Lynn 2017
Lynn 2017
Lynn 2017
To cite this article: Steven Jay Lynn, Reed Maxwell & Joseph P. Green (2017) The Hypnotic
Induction in the Broad Scheme of Hypnosis: A Sociocognitive Perspective, American Journal of
Clinical Hypnosis, 59:4, 363-384, DOI: 10.1080/00029157.2016.1233093
Joseph P. Green
Ohio State University at Lima, Lima, Ohio, USA
Researchers and clinicians typically divide hypnosis into two distinct parts: the induction and the
suggestions that follow. We suggest that this distinction is arbitrary and artificial. Different definitions
of hypnosis ascribe different roles to the hypnotic induction, yet none clearly specifies the mechanisms
that mediate or moderate subjective and behavioral responses to hypnotic suggestions. Researchers have
identified few if any differences in responding across diverse hypnotic inductions, and surprisingly little
research has focused on the specific ingredients that optimize responsiveness. From a sociocognitive
perspective, we consider the role of inductions in the broader scheme of hypnosis and suggest that there is
no clear line of demarcation between prehypnotic information, the induction, suggestions, and other
constituents of the hypnotic context. We describe research efforts to maximize responses to hypnotic
suggestions, which encompass the induction and other aspects of the broader hypnotic framework, and
conclude with a call for more research on inductions and suggestions to better understand their role
within hypnotic interventions in research and clinical contexts.
Keywords: hypnosis, hypnosis definition, hypnotic responsiveness, hypnotic suggestion, induction
Hypnosis has fueled the popular and scientific imagination for centuries. The roots of
hypnosis stretch back to prehistory and surely to ancient times, and the ritual induction
of altered experiential states for spiritual and physical healing remains a key feature in
Western and non-Western religious practices as well (Pintar & Lynn, 2008). The now
virtually mythic figure, Mesmer, was an icon of the charismatic person who wields
amazing and ineluctable power over the participant. This image was firmly implanted in
the public mind following the publication of De Maurier’s novel, Trilby (1894/2009), in
which Svengali—whose name today connotes a ruthless manipulator—uses hypnosis to
dominate the hapless Trilby.
The very idea of inducing hypnosis still reverberates with connotations of power and
control, as depicted stereotypically in popular culture, dramatized vividly in books,
Address correspondence to Steven Jay Lynn, Ph.D., ABPP, Psychology Department, Binghamton University,
Binghamton, NY 13902. E-mail: [email protected]
364 LYNN ET AL.
movies, and television. In recent years, the Internet has touted hypnotic inductions that
push the envelope of the bizarre and absurd, claiming that conversational hypnosis can
be used to “get people to do anything you want” (http://undergroundhypnosissecretsre
vealed.blogspot.com/), and males can use special induction techniques to feminize their
bodies to transform each body part into a desired feminine shape (http://www.hypnosis
blacksecrets.com).
Such nonsense aside, practitioners of hypnosis have concocted a potpourri of approaches
to produce whatever at the historical time or the whim of the hypnosis operator has been
considered to be “hypnotic.” A small sample of these inductions include flashing lights;
focusing on the breath; fixating the eyes on an object, such as a moving watch popularized
in the media; applying pressure to participants’ heads; clanging gongs; and suggesting
relaxation and alertness (Gibbons & Lynn, 2010). It seems that the only constraint on the
nature of hypnotic inductions is the imagination of the hypnotist, although the inductions
used in standardized scales of hypnosis, and incorporated in the repertoire of many clinical
practitioners, are generally quite tame, emphasizing relaxation, sleepiness and drowsiness,
imagination, focused attention, and cooperation. Moreover, as we will describe, inductions
differ along multiple dimensions, such as their length, whether the wording is authoritative
or permissive, whether they are individualized, and whether they are standardized for
research or clinical purposes or administered on an individual or group basis or on a self
versus hypnotist initiated basis. These highly diverse possibilities for inductions underscore
the problems inherent in making generalizations about hypnotic inductions and in finding
commonalities among them. Nevertheless, we attempt to identify such commonalities in
our account of the role of the induction in the broader hypnotic scheme in an effort to
move toward a more comprehensive understanding of how the induction works from a
sociocognitive perspective.
Our review is limited to a consideration of behavioral and subjective responses asso-
ciated with inductions. We do not discuss the thorny question of psychophysiological/
neurological correlates or concomitants of inductions, because interested readers will find
an in depth and critical discussion elsewhere of findings and methodological issues
pertinent to this topic examined from a sociocognitive perspective (Lynn, Kirsch,
Knox, & Lilienfeld, 2006).
Definitions of Hypnosis
Given the rich diversity in inductions, it is not surprising that they have been defined
only in the most general terms by three definitions of hypnosis crafted by the hypnosis
division of American Psychological over more than two decades. The 1994 definition
simply states: “The hypnotic context is generally established by an induction procedure.
Although there are many different hypnotic inductions, most include suggestions for
INDUCTION IN THE BROADER SCHEME OF HYPNOSIS 365
According to this view, simply using the word “hypnosis” in the context of what is
perceived to be an induction may be sufficient to establish a rule-governed context for
responding to imaginative suggestions. Yet, we will argue that what precedes and
follows any ritualized induction may be every bit as influential in determining hypnotic
responding as the induction itself.
Turning now to the “next” definition of the hypnotic induction, a decade later, the
American Psychological Association hypnosis division defined the hypnotic induction this
way: “Hypnosis typically involves an introduction to the procedure during which the subject
is told that suggestions for imaginative experiences will be presented. The hypnotic induction
is an extended initial suggestion for using one’s imagination, and may contain further
elaborations of the introduction …” (Green, Barabasz, Barrett, & Montgomery, 2005, p. 262).
One virtue of this definition is that it correctly implies that the hypnotic induction is
merely one aspect of the hypnotic proceeding and that the induction often proceeds
from the information provided by the hypnotist that precedes the induction. Indeed,
perhaps more often than not, there may be no more than a sliver thin dividing line, if
any at all, between activities that precede and follow the induction, and no boundary of
demarcation between suggestions contained in the induction and those that follow. The
definition is also in keeping with the fact that many inductions have in common explicit
suggestions to deflect attention from the external environment (other than the hypnotist
and suggestions) and narrow and focus attention internally on suggestion-related
imaginings and, in so doing, vividly imagine and experience suggested events.
