OGDEN. An Introduction To English Phonetics. 2009
OGDEN. An Introduction To English Phonetics. 2009
OGDEN. An Introduction To English Phonetics. 2009
Key Features
Richard Ogden is a senior lecturer at the University of York, where he has taught phonetics since
1995.
An Introduction to
English Phonetics
Cover design & illustration: River Design, Edinburgh
Richard Ogden
Edinburgh University Press
Edinburgh
General Editor
Heinz Giegerich, Professor of English Linguistics (University of Edinburgh)
Editorial Board
Laurie Bauer (University of Wellington)
Derek Britton (University of Edinburgh)
Olga Fischer (University of Amsterdam)
Rochelle Lieber (University of New Hampshire)
Norman Macleod (University of Edinburgh)
Donka Minkova (UCLA)
Edward W. Schneider (University of Regensburg)
Katie Wales (University of Leeds)
Anthony Warner (University of York)
An Introduction to
English Phonetics
Richard Ogden
www.euppublishing.com
Typeset in Janson
by Norman Tilley Graphics Ltd, and
printed and bound in Great Britain
by CPI Antony Rowe, Chippenham and Eastbourne
Contents
1 Introduction to phonetics 1
1.1 What is phonetics? 1
1.2 What this book covers 3
1.3 Ways to talk about sounds 3
1.4 An overview of the book 5
Further reading 6
5 Vowels 56
5.1 Introduction 56
5.2 Reference points for vowels: cardinal vowels 56
5.3 The acoustics of vowels 62
5.4 Other vocalic features 63
5.5 Vowels in English ‘keywords’ 64
5.6 Reduced vowels 74
5.7 Voiceless vowels 75
Summary 75
Exercises 76
Further reading 76
6 Approximants 78
6.1 Introduction 78
6.2 The palatal approximant [j] 79
6.3 A doubly articulated sound: the labiovelar
approximant [w] 81
6.4 Laterals 83
6.5 ‘Rhotics’ 89
Summary 94
Exercises 94
Further reading 94
7 Plosives 96
7.1 Introduction 96
7.2 Overview of the production of plosives 96
7.3 Voicing and plosives in English 99
7.4 Glottalisation 104
7.5 Long closure 106
7.6 Place of articulation 106
7.7 Release features of plosives 109
7.8 Taps 114
01 pages i-xiv prelims:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:52 Page vii
CONTENTS vii
Summary 116
Exercises 116
Further reading 117
8 Fricatives 118
8.1 Introduction to fricatives 118
8.2 The production of fricatives 118
8.3 Details of English fricatives 120
8.4 Non-lexical fricatives 131
Summary 136
Exercises 136
Further reading 136
9 Nasals 138
9.1 The production of nasals 138
9.2 Details of English nasals 140
9.3 Nasalised vowels 146
9.4 Syllabic nasals 148
Summary 152
Exercises 152
Further reading 153
11 Conclusion 170
Glossary 173
Discussion of the exercises 181
Further reading 187
Index 191
01 pages i-xiv prelims:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:52 Page viii
Figures
The International Phonetic Alphabet (revised to 2005) xiv
2.1 Cross-section of the vocal tract 10
3.1 Waveform of a vowel 30
3.2 Three types of sound 31
3.3 Spectrogram of the word ‘spend’, with periodic, aperiodic
and transient sounds marked 32
3.4 Expanded version of part of Figure 3.3 32
3.5 Waveform of part of a voiceless fricative 34
3.6 Transient portion (T) for the initial plosive of ‘spend’ 35
3.7 Spectrogram of a production of ‘took off his cloak’ (RP) (IPA) 38
4.1 The larynx (from Catford 1977: 49) 41
4.2 f0 on a linear scale 45
4.3 f0 on a logarithmic scale 45
4.4 1. ‘hello’ [hε\ləυ], 2. ‘hello’ [hε/ləυ], 3. ‘hello there’
[hε/ləυ ðε] 47
4.5 Creaky voice 51
5.1 The vowel quadrilateral 59
5.2 Spectrogram of cardinal vowels 1–8 63
5.3 RP monophthongs 69
5.4 Australian monophthongs 70
5.5 American English monophthongs 70
5.6 RP closing diphthongs 70
5.7 RP centring diphthongs 71
5.8 Australian diphthongs 71
5.9 American English diphthongs 71
5.10 trap vowels 72
5.11 strut vowels 73
5.12 face vowels 73
5.13 goose vowels 74
6.1 ‘A yacht’ 80
viii
01 pages i-xiv prelims:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:52 Page ix
6.2 ‘A win’ 82
6.3 An alveolar lateral with varying secondary articulation,
from palatalised to velarised 87
6.4 ‘Leaf ’ 88
6.5 ‘Feel’ 88
6.6 ‘To lead’ and ‘to read’ 93
7.1 The phases of a plosive 97
7.2 Waveform and spectrogram of the underlined portion
of ‘a good (hobby)’ [ə υd hɒbi] 99
7.3 Voicing for plosives 100
7.4 Fully voiced [], in ‘gig’, [i] 101
7.5 Vocalic portion, closure, plosive release, vocalic portion
from ‘a bit’, [ə bit] 101
7.6 Vocalic portion, closure, plosive release, aspiration, vocalic
portion from ‘a pit’, [ə phit] 102
7.7 Friction, closure, release and vocalic portion from ‘a spit’,
[ə spit] 103
7.8 Preaspiration 105
7.9 Glottalisation in ‘kit’, [kh ʔt h], as spoken by a New Zealand
speaker (IPA) 105
7.10 A sequence of [kt], with two audible releases 113
7.11 A sequence t], with [k] release inaudible.
siɾi],ofas[kproduced 113
7.12 ‘City’, [ by a speaker from southern
Michigan (IPA) 115
7.13 Material for exercise 2 117
8.1 Annotated waveforms for the first 300 ms of ‘sip’ as
produced by an RP speaker (IPA) 121
8.2 Annotated waveforms for the first 300 ms of ‘zip’ as
produced by an RP speaker (IPA) 121
8.3 Spectrograms of ‘sip’ (left) and ‘zip’ (right) (RP) (IPA) 122
8.4 ‘Fie’ (New Zealand) (IPA) 123
8.5 ‘Vie’ (New Zealand) (IPA) 123
8.6 ‘Fie’ (left) and ‘vie’ (right) as spoken by a New Zealander
(IPA) 124
8.7 Spectrogram of ‘looser’, with friction (FRIC) and the offset
and onset of voicing (V off, V on) marked 126
8.8 Spectrogram of ‘loser’, with friction (FRIC) and the offset
and onset of voicing (V off, V on) marked 126
8.9 ‘Sigh’ and ‘shy’ as spoken by a male Australian speaker.
Note the lower frequency energy for [ʃ] than for [s] (IPA) 129
8.10 ‘Kids do i[θ]’. Speaker: 18-year-old male, Dublin (IViE file
f1mdo) 133
01 pages i-xiv prelims:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:52 Page x
Tables
3.1 Systematic transcription of English consonants 26
4.1 Average f0 values (Baken and Orlikoff 2000) 46
5.1 Anglo-English vs. American homophones 66
5.2 Vowels in English keywords 67
6.1 Approximants in English at the systematic level 78
7.1 Plosives in English 96
7.2 Differences between [t + r] and [tɹ ] 111
7.3 Phonetic characteristics of voicing with English plosives 116
8.1 Fricatives in English 118
8.2 Voiced and voiceless fricatives 125
8.3 Fricatives from undershoot 135
9.1 English nasals 138
01 pages i-xiv prelims:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:52 Page xi
To readers
enon, recent work shows that it’s much more complex than traditional
descriptions imply. The chapters here, I hope, will give students enough
grounding in observing and understanding the phonetic organisation of
talk so that understanding phenomena such as assimilation will be easier.
01 pages i-xiv prelims:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:52 Page xiii
Acknowledgements
I owe a great debt of thanks to many people who have helped me with
data for this book. These include the secretary of the IPA, Katerina
Nicolaidis; Dom Watt; Esther Grabe; and many of my own students, who
over the years have collected a lot of material full of wonderful detail.
Thanks also to Alex, Hazel, Jennifer, Julianne, Lis, Malcolm, Nan and
Roger, my panel of non-phonetician readers who took the time to read
parts of this and helped to make it understandable; to my colleagues
who let me have the time to bring this to completion; and to fellow
phoneticians who have kept me enthused about working with speech.
The acoustic representations in the book were made using PRAAT
(www.praat.org), developed by Paul Boersma and David Weenink. Ester
Grabe kindly gave permission to use files from the IViE Project (www.
phon.ox.ac.uk). Where recordings from this have been used, they are
referred to with the preface IViE, followed by the identifier.
The IPA chart is reprinted with permission of the International
Phonetic Association. Copyright 2005 International Phonetic Asso-
ciation. I am grateful to the IPA for permission to use material from the
Journal of the IPA, the Handbook of the IPA and the accompanying record-
ings, which are available to members via the IPA website. Where images
are based on IPA recordings from the website above, they are marked
(IPA) in the accompanying captions. Information about IPA membership
can be obtained from the IPA website: http://www.langsci.ucl.ac.uk/
ipa/index.html.
xiii
01 pages i-xiv prelims:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:52 Page xiv
Bilabial Labiodental Dental Alveolar Post alveolar Retroflex Palatal Velar Uvular Pharyngeal Glottal
Plosive p b t d Ê c Ô k g q G /
Nasal m μ n = N –
Trill ı r R
Tap or Flap v | «
Fricative F B f v T D s ¬z S Z ß ç J x V X Â © ? h H
Lateral
fricative Ò L
Approximant ¥ ® ’ j ˜
Lateral
approximant l Ò ¥ K
Where symbols appear in pairs, the one to the right represents a voiced consonant. Shaded areas denote articulations judged impossible.
ò
Alveolar lateral Uvular Alveolar fricative Open-mid
OTHER SYMBOLS
œ å
Voiceless labial-velar fricative Ç Û Alveolo-palatal fricatives
Open a ” A Å
Where symbols appear in pairs, the one
to the right represents a rounded vowel.
w ¬ Voiced labial-velar approximant » Voiced alveolar lateral flap
Á Voiced labial-palatal approximant Í Simultaneous S and x SUPRASEGMENTALS
Secondary stress
can be represented by two symbols kp ts ÆfoUn´"tIS´n
(
8 5
low falling
Non-syllabic e8 Advanced Tongue Root e5 Õ Downstep ã Global rise
1 Introduction to phonetics
INTRODUCTION TO PHONETICS 3
INTRODUCTION TO PHONETICS 5
context they are in. The differences are not seen as linguistically im-
portant, because they are predictable.
Another way to look at this is to think of the consonant as telling us
something about the vowel that is coming: if you hear the kind of [k]
which goes in the word ‘kit’, then before you even hear the vowel sound
for real, you can tell what kind of vowel sound is coming. So in a way, the
consonant and the vowel are being produced at the same time.
The question for us as phoneticians is what we make of this, and how
we explain it. In this book, we will use the word ‘sound’ as an essentially
neutral word which does not take one stance or another towards what we
hear. It is a term chosen so as to allow us to be as descriptively rich as we
would like, without committing us one way or another to whether the
best account is a phonemic one or something else.
Sounds will be written enclosed in square brackets, such as [k], [a], [t]
or [kat]. Phonemes, where we refer to them, will be enclosed in slash
brackets such as /k/, /a/, /t/. And letters will from now on be enclosed
between angled brackets like this: <c> <a> <t>; but when referring to
words, the convention will be: ‘cat’. We will use English spelling quite a
lot, and this might seem counterintuitive in a book on English phonetics.
But remember that speakers of English do not all pronounce the same
words with the same phonemes, let alone the same sounds; and the only
neutral way to write English is in fact its orthography: this is one reason
why English spelling has been so resistant to change over the years.
the air is moved into or out of the vocal tract by some other mechanism
than the lungs.
Each chapter ends with some exercises and suggestions for further
reading. Discussion of the exercises can be found at the end of the book,
though for many of the exercises there are no clear-cut answers. When
terms appear in bold, this is a first mention, and a glossary containing
these terms is provided at the end of the book.
By the end of this book, you should have some understanding of ways
to represent spoken English. You should understand something about
the way sounds are made in the vocal tract, and something of the
complexity and detail of spoken English. Most importantly of all, by the
end of the book you should have some skills for making some phonetic
observations of your own.
Further reading
Many books on linguistics provide an overview of the place of phonetics
and phonology within linguistics, and the relations between them, e.g.
Fromkin et al. (2007), which also discusses the phoneme.
English phonetics is discussed from a phonemic point of view in e.g.
Jones (1975) and Cruttenden (2001).
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:52 Page 7
2.2 Breathing
Speech sounds are made by manipulating the way air moves out of (or
sometimes into) the vocal tract. There are a number of ways of doing
this, as we will see in Chapter 10, but universally across languages sounds
7
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:52 Page 8
3 M → [h↓
4 P [well is that right]
5 M [you wouldn’t] with redcurrants
P and M are talking about making jam. In line 1, P asks M whether some
acid is needed to make it set. In line 3, M marks that she is about to speak,
by producing an audible in-breath (transcribed [h] with [↓] to indicate
that the air is coming into the body, not out) and then gives her answer
while P1 in line 2 produces the end of his question. Audible in-breaths
like this are one way for a speaker to display “I have something (more) to
say.” Here, the “something to say” is an answer, and M produces her
in-breath at a point relative to P’s talk where it is clear what kind of
answer is relevant in the context. Producing audible in-breaths is a
common device that allows speakers to co-ordinate turn-taking in
conversation.
nas
nasal
sal cavities
alveolar
ridge
velum
(soft palate)
bla
ade
blade front
teeth tip
uvula
backk
tongue
body
lower pharynx
lip tonguue
tongue
roo
roott
epiglottis
hyoid bone
thyroid cartilage
larynx
laryn
nx
cricoid cartilage
2.4 Airflow
Air passes out of the vocal tract through the mouth or the nose. The way
that it comes out affects the sound generated, so we need a framework to
describe this aspect of speech.
the middle of the roof of your mouth. The cold and dry patches will be
more or less symmetrical on each side of your mouth. All languages have
sounds with central airflow.
Lateral airflow is when the air flows down one or both sides of the
vocal tract. If you say the sound [l], hold the articulation and then suck
air in, you should feel this time that it goes cold and dry down one or
both sides of the mouth, but not down the middle. The sides of the
tongue are lowered, and the air passes out between the back teeth.
In theory, lateral airflow can be produced at the lips too: to do this,
keep the sides of the lips together and try saying something like ‘Pepé
bought a pencil’. It will both sound and look strange. It is probably not a
surprise that no language has lateral airflow caused by constricting the
lips at one side, and this combination is blocked out in the chart of the
International Phonetic Association.
2.5.1 Bilabial
Bilabial sounds are sounds made at the lips. ‘Bi-’ means ‘two’, and ‘labial’
is an adjective based on the Latin word for ‘lips’. In English, the sounds
[p b m] are bilabial. If you say [apa aba ama] and look in the mirror, you
will see that they look identical. If you say the sounds silently to your-
self and concentrate on your lips, you will feel that the two lips touch one
another for a short period, and the action is basically the same for all
three sounds.
2.5.2 Labiodental
Labiodental sounds are made with the upper teeth (‘dental’) against the
lower lip (‘labio’). In English the labiodental sounds [f v] occur. Logically
speaking, labiodental sounds could involve the lower teeth and the upper
lip, but this is difficult for most people to do: it involves protruding the
jaw, and most people have upper teeth that sit in front of the lower teeth.
Labiodental sounds can be made with the teeth against either the
inside surface of the lip (endolabial) or the outside edge of the lip
(exolabial).
2.5.3 Dental
Dental sounds involve an articulation made against the back of the upper
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 13
teeth. [θ ð] in English (as in the initial sounds of ‘think’ and ‘then’) are
often dental; they can also be interdental, that is, produced with the
tongue between (‘inter’ in Latin) the teeth, especially in North America.
Dental forms of [l] and [n] are used in words like ‘health’ and ‘tenth’,
where they are followed by a dental; and dental forms of [t] and [d] are
regularly used in many varieties of English (e.g. some forms of Irish or
New York English, and in Nigeria) as forms of [θ ð].
2.5.4 Alveolar
Alveolar sounds are made at the alveolar ridge. This is a bony ridge
behind the upper teeth. If you rest your tongue on the upper teeth then
gradually move it backwards, you will feel a change in texture from the
smooth enamel to the bumpier gum. Just behind the teeth you should be
able to feel the alveolar ridge. This sticks out a bit just behind the teeth.
People’s alveolar ridges are very variable: some are very prominent,
others hardly noticeable. Alternatively, try isolating the consonant
sounds in the word ‘dent’, and you should feel that the tongue tip is
making contact with the alveolar ridge. Sounds with an alveolar place of
articulation in most varieties of English are [t d n l r s z].
2.5.5 Postalveolar
Postalveolar sounds are made just behind (‘post’) the alveolar ridge.
There are four of these in English, [ʃ] and [], the sounds spelt <sh> in
‘ship’, [ʃip], and <si> in ‘invasion’, [iveiən], and the sounds [tʃ d] as
in ‘church’ and ‘judge’. It can be hard to feel the difference in place
of articulation between alveolar and postalveolar sounds, but if you
produce a [s] sound, then a [ʃ] sound, and suck air in immediately after
each sound, you should feel that part of the roof of the mouth which goes
cold and dry is further back for [ʃ] than for [s].
Special symbols for dentals and postalveolars only exist for the frica-
tives. If dental or postalveolar articulations need to be distinguished, this
can be done using diacritics – characters which modify the basic value
of letters, and are placed over or under simple letters. For [t], we use
[t
t t] respectively, where [
] marks ‘dental’, and [] marks ‘retracted’ (i.e.
further back), i.e. postalveolar.
Postalveolars are reported occasionally in dialects which are on their
way to losing distinct [r] sounds. Hedevind (1967) reports a contrast
between dentals/alveolars and postalveolars (transcribed [n , z , t] in
pairs such as those below in a dialect from Dent (Cumbria, Northern
England).
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 14
2.5.6 Retroflex
Retroflex sounds are made with the tongue curled (‘flex’) back (‘retro’)
to the hard palate. (This is one case where the ‘place of articulation’
refers to the active articulator.) The symbols for retroflex sounds are
easy to remember: they all have a rightward-facing hook on the bottom:
[
].
Retroflex [
] are frequently used in Indian varieties of English
instead of alveolars for the sounds [t d n]. (Many Indian languages have
dental and retroflex or postalveolar sounds, but not alveolar.) The
retroflex fricative sound [] also occurs in some varieties of English,
notably some Scottish and North American varieties, as a combination
of [r] + [s], as in ‘of course’, [əv kɔ]. And many varieties of American
English use [] for the r-sound; this is also known as ‘curled-r’.
2.5.7 ‘Coronal’
On the IPA chart, sounds are described according to where in the mouth
they are made; but it is equally important to think about which part
of the tongue is used to make them. Dental, alveolar, postalveolar and
retroflex sounds are all made with the front part of the tongue, the tip
(the very frontmost part of the tongue) or the blade (the part just behind
the tip). There is a lot of variability among English speakers as to which
part of the tongue they use to articulate dental, alveolar and postalveo-
lar sounds, so usually this factor is ignored, since it seems to play no
linguistic role for English. In the phonology literature, sounds made with
the front part of the tongue are often called coronal, a term which does
not appear on the IPA chart. (The Latin word ‘corona’ means ‘crown’;
this is the term used to refer to the front part of the tongue.)
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 15
2.5.8 Palatal
Palatal sounds are made with the tongue body, the massive part of the
middle of the tongue, raised up to the hard palate, or the roof of the
mouth. Palatal sounds aren’t common in English, except for the sound
[j], which is usually spelt <y>, as in ‘yes’, ‘yacht’, ‘yawn’; or as part of the
sequence [ju] represented by the letter <u> in words like ‘usual’,
‘computer’.
2.5.9 Velar
Velar sounds are made with the tongue back (or dorsum) raised towards
the soft palate. The soft palate is at the back of the roof of the mouth, and
is also known as the velum. The sounds [k ] are velars, as is the sound
[ŋ], represented by <ng> in words like ‘king’, ‘wrong’, ‘hang’; but as we
will see in Chapter 7, there are in fact many variations in the precise
place of articulation in English.
