size estimate. In this regression analysis, the observed
effect sizes are regressed on their sampling variances. Simulation studies have shown that
PET-PEESE substantially reduced the overestimation caused by small-study effects [65]. How-
ever, PET-PEESE becomes biased when less than 10 effect sizes are included in a meta-analysis,

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 6 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

the between-study variance in true effect size is large, and the sample size of primary studies
included in a meta-analysis is rather similar [56–59].
The p-uniform method can also be used for estimating effect size (and a confidence inter-
val) and testing the null hypothesis of no effect corrected for publication bias. P-uniform’s
effect size estimate is equal to the effect size for which the p-values conditional on being statis-
tically significant are uniformly distributed. A similar method that uses the distribution of con-
ditional p-values for estimating effect size in the presence of publication bias is p-curve [53].
This method is similar to the p-uniform method, but differs in implementation (for a descrip-
tion of the difference between the two methods see [52]). A limitation of p-uniform and p-
curve is that effect sizes are overestimated in the presence of heterogeneity in true effect size
[52]. Especially if the heterogeneity in true effect size is more than moderate (I2 > 50%; more
than half of the total variance in effect size is caused by heterogeneity) both methods overesti-
mate the effect size, and their results should be interpreted as a sensitivity analysis. Another
limitation of both methods is that they are not efficient if many nonsignificant effect sizes
exist. Such results are discarded by the methods, yielding imprecise estimates and wide confi-
dence intervals of p-uniform (p-curve does not estimate a confidence interval). P-uniform and
p-curve both outperformed trim and fill in simulation studies [5, 53].
A selection model approach [45] can also be used for estimating effect size corrected for
publication bias. A selection model makes assumptions on the distribution of effect sizes (i.e.,
effect size model) and the mechanism that determines which studies are selected (for publica-
tion) and hence observed (i.e., selection model). The effect size estimate (and confidence inter-
val) corrected for publication bias is obtained by combining the effect size and selection
model. Many different selection model approaches exist (e.g., [73–78]). Some approaches esti-
mate the selection model [74, 77] whereas others assume a known selection model [79]. A
recently proposed selection model approach [80] estimates effect size corrected for publication
bias by using Bayesian model averaging over multiple selection models. Selection model
approaches are hardly used in practice, because it requires sophisticated assumptions and
choices [39] and a large number of effect sizes (more than 100) to avoid convergence problems
[55, 60]. However, two recent simulation studies [61, 62] were conducted that included the
three-parameter selection model approach by Iyengar and Greenhouse [74, 81] and showed
that convergence problems of this approach were only severe for conditions that included only
10 studies, or conditions wherein publication bias was extreme.
Stanley, Jarrel, and Doucouliagos [63] proposed to correct for publication bias in the effect
size estimate by computing the unweighted mean of the 10% most precise observed effect
sizes, or the single most precise study in case of less than ten effect sizes. The rationale underly-
ing only using the 10% most precise observed effect sizes is that these primary study’s effect
sizes are less affected by publication bias than the 90% less precise discarded effect sizes. We
propose to not combine the 10% most precise observed effect sizes with an unweighted mean,
but with a random-effects model to take differences in primary study’s sampling variances and
heterogeneity in true effect size into account. A disadvantage of this method is that it is not
efficient leading to imprecise estimates and wider confidence intervals than estimation based
on all effect sizes since up to 90% of the data is discarded. Moreover, bias in the method’s esti-
mates increases as a function of the heterogeneity in true effect size [63].

Methods
Data
A large-scale data set was created with meta-analyses published between 2004 and 2014 in Psy-
chological Bulletin and in the Cochrane Library to study the extent and prevalence of

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 7 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

publication bias in psychology and medicine. Psychological Bulletin was selected to represent
meta-analyses in psychology, because this journal publishes many meta-analyses on a variety
of topics from psychology. Meta-analyses published in the Cochrane Database of Systematic
Reviews (CDSR) of the Cochrane Library were used to represent medicine. This database is a
collection of peer-reviewed systematic reviews conducted in the field of medicine.
A first requirement for the inclusion of a meta-analysis was that either fixed-effect or ran-
dom-effects meta-analysis had to be used in the meta-analysis (i.e., no other meta-analytic
methods as, for instance, meta-analytic structural equation modelling or multilevel meta-anal-
ysis). Another requirement was that sufficient information in the meta-analysis had to be avail-
able to compute the primary study’s standardized effect size and its sampling variance. The
same effect size measure (e.g., correlation and standardized mean difference) as in the original
meta-analysis was used to compute the primary study’s effect size and its sampling variance.
Formulas as described in [82], [83], and [84] were used for computing the standardized effect
sizes and their sampling variances. For each included primary study, we extracted information
on effect size and sampling variance, as well as information on all categorical moderator vari-
ables. Based on these moderators, we created homogeneous subsets of effect sizes. That is, a
homogeneous subset consisted of the effect sizes that had the same scores on all the extracted
moderators. Consequently, each meta-analysis could contain more than one subset of effect
sizes if multiple homogeneous subsets were extracted based on the included moderators.
We only included subsets with less than moderate heterogeneity (I2<50%) [85], because
none of the publication bias methods has desirable statistical properties under extreme hetero-
geneity in true effect size [5, 32, 50, 52]. This implied that the population that we study is the
homogeneous subsets of meta-analyses that were published in the psychological and medical
literature. Drawbacks of examining heterogeneity in true effect size with the I2-statistic are that
its value heavily depends on the sample size of the primary studies in case of heterogeneity
[86] and the statistic is imprecise in case of a small number of primary studies in a meta-analy-
sis [87, 88]. However, the I2-statistic enables comparison across meta-analyses that used differ-
ent effect size measures which is not possible by comparing estimates of the between-study
variance (τ2) in true effect size of meta-analyses. Different effect size measures were sometimes
used within a meta-analysis. This may cause heterogeneity in a meta-analysis, so the type of
effect size measure was also used for creating homogeneous subsets. Publication bias tests have
low statistical power (e.g., [5, 47, 89]) if the number of effect sizes in a meta-analysis is small.
Hence, another criterion for including a subset in the analyses was that a subset should contain
at least five effect sizes.
We searched within the journal Psychological Bulletin for meta-analyses published between
2004 and 2014 by using the search terms “meta-analy� ” and not “comment”, “note”, “correc-
tion”, and “reply” in the article’s title. This search resulted in 137 meta-analyses that were pub-
lished between 2004 and 2014 and that were eligible for inclusion. A flowchart is presented in
Fig 2 describing the data extraction for the meta-analyses published in Psychological Bulletin.
Eighty-three meta-analyses met the inclusion criteria and could be included since the data
were available in the paper or were obtained by emailing the corresponding author. Data of
these meta-analyses were extracted by hand and resulted in 9,568 subsets. Data from a random
sample of 10% of the included meta-analyses was extracted a second time by a different
researcher to verify the procedure of extracting data. Four additional subsets were excluded
after verifying the data, because these subsets were heterogeneous instead of homogeneous.
After excluding subsets with less than five effect sizes and heterogeneous subsets, a total num-
ber of 366 subsets from 83 meta-analyses were available for the analyses.
Data of all systematic reviews in the CDSR are stored online in a standardized format, and
data of these reviews can therefore be extracted by an automated procedure. We used the

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 8 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

Fig 2. Flowchart illustrating the extraction procedure of data from meta-analyses published in Psychological Bulletin between 2004 and 2014.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052.g002

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 9 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

Cochrane scraper developed by Springate and Kontopantelis [90] to automatically extract data
from systematic reviews. The total number of meta-analyses in the CDSR is larger than in Psy-
chological Bulletin, so we drew a simple random sample without replacement of systematic
reviews from the CDSR to represent meta-analyses published in medicine. Each systematic
review in the database has an identification number. We sampled identification numbers,
extracted subsets from the sampled systematic review, and included a subset in our study if (i)
I2<50%, (ii) the number of effect sizes in a subset was at least five, and (iii) the subset was inde-
pendent of previous included subsets (i.e., no overlap between effect sizes in different subsets).
We continued sampling systematic reviews and extracting subsets till the same number of eli-
gible subsets for inclusion were obtained as extracted from Psychological Bulletin (366). Data
and/or descriptions of the data of the meta-analyses are available at https://osf.io/9jqht/. The
next section describes how the research questions were answered, and how the variables were
measured.

Analysis
Prevalence of publication bias. The prevalence of publication bias in homogeneous sub-
sets from meta-analyses in the psychological and medical literature was examined to answer
research question 1 by using the methods listed in the last column of Table 1. Egger’s test and
the rank-correlation test were used in the analyses to test for funnel plot asymmetry instead of
eyeballing a funnel plot. P-uniform’s publication bias test can be applied to observed effect
sizes in a subset that are either significantly smaller or larger than zero. Hence, p-uniform was
applied to negative or positive statistically significant effect sizes in a subset depending on
where the majority of statistically significant effect sizes was observed (using a two-tailed
hypothesis test with α = .05). The estimator based on the Irwin-Hall distribution was used for
p-uniform, because this estimator seemed to have the best statistical properties and provides a
confidence interval [52]. Publication bias tests have low statistical power, so we followed a rec-
ommendation by Egger and colleagues [43] to conduct two-tailed hypothesis tests with α = .1
for all methods. Unintentionally, one-tailed p-values of p-uniform’s publication bias test were
computed in the preregistered R code for subsets of CDSR instead of the intended two-tailed
p-values. Since two-tailed p-values were computed for all the other publication bias tests, we
corrected the pre-registered R code such that two-tailed p-values were also computed for p-
uniform’s publication bias test.
We answered research question 1a about the prevalence of publication bias in meta-analy-
ses published in Psychological Bulletin and CDSR by counting how often each method rejects
the null hypothesis of no publication bias. Agreement among the publication bias tests was
examined by computing Loevinger’s H values [91] for each combination of two methods. Loe-
vinger’s H is a statistic to quantify the association between two dichotomous variables (i.e., sta-
tistically significant or not). The maximum value of Loevinger H is 1 indicating a perfect
association where the minimum value depends on characteristics of the data. For subsets with
no statistically significant effect sizes, p-uniform could not be applied, so we computed the
association between the results of p-uniform and other methods only for subsets with statisti-
cally significant effect sizes.
We studied whether publication bias was more prevalent in homogeneous subsets from
Psychological Bulletin and CDSR (research question 1b) by conducting for each publication
bias test a logistic regression with as dependent variable whether a publication bias test was sta-
tistically significant or not and as predictor a dummy variable indicating whether a subset was
obtained from Psychological Bulletin or CDSR (reference category). The number of effect sizes
in a subset (or statistically significant effect sizes for p-uniform) was included as control

