Teaching English As A Foreign Language To Students With Learning Disabilities at The Intermediate and Advanced Levels: A Multiple-Strategies Approach

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 241

Teaching English as a Foreign Language to

Students with Learning Disabilities at the


Intermediate and Advanced Levels: A
Multiple-Strategies Approach

Abdel Salam A. El-Koumy


2016
Teaching English as a Foreign Language to
Students with Learning Disabilities at the
Intermediate and Advanced Levels: A
Multiple-Strategies Approach

Abdel Salam A. El-Koumy


Professor of Teaching English as a Foreign Language
Suez University, Egypt

Dar An-Nashr for Universities


2016
Published by Dar An-Nashr for Universities, Cairo, Egypt.

All rights reserved under Egyptian and international copyright laws. No


part of this publication may be reproduced, distributed, or transmitted in
any form or by any means, including photocopying, recording, or other
electronic or mechanical methods, without the prior written permission of
the author, except in the case of quotations permitted by copyright laws.
First published 2016
Deposit Number: 17117/2016
ISBN: 978-977-316-542-0

Cataloging-in-Publication Data:
El-Koumy, Abdel Salam A., 2016.
Teaching English as a Foreign Language to Students with Learning
Disabilities at the Intermediate and Advanced Levels: A Multiple-Strategies
Approach/Abdel Salam A. El-Koumy—1st ed. p. cm.
1. Teaching English as a Foreign Language—Oral Communication, Reading
Comprehension, Writing, A Multiple-Strategies Approach, etc.
2. Learning Strategies—Language Learning Strategies, Strategies for
Language Use, Multiple-Strategies Models, etc.
3. Learning Disabilities—What are Learning Disabilities? What Works for
Students with Learning Disabilities at the Intermediate and Advanced
Levels? etc.

Printed in Egypt
First Edition 2016
Cover Image: Retrieved from http://sites.psu.edu/rashidrcl/wp-
content/uploads/sites/15499/2015/04/dyslexia-learningdisabilities-27831317
.jpg
Table of Contents
Table of Contents iv

Overview vi

Chapter One: Learning Disabilities


1.0. Introduction 1
1.1. What are learning disabilities? 1

1.2. What works for students with learning disabilities at the


intermediate and advanced levels? 7

Chapter Two: Teaching Learning Strategies to


Students with Learning Disabilities
2.0. Introduction 10
2.1. Definition of learning strategies 13
2.2. Benefits of learning strategies 14
2.3. Classification of learning strategies 19
2.3.1. Cognitive strategies 19
2.3.2. Metacognitive strategies 20
2.3.2.1. Planning 22
2.3.2.2. Self-monitoring 23
2.3.2.3. Self-assessment 25
2.3.3. Social strategies 34
2.3.4. Affective strategies 35
2.4. Models of learning strategies instruction 39
2.5. Methods of identifying and assessing learning strategies 44
2.6. Research into effective/ ineffective learning strategies 47
2.7. Research on teaching learning strategies to students
with language/learning disabilities 51

Chapter Three: Teaching Communication


Strategies to Students with Oral Communication
Disabilities
3.0. Introduction 56
3.1. Definition of communication strategies 59
3.2. Classification of communication strategies 62
3.3. Benefits of communication strategies 73
3.4. A model for teaching communication strategies to
students with communication disabilities 77
3.5. Research on teaching communication strategies to students
with learning disabilities 86

Chapter Four: Teaching Reading Strategies to


Students with Comprehension Disabilities
4.0. Introduction 88
4.1. Definition of reading comprehension strategies 94
4.2. Classification of reading comprehension strategies 95
4.3. Reading comprehension strategies that work 97
4.4. Benefits of reading comprehension strategies 100
4.5. A model for teaching reading comprehension strategies
to students with comprehension disabilities 102
4.6. Directed reading-thinking activity 114
4.6.1. Definition of the directed reading-thinking activity 114

iv
4.6.2. Benefits of the directed reading-thinking activity 114
4.6.3. Procedures of the directed reading-thinking activity 115
4.6.4. Research on the directed reading-thinking activity
with learning-disabled students 117
4.7. Reciprocal teaching 119
4.7.1. Definition of reciprocal teaching 119
4.7.2. Reciprocal teaching strategies 120
4.7.3. Theoretical foundation of reciprocal teaching 123
4.7.4. Benefits of reciprocal teaching 124
4.7.5. Procedures of reciprocal teaching 125
4.7.6. Research on reciprocal teaching with students with
reading disabilities/difficulties 126
Chapter Five: Teaching Writing Strategies to
Students with Writing Disabilities
5.0. Introduction 135
5.1. Definition of writing strategies 141
5.2. Classification of writing strategies 142
4.3. Benefits of writing strategies 151
5.4. A model for teaching writing strategies to students
with writing disabilities 153
5.5. Research on teaching writing strategies to students
with writing disabilities 166

References 176

v
Overview
The idea of this book arose out of an awareness that students with
language learning disabilities are completely ignored in the
Egyptian school system and there are no special programs that
cater to these students. They are placed in normal schools that are
not prepared to deal with their unique difficulties. This book,
therefore, is an attempt to provide teachers with multiple-
strategies models for teaching English language skills to these
students at the intermediate level and beyond. More specifically,
this book will help pre-and in-service teachers to:
identify effective strategies for learning and using language
skills,
use multiple-strategies models for teaching language skills,
strategies for language learning and language use
into regular language activities, and finally
both the processes and products of language learning
of students with learning disabilities.
Thus, the target audience of this book includes pre-and in-
service regular teachers, special education teachers, school
psychologists, counselors, and administrators.

The Author

vi
Chapter One
Learning Disabilities

1.0. Introduction
This introductory chapter presents the definition of learning
disabilities in different countries. It also presents an overview of
the most effective intervention for students with learning
disabilities.

1.1. What are learning disabilities?


The term learning disabilities is defined in different ways in
different countries. In Australia, the term refers to a small sub-
group within the general area of learning difficulties. This sub-
group involves students who have difficulties in specific areas as a
result of impairment in one or more of the cognitive processes
related to learning. From the Australian perspective, these specific
areas of learning difficulties (known as learning disabilities) share
the following characteristics (Commonwealth of Australia 1992,
2005):

• are considered to be intrinsic to the individual,

• can cause a person to learn differently,

• are not linked to intellectual impairment (except incidentally),

• may coincidently exist with problems in self-regulatory


behaviors, social perception and social interaction,

• are life-long, and


• result in difficulty accessing the curriculum unless educational
adjustments appropriate to individual need are provided to
prevent failure.

In the American context a variety of definitions of the term


learning disabilities have been proposed. The American National
Joint Committee on Learning Disabilities (1994), for example,
defines this term in the following way:
Learning disabilities is a general term that refers to a
heterogeneous group of disorders manifested by
significant difficulties in the acquisition and use of
listening, speaking, reading, writing, reasoning, or
mathematical abilities. These disorders are intrinsic to the
individual, presumed to be due to central nervous system
dysfunction, and may occur across the life span. (p. 65)
Also in the USA, the Individuals with Disabilities Education
Act (IDEA) (2004, cited in Wright, 2005, p. 9) defines the term
specific learning disability as “a disorder in one or more of the
basic psychological processes involved in understanding or in
using language, spoken or written, that may manifest itself in the
imperfect ability to listen, think, speak, read, write, spell, or do
mathematical calculations”.
In Canada, the Learning Disabilities Association of Ontario
(2001) defines the term learning disabilities as follows:
Learning Disabilities refers to a variety of disorders that
affect the acquisition, retention, understanding,
organization or use of verbal and/or non-verbal
information. These disorders result from impairments in
one or more psychological processes related to learning,
in combination with otherwise average abilities essential
for thinking and reasoning. Learning disabilities are
specific not global impairments and as such are distinct
from intellectual disabilities. (p. 1)

2
Similarly, the Learning Disabilities Association of Alberta
(2010) defines the term learning disabilities as “a number of
disorders which may affect the acquisition, organization,
retention, understanding or use of verbal or nonverbal
information” (p. 3).
In the UK the term learning disabilities is used differently to
refer to what is known in Australia and many other countries as
intellectual disabilities which are out of the scope of this book;
whereas the term specific learning difficulties (SpLDs) is used to
refer to difficulties with certain aspects of learning. These SpLDs
include dyslexia, dyspraxia, dyscalculia and dysgraphia
(Department of Health, 2010).
Many neuropsychologists and psychiatrist associations (e. g.,
Cortiella and Horowitz, 2014; Learning Disabilities Association of
Alberta, 2010) agree that students with learning disabilities have
average or above average cognitive ability, but they have
neurologically-based disorders in one or more of the processes
related to information processing such as perceiving, storing,
remembering, retrieving, and communicating information. These
disorders manifest themselves in significant difficulties with
listening, speaking, reading, writing, reasoning, or mathematical
abilities. Specifically, they interfere with the acquisition and use of
one or more of the following language skills: (1) oral
communication (e.g., listening, speaking); (2) reading (e.g.,
decoding, comprehension); and (3) written language (e.g., spelling,
written expression) (National Dissemination Center for Children
with Disabilities, 2004). More specifically, research (e.g., Chalk,
Hagan-Burke, and Burke, 2005; Gerber, 1998; Graham, Schwartz,
and MacArthur, 1993) has shown that students with learning
disabilities often experience difficulty in the following areas:
• Comprehending what is read,
• Understanding what is said,
• Oral expression,

3
• Written expression,
• Generating ideas,
• Organizing ideas logically,
• Writing in stages,
• Understanding inferences, jokes or sarcasm,
• Planning, and decision-making (executive functions),
• Repairing breakdowns in interaction,
• Monitoring and self-evaluating performance,
• Identifying and recognizing strengths and weaknesses,
• Communicating thoughts and ideas,
• Expressing opinions, feelings, and ideas adequately on common
topics,
• Requesting and giving clarification,
• Repairing breakdowns while interacting with others.
However, as Gerber (1998) states, “Learning disabilities are
not a unitary construct. An individual can have one specific
problem or constellation of problems. Moreover, learning
disabilities do not manifest themselves in individuals in exactly the
same way. Some learning disabilities can be mild, while others can
be quite severe” (p. 9). Furthermore, the severity of learning
disabilities can influence many personal aspects including: (a) self-
esteem, (b) personal relations, (c) social interactions, and (d)
employment, as well as (e) educational pursuits (Comstock and
Kamara, 2002).

4
Some psychotherapists, learning disabilities associations, and
educators (e.g., Commonwealth of Australia, 1992, 2005; Gerber
and Reiff, 1994; Gerber, Schnieders, Paradise, Reiff, Ginsberg,
and Popp, 1990; Wilson and Lesaux, 2001) believe that learning
disabilities are long-lasting and that interventions only prevent
complications and help students to cope with their disabilities. In
contrast, some other psychotherapists, learning disabilities
associations, and educators (e. g., Graham and Harris, 2005;
Learning Disabilities Association of Alberta, 2010; Mishna, 1998)
believe that students with learning disabilities have the potential to
achieve at age-appropriate levels once provided with programs
that incorporate appropriate support and evidence-based
instruction because these disabilities have nothing to do with a
student’s intelligence. Mishna (1998), for example, believes that
students with learning disabilities can be very successful when
they are provided with strategies that support their learning. The
Learning Disabilities Association of Canada (1999) goes so far as
to say:
Adults with learning disabilities have average, above-
average, or even exceptional intelligence. They may be
highly artistic, musical, or gifted in a specific academic
area. Their general intellectual functioning is not
impaired and they are able to reason and make
judgments at least within the average range. In other
words, people with learning disabilities are not slow
learners. They just learn in a different way. They learn
inefficiently, due to inefficiencies in the functioning of the
brain. (p. 13)
The Learning Disabilities Association of Ontario (2001) also
believes that persons with learning disabilities can overcome their
learning disabilities and achieve academic success if they are
provided with specialized interventions, appropriate to their
individual strengths and needs. In the same vein, Nichols (2002)
states that all students with learning disabilities can acquire
literacy skills, provided that they are taught appropriately. She

5
maintains that these students “are able to participate in secondary
education successfully and graduate, provided that they are taught
the way that they learn best and that they are guaranteed access to
the accommodations which they have a right to have and without
which they may turn out to be unsuccessful” (p. 5). In addition, the
Learning Disabilities Association of America believes that every
person with learning disabilities can succeed in school, at work, in
relationships, and within the community when provided the right
supports and the right opportunities.
Over and above, the National Dissemination Center for
Children with Disabilities (2004) believes that children with
learning disabilities are not dumb or lazy and that they can learn
successfully with the right help. It states, “Children with learning
disabilities are not “dumb” or “lazy.” In fact, they usually have
average or above average intelligence. Their brains just process
information differently…. With the right help, children with LD
can and do learn successfully” (p. 2).
Moreover, the Learning Disabilities Association of Alberta
(2010) believes that with the right support and intervention, people
with learning disabilities can succeed in school, work, and life.
This association puts it simply in the following way:
Simply put, a person with a learning disability may be
just as intelligent, or even more intelligent, than most
people. However, certain skills or subjects pose
uncommon challenges. The important thing to remember
is that learning disabilities can cause people to learn
differently from others. (p. 3)
To date, research findings indicate that students classified as
having language learning disabilities could: (a) acquire FL skills,
(b) achieve at levels that match their peers in regular FL classes,
and (c) satisfy university FL requirements by participation in the
modified FL classes with proper instruction and accommodation
(Downey and Snyder, 2000; Sparks, Philips, and Javorsky, 2003).

6
To sum up, although there are various definitions for learning
disabilities, most of these definitions excluding the UK definition,
share these key elements: (1) Learning disabilities are a group of
neurological disorders in the information processes and these
disorders manifest themselves in significant difficulties with
listening, speaking, reading, writing, or mathematics; therefore,
the problems of students with learning disabilities lie with the
processing of information, not with intelligence; (2) Learning
disabilities are specific, not global impairments and as such are
distinct from intellectual disabilities; (3) Although learning
disabilities are not caused by environmental factors (e.g.,
insufficient/inappropriate instruction), such environmental factors
may contribute significantly to the negative impact of a learning
disability on a student’s life and make it worse; (4) Students with
learning disabilities have average to above average intelligence
and demonstrate at least average abilities essential for thinking
and reasoning; (5) With appropriate educational support, students
with learning disabilities can attain average or above average
achievement.

1.2. What works for students with learning


disabilities at the intermediate and advanced
levels?
There is no set recipe for teaching English as a foreign language to
students with learning disabilities at the intermediate and
advanced levels. However, research indicates that these students
do well with decoding but have difficulties with language
comprehension and production because they lack effective
cognitive and metacognitive strategies (Schoenbach, Greenleaf,
Cziko, and Hurwitz, 1999). In other words, these students often
possess a good collection of vocabulary and grammatical
structures, but they cannot comprehend oral or written language,

7
or express themselves orally and in writing because they lack
strategies for planning, setting priorities, monitoring, predicting
and self-assessment. Therefore, they more than their peers
without learning disabilities are in need of interventions that
explicitly teach strategies for language learning and language use
in conjunction with language skills. In support of this type of
intervention, Price and Cole (2009) suggest that “[e]ffective
instruction for students with learning disabilities is explicit and
intensive and combines direct instruction with strategy
instruction…. and responsive to the specific information
processing and learning needs of students” (p. 31). Likewise,
Fowler and Hunt (2004) assert that “[i]ndividuals with learning
disabilities have skills that make it possible for them to learn how
to use strategies and accommodations to help them pursue their
goals” (p. 30). In the same vein, Sturomski (1997) states that due to
the information processing difficulties that students with learning
disabilities often experience with learning, they more than their
peers without learning disabilities are in need of effective learning
strategies instruction. He states:

Because of the nature of their learning difficulties,


students with learning disabilities need to become
strategic learners, not just haphazardly using whatever
learning strategies or techniques they have developed on
their own, but becoming consciously aware of what
strategies might be useful in a given learning situation
and capable of using those strategies effectively. (p. 4)
In addition, the National Information Center for Children and
Youth with Disabilities (1997) expresses the need for teaching
students with learning disabilities to learn how to learn in the
following way:

8
Notwithstanding the difficulties that students with
learning disabilities often experience with learning, they
have the same need as their peers without disabilities to
acquire the knowledge, skills, and strategies both
academic and nonacademic that are necessary for
functioning independently on a day-to-day basis in our
society. Perhaps one of the most important skills they
need to learn is how to learn. Knowing that certain
techniques and strategies can be used to assist learning,
knowing which techniques are useful in which kinds of
learning situations, and knowing how to use the
techniques are powerful tools that can enable students to
become strategic, effective, and life long learners. (p. 3)
In support of strategy instruction as an intervention for
students with learning disabilities, many research studies in the
field of learning disabilities recommend this type of intervention.
In their review of research on learning disabilities and adult
literacy, Corley and Taymans (2002) conclude that research on
instructional variables positively associated with successful
learning for students with learning disabilities strongly support
combining direct instruction with strategy instruction. Many
meta-analytic reviews (e.g., Hughes, 1998; Swanson, 1999) also
suggest that a combination of both direct instruction and strategy
instruction for students with learning disabilities produce a larger
effect than either instructional method by itself.

It is thus clear that successful interventions for students who


have learning disabilities at the intermediate and advanced levels
emphasize helping students learn how to learn to enable them to
change their ineffective learning processes and to learn
independently throughout their entire lives. The focus of this book,
therefore, will be on helping students with learning disabilities
become more efficient and effective learners by equipping them
with a repertoire of strategies for language learning and language
performance.

9
Chapter Two
Teaching Learning Strategies to Students
with Learning Disabilities

2.0. Introduction
Students with learning disabilities are not aware of how their
minds work and fail to use strategies that represent the dynamic
processes underlying effective learning and academic
performance. In support of this, many neuropsychologists and
psychiatrist associations (e. g., Allsopp, Minskoff, and Bolt, 2005;
Cortiella and Horowitz, 2014; Learning Disabilities Association of
Alberta, 2010) agree that individuals with learning disabilities
have neurologically-based processing disorders which means that
their brains process information differently than most people.
These information processing disorders manifest themselves in
students’ failure to independently apply effective learning
processes and to monitor their own learning. More specifically,
these information processing disorders manifest themselves in
students’ failure to: (a) apply learning strategies, (b) orchestrate
among various strategies, and (c) change strategies when they
don't work, or make adaptations to them when necessary. These
failures in turn interfere with acquisition and use of one or more
of the following language skills: (1) oral communication, (2)
reading comprehension, and (3) written expression (e.g., Fowler,
2003; Learning Disabilities Association of Canada, 2005; National
Dissemination Center for Children with Disabilities, 2004; Reid,
Lienemann, and Hagaman, 2013; Torgesen and Kail, 1980; Wong,
2000). The information processing disorders can also lead to
frustration, disappointment, low self-esteem and withdrawal from
school (Fiedorowicz, Benezra, MacDonald, McElgunn, Wilson,
and Kaplan, 2001).
In simple words, having a learning disability means that the
brain does not process information normally. This, of course,
requires modeling effective cognitive processes through learning
strategies instruction to help students with learning disabilities
change their ineffective learning processes and employ effective
ones in a reflective, purposeful way. In support of this solution,
Neil Sturomski (1997), the former director of the National Adult
Literacy and Learning Disabilities Center, who has more than
thirty-five years of experience related to individuals with learning
disabilities and other special learning needs, states that students
with learning disabilities can learn strategies, which can in turn
improve their language skills. He further explains:
Because of the nature of their learning difficulties,
students with learning disabilities need to become
strategic learners, not just haphazardly using whatever
learning strategies or techniques they have developed on
their own, but becoming consciously aware of what
strategies might be useful in a given learning situation
and capable of using those strategies effectively. Teachers
can be enormously helpful in this regard. They can
introduce students to specific strategies and demonstrate
when and how the strategies are used. Students can then
see how a person thinks or what a person does when using
the strategies. Teachers can provide opportunities for
students to discuss, reflect upon, and practice the
strategies with classroom materials and authentic tasks.
By giving feedback, teachers help students refine their use
of strategies and learn to monitor their own usage.
Teachers may then gradually fade reminders and
guidance so that students begin to assume responsibility
for strategic learning. (p. 3)
Mothus and Lapadat (2006) also refer to learning strategies
instruction as a solution to the processing difficulties experienced
by students with learning disabilities in the following way:

11
The Strategies Intervention Model (SIM), developed by
researchers at the University of Kansas, is based on the
theory that students with LD [Learning Disabilities] have
information processing difficulties, are strategy deficient,
and are inactive learners. That is, they do not create or
use appropriate cognitive and metacognitive strategies
spontaneously to process information, to cope with
problems they encounter, or to learn new material (Alley
& Deshler, 1979; Bender, 1995; Clark, 1993; Deshler,
Schumaker, Lenz, & Ellis, 1984; Ellis, Deshler, &
Schumaker, 1989; Shaw et al., 1995; Palincsar & Brown,
1987; Torgesen, 1988a, 1988b). (p. 14)
To help students with language learning disabilities overcome
their own learning difficulties, instruction should take as its aim
the improvement of the underlying processes and strategies these
students depend upon to learn language skills as these skills are
rooted in complex processes. In support of learning strategies
instruction as an intervention for improving language skills, many
studies have shown that: (a) learners' awareness of their own
learning processes plays a significant role in improving language
performance (e.g., Baker and Brown, 1984; Bereiter and Bird,
1985); (b) greater strategy use is related to better language
learning and good language learners apply multiple strategies
more frequently and more effectively than poor language learners
(e.g., Kaufman, Randlett, and Price, 1985; Lau, 2006; Paris,
Lipson, and Wixson, 1983); and (c) struggling language learners
have difficulty in using learning strategies (e.g., Brown and
Palincsar, 1982; Chan and Lan, 2003). Therefore, the present
chapter focuses on learning strategies in general and language
learning strategies in particular to help teachers become more
aware of the various strategies that they can use to enable students
to learn independently and effectively. More specifically, this
chapter deals with the definition of learning strategies and
discusses the benefits and types of these strategies. It also
addresses the most-widely used models of learning
strategies instruction. Then, it discusses the methods of

12
identifying and assessing these strategies. Finally, it reviews
research into effective/ineffective learning strategies as well as
research on teaching learning strategies to students with learning
disabilities.

2.1. Definition of learning strategies


Definitions of learning strategies are many. According to Chamot
(1987), “Learning strategies are techniques, approaches, or
deliberate actions that students take in order to facilitate the
learning and recall of both linguistic and content area
information” (p. 71). For Wenden and Rubin (1987) learning
strategies are "any sets of operations, steps, plans, routines used
by the learner to facilitate the obtaining, storage, and use of
information" (p. 19). Along the same line, Schmeck (1988) defined
the term learning strategy in relation to learning tactics in this
way:
The term strategy was originally a military term that
referred to procedures for implementing the plan of a
large scale military operation. The more specific steps in
implementation of the plan were called tactics. More
generally the term strategy has come to refer to the
implementation of a set of procedures (tactics) for
accomplishing something. Thus a learning strategy is a
sequence of procedures for accomplishing learning and
the specific procedures within the sequence are called
learning tactics. (p. 5)
In a similar way, learning strategies were defined by O'Malley
and Chamot (1990) as "special thoughts or behaviors that
individuals use to comprehend, learn, or retain new information"
(p. 1). In Wenden’s (1991a) view, "Learning strategies are mental
steps or operations that learners use to learn a new language and
to regulate their efforts to do so" (p. 18). In addition, Cohen (1998)
defined language learning strategies as conscious behaviors used to

13
learn the language. In the Concise Encyclopedia of Educational
Linguistics, Oxford (1999) defined learning strategies for second
or foreign language as “specific actions, behaviors, steps, or
techniques that students use to improve their own progress in
developing skills in a second or foreign language" (p. 518). In the
same vein, Weisnstein, Husman, and Dierking (2000) defined
learning strategies as “any thoughts, behaviors, beliefs, or
emotions that facilitate the acquisition, understanding, or later
transfer of new knowledge and skills” (p. 727). Furthermore,
Anderson (2005) defined learning strategies as "the conscious
actions that learners take to improve their language learning” (p.
757). By the same token, Chamot (2005) defined learning strategies
as "procedures that facilitate a learning task" (p. 112).
As indicated above, although language learning scholars
define learning strategies differently, there are a number of basic
characteristics accepted by all of them. Oxford (1990, p. 9) lists
these basic characteristics as follows:
• They allow learners to become more self-directed;
• They are specific actions taken by the learner;
• They involve many aspects of the learner, not just the cognitive;
• They support learning both directly and indirectly;
• They are not always observable;
• They are often conscious;
• They can be taught;
• They are flexible;
• They are influenced by a variety of factors.

4.2. Benefits of learning strategies


Learning strategies are a means of enhancing successful learning.
As Oxford (1990) states, learning strategies “make learning easier,

14
faster, more enjoyable, more self-directed, more effective, and
more transferable to new situations” (p. 8). Rubin (1996) also
believes that strategy instruction is a means of enhancing learners'
procedural knowledge, which leads to more successful learning.
She states:
Strategy instruction is one way to work towards
enhancing your procedural knowledge. Since many adults
are "language phobic" or inexperienced with language
learning, they need to gain more procedural knowledge to
deflect negative affective influences and to begin to
experience some success. (p. 151)
Specifically, the use of strategies for language learning is a
fundamental requirement for successful learning. They contribute
to the development of comprehension and production of the target
language. In this respect, Oxford (1990) states that learning
strategies are "especially important for language learning because
they are tools for active, self-directed movement, which is essential
for developing communicative competence" (p. 1). According to
Long and Crookes (1992), learning strategies instruction "clearly
improves rate of learning" and "probably improves the ultimate
level of SL (second language) attainment" (p. 42). In the Concise
Encyclopedia of Educational Linguistics (1999), Oxford adds that
"language learning strategies can facilitate the internalization,
storage, retrieval, or use of the new language” (p. 518). In support
of this, researchers found that skilled language learners are
masters of learning strategies (Bereiter and Scardamalia, 1987),
and that a positive correlation exists between strategy use and
second language proficiency (Oxford, Cho, Leung, and Kim,
2004). The literature on metacognition also suggests that the use
of metacognitive strategies positively influences learners'
academic performance (Pintrich, 1994, 2002; Pintrich and
Schunk, 1996).

15
Learning strategies are also seen as a means of enhancing self-
efficacy, motivation and self-confidence of learners. In this
respect, Schunk (1989) argues that strategy instruction can
positively influence students’ self-efficacy, which can in turn lower
their level of anxiety. In the same vein, Chamot, Barnhardt, El-
Dinary, and Robbins (1996) argue that access to appropriate
strategies leads to students gaining a higher expectation of
learning success, which is central to motivation. That is, strategic
learners perceive themselves as more able to succeed academically
than students who do not know how to use strategies effectively,
which in turn increases their motivation. Likewise, Oxford,
Crookall, Cohen, Lavine, Nyikos, and Sutter (1990) believe that
"strategy training can enhance both the process of language
learning (the strategies or behaviors learners use and the affective
elements involved) and the product of language learning (changes
in students' language performance)" (p. 210).
Besides, learning strategies instruction is an important factor
for developing independent learning as it helps learners discover
what particular strategy works for them in a particular situation
and develops their control over their own learning The more
students become aware of the their own learning strategies, the
greater the control they develop over their own learning. In this
regard, Wenden (1986) says, “[T]o be self-sufficient, learners must
know how to learn” (p. 315). Along the same line, Cohen (1998)
argues that strategy instruction helps learners explore ways in
which they can learn the language more effectively. He further
emphasizes the significant role that strategy instruction plays in
developing learners' independence in the following way:
The strategy training movement is predicated on the
assumption that if learners are conscious about and
become responsible for the selection, use, and evaluation
of their learning strategies, they will become more
successful language learners by [...] taking more
responsibility for their own language learning, and
enhancing their use of the target language out of class. In

16
other words, the ultimate goal of strategy training is to
empower students by allowing them to take control of the
language learning process. (p. 70)
Research has also shown that self-regulated learners engage in
the use of both cognitive and metacognitive strategies for learning
and that students who use effective strategies are better able to
work outside the classroom, where teacher direction and teacher
input are not present, because learning strategies enable them to
become more independent, lifelong learners (Pintrich and De
Groot, 1990).
Moreover, learning strategies enable students to stretch their
own learning styles. In this respect. Oxford (2003) states that
teachers can actively help students “stretch” their learning styles
by trying out some strategies that are outside of their primary
style preferences. She adds that this can happen through strategy
instruction.
Furthermore, learning strategies instruction holds a
significant benefit to students with learning disabilities, because
these students often lack effective learning strategies. In this
regard, Beckman (2002) points out that when students with
learning disabilities become strategic learners, they become
productive lifelong learners, and as a result of strategy use, they
trust their own minds, know that there's more than one right way
to do things, acknowledge their mistakes, evaluate their products
and behavior, feel a sense of power, and know how to try.
Protheroe and Clarke (2008) also assert the importance of
teaching students with learning disabilities to use learning
strategies in the following way:
An increasingly strong research base points to the
potential of strategy instruction to help support struggling
learners, including students with learning disabilities.
Specifically, teaching students how to use learning

17
strategies, and helping them choose and implement them
effectively, helps [sic] to strengthen their metacognitive
abilities—and this, in turn, connects to improved student
learning. (p. 34)
In support of the benefits of learning strategies instruction for
students with learning disabilities, Proctor, August, Carlo, and
Snow (2006) found that these students scored lower on the
measures of learning strategy use than did their non-disabled
peers as a result of comparing the learning strategies used by 79
postsecondary students with disabilities to those used by 139
students without disabilities. Vann and Abraham (1990) also
found evidence that unsuccessful learners
"apparently...lacked...what are often called metacognitive
strategies...which would enable them to assess the task and bring
to bear the necessary strategies for its completion" (p. 192).
Likewise, Vandergrift (2003) found that the more-skilled listeners
used more meta-cognitive strategies over time than the less-skilled
listeners. Therefore, identifying the cognitive and metacognitive
strategies successful learners use makes it possible to help
unsuccessful language learners to become more successful,
through the deliberate teaching of these strategies. This deliberate
teaching can benefit students with learning disabilities in
particular because it will help them to become more aware of
their thinking processes, to recognize when meaning breaks down,
and to understand what strategies work best for them. It has also
been suggested that strategy instruction can help learners with
disabilities to overcome certain psycholinguistic and affective
constraints in the classroom. As Nyikos (1996) states, strategy
instruction "helps overcome nervousness, the inability to
remember and the need to immediately produce language during
oral communication. Being able to overcome these limitations will
obviously make learning more efficient" (p. 112). In short,
learning strategies instruction benefits all students, including
those with learning disabilities. Learning strategy research also

18
suggests that less competent students improve their skills through
training in strategies used by more successful learners. Therefore,
many educators propose that learning strategy instruction should
be integrated into regular courses.

2.3. Classification of language learning


strategies
There are many taxonomies of language learning strategies (e.g.,
Chamot and Küpper, 1989; O’Malley, Chamot, Stewner-
Manzanares, Küpper, and Russo, 1985). However, most of these
taxonomies reflect more or less the same categories of language
learning strategies. In an attempt to bring many of the
categorizations of learning strategies together, Dornyei (2006)
proposes a taxonomy which consists of four categories: Cognitive
strategies, metacognitive strategies, social strategies and affective
strategies. These four categories are the next topics of discussion.

2.3.1. Cognitive strategies


Cognitive psychologists generally agree that cognitive strategies
are behaviors, techniques, or actions used by learners to "operate
directly on incoming information, manipulating it in ways that
enhance learning" (O’Malley and Chamot, 1990, p. 44). These
strategies are closely linked to language skills. More specifically,
there are clusters of learning strategies for each language skill. For
example, the reading comprehension strategies that have
consistently been advocated as playing an important part in the
reading comprehension include: making predictions about the
likely content of a text, clarifying, guessing, questioning, and
summarizing. The choice from these cognitive strategies depends
on the demands of the reading task, among many other factors.
Over and above, the learner should employ a variety of strategies
to comprehend what s/he reads on condition that these strategies
should support and complete each other. In support of this,
research suggests that teaching a repertoire of strategies enhances

19
the performance of academic tasks (Dole, Nokes, and Drits, 2009;
Duke, Pearson, Strachan, and Billman, 2011; McNamara and
Magliano, 2009) and is more effective than individual strategy
instruction (e.g., Duke and Pearson, 2002; Pressley and
Afflerbach, 1995).
Cognitive strategies are very important for all students. In
general, these strategies enable students to learn better because
they help them process (organize, understand, retain and retrieve)
the information they are actually learning. In addition, the use of
these strategies enables students to perform efficiently on learning
tasks as they help them “develop the necessary skills to be self-
regulated learners, to facilitate comprehension, to act directly on
incoming information, and ultimately improve academic
performance” (Khoshsima and Tiyar, 2014, p. 90). Meltzer and
Krishnan (2007) also assert that “effective cognitive strategies help
students bridge the gap between their weak executive function
skills and the academic demands they face” (p. 88). Moreover,
cognitive strategies instruction is one of the most effective ways of
improving the academic performance of students with learning
disabilities. Many scholars and researchers agree that teaching
those students the very cognitive strategies used by successful
students should be the key focus of their interventions. In essence,
cognitive strategies can help students with and without learning
difficulties learn better and enable them to become independent
learners.

2.3.2. Metacognitive strategies


Metacognition includes awareness of one’s own cognitive processes
and one’s own affective processes. As Williams and Burden (1997)
put it:

20
Metacognition . . . includes not only a knowledge of
mental processes, as these are necessarily linked to and
affected by emotions and feelings. It must also encompass
a knowledge of factors relating to the self, and the way in
which these affect the use of cognitive processes. Thus, an
awareness of one’s personality, feelings, motivation,
attitudes and learning style at any particular moment
would be included within such a concept of metacognitive
awareness. (p. 155)
Moreover, metacognition includes not only the conscious
awareness of one’s own cognitive and affective processes, but also
the management of one's own learning through the use of
metacognitive strategies. These strategies involve “planning for
learning, […] self-monitoring during learning and evaluation of
how successful learning has been after working on language in
some way” (Hedge, 2000, p. 78). While these strategies (i.e.,
planning, self-monitoring, and self-assessment) are distinct, they
are also interdependent because (1) planning informs and
promotes self-monitoring; (2) self-monitoring helps students attain
learning goals; and (3) self-assessment enhances students’
motivation to set new goals (Schunk, 1994).
Metacognitive strategies are very important for language
learners because they help them select, monitor and regulate
cognitive strategies. This in turn assists them in becoming more
responsible for their own learning and enables them to change or
modify their own cognitive processes. In support of this, there is a
rich body of literature showing that higher proficiency students
use metacognitive strategies more than lower proficiency ones and
that the former tend to use them more flexibly and effectively
(Bernhardt, 1991; Chamot, 2005; Zhang, 2008). Moreover,
metacognitive strategies can positively impact students with
learning disabilities by helping them become independent learners.

21
As Lerner and Kline (2006) state, "Efficient learners use
metacognitive strategies but students with learning disabilities
tend to lack the skills to direct their own learning. However, once
they learn the metacognitive strategies that efficient learners use,
students with learning disabilities can apply them in many
situations” (p. 184). Due to the importance these strategies, the
next three sections will present them in some details.

2.3.2.1. Planning
2.3.2.1.1. Definition and types of planning
Planning is a metacognitive strategy used by learners before doing
a task to set goals and consider the ways these goals will be
achieved (Zimmerman, 2000). However, planning-in-action may
take place while doing the task to change goals and reconsider the
ways of achieving them. As Humes (1983) claims with respect to
writing, planning occurs before, during, and after putting words
on a page. Humes maintains:
Planning is a thinking process that writers engage in
throughout composing, before, during, and after the time
spent in putting words on a page. During planning,
writers form an internal representation of knowledge that
will be used in writing. (p. 205)
There are two types of planning. One type is process-oriented.
With this type of planning students look for ways to help them
perform a task more skillfully. The other type is outcome-oriented.
With this type of planning, students are concerned about their
overall outcome (Seijts and Latham, 2006).

2.3.2.1.2. Benefits of planning


The benefits of planning for learning include: (1) giving students
the opportunity to set their own personal goals and to see their

22
own progress, which in turn foster their self-regulation skills and
increase their motivation for learning, and (2) reducing the
cognitive strain while learning which in turn improves academic
achievement (Zimmerman, 1998). In support of these benefits,
many research studies indicate that successful learners utilize
planning for language learning (Graham and Harris, 1996;
Zimmerman and Risemberg, 1997) and that students' planning
positively affects the comprehension and production of language.
Ellis (1987) and Crookes (1989), for example, found that planning
positively affected students’ oral performance. In a similar vein,
Pintrich, Smith, Garcia, and McKeachie (1991) found that
learners' use of planning resulted in deeper processing and higher
levels of understanding the materials being learned. Moreover,
Dellerman, Coirier, and Marchand (1996) found that planning
was most effective for nonproficient writers. Furthermore, Asaro-
Saddler (2008) found that planning was beneficial in improving
the writing skills of second and fourth grade students with autism
spectrum disorders.

2.3.2.2. Self-monitoring
2.3.2.2.1. Definition and types of self-monitoring
Self-monitoring is defined as a metacognitive strategy utilized to
observe and regulate cognitive strategies while doing a task to fine
tune strategies and effort as needed in order to achieve learning
goals. For example, when reading, a student can use the context to
guess the meaning of difficult words. To monitor her/his use of this
strategy, s/he should pause and check to see if the meaning s/he
guessed makes sense in the text and if not, s/he goes back to modify
or change this strategy. Thus, self-monitoring enables students to
track understanding as they read and to implement repair
strategies when understanding breaks down (Zimmerman, 1998,
2000).

23
There are two types of self-monitoring procedures: self-
monitoring of attention (SMA) and self-monitoring of
performance (SMP). SMA procedures are used for learning
disabilities who might be easily distracted, get up from their seats,
bother other students, or fiddle with objects. The student can
monitor the frequency or duration of these behaviors. SMP
procedures are used for students who need to monitor some
aspects of academic performance to enhance active learning
(Harris, 1986).

2.3.2.2.2. Benefits of self-monitoring


Self-monitoring is necessary for academic success (Conley, 2007).
Students who successfully use self-monitoring actively adjust
performance and strategies as needed in order to attain goals
without the need for external feedback, thus increasing the
probability of success in achieving learning goals (Zimmerman,
1998) and fostering autonomy in learning (Cresswell, 2000). In
support of this, researchers found positive relationships between
self-monitoring and academic achievement (e.g., Kauffman, 2004;
Schraw and Nietfeld, 1998). Research also showed that self-
monitoring positively affected both the length and quality of the
written stories of fifth and sixth graders with learning disabilities
(Harris, Graham, Reid, McElroy, and Hamby, 1994), and both the
quantity and quality of the narrative and expository writing of
fourth-grade students with learning disabilities (Goddard
and Sendi, 2008).
Additionally, self-monitoring often reveals valuable
information regarding one’s own deficiencies and goes beyond the
detection to the repair of these deficiencies, thus increasing the
probability of improving the learning of students with learning
disabilities because it enables these students to track
understanding as they read and to implement repair strategies
when understanding breaks down. It also encourages them to look
critically and analytically at their writing and to place themselves
in the position of the reader. In support of this, research revealed
that students at all grade levels, including those who had cognitive

24
or behavioral disabilities, could successfully learn to use and
benefit from self-monitoring interventions (e.g., Hughes and Boyle,
1991; Hughes, Copeland, Agran, Wehmeyer, Rodi, and Presley,
2002; Reid, 1996). Moreover, research on self-monitoring with
students of learning disabilities (e.g., Harris, Graham, Reid,
McElroy, and Hamby, 1994; Gumpel and Shlomit, 2000; Hughes,
Copeland, Agran, Wehmeyer, Rodi, and Presley, 2002; Goddard
and Sendi, 2008) demonstrated that self-monitoring led to positive
changes in social behaviors, aggressive behaviors, disruptive
behaviors, on-task behaviors, and academic performance
behaviors.
Furthermore, based on learners' perceptions of their progress,
self-monitoring can positively affect the level of students’ self-
efficacy. When learners perceive satisfactory progress, their
feelings of competence and efficacy may be strengthened. In sum,
self-monitoring empowers students to be in control of their
learning.

2.3.2.3. Self-assessment
2.3.2.3.1. Definition of self-assessment
Self-assessment can be defined as information about the learners
provided by the learners themselves, about their abilities, the
progress they think they are making and what they think they can
or cannot do yet with what they have learned in a course (Blanche
and Merino, 1989). Harris and McCann (1994) also define self-
assessment as “information about students’ expectations and
needs, their problems and worries, how they feel about their
own [learning] process, their reactions to the materials and
methods being used, what they think about the course in general”
(p. 36). According to Oscarson (1997), self-assessment is concerned
with knowing how, under what circumstances and with what
effects learners and users of language judge their own
performance.

25
In light of the previous definitions, it is clear that self-
assessment is self-judgment of one's own learning processes and
products for the purpose of improving them, not for a grade or
placement.

2.3.2.3.2. Merits and demerits of self-assessment


Self-assessment is a key to academic success because it helps
learners to identify their own strengths and weaknesses, thus
assisting them in addressing areas in need of improvement to
adjust effort, resources, and strategies accordingly. In other
words, self-assessment helps learners to gain insight not only in the
processes of learning (how they learned and what did or did not
work for them) but also in the products of their own learning
(what they have learned or not learned). Moreover, the greater
students’ ability to accurately self-assess their potential for success
at a specific task, the more likely it is that they will work hard
and expend extra effort in order to maximize the chances of
mastery over the task (Bandura, 1991). In support of this, studies
showed that self-assessment positively affected achievement
outcomes, skill acquisition, and self-regulation (Schunk, 1994;
Kitsantas, Reiser, and Doster, 2004).
Self-assessment is also one of the cornerstones of self-directed
language learning. In order for self-directed learning to occur,
learners must be able to determine accurately what their needs
are, and to take action to meet these needs. In other words,
students need to be able to self-assess their own learning processes
and outcomes accurately so that they themselves understand what
they need to learn without the help of their teachers. In this
respect, Gottlieb (2000) states that “[m]ultiple opportunities for
self-assessment within instruction allow second language students
to develop as independent learners while acquiring English” (p.
97). Likewise, Rivers (2001) speaks of self-assessment as the most
salient skill for self-regulation and self-directed learning to take
place. He goes so far as to say, "In the absence of either accurate
self-assessment or genuine autonomy, self-directed language

26
learning will not occur” (p. 287). Therefore, self-assessment
practices are considered an essential component of self-regulated
learning.
Additionally, the utilization of self-assessment practices is
vitally important to the development of critical thinking skills.
According to McMahon (1999), in order to develop critical
thinkers, assessment procedures must include self-assessment
practices. He believes that these practices lead to far more critical
analysis of assessment criteria on behalf of the learner and
encourages the learner to challenge assumptions. Fitzpatrick
(2006) also contends that self-assessment helps to develop students’
critical thinking and sense of autonomy. In support of this, some
studies reported improved higher order cognitive skills by self-
assessment but the outcomes were mostly supported by student
self-reports (See Falchikov, 2005, for a review of these studies).
Moreover, motivation and self-efficacy can be fostered
significantly more with continuous self-assessment than without it.
Teachers can enhance students’ motivation for learning when self-
assessment becomes part of day-to-day teaching and when
learners do it for monitoring progress and improvement, not for a
grade or placement. The results of several studies also established
that self-assessment practices in the field of language learning had
increased student motivation (Black and Wiliam, 1998; Blanche
and Merino, 1989; von Elek 1985). Self-assessment can also
enhance students’ self-efficacy because it gives students a voice in
their learning which contributes to their self-efficacy (Bandura,
1997). In this respect, Dodd (1995) believes that self-assessment
promotes self-efficacy, supporting the belief that students who feel
ownership for the class or task and believe they can make a
difference, become more engaged in their own learning process,
which in turn enhances their self-efficacy. In support of this,
Coronado-Aliegro (2006) found that Spanish undergraduate
students’ self-efficacy seemed to be strengthened significantly
more with continuous self-assessment than without it.