The velum also acts as a kind of valve, because it can be raised and
lowered. When it is lowered, air can pass into the nasal cavities and
escape through the nose. When it is raised, the nasal cavities are sealed
off, and air can only escape through the mouth.
2.5.10 Uvular
Uvular sounds are made with the uvula (which is Latin for ‘little egg’, the
shape of the uvula). The uvula is the little fleshy appendage that hangs
down in the middle of your mouth at the back. If you gargle, the uvula
vibrates. French, German, Dutch and Danish all use uvular articulations
for orthographic <r>; and in fact, one variety of English (around the
north east of England) has, in its more archaic forms, a uvular sound too
in this position.
2.5.11 Pharyngeal
The pharynx is the cavity behind the tongue root and just above the
larynx. Pharyngeal sounds are made by constricting the muscles of the
neck and contracting the pharynx; this kind of articulation occurs rarely
in English.
2.5.12 Glottal
Glottal sounds are made at the glottis, the space between the vocal folds,
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 16
which are located at the larynx. English uses a number of such sounds:
[h] as in ‘head’ and its voiced equivalent between two vowels, [], as in
‘ahead’; and the glottal stop [ʔ], which is often used alongside or in place
of [t] (as in many Anglo-English – that is, the English of England –
pronunciations of words like ‘water’, [wɔtə, wɔʔə]), and in words that
begin with vowels (as in many American and Australian pronunciations
of phrases like ‘the [ʔ]apple’).
Summary
There are three main aspects of the production of speech sounds in
English: voicing, place of articulation and manner of articulation. We
have introduced much terminology for describing speech sounds. In
later chapters, we will look at place, manner and voicing in much more
detail. We will focus on those aspects of the sound of English which
relate to meaning in its broadest sense: word meaning, utterance mean-
ing and social meaning. To do this, we will make extensive use of the
categories of the International Phonetic Alphabet.
Exercises
1. What is the place and manner of articulation of the consonants in the
following words? Remember to refer to the sounds you make in pronun-
ciation, which do not always straightforwardly correspond with the
letters in the spelling!
a. club f. Dutch k. psychology
b. heavy g. contact l. hearing
c. deaf h. community m. perform
d. kiss i. industry n. translate
e. raised j. night
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 19
2. Divide each of the following groups of symbols into two sets of three,
each of which has something in common phonetically. The first one is
done for you.
Symbols Set 1 Set 2
a. pmtnkŋ p t k (oral plosives) m n ŋ (nasals)
b. slpmvʃ
c. fjwlzθ
d. svhðθ
e. rknlw
f. tmbs
g. ʃ t
θ ð t
h. hzlʔs
i. napkjw
j. jwbdr
Further reading
Overviews of the production of speech and discussion on the classifi-
cation of speech sounds can be found in Abercrombie (1967), Catford
(2001) and Ladefoged (2005, 2006). Ball (1993) is aimed at clinicians, but
is very approachable. More advanced readings include Laver (1994) and
Pike (1943). For discussion relating to English more specifically, Jones
(1975) and Gimson’s work (Cruttenden 2001) are classics.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 20
3.1 Introduction
One of the problems that phonetics needs to solve is how to represent
speech, an ephemeral and time-bound phenomenon, so that it is avail-
able in a more permanent form.
In this book we will look at two ways to represent speech. The first is
phonetic transcription: the use of alphabetic symbols to represent the
sounds of speech. This is the kind of representation found in dictionary
entries, for instance, to represent the pronunciation of words with incon-
sistent spellings, like ‘plough’, ‘tough’, ‘trough’, ‘cough’ and ‘although’.
English, like all languages, has a set of conventions to relate letters to
sounds; but it has fewer one-to-one mappings between letter and sound
than many other languages that use the Roman alphabet. Phonetic tran-
scriptions are built on the apparently simple alphabetic principle of
one symbol for each sound.
The second kind of representation we will look at gives us quite
different information. These are representations that have a basis in
acoustic analysis, such as waveforms and spectrograms. They provide a
different perspective on the organisation of speech. Acoustic represen-
tations help us to see that despite our impressions, reinforced by an
alphabetic writing system, the sounds of speech are constantly changing,
are interwoven with one another, and are not discrete in the way that
letters are. Acoustic representations are commonly used in phonetics,
and they make it possible to see individual aspects of sounds separately.
ionistic and try to capture what we hear rather than make claims about
the significance of what we hear for making meaning.) It is useful for
phoneticians to write down what we can hear, and we need to do this in
a way that is systematic, easy to use, easily understood by others, and
portable – a notepad and a pencil predate modern recording equipment
by many years, and remain the cheapest tools of the phonetician’s trade.
How we transcribe is not a simple matter. Using just the letters of the
Roman alphabet is problematic for a number of reasons.
First, the phonetic values of letters are variable. For instance, the
letter <g> is regularly used in most European languages with the value
of a voiced velar plosive, []. In Dutch <g> is pronounced like the <ch>
in Scottish ‘loch’; in French and Portuguese before an <e> or <i> it has
the same value as <si> in ‘invasion’, []; in Swedish in the same context
<g> is pronounced like English <y> in ‘yes’; in English (sporadically)
and Italian (regularly) [d], as in ‘gem’.
Within English, letters can have very different values, as in <g> in
‘get’ and ‘gem’, or <a> in ‘sofa’, ‘hat’ and ‘hate’. These differences are
due to different spelling conventions being used at different times in the
history of the language, or spelling conventions reflecting the etymology
of words, and through the conservative approach to spelling reform
adopted in the English-speaking world.
Secondly, the Roman alphabet has no symbol for some sounds of
English, so that we use digraphs (combinations of two letters) like <th>
for the different sounds of ‘thick’ ([θ]) and ‘this’ ([ð]) or <sh> for the
[ʃ] sound in ‘ship’; but ‘facial’, ‘admission’, ‘station’ and ‘louche’ also
contain this sound, where it is represented differently. So the alphabetic
principle in English writing is weak.
A number of writing systems built on phonetic principles have been
invented over the centuries, but the one that is most widely used is the
alphabet of the International Phonetic Association.
‘Alphabet’ is perhaps also not the best way to refer to the IPA. The
letters of the alphabet, {A, B, C …}, occur in a random order, with
vowels scattered among consonants, and the consonants not grouped
according to any linguistic principle. The IPA, however, is a set of tables
containing symbols organised into rows and columns which are labelled
with terms that have agreed meanings.
The rows of the Consonant chart groups sounds according to manner
of articulation. The first row contains plosives: [p b t d
c k q g ʔ].
The rows below have sounds with progressively more open stricture.
The columns organise symbols by place of articulation, with the leftmost
column containing symbols that stand for bilabial sounds, and sub-
sequent columns containing symbols for sounds made progressively
further down the vocal tract, so that the rightmost column contains
symbols for glottal sounds.
The symbols of the IPA are presented in a number of tables, the main
ones being pulmonic egressive consonants and vowels. The other tables
contain non-pulmonic consonants, diacritics (small marks that combine
with letter symbols to represent sounds not on the chart, as we have
already seen) and suprasegmentals, aspects of sound which relate to
things like length, phrasing, intonation and so on. There is also a collec-
tion of ‘other symbols’, which stand for sounds that do not easily fit in the
main scheme.
lar, how are the two words joined? In the north west of England, you
might hear a [ɹ] sound (as if it were written ‘shurrup’); in many parts of
the English-speaking world, you will hear a glottal stop, [ʔ], or a tap, [ɾ]
(as in ‘shuddup’, defined in the online Urban Dictionary as ‘what Donald
Duck says to Goofy Dog’). In most places, you could hear an alveolar
plosive with a puff of air (aspiration), [th]. Most speakers will have a
choice about how to join these words, with [th] probably being the sound
that has the highest social status. These differences are certainly socio-
linguistically meaningful, and for that reason, phoneticians want to be
able to represent them.
Secondly: ‘Speech can be represented partly as a sequence of discrete
sounds or segments.’ ‘Segment’ means a piece of something that has
been chopped up: in the case of speech, ‘segments’ means a piece of the
speech signal, which is actually continuous. This is the principle that
makes the use of the IPA alphabetic: the claim is not that speech is
made of segments, but that we can represent it as segments. It is a useful
working assumption in many ways, and it is familiar to people who use
an alphabetic writing system.
Thirdly: the IPA establishes two major types of segment, consonant
and vowel. Consonants are those sounds which are produced with
some kind of constriction in the vocal tract. We can feel, see and hear
where these constrictions are made, and what kind of constriction they
are.
Vowels, by contrast, are produced without a constriction in the
vocal tract, and it is harder to sense how they are articulated. The IPA’s
terminological framework for describing consonants and vowels is
different.
Suprasegmentals are aspects of speech which persist over several
segments, such as duration, loudness, tempo (speed), pitch character-
istics and voice quality; they are often thought of as the ‘musical’ aspects
of speech, but may include other properties like lip-rounding. They are
called suprasegmentals because they function over (‘supra’ in Latin)
consonants and vowels.
The effect of suprasegmentals is easy to illustrate. In talking to a cat,
a dog or a baby, you may adopt a particular set of suprasegmentals.
Often, when doing this, people adopt a different voice quality, with high
pitch register, and protrude their lips and adopt a tongue posture where
the tongue body is high and front in the mouth, making the speech sound
‘softer’.
Suprasegmentals are important for marking all kinds of meanings, in
particular speakers’ attitudes or stances to what they are saying (or the
person they are saying it to), and in marking out how one utterance
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 24
of ‘shoe[z]’) to the lips and teeth (for ‘[f ]or’). These things do not happen
simultaneously (as the transcription [z f ] implies), so that first we get
[alveolarity +friction +voicing], [z], but then the voicing stops, so we
have [alveolarity +friction –voicing], [z]. Since labiodental articulations
do not involve the same articulators as alveolar ones, the two articu-
lations can overlap, so we get a short portion of [alveolarity +labioden-
tality +friction –voicing]. We can represent this as [zf ]: the symbol
[ ]means that two articulations occur simultaneously. The alveolar
constriction is then removed, leaving just labiodental friction. So in all,
the fricative portion between these two words can be narrowly tran-
scribed as [z z z f f]. This could imply four different ‘sounds’, and at some
level, there are: there are four portions that are phonetically different
from each other, but really there are only two parameters here: voicing
goes from ‘on’ to ‘off ’, and place of articulation changes from ‘alveolar’
to ‘labiodental’.
The end of this utterance is produced with creaky voice. This is where
the vocal folds vibrate slowly and randomly (Chapter 4). As well as
this, the final plosive is not in fact alveolar; like many speakers, this
one uses a glottal stop instead. So the last two syllables can be partially
transcribed as [f"əð"a"ʔ]. The dental sound in ‘that’ is produced without
friction: it is a ‘more open’ articulation (i.e. the tongue is not as close to
the teeth as it might be, and not close enough to produce friction): this is
transcribed with the diacritic [#] (‘more open’); and there is at least a
percept of nasality throughout the final syllable. This might be because
the velum is lowered (the usual cause of nasality), but sometimes glottal
constrictions produce the same percept. We can’t be sure which is the
correct account, but the percept is clear enough, and in an impression-
istic transcription, it is best not to dismiss any detail out of hand. (For all
we know, the percept of nasality might be a feature regularly used by this
speaker to mark utterance finality.)
(4) Impressionistic transcription:
[a θĩŋk a nid sə̃m nju ʃuzzzff"əð#"˜ "a˜ʔ].
This probably looks a bit frightening, but it is worth remembering
that (a) this is a transcription of one utterance on one occasion by
one speaker, and (b) the transcription is based on a set of rather simple
observations of what we can hear: it’s more important to understand that
relationship than to worry about the details of the transcription. It is
important not to fetishise transcriptions, but to see the linguistic patterns
that lie beyond them.
These impressionistic transcriptions, as can be seen, use the full range
of IPA symbols and diacritics in an attempt to capture details of pronun-
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 29
between articulation and acoustics. There are two main kinds of acoustic
representations we will use in this book: waveforms and spectrograms.
We will approach these representations as pictures which can show
us particular aspects of speech and as a useful complementary tool to
transcriptions.
3.3.1 Waveforms
Waveforms are a kind of graph. Graphs have an x-axis, which runs
horizontally, and a y-axis, which runs vertically. In waveforms of speech,
the x-axis represents time and is usually scaled in seconds or milli-
seconds, while the y-axis shows (to simplify a great deal) amplitude, a
representation of loudness.
0.3593
–0.4704
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5
Time (s)
Figure 3.1 Waveform of a vowel.
3.3.2 Spectrograms
Spectrograms are pictures of speech: in spy movies they are often called
‘voiceprints’, which although inaccurate conveys the idea that it shows a
picture of someone speaking.
Spectrograms provide more complex information than waveforms.
Time, as in waveforms, is marked on the x-axis. The y-axis shows
frequency. Amplitude is reflected in darkness: the louder a given
component in the speech signal is, the darker it appears on the spectro-
gram.
Sound
Periodic Aperiodic
Aperiiodic
Transient
Transient Continuous
Figure 3.2 Three types of sound.
Periodic noise.
nooise.
Each vertical striation correspoonds to to an opening of
corresponds
the vocal folds: note alignment
alignm
ment with peaks in the
forrmant structure.
waveform. Distinct formant
Aperiodic friction
frictio
on noise Transient
Transient burst spike.
5000
4000
3000 F3
2000
F2
1000
F1
0.2688
–-0.2919
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0
0.6 0.7 0.7633
Time
Tim
me (s)
Figure 3.3 Spectrogram of the word ‘spend’, with periodic, aperiodic and
transient sounds marked.
0.3 0.31 0.32 0.33 0.34 0.35 0.36 0.37 0.38 0.39 0.4
10.5 cycles in Figure 3.4. This reflects the number of times the vocal folds
open in the time represented. The number of complete cycles the vocal
folds make in one second is called the fundamental frequency (f0); it is
measured in Hertz (Hz). A frequency of 1 Hz means that there is one
complete cycle per second. A frequency of 100 Hz means that there are
one hundred complete cycles per second, or alternatively one complete
cycle every 0.01 s (every one hundredth of a second). In the waveform in
Figure 3.4, there are approximately 10.5 cycles in 0.1 s, which means the
fundamental frequency in this stretch of speech is about 105 Hz.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 33
note. This is because the shape of the vocal tract has been changed by
moving the tongue and opening the jaw. If you make a vowel like [u] you
will hear a lower note again.
In summary: periodic sounds in the vocal tract are caused by voicing.
Periodicity is seen in a regular waveform, striations in the spectrogram,
and visible formants in the spectrogram. Vowels and the sounds [w j l r
m n ŋ] all illustrate these properties well.
0.05 0.06 0.07 0.08 0.09 0.1 0.11 0.12 0.13 0.14 0.15
periodic T
0.55 0.56 0.57 0.58 0.59 0.6 0.61 0.62 0.63 0.64 0.65
Time (s)
Figure 3.6 Transient portion (T) for the initial plosive of ‘spend’.
For all these reasons, it is wise be wary of ascribing to any one form of
representation some kind of primacy. Made and used carefully, they are
all informative in some way.
Summary
In this chapter we have looked at two forms of representation of speech:
transcription and acoustic representations. We have seen that each has a
place, and each type of representation has both advantages and draw-
backs.
In later chapters, we will use verbal descriptions, transcriptions and
acoustic representations to try to give some impression of the way the
sounds of English are produced, and to try to show some of the details
of those sounds where using words is not straightforward. Phonetics
is special in linguistics for the way it combines the production and per-
ception of sounds, the auditory, visual and kinaesthetic aspects of the
subject: this means that learning phonetics can be a multi-sensory
experience. It is worth persisting, if frustration sets in, to try to put
the various forms of phonetic description and representation together,
because it results in a richer understanding of the embodied nature of
human speech.
Exercises
1. Consider the functions of phonetic transcriptions in the following
circumstances: a speech therapist with a client; a fieldworker working
out a writing system for an unwritten language; a dictionary aimed at
learners of English as a foreign language. What demands and needs
might each situation make?
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 0.96718
Time (s)
Figure 3.7 Spectrogram of a production of ‘took off his cloak’ (RP) (IPA).
Further reading
Bell (2004) discusses English spelling in an approachable but critical way.
The Handbook of the IPA (1999) provides a short overview of the prin-
ciples of the IPA and transcription styles. Abercrombie (1967), Kelly and
Local (1989), Laver (1994) and Jones (1975) contain more thorough
discussion of transcription styles, and Pullum and Ladusaw (1996) is a
useful guide to IPA and other phonetic symbols. For more practice at
transcription, Lecumberri and Maidment (2000) has lots of exercises and
discussion.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 39
Figure 4.1 The larynx (from Catford 1977: 49). The most important labels
for our purpose are: vf: vocal folds; hy: hyoid bone; tc: thyroid
cartilage; cc: cricoid cartilage; ac: arytenoid cartilages.
forms the bottom part of the ‘box’. It has two spurs at the back, one on
each side, which reach up to behind the bottom part of the thyroid. The
two artytenoid cartilages sit on top of the back of the cricoid cartilage.
They can move together and apart, rock backwards and forwards as well
as rotate.
The vocal folds are two ligaments (fibrous tissues) which are covered
in mucous membrane. They are attached to the arytenoids at the back
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 42
and the thyroid at the front. At the side, they are attached to muscle in
the larynx. In the middle they are free, so that there is a gap or a space
between them, known as the glottis. The arytenoids can move, but the
thyroid is static; by manipulating the arytenoids, the tension across the
vocal folds can be changed, as can their thickness and the way they
vibrate.
glottis and below the glottis equalises. Now the tension across the vocal
folds forces them back together again, making a closure again. The
process now repeats itself: the folds are closed, air cannot escape through
the glottis, so the pressure builds up, the folds are forced open, the
pressure equalises, the folds close again.
This cycle of opening and closing is an aerodynamic effect called the
Bernoulli effect.
When the vocal folds vibrate making complete closure along their full
length (that is, with no gaps in contact between the vocal folds), with
regular vibration, and with no particular tension in the folds to make
them especially thick (and short) or thin (and long), this is called modal
voicing. Few speakers really achieve modal voicing, but since most
people have a ‘normal’ setting (that is, one that has no particular dis-
tinguishing features for them), we often speak of modal voicing to mean
a person’s default voice quality.
will be low. Speech produced like this is more likely to sound ‘tired’
because it requires less energy to produce. But this can also be used as a
more linguistic device: when coming to the end of a topic, one iconic
device we can use to mark this in our speech is to talk quietly and with a
low pitch.
100 80
585
500
400
300
200 220
100
80
600
300
200
100
80
hello (1) hello (2) hello (3) there
0 1.3424
Time (s)
Figure 4.4 1. ‘hello’ [hε\ləυ], 2. ‘hello’ [hε/ləυ], 3. ‘hello there’ [hε/ləυ ðε].
agrees with this, but her agreement comes late (almost half a second
later). Agreeing late weakens the sense of agreement; notice that Helen
comes in and explains her assessment of the new system as ‘better’, and
the next thing Elizabeth does is to find a reason why the new system
is not very good (lines 7–8). So the ‘yes’ here is not whole-hearted,
especially in comparison with the ‘yes’ of the first example. Perhaps
unsurprisingly, the strength of agreement is audible too: the ‘yes’ in
line 3 of the ‘washing machine’ extract is quiet, low pitched, and slow –
a direct contrast with the ‘yes’ in line 5 of the ‘nice feet’ extract.
Here, then, we have two instances where ‘yes’ is produced, but the
intonation, along with other things, affects the ‘meaning’ of the ‘yes’,
making it stronger and more affirmative, or weaker and prefacing a
disagreement.
Phrasing and intonation give speakers clues about the syntax that
organises words into structures. This is one of the main interfaces
between phonetics and syntax and semantics. Talk is chunked up into
phrases, whose boundaries reflect major syntactic boundaries. The
symbols [| ||] mark minor and major intonational phrase boundaries.
Phrases have some or all of the following characteristics (roughly):
• at the start: speeding up, re-setting of pitch
• at the end: slowing down, quieter, near the bottom or top of the
speaker’s pitch range
• a pause before or after
• congruence with syntactic or pragmatic boundaries
Take the following sentence:
(5) We didn’t go to the museum because it was raining.
