Basadur Simplex Problem Solving Process
Basadur Simplex Problem Solving Process
; ..-� (
��--��
'�"'·�-.��-���·��--·-��--���·---��-·--�-
� M �1-�_!_:_R �
(·
( ' \. '
t ' ,
'
-,
; f"
:
.
\
THE BASADUR SIMPLEX CREATIVE
PROBLEM-SOLVING PROFILE INVENTORY:
DEVELOPMENT, RELIABILITY
· 1 ·:
AND VALIDITY
.• J'.
by
Min Basadur
,:_
•I , !
1 998
� I · MCMASTER
• U N I V E R S I T Y •
I n nis M I C HA E L G . D e G R O O T E
by
Min Basadur
The Working Paper series is intended as a means whereby a researcher may communicate his or her
thoughts and findings to interested readers for their comments. The paper should be considered
preliminary in nature and may require substantial revision. Accordingly, this Working Paper should
not be quoted nor the data referred to without the written consent of the author. Your comments and
suggestions are welcome and should be directed to the author.
TH E BASADUR SIMPLEX C REATIVE PROBLEM-SOLVI NG
PROFILE I NVENTORY: D EVELOPMENT, RELIABI LITY AND VALI DITY
ABSTRACT
The underlying theory and early development and internal consistency testing of the
Basadur CPSP (Creative Problem Solving Profile) inventory is reviewed, then new expanded
reliability and validity research is summarized. The four col umns, the four q uadrants, and
the two bi-polar scales of the C PSP demonstrated satisfactory reliability via test-retest,
The C PSP correlated with the Kirton Adaptation I nventory (KAI) and the Myers-Briggs
Type I ndicator (MBTI) in predicted ways. Also, the relative impact of creativity training was
moderated according to CPSP style in expected ways. Assessments of CPSP style by self
alone and in consensus with expert partner significantly agreed with the assessments made
organizational responsibility for strategic thin king by preferred C PSP style and preferred ratio
of ideation-eval uation polar opposites were strongly supported. Opportunities for improvi ng
I NTRODUCTION
Basadur ( 1 979; 1 982; 1 987; 1 995) modeled organizational, g roup and individual
thinking. Basadur, Graen and Green ( 1 982) and Basadur ( 1 994) showed that skills i n this
process, cal led Si mplex, can be i ncreased through training, practice and organizational
n u rturi ng. The process begins with generation and conceptual ization activity, then cycles
through optimization and i mplementation activity. Basad ur, Graen and Wakabayashi
( 1 990) published the Basadur Simplex Creative Problem-Solving Profile I nventory (CPSP) , an
instrument which helps i ndividua ls, teams and organizations discover their own unique
the instrument and recommended fu rther research. The pri mary purpose of this paper is to
worldwide are using the CPSP for various additional applications, including increasing
increase creativity by synerg izi ng individual similarities and differences; providing g roups
with a method to i mprove the q uantity and quality of their interactions; providing a process
model used by individuals and teams to generate and complete important projects;
identifying training and development opportunities for individuals, teams and organizations;
3
measuring i n novative style i n research; and diagnosi ng organizations and teams for
An objective of this paper is to fi rst document the underlying theory and the early
development and i nternal consistency testing of the instrument and then to summarize
ongoing reliability and validity research. This research includes test-retest and random
para l lel split-half correlations and polar, adjacent, and bipolar scale inter-correlations;
the Myers-Briggs Type I ndicator and the Kirton Adaptation-Innovation I nventory; various
customized predictive validity experiments; and standardization studies linking various fields
non-operational thinking to different innovation styles and to different polar scale ratios.
The Si mplex process approach uses interchangeably the terms i nnovative thin ki ng,
creative problem solving, change making, creative thinking, creativity and innovation. The
Si mplex process is a system of synchronized attitudinal and behavioral thi nking skills which
can be lea rned, developed, shared, managed and continually applied by individuals, teams
and entire o rganizations to produce ongoing innovative results (Basadur, 1 982; 1 987;
BACKGROUND
sol ution implementation activity is shown in Figure 1 . This process is identified as a complete
4
F i g ure 1
Problem
� Finding
Phase I
Problem
� Solving
Phase I I
� Phase Ill
�
5
process of creative problem solving, which means that it is based on two fundamental
concepts. First, it has distinctly different phases: problem fi nding, problem solving a nd
sol ution implementation. Second, each phase contains a fundamental , sequential, two-step
generation without evaluation (putting aside the judgment capability) . This is the divergi ng
aspect of the two-step process. Eval uation is the reverse. It is defined as the application of
judgment to the generated options to select the best one(s) . This is the converging aspect
creative process. Cooper ( 1 993) and his colleagues have written extensively a bout the vital
importance of evaluative skills in creating successful new products. This a pproach also
emphasizes that there a re phases to the creative process beyond simply finding solutions to
a l ready identified problems. Skills in discov�ri ng new i mportant problems to solve and in
implementing new sol utions have both been identified as even more vital phases of the
creative process (than creating the solutions) by many researchers (Mackworth, 1 965;
Livingston, 1 9 7 1 ; Getzels, 1 975; Leavitt, 1 975; Simon, 1 960, Levitt, 1 963; Ackoff, 1 979).
In other words, there is more to the creative process than simply "brainstorming"
ideas to solve a presented, defined problem (contrary to much of the earlier research
Osborn ( 1 963), and Parnes, Noller and Biondi ( 1 977) provided linear three- and five-step
creative problem-solving process models, beginning in fact finding and ending in gaining
acceptance.
6
Gordon ( 1 956, 1 9 7 1 ) modeled learning and inventing as two opposite forces which
feed upon each other. Learning was characterized as a process of m aking new connections
while invention was characterized as a process of breaking old connections. When we learn,
we "make the strange familiar" (by making new connections between new phenomena and
current understanding) . This permits us to view new phenomena in old ways. In contrast,
when we invent, we "make the familiar strange" (by breaking old con nections that
compromise new understanding). This permits us to view old phenomena in new ways.
Thus, the processes of learning and inventing flow conti nually i nto one another in sequence.
the research and development process as a continuous, circular flow of manufactu ring new
knowledge.
Basadur ( 1 9 74; 1 98 1 ; 1 983) integrated the circular approach into the continuous,
complete process (see Figure 2) and evolved and developed the ful l Simplex process
(described later) beginning with the basic Osborn-Parnes three- and five-step linear models
using his extensive field application experience within Procter & Gamble, Ford and other
companies, and culminating in his doctoral dissertation ( 1 979) . Named Simplex™ in 1 981 ,
the process recognizes that, as new problems are sought and discovered and new solutions
subsequently i mplemented, new problems and opportunities a rise. For example, the
automobi le's i nvention provided not only a new solution to an old problem (improving
.i.------...i.-.i.:�-\t... . ..i. 1....---....l _ _,,, �r"t...I�....,..,. t�,.,JI, ....:,..... ,,. ... ,,.,.,... , ,.,,..,..;rlon+.,\ R,,.,,.,,..i , ,r fl• OOA\
11Ull:>f.JVI1u11u1 IJ uu1 u1 u11u-1 n:;vv tJ vu1v111;, \pv11v11v1 11 011-v-1�T1 "-'I'"""''""""""• llWI'/. ..,""'WI'""''-"'"'• \ , ,,. --r I
described his experiences in learning how to help such organizations as Procter & Gamble
..<.+!A!PV
6u!AIOS
we1q0Jd
..<.+!A!PV
uo!+o+uewe1dw1
uo!+n1os
..<.+!A!PV
6U!PLI!:1
wa1q0Jd
L
8
and Frito-Lay to increase short- and long-term profitability. They did so by mainstreaming
Mott's (1 9 72) organizational effectiveness resea rch. Mott showed that effective organizations
know how to synchronize two vital but very different characteristics: efficiency and
methods and routines to attain the highest quantity and quality for the lowest possible cost.
High efficiency means mastery of routine (standard, prescribed methods by which the
organizational unit carries out its '!lain tasks) . Adaptability mea ns continually a nd
i ntentionally changing routines and finding new, continuous and better ways to do business.
environment to anticipate new opportunities and problems and changing methods to attai n
new levels of quantity, quality and cost: adaptability yields both new methods and new
products. High adaptability means a high rate of deli berate positive change of routine --
effort to mai nstream deli berate change in organizations and suggested that OD is a n
ongoing cycle of problem finding, problem solving and solution implementation. Problem
fi nding means continuously finding new problems to address, incl uding not just things that
9
are goi ng wrong but current or future changes, trends, challenges and opportunities. This
incl udes the concept of "looki ng for the golden eggs" that is practised by leading Japanese
companies (Basadur, 1 992). Senge, Klei ner, Roberts, Ross and Smith ( 1 994) a lso emphasize
the i mportance of problem finding i n adaptable organizations. These organizations take the
time to explore background ca uses of i nternal problems to identify bigger, long-term issues
and recog nize the i nterconnectedness of decisions withi n the organization. This recognition
is the essence of systems thinki ng and the sta rting point for making long-term, permanent
i mprovements to the organization. Problem solving means developing new and useful
sol utions to identified problems. Sol ution implementation means making new solutions
succeed. I mplementation usual ly leads the organization to find new problems to solve. New
problems a rise as the system and its environment react to each newly i mplemented sohJtion.
change-making and adaptabi lity; it is a ci rcular process of conti nuously finding and solving
problems and i mplementi ng new sol utions which represent val uable changes that enable
the organization to succeed (Figure 2) . This definition also removes any distinction between
creativity and innovation (despite views of some researchers who distinguish between
phase, complete a nd conti nuous process. Each phase, i ncluding problem generation ,
Specific OD i nterventions a re discrete tools used within this creative process. Some
of these tools a re used within the problem-finding phase, others within the problem-solving
10
and implementation phases. For example, diagnostic interventions such as su rvey feedback
or the confrontation meeting a re tools for problem generation; used regularly, they help
antici pate and expose organizational issues that might otherwise have remained buried.
Group development (or team-building) process interventions help solve identified problems
such as slow project completion by improving the way people work together. When the
might use structural interventions such as job enrichment. Structural interventions can a lso
hel p implement other i ntervention sol utions. For example, a new reward system like the
Sca nlon plan, i ntended to encourage employees to use their creativity by sharing the profits
from their productivity i mprovements, can ensure that employees permanently transfer
creativity skills learned in tra i ning to their jobs. Elsewhere, Japan's Toshiba Corp. places
newly h ired scientists and engineers directly into the sales department to reinforce the
i mportance of problem finding (learning customers' problems) before they begin developing
new products to solve those problems within research and development. Finally, individual
organizational creativity process. For junior managers who have been trained in facilitative
intervention), counseling and coaching by senior managers (individual intervention) can help
dynamic for organizational adaptabil ity and effectiveness. This suggests that an organization
can achieve adapta bility if it mainstreams the creative process. For this to happen, two
11
thi ngs a re necessary. Fi rst, employees must obtain new thinking ski lls (and reframe their
jobs, i .e. , become creative problem finders and solvers and solution implementers) . Second,
the organization must provide a framework for directing these creative thinking skills to
Training in a multi-phase, ci rcular, "complete" process (Si mplex™) has been shown
functional specialties (Basadur, Graen & Green, 1 982; Basadur, Graen & Scandura, 1 986;
Basadur, l 988a,b; Runco & Basadur, 1 993). Such training attempts to increase
appreciation of the value of a l l such multiple phases, and of separating and synchronizing
divergent and convergent thinking. If training is sufficiently i ntensive and lasts long enough,
participants develop not only this a ppreciation but the corresponding skills (Basadur, 1 979;
Basadur et al, 1 982). However, experience shows that most people feel less comfortable
within some phases of the "complete" process than in other phases. They also find that their
own preferences for various phases differ from those of others: styles of using a complete
breaking the three phases i nto eig ht steps as follows: 1 ) problem finding (anticipating future
problems and seeking current problems); 2) fact finding; 3) problem defining; 4) generati ng
potential sol utions; 5) eval uati ng potential sol utions; 6) action planning; 7) gaining
acceptance; and 8) taking action. Because the enti re process is circular and conti nuous, the
12
eighth step is fol lowed by the fi rst step to begin a new cycle. Each action taken to implement
a new sol ution automatical ly leads to new problems, changes and opportunities for a new
round of creativity. Figure 3 displays the eight steps within the complete Si mplex creative
p rocess, begi nning with problem finding and prog ressing toward action in a never-ending
flow. One of the two key ideas upon which the Creative Problem Solving Profile (CPSP) rests
is this circular model of the "complete process." The other key idea underpinning the C PSP
Dynamic Tension
suggests that, to perform creatively (C) , one must begin with appropriate knowledge (K) . As
in a kaleidoscope, one's i magination (I) must transform what is known into new, different
combinations, cal led new pattern ideas, options or points of view. Final ly, evaluation (E) is
needed : one must exercise good j udgment to select the most appropriate patterns, ideas,
Let us first focus on the knowledge component of this kaleidoscope analogy. How
people gain knowledge (learn) is personal and individualistic. That is, the bits and pieces
of knowledge one absorbs and retai ns can be gained in contrasting ways. As discussed
above, one way is by direct, concrete experience (getting personally involved in the task and
"getting one's hands dirt-;"). An opposite \Atay is by detached, abstract thin king (standing
back, observing, analyzing and theorizing to understand). Some people have used the terms
13
Figure 3
problem PHASE I
PHASE Ill finding
SOLUTION "fuzzy" PROBLEM FINDING
situation
IMPLEMENTATION
idea
finding
PHASE II
PROBLEM SOLVING
14
"tacit" versus "explicit" knowledge to explain these two notions. For example, some
knowledge can be discovered only by experiencing first-hand new and unexpected things.
