PED 101 Lesson 6
PED 101 Lesson 6
PED 101 Lesson 6
PED 101- The Child and Adolescent Learner and the Learning Principle
The work of Lev Vygotsky (1934) has become the foundation of much research and theory in
cognitive development over the past several decades, particularly of what has become known as
sociocultural theory.
Sociocultural theory views human development as a socially mediated process in which
children acquire their cultural values, beliefs, and problem-solving strategies through
collaborative dialogues with more knowledgeable members of society.
Vygotsky's theories stress the fundamental role of social interaction in the development of
cognition (Vygotsky, 1978), as he believed strongly that community plays a central role in the
process of "making meaning."
Unlike Piaget's notion that childrens' development must necessarily precede their learning,
Vygotsky argued, "learning is a necessary and universal aspect of the process of developing
culturally organized, specifically human psychological function" (1978, p. 90). In other words,
social learning tends to precede (i.e., come before) development.
Vygotsky has developed a sociocultural approach to cognitive development. He developed his
theories at around the same time as Jean Piaget was starting to develop his ideas (1920's and
30's), but he died at the age of 38, and so his theories are incomplete - although some of his
writings are still being translated from Russian.
No single principle (such as Piaget's equilibration) can account for development. Individual
development cannot be understood without reference to the social and cultural context within
which it is embedded. Higher mental processes in the individual have their origin in social
processes.
This contradicts Piaget's view of universal stages and content of development (Vygotsky does
not refer to stages in the way that Piaget does).
Hence Vygotsky assumes cognitive development varies across cultures, whereas Piaget states
cognitive development is mostly universal across cultures.
According to Piaget, language depends on thought for its development (i.e., thought comes
before language). For Vygotsky, thought and language are initially separate systems from the
beginning of life, merging at around three years of age, producing verbal thought (inner speech).
For Vygotsky, cognitive development results from an internalization of language.
Adults transmit their culture's tools of intellectual adaptation that children internalize. In contrast,
Piaget emphasizes the importance of peers, as peer interaction promotes social perspective
taking.
Vygotsky, therefore, sees cognitive functions, even those carried out alone, as affected by the
beliefs, values, and tools of intellectual adaptation of the culture in which a person develops and
therefore socio-culturally determined. The tools of intellectual adaptation, therefore, vary from
culture to culture - as in the memory example.
Shaffer (1996) gives the example of a young girl who is given her first jigsaw. Alone, she
performs poorly in attempting to solve the puzzle. The father then sits with her and describes or
demonstrates some basic strategies, such as finding all the corner/edge pieces and provides a
couple of pieces for the child to put together herself and offers encouragement when she does so.
As the child becomes more competent, the father allows the child to work more independently.
According to Vygotsky, this type of social interaction involving cooperative or collaborative
dialogue promotes cognitive development.
In order to gain an understanding of Vygotsky's theories on cognitive development, one must
understand two of the main principles of Vygotsky's work: the More Knowledgeable Other
(MKO) and the Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD).
concept of the More Knowledgeable Other is integrally related to the second important principle
of Vygotsky's work, the Zone of Proximal Development.
This is an important concept that relates to the difference between what a child can achieve
independently and what a child can achieve with guidance and encouragement from a skilled
partner.
For example, the child could not solve the jigsaw puzzle (in the example above) by itself and
would have taken a long time to do so (if at all), but was able to solve it following interaction
with the father, and has developed competence at this skill that will be applied to future jigsaws.
Vygotsky (1978) sees the Zone of Proximal Development as the area where the most sensitive
instruction or guidance should be given - allowing the child to develop skills they will then use
on their own - developing higher mental functions.
Vygotsky also views interaction with peers as an effective way of developing skills and
strategies. He suggests that teachers use cooperative learning exercises where less competent
children develop with help from more skillful peers - within the zone of proximal development.
Vygotsky (1987) differentiates between three forms of language: social speech which is external
communication used to talk to others (typical from the age of two); private speech (typical from
the age of three) which is directed to the self and serves an intellectual function; and finally
private speech goes underground, diminishing in audibility as it takes on a self-regulating
function and is transformed into silent inner speech (typical from the age of seven).
For Vygotsky, thought and language are initially separate systems from the beginning of life,
merging at around three years of age. At this point speech and thought become interdependent:
thought becomes verbal, speech becomes representational. When this happens, children's
monologues internalized to become inner speech. The internalization of language is important as
it drives cognitive development.