Did we go or not? If the sentence is spoken on a monotone, it is not
possible to tell. But if an intonation contour is placed on the sentence,
then two different meanings are possible:
(5a) || We `didn’t go to the mu`seum be`cause it was \/raining ||
(5b) || We \didn’t go to the museum | because it was \raining ||
In the first one, there is one phrase and the last word has a falling + rising
intonation contour on it. It means: we did go to the museum, but for some
other reason than the fact that it was raining. In the second one, there are
two phrases, with a slowing down at the end of ‘museum’, and two falling
contours, one on ‘didn’t’, the other on ‘raining’. This means: we did not
go to the museum and the reason is that it was raining.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 50
4.5.2 Creak
Creaky voice can be produced in a number of ways. It involves closure
along the vocal folds leaving an opening at the front end; the folds are
loosely pressed together and are thicker than in other settings. The
subglottal pressure is often low. Creak often leads to a more irregular
pattern of vibration, and always to a slower one than is normal for the
speaker. This means that the f0 of creaky voice is low, and in fact it is
sometimes possible to hear individual pulses as the folds open. When
speakers reach the low part of a falling f0 contour, they may switch into
creaky voice: there is a close relationship between low f0 and creak.
The symbol for transcribing creak is the diacritic [+], which sits
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 51
4000
3000
2000
1000
disgusting yeuagh
creak
6.673 8.208
Time (s)
4.5.3 Whisper
Whisper is produced by narrowing the vocal vocal folds so that the
glottis is not closed, and the folds do not vibrate; none the less the glottis
is narrow enough so that when air passes through it, the airflow becomes
turbulent. Whisper is used by speakers as a way to speak ‘quietly’, or
‘secretively’: this seems to be a very widespread practice among linguis-
tic communities. It is sometimes also used to mark stance, as in (6) below.
The IPA has no symbol for marking whisper.
(6) tlj-sum04-damsongin
1 if you like /damson /jam
2 if you like stewed /damsons
3 if you like \{whdawh}mson \{wh giwh }n,
4 which is gorgeous …
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 52
4.5.4 Falsetto
Falsetto involves the raising of a speaker’s average f0 to way beyond their
normal range. To produce falsetto, the vocal folds are stretched and
lengthened and the glottis is not completely closed. Falsetto can be used
in singing, but also occurs in conversational speech. The IPA has no
symbol for marking falsetto.
Here are two cases of falsetto from everyday talk. The first one is part
of a complaint from Lesley to her friend Joyce about an acquaintance of
theirs, who has offended Lesley. At line 8, she goes into falsetto, probably
marking some kind of stance such as ‘outrage’ at the way she has been
treated. (We will return to this story in Chapter 10.)
(7) Field C85.4
1 L and he came up to me and he said
2 “oh hello Lesley, still trying to buy something for
3 nothing?”
4 J [| ɑ↓] ((a click followed by a sharp in-breath))
5 [ooh
6 L [ooh
7 J isn’t [he
8 L [{Falsetto what do you say Falsetto}
9 J oh isn’t he dreadful
The second example is a woman describing to a friend her feelings
towards a man. She compares her earlier stance towards him with her
current view of him: she is now much more favourably disposed towards
him than she was. She uses the same words to express her stance towards
him, but the second time round, she speaks in falsetto, as well as speaking
much more slowly and loudly.
(8) smc/sweet guy
1 B much more attractive in every way
2 A hahahaha
3 B not even just physically just like in every way than
4 he was before
5 it’s like before when he did something that was
6 really sweet it was just like
7 “oh that’s really sweet=that’s Tim”
{fast———————————–}
8 .hh and now it’s like “o:h that’s ↑really sweet”
{falsetto ——————}
{loud, slow—————}
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 53
Summary
In this chapter, we have seen how speakers control the vibration of the
vocal folds to bring about changes in pitch and changes in voice quality.
Voicing is also implicated in distinguishing certain pairs of sounds in
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 54
English. Both pitch and voice quality are used linguistically in English,
but with a complex range of meanings, none of them lexical.
There is comparatively little work on voice quality in English, either
in terms of its functions in small stretches of conversation or in terms
of its more generic function in marking speakers as belonging to a par-
ticular community: this is another area of English phonetics which is
ripe for research.
Exercises
1. Using the IPA chart, identify which of the sounds of English we
looked at in Chapter 3 are voiced, and which voiceless. For each sound,
find a pair of words or phrases which highlights the contrast. The pairs
should be as alike as possible. For example: [f – v]: ‘proof ’, ‘prove’. For
some sounds, you will not be able to find pairs; try to produce the sound
with/without voicing.
Further reading
Baken and Orlikoff (2000), primarily aimed at clinicians, is an extensive
survey of the voice and its measurement. Laver (1994: ch. 7) provides
a classificatory overview of voicing and voice quality. For more
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 55
5 Vowels
5.1 Introduction
In the remaining chapters we turn our attention to the vowels and
consonants of English, beginning with vowels. Vowels play a central
role in the phonetics of English. While words can consist of vowels
alone (e.g. ‘eye’, ‘awe’), they cannot consist of consonants alone.
Typically, consonants adapt to an adjacent vowel, but not vice versa.
When an English speaker starts talking, we can often tell within a few
syllables where they are from because of the vowels they use.
Vowels are syllabic sounds made with free passage of air down the
mid-line of the vocal tract, usually with a convex tongue shape, and
without friction. They are normally voiced; and they are normally oral.
As we will see, there are exceptions to this generalisation.
There is considerable discussion about the definition of vowels which
is beyond the scope of this chapter; suggestions for further reading are
given at its end.
The vowels of English vary enormously by variety. In this chapter we
introduce the concept of keywords, a way of referring to whole sets of
vowels by using the spelling of English. Keywords are written here in
small capitals. When we say ‘The vowel of goose’, we mean the vowel of
‘goose’ and words like it, such as ‘loose’, ‘boot’, and ‘rude’. More details
are set out in Section 5.5.
56
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 57
VOWELS 57
ing and perfecting one’s cardinal vowels is still strong among pho-
neticians in Britain, the USA, Germany, Australia and elsewhere who are
trained in the ‘British’ tradition.
Cardinal vowels are a set of reference vowels that have pre-
determined phonetic values. Other vowels are described with reference
to the cardinal vowels. A phonetician can say: this vowel sounds like
cardinal vowel 2, but is a little more open; or, this vowel is half way
between cardinals 6 and 7. One phonetician can replicate the sound
described by another following the instructions given alongside the
transcription.
The cardinal vowels represent possibilities of the human vocal tract
rather than actual vowels of a language because they are established on
theoretical grounds. They are independent of any particular language.
Cardinal vowels are best learnt from a trained phonetician. It takes
much practice to get them right, and to learn them well, good feedback
is needed. First we take a practical look at three of them; move on to look
at the full system; then see how it has been applied to a few varieties of
English.
the [w] sound silently, and reflect on your tongue. The back of the
tongue is raised up towards the velum (or soft palate). Suck air in
vigorously, and you should feel that the back of the tongue and the rear
part of the roof of the mouth go cold and dry. The lips are pursed: you
may need to purse them a bit more, as if you were about to blow out a
candle, or as if holding a pen in your mouth. This is close to cardinal
vowel 8 (CV8), [u].
Another way to approach this vowel is to whistle the lowest note
possible, hold that posture, and then try to produce a vowel.
This vowel is cardinal vowel 8 (CV8), [u]. It has a close (or high) and
back tongue position; and it is made with rounded lips. English does not
really use this vowel sound, although very conservative varieties of both
RP and General American come close to it. If you use it in words like
‘soon’, ‘cool’ or ‘rude’, you will probably sound very ‘posh’, ‘conservative’
or ‘old fashioned’. In any case, do not be tempted to think of the sound
of words like these as ‘CV8’: the English versions of this vowel are much
too front for CV8.
Now move silently back and forth between [i] and [u]. The backward
and forward movement of the tongue should give you a sense of the
back–front dimension.
VOWELS 59
Close i y u
Close-mid e ø o
Open-mid œ
Open a
The three vowels we have just described form three corners of the IPA’s
vowel quadrilateral. This represents, schematically, the vowel space:
sounds articulated at the edge of or inside the box are vowels. The vowel
space uses three dimensions for describing vowels: vowel height, front-
ness and backness, and lip posture (or rounding). If the tongue is raised
any higher than the sounds along the [i]–[u] axis, then friction is gener-
ated, and so a fricative (i.e. a consonant sound) is produced. If the tongue
is lowered or backed beyond the vowel [ɑ], then friction is also produced.
The cardinal vowels take pre-determined positions in the quadrilateral;
other vowels are fitted in the spaces in between.
Vowel height is represented on the vertical dimension: from close
through close-mid, and open-mid to open, along a continuum. The
horizontal dimension of the chart (front – central – back) represents
the second aspect of vowel description: vowel frontness/backness.
The points between the extremes are chosen because they are useful
reference points. In theory there is an infinite number of points between
the extremes on each dimension.
The third aspect of vowel description is lip posture. The lips can
be held in a large number of postures. Here are a few: spread and
close (as if smiling), spread and protruded (as if to make a rectangular
box between the lips and teeth), compressed and protruded (as if to
make a polite kiss on someone’s cheek), and open and rounded (as if
to make a big O-shape). The IPA represents lip posture implicitly in
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 60
VOWELS 61
has the tongue height and frontness of CV1, [i], but it has the rounding
of its opposite number, [u]. This is close to vowel in the French word ‘tu’,
[ty], ‘you’; or the sound written <ü> in German. Vowels like [y] occur in
English, especially in [ju] sequences in words like ‘use’, ‘computer’, ‘you’,
which we could more narrowly transcribe with [jy].
Secondary CV2, [ø], has the tongue height and frontness of CV2, [e],
but the rounding of [o], which is the same height, but is back not front
and rounded. It is close to the French vowel in the word ‘feu’, [fø],
‘fire’; or the German sound written <ö>. Vowels like [ø] occur in some
varieties of English: for example, in broad Australian English and in
some northern Anglo-English varieties, the vowel in words like ‘bird’ is
often close to [ø]. In New Zealand, the vowel in this set of words is tran-
scribed as [ɵ] and described as a front or central close-mid rounded
vowel.
Conversely, secondary CV8, [ɯ], has the tongue height and backness
of [u], but the spread lips of [i], and secondary CV7, [&], has the tongue
height and backness of [o], but the spread lip posture of [e].
There are two other vowels between CV1 and CV8, [3] and [4]. These
represent close central unrounded and rounded vowels respectively.
Vowels like these are not ones we expect to find in stressed syllables in
English, but they are rather common in conversational productions of
the word ‘because’. It is often pronounced ‘bec[ɒ]se’, ‘bec[ə]se’, ‘bec[3]se’
or ‘bec[&]se’.
Many varieties have a rather front vowel in words like goose: even in
varieties where this vowel is by convention transcribed [u], the sound
is often closer to [y] than to [u]. The symbol [4] represents a rounded
vowel half way between the two: a close central rounded vowel. This
symbol is commonly used in representing the goose vowel of Australian
and New Zealand English. Many varieties of English (including Scottish
and North American) use a similar, but unrounded, vowel for the close
unstressed vowel in words like ‘fitt[3]d’, ‘clos[3]s’: other varieties, such as
RP, use a fronter vowel, [i], in this position.
Many varieties of English (including RP, some Canadian varieties,
Australia and New Zealand) have a back open rounded vowel, [ɒ], for
the vowel of ‘hot’; and the vowel of ‘strut’ is frequently transcribed as [],
the unrounded sister of CV6, [ɔ].
VOWELS 63
at all points in the graph. Our hearing, however, is not like this: the
human ear is more sensitive to changes in frequency in the lower ranges
than in upper ranges; so the difference between e.g. 400 Hz and 600 Hz
sounds much bigger than the difference between e.g. 2100 Hz and
2300 Hz. So although the differences between the F1 values for CV1 and
CV4 do not look great, they are quite significant from the point of view
of hearing.
The second formant relates to frontness and backness. Front vowels
have a high F2, but back vowels have a low F2. Rounding the lips also
lowers F2, so as we move through cardinals 1–8, F2 gets progressively
lower.
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
5.4.1 Retroflexion
Retroflexion means curling the tongue back. It is not represented
directly on the IPA chart, but it is an important feature of [vowel] +<r>
sequences in rhotic varieties, such as General American, Irish and
Scottish English, where <r> is always pronounced after vowels. Any
vowel sound can in principle be produced with retroflexion. The
diacritic is [7], added to the right of the vowel symbol. It is added mostly
to the mid central vowels [ə] and [8]: [ə7 87]. These stand for the vowels
found in American English words like the second syllable of letter,
[lεɾə7], and in the word nurse, [n87s].
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 64
5.4.2 Diphthongs
The vowels so far are monophthongs: they are ‘pure’ and do not change.
Diphthongs are monosyllabic vowels which have two discernibly differ-
ent points, one at the start and one at the end. Most varieties of English
have several diphthongs. The most obvious diphthongs are the vowels of
choice, mouth and price in most standard varieties of English. These
diphthongs start with open vowels and then raise to close vowels, gener-
ally in the area of [i] or [u]. These are called closing diphthongs for this
reason. Diphthongs are transcribed by the start and end points. E.g. the
vowel of choice is transcribed in RP as [ɔi]: it starts with [ɔ] and ends
with [i].
5.4.3 Triphthongs
Diphthongs are vowels that have a start point different from their end
point. Triphthongs get from the start point to the end point via some
other, third, vowel in the middle. Or, alternatively, they are diphthongs
with a vowel that forms an extension.
Triphthongs have been described for RP in words such as ‘fire’ and
‘power’, i.e. diphthongs which are followed by an <r> in the spelling.
These words are pronounced monosyllabically with triphthongs such as
[ɑiə] and [aυə]; but they are also susceptible to ‘smoothing’, giving
pronunciations such as [fa] and [pɑ] respectively.
Triphthongs also occur in southern US varieties. For example, in
Alabama, the vowel of mouth is produced as [æεɑ], that of choice as
[ɒo3] and that of square as [aiæ].
Triphthongs are a more controversial unit than diphthongs, because
they are not considered to have a phonological status. In RP, triphthongs
can be analysed as diphthongs + phonemic /r/. The Alabaman triph-
thongs can be thought of as complex realisations of simpler underlying
phonological units. Tripthongs can, at least in some varieties, distinguish
words. For example, in RP, ‘hire’ (one morpheme) has a single syllable,
[ɑiə], and never two; but the word ‘higher’ (two morphemes, ‘high+er’)
can be disyllabic, [ɑi.ə], which ‘hire’ cannot be.
VOWELS 65
in the south.
Finally, varieties vary in realisation; that is, in the way phonologically
equivalent vowels are produced. Australian English has virtually the
same vowel system as southern Anglo-English; but as we will see in
Section 5.5.3, their realisations in these varieties are different.
So we cannot state what ‘the vowels of English’ are, because they vary
so much, and along many dimensions.
In this book, we will adopt the system of so-called ‘keywords’. The
concept comes from John Wells’s work (1982), and it makes use of
English spelling, which is independent of dialect.
Keywords exploit the fact that the spelling often captures potential
(or English-wide) differences which are not exploited in all varieties
of English. Referring to vowels by keyword makes it easier to compare
across dialects. If we referred to ‘the phonemes of American English’ and
‘the phonemes of RP’, there would be different sets, and the phonemes
would not be distributed in the same way through the lexicon. Keywords
make it easier to see what the distribution is and provide a way to refer
to classes of vowels without using phonemes.
VOWELS 67
Table 5.2 Vowels in English keywords.
General
Tyneside American Australian
RP (Tyn) (US) (Aus) New Zealand
(Roach (Watt and (Laderfoged (Cox et al. (NZ) (Bauer
Keyword 2004) Allen 2003) 1999) 2007) et al. 2007)
kit i i i i ə
dress e ε ε e e
trap æ a æ æ ε
lot ɒ ɒ ɑ ɔ ɒ
strut υ ɐ ɐ
foot υ υ υ υ υ
bath ɑ a æ ɐ ɐ
cloth ɒ ɒ ɑ ɔ ɒ
nurse 8 ø ə7 8 ɵ
fleece i i i i i
face ei e e æi æe
palm ɑ ɒ ɑ ɐ ɐ
thought ɔ ɔ ɑ o o
goat əυ o o ə4 ɐ4
goose u u u 4 4
price ai ai ai ɑe ɑe
choice ɔi oe ɔi oi oe
mouth aυ æυ aυ æɔ æo
near iə iɐ ir iə iə
square eə ε er e eə
start ɑ ɒ ɑr ɐ ɐ
north ɔ ɔ or o o
force ɔ ɔ or o o
cure υə uɐ ur 4.ə or o 4ə
happy i i i i i
letter ə ə ə7 ə e
comma ə ə ə ə e
captures the contrasting length ([] is the diacritic for long) but not
quality; or as [i – i], which captures quality but not quantity; or as [i – i],
which captures both quality and quantity. Representing either quality or
quantity (but not both) makes the transcription simpler.
Secondly, we have to decide whether to use simple, Roman-shaped
vowel symbols, or to use other shapes. For example, most varieties of
English have in words like goose a vowel that is much fronter than CV8;
[4] is a more accurate representation of it than [u]. But it could also be
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 68
VOWELS 69
i u
,
e
æ
i
o
e
æ
i u
e
o
æ
e
a a
VOWELS 71
e
o
æ
e
æ
a a
(NZ)
æ (US)
æ (RP) a (Tyn))
æ (A
(Aus)
Aus)
VOWELS 73
Australia and New Zealand, the strut vowel is even more open, and is
transcribed with [ɐ], an open central vowel.
(T
(Tyn)
yn)
(US, RP)
(Aus, NZ)
e (US))
e (Tyn)
e (RP)
æe (NZ)
æii (Aus)
u (RP, Tyn)
(Aus) u (US)
(NZ)
VOWELS 75
Summary
In this chapter we have looked at the theory of cardinal vowels as a
framework for describing vowels, and then seen it applied to a small
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 76
Exercises
1. Try to complete the chart of keywords with vowels appropriate for
your own variety of English. (You could use one of the dialects as a
model, and adjust as necessary.) For diphthongs, remember to locate the
start and end points of the vowel.
4. Find an English text and (a) identify those words which have strong
and weak forms, then (b) transcribe the strong and weak forms using
symbols appropriate for your own variety.
Further reading
The history and theory of cardinal vowels is discussed in Abercrombie
(1967), Catford (2001), Jones (1975), Ladefoged (2006), Laver (1994)
and IPA (1999). Laver (1994) also discusses problems in the definition
of vowels. Overviews of the acoustics of vowels can be found in e.g.
Johnson (2002), Ladefoged and Maddieson (1996) and Ladefoged (2005).
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 77
VOWELS 77
6 Approximants
6.1 Introduction
As we have seen, the IPA distinguishes between consonants and vowels
as two different kinds of segment. The distinction seems real enough
if we consider obvious vowels like [ɑ u i], or obvious consonants like
[p f m]. In fact, the distinction between vowel and consonant is not
so straightforward, and this point is most clearly seen with the approxi-
mants.
78
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 79
APPROXIMANTS 79
of air means that the pressure behind a constriction can build up more
quickly; and in turn, this means that it is easier to generate noisy turbu-
lent airflow with voiceless sounds than with voiced sounds. So strictly
speaking, most of the sounds that result from the combination of voice-
lessness + palatality + open approximation are in fact not approximants
but fricatives.
There are several possible ways to transcribe the combination voice-
lessness + palatality + friction. An allophonic transcription
could
modify [j] by marking voicelessness using the diacritic [ ] or []. Words
like ‘pewter’, ‘few’, ‘cue’ and so on can be transcribed with [; ]. This
captures the structural similarity between the voiced and the voiceless
pairs, so for instance
for ‘beauty’ we would have [bju-] and for ‘pewter’
we would have [p; u-], and it makes it obvious in a visual way that there
is a relationship between the sequences in both cases.
More narrowly and impressionistically, we could use the symbol for
a voiceless palatal fricative, [ç]. This makes it more obvious that as well
as palatality there is voicelessness and friction; in the transcription [; ],
friction is more implicit than explicit.
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
PAL approx
For [j], as for [i], there is a low F1, because the tongue body is close
(i.e. raised), and a high F2, because the tongue body is front in the mouth.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 81
APPROXIMANTS 81
The portion labelled ‘PAL approx’ is part of the spectrogram where the
palatal approximation is clearly visible on the spectrogram. Notice the
voicing throughout, seen in the vertical striations that continue through-
out the marked segment.
