Understanding Student Different
Understanding Student Different
RICHARD M . FELDER complaints being variations of'They can memorize and plug num-
Department of Chemical Engin bers into formulas but they don't know how to think!" And yet,
North Carolina State University most engineering departments have one or more faculty members
who manage to get many of those same students to perform at re-
REBECCA BRENT markably high levels, displaying first-rate problem-solving and crit-
Education Designs, Inc. ical and creative thinking skills. Skill deficiencies observed in engi-
neering graduates must therefore aiso be attributable in part to what
instmctors are doing or failing to do.
ABSTRACF An implication of these observations is that to reduce enroll-
ment attrition and improve the thinking and problem-solving skills
Students have diíFerent levels of motivation, different attitudes of engineering graduates, engineering schools should attempt to
about teaching and learning, and different responses to specific improve the qualityoftlieir teaching, which in turn reqtiires under-
elassroom environments and instructional practices. The more standing the learning needs of today's engineering students and de-
thoroughly instructors understand the differences, the better signing instruction to meet those needs. The problem is that no two
chance tliey have of meering the diverse leartiing needs of all of students are alike. They have different backgrounds, strengtlis and
their students. TTiree categories of di\'ersit}' that have been shown weaknesses, interests, ambitions, senses of responsibility, levels of
to ha\'e important implications for teaching and learning are motivation, and approaches to stud\'ing. Teaching methods also
differences in students' learning styles (characteristic ways of vary. Some instructors mainly iecture, while others spend more
taking in and processing information), approaches to learning time on demonstrations or activities^ some focus on pnnctples and
(surfïtce, deep, and strategic), and intellectual development levels others on applications; some emphasize memory and others under-
(attitudes ahout the nature of knowledge and how it should be standing. How much a given student learns in a class is govemed in
acquired and evaluated). This article reviews models that have part by that student's native ability and prior preparation but also by
been developed for each of these categories, outlines their the compatibility ofthe student's attributes as a learner and the in-
pedagogical impÜcadons, and su^ests areas for fiirther study. structor's teaching style.
designed to address t h e learning needs of stj'les less favored by m t h results summarized in Table 1 [50]. Unless othenwse indicat-
traditional instruction, and Sharp [40] descrihes an instructional ed, the population samples shovm in Table 1 are undergraduates.
module based o n the Felder-Silverman model that makes students T h u s , for example, o f t h e 129 undergraduate engineering students
aware of differences in learmng styles and h o w they may afïêct per- w h o completed the I L S in a study conducted at Iowa State Univer-
sonal interactions, teamwork, interactions with professors, a n d sity-, 63 percent were classified as active (A) iearnet^ (and by impli-
learning difficulties and successes. cation 37 percent were classified as reflecta'C learners), 67 percent
2) The index of learning styles: T h e Index of Learning Styles® were sensing (S) learners (33 percent intuitive learners), 85 percent
(ILS) is a forty-four-item forced-choice instrument developed in were visual (Vs) learners (15 percent verbal), and 5 8 percent were
1991 by Richard Felder and Barbara Soloman to assess preferences sequential (Sq) learners (42 percent global).
on the four scales of the Felder-Silverman model. In 1994 several Table 1 iUustiates several o f t h e mismatches described by Felder
hundred sets of responses to the initial twent)'-eight-itern version of and Silverman [13] between learning styles of most engineering u n -
the instrument were collected and subjected to factor analysis. Items dergraduates and traditional teaching styles in engineering educa-
that did not load significantly on single factors were discarded and tion, Si)rt\'-three percent o f t h e undergraduates vi'ere sensore, while
replaced by Tie.vi items to create the current wrsion, which was put tradidonal engineeting instruction tends to be heavily otiented t o -
on the W o r l d W i d e W e b in 1997 [49]. T h e I L S is availahle at no ward intuitors, emphasizing theory and mathematical modeling
cost to individuals who wish to assess their own preferences and to
over experimentation and practical apphcattons in most courses^
instmctors or students who wish to use it for dassroom instmction or
82 percent o f t h e undergraduates were i^sual learners, while most
research, and it may be licensed by non-educational o i ^ n i z a t i o n s .
engineering instruction is overwhelininglv verbal, emphasizing
Learning style preferences of numerous students and faculty written explanations and mathematical formulations of phi.'sical
members have been determined using the Index of Learning Styles, p h e n o m e n a over demonstrations and visual illustrations^ and 64
•I
Miistery Kece vine 1 111 personal InUTpersonal Individual Interinüividual
King-
Kitcliener
H H Ihi
kiw« cdce
1 ate
Prea
Th nking
Paiiern
Thinking
Paiiern
Quasi-Rclleclive
knowlcdye
Relleetivc
1 hinking
assign levels to them. The neural net is trained on a set of responses the subjects. Most ofthe entering students were near Pern' Level 3,
submitted by individuals with known levels on the Reflective Judg onl)' beginning to recognize that not all knowledge is certain and
ment and Periy models (based on structured interviews). In initial still relying hea\'ily on autliorities as sources of truth. The average
tests, the maximum correlation coefficient of about 0,5 between the change after four years of college was one level, with most of the
interview-based levels and the Cogito-as signed levels was indeed change occurring in the last year. Neither instructional approach
higher than the best values obtained for the penciJ-and-paper in- met its goal of elevating a significant number of students to Level 5,
struments, but was still well below the desired minimum value of As discouraging as these resitlts might seem, one could speculate
0,S. The authors speculated that 0.5-0.6 might be an upper bound that a cunicuJum lacldng such features as the experiential leaming
to the correlation coefficient between ratings obtained using inter- envirotiment at Mines or the project-based first-year experience at
views and objectively-scored instruments. Penn State (in Wankat's term, a "dualistic curriculum" [91]) would
lead to even less growth than was observed in tlie two studies in
C. Levels ofDevelopment of Engineering Students quesáon.
Table 3 summarizes results of two studies in which the Periy Wise et •ú- [106] also report Peny ratings of eight male engineer-
levels of beginning and advanced engineering undergraduates were ing students and eight female engineering students who completed
measured. Pavelich's study [102] was carried out to assess the effect the first-year project-based design course. There was initially no ap-
on intellectual development ofthe strong experiential learning en\'i- preciable difference between the two groups in average Peny rating
ronment at the Colorado School of Mines. The study by Wise et al, or SAT scores. At the end of die first year, tlie arerage Peny rating
[106] was intended to determine the effect ofa first-year project- was 3.50 for die men and 3.16 for the women; at the end ofthe third
based design course at Penn State. The studies are remarkably con- year the ratings were 3.50 (men) and 3.00 {women); and at tlu- end
sistent in their assessments ofthe initial and final average levels of ofthe fourth year the ratings were 4.00 (men) and 4.50 (women)
whilçj the flill spectrum of learning stj'les, with some problems emphasiz-
ing practical considerations and requiring carefiil attention to details
Tahle 4. IlStructional conditiot U thatfacilitate intellectual
(sensing strengths) and odiers calling fbr tlieorerical interpretation
growth [95],
and mathematical modeling (intuitive strengths), some involving