Kasdan 2016 - DPM Socio-Cultural Factors of DRM

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

Disaster Prevention and Management

Considering socio-cultural factors of disaster risk management


David Oliver Kasdan
Article information:
To cite this document:
David Oliver Kasdan , (2016),"Considering socio-cultural factors of disaster risk management",
Disaster Prevention and Management, Vol. 25 Iss 4 pp. 464 - 477
Permanent link to this document:
http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/DPM-03-2016-0055
Downloaded on: 10 July 2016, At: 17:51 (PT)
Downloaded by Incheon National University At 17:51 10 July 2016 (PT)

References: this document contains references to 40 other documents.


To copy this document: [email protected]
The fulltext of this document has been downloaded 43 times since 2016*
Users who downloaded this article also downloaded:
(2016),"Community-based disaster communication: how does it become trustworthy?", Disaster
Prevention and Management: An International Journal, Vol. 25 Iss 4 pp. 478-491 http://
dx.doi.org/10.1108/DPM-02-2016-0026
(2016),"Getting ready for mega disasters: the role of past experience in changing disaster
consciousness", Disaster Prevention and Management: An International Journal, Vol. 25 Iss 4 pp.
492-505 http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/DPM-01-2016-0008
(2016),"Factors influencing responders’ perceptions of preparedness for terrorism", Disaster
Prevention and Management: An International Journal, Vol. 25 Iss 4 pp. 520-533 http://
dx.doi.org/10.1108/DPM-12-2015-0280

Access to this document was granted through an Emerald subscription provided by emerald-
srm:452836 []
For Authors
If you would like to write for this, or any other Emerald publication, then please use our Emerald
for Authors service information about how to choose which publication to write for and submission
guidelines are available for all. Please visit www.emeraldinsight.com/authors for more information.
About Emerald www.emeraldinsight.com
Emerald is a global publisher linking research and practice to the benefit of society. The company
manages a portfolio of more than 290 journals and over 2,350 books and book series volumes, as
well as providing an extensive range of online products and additional customer resources and
services.
Emerald is both COUNTER 4 and TRANSFER compliant. The organization is a partner of the
Committee on Publication Ethics (COPE) and also works with Portico and the LOCKSS initiative for
digital archive preservation.

*Related content and download information correct at time of download.


The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available on Emerald Insight at:
www.emeraldinsight.com/0965-3562.htm

DPM
25,4
Considering socio-cultural
factors of disaster risk
management
464 David Oliver Kasdan
Received 23 March 2016
Department of Public Administration, Incheon National University,
Revised 9 May 2016 Incheon, Republic of Korea
10 May 2016
Accepted 10 May 2016
Abstract
Purpose – The purpose of this paper is to explore the relationship between factors of socio-cultural
Downloaded by Incheon National University At 17:51 10 July 2016 (PT)

contexts and disaster risk. Recent efforts by international organizations and research scholarship have
emphasized that applying contextual understandings of human behavior can improve the
effectiveness of disaster risk management (DRM).
Design/methodology/approach – The research employs multiple correlation analysis to find
significant relationships between two sources of socio-cultural data and the World Risk Index scores.
Findings – There are interesting relationships between various measures of socio-cultural context and
disaster risk, such as correlations with levels of individualism, self-expression, and secular-rational values.
Research limitations/implications – While using the broadest sample available with the data
sources, generalizations about the relationships must be tempered as inherently anecdotal and needing
greater depth of study. The national level of analysis is controversial.
Practical implications – Emergency managers can extend the knowledge about socio-cultural
influences on disaster risk to tailor policy for effective practices.
Social implications – Societies may recognize their behaviors as being conducive or obstructive to
DRM based on their socio-cultural characteristics; governments may operationalize the findings into
policy responses for more nuanced mitigation efforts.
Originality/value – This research adds to the momentum for considering non-technical approaches
to DRM and expands the potential for social science derived variables in DRM.
Keywords Disaster management, Cultural behaviour, Emergency management theory,
Risk behaviour
Paper type Research paper

Introduction
The growth of disaster occurrences in contemporary societies is largely an artifact of
human activity, rather than an increase in the frequency of the phenomena themselves
(Oliver-Smith, 1996; Perrow, 1984/1999; Tierney, 2014). This prompts the need to
integrate social sciences into disaster risk management (DRM) to improve
understanding of the phenomena in societies, in contrast to the technological
approach that attempts to engineer DRM solutions apart from subjective realities. The
Sendai Framework for Disaster Risk Reduction 2015-2030 emphasizes that future
emergency management should integrate behavioral aspects, including the concepts of
understanding risk, governance, investing, and preparedness for disasters (UNISDR,
2015). This research considers two different ways of measuring socio-cultural contexts
and how they may inform better approaches to DRM governance: the World Values
Disaster Prevention and
Survey (WVS) and Dimensions of National Culture (DNC). This is not to say that
Management previous research has ignored cultural contexts – a notable recent entry is the World
Vol. 25 No. 4, 2016
pp. 464-477
© Emerald Group Publishing Limited
0965-3562
This research has been supported by the National Research Fund through Incheon National
DOI 10.1108/DPM-03-2016-0055 University, Republic of Korea.
Disasters Report: Focus on Culture and Risk (Cannon and Schipper, 2014) that “looks at Considering
different aspects of how culture affects disaster risk reduction (DRR) and how disasters socio-cultural
and risk influence culture” (p. 8) – but the mass of DRM research has come from natural
science perspectives. This research aims to add depth to social science concerns for
factors of
DRM in the hopes of building a more holistic (Quarantelli, 1979) and strategic (Choi, DRM
2008) approach that is adaptable to a variety of contexts.
Recently, DRM research has in fact begun exploring the influence of socio-cultural 465
variables on mitigation outcomes (Comfort, 2012a, b; IFRC, 2014; Tierney, 2014; Cannon
and Schipper, 2014). The variety of factors that are coming into play for DRM research
include a population’s resilience (Donahue et al., 2013; Norris et al., 2008), its level of
economic development (Kellenberg and Mobarak, 2008), and its patterns of behavior in
response to risk (Dillon et al., 2014; Kunreuther et al., 2013; Paton and Johnston, 2001).
Emerging theories have focussed on how to integrate the empirical lessons about
Downloaded by Incheon National University At 17:51 10 July 2016 (PT)