'Inner speech is not the interiour aspect of external speech - it is a function in itself. It still
remains speech, i.e., thought connected with words. But while in external speech thought is
embodied in words, in inner speech words dies as they bring forth thought. Inner speech is to
a large extent thinking in pure meanings.'
(Vygotsky, 1962: p. 149)
Vygotsky (1987) was the first psychologist to document the importance of private speech. He
considered private speech as the transition point between social and inner speech, the moment in
development where language and thought unite to constitute verbal thinking.
Thus private speech, in Vygotsky's view, was the earliest manifestation of inner speech. Indeed,
private speech is more similar (in its form and function) to inner speech than social speech.
Private speech is 'typically defined, in contrast to social speech, as speech addressed to the
self (not to others) for the purpose of self-regulation (rather than communication).' (Diaz,
1992, p.62)
Unlike inner speech which is covert (i.e., hidden), private speech is overt. In contrast to Piaget’s
(1959) notion of private speech representing a developmental dead-end, Vygotsky (1934, 1987)
viewed private speech as:
'A revolution in development which is triggered when preverbal thought and preintellectual
language come together to create fundamentally new forms of mental functioning.'
(Fernyhough & Fradley, 2005: p. 1).
In addition to disagreeing on the functional significance of private speech, Vygotsky and Piaget
also offered opposing views on the developmental course of private speech and the
environmental circumstances in which it occurs most often (Berk & Garvin, 1984).
Through private speech, children begin to collaborate with themselves in the same way a more
knowledgeable other (e.g., adults) collaborate with them in the achievement of a given function.
Vygotsky sees "private speech" as a means for children to plan activities and strategies and
therefore aid their development. Private speech is the use of language for self-regulation of
behavior. Language is, therefore, an accelerator to thinking/understanding (Jerome Bruner also
views language in this way). Vygotsky believed that children who engaged in large amounts of
private speech are more socially competent than children who do not use it extensively.
Vygotsky (1987) notes that private speech does not merely accompany a child’s activity but acts
as a tool used by the developing child to facilitate cognitive processes, such as overcoming task
obstacles, enhancing imagination, thinking, and conscious awareness.
Children use private speech most often during intermediate difficulty tasks because they are
attempting to self-regulate by verbally planning and organizing their thoughts (Winsler et al.,
2007).
The frequency and content of private speech are then correlated with behavior or performance.
For example, private speech appears to be functionally related to cognitive performance: It
appears at times of difficulty with a task.
For example, tasks related to executive function (Fernyhough & Fradley, 2005), problem-solving
tasks (Behrend et al., 1992), schoolwork in both language (Berk & Landau, 1993), and
mathematics (Ostad & Sorensen, 2007).
Berk (1986) provided empirical support for the notion of private speech. She found that most
private speech exhibited by children serves to describe or guide the child's actions.
Berk also discovered than child engaged in private speech more often when working alone on
challenging tasks and also when their teacher was not immediately available to help them.
Furthermore, Berk also found that private speech develops similarly in all children regardless of
cultural background.
Vygotsky (1987) proposed that private speech is a product of an individual’s social environment.
This hypothesis is supported by the fact that there exist high positive correlations between rates
of social interaction and private speech in children.
Children raised in cognitively and linguistically stimulating environments (situations more
frequently observed in higher socioeconomic status families) start using and internalizing private
speech faster than children from less privileged backgrounds. Indeed, children raised in
environments characterized by low verbal and social exchanges exhibit delays in private speech
development.
Childrens’ use of private speech diminishes as they grow older and follows a curvilinear trend.
This is due to changes in ontogenetic development whereby children are able to internalize
language (through inner speech) in order to self-regulate their behavior (Vygotsky, 1987).
For example, research has shown that childrens’ private speech usually peaks at 3–4 years of
age, decreases at 6–7 years of age, and gradually fades out to be mostly internalized by age 10
(Diaz, 1992).
Vygotsky proposed that private speech diminishes and disappears with age not because it
becomes socialized, as Piaget suggested, but rather because it goes underground to constitute
inner speech or verbal thought” (Frauenglass & Diaz, 1985).
Classroom Applications
Vygotsky's approach to child development is a form of social constructivism, based on the idea
that cognitive functions are the products of social interactions.
Vygotsky emphasized the collaborative nature of learning by the construction of knowledge
through social negotiation.