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
LV approx
The portion labelled ‘LV approx’ is the portion where there are clear
signs of labiovelar approximation. As the lips are rounded and the
tongue back is raised towards the velum, the first formant moves down-
wards ([u], like [i], has a low F1), while F2 gets much lower, because of
both tongue backing and lip-rounding. As the approximations are made,
the vocal tract narrows, and the amplitude diminishes: it can be seen
from the spectrogram that there is almost no visible information much
above 1000 Hz.
Notice that for both [j] and [w] the formants are moving throughout
the labelled portions. The point labelled ‘1’ for [j] is where F2 is highest
and F1 is lowest: this is the most ‘[j]-like’ part of the portion. The point
labelled ‘1’ for [w] is where F1 and F2 are lowest: the most ‘[w]-like’ part
of the portion.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 83
APPROXIMANTS 83
6.4 Laterals
Lateral approximants, more frequently called just ‘laterals’, occur in
English. Laterals are represented in English orthography with the letter
<l>. Phonetically they are very variable sounds both for individual
speakers and across varieties of English.
Laterals are made with a complete closure of the tongue front (either
the tip or the blade) against the alveolar ridge, which makes this part of
the tongue gesture the same as that for [t d n]. But for laterals, one or
both sides of the tongue is kept down, allowing air to escape. If you say
the word ‘leaf ’, and isolate the first consonantal sound, then suck air in,
you should feel that one or both sides of the mouth go cold and dry. Just
as some people are left-handed and some are right-handed (and some
are ambidextrous), so some predominantly produce laterals with one or
the other side of their tongue.
Lateral approximants are a little different from the other approxi-
mants in terms of their overall gesture: they do not have a stricture
of open approximation. On the other hand, the acoustic effect of open
approximation is that it produces frictionless airflow, and laterals in
English do not have friction, so they are classified as approximants.
Laterals vary in several ways: voicing, place of articulation, secondary
articulation and syllabicity. In the next sections, we will look these
features one by one.
6.4.1 Voicing
Laterals are generally voiced throughout, i.e. they are accompanied by
vocal fold vibration: the laterals in ‘loose’ (a simple syllable initial),
‘hollow’ (between vowels after a stressed syllable), ‘allow’ (between
vowels before a stressed syllable) and ‘fall’ (syllable final) are all fully
voiced.
Laterals may be produced with voicelessness. This happens after
voiceless plosives in a cluster syllable-initially, as in ‘play’, ‘plum’, ‘clay’,
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 84
APPROXIMANTS 85
What you should hear is that the ‘tone’ of the sound changes constantly
with the changes in the shape and size of the vocal tract caused by the
movement of the tongue body. All of these different sounds are voiced
alveolar lateral approximants.
In making [l], there are actually two (or three) different articulations.
The primary articulation is the closure of the tongue tip against the
alveolar ridge. The other articulations are called secondary articu-
lations – so called because they are more open articulations than the
primary one.
Secondary articulations are very similar to vocalic articulations. To
make a secondary [i] articulation, spread the lips and push the tongue
body forward and high in the mouth; to make a secondary [ɯ] articu-
lation (like [u] without the rounding), raise the tongue back up towards
the velum while keeping the tongue tip fixed to the alveolar ridge. These
two secondary articulations are called palatalisation and velarisation
respectively, and are transcribed as [j] and [γ] respectively. Velarisation is
often accompanied by labialisation (i.e. lip-rounding; symbolised with
the diacritic [w]), giving labiovelarisation, which can be transcribed
as [γw]. Phoneticians often refer to the auditory impression of these
qualities: palatalised laterals are often called ‘clear’ (often also ‘light’, by
American linguists) and velarised laterals are often called ‘dark’.
Palatalisation and velarisation are at opposite ends of a scale. Palatal-
isation involves tongue-body fronting and raising, while velarisation
involves tongue-body backing and raising. In between, as you will have
observed, there are many shades of difference.
Now say a few pairs of words: ‘leaf, feel; loaf, foal; lot, toll’. If you
compare the laterals at the start and the end of these words, you will
notice that they sound different. If you hold the articulation, you will
feel a different tongue shape and posture. Syllable-final laterals are
backer, more velarised, or ‘darker’ than syllable-initial ones. They are
also longer in duration. Conversely, syllable-initial laterals are usually
more palatalised, or ‘clearer’, than syllable-final ones. It is important to
note that these are terms are relative. In some varieties of English, all
laterals are dark, even when syllable initial. This is the case for e.g. many
varieties of North American English (but perhaps especially New York),
Manchester and Leeds (both in northern England). However, there are
still relativities within these varieties, so while the initial laterals are
dark, the final ones are even darker. Conversely, some varieties have
clear laterals in all positions, such as many varieties of Irish English
and Newcastle (England). Likewise, although these varieties have clear
laterals finally, the final laterals are still darker than the initial laterals,
without actually being dark.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 86
APPROXIMANTS 87
6.4.5 Acoustics
The articulatory variability is reflected acoustically. Figure 6.3 shows the
effect of secondary articulations on the acoustics of laterals. This is a
spectrogram of a production of [l] whose secondary articulation is
shifted from palatalised to velarised. Notice that F2 – associated with
frontness and backness – changes, moving from rather high, at around
2100 Hz, to low, at under 1000 Hz.
5000
4000
Frequency (Hz)
3000
2000
1000
0
0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 0.9533
Time (s)
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
LAT
than the following vocalic portion. F2 during the lateral portion is just
about visible at around 1600 Hz; this value is consistent with a relatively
clear lateral.
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
O-G LAT
APPROXIMANTS 89
6.5 ‘Rhotics’
The last set of approximants we will look at is not one that appears on
the IPA chart. The term ‘rhotics’ comes from the name for the Greek
letter rho, <ρ>, and has been the topic of quite considerable discussion.
There are many symbols on the IPA chart which are based on the letter
<r>: [r ɹ r ʁ ɾ ɺ ]. Many of these represent the phonetic values of the
letter <r> in many European languages. For instance, English has [ɹ],
French has [r ʁ], Finnish and Spanish have [ɾ r], and so on. If you look
these symbols up on the IPA charts, however, you will see that they do
not fit under a single heading for either place or manner of articulation.
The term ‘rhotic’ has been coined to cover this set of sounds, and when
it comes to English, we need to consider some extra sounds which are not
part of the set just given.
The second thing to say is that not all rhotics are in fact approximants.
For example, the trill [r] involves several short periods of closure of the
tongue tip against the alveolar ridge, while the tap [ɾ] has just one. In
English, most rhotics are approximants and they have a similar distri-
bution in words to laterals: for example, they occur in similar environ-
ments in consonant clusters, and behave similarly with respect to voicing
in such clusters. So in this chapter, we will group rhotics together on
functional grounds.
At the systematic level, we transcribe rhotics broadly with [r], since
there is rarely any ambiguity about its value; narrower transcriptions are
used below so that different types of [r] can be compared across varieties
and to convey more of the phonetic detail.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 90
APPROXIMANTS 91
and is used in Britain, Ireland, North America and most parts of the
Southern Hemisphere. To produce this sound, the tongue tip or blade is
raised up towards the alveolar ridge in a stricture of open approximation,
the velum is raised and the vocal folds vibrate.
However, this is a simplified description. [ɹ] is frequently accom-
panied by other articulations. If you compare ‘red’ and ‘led’ and watch
your lips, you may well see that for ‘red’ there is lip movement, whereas
for ‘led’ there is not. This could be more accurately transcribed as [ɹw].
The movement is often protrusion or rounding, and the rounding can, in
more extreme cases, involve puckering of the lips or contact between the
upper teeth and lower lip. These more extreme cases could be tran-
scribed as [ɹ@, ɹ@ ]. [@] is the IPA symbol for a labiodental approximant, i.e.
a sound made between the lips and the teeth which is made with open
approximation and no friction noise generated. (Some speakers actually
do produce friction: this could be transcribed as [ɹv]. The symbol [ ]
represents the fact that the two articulations are produced at the same
time, rather than one after the other in sequence, as implied by [ɹ@].
Alveolar approximants often have another secondary articulation.
Recall that for laterals, the main place of constriction is at the tongue tip,
leaving the body and back of the tongue free to form other articulations.
The same is equally true for [ɹ]. [ɹ] is frequently dark, or velarised. This
is marked in trancription as [ɹγ]. As with laterals, the degree of velaris-
ation is variable. Typically, speakers with clear initial laterals have darker
initial rhotics; and speakers with darker initial laterals have clearer initial
rhotics.
Commonly, [ɹ] is both labialised and velarised: [ɹγw]. Just as [l] often
loses its primary articulation at the tongue tip and instead the
‘secondary’ articulations are retained (so-called vocalisation), so also [ɹ]
sometimes loses its primary articulation at the tongue tip, but the
secondary articulation of labialisation is retained. Many speakers
produce [r] as a labiodental approximant, [@]; or as a velarised labio-
dental approximant, [@γ].
One way to understand non-rhotic accents is as varieties which have
lost the tongue-tip articulation of [ɹ] syllable finally, leaving just vowel
colouring. This is parallel with loss of tongue-tip articulation in the case
of vocalised [l].
usually clear; but other articulations are found too, such as alveolar
fricatives. These are not like [z], because the tongue shape is not right to
produce the narrow channel needed for this. The friction is generated
with the tongue tip at the alveolar ridge, and the articulation is the same
as that for [ɹ] except that there is close instead of open approximation.
There is no specific symbol for this sound, but one can be composed
from the symbol [ɹ] with the diacritic [9], which stands for a closer
articulation, giving [9ɹ ].
Another common variant of [r] in South Africa is a tap, [ɾ]. Taps are
produced with a short movement of the tongue tip towards the alveolar
ridge which makes a closure of short duration. Taps are therefore not
approximants, but stops. Taps occur in very conservative varieties of RP
especially between vowels, and can be commonly heard in old British
films. Nowadays, taps for rhotics most commonly occur in Anglo-
English only after [θ], where they tend also to be voiceless, as in ‘three’,
[θɾi]. Taps are also common realisations of [r] in Liverpool (England)
and Scotland. They may be voiced or voiceless, and when voiceless they
often have a lot of friction accompanying them. The voiceless taps seem
to come utterance finally or next to a voiceless consonant, as in ‘winter
time’ and ‘shirt’, which can both have the cluster [ɾt]. Taps are also
variants of [t] and [d] and are discussed in more detail in Chapter 7.
APPROXIMANTS 93
6.5.5 Acoustics
A common property of rhotics is that they have a low F3 (around
1800 Hz). Because most versions of rhotics involve movement of the
tongue body, which is relatively massive and slow to move, their acoustic
properties tend to be very extensive in the time domain.
We illustrate this with the approximants [ɹ] and [l]. Figure 6.6 shows
two utterances: ‘to lead’, [tə li:d], and ‘to read’, [tə ɹi:d]. The portion of
laterality is marked LAT. Notice that it has more or less clear boundaries
on the spectrogram, corresponding to the fairly abrupt onset and offset
of lateral airflow. [ɹ], like [j] and [w], has no clear beginning or end. The
portion marked RHO surrounds obvious formant transitions where F3
moves downwards, and the point marked ‘1’ is the place where F3 is
lowest on the spectrogram.
Without either labialisation or velarisation, [ɹ] has a low F3. With
these secondary articulations, F3 is lowered still further. So one explan-
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
LAT RHO
ation for the secondary articulations is that they enhance the low F3 of
[ɹ] and so make it more perceptually salient.
Summary
In this chapter, we have looked at approximants, which have similarities
with vowels but which function as consonants in English. We have seen
that two approximants, [j] and [w], are close to cardinal vowels. The
other two approximants, [r] and [l], are very complex in their articu-
lations, and in the range of possible articulations across varieties of
English.
We have also discussed primary, secondary and double articulations.
We have seen that approximants combine consonantal and vocalic
elements in their articulation, and the vocalic elements are a property
of the sounds in English: [j] always has palatalisation; [w] always has
labiovelarisation; [l] is darker or more velarised syllable finally than
syllable initially; [r] typically has labialisation and/or velarisation as part
of its phonetic make-up. In later chapters, we will see that secondary
articulations are an important factor in the make-up of English sounds;
but while approximants have inherent secondary aritculations, most (but
by no means all) other consonants share their secondary articulations
with adjacent vowels.
Exercises
1. Give a phonetic description of the laterals in the following words
and phrases as you produce them. Pay particular attention to voicing,
duration, and the degree of velarisation.
I like it; I’ll hike it; I’ll wipe it; he’d fill them; he’d fill it; he’d fill ink pens; all
those; all of them; it’s all a memory; it’s all the memory
2. Make a list of words which have <r> (a) initially (like ‘read’), (b) after
a plosive (like ‘creak’), (c) after a fricative (like ‘three’), (d) after a vowel
(like ‘star’). Describe in as much detail as you can how you produce these
words, paying attention to secondary articulations. Can you extend any
words in (d) (e.g. ‘star/starry’)? What happens to rhoticity in that case?
Further reading
For more on primary, secondary and double articulations, see Ladefoged
and Maddieson (1996), and Laver (1994). Jones (1975) and Cruttenden
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 95
APPROXIMANTS 95
7 Plosives
7.1 Introduction
Plosives are among the most variable and complex sounds in English.
The aim of this chapter is to demonstrate and explore some of this
complexity.
96
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 97
PLOSIVES 97
Open
Closing
Hold
Release
Figure 7.1 The phases of a plosive. During the hold phase, the pressure
behind the closure builds up. The greater pressure behind the
closure produces a burst of noise, plosion, on release.
trapped behind the closure. However, the lungs are still forcing air out
of the vocal tract, so the pressure behind the closure builds up. You can
feel the build-up of pressure behind a closure if you make a closure at
the lips (as for [p] or [b]), and then make a conscious effort to breathe
out. This is an exaggerated version of what happens in speech, but the
mechanism is essentially the same.
The duration of the hold phase depends on many factors. In citation
form and when before a pause, the hold phase is longer for [p t k] than
for [b d ]. In other conditions (such as connected speech, like conver-
sation), the average hold duration for all kinds of plosive is somewhere
around 50 ms.
PLOSIVES 99
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
C H R
+voice
+vo
oice –voice +voice
+v
voice
0.1 0.2 0.3
Time (s)
Although there is some voicing during the closure (notice the regular
peaks in the waveform), it is quiet and its amplitude diminishes until
eventually it stops. Voicing starts again very soon after the release of the
closure.
C H R
Time
Time
Fully voiced
v
Partially voiced, e.g. [b
bd ]
Voiceless,
Voiceless,
o unaspirated
unaspirated
Voiceless,
Voiceless,
o aspirated
aspirated
Voiceless,
Voiceless,
o preaspirated
preaspirrated
Figure 7.3 Voicing for plosives. Dotted line = voicing. The upper lines
represent two articulators moving together and apart again.
PLOSIVES 101
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
H R
+voice
0 0.05 0.1 0.15 0.2 0.25 0.3
Time (s)
Figure 7.4 Fully voiced [], in ‘gig’, [i]. (After a pause; no visible C phase.)
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
4000
3000
2000
1000
Figure 7.6 shows the lag between release and onset of voicing. After
the release, there is a period of noise before voicing begins. This is called
aspiration and it is generated by air passing through the glottis and then
the vocal tract. Aspiration is a product of turbulent airflow, and some-
times it persists even after the voicing has started.
If you say the phrases ‘a pick, a tick, a kick’ with the back of the hand
just in front of the mouth, you will probably feel aspiration as a puff of
air. Aspiration is transcribed with a superscript [h]: [ə phik, ə thik, ə khik].
The quality of this aspiration depends on the accompanying vocalic
articulation: with front, close vowels (in words like ‘peat’, ‘tick’, ‘king’,
‘cute’ in most varieties), the aspiration has qualities of palatalisation;
with back, close vowels, the aspiration has qualities of labiovelarisation
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 103
PLOSIVES 103
(in words like ‘port’, ‘took’, ‘queen’ in most varieties); with [r]-sounds,
there is accompanying retroflexion, and possibly also labiovelarisation,
as in ‘prey’, ‘treat’, ‘creep’.
The degree and duration of aspiration depend on word and sentence
stress. The more prominent a word is, the more aspiration with any
voiceless plosive in it it is likely to have.
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
Figure 7.7 Friction, closure, release and vocalic portion from ‘a spit’, [ə
spit].
7.4 Glottalisation
Closures of voiceless plosives [p t k] and the affricate [tʃ] are often
accompanied by glottalisation. This involves the adduction of the vocal
folds usually before the oral closure is made. If complete, the adduction
results in a glottal stop; if incomplete, there is a portion of creaky voice.
Both of these give an auditory impression of the vowel being cut short.
This is often called ‘glottal reinforcement’.
Glottalisation is limited to syllable-final voiceless plosives.
Figure 7.9 shows a New Zealand speaker’s production of the vowel +
plosive portion the word ‘kit’, [kh3ʔth] with glottal reinforcement. There
is a short portion between the segments labelled [+voice] and [–voice]
where there is a glottal stop (labelled [?]): in the spectrogram, it shows up
as an irregular and distinct vertical striation. On the waveform, notice
the similarly abrupt pulse during that segment; the amplitude drops
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 105
PLOSIVES 105
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
+voice –voice
C H R
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5
Time (s)
Figure 7.8 Preaspiration. The hold phase (H) starts after the offset of
voicing, producing a short portion of voiceless friction while
the closure is made.
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
7.6.1 Labial
Most labial plosives in English are bilabial, [p b]. Occasionally they may
be labiodental. This might not be a straightforward labiodental closure,
but rather a bilabial closure during which time the upper teeth are
resting on the inside of the lower lip. This kind of production can come
in sequences such as [b] + [v], e.g. ‘obvious’. These plosives take longer
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 107
PLOSIVES 107
to release, since there is more contact between the upper and lower
articulators than if the closure were purely bilabial. There is no separate
IPA symbol for labiodental plosives, but the symbols [p b] can be
modified with the ‘dental’ diacritic [
], giving [b
p
], or perhaps [bb
, pp
].
7.6.2 Coronal
Coronal plosives can be made with either the tongue tip or the tongue
blade. This seems to be a matter of individual habit. In using the word
coronal, we are recognising that the tip or the blade of the tongue can be
used to make closures at a number of different places. The main ones are
dental, alveolar and postalveolar.
Dental plosives occur in a number of places in English: before the
sounds [θ] and [ð], as in ‘width’, ‘breadth’; and often as an alternative
production of [ð] in utterance-initial position. The IPA does not provide
special symbols for dental plosives, so the diacritic [
] is added below the
symbol to mark a dental place of articulation, as in [d
θ] for ‘breadth’ or
[t
θ] for ‘eighth’.
The voiceless alveolar plosive and the voiced alveolar plosive ([t d]
respectively) have slightly different tongue shapes in many varieties. For
[t], the tongue tip tends to have a little slit in it, so that on release, there
is often a short period of friction (affrication), which we could transcribe
as [ts]. [d] on the other hand usually does not have this tongue shape, so
its release is less affricated and sounds ‘flatter’.
Postalveolar plosives occur as part of affricates [tʃ d] (which we will
discuss in Chapter 8), and in clusters before [ɹ], as in ‘train’, ‘drain’. If you
compare the tongue postures for the initial plosives of ‘tie’ and ‘try’, you
will notice that for ‘try’ the tongue tip or blade is making contact a bit
behind the alveolar ridge; you may also notice that the sides of the
tongue are curled up a little and the part of the tongue that makes
contact with the roof of the mouth might be different from the part for
‘tie’: in my own production, the frontmost underside of the tongue makes
contact for the postalveolar plosive. Postalveolar plosive can be tran-
scribed [t d ]; the diacritic [] means ‘retracted’ (i.e. further back).
7.6.3 Dorsal
The back of the tongue (dorsum) and the roof of the mouth both have
large surface areas compared to e.g. the lips. In English, dorso-velar
plosives can be made at a number of places along the roof of the mouth,
and this is especially so for combinations of [k] + vowel.
If you say the words ‘key’, ‘cat’ and ‘court’ (or words with vowels close
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 108
to CV1, CV4 and CV7 or CV8), and isolate the initial consonant articu-
lation, you should be able to feel that the back of the tongue makes
contact with the roof of the mouth in different places. For ‘key’, the artic-
ulation is quite far forward (advanced), which can be transcribed with
the diacritic [']: [k' ]. For ‘court’, the articulation is much further back
(retracted), and if you compare this [k] sound with that of ‘key’, you will
hear that it has a lower-pitched ring to it. This is partly because the lips
are rounded (the vocalic articulation includes a high tongue back and
lip-rounding); but even if you unround your lips, the sound is still differ-
ent. The diacritic for this is []: [k]. The sound in ‘cat’ is ‘neutral’: neither
particularly front nor back when compared to the others.