population behaviors into effective DRM policy (Donahue et al., 2013; Ha, 2015;
Vermeulen, 2014; Viscusi and Zeckhauser, 2006). There is also a growing field of work
about how government agencies can provide effective governance for disasters based
on organizational capacity and standard models of risk behavior (Shavell, 2014;
Smith et al., 2006; Wang and Kuo, 2014; Wilson and McCreight, 2012). Despite this
range and depth of research, the field has not offered a coherent analysis of how
socio-cultural variables can be used to improve DRM from an interdisciplinary
perspective. The interplay of disasters with a population’s politics, economics,
community, and administration is a complex calculus that cannot be served from
a single perspective.
This paper attempts to bring together the ideas about socio-cultural influences on
DRM. The expectation is an enhanced understanding of the importance of those
influences on DRM effectiveness in order to better tailor DRM to the context it serves.
This research discusses how those contexts can differ and what those differences mean
for organizations tasked with DRM. It is worth emphasizing that this work is
promoting an interdisciplinary approach to DRM in order to complement the
necessarily cross-cultural nature of the topic (Hofstede, 2001, p. 19). That is to say that
the comparative nature of the study demands that no single discipline can provide an
appropriately comprehensive frame of analysis.
The basic argument is that, given there may be such a thing as a measurable notion of
“national culture” (Hofstede, 2001; Inglehart, 1997) and that differences in a nation’s
disaster risk exist (Mucke et al., 2014), the ideal approach to DRM should recognize the
unique contexts of a society. Thus, there is a need to investigate the relationship between
social culture and disaster risk to find any patterns of association. The concepts
considered as part of a social culture include such things as a population’s social
attitudes, values, ideological foundations, acceptance of authority, risk orientation,
and overall development, as well as how they are manifest in behaviors and action.
This research raises some simple questions that have complex answers: Which societal
qualities are related to better DRM effectiveness? What human behaviors can be
exploited or confronted to improve DRM? Underlying these questions is the broader
challenges of figuring out what, if anything, can DRM do to integrate the findings for
improved practice?
To these interests, two significant and well-established socio-cultural measures are
analyzed and posed as potential factors of the DRM model. Hofstede’s (2001) (DNC) look
at several dichotomous-spectrum characteristics of a population to inform their relative
collective patterns of thinking, which “are reflected in the meaning people attach to
DPM various aspects of life and which become crystallized in the institutions of a society”
25,4 (http://geert-hofstede.com). WVS links people’s beliefs to their societal development,
using two dimensions of cross-cultural variation that are informed by a continuous
monitoring of myriad aspects by widespread surveys and evolving analyses (www.
worldvaluessurvey.org/). The two sources have some overlap, but together they
provide a broad-based characterization that can then be used to operationalize each
466 nation as a unit of analysis against other measures. In this case, the measures are
juxtaposed with the World Risk Report 2015 Index scores (WRI) that indicate the
exposure and vulnerability of a nation based on an array of demographic, economic,
political, and disaster data (www.worldriskreport.org/).
This is exploratory research as there has yet to be established an empirically
grounded theory of how any of these particular measures would be directly related to
disaster risk. This is partly due to the sporadic and somewhat infrequent occurrence of
Downloaded by Incheon National University At 17:51 10 July 2016 (PT)

disasters as a dependent variable (hence, the use of the WRI scores as a proxy), as well
as the limitations of sample size and the variation in type of measurement. If a theory
were advanced, then it would postulate that disaster risk is mitigated by societies that
are more cautious, educated, and wealthy. Yet this claim cannot account for the time
order of causality, nor is it particularly useful as a prescriptive idea. The objective
herein is merely to describe some of the socio-cultural characteristics of nations at risk
in order that the context can be accounted for in more effective DRM planning.
It is necessary to qualify the study being conducted at the national level of
measurement. The convenience and robustness of the data makes for reliability in the
analysis regardless of any assessments of global validity. This is not to excuse the fallacy
of summarily characterizing the socio-cultural contexts of large, heterogeneous nations
according to their political borders; it is certainly presumptuous to say that all people of a
certain country are wholly assimilated to a dimension or value. Nevertheless, there are
countries that exhibit strong homogeneity in such measures (e.g. Iceland, Japan, and
Korea). Yet it is also missing the point to dismiss such attempts without conceding that
there may be a starting place for such an approach to DRM policy, notwithstanding the
possibility that regional (sub-national) socio-cultural contexts can be studied. Therefore
this research does not make prescriptions according to national boundaries, opting
instead for a more conservative goal of finding associations between generalized
socio-cultural contexts and disaster risk for future application to DRM policy.