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 10 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

variable, because statistical power of publication bias tests depends on the number of effect
sizes in a subset and the number of effect sizes in subsets from meta-analyses published in Psy-
chological Bulletin and CDSR were expected to differ. We hypothesized that publication bias
would be more severe in subsets from Psychological Bulletin than CDSR after controlling for
the number of effect sizes in a subset (or number of statistically significant effect sizes for p-
uniform). This relationship was expected because medical researchers have been longer aware
of the consequences of publication bias whereas broad awareness of publication bias recently
originated in psychology. One-tailed hypothesis tests with α = .05 were used for answering
research question 1b. As a sensitivity analysis, we also conducted for each publication bias test
a multilevel logistic regression where we take into account that the subsets were nested in the
meta-analyses. This analysis was not specified in the pre-analysis plan.
Predicting effect size estimation. Characteristics of subsets were used to predict the esti-
mates of random-effects meta-analysis and estimates of p-uniform in research question 2. All
effect sizes and their sampling variances were transformed to Cohen’s d to enable interpreta-
tion of the results by using the formulas in section 12.5 of [82]. If Cohen’s d and their sampling
variances could not be computed based on the available information, Hedges’ g was used as an
approximation of Cohen’s d (6.4% of all subsets).
Random-effects meta-analysis was used to estimate the effect size rather than fixed-effect
meta-analysis. Random-effects meta-analysis assumes that there is no single fixed true effect
underlying each effect size [92], and was preferred over fixed-effect meta-analysis because a
small amount of heterogeneity in true effect size could be present in the subsets. The Paule-
Mandel estimator [93] was used in random-effects meta-analysis for estimating the amount of
between-study variance in true effect size since this estimator has the best statistical properties
in most situations [94, 95]. Effect sizes corrected for publication bias were estimated with p-
uniform and based on the 10% most precise observed effect sizes (see last column of Table 1).
Estimation based on the 10% most precise observed effect sizes was included as an exploratory
analysis to examine whether estimates of p-uniform were in line with another method to cor-
rect effect sizes for publication bias. If the number of observed effect sizes in a subset was
smaller than ten, the most precise estimate was interpreted as estimate of the 10% most precise
observed effect sizes. For applying p-uniform, the estimator based on the Irwin-Hall distribu-
tion was used, and two-tailed hypothesis tests in the primary studies were conducted with α =
.05. The underlying true effect size in a subset can be either positive or negative. Hence, the
dependent variables of these analyses were the absolute values of the estimates of random-
effects meta-analysis and p-uniform.
Selection model approaches and PET-PEESE methods were not incorporated in the analy-
ses, because the number of effect sizes included in meta-analyses in medicine is often too small
for these methods. Selection model approaches suffer from convergence problems when
applied to data with these characteristics (e.g., [60, 61]), and PET-PEESE is not recommended
to be used since it yields unreliable results if there are less than 10 observed effect sizes [56]. P-
uniform was preferred over trim and fill and p-curve, because applying trim and fill is discour-
aged [5, 53, 54] and p-curve is not able to estimate a confidence interval around its effect size
estimate.
Two weighted least squares (WLS) regressions were performed with as dependent variables
the absolute values of the effect size estimates of either random-effects meta-analysis or p-uni-
form. Since we meta-analyze the effect sizes estimated with meta-analysis methods, we refer to
these analyses as meta-meta-regressions. The inverse of the variance of a random-effects
model was selected as weights in both meta-meta-regressions, because it is a function of both
the sample size of the primary studies and the number of effect sizes in a subset. P-uniform can
only be applied to subsets with statistically significant effect sizes, so the meta-meta-regression

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 11 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

with the effect size estimates of p-uniform as dependent variable was only based on these
subsets.
Four predictors were included in the meta-meta regressions. The predictors and the
hypothesized relationships are listed in the first two columns of Table 2. The meta-meta-ana-
lytic effect size estimate was expected to be larger in subsets from Psychological Bulletin,
because publication bias was expected to be more severe in psychology than medicine. No rela-
tionship was hypothesized between the I2-statistic and the meta-analytic effect size estimate,
because heterogeneity can be either over- or underestimated depending on the extent of publi-
cation bias [96, 97]. Primary studies’ precision in a subset was operationalized by computing
the harmonic mean of the primary studies’ standard error. A negative relationship was
expected between primary studies’ precision and the meta-analytic estimate, because less pre-
cise effect size estimates (i.e., larger standard errors) were expected to be accompanied with
more bias and hence larger meta-analytic effect size estimates. The number of effect sizes in a
subset was included to control for differences in the number of studies in a meta-analysis.
The hypotheses concerning the effects in the meta-meta regression on p-uniform’s esti-
mates are presented in the third column of Table 2. No hypothesis was specified for the effect
of discipline since p-uniform is supposed to correct for possible differences between both dis-
ciplines in effect sizes due to publication bias. We expected a positive relationship with the I2-
statistic, because p-uniform overestimates the true effect size in the presence of heterogeneity
in true effect size [5, 52]. No specific relationship was predicted with primary studies’ precision
as p-uniform is supposed to correct for publication bias. A specific relationship was also not
hypothesized for the effect of the proportion of statistically significant effect sizes in a subset.
Many statistically significant effect sizes in a subset suggest that the studied effect size is large,
sample size of the primary studies are large, or there was severe publication bias in combina-
tion with many conducted (but not published) primary studies. These partly opposing effects
might have canceled each other out or there can be a positive or negative relationship. The
number of effect sizes in a subset was again included as control variable.
The effect size estimate of p-uniform can become extremely positive or negative if there are
multiple p-values just below the α-level [5, 52]. These outliers may affect the results of the
meta-meta-regression with p-uniform’s estimate as dependent variable. Hence, we used quan-
tile regression [98] as a sensitivity analysis, because this procedure is less influenced by outliers
in the dependent variable. In quantile regression, the predictors were regressed on the median
of the estimates of p-uniform. Moreover, we also conducted another meta-meta-regression as
a sensitivity analysis where we added a random effect to take into account that the subsets
were nested in meta-analyses. Both sensitivity analyses were exploratory analyses that were not
specified in the pre-analysis plan.
Overestimation of effect size. Estimates of random-effects meta-analysis and p-uniform
obtained for answering research question 2 were used to examine the overestimation caused

Table 2. Hypotheses between predictors and effect size estimate based on random-effects model, p-uniform, and overestimation in effect size when comparing esti-
mate of the random-effects model with p-uniform (Y).
Hypotheses
Predictor Random-effects model p-uniform Overestimation (Y)
Discipline Larger estimates in subsets from Psychological No specific Overestimation more severe in Psychological
Bulletin expectation Bulletin
I2-statistic No relationship Positive relationship Negative relationship
Primary studies’ precision Negative relationship No relationship Negative relationship
Proportion of significant effect Predictor not included No specific No specific expectation
sizes expectation
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052.t002

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 12 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

by publication bias. As an exploratory analysis, overestimation was also studied by comparing


estimates of random-effects meta-analysis with those of 10% most precise observed effect sizes.
It is possible that especially estimates of the meta-analysis and p-uniform have opposite signs
(i.e., negative estimate of p-uniform and positive meta-analytic estimate or the other way
around). An effect size estimate of p-uniform in the opposite direction than the meta-analytic
estimate is often unrealistic, because this suggests that, for instance, a negative true effect size
results in multiple positive observed effect sizes. Effect size estimates in opposing directions by
meta-analysis and p-uniform may be caused by many p-values just below the α-level [52].
Hence, p-uniform’s estimate was set equal to zero in these situations. Setting p-uniform’s esti-
mate to zero when its sign is opposite to that of random-effects meta-analysis is in line with
the recommendation in [52]. We did not set estimates based on the 10% most precise observed
effect sizes to zero, because this estimator will not yield unrealistic estimates in the opposite
direction than random-effects meta-analysis in the absence of heterogeneity. Such an estimate
in the opposite direction based on the 10% most precise observed effect sizes is also unlikely to
occur. The most precise observed effect sizes get the largest weight in a random-effects meta-
analysis and the sign of these precise observed effect sizes is for the vast majority of cases in
line with the sign of the random-effects meta-analysis.
A new variable Y was created to reflect the overestimation of random-effects meta-analysis
when compared with p-uniform and the 10% most precise observed effect sizes. Such a Y-vari-
able was created for both methods that correct effect size estimates for publication bias. If the
meta-analytic estimate was larger than zero, Y = MA-corrected where “MA” is the meta-ana-
lytic estimate and “corrected” is the estimate of either p-uniform or the 10% most precise
observed effect sizes. If the meta-analytic estimate was smaller than zero, Y = -MA+corrected.
Variable Y was zero if the estimates of the random-effects meta-analysis and an estimate cor-
rected for publication bias were the same, positive if a corrected effect size estimate was closer
to zero than the meta-analytic estimate (if they originally had the same sign), and negative if a
corrected estimate was farther away from zero than the meta-analytic estimate (if they origi-
nally had the same sign). The Y variable based on p-uniform was computed for each subset
with statistically significant effect sizes. We computed the mean, median, and a 95% confi-
dence interval by using a normal approximation and estimated standard error equal to the
standard deviation of Y divided by the square root of the number of homogeneous subsets.
These estimates and 95% confidence intervals were computed for subsets from Psychological
Bulletin and CDSR in order to gather insight in the amount of overestimation in effect size
(research question 3a).
To answer research question 3b, we carried out meta-meta regressions on Y based on p-uni-
form with the inverse of the variance of the random-effects meta-analytic estimate as weights.
We used the predictors that we also included in research question 2. The hypothesized rela-
tionships are summarized in the fourth column of Table 2. A larger value on Y was expected
for subsets from Psychological Bulletin than CDSR, because overestimation was expected to be
more severe in psychology than in medicine. We hypothesized a negative relation between the
I2-statistic and Y, because p-uniform overestimates the effect size in the presence of heteroge-
neity in true effect size [5, 52]. Primary studies’ precision was hypothesized to be negatively
related to Y, because overestimation of the meta-analytic estimate was expected to decrease as
a function of primary studies’ precision. We had no specific expectations on the relationships
between the number of effect sizes in a subset and the proportion of statistically significant
effect sizes in a subset. Although a positive effect of this proportion on the meta-analytic effect
size estimate was expected, the effect of the proportion on p-uniform’s estimate was unclear.
We included the number of effect sizes in a subset in the meta-meta-regression as a control
variable.