This variability arises because the plosive consonant is co-
articulated with vocalic articulations which differ in tongue frontness
and backness and in lip posture. Already in hearing the [k] sound in
‘key’, some secondary articulations associated with the vowel are
audible. Because they anticipate the next sound, this is often called
‘anticipatory co-articulation’. At a narrower, more detailed phonetic
level, then, we have as many ‘kinds of [k]’ as we have kinds of vocalic
articulation.
We have talked about ‘vocalic articulation’ and not ‘vowel’. This is
because velars do not depend on vowels for their place of articulation:
they depend on the subsequent approximant or vowel, whichever is
closer. In the word ‘screen’, there is a retracted [k], not the advanced
[k'] of ‘keen’ or ‘ski’. In this case, [k] is co-articulated with [ɹ], which for
many speakers of English has a secondary articulation of velarisation
and/or lip-rounding. Likewise, in ‘queen’, the velar articulation is co-
ordinated with the labiovelarity of [w] and not the frontness and spread
lips of [i].
Note that this relationship between vocalic and dorsal articulations is
not reciprocal: in sequences of vowel + [k ], the place of articulation of
the dorsal is less adapted to the vocalic articulation than in [k ] + vowel:
compare ‘keep’ and ‘peak’ and ‘caught’ and ‘talk’; you will probably find
that when the velar comes after a vowel, its place of articulation is neither
particularly front nor back as compared to when it precedes a vowel.
7.6.4 Glottal
Glottal stops occur in English, although they are not used to distinguish
one word from another. For this reason, they find no place in a phonemic
analysis of English except as free variants of other sounds, or as pre-
dictable sounds. For instance, if we compare e.g. [il] and [ʔil], these are
just different productions of the word ‘ill’.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 109
PLOSIVES 109
occur before nasals and laterals, though other kinds of release into these
consonants are possible.
PLOSIVES 111
1 1
‘a tree’, [ə tɹ i] vs. ‘at Reeth’, [ət ɹiθ], then you should notice several
differences between them (Table 7.2).
the air to be released through the nose, i.e. nasal release. This is produced
by lowering the velum while keeping the oral closure. The IPA tran-
scription for nasal release is a superscript [n]: [tn].
Nasal release is used in English as a way to join a sequence of plosive
+ nasal consonant, as in the word ‘button’. One way that the plosive [t]
can be released in this word is with an oral, central release, as in [btən].
The second is a nasal release, as in [btnn1 ]. You might be able to isolate
the [tn] sequence and produce a chain of alveolar plosives with voiceless
nasal release, [tnn
]. When you do this the tongue is kept in place against
the alveolar ridge. If you pinch your nose, you will not be able to produce
it because the air will remain trapped in the nasal cavities.
Note that the place of articulation for the nasal and the plosive are the
same, and this is the regular pattern: ‘happen’, [hapnm], ‘bacon’, [beiknŋ]
exhibit the same pattern. One ‘weak’ form of the word 1 ‘can’ (whose
‘strong’ form is [kan]) is with a syllabic nasal: ‘I [knŋ] buy them’. The
place of articulation is the same across the plosive+nasal sequence, but
the nasal could also have the same place of articulation as the bilabial
plosive in ‘buy’, giving e.g. [kəm bai]; this is the pattern we regularly find
when the nasal is not syllabic.
PLOSIVES 113
[k] C H R
[t] C H R
[k] C H R
[t] C H R
In
Inaudible
naudible
Figure 7.11 A sequence of [k(t], with [k] release inaudible.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 114
7.8 Taps
Taps are stop articulations, but unlike plosives, the closure for a tap
is non-maintainable. For taps, the closure is made by two articulators
when one articulator – usually the front of the tongue – strikes another
in passing. The closure lasts for about 30–40 ms. Because the active
articulator is in motion, the closure cannot be maintained.
Taps occur very commonly all over the English-speaking world as
forms of [t] and [d] between vowels. The cluster [nt] is sometimes
produced with a nasalised tap [ɾ"], so that e.g. ‘winter’ can be pronounced
[w""
i ɾə7]. Taps are especially common in many varieties of American
English, but are by no means limited to those varieties. 1
Figure 7.12 shows a production of the word ‘city’, [ siɾi], by a speaker
from southern Michigan. The portion labelled C corresponds to the short
closure period characteristic of taps. Notice how short it is (about 25 ms).
It is voiced all the way through. Immediately after the labelled portion
there is a transient which corresponds to the release of the tongue.
There are many pairs of words distinguished by [t] and [d] which
sound very alike or even identical when pronounced with taps: ‘writer’
and ‘rider’ ([raiɾə7]) are well-known cases. Sometimes, however, the
distinction is still made through e.g. vowel duration (longer duration in
[t/ɾ] words) and vowel quality (for example, Canadian speakers use [ai]
in ‘rider’, but [i] in ‘writer’). This loss of distinction between originally
different words has given rise to some interesting ‘eggcorns’ – words
which are misheard and then given peculiar spellings. (‘Eggcorn’ is
an eggcorn from ‘acorn’.) Examples arising from confusion over [t/d]
include: ‘Catillac’, ‘radify’, ‘color-coated’, and ‘don’t know X from atom’.
(See eggcorns.lascribe.net for more.)
Here are some taps as produced by Anglo-English speakers in
unscripted conversation in places where [t] might also be expected:
(4) VT 020104
a little bi[ɾ] of acid
I’ve go[ɾ]a go by my husband’s opinion
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 115
PLOSIVES 115
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
C
0 0.733
Time (s)
1
Figure 7.12 ‘City’, [ siɾi], as produced by a speaker from southern
Michigan. C marks the closure portion for the tap [ɾ] (IPA).
le[ɾ] it cool
I think I know wha[ɾ] i[ɾ] is
i[ɾ] is del:icious
Taps occur in conservative varieties of Anglo-English as one form of
rhotic. They are commonly heard in old British black-and-white films
where the actors speak RP. Taps also occur dialectally in England as
tokens of rhotics, e.g. in many parts of Yorkshire and Liverpool. Many
speakers who do not normally produce <r> as a tap use a voiceless tap
in the cluster [ɾ], as in ‘three’.
Here are some narrow transcriptions of words with taps in Liverpool
English (Scouse) (Watson 2007). Scouse is known for its open articu-
lations, of which there are many here, including aspiration for [d] in
‘around’.
(5) Liverpool English
free 1 i h
fɾi
around ɾɑ̃nγd
1s
t
ɾɒ̃ŋə'
stronger 1
agreed ə ɾidh
1pɾɒβəβli ɒn 1aɾiγəh
probably on aggregate 1stEɹ ɒŋəɾə9vði 1tsi4
stronger of the two
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 116
Summary
We have seen in this chapter that the phonetic details of plosives depend
very much on the context and variety of English. One of the most
complex aspects of plosives is the way that the voicing contrast is
produced. These differences are summarised (for Standard Southern
British English) in Table 7.3.
Exercises
1. Give a phonetic description of the plosives in your production of the
words below, paying particular attention to how you get into and out of
them:
lock – log – locking – logging
ramp – ramping – rant – ranting – rank – ranking
hump – Humber – hunt – hunter – under – hunk – hunger
Are there any pairs where the voice quality is different?
PLOSIVES 117
d. robin
e. pecking
f. tapestries
g. and trust
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
1 2 3 4 5
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1 1.1 1.2
Time (s)
Further reading
For more on the general phonetics of plosives, see especially Jones
(1975), Cruttenden (2001) and Laver (1994). For an acoustic description
of plosives, see Johnson (2002). Crystal and House (1988) give a good
overview of instrumental findings on plosives. For a comprehensive
description of the phonetics of glottal stops in English, see Dilley et al.
(1996).
The voicing of English plosives is highly variable by dialect, so
general references such as Wells (1982), Foulkes and Docherty (1999)
and Wolfram and Fasold (1974) are very useful.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 118
8 Fricatives
FRICATIVES 119
[ʃ]), the pressure increases, and so the turbulence increases too. With a
wider channel (e.g. for [f ] and [θ]), there is less pressure and therefore
also less friction.
Secondly, the volume of air affects the volume of friction generated.
The more air forced through the constriction, the greater the pressure,
and therefore the greater the turbulence and the amount of friction
noise. This will depend on the amount of air being expelled from the
lungs.
If this seems a bit abstract, imagine watering a garden with a hosepipe.
If the pipe is not blocked and the water pressure is not too high, water
flows through the hose and comes out in a steady stream. If you narrow
the end of the pipe by putting your finger over it, the pressure within
the pipe increases, generating turbulence in the flow of water, and pro-
ducing a spray. You can also increase pressure in the hose by opening the
tap, which increases the flow of water through the pipe and therefore
also the pressure behind the stoppage. To get a really good spray (in
other words, a lot of turbulent water), you can increase the pressure by
both opening up the tap and making the constriction narrower. Air
moving through the vocal tract is a little similar: the constriction in the
vocal tract corresponds to your finger at the end of the hosepipe, and the
water moving through the pipe corresponds to the air being pushed out
of the vocal tract.
Fricatives are often classed as strident or non-strident. Strident
fricatives, [s z ʃ ], have a lot of friction noise, especially at higher
frequencies, caused by a comparatively narrow constriction. Non-
strident fricatives are [f v θ ð]. If you compare the sounds [θ] and [s] in
particular, you will hear that one of the main differences between them
auditorily is that [θ] has a much ‘flatter’, quieter sound than [s], which
sounds ‘sharper’ or ‘brighter’ and louder.
Fricatives in English all have airflow down the mid-line of the vocal
tract. You can tell this by producing a fricative, holding the articulators
in place and then sucking air in. The part of the mouth that goes cold and
dry as you do this should be symmetrical around the middle of the vocal
tract.
Some speakers use fricatives with lateral airflow: the voiceless lateral
fricative [F] is a Welsh sound (spelt <ll>) that occurs in many place-
names, such as ‘Llandudno’, ‘Llangollen’ and ‘Llanfair’. Many English
speakers replace this sound with a sequence like [tl kl θl]; but it is not
difficult to produce [F]. First, make a [l] sound, and hold it; secondly,
remove the voicing (as in the [l] of ‘p[l]ay’); thirdly, raise and tense the
sides of the tongue a little. This should produce a good [F].
English fricatives all have oral airflow: the velum is raised and forms a
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 120
tight seal preventing air from flowing through the nose. This can be
demonstrated by pinching the nose while producing a sustained fricative
such as [s:::]. It makes no difference to the sound that comes out because
[s] requires a good seal, preventing leakage of air through the nasal
cavities and therefore weakening the friction.
Affricates have no place on the IPA chart, because they are sounds
composed of two elements, plosive+fricative. In English, there are two
such sounds at the systematic level, [tʃ d], usually represented by <ch
-tch> and <j ge- gi- -dg-> respectively in the spelling. Affricates are also
discussed in Chapter 7, but their fricative components [ʃ ] are described
in this chapter.
There are fricatives at five places of articulation in most standard
varieties of English: labiodental, dental, alveolar, postalveolar and
glottal; and two degrees of voicing: voiced and voiceless.
Other fricatives also occur in English, for a range of reasons. These are
discussed towards the end of this chapter.
8.3.1 Voicing
English has four pairs of fricatives which are traditionally said to be
distinguished by voicing: [f – v; θ – ð; s – z; ʃ – ]. Say the sounds con-
tinuously, alternating between the voiced and voiceless sound: e.g.
[s::z::s::z::]. If you do this while covering your ears, you will hear the vocal
fold vibration for the voiced sound conducted through your bones, but
otherwise, the articulators remain in the same configuration.
The phonetic detail of voicing is more complex for many speakers.
We will see that the contrast actually involves a cluster of phonetic
features, only one of which is vocal fold vibration.
If you compare a voiced and a voiceless pair of fricatives, such as
[s z] or [f v], you will notice that there is less friction noise for a voiced
fricative than for a voiceless one. The best way to hear this is to compare
pairs, or near-pairs like ‘loser’, ‘looser’; ‘ever’, ‘heifer’. The reason for this
difference is that when there is vocal fold vibration, the vocal folds are
closed for about half of the time, and so there is less air flowing through
the vocal tract. With voicing, the flow of air is reduced, and consequently
the amount of friction noise is reduced.
Figures 8.1 and 8.2 show waveforms of a stretch of about 300 ms from
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 121
FRICATIVES 121
fric
–voice +voice
0.04 0.35
Time (s)
Figure 8.1 Annotated waveforms for the first 300 ms of ‘sip’ as produced
by an RP speaker (IPA).
fric
+ voice
0.76 1.07
Time (s)
Figure 8.2 Annotated waveforms for the first 300 ms of ‘zip’ as produced
by an RP speaker (IPA).
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
fric fric
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1 1.1 1.2
Time (s)
Figure 8.3 Spectrograms of ‘sip’ (left) and ‘zip’ (right) (RP) (IPA).
The differences that have already been mentioned can be seen here
too. [s] and [z] have turbulence centred at 4000 Hz and above. For
[z], voicing can also be seen below 1000 Hz. The duration of friction is
much longer in ‘sip’ (between about 0.05 s and 0.3 s on the spectrogram)
than in ‘zip’; and it is louder, which means it appears as darker on the
spectrogram.
Now by way of contrast let us look at a similar pair, also citation forms,
as spoken by a New Zealander. This speaker often produces the fricatives
transcribed as [v ð z ] without vocal fold vibration, but the other differ-
ences are still found – the amount of friction noise produced is lower for
[v ð z ], and the duration of the frication is shorter.
Figures 8.4 and 8.5 show productions of ‘fie’ and ‘vie’ by a New
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 123
FRICATIVES 123
fric
–voice +voice
Time (s)
Figure 8.4 ‘Fie’ (New Zealand) (IPA).
fric
–voice +voice
Time (s)
Figure 8.5 ‘Vie’ (New Zealand) (IPA).
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
fric fric
Time (s)
Figure 8.6 ‘Fie’ (left) and ‘vie’ (right) as spoken by a New Zealander (IPA).
longer in duration than that for [v], which is about 110 ms. These
productions of voiced fricatives, where there is little or no voicing along
with friction, are very common in English, so we will look at them in a
bit more detail.
Figure 8.6 shows a spectrogram of the utterances in Figures 8.4 and
8.5: note the lower-amplitude friction for [v] than for [f ]: between about
0.1 and 0.2 s the fricative portion corresponding to [f ] on the spectro-
gram is darker than the corresponding portion between about 0.8 and
0.9 s, which corresponds to [v].
These patterns of partial voicing are recurrent in English. On first
noticing them, it is common to worry about how to name and transcribe
these differences. There are a number of solutions to this problem. First,
the difference has often been referred to as ‘tense’ [f θ s ʃ] and ‘lax’
[v ð z ] (rather than ‘voiced’ and ‘voiceless’), to avoid the implication
that [v ð z ] are necessarily accompanied by regular vocal fold vibration.
This is not a solution that has been universally accepted, because it can
be seen as just a difference in the way names are used, and is not much
more descriptive or factually accurate than the term ‘voiced’. Secondly,
conventions should be stated for transcription symbols so that they can
be interpreted accurately. The details are set out in Table 8.2.
We may want to represent the overlap of friction and voicing more
accurately in our transcriptions: for instance, there may be variability
within a speaker’s productions so that some instances of [v ð z ] are fully
voiced while others are not. In this case, we might transcribe [v ð z ], but
use the diacritic for ‘voiceless’, [], along with the symbol for ‘voiced’
fricatives: [v ð z ]. These symbols might seem to be equivalent to
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 125
FRICATIVES 125
Table 8.2 Voiced and voiceless fricatives.
Symbols Conventions
[f θ s ʃ] Voicing and friction do not overlap
Friction is loud and turbulent as compared to [v ð z ]
Friction is long in duration as compared to [v ð z ]
[v ð z ] Voicing and friction may (but do not necessarily) overlap
Friction is quiet (low amplitude) as compared to [f θ s ʃ]
Friction is short in duration as compared to [f θ s ʃ]
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
FRIC
V off V on
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6
Time (s)
Figure 8.7 Spectrogram of ‘looser’, with friction (FRIC) and the offset and
onset of voicing (V off, V on) marked.
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
FRIC
V off V on
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6
Time (s)
Figure 8.8 Spectrogram of ‘loser’, with friction (FRIC) and the offset and
onset of voicing (V off, V on) marked.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 127
FRICATIVES 127
Labiodental
For labiodental fricatives [f v], air passes between the upper teeth and
lower lip. Labiodental articulations are made with the upper teeth on
either the outside or the inside of the lower lip. The two do not sound
very different from each other and, as far as is known, no variety of
English exploits the difference.
Because they are made without involvement of the tongue, labio-
dental fricatives are highly susceptible to secondary articulations made
by different tongue postures: for example, compare the sounds at the
beginning of ‘feast’ and ‘fool’, and you will probably hear an [i]-like
secondary articulation in ‘feast’ – with the tongue close and front in the
mouth, [f j] – but an [u]-like one in ‘fool’, with the tongue body raised
towards the velum, and perhaps with some lip-rounding [f γw]. Retroflex-
ion can also co-occur with labiodental articulation, and is common in
productions of the word ‘from’, where there is often a period of friction
and rhoticity simultaneously: [f ɹəm, fɹəm].
Dental
The fricatives [θ ð] can be made at a couple of places of articulation. For
many speakers, the articulation is interdental, i.e. made with the tongue
blade between (‘inter-’) the upper and lower teeth. In this case, it may
protrude, or be barely visible between the teeth. Such articulations are
frequently reported for varieties of American English.
In other varieties, the friction is generated against the back of the
teeth and the tongue is held relatively flat so that the air escapes through
quite a wide channel. This wide channel is what makes the fricatives
[θ ð] so quiet in comparison with [s z]. If you make a [θ] sound and
then suck air in, you should be able to feel the place where friction is
generated: it is the part of the mouth which goes cold and dry. In the case
of dental fricatives, this is a wide area at the front of the tongue.
Some speakers do not use dental fricatives, replacing them with labio-
dental ones instead, giving [fiŋ] for ‘thing’, [faŋks] for ‘thanks’, [və]
for ‘the’, and [fɑvə] for ‘father’. This phenomenon has gained a lot
of attention in sociolinguistic literature and is commonly known as
th-fronting.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 128
Alveolar
The alveolar fricatives [s z] are made with a groove in the centre of the
tongue. This groove directs air towards the alveolar ridge, and the main
source of turbulence for these fricatives is the air striking the alveolar
ridge, which is an obstacle in its path. (It is hard for people without front
teeth to produce good [s z] sounds.) The jaw is fairly close, so that the
upper and lower teeth are close together: to hear the effect of jaw height,
say a [s] sound and slowly open the jaw, and you will notice that the
friction decreases in loudness. The second feature of these fricatives with
respect to tongue shape is that there is a hollow behind the groove, so the
tongue has a concave shape, with the tongue sides raised and pressed
against the upper teeth to produce a good seal. Without this, air would
leak out, and the sibilance of the fricatives would be much diminished.
[s] and [z] can be made with the tongue tip either up or down; in
fact, the precise articulation of these sounds is very variable between
individuals. The huge variety in individuals’ dentition and the shape of
the inside of the mouth leads to a great variety in articulation, but the
resulting acoustics are similar.
[s] and [z] are made with the front part of the tongue, leaving the lips
and tongue back free. These sounds can take on a range of secondary
articulations, especially before a vowel: compare the [s] sounds of ‘see’,
‘saga’ and ‘soar’, and you will hear something like [s j, s, sγw].
Postalveolar
The fricatives [ʃ ] are made with a constriction that is further back than
[s z]. Their place of articulation is variously described as palatoalveolar
or postalveolar. The tongue has a wider channel than for [s z], and it
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 129
FRICATIVES 129
is convex behind the groove, rather than concave as for [s z]. Like [s z],
[ʃ ] can be produced with the tongue tip either up or down.
[ʃ] is usually accompanied in English by a secondary articulation
of labialisation (lip-rounding). If you compare the words ‘lease’ – ‘leash’,
and ‘said’ – ‘shed’, you will probably notice quite different lip postures.
