Hertz-Picciotto Et Al. 2018
Hertz-Picciotto Et Al. 2018
Hertz-Picciotto Et Al. 2018
The complexity of neurodevelopment, the rapidity of early neurogenesis, and over 100 years of research identifying
environmental influences on neurodevelopment serve as backdrop to understanding factors that influence risk and
severity of autism spectrum disorder (ASD). This Keynote Lecture, delivered at the May 2016 annual meeting of the
International Society for Autism Research, describes concepts of causation, outlines the trajectory of research on non-
genetic factors beginning in the 1960s, and briefly reviews the current state of this science. Causal concepts are intro-
duced, including root causes; pitfalls in interpreting time trends as clues to etiologic factors; susceptible time
windows for exposure; and implications of a multi-factorial model of ASD. An historical background presents early
research into the origins of ASD. The epidemiologic literature from the last fifteen years is briefly but critically
reviewed for potential roles of, for example, air pollution, pesticides, plastics, prenatal vitamins, lifestyle and family
factors, and maternal obstetric and metabolic conditions during her pregnancy. Three examples from the case-control
CHildhood Autism Risks from Genes and the Environment Study are probed to illustrate methodological approaches
to central challenges in observational studies: capturing environmental exposure; causal inference when a random-
ized controlled clinical trial is either unethical or infeasible; and the integration of genetic, epigenetic, and environ-
mental influences on development. We conclude with reflections on future directions, including exposomics, new
technologies, the microbiome, gene-by-environment interaction in the era of –omics, and epigenetics as the interface
of those two. As the environment is malleable, this research advances the goal of a productive and fulfilling life for
all children, teen-agers and adults. Autism Res 2018, 0: 000–000. V C 2018 International Society for Autism Research,
Lay Summary: This Keynote Lecture, delivered at the 2016 meeting of the International Society for Autism Research,
discusses evidence from human epidemiologic studies of prenatal factors contributing to autism, such as pesticides,
maternal nutrition and her health. There is no single cause for autism. Examples highlight the features of a high-
quality epidemiology study, and what comprises a compelling case for causation. Emergent research directions hold
promise for identifying potential interventions to reduce disabilities, enhance giftedness, and improve lives of those
with ASD.
Keywords: autism spectrum disorder; environmental risk factors; causal inference; pre- and peri-natal risk factors; pes-
ticides; nutrition; diabetes; gene-environment interaction; epigenetics
From the Department of Public Health Sciences, MIND Institute (Medical Investigations of Neurodevelopmental Disorders), University of California,
Davis, Davis, California
Grant sponsor: National Institutes of Health; Grant numbers: UG3-OD023365; UL1-TR000002; Grant sponsor: National Institute of Environmental
Health Sciences; Grant numbers: P01-ES011269; P30-ES023513; R01-ES015359; R01-ES020392; R01-ES028089; R01-ES025574; R21-ES021330; T32-
MH073124; Grant sponsor: Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development; Grant number: U54-HD079125;
Grant sponsor: U.S. Environmental Protection Agency; Grant numbers: R829388; R833292; RD-83543201; Grant sponsor: U.S. Department of
Defense; Grant number: AR110194; Grant sponsor: The Allen Foundation.