For example, Mi ntzberg (1 989) related how Japan's Honda Corp. stumbled across the
opportunity for its smal l scooter in North America just by being there. The company had
sent four marketi ng managers to Los Angeles to establish the market for Honda's new giant
"macho" motorcycles. To reduce taxi costs, the fou r managers rode errands a round the city
on Honda's small scooters. (Honda headquarters had assumed there would be little market
for such a small vehicle on the open roads of North America.) The scooters attracted
attention and finally turned i nto a giant market success for Honda, but only when the bigger
machines developed mechanical problems and the marketing managers pursued their newly
discovered opportunity. Thei r real mission, in retrospect, had been to learn whether they
could sell something - anything- i n North America. Rather than attempt to cover every base
early, the company resisted the l ure of over-rationality and came to America prepared to
Second, let us focus on the i magination and judgment components of the analogy.
How people use knowledge is q uite personal and individual istic. Two opposite ways of using
one's knowledge a re for ideation (to generate more options, ideas and diverse points of view
while deferring judgment) and for evaluation (using judgment to select from among those
g aining knowledge and ideation-eval uation for using knowledge), as in Figure 4. This set
15
F i g ure 4
Gaining Knowledge
(Learning) by
Direct Concrete Experience
Gaining Knowledge
(Learning) by
Detached Abstract Thi n king
16
would describe the individual's preferred way of learning and usi ng knowledge. Each
person 's u nique set of preferences might then help identify h is or her unique style of creative
preferences for these four concepts can portray his or her own blend of preferences. The
resulti ng blend is cal led the Creative Problem Solving Profi le, which provides a basis for
understanding preferences in using the Simplex process (see Figures 5 and 6).
RATIONALE
thi n ki ng) and the opposi ng methods of using knowledge (ideation vs. evaluation) yield four
dominant styles of creative problem solving. The fou r styles correspond to the different
El lspermann & Evans, 1 994). The first is the active sensing, seeki ng or anticipati ng of
termed problem generation. The second is the formulation of these problems, termed
problem formulation or problem conceptual ization. Al bert Einstein reputedly said that
merely formulati ng a problem is often far more essential than its solution. He said that,
g iven one hour to save the \Vorld, he would spend 55 minutes defining the problem and only
5 minutes solving it. Based on Ei nstein's statement (and John Oewey's quote: "A problem
17
Figure 5
wel l stated is half solved") , the proportions of Figures 2 and 3 bear modification. Thus, i n
Fig u re 7, the problem-fi nding phase represents half of the circular process; splitting that
Figure 7
I mplementation
Problem
Finding
Problem
Figure 8
Problem
I mplementation Generation
P roblem Problem
Solving Formulation
20
Opti mizer, and Implementer to complete the Creative Problem Solving Profile (CPSP) . How
Figure 9
8 l
Action Problem
'< > < >7
Finding
Quadrant I 7
Gain "-.. /Fad
(Generator)
< > < >
Acceptance"-.. / Finding
< >
Plan "-.. Problem
/
'< > 3
,Definition
/
/Evol. Idea "-..
< >
Select Finding
The Creative Problem-Solving Profile inventory was developed as fol lows. First, 20
thinking to generate a list of descriptors for each of the fou r concepts representing opposing
ways of gaining and using knowledge. Second, the group reached consensus on the 12
words from each of the four l ists that most appropriately described the fou r concepts. One
important criterion for selecti ng a word was its abi lity to be coupled with a word from the
opposing list. Final ly, 24 additional words were incl uded in the fou r lists (six words each)
21
as distractors. These words were deliberately generated and chosen as either total ly
unrelated or opposite to the 12 initial words on that l ist. The result: fou r col umns, each
containing 18 words, i ncl uding six distractors i ntended to help i nventory users remai n
Use of a forced-choice scale ensured that the inventory reflected relative preferences
for different phases of the complete creative problem-solving process (the CPSP is i ntended
to reflect a person's relative preferences within this complete process) . On this 4-3-2-1
forced-choice sca le, 4 denoted the most-preferred item and 1 the least-preferred within each
g roup of fou r items. The resultant inventory is shown in Figure 10. Col umns 1, 2, 3 and
4 represent the experiencing (X) , ideation (I), thinking (T) and evaluation (E) concepts
respectively. Sets of four words corresponding to items3, 4, 6, 7, 8, 9, 11, 12, 13, 15, 16
and 18 were compiled by pai ring words judged as the most a ppropriate opposites between
col umns 1 and 3, and between 2 and 4, respectively. The distractor sets correspond to items
1, 2, 5, 10, 14 and 17. These 18 item groups were presented i n a way that ensured
Selecting a forced-choice scale (also called an ipsative scale) requi res users to state
some preference level for each of the four quadrants of the complete process of creative
probl em solvi ng. This is consistent with the theory that, although each person has some
u ni que combi nation (profile) of relative preferences among the different phases, all phases
This i nventory is designed to describe your method of problem solving. Give a high rank to those words which best
characterize the way you problem solve and a low rank to the words which are least characteristic of your problem
solving style.
You may find it hard to choose the words that best describe your problem solving style because there are no right
or wrong answers. Different characteristics described in the inventory are equally good. The aim of the i nventory
is to describehow you solve problems, notto evaluateyour problem solving ability.
Instructions:
E ighteen sets of four words are listed horizontally below. In each horizontal set assign a 4 to the word which best
characterizes your problem solving style, a 3 to the word which next best characterizes your problem solving style,
a 2 to the next most characteristic word, and a 1 to the word which is least characteristic of you as a problem solver.
Be sure to assign a different number to each of the four words in each horizontal set. Do not make ties.
2. Patient Diligent --
Forceful __ Prepared
7. Feeli ng --
Alternatives __ Analyzing __ Evaluating
8. Action Divergence --
Abstract __ Convergence
14. Impersonal --
Proud __ Hopeful Fearful
16. --
Present-oriented --
Future-oriented --
Rational --
Detail-oriented
p reference for gaining knowledge and using knowledge. Thus, not only does the profile
reflect relative preferences for the two opposing ways of gaining knowledge and the two
opposing ways of usi ng knowledge, but it more genera l ly reflects relative preference for
The inventory was tested for face validity with attendees of a training conference
(n=30). The respondents u nderstood the inventory and its purpose, and reported that the
instrument was highly accurate in assessi ng their creative problem-solving process styles.
psychometric tools because of the selection of a forced-choice (ipsative) scale to rank items
within each of the 12 item groups. For example, to make a prelimi nary test for i nternal
consistency of the fou r col umns, a special, non-forced choice (non-ipsative) Likert scale
format was created. Data were gathered using this non-ipsative version of the i nventory
using the same 18 item groups i ncluding the six distractor groups. Respondents were asked
to assig n a value (1 to 5) for each of the 72 items i n isolation (i ncl uding the 24 distractor
Figure 1 1
�--------------t--------------t--------------t-------------4
2 3 4 5
This non-forced choice format (non-ipsative) permits respondents to rate each item
independently withi n each of the 12 item groups. Because each item is independent of all
others in its g roup, a standard Cronbach Al pha can be appropriately calculated for each
col umn. By contrast, the forced-choice scale by defi nition yields non-independent ratings
for each item. It is expected that, despite individual differences, most people wil l give each
col umn a significant score as most people would regard favora bly all fou r quadrants and
both ends of both dimensions of using/gaining knowledge. Thus, the format selected must
encou rage respondents to choose from within each item group yet vary their pattern of
choices from one item g roup to the next. These two requirements are met with a well
constructed set of horizontal item groups that offers variety among items in each vital column
measure of column reliability using the ipsative, forced-choice format, as it reflects the
simi larity of val ues a l lotted to a l l items in a column . The CPSP forced-choice format
encourages people to do the opposite. For example, one would expect that persons who
favour ideation and eval uation eq ually as a way of using knowledge would rank ha lf (6) of
the 1 2 item pai rs higher i n Column 2 than Column 4 and half (6) higher in Column 4 than
Col umn 2. The specific g roup of six likely would vary randomly from one person to the next
according to their u nique experience and interpretation of the words. This random way of
deployi ng equal weights (scores) to each column for each person in such a sub-group would
yield excessively low alphas for each col umn, but it would accurately assess relative
Thus, as discussed further i n the section on Reliabi lity and Validity, for the forced-
choice i psative sca le, the random paral lel split-half reliability method employi ng the
Spearman-Brown corrected correlation co-efficient was used to assess reliability for each of
the columns and the bi- polar scales rather than C ronbach Alpha. C ronbach Al pha would
be either i na ppropriate or excessively low for measuring the "complete process of creative
In using C ronbach Alpha and i nter-item correlation to screen the items selected for
a ppropriateness, the non-ipsative version of the CPSP inventory was used. First, on a base
responses to each item (including the distractors) was run for each of the 72 items. The
response distribution for each item was checked to ensure that it met the required desirable
proportion of 80%-20%. That is, for any item, the total percentage of respondents who
a nswered 1, 2 or 3 should fal l between 20% and 80%, as should the total percentage that
answered 3, 4 or 5. I nspection showed that, althoug h some of the distractors fai led to
Because very few people selected 1 on any item, all 1 and 2 responses were
combi ned i nto a single response. A computer program was written to recode responses 1
The same work and results were conducted on a second sample (n= 107) . On each
of these two samples, using the recoded responses (1-4), a Cronbach alpha was calculated
26
on each of the four columns of the non-i psative form of the inventory. All fou r col umns had
reasonably high alphas in both samples, ranging from . 76 to .83, as shown in Table 1. The
correlation matrix showing inter-item correlations and the Reliability Analysis for each
Table 1
Reviewi ng Table 1 , the alphas for col umn 1 were .78 and . 76. All 12 items
correlated with the rest of the column l items taken as a group at least .20 or g reater in
both samples. Scanning the matrices, about seven inter-item correlations were
The al phas for column 2 were .82 and .80. In both samples, all 12 items correlated
with the remai ning items in column 2 taken as a group a bove .20. Two individual inter-item
correlations were nearly zero i n sample 1 and three in sample 2. In both samples, the
For column 3 , the Cronbach alphas were . 76 and .80. All of the items correlated
above .20 with all the other items taken as a g roup with one minor exception in sample 2
(.19). About 10 of the inter-item correlations were close to zero in sample l and about eight
27
in sample 2. All the rest were above . 1 0 and positive except one item in each sample (-. 1 5
and - . 1 4 respectively) .