For the alveolar sounds, the lips have the same shape as for the vowel; but
for the postalveolars, the lips are rounded, even though the vowels are
not. So a narrower transcription would be [li:ʃw]. One possible reason for
this secondary articulation is that the postalveolars have friction at a
lower frequency than the alveolars. If the lips are rounded, then the
friction sounds as though it has a lower pitch. Try this for yourself: isolate
the [ʃw] sound of ‘shed’ and then unround and round the lips so you can
hear the acoustic effect.
Figure 8.9 shows a spectrogram of an Australian male speaker saying
the words ‘sigh’ and ‘shy’. Note that the centre of the energy in the
fricative portion is lower for [ʃ] than for [s], having significant energy at
2000 Hz and above. Lip-rounding for [ʃ] can be thought of as enhancing
one of the acoustic differences between [s] and [ʃ].
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
sigh shy
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1 1.1 1.2 1.3 1.4 1.5
Time (s)
Figure 8.9 ‘Sigh’ and ‘shy’ as spoken by a male Australian speaker. Note
the lower frequency energy for [ʃ] than for [s] (IPA).
Not all [ʃ] sounds in English are rounded. This reflects one source of
the sound [ʃ] historically, which is a combination of [s]+[i] or [j]. Many
words across varieties of English can vary between the alveolar+palatal
sequence and the postalveolar sound: ‘tissue’, [tisju: tiʃu:], illustrates this.
In some words one or the other possibility has become lexicalised, that
is, it has become the normative pronunciation of the word: examples
include ‘sugar’, where modern English has [ʃ], but the spelling indicates
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 130
that it was originally [sj-], and ‘suit’, which is usually [su:t], can be [sju:t]
but cannot be [ʃu:t].
In the words with the alveolar+palatal sequence, the rounding does
not start till the frication ends. In words like ‘tissue’, the rounding can
start later during the fricative, so that the last syllable of ‘tissue’ is not
homophonous with the word ‘shoe’. For some speakers, there are the odd
pairs like ‘fisher’ [fiʃ wɵ] and ‘fissure’ [fiʃ jə], where in the first case there
is rounding throughout the friction and in the second syllable ([ɵ] stands
for a rounded version of [ə]), while in ‘fissure’ the friction is slightly
longer than in ‘fisher’ and has a palatal off-glide and lip-rounding with
a later onset. These are small, subtle details which not all speakers of
English have in their speech.
Another place where there is some subtlety about the lip-rounding
is across words, in phrases where in between [s] and [j] there is a word
boundary, as in ‘I miss you’. Here, a wide range of possibilities occurs,
from [s j] through to articulations that are more like [ʃ] throughout.
The fricative [] in many instances derives historically from [z]+[i],
which makes it somewhat parallel to [ʃ] (which has other sources as
well). It has a much more restricted distribution than [ʃ], however: the
main difference is that [] cannot be word initial or word final in native
English
1 words. 1 [] often alternates with [ʃ] and/or [zi]: ‘Parisian’
[pə riziən, pə ri:ən], ‘nausea’, ‘anaesthesia’, [-ziə -ə -ʃə]; also with [d]
in some loanwords like ‘garage’, whose last syllable can be [-ɑ -id]).
The other main source of [] is indeed loanwords, such as ‘negligée’,
‘beige’, ‘rouge’. It does not occur, however, at the boundary of
morphemes where the rightmost morpheme is <-er>: words like ‘rosy’,
1 ‘cheesy’1have comparative forms with [-zi-], and not [--] (so, e.g.,
‘cosy’,
[ rəυziə], not [ rəυə]).
Glottal
Finally, we come to the glottal fricative, [h], which does not occur at
all in some varieties of English. This is usually classed as a fricative on
the grounds that its friction noise is generated at the glottis. Because
of this, its quality is very different depending on what follows. If you say
the words ‘heat, heart, hoot’, and isolate the initial fricative, you will
hear very different qualities, which are determined by the quality of
the vowel that follows. This is because although there is friction being
generated at the glottis, the rest of the vocal tract above the glottis (the
supralaryngeal tract) excites and amplifies some parts of the friction
more than others. One commonly suggested analysis of [h] is that it is
a period of voicelessness superimposed on a vowel, so that it might also
be transcribed as e.g. [i ɑ u]. Between vowels, voiced glottal friction is
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 131
FRICATIVES 131
Palatoalveolar fricatives
The sequence [t + j] sometimes gives rise to palatoalveolar fricatives, as
in words like ‘Tuesday’ and ‘question’, which can be pronounced with
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 132
[tG] or with the sequence [tj]. For some speakers, the first syllable of
‘Tuesday’ may be homophonous with ‘chews’; but for others, there is a
distinction between the two kinds of word. ‘Chews’ has lip-rounding
throughout and an overall dark resonance; on the other hand ‘Tues-’ has
increasing lip-rounding throughout the consonantal portion until the
[u] vowel is reached, when lip-rounding is at a maximum. The friction is
also not in the same place as for [ʃ]: it is further forward and is made with
the blade of the tongue. It is alveolopalatal and transcribed as [G].
Similar observations could be made for ‘question’. Phonemicising
this word as /kwεstʃən/ predicts that the consonant cluster /stʃ/ in the
middle of the word should sound the same as in the sequence ‘nice
church’. However, this does not seem quite accurate for all speakers,
because the lip-rounding and tongue body posture in the two are differ-
ent. In ‘question’, the friction is front and clear, and close to palato-
alveolar, [G].
Palatal fricatives
In discussing the approximant [j], we noted that there is the possibility
of voicelessness + palatality, as in words like ‘pew’. Voiceless palatal
fricatives are also common in English as realisations of the sequence
/hj/, as in ‘hue’, ‘huge’, ‘human’, etc. This is not surprising: as we have
seen, [h] has no associated tongue shape and represents voicelessness. It
must be accompanied by some supralaryngeal articulation, and in the
case of the sequence [ju] (represented in English orthography as ‘long
<u>’), this means that the accompanying supralaryngeal articulation
will be that for a palatal approximant. If this is produced without voicing,
there will be greater airflow through the vocal tract, and in turn this will
generate friction, resulting in voicelessness, palatality and friction, which
is represented as [ç]. Notice that (as with [pj-], etc.) other transcriptions
suggest themselves on phonological grounds, such as [hj] or [; ]. It may
not be a coincidence that there are varieties of English where these
words have initial voicing, not voicelessness: [jud, jumən].
FRICATIVES 133
and Irish, such sounds are normal productions, not just common pro-
ductions, of the sound [p t k b d ], but especially [t], in many contexts.
In all varieties of English, fricatives like these arise in speech that is often
described as ‘fast’ or ‘casual’, though perhaps a more accurate description
would be ‘ordinary’. What is special about Irish and Liverpool English is
that these are normal productions of ‘plosives’.
Alveolar: ‘slit-t’
Irish English has a form of [t] which is sometimes described as ‘slit-t’.
It is made with an incomplete closure. The tongue shape is flatter than
for [s], and the onset of the friction is rather sudden: this gives a very
different impression from a ‘real’ [s], which has a more gradual build-up
of friction. The period of friction is shorter in duration than for [s].
There is no agreed way to transcribe this sound. Possibilities include
modifications of [s], but [s] implies a grooved tongue shape, which is not
accurate; variations of [t], which captures the fact that the sound does the
work of [t] in other varieties; and [θ], which implies a sound with the
tongue shape of [θ-], but an alveolar place of articulation, marked with
the diacritic [-], taken from the Extensions to the IPA (for the tran-
scription of clinical and disordered speech). This sound occurs between
vowels and word finally, but not if another consonant occurs either
before or after. A similar sound can also be heard in some varieties of
Australian English.
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
kids do it
FRIC
Figure 8.10 ‘Kids do i[θ ]’. Speaker: 18-year-old male, Dublin (IViE file
f1mdo).
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 134
Velar
Velar fricatives are regularly found in at least two varieties of British
English: Scottish English and Liverpool English. In Scottish English, the
velar fricative occurs in a few peculiarly Scottish lexical items such as
‘driech’, [drix], a term to describe dark, grey, cloudy weather, and ‘loch’,
[lɔx], a kind of lake or inlet from the sea.
Liverpool also has fricatives where other varieties have [k] after
vowels. The friction is made with the tongue body or back, from palatal
through to uvular articulations, depending on the preceding vowel: a
more forward place of articulation with front vowels, a backer place of
articulation with back vowels, as in ‘week’, [wi:ç], ‘back’, [bax], ‘dock’,
[dɒχ]. Figure 8.11 shows a spectrogram of a Liverpool speaker saying
‘I don’t smoke’ [smə4x]. Notice the last portion, labelled FRIC, which
has no voicing, but has prolonged friction all the way through.
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
I don’t smoke
FRIC
0 0.9877
Time (s)
FRICATIVES 135
Table 8.3 Fricatives from undershoot.
Labial Coronal Dorsal
Complete closure pb td k
Incomplete closure φβ 9s #t 9z d
# xγ
Summary
As with plosives and approximants, we have seen that fricatives form a
phonetically rich set of sounds in English. This richness stems in part
from the fact that tongue shape, the volume of air flowing through the
vocal tract and place of articulation all affect the quality of friction. The
way that fricatives are entered and exited is also a linguistically signifi-
cant feature, distinguishing ‘real’ fricatives from those which arise from
the failure to achieve a complete closure to form a plosive. Although the
number of fricatives in the words of English (nine) is larger than that of
plosives (six), we have also seen that some of these fricatives cannot
occur everywhere in words, and sometimes alternate with other frica-
tives. Fricatives, then, are also a phonologically complex set of sounds.
Exercises
1. Transcribe, broadly and then more narrowly, your productions of
the words and phrases below. Pay particular attention to the secondary
articulations. The first one is done for you.
1
future fjutjə f jj4'ʔtGə
shop
this shop
disgusting
sneezing
all the horses
from the summer
a thin veil
for his father
to hush up
thank you
Further reading
For a more complete description of Liverpool English see Watson (2007)
and references therein. Pandeli et al. (1997) discuss the production of
Irish English ‘slit-t’.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 137
FRICATIVES 137
9 Nasals
138
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 139
NASALS 139
they require the velum to be lowered. This lowering of the velum is what
allows air to flow out of the vocal tract via the nasal cavity. Learning to
control the raising and lowering of the velum is not very easy, since most
people are not aware of the velum. One way to become more aware is to
open the jaw (as if to make an [ɑ] sound), and then breathe in through
the nose, but breathe out through the mouth, and then repeat this. At the
transition between breathing in through the nose and out through the
mouth, you might hear a quiet popping sound as the velum is lowered.
This can make you more aware of the velum’s position.
Secondly, nasal consonants require a complete closure to be made
somewhere in the vocal tract: the oral gestures for [m n ŋ] are the same
as those for [b d ] except that the velum is raised for [b d ] and lowered
for [m n ŋ]. For this reason, nasals are often classed as ‘stops’, alongside
plosives: in this case, the term ‘stop’ refers to a sound with a complete
constriction in the oral tract, rather than a complete constriction in the
vocal tract as a whole.
Thirdly, nasals in English are voiced.
Most of these the properties of nasals can be tested simply. Make a
long nasal sound such as [m] or [n], and then pinch your nose. You
should find that the sound cannot be sustained for very much longer
once the nose is pinched: this is because the closure in the vocal tract
seals off airflow through the mouth, so there is no oral airflow; and by
pinching the nose, airflow through the nose is also blocked.
Next, make a [s] sound. You may be able to make a nasalised [s]
sound, [s̃], by lowering the velum. If you achieve this, the amount of fric-
tion will diminish rapidly as you move from [s] to [s̃], and the fricative
will sound weak and ‘thin’ compared to [s]. However, you can restore
some of the volume to [s̃] by pinching the nose again, blocking nasal
airflow.
Finally, try a simple experiment with a friend. Make nasals at a few
places of articulation, including labial, labiodental, dental, alveolar and
postalveolar; but do this out of sight, e.g. with your back turned or with
your hands in front of your mouth, so that they cannot see the articu-
lation. Ask them to guess the place of articulation. Most likely, they will
not be very successful. The reason is that that nasals are not, by them-
selves, very distinctive, because the nasal cavity absorbs a lot of acoustic
energy. Most information about place of articulation for nasals is located
in the transitions into and out of the nasal occlusion which correspond
to the closing phase of plosives.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 140
Raised
Velum
Velum
Lowered
Together
Toogether
Lips
Apart
[m] [a]
NASALS 141
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
0
m a
0.186 0.34
0.3
34
Time (s)
that during the spectrogram of the portion labelled [m], there are some
areas of low amplitude, such as around 1600 Hz. This is because the nasal
cavities absorb some of the acoustic energy, and gaps like this (called
zeroes) are often evident on spectrograms. Although there are formants
visible during the nasal portion, they are less distinct than in the vocalic
portion; this is because the formant peaks are wider, which makes them
also quieter and less visually prominent. This is particularly noticeable
for F1.
lower the velum rather quickly and the vowel + nasal sequence is more
like a reflection of the nasal + vowel sequence.
Figure 9.3 represents vowel + nasal sequences schematically. The
lowering of the velum is represented as starting early, and happening
slowly.
Raised
Veelum
Velum
Lowered
Together
Together
Articulators
Apart
[a] [ã]
[ã
ã] [m,, n, ]
Figure 9.4 shows the vowel + nasal portion for a male Australian
English speaker saying the word ‘hang’.
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
0
a ng
Figure 9.4 Vowel + nasal portion from the word ‘hang’ [(h)æ̃ŋ]. Speaker:
Australian male (IPA).
The vowel + nasal portions are much less distinct than in the case of
nasal + vowel sequences. F1, around 1000 Hz, gently falls, but its ampli-
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 143
NASALS 143
tude fades away too (that is, it gets lighter in colour on the spectrogram).
The waveform has a smooth diminuendo, and the amplitude falls off
gradually. F2 and F3 come together into a pattern distinctive for velars
at around 0.87 s, where the boundary between [a] and [ŋ] is marked, and
then they fade away in amplitude and are no longer visible.
The nasalisation of vowels before nasal consonants is an example of
anticipatory co-articulation (cf. Section 7.6.3): a vowel anticipates a later
sound (in this case a nasal) by adopting some aspect of the production of
the later sound.
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
the m ore
21.66 21.86
Time (s)
Figure 9.5 ‘The more (he blew)’. Speaker: RP female (IPA).
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 144
Bilabial
Bilabial nasals occur both syllable finally and syllable initially.
Labiodental
Labiodental nasals are sometimes found before labiodental fricatives, in
words like ‘emphasis’, [εfəsis], and ‘invariant’, [ivεəriənt].
Dental
Dental nasals are rather common in English but are limited to word-final
clusters within words and morpheme boundaries between words.
Within words, they occur before the dental fricative [θ] in words like
l n
θ], ‘tenth’, [tε̃n
θ] – which contains two morphemes, ‘ten’
‘plinth’, [pĩ
and ‘-th’.
The fricative [ð] occurs initially only in function words, such as ‘this,
that, the, those, they’. When nasals occur in this context, there may or
may not be friction. So in phrases like ‘in the –’, a range of pronunciations
is possible, including [-n
dð-], [-n
ð-] and [-n
-]. In addition to this,
dental nasals in English typically have a ‘dark’ secondary resonance, with
some degree of velarisation. The entry and exit into dental nasals is also
slower and less crisp than for alveolar consonants. These details make
quite a strong contrast between the definite form ‘in the –’ and the
indefinite form ‘in a –’.
Alveolar
Alveolar consonants occur syllable initially and syllable finally in
English.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 145
NASALS 145
Postalveolar
Postalveolar nasals (transcribed [n]) occur before the postalveolar sounds
[ʃ t ʃ d], in words such as ‘French’, ‘lunch’, ‘grunge’ (narrowly: [frεn tʃ,
lntʃ, grn d]).
They are also reported as variants of alveolar nasals in Indian English;
and as variants of [r] + nasal in some rhotic dialects.
Velar
Velar nasals in English, like velar plosives, have a range of places
of articulation, from rather front ([ŋ]) through to rather retracted or
⫹
back ([ŋ]). Velar nasals, however, must follow a vowel (i.e. they are not
syllable initial or word initial), and the range of vowels that can precede
then is limited: they must be short, and are the vowels of kit, trap, lot
and strut. Compare the place of articulation for ‘sing, sang, song, sung’,
and they should feel different: probably frontest for ‘sing’, backest for
‘song’, and roughly the same for ‘sang, sung’, depending on the quality
you have for these vowels.
The place of articulation of velar plosives is more dependent on the
vowel that comes after than before them; and when they occur between
vowels, the place of articulation is somewhere between the places of
articulation expected for the vowel sounds. The same is roughly true for
velar nasals. Compare the place of articulation for ‘singer’ and ‘singing’:
you should notice that it is slightly advanced for ‘singing’ as compared to
‘singer’. However well these differences can be felt in production, in
perception they are less easy to notice, especially as compared to the
various [k] sounds.
NASALS 147
drink [drĩk]
tint/tent [tĩə̃t]
glance [læ̃ ĩs]
can’t [kæ̃ə̃t, kε̃ĩ t]
This can be seen as a ‘natural’ process, in that it has a simple phonetic
explanation: the velum lowers early, producing nasality + vocalicity
followed by nasality + a stop articulation.
Here are some examples taken from Cockney English (the traditional
dialect of London, especially the eastern part of the city), recorded in
the early 1950s from the speech of people born in the 1890s (Sivertsen
1960). What is recorded here, then, is ‘conservative’ or ‘traditional’, from
a modern perspective. The distribution of nasalisation in Cockney is
under-researched: it occurs in the context of nasal consonants, and is
commonly found on open vowels; it is also a property of the voice
quality used by Cockney speakers. The transcriptions have been
simplified a little.
(4) Cockney (Sivertsen 1960):
1
ə pe1 ərə̃ʔləi apparently
1 wẽ ʔ
ɑ I went
n1θĩ k 1 nothing
ə pɑ̃Aʔ n1 ə ɑAf a pint and a half
i1ts nɑ̃A1s it’s nice
f˜1nĩ ĩn1ĩʔ funny isn’t it
ði ɔod1 ε̃əs the old house
əz ðεi lɑ̃iʔ as they like
It can be seen from these transcriptions that some vowels are nasalised
in words which in the spelling contain nasals: ‘apparently’, ‘went’, ‘pint’
are all examples of this. Then there are words where there is nasalisation
also after a nasal consonant, as in ‘funny’, ‘isn’t it’ and ‘nice’. But there are
also words with nasalised vowels where there is no nasal in the spelling:
‘house’ and ‘like’ are examples of this. These details are very character-
istic of this dialect, and they show that sometimes phonetic parameters
do not behave in the same way in all varieties.
NASALS 149
4000 4000
Frequency (Hz)
Frequency (Hz)
3000 3000
2000 2000
1000 1000
0 0
0.55 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9
0.92 0.72 0.8 0.9 1 1.1
Time (s) Time (s)
Figure 9.6 ‘Bottom’ [bɒɾəm] and ‘button’ [bʔt nn1]. Speaker: Australian
male (IPA).
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 150
NASALS 151
correspondingly [braitnn1 d]. Words like this can also be produced with-
out the syllabic nasal, in which case the release of the alveolar closure is
different: compare [braitnn1 d and [braitənd].
Syllabic consonants are not obligatory in these contexts, and in cases
where the morphological form is less transparent, syllabic nasals are less
likely. A well-known pair is ‘lightening’ (‘light’ + ‘-en’ + ‘ing’), [laitnn1iŋ]
(with three syllables), and ‘lightning’, [laitnniŋ] (with two syllables).
Another example is ‘frightening’ – although this is derived from ‘fright’
+ ‘-en’ + ‘-ing’, it behaves more like a monomorphemic word, and is
more likely to be [fraitnniŋ] (parallel with ‘lightning’) than [fraitnn1 iŋ]
(parallel with ‘lightening’).
Summary
Although there are only three nasal phonemes in English, we have seen
that nasal airflow is common in a range of sounds in English, and syllabic
nasals are an important feature of everyday speech.
Exercises
1. Transcribe the words below, paying particular attention to the place
of articulation for the nasal consonants.
emphasis; hangar; unreal; unbalanced; in bad shape; in good shape; in dubious
taste; ungrateful; hymn; mind; anthropology; in that manner; I’m going to (or:
gonna) mime it
NASALS 153
Further reading
As ever, Cruttenden (2001) and Jones (1975) cover the main details
and discuss assimilation in some detail. Sivertsen (1960) is a study of
Cockney English. For discussion of response particles like ‘mhm’, see
Jefferson (1985). For a phonological account of assimilation, see e.g.