Accordingly, a key point of our thesis is that although hypnosis is typically described as
involving two crucial phases or components—the induction and suggestions—these
components are actually only two of numerous threads in a much broader “hypnotic”
skein. Indeed, it is often imprecise to describe what transpires in terms of clear-cut phases
or stages, so that where the “induction” begins and ends is often a strained, arbitrary, and
artificial distinction. Rather, the induction is often preceded by an informed consent
procedure in research settings; an introduction by the hypnotist, which frames and
contextualizes hypnosis; and questions the participant asks about hypnosis followed by
the hypnotist’s responses. The induction itself is typically comprised of suggestions that
often stream unobtrusively into experimental test or clinical suggestions that are targeted
to achieve research or clinical objectives. As we will illustrate, all of these components
seem to serve a common purpose: (1) to build and validate the top-down constructed
cognitive-affective context—loosely speaking the participants’ “experiential world”—
from which the hypnotic behavior arises, and (2) perhaps to also reduce the impact of
bottom-up input (e.g., distracting sensations) on hypnotic responding.
In certain clinical contexts, the lines between the induction and other aspects of
hypnosis may be further blurred or eliminated. For example, in the popular Ericksonian
tradition the therapist may attempt to induce hypnosis on an entirely informal and
conversational basis, integrating suggestions into stories and anecdotes, metaphors,
and information derived from the person’s past and current everyday experiences in a
INDUCTION IN THE BROADER SCHEME OF HYPNOSIS 367
seamless manner (see Matthews, Lankton, & Lankton, 1993; Yapko, 2012). In research
settings, the induction will typically be more standardized and more formally demar-
cated from other aspects of the induction of hypnosis than in clinical contexts, although
this issue has not been explored on an empirical basis.
Unfortunately, researchers have not subjected more informal conversational
approaches to hypnosis to scrutiny in a systematic fashion. Nor has the role of socio-
cognitive variables been studied extensively in Ericksonian-type hypnosis, although the
literature we review below on the equivalence of traditional authoritative direct versus
indirect and permissive suggestions, which are mainstays of the Ericksonian approach,
suggests that there is nothing “special” about the latter type of suggestions in terms of
their ability to increase hypnotic responsiveness.
But to return to our point of emphasis, the hypnotic induction and suggestions are
often enfolded within a larger framework of events and potentially complex interperso-
nal transactions (particularly in clinical contexts). Hypnosis unfurls from the inception
of the hypnotist-participant contact and extends to suggestions or instructions to termi-
nate hypnosis (i.e., at the count of 5, you will “come out of hypnosis”), which
effectively deactivates the top-down constructed context, and the parting interaction
of the hypnotist and participant. Moreover, hypnosis—particularly in clinical settings in
which suggestions are typically tailored to the individual—often unfolds as a reciprocal
process in which the patient’s responses influence subsequent hypnotist interventions in
a recursive manner on a moment-to-moment basis.
The second definition also hints at the role of imagination as a mechanism in
producing hypnotic responses. Indeed, many inductions include suggestions that
actively encourage use of the imagination and thinking and imagining along with
suggestions, as well as suggestions that imply that suggested events will occur invo-
luntarily or automatically. Nevertheless, the definition does not explain how using one’s
imagination facilitates responsiveness to suggestions and begs the question of how
participants translate what is imagined into felt sensations and involuntary actions
consistent with the aim of suggestions. Nor is it always the case that inductions
emphasize specifically the use of imagination.
The third and most recent definition of hypnosis (Elkins, Barabasz, Council, &
Spiegel, 2015) states that the hypnotic induction is “a procedure designed to induce
hypnosis.… The definition is designed such that it does not specify what procedures
may be involved or the context in which they occur. This may allow for procedures that
involve interaction between a therapist and client, self-hypnosis procedures, or other
procedures that may be identified in the future in clinical practice of research (such as
through advances in technology” (p. 6).
This definition skirts any attempt to specify attributes of the induction, mechanisms
associated with successful responding, and the place of the induction in the broader
procedures. This latter definition, in an attempt to be “theoretically neutral,” is arguably
a step backwards in developing an operational understanding of inductions and arriving
368 LYNN ET AL.
at even a rudimentary mechanistic explanation for their role and function (see Lynn,
Green, et al., 2015). Generally speaking, the lack of specificity and elaboration in these
three definitions reflects the sparse attention in the literature paid to the induction per se.
To better understand the role of the induction and its place in the overall scheme of hypnosis,
we will first examine the typical hypnosis setting and the effects of different inductions on
hypnotic responsiveness. We will suggest that the specific nature of the induction appears to
play a negligible role in affecting hypnotic responding. In contrast, we will suggest that
successful responding depends on a constellation of attitudes, beliefs, understandings,
expectancies, motivation, and response sets of participants, and on rapport with the hypnotist
in clinical settings, in particular. These variables, clearly identified with the sociocognitive
perspective of hypnosis (e.g., Lynn, Laurence, & Kirsch, 2015; Kirsch, 1990; Spanos, 1986),
promote cognitive-affective-behavioral plasticity that fosters increased responsiveness to
specific suggestions within the broader hypnotic context. This broad context sets new
parameters for how “reality” is temporarily experienced and how information is processed.
We will further develop this argument in the course of our discussion of commonalities
associated with diverse inductions and the broader scheme of hypnosis, research on the
hypnotic induction, and empirical efforts to increase hypnotic responsiveness.
The setting in which hypnosis is typically conducted crucially marks the situation as
“hypnotic.” For example, the setting is characteristically designed to enhance physical
comfort, relaxation, and quiet to limit distractions. The participant may be seated in a
comfortable chair or couch and be encouraged to pay attention to both the hypnotist’s voice
and to get involved with the suggestions and relax. The lighting may be soft or dimmed. The
office or experimental room appointed with paintings or photographs selected to promote a
calm and relaxed state of mind (e.g., bucolic or beach scenes, sunsets). Eye closure is often
one of the first suggestions or instructions presented, and is geared to at once to direct
attention away from the external milieu and to facilitate a more narrow or concerted focus
on imagery and internal experiences as they arise. The hypnotist’s voice, often presented in
a “hypnotic like” pitch and cadence—which mirrors cultural beliefs regarding what is
“hypnotic”—also sets the situation apart from the mundane and can act as nonverbal
suggestion to “go into hypnosis” (Barber, 1985). Together, the effect of the hypnotist’s
presentation may be to amplify the effects of suggestions and top-down influences while
depreciating the impact of bottom-up input (e.g., distracting sensations, perceptions, affects)
that interfere with involvement in suggestions.
INDUCTION IN THE BROADER SCHEME OF HYPNOSIS 369
Prehypnotic Information
From the first to the last words the hypnotist utters, the intention is to make the
experience of hypnosis less foreign and more palatable to participants and to minimize
anxiety and oppositional resistance while increasing curiosity about hypnosis and
maximizing expectations for positive responding. To facilitate comfort with the idea
of “being hypnotized,” the preamble may liken hypnosis to familiar scenarios, such as
involvement in watching a movie or getting absorbed while driving, or experiencing a
beautiful sunset or a visit to a favorite place. This information counters the widely
prevailing myth that hypnosis involves going into a “trance” far removed from everyday
experience. Alternatively, hypnosis may be presented as involving cooperation, an
altered state of consciousness, or a state of dissociation or heightened suggestibility.