For col umn 4, the Cronbach alphas were .83 and 77 . . All of the items in both
samples correlated a bove .20 with the remainder of the items i n the col umn taken as a
g roup except one, which correlated . 1 9 and .09 in samples 1 and 2 respectively. About six
of the inter-item correlations were close to zero in sample 1 and nine in sample 2. All the
Bi-pol a r Scales
Stil l using the non-ipsative scale, the i nternal consistency properties of the bi-pola r X-T
and 1 -E scales were i nvestigated. After subtracti ng each of the items of col umn 3 (T) from
its counterpart from column 1 (X), a single scale or factor, X-T, was developed. Likewise, a
second single scale or factor, 1- E, was developed from ·Columns 2 and 4. This provided
items in the two new scales with a range of possible val ues from -3 to +3. To create ordinal
scales, the quantity 4 was added to each new item in the two new scales, providing a val ue
The Pearson correlation coefficient between the two bi-pola r scal es was computed as
. 1 3 for sample 1 and .07 for sample 2. This indicated the independence of the two scales.
An i nternal rel iability analysis on the bi-polar X-T scale revealed Cronbach Al phas of
. 62 for sample 1 and . 73 for sample 2. With two relatively minor exceptions, all X-T item
pai r correlations with the remaining scale item pai rs as a group were quite strong. One of
the exceptions was an item pair that was weak in sample 1 (. 08) but was much stronger i n
28
sample 2 (.30). This was item pair 1 5 (implementi ng-modeling). The other exception was
an item pai r that was slightly weak in sample 1 (. 1 6) but stronger in sample 2 (.22) . This was
item pair 1 8 (aware-orderly) . The rest of the item pairs were all above .20 in both samples.
The second scale, 1-E, had a Cronbach alpha of . 74 in sample 1 and . 7 6 in sample
2. Al l of the item pairs correlated above 20 with the rest of the item pairs in the scale taken
as a g roup in both samples. These data a re available in Appendices XVl l through XX, a nd
previous successful results experienced for face validity described above using the regular,
i psative, forced-choice i nventory format. The further reliability testing of the ipsative version
Table 2
circular process i nvolving two opposite ways of gaining knowledge and two opposite ways
of using knowledge. These two dimensions can be portrayed as two perpendicular axes with
29
fou r q uadrants as shown in Figu re 2. I ndividuals can use the Creative Problem Solvi ng
Profile I nventory discussed below to plot thei r own relative preferences on each of four
scales: Experience, Ideation, Thinking and Eval uation. The shape of the profile a nd the
dominant quadra nt that result from connecting the fou r scores provide i nformation about
one's own� or profile of creative problem solvi ng. See Figures 5 and 6.
Quadra nt D escriptions
The Quadra nt I orientation toward creative problem solving is cal led generator. I n
and gatheri ng i nformation personal ly) and (2) using knowledge for ideation (imagining
possibilities and sensing releva nce in a lmost everything; seeing many different points of view;
dreaming a bout what might be; wondering why things seem to be what they a re; speculati ng
about the future) . Combining these two i nclinations i ndicates a preference for problem
sensing and fact-fi nding activities in the creative process. When operating as a generator,
to the environment, generators a bsorb diverse i nformation and possibi lities that might
pertain to their i nterests and goals. Generators a re comfortable with high ambiguity and
dealing with much i nformation and potential opportunity. In a generator mode, people love
to start things, are comfortable in the early phases of creative problem solving, and a ct as
probl em starters and challenge finders. They antici pate and sense new problems, changes
The Quadrant II orientation toward creative problem solvi ng is cal led conceptualizer.
inclinations are: ( 1 ) using knowledge for ideation (as above) and (2) gaining knowledge by
problem formulation and defin ition and idea generation. Conceptualizers tend to absorb
a wide range of seemingly unrelated facts or idea fragments and possibilities, and assimilate
them into an i nteg rated explanation, hypothesis, theory, q uestion, chal lenge, problem
definition or idea. They l i ke viewing the big picture and extracting and defining the essence
of the opportunity or problem, and generating ideas to solve it. They are problem definers
and idea developers and a re comfortable in the early to middle phases of creative problem
solving.
The Quadrant Ill orientation toward creative problem solving is called optimizer.
a re: (1) learning by detached dominant abstract thi nking (as above) and (2) using such
knowledge primari ly for evaluation (developing criteria for assessing alternatives, being
aware of possible pitfalls and deficiencies in potential sol utions, and looking for a single
optimum or best answer) . These two inclinations indicate a preference for being i nvolved
i n the practical sol utions of a well-defi ned problem, and planning and organizing concrete
staps for making them implementable. Thus, optimizers a re problem solvers, and a re more
When one is operating in an implementer mode, one's combi nation of inclinations toward
usi ng knowledge for evaluation and gaining knowledge by direct concrete experience
i ndicates a preference for implementation activity - gaining acceptance from others for new
sol utions and changes, and taking action to make sure those solutions and changes work
and stick. I n an implementer mode, one does not generally dwell on understanding the
theory behind a new idea or plan. I nstead, one wants to take it and "run with it," experience
it, work with it, show others how to use it, shape it and fit it to needs, adapt it to various
changing circumstances, and try and retry it (and not worry about why it didn't work the first
way) . I n a strong implementer mode, people wil l do whatever it takes (including alteration)
to implement the plan, idea, new product or new solution. They wil l become directly
finishers and a re most comfortable in the later phases of creative problem solving.
Scori ng a nd Plott i ng
The inventory i n Figu re 10 is scored and plotted as follows: I n each col umn, all the
items are summed except items 1, 2, 5 1 10 1 14 and 17 (the distractors) to yield col umn
scores. Each column score is plotted on the appropriate axis of the Basadur Creative
Problem Solving Profile shown i n Figu re 10 below. Note that the col umn 1 score indicates
the individual's orientation toward gaining knowledge for creative problem solving by di rect
concrete Experiencing; the column 2 score indicates the orientation toward using Ideation
i n creative problem solving; the col umn 3 score indicates the orientation toward gaining
knowledge for creative problem solving by detached, abstract Thinking; the col umn 4 score
32
indicates the orientation towa rd using Evaluation in creative problem solving. Connecti ng
the four points in sequence with curved lines makes an i rregular circle that represents one's
personal creative problem-solving profile. Four identical col umn scores would result in a
perfect ci rcle. This is unlikely to occur (but is a perfectly legitimate profile). The q uadra nt
in which the profile is most dominant indicates one's strongest orientation. The other
q uadrants represent secondary orientations. One's profile is one's unique blend of the four
experience and (2) by abstract analytical, logical thinking; and two opposite ways of
diverging) a nd (2) by eval uation (testing and verifying new possibilities, making
2. As a complete process, creativi1y is about more than just "getting ideas." It starts with
problem sensing and ends in action, and involves evaluation and convergent thin ki ng
different stages.
33
3. Different people have differing skills i n the various stages of the creative process. We
incl i nations and ski l ls, the CPSP enables individuals to understand how to team up
for synergy and how to combine their strengths for g reater creativity.
RELIABILITY AN D VALIDITY
application sessions has demonstrated the instrument's excellent reliability and face validity.
People who take the i nventory more than. once usual ly report stable scores over time.
Changing scores a re usua l ly explained by such factors as job changes that require different
thinking and problem-solving modes. Most users state that the profile makes much sense
to them. It helps them better u nderstand the concept of a "complete process" of creative
problem solving and appreciate individual differences within such a complete process.
Excellent learnings result when individuals complete the CPSP within a group, in which they
A more formal program to investigate reliabi l ity and validity psychometrical ly has
been under way for some time. This section reports findings to date.
34
RELIABI LITY
To estimate test-retest rel iability, two samples, n=129 and n=40, representi ng a wide
variety of business and industrial organizations and job types were administered the CPSP
i nventory twice, one week apart. Correlations of the fou r columns and two bi-polar column
scores for the two occasions were then calculated, as shown in Ta ble 3.
Table 3
Thus, the correlations i n Table 3 range from .66 to . 75 for the bi-pola r scales, and
from . 58 to .69 for the columns, respectively. This represents satisfactory test-retest
reliability. The variations across the four columns, two bi-polar scales and two samples are
relatively small.
2. Solit-Half Reliability
The i nternal consistency of the instrument was investigated using the random,
para l lel, split-half method for four separate, i ndependent sa mples and one large sample
35
which combi ned the fi rst three of the four samples with several additional samples. The 1 2
item g roups of the i nventory were randomly assigned to two parallel, split-half inventories
of six item g roups each. From each of the two parallel, six-item, split-half i nventories that
resulted , each partici pant's fou r q uadrant scores were computed, as were correlation
coefficients for each of the four quadrants. The Spearman-Brown corrected correlation
coefficient (rxx) was calculated for each q uadrant as the reliability estimate for the composite
1 2-item inventory. (The composite inventory was the sum of the scores of the two parallel
Table 4
Thus, the Spearman-Brown corrected reliability estimates calculated above for the
four q uadrants of the Creative Problem-Solving Profile Inventory ranged across the quadrants
within each of the five samples from .62 to .65, .66 to . 69, .67 to . 73, .64 to . 72, and .62
to . 70, and within the four quadrants across the samples from . 64 to .71 , .62 to .73 , .62 to
. 69 and .62 to . 71 . This i ndicates satisfactory levels of consistency reliability within each
Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated among the fou r q uadrants and
among the four basic scales (col umns) and between the two bi-polar scales for the five
I n Table 5, as expected from both theory and scale construction, the correlations
between the scores on the pairs of opposite q uadrants ( 1 and 3, 2 and 4) were strongly
negative across all five samples (-. 98, -.92, -.90, -.96, -.98, and -.97, -.92, -.87, -.97, -.97,
respectively) . Also as expected from theory, the correlations between scores on pai rs of
adjacent quadrants were low. I n sample 5, the range was - . 1 1 to .05; in sample 4, -.02 to
The strongly negative correlations between opposite q uadrant scores indicate that the
represented by these q uadra nts. The low adjacent q uadrant correlations indicate satisfactory
i ndependence of adjacent quadrants despite the sharing of one col umn (scale) between each
I n Table 6, as expected from theory, the correlations between the Bi-polar X-T
(column 1 -3 ) and the Bi-polar 1- E (col umn 2-4) scales were very low in all five samples (.06,
. 05, . 1 1 , -.07 and . 1 1 , respectively) . This indicates that the two scales are independent a nd
that the two dimensions of the CPSP -- gaining and using knowledge (see Figure 4) -- a re
This supports the notion that opposing concepts a re being measu red at the poles of each of
Table 5
Table 6
and between adjacent scales X and E were -. 1 6, - . 24, -. 1 2, - .07 and -. 26, respectively. The
correlations between adjacent scales T and I , and T and E, were - . 20, -. 1 2, - . 30, -. 1 6, -. 26
a nd .04, . 02, . 05, - . 1 4, . 03, respectively. As predicted by theory, most of these correlations
a re quite low and represent satisfactory independence and internal consistency. The T and
The low correlations between the bi-polar X-T and 1-E scales (ranging from -.07 to
. . 1 1 ), the strongly negative correlations between the opposite poles on the two dimensions
(ranges -.65 to -.69 and - . 64 to - . 78), and the relatively low correlations between the pai rs
of adjacent columns (XI, IT, TE and EX) indicate that ( l ) the two bi-polar scales and the four
colu mns taken in adjacent pai rs are satisfactorily independent; and ( 2 ) the opposing columns
within each of the two dimensions measure strongly contrasting concepts. This represents
good internal consistency for the CPSP instrument shown in Figure l 0. Furthermore, the
consistency of results across the five samples also indicates good parallel test rel iability for
i ntercorrelations of scales.
VALI D ITY
A. The Kirton Adaptation- Innovation I nventory Theory (KAI) defines and measures two
personal ity types relative to creative decision making (Kirton, 1 9 7 6 ) . Accoiding to the
At one end of this conti n uum, adaptors characteristically use accepted definitions of
the problem and likely solutions in generating ideas. They examine these ideas closely a nd
proceed withi n the established theories, policies and practices of thei r organizations. Much
of thei r chan ge effort involves im provi ng and "doing better." This description is very
consistent with the description of the Optimizer stage of the CPSP (and opposite to the
Generator stage) .
Innovators are more l i kely to reconstruct the problem. By considering the problem
free of accepted thought, paradigms and customary viewpoints, they generate much less
expected, and probably less acceptable, sol utions. They think less about "doing things
better" than "doing things differently." This description is very consistent with the description
of Generators (opposite to Optimizers), who continually find new problems to solve and
Similarly, the I and E scales which measure opposing ways of using knowledge
a ppear consistent with the I nnovator and Adaptor KAI styles, respectively. The KAI
a pparently measures not different ways of gaining knowledge but different ways of using it.