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 13 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

Estimates of p-uniform that were in the opposite direction than traditional meta-analysis
were set equal to zero before computing the Y-variable. This may have affected the results of
the meta-meta-regression since the dependent variable Y did not follow a normal distribution.
Hence, quantile regression [98] was used as sensitivity analysis with the median of Y as depen-
dent variable instead of the mean of Y in the meta-meta regression. We also conducted another
meta-meta-regression as a sensitivity analysis where a random effect was included to take into
account that the subsets were nested in meta-analyses. Both sensitivity analyses were explor-
atory analyses that were not specified in the pre-analysis plan.
Monte-Carlo simulation study. Following up on the comments of a reviewer we exam-
ined the statistical properties of our preregistered analyses by means of a Monte-Carlo simula-
tion study. More specifically, we examined the statistical power of publication bias tests and
properties of effect size estimation based on the 10% most precise observed effect sizes, both as a
function of publication bias and true effect size. As the analysis based on the 10% most precise
estimates does not make any assumptions about the publication process (like the publication
bias methods, including p-uniform), we consider this analysis to provide additional valuable
information about the extent of publication bias in the psychology and medicine literature.
Cohen’s d effect sizes were simulated under the fixed-effect meta-analysis model using the
number of observed effect sizes and their standard errors of the homogeneous subsets included
in our large-scale dataset. That is, effect sizes were simulated from a normal distribution with
mean μ and variance equal to the ‘observed’ squared standard errors of each homogeneous
subset. Publication bias was introduced by always including statistically significant effect sizes
where significance was determined based on a one-tailed test with α = .025 to resemble com-
mon practice to test a two-tailed hypothesis with α = .05 and only report results in the pre-
dicted direction. All generated nonsignificant effect sizes had a probability equal to 1-pub to be
included. For each effect size in the homogeneous subset, the observed effect size was simu-
lated until it was ‘published’; as a result the simulated homogeneous subset had the same prop-
erties (number of studies, standard errors of the studies but not the effect sizes and their
corresponding p-values) as the observed homogeneous subset.
The publication bias tests (see Table 1 for the included methods) and methods to correct
effect size for publication bias (p-uniform and meta-analysis based on the 10% most precise
observed effect sizes) were applied to data of each generated homogeneous subset. We exam-
ined Type-I error rate and statistical power of the publication bias tests using the same α-level
(i.e., 0.1) as for testing for publication bias in the homogeneous subsets. We also assessed the
overestimation of the random-effects model with the Paule-Mandel estimator [93] for the
between-study variance when compared with the 10% most precise observed effect sizes by
computing the earlier introduced Y-variable.
Data of homogeneous subsets were simulated for characteristics of all 732 homogeneous
subsets and repeated 1,000 times. Values for μ were selected to reflect no (μ = 0), small (μ =
0.2), and medium (μ = 0.5) effect regarding the guidelines by Cohen [99]. Publication bias
(pub) was varied from 0, 0.25, 0.5, 0.75, 0.85, 0.95, and 1, with pub = 0 implying no publication
bias and 1 extreme publication bias. The Monte-Carlo simulation study was programmed in R
[100] and the packages “metafor” [101], “puniform” [102], and “parallel” [100] were used (see
https://osf.io/efkn9/ for R code of the simulation study).

Results
Descriptive statistics
The total number of included homogeneous subsets was 732 (366 representing Psychological
Bulletin and 366 representing CDSR). Table 3 shows descriptive results (number of effect

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 14 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

Table 3. Percentage of statistically significant effect size estimates, median number of effect sizes and median of average sample size per homogeneous subset, and
mean and median of effect size estimates when the subsets were analyzed with random-effects meta-analysis, p-uniform, and random-effects meta-analysis based
on the 10% most precise observed effect sizes.
RE meta-analysis p-uniform 10% most precise
Psychological Bulletin
28.9% statistically significant
Median (IQR) number of effect sizes 6 (5;9) 1 (0;4) 1 (1;1)
Median (IQR) sample size 97.8 (52.4;173.2) 109 (56.5;206.2) 207.3 (100;466)
Positive RE meta-analysis estimates:
67.2% of homogeneous subsets
Mean, median, [min.;max.], (SD) of estimates 0.332, 0.279, [0;1.456] (0.264) -0.168, 0.372, 0.283, 0.22,
[-21.584;1.295] (2.367) [-0.629;1.34] (0.289)
Negative RE meta-analysis estimates:
32% of homogeneous subsets
Mean, median, [min.;max.], (SD) of estimates -0.216, -0.123, -0.041, -0.214, -0.228, -0.204,
[-1.057;-0.002] (0.231) [-5.166;13.845] (1.84) [-0.972;0.181] (0.247)
CDSR
18.9% statistically significant
Median (IQR) number of effect sizes 6 (5;8) 1 (0;2) 1 (1;1)
Median (IQR) sample size 126.6 (68.3;223.3) 123.3 (71.9;283.5) 207 (101.2;443)
Positive RE meta-analysis estimates:
45.1% of homogeneous subsets
Mean, median, [min.;max.], (SD) of estimates 0.304, 0.215, [0.001;1.833] (0.311) -1.049, 0.323, 0.284, 0.201,
[-60.85;1.771] (6.978) [-0.709;1.757] (0.366)
Negative RE meta-analysis estimates:
54.9% of homogeneous subsets
Mean, median, [min.;max.], (SD) of estimates -0.267, -0.19, 1.51, -0.239, -0.214, -0.182,
[-1.343;0] (0.253) [-1.581;163.53] (15.064) [-1.205;0.644] (0.286)

RE meta-analysis is random-effects meta-analysis, IQR is the interquartile range, min. is the minimum value, max. is the maximum value, SD is the standard deviation,
and CDSR is Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. The percentages of homogeneous subsets with positive and negative RE meta-analysis estimates do not sum to
100%, because the estimates of three homogeneous subsets obtained from the meta-analysis by Else-Quest and colleagues [103] were equal to zero. These authors set
effect sizes to zero if the effect size could not have been extracted from a primary study but was reported as not statistically significant.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052.t003

sizes, percentage of statistically significant effect sizes, primary study sample sizes, and positive
and negative meta-analytic effect size estimates) of applying random-effects meta-analysis, p-
uniform, and random-effects meta-analysis based on the 10% most precise observed effect
sizes.
The percentage of effect sizes (across all homogeneous subsets) that was statistically signifi-
cant was 28.9% and 18.9% in Psychological Bulletin and CDSR, respectively. These percentages
were lower than those based on the excluded heterogeneous subsets (44.2% and 28.9%, respec-
tively). The number of effect sizes in subsets was similar in Psychological Bulletin and CDSR.
The majority of subsets contained less than 10 effect sizes (third quartile 9 for Psychological
Bulletin and 8 for CDSR) meaning that the characteristics of the subsets were very tough for
publication bias methods. Statistical power of publication bias is low in these conditions [47,
49] and effect size estimates corrected for publication bias are imprecise [5, 52]. The number
of statistically significant effect sizes in the subsets based on a two-tailed hypothesis test with
α = .05 was also small (listed in column with results of p-uniform). The median number of sta-
tistically significant effect sizes in the subsets was 1 for both Psychological Bulletin and CDSR.
Moreover, 267 (73%) of the subsets from Psychological Bulletin and 214 (58.5%) of the subsets
from CDSR contained at least one statistically significant effect size; hence 27% and 41.5% of
subsets did not contain a single statistically significant effect size. Consequently, p-uniform

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 15 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

could only be applied to 481 (65.7%) of the subsets. Of these subsets 180 (37.4%) included only
one statistically significant effect size, so the characteristics of the subsets were very challenging
for p-uniform. However, methods based on similar methodology as p-uniform to, for instance,
compare an original study and replication and to determine the required sample size in a
power analysis showed that one or two effect sizes can be sufficient for accurate estimation of
effect size [5, 104–106]. The median and interquartile range of the 10% most precise effect size
estimates were all equal to one, and estimates of this method were for 676 (92.3%) subsets
based on only one effect size.
The median of the average sample size per subset was slightly larger for CDSR (126.6) than
for Psychological Bulletin (97.8). The interquartile range of average sample size within subsets
from CDSR (68.3; 223.3) was also larger than for subsets from Psychological Bulletin
(52.4;173.2). Psychological Bulletin and CDSR showed small differences in the median and
interquartile range of the average sample size in subsets if computed based on only the statisti-
cally significant effect sizes (p-uniform) or the 10% most precise effect size estimates.
Results of estimating effect size in subsets with random-effects meta-analysis, p-uniform,
and random-effects meta-analysis based on the 10% most precise observed effect sizes (explor-
atory analysis) are also included in Table 3. To increase interpretability of the results, estimates
were grouped depending on whether the effect size estimate of random-effects meta-analysis
was positive or negative. The mean and median of the effect size estimates of random-effects
meta-analysis and those based on the 10% most precise observed effect sizes were highly simi-
lar (difference at most 0.053). However, estimates of p-uniform deviated from the other two
methods, because p-uniform’s estimates were in some subsets very positive or negative (i.e., 4
estimates were larger than 10 and 7 estimates were smaller than -10) due to p-values of the pri-
mary study’s effect sizes close to the α-level. Consequently, the standard deviation and range
of the estimates of p-uniform were larger than of random-effects meta-analysis and based on
the 10% most precise observed effect sizes.

Prevalence of publication bias


Table 4 shows the results of applying Egger’s regression test, the rank-correlation test, p-uni-
form’s publication bias test, and the TES to examine the prevalence of publication bias in the
meta-analyses. The panels in Table 4 illustrate how often each publication bias test was statisti-
cally significant (marginal frequencies and percentages) and also the agreement among the
methods (joint frequencies). Agreement among the methods was quantified by means of Loe-
vinger’s H (bottom-right cell of each panel).
Publication bias was detected in at most 94 subsets (12.9%) by Egger’s regression test. The
TES and rank-correlation test were statistically significant in 40 (5.5%) and 78 (10.7%) subsets,
respectively. In the subsets with at least one statistically significant effect size, p-uniform’s pub-
lication bias test detected publication bias in 42 subsets (9%), which was more than TES (40;
8.6%) and less than both the rank-correlation test (55; 11.8%) and Egger’s regression test (78;
16.7%). Since the estimated prevalence values are close to 10%, which equals the significance
threshold of each test, we conclude there is at best weak evidence of publication bias on the
basis of publication bias tests. Associations among the methods were low (H < .168), except
for the association between Egger’s regression test and the rank-correlation test (H = .485).
To answer research question 1b we examined whether publication bias was more prevalent
in subsets from Psychological Bulletin than CDSR. Publication bias was detected in 13.4%
(Egger’s test), 12.8% (rank-correlation test), 11.4% (p-uniform), 6.6% (TES) of the subsets
from Psychological Bulletin and in 12.2% (Egger’s test), 8.5% (rank-correlation test), 5.9% (p-
uniform), and 4.4% (TES) of the subsets from CDSR. When testing for differences in

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 16 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