Literature review
The research literature on socio-political, cultural, and other “ancillary” factors of
disaster management is gaining prominence. The globalization effort of the UNISDR
and other agencies notwithstanding, there is still little scholarship that discusses the
relative effectiveness of policies between countries. The reasons for this shortfall start
with the acknowledgment that social and cultural elements are unique to each context
(and even by regions within nations), closely followed by the extension of those unique
elements into the unique processes that form DRM policies. As stated in the World
Disaster Report (IFRC, 2014, p. 8): “The one thing that is certain is that we will have less
sustained impact if we do not adequately take account of people’s cultures, beliefs, and
attitudes in relation to risk.”
This prompts the question of whether or not we can have a global perspective
without first compiling a systematic analysis of the various national or local (Torry,
1979) perspectives to DRM. Quarantelli (1979) initialized ideas for cross-cultural studies
in disaster response behaviors, but the small number of comparative studies that have
followed tend to focus on recovery efforts across different contexts (Kearns, 2011) with Considering
few exceptions. Another cluster of research was presented in a two-part series in the socio-cultural
Journal of Comparative Policy Analysis, wherein Comfort (2012a) recognized that,
“reducing the risk of losses in lives, property, and disruption from extreme events is a
factors of
global policy problem that can best be studied in comparative perspective” (p. 109) and DRM
noted that, “rapid technical, organizational, and cultural changes often intersect to
create novel patterns of risk” (Comfort, 2012b, p. 199). 467
Wisner (1978) made “an appeal for a significantly comparative method in disaster
research” that introduces the notion of alternative study frameworks for DRM. Torry
(1979) may be one of the original “interdisciplinary” DRM researchers, invoking an
anthropological consideration of DRM that was followed by a significant study by
Oliver-Smith (1996), among others. Quarantelli (1979) marks an early call for the study
of cross-cultural behavior in the context of disasters. These early works were mostly
theoretical propositions that DRM was not solely a “hard science” issue.
Downloaded by Incheon National University At 17:51 10 July 2016 (PT)

Perrow’s Normal Accidents (1984/1999) introduced the idea that socio-cultural


behaviors have significant influence on disaster risk and its management. His case
studies demonstrated that human action and reaction are integral to the incidence and
effects of disaster. Tierney’s (2014) study from the sociology perspective took the
notion further, laying significant blame for disasters on social practices. Between these
two texts, there exists a robust argument for understanding DRM as a socio-cultural
problem before trying to apply technological solutions. Their work can be bolstered by
a recent work by Ha (2015, p. 75) that dichotomized the “hardware” and “software” of
DRM: “The software-oriented approach supports social aspects of emergency
management. The position of emergency management is not neutral among
individual values or organizational values.”
Cutter et al. (2010) studied disaster resilience using local communities as the unit of
analysis. They focussed on a region in the USA and found variation was related to
urban/rural classification of communities, but they were unable to isolate the effects of
socio-cultural indicators on disaster resilience. The call for further research from their
work was to “suggest that the baseline indicators provide the first “broad brush” of the
patterns of disaster resilience within and between places and the underlying factors
contributing to it” (Cutter et al., 2010, p. 18). Their work utilized a number of measures
that are part of any socio-cultural study of a population, such as governance quality
and welfare characteristics within resilience components.
Looking at comparative social studies for disasters, Yoon (2012) took aim at the
methodologies of social vulnerability research. The research develops a framework for
assessing inductive and deductive indices, then advances a factor-analysis to produce
dimensions of social vulnerability that informs a study of property damages in coastal
counties of the USA. Yoon (2012, p. 840) concludes that social vulnerability is a
necessary part of DRM and that, “social vulnerability is often hidden and complex,
nested in various human aspects, and place-sensitive.”
Eiser et al. (2012) drilled down to individual perceptions of risk and action, finding
that behavioral factors and non-rational decision making in the context of disasters are
vital considerations. They reiterate that the ideal is often (wrongly) modeled from
instances where populations have perfect information and that DRM practices do not
adequately integrate variance in socio-cultural factors. Kunreuther (1996) and
Kunreuther et al. (2013), using a behavioral economics perspective, also focussed on
individual understandings of disaster risk and found that people have difficulty
judging the efficiency of mitigation efforts in DRM, often to their own disadvantage.
DPM The World Disasters Report (Cannon and Schipper, 2014) is subtitled, “Focus on
25,4 culture and risk,” suggesting that our understanding of DRM is moving from a
mechanistic approach to something more heuristic and contextually based. This
introduces a particular challenge for agencies and governments that must adapt
DRM models to fit circumstances, not only including considerations of the
quantifiable measures of development, but also considering culture and other
468 intangible qualities of a society. Other organizations have taken up the humanitarian
aspects of DRM and published reports that highlight the non-technical aspects to
mitigate risk. DARA’s reports and Risk Reduction Index (www.daraint.org) are a
solid effort toward operationalizing the idea behind the WRI, insofar as taking the
data to task for direct action in developing societies. The UNISDR publishes a
bi-annual Global Assessment Report with a specific focus on the development of its
global DRR initiative in each volume. The aforementioned Sendai Framework
Downloaded by Incheon National University At 17:51 10 July 2016 (PT)

(UNISDR, 2015, p. 5) integrated some of the interests and messages of the prior four
Global Assessment Reports in its call for “improved understanding of disaster risk in
all its dimensions.” The UNDP report (Pelling et al., 2004) emphasized DRM as a part
of modern development, rather than an afterthought to nations’ economic agendas.
This report also featured a “disaster risk index” that made connections between
characteristics of societies and their risk profiles.

Data and methodology


This section describes the data in this study and how the measures are connected to
concerns of DRM. For this study, the WRI is essentially a dependent variable and the
two other data sources are examined for their relationship to the WRI. There is a valid
objection to such an analysis being conducted with observations at the national level;
nobody would agree that the socio-cultural environment of, say, the Northeastern USA
is the same as that of the Southwestern region. Yet the preponderance of data collected
at this level and the reserved claims from the forthcoming analysis allow for some
latitude to say that the “labels” of a country based on the data sources is not meant to
serve as anything more than a proxy for a type of behavioral context that seems to be
located in that place. Thus, the conclusions drawn from the eventual analysis are not
intended to make specific claims about a country, but rather to offer some connections
between certain kinds of socio-cultural characteristics and effective DRM efforts.