Still, in keeping with our earlier discussion, we suspect that the particular way in which
hypnosis is described may be less important than casting the situation as “hypnotic,”
thereby locating hypnosis within a broader cultural frame, with attendant connotations
of increased responsiveness and heightened expectations. The single, powerful word
“hypnosis,” by meaningfully evoking culture-bound associations regarding what it
connotes to be hypnotized, may, therefore, account for many of the impressive effects
we attribute to the broader, sometimes lengthy and elaborate induction procedure.
An important goal of facilitative prehypnotic information is to counteract misconceptions
about hypnosis and negative beliefs and attitudes that may impede optimal responding.
These misconceptions include the culturally prevalent ideas that (1) hypnosis robs people of
their willpower and they cannot resist or oppose suggestions, and, instead, respond roboti-
cally; (2) hypnosis is associated with gullibility, low intelligence, or a lack of willpower;
(3) people lose awareness of their surroundings or develop spontaneous amnesia for what
occurred during hypnosis; and (4) people will “not be able to come out of hypnosis”
(see Green, Page, Rasekhy, Johnson, & Bernhardt, 2006; Lynn, Rhue, & Kirsch, 2010).
Prehypnotic motivational information can further convey the expectation that many
people are able to respond to a variety of suggestions, the experience of hypnosis will
be enjoyable and relaxing—often directly reinforced with suggestions for relaxation in
the induction—and that people respond to suggestions to different degrees, so as to
minimize negative expectancies arising from less than “perfect” or sub-optimal respond-
ing. Participants may also be instructed to think and imagine along with suggestions and
to experience them to the fullest extent possible, thereby motivating goal-directed and
active responses to suggestions and minimizing bottom-up influences (Barber &
Calverley, 1964; Barber, Spanos, & Chaves, 1972). These efforts are designed to
encourage participants to approach the experience with a positive, motivated, cogni-
tively committed set to respond, rather than face the prospect of hypnosis with fear and
trepidation or passively wait for a response to “just happen.”
To further minimize response-interfering concerns and potential resistance, partici-
pants may benefit from a demonstration that they can respond easily to nonhypnotic
370 LYNN ET AL.
suggestions, such as an easy to pass suggestion for arm levitation, or head falling, as
included in the Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility: Form A (HGSHS:A;
Shor & Orne, 1962). Or to counteract common fears of relinquishing control, the
hypnotist may encourage deliberate and active resistance to a suggestion, such as
hands moving together (as if pulled by a magnetic force), to “prove to participants
and the hypnotist” that they can actively resist (Gfeller, Lynn, & Pribble, 1986). For
some participants (typically highly suggestible) who respond—despite instructions to
resist the hypnotist (see Lynn, Nash, Rhue, Frauman, & Sweeney, 1984)—the hypnotist
can comment on how their strong response suggests that they are excellent hypnotic
subjects. The hypnotist might add that if a suggestion were provided to engage in an
action that violated their ethics or personal standards or values, they could easily resist
(Coe, Kobayashi, & Howard, 1973).
Can the preamble make a difference? Lynn, Vanderhoff, Shindler, and Stafford (2002)
compared the impact of prehypnotic instructions that informed participants that the ability
to experience hypnosis depended on their ability to achieve an altered state of conscious-
ness or “trance” versus their willingness to cooperate. The latter instructions are included
in the Carleton University Scale of Hypnotic Responsiveness (CURSS; Spanos, Radtke,
Hodgins, Stam, & Bertrand, 1983). Participants who received the altered state set
indicated prior to hypnosis that they would respond to fewer suggestions and later
responded to fewer suggestions compared with participants who received the standard
instructions that emphasized cooperation. We interpreted these findings as indicating that
participants’ expectancies about successful responding are dampened by the prospect of
entering a trance state that may be perceived to be difficult to achieve, or associated with
negative connotations, and that when their experience fails to match their expectations, it
diminishes their responsiveness to suggestion (see Lynn, Green, Jaquith, & Gasior, 2003).
In another study, Accardi, Cleere, Lynn, and Kirsch (2013) found no differences in
participant responsiveness across rationales that experimenters provided regarding hypnosis
when we equated the rationales for induced expectancies about successful responding.
More specifically, in one condition we presented hypnosis in keeping with the preamble that
accompanies the HGSHS:A (Shor & Orne, 1962), which informs participants that hypnosis
is “largely a question of willingness to be receptive and responsive to ideas, and to allow
these ideas to act on you without interference.” We compared the effects of this rationale
with one that indicated that hypnosis could be used as a non-deceptive placebo (Kirsch,
1994). This rationale informed participants, “Hypnosis acts very much like a placebo … in
this study, we can use hypnosis as a placebo because hypnosis can be used without any
deception at all … and like a placebo, hypnosis seems to work by mobilizing people’s
positive expectancies” (p. 108).
To control for expectations, in both conditions we told participants that hypnosis
works by mobilizing participants’ positive expectancies, but no mention of hypnosis
being a placebo was presented in the standard condition. We found that participants
responded comparably in terms of average hypnotic attitudes, prehypnotic expectances,
INDUCTION IN THE BROADER SCHEME OF HYPNOSIS 371
Given commonalities we referred to across inductions and other aspects of the hypnotic
proceedings, it follows to ask whether the nature of the induction per se has much of a
bearing on hypnotic responsiveness. Researchers have discovered that it appears to
make no difference in behavioral responding whether the induction is (1) administered
in a group or on an individual basis (Bentler & Hilgard, 1963); (2) relatively long or
short (Klinger, 1970); (3) administered live or via audio tape (Fassler, Lynn, & Knox,
2008); (4) administered using a traditional eye closure induction or a standardized
Chiasson (1973) technique (hand moves toward face and when it reaches the face, the
participant experiences a deep state of hypnosis; Page & Handley, 1991); (5) taken from
the Carleton University Responsiveness Scale induction or a modified version of the
Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale, Form C; and (6) presented as heterohypnosis or
self-hypnosis (Hammond, Haskins-Bartsch, Grant, & McGhee, 1988).
Additionally, researchers have examined the effects of inductions in terms of whether
the suggestions contained in them are direct versus indirect. Direct suggestions are
statements that are specific, clear, directive, and command like, often delivered in an
authoritative manner (O’Hanlon, 1987), such as “Your arm will rise up off the resting
surface” and “You are very deeply relaxed.” In contrast, indirect suggestions (Matthews
et al., 1993; Stone & Lundy, 1985) typically share one or more of the following
features. They are permissive, often nonspecific or ambiguous, and allow for choice
and a range of appropriate responses, such as “You may experience a light, deep, or
medium trance today. I wonder just how deep you will go?”