27
Besides, self-assessment can boost students' confidence. In
relation to writing, Pajares, Johnson and Usher (2007) mention
this benefit as follows:
Frequent self-assessment leads to more successful writing.
[…] as students learn to evaluate themselves as writers,
they also learn to set goals and strategies for improving
their writing and to document their growth. This self-
awareness helps students to interpret their achievements
in ways that will boost their confidence. (p. 116)
Furthermore, self-assessment is seen by Boud (2000) as a
“necessary skill for lifelong learning” (p. 159) because it is
beneficial for people in their daily lives and helps them to meet the
challenges of a changing society. Ellis (1999) also asserts that
knowing one's strengths and weaknesses can make a difference in
the real world. He adds that when people carry out self-evaluation
they will have a truer sense of what is good or better for them,
whether in a work situation or an academic one. Therefore, Boud,
Cohen, and Sampson (1999) state that without fostering self-
assessment, assessment will “undermine an important goal of
lifelong learning” (p. 419).
Over and above, self-assessment practices in the classroom
also make teachers aware of individual students’ learning
processes and needs. They also help them gather information
about learners from another perspective, in this case, the learners
themselves (Black, Harrison, Lee, Marshall, and Wiliam, 2003).
Furthermore, self-assessment alleviates the assessment burden on
teachers and saves their time because it spares them from
assessing students’ language learning progress continuously and
regularly in all areas. In addition, rather than giving a
comprehensive diagnostic test to have a glimpse of students’
problem areas, it is much faster to ask them directly what
problems they feel they have (Harris and McCann, 1994).

28
Additional benefits of involving students in assessment
include: developing students’ reflection, reducing their anxiety,
raising their awareness of learning strategies, providing the basis
for agreement between student and teacher on academic priorities,
encouraging objective analysis of one’s own attitudes and
aptitudes, encouraging individual goal setting, acknowledging
differences in learning styles, and developing democratic citizens
who know how to evaluate different views for the public good.
However, critics of self-assessment argue that there are many
obstacles that prevent its application. The most serious one among
these obstacles is that students may either overestimate or
underestimate their own progress. In support of this, research has
shown that students with elementary skills and students with low
self-esteem tend to overestimate their abilities, placing emphasis
on effort rather than achievement, while students who are more
proficient tend to underestimate their abilities (Boud, 1995;
Falchikov and Boud, 1989).
To surmount the obstacle of students’ inaccurate estimation of
their own progress, many assessment experts (e.g., McDevitt and
Ormrod, 2004; Paris and Ayres, 1994; Winne, 1995) suggest ways
such as (a) providing students with self-assessment training, (b)
identifying appropriate criteria for self-assessment, (c) explaining
and modeling these criteria, and (d) giving students feedback on
their self-assessments. In support of self-assessment training as a
way for surmounting inaccurate estimation, research showed that
self-assessment training had a positive effect on the quality
of self-assessment and learning. In her intervention study, which
focused on self-assessment training with a group of adult
immigrants learning Dutch, Dieten (1992) concluded that
“training can have a positive effect on the quality of
self-assessment, provided it is conducted in the way intended” (p.
220). Along the same line, Ross, Rolheiser and Hogaboam-Gray
(1999) found that teaching self-assessment skills increased

29
accuracy, especially for those who tended to overestimate, and had
a positive effect on achievement among low achievers as it helped
them better understand teacher expectations. They (Ross,
Rolheiser and Hogaboam-Gray) concluded that language students
have to be taught to self-assess their work correctly. Likewise,
McDonald and Boud (2003) found that self-assessment training
had a significant impact on student performance in all curriculum
areas and students with training in self-assessment outperformed
students without similar training.
Research also reveals that using assessment criteria can
enhance the quality of self-assessment practice and improve
student learning. Eighty-four percent of the students in a study by
Orsmond, Merry, and Reiling (2000) who self-assessed
their progress in relation to set criteria thought that the
exercise had been beneficial and made them better
critical thinkers. Orsmond et al. (ibid.) also concluded that
“[d]eveloping an appreciation of criteria may enhance the
quality of the assessment practice and have a major impact on
student learning” (p. 24). Along the same line, in a study with
high school students, Andrade and Boulay (2003)
established that simply giving and explaining assessment
criteria gave students a deeper understanding of the
qualities evaluated. In another study with undergraduate
students, Andrade and Du (2005) found that having a good grasp
of assessment criteria made the students able to self-assess their
work in progress and helped them “identify strengths and
weaknesses in their work” (p. 3). They also found that
students reported positive attitudes when they were involved in
criteria-referenced self-assessment.
Research also indicates that teacher feedback on student self-
assessment improves the quality of self-assessment. Taras
(2003), for example, found that minimal integrated tutor feedback
allowed the students a high level of independence to consider
their errors, understand assessment procedures including
criteria and feedback, and realize what their strengths

30
and weaknesses were before being given a grade. She (ibid.)
concluded that “SA [self-assessment] without tutor feedback
cannot help students to be aware of all their errors” (p. 561), and
that “student self-assessment with integrated tutor feedback is
one efficient means of helping students overcome unrealistic
expectations and focus on their achievement rather than on the
input required to produce their work.” (p. 562). In the same vein,
El-Koumy (2010) found that self-plus-teacher assessment was
more effective than either alone. He offered the following
recommendation:
Rather than viewing self and teacher assessments as
opposing strategies, it is more useful to capitalize on the
advantages of both. In other words, for self-assessment to
be effective, students are in need to practice it with
teacher feedback. The teacher feedback can be decreased
gradually, and the student can take greater responsibility
for assessment as her/his self-assessment skill is
developed. (p. 16)
To summarize, it seems that self-assessment has advantages
and disadvantages However, based upon the literature reviewed
before, the advantages of using self-assessment as a tool for
improving students' learning seem to outweigh the disadvantages.
There are no consistent conclusions regarding the reliability and
validity of self-assessment. However, this should not prevent
educators from using it in teaching and learning.

2.3.2.3.3. Self-assessment tools


Many educators believe that students need instruments in order to
be able to independently take charge of the assessment of their
own learning. Checklists, learning logs and learning strategies
questionnaires are examples of the tools used to help students
reach this goal. (For additional information on these self-
assessment tools, see El-Koumy, 2004b).

31
2.3.2.3.4. Self-assessment of one’s own metacognitive
strategies
Teachers should encourage students to evaluate the whole cycle of
planning, monitoring and assessment through self-questioning. In
this respect, Schraw (1998) suggests that for promoting
metacognitive awareness, students should ask themselves the
following questions that trigger each stage of their thoughts from
planning to approach a particular task, monitoring the strategies
being applied to the task, and evaluating their learning outcomes:
(1) Planning
฀ What is the nature of the task?
฀ What is my goal?
฀ What kind of information and strategies do I need?
฀ How much time and resources do I need?
(2) Monitoring
฀ Do I have a clear understanding of what I am doing?
฀ Does the task make sense to me?
฀ Am I reaching my goals?
฀ Do I need to make changes?
(3) Evaluating
฀ Have I reached my goal?
฀ What worked?
฀ What didn’t work?
฀ Would I do things differently the next time?

32
Likewise, Anderson (2002) suggests that teachers should have
students respond thoughtfully to the following questions: “(1)
What am I trying to accomplish? (2) What strategies am I using?
(3) How well am I using them? (4) What is the outcome? (5) What
else could I do?” (p. 3). In responding to these questions, students
reflect on their own metacognitive strategies. The first question
relates to planning; the second and third questions correspond to
self-monitoring; and the fourth and fifth questions relate to
evaluation of their own learning. Similarly, Thamraksa (2005)
suggests that students should ask themselves the following
questions that trigger their thoughts before, during and after
doing a task:
(1) Before: When students are developing a plan of action, they
can ask themselves:
฀ What is my prior knowledge that will help me do this task?
฀ What should I do first?
฀ What is my expectation in doing this task?
฀ How much time do I need to complete this task?
(2) During: When students are doing the task, they can ask
themselves:
฀ How am I doing?
฀ Am I on the right track?
฀ What strategies am I using?
฀ Should I use a different strategy to complete this task?
฀ What other things/information should I need?
(3) After: After doing their task, students can ask themselves:
฀ How well did I do?
฀ What did I learn from doing this task?

33
฀ Did I learn more or less than I had expected?
฀ Do I need to redo the task?
฀ What could I have done differently?

2.3.3. Social strategies


Social strategies are actions that involve other people. These
strategies include, but are not limited to, asking others for help,
speaking together in the target language, working with fellow-
students, and reviewing others’ work. According to Oxford (1990),
social learning strategies include four main categories. The first is
asking questions; for instance, asking for clarification, verification,
or correction. The second is cooperating with others; for example,
cooperating with peers and proficient users of the new language.
The third is empathizing with others like developing cultural
understanding and becoming aware of others´ thoughts and
feelings. The fourth is seeking opportunities; for example, reading,
writing and speaking with natives, teachers and proficient peers.
From the social constructivist viewpoint, social strategies are
extremely important for all learning because learning is a social
process and people learn by becoming members of communities.
In these communities, social strategies play a major role as they
allow people to construct meaning together and to learn from each
other. Walqui (2006) expresses this idea in the following way:
The basis for all learning is social interaction. Vygotsky
emphasizes that social interaction precedes the
development of knowledge and ability. Consciousness, the
notions of self and identity, physical skills and mental
abilities, all these have their origin in social interaction
between the child and parent, and between the child,
peers and others, including teachers. (p.162)

34
Social strategies are particularly more important for language
learning because language is a social behavior and cannot be
separated from its social context. As Williams (1994) points out,
“[T]here is no question that learning a foreign language is
different to learning other subjects. This is mainly because of the
social nature of such a venture. Language, after all, belongs to a
person’s whole social being; it is part of one’s identity” (p. 77).
Therefore, many scholars (e.g., Ellis, 1988; Ligthbown and Spada,
1993; Strickland and Shanahan, 2004; Williams, 1994) emphasize
the importance of social interactions for developing the learner´s
communicative and linguistic competence. Ellis (1988), for
example, claims that second language development in classroom
can be successful when the teacher not only provides an input with
features of a target language, but also makes conditions necessary
for reciprocal interaction. “Without interaction, [language]
teaching becomes simply [...] passing on content as if it were
dogmatic truth” (Shale and Garrison, 1990, p. 29).
Over and above, social strategies help students communicate
more effectively and successfully and permit them to actively
engage with the emerging themes and issues that exist in real-
world contexts. They also allow them to use language in non-
threatening communities, and afford them more opportunities for
interactions. In these interactions students feel more comfortable
and more confident to share their own thoughts, opinions, and
ideas; and use the language openly and freely.

2.3.4. Affective strategies


Affective strategies are learning strategies concerned with
managing one's own emotions, motivations, and attitudes. These
strategies include, but are not limited to, activating supportive
emotions, beliefs and attitudes; generating and maintaining
motivation; positive self-talk; anxiety reduction; self-reward;
taking wise risks; self-encouragement; relaxation; sharing worries

35
with tutor and other students; ticking off completed tasks; joining
a self-help group, and engaging in leisure activities such as
gardening; encouraging one's self, and taking one's own emotional
temperature (Oxford, 1990, 2013; Oxford and Crookall, 1989).
The importance of affective strategies is widely recognized in
all areas of learning. The literature indicates that positive affect
can play a key role in stimulating critical and creative thinking
(Isen, 1999, 2000; Isen, Daubman, and Nowicki, 1987; Kahn and
Isen, 1993); broadening attention and steering mental processes
and cognitive decisions (Isen, 1984; Isen and Reeve, 2005; Isen and
Shalker, 1982; Rowe, Hirsch, and Anderson, 2007); maintaining
commitment to task and boosting problem-solving skills (Isen,
2000; Isen and Patrick, 1983; Isen and Reeve, 2005); fostering
helpfulness, kindness and flexibility during group interaction and
cooperative work (Isen, 2001); improving achievement (Hamre
and Pianta, 2005); and boosting language learning (Crooks and
Schmidt, 1991; Dornyei, 2008; Gardner, 2001, 2010). In contrast,
negative affect states such as anxiety and depression leads to
deficits in attentional and cognitive control mechanisms, closing
off, withdrawal, and low language achievement (Horwitz, 2001;
Horwitz, Horwitz, and Cope, 1986; Hurd, 2007; MacIntyre, 1995,
1999; Matsuda and Gobel, 2004; Mayberg et al., 1999; Zheng,
2008).
Moreover, a review of neuroscience research (e. g., Ashby,
Isen, and Turken,1999; Estrada, Isen, and Young, 1997; Gray,
Braver, and Raichle, 2002) also revealed the beneficial role of
positive affect in the workings of the neural pathways that in turn
resulted in improving the performance of cognitive tasks. Ashby
et al. (1999), for example, provided evidence demonstrating that
positive affect influenced everyday cognitive processes and
improved episodic and working memory. Their study also showed
the effect of positive affect on neural and chemical pathways that
resulted in improving creativity, problem solving, social
interaction and emotional reactions associated with changes in
brain activity.

36
Furthermore, affective strategies are particularly important
for foreign language learners because learning a new language can
be highly stressful (Arnold, 1998). The literature indicates that
these strategies can play a key role in helping students gain better
control over their own emotions, overcome their negative
attitudes, increase their self-confidence and reduce their anxiety
(Hurd, 2006; Oxford and Crookall, 1989). This in turn enhances
their learning, as Bolitho, Carter, Hughes, Ivanic, Masuhara, and
Tomlinson (2003) put it, "[M]ost learners learn best whilst
affectively engaged, and when they willingly invest energy and
attention in the learning process" (p. 252). Furthermost, affective
strategies are important for students with learning disabilities
because research indicates that these students enter into foreign
language learning with a history of failure and frustration and
perceive themselves as less capable, more anxious, and as
possessing fewer capabilities to master oral and written language
skills as compared to their non-LD peers (Javorsky, Sparks, and
Ganschow, 1992). Therefore, affective strategies are essential for
them to build their confidence, increase their motivation to learn a
foreign language and lessen their anxiety.
It may be appropriate here to point out that although affective
strategies are actions taken in relation to self, the teacher can play
an important role in raising students’ awareness of these
strategies. More importantly, s/he can create a relaxed
atmosphere conducive to learning through her/his non-verbal
behaviors (e.g., reducing physical distance, displaying relaxed
postures, smiling, engaging in eye contact during interactions) and
verbal behaviors (e.g., addressing students by name, praising
them, using inclusive pronouns) (Gorham, 1988).
In addition, the teacher can create a non-threatening and low-
anxiety classroom atmosphere by tolerating students’ linguistic
errors to remove their fear of being wrong. S/he should "pay
attention to the message of students' utterances rather than to the
form in which the utterances are cast… [and] treat the correction
of errors as a 'pragmatic' or interactional adjustment, not as a

37
normative form of redress, for example, by restating the incorrect
utterance in a correct manner rather than pointing explicitly to
the error" (Kramsch, 1987, p. 17). Macaro (2003), too, casts doubt
on the importance of error correction saying, “I would argue that
we should focus on forms in order to generate more learner errors,
more inaccuracy”. Therefore, the teacher should convey to the
students that making errors is normal and a signal of progress in
learning, rather than seeing these errors as sins. As Lewis (2002)
puts it,
Error is intrinsic to learning, and any strategy of error
avoidance will be counter-productive. Anyone who learns
a foreign language to a reasonable degree of proficiency
will inevitably make thousands of mistakes on the way.
Correcting every one of them is an impossibility.
Fortunately it is also highly undesirable. (p. 173).
Most importantly, the teacher should respect every student’s
thinking, deal with every student as an individual and value
her/his individuality, make her/him feel accepted as a whole
person by encouraging a realization in her/him that s/he is valued
as other people, and share power and authority with her/him by
allowing her/him to explore issues, make judgments and propose
strategies to achieve justifiable goals. The teacher should also
create opportunities for success to build self-esteem and self-
confidence, praise every student frequently for successful work,
and provide motivating texts and contexts for reading and
motivating topics for speaking and writing.
Finally, the teacher should help students take control of and
reflect on their affective strategies. To do so, Oxford (2013)
suggests an eight-step model for meta-affective strategy
instruction. The steps of this model are the following:
(1) Paying attention to affect,
(2) Planning for affect,
(3) Obtaining and using resources for affect,

38
(4) Organizing for affect,
(5) Implementing plans for affect,
(6) Orchestrating affective strategy use,
(7) Monitoring affect,
(8) Evaluating affect.
In sum, it appears that educating the heart is as important as
educating the mind and that educating the latter without
educating the former is no education at all. As Stern (1983) states,
“The affective component contributes at least as much and often
more to language learning than the cognitive skills” (p. 386). In the
same vein, Harris (1997) draws attention to the importance of both
affective and cognitive components saying, "If we attend to the
affective and cognitive components … we may be able to increase
the length of time students commit to language study and their
chances of success in it” (p. 20). Similarly, Arnold (1999) points
out, “Neither the cognitive nor the affective has the last word, and,
indeed, neither can be separated from the other” (p.1). Therefore,
she (Arnold) emphasizes the need to treat students as whole
persons, referring to the complex relationship between affect,
learning and memory, and the inseparability of emotion and
cognition in the workings of the human brain. It is clear then that
the affective dimension is just like the blood beneath the skin or
the soul inside the flesh, nourishing our interface with the world;
therefore, it is fundamental to living in general and learning in
particular.

2.4. Models of learning strategies instruction


A variety of instructional models have been developed for learning
strategies instruction (e.g., Chamot and O`Malley, 1994; Collins,
1998; Duke, 2001; Fowler, 2003; Kiewra, 2002; Oxford, 1990;
Santangelo, Harris and Graham, 2008; Wenden, 1985). These
models stress that learning strategies must be brought to a
conscious level in the learner’s mind to enable him/her to

39
implement these strategies independently in accordance with
her/his needs and the requirements of the learning task. As Oxford
(1990) states, "Learners need to learn how to learn, and teachers
need to learn how to facilitate the process. Although learning is
certainly part of the human condition, conscious skill in self-
directed learning and in strategy use must be sharpened through
training" (p. 201). Therefore, this section presents the most
famous ones among the models of learning strategies instruction.
Oxford (1990) proposes an eight-step model for learning
strategy instruction, in which the first five steps involve planning
and preparation, and the last three ones concern conducting,
evaluating, and revising the training program. The steps of this
model are the following:
(1) Determining the learners’ needs and the time available,
(2) Selecting strategies,
(3) Considering integration of strategy training,
(4) Considering motivational issues,
(5) Preparing materials and activities,
(6) Conducting completely informed training,
(7) Evaluating,
(8) Revising the strategy training.
Chamot and O’Malley’s (1994) suggest a five-phase model for
teaching learning strategies. These phases are the following:
(1) Preparation: In this phase the teacher activates students’
background knowledge about current use of learning
strategies. Activities in this phase include class discussions
about strategies used for recent learning tasks, group or
individual interviews about strategies used for particular

40
tasks, think aloud sessions in which students describe their
thought processes while they work on a task, questionnaires or
checklists about strategies used, and diary entries about
individual approaches to language learning.
(2) Presentation: In this phase the teacher explains and models the
learning strategies. S/he communicates to students
information about the characteristics, usefulness, and
applications of the strategies to be taught.
(3) Practice: In this phase, students have the opportunity of
practicing the learning strategies with an authentic learning
task.
(4) Evaluation: In this phase, the teacher provides students with
opportunities to evaluate their own success in using learning
strategies, thus developing their metacognitive awareness of
their own learning processes.
(5) Expansion: In this phase students make personal decisions
about the strategies that they find most effective and apply
these strategies to new contexts.
Collins (1998) suggests a four-phase model for teaching
strategic writing to struggling writers. These phases are the
following:
(1) Identifying a strategy worth teaching,
(2) Introducing the strategy by modeling it,
(3) Helping students to try the strategy out with workshop-style
teacher guidance,
(4) Helping students work toward independent mastery of the
strategy through repeated practice and reinforcement.

Duke (2001) proposes that teachers should follow these five


steps when introducing a new strategy: (1) explicitly describing the
strategy and stating when and how it should be used, (2) modeling
the use of the strategy in action, (3) collaborative use of the

41
strategy in action, (4) guided practice using the strategy with
gradual release of responsibility, and (5) independent use of the
strategy.

Fowler (2003) proposes a strategic instruction model (SIM) for


students with learning disabilities. This SIM incorporates both
strategic instruction and content enhancement. It helps to meet the
performance gaps through the teaching of learning strategies and
the information demands through content enhancement routines.
This model involves six main steps:
(1) Pretesting learners and encouraging them to become interested
in learning the strategy: The teacher determines how much
the learners might already know about using the strategy and
secure their commitment to learning the strategy from top to
bottom. S/he explains to the learners what strategy they are
going to learn and how it can help them in skills development.
(2) Describing the strategy: The teacher gives a clear explanation
of the strategy, identifies real tasks where the learners can
apply the strategy and asks them if they can think of other
work where the strategy might be useful.
(3) Modeling the strategy: In this step, the teacher models the
strategy for learners through thinking aloud as s/he works so
that they can observe how a person thinks and what a person
does while using the strategy.
(4) Practicing the strategy: The teacher provides repeated
opportunities to practice the strategy. The more learners and
the teacher work together to use the strategy, the more the
learners will internalize the strategy. Initial practice may be
largely practitioner-directed, with teachers continuing to
model appropriate ways of thinking about the task at hand
and deciding (with increasing student direction) which
strategy or action is needed to work through whatever
problems arise in completing the task.

42
(5) Providing feedback: The teacher provides feedback to learners
on their strategy use. Much of the feedback can be offered as
learners become involved in thinking aloud about the task and
about strategy use.
(6) Promoting generalization: In this step, learners apply the
strategy in various situations and with other tasks. This
transfer is not automatic for students with learning
disabilities. Consistent and guided practice at generalizing
strategies to various settings and tasks is therefore vital for
students with learning disabilities.
Vacca, Vacca, Gove, McKeon, Burkey, and Lenhart (2006)
suggest four steps for learning strategies instruction. These steps
are:
(1) Creating awareness of the strategy: This step is a give-and-
take exchange of ideas between teacher and students.
These exchanges may include explanations and strategy
tips and are built around questions such as "Why is the
strategy useful?" "What is the payoff for students?" and
"What are the rules, guidelines, or procedures for being
successful with the strategy?"
(2) Modeling the strategy,
(3) Providing practice in the use of the strategy,
(4) Applying the strategy in authentic situations.
Santangelo, Harris, and Graham (2008) suggest a six-phase
model for explicitly teaching learning strategies to students with
learning disabilities. These phases are:
(1) Developing preskills: At this phase, students’ prior knowledge
about the task and strategy is assessed and remediation is
provided when needed.
(2) Discussing the strategy: The strategy to be learned is
described, a purpose for using the strategy is established, and
the benefits of using the strategy are presented.

43
(3) Modeling the strategy: The teacher cognitively models (while
thinking out loud) how to use and apply the strategy for the
task.
(4) Memorizing the strategy: Students should be provided time
to memorize the strategy until they are fluent in
understanding each step. The use of mnemonics (e.g., POWER
for Plan, Organize, Write, Edit, and Revise) and graphic
organizers can help them memorize the steps of strategy.
(5) Guided practice: The teacher guides learners through a series
of prompts and/or questions to apply the strategy.
(6) Independent practice: The teacher provides independent
practice across task and settings to foster generalization and
maintenance.
In summary, many of the previously mentioned models show a
remarkable similarity with respect to the basic stages of strategy
instruction. Most of them agree that effective strategy instruction
must include four basic stages. At the first stage, the teacher raises
students’ awareness of the strategy/ies under focus by providing
them with declarative knowledge about the strategy/ies (i.e., what
strategy/ies they are learning), procedural knowledge (i.e., how the
strategy/ies should be used and why) and conditional knowledge
(i.e., in which contexts should the strategy/ies be used). At the
second stage, students use the strategy/ies under focus in
contextualized tasks under teacher guidance and/or in
collaboration with more capable peers. At the third stage, each
student uses the strategy/ies individually and independently in
contextualized tasks. The final stage involves each student in
evaluating the success of the strategy/ies s/he has already used in
relation to task performance.

2.5. Methods of identifying and assessing


learning strategies
Language learning strategies are generally internal processes.
They are for the most part unobservable, though some may be
44
associated with an observable behavior. For example, a student
listening to information may use selective attention, which is
unobservable strategy, to focus on the main ideas; and s/he might
then decide to take notes of these main ideas, which is an
observable strategy. The only way to find out whether students are
using selective attention during a listening comprehension task is
to ask them to voice or verbalize their inner processes during or
after performing the task (Chamot, 2004). Mere observation will
be unsatisfactory in identifying these mental processes because
they are invisible and take place in the learners’ heads. Given the
covert nature of most of the learning strategies, some techniques
have been developed to uncover such strategies. These techniques
fall into two broad categories: (1) retrospective self-reports and (2)
concurrent self-reports. Each of these techniques has its merits
and demerits. Retrospective reporting gives learners the
opportunity to think back and report on the strategies they believe
they used before, during, and after doing a task. This type of
reporting can be elicited through such tools as interviews and
questionnaires. These tools provide basic information about
students’ awareness and use of learning strategies in general or in
relation to a specific task. As Ellis (1994) says, “Interviews and
questionnaires can require learners to report on the learning
strategies they use in general or in relation to a specific activity”
(p. 534). They also allow instructors to identify the cognitive
processes responsible for students’ behavior. Moreover, Jacobs
and Paris (1987) have defended self-reports via questionnaires or
interviews as a means to peer into the minds of those who are
markedly shy or unable to speak. Though retrospective reporting
can make the invisible visible and the implicit explicit and provide
valuable insights into students’ learning strategies, the very act of
trying to remember what was happening can be taxing to some
learners, particularly those with learning disabilities for whom
retrospective recall is problematic. In addition, learners may fall
into the trap of responding in the way they believe the instructor
expects them to do, just to please her/him, whether they have
answers or not. That is, in response to questionnaires learners may
report using strategies that, in fact, they have not used.

45
Furthermore, if a questionnaire is written in the FL, learners may
lack adequate proficiency to comprehend some questions or
Likert-scaled statements and thus give inaccurate responses.
Finally, certain types of learners may under- or over-estimate
their use of learning strategies. In support of this, Meltzer, Katzir-
Cohen, Miller, and Roditi (2001) found that fourth through ninth
graders with learning disabilities rated themselves as highly
strategic on a self-report measure using a Likert-like scale, reports
with which their grades and teachers' reports did not concur.
Concurrent reporting is another means of making learners'
cognitive processes overt via thinking-aloud while performing a
task. This type of reporting is beneficial for both learners and
teachers. For learners, thinking-aloud induces them to become
more aware of what they think, what they understand and what
they do not, and what they need to do when misunderstanding
occurs. It also raises their awareness of the mental processes that
occurs during performing a specific task. In other words, it helps
them to be cognizant of what occurs in their own minds during
performing a task and what strategies they call into play to boost
their performance (Anderson and Vandergrift, 1996). For
teachers, thinking-aloud allows them to identify and assess
learners’ cognitive and metacognitive strategies, which in turn
helps them to change and/or improve the covert processes
responsible for learners’ behavior. Moreover, teachers themselves
may use thinking-aloud as an intervention or instructional
technique to model effective strategies to their students (Ericsson
and Simon, 1984; Pressley and Afflerbach, 1995). However,
several drawbacks to this particular type of reporting must be
noted. Learners may have difficulty verbalizing their own
thoughts while doing the task because they think faster than they
talk, and their oral language proficiency level may not help them
to express their mental processes precisely and accurately.
Furthermore, thinking-aloud slows down cognitive processes
(Nielsen, Clemmensen, and Yssing, 2002), and concurrent
verbalization may be problematic when the task involves “a high
cognitive load, when the information is difficult to verbalize

46
because of its form” (Branch, 2000, p. 379). Moreover,
concurrent verbalization puts a cognitive load on and requires a
cognitive involvement from the user, which may interfere or even
compete with the cognitive requirements of the task (Karsenty,
2001). In addition, Ericsson and Simon (1993) concede that
concurrently elicited verbal reports are not likely to be complete.
Although the previously-mentioned types of reporting lack
standardization, they are the only ways available to teachers to
generate insights into unobservable learning strategies, to collect
information from students about their learning strategies, and to
develop some understanding of learners’ mental processes. As
Grenfell and Harris (1999) point out, “It is not easy to get inside
the ‘black box’ of the human brain and find out what is going on
there. We work with what we can get, which, despite the
limitations, provides food for thought” (p. 54). Despite their
limitations, the two types of reporting can adequately reveal
learning strategies by allowing learners to use their native
language for reporting, combining two or more of the protocols
used for assessing learning strategies (e.g., interviews in
conjunction with videotaped thinking-aloud), providing learners
with training in thinking-aloud via modeling to enable them to
describe their thinking clearly and explicitly, and giving them
prompts if they fall silent for more than 30 seconds during
thinking-aloud (e.g., "What are you thinking now?" "What's
going through your mind?" "Say it aloud.").

2.6. Research into effective/ineffective learning


strategies
An abundant body of research has examined the strategies
successful language learners tend to use in order to manage their
overt behaviors. As Ellis (1994) states, much of the research on
language learning strategies "has been based on the assumption
that there are 'good' learning strategies" (p. 558). Rubin (1975)
was among the first researchers who focused on strategies of
successful language learners because, as she stated, once identified,

47
such strategies could be made available to less successful learners
so that they could increase their success rate. Her findings
revealed that the good language learner was willing and accurate
guesser; had a strong persevering drive to communicate; was often
uninhibited and willing to make mistakes in order to learn or
communicate; took advantage of all practice opportunities;
monitored her or his own speech as well as that of others; and paid
attention to meaning. Later on, studies into effective and
ineffective learning strategies focused on learning strategies used
in specific language areas. In the area of reading, Hosenfeld (1977)
found that successful L2 readers kept the meaning of the passage
in mind, skipped words that they believed to be unimportant to the
meaning of the sentence or text, read in broad phrases, and used
context to determine the meaning of unknown words. Less
successful readers, on the other hand, translated sentences on a
word-for-word basis, rarely skipped words, and looked up
unknown words in a glossary. In a like manner, Kaufman,
Randlett, and Price (1985) found that high comprehenders used
more strategies than low comprehenders when confronted with
comprehension difficulties and that although both groups reported
using equal amounts of concrete, observable strategies when
reading (e.g., skimming, re-reading, slowing down reading pace),
high comprehenders were more likely to report using strategies
that involve complex, unobservable mental operations to repair
their misunderstandings (e.g., visualizing, perspective-taking,
making predictions, drawing inferences). According to the
researchers, these findings provide evidence that good readers use
different sets of strategies when confronted with a comprehension
problem. Specifically, they noted that higher order, complex
thinking skills are required to achieve an accurate and thorough
understanding of difficult passages.
In addition, Block (1986) found that more successful readers
used global strategies such as anticipating content, recognizing text
structure, identifying main ideas, using background knowledge,
monitoring comprehension, and reacting to the text as a whole. In
contrast, less successful readers used local strategies such as
questioning the meaning of individual words and sentences.
48
By the same token, Barnett (1989) found that successful
readers hypothesized about what might come next, and guessed
the meaning of unknown words. Unsuccessful readers, on the
other hand, focused on the meaning of individual words, paid
attention to text structure, reread isolated difficult parts only,
never or rarely hypothesized, and resisted skipping unknown
words. Furthermore, Pressley (1995) found that good readers and
writers selectively and flexibly applied a vast array of strategies to
every reading or writing event. In contrast, students who
experienced difficulty with reading and writing typically used
fewer strategies and their strategy use tended to be rigid rather
than flexible. Moreover, in their study of the behaviors of effective
readers, Pressley and Afflerbach (1995) found that expert and
highly skilled readers used metacognitive strategies before, during,
and after reading.
Along the same line, recent studies tend to support the
findings of the previous studies conducted in the 1980s and 90s. In
a meta-analysis of research on reading strategies, Singhal (2001)
concluded that it is “clear that there are indeed differences
between successful or good readers, and less successful or poor
readers in terms of strategy use” (p. 4). Specifically, successful
readers had been found to rely primarily upon top-down
strategies. In contrast, less proficient readers’ strategies tended to
be more local, reflecting a desire to treat reading as a decoding
process rather than a meaning-making process. Saricoban (2002)
examined the strategy use of post-secondary ESL students and
found that successful readers engaged in predicting and guessing
activities, made use of their background knowledge related to the
text’s topic, guessed the meaning of unknown words, and skimmed
and scanned the text. In contrast, less successful readers focused
on individual words, verbs in particular. In the same vein, Lau
(2006) examined the reading strategies used by good and poor
Chinese readers in Hong Kong. The results from the study showed
that good readers used more strategies during reading than did
poor readers. Good readers were also more knowledgeable about
reading strategies, which presumably allowed them to apply
strategies more effectively.
49
In the area of writing, Raimes (1987) examined ESL learners
at different levels in order to compare a wide range of their
composing behaviors with native speakers and to describe their
writing strategies through think-aloud protocols. Her research
findings revealed that skilled L2 writers engaged in more
interaction with the text and were consistently involved in more
strategies, including planning, rehearsing, rescanning, revising,
and editing. Simultaneously, they were well aware of the audience
and the purpose of a given task. In contrast, the unskilled L2
writers seemed to attach to their already produced text, with the
result that they failed to be flexible to edit or reformulate their
writing. These findings are consistent with those of many other
studies (e.g., de Bot, 1996; Schoonen and De Glopper, 1996). The
findings of these studies revealed that more proficient writers paid
more attention to higher processes while less proficient writers
were more concerned with lower processes.
In the area of listening, Vandergrift (2003) compared the
listening comprehension strategies of more- and less-skilled
Canadian seventh-grade students of French. Students listened to
several French texts and were prompted to think aloud during the
process. The more skilled listeners used more metacognitive
strategies, especially comprehension monitoring, than did their
less skilled peers. In addition, more skilled listeners engaged in
questioning for clarification, whereas the less skilled listeners used
more translation.
In sum, research has shown that good language learners
engage in the use of both cognitive and metacognitive strategies for
learning and that the major difference between successful and
unsuccessful language learners is that successful learners employ
appropriate strategies for language learning while unsuccessful
learners employ inappropriate ones. More specifically, successful
language learners rely more on top-down processing strategies
while unsuccessful language learners rely more on bottom-up
processing strategies.

50
2.7. Research on teaching learning strategies to
students with language/learning disabilities
A large body of research supports the positive effects of strategy
training on the language learning of students with and without
learning disabilities. This section presents studies conducted only
with students with language/learning disabilities.
Borkowski, Weyhing, and Carr (1988) taught summarization
and attribution strategies to 10- to 14-year-old students with
learning disabilities. The attributional components included the
instructor modeling statements like, "I need to try and use the
strategy," and, "I tried hard, used the strategy, and did well" (p.
49). In addition, in an attribution-plus condition, students were
reminded that strategy use would be beneficial to them and were
prompted to attribute success and failure to controllable factors.
Findings indicated that the attribution-plus condition combined
with the summarization treatment condition maintained superior
performance effects over other conditions on summarization
measures and standardized measures of reading comprehension.
Malone and Mastropieri (1992) taught middle school students
with learning disabilities how to self-question and summarize
while reading. The summarization instruction condition taught
students to ask and answer these two questions: (a) Who or what is
the passage about? and (b) What is happening to the who or what?
In another condition, self-monitoring was added to the
summarization condition, whereas a third condition served as a
traditional instruction comparison condition. Both summarization
conditions outperformed traditional instruction on free-recall
measures of passage content, whereas students in the
summarization plus self-monitoring condition outperformed both
comparison conditions on a transfer measure.
Rich and Blake (1994) implemented a comprehension
intervention that included instruction in some comprehension
strategies. The participants for their study consisted of five
students with language/learning disabilities. These students

51
received instruction in identifying main ideas, self-questioning,
and paraphrasing. During the intervention, students kept daily
journals evaluating their cognitive and affective behaviors.
Reading outcomes were measured with expository passages
excerpted by the researchers from informal reading inventories
and students responded to eight questions about each passage. The
researchers reported that all five students made improvements
from the pretest to the posttest in listening comprehension with
scores on the outcome measure ranging from 56–100% (2 students
below 75% on the posttest). Four of the students also improved
from pretest to posttest in reading comprehension with scores
ranging from 63–100% on the posttest measure (1 student below
75% on the posttest).
Mendelsohn (1995) investigated the effects of listening
strategies instruction on normal and poor listeners’
comprehension. Two experiments are reported. Four text
comprehension strategies, question generation, summarizing,
clarification, and predicting were taught through direct
instruction and reciprocal teaching. Dependent variables were
experimenter-developed strategic reading and listening tests, and
standardized reading and listening comprehension tests. In the
first experiment the subjects were 9 to 11-year-old poor readers
from special schools for children with learning disabilities. In this
experiment, the intervention program’s texts and strategy
instructions were presented in listening settings only. The subjects
in the second experiment were 10-year-old children from regular
elementary schools and 9 to 11-year-old children from special
schools. They were also poor readers but their decoding
performance was not as poor as in the subjects in experiment 1. In
experiment 2, the intervention program involved text
presentations in alternating reading and listening lessons.
Although in general, normal listeners performed better on all
comprehension tests than poor listeners, there were no differential
program effects for the two listening levels. Clear effects of both

52
programs were found on strategic reading and listening tests
administered directly after the interventions. In the first
experiment, maintenance test performance showed prolonged
program benefits, whereas in the second experiment these
maintenance effects were blurred by unexpected gains of the
control groups, especially from regular schools.
Aarnoutse (1997) investigated the effectiveness of teaching
reading comprehension strategies to very poor decoders in a
listening situation. The subjects, 95 students from 6 special schools
for children with learning disabilities, were chosen based on their
very low scores on a decoding test, low scores on a reading
comprehension test, and low or average scores on a listening
comprehension test. The subjects were administered pretests,
posttests, and retention tests. The 48 students in the experimental
group were instructed in a listening program consisting of 20
lessons of 30 minutes each. The 47 students in the control group
attended regular reading lessons, which did not contain
comprehension strategy instruction. Results indicated that
students trained by the program performed better during the
posttest on the strategic listening and reading tests than the
control group, and the better performance was maintained on the
strategy retention tests (3 months after the posttest).
In a synthesis of research on metacognition, Collins, Dickson,
Simmons, and Kameenui (1998) identified a body of research
indicating that individuals with reading disabilities can learn to
become effective and active readers through instruction aimed at
increasing such metacognitive skills as self-regulation. Based on
their synthesis of research, they recommended that it is crucial for
adult literacy programs to incorporate the direct teaching of
reading strategies in a way that helps adults with learning
disabilities apply strategies to meet their specific reading needs.
Bryant, Vaughn, Linan-Thompson, Ugel, and Hamff (2000)
reported using collaborative strategic reading as part of a multi
component reading intervention strategy with students with

53
reading disabilities, low-achieving students, and average-achieving
students in the middle years and found that all students’ reading
outcomes (i.e., word identification, fluency, and comprehension)
increased significantly as a result of the intervention although a
subgroup of very poor readers made little progress. Moreover,
teachers reported that the percentage of their students who passed
high-stakes tests increased from the previous year as a result of
their participation in the intervention
Jitendra, Hoppes, and Xin (2000) taught middle-school
students with disabilities a main idea identification strategy either
with or without a monitoring component. Students in the
monitoring group outperformed those in the control group in both
near and far measures of reading comprehension.
Burchard and Swerdzewski’s (2009) study of a postsecondary
strategic learning course, including students with and without
disabilities, demonstrated that students who participated in the
course made statistically significant gains in metacognitive
regulation and metacognitive awareness from the beginning to the
end of the course. Course participants made greater gains in
metacognitive regulation than did students in the general
population at the university. This study also revealed that gains by
students with disabilities were not different from gains made by
students without disabilities, suggesting that students with
disabilities benefit just as much as students without disabilities
from participation in learning strategies courses.
Crabtree, Alber-Morgan, and Konrad (2010) used a multiple
baseline across participants design to examine the effects of self-
monitoring and active responding on the reading comprehension
of three high school seniors with learning disabilities and
significant attention problems. The self-monitoring intervention
required the participants to read a story and stop reading at three
pre-determined places in the text. At each stopping point, the
participants used a form to record the answers to five questions

54
focusing on narrative story elements (e.g., Who are the main
characters? What is the setting?). Reading comprehension was
measured by (a) number of story facts the participants were able
to recall and (b) number of correct responses on a 10-item reading
comprehension quiz. Findings demonstrated a functional relation
between the self-monitoring intervention and reading
comprehension performance.
In short, it has been demonstrated that when students with
learning disabilities are taught language learning strategies and
are given ample opportunities to use them, they improve in their
ability to process information, which in turn improves their
comprehension and production of the language. However, it is a
known fact that different language skills involve varied processes
and need different learning strategies. Therefore, the remaining
chapters of this book will address the strategies related to major
language skills, namely, (1) oral communication (2) reading
comprehension and (3) writing.

55
Chapter Three
Teaching Communication Strategies to
Students with Oral Communication
Disabilities
3.0. Introduction
The primary aim of teaching and learning English as a foreign
language is to develop students’ communication skills because
these skills are necessary in school and society. In the
globalization era, English has become one of the most important
languages of communication in the world. As Lewis (2011) puts it,
“In today’s world where a high percentage of students need or
will need to be able to speak English outside the classroom, there
is an absolute necessity to develop communicative competence as
an integral part of an effective EFL syllabus” (p. 54). In school,
oral communication skills are the bridge to literacy because they
form the basis for literacy development at the beginning-level.
Students cannot write what they cannot say. Oral language is a
precursor to written language even if we do not write exactly the
way we speak (Williams and Roberts, 2011). Moreover, both
teacher-student and student-student interactions are important
sources for EFL learning in the classroom. Through such
interactions, input can be made comprehensible and meaning can
be made clearer. Most importantly, communication makes
language teaching more thoughtful, involves students in thinking
and turns the language classroom into a community of thinkers.
Therefore, it can develop students’ higher order thinking skills.
As Logan (2007) states, "Communication facilitates thinking and
thinking facilitates communication. Dialogue and questions
provoke new thoughts, new ideas, and new forms of language
which require new vocabularies, and those new vocabularies then
make new thoughts and insights possible" (p. 104). Therefore, in
order for language learning and thinking to go hand-in-hand,
students need to share their ideas with their teacher and other
students.
In the global society, English communication skills have
become essential for attaining and performing many high-
level jobs. They are amongst the most sought after skills by many
employers. Many if not all employers rank communication skills
among the most important skills for graduates to possess upon
their entry in the workplace. Furthermore, a variety of reports
identify oral communication skills as the most important
workplace skills for employees (e.g., Bauer 1995; Howe 2003;
Wayne and Mitchell 1992). Therefore, Benson (1983), among
many others, regard communication skills as one of the most
important courses, business schools can teach their students, to
prepare them for management positions, and to increase their
occupational success.
Communication skills are also central in developing informed
citizens who are capable of participation in the global society and
democratic deliberation. Through communication, citizens can
share perspectives for the benefit of the society as a whole. In
recognition of this, the European Parliament and the Council of
the European Union (2006) recommend that lifelong learning
skills should include communication in the mother tongue and
communication in an international foreign language.
Moreover, communication is a vital part of all aspects of life.
As Bakhtin (1984) states, life by its very nature is dialogic and we
need to freely engage in open ended dialogue to fully engage with
life and learning. He states:
To live means to participate in dialogue: to ask questions,
to heed, to respond, to agree, and so forth. In this
dialogue a person participates wholly and throughout his
whole life: with his eyes, lips, hands, soul, spirit, with his
whole body and deeds. He invests his entire self in
discourse, and this discourse enters into the dialogic
fabric of human life, into the world symposium. (p. 293)

57
In a nutshell, it is clear that communication skills are vital to
student success within and beyond school. These skills have been
shown to increase academic, occupational and personal success.
Therefore, Freire (2000) claims that “without communication
there can be no true education” (p. 92).
Despite the fact that the importance of communication skills
within all facets of life has been well documented, many students
with learning disabilities have trouble understanding others and
expressing themselves normally in a meaningful way. More
specifically, speech researchers and pathologists (e.g., David,
1975; Harris, 1994; Lahey, 1988) state that students with learning
disabilities avoid speaking in class and experience difficulties with
oral communication in the following areas:
• Exchanging information on a wide variety of topics,
• Requesting and giving clarification,
• Expressing opinions, ideas, or feelings adequately on everyday
topics,
• Telling a story or talking about an incident in sequence;
• Interacting with peers,
• Responding to requests and open-ended questions,
• Requesting and giving clarification,
• Repairing breakdowns during interaction,
• Using turn taking appropriately,
• Keeping a conversation going,
• Using appropriate eye contact,
• Comprehending spoken language,
• Expressing understanding.