Table 4. Results of applying Egger’s regression test, rank-correlation test, p-uniform’s publication bias test, and test of excess significance (TES) to examine the
prevalence of publication bias in meta-analyses from Psychological Bulletin and Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews.
Rank-correlation p-uniform
Not sig. Sig. Not sig. Sig.
Egger Not sig. 600 35 635; 87.1% Egger Not sig. 354 34 388; 83.3%
Sig. 51 43 94; 12.9% Sig. 70 8 78; 16.7%
Total 651; 89.3% 78; 10.7% H = .485 Total 424; 91% 42; 9% H = .028
TES p-uniform
Not sig. Sig. Not sig. Sig.
Egger Not sig. 609 29 638; 87.2% Rank-corr. Not sig. 377 34 411; 88.2%
Sig. 83 11 94; 12.8% Sig. 47 8 55; 11.8%
Total 692; 94.5% 40; 5.5% H = .168 Total 424; 91% 42; 9% H = .082
TES TES
Not sig. Sig. Not sig. Sig.
Rank-corr. Not sig. 620 31 651; 89.3% p-uniform Not sig. 393 31 424; 91%
Sig. 69 9 78; 10.7% Sig. 33 9 42; 9%
Total 689; 94.5% 40; 5.5% H = .132 Total 426; 91.4% 40; 8.6% H = .148

H denotes Loevinger’s H to describe the association between two methods. The rank-correlation could not be applied to all 732 subsets, because there was no variation
in the observed effect sizes in three subsets. All these subsets were part of the meta-analysis by Else-Quest and colleagues [103] who set effect sizes to zero if the effect size
could not have been extracted from a primary study but was reported as not statistically significant.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052.t004

publication bias we controlled for the number of effect sizes (or for p-uniform statistically sig-
nificant effect sizes) in a meta-analysis. Publication bias was more prevalent in subsets from
Psychological Bulletin if the results of p-uniform were used as dependent variable (odds
ratio = 2.226, z = 2.217, one-tailed p-value = .014), but not for Egger’s regression test (odds
ratio = 1.024, z = 0.106, one-tailed p-value = .458), rank-correlation test (odds ratio = 1.491,
z = 1.613, one-tailed p-value = .054), and TES (odds ratio = 1.344, z = 0.871, one-tailed p-value
= .192). Tables with the results of these logistic regression analyses are reported in S1–S4
Tables. Note, however, that if we control for the number of tests performed (i.e., 4) by means
of the Bonferoni correction (p = .005 < .05/4 = .0125), the result of p-uniform was no longer
statistically significant.
We also conducted multilevel logistic regression analyses to take into account that the sub-
sets were nested in meta-analyses. The intraclass correlation can be used to assess to what
extent the subsets within a meta-analysis were related to each other. These intraclass correla-
tions were 14.9%, 25.6%, 0%, and 0% for Egger’s test, the rank-correlation test, p-uniform, and
TES, respectively. Taking into account the nested structure hardly affected the parameter esti-
mates and did not change the statistical inference (see S5–S8 Tables). All in all, we conclude
that evidence of publication bias was weak at best and that we found no evidence of a differ-
ence in the extent of publication bias existed between subsets from Psychological Bulletin and
CDSR.

Predicting effect size estimation


To answer research question 2, absolute values of the effect size estimates of random-effects
meta-analysis and p-uniform were predicted based on characteristics of the subsets. One-tailed
hypothesis tests were used in case of a directional hypothesis (see Table 2 for a summary of our
hypotheses). Table 5 presents the results of the meta-meta-regression on the absolute value of
the effect size estimates of random-effect meta-analysis. The variables in the model explained

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 17 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

Table 5. Results of meta-meta regression on the absolute value of the random-effects meta-analysis effect size esti-
mate with predictors discipline, I2-statistic, harmonic mean of the standard error (standard error), and number
of effect sizes in a subset.
B (SE) t-value (p-value) 95% CI
Intercept 0.035 (0.018) 1.924 (.055) -0.001;0.07
Discipline 0.056 (0.014) 3.888 (< .001) 0.028;0.084
I2-statistic 0.002 (0.0004) 3.927 (< .001) 0.001;0.002
Standard error 0.776 (0.073) 10.685 (< .001) 0.633;0.918
Number of effect sizes -0.002 (0.0005) -4.910 (< .001) -0.003;-0.001

CDSR is the reference category for discipline. p-values for discipline and harmonic mean of the standard error are
one-tailed whereas the other p-values are two-tailed. CI = Wald-based confidence interval.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052.t005

15.2% of the variance in the estimates of random-effects meta-analysis (R2 = 0.152; F(4,727) =
32.6, p < .001). The absolute value of the meta-analytic estimate was 0.056 larger for subsets
from Psychological Bulletin compared to CDSR, and this effect was statistically significant and
in line with our hypothesis (t(727) = 3.888, p < .001, one-tailed). The I2-statistic had an unex-
pected positive association with the absolute value of the meta-analytic estimate (B = 0.002, t
(727) = 3.927, p < .001, two-tailed). The harmonic mean of the standard error had, as
expected, a positive effect (B = 0.776, t(727) = 10.685, p < .001, one-tailed). The intraclass coef-
ficient that was obtained with the sensitivity analysis where a random effect was included to
take into account that the subsets were nested in meta-analyses was equal to 1.1%. The results
of this sensitivity analysis are shown in S9 Table and were highly similar to the results of the
analyses where the hierarchical structure was not taken into account.
Table 6 shows the results of meta-meta regressions on the absolute value of p-uniform’s esti-
mate as the dependent variable. The proportion explained variance in p-uniform’s estimate
was R2 = .014 (F(5,475) = 1.377, p = .231). None of the predictors was statistically significant.
The results of the sensitivity analysis where a random effect was included to take into account
that the subsets were nested in meta-analyses were highly similar (see S10 Table). This was no
surprise as the intraclass correlation was estimated as 0%. Quantile regression was used as sen-
sitivity analysis to examine whether the results were distorted by extreme effect size estimates
of p-uniform (see S11 Table). The results of the predictors discipline and I2-statistic were also
not statistically significant in the quantile regression. The association of the harmonic mean of
the standard error was lower in the quantile regression but statistically significant (B = 2.021, t

Table 6. Results of meta-meta-regression on the absolute value of p-uniform’s effect size estimate with predictors
discipline, I2-statistic, harmonic mean of the standard error (standard error), proportion of statistically signifi-
cant effect sizes in a subset (Prop. sig. effect sizes), and number of effect sizes in a subset.
B (SE) t-value (p-value) 95% CI
Intercept 0.77 (0.689) 1.118 (0.264) -0.584;2.124
Discipline 0.001 (0.497) 0.001 (0.999) -0.975;0.976
I2-statistic 0.013 (0.014) 0.939 (0.174) -0.014;0.039
Standard error 3.767 (2.587) 1.456 (0.146) -1.316;8.851
Prop. sig. effect sizes -1.287 (0.797) -1.615 (0.107) -2.853;0.279
Number of effect sizes -0.02 (0.015) -1.363 (0.173) -0.049;0.009
2
CDSR is the reference category for discipline. p-value for the I -statistic is one-tailed whereas the other p-values are
two-tailed. CI = Wald-based confidence interval.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052.t006

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 18 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

Table 7. Mean, standard deviation (SD), 95% confidence interval (CI), and median of the Y variable computed with p-uniform and the 10% most precise observed
effect sizes.
p-uniform 10% most precise
Psy. Bull. CDSR Psy. Bull. CDSR
Mean (SD) -0.007 (0.412) 0.042 (0.305) 0.030 (0.181) 0.038 (0.220)
(95% CI) (-0.056;0.043) (0.002;0.083) (0.011;0.048) (0.016;0.061)
Median 0.019 0.051 0.024 0.023

Results are reported for homogeneous subsets of meta-analyses published in Psychological Bulletin (Psy. Bull.) and Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (CDSR).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052.t007

(475) = 7.969, p < .001, two-tailed) and the predictor “proportion of statistically significant
effect sizes” was statistically significant (B = 0.196, t(475) = 2.353, p = .019, two-tailed).

Overestimation of effect size


Table 7 shows descriptive statistics and 95% confidence intervals of the Y-variables comparing
estimates of random-effects meta-analysis with p-uniform (first two columns) and the 10%
most precise observed effect sizes (last two columns). Results of p-uniform suggest that possible
overestimation because of publication bias was at most minimal for subsets from Psychological
Bulletin: mean = -0.007, 95% CI = (-0.056;0.043), and median = 0.019. Overestimation for sub-
sets from CDSR was larger but still small, and statistically significant (mean = 0.042, 95% CI =
(0.002;0.083), median = 0.051). The overestimation based on the 10% most precise observed
effect sizes was also small (d<0.04) and statistically significant for subsets from Psychological
Bulletin (mean = 0.030, 95% CI = (0.011;0.048), median = 0.024) and CDSR (mean = 0.038,
95% CI = (0.016;0.061), median = 0.023). The slight overestimations provide indirect evidence
of publication bias that appears to be similar in both fields (research question 3a).
Table 8 presents the results of the meta-meta regression on Y to answer research question
3b on predictors of the overestimation in effect size caused by publication bias. The predictors
explained 11.8% of the variance of Y (F(5,475) = 12.76, p < .001). The effect size in subsets
from Psychological Bulletin was not significantly larger than from CDSR (B = -0.040, t(475) =
-1.651, p = .951, one-tailed). We found a negative effect of the I2-statistic on Y (B = -0.004,
t(475) = -5.338, p < .001, one-tailed). The hypothesized relationship between the harmonic
mean of the standard error and Y was not statistically significant (B = 0.172, t(475) = 1.371, p =
.086, one-tailed). The proportion of statistically significant effect sizes in a subset was positively
associated with Y (B = 0.182, t(475) = 4.713, p < .001, two-tailed).

Table 8. Results of meta-meta-regression on the effect size overestimation in random-effects meta-analysis when
compared to p-uniform (Y) and predictors discipline, I2-statistic, harmonic mean of the standard error (standard
error), proportion of statistically significant effect sizes in a subset (Prop. sig. effect sizes), and number of effect
sizes in a subset.
B (SE) t-value (p-value) 95% CI
Intercept -0.017 (0.033) -0.517 (.605) -0.083;0.048
Discipline -0.04 (0.024) -1.651 (.951) -0.087;0.008
I2-statistic -0.004 (0.001) -5.338 (< .001) -0.005;-0.002
Standard error 0.172 (0.126) 1.371 (.086) -0.074;0.419
Prop. sig. effect sizes 0.182 (0.039) 4.713 (< .001) 0.106;0.258
Number of effect sizes -0.001 (0.001) -2.064 (.04) -0.003;-0.0001

CDSR is the reference category for discipline. p-values for discipline, the I2-statistic, and the harmonic mean of the
standard error are one-tailed whereas the other p-values are two-tailed. CI = Wald-based confidence interval.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052.t008

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 19 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

Fig 3. Type-I error rate and statistical power of the rank-correlation test (open bullets), Egger’s test (triangles), test of excess significance (TES; diamonds), and p-
uniform’s publication bias test (solid bullets) in the Monte-Carlo simulation study. pub and μ are the extent of publication bias and the average true effect size,
respectively.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052.g003

The results of the sensitivity analysis where a random effect was included to take into
account that subsets were nested in meta-analyses were highly similar (see S12 Table). Again,
this was no surprise as the intraclass correlation coefficient was estimated as practically 0
(0.04%). Quantile regression with the median of Y as dependent variable was conducted to
examine whether the results of the meta-meta-regression were affected by truncating the esti-
mate of p-uniform to zero (see S13 Table). This truncation occurred in 136 subsets before
computing Y. The predictor discipline was not statistically significant in the quantile regres-
sion. In contrast to the results of the meta-meta-regression, the effects of the I2-statistic (B =
-0.0003, t(475) = -0.2, p = .579, one-tailed) and proportion of statistically significant effect size
in a subset (B = -0.002, t(475) = -1.53, p = .127, two-tailed) were no longer statistically signifi-
cant, whereas the predictor harmonic mean of the standard error was statistically significant
(B = 0.279, t(475) = 1.889, p = .03, one-tailed).