World Risk Index (WRI)


The World Risk Report 2015 (Welle and Birkmann, 2015) is in a series of annual
publications that calculates the WRI from 28 indicators. The WRI is a compilation of
those indicators according to weighted values measured at the national level that yield
“dimensionless index values” (Welle and Birkmann, 2015, p. 42). The WRI is posited as
a measure of a country’s exposure and vulnerability to natural events, but it “is not a
forecasting model […] Rather, it demonstrates that risks in the context of natural
hazards and the potential effects of climate changes are not solely the result of extreme
natural events but that they are also determined by societal conditions” (Welle and
Birkmann, 2015, p. 42). The WRI score is akin to knowing the climate of a nation; a
country may have a tropical climate, but that does not mean we know whether it will
rain on any particular day next year. While the WRI is valuable to DRM, it is missing
some notable qualitative elements in its array of data. The WRI captures some of the
lived behavioral characteristics of societies, but not so directly the cultural dimensions.
As admitted by the WRI authors, several factors that would ideally be included in its Considering
calculation, such as social networks and adaptation strategies, do not have sufficient socio-cultural
global data available (Welle and Birkmann, 2015, pp. 42-43). The description of the
main components of the WRI are as follows (Welle and Birkmann, 2015, pp. 42-43):
factors of
DRM
Exposure means that an entity (population, built-up area, infrastructure component,
environmental area) is exposed to one or more natural hazards.
469
Susceptibility is understood here as the likelihood of experiencing harm in the event of a
natural hazard process. Thus, susceptibility describes structural characteristics and
framework conditions of a society.
Coping and coping capacities comprise various abilities of societies and exposed elements to
minimize negative impacts of natural hazards and climate change through direct action and
the resources available. Coping capacities encompass measures and abilities that are
Downloaded by Incheon National University At 17:51 10 July 2016 (PT)

immediately available to reduce harm and damages in the occurrence of an event.


Adaptation, unlike coping, is understood as a long-term process that also includes structural
changes and measures and strategies dealing with and attempting to address the negative
impacts of natural hazards and climate change in the long run.
There is an interesting facet to a sub-analysis of the WRI components: exposure has
the weakest correlations to the other components, but the strongest correlations to the
WRI score itself. This is an artifact of the WRI methodology, but it does suggest that
the non-human elements of disaster risk stand apart from the other three components
(see Table I).

WVS
The Worlds Values Survey project has a history dating to the early 1980s with a
conglomerate of international social scientists who have conducted hundreds of
thousands of interviews. Its purpose is, “to help scientists and policy makers
understand changes in the beliefs, values and motivations of people throughout the
world” for “the full range of global variations, from very poor to very rich countries, in
all of the world’s major cultural zones” (www.worldvaluessurvey.org). The version
used in this study is the sixth iteration for 2015 that plots countries on a coordinate
system according to the spectra of traditional vs secular-rational values and survival vs
self-expression values. The WVS observations were recorded as coordinate locations
on the Inglehart-Welzel Cultural Map as found on the website. The definitions of the
values are best quoted at length:
Traditional values emphasize the importance of religion, parent-child ties, deference to
authority and traditional family values. People who embrace these values also reject divorce,
abortion, euthanasia and suicide. These societies have high levels of national pride and a
nationalistic outlook.
Secular-rational values have the opposite preferences to the traditional values. These societies
place less emphasis on religion, traditional family values and authority. Divorce, abortion,
euthanasia and suicide are seen as relatively acceptable.
Survival values place emphasis on economic and physical security. It is linked with a relatively
ethnocentric outlook and low levels of trust and tolerance.
Self-expression values give high priority to environmental protection, growing tolerance of
foreigners, gays and lesbians and gender equality, and rising demands for participation in
decision-making in economic and political life.
Downloaded by Incheon National University At 17:51 10 July 2016 (PT)

25,4

470
DPM

Table I.
Multiple correlation
of all study variables
WRI components WVS DNC
A B C D E F G H I J K L M N

A. World Risk Index 2015 1.000


B. Exposition 0.919 1.000
C. Vulnerability 0.618 0.330 1.000
D. Susceptibility 0.672 0.415 0.926 1.000
E. Lack of coping capacities 0.574 0.290 0.975 0.857 1.000
F. Lack of adaptive capacities 0.567 0.291 0.955 0.860 0.886 1.000
G. Survival/self-expression −0.220 −0.097 −0.485 −0.338 −0.481 −0.527 1.000
H. Traditional/Secular-rational −0.431 −0.212 −0.693 −0.593 −0.742 −0.593 0.090 1.000
I. Power distance 0.452 0.284 0.629 0.497 0.646 0.610 −0.564 −0.448 1.000
J. Individualism −0.490 −0.324 −0.706 −0.589 −0.719 −0.670 0.520 0.537 −0.673 1.000
K. Masculinity 0.106 0.144 0.081 0.083 0.095 0.046 −0.016 −0.239 0.143 0.050 1.000
L. Uncertainty avoidance 0.002 0.040 −0.007 −0.031 0.000 0.001 −0.311 −0.169 0.300 −0.325 −0.011 1.000
M. Long-Term orientation −0.209 −0.090 −0.327 −0.309 −0.406 −0.173 −0.287 0.706 −0.025 0.198 −0.002 0.015 1.000
N. Indulgence 0.017 0.082 −0.169 −0.118 −0.105 −0.281 0.708 −0.341 −0.243 0.090 0.106 −0.106 −0.558 1.000
Inglehart’s (1997) long-term study of culture and change argues that it is possible Considering
to provide a current snapshot of societies relative to each other in terms of socio-cultural
development values. The “map” shows positions of countries according to their
values and allows the rough grouping of countries by geographic and dominant value
factors of
commonalities. While it does not purport to affix countries to labels for ready DRM
identification, the WVS is more than an anecdotal scheme for classifying nations.
In general, more developed nations tend to score higher for both secular-rational 471
and self-expression values.