58
In the Egyptian context, the oral communication difficulties,
faced by struggling EFL students particularly those with
communication disabilities, are due to two causes. The first cause
is that teachers always view students as passive recipients of
information. They do not interact with them; nor do they provide
them with opportunities to interact with each other. That is,
interaction is completely neglected in Egyptian classrooms. The
second cause is that Egyptian students are always fearful of
expressing their own opinions because teachers penalize them for
their mistakes. Therefore, they prefer to be reticent to avoid
humiliation, embarrassment, and criticism. This results in the
vicious circle, “the less they speak, the less they improve their
speaking skills, and the more they are afraid of speaking‟
(Jianing, 2007).
To help students who struggle with oral communication
overcome their difficulties, many educators and researchers (e.g.,
Dornyei, 1995; Kongsom, 2009; Lewis, 2011; Nakatani, 2005;
Rababah, 2002) recommend using communication strategies as an
instructional intervention for them to develop their
communication skills. Lewis (2011), for example, expresses this
recommendation as follows:
If we are to help students develop their communicative
competence, it is essential that we expose them to and
draw their attention to a variety of communicative
strategies, give them opportunities to apply the strategies
in similar contexts and give them structured feedback on
their performance. (p. 54)
In light of the above, the rest of this chapter will address
communication strategies from all aspects. It will also detail a
four-step model for teaching these strategies and review research
on their impact on students with learning disabilities.
3.1. Definition of communication strategies
There are a number of definitions for communication strategies.
From different perspectives, linguists defined communication

59
strategies in different ways. From a psycholinguistic perspective,
communication strategies are defined as internal cognitive
techniques used by a speaker to solve communication problems.
In this respect, Corder (1981) defines communication strategies as
systematic techniques employed by a speaker to express her/his
meaning when faced with some difficulties. In a similar way,
Færch and Kasper (1983b) define communication strategies as
“potentially conscious plans for solving what to an individual
presents itself as a problem in reaching a particular
communicative goal” (p. 36). Likewise, Wenden (1986) defines
communication strategies as techniques used by learners when
there is a gap between their knowledge of the language and their
communicative intent. Along the same line, Bialystok (1990)
defines communication strategies as “strategies [that] are used
only when a speaker perceives that there is a problem which may
interrupt communication” (p. 3).
From a sociolinguistic perspective, communication strategies
are defined as techniques that both speakers and listeners use to
solve their problems during the course of communication. Tarone
(1980), for example, defines communication strategies as “mutual
attempts of two interlocutors to agree on a meaning in situations
where the requisite meaning structures do not seem to be shared”
(p. 420). This sociolinguistic view suggests that communication
strategies are not unique to speakers and that both speakers and
listeners mutually use communication strategies to solve problems
while negotiating for meaning. As Kraat (1985) states, “What is
said or done by a ‘speaker’ at any point in an interaction is often
the result of what was said and done by both partners in earlier
segments of that exchange. That, in turn, influences the
subsequent behaviors that occur” (p. 21). Moreover, the listener’s
verbal and non-verbal behaviors also influence the speaker’s
behavior at any point in an interaction. For example, the

60
listener’s non-verbal behaviors (e.g., eye-gaze, body shifts, puzzled
facial expressions, head shakes) make the speaker “shift style as
he or she perceives the ‘listener’ to be reacting badly to a request;
or become more explicit as he or she sees a puzzled look appear;
or shift topic or begin to terminate the conversation perceiving
that the partner is bored or inattentive” (ibid., p. 135). The
sociolinguistic view also suggests that communication strategies
can occur in situations where problems are not involved and can
serve as non-problem-solving strategies to enhance the
effectiveness of communication, in that speakers can employ these
strategies to clarify or elaborate on the intended message (Canale,
1983).
From a psycho-social perspective, a broader definition of
communication strategies was proposed by some linguists (e.g.,
Chuanchaisit and Prapphal, 2009; Dornyei and Scott, 1997;
Malasit and Sarobol, 2013) who believe that a speaker does not
only cooperate with his or her interlocutor to solve
communication problems, but also finds a solution without the
help of others and that communication requires the speaker to use
inter- and intra- individual communication strategies.
It is clear then that there is no universally accepted definition
of communication strategies because these definitions, as
mentioned before, are derived from different theoretical
perspectives. The psycholinguistic perspective views
communication strategies as individual or cognitive processes for
overcoming communication barriers, and neglects the role of
the partner despite the fact that “language is […] a living
organism created by both speaker and hearer” (Tarone, 1981, p.
288) and that communication strategies and negotiation of
meaning cannot occur in isolation from each other. In contrast,
sociolinguists view communication strategies as bi-directional
techniques between at least two partners, who mutually
influence each other in a reactive and interactive way (Kraat,
1985). From a psycho-social perspective, a speaker does not only

61
cooperate with his or her interlocutor to solve communication
problems, but also finds a solution without the help of others. This
standpoint integrates the psycholinguistic and sociolinguistic
perspectives in an attempt to overcome the limitations of both.

3.2. Classification of communication strategies


Many different taxonomies of communication strategies have been
suggested over years (e.g. Bialystok, 1983; Corder, 1983; Færch
and Kasper, 1983a; Poulisse, 1987; Tarone, 1983). Some of these
classifications have been organized around various theories, while
others have been organized around various research purposes.
This section presents some of these taxonomies in a chronological
order.
From a psycholinguistic perspective, Færch and Kasper
(1983b) classified communication strategies into two types:
achievement strategies and reduction strategies. The former set of
strategies allows the learner to have an alternative plan to reach
an original goal using available resources. This set consists of
compensatory strategies and retrieval strategies. The
compensatory strategies include code switching, interlingual
transfer, interlanguage–based strategies, and nonlinguistic
strategies. Retrieval strategies are used when the learner has
difficulties in retrieving specific interlanguage items. The latter set
of strategies is used by the learner to avoid solving a
communication problem and to allow her/him to give up
conveying an original message. This set consists of formal
reduction strategies (e.g., using a reduced system to
avoid producing non-fluent or incorrect utterances) and function
reduction strategies (e.g., giving up a message or avoiding a
specific topic).
In her book, Communication strategies: A psychological
analysis of second-language use, Bialystok (1990, pp. 132-134)
developed a taxonomy of communication strategies that consists of
analysis- and control-based strategies. The analysis-based

62
strategies imply the manipulation of the intended concept to
“convey the structure of the intended concept by making explicit
the relational defining features" (ibid:133). The control-based
strategies, on the other hand, imply the manipulation of form
while keeping the intention constant. These two types of strategies
are further described below.
1. Analysis-based strategies are attempts to convey the structure
of the intended concept by making explicit the relational
defining features. These strategies include:
a. Circumlocution,
b. Paraphrase,
c. Transliteration,
d. Word coinage,
e. Mime (i.e., using a nonverbal behavior in place of a lexical
item).
2. Control-based strategies are the uses of a representational
system that is possible to convey and that makes explicit
information relevant to the identity of the intended concept.
These strategies include:
a. Language switch (i.e., using the native language term),
b. Ostensive definition (i.e., pointing to real objects),
c. Appeal for help (i.e., asking for the correct item),
d. Mime.
From a psycho-social perspective, Dornyei and Scott (1997, p.
197) classified communication strategies used by both the speaker
and the listener. Their taxonomy comprises three main categories.
These categories, along with sample strategies from the fifty-nine
communication strategies identified by them, are displayed in
Figure 3.1.

63
Figure 3.1: Dornyei and Scott’s classification of communication
strategies (Dornyei and Scott, 1997, p. 197)
Categories Sample strategies

Direct • Message reduction,

Strategies • Circumlocution,

• Approximation,

• Code-switching,

• Mime,

• Self-rephrasing,

• Self-repair,

• Other-repair: For politeness' sake, other-repairs are


often phrased as confirmation requests in which the
trigger is changed, using oh you mean.

Interactional • Appeals for help,

Strategies • Asking questions to check that the interlocutor can


follow you,

• Asking for repetition,

• Asking for clarification,

• Asking for confirmation,

• Interpretive summary,

• Repairing the response of the interlocutor,

• Rephrasing the response of the interlocutor,

• Expanding the response of the interlocutor,

• Confirming the response of the interlocutor,

• Rejecting the response of the interlocutor.

64
(Figure 3.1 Continued)

Categories Sample strategies

Indirect • Use of fillers,

Strategies • Repeating a word or a string or words immediately after


they were said,

• Repeating something the interlocutor said to gain time,

• Use of verbal and non-verbal strategy markers to signal


that the word or structure does not carry the intended
meaning perfectly in the L2 code,

• Self-confirmation,

• Feigning understanding.

In compliance with the psycholinguistic perspective, Bygate


(2001) identified two main categories of communication strategies:
achievement strategies and reduction strategies. The following is a
brief description of these two categories:
(1) Achievement strategies:
Strategies in this category are grouped into the following
subcategories:
(a) Guessing strategies, e.g. borrowing or foreignizing a
mother-tongue word, literal translation and coining or
inventing a word,
(b) Paraphrasing strategies, e.g. circumlocuting around a
word when we don’t know the exact word,
(c) Cooperative strategies, e.g. signaling to the interlocutor
for help.
(2) Reduction strategies:
Strategies in this category are grouped into the following
subcategories:
(a) Avoidance strategies, e.g. changing one’s message to avoid
using certain language or to make it more manageable,

65
(b) Compensatory strategies, e.g. organizing one’s message in
order to buy thinking time.
In line with the psycho-social perspective, Nakatani (2006)
developed an oral communication strategy inventory (OCSI)
which consists of two parts. The first part comprises eight
categories of communication strategies for coping with speaking
problems, and the second part comprises seven categories of
communication strategies for coping with listening problems. The
following is a list of these strategies (ibid, pp. 155-157):
(1) Strategies for coping with speaking problems
1.1. Social-affective strategies,
1.2. Fluency-oriented strategies,
1.3. Negotiation for meaning while speaking strategies are
relevant to the participants’ attempts to negotiate with their
interlocutors,
1.4. Accuracy-oriented strategies,
1.5. Message reduction and alteration strategies,
1.6. Nonverbal strategies,
1.7. Message abandonment strategies,
1.8. Thinking in the foreign language strategies.
(2) Strategies for coping with listening problems
2.1. Meaning-negotiation strategies,
2.2. Fluency-maintaining strategies,
2.3. Scanning strategies,
2.4. Getting-the-gist strategies,
2.5. Nonverbal strategies,

66
2.6. Affective strategies,
2.7. Word-oriented strategies.
From a sociolinguistic perspective, Khamwan (2007)
classified communication strategies into four types. These four
types, along with their functions and examples of language, are
displayed in Figure 3.2 below.
Figure 3.2: Khamwan’s classification of interactional strategies
(pp. 16-17)
Strategies Functions Examples of language

1. Appeals for help The learner asks for aid - How do you say…?
by asking an explicit
question concerning a - What do you call…
specific gap in one’s
understanding. in English?

- Could you tell me what

is… called?

-What does the word mean?

2. Repetition requests The learner uses them when - Pardon?


not hearing or
understanding something - Could you say that
properly.
again, please?

- Again, please?

- Again, please

- What?

- Excuse me?

67
(Figure 3.2 Continued)

Strategies Functions Examples of


language

3. Clarification requests The learner requests the - What do you mean


explanation of an unclear by…?
point.
- - Could you explain to
me, what is…?

- I don’t understand__.

- I’m not sure what you


mean by saying
“______”.

- I’m not following you.

- It’s not clear enough


yet.

- Could you make that


clearer, please?

- Could you tell me


more?

4. Comprehension checks The learner asks questions - Am I correct?


to check if understanding
is correct. - Did you say…?

- You said…?

- You mean…?

In consonance with the psycholinguistic perspective, Dobao


and Martínez (2007, p. 90), developed a classification of
communication strategies. This classification consists of two major
types of strategies. These two types, along with their
subcategories, are listed below.
I. Avoidance Strategies

(a) Topic avoidance,

68
(b) Message abandonment,

(c) Semantic avoidance,

(d) Message reduction.

II. Achievement Strategies

(1) Paraphrase

(a) Approximation,

(b) Word coinage,

(c) Circumlocution.

(2) Conscious transfer

(a) Borrowing,

(b) Language switch.

(3) Appeal for assistance

(4) Mime

In conformity with the sociolinguistic perspective, Douglas


(2007, p. 332) developed a very simple classification of
communication strategies. This classification consists of nine
strategies. These strategies are:
(1) Asking for clarification (e.g., what?),
(2) Asking someone to repeat something (e.g., huh? Excuse me?),
(3) Using fillers (e.g., uh, I mean, well,) in order to gain time to
process,
(4) Using conversation maintenance cues (e.g., uh-huh, right, yeah,
okay, hm),

69
(5) Getting someone’s attention (e.g., hey, Say, So),
(6) Using paraphrases for structures one can’t produce,
(7) Appealing for assistance from the interlocutor (to get a word
or phrase, for example),
(8) Using formulaic expressions (at the survival stage) (e.g., how
much does ______ cost? How do you get to the ___?),
(9) Using mime and nonverbal expressions to convey meaning.
In agreement with the psycho-social perspective,
Chuanchaisit and Prapphal (2009, pp. 102-103) categorized
communication strategies into two major types: (1) risk-taking
strategies, and (2) risk-avoidance strategies. The former set of
strategies refers to strategies speakers use to expand their
linguistic resources to achieve communicative goals. These
strategies include:
(1) Social-affective strategies for dealing with emotions and
attitudes,
(2) Fluency-oriented strategies emphasizing speech clarity and
pronunciation,
(3) Accuracy-oriented strategies for paying attention to forms of
speech,
(4) Non-verbal strategies such as giving hints by using gestures
and facial expression,
(5) Help-seeking strategies such as asking for repetition,
clarification or confirmation,
(6) Circumlocution strategies for paraphrasing or describing the
properties of target objects.
The latter set of strategies--risk-avoidance strategies--refers to
strategies speakers use to adjust the message to match their
linguistic resources. These strategies include:

70
(1) Message abandonment strategies for leaving a message
unfinished,
(2) Message reduction and alteration strategies to allow the
substitution of familiar words,
(3) Time-gaining strategies, consisting of gambits or fillers, to
keep the communication channel open and maintain discourse
in times of difficulty.
In line with the sociolinguistic perspective, Nguyet and Mai
(2012) classified communication strategies into four types. These
four types are:
(1) Checking for comprehension,
(2) Confirming,
(3) Asking for clarification,
(4) Using fillers/hesitation devices.
In accordance with the psycho-social perspective, Malasit and
Sarobol (2013, p. 805) developed a taxonomy of communication
strategies that consists of two major types of strategies: (1)
avoidance strategies and (2) compensatory strategies. The
following is a brief description of these strategies along with their
subcategories (See Figure 3.3).
Figure 3.3: Malasit and Sarobol’s taxonomy of communication
strategies (p. 805)
1. Avoidance strategies
1.1. Topic avoidance,

1.2. Message abandonment.

2. Compensatory strategies

71
(Figure 3.3 Continued)

2.1. Intra-actional strategies


2.1.1. Word coinage: Making up a non-existing new word,

2.1.2. Code-switching: Switching to L1 without bothering to translate,

2.1.3. Foreignizing: Adjusting L1 to L2 phonologically and/or morphologically,

2.1.4. Use of non-linguistic means: Replacing a word with non-verbal cues,

2.1.5. Self-repair: Making self -correction of one’s own speech,

2.1.6. Mumbling with inaudible voice,

2.1.7. Use of all-purpose words to extend a general, empty item to the exact word,

2.1.8. Approximation: Substituting the L2 term with the item which shares the same
meaning,

2.1.9. Circumlocution: Describing the properties of the object instead of the exact
target item,

2.1.10. Literal translation: Translating word for word from L1 to L2,

2.1.11. Use of fillers/hesitation devices to gain time to think,

2.1.12. Self-repetition: Repeating words or phrases of one’s own speech,

2.1.13. Other–repetition: Repeating something the interlocutor said to gain time,

2.1.14. Omission: Leaving a gap when not knowing a word or continuing as if it was
understandable.

2.2. Inter-actional strategies


2.2.1. Asking for repetition when having a comprehension difficulty,

2.2.2. Appeal for help: Requesting direct or indirect help from the interlocutor,

2.2.3. Clarification request: Asking for (more) explanation to solve a comprehension


difficulty,

2.2.4. Asking for confirmation: Requesting a confirmation that something is


understood correctly,

2.2.5. Comprehension check: Checking the interlocutor’s understanding,

2.2.6. Expressing non-understanding to show one’s own inability to understand


messages.

72
In summary, it must be noted that there is no consensus
on the classification of communication strategies because
classifications as shown before are organized around various
theoretical perspectives and various research purposes. However,
Bialystok (1990) believes that
the variety of taxonomies proposed in the literature differ
primarily in terminology and overall categorizing
principle rather than in the substance of the specific
strategies. If we ignore, then, differences in the structure
of the taxonomies by abolishing the various overall
categories, then a core group of specific strategies that
appear consistently across the taxonomies clearly
emerges. (p. 61)

3.3. Benefits of communication strategies


The benefits of communication strategies are many and varied.
First and foremost, these strategies are important for the
development of strategic competence which is an important
component of communicative competence. As Savignon (1983)
puts it, “The inclusion of strategic competence as a component of
communicative competence at all levels is important because it
demonstrates that regardless of experience and level of
proficiency one never knows all a language. The ability to cope
within limitations is an ever present component of
communicative competence” (p. 46). Tarone (1984) also argues
that one of the aims of L2 teaching should be the development of
the student´s use of communication strategies as a way of
enhancing their communicative competence. Along the same line,
Dornyei and Thurrell (1991) refers to the crucial importance of
strategic competence for successful communication in the
following way:
Strategic competence is relevant to both L1 and L2, since
communication breakdowns occur and must be

73
overcome not only in a foreign language but in one’s
mother tongue as well. However, since strategic
competence involves strategies to be used when
communication is difficult, it is of crucial importance for
foreign language learners. A lack of strategic competence
may account for situations when students with a firm
knowledge of grammar and a wide range of vocabulary
get stuck and are unable to carry out their communicative
intent. At oral language exams such students may even
fail, and their teachers often cannot comprehend how that
could happen to their ‘best students’. On the other hand,
there are learners who can communicate successfully with
only one hundred words--they rely almost entirely on
their strategic competence. (p.17)
Dornyei and Scott (1997) also conceive communication
strategies to be "the key units in a general description of problem-
management in L2 communication" (p. 179). Wagner and Firth
(1997) echo this point by stating that communication strategies
are a very prominent element in speech production and natural
discourse (p. 342). Specifically, Rababah (2002) asserts that
communication strategies are essential for developing students’
strategic competence in the following way:
All teachers and learners need to understand that
successful language learning is not only a matter of
developing grammatical competence, sociolinguistic
competence and semantic competence, but also strategic
competence which involves the use of CSs and their role
in transmitting and comprehending messages
successfully. (p. 10)
In support of the benefit that teaching communication
strategies can enhance students’ strategic competence, Dornyei
(1995), Nakatani (2010) and Rabab'ah (2015) found
that communication strategies instruction improved students’ oral
communicative ability and strategic competence.

74
Furthermore, communication strategies can “develop
linguistic and sociolinguistic competence in the target
language" (Faerch and Kasper, 1983b, p. 67) because they
“encourage risk-taking and individual initiative and this is
certainly a step towards linguistic and cognitive autonomy”
(Mariani, 1994). They also serve as an excellent means for less
proficient learners to maintain interaction, thus providing them
with opportunities to receive more input of the target language,
which can in turn develop their conversational ability. Faucette
(2001) puts this benefit in the following way:
If learners soon give up without achievement or
interactive strategies at their disposal, then it is unlikely
they will develop their conversational ability. Through CS
[Communication Strategies] use, the channel will remain
open. Hence, learners receive more input, can stay in the
conversation, and develop their ability. Communication
strategies are the means by which learners can act on
Hatch’s (1978) advice that “Finally, and most important,
the learner should be taught not to give up. (p. 6)
In addition to keeping a conversation going and ensuring
more input for students, de Quesada (2009) adds that
communication strategies are also an important vehicle that
produces pushed output, and this can in turn develop
communicative ability, foster language acquisition and increase
fluency and ability to manage conversations more effectively.
Besides, communication strategies are very important for
enhancing students’ self-confidence and building their security
because they help them overcome their communication barriers
and allow them to maneuver in times of difficulty (Dornyei, 1995).
This in turn motivates them to communicate in the
foreign language and to remain in the conversation to achieve
their communicative goals, rather than giving up their
messages. In support of this, some researchers (e.g., Dornyei,

75
1995; Le, 2006; Nakatani, 2005; Kongsom, 2009) found that
communication strategies instruction in the classroom helped
students to communicate more and enhanced their confidence in
speaking in English.
Moreover, communication strategies can develop students’
higher-order thinking skills as they allow them to exchange
thoughts. This in turn broadens their perspectives, sharpens their
own thoughts, turns the language classroom into a community of
thinkers and develops their oral language skills.
Over and above, communication strategies bridge the gap
between classroom and real-life communication and help students
to overcome their communication problems in real life situations.
If students lack these strategies, they will not be able to solve
problems during face-to-face interaction; and as a result, they will
avoid such interaction. Mariani (1994) expresses this benefit in the
following way:
Communication strategies train learners in the flexibility
they need to cope with the unexpected and the
unpredictable. At the same time, they help students get
used to the non-exact communication, which is perhaps
the real nature of all communication. In this way, they
help to bridge the gap between the classroom and the
outside reality, between formal and informal learning.
Finally, communication strategies are particularly useful for
students with language learning disabilities who experience
communication difficulties on a regular basis. This is simply
because these strategies: (a) provide them with an efficient tool for
dealing with knowledge gaps emerging in talk exchanges, thus
affording them a sense of security in the language and extra room
to maneuver in times of difficulty and (b) increase their

76
confidence to communicate in English (Doqaruni and Yaqubi,
2011). In support of this, Lam (2010) found that low-proficiency
students did benefit from teaching communication strategies: “(a)
reporting consistent increases in their frequency and variety of
use of the whole range of target strategies, using consistently more
resourcing to help them with ideas and language, and
demonstrating enhanced ability to reflect on and evaluate their
performance; and (b) making greater improvements, especially in
the English score, in group discussion tasks than the high-
proficiency students” (p. 23-24).

3.4. A model for teaching communication


strategies to students with communication
disabilities
Many communication scholars and researchers (e.g., Dornyei,
1995; Dornyei and Thurrell, 1991; Manchón, 1988; Tarone, 1984)
agree that students with oral communication disabilities should be
trained to be effective communicators and that effective
communication strategies need to be taught to those students to
improve their communication skills. They further agree that if
students are not taught effective communication strategies, they
will rely on the strategies that do not work well, such as
borrowing from Ll and avoidance strategies. Therefore, the
author developed a model for teaching communication strategies
to students with oral communication disabilities. This model is
based on Wood, Bruner, and Ross's concept of scaffolding, Long’s
interaction hypothesis and the social cognitive theory. That is,
the gradual release of responsibility from the teacher to the
student lies at the heart of this model. To make this happen, the
model proceeds through these four steps: (1) direct instruction of
communication strategies, (2) application of communication
strategies in teacher-student interaction, (3) application of
communication strategies in student-student interaction, and (4)
self-assessment. These four steps are discussed in detail in the next
sections.

77
3.4.1. Direct instruction of communication strategies
In this step, the teacher raises students' awareness of one or more
of the communication strategies at a time over a semester, school
year, or course. S/he makes them aware of how, when, and why
this/these strategy/ies are used to facilitate oral communication.
S/he also provides students with the necessary words and
expressions of this/these strategy/ies. Finally, s/he involves
students in observing a video of an authentic conversation, and
gets them to identify, categorize, and evaluate the strategy/ies
explained to them before and used by interlocutors in this
conversation. In support of this step, Oxford (1990) states that
“[r]esearch shows that strategy training which fully informs the
learner (by indicating why the strategy is useful, how it can be
transferred to different tasks, and how learners can evaluate the
success of this strategy) is more successful than training that does
not” (p. 207).

3.4.2. Application of communication strategies in


teacher-student interaction
Teacher-student interaction is a two-way process in which the
teacher and the students participate in oral exchanges. It involves,
according to Ellis (1990), not only authentic exchanges but also
every oral exchange that occurs in the classroom, including those
that arise in the course of formal drilling. This type of interaction
is based on the concept of scaffolding, introduced by Wood,
Bruner and Ross (1976), within the student's zone of proximal
development (ZPD). In this type of interaction the teacher
interacts with students by using interactional patterns that elicit
students' use of the communication strategies explained to them in
step one. In other words, s/he supports students (within their
ZPD) to develop their strategic competence through the
questions s/he asks, the speech modifications s/he makes, and the
way s/he reacts to student responses and mistakes. S/he also
provides needed language to help them pre-empt communication

78
breakdowns and offers communication strategies to help them
maintain and extend their turns. This in turn can make
communication strategies meaningful and utilizable to the
students.
Moreover, teacher-student interaction can indirectly
contribute to the development of communication strategies
because students can absorb these strategies through observation
of the teacher’s verbal behavior. During this type of
interaction, students notice the gap between the strategies they use
and the strategies used by the teacher. When they notice the gap
and realize that their message is not understood as intended, or
that the teacher is using a different strategy, they can modify their
message and/or strategy accordingly. This in turn leads to the
development of both the processes and outcomes of their
interactions. As Hall and Verplaetse (2000) state, “It is in their
interactions with each other that teachers and students work
together to create the intellectual and practical activities that
shape both the form and the content of the target language as well
as the processes and outcomes of individual development” (p. 10).
More importantly, teacher-student interaction allows the
teacher to continually and informally assess what students
comprehend and express as well as the strategies they employ in
expressing their own thoughts. This in turn allows the teacher to
determine where scaffolding is needed to help students perfect
their use of communication strategies. It also allows teachers to
give feedback to the students to help them maintain interaction
and expand their use of these strategies.
Furthermore, teacher-student interaction is extremely
important for a positive relationship between students and
teachers, which can in turn lead to better learning in general. In
support of this benefit, many researchers (e.g., Christophel, 1990;
Gorham, 1988; Kelley and Gorham, 1988; Rodriguez, Plax, and
Kearney, 1996) found that teachers’ verbal (e.g., giving

79
praise,self-disclosure) and non-verbal (e.g., eye contact, facial
expressions) immediacy behaviors could lessen the
psychological distance between themselves and their students,
leading to greater learning. This positive relationship may even
have a mediating effect for students with developmental
vulnerabilities and insecure maternal attachments. In some cases,
high quality teacher-student interactions provided a “protective
effect” for at-risk students in comparison to similar students
who lacked these interactions (Baker, 2006, cited in Smart, 2009,
p. 11). In addition, Savage (1998) found that students retained
new information better when they interacted with the instructor
by questioning the new information. He further found that when
questions elicited higher cognitive processes students could retain
80% to 85% of new materials.
In sum, the teacher-student interaction plays a key role in
supporting students in attaining a higher level of communication
skills and strategies, which could be impossible if students work
on their own. It also has a positive effect on students’ affective and
cognitive outcomes. Moreover, it gives the instructor an
opportunity to easily and quickly assess students’ communication
skills and strategies. However, for the teacher-student interaction
to harvest its own benefits, the teacher should regard students’
linguistic mistakes as a natural part of the learning process
because students do not want to feel embarrassed in front of their
classmates. “Above all, criticism is usually counter-productive”
(Gipps, 1994, p. 39).
Despite the importance of teacher-student interaction in
scaffolding students’ communication skills and strategies, the
teacher should gradually diminish this scaffolding assistance as
students begin to assume full control of the communication
strategies. S/he should move from this step to the next which is
student-student interaction, where students interact with each
other in pairs or groups to achieve a clear communicative goal.

80
3.4.3. Application of communication strategies in
student-student interaction
In this step, the teacher gives students opportunities to use the
communication strategies in interactional activities to achieve an
authentic communicative goal. In such interactional activities,
students are responsible for selecting and implementing the most
appropriate strategies for performing the activity. Meanwhile, the
teacher acts as a facilitator, offering suggestions
and encouragement while following and observing all of the
interactions. s/he also diagnoses both strengths and weaknesses in
students’ communicative competence, including their
communication strategies to (re)teach weak areas in the direct
instruction step in the future.
Student-student interaction could play a key role in
developing students’ communication skills and strategies. In this
respect, many applied linguists (e.g., Canale and Swain,1980;
Ellis, 1999; Hatch, 1978; Krashen,1988; Long, 1985, 1996; Rivers,
2000) assert that this type of interaction is essential for developing
the learner´s communicative competence of which strategic
competence is an important part because it secures the reception
of comprehensible input and the production of
meaningful output. It also provides opportunities for actual
practice in the use of communication strategies which in turn
improves the use of these strategies because learners learn to use
communication strategies effectively through participating in
communication activities where a real communicative goal has to
be achieved. As students negotiate for meaning and try to produce
comprehensible output, they use communication strategies to
repair breakdowns and misunderstandings during the course of
interaction. To name only a few, they ask for help, repetition,
clarification and confirmation. They also use miming and
nonverbal expressions to convey meaning and fillers to gain time
to think. In this way, student-student interaction increases
students’ chances to use communication strategies

81
in real situations, which in turn develops their strategic
competence. Students also receive feedback directly or indirectly
from their partners on their communication strategies and
language output and then modify the two based on this feedback.
In addition to allowing students to practice and apply
communication strategies, student-student interaction also plays
an important role in developing students’ linguistic competence.
Through interaction, students can absorb new grammatical
forms, words, and expressions, thus expanding their language
ability. In this regard, Hatch (1978) states, “One learns how to do
conversation, one learns how to interact verbally, and out of this
interaction syntactic structures are developed” (p. 404). In the
same vein, Richards and Lockhart (1996) state:
Through interacting with other students in pairs or
groups, students can be given the opportunity to draw on
their linguistic resources in a nonthreatening situation
and use them to complete different kinds of talks. Indeed,
it is through this kind of interaction that researchers
believe many aspects of both linguistic and
communicative competence are developed. (p. 152)
In support of the notion that student-student interaction
develops students’ linguistic competence, Mackey (1999) found a
link between interaction and grammatical development. She
further concluded that “[o]ne of the features that facilitate
language development is learner participation in the interaction”
(p. 573).
Furthermore, student-student interaction is a tremendously
important source, if not the most important, of language use for
foreign learners. This is because it is difficult for those learners to
use the foreign language outside the classroom and because this
type of interaction increases the amount of each student’s
participation time and her/his chances to use the foreign language
for purposes associated with foreign language acquisition.

82
This, in turn, leads to the development of linguistic and strategic
competence. In support of this, Pica and Doughty (1985) found
that
individual students appeared to have more opportunities
to use the target language in group than in teacher-
fronted activities, through either taking more turns or
producing more samples of their interlanguage. Such
opportunities may have had a positive effect on students’
development of linguistic and strategic competence in
giving them practice in hypothesizing about interlanguage
structures which were still at variable levels of accuracy,
or in enhancing their development of second language
fluency. (p. 131)
Moreover, student-student interaction encourages students,
especially introvert ones who are irresolute to talk in front of the
whole class or teachers, to participate in communication activities
using their available language skills. In this non-threatening
atmosphere, students can speak freely and openly without being
afraid of making mistakes. This, according to Dornyei (1995),
encourages students to take risks and use communicative
strategies.
The importance of student-student interaction is not confined
to the language acquisition, but extends to the development of
thought and problem-solving abilities as well. It makes language
learning more thoughtful, involves students in thinking and turns
the language classroom into a community of thinkers, and this in
turn leads to the development of their higher order thinking skills.
This benefit is in line with the Vygotskian perspective which views
the development of language and thought as a result from social
interaction and the growth of the student’s mind as a product of
interaction with other minds. The importance of social interaction
in learning has also been recognized by social cognition theorists
as necessary to the development of higher mental processes. In

83
student-student interaction, students exchange
multiple perspectives, consider these perspectives and select the
best one based on reasoning and evidence. This, in turn, extends
and refines their thinking and decision-making capabilities, and
sharpens their own thought. In support of this benefit, Greene and
Land (2000) found that peer interaction developed reflective
thinking and problem solving skills. Anderson, Howe, Soden,
Halliday and Low (2001) also found that peer interaction
developed students’ critical thinking skills.
Additional advantages of student-student interaction include:
developing students’ self-confidence and social skills, establishing
positive attitudes toward school, fostering motivation for
learning, improving retention of information, valuing students’
past experiences and respecting their abilities, creating a sense of
learning community that reduces learners’ isolation and anxiety,
preparing students to be effective citizens in a democratic society,
and promoting students’ independence (Harmer, 2001; Johnston
and Rogers, 2001).
To conclude, the value of student-student interaction for the
development of students’ communicative competence in general
has been highlighted with communicative language teaching and
with the advent of theories that emphasize the social nature of
language learning. In support of the value of this type of
interaction, Rivers (2000) states that “communication derives
essentially from interaction" (p. xiii). Brown (2001) also states, “In
the era of communicative language teaching, interaction is, in fact,
the heart of communication: it is what communication is all
about” (p. 165). Moreover, Strickland and Shanahan (2004) assert
that "[o]ral language development is facilitated when children
have many opportunities to use language in interactions with
adults and with one another" (p. 76).

4.4. Self-assessment
In this step the teacher involves each student in assessing the
quality of her/his oral performance in relation to the

84
communication strategies s/he employed during student-student
interaction. This step helps each student to draw a profile of
her/his lacks of communication skills and strategies. It also
promotes the learner’s responsibility and independence, as Hunt,
Gow, and Barnes (1989) claim, without learner self-assessment
"there can be no real autonomy" (p. 207). Specifically self-
assessment can help students to:
(1) identify their strengths and weaknesses in
communication,
(2) document their progress,
(3) identify effective language learning strategies and
materials,
(4) become aware of the language learning contexts that
work best for them,
(5) establish goals for future independent learning.
(McNamara and Deane 1995, p. 17)
To make it easy for the student to self-assess her/his own
communicative performance in relation to the communication
strategies s/he has already used, the teacher should provide
her/him with an assessment tool such as the one given in Figure
3.4 below.
Figure 3.4: A self-assessment tool of communication strategies
Name: -------------------------------------. Date: -----------------------.

Topic of Interaction: -------------------------------------------.

_______________________________________________

I used a repertoire of strategies to communicate with other members in my group.

85
(Figure 3.4 Continued)

The strategies I used helped me communicate with other members in my group


more easily and thoughtfully.

The communication strategies I found most useful were:

----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
-------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------.

The communication strategies I found difficult to use were:

----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------.

3.5. Research on teaching communication


strategies to students with learning
disabilities
Many researchers studied communication strategies and their
effect on students' oral communicative ability (e.g., Dornyei, 1995;
Nakatani, 2010; Teng, 2012; Tian and Zhang, 2005). However, the
author found that only one of these studies was conducted with
students with learning disabilities. In this study, Cohen (2011)
examined teacher-student verbal interaction in classrooms of high

86
school students with specific learning difficulties (SpLDs) learning
English as a foreign language. The aims of the study were twofold:
(a) to examine teachers' interactional strategies when teaching a
foreign language to those students and (b) to search for strategies
which appeared to support and activate students' participation in
language learning activities. The study was conducted in
classrooms of low ability groups of high-school students over a
period of four months. The classrooms were composed of
students with SpLDs which affected their ability to acquire proper
literacy skills in English as a foreign language and whose literacy
and communication skills in English were therefore limited. The
main sources of data were transcripts of observation notes and
video-taped classroom interactions. The conceptual framework
for analysis and interpretation of the data included a socio-
cultural model of teacher-student interaction and examination of
deviations from the traditional, restricted Initiation-Response-
Feedback (IRF) classroom sequence. The results suggest that
instructional strategies and the quality of teacher verbal
interaction have a potential to open up and increase learning
opportunities for SpLD students despite their limited literacy and
communication skills.

87
Chapter Four
Teaching Reading Strategies to Students
with Comprehension Disabilities

4.0. Introduction
Reading is a process of constructing meaning from a written text.
As Wixson and Peters (1984) define it, reading is "the process of
constructing meaning through the dynamic interaction among the
reader's existing knowledge, the information suggested by the
written language, and the context of the reading situation” (p. 4).
In the same vein, Durkin (1993) defines it as an “intentional
thinking during which meaning is constructed through
interactions between text and reader” (p. 5). Along the same line,
Harris and Hodges (1995) define reading as “the construction of
the meaning of a written text through a reciprocal interchange of
ideas between the reader and the message in a particular text” (p.
39).
As indicated, the previously-mentioned definitions of reading
have at their core the idea that reading is comprehension and
without comprehension the reader is not truly reading, but
following words on a page from left to right. As Trehearne (2015)
states, “Comprehension is what reading is all about. Decoding
without comprehension is simply word barking—being able to
articulate the word correctly without understanding its meaning”
(p. 423). The definitions of reading also have at their core the idea
that comprehension results from the mental processes and the
strategies the reader employs to interact with the text. Readers
who are successful in understanding what they read use various
strategies to construct meaning from the text and to repair
misunderstanding. In contrast, struggling readers often lack these
strategies and strictly focus on the decoding aspect of reading.
This, in turn, makes them just “word callers” rather than readers
because reading goes beyond decoding words. This problem is
stated by Friend (2005), as cited in Pierangelo and Giuliani (2008),
in the following way:
Reading comprehension refers to a student’s ability to
understand what he or she is reading. Some students with
reading comprehension difficulties are able to read a
passage so fluently that you might assume they were
highly proficient readers. However, when they are asked
questions about what they have read, they have little or
no understanding of the words. Students with this
problem sometimes are referred to as word callers.
(Friend, 2005, cited in Pierangelo and Giuliani, 2008)
Despite the fact that the essence of reading is comprehension
and that comprehension is fundamental to success in education,
most Egyptian students with and without learning disabilities face
many difficulties in EFL reading comprehension. In support of
this, El-Koumy (the author), found that approximately 70% of
secondary school students without learning disabilities and more
than 98% of students with learning disabilities at the secondary
level in Egypt have reading comprehension difficulties in the
following areas:
• Identifying the relationship of each sentence to its predecessor in
the text,
• Identifying relationships between and among paragraphs in the
text,
• Identifying the logical connection between ideas in the text,
• Inferring the author’s purpose for writing the text,
• Inferring ideas that are not explicitly stated in the text,

89
• Inferring the author’s attitude, tone and bias within the text,
• Inferring the author’s assumptions that are not explicitly stated
in the text,
• Identifying similarities and differences among ideas,
• Distinguishing relevant from irrelevant information
• Relating what is being read to background knowledge,
• Identifying the author’s viewpoint,
• Identifying relationships among ideas in the text,
• Comparing and contrasting the main ideas in two texts on the
same topic.
Much of the literature suggests that a prominent cause of the
reading comprehension difficulties for students with reading
disabilities is their lack of appropriate comprehension strategies.
Many reading scholars (e.g., Biancarosa and Snow, 2004; Bos and
Anders, 1990; Fowler, 2003; Roberts, Torgesen, Boardman, and
Scammacca, 2008) agree that students with learning disabilities
who continue to struggle with reading comprehension after the
primary grades lack reading comprehension strategies. Bos and
Anders (1990) put this cause in the following way:
Students with learning disabilities face challenging
reading and learning demands as they move beyond the
primary grades. While many of these students continue to
encounter difficulties with basic reading skills, moving
into the intermediate and secondary grades means they
also need to use a cadre of cognitive and metacognitive
strategies for negotiating informational text. (p. 166)
In the same vein, Thompson (1993) asserts that problems in
comprehension are a result of the lack of instruction in reading
strategies and that students who lack adequate or effective
comprehension strategies necessarily struggle to achieve
comprehension. Likewise, Roberts, Torgesen, Boardman and

90
Scammacca (2008) agree that poor readers cannot use reading
comprehension strategies in order to make sense of what they
read. They state:
Reading well is a demanding task requiring coordination
of a diverse set of skills. Good readers monitor their
understanding by linking new information with prior
learning and, when comprehension breaks down, by
deploying appropriate repair strategies, like adjusting
their reading rate or strategically rereading passages.
Struggling readers, even those with adequate word-level
skills and acceptable fluency, often fail to use these types
of strategies, either because they do not monitor their
comprehension or because they lack the necessary tools to
identify and repair misunderstandings when they occur.
(p 66)
Research also showed that students with learning disabilities
did not apply strategies to help themselves comprehend what they
read (Englert et al., 2009; Englert and Thomas, 1987). More
specifically, research showed that these students experienced
serious difficulties in making inferences (Holmes, 1985), relating
new information with background knowledge (Johnson, Graham,
and Harris, 1997), predicting text ideas, clarifying, and
summarizing (Mastropieri and Scruggs, 1997), guessing the
meaning of difficult words from the context (Meng, 2002) and
applying various strategies in different reading phases, before,
during, and after reading (Gersten, Fuchs, Williams, and Baker,
2001; Mastropieri, Scruggs, Bakken, and Whedon, 1996). In
addition, research showed that students with learning disabilities
could not monitor their own comprehension (Bos, Anders, Filip,
and Jaffe, 1989; Fowler, 2003; Wong, 1994) and had less
metacognitive knowledge and weaker control of their reading
comprehension processes than normal students (Baker and
Brown, 1984; Garner, 1987; Hacker, 1998).

91
What complicates the problem of students’ lack of effective
reading strategies in the Egyptian context is that EFL teachers
themselves lack these strategies and are not prepared to teach
them. The following quote from Kamil, Borman, Dole, Kral,
Salinger, and Torgesen (2008) typically applies to the Egyptian
context:
Most teachers lack the skills to provide direct and explicit
comprehension strategy instruction. Most teacher
education programs do not prepare preservice teachers to
teach strategies. In addition, teachers may find it
particularly challenging to model their own thinking by
providing think aloud of how they use strategies as they
read. Many teachers use various strategies automatically
as they read and are not aware of how they use the
strategies they are teaching. (p. 19)
Another probable cause of the poor reading comprehension of
students with learning disabilities in the Egyptian context is that
EFL teachers always focus on decoding skills, rather than
comprehension. Therefore, Egyptian EFL students often do well
with decoding but struggle with comprehension. In other words,
they decode words individually, but they cannot make sense of
entire paragraphs. In support of the notion that mere decoding
does not lead to comprehension, Schoenbach, Greenleaf, Cziko,
and Hurwitz (1999, cited in Herczog and Porter, 2010, p. 9) are of
the opinion that mere decoding does not guarantee comprehension
in the following way:
Most older students who struggle with reading do not
have decoding problems; they struggle with
comprehension. Consequently, these students do not need
assistance with decoding. In fact, focusing on decoding
skills with these students is counterproductive because it
sends a message that reading is mainly about correct
pronunciation, not understanding content. (p. 9)

92
To help students with learning disabilities overcome their
reading comprehension difficulties, many reading scholars (e.g.,
Bongratz, Bradley, Fisel, Crcutt, and Shoemaker, 2002; Cramer,
Fate, and Lueders, 2001; Lysynchuk, Pressley, and Vye, 1990;
Marchand-Martella and Martella, 2012) recommend
comprehension strategies instruction to enable these students to
select and use appropriate strategies and as such gain better
understanding of what they read. It is also evident from the review
of the previous research that the use of comprehension strategies
as an instructional intervention improves the reading
comprehension of students with learning disabilities. In their
review and synthesis of the research on reading comprehension
interventions for students with learning disabilities, Gersten and
Baker (1999a) recommended that students with learning
disabilities “need to learn an array of strategies to enhance their
understanding of the narrative and expository material they read”
(p. 5). They maintained that successful interventions teach
students “multiple strategies with the goal of having them
internalize the strategies” (loc. cit.). Likewise, in his meta-analysis
of the reading comprehension interventions conducted between
1985 and 2005 for students with learning disabilities, Sencibaugh
(2005) concludes:
Impressive gains in reading for students with learning
disabilities are possible (Torgesen et al., 2001; Vaughn et
al., 2002) especially if the instructional process utilizes
strategy instruction to assist the students with organizing
the material. As revealed in the results of this study,
strategy instruction strongly impacts the reading
comprehension of students with learning disabilities
based on the notion that students with learning
disabilities are inactive learners with metacognitive
deficits (Deshler, Ellis, & Lenz, 1996); therefore, they
benefit greatly from training in such strategies as
activating prior knowledge and organizing and
summarizing text (Mastropieri & Scruggs, 1997). (p. 11)

93
Moreover, in her review of the reading comprehension
interventions for students with learning/reading disabilities
between 2006 and 2011, Scott, (2012) concludes that “extensive
research has shown large effect sizes on reading comprehension of
students with learning disabilities and reading difficulties when
the students were given explicit instruction in comprehension
strategies” (p. 25). Over and above, research on text
comprehension demonstrates that students with learning
disabilities can be taught to use comprehension strategies (e.g.,
Bakken, Mastropieri, and Scruggs, 1997; Englert and Mariage,
1991; Gardill and Jitendra, 1999; Johnson, Graham, and Harris,
1997; Nelson, Smith, and Dodd, 1992). Therefore, the present
chapter will deal with reading strategies from all aspects. It will
also detail a four-step model for teaching these strategies and
review research on their impact on the reading comprehension of
students with learning difficulties.