As a result, the X and T scales of the CPSP are apparently unrelated to the KAI .
Because of their interest i n new ideas and their desire to correctly define the problem
and see the big picture, Conceptualizers are also consistent with the KAI I nnovator style.
I mplementers, who confine their activity to converting already developed plans and ideas
Compa rin g the KAI I nnovator and Adaptor descriptions to the descriptions of the four
CPSP quadrants and to the I , E and 1-E bi-pola r use of knowledge distinctions leads to the
followin g hypotheses:
40
correlate with the KAI, the former positively and the latter negatively.
Hypothesis #2 : The 1- E and I scale scores should correlate positively with the KAI
Hypot hesis #3 : The X, T and X-T scale scores should not correlate substantial ly with
the KAI (because the KAI relates not to gaining knowledge but to usi ng it) .
with the KAI , as do the Generator and Optimizer styles, but to a lesser degree.
Testing. Two samples were administered both the KAI and the CPSP. The first
n= 1 85, consisted of supervisors a nd managers from several busi ness and government
organizations and functions (and included the n = 1 1 0 sample described in the appendix).
Pearson Product Moment correlation coefficients were calculated for the following 1 1
variables and formed a correlation matrix for each sample: KAI score; total scores on each
of the fou r CPSP col u mns (X, I , T, E); scores calculated for each of the four C PSP Quadrants
product of colu mns 3 and 4 scores; Quadrant 4, I mplementer style, product of col umns 4
a nd 1 scores) ; and scores for the X-T and 1-E bi-polar scales developed by subtracting T
The correlation matrices are shown here as Tables 7 and 8. The key findings noted
from scanning the tables are as follows. As predicted, Quadrant 1 , the Generator style,
Table 7
Correlation of CPSP Columns a nd Quadra nt Scores with KAI Scores for Sample l, n = 1 01
Conceptualizer (I xT) 8 70. 1 208 . 9 -. 60 *** . 77*** .45 *** - . 68 *** -. 5 8 *** . 77 *** .12 .22 *
Optimizer (Tx E) 933.3 224.0 - .55 *** -. 7 1 *** . 66 *** . 77 *** -. 66 *** - . 78 *** - . 35 *** -. 98 *** -.23 **
I m plementer (ExX) 91 8.9 2 1 1 .0 . 60 *** -. 75 *** -.48 *** . 6 8 *** . 59 *** - . 76 *** -.08 - .23 ** -.9 7 *** .20 *
Coefficients marked *** a re statistically significant at p < .00 1 ; ** at p < .0 1 ; and * p < .0 5
.i::...
__,
Table 8
Correlation of C PS P Col umns and Quadra nt Scores with KAI Scores for Sample 2. n= 1 85
Generator (Xx I ) 9 74.3 20 7 .4 . 6 1 *** . 6 7 *** -. 68 *** - . 74 *** . 70 *** . 7 7 *** .43 ***
Conceptualizer (I ><T) 82 1 . 8 1 88.4 - . 66 *** . 6 8*** . 5 7 *** - . 53 *** -. 6 7 *** . 66 *** . 25 ** .03 *
Optimizer (Tx E) 825 . 6 1 94.4 - . 67 *** -. 6 1 *** . 7 1 *** . 7 2 *** -. 74 *** -. 7 3 *** -.42 *** -. 9 7 *** .01
I m plementer (E xX) 9 74.5 1 95 .0 . 6 3 *** -. 7 0 *** - . 55 . 56 *** . 64 *** - . 6 9 *** -.28 *** - .09 ** -.9 7 *** .01
Coefficients marked *** are statistically significant at p< . 00 1 ; ** at p< .0 1 ; and * p< .05
�
""
43
positively correlated with the KAI, in both samples (.33 and .43), and significant positive
correlations were found between KAI and 1 - E scores in both samples (.29 and .49) and
between KAI and I scores (.29 and .44) . Also as predicted, the Quadrant 3 (Optimizer style)
correlation with KAI was significantly negative i n both sa mples (-.35 and -.42) . Quadra nt
2, the Conceptualizer style, also correlated positively with KAI in both samples, but, as
predicted, at lower levels (. 1 2 , not significant, and .25) . The Quadrant 4 style, I mplementer,
also correlated negatively at lower levels (- . 08, not significant, and -.28). The correlations
between KAI and Quadrant 2 (Conceptualizer) and Quadrant 4 (Implementer) lend some
support to Hypothesis #4. As expected, there were low correlations between KAI a nd the
X-T, X, and T scales. The sample 2 correlations were lower (. 1 4, . 1 2, and -. 1 4) than
sample 1 (. 2 1 , . 1 4, and -.25) respectively. General ly, all four hypotheses were supported.
theory of psychological types that is widely used in organizational psychology. The MBTI
produces fou r bi pola r scales measuring Extroversion-I ntroversion (El), Sensing-Intuition (SN) ,
by these scales (Myers, 1 994) appear highly relevant to the CPSP quadrant styles.
The El scale i ndicates how strongly an i ndividual focuses internally or externally. The
Extrovert type focuses on external events, people, things and experiences, and prefers to
learn by doing. The I ntrovert type focuses on the in ner world of concepts and ideas, and
prefers to learn by reflection and thought. Extroverts a re said to be sociable and expressive,
and more i nterested and comfortable when they are working actively with people or thi ngs.
44
I ntroverts are said to be private and contained, and more interested and comfortable when
The SN scale defines preferences for "fi nding out." Sensing types are said to be
factual , concrete and oriented to today, to val ue practical applications, and to trust
experience. They rely on what their senses tell them about their environment. I n contrast,
l ntuitives a re future-oriented, abstract and theoretical, concerned with the big picture, and
a ble to see patterns and meaning in facts; they a re also said to enjoy defining and solving
new, complex problems. They use thei r intuition to gain meaning, propose relationships and
possibilities beyond their physical senses, and consider what they might do about a situation.
The TF scale defines an individual's approach to decision making. Thinking types are
said to rely on logic, analysis and cause-a nd-effect reasoning when making decisions a nd
solvi ng problems, and to seek a detached and impersonal standard of truth . Feeling types
a re said to make decisions based on affective and person-centered values rather than on
objective standards of logic. They incorporate anything that matters or is important to them
The J P scale measu res orientation to the outer world. Judging types seek to control
and regulate life, to make clear decisions and move on; they plan extensively, and a re
a nd cu rious, seeking to understand, experience and adapt rather than to control life; they
prefer to keep thei r options open rather than restrict themselves to fixed plans or schedules.
The MBT! Form G (I-Ayers & Myers, 1 984 ; 1-llyers & McCaulley, 1 985) is a 1 26-item
inventory produci ng four pai rs of preference scores. MBTI practitioners usual ly convert each
of the fou r pai rs of scores to a dichotomous preference, then assign an individual to one of
45
1 6 personality types. A summary of the four bipolar MBTI type preferences is provided in
the MBTI Users Manual by Consulti ng Psychologists Press, I nc., and is shown in Tables 9 a nd
1 0. Several personality researchers (Hicks, 1 984; 1 985; McCrea & Costa, 1 989; Stricker
& Ross, 1 964) have suggested that rather than disti nct MBTI types, the MBTI really measures
fou r i ndependent and contin uous personality traits. Following this line of reasoning, a
research a lternative to dichotomizi ng the MBTI scores was adopted. Four continuous trait
scores, one for each dimension (shown in Table 1 1 ) were obtained by subtracting the two
scores representing the opposing poles of the dimension, and resetting the origin to a
neutral point of zero. MBTI conti nuous scores are scored in the direction of the second
named pole. For example, a score g reater than zero on the El scale represents a preference
for I ntroversion (I) and a score less than zero a preference for Extroversion (E).
A correlational study was performed using the MBTI and CPSP. The total number of
partici pants was 1 34 MBA students, most of them enrolled i n the co-operative education
The correlations i n Table 1 2 show that the Generator style was associated significantly
with a l l fou r MBTI scales, most strongly with Extroversion (.35) and Perception (.30) (both
p < . 0 1 ) but also with I ntuition (.25, p< . 0 1 ) and Feeling (.22, p < .05). Generators may,
therefore, characteristically focus externally rather than internally and prefer to employ their
Perceivi ng fun ction rather than thei r Judgi ng function. This is consistent with the idea that
they excel in the initial phase of the Simplex creative problem-solving process, which involves
scanning the external envi ronment to identify new facts and new problems to work on. This
is also consistent with the Feel ing characteristic of accommodating any relevant points no
Table 9 46
EXTROVERTS I NTROVERTS
Like variety and action. Like quiet for concentration.
Tend to be faster, dislike complicated Tend to be careful with details, dislike
p roced ures. sweepi ng statements.
Are often good at g reeting people. Have trouble remembering names and faces.
Are often impatient with long slow jobs. Tend not to mind working on one project for
a long time uninterruptedly.
Are interested in the results of their job, in Are interested in the idea behind their job.
getting it done and in how other people do it.
Often do not mind the interruption of Dislike telephone intrusions and interruptions.
answering the telephone.
Often act quickly, sometimes without Like to think a lot before they act, sometimes
thinking. without acting.
Like to have people a round. Work contentedly alone.
Usual ly communicate freely. Have some problems communicating.
Table 1 2
C PSP Styles
MBTI Sca l es
Generator Conceptual izer Optimizer Implementer
(Quadrant 1 ) (Quadrant 2 ) (Quadrant 2) (Quadrant 4 )
Table 1 3
Table 1 4
C PS P Colu mns
MBTI Scal es
Col u m n l Col u m n 2 Col u m n 3 Col u m n 4
Gaining Using Gaining Using
Knowledge by Knowledge for Knowledge by Knowledge for
Experiencing Ideation Thi n ki ng Eval uation
(Physical (Creating (Mental (Choosing
Processing 1 ) Options) Processing) Options)
matter how il logical, and with the I ntuitive characteristics of enjoying new problems and of
The Optimizer style correlations were exactly opposite to the Generator style. All four
correlations with the MBTI were significant (I , .37, p < . 0 1 ; J, . 3 1 , p < . 0 1 ; S, . 24, p < . 0 1 ; a nd
T, - . 1 7 , p < . 05) . Thus, the MBTI preferences most strongly associated with the Optimizer
style are I ntroversion and J udging. An introverted focus is consistent with the Optimizer style
preferences for mental testing of ideas, worki ng on one project at a time, and for dealing
with thi ngs rather than people. Their preference for J udging is consistent with their
51
preference for the evaluati ng, selecti ng, planning a n d organizing stage of the Simplex
creative problem-solving process. The Thi nking preference is consistent with the Optimizer's
reliance on logic, analysis and reasoning in problem solving, and the Sensing preference is
consistent with the orientation toward practical solutions and the aversion toward add ressi ng
The results also show that Conceptualizers are characterized by a clear preference
(.38, p < . 0 1 ) for I ntuition. I ntuitive individuals seek to grasp patterns, and try to understand
relationshi ps and make connections between facts; they a re also said to do wel l at seeing
new possi bil ities and different ways of doing things. This is entirely consistent with the
thinking style of the Conceptualizer, and the Conceptua lizer's preference for the problem
definition and idea-finding steps of the Simplex creative problem-solving process. There was
a lesser but significant (.20, p < . 05) correlation with the Perception type, consistent with the
The I mplementer style (the opposite of the Conceptualizer style) was associated a lmost
equal ly strongly with the opposite types -- that is, Sensi ng (.36, p < . 0 1 ) and Judging (. 2 1 ,
p < . 05) . People who prefer Sensing are said to be factual, concrete and practical , a nd to
val ue experience and application -- characteristics that seem consistent with a preference for
desire to move on, get things finished, and follow through thei r plans without interruption.