He rejected the assumption made by Piaget that it was possible to separate learning from its
social context.
Vygotsky believed everything is learned on two levels. First, through interaction with others, and
then integrated into the individual’s mental structure.
Every function in the child’s cultural development appears twice: first, on the social level,
and later, on the individual level; first, between people (interpsychological) and then inside
the child (intrapsychological). This applies equally to voluntary attention, to logical memory,
and to the formation of concepts. All the higher functions originate as actual relationships
between individuals. (Vygotsky, 1978, p.57)
Teaching styles based on constructivism mark a conscious effort to move from ‘traditional,
objectivist models didactic, memory-oriented transmission models’ (Cannella & Reiff, 1994) to a
more student-centred approach.
A contemporary educational application of Vygotsky's theory is "reciprocal teaching," used to
improve students' ability to learn from text. In this method, teachers and students collaborate in
learning and practicing four key skills: summarizing, questioning, clarifying, and predicting. The
teacher's role in the process is reduced over time.
Also, Vygotsky theory of cognitive development on learners is relevant to instructional concepts
such as "scaffolding" and "apprenticeship," in which a teacher or more advanced peer helps to
structure or arrange a task so that a novice can work on it successfully.
Vygotsky's theories also feed into the current interest in collaborative learning, suggesting that
group members should have different levels of ability so more advanced peers can help less
advanced members operate within their ZPD.
Critical Evaluation
Vygotsky's work has not received the same level of intense scrutiny that Piaget's has, partly due
to the time-consuming process of translating Vygotsky's work from Russian. Also, Vygotsky's
sociocultural perspective does not provide as many specific hypotheses to test as did Piaget's
theory, making refutation difficult, if not impossible.
Perhaps the main criticism of Vygotsky's work concerns the assumption that it is relevant to all
cultures. Rogoff (1990) dismisses the idea that Vygotsky's ideas are culturally universal and
instead states the concept of scaffolding - which is heavily dependent on verbal instruction - may
not be equally useful in all cultures for all types of learning. Indeed, in some instances,
observation and practice may be more effective ways of learning certain skills.
References
Behrend, D.A., Rosengren, K.S., & Perlmutter, M. (1992). The relation between private
speech and parental interactive style. In R.M. Diaz & L.E. Berk (Eds.), Private speech:
From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 85–100). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Berk, L. E. (1986). Relationship of elementary school children's private speech to
behavioral accompaniment to task, attention, and task performance. Developmental
Psychology, 22(5), 671.
Berk, L. & Garvin, R. (1984). Development of private speech among low-income
Appalachian children. Developmental Psychology, 20(2), 271-286.
Berk, L. E., & Landau, S. (1993). Private speech of learning-disabled and normally achieving
children in classroom academic and laboratory contexts. Child Development, 64, 556–571.
Cannella, G. S., & Reiff, J. C. (1994). Individual constructivist teacher education:
Teachers as empowered learners. Teacher education quarterly, 27-38.
Diaz, R. M., & Berk, L. E. (1992). Private speech: From social interaction to self-
regulation. Lawrence Erlbaum.
Frauenglass, M. & Diaz, R. (1985). Self-regulatory functions of children's private
speech: A critical analysis of recent challenges to Vygotsky's theory. Developmental
Psychology, 21(2), 357-364.
Fernyhough, C., & Fradley, E. (2005). Private speech on an executive task: Relations
with task difficulty and task performance. Cognitive Development, 20, 103–120.
Freund, L. S. (1990). Maternal regulation of children's problem-solving behavior and its
impact on children's performance. Child Development, 61, 113-126.
Ostad, S. A., & Sorensen, P. M. (2007). Private speech and strategy-use patterns:
Bidirectional comparisons of children with and without mathematical difficulties in a
developmental perspective. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40, 2–14.
Piaget, J. (1959). The language and thought of the child (Vol. 5). Psychology Press.
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeships in thinking. New York: Oxford University Press.
Schaffer, R. (1996). Social development. Oxford: Blackwell.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Thinking and speech. In R.W. Rieber & A.S. Carton (Eds.), The
collected works of L.S. Vygotsky, Volume 1: Problems of general psychology (pp. 39–
285). New York: Plenum Press. (Original work published 1934.)
Winsler, A., Abar, B., Feder, M. A., Schunn, C. D., & Rubio, D. A. (2007). Private
speech and executive functioning among high-functioning children with autistic
spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1617-1635.