4.1. Definition of reading comprehension


strategies
Reading comprehension strategies are specific procedures used by
readers to help them make sense of written texts. Pani (2004)
defines these strategies as “the mental operations involved when
readers approach a text effectively to make sense of what they
read”. For Kamil et al. (2008), “Comprehension strategies are
routines and procedures that readers use to help them make sense
of texts” (p. 16). Likewise, Pilonieta (2010) defines comprehension
strategies as the “conscious, deliberate, and flexible plans readers
use and adjust with a variety of texts to accomplish specific goals”
(p. 152). According to Trehearne (2015), “Comprehension
strategies are conscious plans—sets of steps that good readers use
to make sense of text when reading” (p. 446). Similarly, the
Alabama Department of Education (2015) defines comprehension
strategies as “specific actions that readers use as they attempt to

94
make sense of text” (p. 68). In essence, reading strategies are
conscious procedures that help readers to comprehend what they
read and to repair breakdowns in comprehension. Thus, reading
comprehension strategies are a means to an end rather than the
end itself.

4.2. Classification of reading comprehension


strategies
Reading comprehension strategies have been classified differently
by different reading scholars. Some scholars (e.g., Levin and
Pressley, 1981; Palincsar and Brown, 1984; Paris, Lipson, and
Wixson, 1983; Paris, Wasik, and Turner, 1991; Pressley, 2000;
Vaughn and Linan-Thompson, 2004) classified reading strategies
on the basis of the order they take place, i. e. pre-reading, while-
reading and post-reading strategies. The pre-reading
comprehension strategies include, but are not limited to, setting
goals for reading, activating prior knowledge by thinking about
what is already known about the topic, making predictions,
previewing, asking questions based on the title of the text, and
skimming. The while-reading strategies include, but are not
limited to, guessing meaning of words from the context,
identifying the main idea of a paragraph, identifying supporting
details, visualizing, making connections among important ideas,
resolving comprehension difficulties, underlining important
portions of the passage, comprehension monitoring, clarification,
making inferences, and note-taking. The post-reading strategies
include, but are not limited to, summarizing, reviewing,
elaborating, re-reading key points, and evaluating.
Some other reading scholars (e.g., Abbott, 2006, 2010; Eskey,
2005; Lee-Thompson, 2008; Rao, 2003) classified reading
strategies on the basis of the bottom-up and top-down processing
theories into two groups: bottom-up (local) strategies and top-
down (global) strategies. “Bottom-up reading comprehension
strategies are data-driven, whereas top-down strategies are

95
conceptually or hypothesis-driven” (Abbott, 2010, p. 15). The
bottom-up strategies require readers to break down texts into
their most basic elements of meaning. They include, but are not
limited to, dividing unknown words into their component
morphemes and analyzing each morpheme to identify the meaning
of these words, analyzing sentences in order to determine what is
happening in them and how they relate to neighboring sentences,
scanning the text for specific details or explicitly stated
information, vocalizing, rereading, and translating. In contrast,
the top-down strategies are holistic and all require readers to
combine and synthesize different pieces of information. They
include, but are not limited to, predicting, previewing, planning,
paraphrasing, using background knowledge to speculate beyond
the text, hypothesizing, taking notes, formulating questions,
summarizing, monitoring comprehension, identifying problems,
and evaluating strategy use and performance
On the basis of the cognitive theory of learning, some other
reading scholars (e.g., Aghaei and Pillaie, 2011; Fowler, 2003)
classified reading strategies into two general categories: cognitive
strategies and metacognitive strategies. The cognitive strategies
include previewing, making predictions, translating, summarizing,
linking with prior knowledge or experience, and guessing meaning
from contexts; whereas the metacognitive strategies include self-
regulation, planning, self-monitoring, and self-assessment.
On the basis of the socio- and psycho-cognitive theories of
learning, some researchers (e.g., Fotovatian and Shokrpour, 2007;
Zeynali, Zeynali, and Motlagh, 2015) incorporated the socio-
affective strategies into their classification of the reading
strategies. According to them, the socio-affective strategies
represent a broad group that involves either interaction with
another person or regulation of emotions, motivation and attitude
toward reading. This category consists of strategies like
cooperative reading, asking questions for clarification, and self-
talk.

96
The difference in the previously mentioned categories of
comprehension strategies is a result of the distinct frameworks
that were used to classify these strategies. However, the previously
mentioned categories can be complementary because students with
and without reading disabilities need to apply various cognitive
and metacognitive strategies before, during, and after reading.
They also need a repertoire of strategies to read strategically with
comprehension. Therefore, the next section will explore the
literature around which strategies to include in this repertoire.

4.3. Reading comprehension strategies that work


As cited in the previous section, the reading comprehension
strategies are many; therefore, it is important here to decide what
reading comprehension strategies to teach. In this respect, some
reading scholars (e. g., Cunningham and Allington, 1994;
Trehearne, 2015) identify reading comprehension strategies that
are worth teaching on the basis of theoretical analyses of reading
comprehension. Cunningham and Allington (1994), for example,
suggest what they call “necessary strategies” that readers need to
use when reading. These strategies are: calling up relevant
background knowledge, predicting what will be learned and what
will happen, making mental pictures, self-monitoring and self-
correction, using fix-up strategies such as re-reading or asking for
help, determining the most important ideas and events and seeing
how they are related, questioning; drawing conclusions and
making inferences, comparing and contrasting what is being read
and what is known, figuring out unknown words and
summarizing what has been read.
Along the same line, Hollas (2002) suggests that a strategic
reader in the content areas employs seven reading strategies.
These strategies are: (1) predicting, (2) visualizing, (3) connecting,
(4) questioning, (5) clarifying, (6) summarizing and (7) evaluating.
In the same vein, Trehearne (2015) recommends eight strategies that

97
work for reading comprehension. These strategies are
(1) monitoring comprehension, (2) using mental imagery, (3) using
visual representation of text, (4) using prior
knowledge/predicting, (5) summarizing/retelling, (6) using text
structure, (7) generating questions, and (8) answering questions.
Other reading scholars (e.g., Palincsar and Brown, 1984;
Pressley, Johnson, Symons, McGoldrick, and Kurita, 1989;
Pressley, Levin, and Ghatala, 1984; Swanson and De La Paz, 1998)
identify reading comprehension strategies that work based on
research evidence. According to these scholars effective strategies
are those strategies supported by research evidence and only
strategic procedures that enjoy clear scientific support should be
recommended to teachers. They further emphasize that such
recommended strategies must have proven their worth in studies
that permit cause-and-effect conclusions. Swanson and De La Paz
(1998), for example, point out that each recommended strategy
should have been formally evaluated and found to be effective for
improving learners' reading comprehension. They add that the
compilation of the reading strategies should be inspired by
contemporary reading research and recurrent strategies in explicit
strategy instruction programs.
Based upon research-based evidence, Palincsar and Brown
(1984) identified four important reading strategies for teaching
reading comprehension to special needs students. These strategies
are: predicting, summarizing, clarifying, and asking questions.
The teaching of these four strategies is known as reciprocal
teaching in the literature. They (Palincsar and Brown) conducted
a series of studies in which they taught special education middle-
school students to use these strategies over an extended period of
time. These studies revealed that the teaching of this repertoire of
strategies had beneficial effects on the reading performance of
special needs students.

98
In the same vein, Pressley, Johnson, Symons, McGoldrick, and
Kurita (1989) surveyed and reviewed relevant experimental
studies that demonstrated the potency of a range of reading
comprehension strategies. They identified summarization,
representational- and mnemonic-imagery, story grammar,
question-generation, question-answering, prior-knowledge
activation strategies and making inferences as being supported by
substantial evidence base.
By the same token, based on an analysis of more than 200
published studies from the past two decades, the National Reading
Panel (2000) found eight comprehension strategies that were most
effective and most promising for instruction. These strategies are:
comprehension monitoring, cooperative learning, graphic and
semantic organizers, story mapping, question answering, question
generation, summarizing, and multiple strategies. In addition to
these strategies, the National Reading Panel found varying degrees
of scientific research support for several additional strategies,
including activating and using prior knowledge, and mental
imagery and mnemonics.
Likewise, in their review of the effective practices for
developing reading comprehension, Duke and Pearson (2002)
identified six individual comprehension strategies that research
suggests for developing reading comprehension. These strategies
are prediction, think-aloud, text structure, visual representations,
summarization, and questioning. Furthermore, in their review of
the essential elements of fostering and teaching reading
comprehension, Duke, Pearson, Strachanm, and Billman (2011)
point out that the list of strategies that research indicated are
worth teaching–that is, if taught, they improve reading
comprehension--varies from one research review to another but
often includes the following:
• Setting purposes for reading,
• Previewing and predicting,
• Activating prior knowledge,

99
• Monitoring, clarifying, and fixing,
• Visualizing and creating visual representations,
• Drawing inferences,
• Self-questioning and thinking aloud,
• Summarizing and retelling. (p. 64)
In essence, with the fact in mind that different terminologies
are sometimes used for the same strategy, the author can say that
reading scholars and reading researchers seem to agree to a great
extent on the effective strategies that should be taught for
improving reading comprehension.

4.4. Benefits of reading comprehension


strategies
The importance of reading comprehension strategies is widely
recognized in the literature. First and foremost, these strategies
are necessary for meaningful learning and understanding from
text to occur. As Alexander and Jetton (2000) point out,
“Strategies are essential tools in learning. It is unfathomable to
expect meaningful learning from text to occur without some
evidence of strategic processing. Likewise, when readers employ
strategies efficiently and effectively, these procedures are
facilitative, promoting deeper and better understanding” (p. 295).
Similarly, Thompson (2000) puts this benefit as follows:
Comprehension strategies are useful in helping students
in the preparing, organizing, elaborating, rehearsing, and
monitoring of text as it is being read. Student should be
taught how to use comprehension strategies and
typographical signals to understand the author's intended
message. Teaching strategies will not only help students
develop comprehension, but develop other attributes as
skillful readers. (p. 6)

100
In the same vein, Gooden (2012) asserts that “[i]nstruction in
comprehension strategies helps children become flexible thinkers
who can approach a variety of texts with a repertoire of strategies,
thus helping them to better comprehend those texts” (p. 17). In
support of this benefit, many studies (e.g., Baker and Brown, 1984;
Bereiter and Bird, 1985; Pressley et al., 1989; Rosenshine and
Meister, 1994; Rosenshine, Meister, and Chapman, 1996) showed
that when readers received comprehension strategy instruction
instead of conventional comprehension instruction, they improved
significantly on reading comprehension measures. Various studies
(e.g., Arabsolghar and Elkins, 2001; Dreyer, 1998; Kozminsky and
Kozminsky, 2001) also showed a positive relationship between
reading strategy use and reading comprehension.
In addition, the use of reading comprehension strategies helps
students absorb grammar, sentence structure, and discourse
structure as they occur in meaningful authentic contexts. Students
thus gain a more complete picture of the ways in which the
elements of reading work together to convey meaning. It is also
argued that vocabulary is learned through context. Furthermore,
comprehension strategies instruction can promote self-regulation
in learners, foster independent reading, and lay the foundation for
students to become lifelong readers, not simply school time
readers. This in turn can positively influence their self-efficacy
beliefs and encourage them to participate as thoughtfully literate
members of our complex world.
Furthermore, reading comprehension strategies instruction is
especially beneficial for students with learning disabilities as it
enables them to become aware of their reading processes and
offers avenues for improving their reading comprehension. Ruffin
(2009) expresses this benefit in the following way:
Students with learning disabilities often experience
deficits in comprehension; therefore, reading
comprehension strategies are relevant. Reading without
comprehension seems pointless and not reading
strategically or employing a technique to monitor

101
comprehension is likely to add frustration and anxiety to
the reader causing significant difficulty with
understanding in the reading process. Students with
learning disabilities must find meaningful ways to
complete the task of gaining understanding from written
text, and reading comprehension strategies offer avenues
for improving or increasing reading comprehension. (p.
24)
Likewise, Pilonieta (2010) argues that “[i]nstruction in
comprehension strategies is particularly important for struggling
readers as they are unlikely to discover these strategies on their
own” (p. 152). In support of this benefit, many studies provided
evidence that struggling readers improved their reading skills
through training in reading comprehension strategies (For a
review of these studies, see sections 4.6.4. and 4.7.6. of this
chapter).
4.5. A model for teaching reading
comprehension strategies to students with
reading disabilities
Reading comprehension strategies do not seem to come naturally
or easily to students, particularly those with learning disabilities.
They are “neither easy nor automatic” (Pintrich, 1999, p. 7).
Therefore, learners need the teacher’s support, with a
gradual reduction in the amount of this support. Eventually this
support should be removed when they are able to apply the
strategies without assistance. In this light, the author developed a
four-step model for teaching and applying comprehension
strategies on the basis of Wood et al.'s concept of scaffolding
within Vygotsky’s zone of proximal development. These steps are:
(1) teacher modeling, (2) peer modeling, (3) independent use of the
strategies in action, and (4) self-assessment. These four steps are
discussed in detail in the next sections.

102
4.5.1. Teacher modeling of reading comprehension
strategies
Teacher modeling lies at the heart of teaching learning strategies
in general and reading strategies in particular. In this step, the
teacher assumes the major responsibility for instruction by
offering a demonstration of the application of the targeted
strategies in a real context. In doing so, s/he thinks aloud to make
the mental processes underlying reading comprehension strategies
overt, and to help students gain insights into the decisions of
experienced readers including: deciding which strategies to use to
perform the task at hand and the way they should be used.
Meanwhile, the students observe how the teacher uses the
strategies and listen to her/his thinking.
The modeling of reading strategies is recommended by many
scholars and researchers because it makes strategies concrete and
overt and promotes student engagement when they see teachers
practice what they preach. As Schoenbach, Greenleaf, Cziko, and
Hurwitz (1999) point out, teacher modeling can demystify the
reading process by explaining the behind-the-scenes thinking
required for good comprehension. They add that during modeling,
the teacher should offer comments on the strategies s/he uses to
work through the material. Similarly, Lenz, Ellis, and Scanlon
(1996) suggest that teachers should model the strategy more than
once and involve students in discussing the steps of the strategy
during modeling. They add that discussion will help the teacher
determine how well the students understand when and where they
should use the strategy, as well as the steps involved in the
strategy. Likewise, Blair, Rupley, and Nichols (2007) point out that
teacher modeling should go beyond showing the student what to
do and involve discussions of the steps in a process. They maintain
that such discussions allow the students to get inside the teacher's
mind so that they can develop similar strategies and apply them in
their own learning.

103
For teacher modeling to achieve its goals, the teacher should
model various strategies in different contexts, before, during, and
after reading. In support of this, an analysis of proficient readers'
behavior revealed that skilled reading does not involve the use of a
single potent strategy but the coordination of multiple strategies
(Brown, Pressley, Van Meter, and Schuder, 1996). Effective
modeling also “goes well beyond merely presenting the steps in a
strategy. It provides students with the "why" and "how" of
various strategy steps. It also demonstrates that student effort is
essential, and shows that strategy use results in better
performance” (Department of Special Education and
Communication Disorders, 2016). Sturomski (1997) adds that for
modeling to be effective, the teacher and the students should
collaborate to use the strategy; the more students and teachers
collaborate to use the strategy, the more students will internalize
the strategy in their strategic repertoire. Moreover, teachers must
be aware of their own cognitive process during reading, and must
be strategic readers and thinkers themselves in order to be
effective instructors of comprehension strategies (Sturomski,
1997). Over and above, teacher modeling should be temporary not
permanent just like scaffolding in the area of constructive
engineering. As Nunan and Bailey (2009) put it, “The scaffold is
there to help the workers reach the problem areas or unfinished
areas that need attention. When those areas have been dealt with,
the scaffolding is removed. It is an intentionally temporary
structure” (p. 178).

4.5.2. Peer modeling of reading comprehension


strategies
After modeling the strategies one or two times, the teacher should
involve students in modeling these strategies to each other. In this
step the teacher relinquishes control and encourages students to
model the reading strategies alternately to each other while
reading a new text. This takes place in the form of a reciprocal
dialogue in which the participants take turns assuming the role of

104
a teacher in leading the dialogue about a part of this text. In such a
dialogue, one of the students models the reading strategies to the
other and both share the responsibility for their own learning.
This, in turn, increases their chances to observe and use reading
strategies in real situations. In this step, the teacher takes on a
facilitator role. S/he also observes and assesses students’ use of
strategies, paying particular attention to the strategies students are
using (or not using) to determine where scaffolding is needed to
help students perfect their uses of the comprehension strategies
and to follow-up with appropriate instructional activities.
While peer modeling, Sturomski (1997) suggests that students
should be called upon to think aloud to reveal what goes on in
their minds as they read and the strategy being used to
comprehend what they read because comprehension occurs in
one’s head, and thinking-aloud helps to remove the mystery of
what readers do and makes the covert overt. He (Sturomski) adds
that the more learners work together to use the strategy, the more
they will internalize the strategy in their strategic repertoire.
In addition to helping students better understand and apply
new strategies, the reciprocal dialogue in which peer modeling
takes place allows them to construct meaning from the text and to
listen to each other’s interpretations, thereby assisting each other
in attaining a higher level of understanding which will be
impossible if students work individually. As Tovani (2004) points
out, "Good readers use talk and collaboration with peers to extend
their thinking about text" (p. 98). Peterson and Eeds (1990) also
assert that:
Dialogue puts forward a new story line, puts events and
relationships into a new light. Our basis for interpreting
the text is broadened. In working together to disclose a
deeper level of meaning, each participant's imagination is
enriched and the potential for meaning construction is
expanded. (p. 29)

105
The reciprocal dialogue also involves students in the
negotiation of meaning which is one of the keys to promoting
understanding. As Brown and Campione (1986) state,
“Understanding is more likely to occur when a student is required
to explain, elaborate or defend his or her position to others; the
burden of explanation is often the push needed to make him or her
evaluate, integrate, and elaborate knowledge in new ways” (p.
1066).
Moreover, participation in the peer-led dialogue plays a key
role in developing students’ communication skills and fostering
their self-confidence and self-efficacy. It also provides the
opportunity for greater amounts of student verbalization, which
can promote students’ linguistic competence. Moreover, it involves
linking to other ideas and providing evidence from the text to
support one’s thinking, which can in turn develop students’
critical thinking and resolve the conflicts between the students’ old
beliefs and the new information gained from text. In support of the
effectiveness of peer modeling for scaffolding reading strategies,
Palincsar, Brown, and Martin (1987) found that peer interaction
resulted in equal gains in reading comprehension comparable to
interactions between the teacher and the students. Furthermore,
Fuchs, Fuchs, and Kazdan (1999) found a statistically significant
positive effect of peer-assisted learning strategies on the reading
comprehension of high school students with serious reading
problems. Chinn, Anderson, and Waggoner (2001) also found that
text-based discussion emphasizing collaborative reasoning
increased higher level thinking and overall reading engagement
more than recitation styles of interaction (e.g., Initiate-Respond-
Evaluate). Similarly, Van den Branden (2000) found that primary-
grade students who engaged in conversation around texts had
higher comprehension than those who did not collaboratively
negotiate meaning. She concluded that higher comprehension may
have resulted from the challenges of explaining oneself to others or
the collaborative effort to repair breakdowns in comprehension.

106
4.5.3. Independent use of the strategies in action
Independent use of the strategies in action occurs once the teacher
is convinced that students can use the strategies on their own
(Kamil et al., 2008). In this step, each student applies the
comprehension strategies independently in reading a new text; and
the teacher completely releases the responsibility to each student
to use the strategies on her/his own. That is, each student actually
practices the strategies s/he has been taught in the previous steps
(teacher and peer modeling) on her/his own, without the help of
others. While so doing, the teacher moves among individuals to
make sure that each student is actually applying the strategies in
action. The teacher also assesses students’ use of strategies, paying
particular attention to the strategies they are using (or not using)
to determine where scaffolding is needed to help students perfect
their uses of the comprehension strategies and to follow-up with
appropriate instructional activities.

4.5.4. Self-assessment
In this step, each student assesses her/his own comprehension in
relation to the reading strategies s/he employed before, during and
after reading. This step is necessary for the student to know
whether or not comprehension has occurred; and whether or not
the strategies s/he employed before, during and after reading were
effective. It also develops the student’s ability to self-reflect and to
become more aware of and insightful about her/his use of reading
strategies. This, in turn, helps the student to use the strategies
effectively and to change or modify the strategy which does not
work.
To make it easy for the student to self-assess her/his own
reading comprehension in relation to the reading strategies s/he
has already used, the teacher should provide her/him with an
assessment tool. If such a tool is developed in collaboration with
students, they will gain a clearer understanding of how to use it
and develop a sense of its ownership. The teacher should provide

107
an opportunity for each student to use this tool after the
independent use of the strategies in action. S/he should also read
the student’s responses to this tool and discuss them with her/him
in individual conferences. If the teacher has no time to develop a
tool for self-assessment, s/he can select or adapt an existing one,
such as the ones presented in the figures below.
Figure 4.1: A self-assessment tool of reading strategies (Adapted
from Williamson, McMunn, and Reagan, 2004, p. 17)
Name: -----------------------------------------. Date: ---------------------.

Title of the reading passage: -------------------------------------------.

-------------------------------------------------------

I used a repertoire of comprehension strategies with the text I have read.

The strategies I used helped me understand what I read.

The reading strategies made me read more thoughtfully.

The reading strategies I found most useful were:

---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
--------------------------------------------------------------------------.

The reading strategies I found difficult to use were:

---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
--------------------------------------------------------------------------.

108
Figure 4.2: A self-assessment checklist of reading strategies
(SABES, 2008, p. 10)

Name: -----------------------.

Date: --------------------------.

Check () the box that indicates how you read.

Reading Strategies Often Sometimes Never

1. I think about what I already know


about the topic.

2. I make predictions and read to find out


if I was right.

3. I reread the sentences before and after


the word I do not know.

4. I ask another student for help.

5. I look for the main idea.

6. I take notes.

7. I discuss what I read with others.

109
(Figure 4.2 Continued)
8. I stop and summarize.

9. I choose material to read on my own.

10. I make outlines of what I read.

To conclude this section, there are two important


considerations that should be taken into account when using the
author’s model for teaching reading comprehension strategies to
students with reading comprehension disabilities. These
considerations are: (1) reading comprehension requires the
orchestration of a repertoire of strategies, and (2) reading
strategies instruction must not occur in isolation from context.
Each of these considerations is discussed in more details below.
(1) Reading comprehension requires the orchestration of a
repertoire of strategies. Successful readers apply a wide range
of strategies before, during, and after reading to comprehend
what they read. In other words, they use a collection of
strategies and do not rely on one or two strategies. They also
know how to use different strategies at different points in a
text and how to switch strategies as the text demands. As
Williamson, McMunn, and Reagan (2004) put it:
Before, during, and after reading, effective readers
apply multiple strategies flexibly, selectively,
independently, and reflectively. For example, they
identify purposes in reading, make predictions, and
check them as they read, and they ask insightful,
reflective questions about what they are reading.

110
They have a number of strategies they use and do not
over rely upon one or two strategies. They also know
how to use different strategies in different contexts.
(p. 6)
Therefore, the teacher should teach learners a repertoire
of strategies that complete and support each other. Although
each strategy may be taught singly, the teacher should model
and demonstrate the use of a repertoire of strategies while
reading authentic texts. In other words, s/he should teach
students how to integrate and use multiple strategies flexibly.
Along this line of thinking, Grabe (2009) states that “the
combination of strategic responses to texts appears to be more
effective in supporting comprehension development” (p. 445).
Likewise, Gooden (2012) states, “Instruction in
comprehension strategies helps children become flexible
thinkers who can approach a variety of texts with a repertoire
of strategies, thus helping them to better comprehend those
texts” (p. 17). In essence, comprehension requires a repertoire
of strategies from which students can draw during
independent reading. The more strategies a reader has at her
or his disposal, the more likely she or he is to interact
meaningfully with a given text.
In support of the development of a repertoire of reading
strategies, research indicates that successful readers apply a
wide range of strategies to comprehend what they read and
that the ability to coordinate and make associations among
various reading strategies is a major distinction between good
and poor readers. In an analysis of proficient readers' reading
behavior, Brown et al. (1996), for example, found that skilled
reading did not involve the use of a single potent strategy but
the coordination of multiple strategies.
Moreover, research showed that teaching a variety of
strategies was more effective than individual strategy
instruction (e.g., Brown, 2008; Duke and Pearson, 2002;
Pressley and Afflerbach, 1995; Reutzel, Smith, and Fawson,

111
2005; Sporer, Brunstein, and Kieschke, 2009). Reutzel, Smith,
and Fawson, (2005), for example, found that introducing a set
of strategies briefly and then quickly moving students to
applying or juggling multiple strategies simultaneously was
more effective than spending several weeks focusing on a
single strategy. In addition, studies and reviews of integrated
methods/activities for strategy instruction, such as reciprocal
teaching, revealed that teaching students a repertoire of
strategies from which they could draw during independent
reading could lead to improving the reading comprehension of
students with and without reading disabilities (e.g., Sporer,
Brunstein, and Kieschke, 2009).
From a review of literature on reading strategy
instruction, the National Reading Panel (2000) supports the
same consideration in the following way:
Reading requires the coordinated and flexible use of
several different kinds of strategies. Considerable
success has been found in improving comprehension
by instructing students on the use of more than one
strategy during the course of reading. Skilled reading
involves an ongoing adaptation of multiple cognitive
processes….Being strategic is much more than
knowing the individual strategies. When faced with a
comprehension problem, a good strategy user will
coordinate strategies and shift strategies as it is
appropriate to do so. (p. 4-47)
Numerous reading researchers also agree that teaching
repertoires of strategies improves reading comprehension and
recall of information from texts. The National Reading Panel’s
report on effective instructional practices, for example,
demonstrated the value and usefulness of teaching a variety of
reading comprehension strategies to students of all ages as
follows:

112
The empirical evidence reviewed favors the
conclusion that teaching of a variety of reading
comprehension strategies leads to increased learning
of the strategies, to specific transfer of learning, to
increased memory and understanding of new
passages, and, in some cases, to general
improvements in comprehension. (NRP, 2000, p. 4-
51)
In the same vein, on the basis of their review of research
evidence from many studies, Kamil et al. (2008) concluded
that “multiple-strategy training results in better
comprehension than single-strategy training” (p. 17).
(2) Reading strategies instruction must not occur in isolation from
context. It should be integrated into the regular classroom
activities. More specifically, the teacher should integrate
reading strategies instruction with the objectives, tasks, and
materials used in the regular reading program because this
facilitates the transfer of strategies to similar tasks, makes the
strategies more meaningful, allows learners to see how these
strategies can be applied to real life reading, and assists them
in becoming autonomous readers. Along this line of thinking,
many practitioners and researchers recommend teaching
learning strategies in contexts that are relevant and
appropriate for their use. Ehrman, Leaver, and Oxford
(2003), for example, state, “A given learning strategy is neither
good nor bad; it is essentially neutral until it is considered in
context” (p. 315). Further, Oxford (1993) suggests that the
strategies chosen should mesh with and support each other,
whilst fitting the requirements of the language task, the
learners' goals, and their styles of learning. In the same vein,
O'Malley (1987) asserts that strategy training should be
interwoven into regular L2 activities and be undertaken over a
long period of time (a semester or a year) rather than taught
as a separate, short intervention.

113
Bearing in mind the previously mentioned considerations, it
appears that multiple reading strategies instruction in authentic
reading situations will be highly effective for students with and
without reading disabilities. In this light, the next two sections will
address an activity and a method that take these two
considerations into account (i.e., directed reading-thinking activity
and reciprocal teaching).

4.6. Directed reading-thinking activity


4.6.1. Definition of the directed reading-thinking
activity
The directed reading-thinking activity (DR-TA) is a
comprehension activity that guides students in making predictions
about a text, and then reading to confirm or refute their
predictions. The creator of the DR-TA method, Russell Stauffer,
defines it as a cycle that students go through when they read. This
cycle starts out by having students predict, then read a paragraph
of a text and evaluate their predictions, make new predictions and
read another paragraph and evaluate their predictions, and so on.

4.6.2. Benefits of the directed reading-thinking


activity
Much information has been written about the directed reading-
thinking activity and its importance for improving reading
comprehension in general. According to Tierney, Readence, and
Dishner (1995), the DR-TA is a method for building independent
readers. Similarly, Richardson and Morgan (1997) state that the
DR-TA engages students in higher order thinking skills and that
these skills include: making connections between interrelated
elements of the text, justifying thought processes and drawing
logical conclusions. They maintain that these skills can set the
pathway toward independent reading, foster learner responsibility
and improve reading comprehension.

114
Furthermore, Jennings and Shepherd (1998) state that the
DR-TA helps students become aware of reading strategies,
understand the reading process, and develop prediction skills.
They add that this activity stimulates students' thinking and
makes them listen to the opinions of others and modify their own
in light of additional information. By the same token, El-Koumy
(2006) states that the DR-TA engages students in higher-order
thinking about what they read and involves them in using a
repertoire of reading comprehension strategies. These strategies
include: (1) setting purposes for reading through predictions and
activation of content schemata; (2) generating and answering
questions before and during reading to confirm, disconfirm or
extend predictions; (3) self-monitoring (i.e., use of predictions to
monitor comprehension); (4) clarifying (i.e., making text less
confused and more comprehensible through peer and/or group
discussion of predictions); (5) inferring (i.e., reading between the
lines to confirm or disconfirm predictions); and (6) evaluation
(i.e., assessing the goodness of predictions through peer- and/or
self-assessment). The application of this repertoire of strategies
can improve reading comprehension, recall of information from
texts, and higher-order thinking skills.
Along the same line, Tankersley (2005) states that the DR-TA
extends reading to higher-order thought processes and provides
teachers with a great deal about each student's thought processes,
prior knowledge and thinking skills. Moreover, many educators
believe that the DR-TA helps students to interact with
information, to read more actively and enthusiastically, and to
assess their own comprehension. It is also useful for processing all
types of text and fostering students’ reflective and analytical
thinking skills. Finally, the DR-TA can be easily adapted for a
variety of subjects and reading levels.

4.6.3. Procedures of the directed reading-thinking


activity
Based on an extensive review of the relevant literature and his
personal experience, the author suggests the following eight steps

115
for modeling the DR-TA in the EFL classroom:
1. The teacher writes the title of the reading passage on the board
and asks students to read it,
2. The teacher asks students to make predictions about the title
using the following questions:
a. What do you think a passage with a title like this might be
about?
b. Why do you think so?
3. The teacher lists predictions on the board and initiates a
discussion with some of the students by asking them to respond
to the following questions:
a. Which of these predictions do you think would be the likely
one?
b. Why do you think this prediction is a good one?
4. The teacher invites students to work in pairs to complete the
discussion following the same format.
5. The teacher asks each student to read the passage silently to
confirm or reject her/his own predictions. S/he then initiates a
discussion with some of the students by asking them the
following questions:
a. Were you correct?
b. What do you think now?
c. Why do you think so?
6. The teacher asks students to discuss and assess their own
predictions in pairs through asking and responding to the
following questions:
a. What prediction did you make?

116
b. What made you think of this prediction?
c. What in the passage supports this prediction?
d. Do you still agree with this prediction? Why?
7. The teacher gives each student the chance to independently
apply the strategies s/he has already known to a new text.
8. The teacher asks each student to self-assess the strategies s/he
has applied in relation to her/his comprehension of the text.

4.6.4. Research on the directed reading-thinking


activity with learning-disabled students
Research literature on the DR-TA reveals that many studies have
examined the effect of this activity on students’ reading
comprehension, but no studies specifically investigated its effect on
the reading comprehension of students with learning disabilities.
Therefore, this section reviews the DR-TA research conducted
with struggling readers in general.
Tancock (1994) developed a lesson for low-achieving children
with reading problems in an after school tutoring program. Her
lesson consisted of directed reading-thinking activities. The lesson
included prereading activities such as generating questions and
making predictions. Students answered questions that were
generated during prereading activities. Postreading activities
included evaluating predictions, clarifying, and extending or
refining thinking about the story. The findings revealed a
significant improvement in the students' reading skills. The
students also began viewing the reading process as a meaningful
experience rather than just rapidly going through reading
material to get specific answers to assignments.
Sears, Carpenter, and Burstein (1994) used the DR-TA
simultaneously with summarizing, questioning and clarifying
strategies, and investigated their effect on the reading
comprehension of eighth graders with special needs. Results

117
showed a measurable gain on word recognition and reading
comprehension.
Fabrikant, Siekierski, and Williams (1999) used the DR-TA in
combination with brainstorming of prior knowledge, Question
Answer Relationship (QAR), self-monitoring questions and
literature circles, and investigated their effect on the reading
comprehension skills of third, fourth and fifth grade students who
had poor literal and inferential comprehension skills. Results
indicated that this package of strategies improved students'
intrinsic motivation to read as well as their literal and inferential
reading comprehension skills.
El-Koumy (2006) investigated the effects of the directed
reading- thinking activity on first-year secondary poor
comprehenders. The study utilized a pretest-posttest control group
experimental design. The subjects consisted of 72 first-year
secondary poor comprehenders in Menouf Secondary School for
Boys at Menoufya Directorate of Education (Egypt) during the
academic year 2005/2006. These subjects were randomly assigned
to an experimental group and a control group. Both groups were
pre-tested to measure their referential and inferential reading
comprehension before conducting the experiment. During the
experiment, the experimental group students were exposed to the
DR-TA, whereas the control group students were exposed to the
conventional method of teaching reading comprehension. The
experiment lasted for about five months. After treatment, the two
groups were post-tested to investigate any significant differences in
their referential and inferential reading comprehension. The
obtained data were analyzed using the Multivariate Analysis of
Variance (MANOVA) and the T-test. The findings showed that
there were statistically significant differences in both referential
and inferential reading comprehension in favor of the
experimental group.

118
Van Riper (2010) investigated the functional relationship
between the directed reading-thinking activity and the reading
comprehension skills of students with Autism Spectrum Disorder
(ASD). Some of these students decoded with ease, and some read
falteringly. The DR-TA intervention, which occurred in a small
group setting, included use of non-linguistic visual supports, such
as graphic organizers, pictures, and objects. A reading inventory
and chapter comprehension checks revealed that students with
ASD made gains in reading comprehension with the use of the DR-
TA.

4.7. Reciprocal teaching


4.7.1. Definition of reciprocal teaching
Reciprocal teaching (RT) is a multiple-strategy method developed
by Palincsar and Brown (1984). In this method, the teacher and
the students take turns leading a dialogue about the text they are
working with. The teacher then gradually turns over responsibility
to the students to have this dialogue with each other. The purpose
of this dialogue is to achieve joint understanding of the text
through the application of four comprehension strategies that aid
in developing reading comprehension. As Palincsar, David, Winn,
and Stevens (1991) put it:
In reciprocal teaching teachers and students take turns
leading a dialogue about the meaning of the text with
which they are working. The discussion focuses on (1)
generating questions from the text, (2) summarizing the
text, (3) clarifying portions that impair understanding,
and (4) predicting upcomimg content. … When these
dialogues begin, the teacher assumes principal
responsibility for leading and sustaining the discussion,
modeling skilled use of the strategies for the purpose of

119
understanding the content. Even from the first day of
instruction, however, the children are encouraged to
participate in the discussion by generating their own
questions, elaborating upon or revising the summary, or
suggesting additional predictions. (p. 46)
In a nutshell, RT is a scaffolded discussion method built on
four strategies that good readers employ to comprehend texts. In
this method the teacher models, through thinking aloud, the
application of these strategies, gradually transferring control to
students. That is, students gradually take on the teacher's role as
they become more confident and proficient.

4.7.2. Reciprocal teaching strategies


The four strategies that traditionally constitute reciprocal teaching
are: questioning, summarizing, clarifying, and predicting.
Palincsar and Brown (1984) selected these four strategies because
they provide a dual function; that is, they embody both
comprehension-monitoring and comprehension-fostering
activities. Moreover, each of these strategies has an important role
in the reading comprehension process. This role is discussed
below.
Questioning focuses students’ attention on the main ideas,
provides a check on their current understanding of what they are
reading and enables them to monitor comprehension. When
generating questions, one must first identify the important points
that are worthy of questioning by understanding the basic gist of
the text and the main points that support it. Moreover, students
tend to read with greater purpose when they know that they are
expected to frame questions about what they have read. They also
learn to take responsibility for the comprehension of what they
read (Hashey and Connors, 2003). With questioning, students
come to realize that both information from the text and their own
schema are important when answering questions. Generating
questions about a text and answering them also make the
information easier to remember and provide more opportunities

120
to interact with the content of the text (Graham and Hebert, 2010,
Swanson and De La Paz, 1998). Questioning is also an effective
strategy that can be used with students of all ages and levels.
According to Clark, Deshler, Schumaker, and Alley (1984),
learning disabled students can increase their reading
comprehension via self-questioning. In support of this, research
has shown that training in question and answer relationships
benefit average and lower level performing readers (Raphael and
Pearson, 1985). Moreover, Wong and Jones (1982) found that
eighth- and ninth-grade students with learning disabilities who
were taught to use the self-questioning strategy performed better
on a number of measures, including gist recall, idea unit
identification, and factual recall, than students who received no
training. There is also a positive relationship between generating
higher order questions and reading comprehension; that is, the
more higher order questions students generate, the better reading
performance they have. Davey and McBride’s (1986) study also
showed that students’ reading comprehension in the question-
generation group was better than students in the read-reread
group. Moreover, Rost and Ross’s (1991) study revealed that
“prior training of learners in specific questioning strategies can
exert an effect on their subsequent behavior in interactions and
can influence their immediate comprehension of a text as well”.
Summarizing a text “requires readers to sift through large
units of text, differentiate important from unimportant ideas, and
then synthesize those ideas and create a new coherent text that
stands for, by substantive criteria, the original” (Dole, Duffy,
Roehler, and Pearson, 1991, p. 244). Therefore, teaching students
to summarize what they read can improve their overall
comprehension of text. In support of summarizing as one of the
most successful strategies for teaching reading comprehension,
Gajria and Salvia (1992) taught middle school students with
learning disabilities to summarize expository prose passages.
Findings indicated that trained students performed better on
multiple choice questions than those in a no-treatment comparison

121
group and that the summarization strategy was maintained over
time and students generalized its use to other tasks. Graham and
Hebert (2010) also found that writing summaries about what was
being read was associated with improvements in reading
comprehension. They also found that writing summaries was
better than simply reading and rereading the text.
Clarification is needed when new vocabulary is encountered,
difficult concepts are presented, or ideas are not understood.
When readers use clarification, they unpack ambiguous, confusing
sections of text and identify when comprehension is not
progressing before excessive breakdowns occur. Clarification also
engages students in a critical evaluation of their own reading.
With respect to prediction, it occurs when the students
hypothesize what the author will discuss next in the text. The
literature confirms that this strategy is effective and facilitates
comprehension. When students make predictions about texts, they
are tapping into their own prior knowledge to help make
connections between the knowledge they possess and the
knowledge they will acquire from reading a text. When students
activate their schema and make connections with the text, they
develop a deeper understanding of the information they receive. If
the schema is not activated properly, the student’s comprehension
will suffer (McNeil, 1992). In support of this, research has shown
that good readers activate their schema before, during, and after
reading. The strategy of prediction also makes the reader alert to
cues that aid in confirmation or adjustment of the prediction, thus
increasing the active stance of the reader and offering checks for
clarification. Prediction also allows students of varying abilities to
contribute their ideas and helps them to set a purpose for reading
(Hashey and Connors, 2003; Oczuks, 2003). Therefore, reciprocal
teaching uses prediction as a strategy to set the stage for students
to comprehend what they read.

122
In sum, Palincsar and Brown (1984) describe the reasons for
choosing the four strategies mentioned earlier as "comprehension
fostering and comprehension-monitoring" (p. 121). The order in
which the reciprocal teaching strategies are used is not fixed; it
depends on the text and the reader (Oczkus, 2003). The strategies
are merely the steps good readers take on their way to
comprehension. As such, they are a means to an end in the
comprehension process. These strategies not only assist reading
comprehension but also provide opportunities for students to
monitor their own learning and thinking processes with or without
assistance.
As mentioned before, there are four main strategies
that traditionally constitute reciprocal teaching. However, these
strategies have had many adaptations and extensions over time.
Many practitioners and researchers (e.g., Coley, DePinto, Sharon,
and Gardner, 1993; Meyer, 2010) have adapted these strategies to
suit their local context, subject area and student learning needs.
Meyer (2010), for example, extended these strategies to include
orientating, connecting and giving feedback.

4.7.3. Theoretical foundation of reciprocal teaching


Reciprocal teaching appears to reflect the learning principles
derived from theories such as social constructivism, cognitivism
and metacognitivism. The reciprocal nature within reciprocal
teaching is rooted in Vygotsky’s theory of ‘Zone of Proximal
Development’ which refers to the support that should be given to
students under adult guidance or in collaboration with more
capable peers to make it possible for the novice to participate in
mature tasks from the very beginning. According to Vygotsky,
cognitive practices such as reading comprehension are acquired
through social interaction in which students negotiate for meaning
with more knowledgeable others. In doing so, students are
initiated into the cognitive practices and strategies of skilled
comprehenders, which are then internalized. In this way,
scaffolding can help students to exceed what can be attained alone,

123
develop higher skills and increase their knowledge base. In this
respect, Alfassi, Weiss, and Lifshitz (2009) state that during the
early stages of reciprocal teaching the instructor should assume
the major responsibility for instruction by explicitly modeling the
four strategies on a reading passage. S/he should gradually
diminish scaffolding as students move from what Vygotsky (1978)
called the ‘other directed’ to ‘self directed’ stages of
understanding. Eventually, the student assumes most of the
comprehension responsibilities and the teacher becomes a
supportive and sympathetic audience. That is, as the skill level of
the learner increases, the level of teacher support decreases. With
diminished assistance, the student gradually assumes total
responsibility for the application of the reciprocal strategies.
Moreover, reciprocal teaching is based on the cognitive theory
which contends that all students need to learn a range of cognitive
strategies so that they will be able to select from an extensive
repertoire to address their particular learning needs and abilities
(Graves and Graves, 1994). A major assumption underlying
reciprocal teaching is that students will eventually internalize the
use of the four supporting strategies and utilize them effectively.
The strategies that are practiced between learners in the group
will eventually be accomplished within the individual students.
Reciprocal teaching is also metacognitive in that students are
required to monitor their own comprehension performance and
decide whether it is appropriate to apply a specific strategy at a
particular time. The teacher slowly decreases modeling of the
strategies and relinquishes more control to students as they
demonstrate independence with applying these strategies. The
students become independent when the strategies they have been
taught cognitively are used metacognitively.