Monte-Carlo simulation study


Fig 3 shows the average Type-I error rate and the average statistical power across all 732 data
sets of the rank-correlation test (open bullets), Egger’s test (triangles), the TES (diamonds),
and p-uniform’s publication bias test (solid bullets) as a function of publication bias (pub) and
average true effect size (μ). Average statistical power of all methods increased as a function of
pub and decreased as a function of μ. None of the publication bias tests achieved an average
statistical power larger than 0.5 if pub � 0.95, and average statistical power did not exceed 0.2
for pub � 0.75 for any true effect size examined.
Our Monte-Carlo simulation study confirms the low statistical power of the publication
bias tests to detect publication bias in the small homogeneous subsets examined in this study.
The results of the simulations also indicate that the observed proportions of statistically signifi-
cant publication bias tests for the homogeneous subsets could have occurred for a large range
of values for pub. The observed proportions of statistically significant results for each publica-
tion bias test (ranging from 0.055 to 0.167) are in line with publication bias varying from
pub = 0 to pub = 0.95 for μ = 0 and 0.2 and pub = 0.85 for μ = 0.5. This means that, even in our
large data set with 732 subsets, the proportion of statistically significant results of publication
bias tests does not provide much information on the presence of publication bias in homoge-
neous subsets of meta-analyses as published in Psychological Bulletin and CDSR.

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 20 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

Fig 4. Overestimation (Y) of the random-effects model when compared with the 10% most precise observed effect sizes for simulated data based on
characteristics of subsets from meta-analyses published in Psychological Bulletin (left panel) and Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (CDSR; right panel).
pub refers to the extent of publication bias and open bullets, triangles, and diamonds indicate no, small, and medium average true effect size. The solid line indicates the
mean of the Y-variable observed in the homogeneous subsets and the dashed lines are the upper and lower bound of a 95% confidence interval (CI) around the mean of
the Y-variable.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052.g004

Fig 4 illustrates the overestimation of effect size caused by publication bias for no (μ = 0;
open bullets), small (μ = 0.2; triangles), and medium (μ = 0.5; diamonds) average true effects.
We computed the mean of the Y-variable for each simulated homogeneous subset based on
characteristics of subsets from meta-analyses published in Psychological Bulletin (left panel)
and CDSR (right panel). The solid horizontal lines and dashed horizontal lines in Fig 4 refer to
the mean and 95% confidence interval around this mean of the Y-variable observed in the
homogeneous subsets (see Table 7). A larger value for Y refers to a larger overestimation by
the random-effects model when compared with the 10% most precise observed effect sizes.
Overestimation of effect size increased as a function of pub, but it was small for moderate pub-
lication bias (pub = 0.5) or even quite strong publication bias (pub < .85) for a zero true effect
size.
The overestimation observed in the homogeneous subsets is in line with a large range of val-
ues for pub. For homogeneous subsets of meta-analyses published in Psychological Bulletin
(left panel), the lower and upper bound of the 95% confidence intervals could be observed in
combination with pub > 0.25 and < 0.95 for μ = 0, pub > 0.5 and < 0.95 for μ = 0.2, and
pub > 0.25 and < 0.85 for μ = 0.5. The same holds for the subsets of meta-analyses from CDSR
where the 95% confidence intervals could be observed when pub > 0 and < 0.95 for μ = 0,
pub > 0.5 and < 0.95 for μ = 0.2, and pub > 0.25 and < 0.85 for μ = 0.5. Note, however, we
should be careful with overinterpreting the results depicted in Fig 4 as estimated overestima-
tion quite strongly depends on the assumed true effects, and we do not know the distribution
of true effect size for the included homogeneous subsets. Nonetheless, we believe our Monte-
Carlo simulation study revealed that the results in our study are in line with a wide range of
publication bias, albeit not with scenarios where statistically nonsignificant effect sizes hardly

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 21 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

ever get published. Specifically, our empirical results suggest at least mild publication bias as
estimated overestimation was often not in the observed overestimation’s CI for pub � 0.25
(Fig 4).

Conclusion and discussion


Publication bias is a major threat to the validity of meta-analyses. It results in overestimated
effect sizes in primary studies which in turn also biases the meta-analytic results (e.g., [3, 5]).
Indications for the presence of publication bias have been observed in many research fields
(e.g., [7, 8, 11, 14]), and different methods were developed to examine publication bias in a
meta-analysis (for an overview see [2]). We studied the prevalence of publication bias and the
overestimation caused by it in a large number of meta-analyses published in Psychological Bul-
letin and CDSR by applying publication bias methods to homogeneous subsets of these meta-
analyses. Homogeneous subsets were created, because publication bias methods have poor sta-
tistical properties if the true effect size is heterogeneous [5, 32, 50, 52]. The prevalence of publi-
cation bias was studied by means of Egger’s test [43], the rank-correlation test [47], TES [50],
and p-uniform’s publication bias test [5]. We used p-uniform and a meta-analysis based on the
10% most precise effect size estimates of a meta-analysis to estimate the effect size corrected
for publication bias. The statistical properties of our preregistered analyses were also examined
by means of a Monte-Carlo simulation study. Our paper is different from previous work [15–
21] that studied the presence of questionable research practices and publication bias based on
the distribution of p-values, because we did not analyze the distribution of p-values of studies
published in a whole research field.
The results of our paper are not in line with previous research showing rather strong indica-
tions for publication bias in numerous research fields (e.g., [7, 8, 11, 14]) and revealing the
presence of small-study effects in another meta-meta-analysis [107]. The best illustration of
the diverging results of previous research and our study is that many (subsets of) meta-analyses
did not contain a single statistically significant effect size (41.5% in CDSR and 27% in Psycho-
logical Bulletin), and only a minority of observed primary effect sizes was statistically signifi-
cant (18.9% in CDSR and 28.9% in Psychological Bulletin). Although the percentage of
statistically significant findings was slightly higher before excluding heterogeneous subsets
(28.9% in CDSR and 44.2% in Psychological Bulletin), these percentages are substantially
lower than the percentage of times the main hypothesis of a paper was deemed to be supported
according to previous research. For example, approximately 90% of these hypotheses were sta-
tistically significant in the psychology and psychiatry [7, 8] and clinical medicine literature
[108]. To put the percentages of statistically significant effect sizes in perspective, imagine that
only 25% of all effects examined in a field have a nonzero true effect size and that statistical
power equals 0.5 for a one-tailed hypothesis test with α = .025 to resemble researchers con-
ducting a two-tailed test and reporting only the results in the predicted direction. Then 18.9%
and 28.9% significant effects imply that 72% and 41.3% of nonsignificant effects get published,
respectively (pub equals 0.28 and 0.587). Assuming higher prevalence of nonzero true effects,
higher power, or assuming p-hacking implies less publication bias than calculated here. Thus,
these numbers alone imply that if publication bias exists in homogeneous (subsets of) meta-
analyses in psychology and medicine, then statistically nonsignificant effects still have a non-
zero probability of getting published.
Only weak evidence for the prevalence of publication bias was observed in our large-scale
data set of homogeneous subsets of primary studies. No evidence of bias was obtained using
the publication bias tests. Overestimation was minimal but statistically significant (except for
subsets from Psychological Bulletin in combination with p-uniform as estimator), providing

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 22 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

evidence of publication bias that appeared to be similar in both fields. The simulation study (not
preregistered) showed that the publication bias tests were only reasonably powered to detect
extreme publication bias where all statistically nonsignificant effect sizes remain unpublished. No
evidence for this extreme publication bias was present in our large data set. It also showed that the
observed overestimation in the large data set was consistent with a wide range of relatively mild
publication bias scenarios (but not with a scenario with extreme publication bias). Based on these
findings in combination with the small percentages of statistically significant effect sizes in psy-
chology and medicine, we conclude that evidence for publication bias in the studied homoge-
neous subsets is weak, but suggestive of mild publication bias in both disciplines.
A meta-meta-regression on the random-effects meta-analytic estimates revealed, in line
with the hypothesis, a negative association of primary studies’ precision with a meta-analytic
estimate. Since only weak evidence for publication bias was observed, this association was
most likely mainly caused by differences in sample sizes between research fields. For instance,
if researchers use statistical power analysis to determine the sample size of their study or if
researchers in fields characterized by lower effect sizes use larger sample sizes by habit, larger
true effect sizes will be associated with studies using smaller sample sizes (see supplemental
materials of [44] and [45]).
The same predictors used for predicting the random-effects meta-analytic effect size esti-
mate were also used in a meta-meta-regression on p-uniform’s estimate. None of the predic-
tors statistically significantly predicted p-uniform’s effect size estimate. This was in line with
our hypothesis on the relationship with primary studies’ precision, but in contrast to the
expected positive relationship between the I2-statistic and p-uniform’s effect size estimate. The
absence of such a positive relationship indicates that p-uniform did not overestimate the effect
size in the presence of heterogeneity in true effect size. The initially unexpected positive associ-
ation between the meta-analytic estimate and I2-statistic is in line with two very recent studies
on multi-lab replication [109, 110], which showed absence of an effect in combination with
homogeneous effect size, and heterogeneous effect sizes only in combination with some non-
zero average effect size. The explained variance in the meta-meta-regression with p-uniform’s
estimate as dependent variable (1.4%) was substantially lower than with the estimate of ran-
dom-effects meta-analysis as dependent variable (67.6%). This difference in explained variance
was mainly caused by the large variance in p-uniform’s effect size estimates across homoge-
neous subsets. The variance in these estimates was large, because estimates of p-uniform were
based on a smaller subset of (statistically significant) effect sizes and were sometimes extremely
positive or negative caused by observed effect sizes with p-values just below the α-level.
The different publication bias methods were not always in agreement with each other,
which is caused by the absence of clear publication biases in the meta-analytic homogeneous
subsets. An exception was the association between the results of Egger’s test and the rank-cor-
relation test, but this association was expected since both methods are very similar in method-
ology (i.e., testing for publication bias by examining the relationship between observed effect
size and some measure of its precision). Substantial differences were also observed among the
two methods that we used to correct effect size estimates for publication bias (i.e., p-uniform
and the 10% most precise observed effect size estimates). Effect size estimates based on the
10% most precise observed effect sizes were close to estimates of the random-effects meta-anal-
ysis whereas estimates of p-uniform were imprecise and sometimes very different from ran-
dom-effects meta-analysis and the 10% most precise observed effect sizes. This suggests that p-
uniform overcorrected for publication bias because of a small number of observed effect sizes
in homogeneous subsets combined with p-values of primary studies being close to the α-level,
because estimates based on the 10% most precise observed effect sizes are expected to be closer
to p-uniform’s estimates in case of publication bias.