Received May 24, 2017; accepted for publication October 19, 2017
Address for correspondence and reprints: Irva Hertz-Picciotto, Department of Public Health Sciences, MIND Institute (Medical Investigations of
Neurodevelopmental Disorders), University of California, Davis, Med Sci 1C, Davis, CA, 95616. E-mail: [email protected]
Published online 00 Month 2018 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com)
DOI: 10.1002/aur.1938
C 2018 International Society for Autism Research, Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
V
INSAR
confounding at the group-level, reverse causation using the nationally representative NHANES (National
(when exposure and outcome are measured in the same Health and Nutrition Examination Survey) provided
time interval, the outcome could be influencing the unequivocal evidence that household income is posi-
exposure), and cross-level confounding (the latter tively correlated with numerous serum nutrients, and
referred to as “ecologic” bias). [Morgenstern, 1995]. negatively correlated with blood and urine measure-
ments of metals, hydrocarbons and volatile organics
that are typical constituents of ambient air pollution,
Birth. In 2006–2007, high quality individual-level
along with a wide range of viral and inflammatory
studies emerged. Within a decade, scores of reports pro-
markers [Patel et al., 2015]. Notably, nutritional, viral
duced results on environmental chemical and physical
and inflammatory exposures have each been associated
agents, lifestyle, infectious agents, trauma, and medical
with ASD. Moreover, two large rigorous studies con-
conditions or interventions. We review major classes of
ducted in Europe, where there is less of a link between
risk or protective factors occurring in the prenatal
wealth and clean air, showed no association with ASD
period in Tables 1, 2, and 3. This literature, represents
[Gong et al., 2017; Guxens et al., 2016]. Additionally, a
the Infancy & Childhood of the field. A separate sec- troubling discrepancy is the inconclusive literature on
tion probes, more deeply, methodologic issues relevant cigarette smoking in relation to ASD, even though
to causal inference using three instructive examples of tobacco smoke and ambient air pollution contain thou-
environmental chemicals, nutrition, and maternal med- sands of chemicals in common, some associated with
ical conditions, with special reference to work con- ASD in epidemiologic studies and many with biologic
ducted at the UC Davis MIND Institute. plausibility. Moreover, many studies on tobacco smoke
Chemicals and Pollutants (Table 1) failed to control for confounding or inappropriately
controlled for potential intermediates (such as birth-
Air pollution. Over a dozen full-scale epidemiologic weight). In short, unanswered questions about con-
studies have been published, most indicating that pre- founding in studies of air pollution and about the true
natal exposure to air pollutants increased risks for ASD association of smoking with ASD remain.
[Becerra, Wilhelm, Olsen, Cockburn, & Ritz, 2013;
Hartz, Heinonen, Shapiro, Siskind, & Slone, 1975; Kalk-
brenner et al., 2010; Kalkbrenner, Schmidt, & Penlesky, Pesticides. Studies of pesticides and ASD are dis-
2014; Raz et al., 2015; Talbott et al., 2015; Volk, Hertz- cussed under “Methodologic Challenges” below.
Picciotto, Delwiche, Lurmann, & McConnell, 2011;
Volk, Lurmann, Penfold, Hertz-Picciotto, & McConnell, Phthalates. Phthalates are a ubiquitous class of
2012; Windham, Zhang, Gunier, Croen, & Grether, chemicals used as plasticizers, stabilizers, emulsifiers,
2006]. However, different pollutants were evaluated, dispersants, solvents, and lubricants and as enteric coat-
including coarse and fine particles, nitrogen dioxide, ings on pharmaceuticals and nutritional supplements.
ozone, metals, solvents, and diesel particles. Metrics of They are in vinyl flooring, adhesives, pesticides, food
exposure varied from proximity to freeways to model- packaging, children’s toys, medical tubing, air fresh-
based estimates of concentrations, sometimes based on eners, toiletries (soaps, shampoos, hair products), and
annual averages, and in other studies based on more cosmetics, [Centers for Disease Control and Prevention,
temporally sensitive models. The models are con- 2016; Kim, Hong, Bong, & Cho, 2015; U.S. Environ-
structed from air monitoring, source information, dis- mental Protection Agency Office of Prevention, 2006].