4.7.4. Benefits of reciprocal teaching


Reciprocal teaching is an effective instructional method that can
improve the reading comprehension of students of varying levels

124
and abilities (Brown and Palincsar, 1985). The
advantages of this method include: (1) building students’
comprehension skills through the use of cognitive strategies, (2)
integrating language skills, (3) developing students’ social skills,
(4) developing students’ sociolinguistic competence and .improving
their oral language skills, and (5) allowing the teacher to assess
students in a non-threatening atmosphere.
In addition to the previously mentioned benefits, reciprocal
teaching meets the needs of students with inferior reading abilities.
It is perhaps the first formal instructional method targeted at
struggling readers (Palinscar and Brown, 1984). More specifically,
the four strategies that constitute reciprocal teaching best address
the deficiencies of poor readers (Hart and Speece, 1998). In
support of this, Carter (1997) found that “[r]eciprocal teaching
helps novice readers learn and internalize the strategies excellent
readers employ” (p. 65). In a research synthesis of twenty-nine
studies, Gajria, Jitendra, Sood, and Sacks (2007) found that
students with learning disabilities (though fluent in text decoding)
tended to be passive readers who did not automatically engage
with the text at a deep level. They were unable to relate new
information to prior knowledge and exhibited no self-monitoring
skills for reading. Yet when exposed to RT, notable improvements
in reading comprehension were recorded. However, there are
some disputes among educators on the order in which the
reciprocal teaching strategies should be used. Should the
prediction strategy, for instance, be used before reading the text to
activate students’ prior knowledge, or should it be used during
reading where students are required to predict what will happen
next? Another drawback to this method is that students may have
reservations to learning how to use the reciprocal teaching
strategies which are time consuming to learn (Hashey and
Conners, 2003).

4.7.5. Procedures of reciprocal teaching


The procedures of reciprocal teaching run as follows: The students
and the teacher read a paragraph of a text. Next, the teacher

125
models the four strategies aloud by summarizing the paragraph
they read, clarifying anything that needs to be clarified, generating
questions about this paragraph, and predicting what will happen
in the next paragraph. After that the teacher asks one specific
student to read the next paragraph aloud, and engages in coaching
and scaffolding to help this student proceed through the RT steps.
The teacher then does the same thing with another student. Once
several students have engaged in RT, the teacher begins to fade
her/his support and gradually diminishes the scaffolding
assistance as students begin to assume full control of the four
strategies. In this step, students take turns modeling the strategies
in peer groups and the teacher becomes a mediator who provides
guidance and feedback tailored to the needs of the current
dialogue leader and her/his partner. Each student then applies the
four strategies independently to a new text. Lastly, each
student self-assesses the strategies s/he has used in relation to
her/his comprehension of the new text (Adapted from Palincsar
and Brown, 1984).
In short, because it is particularly developed for struggling
middle school readers, the reciprocal teaching method
incorporates teacher and peer modeling. This modeling is
withdrawn gradually and systematically, passing responsibility to
the individual learner. In this way, struggling readers can master
the four supporting strategies of reciprocal teaching and use them
independently for all of their reading assignments.

4.7.6. Research on reciprocal teaching with


students with reading disabilities/difficulties
Reciprocal teaching has been demonstrated as an effective
teaching method in a variety of settings, by countless researchers
with students of varying levels and abilities. However, this section
only offers a review of the research on reciprocal teaching with
students with reading disabilities/difficulties.

126
Originally, Palincsar and Brown (1984) investigated the effects
of reciprocal teaching on the reading comprehension of two
different groups of struggling seventh-grade students. In the first
study, Palincsar and Brown (1984) investigated the change RT
made in the reading comprehension of expository text for seventh
graders with adequate decoding but poor comprehension.
Comparison groups participated in a "locating information"
intervention, regular classroom instruction or unmet control.
Participants in the RT group, who met in pairs with an instructor,
received 20 lessons over a four-week period, reading expository
passages averaging 1500 words each. The RT students became
progressively more proficient at implementing the cognitive tactics
taught to them. During daily reading assessments following
instruction, RT students made striking improvements in their
ability to answer comprehension questions on the passages read.
RT readers’ abilities to summarize, detect anomalous information
in text, and answer comprehension questions were significantly
improved. Summaries given by RT students contained more main
ideas and fewer incorrect or incomplete details. Additionally, the
six students in the RT group significantly improved from pre to
post-test in their ability to (a) answer comprehension questions
and (b) identify text incongruities. These changes were maintained
over time. On standardized tests of reading achievement, four of
six RT students made substantial gains averaging 15 months'
growth. Control students evidenced no corresponding change.
Palincsar and Brown observed that their intervention accelerated
the progress of the lower-achieving readers. Readers with and at
risk for disabilities in the RT group improved to the level set by
the average readers, whereas marginalized readers in the other
two groups did not.
In their second study, Palincsar and Brown (1984) moved the
RT intervention into the classroom (i.e., resource room
instruction) with their regular teacher in the facilitator's role.
Intervention materials and procedures were identical to Study 1.
Palincsar and Brown found the same trends for improved reading
comprehension for sixth, seventh, and eighth graders with

127
adequate decoding but poor comprehension who received training
in RT. In early lessons, the teacher tended to retain a "pivotal"
role in RT, with students interacting with her/him rather than
with one another. By lesson 10, however, student-participants
initiated and responded independent of the teacher's guidance,
acting as agents of their own learning, with teachers redirecting
discourse only as needed. Students in the RT condition improved
their ability to summarize, answer comprehension questions, and
state main ideas. The quality of their text-centered discourse also
improved.
The use of reciprocal teaching to improve the standardized
reading comprehension performance of poor readers was studied
by Lysynchuk, Pressley, and Vye (1990). In their study, 72 grade
four and seven students in Canada participated in 13 sessions of
reciprocal teaching reading instruction. Those students, as
characterized by their teachers, were adequate decoders but poor
comprehenders. All of them received scores below the 50th
percentile on standardized achievement tests on the
comprehension subtest. Of the 72 students, 36 were assigned to the
reciprocal teaching intervention, while others worked in small
groups, with the teacher offering assistance if needed in decoding
and passage understanding (i.e., the guided reading model).
Thirteen sessions were administered to both groups, with daily
dependent measures being taken (i.e., retelling and questions), as
well as pre and post standardized reading measures (i.e., Gates-
MacGinitie Reading Comprehension Test). Results revealed that
the mean pretest to posttest gain for the reciprocal teaching
students was significant with a 9.97 percentile point gain, whereas
the control group received a 1.63 percentile point increase, which
was not significant.
Westera and Moore (1995) investigated the effect of reciprocal
teaching on the reading comprehension of 46 high school students
in New Zealand. These students were adequate decoders but
scored the lowest of 300 students on a standardized comprehension
test. Eleven of the 46 students served as the control group. The rest

128
of the students were divided into two groups where one group
received between 12 and 16 sessions and the other group received
6 to 8 sessions of reciprocal teaching instruction. An analysis of
pretest posttest scores revealed that the extended reciprocal
teaching group outperformed the control group significantly.
Ninety-five percent of the extended strategy group gained an
average of more than one age equivalent year in reading
comprehension performance over the five-week period. The short
strategy group showed gains in reading comprehension of 47 %,
where the control group demonstrated gains of 45 %.
Klingner and Vaughn (1996) used reciprocal teaching as an
intervention for poor decoders with learning disabilities at the
middle school level. A small sample size of 26 students was treated
with 15 sessions of reciprocal teaching. For these sessions, students
were randomly assigned to one of two groups (i.e., reciprocal
teaching with cooperative tutoring or reciprocal teaching with
cross-age tutoring). Three sessions were used for strategy
instruction, while the remaining 12 sessions implemented
reciprocal teaching. The instruments used as dependent measures
included the Gates-MacGinitie standardized reading tests and
teacher-made comprehension questions on reading passages as
developed by Palincsar and Brown (1984). The results indicated
that reciprocal teaching improved reading comprehension even
with only minimal adult support. They (Klinger and Vaughn)
concluded that reciprocal teaching was especially important for
ESL learners as it improved their meta-cognitive skills and gave
them voice to what they were doing while they were reading.
Alfassi (1998) investigated the effects of reciprocal teaching in
comparison to traditional methods used in remedial reading in
large intact high school remedial reading classes. The results
showed that the students who participated in the reciprocal
teaching classes obtained higher post intervention comprehension
scores than their peers who participated in traditional reading
instruction.

129
Brand-Gruwel, Aarnoutse, and Van Den Bos (1998)
investigated the effects of reciprocal teaching on the
comprehension of students with poor decoding skills and poor
reading comprehension. One hundred fifty-seven fourth-grade
students participated in this study; half of them received the
reciprocal teaching intervention program and half of them acted
as a control group and received their school reading curriculum
instruction. The intervention program consisted of twenty 45-
minute lessons, ten of which were reading lessons, five were
listening lessons, and five were integrated reading and listening
lessons. During the lessons clarifying, questioning, summarizing,
and predicting strategies were first explicitly taught and modeled
by the teacher. They were then practiced through reciprocal
teaching in small groups of students. When compared to a control
group, the students who received the explicit and reciprocal
training performed better on tests of comprehension.
Hart and Speece (1998) used reciprocal teaching with college
students who were at risk of academic failure and compared them
to a group of students who participated in cooperative learning
groups where students were not trained to use reciprocal teaching
strategies. The reciprocal teaching groups performed significantly
better than their peers in the cooperative groups on reading
comprehension and strategy acquisition measures. Moreover, the
poorer readers in the reciprocal teaching groups performed
significantly better than poorer readers in the cooperative groups.
Johnson-Glenberg (2000) conducted a study to determine the
effects of reciprocal teaching on the reading comprehension of
students with poor text comprehension skills. The sample of the
study consisted of 59 third, fourth, and fifth graders from three
different schools. Over a ten-week period, 22 students received
reciprocal teaching intervention, 23 students received visualizing-
verbalizing intervention, where students formed mental images in
their minds of important text segments and then verbalized their

130
understanding, and 14 students were untreated and served as the
control group. Both strategy groups made statistically significant
gains that were greater than the control group on four measures
including word recognition, question generation, answering
explicit open-ended questions and visual open-ended questions.
Additionally, the reciprocal teaching group demonstrated
significant gains over the control group on answering questions
involving implicit open-ended questions.
The use of reciprocal teaching within inclusive social studies
classrooms was investigated by Lederer (2000). The sample
consisted of 128 students in the intermediate grades (4th, 5th, and
6th), of whom some were identified as learning disabled. At each
grade level, two classrooms were inclusive, (i.e., general education
and special education students) and the other two were non-
inclusive (i.e., general education students only). In the study, the
experimenter/researcher administered approximately 15
reciprocal teaching sessions across the three grade levels. The
results indicated that the experimental group scored higher than
the control group at all grade levels. The study also revealed
positive changes in students' abilities to generate questions,
respond to questions, and summarize information. These results
suggested that reciprocal teaching was an effective whole class
intervention that improved the reading comprehension of students
with learning disabilities.
A multiple-baseline across groups design was employed by
Kelly, Moore and Tuck (2001) to gauge the effects of reciprocal
teaching. Eighteen poor readers in fourth and fifth grades were
selected to participate in the study in an urban elementary school
in New Zealand. Three groups were formed—two receiving the
reciprocal teaching intervention (n= 6 each) and one receiving
their regular reading instruction (n= 6). The results showed that
both groups receiving the reciprocal teaching intervention made
significant gains in reading comprehension based on daily teacher-
made comprehension tests. These gains were not seen for the third
group, which received its regular reading instruction. Treatment

131
integrity was addressed by gathering data on the use of the four
strategies by teacher and student during the reciprocal teaching
intervention. These data indicated an increase in teacher-directed
strategy use during baseline rather than during the intervention
phase.
Fung, Wilkinson, and Moore (2003) used RT with
intermediate-level students in heterogeneous groups of students
with and without limited English proficiency (LEP). Students with
LEP participated in discussions of texts either in their first
language (Chinese) or English. The statistical analysis employed
was multiple t-tests for non-independent samples to analyze
whether posttest scores of the strategy classes as a whole were
significantly higher than pretest scores, compared to a class that
did not use the reciprocal teaching method. The results indicated
that reciprocal teaching was highly effective for fostering and
strengthening reading comprehension skills even though students
were poor decoders.
LeFevre, Moore, and Wilkinson (2003) applied a modified
reciprocal teaching intervention with students who had limited
decoding and comprehension skills. Two single subject
experiments, one with an ABC design (featuring baseline,
Condition 1, Condition 2, follow-up, and maintenance), and one
using a multiple baselines across groups of students (as suggested
by Palincsar and Brown, 1984) evaluated reciprocal teaching.
Study one assessed six students in 3rd grade in an urban school in
Auckland, New Zealand. Students were first assessed with no
treatment during baseline. Then during Condition 1, the
traditional reciprocal teaching intervention was applied. Condition
2 consisted of tape-assisted reciprocal teaching where students
listened to the story via a tape recorder and followed the
conventional reciprocal teaching method. There was no change
from baseline (14%) to condition 1 (15%) on the percentage of
comprehension questions answered correctly based on daily
repeated measures. Conversely, during Condition 2 (tape-assisted),

132
improvement was noted, with students attaining a mean
performance of 47% correct on the daily comprehension test. The
second study was composed of 18 students in the same age range
and social setting from three different schools. This second study
was conducted to provide some generalizability based on the
previous experiment. The results on the daily short answer
comprehension tests showed systematic improvement on
performance, as well as significant gains when compared to
baseline data.
Diehl (2005) investigated the effects of reciprocal teaching on
strategy acquisition of fourth-grade struggling readers. Six,
fourth-grade struggling readers from Glendale School
participated in the study. Specifically, these students were
identified because they could decode words adequately but
comprehended text poorly. Identified students participated in 20
sessions following the reciprocal teaching framework—a reading
intervention program that incorporates direct instruction in four
comprehension strategies, questioning, predicting, clarifying, and
monitoring. The teacher explicitly demonstrated how, when, and
why to apply each strategy while reading a text. After the initial
demonstrations, the teacher slowly withdrew her support as the
students began to take turns modeling the strategies and offering
feedback to each other. Results indicated that direct strategy
instruction appeared to affect strategy acquisition which then led
to improvement in the students’ abilities to comprehend what they
read. Further, it seemed that these six students relied heavily on
their world knowledge, manifested through the strategy of
prediction, at the early stages of strategy acquisition. Finally,
questioning to clarify an idea seemed to be an important function
as the impetus for group discussions, which led to the joint
construction of meaning. The other strategies were embedded
within the discussion, and the joint construction of meaning
appeared to result from the mutual dependencies of all four

133
strategies. The results of this study may be important to
practitioners interested in developing reading instruction that
meets the needs of students who can decode words adequately but
comprehend text poorly.
To sum up, the previously-mentioned studies on reciprocal
teaching revealed that reading strategies can successfully be
taught to students with reading disabilities/difficulties and that
these strategies can improve their reading comprehension. These
studies provided further evidence of the effectiveness of reciprocal
teaching as an instructional procedure for students experiencing
difficulty with reading comprehension in a variety of contexts
including regular classrooms where teachers are forced to provide
instruction to diverse populations of students.

134
Chapter Five
Teaching Writing Strategies to Students
with Writing Disabilities

5.0. Introduction
Today, we live in the information age, where computers are used
for teaching, learning and communication; therefore, writing in
English has become essential to enable students to meet the
challenges of this age and to use its new communication
technologies for learning and communication. In this respect,
Björk and Räisänen (1997) argue that writing is a need today due
to internationalization development of computer communication
and the mobility of both students and faculty.
Moreover, Bello (1997) proposes that writing in general is an
important skill whether in students’ native or foreign language. He
argues that writing helps students to communicate their feelings
and thoughts with others and enhances their linguistic competence
as they use vocabulary and grammar to communicate their own
ideas. Likewise, Stirling (2003) claims that writing is a very
important skill for EFL students as they need to write in English
in both academic and everyday life. She adds that this skill is an
important tool for communication and it gives students more self-
confidence to experiment with language. Graham and Perin
(2007b), too, contend that the “writing skill is a predictor of
academic success and a basic requirement for participation in civic
life and in the global economy” (p. 3).
By the same token, Nik, Hamzah, and Rafidee (2010) state that
writing reinforces students’ grammatical structures and develops
their language in terms of fluency, accuracy, and appropriateness
in the communication of meanings and messages. They claim that
since English language is essential for attaining academic degrees,
students should acquire and achieve some kind of satisfactory level
of writing proficiency. They add that writing helps students use
English frequently and provides them with opportunities to form
their own ideas clearly.
Over and above, Graham and Harris (2011) state that the
writing skill is needed for acquiring jobs. They maintain that there
are many jobs, regardless of the nature of the job, which require
effective writing. They also state that nineteen out of twenty
students with learning disabilities are not good writers and this
puts them at an academic disadvantage and makes them less likely
to successfully enter into employment due to their difficulties with
written expression. In essence, writing is essential for students with
and without disabilities because it is more than a requirement for
school, it is also a part of our everyday life.
Although the importance of writing is widely recognized as
mentioned above, EFL students, particularly those with learning
disabilities, face many difficulties in writing paragraphs and
essays. These difficulties include, but are not limited to expressing
and organizing thoughts and ideas on paper in a coherent,
meaningful, logical and comprehensible way in terms of the genre
of the topic they are working with. More specifically, many
researchers (e.g., Englert and Raphael, 1988; Gleason, 1999;
Graham and Harris, 1989b, 1991; TATN, 2012; Thomas, Englert
and Gregg, 1987; Troia, 2007; Wong, 2000) found that students
with writing disabilities are characterized by the following:
• They have difficulty expressing their ideas;

• They have difficulty organizing thoughts on paper;

• They have difficulty generating ideas;


• They lose track of the ideas they put on paper;
• They spend too much time on producing legible handwriting and
proper spelling;

136
• They lack procedural knowledge about the writing process;
• They fail to use the appropriate strategies as they are writing;
and
• They are unaware of specific cognitive writing strategies that
assist with writing expression.
The aforementioned difficulties experienced by students with
learning disabilities at the intermediate level and beyond are
attributable, in part, to their difficulties with executing and
regulating the writing processes, especially planning, drafting and
revising because they write without strategies that can help them
carry out these processes (Golley, 2015; Graham and Harris, 1997,
2009; Graham, Harris, and Troia, 1998; Troia, 2007). In other
words, they just retrieve-and-write without strategies that help
them plan, generate, organize and revise their writing. This cause
is expressed by Dean (2010) in the following way:
Our experience as teachers shows us that use of effective
strategies can be a distinguishing characteristic between
experienced and novice writers…. We have probably all
observed how students with few strategies approach a
writing task; they may begin writing what comes to mind
(what Scardamalia and Bereiter call knowledge telling) or
they may never complete the writing task…. They have
no effective strategies to use. (pp. 4-5).
The same cause is expressed by TATN (2012) in the following
way:
One of the biggest differences between the struggling
writer and the more skilled writer is that the struggling
writer is less strategic. The struggling writer uses very
few strategies and is comfortable with using the
knowledge-telling strategy. Also, he/she is reluctant to use
unfamiliar strategies or those that require any effort.
(Slide 15)

137
Similarly, Golley (2015) expresses the same cause in the
following way:
Students with learning disabilities often struggle with
writing. They lack the appropriate strategies to use while
writing, which leaves them frustrated and unwilling to
continue writing. Teachers need to find strategies that will
help their students become more engaged and excited
about their writing. Finding effective strategies for
planning, composing, and revising writing pieces will help
students with learning disabilities become more proficient
writers…. In order for students with learning disabilities
to become better writers, they need to be given
appropriate strategies in planning, composing, and
revising written pieces. (p. iii)
In support of the above-mentioned cause, research showed
that struggling writers, including students with writing disabilities,
dived into writing assignments without planning or setting writing
goals (Wong, 1988, 1994, 2000), wrote without strategies for
generating and organizing ideas (Graham and Harris, 2005;
MacArthur, Ferretti, Okolo, and Cavalier, 2001), had difficulty
self-monitoring their writing (Hacker, Plumb, Butterfield,
Quathamer, and Heineken, 1994), and lacked strategies for
revising what they had written (MacArthur, Ferretti, Okolo, and
Cavalier, 2001; Peterson-Karlan and Parette, 2007).
Another cause that accounts for the writing difficulties faced
by struggling writers in the Egyptian context is that their teachers
focus only on discrete skills and value product over process. They
teach the students more bits of language and completely neglect
the writing process. They also measure their students’ writing
against criteria of micro-structural elements such as handwriting,
grammar, and spelling. Therefore, students spend too much time
producing legible handwriting and proper spelling. As they
become over involved in producing legible handwriting and
properly spelled words, they neglect cognitive strategies that
generate and organize ideas and limit the ideas they choose to

138
write about. They also focus more on the mechanical revisions,
rather than content revision to make their piece look better
(Wong, 2000). As Troia, (2007) puts it, “A strong emphasis on
mechanics by teachers who work with struggling writers serves to
bias their students’ views of writing, leading them to believe that
text appearance is paramount” (p. 135). In support of the negative
effect of focusing on the micro-skills of writing, Hillocks (1984)
found that students in writing programs that emphasized
mechanics and grammar achieved significantly lower qualitative
gains in writing than students who received instruction that
emphasized the organization of ideas and the process of writing. In
addition, students taught in the product-driven group came to
dislike writing, especially school writing; whereas students in the
writing process group developed positive attitudes towards
writing.
Still another cause that leads to the impoverished writing
performance of students with learning disabilities is that they lack
genre-specific strategies that help them organize their ideas and
enable them to write cohesive texts (Troia, 2007; Wong, 1997). In
support of this cause, Barenbaum, Newcomer and Nodine (1987)
found that stories written by students with learning disabilities
frequently lack even the most basic story parts such as character
and goals. Gleason (1999) also found that students with learning
disabilities have trouble with all genres of writing in general and
the persuasive genre in particular.
To overcome the writing difficulties experienced by students
with learning disabilities, many writing scholars and researchers
(e.g., Graham, Bollinger, Booth Olson, D’Aoust, MacArthur,
McCutchen, and Olinghouse, 2012; Graham, Harris, and Mason,
2005; Harris, Graham, and Mason, 2006; Troia and Graham,
2002; Troia, Graham, and Harris, 1999) suggest incorporating the
teaching of writing strategies and genre-specific strategies within
the writing process to enable students with writing difficulties to
be effective writers. Harris, Graham, and Mason (2006), for
example, state that instruction involving general strategies and
genre-specific strategies within the writing process can have

139
positive effects on the writing performance of students
experiencing difficulty learning to write, including those with
learning disabilities. Graham et al. (2012) call for the same
solution in the following way:
Teachers can help students become effective writers by
teaching a variety of strategies for carrying out each
component of the writing process and by supporting
students in applying the strategies until they are able to
do so independently. Over time, students will develop a
repertoire of strategies for writing. Teachers should
explain and model the fluid nature in which the
components of the writing process work together, so that
students can learn to apply strategies flexibly—separately
or in combination—when they write. (p. 12)
It is also evident from the review of the previous research that
the use of writing strategies as an instructional intervention to
improve the writing difficulties of struggling writers is firmly
established. Previous research found that instruction in writing
strategies led to improvements in (a) four aspects of students'
writing performance: quality of writing, knowledge of writing,
approach to writing, and writing self-efficacy (Troia, Graham,
and Harris, 1999), (b) written expression skills for students with
writing deficits (Graham and Harris, 1996; 2000; Graham, Harris
and Troia, 1998; Graham, Harris, MacArthur, and Schwartz,
1991; Sawyer, Graham, and Harris, 1992), and (c) writing
performance of adolescents with learning disabilities (Graham and
Harris, 1989a). In addition, meta-analytic reviews revealed that
instruction in writing strategies outperformed other approaches in
both struggling and typically-developing students at both primary
and secondary levels (Graham, McKeown, Kiuhara, and Harris,
2012; Graham and Perin, 2007a; Rogers and Graham, 2008). For
example, in a review of studies over the last two decades, Graham
and Perin (2007a) found that the effects of strategy instruction
across studies was large and statistically significant and that
“[e]xplicitly teaching adolescents strategies for planning, revising,

140
and/or editing had a strong impact on the quality of their writing”
(p. 463). Furthermore, research showed that genre-specific
strategies enhanced writing in adolescents with learning
disabilities (Wong, 1997).
With the above in mind, it appears that students with learning
disabilities are in need of embedding writing strategies and genre-
specific strategies in the context of a process approach to writing
to enable them to use these strategies for carrying out each phase
of the writing process, namely, planning, drafting, revising and
editing. For each of these phases there are a number of general
and specific writing strategies that can assist the students to carry
it out successfully. Therefore, teaching students with writing
difficulties to use these strategies, through a gradual release of
responsibility from the teacher to the student can enable them to
clearly express their thoughts and ideas. In this light, the
remainder of the present chapter will deal with writing strategies
from all aspects. It will also detail a four-phase model for teaching
these strategies and review research on their impact on the writing
of students with learning disabilities.

5.1. Definition of writing strategies


Some writing scholars and researchers (e.g., Graham et al., 2012;
Hedge, 2000; Williams, 2003) view writing strategies as conscious
procedures or actions that learners employ to control their writing
process. Graham et al. (2012), for example, define writing
strategies as “tools that can help students generate content and
carry out components of the writing process” (p. 42). That is,
within each stage of the writing process good writers use a variety
of strategies. They, for example, use strategies such as
brainstorming for generating ideas, and webbing for organizing
these ideas at the planning stage. The types of strategies good
writers use within each stage of the writing process depend on the
genre of the topic, purpose of writing and the audience for the
writing assignment.

141
Some other writing scholars and researchers (e.g., Arndt,
1987; Torrance, Thomas, and Robinson, 2000) view writing
strategies as stages of the writing process. As Torrance et al. (2000)
put it, a writing strategy is “the sequence in which a writer enga-
ges in planning, composing, revising and other writing related
activities” (p. 182).
It is obvious then that some writing scholars view writing
strategies as the techniques writers use to carry out and control
the stages of the writing process; while others view the stages of
the writing process (particularly planning, revising, and editing) as
writing strategies. It seems to the author that the former view is
broader than the latter. Therefore, this view (the former one) is
adopted by him for developing a multiple-strategies model for
teaching writing strategies to students with writing disabilities (See
section 5.4 of this chapter).

5.2. Classification of writing strategies


Many different taxonomies of writing strategies have been
suggested over years. Some writing scholars (e. g., Geladari and
Mastrothanasis, 2011; Wenden, 1991b) classified writing strategies
on the basis of cognitive and metacognitive theories of learning.
For example, Wenden (1991b) categorized ESL writing strategies
into cognitive and metacognitive strategies. According to her,
metacognitive strategies are strategies that writers use to control
and regulate the writing process and cognitive strategies are those
that writers use to implement actual writing. She further states
that metacognitive strategies are directly responsible for the
execution of a writing task and that cognitive strategies are
auxiliary strategies that aid in the implementation of the
metacognitive strategies. That is, the function of cognitive
strategies is narrower in scope than metacognitive strategies. The
metacognitive and cognitive writing strategies identified by
Wenden are summarized in Figure 5.1.

142
Figure 5.1: Wenden’s cognitive and metacognitive writing
strategies (Adapted from Wenden, 1991b)

Metacognitive writing strategies Cognitive writing strategies

(1) Clarification

Planning a. Self-questioning,

Evaluation b. Hypothesizing,

Monitoring c. Defining terms,

d. Comparing.

(2) Retrieval

a. Rereading aloud or silently what had


been written,

b. Writing in a lead-in word or


expression,

c. Rereading the assigned question,

d. Self-questioning,

e. Writing till the idea would come,

f. Summarizing what had just been


written (in terms of content or of
rhetoric),

g. Thinking in one’s native language.

(3) Resourcing

a. Asking researcher,

b. Referring to dictionary.

(4) Deferral

(5) Avoidance

(6) Verification

143
Some other writing scholars (e.g., Chen, 2011; Graham et al.,
2012) classified writing strategies on the basis of the basic phases
identified by the process theory of writing. Chen (2011), for
example, identified twelve writing strategy groups and twenty
eight individual strategies across three basic phases of the writing
process (pre-writing, writing, and revising). Figure 5.2 below
presents these strategies.
Figure 5.2: Chen’s classification of writing strategies (Chen, 2011,
p. 246)
Stage Strategy group Individual strategy

Pre-writing strategies Metacognitive Planning,

Identifying,

Overviewing,

Organizing.

Cognitive Resourcing,

Translating.

While-writing Metacognitive Goal-setting,

Strategies Self-monitoring,

Organizing,

Overviewing.

Cognitive Repeating,

Recognizing,

Translating,

Resourcing.

Memory New-word

144
(Figure 5.2 Continued)
Stage Strategy group Individual strategy

Compensation Approximating,

Synonym.

Revising Metacognitive Goal-setting,

Strategies Self-monitoring,

Paying attention,

Identifying.

Cognitive Resourcing,

Repeating.

Memory Keywords

Social Teacher-cooperating,

Peer-cooperating.

Affective Self-rewarding

Along the previous line of classifying writing strategies on the


basis of the basic phases of the writing process, Graham et al.
(2012) identified ten writing strategy groups, and twenty three
individual strategies across five stages of the writing process:
planning, drafting, sharing, evaluating, revising and editing (See
Figure 5.3).

145
Figure 5.3: Graham et al.’s writing strategies (Graham et al. 2012,
p. 16)
Stage of Writing Writing How Students Can Use the Strategy
Process Strategy

Planning POW ฀ Pick ideas (i.e., decide what to write about),

฀ Organize notes (i.e., brainstorm and organize


possible writing ideas into a writing plan),

฀ Write and say more (i.e., continue to modify


the plan while writing),

Ordering ฀ Brainstorm/generate ideas,


ideas/outlining
฀ Review ideas and place a number by what will
go first, second, third, and so on,

฀ Decide which are main ideas and which are


supporting ideas,

฀ Create an outline that shows the order of the


main ideas and the supporting details for each
main idea.

Drafting Imitation ฀ Select a sentence, paragraph, or text excerpt


and imitate the author’s form.

Sentence ฀ Try out sentences orally before writing them


on paper,
generation
฀ Try multiple sentences and choose the best one,

฀ Use transition words to develop different


sentence structures,

฀ Practice writing good topic sentences.

Sharing Peer sharing ฀ In pairs, listen and read along as the author
reads aloud,

฀ Share feedback with a writing partner.

146
(Figure 5.3 Continued)

Stage of Writing Writing How Students Can Use the Strategy


Proces Strategy

Evaluating Self- ฀ Reread and ask these questions:


evaluating
฀ Are the ideas clear?

฀ Is there a clear beginning, middle, and end?

฀ Does the writing connect with the reader?

฀ Are sentence types varied?

Self- ฀ Self-assess and ask these questions, either out


monitoring
loud or internally:

฀ Did I meet the goals I developed for my writing?

If not, what changes should I make to meet my goals?

฀ Did I correctly use strategies that were

appropriate for this task? If not, what should I change?

฀ Record your answers to self-assessment questions on a

chart or teacher-provided questionnaire in order to

track progress toward writing goals and strategy use,

฀ Congratulate yourself, and inform the teacher, when you


meet your goals.

Revising Peer ฀ Place a question mark (?) by anything you do not


revising
and editing understand in the writing partner’s paper,

฀ Place a carat (^) anywhere it would be useful to have

the author include more information.

COPS ฀ Ask the COPS editing questions:


(editing)
฀ Did I Capitalize the first word in sentences

and proper names?

฀ How is the Overall appearance of my paper?

฀ Did I use commas and end-of-sentence Punctuation?

฀ Did I Spell each word correctly?

147
Still, some writing scholars (e.g., Li-xia, 2016; Mu, 2005)
extended the view of writing strategies by taking rhetorics,
communication and social constructionist theories into account in
their taxonomies of writing strategies in addition to the cognitive-
processing theory. Mu (2005), for example, categorized writing
strategies into rhetorical, metacognitive, cognitive,
communicative, and social/affective strategies, each of which
includes substrategies as shown in Figure 5.4 below.
Figure 5.4: Mu’s taxonomy of ESL writing strategies (Mu, 2005, p.
9)
Writing strategies Sub-strategies

Rhetorical strategies Organization,

Use of L1,

Formatting/Modelling,

Comparing different rhetorical


conventions.

Meta-cognitive strategies Planning,

Monitoring,

Evaluating.

Cognitive strategies Generating ideas,

Revising,

Elaborating,

Clarification,

Retrieval (getting information from


memory),

Rehearsing,

Summarizing.

148
(Figure 5.4 Continued)
Communicative strategies Avoidance,

Reduction,

Sense of readers (anticipating

readers’ response).

Social/affective strategies Resourcing,

Getting feedback,

Assigning goals,

Rest/deferral (reducing anxiety).

Some writing researchers (e.g., Arndt, 1987; Victori, 1995)


identified writing strategies based on interviews and think-aloud
protocol analysis. Arndt (1987), for example, identified eight
categories of writing strategies. These strategies are planning (i.e.,
deciding what to write about), global planning (i.e., deciding how
to organize the text as a whole), rehearsing, repeating, re-reading,
questioning, revision, and editing. On the same basis as that of
Arndt, Victori (1995) identified seven categories of writing
strategies. These categories are: planning, monitoring, evaluating,
resourcing, repeating, reduction, and use of L1 strategies. Along
the same line, Riazi (1997) classified writing strategies into macro
and micro strategies on the basis of students' perceptions of their
own writing strategies. He divided students’ macro writing
strategies into cognitive, metacognitive, and social strategies in
addition to a fourth category, "search strategies," he himself
discerned. These four macro-level strategies are subdivided into
fourteen micro-level strategies as shown in Figure 5.5.

149
Figure 5.5 Riazi’s classification of writing strategies (Adapted
from Riazi, 1997, p. 122)

Macro-strategies Micro-strategies

Cognitive Strategies (a) Note-taking,

(b) Elaboration,

(c) Use of mother tongue knowledge and


skill transfer from L1,

(d) Inferencing,

(e) Drafting (revising and editing).

(a) Assigning goals,


Metacognitive Strategies
(b) Planning (making and changing
outlines),

(c) Rationalizing appropriate formats,

(d) Monitoring and evaluation.

Social Strategies (a) Appealing for clarifications,

(b) Getting feedback from professors


and peers.

Search Strategies (a) Searching and using libraries


(books, journal, ERIC),

(b) Using guidelines,

(c) Using others’ writing as a model.

150
It is evident that some classifications of writing strategies
consider writing strategies as different from the stages of the
writing process while others view the stages of the writing process
(particularly planning, drafting and revising) as writing strategies.
For example, while Victori (1995) and many others regard
planning as a strategy, Graham et al. (2012) regard it as a
subprocess or a stage of the writing process. Furthermore, some
researchers (e.g., Mu, 2005) take rhetorical and communication
theories into account while others (e.g., Wenden, 1991b) do not.
However, “such multiplicity of categorizations have no doubt
helped to build a composite picture of the writers’ behaviours
while writing” (Peñuelas, 2012, p. 84).

5.3. Benefits of writing strategies


The importance of writing strategies is widely emphasized in the
literature. Many benefits of these strategies have been suggested
by various authors and researchers (e.g., Bos, 1988; Cihak and
Castle, 2011; Grabe and Kaplan, 1996; Graham and Harris, 1996;
Graham, Harris, MacArthur, and Schwartz, 1991; Harris and
Pressley, 1991; Santos, 2010; Sawyer, Graham, and Harris, 1992;
Schmidt, Deshler, Schumaker, and Alley, 1988/1989). These
benefits include:
• Improving the writing of students with and without learning
disabilities,
• Providing students with effective ways of overcoming the
obstacles that the process of writing tends to pose to any writer,
• Enabling learners to take control of their writing process,
• Increasing learners' confidence as independent writers,
• Facilitating the execution of planning, drafting, and revising the
text,
• Helping students discover new ideas, generate appropriate ideas
and organize these ideas,

151
• Helping students become independent writers,
• Offering students with learning disabilities the tools they need to
become successful, independent writers,
• Developing students’ self-confidence as independent writers,
• Enhancing motivation and developing positive attitudes towards
writing,
• Removing writing anxiety,
• Promoting students’ critical reflection,
• Improving the written expression skills for students with writing
deficits,
• Helping students know their writing strengths and limitations,
• Developing a sense of audience for writing,
• Allowing teachers to assess students’ needs and to offer learning
directions based on these needs, and
• Creating lifelong authentic writers.
To the above-mentioned list, Santangelo, Harris, and Graham
(2008) add that writing strategies instruction has been shown to be
an effective instructional intervention for students with learning
disabilities. They further mention a number of reasons why these
strategies are especially beneficial for these students in the
following way:
First, they help simplify and organize the complex tasks
such as planning, generating, and revising text. Second,
they define a course of action for successfully completing
all, or part, of a writing assignment. Third, they make the
mental operations that occur during planning, composing,
evaluating, and revising visible and concrete. This is
particularly salient because contemporary approaches to
writing instruction (e.g., Writer’s Workshop) encourage

152
students to plan, draft, edit, revise, and publish their
written work, yet surprisingly little attention is devoted to
explicitly teaching these processes (Graham & Harris,
1997). Finally, strategies enhance students’ knowledge
about writing genres and devices, the writing process, and
their capabilities as writers. (p. 81)
In support of the benefits of writing strategies, many research
studies (e.g., Fidalgo, Torrance, and García, 2008; Graham,
Harris, and Larsen, 2001; Wong, Wong, and Blenkinsop, 1989)
found that writing strategies instruction improved both the
quantity and quality of the writing of students with and
without disabilities. Fidalgo, Torrance, and García (2008), for
example, found that strategy-based instruction potentially
impacted student writing beyond a short-term experimental
context or classroom. The results of their study provided a “robust
evidence that strategy-focused instruction delivered to sixth-grade
students results in an increased tendency to pre-plan and in
improvements in text quality that persist at least until eighth
grade” (p. 688).
Due to the benefits of writing strategies for students with and
without writing disabilities, the next section will offer a multiple-
strategy model that combines the features of the process approach
and the genre approach to teaching these strategies to students
with writing disabilities.

5.4. A model for teaching writing strategies to


students with writing disabilities
In this section, the author proposes a four-phase model for
teaching both writing strategies and genre-specific strategies for
each stage of the writing process through a gradual release of
responsibility from the teacher to the student. In this model, the
teacher demonstrates the use of the various strategies specific to
planning, drafting, revising, and editing (one or more at a time for

153
each stage) while writing an actual composition. In addition to
teaching and modeling general writing strategies, the teacher also
teaches and models the genre-specific strategies that fit the writing
topic. These genre-specific strategies include argumentating,
narrating, comparing/contrasting, and reporting. While modeling,
the teacher thinks aloud to draw students’ attention to the
strategies s/he employs and to the features of the genre under
focus. S/he also articulates the purpose of each strategy and
explains why s/he uses it. While doing so, the students observe
her/his modeling of the writing strategies, listen to her/his thinking
aloud, and ask for clarification if they don't understand anything.
Next, the teacher provides opportunities for students to use these
strategies in the writing process, moving from joint writing in
which the students and the teacher work together to construct a
new composition of the same genre, to independent writing in
which each student writes individually about another topic of the
same genre. Finally, the teacher provides opportunities for each
student to self-assess her/his writing performance in relation to the
writing strategies s/he employed. These four phases are the next
topics of discussion.

5.4.1. Teacher modeling of writing strategies


Teacher modeling is the core of teaching writing strategies because
it makes the invisible visible and the implicit explicit to the
students. Moreover, it is rooted in theory and research on
learning. More specifically, it is rooted in Wood et al.'s (1976)
scaffolding theory, Bandura’s (1977) social learning theory and
Vygotsky’s (1978) sociocultural theory.
In support of teacher modeling of writing strategies for each
stage of the writing process for students with learning disabilities,
many writing scholars and researchers (e.g., Dowell, Storey, and
Gleason, 1994; Gambrell and Chasen, 1991; Gleason and Isaacson,
2001; Golley, 2015; Wolf and Gearhart, 1994) note that modeling
the writing strategies within the writing process is a core element
because simply being exposed to the writing process is insufficient

154
for most students, particularly those with learning disabilities. As
Golley (2015) states, “Teaching students with learning disabilities
to use strategies to help them plan and organize their writing will
help them become more effective writers and will enable them to
clearly express their thoughts and ideas” (p. 20). Writing scholars
and researchers further note that explicit modeling of how to write
in varied genres should be another ingredient of writing
instruction to students with learning disabilities.
With the above in mind, the author’s model begins with
teacher modeling of one of the general writing strategies at a time
for each stage of the writing process. The teacher also models the
genre-specific strategies that fit the topic s/he is working with.
While doing so, s/he thinks aloud and verbalizes everything that
goes in her/his mind at the various stages of the writing process.
During teacher modeling, the students watch, listen and ask for
clarification if they don't understand anything. In the suggested
model, the teacher modeling phase is divided into four stages as
research continually emphasizes that the most successful
intervention is using a basic framework for writing that includes
planning, writing, revising, and editing. The teacher modeling of
general and specific writing strategies at these four stages is
explained in details below.

5.4.1.1. Planning
At this stage, the teacher models how to plan for the topic s/he is
going to write about. S/he first of all sets a purpose and identifies
an audience for her/his writing. S/he then uses one of the strategies
for generating ideas about this topic (e. g., brainstorming, free
writing, jotting down notes, etc.). After that, s/he uses one of the
strategies for organizing the ideas, s/he has already generated,
according to the genre of the topic in action (e.g., webbing,
clustering, tree-mapping, Venn diagramming, wheel writing,
etc.). While modeling how to plan, the teacher makes the
invisible visible by thinking aloud and verbalizing everything

155
that goes in her/his mind; and the students listen and watch. It is
important to note here that the teacher should model all strategies
alternately and systematically, one by one in each session over the
course, until all are over.
At this stage, the teacher can also model the use of a "Planning
Think Sheet" that contains a series of sequential questions as
prompts for planning. Examples of these questions are: "Who am
I writing for?" "Why am I writing?" "What do I know?" "How
can I group my ideas?" and "How will I organize my ideas?"
(Englert, Raphael, and Anderson, 1992).

5.4.1.2. Drafting
At this stage, the teacher models how to elaborate the ideas s/he
has generated in the planning stage, to fit the purpose for writing
as well as the genre under focus, but s/he may make changes to
the plan when it is necessary. While drafting, the teacher places
her/his thoughts on a whiteboard or a chart paper and writes
without worrying about form. S/he also uses strategies such as self-
questioning and asking for clarification where necessary. All this is
accompanied by thinking aloud to make the reasoning behind
what s/he does explicit. S/he also models genre-specific strategies
(e.g., persuading, describing, comparing/contrasting,
narrating, informing, explaining, convincing, etc.) depending on
the genre s/he is working with, and draws students attention to the
procedures of this genre by verbalizing thoughts as s/he writes to
make the invisible visible.

5.4.1.3. Revising
At this stage, the teacher models how to revise her/his first draft.
S/he reads aloud this draft to add, substitute, delete, modify,
expand and/or rearrange ideas to be more understandable to the
reader. While doing so, s/he verbalizes the strategies s/he applies to

156
help students know what to do when revising their own drafts.
S/he also uses strategies such as asking for clarification, self-
questioning and sharing the rough draft in a writing group
(Mather, Wendling, and Roberts, 2009).

At this stage, the teacher can model the use of a prompt sheet
that guides her/him to revise what s/he has written in terms of
purpose, audience, and genre of writing. Such a prompt
sheet should contain questions such as the following (Poway
Unified School District, n.d.):
• Is my purpose clear to the reader?
• Did I clearly maintain for that purpose throughout the essay?
• Does all my supporting information clearly relate to my purpose?
• Did I organize my ideas to best fulfill my purpose?
• Is the level of detail appropriate to the audience (not too general
or too specific)?
• Are my ideas presented in a logical order that will be evident to
the reader?
• Did I say what I mean and mean what I say?
• Is my tone and style appropriate to the audience?
• What misconceptions might readers have of my topic and/or my
approach to it? How can I dispel these misconceptions?
• Did I follow the genre of the topic I am writing about?
• Did I use clear transitions to help the reader follow my
train of thought?
• Did I maintain balance among my points, developing each to the
same extent?
• Did I separate ideas into paragraphs with clear topic sentences?