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 23 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

Our weak evidence of publication bias is at odds with meta-meta-analyses that found evi-
dence for the presence of small-study effects (i.e., [107, 111]). One possible explanation for this
disparity is that we created homogeneous subsets of primary studies in this paper. Small-study
effects can be caused by heterogeneity in true effect size, so eliminating this heterogeneity by
creating homogeneous subsets may have led to not observing small-study effects. Publication
bias could, however, have gone undetected due to a variety of reasons. First, publication bias is
less of an issue if the relationship of interest in a meta-analysis was not the main focus of the
primary studies. Statistical significance of the main result in a primary study probably deter-
mines whether a result gets published, rather than whether a secondary outcome or supple-
mentary result is significant. For instance, a meta-analysis might be about gender differences
where data is extracted from studies that used gender only as a control variable. Second, meta-
analysts included many unpublished studies in their meta-analyses, which might have
decreased the severity and detectability of publication bias in our selected meta-analyses.
Third, questionable research practices may have also decreased the detectability of publication
bias in the meta-analyses, because questionable research practices may bias the effect size esti-
mates of meta-analysis methods in any direction [52]. However, we do not believe that ques-
tionable research practices played a major role in the subsets in our paper, as relatively few
effect sizes were statistically significant. The weak evidence for publication bias may be also
caused by the challenging characteristics of the homogeneous subsets. Our Monte-Carlo simu-
lation study revealed that publication bias tests did not achieve reasonable statistical power to
detect moderate publication bias, and overestimation of effect size only became apparent in
conditions where at most 1 out of 4 statistically nonsignificant effect size was included in a
meta-analysis (pub � 0.75).
Our focus on homogeneous (subsets of) meta-analyses limits the generalizability of our
findings, because our conclusions can only straightforwardly be generalized to the population
of subsets of primary studies of meta-analyses without evidence for medium or higher hetero-
geneity. However, limiting our population was necessary because publication bias methods’
statistical properties deteriorate if heterogeneity in true effect size is moderate or large. Deter-
mining whether primary studies in a subset were homogeneous or not was based on the I2-sta-
tistic. Homogeneous subsets that belong to the population under study can readily be
identified in a meta-analysis, because the I2-statistic is now almost always reported in a meta-
analysis [112] and routinely included in any Cochrane systematic review [87]. Another option
for assessing the heterogeneity in true effect size is the Q-test for homogeneity [113], but this
hypothesis test suffers from (too) low statistical power if a small number of primary studies are
included in the meta-analysis [85, 114, 115]. However, drawbacks of the I2-statistic are that it
heavily depends on the sample size of the primary studies [86] and the statistic is imprecise in
case of a small number of primary studies in a meta-analysis [87, 88]. As a consequence, we
may have wrongly included subsets that were heterogeneous and excluded subsets that were
homogeneous. Moreover, our selection of homogeneous subsets could have led to the exclu-
sion of subsets with severe publication bias. Imagine a subset with a number of statistically sig-
nificant effect sizes that were published in a field with considerable publication bias, and a few
statistically nonsignificant effect sizes that were obtained from unpublished research. The
inclusion of the effect sizes from the unpublished research may cause heterogeneity in true
effect size, and therefore a subset with potentially severe publication bias was excluded from
our study.
Although no convincing evidence for publication bias was observed in our study, we agree
with others (e.g., [34, 60, 116]) that publication bias should be routinely assessed in every
meta-analysis. Moreover, a set of publication bias methods is recommended to be applied and
reported in each meta-analysis, because each method assesses publication bias in a different

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 24 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

way and one method might detect or correct for publication bias in a meta-analysis whereas
another method might not [35, 36]. Future research should focus on developing publication
bias methods that are able to examine publication bias in meta-analyses with heterogeneous
true effect size, because effect size estimators that correct for publication bias are often biased
in these conditions (e.g., [5, 52, 61, 62]). However, p-uniform was recently extended such that
it can also deal with heterogeneous true effect size [106]. Other promising developments are
the recently increased attention for selection model approaches (e.g., [61, 62, 106, 117]) and
the development of a Bayesian method to correct for publication bias [80]. Albeit meta-ana-
lysts will greatly benefit from improved methods to assess publication bias, attention should
also be paid to registering studies to make unpublished research readily accessible (e.g., [118]).
Such a register enables meta-analysts to also include unpublished research in their meta-analy-
sis and will improve the validity of meta-analytic results.

Supporting information
S1 Table. Results of logistic regression predicting statistical significance of Egger’s regres-
sion test with discipline and control variable number of effect sizes in a subset.
(DOCX)
S2 Table. Results of logistic regression predicting statistical significance of rank-correla-
tion test with discipline and control variable number of effect sizes in a subset.
(DOCX)
S3 Table. Results of logistic regression predicting statistical significance of p-uniform’s
publication bias test with discipline and control variable number of statistically significant
effect sizes in a subset.
(DOCX)
S4 Table. Results of logistic regression predicting statistical significance of test of excess
significance with discipline and control variable number of effect sizes in a subset.
(DOCX)
S5 Table. Results of multilevel logistic regression predicting statistical significance of
Egger’s regression test with discipline and control variable number of effect sizes in a sub-
set.
(DOCX)
S6 Table. Results of multilevel logistic regression predicting statistical significance of
rank-correlation test with discipline and control variable number of effect sizes in a subset.
(DOCX)
S7 Table. Results of multilevel logistic regression predicting statistical significance of p-
uniform’s publication bias test with discipline and control variable number of statistically
significant effect sizes in a subset.
(DOCX)
S8 Table. Results of multilevel logistic regression predicting statistical significance of test
of excess significance with discipline and control variable number of effect sizes in a sub-
set.
(DOCX)
S9 Table. Results of meta-meta regression with a random effect to take into account that
the subsets were nested in meta-analyses. The dependent variable is the absolute value of the
random-effects meta-analysis effect size estimate with predictors discipline, I2-statistic,

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 25 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

harmonic mean of the standard error (standard error), proportion of statistically significant
effect sizes in a subset (Prop. sig. effect sizes), and number of effect sizes in a subset.
(DOCX)
S10 Table. Results of meta-meta regression with a random effect to take into account that
the subsets were nested in meta-analyses. The dependent variable is the absolute value of p-
uniform’s effect size estimate with predictors discipline, I2-statistic, harmonic mean of the
standard error (standard error), proportion of statistically significant effect sizes in a subset
(Prop. sig. effect sizes), and number of effect sizes in a subset.
(DOCX)
S11 Table. Results of quantile regression with the median of p-uniform’s effect size esti-
mates and predictors discipline, I2-statistic, harmonic mean of the standard error (stan-
dard error), proportion of statistically significant effect sizes in a subset (Prop. sig. effect
sizes), and number of effect sizes in a subset.
(DOCX)
S12 Table. Results of meta-meta-regression with a random effect to take into account that
the subsets were nested in meta-analyses. The dependent variable is the effect size overestima-
tion in random-effects meta-analysis when compared to p-uniform (Y) and predictors discipline,
I2-statistic, harmonic mean of the standard error (standard error), proportion of statistically sig-
nificant effect sizes in a subset (Prop. sig. effect sizes), and number of effect sizes in a subset.
(DOCX)
S13 Table. Results of quantile regression with the median of effect size overestimation in ran-
dom-effects meta-analysis when compared to p-uniform (Y) and predictors discipline, I2-sta-
tistic, harmonic mean of the standard error (standard error), proportion of statistically
significant effect sizes in a subset (Prop. sig. effect sizes), and number of effect sizes in a subset.
(DOCX)
S1 File. List of references of meta-analyses where the data of the primary studies were
obtained after contacting the corresponding author.
(DOCX)

Author Contributions
Conceptualization: Robbie C. M. van Aert, Jelte M. Wicherts, Marcel A. L. M. van Assen.
Formal analysis: Robbie C. M. van Aert.
Methodology: Robbie C. M. van Aert, Jelte M. Wicherts, Marcel A. L. M. van Assen.
Project administration: Robbie C. M. van Aert.
Software: Robbie C. M. van Aert.
Supervision: Jelte M. Wicherts, Marcel A. L. M. van Assen.
Writing – original draft: Robbie C. M. van Aert.
Writing – review & editing: Robbie C. M. van Aert, Jelte M. Wicherts, Marcel A. L. M. van
Assen.

References
1. Glass GV. Primary, secondary, and meta-analysis of research. Educational Researcher. 1976; 5
(10):3–8.

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 26 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