Dimensions of national culture


Hofstede’s (2001) ambitious work not only deems to categorize and summarize national
cultures in a handful of measurable dimensions, but it does an admirable job of
reconciling the inherently elitist conceit of such a task with substantial empiricism and
Downloaded by Incheon National University At 17:51 10 July 2016 (PT)

objectivity. Stemming from his research into corporate cultures for multinational
companies in the 1960s, Hofstede has continued the project over five decades to develop
a comprehensive corpus that is accessible to a variety of purposes.
Hofstede’s work provides a relative indication of a nation’s culture also based on
spectra of dimensions. The latest iteration of the study has six dimensions (http://geert-
hofstede.com)[1]. Again, the definitions are best given by direct quote at length from
the website:

Power Distance Index: This dimension expresses the degree to which the less powerful
members of a society accept and expect that power is distributed unequally […] People in
societies exhibiting a large degree of power distance accept a hierarchical order in which
everybody has a place and which needs no further justification. In societies with low power
distance, people strive to equalize the distribution of power and demand justification for
inequalities of power.
Individualism vs Collectivism: The high side of this dimension, called individualism, can be
defined as a preference for a loosely knit social framework in which individuals are expected
to take care of only themselves and their immediate families. Its opposite, collectivism,
represents a preference for a tightly-knit framework in society in which individuals can
expect their relatives or members of a particular in-group to look after them in exchange for
unquestioning loyalty.
Masculinity vs Femininity: The Masculinity side of this dimension represents a preference in
society for achievement, heroism, assertiveness and material rewards for success. Society at
large is more competitive. Its opposite, femininity, stands for a preference for cooperation,
modesty, caring for the weak and quality of life. Society at large is more consensus oriented.
Uncertainty Avoidance Index: The uncertainty avoidance dimension expresses the degree to
which the members of a society feel uncomfortable with uncertainty and ambiguity. The
fundamental issue here is how a society deals with the fact that the future can never be known
[…] Countries exhibiting strong UAI maintain rigid codes of belief and behavior and are
intolerant of unorthodox behavior and ideas. Weak UAI societies maintain a more relaxed
attitude in which practice counts more than principles.
Long Term Orientation vs Short Term Normative Orientation: Every society has to maintain
some links with its own past while dealing with the challenges of the present and the future
[…] Societies who score low on this dimension prefer to maintain time-honored traditions and
norms while viewing societal change with suspicion. Those with a culture which scores high,
on the other hand, take a more pragmatic approach: they encourage thrift and efforts in
modern education as a way to prepare for the future.
DPM Indulgence vs Restraint: Indulgence stands for a society that allows relatively free gratification
of basic and natural human drives related to enjoying life and having fun. Restraint stands
25,4 for a society that suppresses gratification of needs and regulates it by means of strict
social norms.
The validity of Hofstede’s cultural dimensions is not without contention (e.g. Blodgett
et al., 2008; McSweeney, 2002); however, it does provide a framework for classifying
472 populations in the abstract and finding cultures that are alike or dissimilar. DNC is
positioned as a management tool; the theory of his work seeks to support
the effect of society’s culture on its values and how this is realized in its behavior
(Hofstede, 2001). For DNC, the general trend is that more developed nations will exhibit
lower power distance, masculinity, and uncertainty avoidance scores with higher
Individualism (long-term orientation and indulgence dimensions do not show any
trends with respect to development).
Downloaded by Incheon National University At 17:51 10 July 2016 (PT)

Putting it all together


It is tempting to try and build a model that extends the analysis of the independent
socio-cultural factor variables of DRM to predict the dependent variable value of the
WRI score. Such a model would necessarily include a control for the variation in the
disaster phenomena themselves, as well as demographic qualities that are not already
included in the other variables. This would dictate a wide array of independent
variables while having a limited sample of countries for which the information is
available. Variations of such a model were attempted, but there was no reliable and
statistically significant relationship to be distilled; this can be attributed to a number
of problems, the least of which is the small sample size. It is also conceivable that
statistical relationships could be examined individually for each of the two sources
(or select variables from each of the sources) against the WRI, but that leaves
problems pertaining to the variety of scoring methods involved in the levels of
measurement and scale. Parsing out all the elements of the indexes and factors to
rebuild the research model is also not possible, as the raw data are in some cases not
available and it would defeat the objective of using readily available analyses to
understand the relationship of intangible factors on material outcomes. In other
words, this research is neither designed nor amenable to predicting causal
relationships. The indices are considered at the macro level to illuminate relationships
for a holistic understanding of DRM effectiveness, rather than the more mechanistic
goals of DRM solutions engineering.
As it is, the analysis conducted here is a multiple correlation of the various
measures. The sample is restricted to the 62 nations that had complete data for all
measures[2]. The measures are all varieties of index scores that are neither consistent in
scale nor meaning. For example, a high score on one DNC value indicates a position on
a spectrum from one dominant characteristic to another, while a high WRI score
indicates that there are significant factors that would put that nation at risk on an
absolute basis. The data were cleaned and arranged to conduct statistical analysis.
A multiple correlation analysis (Table I) did reveal a number of strong relationships
that are discussed below. The correlations show that there may be duplication, if not
triangulation, of certain qualities between the measures which helps confirm the
reliability and validity of some measures. There is an implied hypothesis supported by
the wave of recent research in the socio-cultural considerations that these measures –
individually and collectively – have some import to DRM. The WRI’s relevance is
obvious, while there may be some question as to how “masculinity” is a consideration Considering
for mitigating earthquakes. Indeed, the forthcoming analysis will show where the socio-cultural
strongest relationships exist and whether the less pertinent variables’ values can be
redeemed in the bigger picture.
factors of
DRM
Analysis and discussion
There are two ways to analyze the data in this study. The first is to consider each of the 473
sources independently from each other and look for relationships to the WRI scores for
each country. In this approach, certain measures may show some relationship while others
do not. This is problematic insofar as the level of measurement generalizes both the
characteristics of a nation’s population and its disaster risk as being homogeneous
throughout its borders, i.e. it may lead to conclusions that ignore the reality of sub-national
diversity in behavior and vulnerability. The second way employs a holistic interpretation,
Downloaded by Incheon National University At 17:51 10 July 2016 (PT)