persion models, and sometimes also land topography In short, they are ubiquitous. Four studies have
and meteorologic variables; model validation was per- addressed phthalates in relation to ASD, autistic symp-
formed for many, though for limited time periods and toms, or common co-occurring behaviors. In a Scandi-
geographic areas. Outcomes also varied from maternal navian population-based sample, maternal report of a
response to a single question about whether they have child with ASD was associated with vinyl flooring in
a child with autism to a full clinical evaluation using the bedrooms [Larsson, Weiss, Janson, Sundell, & Bor-
established standardized instruments (e.g., ADOS and nehag, 2009]. Phthalates are a major component in
ADI-R). vinyl flooring and off-gas continuously. Two cohort
The pattern of findings is generally consistent across studies measured phthalate metabolites in maternal 3rd
U.S. studies, though not necessarily for the same pollu- trimester urine samples: one found poorer scores on all
tants. Although virtually all studies adjusted for at least domains of the Social Responsiveness Scales (SRS) [Mio-
one socioeconomic variable, concerns about residual dovnik et al., 2011], whereas the other measured differ-
confounding remain: Air pollution is highest in com- ent phthalates, and showed no association with the
munities with the lowest socioeconomic levels in most composite SRS [Braun et al., 2014b]. In a fourth study,
of the U.S., and, a recent analysis of correlates for SES phthalates measured in house dust samples collected
Air pollution Associated with increased risk of autism in close to a dozen studies in the Windham et al. [2006]; Kalkbrenner et al.
U.S. and Asia, but not in several European populations. Different exposure [2010]; Volk et al. [2011, 2012]; Roberts
measures have included: (a) modeled estimates by Census tract; (b) prox- et al. [2013]; von Ehrenstein et al.
imity of geocoded home address to freeway; (c) model-based estimates of [2014]; Raz et al. [2015]; Guxens et al.
NO2, ozone, PM2.5, and PM10 at geocoded home address; (d) other air con- [2016]; Gong et al. [2017]
taminant monitoring and land use regression. Results most consistent for
PM2.5; other pollutants not entirely consistent. Two European studies do
not confirm air pollution associations with ASD. Because of strong correla-
tions between air pollution and socioeconomic status, residual confound-
ing may be affecting multiple studies. Apparent discrepancy with cigarette
smoke (see below) also raises questions. Air pollutants induce oxidative
stress and have been associated with cognitive and neurobehavioral
deficits.
Pesticides: Organophos- Four studies provide evidence, all used objective measures of exposure. Two Rauh et al. [2011]; Eskenazi et al. [2007];
phates (OP’s) cohort studies reported prenatal OP exposures associated with PDD symp- Roberts et al. [2007]; Shelton et al.
toms: one measured chlorpyrifos in plasma (cord or maternal); the other [2014]
measured metabolites (nonspecific for organophosphate pesticides) in
urine. Two case-control studies linked proximity to agricultural applica-
tions of OP’s with autism risk. Third trimester exposures to chorpyrifos,
the most commonly used OP, showed OR>3.0. Pesticide drift measured in
air samplers at distances several kilometers away from agricultural applica-
tions correlates with amounts applied [Wofford et al., 2014]. Organophos-
phate exposures during gestation have also been associated with
cognitive impairment, volumetric differences in brain regions, attention
deficits, and tremor (see Table 4).
Pesticides: Pyrethroids Association observed with applications of pyrethroids in two studies, includ- Roberts et al. [2007]; Shelton et al. [2014]
ing two large case-control studies. Both geocoded residential addresses
and assessed proximity to agricultural applications during the pregnancy.
One observed association with bifenthrin, which degrades most slowly of
all pyrethroids. The other study found total pyrethroids or Type II pyreth-
roids associated primarily when exposures were in the pre-conception or
3rd trimester, after adjustment for confounders. Numerous mechanisms
have been proposed for neurodevelopmental toxicity [Shelton et al.,
2012].
Phthalates Three of four studies suggest an association with neurobehavior; one of two Larsson et al. [2009]; Miodovnik et al.
reported an association with ASD diagnosis, and one of two found mater- [2011]; Braun et al. [2014a]; Philippat
nal prenatal urinary phthalate metabolites associated with higher SRS et al. [2015]
scores. One observed greater hyperactivity/impulsivity and inattention.
Studies used: (a) presence of vinyl flooring; (b) phthalate metabolites
measured in maternal prenatal urine; and (c) house dust phthalates. Given
the continuous nature of most phthalate exposure sources, urinary meas-
urements may provide reasonable approximations to chronic long-term
exposures. Phthalates disrupt androgen activity [Main et al., 2006;
National Research Council, 2008; Miodovnik et al., 2014].