Researchers have generally found no differences in behavioral responses to direct
versus indirect inductions and suggestions (see Lynn, Neufeld, & Mare, 1993). Lynn,
Neufeld, and Matyi (1987) is one of the few studies that manipulated the induction
wording independent of suggestion wording. The wording of the induction had no effect
on subjects’ responsivity and their ratings of subjective involvement or suggestion-
related involuntariness.
Nevertheless, aside from the induction, some studies (Lynn et al., 1987; Lynn,
Weekes, Matyi, & Neufeld, 1988; Matthews, Bennett, Gallagher, & Bean, 1985;
Matthews & Mosher, 1988; Weekes & Lynn, 1990) have found that direct suggestions
produce greater subjective involvement, feelings of involuntariness, and diminished
resistance (Matthews & Mosher, 1988). Yet, when Matthews and Mosher (1988)
equated the inductions for length and delivery time, they failed to replicate their earlier
372 LYNN ET AL.
findings. And still other research has found no difference between direct and indirect
suggestions on measures of involuntariness (Stone & Lundy, 1985) and subjective
experience (Matheson & Shue, 1987).
Since Lynn et al.’s (1993) review, the majority of studies have found no differences in
behavioral and/or subjective responses between direct suggestions and inductions and
their indirect counterparts (Groth-Marnat & Mitchell, 1998; Hawkins, Liossi, Ewart,
Hatira, & Kosmidis, 1998; Maurer, Santangelo, & Claiborn, 1993; Pitsch, Sapp, &
McNeely, 2001; Szabó, 1993). Although the findings regarding subjective measures are
somewhat mixed, there is little ambiguity regarding conclusions that can be drawn with
regard to behavioral (i.e., objective) responses: virtually no support exists for the advan-
tage of indirect suggestions over direct suggestions. It appears to be the case that the
differences in wording between direct and indirect inductions and suggestions are incon-
sequential at the level of semantic processing and activation in the hypnotic context.
This lack of difference among inductions does not preclude differences in individual
preferences for one induction over another. Spinhoven, Baak, Van Dyck, and Vermeulen
(1988) used a version of the Harvard scale adapted with indirect suggestions and found
that subjects who received the indirect and direct Harvard scales had identical average
hypnotizability scores. Nevertheless, 20 subjects indicated a preference for the Harvard,
whereas 15 subjects preferred the indirect scale, and five were neutral in their
preference.
Researchers have also compared a traditional induction with a more unconventional and
arguably indirect induction. Matthews, Kirsch, and Mosher (1985) tested Bandler and
Grinder’s (1975) claim that the so-called double induction is superior to traditional hypno-
sis. Half of the right-handed subjects received the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale,
Form C (SHSS:C; Weitzenhoffer & Hilgard, 1962) induction, and the other half received a
stereo delivery of two simultaneously presented audio tracks. The audio track that was
directed to the subjects’ dominant hemisphere (left ear) used Erickson’s hand levitation
induction, while what the researchers referred to as “child-like grammar messages,” such as
“wondering,” ‘‘hands lifting,” “sleep,” and so forth, were delivered to the nondominant
(right ear) hemisphere. The following day the order of inductions was reversed. No
differences in hypnotizability scores were evidenced following either induction; however,
subjects who received the double induction first were later less responsive to suggestions
following both inductions than were subjects who received the standard induction first. The
findings contradict Bandler and Grinder’s (1975) assertion that the double induction is an
especially powerful means of inducing hypnosis (… one of the most powerful techniques
which we have developed is the double induction…, p. 194).
As many different hypnotic inductions share a common element—they provide
suggestions for physical and/or mental relaxation and behavioral stillness—an arguably
greater departure from the traditional relaxation-based induction is the so-called active-
alert induction. Bányai and Hilgard (1976) compared an active-alert induction, in which
participants received suggestions for increased alertness with their eyes open, while
INDUCTION IN THE BROADER SCHEME OF HYPNOSIS 373
riding a bicycle ergometer, with a traditional relaxation induction (i.e., eye fixation with
instructions to relax) followed by suggestions from a standardized scale (SHSS:B;
Weitzenhoffer & Hilgard, 1959). The researchers found no differences in hypnotic
responding between the two inductions in the context of a within-subjects design with
randomized alternation between alert-active and traditional inductions. Across both
inductions, highly suggestible participants reported that they achieved “altered states.”
More recently, Wark (2006) summarized the findings across multiple studies (e.g.,
Cardeña, 2005; Malott, 1984; Miller, Barabasz, & Barabasz, 1991) that substituted an
alert or hyper-alert induction for a more traditional relaxation-based induction and
concluded that there were practically no significant differences across suggestion types.
Cardeña, Alarcon, Capafons, and Bayot (1998) conducted an interesting study that
provided a creative demonstration of the value of induction preparation. The researchers
developed a set of energizing exercises to prepare subjects for an “active hand”
induction in which participants were invited to recall or imagine experiences intended
to increase heartbeat, respiration, and wide visual expansion. The actual induction
involved moving the right hand vertically up and down rapidly and then suggesting
increased heartbeat, respiration, and alertness. The researchers found that participants
were significantly more responsive and more likely to stay in the study compared with
Bányai and Hilgard’s (1976) bicycle induction.
Our discussion has emphasized the value of considering the induction in terms of the
broader events of hypnosis. Cardeña et al.’s study highlights the potential of instruc-
tional efforts to increase responsiveness as influential in the induction of hypnosis.
Relatedly, Vickery, Kirsch, Council, and Sirkin (1985) provided participants with
information and experimenter demonstration regarding imaginative cognitive strategies
to respond effectively to suggestions followed by practice in using the cognitive
strategies. When this “cognitive skill induction” was preceded by a traditional hypnotic
induction, subjective responses to hypnosis were enhanced significantly and objective
response was enhanced marginally, compared with the traditional induction. However,
when the order was reversed, the inductions did not produce differences in subjective or
objective responses. The traditional induction engendered greater alterations in con-
sciousness, prompting the researchers to comment, “… the skill induction teaches
subjects cognitive strategies that enhance responsivity to suggestions in subsequent
hypnotic experiences independent of the alterations in consciousness elicited by trance
induction” (p. 131).