potentially illuminating how different measures contribute to understanding the socio-


cultural influences on DRM. Again, the objective is not to establish a time order and
determine causality of disaster risk, but rather to find extant variables in populations that
may be integrated into other models for greater DRM effectiveness.
Table I shows how the various measures relate to each other. One feature of the
analysis stands out: the significant correlations between almost all of the measures,
save three of the DNC. Of particular interest is how the relationships stack up against
the normative theory of how they should be and if there are any significant surprises
therein. With the definitions of the variable components given above, we would expect
the following kinds of relationships:
WRI versus WVS: those populations with higher Self-Expression and Secular-Rational values
should have lower scores for WRI while populations that exhibit more Traditional and
Survival oriented values will have higher WRI scores. This roughly corresponds to the trends
between WVS and development status, as various measures of development are contained
within the components of the WRI.
WRI versus DNC: populations that tend to have lower scores for Power Distance,
Individualism, Masculinity, Uncertainty Avoidance, and Indulgence dimensions with a higher
Long-Term Orientation score should exhibit lower WRI scores. These suppositions loosely
follow development levels, but are not as consistent as the WVS.
A study of the correlation matrix shows both confirmation and rejection of the
normative expectations. If the WRI is held as an efficient proxy for measuring the need
of DRM, then those populations that are characterized by Survival and Traditional
values (“low” WVS scores) will have higher WRI scores, as shown by the negative
correlation coefficients. This is in accordance with expectations, suggesting that
survival and traditional oriented cultural contexts may need greater DRM efforts.
Looking at the DNC components, greater power distance and lower Individualism
values (strong positive and strong negative correlation coefficients, respectively) are
associated with higher WRI scores. The relationship between Individualism and WRI is
unexpected; nations with a loosely-knit social framework actually correspond to lower
WRI scores. This finding could be investigated further with other techniques to better
understand the dynamics of social capital for DRM (Kapucu, 2008; Paton and Johnston,
2001; Tierney, 2014; Viscusi and Zeckhauser, 2006). Although the correlation coefficient
is not strong, the negative value for long-term orientation agrees with expectations
insofar as populations with a more strategic outlook should have lower WRI scores.
It is curious that the other DNC variables do not have significant correlations to WRI,
DPM especially as one would expect uncertainty avoidance to show some relationship
25,4 perceptions of disaster risk.
Putting the analysis together into a cautious summary, we can say that lower WRI
scores are correlated to greater self-expression, secular-rational, and individualism
values, as well as lower power distance values. A greater long-term orientation value
shows a weak relationship with lower WRI scores. While no single nation “wins” by
474 ticking off all the right values for the trigger variables and having a low WRI score,
among those with veritable threats of disaster (as seen by above average Exposition
values in the WRI component), Australia, New Zealand, and The Netherlands stand
out. The socio-cultural characteristics of those nations, as measured by the variables
with statistical significance, display correlations that tend toward lower WRI scores.
The value in applying WVS and DNC is that emergency managers can “play” to a
population’s interests more effectively to improve DRM outcomes. Trying to change a
Downloaded by Incheon National University At 17:51 10 July 2016 (PT)

nation’s culture may be an overwhelming task, but knowing the culture in order to tailor
DRM policy for a particular context can serve as an effective approach. A generalized
policy prescription would advocate more opportunities for citizen-government discourse
about disaster management centered on reinforcing individual responsibility and
communicating empirical understandings of disaster risk. A society with high
uncertainty avoidance might respond better to mitigation efforts, while a population that
displays survival values would benefit from guidance in aspects that emphasize personal
accountability. These lessons apply across disciplines; policy makers, civil engineers,
security forces, and other agencies can all benefit by knowing what kinds of DRM
strategies will be most necessary and effective. The direct connection between a
particular WVS or DNC score and any single DRM practice is far from evidentiary, but
that such a connection might be tested and tenable is surely attractive for future studies.