Persistent pollutants: Three studies of PCBs in association with ASD or SRS scores, one with very Cheslack-Postova et al. [2013]; Braun et al.
PCBs low power. Two others (one using SRS, one using ASD diagnosis) found [2014a]; Lyall et al. [2017]; Jolous-
associations of higher scores or risk for the same congener peak (IUPAC Jamshidi et al. [2010]
#138/#158), while one found lower SRS and the other found higher ASD
risk with #153. Nondioxin-like PCBs can influence neuronal dendritic
development. Toxicologic studies demonstrate alterations in social behav-
iors in rodents. Disruption of thyroid hormone homeostasis is a mecha-
nism of neurodevelopmental toxicity in rodents.
ASD, autism spectrum disorder; PCBs, polychlorinated biphenyls; PDD, pervasive developmental delay.
after the child was diagnosed were associated with to obtain deep dust deposited over a period of years
other developmental delay but not autism; however, in that included, but was not specific to, prenatal expo-
both groups, higher phthalate levels were associated sures. Thus, three of four studies in different popula-
with hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention [Philip- tions, using three different methods to assess early life
pat et al., 2015]. The house dust protocol was designed exposure suggest potential impact on neurobehavior,
Interpregnancy interval Shorter intervals (<12 months) from end of the previous pregnancy to the Cheslak-Postava et al. [2011]; Dodds et al.
start of pregnancy associated with about two-fold higher autism risk in [2011]; Gunnes et al. [2013]; Cheslack-
8 studies. Some evidence for dose-response, however three studies also Postava et al. [2014]; Coo et al. [2015];
showed higher risk with longer intervals. Associations could result from Durkin et al. [2015]; Zerbo et al. [2015];
maternal nutrient depletion or cultural, family planning, or lifestyle Conde-Agudelo et al. [2016]
factors.
Maternal folic acid and Higher folic acid intake and folic acid supplements associated with reduced Schmidt et al. [2011, 2012, 2017]; Suren
prenatal vitamins risk for ASD or autistic traits in several population-based studies; one et al. [2013]; Braun et al. [2014a];
study had null findings. Association with ASD diagnosis specific to expo- Steenweg-de Graaff et al. [2015]; Virk
sure near conception in two studies. Evidence for a stronger association et al. [2016]
when mother or child has genetic or environmental susceptibility. Two
studies observed no association of autistic traits with maternal serum
folate collected after the first trimester of pregnancy, similar to reported
lack of association with folic acid intake or supplements after first two
months. Mechanisms for folic acid protection may involve methylation of
DNA and altered gene expression. One study of interactions showed folic
acid protection against pesticide-related increased risk of ASD.
Maternal iron In a large case-control study, mothers of children with ASD were less likely Schmidt et al. [2014b]; Suren et al. [2013]
to report taking an iron supplement; lowest quintile of supplemental iron
associated with ASD, especially in older mothers and mothers with meta-
bolic conditions. A large birth cohort found no association with minerals.
Maternal fatty acids and Literature is inconsistent. Exposures have included (a) prenatal fish oil sup- Suren et al. [2013]; Lyall et al. [2013];
fish consumption plements, (b) intake of polyunsaturated fatty acids, omega-6 and omega-3 Steenweg-de Graaff et al. [2016]; Julvez
fatty acids and/or linoleic acid, and (c) fish consumption. Self-reported et al. [2016]
intake of polyunsaturated fatty acids, omega-6 fatty acids and linoleic
acid associated with lower ASD risk in one study, but in another, total
omega-6 and linoleic acid associated with more ASD traits. Another
observed increased ASD risk with very low omega-3 intake. Fish intake not
associated with ASD or traits in two studies, while another found fewer
autistic traits in association with high fatty fish consumption; this latter
was the only study to adjust for mercury.
Maternal smoking Evidence is inconsistent. Two recent meta-analyses, each based on fifteen Rosen et al. [2015]; Tang et al. [2015].
studies, reported the same OR of 1.02 (95% CI 5 0.93, 1.12 or 1.13).