157
• Do ideas flow from one to another in a recognizably organized
way according to the genre under focus?
• Do paragraphs create a chain?
• Is old and new information balanced and manipulated?

5.4.1.4. Editing
At this final stage of teacher modeling, the teacher models
proofreading the final draft. S/he proofreads her/his draft and
corrects the mechanical mistakes s/he notices and thinks aloud
about the reasons for her/his editorial changes. Then s/he asks one
of the students to proofread this draft to identify and correct
remaining mistakes. As Widodo (2008) states:
In editing, students get involved in fine tuning their own
drafts as they prepare the final drafts for a product
assessment by the teacher. In this regard, the students are
required to check minor mistakes related to grammar
(i.e., tenses or subject-verb agreements), spellings,
punctuations, dictions, and contractions. Thus, the goal of
this activity is to produce well-written essays before the
students submit the work to the teacher. (p. 104)

5.4.2. Joint application of writing strategies


Armed with a clear understanding of the writing strategies and
genre-specific strategies students should use before, during and
after writing, they are now ready to work—but not to work
independently yet. At this phase, the students and the teacher
work together to construct a new text. While doing so, they go
through the writing stages of planning, drafting, revising, and
editing and use the modeled strategies with a new writing task.
The students contribute information and ideas, and the teacher
writes the generated text on the board. The teacher can also guide
students to co-operate in small or peer groups, to practice applying

158
the modeled strategies in a workshop, with her/his assistance and
guidance to each group or pair by turn (Badger and White, 2000).
During the workshop, the teacher keeps students on track,
contributes insight and further knowledge, asks for further
elaboration on an idea to encourage students to more fully explore
the topic they are working with.

5.4.3. Independent application of writing strategies


At this phase, each student works individually and independently
to produce her/his own text. S/he writes on a related topic of
her/his choice applying the same processes and strategies modeled
and practiced in the previous stages. While doing so, the teacher
can move among students to assess students' writing needs and
strengths throughout their writing process. If class time is not
enough for the completion of this phase, the writing task can be set
as a homework.

5.4.4. Self-assessment
At this phase, the student self-assesses her/his writing performance
in relation to the strategies s/he employed before, during and after
writing. This phase is necessary because it helps the learner to
know his own strengths and weaknesses in writing strategies,
which in turn motivates further learning. For self-assessment to be
effective, according to Boud (1995), students need:
• A clear rationale of this particular activity,
• Explicit procedures of what is expected of them,
• Reassurance of a safe environment in which they can be
honest about their own performance without the fear that
they will expose information which can be used against
them,
• Confidence that other students will do likewise, and that
cheating or collusion will be detected and discouraged. (p.
182)

159
Further, Sturomski (1997) suggests providing students with
key questions that can be used as prompts for self-evaluation to
draw their attention to reflect upon the strategies used to complete
the task. He further suggests that it is important to incorporate the
following questions into a self-evaluation sheet for the learners’
reference:
• What aspects of the task did I complete well?
• What aspects were difficult?
• Did any problems arise, and what did I do to solve the problems?
• What might I do differently the next time I have to complete a
similar task?
In the same vein, Finch and Sampson (2003) suggest providing
each student with an assessment tool, such as the one given below,
to make it easy for her/him to self-assess her/his own writing in
relation to the writing strategies s/he has already used before,
during, and after writing.
Figure 5.6: A self-assessment tool of writing strategies (Adapted
from Finch and Sampson, 2003, pp. 82-83)
Name: ----------------------------------. Date: -----------------.

Title of piece of writing: --------------------------------------.

________________________________________________

Before writing:

1. I talked to a friend or partner about the topic.

2. I made a list of ideas on the topic.

3. I made an outline or thinking map.

160
(Figure 5.6 Continued)
During writing:

4. I skipped words I didn’t know and went

back to them later.

5. I substituted a word from my own language.

6. I used drawings or pictures in my writing.

After writing

7. I checked to see if the writing met my purpose.

8. I reread to see if it made sense.

9. I added information.

10. I corrected mechanical mistakes.

Other strategies I used:

------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
-----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------.

The strategies I found most useful for expressing my thoughts were:

------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------.

The strategies I found difficult to use were:

------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
---------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------.

161
To conclude this section, there are two important
considerations that should be taken into account when using the
above model to help students with learning disabilities at the
intermediate level and beyond to improve their writing. These two
considerations are that the teacher should (1) model both writing
strategies and genre-specific strategies for each stage of the writing
process; and (2) gradually release responsibility to the students.
Each of these considerations is discussed in more details below.
(1) The teacher should model strategies for each stage of the
writing process. The case is very strong that effective writers
apply a series of writing strategies selectively and
independently before, during and after writing. Therefore,
“Teaching them [students with learning disabilities] strategies
for each stage of writing will provide them with the
opportunity to get through each part with ease” (Golley, 2015,
p. 1). Although each strategy may be modeled singly, the
teacher should teach students how to coordinate these
strategies. As the teacher models and demonstrates the
coordinated use of strategies, s/he should use strategies that
complete each other and fit the genre of the writing topic. S/he
should also teach students how to use different strategies in
different contexts and different stages of the writing process.
If there are many strategies that can be used for achieving the
same purpose within one stage of the writing process like
planning; for example, the teacher should model one of these
strategies at a time in each session over the course. In support
of the effectiveness of embedding strategy instruction in the
context of the process approach to writing, Danoff, Harris,
and Graham (1993) state that such incorporation of strategy
instruction within the writing process helps students to use
writing strategies in the context in which they are expected to
apply, “increasing the likelihood that they will see the
relevance of the strategies and be more likely to maintain and
generalize their use” (p. 296). Chalk, Hagan-Burke, and Burke

162
(2005 ) also state, “Many students with learning disabilities
(LD) exhibit deficiencies in the writing process. In order to
achieve an adequate level of writing competence, these
students must apply strategies that enable them to effectively
plan, organize, write, and revise a written product” (p 75).
Likewise, Graham and Harris (2009) state that the teaching of
writing strategies which help students with the stages of the
writing process seem to generate marked increases in student
writing quality. In support of incorporating strategy
instruction within the writing process, Graham and Perin
(2007a) found in their meta-analysis of writing instruction that
“[e]xplicitly teaching adolescents strategies for planning,
revising, and/or editing had a strong impact on the quality of
their writing” (p. 463).

In addition to modeling general writing strategies, the


teacher should also model the genre-specific strategies within
the writing process. Each genre-specific text structure should
be modeled to the students (one per session) throughout the
various stages of the writing process. At the planning stage,
the teacher models the use of the graphic organizer that best
suits the genre of the topic s/he is working with (e.g.,
sequential paragraph organizer, compare and contrast
organizer, descriptive organizer, cause and effect organizer,
etc.). At the drafting stage, the teacher models the
development of ideas from the graphic organizer with enough
supporting details to fit the genre under focus by using
genre-specific strategies (e.g., persuading, describing,
comparing/contrasting, narrating, informing, explaining,
convincing, etc.). For example, if the topic is argumentative,
the teacher should model one of the two main methods of
presenting an argument: the balanced view or the persuasive
view. The one s/he chooses should depend on how the essay
title is worded. If s/he chooses the balanced view, her/his essay
should run as follows:

163
a. Introducing the argument to the reader, explaining why it
is particularly a relevant topic nowadays with reference to
some of the comments that have been voiced on it recently,
b. Offering reasons in favor of the issue under
argumentation,
c. Offering reasons against the issue,
d. Summarizing the two sides while pointing out the
strengths and limitations of both.
On the other hand, if the teacher chooses the persuasive
view, her/his essay should run as follows:
a. Introducing the argument to the reader, explaining why it
is particularly a relevant topic nowadays with reference
to some of the comments that have been voiced on it
recently,
b. Offering reasons in favor of or against the issue under
argumentation,
c. Providing evidence that clearly support her/his reasons to
align the reader with her/his point of view.

While implementing the procedures of the genre under


focus, such as the ones mentioned above, the teacher
verbalizes her/his thoughts as well as the genre conventions.
At the revision stage, s/he checks to see if the organizational
pattern fits the purpose and the genre of the topic.
The teacher should systematically shift from one genre to
another. This shift is highly significant until the students know
how to apply writing strategies to various writing situations
that involve different genres. After modeling and applying the
most common types of writing genres, the teacher may move
to multigenre topics according to students’ needs.

164
In support of the effectiveness of incorporating genre-
specific strategies instruction within the writing process in the
regular classroom, Wong (1997) found that teaching students
with learning disabilities in the ninth, eighth, and tenth
grades how to write three different genres of expository essays
(reportive, persuasive, and compare/contrast) over a three-
year period (one per year) increased their mean scores for
writing clarity and other genre-specific variables (e.g.,
thematic salience, organization of ideas) from pretest to
posttest (For more details of this study, see section 5.5 of this
chapter).
(2) The teacher should gradually release responsibility to the
students. The gradual release of responsibility from the
teacher to the student lies at the heart of this model. As the
model proceeds, the responsibility shifts more and more to the
student who eventually ends up with full responsibility. In
each writing session, the teacher gradually releases control to
enable the student to make progress and gain independence in
using the writing strategies. The teacher is in control of the
writing event when s/he models and demonstrates for the
student. This assistance is withdrawn gradually and
systematically passing responsibility to the individual student
as s/he gains control and becomes able to work and apply
strategies independently. In other words, the teacher shifts
gradually from the role of a supporter to the role of a
sympathetic audience (Palinscar and Brown, 1984) and the
role of the student increases as that of the teacher diminishes.
This is exactly the core idea behind scaffolding. Leong,
Bodrova, Hensen and Henninger (1999) explain this idea in
the following way:
When you build a building, you build a scaffold with
the size and shape of the building in mind. In the
initial stages, the contractor provides more scaffolding
than later, when the walls are established and the
foundation is secure. If the scaffolding is removed too
early, the building will also suffer. If the scaffolding is
not removed, the contractor cannot build another
165
building. In teaching, we provide more support at the
beginning stages of the skill/concept formation. If we
remove the support too early the child may have
incomplete or incorrect understanding. If we leave the
supports too long, the child will not be encouraged to
move on to new learning. (p. 3)
With the above in mind, the teacher should make sure
that s/he does not release responsibility to the students too
early. In some cases, this means that “[t]eachers may need to
model an entire strategy or parts of a strategy again before
students can work independently” (Graham et al. 2012, p. 17).

5.5. Research on teaching writing strategies to


students with writing disabilities
The teaching of writing strategies has been demonstrated as an
effective writing intervention, by many researchers, for students of
all ages and abilities. However, this section only offers a review of
writing strategies research in the area of learning disabilities.
Graham and Harris (1989a) investigated the effect of story
grammar instruction on the story writing of students with learning
disabilities. Twenty two students with learning disabilities in the
fifth and sixth grades were taught narrative text-structure (i.e.
story grammar instruction) in order to improve the overall quality
of their stories. The students were instructed in small groups in
their resource rooms on these eight story-grammar elements: main
character, locale, time, starter event, goal, action, ending, and
reaction. The results of the study indicated that, the inclusion of
story-grammar elements at posttest, generalization, and
maintenance was significantly higher than at pretest for twenty of
the twenty two students with learning disabilities. With regard to
quality, the students with learning disabilities increased their
average scores from 2.14 at pretest to 2.91 at posttest.

166
MacArthur, Graham, and Schwartz (1991) investigated the
effect of revision strategy instruction on the narratives written by
students with learning disabilities. The participants for this study
consisted of four classes, who were randomly assigned to an
experimental group and a control group. The experimental
students received explicit instruction, modeling, and guided
practice in the collaborative use of the strategy. The students of
the control group used the strategy individually. The paired
students also received interaction-instruction (e.g., “Tell the
author what the paper is about and what you liked best”). To
assess writing and revision quality, two writing assignments were
administered as both a pre and post-test. The final drafts were
assessed on overall quality and on the number and quality of
revisions (content and editing aspects; spelling, use of capitals,
punctuation). Revisions were categorized by text level, impact on
meaning, and quality. The results of the post-test showed that the
peer response students produced texts of higher quality and made
more and better revisions than the students who used the strategy
individually. Transcripts of peer interactions suggested that the
performance of the peer response students was mediated by use of
the strategy. All students followed the strategy and gave
suggestions for adding information or detail and for improving
clarity or organization as well. Results of a metacognitive
interview on the knowledge of criteria for good writing indicated
that the peer response students demonstrated greater awareness of
criteria for evaluating writing.
Danoff, Harris, and Graham, (1993) examined the
effectiveness of embedding strategy instruction in the context of a
process approach to writing in inclusive classrooms. Through a
series of extended mini-lessons during writers' workshop, both
students with and without a learning disability were taught a
previously validated writing strategy and procedures for
regulating the strategy and the writing process. The strategy
instructional procedures had a positive effect on the participating
fourth- and fifth grade students' writing. The schematic structure

167
of their stories improved substantially following instruction and
remained improved over time and with a different teacher. The
quality of what was written also improved for all but two of the
students following instruction. Overall, improvements in story
quality were maintained and generalized by all of the students,
except for the younger fourth graders and one fifth-grade student
who failed to maintain quality gains on a generalization probe. In
addition, one of the students who had not evidenced quality gains
immediately following instruction, wrote qualitatively better
stories on the generalization and maintenance probes. Data
collected during instruction demonstrated that the best results
were obtained when all stages and components of instruction were
enacted. Finally, Danoff et al. concluded that “incorporating
strategy instruction into a process approach to writing can
meaningfully augment students' composition skills” (p. 319).
Stoddard and MacArthur (1993) examined the effects of an
approach that integrated strategy-instruction, peer response, and
word processing on the revision of narratives of six learning
disabled students (age 13-15). Students used a revision strategy
consisting of questions which incorporated criteria for evaluation
(e.g. “Does the text follow a logical sequence?” “Where could more
details be added?”), and an overall strategy for regulating the
revision process (a prompting sheet with key words for the
revision of meaning and mechanical errors). The students received
explicit instruction, modeling, and guided practice in the use of the
strategy. They were instructed in rules for regulating the
interaction process as well. Pre- and post-test performances on
writing and revision tasks were compared. On the pre-tests, the
students made few substantive revisions and did not improve the
quality of their papers by revising them. On the post-tests all
students made more substantive revisions, the proportion of
revisions rated as improvements increased from 47% to 83%.
Second revised drafts were rated as significantly better than first
drafts. Furthermore, the overall quality of final drafts increased
substantially from pre-tests to post-tests.

168
Hallenbeck (1995) adapted the Cognitive Strategy in Writing
(CSIW) program, which had been effectively used with elementary
students with learning disabilities, to an older population of
students. The CSIW embodies three guiding principles: (1)
effective writing is seen a holistic enterprise involving the
processes of planning, organizing, writing, revising, and editing;
(2) teachers scaffold students' use of specific writing strategies;
and (3) students write for authentic purposes and real audiences
and collaborate with each other. Subjects included seven junior
high and high school students with learning disabilities who
demonstrated difficulties with written expression. The students
learned CSIW and practiced the strategies on two text structures
(one requiring explaining a process and the other discussing what
they know about a topic) over the course of a school year. Pretest
and posttest assessments of overall quality, structure-specific
primary traits, paper length, and reader sensitivity indicated
improvement in students' writing during the year. T-tests
demonstrated that students showed significant improvement on all
measures of their writing ability.
Dellerman, Coirier, and Marchand (1996) examined the
effects of planning on the argumentative writing of nonproficient
writers. They hypothesized that the quality of an argumentative
text is dependent on prior planning of the argumentative
relationships (logical, thematic, and directional) and the writer’s
proficiency. They also expected that planning would be most
beneficial to nonproficient writers on the basis of the assumptions
that planning would improve the organization of information and
increase the available cognitive resources for high-level processes.
The participants were asked to complete a constrained
argumentative composition based on 13 arguments that were
provided in 30 minutes. Although there was no global effect of
planning on the quality of written texts, the results showed that
planning focused on logical relationships had a significant effect
on the argumentative texts produced. As Dellerman et al.
expected, planning was most effective for nonproficient writers.

169
Wong (1997) investigated the effect of genre instruction on the
writing of adolescents with learning disabilities. Fifteen students
with learning disabilities in the ninth, eighth, and tenth grades
were taught how to write three different genres of expository
essays (reportive, persuasive, and compare/contrast) over a three-
year period (one per year). Within each intervention, during the
planning phase, Wong explained the writing process to the
students, emphasizing the recursive nature of the various stages of
planning, writing, and revising through thinking aloud.
Throughout the writing process, students received assistance from
members of the intervention team in articulating their
communicative intent and ideas, structuring sentences, choosing
appropriate words, and spelling. The results of the study indicated
that across the three types of essays, the students were able to
increase their mean scores for writing clarity and other genre-
specific variables (e.g., thematic salience, organization of ideas)
from pretest to posttest. Wong gave the following three reasons
that contributed to the success of the interventions:
(1) Use of one appropriate way of instructing adolescents with
learning disabilities and low achievers to write one particular
genre,
(2) Focused and intensive nature of the writing instruction, and
(3) Use of interactive dialogues in conferences between students
and intervention researchers that contributed much to the
writing enhancement.
Gersten and Baker (1999b) conducted an exploratory meta-
analysis of experimental and quasi-experimental studies to
investigate the research-based instructional approaches to
teaching written expression to students with learning disabilities.
Expressive writing was defined as writing for the purpose of
displaying knowledge or supporting self-expression. The meta-
analysis addressed this question, "Given a group of studies
designed explicitly for the purpose of improving the writing of
students with learning disabilities, which interventions and

170
components were found to be most effective, and what is the
strength of their effects?" The findings revealed the following
three components as ones that reliably and consistently led to
improved outcomes in teaching expressive writing to students with
learning disabilities:

(1) Adhering to a basic framework of planning, writing, and


revision,

(2) Explicitly teaching critical steps in the writing process, and

(3) Providing feedback guided by the information explicitly


taught.

De La Paz (1999) investigated the effect of self-regulated


strategy instruction on the writing outcomes of students with and
without learning disabilities. She taught middle school students
with and without learning disabilities a strategy for planning and
writing expository texts, using the Self-Regulated Strategy
Development (SRSD) model within a general education setting.
All of the students were taught how to plan for the writing genre
(i.e., expository) and the writing task used on the statewide writing
competency test by their general education teachers who followed
scripted lesson plans. The intervention included strategies to help
students plan in response to the assessment prompt and to
encourage them to continue planning while writing their essays.
The results of the study indicated that after the intervention, all of
the students generated pre-writing plans, and approximately half
of the students' plans were appropriately relevant to the topic
prompt. All of the students increased the length of their essays,
and the students with LD increased the length of their essays by
250 percent. All of the students also doubled and/or tripled the
average number of functional expository elements (e.g., premise,
reason) present in their essays. These positive gains were
maintained four weeks later.

171
Troia, Graham, and Harris (1999) examined the effect of
planning instruction on the writing of students with learning
disabilities. The subjects of the study consisted of three 5th-grade
students with learning disabilities. These students were
individually taught methods for planning narrative and expository
essays over a three-week period. Instruction in the planning
strategies followed the Self-Regulated Strategy Development
(SRSD) model (Harris and Graham, 1996), and the students were
instructed to set goals, brainstorm ideas, sequence their ideas, and
complete self-selected homework assignments. The intervention
also included the use of acronyms and mnemonics to help students
within the planning process. The results of the study indicated that
after the intervention, the students dramatically changed their
pre-writing planning behavior, and this favorably impacted their
writing. Following instruction, the students increased their
planning time and devoted as much time to their planning as they
did to writing. They also increased the length of their stories and
made an average gain of 3.1 points on their story-grammar scores
(i.e. inclusion of basic story elements) from 7.1 at baseline to 10.2
at post-instruction (total possible score was 21 points). In addition,
they were able to generalize these effects to writing persuasive
essays and made an average gain of 3.8 points on the number of
functional expository elements present (e.g., premise, line of
argument) from 7.0 at baseline to 10.8 at post-instruction. These
positive effects were maintained three weeks later.

Gersten and Baker (2001) conducted a meta-analysis of 13


intervention studies with students with learning disabilities to
determine the impact of writing strategy interventions on the
writing of these students and to identify instructional components
associated with the best writing outcomes for them. They reported
overall weighted effect sizes ranging from 0.41 to 1.17 with an
aggregate effect size of 0.81, which represents a large effect
favoring the selected interventions, for varied measures of writing

172
including standardized writing tests, quality ratings of student
papers, and scores on trait and genre structure rubrics. Based on
the results of their meta-analysis, Gersten and Baker identified
five components that appeared to be associated with strong
positive writing outcomes for poor writers in the set of studies they
examined. These components are:
(1) Explicit teacher modeling of the writing process and composing
strategies,
(2) Peer collaboration and teacher conferencing to gain
informative feedback,
(3) Use of procedural prompts (e.g., graphic organizers,
mnemonics, outlines, checklists) to facilitate planning and
revising,
(4) Limiting barriers produced by poor text transcription (e.g.,
dictating), and
(5) Self-regulation (e.g., self-statements and questions).
ERIC Clearinghouse on Disabilities and Gifted Education
(2002) conducted a meta-analysis of research on teaching
expressive writing to students with learning disabilities. Virtually
all the interventions analyzed were multifaceted and involved
students writing everyday as part of the curriculum. The meta-
analysis identified several themes critical to effective writing
instruction: (1) adherence to a basic framework of planning,
writing, and revision; (2) explicit instruction of critical steps in the
writing process, as well as the features and conventions of the
writing genre or text structure; and (3) provision of feedback
guided by the information explicitly taught.
Chalk, Hagan-Burke, and Burke (2005) examined the effects
of the Self-Regulated Strategy Development (SRSD) model on the
writing performance of 15 high school sophomores with learning
disabilities. Students were taught to apply the SRSD model as a
strategy for planning and writing essays and to self-regulate their

173
use of the strategy and the writing process. The results of the study
indicated that “students benefited from an approach to writing
that helped them develop strategies for brainstorming, semantic
webbing, setting goals, and revising” (p 86). The repeated
ANOVA revealed a significant main effect, indicating that quality
improved over time, F (10, 140) = 21.5, p = 0.000. Follow-up trend
analysis revealed a linear trend, F (1.14) = 115.9, p = 0.000, with an
eta squared explaining 89% of the variance.
Cihak and Castle (2011) investigated the effect of explicit
strategy instruction on the writing of students with and without
learning disabilities. Forty eighth grade students with and without
learning disabilities in an inclusive classroom participated in the
study. Five students without disabilities were dropped from the
data analysis because of absenteeism during the posttest probe.
The intervention targeted expository essays and composing topic,
detail, transitional, and concluding sentences. A repeated-
measures ANOVA indicated that both students with and without
disabilities made significant improvements in expository writing
skills as measured on the state’s criterion reference test for written
expression. Improvements in the quality of writing emerged after
students had received the writing intervention. In pretest analysis,
students with disabilities lacked the writing skills of how to create
a topic sentence, how to use supporting details, how to use
transitions, and how to conclude a composition. In posttest
analysis, students with disabilities made significant writing
improvements. They demonstrated the skills of writing a topic
sentence, supporting the topic with details, using transitions, and
effectively concluding the composition. Moreover, students
without disabilities made significant writing improvements from
pretest to posttest. For both students with disabilities and students
without disabilities, the greatest developments between pretest and
posttest compositions were paragraph structure. Essays were
organized and themes well developed. Compositions including the
presence of an introductory sentence and central ideas were
expanded coherently using detailed sentences. Transition and

174
concluding sentences were also exhibited. Moreover, sentence
structure and syntactic variety improved. Overall, students wrote
expository essays that were qualitatively better, which were
generally free from mechanical errors and language misusage.

To sum up, it is evident from the previous research on


teaching writing strategies to learning-disabled students that: (1)
most research into writing strategies focused almost exclusively on
writing strategies in isolation rather than in combination; (2) with
the exception of Danoff, Harris, and Graham’s study (1993),
multiple strategies instruction has not yet been investigated in
conjunction with the writing process to determine
their combined impact on the writing performance of students
with learning disabilities; and (3) there are no studies on the effect
of combinations of writing strategies and genre-specific strategies
within the writing process on the writing performance of students
with learning disabilities.

175
References
Aarnoutse, C. (1997). Teaching reading comprehension strategies to very poor
decoders in a listening situation. Paper presented at the annual meeting
of the American Educational Research Association, Chicago.

Abbott, M. L. (2006). ESL reading strategies: Differences in Arabic and


Mandarin speaker test performance. Language Learning, 56(4), 633-
670.
Abbott, M. (2010). An introspective study of Arabic and Mandarin-speakers
reading comprehension strategies. TESL Canada Journal/Revue TESL
Du Canada, 28(1), 14-40.

Aghaei, R., and Pillaie, S. (2011). On the explicit instruction of cognitive and
metacognitive reading strategies in reading performance and self-
efficacy. The Iranian EFL Journal, 7(5), 98-118.
Alabama Department of Education (2015). Foundations for beginning
reading: Modules for professional learning. Montgomery, AL.: Alabama
Reading Initiative.

Alexander, P.A., and Jetton, T. L. (2000). Learning from text: A


multidimensional and developmental perspective. In Kamil, M. L.,
Mosenthal, P. B., Pearson, P. D., and Barr, R. (eds.), Handbook of
reading research, Vol. 3 (pp. 285-310). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Alfassi, M. (1998). Reading for meaning: The efficacy of reciprocal teaching
in fostering reading comprehension in high school students in remedial
reading classes. American Educational Research Journal, 35(2), 309–
332.
Alfassi, M., Weiss, I., and Lifshitz, H. (2009). The efficacy of reciprocal
teaching in fostering the reading literacy of students with intellectual
disabilities. European Journal of Special Needs Education, 24(3), 291-
305.
Allsopp, D. H., Minskoff, E. H., and Bolt, L. (2005). Individualized course-
specific strategy instruction for college students with learning
disabilities and ADHD: Lessons learned from a model demonstration
project. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 20(2), 103-118.
Allwright, D. (1984). Why don't learners learn what teachers teach? The
interaction hypothesis. In D. M. Singleton and D. G. Little (eds.),
Language learning in formal and informal contexts. Dublin, Ireland:
Irish Association for Applied Linguistics.

American Psychiatric Association (APA). (2000). Diagnostic and statistical


manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR (4th edition). Washington DC:
American Psychiatric Association.

Anderson, A., Howe, C., Soden, R., Halliday, J., and Low, J. (2001). Peer
interaction and the learning of critical thinking skills in further
education students. Instructional Science, 29, 1–32.
Anderson, N. J. (2005). L2 learning strategies. In E. Hinkel (ed.), Handbook
of research in second language teaching and learning (pp. 757-
771). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Anderson, N. J., and Vandergrift, L. (1996). Increasing metacognitive


awareness in the L2 classroom by using think-aloud protocols and
other verbal report formats. In R. L. Oxford (ed.), Language learning
strategies around the world: Cross-cultural perspectives (pp. 3-18).
National Foreign Language Resource Center. Manoa: University of
Hawai'i Press.

Andrade, H., and Boulay, B. (2003). Gender and the role of rubric-
referenced self-assessment in learning to write. Journal of Educational
Research, 97(1), 21-34.

Andrade, H. and Du, Y. (2005) Knowing what counts and thinking about
quality: students report on how they use rubrics, Practical Assessment,
Research and Evaluation, 10(4). Retrieved from
http://PAREonline.net/getvn.asp?v=10&n=3.

Andrade, H., and Du, Y. (2007). Student responses to criteria-referenced


self-assessment. Assessment and Evaluation in Higher Education, 32(2),
159-181.

Arabsolghar, F. and Elkins, J. (2001). Teachers' expectations about


students' use of reading strategies, knowledge and behavior in Grades
3, 5 and 7. Journal of Research in Reading, 2, 154-162.
Arndt, V. (1987). Six writers in search of texts: A protocol-based study of L1
and L2 writing. ELT Journal, 41, 257-267.

177
Arnold, J. (1999). Affect in language learning. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.

Arries, J. F. (1999). Learning disabilities and foreign languages: A


curriculum approach to the design of inclusive courses. The Modern
Language Journal, 83, 98-110.

Asaro-Saddler, K. (2008). The effects of planning instruction and self -


regulation training on the writing performance of young writers with
autism spectrum disorders. New York: State University of New York at
Albany. Retrieved from
http://search.proquest.com/docview/304356319.
Asaro-Saddler, K. and Saddler, B. (2010). Planning instruction and self-
regulation training: Effects on writers with autism spectrum disorders.
Exceptional Children, 77(1), 107-124.

Asato, Moriatsu (2003). Challenge and change for EFL oral communication
instruction. Washington DC: ERIC Clearinghouse on Languages and
Linguistics. [ERIC Document No. ED 475 020]. Available at:
http://www.eric.ed.gov/

Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. and Turken, A. (1999). A neuropsychological theory


of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychological Review,
106, 529–550.

Badger, R. and White, G. (2000). A process genre approach to teaching


writing. ELT Journal, 54(2), 153–60.

Baker, L. and Brown, A. L. (1984). Metacognitive skills and reading. In P.


D. Pearson, R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, and P. Mosenthal (eds.), Handbook
of reading research (pp. 353-394). White Plains, NY: Longman.
Baker, S., Gerstein, R., and Graham, S. (2003). Teaching expressive writing
to students with learning disabilities: Research-based applications and
examples. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, 109-123.

Baker, L. and Brown, A. (1984). Metacognitive skills and reading. In P.D.


Pearson (ed.), Handbook of reading research. White Plains, NY:
Longman.
Bakken. J., Mastropieri, M., and Scruggs. T. (1997). Reading
comprehension of expository science material and students with
learning disabilities: A comparison of strategies. The Journal of Special
Education, 31, 300-324.

178
Bakhtin, M. (1984). Problems of Dostoevsky's poetics. (Ed. and Trans. by C.
Emerson), Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.

Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of self-regulation. Organizational


Behavior and Human Processes, 50, 248-287.
Barenbaum, E., Newcomer, P., and Nodine, B. (1987). Children's ability to
write stories as a function of variation in task, age, and development
level. Learning Disability Quarterly, 7, 175-188.

Barnett, M. (1989). Teaching reading strategies: How methodology affects


course articulation. Foreign Language Annals, 21, 109-121.

Bauer, T. N. (1995). How three factions rank job and applicant attributes.
Journal of Career Planning and Employment, 55(1), 43-46.

Beckman, P. (2002). Strategy instruction. Arlington, VA: ERIC


Clearinghouse on Disabilities and Gifted Education, Council for
Exceptional Children. [ERIC Document Number: ED474302].
Available at: http://www.eric.ed.gov/

Bello, T. (1997). Improving ESL learners’ writing skills. Washington DC:


National Clearinghouse for ESL Literacy Education [ERIC Document
No. ED 409 746]. Available at: http://www.eric.ed.gov/

Benson, G. (1983). On the campus: How well do business schools prepare


graduates for the business world? Personnel, 60, 61-65.

Bereiter, C. and Bird, M. (1985). Use of thinking aloud in identification and


teaching of reading comprehension strategies. Cognition and
Instruction, 2(2), 131–156.
Bereiter, C., and Scardamalia, M. (1987). The psychology of written
composition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Bernhardt, E. B. (1991). Reading development in a second language:


Theoretical, research, and classroom perspectives. Norwood, N.J.: Ablex.

Bialystok, E. (1979). The role of conscious strategies in second language


proficiency. Canadian Modern Language Review , 35, 372-394.
Bialystok, E. (1983). Some factors in the selection and implementation of
communication strategies. In Faerch, C. and Kasper, G. (eds.).
Strategies in interlanguage communication (pp. 100–118). New
York: Longman.

179
Bialystok, E. (1990). Communication strategies: A psychological analysis of
second language use. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

Biancarosa, G. and Snow, C. E. (2006). Reading next: A vision for action and
research in middle and high school literacy — A report from Carnegie
Corporation of New York (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: Alliance for
Excellent Education.

Björk, L. and Räisänen, C. (1997). Academic writing: A university writing


course. Lund: Student literature.

Black, P., Harrison, C., Lee, C., Marshall, B. and Wiliam, D. (2003).
Assessment for learning: Putting it into practice. Buckingham, UK:
Open University Press.

Black, P. and Wiliam, D. (1998). Assessment and classroom learning.


Assessment in Education, 5(1), 7-74.

Blair, T. R., Rupley, W. H., and Nichols, W. (2007). The effective teacher of
reading: Considering the "what" and "how" of instruction. Reading
Teacher, 60, 432-438.

Blanche, P. and Merino, B. (1989). Self-assessment of foreign-language


skills: implications for teachers and researchers. Language Learning,
39(3), 313-340.

Block, E. (1986). The comprehension strategies of second language readers.


TESOL Quarterly, 20(3), 463-94.
Bolitho, R., Carter, R., Hughes, R., Ivanic, R., Masuhara, H., and
Tomlinson, B. (2003). Ten questions about language awareness. ELT
Journal, 57(3), 251-259.
Bongratz, K., Bradley, J., Fisel, K., Crcutt, J. and Shoemaker, A. (2002).
Improving student comprehension skills through the use of reading
strategies. Washington DC: ERIC Clearinghouse on Languages and
Linguistics. [ERIC Document No. ED 471073]. Available at:
http://www.eric.ed.gov/
Borkowski, J., Estrada, M. T., Milstead, M., and Hale, C. A. (1989). General
problem-solving skills: Relations between metacognition and strategic
processes. Learning Disability Quarterly, 12, 57-70.

180
Borkowski, J., Weyhing, R., and Carr, M. (1988). Effects of attributional
retraining on strategy-based reading comprehension in learning
disabled students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 46-53.

Bos, C. S. (1988). Process-oriented writing: Instructional implications for


mildly handicapped students. Exceptional Children, 54, 521-527.

Bos, C., and Anders, P. (1990). Interactive teaching and learning:


Instructional practices for teaching content and strategic knowledge.
In T. E. Scruggs and B. Y. L. Wong (eds.), Intervention Research in
Learning Disabilities (pp. 166-185). Springer New York.
Bos, C., and Fili, D. (1984). Comprehension monitoring in learning disabled
and average students. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 17, 229-233.

Boud, David (1995). Enhancing learning through self-assessment. London:


Kogan Page.

Boud, D. (2000). Sustainable assessment: Rethinking assessment for the


learning society. Studies in Continuing Education, 22, (2), 151-167.
Boud, D., Cohen, R., and Sampson, J. (1999). Peer learning and assessment.
Assessment and Evaluation in Higher Education, 24, (4), 413-426.
Branch, J. L. (2000). The trouble with think alouds: Generating data using
concurrent verbal protocols. In A. Kublik (ed.), Dimensions of a global
information science. Edmonton, Alberta: Canadian Association for
Information Science.
Brand–Gruwel, S., Aarnoutse, C., and Van Den Bos, K. (1998). Improving
text comprehension strategies in reading and listening settings.
Learning and Instruction 8(1), 63 – 81.
Brown, A., and Campione, J. C. (1986). Psychological theory and the study
of learning disabilities. American Psychologist, 41(10), 1059–1068.

Brown, A. L., and Palincsar, A. S. (1982). Inducing strategic learning from


texts by means of informed, self control training. Topics in Learning
and Learning Disabilities, 2, 1-17.

Brown, A., and Palincsar, A. (1985). Reciprocal teaching of comprehension


strategies: A natural history of one program for enhancing learning.
Urbana: University of Illinois Center for the Study of
Reading. Technical Report No. 334.

181
Brown, D. (2001). Teaching by principles: An interactive approach to
language pedagogy (2nd ed.). New York: Addison Wesley Longman,
Inc.

Brown, K. and Hood, S. (1997). Writing matters: Writing skills and strategies
for students of English. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Brown, R. (2008). The road not yet taken: A transactional strategies


approach to comprehension instruction. The Reading Teacher, 61(7),
538–547.

Brown, R., Pressley, M., Van Meter, P., and Schuder, T. (1996). A quasi-
experimental validation of transactional strategies instruction with
previously low-achieving second-grade readers. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 88, 18-37.

Bryant, D. P., Vaughn, S., Linan-Thompson, S., Ugel, N., and Hamff, A.
(2000). Reading outcomes for students with and without learning
disabilities in general education middle school content area classes.
Learning Disability Quarterly, 23(3), 24-38.

Burchard, M. S. and Swerdzewski, P. (2009). Learning effectiveness of a


strategic learning course, Journal of College Reading and Learning,
40(1), 14-21.

Bygate, M. (2001). Speaking. In R. Carter and D. Nunan (eds.) Cambridge


guide to teaching English to speakers of other languages (pp. 14-20).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Canale, M., and Swain, M. (1980). Theoretical bases of communicative
approaches to second language teaching and testing. Applied
Linguistics, 1(1), 1-47.
Cappello, M., and Moss, B. (eds.) (2010). Contemporary readings in literacy
education. New York: Sage.

Carter, C. (1997). Why reciprocal teaching? Educational Leadership, 54, 64-


69.
Chalk, J., Hagan-Burke, S. and Burke, M. (2005). The effects of self-
regulated strategy development on the writing process for high school
students with learning disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 28(1),
75-87.

182
Chamot, A. U. (1987). The learning strategies of ESL students. Prentice Hall,
New Jersey.

Chamot, A. U. (2004). Preparing language teachers to teach learning


strategies. Singapore: National University of Singapore.
Chamot, A. U. (2004). Issues in language learning strategy research and
teaching. Electronic Journal of Foreign Language Teaching, 1(1), 14-20.

Chamot, A. (2005). Language learning strategy instruction: Current issues


and research. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 25, 112-130.

Chamot, A.U., Barnhardt, S., El-Dinary, P., and Robbins, J. (1996).


Methods for teaching learning strategies in the foreign language
classroom. In R. Oxford (ed.), Language learning strategies around the
world: Cross-cultural perspectives (pp. 175-187). Manoa: University of
Hawaii Press.

Chamot, A. U., and Küpper, L. (1989). Learning strategies in foreign


language instruction. Foreign Language Annals, 22, 13-24.
Chamot, A. U., and O'Malley, J. M. (1994). Language learner and learning
strategies. In N.C. Ellis (ed.), Implicit and explicit learning of languages
(pp. 371-392). London: Academic Press.

Chamot, A. and O’Malley, J. M. (1994). The CALLA handbook:


Implementing the cognitive academic language learning approach.
White Plains, NY: Addison Wesley Longman.

Chan, D. (1998). Perceived competence of students with learning difficulties


in Hong Kong. In D. W. Chan (ed.). Helping students with learning
difficulties. Hong Kong: Chinese University Press.

Chan, D. and Lan, K. (2003). Reading strategy use and motivation among
Chinese good and poor readers in Hong Kong. Journal of Research in
Reading, 2, 177-190.

Chalk, J. C., Hagan-Burke, S., and Burke, M. D. (2005). The effects of self-
regulated strategy development on the writing process for high school
students with learning disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 28,
75-87.

Chen, Y. (2011). Study of the writing strategies used by Chinese non-


English majors. Theory and Practice in Language Studies, 1(3), 245-
251.

183
Cheng, P.W (1998). Primary teachers percentages and understanding of
learning difficulties. In D.W. Chan (ed.). Helping students with
learning difficulties. Hong Kong: Chinese University Press.

Chinn, C. A., Anderson, R. C., and Waggoner, M. A. (2001). Patterns of


discourse in two kinds of literature discussion. Reading Research
Quarterly, 36(4), 378–411.

Christophel, D. (1990) The relationship among teacher immediacy


behaviors, student motivation, and learning, Communication
Education, 39, 323–240.

Chuanchaisit, S., and Prapphal, K. (2009). A study of English


communication strategies of Thai university students. Journal of
Humanities, 17, 100-126.

Cihak, D. and Castle, K. (2011). Improving expository writing skills with


explicit and strategy instructional methods in inclusive middle school
classrooms. International Journal of Special Education, 26(3), 106-113.

Clark, F. L., Deshler, D. D., Schumaker, J. B., and Alley, G. R. (1984).


Visual imagery and self-questioning: Strategies to improve
comprehension of written materials. Journal of Learning Disabilities,
17(3), 145-149.

Cohen, A. (1998). Strategies in learning and using a second language.


Harlow, Essex: Longman.

Cohen, I. (2011) Teacher-student interaction in classrooms of students with


specific learning disabilities learning English as a foreign language.
Journal of Interactional Research in Communication Disorders, 2(2),
271-292.

Coley, J. D., DePinto, T., Sharon, C., and Gardner, R. (1993). From college
to classroom: Three teachers’ accounts of their adaptations of
reciprocal teaching. The Elementary School Journal, 94(2), 255–266.

Collins, J. L. (1998). Strategies for struggling writers. New York: Guilford.

Collins, V. L., Dickson, S. V., Simmons, D. C., and Kameenui, E. J. (1998).


Metacognition and its relation to reading comprehension: A synthesis of
the research. Eugene, OR: National Center to Improve the Tools of
Educators, University of Oregon.

184
Commonwealth of Australia (1992). Disability discrimination act. Australian
Government, Canberra.

Commonwealth of Australia (2005). Disability standards for education.


Australian Government, Canberra.

Comstock, R., and Kamara, C. (2003). Adult language/learning disability:


Issues and resources. Retrieved from ERIC database. ERIC Document
Number: ED 482 311.

Conley, D. (2007). Redefining college readiness. Retrieved on June 6, 2015,


from www.ctdhe.org/info/pdfs/2010/RedefiningCollegeReadiness.pdf.

Corder, S. P. (1981). Error analysis and interlanguage. Oxford: Oxford


University Press.

Corley, M., and Taymans, J. (2002). Adults with learning disabilities: A


review of the literature (Online). Retrieved from
www.ncsall.net/?id=575.

Coronado-Aliegro, J. (2006). The effect of self-assessment on the self-efficacy


of students studying Spanish as a foreign language. Doctoral
dissertation, University of Pittsburgh. Retrieved from
http://challenger.library.pitt.edu/ETD/available/etd12012006-
121145/unrestricted/Coronado-AliegroJ2_etd2006.pdf/

Cortiella, C. and Horowitz, S. H. (2014). The state of learning disabilities:


Facts, trends, and emerging issues (3rd ed.). New York, NY: National
Center for Learning Disabilities. Retrieved from
http://www.ncld.org/wp-content/uploads/2014/11/2014-State-of-
LD.pdf.

Crabtree, T., Alber-Morgan, S. R., and Konrad, M. (2010). The effects of


self-monitoring of story elements on the reading comprehension of
high school seniors with learning disabilities. Education and Treatment
of Children, 33, 187-203.

Cramer, C., Fate, J. and Lueders, K. (2001). Improving reading achievement


through the implementation of reading strategies. Retrieved from
http://www.eric.ed.gov/ [ED 454503].

Cresswell, A. (2000). Self-monitoring in student writing: Developing learner


responsibility. ELT Journal, 54, 244-253.

185
Crookes, G. (1989). Planning and inter-language variation. Studies in
Second Language Acquisition, 11, 367-383.

Crookes, G., and Schmidt, R. W. (1991). Motivation: Reopening the


research agenda. Language Learning, 41(4), 469-512.

Cunningham, P. M., and Allington, R. L. (1994). Classrooms that work: They


can all read and write. New York: Harper Collins.

Danoff, B., Harris, K. R., and Graham, S. (1993). Incorporating strategy


instruction within the writing process in the regular classroom: Effects
on the writing of students with and without learning disabilities.
Journal of Reading Behavior, 25, 295-319.

Davey, B., and McBride, S. (1986). Generating self-questions after reading:


A comprehension assist for elementary students. Journal of Education
Research, 80(1), 43-46.

David, V. (1975). Including the language learning disabled student in the


college English class. ERIC Document Number: ED114 823.

Dean, D. (2010). What works in writing instruction: Research and practices.