Around the time of this research, Gorassini and Spanos (1986) initiated a line of studies
that focused on efforts to systematically enhance hypnotic suggestibility by teaching not
only cognitive strategies, but also enhancing response expectancies, minimizing or
374 LYNN ET AL.
eliminating negative beliefs about hypnosis, and enhancing rapport and minimizing
resistance to responding to suggestions. More specifically, the Carleton Skills Training
Program (CSTP) the researchers developed includes the following components: (1)
information to instill positive attitudes about hypnosis and motivation to respond;
(2) instructions regarding how to use imaginal strategies to promote successful respond-
ing and instructions regarding how to interpret suggestions (e.g., enactment instructions
that one must actually lift the hand while imagining it is rising by itself); (3) exposure to a
video-taped model who enacts successful responses to suggestions and verbalizes
imagery-based strategies to facilitate subjective response; and (4) practice in responding
to suggestions. The selection of variables to target in this program was inspired by the
sociocognitive model of hypnosis (see Kirsch, 1990; Lynn, Laurence, & Kirsch, 2015;
Spanos, 1986, 1991), which holds that attitudes, beliefs, expectancies, motivation,
responsiveness to nonhypnotic imaginative suggestions, and cognitive strategies are
variables key to understanding and potentially facilitating hypnotic responsiveness.
Spanos and his associates (see Gfeller & Gorassini, 2010; Gorassini & Spanos, 1999)
have determined that approximately 50% of initially low suggestible subjects who
underwent the CSTP scored as high suggestible at post-testing with standardized
inductions. Importantly, researchers have documented large magnitude CSTP treatment
gains that have persisted for two and a half years (Spanos, Cross, Menary, & Smith,
1988) across studies in Spain (Cangas & Pérez, 1998), Poland (Niedzwienska, 2000),
and England (Fellows & Ragg, 1992). Because the CSTP produces increases in both
objective and subjective responsiveness to hypnotic suggestions, with gains general-
izing to novel test suggestions (Spanos, 1986), it is possible that suggestibility training
can increase responsiveness to clinically relevant suggestions related to hypnotic
suggestibility, such as for pain control (see Lynn, 2004; Lynn & Sherman, 2000).
Before leaving this topic, it bears mention that rapport is another aspect of the
hypnotic situation that is amenable to modification. Lynn et al. (1991) tested high and
low suggestible participants in either a high or a low rapport condition. In the high
rapport condition, the hypnotist was friendly, smiled, and disclosed to the participant on
relatively intimate topics selected from a list ranked in intimacy. In the low rapport
condition, the hypnotist was formal in demeanor and disclosed on only the most
superficial topics. Initially low responsive participants passed more than 2.5 suggestions
and, on average, scored in the medium hypnotizable range after the manipulation of
positive rapport, whereas the initially highly suggestible participants were unaffected by
the rapport manipulation.
Researchers have also shown that rapport also exerts an effect on the impact of CSTP
training. Gfeller et al. (1987) reported that 25% of initially low suggestible participants
tested as highly suggestible after training in a relatively “neutral” rapport condition
compared with 50% of participants who tested as highly suggestible after training in a
condition designed to facilitate rapport with the trainer. Unfortunately, typically little is
done to increase rapport, at least in the context of experimental hypnosis, which is
INDUCTION IN THE BROADER SCHEME OF HYPNOSIS 375
perhaps one reason why it seems to matter little whether instructions are provided in a
live format or via an audio recording (Fassler et al. 2008), or whether hypnosis is
defined as self-hypnosis and participants are trained to administer self-suggestions
(Barber, 1985).
The CSTP program has not been without its critics, as justifiable questions linger
regarding the extent to which post-training gains reflect instructed versus more sponta-
neous, ability-related cognitive and experiential processes (see Lynn et al., 2015).
Nevertheless, following Gfeller and Gorassini (2010), we suggest that it may be fruitful
to include many of the elements associated with the CSTP into relatively brief proce-
dures initiated prior to and during inductions, because research has substantiated the role
of elements of the CSTP as accounting for substantial variability in hypnotic responding
(see Lynn, Laurence et al., 2015).
As we have indicated, prehypnotic information can be used to bolster positive expec-
tancies, increase motivation, correct misconceptions about hypnosis, and facilitate rapport
with participants. It may also be worthwhile to attempt to minimize top down impedi-
ments to hypnotic responding in hypnotic suggestibility modification programs and
inductions more generally. For example, participants could be instructed to accept that
initially distracting thoughts and sensations may arise in the course of events, and “that is
perfectly fine, just know that they are natural occurrences and should not get in the way at
all with your ability to experience suggestions … you can easily become absorbed in your
experiences as you find that these thoughts and sensations recede into the background of
your consciousness as you get more deeply involved in your experience of hypnosis.”
Methods and suggestions intended to enhance rapport, motivation, and positive
expectancies, could be included in hypnotic inductions and conceivably be individua-
lized based on a careful assessment of the person with an eye toward optimizing
hypnotic performance. The effects of individualizing inductions and procedures is
clearly an understudied area, with only one study to date comparing a standardized
hypnotic suggestibility scale, based on a relaxation induction, with an individualized
version of the same scale to determine whether individualizing the induction enhances
hypnotic responsiveness. In research conducted by Van Der Does, Van Dyck,
Spinhoven, and Kloosman (1989), participants were tested twice; once with the stan-
dardized Stanford Hypnotic Clinical Scale: Adult (Morgan & Hilgard, 1978) and once
with an adapted, individualized version of the same scale in which the hypnotherapists
(experienced members of the Netherlands Society of Hypnosis) were “… free to use
whatever induction procedures and wording of suggestions seemed appropriate for a
particular S” (p. 183). The researchers found no differences across the standardized and
individualized version of the scale. Nevertheless, the criteria for individualizing the
procedures were neither standardized nor theory-based, so much research needs to be
done on individualizing suggestions to guide and facilitate clinical work.
A novel way to increase hypnotic suggestibility may be to provide instructions or
suggestions to facilitate goal-directed behavior in the hypnotic context. Gollwitzer
376 LYNN ET AL.
In this article, we have taken note of the tremendous variability in hypnotic inductions
and procedures that render it difficult to draw unambiguous conclusions and interpreta-
tions regarding the nature, role, intended purpose, and impact of inductions on hypnotic
responding. We now suggest that the problem of disambiguating the induction from
other elements of the broader scheme of hypnosis begins even prior to the hypnotic
session. More specifically, it is difficult to disentangle the effects of an induction from
information provided prior to even starting the hypnosis session in the informed consent
procedures in a research context, or in the earliest informal or nonstandardized com-
ments by the hypnotist in a clinical setting. Indeed, how the session is cast (e.g., what is
the nature of hypnosis; why is it being used; what is the probability that it will help in a
particular circumstance), as well as information about myths and potential risks, may
vary widely among hypnosis sessions and studies and exert important and as yet often
under-appreciated effects on hypnotic responding.