However, two-thirds of original analyses either did not adjust for any con-
founders, or incorrectly adjusted for birthweight or gestational age,
potential intermediates, for which adjustment can induce bias. All relied
on self-report of smoking, known to be prone to underascertainment. No
studies examined genetic susceptibility, nor have objective exposure
markers been utilized in smoking assessment.
2001; van Eijsden, Smits, van der Wal, & Bonsel, 2008]. decreased social interaction [Pollitt, 1993]. Iron contrib-
Short IPI could also be a marker for cultural, family utes to neurotransmitter production, myelination, and
planning or lifestyle factors [Cheslack-Postava et al., immune function [Beard, 2000], processes shown to be
2011; Dodds et al., 2011], and evidence for a U-shaped dysregulated in autism [Beard, 2000].
relationship between IPI and ASD supports a contribu- Results from two large studies of maternal iron during
tion from such influences [Cheslack-Postava et al., pregnancy and ASD risk are inconsistent. In the
2014; Durkin et al., 2015; Zerbo et al., 2015]. California-based CHARGE case-control study (520 cases,
346 typically developing controls), children of mothers
with the highest versus lowest iron intake from supple-
Iron. Iron is critical for fetal and placental growth ments and cereals had half the odds of developing ASD,
and brain development and functioning. During preg- especially when mothers were of advanced age, obese,
nancy, iron deficiency, which is common [O’Brien, diabetic, or hypertensive [Schmidt, Tancredi, Krako-
Zavaleta, Abrams, & Caulfield, 2003; Stoltzfus, 2001] wiak, Hansen, & Ozonoff, 2014b]. In contrast, in the
can induce fetal and infant iron-deficiency [Allen, 2000; Norwegian birth cohort study [Suren et al., 2013], ASD
Colomer et al., 1990; Millard, Frazer, Wilkins, & Ander- risk did not differ for those taking vitamin and mineral
son, 2004; Tchernia, Archambeaud, Yvart, & Diallo, supplements likely to contain iron, compared with
1996]. Low iron has been associated with developmen- those taking no vitamins or minerals. The studies dif-
tal delays and behavioral disturbances including fered on obesity prevalence; potential for recall bias;
Thalidomide Case reports of increased autism prevalence in children of mothers given Stromland et al. [1994]
Valproic acid/anti- these drugs during pregnancy. With low prevalence of use, these factors Moore et al. [2000]; Rodier et al. [1996]
epileptic drugs can account for very few cases of autism today.
Selective Serotonin Most studies found an association of SSRI use during pregnancy and Croen et al. [2011]; Hviid et al. [2013]; El
Reuptake Inhibitors increased risk in either a specific time period or a specific subset. Meta- Marroun et al. [2014]; Harrington et al.
analysis of four case-control studies produced and adjusted OR51.8 (95% [2014]; Rai et al. [2013]; Malm et al.
CI 5 1.5, 2.2). As with individual studies, confounding by indication could [2016]; Man et al. [2015]
not be excluded. Studies attempting to assess such confounding yielded
conflicting results.
Pre-eclampsia Several large studies provide evidence of increased risk for ASD. Studies Walker et al. [2015]; Mann et al. [2010];
relied on (a) administrative medical databases (e.g., Medicaid), or (b) Burstyn et al. [2010]; Buchmayer et al.
detailed abstraction of medical records including placental insufficiency. [2009]; Dodds et al. [2011]
The latter produced a larger effect size than administrative databases,
possibly because detailed abstraction of notes in medical charts beyond
standard codes, is more complete. RR’s ranged from 1.2 to over 2.
Gestational diabetes High degree of consistency in associations with ASD across studies, includ- Krakowiak et al. [2012]; Lyall et al. [2012];
ing two meta-analyses, several large cohorts, and a few moderate-sized Xiang et al. [2015]; Connolly et al. [2016];
studies. Meta-analysis, two large cohorts and one moderate-sized case- Li et al. [2016]; Nahum Sacks et al. [2016]
control study found RRs/OR of 1.3–1.5; two studies with <30 cases Meta-analysis: Xu et al. [2014]
observed RRs of 3–4 (with wide CI’s). Strong biologic plausibility; co-
occurring obesity may exacerbate effect. Whether controlled versus uncon-
trolled diabetes alters association has not been examined.