Giegerich (1992).
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 154
154
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 155
tongue’s contact from the upper surfaces of the mouth. These sounds
are not known to occur in the world’s languages, although a velarically
initiated, bilabial egressive plosive or trill can be used in English to mean
something like ‘nothing’, or ‘I have no idea’ (‘and I don’t care’).
In learning to produce clicks in phonetics classes, students commonly
allow the tongue to plop down on to the floor of the mouth. This
produces a secondary sound known as a percussive. Percussives are
sounds made when two articulators strike each other, but there is no
airstream necessarily involved in producing the noise. When English-
speaking children mimic the sound of a horse walking on a road, they
often make a sequence of click + percussive, which mimics the ‘clip clop’
of the horse’s feet. In producing clicks, it is important to try to eliminate
any percussive element.
Clicks can be produced at a range of places of articulation. In English,
the commonest ones are probably dental [J], bilabial [◎] and alveolar
lateral [L]; other possibilities include palatoalveolar [M] and alveolar [!].
We will look at dental clicks in Section 10.2.1 below.
Clicks are complex articulations, but in their simplest form, we
can think of them as a velar plosive accompanied by another complete
closure which is released before the velar closure. It is quite possible to
produce more complex click articulations by changing the ‘accompani-
ments’ that go with them. For instance, it is possible to produce a click
with voicing, by combining velar closure and voicing: so instead of
voicelessness at the same time as velar closure, [k], as the accompani-
ment, we have voicing and velar closure, []. Voicing and nasality are
also combinable with clicks. These are easier to produce than might be
thought. Start off by making a protracted [ŋ] sound, and simultaneously
start to make a clicking sound: this should be easy to do, because the
airstream needed for clicks is located forward of the velum, so air can
flow through the nasal cavities while a click is being made. The resulting
sound can be transcribed [ŋJ].
It has become usual to think of clicks as ‘click + accompaniment’
(a point of view that originated in the work of Ladefoged and Traill
1994), and it is common to transcribe clicks in two parts: the first part
describing the velar (or uvular) articulation (as e.g. [k ŋ], the second
part describing the click type. The two parts are joined with a ‘tie bar’,
[ ]. So a fuller transcription of the click [J] (the ‘tut’ of English) would
be [k J].
Figure 10.1 shows a spectrogram of a click (from extract (5) below).
Point 1 marks the release of the forward closure, in this case a dental
closure. This closely resembles the burst of the release of [t]: it has most
energy higher up in the frequency range. Notice that there is a transient
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 157
4000
3000
2000
1000
1 2
burst followed by a longer period of noise. Very soon after that comes the
second release, that of the velar closure. This is marked 2. Notice the
centre of energy is very different: around 1500 Hz. This is consistent
with the kind of burst we find for velars. After the release of the velar
closure there is a lot of ingressive aspiration noise, which is the sound of
air entering the vocal tract; and then the particle ‘ah’. This spectrogram,
then, shows clearly the two releases associated with the velaric airstream
mechanism.
Agnes’s talk, and in doing that, going along with the complaint made by
Agnes. This is consistent with her display of sympathy in her next turn:
‘that’s a shame’. So here we have a click which seems to be produced as
the first part of a display of sympathy with another’s situation, and which
seems to be temporally placed to display attentiveness to that other
person’s telling of the situation.
Clicks can also be used to mark receipt of positive news. In the next
fragment, Jade tells Kate how two people they know became a couple.
Once she has completed her story, Kate offers an appreciation of the
story (line 6), which starts with a click followed by a long, breathy open
vowel with falling pitch. If clicks marked ‘disapproval’, then Kate would
be treating this story as bad news; but in fact she makes a positive assess-
ment of the story: ‘that’s lovely’. Indeed, we might even say that the
stretch [ʔɑ*] marks a positive receipt of the news.
(5) nrb reluctant lover
1 J he wouldn’t stop asking her out
2 he used to ring her like three times a day
3 and she’d go “no no”
4 or she’d say yes and not turn up
5 and then she just completely fell for him
6 K → [Jh (.) ʔɑ*] that’s lovely
So, here we have a few instances where clicks initiate turns that
provide assessments or appreciations of a telling; in these turns, the
recipient of a story demonstrates their understanding of the kind of
story it was, which can include ‘disapproval’, but also ‘a story deserving
of sympathy’ and ‘good news’.
Another common use for clicks is to signal the transition from one
activity to another (Wright 2007). A good place to observe this is at the
end of phone calls, where there is a sequence that English speakers
commonly use to manage how they will both get to the point of putting
the phone down. (This may seem trivial, but putting the phone
down before it is due is a big faux pas in English-speaking societies.)
The fragment in (6) below displays this well. The current topic is
closed down in lines 8–9 (‘oh splendid’/‘yahp’); then there follows a click
in line 9; then there is a short sequence in lines 9–10 where a proposal
is made to close the call (‘Okay then’), which is accepted (‘right’), and
then greetings are exchanged, and the speakers hang up the phone.
Clicks are common in this sequence in between the closing down of the
topic and the ‘OK’:
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 161
and lowered for the ‘low’ note. If you can do these two things in quick
succession, you will feel the larynx bobbing up and down. To make good
ejectives, you need to be able to control the upward movement of the
larynx.
Now make a glottal stop again, and this time combine it with another
complete closure, for instance at the velum, as for [k]. (We could tran-
scribe this as [ʔ( k(].) Having two complete closures – the glottis at the
lower end and the velum at the upper end – means that the air between
them is trapped. If the larynx is now raised, the air pressure between the
closures will increase, because there is the same amount of air, but in a
smaller space. In the case of a pulmonically initiated plosive, the increase
in air pressure behind a closure comes from the lungs; but in the case of
a glottalically initiated egressive plosive, the increase in air pressure
comes from the raising of the larynx. If the velar closure is now released,
there will be a sound rather like a [k] sound, but it has a much harsher,
louder character than a pulmonically initiated plosive.
Egressive, glottalically initiated plosives like this are called ‘ejectives’.
(The Latin origin of this term is quite descriptive: ‘e-’, ‘out’; ‘-ject-’,
‘thrown’.) The IPA uses the convention of an apostrophe after the symbol
for the corresponding pulmonic sound: in the case of a velar ejective,
then, the symbol is [k’].
Since ejectives require a closure at the glottis, they are necessarily
voiceless sounds.
the word ‘three’ is highly predictable from the context, there is a hitch in
the speaker’s production, and the ejective occurs before a 0.2-s pause.
(9) gw lab 0701 weeks
1 H when are your finals then
2 → what wee[k’]
3 E (0.6 s)
4 H [ʔ]uhm (.) wee[kh] (0.5 s) [b] one (1.2 s)
5 I’ve got my exams from this term
6 E [p t↓] yeah
7 H wee[kh ʔ (] (1.1 s) two [ʔ]and three is then[ʔ]
8 write my open paper
9 (0.3 s)
10 E yeah
11 H ooh I wonder when that’s gotta be in
12 cos that might be quite good
13 (0.5 s)
14 [h↓] cos David’s brother’s: (0.3 s) wedding’s
15 → at– [[ʔ ]on the week]end of wee[k’ (0.3 s) ʔ ]three
16 E [ohh yeah ]
17 H (0.3 s) [h↓] (.) [ʔ ]and the:n (0.8 s)
18 is that right weekend
In line 15, in between the release of the plosive of ‘week’ and the start
of the friction of ‘three’ is a pause of about 0.3 s. The fricative [f ] at the
start of ‘three’ starts with a glottal stop. So the glottal closure made
earlier at the same time as the velar closure in ‘week’ is held until the
speaker starts to speak again with ‘three’. Articulations which are held
across gaps in speaking within a speaker’s turn like this have been shown
for English to mark: ‘I may not be speaking now but I have more to say
and I am keeping the turn’. So perhaps the ejective in this environment
is a side-effect of some other work that is being done by a glottal stop.
We can compare Helen’s production of ‘week’ here with her other
productions of ‘week’ in lines 4 and 7. In line 4, the plosive is produced
with aspiration, followed by a long pause. There is evidently labial
closure too: the word ‘one’ is preceded by a bilabial plosive with
approximately 0.07 s of voicing during the closure. In line 7, the plosive
at the end of ‘week’ is produced with lengthy aspiration (0.45 s), which is
abruptly cut off by a glottal stop before a pause of 1.1 s. The pause is
released into an alveolar plosive.
For comparison, spectrograms of the tokens of ‘week’ from lines 4
([k]) and 15 ([k’]) are shown in Figure 10.2.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 166
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
1 2
0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1 1.1 1.2 1.3
Time (s)
The first production (on the left) shows voicelessness and closure co-
occurring: notice the high-frequency friction around 3000–4000 Hz
between 0.3 and 0.4 s on the spectrogram. The plosive is released with a
considerable amount of aspiration: the release is marked at 1 on the spec-
trogram. The second production is an ejective production, marked at
2 on the spectrogram. Notice the glottal closure at around 1.15 s, and the
very abrupt way that the energy for the vowel dies away. The plosive
release marked at 2 is sharp and loud (darker than at 1), and although
there is some friction noise, it is much shorter in duration than in the
pulmonic production. This is because the amount of air available
between the glottal and velar closures is rather small, which makes it
difficult for friction to be sustained for very long; whereas for pulmoni-
cally initiated productions, there is far more air in the lungs which can
be forced out to produce aspiration noise.
So while not all the tokens of ‘week’ contain ejectives in this fragment,
they do all have some kind of closure which is held in the pause after the
word ‘week’, and two of these, in lines 7 and 15, involve also glottal
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 167
10.3.2 Implosives
Ingressive glottalically initiated plosives (implosives) can also be made.
These sounds are made by lowering the larynx while there is a forward
closure. As the larynx is lowered, the air pressure between the glottal
constriction and supralaryngeal closure drops, and when the supra-
laryngeal closure is released, air flows into the vocal tract. It is possible
to produce voiceless glottalically initiated ingressive plosives, but these
are thought not to occur in the world’s languages. Much more commonly
for these sounds, the vocal folds vibrate as the larynx is lowered. This is
also the case for Jamaican Creole (see below).
Auditorily, these sounds are rather distinctive. As the larynx is
lowered and the vocal folds vibrate, a peculiar ‘swallowing’ sound can be
heard. As might be guessed, voiced implosives are fully voiced. The IPA
represents implosives with modified plosive symbols which contain a
rightward hook on the upper part of the letter: [N O P].
In many languages, implosives seem to have derived historically from
fully voiced plosives. Remember that the vocal folds can only vibrate
when there is a pressure difference above and below the glottis; and
when there is a complete closure in the vocal tract, the pressure above
and below the glottis will eventually equalise. One way to enable the
vocal folds to continue vibrating while there is a supralaryngeal closure
is to expand the size of the vocal tract. If this is done, then there is more
volume, which means that the pressure in the supralaryngeal tract will
fall. Expanding the vocal tract can be achieved by puffing out the cheeks,
or expanding the pharynx by lowering the larynx: this may account
for how some languages develop fully voiced glottalically initiated
ingressive plosives.
Implosives have not been formally reported for any varieties of
English. One reason for this absence is probably that most varieties of
English do not have fully voiced plosives. One prediction that we might
make is that implosives – perhaps even weak ones, with a short hold
period and a short portion of voicing along with larynx lowering – might
be found as an innovation or a development in varieties which do have
fully voiced plosives.
Implosives do, however, occur in at least one English-derived creole,
Jamaican Creole (Harry 2006), where they occur as realisations of
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 168
voiced plosives when they are initial in syllables that are auditorily
prominent, especially word initially.
A spectrogram of an implosive [P] in the word ‘good’ in Jamaican
Creole is shown in Figure 10.3. At the point marked 1, a short transient
noise burst can be seen. This corresponds to the tongue back making a
complete closure against the velum. In between points 1 and 2, there is a
rather lengthy voiced portion, about 135 ms in duration, during which
there is complete closure. Notice how the amplitude of the voicing dies
away shortly before the release of the closure at point 2. This is what we
would expect as the pressure difference above and below the vocal folds
becomes smaller, and the pressure equalises. Point 2 is where the velar
closure is released. The [d] at the end of the word is also fully voiced, but
not implosive.
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
1 2
Summary
In this chapter, we have looked at a group of sounds produced using the
velaric and glottalic airstream mechanisms that are not typically thought
of as ‘sounds of English’. However, it is relatively easy to find examples
of both clicks and ejectives in English. These sounds are not part of the
phonemic inventory of English. Nevertheless, both kinds of sound do
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 169
Exercises
1. Make a collection of stretches of talk with clicks. Good places to start
might be radio phone-in shows, or panel discussions, where the activities
of the show move on rapidly. Word searches are another place where
clicks seem common. Compare what you find with the descriptions in
this chapter.
Further reading
There is currently rather little literature on ejectives and clicks in
English. See Ladefoged and Maddieson (1996) for an overview of clicks,
ejectives and implosives in languages other than English; and Wright
(2007) and Ward (2006) on clicks in spoken English.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 170
11 Conclusion
This book has, I hope, shown some of the richness and complexity of
the sounds of English. We have seen a great deal of phonetic diversity for
sounds which intuitively we think of as ‘the same’. In many cases, what
started off looking like a simple system turned out to be more complex.
For example, /l/ is not always produced as a lateral in English, a fact that
we explained with secondary articulations. Syllable-initial and syllable-
final /l/ have different secondary articulations, with the syllable-final
lateral being (labio)velarised, [lγ], and we saw that in some cases, the
alveolar closure is lost, leaving only the secondary articulation, giving
fairly close, back, vocalic articulations like [& ö] or [υ]. We also noted
that the kind and degree of secondary articulation vary according to
the variety of English. The linguistic interpretation of this kind of
complexity is a problem for phonology, which is largely beyond the
scope of this book.
If we consider critically what we have learnt, we can draw a number
of conclusions.
First, we have seen that sometimes the phonetic details are more
complex than we might first imagine. This is especially obvious for
voiced and voiceless fricatives and plosives (as in ‘race’ vs. ‘raise’ or
‘hit’ vs. ‘hid’). For these sounds, we have seen that the phonetics of the
voicing contrast in English is more complex than just vocal fold
vibration. The timing of the start and end of voicing with, for example,
the onset and offset of friction, or closure and release, is complex in
English. There are also differences in the volume of air passing through
the vocal tract, possible differences in voice quality, and differences in
the duration of resonant articulations that precede such sounds. We have
seen also that in many cases, the location of a sound in a syllable or a
word is an important determinant of how it is pronounced: for example,
syllable-initial nasality and syllable-final nasality are co-ordinated with
oral gestures (such as complete closure) in different ways. Likewise,
syllable-initial [k] matches closely the articulation of an adjacent
170
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 171
CONCLUSION 171
resonant (as in ‘keep’, ‘car’, ‘corn’, ‘creep’), while syllable-final [k] is a lot
less variable in its place of articulation. Phonetic details like these
provide listeners with a lot of information about linguistic structure: and
this is information that they can use to make sense out of the sounds of
speech.
Secondly, we have seen that there are many things we can observe
phonetically that relate to ‘meaning’, but not to word meaning. For
example, the use of voice quality to convey a stance towards the thing
being talked about; the use of in-breaths and clicks to project ‘I have
more to say’; the clustering of ejective sounds with other phonetic details
around pauses of particular types: all these things seem to be used by
speakers to mark something that goes beyond word meaning. Another
kind of non-lexical meaning involves the use of one sound in place of
another that is found in more standard varieties. Examples of this
include the use of glottal stops in the place of [t] or [d], or the more open
articulations found in Liverpool, or the particular voice quality found in
Glasgow. These details index another kind of meaning: ‘I am from
(where I am from)’, or ‘I belong to such-and-such a social group’.
Phonetic details at all levels are not ‘extras’, but are often an
indispensable part of speakers’ phonetic repertoires. Some of the most
exciting work in phonetics at the moment comes from the systematic
exploration of how such details work in everyday talk, taking into
account speakers’ sociolinguistic background, marking ‘stance’, ‘attitude’
and ‘affect’, and looking at how talk is organised in conversation. There
is still a great deal that we do not know, for example, about the way
speakers use intonation, voice quality, sounds made with non-pulmonic
airstreams, and other phonetic parameters to generate meaning.
In our studies, we have used our eyes (through the use of symbols,
spectrograms, waveforms and other diagrams), our ears (listening and
reflecting on our own productions), and our awareness of our own vocal
tracts (reflecting how it feels to make a sound), as well as data from care-
fully controlled recordings and everyday conversation. Phonetics is, by
its very nature, a multisensory discipline. Human communication is also
a multisensory phenomenon: whether our language is spoken or signed,
language is ultimately produced, sensed and mediated through our
bodies. One growing area for research in phonetics is how our speech is
co-ordinated with other physical activities like body posture, pointing
gestures, eye gaze and eyebrow movements; another is how the brain
handles speech and language.
What comes next for you, the reader? This will depend on your own
interests, but a better understanding of acoustics will make it easier for
you to appreciate how speech perception works. There is much more to
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 172
say about the way that phonetics and phonology relate to one another.
We have not even scratched the surface of the phonetics of the inton-
ational and rhythmical systems of English. The small details of today’s
English sometimes become the lexically contrastive of ones tomorrow,
just as the details of today’s English have developed from earlier pronun-
ciations. Historical change and sociolinguistic variability are closely
related, and often have phonetic explanations. There are many varieties
of English (let alone other languages) whose phonetic details have not
been recorded: it’s a healthy stance to approach each variety as a foreign
language, noticing as many details as possible, because in many cases we
still do not know what details are important to speakers and hearers. To
develop as a phonetician, constant practice and observation are needed.
We are lucky that the stuff of our trade surrounds us.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 173
Glossary
173
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 174
cardinal vowels (CVs) – A set of reference vowels used in the description of the
vowels of languages. There are eight primary cardinal vowels, [i e ε a ɑ ɔ o u].
central airflow – Airflow along the centre of the vocal tract. Cf. lateral airflow;
nasal airflow.
citation form – The phonetic form of a word when spoken carefully; the usual
form found in a dictionary.
click – A type of ingressive plosive made on a velaric airstream. In English
speech, clicks do not form parts of words but are common in everyday talk;
often represented as ‘tut’ or ‘tsk’.
close approximation – An articulation which generates turbulent airflow
between two articulators, resulting in friction.
co-articulation – The articulation of aspects of two or more sounds at the
same time. E.g. the velar closure in [k' ] in ‘keep’ is front (advanced); it is co-
articulated with the following front [i] sound.
complete closure – An articulation which blocks the passage of air through the
vocal tract because a seal is made between two articulators.
consonant – One of two kinds of segment recognised by the IPA, the other
being vowels. Consonants are produced with a stricture of at least open
approximation in the vocal tract. Consonants are described in terms of
voicing, place and manner of articulation.
coronal (adj.) – A term used widely in phonology, referring to sounds made
with the tongue tip and blade, i.e. the active articulator, without specifying
what the passive articulator is. Alveolar consonants, e.g. [t z n l], as well as
dental consonants, [θ ð d
l]are coronal.
creaky voice – A voice quality generated by slow, tense, sometimes random
vibration of the vocal folds. Diacritic [+], e.g. [a+].
cricoid cartilage – A ring of cartilage at the base of the larynx.
diacritic – A character which modifies the basic value of a phonetic symbol and
is placed around simple letter shapes. E.g. [] is the diacritic for voicelessness.
Since there is no special symbol for a voiceless alveolar nasal, this diacritic
can be combined with the symbol for the voiced sound [n], giving [n].
diphthong – A sequence of two vowels within the same syllable. The vowels of
choice, mouth and price in most varieties of English are diphthongs, as in
RP [ɔi, aυ, ɑi].
distribution – A statement of where something occurs in a language, e.g.
syllable initially, word medially, utterance finally.
dorsal (adj.), dorsum (n.) – A term referring to sounds made with the tongue
back, i.e. the active articulator, without specifying what the passive articu-
lator is. Velar consonants, [k ŋ], are dorsal.
double articulation (doubly articulated) – A sound with two articulations
with the same degree of stricture. [w] has open approximation at the lips and
the velum, and is a double articulation.
egressive – A sound made with air flowing out of the vocal tract.
ejective – A type of egressive plosive made on a glottalic airstream. Ejectives
occur in some varieties of British English. Diacritic [’], e.g. [k’].