Urbana, Ill.: National Council of Teachers of English.

de Bot, K. (1996). Review article: The psycholinguistics of the output


hypothesis. Language Learning, 46, 529–555.

DeFoe, M. (1999). Using directed reading thinking activity strategies to teach


students reading comprehension skills in middle grades language arts.
ERIC Document Number: ED432011.

De La Paz, S. (1999). Self-regulated strategy instruction in regular


education settings: improving outcomes for students with and without
learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 14(2),
92-106.

De La Paz, S. (2007). Managing cognitive demands for writing: Comparing


the effects of instructional components in strategy instruction. Reading
& Writing Quarterly, 23, 249–266.

De La Paz, S. and Graham, S. (2002). Explicitly teaching strategies, skills,


and knowledge: Writing instruction in middle school classrooms.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(4), 687-698.

186
De La Paz, S., Swanson, P., and Graham, S. (1998). Contribution of
executive control to the revising problems of students with writing and
learning difficulties. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 448–460.

Dellerman, P., Coirier, P., and Marchand, E. (1996). Planning and expertise
in argumentative composition. In G. Rijlaarsdam, H. V. d. Bergh, and
M. Couzijn (eds.), Theories, models and methodology in writing
research (pp. 182–195). Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.

Demuth, K. and Smith, F. (1987). The foreign language requirement: An


alternative program. Foreign Language Annals, 20(1), 67-77.

Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (2014). Learning


difficulties. Retrieved from
https://www.imh.com.sg/uploadedFiles/Clinical_Services/Community-
based_Services/REACH/What-are-Learning-Difficulties.pdf.

Department of Education (2006). Guidance for schools: Recording children


with special educational needs. Retrieved from www.deni .gov.uk.

Department of Health (2001). Valuing people: A new strategy for learning


disability for the 21st century. London: Department of Health.

Department of Health (2009). Valuing people now: A new three year strategy
for people with learning disabilities – making it happen for everyone.
London: Department of Health.

Department of Health (2010). Raising our sights: Services for adults with
profound intellectual and multiple disabilities. London: Department of
Health.

Department of Special Education and Communication Disorders (2016).


Teaching strategies. College of Education and Human Sciences,
University of Nebraska–Lincoln. Retrieved from
http://cehs.unl.edu/secd/teaching-strategies/

de Quesada, S. (2009). Communicative tasks and interaction can contribute to


language acquisition. Retrieved from
http://pirnhua.udep.edu.pe/bitstream/handle/123456789/1413/MAE_E
DUC_074.pdf?sequence=1

Diehl, H. L. (2005). The effects of the reciprocal teaching framework on


strategy acquisition of fourth-grade struggling readers. Dissertation
Abstracts International, 66(04A), 1259.

187
Dieten, Anne-Mieke Janssen-van (1992). Self-Assessment in second language
learning. An empirical investigation into self-assessment by adult
learners of Dutch. Ph.D. dissertation, Katholieke Universiteit,
Nijmegen, The Netherlands.

DiFino, S. and Lombardino, L. (2004). Language learning disabilities: The


ultimate foreign language challenge. Foreign Language Annals, 37(3),
390-400.

Disability Discrimination Act (1992). Retrieved from


http://www.austlii.edu.au/au/legis/cth/consol_act/dda199226/

Dobao, A. and Martínez, I. (2007). Negotiating meaning in interaction


between English and Spanish speakers via communicative strategies.
Atlantis, 29(1), 87–105.

Dodd, A. (1995). Engaging students: What I learned along the way.


Educational Leadership, 53, 65-68.

Dole, J.A., Duffy, G.G., Roehler, L.R., and Pearson, P. D. (1991). Moving
from the old to the new: Research on reading comprehension
instruction. Review of Educational Research, 61, 239–264.

Dole, J. A., Nokes, J. D., and Drits, D. (2009). Cognitive strategy instruction.
In S. E. Israel and G. G. Duffy (eds.), Handbook of research on reading
comprehension (pp. 347-372). New York: Routledge.

Donnelly, M. (1987). At-risk students. Eric Digest, series number 21.

Doqaruni, V. and Yaqubi, B. (2011). A new approach to communication


strategies: The case of Iranian EFL teachers. Iranian Journal of
Applied Linguistics (IJAL), 14(1), 135-158.

Dornyei, Z. (1995). On the teachability of communication strategies. TESOL


Quarterly, 29(1), 55–85.

Dornyei , Z. (2008). New ways of motivating foreign language learners:


Generating vision. Links, 38, 3-4.

Dornyei, Z., and Scott, M. (1995). Communication strategies: What are they
and they not? Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the
American Association for Applied Linguistics, Long Beach, CA.

188
Dornyei, Z., and Scott, M. (1997). Communication strategies in a second
language: Definitions and taxonomies. Language Learning, 47(1), 173-
210.

Dornyei, Z., and Thurrell, S. (1991). Strategic competence and how to teach
it. ELT Journal, 45(1), 16-23.

Dornyei, Z., and Thurrell, S. (1992). Conversation and dialogues in action.


New York: Prentice Hall.

Dornyei, Z., and Thurrell, S. (1994). Teaching conversational skills


intensively: Course content and rationale. ELT Journal, 48(1), 40-49.

Douglas, H. (2007) Teaching by principles: An interactive approach to


language pedagogy. New York. Pearson Education.

Dowell, H. A., Storey, K., and Gleason, M. M. (1994). A comparison of


programs designed to improve the descriptive writing of students
labeled learning disabled. Developmental Disabilities Bulletin, 22(1),
73-91.

Downey, D., and Snyder, L. (2000). College students with LLD: The
phonological core as risk for failure in foreign language classes. Topics
in Language Disorders, 21(1), 82-92.

Downey, D., and Snyder, L. (2001). Curricular accommodations for college


students with language learning disabilities. Topics in Language
Disorders, 21(2), 55-67.

Dreyer, C. (1998). Improving students’ reading comprehension by means of


strategy instruction. Journal for Language Teaching, 31, 18-29.

DSF Literacy and Clinical Services (2014). Understanding learning


difficulties: A practical guide. DSF Literacy Services: AUSPELD.

Duke, N. K., and Pearson, P. D. (2002). Effective practices for developing


reading comprehension. In A. E. Farstrup and S. J. Samuels (eds.),
What research has to say about reading instruction (3rd ed., pp. 205–
242). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

Duke, N. K., Pearson, P. D., Strachan, S.L., and Billman, A. K.


(2011). Essential elements of fostering and teaching reading
comprehension. In S. J. Samuels and A. E. Farstrup (eds.). What

189
research has to say about reading instruction (4th ed., pp. 51–93).
Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

Durkin, D. (1993). Teaching them to read (6th ed.). Needham, MA: Allyn &
Bacon.

Ehrman, M. E., Leaver, B. L., and Oxford, R. L. (2003). A brief


overview of individual differences in second language
learning. System, 31, 313–330.

El-Koumy, Abdel Salam A. (2001). Effects of student self-assessment on


knowledge achievement and academic thinking. Washington DC: ERIC
Clearinghouse on Languages and Linguistics. [ERIC Document No.
ED 452731]. Available at: http://www.eric.ed.gov/

El-Koumy, Abdel Salam A. (2004a). Effect of self-assessment of writing


processes versus products on EFL students’ writing. Washington DC:
ERIC Clearinghouse on Languages and Linguistics. [ERIC Document
No. ED 490559]. Available at: http://www.eric.ed.gov/
El-Koumy, Abdel Salam A. (2004b). Language performance assessment:
Current trends in theory and research. Washington DC: ERIC
Clearinghouse on Languages and Linguistics. [ERIC Document No.
ED 490574]. Available at: http://www.eric.ed.gov/

El-Koumy, Abdel Salam A. (2004c). Metacognition and reading


comprehension: Current trends in theory and research. Washington
DC: ERIC Clearinghouse on Languages and Linguistics. [ERIC
Document No. ED 490569]. Available at: http://www.eric.ed.gov/

El-Koumy, Abdel Salam A. (2006). The effects of the directed reading-


thinking activity on EFL students' referential and inferential
comprehension. Washington DC: ERIC Clearinghouse on Languages
and Linguistics. [ERIC Document No. ED 502645]. Available at:
http://www.eric.ed.gov/
El-Koumy, Abdel Salam A. (2009). The effect of classroom performance
assessment on EFL students' basic and inferential reading skills.
Washington DC: ERIC Clearinghouse on Languages and Linguistics.
[ERIC Document No. ED 514530]. Available at:
http://www.eric.ed.gov/

190
El-Koumy, Abdel Salam A. (2010). Student self-assessment in higher
education: Alone or plus? Paper presented at the CPLA Conference,
October 29-30, 2010, Lebanese American University, Lebanon. [ERIC
Document No. ED 513289]. Available at: http://www.eric.ed.gov/

Ellis, M. (1999). Self-assessment: Discovering yourself and making the best


choice for you! Black Collegian, 30, 30-33.

Ellis, N. (1988). Reading, phonological skills, and short-term memory:


Interactive tributaries of development. Journal of Research in Reading,
13(2), 107-122.

Ellis, R. (1987). Interlanguage variability in narrative discourse: Style


shifting in the use of the past tense. Studies in Second Language
Acquisition, 9, 1-20.

Ellis, R. (1990). Instructed second language acquisition: Learning in the


classroom. Oxford: Basil Blackwell Ltd.

Ellis, R. (1994). The study of second language acquisition. Oxford: Oxford


University Press.

Ellis, R. (1999). Learning a second language through interaction.


Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Englert, C. S., and Mariage, T. V. (1991). Making students partners in the


comprehension process: Organizing the reading “POSSE.” Learning
Disability Quarterly, 14, 123–138.

Englert, C. S., Mariage, T. V., Okolo, C. M., Shankland, R. Moxley, K.,


Courtad, C. A., Meier, B. O'Brien, J. C., Martin, N., and Shin-Yuan,
C. (2009). The learning-to-learn strategies of adolescent students with
disabilities: Highlighting, notetaking, planning, and writing expository
texts. Assessment for Effective Intervention, 4(3), 147-161.

Englert, C. S., and Raphael, T. E. (1988). Constructing well-formed prose:


Process, structure, and metacognitive knowledge. Exceptional
Children, 54(6), 513–520.

Englert, C. S., Raphael, T. E., and Anderson, L. M. (1991). Making


strategies and self-talk visible: Writing instruction in regular and
special education classrooms. American Educational Research Journal,
28, 337-372.

191
Englert, C. S., Raphael, T. E., and Anderson, L. M. (1992). Socially
mediated instruction: Improving students' knowledge and talk about
writing. Elementary School Journal, 92, 411-449.

Englert, C. S., and Thomas, C. C. (1987). Sensitivity to text structure in


reading and writing: A comparison between learning disabled and
non-learning disabled students. Learning Disability Quarterly, 10, 93-
105.

ERIC Clearinghouse on Disabilities and Gifted Education (2002)


Strengthening the second 'R': Helping students with disabilities prepare
well-written compositions. Special Education Programs, Washington,
DC.: Council for Exceptional Children, Reston, VA.[ED 464 444]

Ericsson, K. A., and Simon, H. A. (1984). Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as


data. Cambridge, MA: MIT.

Ericsson, K. A., and Simon, H. A. (1993). Protocol analysis: Verbal reports


as data (rev. ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT.

Eskey, D. (2005). Reading in a second language. In E. Hinkel (ed.),


Handbook of research in second language teaching and learning (pp.
563-359). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Estrada, C. A., Isen, A. M., and Young, M. J. (1997). Positive affect


facilitates integration of information and decreases anchoring in
reasoning among physicians. Organizational Behavior and Human
Decision Processes, 72, 117–135.

European Parliament and Council of the European Union (2006).


Recommendation of the European Parliament and the Council of 18
December 2006 on key competences for lifelong learning. Document
(2006/962/EC), Brussels.

Fabrikant, W., Siekierski, N. and Williams, C. (1999). Improving students'


inferential and literal reading comprehension. ERIC Document
Number: ED 433497.

Faerch, C., and Kasper, G. (1983a). Plans and strategies in foreign language
learning and communication. London: Longman.

Faerch, C. and Kasper, G. (1983b). Strategies in interlanguage


communication. London: Longman.

192
Faerch, C. and Kasper, G. (1984). Two ways of defining communication
strategies. Language Learning, 34(1), 45-63.

Faerch, C. and Kasper, G. (1986). Strategic competence in foreign language


teaching. In G. Kasper. (ed.), Learning, teaching and communication in
the foreign language classroom (pp. 179-193). Aarhus: Aarhus
University Press.

Falchikov, N. (2005). Improving assessment through student involvement:


Practical solution for aiding learning in higher and further education.
New York: Routledge Falmer.

Falchikov, N. and Boud, D. (1989). Student self-assessment in higher


education: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 59(4),
395-430.

Farrell, T., and Marrard, C. (2006). The use of reception strategies by


learners of French as a foreign language. The Modern Language
Journal, 90(3), 338-352.

Faucette, P. (2001). A pedagogical perspective on communication strategies:


Benefits of training and an analysis of English language teaching
materials. Second Language Studies, 19(2), 1-40.

Fidalgo, R., Torrance, M., and García, J. N. (2008). The long term effects of
strategy-focussed writing instruction for grade six students.
Contemporary Educational Psychology. 33, 672-693.

Fiedorowicz, C., Benezra, E., MacDonald, W., McElgunn, B., Wilson, A.,
and Kaplan, B. (2001). Neurobiological basis of learning disabilities:
An update. Learning Disabilities: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 11(2),
61-74.

Finch, A. E. and Sampson, K. (2003). It’s up to you. Seoul: Chonghab


English Publishing.

Firkins, A., Forey, G., and Sengupta, S. (2007). Teaching writing to low
proficiency EFL students. ELT Journal, 61(4), 341-352.

Fisher, D. and Frey, N. (2014). Better learning through structured teaching


(2nd ed.). Alexandria, VA: ASCD.

193
Fitzpatrick, J. (2006). Self-assessment as a strategy to provoke integrative
learning within a professional degree programme. Learning in Health
and Social Care, 5(1), 23-34.

Flavell, J. H. (1976). Metacognitive aspects of problem solving. The nature of


intelligence, 12, 231-235.

Florida Department of Education (1995). Collaborative language and speech


services: Alternative classroom treatment. Tallahassee, FL: Author.

Fotovatian, S. and Shokrpour, N. (2007). Comparison of the efficiency of


reading comprehension strategies on Iranian university students’
comprehension. Journal of College Reading and Learning, 37(2), 47-63.

Fowler, J. (2003). Learning disabilities training: A new approach. London,


ON: Literacy Link South Central. Retrieved from
www.nald.ca/library/research/ldtrain/cover.htm.

Fowler, J., and Hunt, J. (2004). Learning disabilities training, phase II:
screening tools, strategies, and employment. Retrieved from
www.nald.ca/library/research/ldtrain/tools/cover.htm.

Freire, P. (2000). Pedagogy of the oppressed: 30th anniversary edition. New


York: Continuum.

Fuchs, D., Fuchs, L. S., Mathes, P. G., and Simmons, D. C. (1997). Peer-
Assisted Learning Strategies: Making classrooms more responsive to
diversity. American Educational Research Journal, 34(1), 174–206.

Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., and Kazdan, S. (1999). Effects of peer-assisted


learning strategies on high school students with serious reading
problems. Remedial and Special Education, 20(5), 309–318.

Fung, I., Wilkinson, I. and Moore, D. (2003). L1-assisted reciprocal


teaching to improve ESL students' comprehension of English
expository text. Learning and Instruction, 13(1), 1-31.

Gajria, M., Jitendra, A., Sood, S., and Sacks, G. (2007). Improving
comprehension of expository text in students with LD: A research
synthesis. Learning Disabilities, 40(3), 210-225.

Gajria, M. and Salvia, J. 1992. The effects of summarization instruction on


text comprehension of students with learning disabilities. Exceptional
Children, 58, 508-516.

194
Gambrell, L. B., and Chasen, S. P. (1991). Explicit story structure
instruction and the narrative writing of fourth- and fifth-grade below-
average readers. Reading Research & Instruction, 31, 54-62.

Ganshow, L. and Schneider, E. (2005). At-risk students and the study of a


foreign language in school. Fact Sheet, 25, 2005.

Ganschow, L. and Schneider, E. (2006). Assisting students with foreign


language learning difficulties in School. Baltimore, MD:International
Dyslexia Association.

Ganschow, L. and Sparks, R. (1995). Effects of direct instruction in Spanish


phonology on the native language skills and foreign language aptitude
of at-risk foreign language learners. Journal of Learning Disabilities,
28,107-120.

Ganschow, L., Sparks, R., and Javorsky, J. (1998). Foreign language


learning difficulties: A historical perspective. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 31(3), 1-18.

Ganschow, L., Sparks, R., Javorsky, J., Pohlman, J., and Bishop-Marbury,
A. (1991). Identifying native language difficulties among foreign
language learners in college: A foreign language learning disability?
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 24(9), 530–541.

Gardill. M. and Jitendra. A. (1999). Advanced story map instruction:


Effects on reading comprehension of students with learning
disabilities. The Journal of Special Education, 33, 2-17.

Gardner, R. C. (2001). Integrative motivation and second language


acquisition. In Z. Dornyei and R. W. Schmidt (eds.), Motivation and
second language acquisition (pp. 1-19). Honolulu: University of
Hawaii, Second Language Teaching and Curriculum Center.

Gardner, R. C. (2010). Motivation and second language acquisition: The


socioeducational model. New York: Peter Lang.

Garner, R. (1987). Metacognition and reading comprehension. Norwood, NJ:


Ablex.

Geladari A. and Mastrothanasis, K. (2011). The employment of cognitive


and metacognitive strategies in bilingual pupils’ creative writing.
Major Trends in Theoretical and Applied Linguistics, 3, 97- 113.

195
Gerber, P. (1998). Characteristics of adults with specific learning
disabilities. In Lenz, B.K., Sturomski, N.A., and Corley, M.A. (eds.),
Serving adults with learning disabilities: Implications for effective
practice (pp. 8-19). U. S. Department of Education, Office of
Vocational and Adult Education. National Adult Literacy and
Learning Disabilities Center, Academy for Educational Development.

Gerber, P. and Reiff, H. (1994). Learning disabilities in adulthood: Persisting


problems and evolving issues. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.

Gerber, P., Schnieders, C., Paradise, L., Reiff, H., Ginsberg, R., and Popp,
P. (1990). Persisting problems of adults with learning disabilities: Self-
reported comparisons from their school-age and adult years. Journal
of Learning Disabilities, 23(9), 570-573.

Gersten, R., and Baker, S. (1999a). Reading comprehension instruction for


students with learning disabilities: A research synthesis. Retrieved from
http://www.ldonline.org/ld_indepth/teaching_techniques/ncld_summit
99_twode cades.html.

Gersten, R. and Baker, S. (1999b). Teaching expressive writing to students


with learning disabilities: A meta-analysis. Eugene, OR: University of
Oregon.

Gersten, R. and Baker, S. (2000). The professional knowledge base on


instructional practices that support cognitive growth for English-
language learners. In R. Gersten, E. Schiller, and S. Vaughn
(eds.), Contemporary special education research syntheses of the
knowledge base on critical instructional issues. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum.

Gersten, R. and Baker, S. (2001). Teaching expressive writing to students


with learning disabilities: A meta-analysis. The Elementary School
Journal, 101(3), 251-272.

Gersten, R., Baker, S., and Edwards, L. (1999). Teaching expressive


writing to students with learning disabilities. The ERIC Clearinghouse
on Disabilities and Gifted Education, The Council for Exceptional
Children.

Gersten, R., Fuchs, L., Williams, J., and Baker, S. (2001). Teaching reading
comprehension strategies to students with learning disabilities: A
review of research. Review of Educational Research, 71(2), 279–320.

196
Gillberg, C. and Soderstrom, H. (2003). Learning disability.
The Lancet, 362, 811-821.

Gipps, C. V. (1994). Beyond testing: Towards a theory of educational


assessment. Washington D.C.: The Falmer Press.

Gleason, M. M. (1999). The role of evidence in argumentative writing.


Reading & Writing Quarterly, 15, 81-106.

Gleason, M. M., and Isaacson, S. (2001). Using the new basals to teach the
writing process: Modifications for students with learning problems.
Reading & Writing Quarterly, 17, 75-92.

Goddard, Y. L., and Sendi, C. (2008). Effects of self-monitoring on the


narrative and expository writing of four fourth-grade students with
learning disabilities. Reading and Writing Quarterly: Overcoming
Learning Difficulties, 24(4), 408-433.

Golley, Ashley (2015). Teaching writing for students with learning disabilities
in an inclusive classroom setting: A curriculum development project.
Retrieved from
http://digitalcommons.brockport.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1282
&context=surc.

Gooden, S. H. (2012). Comprehension strategies teachers use when they


read. Journal of Reading Education, 37(2), 16-20.

Gorham, J. (1988). The relationship between verbal teaching immediacy


behaviors and student learning. Communication Education, 17(1), 40-
53.

Gottlieb, Margo (2000). Portfolio practices in elementary and secondary


schools: Towards learner-directed assessment. In G. Ekbatani and H.
Pierson (eds.), Learner-directed assessment in ESL (pp. 89-126).
London: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Grabe, W. (2009). Teaching and testing reading. In M. H. Long and C. J.


Doughty (eds.), The handbook of language teaching (pp. 441-462). U.S.:
Wiley Blackwell.

Grabe, W. and Kaplan, R. B. (1996) Theory and practice of writing. London,


New York: Longman.

197
Graham, L. and Bailey, J. (2007). Learning disabilities and difficulties: An
Australian conspectus. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40(5), 386-391.

Graham, S. (1990). The role of production factors in learning disabled


students’ compositions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 781-
791.

Graham, S., Bollinger, A., Booth Olson, C., D’Aoust, C., MacArthur, C.,
McCutchen, D., and Olinghouse, N. (2012). Teaching elementary school
students to be effective writers: A practice guide. Washington, DC:
National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance,
Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.

Graham, S., and Harris, K. (1989a). A components analysis of cognitive


strategy instruction: Effects on learning disabled students'
compositions and self-efficacy. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81,
353-361.

Graham, S., and Harris, K. (1989b). Improving learning disabled students’


skills at composing essays: Self-instructional strategy training.
Exceptional Children, 56(3), 201-214.

Graham, S., and Harris, K. R. (1991). Self-instructional strategy


development: Programmatic research in writing. In B. Y. L. Wong
(ed.), Contemporary intervention research in learning disabilities: An
international perspective (pp. 47-64). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Graham, S., and Harris, K. (1993). Self-regulated strategy development:


Helping students with learning problems develop as writers.
Elementary School Journal, 94, 169-181.

Graham, S., and Harris, K. (1996). Self-regulation and strategy instruction


for students who find writing and learning challenging: In M. Levy
and S. Ransdell (eds.), The science of writing: Theories, methods,
individual differences, and applications (pp. 347-360). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.

Graham, S., and Harris, K. (1997). Self-regulation and writing: Where do


we go from here? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 22, 102-114.

Graham, S., and Harris, K. (2000). The role of self-regulation and


transcription skills in writing and writing development. Educational
Psychologists, 35, 3-12.

198
Graham, S. and Harris, K. (2005). Writing better: Effective strategies for
teaching students with learning difficulties. Baltimore: Brookes
Publishing Company.

Graham, S., and Harris, K. (2006). Students with learning disabilities and
the process of writing: A meta-analysis of SRSD Studies. In H. L.
Swanson, K. R. Harris and S. Graham, Handbook of learning
disabilities. New York: Guilford.

Graham, S., and Harris, K. (2009). Almost 30 years of writing research:


Making sense of it all with the wrath of khan. Learning Disabilities
Research and Practice, 24(2), 58-68.

Graham, S. and Harris, K. (2011). Writing and students with disabilities. In


J. M. Kauffman and D. P. Hallahan (eds.), Handbook of special
education (pp. 422-433). New York, NY: Routledge.

Graham, S., Harris, K., and Fink, B. (2000). Is handwriting causally related
to learning to write? Treatment of handwriting problems in beginning
writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 620-633.

Graham, S., Harris, K., and Fink, B. (2002). Contribution of spelling


instruction to the spelling, writing, and reading of poor spellers.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 669-686.

Graham, S. Harris, K., and Larsen, L. (2001). Prevention and intervention


of writing difficulties for students with learning disabilities. University
of Maryland Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 16(2), 74-84.

Graham, S., Harris, K., MacArthur, C., and Schwartz, S. (1991). Writing
and writing instruction with students with learning disabilities: A
review of a program of research. Learning Disabilities Quarterly, 14,
89-114.

Graham, S., Harris, K. R., and Mason, L. (2005). Improving the writing
performance, knowledge, and self-efficacy of struggling young writers:
The effects of self-regulated strategy development. Contemporary
Educational Psychology, 30(2), 207-241.

Graham, S., Harris, K., and Troia, G. A. (1998). Writing and self-
regulation: Cases from the self-regulated strategy development model.
In D. H. Schunk and B. J. Zimmerman (eds.), Developing self-regulated
learners: From teaching to self-reflective practice (pp. 20-41). New
York: Guilford Press.

199
Graham, S. and Hebert, M. (2010). Writing to read: Evidence for how writing
can improve reading. New York: Carnegie Corporation of New York.

Graham, S., MacArthur, C., Schwartz, S., and Page-Voth, V. (1992).


Improving the compositions of students with learning disabilities using
a strategy involving product and process goal setting. Exceptional
Children, 58, 322–334.

Graham, S., McKeown, D., Kiuhara, S., and Harris, K.S. (2012). A meta-
analysis of writing instruction for students in elementary grades.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(4), 879-896.

Graham, S., and Perin, D. (2007a). A meta-analysis of writing instruction


for adolescent students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(3), 445–
476.

Graham, S., and Perin, D. (2007b). Writing next: Effective strategies to


improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools. New York,
NY: Carnegie Corporation of New York.

Graham, S., Schwartz, S., and MacArthur, C. (1993). Knowledge of writing


and the composing process, attitude toward writing, and self-efficacy
for students with and without learning disabilities. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 26, 237-249.

Graves, M., and Graves, B. (1994). Scaffolding reading experiences: Designs


for student success. Norwood, MA: Christopher-Gordon.

Gray, J. R., Braver, T. S., and Raichle, M. E. (2002). Integration of emotion


and cognition in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99, 4115
- 4120.

Greene, B. A., and Land, S. M. (2000). A qualitative analysis of scaffolding


use in a resource-based learning environment involving world wide
web. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 23(2), 151-179.

Grenfell, M. and Harris, V. (1999). Modern languages and learning


strategies. London: Routledge.

Gresham, F. M., MacMillan, D. L., and Bocian, K. M. (1996). Learning


disabilities, low achievement, and mild mental retardation: More alike
than different? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29(6), 570-581.

200
Gumpel, T., and Shlomit, D. (2000). Exploring the efficacy of self-regulatory
training as a possible alternative to social skills training. Behavioral
Disorders, 25, 131-141.

Hacker, D. J. (1997). Comprehension monitoring of written discourse across


early-to-middle adolescence. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary
Journal, 9, 207-240.

Hacker, D. J., Plumb, C., Butterfield, E. C., Quathamer, D., and Heineken,
E. (1994). Text revision: Detection and correction of errors. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 86, 65-78.

Hall, J. and Verplaetse, L. (2000). Second and foreign language learning


through classroom interaction. Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Hallenbeck, Mark J. (1995). The Cognitive strategy in writing: Welcome relief


for adolescents with learning disabilities. ERIC Document Number:
ED 381981.

Hamre, B. and Pianta, R. (2005). Can instructional and emotional support


in the first grade classroom make a difference for children at risk of
school failure? Child Development, 76, 949-967.

Harmer, J. (2001). The practice of English language teaching. Cambridge:


Pearson Education Ltd.

Harris, K. (1986). Self-monitoring of attentional behavior versus self-


monitoring of production: Effects on on-task behavior and academic
response rate among learning disabled children. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 19, 417-42.

Harris, K. (1994). What is a speech-language pathologist? Spaniards Bay,


NF: Avalon North School District.

Harris, K., and Graham, S. (1996). Making the writing process work:
Strategies for composition and self-regulation. Cambridge, MA:
Brookline.

Harris, K., and Graham, S. (1999). Programmatic intervention research:


Illustrations from the evolution of self-regulated strategy development.
Learning Disability Quarterly, 22, 251-262.

Harris, M. (1997). Self-assessment of language learning in formal settings.


English Language Teaching Journal, 51(1), 12-20.

201
Harris, T. L., and Hodges, R. E. (1995). The literacy dictionary. Newark, DE:
International Reading Association.

Harris, K. R., Graham, S., and Mason, L. (2006). Improving the writing,
knowledge, and motivation of struggling young writers: Effects of self-
regulated strategy development with and without peer support.
American Educational Research Journal, 43, 295–340.

Harris, K. R., and Pressley, M. (1991). The nature of cognitive strategy


instruction: Interactive strategy construction. Exceptional Children,
57, 392-405.

Harris, K., Graham, S., Reid, R., McElroy, K., and Hamby, R. (1994). Self-
monitoring of attention versus self-monitoring of performance:
Replication and cross-task comparison studies. Learning Disability
Quarterly, 17, 121-139.

Harris, M. (1997). Self-assessment of language learning in formal settings.


English Language Teaching Journal, 51(1), 12-20.

Harris, M. and McCann, P. (1994). Assessment. Oxford, U.K.: Heinemann


Publishers.

Hart, E. and Speece, D. (1998). Reciprocal teaching goes to college: Effects


for postsecondary students at risk for academic failure. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 90(4), 670-681.

Hashey, J. and Connors, D. (2003). Learn from our journey: Reciprocal


teaching action research. The Reading Teacher, 57(3), 224-235.

Hatch, E. (1978). Discourse and second language acquisition. In E. Hatch


(ed.), Second language acquisition: A book of readings (pp. 401-435).
Rowley, MA: Newburry House.

Hedge, T. (2000). Teaching and learning in the classroom. Oxford, UK:


Oxford University Press.

Herczog, M. and Porter, P. (2010). Strategies for struggling readers: A


teacher resource guide. California: Center for Civic Education.

Higgins, E. L., and Raskind, M. H. (1997). The compensatory


effectiveness of optical character recognition/speech synthesis on
reading comprehension of postsecondary students with learning
disabilities. Learning Disabilities: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 8, 75-87.

202
Hillocks, G. (1984). What works in teaching composition: A meta-analysis of
experimental treatment studies. American Journal of Education, 93,
133-170.

Holmes, B. C. (1985). The effects of a strategy and sequenced materials on


the inferential comprehension of disabled readers. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 18(9), 542-546.

Hollas, B. (2002). Teaching your below-grade level students how to become


strategic readers. University of Tennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee:
Professional Development Inservice.

Horwitz, E. K. (2001) Language anxiety and achievement. Annual Review of


Applied Linguistics 21, 112-126.

Horwitz, E. K., Horwitz, B. and Cope, J. (1986). Foreign language classroom


anxiety. The Modern Language Journal 70(2), 125-132.

Hosenfeld, C. (1977). A preliminary investigation of the reading strategies of


successful and nonsuccessful second language learners. System, 5(2),
110-23.

Howe, M. (2003). Importance of communication skills. Retrieved from


wysiwyg://9/http://www.howewriteyourare.com/articles/communicatio
nskils0103.html.

Hu, G. (2002). Psychological constraints on the utility of metalinguistic


knowledge in second language production. Studies in Second Language
Acquisition, 24, 347–386.

Hughes, C. (1998). Effective instruction for adults with learning disabilities.


In B. Keith Lenz, Neil A. Sturomski, and Mary Ann Corley (eds.),
Serving adults with learning disabilities: Implications for effective
practice (pp. 34-49). National Adult Literacy and Learning Disabilities
Center, Washington, DC.

Hughes, C., and Boyle, J. R. (1991). Effects of self-monitoring for on-task


behavior and task productivity on elementary students with moderate
mental retardation. Education and Treatment of Children, 14(2), 96-
111.

203
Hughes, C., Copeland, S., Agran, M., Wehmeyer, M., Rodi, M. S., and
Presley, J. A. (2002). Using self-monitoring to improve performance in
general education high school classes. Education and Training in
Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities, 37, 262-272.

Humes, A. (1983). Research on the composing process. Review


of Educational Research, 53(2), 201-216.

Hunt, J., Gow, L. and Barnes, P. (1989). Learner self-evaluation and


assessment: A tool for autonomy in the language learning classroom.
In V. Bickley (ed.), Language teaching and learning styles within and
across cultures (pp. 207-17). Hong Kong: Institute of Language in
Education, Education Department.

Hurd, S. (2006). Towards a better understanding of the dynamic role of the


distance language learner: learner perceptions of personality,
motivation, roles and approaches. Distance Education, 27(3), 299-325.

Hurd, S. (2007) Anxiety and non-anxiety in a distance learning


environment: the distance factor as a modifying influence. System,
35(4), 487-508.

Isen, A. M. (1984). The influence of positive affect on decision making and


cognitive organization. In T. C. Kinnear (ed.), Advances in consumer
research, XI (pp. 534-537). Utah: Association for Consumer Research.

Isen A. M. (1999). On the relationship between affect and creative problem


solving. In Russ S.W. (ed.), Affect, creative experience and
psychological adjustment (pp. 3-17). Phila: Brunner/Mazel.

Isen, A. M. (2000). Positive affect and decision making. In M. Lewis and J.


Haviland-Jone s (eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 417–435).
New York: Guilford.

Isen, A. M. (2001). An influence of positive affect on decision making in


complex situations: Theoretical issues with practical implications.
Journal of Consumer Psychology, 11(2), 75–85.

Isen, A., Daubman, K. and Nowicki, G. (1987). Positive affect facilitates


creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
32, 112-1131.

204
Isen, A. and Patrick, R. (1983). The effect of positive feelings on risk taking:
When the chips are down. Organizational Behavior and Human
Performance, 31, 194–202.

Isen, A. M., and Reeve, J. (2005).The influence of positive affect on intrinsic


and extrinsic motivation: Facilitating enjoyment of play, responsible
work behavior, and self-control. Motivation and Emotion, 29(4), 29-
323.

Isen, A. M., and Shalker, T. E. (1982). The effect of feeling state on the
evaluation of positive, neutral, and negative stimuli: When you
accentuate the positive, do you eliminate the negative? Social
Psychology Quarterly, 45, 58–63.

Jacobs, J. E., and Paris, S. G. (1987). Children’s metacognition about


reading: Issues in definition, measurement, and instruction.
Educational Psychology, 22, 255–278.

Javorsky, J., Sparks, R., and Ganschow, L. (1992). Perceptions of college


students with and without specific learning disabilities about foreign
language courses. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 7, 31-44.

Jennings, C. and Shepherd, J. (1998). Literacy and the key learning areas:
Successful classroom strategies. Australia: Eleanor Curtain Publishing.

Jianing, Xu. 2007. Storytelling in the EFL speaking classroom. The Internet
TESL Journal, 13(11). Available at:
http://iteslj.org/Techniques/Jianing-Storytelling.html.

Jitendra, A. K., Hoppes, M. K., and Xin, Y. P. (2000). Enhancing main idea
comprehension for students with learning problems: The role of
summarization strategy and self-monitoring instruction. Journal of
Special Education, 34(3), 127-139.

Johnson-Glenberg, M. C. (2000). Training reading comprehension in


adequate decoders/poor comprehenders: Verbal versus visual
strategies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(4), 772-782.

Johnson, L., Graham, S., and Harris, K. R. (1997). The effects of goal setting
and self-instruction on learning a reading comprehension strategy: A
study of students with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 30(1), 80–91.

205
Johnston, P. and Rogers, R. (2001). Early literacy assessment. In S. B.
Neuman and D. K. Dickenson (eds.), Handbook for early literacy
research (pp. 377-389). New York, NY: Guilford.

Kahn, B. E., and Isen, A. M. (1993). The influence of positive affect on


variety seeking among safe, enjoyable products. Journal of Consumer
Research, 20, 257–270.

Kamil, M., Borman, G., Dole, J., Kral, C., Salinger, T., and Torgesen, J.
(2008). Improving adolescent literacy: Effective classroom and
intervention practices. Washington, DC: National Center for Education
Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences,
U.S. Department of Education.

Karsenty, L. (2001). Adapting verbal protocol methods to investigate speech


systems use. Applied Ergonomics, 32(1), 15-22.

Kasper, G. and Kellerman, E. (1997). Communication strategies:


Psycholinguistic and sociolinguistic perspectives. London: Longman.

Kauffman, D. F. (2004). Self-regulated learning in web-based environments:


Instructional tools designed to facilitate cognitive strategy use,
metacognitive processing, and motivational beliefs. Journal of
Educational Computing Research, 30(1), 139-161.

Kaufman, N. J., Randlett, A. L., and Price, J. (1985). Awareness of the use
of comprehension strategies in good and poor college readers. Reading
Psychology, 6, 1-11.

Kavale, K., Fuchs, D. and Scruggs, T. (1994). Setting the record straight on
learning disability and low achievement: Implications for policy
making. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 60(10), 717-718.

Kehe, D., and Kehe, P. (2004). Conversation strategies: Pair and group
activities for developing communicative competence. Brattleboro, VT:
Pro Lingua Associates.

Kellerman, E. (1991). Compensatory strategies in second language research:


A critique, a revision, and some (non-) implications for the classroom.
In R. Phillipson, E. Kellerman, L. Selinker, M. Sharwood Smith, and
M. Swain (eds.), Foreign/second language pedagogy research: A
commemorative volume for Claus Færch (pp. 142-161). Clevedon,
England: Multilingual Matters.

206
Kelley, D., and Gorham, J. (1988). Effects of immediacy on recall of
information. Communication Education, 37, 198–207.

Kelly, M., Moore, D., and Tuck, B. (1994). Reciprocal teaching in a regular
primary school classroom. Journal of Educational Research, 88(1), 53 –
62.

Khamwan, T. (2007). The Effects of interactional strategy training on


teacher-student interaction in an EFL classroom. Nakhon Ratchasima:
Suranaree University of Technology. Retrieved from
www.sutir.sut.ac.th:8080/sutir/bitstream/123456789/.../tanaporn_fullte
xt.pdf.

Khoshsima, H. and Tiyar, F. (2014). The Effect of summarizing and


presentation strategies on reading comprehension of Iranian
intermediate EFL learners. International Journal of Applied Linguistics
& English Literature, 3(4), 89-96.

Kiewra, K. A. (2002). How classroom teachers can help students learn and
teach them how to learn. Theory into Practice, 41(2), 71-80.

Kitsantas, A., Reiser, B. and Doster, J. (2004). Goal setting, cues, and
evaluation during acquisition of procedural skills: Empowering
students’ learning during independent practice. Journal of
Experimental Education, 72(4), 269–287.

Klingner, J., and Vaughn, S. (1996). Reciprocal teaching of reading


comprehension strategies for students with learning disabilities.
Elementary School Journal, 96, 275–293.

Klingner, J., Vaughn, S., and Boardman, A. (2007). Teaching reading


comprehension to students with learning difficulties. New York:
Guilford Press.

Kong, J. (2004). A study of communication strategy training. Foreign


Language World, 5, 33-39.

Kong, J. (2006). An investigation of communication strategies used by


Chinese non-English major postgraduates. Teaching English in China,
29(2), 3-9.

Kongsom, T. (2009). The Effects of teaching communication strategies to Thai


learners. Unpublished Ph.D Thesis. University of Southampton.

207
Kozminsky, E. and Kozminsky, L. (2001). How do general knowledge and
reading strategies ability relate to reading comprehension of high
school students at different educational levels? Journal of Research in
Reading, 2, 187-204.

Kraat, Arlene W. (1985). Communication interaction between aided and


natural speakers: A state of the art report. Eric Document Number:
287275.

Kramsch, C. (1987). Interactive discourse in small and large groups. In


Wilga M. Rivers (ed.), Interactive language teaching (pp. 17-30).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Krashen, Stephen D. (1988). Second language acquisition and second


language learning. New York: Prentice-Hall International.

Lahey, Margaret (1988). Language disorders and language development.


New York: Macmillan Publishing Company.

Lam, W. K. (2010). Implementing communication strategy instruction in the


ESL oral classroom: What do low-proficiency learners tell us? TESL
Canada Journal, 27(2), 11-30.

Langtree, I. (2010). Definitions of the models of disability. Retrieved from


http://www.disabled-world.com/definitions/disability-models.php.

Lantoff, J. (2000). Second language learning as a mediated process.


Language Teaching, 33, 79-96.

Lau, K. L. (2006). Reading strategy use between Chinese good and poor
readers: a think aloud study. Journal of Research in Reading, 29(4),
383-399.

Learning Disabilities Association of Alberta (2010). Learning disabilities


reference manual. Retrieved from
http://www.ldalberta.ca/wpcontent/uploads/2010/10/Resources_LDRef
erenceManual.pdf.

Learning Disabilities Association of Canada (1999). Destination literacy:


Identifying and teaching adults with learning disabilities. Ottawa, ON:
The Association.

208
Learning Disabilities Association of Canada (2005). A literature framework
to guide the research study: Putting a Canadian face on learning
disabilities (PACFOLD).
http://pacfold.ca/download/Supplementary/Framework.pdf.

Learning Disabilities Association of Ontario (200l). Learning disabilities: A


new definition. Retrieved from
http://www.ldao.ca/documents/Definition_and_Suporting%20Docume
nt_2001.pdf.

Lederer, J. (2000). Reciprocal teaching of social studies in inclusive


elementary classrooms. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(1), 91 –
107.

Lee-Thompson, L. C. (2008). An investigation of reading strategies applied


by American learners of Chinese as a foreign language. Foreign
Language Annals, 41(4), 702-720.

LeFevre, D., Moore, D., and Wilkinson, I. (2003). Tape–assisted reciprocal


teaching: Cognitive bootstrapping for poor decoders. British Journal
of Educational Psychology, 73(1), 37– 58.

Lenz, B. K., Ellis, E.S., and Scanlon, D. (1996). Teaching learning strategies
to adolescents and adults with learning disabilities. Austin, TX: PRO-
ED.

Leong, D., Bodrova, E., Hensen, R., and Henninger, M. (1999). Scaffolding
early literacy through play. New Orleans, NAEYC 1999 Annual
Conference.

Lerner, J., and Kline, F. (2006). Learning disabilities and related disorders:
Characteristics and teaching strategies (10 th ed.). Boston, MA:
Houghton Mifflin Company.

Levin, J. R., and Pressley, M. (1981). Improving children's prose


comprehension: Selected strategies that seem to succeed. In C. M.
Santa and B. L. Hayes (eds.). Children's prose comprehension:
Research and practice (pp. 44-71). Newark, DE: International Reading
Association.

Lewis, M. (2002). Implementing the lexical approach: Putting theory into


practice. Boston, MA: Heinle.

209
Lewis, S. (2011). Are communication strategies teachable? Encuentro, 20,
46-54.

Lightbown, P., and Spada, N. (1993). Instruction and the development of


question in L2 classroom. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 15,
205-224.

Li-xia, J. (2016). The establishment of strategy-training-based writing


teaching mode via language experiment platform (LEP). US-China
Foreign Language, 14(2), 115-120.

Logan, R. K. (2007). The extended mind: The emergence of language, the


human mind and culture. Canada: University of Toronto Press.

Long, M. (1985). Input and second language acquisition theory. In S. M.


Gass, and C. G. Madden (eds.), Input in second language acquisition
(pp. 377-393). Rowley, MA: Newburry House.

Long, M. (1996). The role of the linguistic environment in second language


acquisition. In W. Ritchie, and T. Bhatia (eds.), Handbook of second
language acquisition (pp. 413-468). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Lysynchuck, L., Pressley, M., and Vye, N. (1990). Reciprocal teaching


improves standardized reading comprehension performance in poor
comprehenders. Elementary School Journal, 90(5), 469 – 484.

Macaro, E. (2003). Teaching and learning a second language. London:


Continuum.

MacArthur, C. (1996). Using technology to enhance the writing


performance of students with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 29, 344-354.

MacArthur, C. (1999). Overcoming barriers to writing: Computer support


for basic writing skills. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 15, 169-192.