Maternal infections/fever Congenital rubella, maternal fever and influenza, and other viruses during Chess et al. [1977]; Desmond et al. [1967];
during pregnancy pregnancy are each associated with autism in multiple studies and in Deykin and MacMahon [1979]; Zerbo
meta-analysis. In two studies, the impactful timing of maternal fever was et al. [2012]; Atladottir [2010, 2012];
2nd trimester, and limited to fevers not treated with antipyretics. Addi- Webb et al. [2017]; Hornig et al. [2017]
tionally, copy number variants and viral infection combined appear to
increase severity of autism symptoms.
2011; Harrington et al., 2014]. A meta-analysis of nine no estimate of duration or intensity of the thermal
studies produced pooled ORs for case-control studies and index, suggest that the exposure metric may have done a
cohort studies, respectively, of 2.1 (95% CI, 1.7, 2.7) and poor job of rank ordering children by their true doses. A
1.8 (1.5, 2.2). As with many of the individual studies, second null study randomized pregnancies to different
this analysis was subject to confounding by indication. ultrasound and Doppler imaging protocols, but took
However, larger studies that did take such confounding place before introduction of higher dose equipment
into account by comparing depressed mothers who did [Stoch et al., 2012]. The third investigation evaluated the
vs. did not medicate (or did not medicate with SSRIs) triple-hit hypothesis, which proposed ASD phenotypes
produced conflicting results [Malm et al., 2016; Rai et al., to be dependent on three insults combined: (a) environ-
2013]; neither study examined timing of exposure. Nev- mental exposures; (b) their specific timing; and (c)
ertheless, mother’s mental state can alter the intra- genetic susceptibility [Williams & Casanova, 2010]. This
uterine (and postnatal) environment for the child. At concept was operationalized for exposures to 1st trimes-
this stage, no conclusion can yet be drawn as to whether ter ultrasound and presence of CNV in an ASD popula-
SSRIs influence risk of ASD. tion (n 5 1749) from the Simons Simplex Collection
[Webb et al., 2017]. Those with vs. without a 1st trimes-
ter ultrasound scored significantly lower in social affect,
Medical interventions. Use of ultrasonography for and significantly higher in repetitive/restricted behav-
imaging the fetus is now nearly universal. Both fre- iors. Limiting to males with CNV, the 1st trimester ultra-
quency of use and thermal intensity have increased, the sound associated with significantly lower nonverbal IQs,
latter by 8-fold during the 1990s as a result of FDA lifting and higher repetitive behavior scores. Without a control
prior restrictions [Webb et al., 2017]. A few studies group, ASD risk could not be assessed, but results suggest
addressed ultrasound in relation to ASD. One found no 1st trimester ultrasound may influence the severity of
association in analyses of the number of ultrasounds impairments, particularly in those with underlying
received, but the study period for these exposures (1994– genetic susceptibility.
1999) covered the changes in equipment [Grether, Li, Infertility treatments have also received attention.
Yoshida, & Croen, 2010]. That problem, combined with Because both ASD and each type of treatment are both
Memory and IQ New York inner-city children age 3 and age 7 years Rauh et al. [2006, 2011]
Brain volumetric measurements Same cohort, age 8–9 years PNAS, Rauh et al. [2012]
Tremor Same cohort, age 11 years Neurotoxicology, Rauh et al. [2015]
Mental & psychomotor development California farmworker children, age 24 months Eskenazi et al. [2007]
Memory and IQ Same cohort, age 7 years Bouchard et al. [2011]
Attention/ADHD Children ages 36 months and 5 years Marks et al. [2010]
Mental and psychomotor development and reasoning Urban child population, ages 12 months and 6–9 years Engel et al. [2011]
provide exposure assignments equal in quality to those doubled with a ten-fold increase in metabolites [Eske-
obtained in prospective studies. nazi et al., 2007].