Conclusion
DRM is an especially pernicious challenge for governance; it must contend with the
disaster itself, the population at risk, and the government agencies tasked with protecting
the public from the disaster. A standard approach is to confront the unpredictable nature
of the disaster phenomena with rational understandings of the population at risk,
technological applications, and the attendant governance capacities. The behaviors of a
population, as affected by socio-cultural factors, can be difficult to ascertain. This leaves
DRM agencies to deal with two unknown values when it comes to the calculus for
effective mitigation: the disaster phenomena and the people. This research illuminates
some of the ways that a population’s behavioral context can vary according to its socio-
cultural character in an attempt to get a better handle on one of these unknowns.
Socio-cultural factors have material influence on a population’s relationship to
disasters, be they natural or man-made. There is a rabbit-hole of blame involved in this
situation, questioning the time order of behaviors and disasters (especially on the man-
made side) that is extremely difficult to ascertain due to the limited sample size for any
sort of causal relationship to be established through quantitative analysis. This has
been a continued limitation to the social sciences’ ability to gain a foothold in DRM
research, notwithstanding the many case studies available for disaster management
study. Nevertheless, some useful relationships are apparent between socio-cultural
aspects of populations and their disaster risk in the foregoing analysis. Accepting the
WRI as a viable and valid assessment of a nation’s exposure, susceptibility, coping
capacity, and adaptation qualities, the analysis shows relationships to underscore that
the socio-cultural context should be considered in DRM profiles. While it would be
convenient if the analysis were able to say something to the effect that, “Country X has Considering
these scores for DNC and WVS, therefore the most efficient and effective DRM socio-cultural
strategies and tactics will be […],” it is quite impossible to make such deductions.
The use of nation and country in this discussion is a convenience that does not suggest
factors of
that the findings can be bluntly applied in a diverse population; most countries are not DRM
uniform in their socio-cultural context.
Despite introducing data and referring to them as potential variables in the equation 475
of effective DRM, the intent of this research was to provide empirically founded
qualitative information about the variety of socio-cultural effects to DRM. More to the
practitioner’s perspective, the research asks what qualities and characteristics of a
society need to be accounted for in applying best practices of DRM? This research is a
brick in the construction of a bigger theory and practice of DRM as a socio-cultural,
technological, and governmental endeavor.
Downloaded by Incheon National University At 17:51 10 July 2016 (PT)

Further research related to this topic could investigate the differences in DRM based on
a nation’s degree of de-/centralized governance (Tsai and Chi, 2012), as it is related to the
population’s preferences for such level of government involvement. There is also more to
be understood at the broader level of governance vis-à-vis the Rule of Law (Western
constitutional) vs Rule of Man (Eastern institutional) approaches to handling DRM.
Another strain of research could build on a feature of one of the sources, such as Hofstede’s
dimension of individualism/collectivism and the literature on group-based risk aversion
characteristics, in order to hypothesize about a population’s understanding of disaster
vulnerability (WRI score) in light of its level of communalism and the individualism of
DRM policy and decision makers. And there is of course opportunity to test the general
idea at lower levels of measurement, comparing sub-national regions as such data may
become available. In other words, there is a wide array of interesting and potentially
educating study to be conducted on socio-cultural facets of DRM in comparative
population contexts. This research aimed to open that discourse by introducing just a few
interdisciplinary approaches to indicate that the relationship between populations and
effective DRM is rich, fascinating, and worthy of continued inquiry.

Notes
1. There is a relationship between the DNC and WVS through one of the latter’s primary
researchers, Michael Minkov. In a sense, the last two dimensions of are extensions based on
the WVS; the correlations between them are apparent in Table I.
2. All 62 nations included in the sample have values for WRI. Four countries – Bangladesh, El
Salvador, Italy, and Venezuela – did not have WVS6 2015 coordinates, but were plotted in
the WVS5 of 2008. Ecuador, Guatemala, and South Africa did not have DNC values for
“long-term orientation” and “indulgence”; the missing values were estimated from those of
comparable countries.

References
Blodgett, J., Bakir, A. and Rose, G. (2008), “A test of the validity of Hofstede’s cultural
framework”, Journal of Consumer Marketing, Vol. 22 No. 6, pp. 339-349.
Cannon, T. and Schipper, L. (Eds) (2014), World Disasters Report: Focus on Culture and Risk,
International Federation of Red Cross and Red Crescent Societies, Geneva.
Choi, S-O. (2008), “Emergency management: implications from a strategic management
perspective”, Journal of Homeland Security and Emergency Management, Vol. 5 No. 1,
pp. 1-21. doi: 10.2202/1547-7355.1372.
DPM Comfort, L. (2012a), “Designing disaster resilience and public policy: comparative perspectives”,
Journal of Comparative Policy Analysis: Research and Practice, Vol. 14 No. 2, pp. 109-113.
25,4
Comfort, L. (2012b), “Designing disaster resilience and public policy: comparative perspectives,
part II”, Journal of Comparative Policy Analysis: Research and Practice, Vol. 14 No. 3,
pp. 199-201.
Cutter, S., Burton, C. and Emrich, C. (2010), “Disaster resilience indicators for benchmarking
476 baseline conditions”, Journal of Homeland Security and Emergency Management, Vol. 7
No. 1, pp. 1-22. doi: 10.2202/1547-7355.1732.
Dillon, R., Tinsley, C. and Burns, W. (2014), “Near‐misses and future disaster preparedness”, Risk
Analysis, Vol. 34 No. 10, pp. 1907-1922.
Donahue, A., Eckel, C. and Wilson, R. (2013), “Ready or not? How citizens and public officials
perceive risk and preparedness”, American Review of Public Administration, Vol. 44 No. 4S,
pp. 89S-111S.
Downloaded by Incheon National University At 17:51 10 July 2016 (PT)