The results suggest that preferences for performing successive stages of the Simplex
creative problem-solving process are related to preferences for different MBTI functions. At
different stages, the poles of various dimensions predominate. The first stage, Generating,
is related to Extroversion and Perceiving and also to I ntuition and Feeling. Generators are
52
sensitive to their envi ronment, scanning for new problems to solve, avoid formi ng
concl usions or closing off options, and focus on absorbing a variety of sti muli. They see
opportunity in al most everything, and enjoy the ambiguity inherent in peoples' problems
which involve feelings and val ues. In the next stage, the I ntuitive function hel ps discern
patterns in data, to make new connections and to conceptualize new insights into how the
problem should be defined and modeled. The Conceptualizer begins to consider the future
by looking at new possibilities, hatching new ideas and keeping them open. Next, the
Thinking and Sensi ng. By focusing inward, the Optimizer subjects solutions and ideas to
sustained and systematic mental scrutiny using concrete, logical criteria, before making a n
opti mal choice. The practicality of that choice is governed by the realities of the situation,
not a plethora of intuitive speculation. Finally, during the implementation stage, the Sensing
function predominates, with Judgment supporting. The I mplementer deals with the concrete
and current and focuses on converging upon a successful finish, not on diverging to open
up new opportunities.
style affects training in creative thinking. This intensive hands-on training emphasized the
problem fi nding, problem solving and i mplementation . T\.YO attitudes associated V'1ith
divergent thinking were measured before and after training. The sample (n= 1 56) consisted
53
of both managers (n= 90) and non-managers (n= 66) and a variety of functional specialties,
The primary hypotheses were that, compared to the other three styles (generator,
conceptual izer and implementer) , participants with an opti mizer style who received trai ning
(premature convergence).
A six-item preference for ideation scale and an eight-item tendency to make premature
critical evaluations of ideas scale were used to measure "active divergence" and "premature
convergence" attitudes. The items a nd scales a re ful ly descri bed in Basadur and Finkbeiner
( 1 985) . The major finding of this study was that, as predicted by hypotheses H10 and H1b ,
practice with creative techniques and processes was particularly effective for people with a n
opti mizer style of creative problem solving. For the entire sample, optimizers made g reater
gains than the other three styles on both attitudes. All four creative problem-solving styles
improved significantly on the premature convergence attitude, but the improvement on the
active divergence attitude was statistical ly significant only for the optimizers. For the non
manager subsample, the advantages for the optimizers were the same as those of the entire
sample; only the advantage over conceptualizers was not statistical ly significant. For the
manager subsample, the advantages for the optimizers were the same as those fou nd for
no advantages for the optimizer. Thus, hypothesis H 1 b was strongly supported for the entire
54
sample and hypothesis H 1 0 (active divergence) was strongly su pported for the entire sample
and the non-manager subsample, but not for the manager subsamples.
Table 1 5 presents the mean differences in the two attitudes for the four creative
problem-solving process styles for the entire sample and for the manager and non-manager
subgroups separately. For the overall sample and the active divergence attitude, only the
optimizer style showed a significant gain, t(30) = 2. 1 , p < .05, fol lowing training. Tukey's
tests indicated that this gain was significantly g reater than the average of the other three
styles and significantly g reater than for either the conceptualizers or implementers (p < .05) .
For the premature convergence attitude, all four styles showed significant gains.
For the non-manager subsample and the active divergence attitude, only the
optimizer style showed a significant gain, t(l 0) = 2.3, p < . 05, following training. Tukey's
tests indicated that this gain was significantly g reater than the averages of the other three
styles and the implementer style. For the premature convergence attitude, all fou r styles
showed significant gains fol lowing training. Here again, Tukey's tests following the ANOVA
indicated (p< . 0 1 ) that the gain by the optimzer style was significantly g reater than the
average of the other three styles, and significantly g reater than the implementer style
(p< . 05).
For the manager subsample, the gain in the active divergence attitude fol lowi ng
training was not significant for the opti mizer, implementer or conceptualizer styles. Only the
gain for the generators was significant, t(9) = 2.6, p < .05. None of the comparisons of the
gains of the four styles vvas significant. For the premature convergence attitude, a ! ! four
styles showed significant gains (p< .0 1 ) following training. Tukey's comparisons indicated
Table 1 5 55
Non-Managers {n = 6 6}
Generator
( n =20/l 9) 2 1 .8 23.0 26.6 1 8. 9 3 .6**
(4.3) (4 .4) (6 .0) (5.8)
Conceptua I izer
(n= 1 5/1 4) 2 2 .4 23.7 24.5 1 7.8 3.6**
(3. 9) (4 .2) (5 . 4) (5. 3)
Optimizer
(n = l l /1 0) 2 2 .0 25.3a,c 2 .3* 24.4 1 4.5a,c 4 . 7**
(4 . 1 ) (3.9) (5.6) (5.5)
Implementer
( n = 2 0/ l 9) 2 1 .6 2 2 .3 b 25.6 20.3b 3.5**
(4.3) (4. 1 ) (5.2) (5 .4)
(ANOVA F) (ns) (3.6)* (3.8) * (4 .9)* (6. 1 )**
Managers {n =90)
Generator
( n = l 0/9) 1 9.4 23.2 2 . 6* 22.8 1 7 .4 3 .4**
(4 .5) (4.2) (6.2) (5 .8)
Conceptualizer
( n = 32/3 1 ) 21 .l 22.8 22.8 1 7 .2 3 .4**
(4 . 1 ) (4 .0) (5.5) (5. 7)
Optimizer
(n=2 0/l 9) 20. 1 22.7 25. l l 6 . 7a 4 .0**
(4. 1 ) (3. 8) (5.2) (5.5)
Implementer
(n=28/2 7) 1 9.6 22.1 24.1 l 9.2b 3 .2 **
(3.9) (4 .0) (5.2) (4. 9)
(ANOVA F) (ns) (ns) (ns) (ns) (5.7) ** (5 .5)**
Note: Values denoted .9. are significantly different from those denoted )2 at the p< .05 level. s indicates that Optimizers are different from
the average of the other three styles at the p < .05 level. Tests were concluded based on Tukey's method following ANOVA.
* and ** denote significant changes at p< .05 and p < . 0 1 respedively comparing After-Before means based on t tests for the
matched sample (Kerlinger, 1 973).
ns - Non-significant
56
that the gain by the opti mizers was significantly greater than the average of the other three
styles, and significantly g reater than the gain by the implementers (p< .05).
The ability of the CPSP i nventory to predict a subject's profile compared to the
estimate of the subject's profile made by consensus with an expert partner (one who knows
Each consensus pair ranked the fou r q uadrants in the order they agreed should
describe each subject best. This ranking was compared to that provided by the inventory
itself for all 24 sets of scores as fol lows. Because there are four quadrants to rank order,
there a re four "slots" for each subject to compare for agreement as shown in Figure 1 2 (total
96 slots for 24 subjects). For each slot, there would be 1 00% agreement (same quadrant
picked for this slot), zero agreement (opposite q uadra nt picked for this slot) or partial
agreement (an adjacent q uadrant picked for this slot) . Then a one-way table chi-squared
The form used for ran king is shown below in Figu re 1 2. Table 1 6 shows the results
of the degree of agreement test between prediction by "i nventory" and prediction by "expert
X2 -
_ (4i:;;: _ l")..t)2 /l") A
.J L..... I L .....
+ / 1 7_
\ I , 1") A )2/'J4
L....,. L
..L
I
( 'l A _ A Q.) 2 /A Q.
V""'T -r\J I """T ....,
- 4.
�
'JA J:\ ')
"""T . '-' L
Fig u re 1 2
Scale: •
a score of 4 indicates the quadrant ranked highest (most like me)
•
a score of 3 indicates the quadrant ranked 2"d highest (second most like me)
•
a score of 2 indicates the q uadrant ranked 3rd highest (third most like me)
•
a score of l indicates the q uadrant ranked lowest (least like me)
2. My partner's prediction of my
profile is
3. Our consensus of my profile is
(by discussion after separate
p redictions i ndependently
made)
4. The CPSP i nventory's prediction
of my profile is
Table 1 6
Discussio n
The null hypothesis i s that there will be random chance (25%/25%/50%) distribution
across the base i.e., 25%/25%/50%. But, if the inventory is valid, we should get significantly
more than 25% in the first cell (agreement) and significantly less than 25% in the second cel l
(disagreement) .
Then % of slots agree is 50%; % of slots disagree (opposite) is 0%; and % of time slots
partially agree (are adjacent) is 50%. This provides a 50%/0%/50% distribution vs the
1 8. 3 75
24
( 1 7 - 24 ) 2 =
Cell 2 : 2.04 2
24
( 34 - 4 8 ) 2 =
Cell 3 : 4 . 083
24
Total 24.52
Total d.f. = 2
X2 = 24.2 > . 01 (al pha = 9. 2 1 ) and the null hypothesis can be rejected (p< .00 1 ) .
Predi ctive Val id ity: Rating of D egree of Fit of C PS P rofile as Compared to Subject's Own
The following table shows a "goodness of fit" between the CPSP styl es i nventory
In Table 1 7, the mean ratings of how wel l the CPS Profile matched individuals'
perceptions of their own creative problem-solving process style are 7 . 1 and 8.3 for
u ndergraduates and managers respectively, based on a 1 0-point scale. The fit was rated
profile as a good predictor of their personal process style of creative problem solving.
Table 1 7
explore the relative contri butions of ideation and evaluation during each of the three phases
of the process of Figure 1 . For exa mple, do these relative contributions differ by task or field
of work? Perhaps in high-pressure jobs oriented strongly toward implementation, eval uation
opti mal ideation-evaluation ratios that differ by phase for any job or organization.
On the contrary, Figure 1 models a process i n which people alternate thei r judgmental and
environment focused on profits and quick action, increasing creative behavior would l i kely
involve i ncreases in both ideation and evaluation skill. However, on a day-to-day basis, this
attitude than a diverging, perceptive (ideational) attitude. Environments that a re less ti me
sensitive, such as pure research, might favor divergence and perceptiveness (ideational) at
eval uation at each phase (see Figu re 1 ) encourages participants to balance ideation a nd
evaluation. This balance is i ll ustrated in the educational film, "The Dot and the Line"
(Norto n , 1 96 5) , which shows the pitfalls of the two extremes of total rigidity and total
flexibility, and promotes an opti mal concept cal led "disci plined freedom."
Second, such training might move individuals from either extreme on an ideation
evaluation spectrum toward some optimum (depending on the type of work or field of
endeavor i nvolved) . The location of this optimum may differ for work requiring more
problem finding (e.g . , pure research), more problem solving (e.g., applied research) or more
sol ution implementation (e. g., manufacturing). This thinking is depicted in Figure 1 3 .
The n otion of different optimal ratios of ideation and evaluation in varying phases is
ill ustrated in yet another way i n Figure 1 4. Figure 1 4 represents a revision of the basic
model in Figu re 1 . The i deation and evaluation symbols a re of equal length in Figure 1
withi n and among the three phases. I n Figure 1 4, however, the lengths are similar only
within Phase I I ; the ideation symbol is longer than the evaluation symbol in Phase I and vice
versa in Phase I l l . These ratio differences in Figure 1 4 illustrate differences in each phase
in the opti mal balance between ideation activity and evaluation activity.
62
Fig u re 1 3
Field of Endeavor
(Phase I l l *)
Preference -Optimum Preference
for for
Evaluation Ideation
* See Figu re 1
63
Fig u re 1 4
N ote :
PROBLEM
Ph ase I I
SOLVING
Ph ase I l l
64
Third, throughout the 11complete process" (Figure 1 ), the relative desirable emphasis
on eval uation with i n a given field of endeavor may increase within each phase but from
different starti ng points (and vice versa for ideation). This concept is illustrated in Figure 1 5.
medici ne and engi neering, the opti mum will tend toward the E side. Training in a 11complete
preference for such an opti mum. The opti mal ratio between time devoted to ideation activity
a nd time devoted to evaluation activity (l/E) would then be relatively lower than the optimal
ratio for more pure research professions. This optimal ratio (l/E) would be even l ower i n
problem fi nding and with fewer time restrictions, such as pu re resea rch, the opti mal l/E
ratios would be relatively higher. In the former cases, the range of 11good judgment"
decisions might be much tighter than in the latter cases, requiring more attention to
eval uation. An a rchitect or writer might face diverse acceptable problems and sol utions.
sol utions may be more heavily constrained by the short-term needs of the patient or
organization.
direction for future research is to determine whether optimal ideation-evaluation ratios exist
on the spectrum for each phase of the 11 complete process of creative problem solving" (see
Figure 1 and Figure 1 3) for various fields of endeavor. Another research route is to
determine whether optimal ideation-eval uation ratios change with increasing eval uation at
each succeeding phase of the 11complete process of problem solving" (see Figure 1 and
Figu re 1 5
Possible Opti m u m I deation-Eva luation Preference Ratios for Each of Three Phases of
"A Complete Process of C reative Problem Solving" for Different Fields of Endeavor
Problem
Solving � -x 1 1 x --{
(Phase I I *) J . � -x� J
E
"a ptimum I E "" .