INDUCTION IN THE BROADER SCHEME OF HYPNOSIS 377
participants are told that “all hypnosis is really self-hypnosis,” as is often the case, likely
activates attitudes, beliefs, and expectancies regarding hypnosis that are deeply rooted in
our culture. Still, limited research has directly compared responsiveness to hypnosis when
the procedures are defined as self- versus hetero-hypnosis, and research that compares the
role of sociocognitive variables across hetero- versus self-hypnosis is certainly warranted.
Clinical practitioners and researchers not infrequently encounter participants who
deny the fact that they were “hypnotized” or minimize their responsiveness to hypnotic
suggestions, despite the fact that they display a high level of behavioral responsiveness
to suggestions. These types of reports have not been studied systematically, and deserve
such scrutiny, although they may simply be indicators of mismatches between partici-
pants’ experience and their expectancies regarding how “good” or “excellent” hypnotic
subjects experience hypnosis. For example, if participants do not believe that they
achieved the proverbial and poorly defined “trance state,” they may report after
hypnosis that they were “not hypnotized.” In fact, researchers have shown that partici-
pants who test at the extremes of hypnotic high suggestibility do not tend to rate their
responsiveness in accordance with their actual objective performance, because there are
no clear-cut widely accepted subjective criteria that determine whether a person is
“hypnotized” or not (Martin & Lynn, 1996).
Clinicians will also encounter individuals who are initially highly skeptical of their
ability to experience hypnosis. However, most initial suggestions in research protocols,
and many suggestions in psychotherapy contexts, are easy to pass (see Barber, 1985) and,
if passed by initially skeptical participants, may provide strong and compelling positive
expectations for subsequent successful responding (“Well, here I thought I could not be
hypnotized, so I must be more hypnotizable than I thought” or, “Hypnosis must be very
powerful if I could respond despite my skepticism”) in these individuals. This phenom-
enon has not been the subject of study in the laboratory. Expectancies arise not only
during the induction, but also as a function of participant self-observation of their
responsiveness to imaginative suggestions, which generally produce equivalent levels
of responsiveness, regardless of whether they are preceded by an induction or not (Meyer
& Lynn, 2011). Studies of the link between different types of inductions and changes in
expectations over the course of a hypnosis session have not been reported to date.
Note that the sociocognitive perspective does not discount the possibility that
researchers will, one day, isolate an as yet unidentified “ability factor” that is reliably
and robustly associated with the transformation of verbal suggestions into involuntary
experiences. Unfortunately, the search for such ability has not proved to be very
rewarding to date. For example, measures of dissociation and absorption account for
only a small percent of variability in hypnotic responding, especially when hypnotic
suggestibility and measures of these constructs are administered in separate test contexts
(see Council, Kirsch, & Grant, 1996; Kirsch & Lynn, 1998). Still, it is worthwhile to
continue to search for one or more abilities, in isolation or in combination, which can
account for significant variability in hypnotic responding and experience.
380 LYNN ET AL.
References
Accardi, M., Cleere, C., Lynn, S. J., & Kirsch, I. (2013). Placebo versus “standard” hypnosis rationale:
Attitudes, expectancies, hypnotic responsiveness, and experiences. American Journal of Clinical
Hypnosis, 56, 103–114. doi:10.1080/00029157.2013.769087
Bányai, E. I., & Hilgard, E. R. (1976). A comparison of active-alert hypnotic induction with traditional
relaxation induction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85, 218–224. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.85.2.218
Barber, T. X. (1985). Hypnosuggestive procedures as catalysts for psychotherapies. In S. J. Lynn & J. P.
Garske (Eds.), Contemporary psychotherapies: Models and methods (pp. 333–376). Columbus, OH:
Charles E. Merrill.
Barber, T. X., & Calverley, D. S. (1964). An experimental study of “hypnotic” (auditory and visual)
hallucinations. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68(1), 13–20. doi:10.1037/h0042175
Barber, T. X., Spanos, N. P., & Chaves, J. F. (1974). Hypnosis, imagination, and human potentialities
(Vol. 46). Oxford, UK: Pergamon.
Bandler, R., & Grinder, J. (1975). Patterns of hypnotic techniques of Milton H. Erickson, M.D. (vol.1., p.
194). Cupertino, CA: Meta Communications.
INDUCTION IN THE BROADER SCHEME OF HYPNOSIS 381
Bentler, P. M., & Hilgard, E. R. (1963). A comparison of group and individual induction of hypnosis with
self-scoring and observer-scoring. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 11,
49–54. doi:10.1080/00207146308409227
Cangas, A. J., & Pérez, M. (1998). The effect of two procedures on hypnotic susceptibility
modification. Contemporary Hypnosis & Integrative Therapy, 15(4), 212–218. doi:10.1002/(ISSN)
1557-0711
Cardeña, E. (2005). The phenomenology of deep hypnosis: Quiescent and physically active. International
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 53, 37–59. doi:10.1080/00207140490914234
Cardeña, E., Alarcon, A., Capafons, A., & Bayot, A. (1998). Effects on suggestibility of a new method of
active-alert hypnosis: Alert hand. International Journal of Clinical & Experimental Hypnosis, 46(3),
280–294. doi:10.1080/00207149808410008
Chiasson, S. (1973). Chiasson’s method. Syllabus on hypnosis and a handbook of therapeutic suggestions.
Bloomingdale, IL: American Society of Clinical Hypnosis Education and Research Foundation.
Coe, W. C., Kobayashi, K., & Howard, M. L. (1973). Experimental and ethical problems of evaluating the
influence of hypnosis in antisocial conduct. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 82(3), 476–482.
doi:10.1037/h0035365
Coe, W. C., Peterson, P., & Gwynn, M. (1995). Expectations and sequelae to hypnosis: Initial findings.
American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 38(1), 3–12.
Council, J. R., Kirsch, I., & Grant, D. L. (1996). Hypnosis and imagination (pp. 41–65). R. Kunzendorf, N.
Spanos, & B. Wallace (Eds.). Amityville, NY: Baywood Publishing.
Du Maurier, G. (1984/2009). Trilby. UK: London, Osgood, Mcilvaine & Company.
Elkins, G. R., Barabasz, A. F., Council, J. R., & Spiegel, D. (2015). Advancing research and practice: The
revised APA Division 30 definition of hypnosis. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 57, 378–385.
doi:10.1080/00029157.2015.1011465
Fassler, O., Lynn, S. J., & Knox, J. (2008). Is hypnotic suggestibility a stable trait? Consciousness and
Cognition, 17, 240–253. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2007.05.004
Fellows, B. J., & Ragg, L. (1992). The carleton skills training program: A preliminary British trial.
Contemporary Hypnosis, 9(3), 169–174.