Eiser, J., Bostrom, A., Burton, I., Johnston, D., McClure, J., Paton, D., van der Pligt, J. and White, M.
(2012), “Risk interpretation and action: a conceptual framework for responses to natural
hazards”, International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction, Vol. 1, pp. 5-16.
Ha, K-M. (2015), “Moving from a hardware-oriented to a software-oriented approach in Korean
emergency management”, Environmental Hazards, Vol. 14 No. 1, pp. 74-85.
Hofstede, G. (2001), Culture’s Consequences: Comparing Values, Behaviors, Institutions and
Organizations Across Nations, 2nd ed., Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA.
IFRC (2014), World Disaster Report: Focus on Culture and Risk, International Federation of Red
Cross and Red Crescent Societies, Geneva.
Inglehart, R. (1997), Modernization and Postmodernization: Cultural, Economic, and Political
Change in 43 Societies, Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.
Kapucu, N. (2008), “Collaborative emergency management: better community organising, better
public preparedness and response”, Disasters, Vol. 32 No. 2, pp. 239-262.
Kearns, C. (2011), “Review of comparative emergency management: examining global and
regional responses to disasters”, Journal of Homeland Security and Emergency
Management, Vol. 8 No. 1, pp. 1-21. doi: 10.2202/1547-7355.1969.
Kellenberg, D. and Mobarak, A. (2008), “Does rising income increase or decrease damage risk
from natural disasters?”, Journal of Urban Economics, Vol. 63 No. 33, pp. 788-802.
Kunreuther, H. (1996), “Mitigating disaster losses through insurance”, Journal of Risk and
Uncertainty, Vol. 12 No. 2, pp. 171-187.
Kunreuther, H., Meyer, R. and Michel-Kerjan, E. (2013), “Overcoming decision biases to reduce
losses from natural catastrophes”, in Shafir, E. (Ed.), Behavioral Foundations of Policy,
Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ, pp. 398-413.
McSweeney, B. (2002), “Hofstede’s model of national cultural differences and their consequences:
a triumph of faith-a failure of analysis”, Human Relations, Vol. 55 No. 1, pp. 89-118.
Mucke, P., Jeschonnek, L., Garschagen, M., Schauber, A., Seibert, T., Welle, T. and Matuschke, I.
(2014), World Risk Report 2014: The City as a Risk Area, Entwicklung Hilft, Bündnis.
Norris, F., Stevens, S., Pfefferbaum, B., Wyche, K. and Pfefferbaum, R. (2008), “Community
resilience as a metaphor, theory, set of capacities, and strategy for disaster readiness”,
American Journal of Community Psychology, Vol. 41 Nos 1-2, pp. 127-150.
Oliver-Smith, A. (1996), “Anthropological research on hazards and disasters”, Annual Review of
Anthropology, Vol. 25, pp. 303-328.
Paton, D. and Johnston, D. (2001), “Disasters and communities: vulnerability, resilience and
preparedness”, Disaster Prevention and Management, Vol. 10 No. 4, pp. 270-277.
Pelling, M., Maskrey, A., Ruiz, P. and Hall, L. (Eds) (2004), Reducing Disaster Risk: A Challenge for
Development, United Nations Development Programme, New York, NY.
Perrow, C. (1984/1999), Normal Accidents, Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. Considering
Quarantelli, E. (1979), “Some needed cross-cultural studies of emergency time disaster behavior: socio-cultural
a first step”, Disasters, Vol. 3 No. 3, pp. 307-314.
factors of
Shavell, S. (2014), “A general rationale for a governmental role in the relief of large risks”, Journal
of Risk and Uncertainty, Vol. 49 No. 3, pp. 213-234. DRM
Smith, V., Carbone, J., Pope, J., Hallstrom, D. and Darden, M. (2006), “Adjusting to natural
disasters”, Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, Vol. 33 Nos 1-2, pp. 37-54. 477
Tierney, K. (2014), The Social Roots of Risk: Producing Disasters, Promoting Resilience, Stanford
University Press, Stanford, CA.
Torry, W.I. (1979), “Anthropology and disaster research”, Disasters, Vol. 3 No. 1, pp. 43-52.
Tsai, J-S. and Chi, C. (2012), “Cultural influence on the implementation of incident command
system for emergency management of natural disasters”, Journal of Homeland Security
and Emergency Management, Vol. 9 No. 1, pp. 1-22. doi: 10.1515/1547-7355.1970.
Downloaded by Incheon National University At 17:51 10 July 2016 (PT)

UNISDR (2015), Sendai Framework for Disaster Risk Reduction 2015-2030, United Nations
International Strategy for Disaster Reduction, Geneva.
Vermeulen, K. (2014), “Understanding your audience: How psychologists can help emergency
managers improve disaster warning compliance”, Journal of Homeland Security and
Emergency Management, Vol. 11 No. 3, pp. 309-315.
Viscusi, W. and Zeckhauser, R. (2006), “National survey evidence on disasters and relief:
Risk beliefs, self-interest, and compassion”, Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, Vol. 33
Nos 1-2, pp. 13-36.
Wang, C. and Kuo, M. (2014), “Strategic styles and organizational capability in crisis response in
local government”, Administration & Society, pp. 1-19. doi: 10.1177/0095399714544940.
Welle, T. and Birkmann, J. (2015), World Risk Report 2015, United Nations University, Bonn.
Wilson, L. and McCreight, R. (2012), “Public emergency laws and regulations: understanding
constraints and opportunities”, Journal of Homeland Security and Emergency Management,
Vol. 9 No. 2, pp. 1-16. doi: 10.1515/1547-7355.2034.
Wisner, B. (1978), “An appeal for a significantly comparative method in disaster research”,
Disasters, Vol. 2 No. 1, pp. 80-82.
Yoon, D. (2012), “Assessment of social vulnerability to natural disasters: a comparative study”,
Natural Hazards, Vol. 63 No. 2, pp. 823-843.

Further reading
UNISDR (2011), Global Assessment Report on Disaster Risk Reduction, United Nations
International Strategy for Disaster Reduction, Geneva.

About the author


David Oliver Kasdan is an Associate Professor in the Department of Public Administration at the
Incheon National University, Korea. His research interests concern behavioral economics,
disaster management, and public administration theory. His previous published work has
spanned from neopragmatism to emergency financial management, appearing in journals
such as Administration & Society and Journal of Urban Affairs. David Oliver Kasdan can be
contacted at: [email protected]

For instructions on how to order reprints of this article, please visit our website:
www.emeraldgrouppublishing.com/licensing/reprints.htm
Or contact us for further details: [email protected]

You might also like