I E
/ I
Optimum Optimum
Solution
I mplementation L _
"'
I I X 1
(Phase I l l *) 11
I /
rx I ' x "'
E I E
Optimum O ptimu m
Opt im um
I
0-
():J
66
Figu re 1 4) and whether these ratios would differ for various fields of endeavor (see Figures
1 3 and 1 5) . The movement of various samples of trai nees from extreme positions of E or
Research has already supported the usefulness of such future exploration. Basadur,
whose d iffering creative probl em solving styles reflect varying preferences for ideation
evaluation. Such different styles may relate to field of work. Kirton ( 1 987) found that
departments more concerned with implementation (such as production) have more adaptive
creative styles. One could speculate that they favor problem-solving activity si nce they prefer
that focus on finding new long-term opportunities (such as research and development) have
more innovative creative styles. It could be said that they favor problem-finding activity,
si nce they prefer to deal with problems . by breaking (redefining) given paradigms.
work with R&D in translating new concepts (problems) into new designs (solutions) a nd work
(implementation) - had mean KAI scores roughly halfway between those of production and
R&D . This supports the usefulness of exploring the concept of optimum l/E ratios, and also
I n Table 1 8, research data on relatively sma l l base sizes showing l/E ratios for various
fields of endeavor tests this theory. The respondents were extracted from the various
samples used in this report. For some occupations, the base sizes were too sma l l to report
and wil l be supplemented in future work. Table 1 8 includes a col umn that predicts, based
67
on judgment and experience, which of the three phases (problem fi nding or solving or
Table 1 8
D iscussion
The data in Table 1 8 suggest that l/E ratios can be meaningfully used to describe the
creative problem solving. I n virtual ly every case, the predicted emphasis was consistent with
. 90 and 1 . 1 0 and are consistent with medium time horizons of solving problems, helping
The reasoning on opti mal l/E ratios above can easi ly be extended to predict the
preferred C PSP styles of people in various fields of endeavor. Table 1 9 shows mean
quadrant scores of individuals in various fields of endeavor. The fields of endeavor are
groupings make reasonable sense. For example, the q uadrant 4 fields a l l emphasize short
i ncluding engineeri ng, finance, accounting, systems development, quality assurance and
progra mmer/analyst, all i nvolve solving problems with precision and eval uating a nd
consultation with clients to define problems accurately, quadrant 2 would tend to dominate,
and development, which require people to generate new big ideas, projects, opportunities
and problems to research further or to stimulate others' interest. Such fields as counseling
and personnel/human resources require the ability to operate with ambiguity, helping
Table 1 9
6. Predi ctive Validity: Mean C PSP Quadra nt (Style) Scores by Organ izational Level
I n Table 20, mean quadrant scores are tabulated for a large sample (n= 1 639) by
operational thin king. The i ncreasing levels are defined as non-management, fi rst-line
higher in the hierarchy, the less important it becomes to implement and optimize day-to-day
operational tasks and current methods, products and services, and the more important to
generate new di rections to pursue, to think strategically about the future, to conceptualize
the "big picture" and to create appropriate goals for others to achieve. The category
selected as the highest i n the hierarchy includes "non-hierarchical" jobs for which highly
specialized technical and professional people, including economists, scientists and planners,
are paid pri mari ly to thin k and to help senior management to thi nk strategical ly.
Accordi ng to this logic, as responsibilities increase, the mean scores for the generator
and conceptualizer q uadrants should i ncrease and those for the implementer and optimizer
should decrease. The data in Table 20 and Figure 1 6 support this prediction, especially for
Ta ble 2 1 and Figure 1 7 show that l/E ratios increase in a similar fashion as
organizational levels gain more responsibili1y for strategic thinki ng rather than operational
thinking.
71
Table 2 0
N on-
management 1 91 888 808 896 989
First Line
Supervision 494 893 81 3 897 980
Middle
Management 232 899 843 893 946
U pper
Management 84 898 875 895 913
Technical/
Professional 474 929 896 873 884
Table 2 1
Means (SO}
O rgan izational Level Base lLE Ratios
Optimizing
900 • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • •
•
• • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • • •
• •
• • •
Generating • •
.,... .,... � r:• .,... ;.I
• ..-.--
• •
• •
--
--
--
--
--
--
--
--
,,,,.
--
--
--
,,,,.
--
--
,,,,.
Co ncept ua I izi ng --
--
--
------ - -�1--�����-��-+-��+-�
8 QQ 1-
"""
t-...)
Level of Organizational Responsibility for Thinking Strategically Rather than Operationa l ly
Fig ure 1 7
i .2 IT----1---+---�---_J
1 .0 r-�r-�����-1-��-::--::t;�Oll-�:;;ootl�===-��+-���_J
.80 1--�1--�����-��-+��+-���-l
SUMMARY
This paper presents evidence of adequate psychometric properties for the CPSP but
there is considerable room for reliabil ity improvement. The validity evidence in support of
the CPSP has been presented from many directions and indicates quite strongly that the
The underlying theory and early development and internal consistency testing of the
Basadur CPSP (Creative Problem Solving Profile) inventory is reviewed, then new expanded
rel iability and validity research is summarized. The four columns, the four quadrants, a nd
the two bi-pola r scales of the CPSP demonstrated satisfactory reliability via test-retest,
The CPSP correlated with the Kirton Adaptation I nventory (KAI) and the Myers-Briggs
Type I ndicator (MBTI) in predi cted ways. Also, the relative impact of creativity training was
moderated according to CPSP style in expected ways. Assessments of CPSP style by self
a lone and i n consensus with expert partner significantly agreed with the assessments made
o rganizational responsibility for strategic thinking by preferred CPSP style and preferred ratio
NOTE
1 . MBTI is a registered trademark of Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc., Palo Alto California
which publishes a users manual for the instrument.
REFERENCES
Ackoff, R. L. ( 1 9 79). The future of operational research is past. Journal of Operational Research
Society, 30, 9 3 - 1 04 .
Basad u r, M.S. ( 1 997) . Org anizational development interventions for enhancing creativity in the
workplace. Journal of Creative Behavior, 3 1 (1 ), 59-7 2 .
Basadur, M.S. ( 1 995). The Power of Innovation. London, UK: Pitman Professional Publishing .
Basadur, M.S. ( 1 994) . Managing the creative process in organizations. In Problem Finding, Problem
Solving, and Creativity. (Editor: Runco, M.J .), New York: Abl ex.
Basadur, M.S. ( 1 992) . Managing Creativity: A Japa nese model. Academy of Management
Executive, 6 (2), 29-42.
Basadur, M.S. ( l 988a). Improving union-management bargaining using a special process of applied
creative thinki ng . McMaster University Faculty of Business Research and Working Paper
Series, No. 300, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada, L8S $M4.
Basad ur, M.S. ( 1 988b) . The new creative thinking skills today's purchasing professionals must have
to be successful . The Cincinnati Purchaser, Cincinnati, OH, P.O. Box 39376, 45230-0376,
November/December.
Basad ur, M.S. ( 1 987) . In Frontiers of Creativity Research : Beyond the Basics. (Editor: Isaksen, S.G.,
Chapter 1 8.) Buffalo, N.Y. : Beerly.
Basadur, M.S. ( 1 983). Employee involvement creative problem-solving workshop. In Ford Education
a nd Training Catalog, Ford Education a nd Personnel Resea rch Department, Dearborn, Ml,
September, 1 1 5 .
Basadur, M.S. ( 1 982 ) . Research in creative problem solving training in business a n d industry.
Proceedings of Creativity Week 4 . Greensboro, NC: Center for Creative Leadership.
Basad ur, M.S. ( 1 979) . Training in creative problem solving : Effects on deferred judgment and
p roblem finding and solving in a n industria l research organization. Doctoral Dissertation,
University of Cincinnati, December.
Basadur, M.S. ( 1 97 4). Think or sink. The Deli berate Methods Change Bulletin, J uly-September,
Procter & Gamble Management Systems Division, Cincinnati, Ohio.
Basadur, M.S., Ellspermann, S.J ., a nd Evans, G.W. ( 1 994) . A new methodology for formulating ill
structured problems. OMEGA: The International Journal of Management Science, 2 2 , 627-
645 .
Basadur, M.S., & Finkbeiner, C.T. ( 1 985) . Measuring preference for ideation i n creative p roblem
solving training. Journal of Applied Behaviora l Science, 2 1 ( 1 ), 37-49.
Basadur, M.S., Graen, G.B., & Green, S.G. ( 1 982 ) . Training i n creative problem solving: Effects on
ideation and problem finding in a n a pplied research orga nization. Organizationa l Behavior
a nd Human Performance, 30, 4 1 -70.
Basad ur, M.S., Graen, G.B., & Scandura, T.A. ( 1 986) . Training effects on attitudes toward divergent
thinking a mong manufacturing engineers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 7 1 (4), 6 1 2 -6 1 7 .
Basadur, M.S., Graen, G . B., & Wakabayashi, M. ( 1 990). Identifying i ndividual differences i n creative
problem solving style. Journal of Creative Behavior, 24 (2) .
Basadur, M.S., Wakabayashi, M., & Graen, G.B. ( 1 990). Attitudes towards divergent thinking before
a nd after training : Focusing upon the effect of individ ua l problem solving styles. Creativity
Research Journa l, 3 (1 ) , 2 2-32 .
Carlsson, B., Keane, P., & Martin, J . B. ( 1 9 76). R&D organization as learning systems. Sloan
Management Review, Spring.
Cooper, R.G. ( 1 993). Winning at New Products: Accelerating the Process from Idea to Launch.
Reading, MA: Perseus Books.
Getzels, J .W. ( 1 9 75). Problem finding a nd the i nventiveness of solutions. The Journal of Creative
Behavior, 9 ( 1 ) .
Gordon, W.J .J. ( 1 9 7 1 ) The Metaphorical Way. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Porpoise Books.
.
Kirton, M.J . ( 1 987). KAI Manual. Occupational Research Centre, P.O. Box 1 09, Hatfield, Hertsford,
AL 1 0 9AB, U . K.
77
Kirton, M.J. ( 1 9 76) . Adaptors and innovators : A description and measure. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 6 1 , 622-629.
Leavitt, H.J. ( 1 9 75) . Beyond the analytic manager. California Management Review.
Livingston, l.S. ( 1 9 7 1 ) . Myth of the wel l -educated manager. Harvard Business Review, Jan-Feb, 79-
89.
McCrea, R.R., & Costa, P.T., J r. ( 1 989). Reinterpreting the Mye rs-Briggs Type I nventory from the
perspective of the five-factor model of personality. Journal of Personality, 57, 1 7-40.
Mott, P.E. ( 1 9 72 ) . The characteristics of effective org anizations. New York: Harper and Row.
Myers, l . B. ( 1 9 94). I ntroduction to Type. (5th ed ., European English Version.) Oxford : Oxford
Psychologists Press.
Myers, l . B . , & McCaulley, M.H. ( 1 985). Manual : A Guide to the Development and Use of the Myers
Briggs Type I ndicator (2"d ed.) . Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psycholog ists Press.
Myers, P.B., & Myers, K.D . ( 1 984) . MBTI Form G. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press, I nc.
Norton, J . ( 1 9 65) . The Dot a nd the Line (Film). Wilmett, IL: Fil ms Incorporated, 1 1 44 Wil mett Ave.
Osborn, A.F. ( 1 963). Applied I magination. New York, NY: Charles Scribner's Sons.
Parnes, S.J., Noller, R. B., & Biondi, A.M. ( 1 977). Guide to Creative Action. New York, NY: Charles
Scribner's Sons.
Runco, M.A. a nd Basadur, M.S. ( 1 993). Assessing ideational and evaluative skills a nd creative styles
a nd attitudes. Creativity a nd I nnovation Management, 2 (3), 1 66 - 1 73.