2. Rothstein HR, Sutton AJ, Borenstein M. Publication bias in meta-analysis. In: Rothstein HR, Sutton
AJ, Borenstein M, editors. Publication bias in meta-analysis: Prevention, assessment and adjust-
ments. Chichester, UK: Wiley; 2005.
3. Lane DM, Dunlap WP. Estimating effect size: Bias resulting from the significance criterion in editorial
decisions. British Journal of Mathematical & Statistical Psychology. 1978; 31:107–12.
4. Nuijten MB, van Assen MALM, Veldkamp CLS, Wicherts JM. The replication paradox: Combining
studies can decrease accuracy of effect size estimates. Review of General Psychology. 2015; 19
(2):172–82. https://doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000034
5. van Assen MALM, van Aert RCM, Wicherts JM. Meta-analysis using effect size distributions of only
statistically significant studies. Psychological Methods. 2015; 20(3):293–309. https://doi.org/10.1037/
met0000025 PMID: 25401773
6. Bouter LM, Tijdink J, Axelsen N, Martinson BC, ter Riet G. Ranking major and minor research misbe-
haviors: results from a survey among participants of four World Conferences on Research Integrity.
Research Integrity and Peer Review. 2016; 1(1). https://doi.org/10.1186/s41073-016-0024-5.
7. Sterling TD, Rosenbaum WL, Weinkam JJ. Publication decisions revisited: The effect of the outcome
of statistical tests on the decision to publish and vice versa. The American Statistician. 1995; 49
(1):108–12. https://doi.org/10.2307/2684823
8. Fanelli D. Negative results are disappearing from most disciplines and countries. Scientometrics.
2012; 90(3):891–904. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11192-011-0494-7
9. Bakker M, van Dijk A, Wicherts JM. The rules of the game called psychological science. Perspectives
on Psychological Science. 2012; 7(6):543–54. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612459060 PMID:
26168111
10. Cohen J. Things I have learned (so far). American Psychologist. 1990; 45(12):1304–12.
11. Franco A, Malhotra N, Simonovits G. Publication bias in the social sciences: Unlocking the file drawer.
Science. 2014; 345(6203):1502–5. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1255484 PMID: 25170047
12. Cooper H, DeNeve K, Charlton K. Finding the missing science: The fate of studies submitted for review
by a human subjects committee. Psychological Methods. 1997; 2(4):447–52. https://doi.org/10.1037/
1082-989X.2.4.447
13. Coursol A, Wagner EE. Effect of positive findings on submission and acceptance rates: A note on
meta-analysis bias. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice. 1986; 17(2):136–7. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0735-7028.17.2.136
14. Franco A, Simonovits G, Malhotra N. Underreporting in psychology experiments: Evidence from a
study registry. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2016; 7(1):8–12. https://doi.org/10.
1177/1948550615598377
15. Jager LR, Leek JT. An estimate of the science-wise false discovery rate and application to the top
medical literature. Biostatistics. 2014; 15(1):1–12. https://doi.org/10.1093/biostatistics/kxt007 PMID:
24068246
16. Head ML, Holman L, Lanfear R, Kahn AT, Jennions MD. The extent and consequences of p-hacking
in science. PLoS Biology. 2015; 13(3):e1002106. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002106 PMID:
25768323
17. Hartgerink CHJ, van Aert RCM, Nuijten MB, Wicherts JM, van Assen MALM. Distributions of p-values
smaller than .05 in psychology: What is going on? PeerJ. 2015; 4:e1935. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.
1935.
18. Hartgerink CHJ. Reanalyzing Head et al. (2015): Investigating the robustness of widespread p-hack-
ing. PeerJ. 2017; 5. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.3068
19. Brodeur A, Le M, Sangnier M, Zylberberg Y. Star Wars: The empirics strike back. American Economic
Journal: Applied Economics. 2016; 8(1):1–32. https://doi.org/10.1257/app.20150044
20. Krawczyk M. The search for significance: A few peculiarities in the distribution of p values in experi-
mental psychology literature. PLOS ONE. 2015; 10(6):e0127872. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.
pone.0127872 PMID: 26061881
21. Auspurg K, Hinz T, Schneck A. Ausmaß und Risikofaktoren des Publication Bias in der deutschen
Soziologie. KZfSS Kölner Zeitschrift für Soziologie und Sozialpsychologie. 2014; 66(4):549–73.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11577-014-0284-3
22. Hopewell S, Clarke M, Mallet S. Grey literature and systematic reviews. In: Rothstein HR, Sutton AJ,
Borenstein M, editors. Publication bias in meta-analysis: Prevention, assessment and adjust-
ments2005. p. 99–110.
23. Dickersin K, Chan S, Chalmers TC, Sacks HS, Smith H. Publication bias and clinical trials. Controlled
Clinical Trials. 1987; 8(4):343–53. PMID: 3442991

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 27 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

24. Jones CW, Handler L, Crowell KE, Keil LG, Weaver MA, Platts-Mills TF. Non-publication of large ran-
domized clinical trials: Cross sectional analysis. British Medical Journal. 2013; 347. https://doi.org/10.
1136/bmj.f6104 PMID: 24169943
25. United States Code (2007) US Public Law 110–85: Food and Drug Administration Amendments Act
2007. http://frwebgate.access.gpo.gov/cgi-bin/getdoc.cgi?dbname=110_cong_public_laws&docid=f:
publ085.110.pdf.
26. Dwan K, Altman DG, Arnaiz JA, Bloom J, Chan A-W, Cronin E, et al. Systematic review of the empiri-
cal evidence of study publication bias and outcome reporting bias. PLoS ONE. 2008; 3(8):e3081.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0003081 PMID: 18769481
27. Kirkham JJ, Dwan KM, Altman DG, Gamble C, Dodd S, Smyth R, et al. The impact of outcome report-
ing bias in randomised controlled trials on a cohort of systematic reviews. BMJ. 2010; 340(c365).
https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.c365 PMID: 20156912
28. Song F, Parekh S, Hooper L, Loke YK, Ryder J, Sutton AJ, et al. Dissemination and publication of
research findings: An updated review of related biases. Health Technology Assessment. 2010; 14(8).
https://doi.org/10.3310/hta14080 PMID: 20181324
29. Aguinis H, Dalton DR, Bosco FA, Pierce CA, Dalton CM. Meta-analytic choices and judgment calls:
Implications for theory building and testing, obtained effect sizes, and scholarly impact. Journal of
Management. 2010; 37(1):5–38. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206310377113
30. Banks GC, Kepes S, Banks KP. Publication bias: The antagonist of meta-analytic reviews and effec-
tive policymaking. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis. 2012; 34(3):259–77. https://doi.org/10.
3102/0162373712446144
31. Aytug ZG, Rothstein HR, Zhou W, Kern MC. Revealed or concealed? Transparency of procedures,
decisions, and judgment calls in meta-analyses. Organizational Research Methods. 2012; 15(1):103–
33. https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428111403495
32. Ioannidis JP, Trikalinos TA. The appropriateness of asymmetry tests for publication bias in meta-anal-
yses: A large survey. Canadian Medical Association Journal. 2007; 176(8):1091–6. https://doi.org/10.
1503/cmaj.060410 PMID: 17420491
33. Ioannidis JPA. Why most discovered true associations are inflated. Epidemiology. 2008; 19(5):640–8.
https://doi.org/10.1097/EDE.0b013e31818131e7 ISI:000258712000001. PMID: 18633328
34. Banks GC, Kepes S, McDaniel MA. Publication bias: A call for improved meta-analytic practice in the
organizational sciences. International Journal of Selection and Assessment. 2012; 20(2):182–97.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2389.2012.00591
35. Kepes S, Banks GC, McDaniel M, Whetzel DL. Publication bias in the organizational sciences. Organi-
zational Research Methods. 2012; 15(4):624–62. https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428112452760
36. Coburn KM, Vevea JL. Publication bias as a function of study characteristics. Psychological methods.
2015; 20(3):310–30. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000046 PMID: 26348731
37. Ferguson CJ, Brannick MT. Publication bias in psychological science: Prevalence, methods for identi-
fying and controlling, and implications for the use of meta-analyses. Psychological Methods. 2012; 17
(1):120–8. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024445 PMID: 21787082
38. Rosenthal R. The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin. 1979; 86
(3):638–41.
39. Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JPT, Rothstein HR. Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester, UK:
John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.; 2009.
40. Becker BJ. Failsafe N or file-drawer number. In: Rothstein HR, Sutton AJ, Borenstein M, editors. Publi-
cation bias in meta-analysis: Prevention, assessment and adjustments. Chichester, UK: Wiley; 2005.
p. 111–25.
41. Light RJ, Pillemer DB. Summing up: The science of reviewing research. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press; 1984.
42. Jürgens G, Graudal NA. Effects of low sodium diet versus high sodium diet on blood pressure, renin,
aldosterone, catecholamines, cholesterols, and triglyceride. Cochrane Database of Systematic
Reviews. 2004;(1). https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD004022.pub2 CD004022. PMID: 14974053
43. Egger M, Smith GD, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical
test. British Medical Journal. 1997; 315:629–34. PMID: 9310563
44. Open Science Collaboration. Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science. 2015;
349(6251). https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aac4716 PMID: 26315443
45. Hedges LV, Vevea JL. Selection method approaches. In: Rothstein HR, Sutton AJ, Borenstein M, edi-
tors. Publication bias in meta-analysis: Prevention, assessment, and adjustments. Chichester: UK:
Wiley; 2005.

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 28 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

46. Terrin N, Schmid CH, Lau J. In an empirical evaluation of the funnel plot, researchers could not visually
identify publication bias. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 2005; 58(9):894–901. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.jclinepi.2005.01.006 PMID: 16085192
47. Begg CB, Mazumdar M. Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Bio-
metrics. 1994; 50(4):1088–101. PMID: 7786990
48. Sterne JAC, Harbord RM, Sutton AJ, Jones DR, Ioannidis JP, Terrin N, et al. Recommendations for
examining and interpreting funnel plot asymmetry in meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials.
British Medical Journal. 2011; 343(7818):1–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.d4002.
49. Sterne JAC, Gavaghan D, Egger M. Publication and related bias in meta-analysis: Power of statistical
tests and prevalence in the literature. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 2000; 53(11):1119–29. https://
doi.org/10.1016/S0895-4356(00)00242-0 PMID: 11106885
50. Ioannidis JP, Trikalinos TA. An exploratory test for an excess of significant findings. Clinical Trials.
2007; 4(3):245–53. https://doi.org/10.1177/1740774507079441 PMID: 17715249
51. Replication Francis G., statistical consistency, and publication bias. Journal of Mathematical Psychol-
ogy. 2013; 57(5):153–69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmp.2013.02.003
52. van Aert RCM, Wicherts JM, van Assen MALM. Conducting meta-analyses on p-values: Reservations
and recommendations for applying p-uniform and p-curve. Perspectives on Psychological Science.
2016; 11(5):713–29. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616650874 PMID: 27694466
53. Simonsohn U, Nelson LD, Simmons JP. P-curve and effect size: Correcting for publication bias using
only significant results. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2014; 9(6):666–81. https://doi.org/10.
1177/1745691614553988 PMID: 26186117
54. Moreno SG, Sutton AJ, Ades AE, Stanley TD, Abrams KR, Peters JL, et al. Assessment of regression-
based methods to adjust for publication bias through a comprehensive simulation study. BMC Medical
Research Methodology. 2009; 9(2). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2288-9-2 PMID: 19138428
55. Terrin N, Schmid CH, Lau J, Olkin I. Adjusting for publication bias in the presence of heterogeneity.
Statistics in Medicine. 2003; 22(13):2113–26. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.1461 PMID: 12820277
56. Stanley TD, Doucouliagos H, Ioannidis JP. Finding the power to reduce publication bias. Statistics in
Medicine. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.7228 PMID: 28127782
57. Stanley TD. Limitations of PET-PEESE and other meta-analysis methods. Social Psychological and
Personality Science. 2017; 8(5):581–91. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617693062
58. Reed WR. A Monte Carlo analysis of alternative meta-analysis estimators in the presence of publica-
tion bias. Economics: The Open-Access, Open-Assessment E-Journal. 2015. https://doi.org/10.5018/
economics-ejournal.ja.2015-30
59. Alinaghi N, Reed WR. Meta-analysis and publication bias: How well does the FAT-PET-PEESE proce-
dure work? Research Synthesis Methods. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.1298 PMID: 29532634
60. Field AP, Gillett R. How to do a meta-analysis. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychol-
ogy. 2010; 63(3):665–94. https://doi.org/10.1348/000711010X502733 PMID: 20497626
61. Carter EC, Schönbrodt FD, Gervais WM, Hilgard J. Correcting for bias in psychology: A comparison of
meta-analytic methods. 2018. Available from osf.io/preprints/psyarxiv/9h3nu
62. McShane BB, Böckenholt U, Hansen KT. Adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis: An evaluation
of selection methods and some cautionary notes. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2016; 11
(5):730–49. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616662243 PMID: 27694467
63. Stanley TD, Jarrell SB, Doucouliagos H. Could it be better to discard 90% of the data? A statistical par-
adox. The American Statistician. 2010; 64(1):70–7. https://doi.org/10.1198/tast.2009.08205
64. Ioannidis JP, Trikalinos TA, Zintzaras E. Extreme between-study homogeneity in meta-analyses could
offer useful insights. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 2006; 59(10):1023–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
jclinepi.2006.02.013 PMID: 16980141
65. Stanley TD, Doucouliagos H. Meta-regression approximations to reduce publication selection bias.
Research Synthesis Methods. 2014; 5(1):60–78. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.1095 PMID: 26054026
66. Higgins JPT. Commentary: Heterogeneity in meta-analysis should be expected and appropriately
quantified. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2008; 37(5):1158–60. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/
dyn204 PMID: 18832388
67. Turner RM, Jackson D, Wei Y, Thompson SG, Higgins JPT. Predictive distributions for between-study
heterogeneity and simple methods for their application in Bayesian meta-analysis. Statistics in Medi-
cine. 2015; 34(6):984–98. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.6381 PMID: 25475839
68. Rhodes KM, Turner RM, Higgins JP. Predictive distributions were developed for the extent of hetero-
geneity in meta-analyses of continuous outcome data. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 2015; 68
(1):52–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2014.08.012 PMID: 25304503