MacArthur, C., Ferretti, R. P., Okolo, C. M. and Cavalier, A. R. (2001).


Technology applications for students with literacy problems: A critical
review. Elementary School Journal, 101(3), 273–301.

MacArthur, C., and Graham, S. (1987). Learning disabled students’


composing under three methods of text production: Handwriting,
word processing, and dictation. Journal of Special Education, 21, 22–
42.

210
MacArthur, C., Graham, S., Haynes, J., and De La Paz, S. (1996). Spelling
checkers and students with learning disabilities: Performance
comparisons and impact on spelling. Journal of Special Education,
30, 35-57.

MacArthur, C., Graham, S., and Schwartz, S. (1991). Knowledge of revision


and revising behavior among students with learning disabilities.
Learning Disability Quarterly, 14(1), 61–74.

MacArthur, C. A., Schwartz, S., and Graham, S. (1991). Effects of a


reciprocal peer-revision strategy in special education classrooms.
Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 6, 201-210.

MacDonald, B. and Boud, D. (2003). The Impact of self-assessment on


achievement: The effects of self-assessment training on performance in
external examinations. Assessment in Education, 10(2), 209-220.

MacIntyre, P.D. (1995). How does anxiety affect second language learning?
A reply to Sparks and Ganschow. The Modern Language Journal,
79(1), 90-99.

MacIntyre, P.D. (1999). Language anxiety: A review of the research for


language teachers. In D. Young (ed.) Affect in foreign language and
second language learning: A practical guide to creating a low-anxiety
classroom atmosphere. Boston: McGraw-Hill College.

Mackey, A. (1999). Input, interaction, and second language development:


An empirical study of question formation in ESL. Studies in Second
Language Acquisition, 21, 557-587.

MacMillan, D. and Siperstein, G. (2002). Learning disabilities as


operationally defined by schools. In R. Bradley, L. Danielson and D. P.
Hallahan (eds.). Identification of learning disabilities: Research to
practice (pp. 287-333). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Malasit, Y., and Sarobol, N. (2013). Communication strategies used by Thai


EFL learners. Retrieved from http://www.fllt2013. org/
private_folder/Proceeding/802.pdf.

Malone, L. D., and Mastropieri, M. A. 1992. Reading comprehension


instruction: Summarization and self-monitoring training for students
with learning disabilities. Exceptional Children, 58(3), 270-279.

211
Manchón, R. (2000). Fostering the autonomous use of communication
strategies in the foreign language classroom. Links & Letters 7, 13-27.

Marchand-Martella, N. and Martella, R. (2012). Important features of


effective adolescent literacy instruction. Desoto, TX: SRA McGraw-Hill.

Mariani, L. (1994). Developing strategic competence towards autonomy in


oral interaction. Perspectives, a Journal of TESOL-Italy, 20 (1).
Retrieved from
http://www.learningpaths.org/papers/papercommunication.htm.

Mastropieri, M. A., and Scruggs, T. E. (1997). Best practices in promoting


reading comprehension in students with learning disabilities. Remedial
& Special Education, 18(4), 197-216.

Mastropieri, M. A., Scruggs, T. E., Bakken, J. P., and Whedon, C. (1996).


Reading comprehension: A synthesis of research in learning
disabilities. In T.E. Scruggs and M.A. Mastropieri (eds.) Advances in
learning and behavioral disabilities: Intervention research (vol. 10, Part
B, pp. 201-227. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Mather, N., Wendling, B. J., and Roberts, R. (2009). Writing assessment and
instruction for students with learning disabilities (2nd ed.). San
Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Matsuda, S. and Gobel, P. (2004) Anxiety and predictors of performance in


the foreign language classroom. System 32, 21-36.

Mayberg, H. S., Liotti, M., Brannan, S. K., McGinnis, S., Mahurin, R. K.,
Jerabek, P.A., J. Silva, J. A., Janet L. Tekell, J. L., Charles C., Martin,
C. C., Jack L., Lancaster, J. L. and Fox, P. T. (1999). Reciprocal
limbic-cortical function and negative mood: Converging PET findings
in depression and normal sadness. American Journal of Psychiatry,
156, 675-682.

McDevitt, T. and Ormrod, J. (2004). Child development: Educating and


working with children and adolescent (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River,
NJ: Pearson.

McMahon, T. (1999). Using negotiation in summative assessment to


encourage critical thinking. Teaching in Higher Education, 4(4), 549-
554.

212
McNamara, M. J., and D. Deane (1995). Self-assessment activities: Towards
autonomy in language learning. TESOL Journal, 5(1), 17–21.

McNamara, D. S., and Magliano, J. T. (2009). Toward a comprehensive


model of comprehension. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 51,
297–384.

McNeil, J. D. (1992). Reading comprehension new directions for classroom


practice (3rd ed.) Los Angles: University of California.

Medical Research Council (2001) Review of autism research: Epidemiology


and causes. London: Medical Research Council.

Meltzer, L. and Krishnan, K. (2007). Executive function difficulties and


learning disabilities: Understandings and misunderstandings. In L.
Meltzer (Ed.), Executive function in education: From theory to practice
(pp. 77–105). New York: The Guilford Press.

Meltzer, L., Katzir-Cohen, T., Miller, L., and Roditi, B. (2001). The impact
of effort and strategy use on academic performance: Student and
teacher perceptions. Learning Disability Quarterly, 24(2), 85-98.

Meyer, K. (2010). “Diving into reading”: Revisiting reciprocal teaching in


the middle years. Literacy Learning: The Middle Years, 18(1), 41-52.

Mishna, F. (1998). Tips for parents – expectations: A delicate balance.


Retrieved from http://www.integra.on.ca/tipseries_12.htm.

Moats, L. (1998). Reading, spelling, and writing disabilities in the middle


grades. In B. Wong (ed.), Learning about learning disabilities (2nd ed.,
pp. 367-389). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Mortimer, E., and Scott, P. (2003). Meaning making in secondary science


classrooms. Maidenhead, UK: Open University Press.

Mothus, T. and Lapadat, J. (2006). A strategy intervention to increase the


reading comprehension of junior high school students with reading
disabilities. ERIC Document Number: 490965.

Mu, C. (2005). A taxonomy of ESL writing strategies. Retrieved from


http://eprints.qut.edu.au/64/1/64.pdf.

Nakatani, Y. (2005). The effects of awareness-raising training on oral


communication strategy use. Modern Language Journal, 89(1), 76-91.

213
Nakatani, Y. (2006). Developing an oral communication strategy inventory.
The Modern Language Journal, 90(2), 151-168.

Nakatani, Y. (2010). Identifying strategies that facilitate EFL learners’ oral


communication: A classroom study using multiple data collection
procedures. The Modern Language Journal, 94(1), 116-136.

Naparstek, N. (2002). Successful educators: A practical guide for


understanding children’s learning problems and mental health issues.
Westpoint, CT: Bergin & Garvey.

National Dissemination Center for Children with Disabilities (2004).


Learning disabilities. Disability Fact Sheet, No. 7. Retrieved from
http://www.spannj.org/transition/TransitionCD/Champion20or%20Pr
ogress/how%20do%20I%20stay%20healthy/ld%20fact%20sheet.pdf.

National Health Service in Scotland (2004). People with learning disabilities


in Scotland. Retrieved from
http://www.gla.ac.uk/media/media_63872_en.pdf.

National Information Center for Children and Youth with Disabilities


(1997). Interventions for students with learning disabilities. ERIC
Document Number: ED 415 607.

National Joint Committee on Learning Disabilities (1991). Learning


disabilities: Issues on definition. Asha, 33, (Suppl.5), 18-20.

National Joint Committee on Learning Disabilities (1994). Learning


disabilities: Issues on definition. In Collective perspectives on issues
affecting learning disabilities: Position papers and statements (pp. 61-
66). Austin, TX: PRO-ED.

National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence-


based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its
implications for reading instruction. Washington, DC: National
Institute of Child Health and Human Development. Retrieved from
www.nationalreadingpanel.org/

Nelson, J., Smith, D., and Dodd. J. (1992). The effects of teaching a
summary skills strategy to students identified as learning disabled on
their comprehension of science text. Education and Treatment of
Children, 15, 228-243.

214
Newhall, P. (2012). Language-based learning disabilities. In P. W. Newhall
(ed.), Language-based teaching series. Prides Crossing, MA:
Landmark School Outreach Program.

Newhall, P. (2013). Language-based learning disability: What to know.


http://www.ldonline.org/article/56113/

Nguyet, N. and Mai, L. (2012). Teaching conversational strategies through


video clips. Language Education in Asia, 3(1), 32-49.

Nichols, E. (2002). On the legislative front: Secondary school reform and


students with learning disabilities. Communique, 31(2), 4-5.

Nielsen, J., Clemmensen, T., and Yssing, C. (2002). Getting access to what
goes on in people's heads? Reflections on the think-aloud technique.
Proceedings of the second Nordic conference on human computer
interaction (pp. 101-110). New York: Association for Computing
Machinery.

Nik, Y., Hamzah, A. and Rafidee, H. (2010). A comparative study on


the factors affecting the writing performance among bachelor
students. International Journal of Educational Research and
Technology, 1(1), 57-63.

Nunan, D. and Bailey, K. M. (2009). Exploring second language classroom


research. Boston: Heinle, Cengage Learning.

Nyikos, M. (1996). The conceptual shift to learner-centered classrooms:


Increasing teacher and student strategic awareness. In R.L. Oxford
(ed.), Language learning strategies around the world: Cross-cultural
perspectives (pp. 109-117). Honolulu: Second Language Teaching and
Curriculum Center.

O'Connor, E., and McCartney, K. (2007). Examining teacher-child


relationships and achievement as part of an ecological model of
development. American Educational Research Journal, 44, 340-369.

Oczkus, L. D. (2003). The four reciprocal teaching strategies. In reciprocal


teaching at work: Strategies for improving reading comprehension (pp.
13-28). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

Ogane, M. (1998). Teaching communication strategies. ERIC Document


Number: ED 419384.

215
O’Malley, J. M. (1987). The effects of training in the use of learning
strategies on learning English as a second language. In Wenden, A.
and J. Rubin (eds.). Learner strategies in language learning. London:
Prentice-Hall.

O’Malley, J. M., and Chamot, A. U. (1990). Language strategies in second


language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

O'Malley, J. M., and Chamot, A. U. (1994). Learning strategies in second


language learning. In A. Lewy (ed.), International encyclopedia of
education. Oxford, England: Pergamon Press.

O'Malley, J. M., Chamot, A. U., and Küpper, L. (1989). Addison-Wesley


ESL: Placement test package. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

O'Malley, J. M., Chamot, A. U., Stewner-Manzanares, G., Küpper, L., and


Russo, R. P. (1985). Learning strategy applications with students of
English as a second language. TESOL Quarterly, 19(3), 285-296.

Ontario Ministry of Education (2003). Early reading strategy. Available on


the Ministry of Education's website at http://www.edu.gov.on.ca.

Orsmond, P., Merry, S., and Reiling, K. (2000). The use of student derived
marking criteria in peer and self-assessment. Assessment and
Evaluation in Higher Education, 25, 23–38.

Oscarson, M. (1989). Self-assessment of language proficiency: Rationale and


applications. Language Testing, 6, 1-13.

Oscarson, M. (1997). Self-assessment of foreign and second language


proficiency. In C. Clapham, and D. Corson (eds.), Encyclopedia of
language and education: Vol. 7. Language testing and assessment (pp.
175-187). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.

Oxford, R. (1990). Language learning strategies: What every teacher should


know. New York: Newbury House Publishers.

Oxford, R. L. (1990). Language learning strategies and beyond: A look at


strategies in the context of styles. In S.S. Magnan (Ed.), Shifting the
instructional focus to the learner (pp. 35-55). Middlebury, VT:
Northeast Conference on the Teaching of Foreign Languages.

216
Oxford, R. L. (1993). Research on second language learning strategies. In
W. Grabe (ed.), Annual review of applied linguistics (pp. 175-187).
Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.

Oxford, R. (1994). Language learning strategies: An update. Retrieved from


http://www.cal.org/resources/digest/oxford/html.

Oxford, R. (1999). Learning strategies. In B. Spolsky (ed.), Concise


Encyclopedia of Educational Linguistics (pp. 518–522). Oxford:
Elsevier.

Oxford, R. L. (2000). Communicative strategies. In M. Byram (ed.),


Routledge encyclopedia of language teaching and learning (pp. 130-
132). London: Routledge.

Oxford, R. L. (2003). Language learning styles and strategies: Concepts and


relationships. International Review of Applied Linguistics and Language
Teaching, 41(4), 271–278.

Oxford, R. (2013). Teaching and researching language learning strategies.


Retrieved from https://books.google.com.eg/books?isbn=131787993.

Oxford, R., Cho, Y., Leung, S., and Kim, H-J. (2004). Effect of the presence
and difficulty of task on strategy use: An exploratory study.
International Review of Applied Linguistics and Language Teaching,
42(1), 1-47.

Oxford, R. and Crookall, D. (1989). Research on language learning


strategies: methods, findings, and instructional issues. Modern
Language Journal, 73, 404-419.

Oxford, R., Crookall, D., Cohen, A., Lavine, R., Nyikos, M., and Sutter, W.
(1990). Strategy training for language learners: Six situational case
studies and a training model. Foreign Language Annals, 22, 197-216.

Pajares, F., Johnson, M. J., and Usher, E. L. (2007). Sources of writing self-
efficacy beliefs of elementary, middle, and high school students.
Research in the Teaching of English, 42, 104–120.

Palincsar, A.S. and Brown, A.L. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of


comprehension fostering and comprehension-monitoring activities.
Cognition & Instruction, 1(2), 117-175.

217
Palinscar, A. and Brown, A. (1985). Reciprocal teaching: A means to a
meaningful end. In J. Osborn, P. Wilson and R. C. Anderson (eds.),
Reading education: Foundations for a literate America (pp. 299-310).
MA: Lexington.

Palincsar, A., Brown, A., and Martin, S. (1987). Peer interaction in reading
comprehension instruction. Educational Psychologist, 22, 231-253.

Palincsar, A., David, Y., Winn, J., and Stevens, D. (1991). Examining the
context of strategy instruction. Remedial and Special Education, 12(3),
41-53.

Palincsar, A., Brown, A., and Martin, S. (1987). Peer interaction in reading
comprehension instruction. Educational Psychologist, 22, 231-253.

Palincsar, A., David, Y., Winn, J., and Stevens, D. (1991). Examining the
context of strategy instruction. Remedial and Special Education, 12(3),
41-53.

Paris, S., and Ayres, L. (1994). Becoming reflective students and teachers with
portfolios and authentic assessment. Hyattsville, MD: American
Psychological Association.

Paris, S., Lipson, M., and Wixson, K. (1983). Becoming a strategic reader.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 8, 293-316.

Paris, S., Wasik, B., and Turner, J. (1991). The development of strategic
readers. In R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, and P.D. Pearson
(eds.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 609-640). White Plains, NY:
Longman.

Peñuelas, A. (2012). The writing strategies of American university


students: Focusing on memory, compensation, social and affective
strategies. ELIA, 12, 77-113.

Peterson, R., and Eeds, M. (1990). Grand conversations: Literature groups in


action. New York: Scholastic.

Peterson-Karlan, G., and Parette, H. P. (2007). Supporting struggling writers


using technology: Evidence-based instruction and decision-making.
Retrieved from
http://www.techmatrix.org/resources/tech_support_writing.pdf.

218
Pica, T., and Doughty, E. (1985). Input and interaction in the
communicative language classroom: A comparison of teacher-fronted
and group activities. In S. M. Gass and E. G. Madden (eds.), Input in
second language acquisition (pp. 115-32). Rowley, MA: Newbury
House.

Pierangelo, R. and Giuliani, G. (2008). Teaching students with learning


disabilities: A step-by-step guide for educators. USA: Corwin Press.

Pilonieta, P. (2010). Instruction of research-based comprehension strategies


in basal reading programs. Reading Psychology, 31, 150-175.

Pintrich, P. R. (1994). Continuities and discontinuities: Future directions for


research in educational psychology. Educational Psychologist, 29, 37-
148.

Pintrich, P. R. (1999). The role of motivation in promoting and sustaining


self-regulated learning. International Journal of Educational Research,
31, 459-470.

Pintrich, P (2002). The role of metacognitive knowledge in learning,


teaching, and assessing. Theory Into Practice, 41(4), 219-225.

Pintrich, P. and De Groot, E. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated


learning components of classroom academic performance. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 82(1), 33-50.

Pintrich, P. R., and Schunk, D. H. (1996). Motivation in education: Theory,


research, and applications. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall Merrill.

Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D., Garcia, T., and McKeachie, W. J. (1991). A


manual for the use of the motivated strategies for learning questionnaire
(MSLQ). Ann Arbor, MI: NCRIPTAL, School of Education, The
University of Michigan.

Poulisse, N. (1987). Problems and solutions in the classification of


compensatory strategies. Second Language Research, 3, 141–153.

Poway Unified School District (PUSD) (n.d.). The Writing process.


http://bowenpeters.weebly.com/uploads/8/1/1/9/8119969/writing_proce
ss_detailed.pdf.

219
Pressley, M. (1995). More about the development of self-regulation:
Complex, long-term, and thoroughly social. Educational Psychologist,
30, 207–212.

Pressley, M. (2000). What should comprehension instruction be the


instruction of? In M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, and R.
Barr (eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 3, pp. 545-461).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Pressley, M., and Afflerbach, P. (1995). Verbal protocols of reading: The


nature of constructively responsive reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Pressley. M., El-Dinary, P.B., Gaskins. I., Schuder, T., Berman, J.L.,
Almasi, J. and Brown. R. (1992). Beyond direct explanation:
Transactional instruction of reading comprehension strategies. The
Elementary School Journal, 92(5), 51 3-555.

Pressley, M., and Ghatala, E. S. (1990). Self-regulated learning: monitoring


learning from text. Educational Psychologist, 25, 19–33.

Pressley, M., Johnson, C. J., Symons, S., McGoldrick, J. A., and Kurita, J.
A. (1989). Strategies that improve children’s memory and
comprehension of text. Elementary School Journal, 90, 3-32.

Pressley, M., Levin, J. R., and Ghatala, E. S. (1984). Memory strategy


monitoring in adults and children. Journal of Verbal Learning and
Verbal Behavior, 23, 270–288.

Pressley, M., Levin, J. R., Ghatala, E. S., and Ahmad, M. (1987). Test
monitoring in young grade school children. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology, 43, 96–111.

Price, A., and Cole, M. (2009). Best practices in teaching students with
learning disabilities. Calgary, AB: Calgary Learning Centre.

Protheroe, N. and Clarke, S. (2008). Learning strategies as a key to student


success. Retrieved from
https://www.naesp.org/resources/2/Principal/2008/NDp33.pdf.

Proctor, C. P., August, D., Carlo, M. S., and Snow, C. E. (2006). The
intriguing role of Spanish vocabulary knowledge in predicting English
reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 159–
169.

220
Proctor, C. P., Dalton, B., and Grisham, D. L. (2007). Scaffolding English
language learners and struggling readers in a universal literacy
environment with embedded strategy instruction and vocabulary
support. Journal of Literacy Research, 39(1), 71-93.

Queensland Studies Authority (2007) Learning difficulties. Brisbane,


Queensland. Retrieved from
http://www.qsa.qld.edu.au/downloads/p9/kla_special_needs_info_learn
ing.pdf.

Rababah, G. (2002). Strategic competence and language teaching. ERIC


Document Number: ED 472 697.

Rababah, G. (2003). Communication problems facing Arab learners of


English: A personal perspective. TEFL Web Journal, 2(1), 16-29.

Rabab'ah, G. (2015). The Effect of communication strategy training on the


development of EFL learners' strategic competence and oral
communicative ability. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, pp. 1-27.

Raimes, A. (1987). Language proficiency, writing ability, and composing


strategies: A study of ESL college student writers. Language Learning,
37, 439–468.

Rao, Z. (2003). Effect of using a "top-down" strategy on Chinese university


students comprehension of English readings. Asian Journal of English
Language Teaching, 13, 29-44.

Raphael, T. and Pearson, P. D. (1985) Increasing students’ awareness of


sources of information for answering questions. American Educational
Research Journal, 22, 217-236.

Reid, R. (1996). Research in self-monitoring with students with learning


disabilities: The present, the prospects, the pitfalls. Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 29(3), 317-331.

Reid, R., Lienemann, T., and Hagaman, J. (2013). Strategy instruction for
students with learning disabilities (2nd ed.). New York, NY: The
Guilford Press.

Reutzel, D. R., Smith, J. A., and Fawson, P. C. (2005). An evaluation of two


approaches for teaching reading comprehension strategies in the
primary years using science information texts. Early Childhood
Research Quarterly, 20(3), 276–305.

221
Riazi, A. (1997). Acquiring disciplinary literacy: A social-cognitive analysis
of text production and learning among Iranian graduate students of
education. Journal of Second Language Writing, 6(2), 105-137.

Rich, R. Z., and Blake, S. (1994). Using pictures to assist in comprehension


and recall. Intervention in School and Clinic, 29, 271–275.

Richards, J. and Lockhart, C. (1996). Reflective teaching in second language


teaching classrooms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Richardson, J. and Morgan, R. (1997). Reading to learn in the content areas.


Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Publishing Company.

Rivers, W. M. (2000). Interaction as the key to teaching language for


communication. In Wilga M. Rivers. (ed.) Interactive language
teaching (pp. 3-16). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Rivers, W. M. (2000). Preface. In Wilga M. Rivers. (ed.) Interactive language


teaching (pp. xi-xvi). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Rivers, William, P. (2001). Autonomy at all costs: An ethnography of


metacognitive self-assessment and self-management among
experienced language learners. The Modern Language Journal, 85(2),
279-290.

Roberts, G., Torgesen, J., Boardman, A. and Scammacca, N. (2008).


Evidence-based strategies for reading instruction of older students
with learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice,
23(2), 63–69.

Rodriguez, J., Plax, T. and Kearney, P. (1996). Clarifying the relationship


between teacher nonverbal immediacy and student cognitive learning:
Affective learning as the central causal mediator, Communication
Education, 45, 293–305.

Rogers, L., and Graham, S. (2008). A meta-analysis of single subject design


writing intervention research. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100,
879–906.

Rosenshine, B., and Meister, C. (1992). The use of scaffolds for teaching
higher-level cognitive strategies. Educational Leadership, 49(7), 26–33.

Rosenshine, B. and Meister, C. (1994). Reciprocal teaching: A review of the


research. Review of Educational Research, 64, 479-530.

222
Rosenshine, B., Meister, C., and Chapman, S. (1996). Teaching students to
generate questions: A review of the intervention studies. Review of
Educational Research, 66, 181-221.

Ross, J., Rolheiser, C., and Hogaboam-Gray, A. (1999). Effects of self-


evaluation training on narrative writing. Assessing Writing, 6(1), 107-
132.

Ross, S. (1999). Self-assessment in second language testing: A meta-analysis


and analysis of experiential factors. Language Testing, 15(1), 1-20.

Rost, M. (1996). Helping learners develop communication strategies. The


Language Teacher, 20 (12), 41-43.

Rost, M., and Ross, S. (1991). Learner strategies in interaction: Typology


and teachability. Language Learning, 41(2), 235-273.

Rowe, G., Hirsch, J. B., and Anderson, A. (2007). Positive affect increases
the breadth of attentional selection. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101, 1, 383-388.

Rubin, J. (1981). Study of cognitive processes in second language


learning. Applied Linguistics, 11, 117-131.

Ruffin, T. (2009). Reading strategies: Adaptations to meet the needs of


secondary English language learners with learning disabilities. The
Reading Matrix, 9(1), 22-30.

SABES (2008). Examples of “authentic” tasks and assessments. Retrieved


from www.quia.com/files/quia/.../sabesse/Authentic_Assessme.

Santangelo, T., Harris, K., and Graham, S. (2008). Using self-regulated


strategy development to support students who have “Trubol Giting
Thangs Into Werds” Remedial and Special Education, 29(2), 78-89.

Santos, S. (2010). EFL writing in Mexican universities: Research and


experience. Tepic, Nayarit, México: Universidad Autónoma de
Nayarit.

Saricoban, A. (2002). Reading strategies of successful readers through the


three phase approach. The Reading Matrix, 2, 1-13.

Sasaki, M. (2000). Toward an empirical model of EFL writing processes: An


exploratory study. Journal of Second Language Writing, 9(3), 259-291.

223
Savage, L. B. (1998). Eliciting critical thinking skills through questioning.
The Clearing House, 71(5), 291-293.

Savignon, S. J. (1972). Communicative competence: An experiment in foreign


language teaching. Philadelphia: Center for Curriculum Development.

Savignon, S. (1983). Communicative competence: Theory and classroom


practice. MA: Addison Wesley.

Savignon, S. (1991). Communicative language teaching: State of the art.


TESOL Quarterly, 25(2), 261-277.

Savignon, S. J. (2001). Communicative language teaching for the twenty-


first century. In M. Celce-Murcia (ed.), Teaching English as a second
or foreign language (3rd ed., pp. 13-28). Boston: Heinle & Heinle.

Sawyer, R. J., Graham, S., and Harris, K. R. (1992). Direct teaching,


strategy instruction, and strategy instruction with explicit self-
regulation: Effects on the composition skills and self-efficacy of
students with learning disabilities. Journal of Educational Psychology,
84(3), 340-352.

Schmeck, R. R. (1988). Learning strategies and learning styles. Plenum Press.

Schmidt, J. L., Deshler, D. D., Schumaker, J. B., and Alley, G. R.


(1988/1989). Effects of generalization instruction on the written
language performance of adolescents with learning disabilities in the
mainstream classroom. Journal of Reading, Writing, and Learning
Disabilities International, 4, 291–309.

Schmidt, R., and Frota, S. (1986). Developing basic conversational ability in


a second language: A case study of an adult learner of Portuguese. In
R. Day (Ed.), Talking to learn: Conversation in second language
acquisition (pp. 237–326). Rowley, MA: Newbury.

Schneider, E. and Crombie. M. (2003). Dyslexia and foreign language


learning. London: David Fulton.

Schoenbach, R., C. Greenleaf, C. Cziko, and Hurwitz, L. (1999). Reading


for understanding: A guide to improving reading in middle and high
school classrooms. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

224
Schoonen, R., and De Glopper, K. (1996). Writing performance and
knowledge about writing. In G. Rijlaarsdam, H. Bergh, and M.
Couzijn (eds.), Theories, models and methodology in writing research
(pp. 87–107). Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.

Schraw, G. (1998). Promoting general metacognitive awareness.


Instructional Science, 26, 113–125.

Schraw, G., and Nietfeld, J. (1998). A further test of the general monitoring
skill hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 236–248.

Schunk, D. (1989). Self-Efficacy and cognitive achievement: Implications for


students with learning problems. Journal of Learning Disabilities 22,
14–22.

Schunk, D. (1994). Self-regulation of self-efficacy and attributions in


academic settings. In D. H. Schunk and B. J. Zimmerman (eds.), Self-
regulation of learning and performance: Issues and educational
applications (chapter 4). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Schunk, D. H. and Rice, J. M. (1992). Influence of reading-comprehension


strategy instruction on children’s achievement outcomes. Learning
Disability Quarterly, 15, 51–64.

Schwarz, R. and Terrill, L. (2000). ESL instruction and adults with learning
disabilities. Retrieved from
www.cal.org/caela/esl_resources/digests/LD2.html.

Scott, J. (2012). Reading comprehension interventions for students


with learning disabilities or reading difficulties in Grades 3-12: A
literature review, 2006-2011. Retrieved from
http://www.lynchburg.edu/wp-content/uploads/volume-6-
2012/ScottJR-Reading Comprehension-Lit-Review.pdf.

Scott, S., McGuire, J., and Foley, T. (2003). Universal design for instruction:
A framework for anticipating and responding to disability and other
diverse learning needs in the college classroom. Equity and Excellence
in Education, 36(1), 40-49.

Sears, S., Carpenter, C. and Burstein, N. (1994). Meaningful reading


instruction for learners with special needs. The Reading Teacher, 47(8),
632-646.

225
Seijts, G. H. and Latham, G. P. (2006). Learning goals or performance
goals: Is it the journey or the destination? Ivey Business Journal, 70(5),
1-6.

Selinker, L. (1984). The current state of IL studies: An attempted critical


summary. In A. Davies, C. Criper and A. P. R. Howatt (eds.),
Interlanguage (pp. 332-343). Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

Sencibaugh, Joseph M. (2005). Meta-Analysis of reading comprehension


interventions for students with learning disabilities: strategies and
implications. Available at: http://www.eric.ed.gov/ [ED 493483]

Shale, D., and D. R. Garrison (1990). Education and communication. In D.


R. Garrison and D. Shale (eds.), Education at a distance. Malabar:
Robert E. Krieger Publishing Company.

Singhal, M. (2001). Reading proficiency, reading strategies, metacognitive


awareness and L2 readers. The Reading Matrix, 1, 1-9.

Skinner, M. E., and Smith, A. T. (2011). Creating success for students with
learning disabilities in postsecondary foreign language courses.
International Journal of Special Education, 26, 42–57.

Smart, J. (2009). Teacher-student interactions and domain-specific


motivation: The relationship between students' perceptions of teacher
interpersonal behavior and motivation in middle school science. A Ph.D.
Dissertation, Graduate School of Clemson University. Retrieved from
http://tigerprints.clemson.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=2271&conte
xt=all_dissertations.

Solomon, A. (2011). The new wave of autism rights activists. Retrieved from
http://nymag.com/news/features/47225/index2.html.

Sousa, D. (2001). How the special needs brain learns. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Corwin Press.

Sparks, R., and Ganschow, L. (1993). Searching for the cognitive locus of
foreign language learning difficulties: Linking first and second
language learning. Modern Language Journal, 77(2), 289-302.

Sparks, R. L., and Ganschow, L. (1993). The impact of native language


learning problems on foreign language learning: Case study
illustrations of the linguistic coding deficit hypothesis. Modern
Language Journal, 77(1), 58-74.

226
Sparks, R. L., Philips, L., Ganschow, L., and Javorsky, J. (1999a).
Comparison of students classified as LD who petitioned for or fulfilled
the college foreign language requirement. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 32, 553-565.

Sparks, R.L., Phillips, L., Ganschow, L., and Javorsky, J. (1999b). Students
classified as LD and the college foreign language requirement: A
quantitative analysis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 566-580.

Sparks, R., Philips, L., and Javorsky, J. (2003). Students classified as LD


who petitioned for or fulfilled the foreign language requirement – Are
they different?: A replication study. Journal of Learning Disabilities,
36, 348-362.

Sporer, N., Brunstein, J. and Kieschke, U. (2009). Improving students'


reading comprehension skills: Effects of strategy instruction and
reciprocal teaching. Learning and Instruction, 19(3), 272-286.

Stern, Hans Heinrich (1983). Fundamental concepts of language teaching.


Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Stoddard, B., and MacArthur, C.A. (1993). A peer editor strategy: Guiding
learning disabled students in response and revision. Research in the
Teaching of English, 27(1), 76-103.

Strickland, D. and Shanahan, T. (2004). Laying the groundwork for


literacy. Educational Leadership, 61(6), 74–77.

Sturomski, Neil (1997). Interventions for students with learning disabilities.


National Information Center for Children and Youth with Disabilities,
Washington [ED 415 607]

Swanson, H. L. (1999). Instructional components that predict treatment


outcomes for students with learning disabilities: Support for a
combined strategy and direct instruction model. Learning Disabilities
Research, 14, 129-140.

Swanson, P. N. and De La Paz, S. (1998). Teaching effective comprehension


strategies to students with learning and reading disabilities.
Intervention in School and Clinic, 33(4), 209–218.

Tancock, S. (1994). A literacy lesson framework for children with reading


problems. The Reading Teacher, 48(2), 130-140.

227
Tankersley, K. (2005). Literacy strategies for grades 4-12: Reinforcing the
threads of reading. Association for Supervision and Curriculum
Development: Alexandria, VA.

Taras, M. (2003). To feedback or not to feedback in student self-assessment.


Assessment and Evaluation in Higher Education, 28 (5), 549–565.

Tarone, E. (1980). Communication strategies, foreigner talk and repair in


interlanguage. Language Learning, 30, 417-431.

Tarone, E. (1983) On the variability of interlanguage systems. Applied


Linguistics, 4, 142-163.

Tarone, E. (1984). Teaching strategic competence in the foreign-language


classroom. In Savignon, Sandra J. and Margie S. Berns. (eds.).
Initiatives in communicative language teaching (pp. 127-136). Reading,
MA: Addison-Wesley.

Tarone, E. (2005). Speaking in a second language. In E. Hinkel (ed.),


Handbook of research in second language teaching and learning (pp.
485-502). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

TATN (Texas Assistive Technology Network) (2012). Technology supports


for struggling writers. Texas Assistive Technology Network. West
Tidwell Houston, Region 4 Education Service Center, Texas. Retrieved
from http://www.texasat.net.

Teng, H. (2012). A study on the teachability of EFL communication


strategies. Social and Behavioral Sciences 46, 3566 – 3570.

Thamraksa (2005) Metacognition: A key to success for EFL learners.


Retrieved from
http://www.bu.ac.th/knowledgecenter/epaper/jan_june2005/chutima.p
df.

Thomas, C., Englert, C., and Gregg, S. (1987). An analysis of errors and
strategies in the expository writing of learning disabled
students. Remedial and Special Education, 8, 21-30.

Thompson, E. (1993). It’s not so hard: Preparing students for reading


comprehension. Schools in the Middle, 2(3), 33-36.

Thompson, S. (2000). Effective content reading comprehension and retention


strategies. ERIC Document Number: ED 440 372.

228
Tian, J., and Zhang, X. (2005). The study of communication strategies used
by non-English majors in group discussion. Foreign Language World,
3, 61-67.

Tierney, R., Readence, J., and Dishner, E. (1995). Reading strategies and
practices: A compendium (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Torgesen, J. and Kail, R. (1980). Memory processes in exceptional children.


In B. K. Keogh (ed.). Advances in special
education, Vol. 1. Greenwich, CT: JAI.

Torrance, M., G., Thomas, V., and Robinson, E. J. (2000). Individual diffe-
rences in undergraduate essay-writing strategies: A longitudinal study.
Higher Education, 39, 181-200.

Tovani, C. (2004). Do I really have to teach reading? Portland, Maine:


Stenhouse Publishers.

Trehearne, Miriam P. (2015). Comprehensive literacy resource for grades 1–


2 teachers. New York: ETA/Cuisenaire.

Troia, G. (2007). Research in writing instruction: What we know and what


we need to know. In M. Pressley, A. K. Billman, K. H. Perry, K. E.
Refitt, and J. M. Reynolds (Eds.), Shaping literacy achievement:
Research we have, research we need (pp. 129-156). New York: Guilford
Press.

Troia, G., and Graham, S. (2002). The effectiveness of a highly explicit,


teacher-directed strategy instruction routine: Changing the writing
performance of students with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 35(4), 290–305.

Troia, G., Graham, S., and Harris, K.R. (1999). Teaching students with
learning disabilities to mindfully plan when writing. Exceptional
Children, 65(2), 235-252.

Troia, G., Roth, F., and Graham, S. (1998). An educator's guide to


phonological awareness: Assessment measures and intervention
activities for children. Focus on Exceptional Children, 30, 1-12.

Vacca, L., Vacca, R. T., Gove, M. K., McKeon, C. A., Burkey, L. C. and
Lenhart, L. A. (2006). Reading and learning to read. Boston: Allyn &
Bacon.

229
Van den Branden, K. (2000). Does negotiation of meaning promote reading
comprehension? A study of multilingual primary school classes.
Reading Research Quarterly, 35(3), 426–443.

Vandergrift, L. (2003). From prediction through reflection: Guiding


students through the process of L2 listening. Canadian Modern
Language Review, 59(4), 425-440.

Vandergrift, L. (2003). Orchestrating strategy use: Toward a model of the


skilled second language listener. Language Learning, 53(3), 463-496.

Vann, R., and Abraham, R. (1990). Strategies of unsuccessful language


learners. TESOL Quarterly, 24(2), 223-234.

Van Riper, I. (2010). The effects of the directed reading-thinking activity on


reading comprehension skills of middle school students with autism.
Ph.D Dissertation, Widener University, PA. Retrieved from ProQuest
Dissertations and Theses database (3414553).

Van Schrojenstein Lantman-de Valk, H. and Walsh, P.N. (2008) Managing


health problems in persons with intellectual disabilities. British
Medical Journal, 337, 1408–1412.

Vaughn, S., and Linan-Thompson, S. (2004). Research-based methods of


reading instruction: Grades k-3. Alexandria, VA: ASCD.

Victori, M. (1995). EFL writing knowledge and strategies: An interactive


study. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Universitat Autonoma de
Barcelona (Spain), Barcelona.

Vogel, S. and Reder, S. (1998). Literacy proficiency among adults with self-
reported learning disabilities. In M. C. Smith (Ed.), Literacy for the
21st Century: Research, policy, practices, and the national adult literacy
survey. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.

von Elek, Tibor (1985). A test of Swedish as a second language. In Y. P. Lee


et al. (eds.) New Directions in Language Testing (pp. 47-57). Oxford:
Pergamon Press.

Wagner, E. D. (1994). In support of a functional definition of interaction.


American Journal of Distance Education, 8(2), 6–26.

230
Wagner, J., and Firth, A. (1997). Communication strategies at work. In G.
Kasper and E. Kellerman (eds.) Communication strategies:
Psycholinguistic and sociolinguistic perspectives (pp. 323-344). New
York: Longman.

Walqui, A. (2006). Scaffolding instruction for English language learners: A


conceptual framework . The International Journal of Bilingual
Education and Bilingualism, 9(2), 159-180.

Wayne, F. S. and Mitchell, R. B. (1992). Vital communication skills and


competencies in the workforce of the 1990s. Journal of Education for
Business, 67(3), 141-147.

Weinstein, C. E., Husman, J., and Dierking, D. R. (2000). Self-regulation


interventions with a focus on learning strategies. In M. Boekaerts, P.
R. Pintrich, and M. Zeidner (eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp.
727-747). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Wenden, A. (1985). Learner strategies. TESOL Newsletter, 19(5), 1-7.

Wenden, A. (1986). Incorporating learner training in the classroom. System,


14(3), 315–25.

Wenden, A. (1991a). Learner strategies for learner autonomy. London:


Prentice-Hall International.

Wenden, A. (1991b). Metacognitive strategies in L2 writing: A case for task


knowledge. In J. E. Alatis (ed.), Georgetown university round table on
languages and linguistics 1991 (pp. 302-322). Washington, D. C.:
Georgetown University Press.

Westera, J. and Moore, D. (1995). Reciprocal teaching of reading


comprehension in a New Zealand high school. Psychology in the
Schools, 32(3), 225 – 232.

Westwood, P. (2008). What teachers need to know about learning difficulties?


Australia: ACER Press.

Widodo, H.P. (2008). Process-based academic essay writing instruction in an


EFL context. BAHASA DAN SENI, 36(1), 101-111.

Williams, J. D. (2003). Preparing to teach writing: Research, theory, and


practice (3rd ed.). Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

231
Williams, M. and Burden, R. L. (1997). Psychology for language teachers: A
social constructivist approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.

Williams, C., and Roberts, D. (2011). Strategic oral language instruction in


ELD: Teaching oracy to develop literacy. California: Ballard & Tighe
Publishers.

Williamson, J., McMunn, N., and Reagan, H. (2004). Tools for providing
feedback in reading: A reading assessment handbook for all teachers in
grades 3–12. Institute of Education Sciences: U.S. Department of
Education.

Williamson , Y. (2015). At-risk students and the study of foreign language in


school. Retrieved from
https://sites.google.com/site/mrswilliamsonmandarinclass/blog/summa
riesofatriskstudentsandthestudyofforeignlanguageinschool.

Wilson, A. M., and Lesaux, N. K. (2001). Persistence of phonological


processing deficits in college students with dyslexia who have age-
appropriate reading skills. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34(5), 394-
400.

Winne, P. H. (1995). Inherent details in self-regulated learning. Educational


Psychologist, 30(4), 173- 187.

Wixson, K. K., and Peters, C. W. (1984). Reading redefined: A Michigan


Reading Association position paper. The Michigan Reading Journal,
17, 4-7.

Wolf, S. A., and Gearhart, M. (1994). Writing what you read: Assessment as
a learning event. Language Arts, 71, 425-444.

Wong, B. (1988). An instructional model for intervention research in


learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 4, 5-16.

Wong, B. (1994). Instructional parameters promoting transfer of learned


strategies in students with learning disabilities. Learning Disability
Quarterly, 17, 110-120.

Wong, B. (1997). Research on genre-specific strategies for enhancing


writing in adolescents with learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities
Quarterly, 20, 140-159.

232
Wong, B. (1998). Learning about learning disabilities (3rd ed.). San Diego:
Academic Press.

Wong, B. (2000). Writing strategies instruction for expository essays for


adolescents with and without learning disabilities. Topics in Language
Disorders, 20(4), 29-44.

Wong, B. (2001). Commentary: Pointers for literacy instruction from


educational technology and research on writing instruction.
Elementary School Journal, 101(3), 359-369.

Wong, B., Butler, D. L., Ficzere, S. A., and Kuperis, S. (1996). Teaching low
achieving students and students with learning disabilities to plan,
write, and revise opinion essays. Journal of Learning Disabilities,
29, 197-212.

Wong, B., Butler, D. L., Ficzere, S. A., and Kuperis, S. (1997). Teaching low
achieving students and students with learning disabilities to plan,
write, and revise compare-and-contrast essays. Learning Disabilities
Research & Practice, 20, 175-183.

Wong, B., and Jones, W. (1982). Increasing metacomprehension in learning-


disabled and normally achieving students through self-questioning
training. Learning Disability Quarterly, 5, 228-240.

Wong, B., Wong, R., and Blenkinsop, J. (1989). Cognitive and metacognitive
aspects of learning disabled adolescents' composing problems. Learning
Disability Quarterly, 12(4), 310-323.

Wood, D., Bruner, J. S., and Ross, G. (1976). Role of tutoring in problem-
solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied
Disciplines, 17(2), 89–100.

Wright, P. W. (2005). IDEA 2004: Proposed changes to the code of federal


regulations. U.S. Department of Education.

Yoshida-Morise, Y. (1998). The Use of communication strategies in


language proficiency interviews. In Richard Young and Agnes Weiyun
He (eds.), Talking and testing: Discourse approaches to the assessment
of oral proficiency (pp. 205–238). Amsterdam: John Benjamin.

233
Zeynali, S., Zeynali, S. and Motlagh, S. (2015). The Effects of socio-affective
strategy in enhancement of reading comprehension in Iranian EFL
Learners. International Journal of Language and Linguistics, 4(2-1), 9-
22.

Zheng, Y. (2008). Anxiety and second/foreign language learning revisited.


Canadian Journal for New Scholars in Education, 1(1), 1-12.

Zhang, L. J. (2008). Constructivist pedagogy in strategic reading


instruction: Exploring pathways to learner development in the English
as a second language (ESL) classroom. Instructional Science, 36(2), 89-
116.

Zimmerman, C. B. (1997). Historical trends in second language vocabulary


instruction. In James Coady and Thomas Huckin (eds.), Second
language vocabulary acquisition (pp. 5-19). Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.

Zimmerman, B. J. (1998). Developing self-fulfilling cycles of academic


regulation: An analysis of exemplary instructional models. In D. H.
Schunk, and B. J. Zimmerman (eds.), Self-regulated learning: From
teaching to self-reflective practice (pp. 1-19). New York: The Guiford
Press.

Zimmerman, B. J. (2000). Attaining self-regulation: A social cognitive


perspective. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, and M. Zeidner (eds.),
Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 13-39). San Diego: Academic Press.

Zimmerman, B. J. and Risemberg, R. (1997). Self-regulatory dimensions of


academic learning and motivation. In G. D. Phye (ed.), Handbook of
academic learning: Construction of knowledge (pp. 105-125). San
Diego, CA: Academic Press.

234

You might also like