Senge, P., Kleiner, A., Roberts, C., Ross, R., & Smith, B. ( 1 994) . The Fifth Discipline Fieldbook. New
York: Doubleday.
Simon, H .A. ( 1 977). The New Science of Management Decisions. Englewood Cliffs, NJ : Prentice
Hal l .
Simon, H .A. ( 1 960) . The New Science o f Management Decision. Englewood Cliffs, NJ : Prentice
Ha l l .
Stricker, L.J . , & Ross, J . ( 1 964). A n assessment of some structural properties o f the Jungian
personality topology. Journal of Abnormal a nd Social Psychology, 68, 62-7 1 .
78
This paper has reported data from various participant samples. Following is a description
of each of these samples. The data were usually gathered in the field d u ri ng training,
Sample D escription
different organizations.
large industrial company, and professionals, lead hands and first-l ine
manufacturer.
79
engineers, first- line supervisors and technical specialists drawn from the
n = 40 This was a small group of partici pants representing a variety of job types
and organizations.
n = 1 639 This sample combi nes all the above samples (n =849) plus several
The items i n the columns for the correlational analyses in Appendices I through XX a re
n umbered as shown below.
x I T E
Column 1 Column 2 Column 3 Column 4
Column 1 (X)
I T E M
I tem 3 4 6 7 8 9 11 12 13 15 16 18
3 1. 0000
4 . 2894 1. 0000
6 . 0618 . 2834 1. 0000
7 . 0967 . 3204 . 5557 1. 0000
8 . 4843 . 19 6 9 . 13 69 . 1294 1. 0000
9 . 2927 . 1167 . 0600 . 0177 . 4549 1. 0000
11 . 1848 . 1153 . 14 9 2 . 0665 . 3489 . 3 17 1 1. 0000
12 . 07 7 6 . 2186 . 2865 . 1128 . 2205 . 2528 . 4489 1. 0000
13 . 4113 . 2 2 64 . 18 15 . 0858 . 5322 . 3773 . 3073 . 2281 1. 0000
15 . 3439 . 19 7 0 . 15 1 6 . 0169 . 5125 . 2905 . 3263 . 1627 . 4 099 1 . 0000
16 . 15 3 8 . 17 3 1 . 2547 . 15 3 0 . 2433 . 22 67 . 18 6 7 . 2 184 . 3 132 . 19 2 5 1 . 0000
18 . 0038 . 18 2 4 . 2 168 . 18 2 1 . 2250 . 2496 . 3 19 4 . 2479 . 14 6 7 . 2 2 05 . 1900 1 . 0000
co
--'
APPENDIX I I
Column 2 (I)
Non-Ipsat ive S c a l e , n= l 4 9
Item
I tem 21 22 24 25 26 27 29 30 31 33 34 36
21 1 . 0000
22 . 2254 1. 0000
24 . 17 3 4 . 2 8 57 1. 0000
25 . 2028 . 2625 . 4356 1. 0000
26 . 0966 . 19 7 0 . 4 050 . 5 0 12 1. 0000
27 . 14 8 9 . 3578 . 3968 . 4525 . 53 13 1. 0000
29 . 3500 . 0671 . 4351 . 2 2 65 . 3 6 07 . 3 105 1. 0000
30 . 2849 . 1610 . 2406 . 2280 . 18 8 9 . 2437 . 4051 1. 0000
31 . 0640 . 1677 . 17 3 2 . 2616 . 34 56 . 2371 . 2388 . 3474 1. 0000
33 . 3 155 . 3 3 98 . 3410 . 3 118 . 2 6 19 . 3228 . 3434 . 4290 . 1389 1 . 0000
34 . 2 07 5 . 2776 . 3 528 . 2989 . 3170 . 3 814 . 2950 . 2431 . 2 6 14 . 4831 1 . 0000
36 . 2564 . 2075 . 2454 . 18 4 7 . 17 5 9 . 09 16 . 2 658 . 2 187 . 12 19 . 2 113 . 174 1 1 . 0000
co
t-..)
APPENDIX I I I
Column 3 (T)
Item
I tem 39 40 42 43 44 45 47 48 49 51 52 54
39 1. 0000
40 . 2830 1. 0000
42 . 16 6 9 . 2898 1 . 0000
43 . 1808 . 2363 . 6753 1. 0000
44 . 2329 . 06 3 1 - . 1497 . 0067 1. 0000
45 . 1211 . 2 14 3 . 13 2 3 . 2469 . 4992 1. 0000
47 . 03 4 1 . 15 9 2 . 17 2 0 . 3 042 . 4455 . 5001 1. 0000
48 . 0986 . 2 3 04 . 2759 . 3877 . 08 19 . 2 155 . 2230 1. 0000
49 . 0069 . 19 4 7 . 16 2 6 . 3 14 3 . 08 0 2 . 0952 . 0975 . 2504 1. 0000
51 . 02 8 3 . 08 9 8 - . 0088 . 14 7 9 . 3 129 . 3528 . 2935 . 1259 . 2744 1 . 0000
52 . 1083 . 24 7 5 . 4 3 15 . 3834 . 0016 . 2737 . 19 7 5 . 3 57 0 . 3 4 50 . 2638 1 . 0000
54 . 1783 . 3209 . 2468 . 3 068 -. 0 2 18 . 1562 . 03 7 5 . 2439 . 1893 . 14 2 6 . 3825 1 . 0000
co
w
APPENDIX IV
Column 4 (E)
Non- Ipsat iv e s c al e , n= 1 4 9
I tem
Item 57 50 62 61 62 63 65 66 67 69 70 72
57 1 . 0000
58 . 3820 1 . 0000
60 . 0048 . 0668 1 . 0000
61 . 3328 . 5101 . 13 4 8 1 . 0000
62 . 3032 . 3 637 - . 0055 . 3800 1. 0000
63 . 2791 . 3 8 67 . 0104 . 2775 . 3945 1. 0000
65 . 2 6 17 . 2661 . 4513 . 2788 . 0948 . 19 17 1. 0000
66 . 3 140 . 4676 . 2753 . 4869 . 3206 . 3448 . 6064 1. 0000
67 . 3261 . 5111 . 0839 . 4598 . 3132 . 3258 . 3725 . 5 194 1. 0000
69 . 15 3 5 . 3 2 52 . 12 8 9 . 4051 . 2 09 1 . 14 15 . 1768 . 2971 . 27 19 1 . 0000
70 . 2975 . 4481 . 0651 . 4670 . 2 625 . 1534 . 14 5 0 . 4042 . 3896 . 14 7 2 1 . 0000
72 . 2989 . 3591 . 03 5 1 . 3477 . 2 63 1 . 4903 . 19 8 0 . 2863 . 3370 . 2 04 3 . 2 158 1. 0000
co
.i::...
APPEND I X V
Re l i abi l i ty Analys i s
Column 1 ( X)
Non- Ip s ative S c al e , n= l 4 9
Re l i ab i l ity C o e f f i c i en t s - 12 I tems
Alpha = . 7803
co
U'I
APPENDIX VI
Rel i ab i l i ty Analy s i s
Column 2 (I)
Al pha = . 8 174
OJ
°'
..
APPENDI X VI I
Re l i abi l i ty Analys i s
column 3 (T)
s c al e Scale Corrected
Mean vari ance I tem- squared Alpha
I f Item I f I tem Total Mul t i p l e I f I tem
I tem Del eted Del eted Correlation Corre l at i o n Del eted
A l ph a = . 7637
co
-...../
APPENDIX V I I I
Re l i abi l i ty Analys i s
Column 4 (E)
Alpha = . 82 65
co
co
•
APPENDIX I X
Column 1 (X)
Item
Item 3 4 6 7 8 9 11 12 13 15 16 18
3 1. 0000
4 . 3634 1. 0000
6 . 1026 . 3943 1. 0000
7 . 17 7 4 . 3 067 . 5665 1. 0000
8 . 50 3 3 . 3769 . 0145 . 1665 1. 0000
9 . 32 1 1 . 02 2 5 -. 0357 . 09 0 2 . 3 8 12 1. 0000
11 . 2329 . 2336 . 17 9 9 . 19 6 0 . 2615 . 3581 1. 0000
12 . 16 3 2 -. 0063 . 2301 . 2551 -. 0345 . 13 7 5 . 2 165 1. 0000
13 . 4708 . 13 6 1 -. 0384 . 1197 . 3762 . 3338 . 3 589 . 13 0 1 1. 0000
15 . 3830 . 2191 . 0159 . 18 4 4 . 4 104 . 2741 . 2 3 54 . 03 4 2 . 3595 1 . 0000
16 . 14 2 6 . 05 0 3 . 0027 . 2 186 . 19 2 2 . 1392 . 07 0 6 -. 0170 . 2881 . 2705 1 . 0000
18 . 157 6 . 13 0 6 . 3 187 . 2434 . 2 3 53 . 2904 . 3613 . 2480 . 17 6 4 . 1644 . 14 7 3 1 . 0000
co
'-0
APPENDI X X
Column 2 (I)
Item 21 22 24 25 26 27 29 30 31 33 34 36
21 1 . 0000
22 . 3058 1 . 0000
24 . 10 3 3 . 1687 1. 0000
25 . 1647 . 3232 . 3 6 14 1. 0000
26 . 2695 . 2 6 18 . 28 7 3 . 2961 1. 0000
27 . 2964 . 2 554 . 27 8 3 . 5083 . 4990 1. 0000
29 . 12 3 8 . 0 64 5 . 17 0 1 . 2 192 . 3985 . 3 109 1. 0000
30 . 17 6 4 . 07 2 7 . 17 1 6 . 2299 . 3 198 . 2 603 . 3 056 1. 0000
31 . 1132 . 12 7 0 . 18 5 6 . 2638 . 2 189 . 3 590 . 17 7 7 . 2940 1. 0000
33 . 3770 . 2638 . 3538 . 47 10 . 3 14 6 . 4 14 4 . 2 184 . 2497 . 4370 1 . 0000
34 . 2 3 17 . 1613 . 2569 . 2907 . 3701 . 3895 . 2 557 . 3229 . 1901 . 3460 1 . 0000
36 . 2650 . 1400 . 12 65 . 2 14 4 . 2236 . 1951 . 1560 . 0884 . 1285 . 2538 . 2299 1 . 0000
'-0
0
APPENDIX XI
Column 3 (T)
I tem
Item 39 40 42 43 44 45 47 48 49 51 52 54
39 1 . 0000
40 . 16 5 3 1 . 0000
42 . 1156 . 5699 1 . 0000
43 . 15 4 3 . 52 3 9 . 7077 1 . 0000
44 . 2 078 . 0602 - . 0377 . 1188 1. 0000
45 . 07 3 2 . 2 659 . 08 2 1 . 2225 . 4 2 57 1. 0000
47 . 18 6 1 . 4379 . 2022 . 3586 . 3022 . 4 567 1. 0000
48 . 15 8 5 . 3876 . 3209 . 3787 -. 0305 . 1674 . 2811 1. 0000
49 . 0000 . 3497 . 2704 . 3087 . 0000 . 2855 . 1206 . 2 3 59 1. 0000
51 . 0674 . 2680 . 0882 . 17 7 5 . 3244 . 3 127 . 2 19 0 . 3 165 . 2224 1 . 0000
52 . 2 097 . 5482 . 64 12 . 5488 -. 0561 . 0528 . 17 9 4 . 3439 . 27 3 9 . 1686 1 . 0000
54 . 0750 . 3528 . 43 2 0 . 4436 -. 1386 -. 0647 . 0976 . 3 8 16 . 0556 . 2 3 97 . 4 161 1 . 0000
'°
APPENDIX X I I
Column 4 (E)
I t em
Item 57 58 60 61 62 63 65 66 67 69 70 72
57 1. 0000
58 . 38 3 1 1 . 0000
60 . 16 3 9 - . 0150 1. 0000
61 . 3021 . 2752 -. 02 10 1. 0000
62 . 3020 . 17 10 -. 1 19 7 . 2 57 3 1 . 0000
63 . 107 3 . 1113 -. 0686 . 3 277 . 2372 1. 0000
65 . 3025 . 2666 . 4 163 . 1613 . 0371 -. 0648 1. 0000
66 . 2573 . 2989 . 1514 . 3 018 . 2 1 14 . 14 0 4 . 4306 1. 0000
67 . 2529 . 6098 -. 0810 . 29 04 . 2465 . 1571 . 2 3 62 . 2894 1. 0000
69 . 2293 . 2 7 17 . 06 5 1 . 3503 . .2 0 5 3 . 1678 . 2 085 . 2348 . 1661 1 . 0000
70 . 17 5 4 . 4 197 . 00 18 . 2990 . 12 5 2 . 14 5 0 . 2311 . 2837 . 4281 . 4265 1 . 0000
72 . 3 59 1 . 3 10 6 . 0887 . 3422 . 0296 . 3340 . 3 559 . 2383 . 4135 . 3 078 . 2821 1 . 0000
'°
"'
APPENDIX XI I I
Rel i ab i l i ty Analys i s
Column 1 (X)
A l pha = 0 . 7 6 2 5
'°
w
APPENDI X XIV
Co lumn 2 (I)
A l pha = 0 . 8027
-0
.i::...