Two case-control studies examined ASD in association
Example #1-Pesticides: The challenge of exposure assessment
with distance of home to agricultural pesticide applica-
Humans exposures to pesticides occur from: (a) insect tions. All commercial pesticide applications in Califor-
or other pest control products (sprays, bombs, etc.) in nia agriculture are reportable by law to the Department
homes, malls, restaurants, or on pets and gardens, even of Pesticide Regulation, including specific chemicals
when used according to instructions; (b) drift from used, amounts, dates of applications, and locations.
applications on orchards or agricultural fields, well- Both studies used Geographic Information Systems to
documented at distances of several kilometers from the overlay maternal addresses with locations of the pesti-
application site [Wofford et al., 2014]; and (c) residues cide applications from this database. Amounts of pesti-
in fruits, vegetables, and, through bioaccumulation, cide applied within pre-defined distances of the
meats, fish, and dairy products [Schafer & Kegley, 2002; residences were compared for cases vs. controls. One
Vogt et al., 2012]. study showed increased risks for residential proximity
Most pesticides are designed to be neurotoxic. Orga- to two organochlorine pesticides in the first trimester,
nophosphates, currently among the most abundantly and for applications of organophosphates and a specific
used insecticides, are acutely toxic due to inhibition of pyrethroid (bifenthrin) at any time during pregnancy
the enzyme acetylcholinesterase (AChE), which breaks [Roberts et al., 2007].
down neurotransmitters at the post-synaptic mem- The second case-control study used CHARGE partici-
brane. However, neurodevelopmental and neurologic pants [Shelton et al., 2014], for whom all diagnoses
dysfunction occur at subacute levels, when AChE inhi- were clinically confirmed. The three chemical classes
bition is absent, [Voorhees, Rohlman, Lein, & Pieper, investigated were: organophosphate, pyrethroid, and
2016]. Hence other toxic mechanisms are at play and carbamate pesticides. Figure 3 demonstrates linkage of
potentially relevant for gestational exposures in early pesticide use reports to the residences. All addresses
brain development, including inflammation, GABA sig- from 3 months before conception through pregnancy
naling, thyroid disruption, oxidative stress and mito- were geocoded and buffers of radii 1.25, 1.5, or 1.75
chondrial dysregulation [Banks & Lein, 2012; Schuh, kilometers were created. Overlaying on this map were
Lein, Beckles, & Jett, 2002; Shelton, Hertz-Picciotto, & the locations of pesticide applications, with each child
Pessah, 2012; Voorhees et al., 2016]. Prenatal organo- assigned exposures within the buffer zone in the win-
phosphate exposures have been associated with cogni- dows of interest: the 3 months before conception, each
tive impairments, attention deficits, and tremor in trimester, or the entire pregnancy. One salient finding
children (Table 4) [Bouchard et al., 2011; Engel et al., was the significant association between organophos-
2011; Rauh et al., 2011]. Some deficits persist into phate exposures at any point during pregnancy and
school ages. 60% higher risk for ASD (Fig. 4, top two panels). The
Three studies evaluated prenatal organophosphate strongest association was for 2nd and 3rd trimester
exposures and either an ASD diagnosis or symptoms. chlorpyrifos (an organophosphate), with a 2.5- to 3-fold
Urine specimens collected from pregnant women in a elevation in ASD risk. Chlorpyrifos was banned in 2001
farmworker community were analyzed for organophos- by the U.S. EPA for household products, and the safety
phate pesticide metabolites. When the children were 24 of its continued use in agriculture has been contested
months of age, pervasive developmental delay (PDD) based on both toxicologic and epidemiologic research.
symptoms were assessed. The proportion scoring In fall 2016, U.S. EPA determined that residues in food
>97.5th percentile was unexpectedly high (possibly and water exceeded standards for safe levels. In March
related to administration of the scale in Spanish) and 2017, the U.S. EPA decided not to follow through on its