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 29 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

69. Duval S, Tweedie RL. Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for pub-
lication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics. 2000; 56(2):455–63. PMID: 10877304
70. Duval S, Tweedie RL. A nonparametric "trim and fill" method of accounting for publication bias in
meta-analysis. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 2000; 95(449):89–98. https://doi.org/
10.1080/01621459.2000.10473905
71. Peters JL, Sutton AJ, Jones DR, Abrams KR, Rushton L. Performance of the trim and fill method in the
presence of publication bias and between-study heterogeneity. Statistics in Medicine. 2007; 26
(25):4544–62. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.2889 PMID: 17476644
72. Rothstein HR, Bushman BJ. Publication bias in psychological science: Comment on Ferguson and
Brannick (2012). Psychological Methods. 2012; 17(1):129–36. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027128
PMID: 22369520
73. Hedges LV. Estimation of effect size under nonrandom sampling: The effects of censoring studies
yielding statistically insignificant mean differences. Journal of Educational Statistics. 1984; 9(1):61–
85.
74. Iyengar S, Greenhouse JB. Selection models and the file drawer problem. Statistical Science. 1988; 3
(1):109–17. https://doi.org/10.1214/ss/1177013012
75. Dear KBG, Begg CB. An approach for assessing publication bias prior to performing a meta-analysis.
Statistical Science. 1992; 7(2):237–45.
76. Hedges LV. Modeling publication selection effects in meta-analysis. Statistical Science. 1992; 7
(2):246–55.
77. Vevea JL, Hedges LV. A general linear model for estimating effect size in the presence of publication
bias. Psychometrika. 1995; 60(3):419–35. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf02294384
78. Copas JB. What works?: Selectivity models and meta-analysis. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society
Series A 1999; 162(1):95–109.
79. Vevea JL, Woods CM. Publication bias in research synthesis: Sensitivity analysis using a priori weight
functions. Psychological Methods. 2005; 10(4):428–43. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.10.4.428
PMID: 16392998
80. Guan M, Vandekerckhove J. A Bayesian approach to mitigation of publication bias. Psychonomic Bul-
letin and Review. 2015:Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0868-6 PMID:
26126776
81. Iyengar S, Greenhouse JB. Selection models and the file drawer problem: Rejoinder. Statistical Sci-
ence. 1988; 3(1):133–5.
82. Borenstein M. Effect sizes for continuous data. In: Cooper H, Hedges LV, Valentine JC, editors. The
Handbook of Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 2009. p.
221–36.
83. Fleiss JL, Berlin JA. Effect sizes for dichotomous data. In: Cooper H, Hedges LV, Valentine JC, edi-
tors. The Handbook of Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis. New York: Russell Sage Foundation;
2009. p. 237–53.
84. Viechtbauer W. Approximate confidence intervals for standardized effect sizes in the two-independent
and two-dependent samples design. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics. 2007; 32(1):39–
60. https://doi.org/10.3102/1076998606298034
85. Higgins JPT, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. British
Medical Journal. 2003; 327(7414):557–60. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557 PMID:
12958120
86. Rücker G, Schwarzer G, Carpenter JR, Schumacher M. Undue reliance on I2 in assessing heteroge-
neity may mislead. BMC Medical Research Methodology. 2008; 8(1):79. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-
2288-8-79 PMID: 19036172
87. Ioannidis JP, Patsopoulos NA, Evangelou E. Uncertainty in heterogeneity estimates in meta-analyses.
BMJ. 2007; 335(7626):914–6. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.39343.408449.80 PMID: 17974687
88. von Hippel PT. The heterogeneity statistic I2 can be biased in small meta-analyses. BMC Medical
Research Methodology. 2015; 15:35. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12874-015-0024-z PMC4410499.
PMID: 25880989
89. Macaskill P, Walter SD, Irwig L. A comparison of methods to detect publication bias in meta-analysis.
Statistics in Medicine. 2001; 20(4):641–54. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.698 PMID: 11223905
90. Springate DA, Kontopantelis E. Cochrane_scraper: tools for downloading data from the Cochrane
Library of Systematic Reviews. 1.1.0 ed2014.
91. Loevinger J. The technic of homogeneous tests compared with some aspects of scale analysis and
factor analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 1948; 45(6):507–29. PMID: 18893224

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 30 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

92. Raudenbush SW. Analyzing effect sizes: Random-effects models. In: Cooper H, Hedges LV, Valen-
tine JC, editors. The Handbook of Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis. New York: Russell Sage
Foundation; 2009. p. 295–315.
93. Paule RC, Mandel J. Consensus values and weighting factors. Journal of Research of the National
Bureau of Standards. 1982; 87(5):377–85.
94. Veroniki AA, Jackson D, Viechtbauer W, Bender R, Bowden J, Knapp G, et al. Methods to estimate
the between-study variance and its uncertainty in meta-analysis. Research Synthesis Methods. 2016;
7(1):55–79. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.1164 PMID: 26332144
95. Langan D, Higgins JPT, Simmonds M. Comparative performance of heterogeneity variance estimators
in meta-analysis: A review of simulation studies. Research Synthesis Methods. 2016; 8(2):181–98.
https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.1198 PMID: 27060925
96. Jackson D. The implications of publication bias for meta-analysis’ other parameter. Statistics in Medi-
cine. 2006; 25(17):2911–21. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.2293 PMID: 16345059
97. Augusteijn HEM, van Aert RCM, van Assen MALM. The effect of publication bias on the Q-test and
assessment of heterogeneity. Psychological Methods. in press.
98. Koenker R. Quantile regression. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2005.
99. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erl-
baum Associates; 1988.
100. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. 2018.
101. Viechtbauer W. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal of Statistical Soft-
ware. 2010; 36(3):1–48.
102. van Aert RCM. puniform: Meta-analysis methods correcting for publication bias. 0.1.0 ed2018.
103. Else-Quest NM, Hyde JS, Goldsmith HH, Van Hulle CA. Gender differences in temperament: A meta-
analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2006; 132(1):33–72. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.33
PMID: 16435957
104. van Aert RCM, van Assen MALM. Examining reproducibility in psychology: A hybrid method for com-
bining a statistically significant original study and a replication. Behavior Research Methods. 2018; 50
(4):1515–39. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0967-6 PMID: 28936638
105. Anderson SF, Kelley K, Maxwell SE. Sample-size planning for more accurate statistical power: A
method adjusting sample effect sizes for publication bias and uncertainty. Psychological Science.
2017; 28(11):1547–62. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617723724 PMID: 28902575.
106. van Aert RCM, van Assen MALM. Correcting for publication bias in a meta-analysis with the p-uni-
form* method. Manuscript submitted for publication Retrieved from: https://osfio/preprints/bitss/
zqjr92018.
107. Fanelli D, Costas R, Ioannidis JP. Meta-assessment of bias in science. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2017.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1618569114 PMID: 28320937
108. Fanelli D. “Positive” results increase down the hierarchy of the sciences. PLoS ONE. 2010; 5(4):
e10068. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0010068 PMID: 20383332
109. Olsson-Collentine A, Wicherts JM, van Assen MALM. Limited evidence for widespread heterogeneity
in psychology. Manuscript submitted for publication. 2019.
110. Klein RA, Vianello M, Hasselman F, Adams BG, Adams RB, Alper S, et al. Many Labs 2: Investigating
variation in replicability across samples and settings. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psycho-
logical Science. 2018; 1(4):443–90. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245918810225
111. Fanelli D, Ioannidis JP. US studies may overestimate effect sizes in softer research. Proc Natl Acad
Sci USA. 2013; 110(37):15031–6. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1302997110 PMID: 23980165
112. Jackson D, White IR, Riley RD. Quantifying the impact of between-study heterogeneity in multivariate
meta-analyses. Statistics in Medicine. 2012; 31(29):3805–20. https://doi.org/10.1002/sim.5453 PMID:
22763950
113. Cochran WG. The combination of estimates from different experiments. Biometrics. 1954; 10(1):101–
29.
114. Hardy RJ, Thompson SG. Detecting and describing heterogeneity in meta-analysis. Statistics in Medi-
cine. 1998; 17(8):841–56. PMID: 9595615
115. Viechtbauer W. Hypothesis tests for population heterogeneity in meta-analysis. British Journal of
Mathematical and Statistical Psychology. 2007; 60:29–60. https://doi.org/10.1348/000711005X64042
PMID: 17535578
116. Sutton AJ. Evidence concerning the consequences of publication and related biases. In: Rothstein
HR, Sutton AJ, Borenstein M, editors. Publication bias in meta-analysis: Prevention, assessment and
adjustments. Chichester, UK: Wiley; 2005. p. 175–92.

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 31 / 32


Studying publication bias in a meta-meta-analysis

117. Citkowicz M, Vevea JL. A parsimonious weight function for modeling publication bias. Psychological
Methods. 2017; 22(1):28–41. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000119 PMID: 28252998
118. Dickersin K. Publication bias: Recognizing the problem understanding its origins and scope, and pre-
venting harm. In: Rothstein HR, Sutton AJ, Borenstein M, editors. Publication bias in meta-analysis:
Prevention, assessment and adjustments. Chichester, England: Wiley; 2005. p. 11–33.

PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215052 April 12, 2019 32 / 32

You might also like