•
APPEND I X XV
Re l i ab i l i ty Analy s i s
Co lumn 3 (T)
A l pha = 0 . 7950
'°
01
APPEND I X XVI
Re l i ab i l i ty Analy s i s
Column 4 (E)
Alpha = 0 . 7724
'°
0.
..
APPENDIX XVI I
Rel i ab i l i ty Analy s i s
B i -p o l ar S c a l e X-T
Alpha = 0 . 6178
'-0
'-.!
APPENDIX XVI I I
B i -p o l ar s c a l e I - E
Non - I p s at ive s c al e , n= l 4 9
A l pha = 0 . 7 3 6 6
'-0
co
" ...
APPENDIX X I X
Re l i abi l i ty Analys i s
Bi -po l ar s c a l e X-T
Alpha = . 7261
'-0
'-0
APPEND I X XX
Bi-po l ar s c a l e I - E
Alpha = . 7638
__,
0
0
..
..
MANAGEMENT OF INNOVATION AND NEW TECHNOLOGY
WORKING PAPER SERIES
1. R.G. Cooper and E.J. Kleinschmidt, "How the New Product Impacts on Success and Failure
in the Chemical Industry", February, 1 992.
2. R.G. Cooper and E.J. Kleinschmidt, "Major New Products: What Distinguishes the Winners
in the Chemical Industry", February, 1 992.
3. J. Miltenburg, " On the Equivalence of IlT and MRP as Technologies for Reducing Wastes
in Manufacturing, March, 1 992.
4. J.B. Kim, I . Krinsky and J. Lee, "Valuation of Initial Public Offerings: Evidence from
Korea", February, 1 992.
5. M . B asadur and S . Robinson, "The New Creative Thinking Skills Needed for Total Quality
Management to Become Fact, Not Just Philosophy" , April, 1 992.
7. A.R. Montazemi and K.M. Gupta, "Planning and Development of Information Systems
Towards Strategic Advantage of a Firm", April, 1 992.
8. A.R. Montazemi, "Reducing the Complexity o f MIS Innovation Through Hypermedia and
Expert Systems" , May, 1 992.
9. M . B asadur and Bruce Paton, "Creativity Boosts Profits i n Recessionary Times - Broadening
the P laying Field" , June, 1 992.
10. Robert G . Cooper and Elko Kleinschmidt, " Stage-Gate Systems for Product Innovation:
Rationale and Results", June, 1 992.
11. S .A.W. Drew, "The Strategic Management oflnnovation in the Financial Services Industry :
An Empirical Study" , July, 1 992.
12. M . Shehata and M.E. Ibrahim, "The Impact of Tax Policies o n Firms' R & D Spending
Behavior: The Case of R & D Tax Credit", July, 1 992.
13. Willi H . Wiesner, "Development Interview Technology: Implications for Innovative
Organizations", July, 1 992.
14. Isik U. Zeytinoglu, "Technological Innovation and the Creation of a New Type of
Employment: Telework", August, 1 992.
15. John W . Medcof, "An Integrated Model for Teaching the Management of Innovation in the
Introduction to Organizational Behaviour Course", October, 1 992.
1 6. Min Basadur, "The Why-What's Stopping Analysis: A New Methodology for Formulating
Ill-Structured Problems", October, 1 992.
1 8. Stephen A.W. Drew, "Innovation and Strategy in Financial Services", November, 1 992.
1 9. Scott Edgett, "New Product Development Practices for Retail Financial Services",
November, 1 992.
20. Robert G. Cooper and Elko J. Kleinschmidt, "New Product Winners and Losers: The
Relative Importance of Success Factors - Perception vs. Reality", November, 1 992.
21. Robert G. Cooper and Elko J. Kleinschmidt, " A New Product Success Factors Model : An �
Empirical Validation", November, 1 992.
22. Robert G. Cooper & Elko J. Kleinschmidt, " Stage Gate Systems: A Game Plan for New
Product Success", November, 1 992.
25. Yufei Yuan, "The Role of lnformation Technology in Business Innovation", July, 1 993 .
26. Isik Urla Zeytinoglu, "Innovation in Employment: A Telework Experiment in Ontario ", July,
1 993 .
27. John Miltenburg and David Sparling, "Managing and Reducing Total Cycle Time: Models
and Analysis", August, 1 993. •
28. R.G. Cooper, C.J. Easingwood, S . Edgett, E.J. Kleinschmidt and C. Storey, "What
Distinguishes the Top Performers in Financial Services", September, 1 993.
31. R.G. Cooper and E.J. Kleinschmidt, "Determinants of Time Efficiency in Product
Development", December, 1 993 .
32. Christopher K. Bart, "Back to the Future: Timeless Lessons for Organizational Success",
February, 1 994.
33. Ken R . Deal and Scott J . Edgett, "Determining Success Criteria for New Financial Products;
A Comparative Analysis of CART, Logit and Discriminant Analysis", February, 1 995.
34. Christopher K. Bart and Mark C. Baetz, "Does Mission Matter?" , February, 1 995.
36. Christopher K. Bart, "Is Fortune Magazine Right? An Investigation into the Application
of Deutschman's 1 6 High-Tech Management Practices", February, 1 995.
37. Christopher K. Bart, "The Impact of Mission on Firm Innovativeness", February, 1 995 .
39. R.G. Cooper and E.J. Kleinschmidt, "Benchmarking the Critical Success Factors of Firms'
New Product Development Programs", April, 1 995.
41. Robert C . Cooper & E.J. Kleinschmidt, "Benchmarking Firms' New Product Performance
& Practices", September, 1 995.
42. Min Basadur and Darryl Kirkland, "Training Effects on the Divergent Thinking Attitudes
of South American Managers" , November, 1 995.
43. Min Basadur, "Organizational Development Interventions for Enhancing Creativity in the
Workplace", November, 1 995.
44. Min Basadur, "Training Managerial Evaluative and Ideational Skills in Creative Problem
Solving: A Causal Model", December, 1 995.
45. Min Basadur, Pam Pringle and Simon Taggar, "Improving the Reliability of Three New
Scales Which Measure Three New Divergent Thinking Attitudes Related to Organizational
Creativity", December, 1 995.
•
46. N. P. Archer and F. Ghasemzadeh, "Proj ect Portfolio Selection Techniques : A Review and
a Suggested Integrated Approach", February, 1 996.
48. Christopher K. Bart, "Industrial Firms & the Power of Mission," April, 1 996.
51. Susan L . Kichuk and Willi H. Wiesner, "Personality and Team Performance: Implications
for Selecting Successful Product Design Teams," May, 1 996.
52. Susan L. Kichuk and Willi H. Wiesner, "Selection Measures for a Team Environment: The
Relationships among the Wonderlic Personnel Test, The Neo-FFI, and the Teamwork KSA
Test, " May, 1 996. '
53. Susan L. Kichuk and Willi H . Wiesner, "Personality, Performance, Satisfaction, and Potential
Longevity in Product Design Teams," June, 1 996.
54. John W. Medcof, "Learning, Positioning and Alliance Partner Selection," June, 1 996.
55. Scott J. Edgett, "The New Product Development Process fo r Commercial Financial
Services," July, 1 996.
56. Christopher K. Bart, "Sex, Lies & Mission Statements," September, 1 996.
57. Stuart Mestelman and Mohamed Shehata, "The Impact of Research and Development
Subsidies on the Employment of Research and Development Inputs," November, 1 996.
58. Mark C. Baetz and Christopher K . Bart, "Developing Mission Statements Which Work,"
�
November, 1 996.
59. Fereidoun Ghasemzadeh, Norm Archer and Paul Iyogun, "A Zero-One Model for Project
Portfolio Selection and Scheduling," December, 1 996.
60. R. G. Cooper, S . J. Edgett, E. J. Kleinschmidt, "Portfolio Management in New Product
Development: Lessons from Leading Firms," February 1 997.
62. C. K. Bart, "A Comparison of Mission Statements & Their Rationales in Innovative and
Non-Innovative Firms," February 1 997.
64. S. Taggar, "Intelligence, Personality, Creativity and Behaviour: The Antecedents of Superior
Team Performance," April 1 997.
65. R. Deaves and I. Krinsky, "New Tools for Investment Decision-Making: Real Options
Analysis," May 1 997.
66. J. W. Medcof (ed.), "Trends and Events in Selected High Technology Industries," May,
1 997. (On the WEB only)
69. John W. Medcof, "Research Intensity and the Identification ofHigh Technology Industries,"
September, 1 997.
70. Christopher K. Bart and John C. Tabone, "Mission Statements in the Not-for-profit Health
Care Sector: A State of the Art Review," September, 1 997.
71. Elko J. Kleinschmidt, "In-house and Partnership New Product Development in Austria: An
Empirical Analysis on Outcome and Explanatory Factors," October, 1 997.
72. Robert G. Cooper, Scott J. Edgett and Elko J. Kleinschmidt, "R&D Portfolio Management
Best Practices: Methods Used & Performance Results Achieved," January, 1 998.
73. Christopher K. Bart and Simon Taggar, "A Model of the Impact of Mission Rationale,
Content, Process and Alignment on Firm Performance," March, 1 99 8 .
74. Christopher K. Bart, John Parkinson and Simon Taggar, "The Implementation of Strategy:
Behavioural vs Budgetary Approaches and the Effect of Participation," March, 1 998.
75. John W. Medcof, "The Resource Based View and the New Competitive Landscape:
1
Characterizing Positions of Dynamic Capability," May, 1 998.
76. F. Ghasemzadeh and N. P. Archer, "Proj ect Portfolio Selection Through Decision Support,"
June, 1 998.
77. Y. Yuan, N. Archer, and R. Bassett, "The Impact of Electronic Commerce Innovations on
Marketing Management," June, 1 998.
78. Kenneth S . Chan, James Chowhan, Stuart Mestelman, Mohamed Shehata, "Value
Orientations and Income and Displacement Effects," July 1 998.
79. Min Basadur, Laurent Lapierre, "Predicting Creative Problem Solving Behaviors within
Teams," September, 1 998.
80. Min Basadur, "Simplex: Modelling the Phases and Stages ofthe Innovation Process in Open
System Organizations, October, 1 998.
81. Ken Deal, Ben Long and Bryan Scott, "New Pricing Product Design for Competitive
Advantage, November, 1 998.
82. Min Basadur, Mark A. Runco and Luis A. Vega, "Understanding How Creative Thinking
Skills, Attitudes and Behaviors Work Together in Real World Managerial Problem Solving,"
November, 1 998. )
innova/papers.irc
..
lllllH�I��lllll'if!�Hiii�II
3 9005 0245 5434 1
-� ��-- ������
� -----------i
l .r., --1 ., Or
t i 1n J f\. � !
1-\ 0
4S
_ Wlo�7
£f\ O . 33
�. ' .
. " . ., �- , -
.'
.
"' . '��l;
D. i
MCMASTER
• U N I V E R S I T Y•
MICHAEL G . D•GROOTE
SCHOOL OF BUSINESS
I N F O R M AT I O N . . .
- . f l -; � .
, (... .
/ , ·� - - F;
*Management of Innovation and New Technology Research Centre
(MINT) is an official mark of the
Michael G. DeGroote School of Business,
McMaster University.
� '
.
I-,
/ ;
(
•