Gfeller, J., & Gorassini, D. R. (2010). Enhancing hypnotizability and treatment response. In S. J. Lynn,
J. W. Rhue, & I. Kirsch (Eds.), Handbook of clinical hypnosis (pp. 339–356). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Gfeller, J., Lynn, S. J., & Pribble, W. (1987). Enhancing hypnotic susceptibility: Interpersonal and rapport
factors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 586–595. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.3.586
Gibbons, D. E., & Lynn, S. J. (2010). Hypnotic inductions: A primer. In Handbook of clinical hypnosis
(2 nd ed., pp. 267–293). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist,
54(7), 493–503. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.493
Gorassini, D. R., & Spanos, N. P. (1986). A social-cognitive skills approach to the successful modification
of hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(5), 1004–1012.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.50.5.1004
Gorassini, D. R., & Spanos, N. P. (1999). The carleton skill training program for modifying hypnotic
suggestibility: Original version and variations. In I. Kirsch, A. Capafons, E. Cardeña-Buelna, & S.
Amigó (Eds.), Clinical hypnosis and self-regulation: Cognitive-behavioral perspectives (pp. 141–177).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Green, J. P., Barabasz, A. F., Barrett, D., & Montgomery, G. H. (2005). Forging ahead: The 2003 APA
Division 30 definition of hypnosis. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 53,
259–264. doi:10.1080/00207140590961321
382 LYNN ET AL.
Green, J. P., Page, R. A., Rasekhy, R., Johnson, L. K., & Bernhardt, S. E. (2006). Cultural views and
attitudes about hypnosis: A survey of college students across four countries. International Journal of
Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 54, 263–280. doi:10.1080/00207140600689439
Groth-Marnat, G., & Mitchell, K. (1998). Responsiveness to direct versus indirect hypnotic procedures:
The role of resistance as a predictor variable. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Hypnosis, 46, 324–333. doi:10.1080/00207149808410012
Hammond, D. C., Haskins-Bartsch, C., Grant, C. W., & McGhee, M. (1988). Comparison of self-directed
and tape-assisted self-hypnosis. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 31, 129–137. doi:10.1080/
00029157.1988.10402879
Hawkins, P. J., Liossi, C., Ewart, B. W., Hatira, P., & Kosmidis, V. H. (1998). Hypnosis in the alleviation of
procedure related pain and distress in paediatric oncology patients. Contemporary Hypnosis, 15(4),
199–207. doi:10.1002/(ISSN)1557-0711
Kirsch, I. (1990). Changing expectations: A key to effective psychotherapy. Pacific Grove, CA: Thomson
Brooks/Cole Publishing Co.
Kirsch, I. (1994). Defining hypnosis for the public. Contemporary Hypnosis, 11(3), 142–143.
Kirsch, I., & Lynn, S. J. (1998). Dissociation theories of hypnosis. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 100–115.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.123.1.100
Kirsch, I., & Lynn, S. J. (1999). Automaticity in clinical psychology. American Psychologist, 54(7),
504–515. doi:037/0003-066X.54.7.504
Klinger, B. I. (1970). Effect of peer model reponsiveness and length of induction procedure on hypnotic
responsiveness. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 75, 15–18. doi:10.1037/h0028683
Lynn, S. J. (2004). Enhancing hypnotizability: The effects of compliance versus imagery. American Journal
of Clinical Hypnosis, 47, 117–128. doi:10.1080/00029157.2004.10403630
Lynn, S. J., & Green, J. P. (2011). The sociocognitive and dissociation theories of hypnosis: Toward a
rapprochement. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 59, 277–293.
doi:10.1080/00207144.2011.570652
Lynn, S. J., Green, J. P., Jaquith, L., & Gasior, D. (2003). Hypnosis and performance standards.
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 51(1), 51–65. doi:10.1076/
iceh.51.1.51.14062
Lynn, S. J., Green, J. P., Kirsch, I., Capafons, A., Lilienfeld, S. O., Laurence, J.-R., & Montgomery, G. H.
(2015). Grounding hypnosis in science: The “new” APA Division 30 definition of hypnosis as a step
backwards. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 57(4), 390–401. doi:10.1080/00029157.2015.1011472
Lynn, S. J., Kirsch, I., Knox, J., & Lilienfeld, S. (2006). Hypnosis and neuroscience: Implications for the
altered state debate. In G. Jamieson (Ed.), Hypnosis and conscious states: The cognitive-neuroscience
perspective. New York/UK: Oxford University Press.
Lynn, S. J., Laurence, J.-R., & Kirsch, I. (2015). Hypnosis, suggestion, and suggestibility: An integrative
model. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 57(3), 314–329. doi:10.1080/00029157.2014.976783
Lynn, S. J., Nash, M. R., Rhue, J., Frauman, D., & Sweeney, C. (1984). Nonvolition, expectancies, and
hypnotic rapport. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 295–303. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.93.3.295
Lynn, S. J., Neufeld, V. R., & Mare, C. (1993). Direct versus indirect suggestions: A conceptual and
methodological review. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 41, 124–152.
doi:10.1080/00207149308414543
Lynn, S. J., Neufeld, V. R., & Matyi, C. L. (1987). Inductions versus suggestions: Effects of direct and
indirect wording on hypnotic responding and experience. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 76–79.
doi:10.1037/0021-843X.96.1.76
Lynn, S. J., Rhue, J. W., & Kirsch, I. (Eds.). (2010). Handbook of clinical hypnosis (2nd ed.). Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association.
INDUCTION IN THE BROADER SCHEME OF HYPNOSIS 383
Lynn, S. J., & Sherman, S. J. (2000). The clinical importance of sociocognitive models of hypnosis:
Response set theory and Erickson’s strategic interventions. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 42,
294–315. doi:10.1080/00029157.2000.10734363
Lynn, S. J., Vanderhoff, H., Shindler, K., & Stafford, J. (2002). Defining hypnosis as a trance vs.
cooperation: Hypnotic inductions, suggestibility, and performance standards. American Journal of
Clinical Hypnosis, 44, 231–240. doi:10.1080/00029157.2002.10403483
Lynn, S. J., Weekes, J., Brentar, J., Neufeld, V., Zivney, O., & Weiss, F. (1991). Interpersonal climate and
hypnotizability level: Effects on hypnotic performance, rapport, and archaic involvement. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 739–743. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.5.739
Lynn, S. J., Weekes, J. R., Matyi, C. L., & Neufeld, V. (1988). Direct versus indirect suggestions, archaic
involvement, and hypnotic experience. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 296–301. doi:10.1037/
0021-843X.97.3.296
Malott, J. M. (1984). Active-alert hypnosis: Replication and extension of previous research. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 93, 246–249. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.93.2.246
Martin, D., & Lynn, S. J. (1996). The successful discrimination of hypnotizable and simulating participants:
The hypnotic simulation index. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 44,
338–353. doi:10.1080/00207149608416097
Matheson, G., & Shue, K. L. (1987, March). “Hypnotic language”: A study of the experience of direct vs.
indirect suggestions. Paper presented to the Fourth European Congress of Hypnosis in Psychotherapy
and Psychosomatic Medicine, Oxford, UK.
Matthews, W. J., Bennett, H., Gallagher, M., & Bean, W. (1985). Indirect versus direct hypnotic sugges-
tions—An initial investigation: A brief communication. International Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Hypnosis, 33, 219–223. doi:10.1080/00207148508406650
Matthews, W. J., Kirsch, I., & Mosher, D. (1985). Double hypnotic induction: An initial empirical test.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94, 92–95. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.94.1.92
Matthews, W. J., Lankton, S., & Lankton, C. (1993). An Ericksonian model of hypnotherapy. In J. W.
Rhue, S. J. Lynn, & I. Kirsch (Eds.), Handbook of clinical hypnosis (pp. 187–214). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Matthews, W. J., & Mosher, D. L. (1988). Direct and indirect hypnotic suggestion in a laboratory setting.
British Journal of Experimental & Clinical Hypnosis, 5(2), 63–71.
Maurer, C., Santangelo, M., & Claiborn, C. D. (1993). The effects of direct versus indirect hypnotic
suggestion on pain in a cold pressor task. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis,
41, 305–316. doi:10.1080/00207149308414560
Meyer, E., & Lynn, S. J. (2011). Responding to hypnotic and nonhypnotic suggestions: Performance
standards, imaginative suggestibility, and response expectancies. International Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Hypnosis, 59(3), 327–349. doi:10.1080/00207144.2011.570660
Miller, M. F., Barabasz, A. F., & Barabasz, M. (1991). Effects of active alert and relaxation hypnotic inductions
on cold pressor pain. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 223–226. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.100.2.223
Morgan, A. H., & Hilgard, J. R. (1978). The Stanford hypnotic clinical scale for adults. American Journal
of Clinical Hypnosis, 21(2–3), 134–147. doi:10.1080/00029157.1978.10403968
Niedzwienska, A. (2000). Goal-directed fantasy does not explain the training effect of the Carleton skills
training package. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 48, 404–417.
O’Hanlon, W. H. (1987). Taproots: Underlying principles of Milton Erickson’s therapy and hypnosis.
New York, NY: WW Norton & Co.
Page, R. A., & Handley, G. W. (1991). A comparison of the effects of standardized Chiasson and
eye-closure inductions on susceptibility scores. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 34, 46–50.
doi:10.1080/00029157.1991.10402959
384 LYNN ET AL.
Page, R. A., & Handley, G. W. (1996). In search of predictors of hypnotic sequelae. American Journal of
Clinical Hypnosis, 39(2), 93–96. doi:10.1080/00029157.1996.10403371
Pintar, J., & Lynn, S. J. (2008). A brief history of hypnosis. New York, NY: Wiley-Blackwell.
Pitsch, E., Sapp, M., & McNeely, R. L. (2001). Effects of locus of control, hypnotizability, transcript type,
and gender on vividness of imagination, hypnotic depth, and inner subjective experiences of
automaticity. Australian Journal of Clinical Hypnotherapy and Hypnosis, 22(1), 1–10.
Sheehan, P. W., & Perry, C. W. (1976). Methodologies of hypnosis (psychology revivals): A critical
appraisal of contemporary paradigms of hypnosis. London, England: Routledge.
Shor, R. E., & Orne, E. C. (1962). Harvard group scale of hypnotic susceptibility. Palo Alto, CA:
Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc.
Spanos, N. P. (1986). Hypnotic behavior: A social-psychological interpretation of amnesia, analgesia, and
“trance logic.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9(03), 449–467. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00046537
Spanos, N. P. (1991). A sociocognitive approach to hypnosis. In S. J. Lynn & J. W. Rhue (Eds.), Theories
of hypnosis (pp. 324–361). New York, NY: Guilford.
Spanos, N. P., Cross, W. F., Menary, E., & Smith, J. (1988). Long term effects of cognitive-skill training for
the enhancement of hypnotic susceptibility. British Journal of Experimental & Clinical Hypnosis, 5(2),
73–78.
Spanos, N. P., Radtke, H. L., Hodgins, D. C., Stam, H. J., & Bertrand, L. D. (1983). The Carleton
University responsiveness to suggestion scale: Normative data and psychometric properties.
Psychological Reports, 53(2), 523–535. doi:10.2466/pr0.1983.53.2.523
Spanos, N. P., Rivers, S. M., & Ross, S. (1977). Experienced involuntariness and response to hypnotic
suggestions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 296(1), 208–221.
Spinhoven, D., Baak, D., Van Dyck, R., & Vermeulen, P. (1988). The effectiveness of an authoritative
versus permissive style of hypnotic communication. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Hypnosis, 36, 182–191. doi:10.1080/00207148808410507
Stone, J. A., & Lundy, R. M. (1985). Behavioral compliance with direct and indirect body movement
suggestions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94(3), 256–263. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.94.3.256
Szabó, C. (1993). The phenomenology of the experiences and the depth of hypnosis: Comparison of direct
and indirect induction techniques. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 41(3),
225–233. doi:10.1080/00207149308414552
Van Der Does, A. J. W., Van Dyck, R., Spinhoven, P., & Kloosman, A. (1989). The effectiveness of
standardized versus individualized hypnotic suggestions: A brief communication. International Journal
of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 37, 1–5. doi:10.1080/00207148908410528
Vickery, A. R., Kirsch, I., Council, J. R., & Sirkin, M. I. (1985). Cognitive skill and traditional
trance hypnotic inductions: A within-subjects comparison. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 53(1), 131–133. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.53.1.131
Wark, D. M. (2006). Alert hypnosis: A review and case report. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis,
48(4), 291–300. doi:10.1080/00029157.2006.10401536
Weekes, J. R., & Lynn, S. J. (1990). Hypnosis suggestion type, and subjective experience—The order-
effects hypothesis revisited: A brief communication. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Hypnosis, 38, 95–100. doi:10.1080/00207149008414504
Weitzenhoffer, A. M., & Hilgard, E. R. (1959). Stanford hypnotic susceptibility scale, forms A and B. Palo
Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Weitzenhoffer, A. M., & Hilgard, E. R. (1962). Stanford hypnotic susceptibility scale, form C (Vol. 27).
Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Yapko, M. D. (2012). Trancework: An introduction to the practice of clinical hypnosis (4th ed.). London,
England: Routledge.