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 175
GLOSSARY 175
Extensions to the IPA (ExtIPA) – Extensions to the IPA system for the descrip-
tion of disordered speech, but often useful in the notation of details from
everyday talk.
falsetto – A voice quality generated by making the vocal folds long, thin and
tense, causing them to vibrate at a much faster rate than usual. May be
marked using ExtIPA conventions, e.g. {falsetto [wɑt də jə sei] falsetto}.
formant – A broad, dark band on a spectrogram. It indicates an area of the
acoustic signal which is boosted by the natural resonances of the vocal tract.
Formants are counted upwards, from the first formant, F1 (the lowest). In
speech, F1–F3 are the most important formants.
frequency – Frequency is a measure of the rate of how many cycles occur per
second. It is measured in Hertz.
fricative – When there is turbulent airflow through the vocal tract, generally as
the result of close approximation, fricatives are generated. English fricatives
include [f v θ ð s z ʃ h].
fundamental frequency (f0) – In speech, the lowest component frequency of
the speech signal, generated by the vibration of the vocal folds.
glottalic (airstream) – Airflow caused by the raising or lowering of the larynx.
When egressive, the glottis is closed. When ingressive, there is usually vocal
fold vibration.
glottalisation – The accompaniment of creaky voice and/or glottal stop while
a sound is made. Common for syllable-final voiceless plosives, e.g. ‘hat’,
[ha+ʔ t], when it is often known as glottal reinforcement.
glottis – The space between the vocal folds.
Hertz (Hz) – A measure of frequency. 1 Hz = one complete cycle per second.
If there is voicing at a frequency of 150 Hz, it means the vocal folds open and
close 150 times in a second.
homophone – Two words which are different but sound alike are homophones,
e.g. ‘whole’ (adj.) and ‘hole’ (n.).
homorganic – Two sounds are homorganic when they share the same place of
articulation, as in the clusters [mp lt ŋk].
implosive – A type of voiced ingressive plosive made on a glottalic airstream.
They are said to occur in parts of the Southern states of the USA, and
occur in some English-derived creoles. The IPA represents implosives with
modified plosive symbols which contain a rightward hook on the upper part
of the letter: [N O P].
ingressive – A sound made with air flowing into the vocal tract. Symbol
(ExtIPA) [↓].
intonation – A linguistic use of pitch, spread out over whole utterances.
intrusive-r – In non-rhotic varieties: a [r] sound (produced as e.g. [ɹ ɾ])
used to join two words where the second starts with a vowel but there is no
historical warrant for the presence of [r]. E.g. ‘China [-r-] and Japan’.
IPA – International Phonetic Association; International Phonetic Alphabet.
labialisation – A secondary articulation where the lips are rounded in a gesture
of open approximation. Diacritic [w], e.g. [ʃ w].
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 176
GLOSSARY 177
sources of sounds are aperiodic. Voiced fricatives contain both periodic and
aperiodic components (voicing and friction respectively).
phoneme – A unit of phonology: sounds which can differentiate one word from
another, e.g. the sounds [t] and [d] in ‘hit’ and ‘hid’. Transcribed between slash
brackets, e.g. /t d/.
pitch – A percept generated by f0. High pitch is caused by high f0, low pitch by
low f0.
plosive – An oral consonant made with complete closure in the vocal tract.
Plosives have three possible stages: closing, holding and release.
preaspiration – A period of voiceless friction as a closure into a plosive is made.
Transcribed with [h] e.g. ‘loop’, [luhp], or with a homorganic fricative symbol,
e.g. [luφp].
primary articulation – In complex articulations, the primary articulation is the
closest of two or more articulations. In English, syllable-final laterals are
velarised: compare ‘leaf ’, [l-], and ‘feel’, [-lγ]. The primary articulation is [l],
the secondary articulation is velarisation.
pulmonic (airstream) – Airflow originating from the lungs; pulmonic airstream;
cf. glottalic; velaric.
Received Pronunciation (RP) – The traditionally prestigious variety of British
English. This is the variety of British English which has most commonly been
described, and is taught to learners of English.
reduced vowel – The use of one of a limited set of vowels (such as [ə i]) in
unstressed syllables.
release, central – The most usual way to release plosives, by letting air flow
down the middle of the vocal tract.
release, fricative – The release of a plosive into a period of friction. Affricates,
such as [tʃ, d], are plosives with fricative release.
release, inaudible – A form of release without audible plosion. Diacritic [ (], e.g.
[-k(t-].
release, lateral – A way to release alveolar plosives, by lowering one side of the
tongue and allowing air to flow down the side(s) of the vocal tract. Diacritic
[l], e.g. [dl].
release, nasal – A way to release plosives, by lowering the velum while retain-
ing the oral closure and allowing air to escape through the nose. Diacritic [n],
e.g. [bn].
resonance – As a physical space, the vocal tract boosts the amplitude (loudness)
of some parts of the speech signal (frequencies) more than others. These
amplified parts are known as resonances. The values of the resonances of the
vocal tract depend on the shape of the vocal tract, which is why different
sounds sound different from one another.
resonant articulation – Sounds made without friction.
retracted – Retracted articulations are further back than might be expected.
Diacritic [], e.g. ‘court’, [k ]. (Cf. ‘cart’ or ‘key’.) Cf. advanced.
retroflexion – Curling back of the tongue tip. Diacritic for vowels [7], e.g.
‘mother’, [mðə7], in rhotic varieties. Symbols for retroflex consonants all
have a right-facing downward hook, e.g. [
].
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 178
rhotic (adj.) – Varieties of English which allow [r] sounds before consonants are
known as rhotic. E.g. in rhotic varieties, the word ‘start’ is pronounced as
[start] or [stɑrt]. Cf. non-rhotic.
rhotic (n.) – A cover term used to refer to an r-sound; one of a family of sounds,
such as [ɹ ɾ ], which is a realisation of <r>.
secondary articulation – The more open of two or more articulations in the
same sound. In English, syllable-final laterals have a secondary articulation
of velarisation: compare ‘leaf ’, [l-], and ‘feel’, [-lγ].
segment – Speech is conventionally thought of as being composed of segments:
consonants and vowels. In acoustic phonetics, ‘segment’ may refer to any
stretch of the sound wave which can be identified consistently, such as the
hold portion of plosives.
spectrogram – A graphical representation with time on the horizontal axis
and frequency on the vertical axis. Amplitude (loudness) is represented by
darkness.
stop articulation – An articulation made with complete closure in the oral tract.
Plosives and taps are stop articulations.
striation – A vertical line on a spectrogram corresponding to one opening of the
vocal folds: a good indication of voicing.
strident – Strident fricatives are those which have a lot of friction noise, caused
by a narrow constriction. Strident fricatives in English are [s z ʃ ].
suprasegmental – Properties of speech which extend over segments. Supra-
segmental features of speech include intonation, loudness, rhythm, voice
quality and tempo.
syllabic lateral/nasal – A nasal or lateral which forms a syllable by itself,
usually after a lateral or nasal release. Diacritic [1], as in ‘bottle’, [bɑtl1l],
‘button’, [btnn1 ].
tap – A sound made by a short complete closure in the vocal tract as one artic-
ulator strikes another.
thyroid cartilage – One of three cartilages that make up the larynx. The thyroid
has two prominent plates which form a notch at the front of the larynx.
transcription – The practice of representing speech in writing.
transcription, allophonic – A transcription which uses the phoneme symbols
of a language and includes some allophonic detail. Allophonic transcriptions
are narrower than phonemic transcriptions.
transcription, broad – A transcription which contains a limited amount of
phonetic detail. Cf. transcription, narrow.
transcription, comparative – A transcription whose purpose is to compare
sounds within or between varieties.
transcription, impressionistic – A transcription which uses the full potential of
the IPA to record much observable detail. Impressionistic transcriptions (or
‘impressionistic records’) are necessarily narrow.
transcription, narrow – A transcription which contains some phonetic detail.
There is a gradual scale from broad to narrow transcription. Cf. transcription,
broad.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 179
GLOSSARY 179
consonants. Vowels are voiced oral resonant sounds made with central
airflow. Vowels may occasionally be voiceless or nasalised. Vowels are
described in terms of height, frontness/backness and lip posture.
vowel frontness/backness – One of three dimensions for describing vowels.
The IPA recognises three arbitrary points along a continuum: front, central
and back.
vowel height – One of three dimensions for describing vowels. The IPA recog-
nises four arbitrary points along a continuum: close, close-mid, open-mid
and open. ‘High’ and ‘low’ are sometimes used instead of ‘close’ and ‘open’
respectively.
vowel – lip posture – One of three dimensions for describing vowels. The lips
can be held in a number of postures, such as spread and close (as for CV1 [i]),
compressed and protruded (as for CV8, [u]) or open and rounded (as for [ɒ]).
The IPA represents lip postures implicitly in symbols.
vowel quadrilateral – The four-cornered chart which is used to represent the
vowel space.
vowel space – An abstract space which represents the possible configurations of
the vocal tract that produce vowels. Articulations outside the vowel space
generate friction and are consonantal.
waveform – A graphical representation with time on the horizontal axis and
sound pressure level on the vertical axis.
whisper – A type of voice quality where the vocal folds are narrowed, so as to
produce turbulent airflow across the glottis. The vocal folds do not vibrate in
whisper.
yod-dropping – A term applied to some varieties of English which have no [j]
sound after alveolars before [u] in words such as ‘dew’, ‘new’, [d(j)u, n(j)u].
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 181
Chapter 2
1a. velar plosive; alveolar lateral; bilabial plosive
b. glottal fricative; labiodental fricative
c. alveolar plosive, labiodental fricative
d. velar plosive; alveolar fricative
e. alveolar approximant; alveolar fricative; alveolar plosive
f. alveolar plosive; postalveolar affricate
g. velar plosive; alveolar nasal; alveolar plosive; velar plosive; alveolar
plosive
h. velar plosive; bilabial nasal; alveolar nasal; alveolar plosive (or tap)
i. alveolar nasal; alveolar plosive; alveolar fricative; alveolar plosive
j. alveolar nasal; alveolar plosive. (NB <g> and <h> are not pro-
nounced.)
k. alveolar fricative; velar plosive; alveolar lateral approximant; post-
alveolar affricate
l. glottal fricative; alveolar approximant; velar nasal
m. bilabial plosive; (alveolar approximant); labiodental fricative;
(alveolar approximant); bilabial nasal. Not all speakers will pro-
nounce this word with [r] sounds.
n. alveolar plosive; alveolar approximant; alveolar nasal; alveolar frica-
tive; alveolar lateral approximant; alveolar plosive
181
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 182
2.
Symbols Set 1 Set 2
a. pmtnk p t k (oral plosives) m n ŋ (nasals)
b. slpmvʃ p m v (labial) s l ʃ (made with tongue tip)
c. fjwlzθ f z θ (fricatives) j w l (approximants)
d. svhðθ v ð (voiced fricatives) s h θ (voiceless fricatives)
e. rknlw k w (velar) r n l (alveolar)
f. tmbs t s (voiceless) m b g (voiced)
g. ʃ t
θ ð t ʃ t (postalveolar) t
θ ð dental)
h. hzlʔs h ʔ (glottal) z l s (alveolar)
i. napkjw a j w (made with open n p k (made with complete
approximation) closure; stops)
j. jwbdr j w r (approximants) b d (plosives)
Chapter 3
2. (a) is a phonemic transcription, based on citation forms. It is a broad,
systematic transcription. (b) is a more allophonic, narrower transcription
which contains some predictable details such as aspiration and the
unstressed vowels in ‘was’ and ‘because’. (c) is much the same, but it is
simpler; it uses the familiar letter shapes [e] and [r]. (d) is a narrow tran-
scription, and is more impressionistic. It uses a full range of diacritics to
mark e.g. voicelessness and lip-rounding; it also shows many details of
the consonants that are not evident in (a)–(c).
3. Transiients
Transients Transients
Transients
High F2
5000
4000
3000
2000 First
rst three formants
Fir
1000
00
Low F2
0
took
too
ok off his cloak
0 0.11
0. 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 0.96718
Time (s)
L
Lower r panel
Periods of voicing in ellipses; aperiodic
aperioodic noise in rectangles. Both
Botth have
corresponding
correspoonding portions of spectrogram
spectrogrram immediately above.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 183
Chapter 4
1. Finding contrasting pairs for [s – z], [f – v] and [θ – ð] should be
straightforward where these are word final. Word initially, it is harder,
because the distributions of [z ð ] are limited in this position. This kind
of test is useful for working out the distribution of sounds.
2. You should find that some kind of phrase boundary must be marked
with punctuation at major syntactic breaks, but not generally between
words that are closely related syntactically. In the sentence below, <|>
marks a possible phrase boundary, <*> where a boundary could not
occur.
you’re * a * caterer | with * a * big * firm | small * firm | your * own * firm |
e.g. You’re a caterer. With a big firm? Small firm? Your own firm?
In spoken language, names used to identify the next speaker are gener-
ally phrases: ‘As I understand it, Marguerite, is that right …’. Certain
displays of hesitation also have phrase boundaries: ‘ she was like a, sort
of, you know, like one of those film stars’. Certain relative clauses also
require phrase boundaries: ‘You grow your own fruit which is fantastic’
(= your fruit is fantastic); ‘You grow your own fruit, which is fantastic’
(= it’s fantastic that you grow your fruit (which may or may not be
fantastic)).
Chapter 5
1. and 2. The answers you come up with will depend on your own
dialect.
Chapter 6
1. Most speakers will find that they have more heavily velarised laterals
when a consonant follows; and that when a vowel follows, the lateral
will be more velarised when syllable final than when syllable initial.
Many speakers will also find that pronouns behave differently from other
words in e.g. ‘fill it’ vs. ‘fill ink …’, with less velarisation before pronouns.
Place of articulation will most probably be dental if there is a following
dental; and these will generally be more velarised than in other positions.
Chapter 7
1. Pay particular attention to the duration of the various articulations.
When considering plosives in verbs, the plosive is in different positions
in each form of the verb. When followed by <-ing>, there is a central
release, with the possibility of aspiration. When word final, or when
followed by <-ed>, the release may not be evident. [t] between vowels
may be produced as a tap. The degree of aspiration will probably be
greatest when the plosive is word final.
Chapter 8
1. The transcriptions below capture some of the details of most Anglo-
English speakers. The last one represents a common pronunciation in
conversational speech.
1
future fjutjə f j;4'ʔtGə
shop ʃɒp ʃwɒpw
˜
this shop ðis ʃɒp ðiʃ ʃwɒpw, d
ðiʃ ʃwɒpw
disgusting disstiŋ, dizstiŋ dizstiŋ
sneezing sniziŋ sniz iŋ, sn niz iŋ
all the horses ɔl ðe hɔsiz ɔ
lə ɔsiAz , ɔ
lə ɔ*ɔsiAz
from the summer frəm ðə smə f γwɹγwəm n
ə smə
a thin veil ə θin veil ə θi b
eil, ə θin b
eil
for his father fər iz fɑðə fɹ1 γwiz fɑð#ə
to hush up tə hʃ p tə ʃ w p
thank you θaŋk ju aŋk1 j4' , *ããŋk1 j4
Chapter 9
1. In the following words, the nasal and following consonant share their
place of articulation:
emphasis [f ], hangar [ŋ], unreal [n r], anthropology [n
θ], in that (manner)
[n
ð]
In other words and phrases, productions may vary between e.g. alveolar
+ another place of articulation; one (non-alveolar) place of articulation;
possibly a double articulation:
unbalanced [nb, mb], ungrateful [n, ŋ], in good (shape) [n, ŋ, nŋ].
In the phrase ‘I’m going to’/‘gonna’, the nasal is often velar. Usually it is
nasals which are basically alveolar which exhibit this change in place of
articulation.
3. In these cases, the complete closure needed for the final nasal is
produced early, and instead of close approximation (which would result
in friction). The result is a cluster of two consonants with a shared place
of articulation. The first part of the cluster has oral airflow, the second
has nasal airflow, and the transition from the oral to nasal airflow is
achieved through lowering of the velum.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 186
4. English does not use nasalised vowels; but some speakers mimic them
in their pronunciation of French words: [gratã, kwasɑ̃]. Another strategy
is to use a vowel similar to the French one and put a nasal consonant after
it, e.g. [bɒn məυ, krutɒn]. Words can also become completely nativised,
such as [lieizən].
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 187
Further reading
Abbreviations
EWW English World-Wide
JIPA Journal of the International Phonetic Association
187
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 188
Foulkes, Paul and Gerard Docherty (1999), Urban voices: accent studies in the British
Isles, London: Arnold.
Fromkin, Victoria, Robert Rodman and Nina Hyams (2007), An introduction to
language, Boston, MA and London: Thomson/Wadsworth.
Giegerich, Heinz (1992), English phonology, Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Harry, O. G. (2006), Jamaican Creole, JIPA 36, 125–31.
Hedevind, Bertil (1967), The dialect of Dentdale in the West Riding of Yorkshire,
Uppsala: Äppelbergs tryckeri, Acta Universitatis Upsaliensis 5, Studia
Anglicistica Upsaliensia.
Hillenbrand, James M. (2003), American English: Southern Michigan, JIPA 33:
121–6.
IPA (International Phonetic Association) (1999), Handbook of the International
Phonetic Association, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Jacewicz, Ewa, Joseph Salmons and Robert A. Fox (2007), Vowel duration in
three American English dialects, American Speech 82: 367–85.
Jefferson, Gail (1985), Notes on a systematic deployment of the acknowledge-
ment tokens ‘Yeah’ and ‘Mmhm’, Papers in Linguistics 17(2): 197–216.
Johnson, Keith (2002), Acoustic and auditory phonetics (2nd edn), Oxford: Blackwell.
Jones, Daniel (1975), An outline of English phonetics (9th edn), Cambridge and New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Kelly, John (1967), On the phonology of an English urban accent, Le Maître
Phonétique 127: 2–5.
Kelly, John and John Local (1989), Doing phonology, Manchester: Manchester
University Press.
Labov, William (1972), Language in the inner city: studies in the Black English
Vernacular, Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Ladd, D. Robert (1996), Intonational phonology, Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Ladefoged, Peter (1995), Elements of acoustic phonetics (2nd edn), Chicago: Univer-
sity of Chicago Press.
Ladefoged, Peter (1999), American English, in IPA 1999: 41–4.
Ladefoged, Peter (2005), Vowels and consonants: An introduction to the sounds of
languages (2nd edn), Oxford: Blackwell.
Ladefoged, Peter (2006), A course in phonetics (5th edn), Boston, MA: Thom-
son/Wadsworth.
Ladefoged, Peter and Ian Maddieson (1996), Sounds of the world’s languages,
Oxford: Blackwell.
Ladefoged, Peter and Anthony Traill (1994), Clicks and their accompaniments,
Journal of Phonetics 22: 33–64.
Laver, John (1994), Principles of phonetics, Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Lecumberri, Maria Luisa Garcia and J. A. Maidement (2000), English tran-
scription course, London: Arnold.
Lindau, Mona (1985), The story of /r/. In V. A. Fromkin (ed.), Phonetic linguis-
tics, London: Academic Press, 157–68.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 189
Nolan, Francis, Tara Holst and Barbara Kühnert (1996), Modelling [s] to [ʃ]
accommodation in English, Journal of Phonetics 24: 113–37.
Pandeli, H., J. Eska, M. J. Ball and J. Rahilly (1997), Problems of phonetic
transcription: the case of the Hiberno-English flat alveolar fricative, JIPA 27:
65–75.
Pike, Kenneth L. (1943), Phonetics: a critical analysis of phonetic theory and a technic
for the practical description of sounds, Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.
Pullum, Geoffrey K. and William A. Ladusaw (1996), Phonetic symbol guide
(2nd edn), Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Roach, Peter (2004), British English: Received Pronunciation, JIPA 34: 239–45.
Rogers, Henry (2000), The sounds of language: an introduction to phonetics, New York:
Longman.
Shockey, Linda (2003), Sound patterns of spoken English, Oxford: Blackwell.
Sivertsen, Eva (1960), Cockney phonology, Oslo: University Press and New York:
Humanities Press.
Slomanson, Peter and Michael Newman (2004), Peer group identification and
variation in New York Latino English laterals, EWW 25: 199–216.
Stuart-Smith, Jane (2007), A sociophonetic investigation of postvocalic /r/ in
Glaswegian adolescents, Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of Phonetic
Sciences, Saarbrücken, 1307–10.
Thomas, Erik (2003), Secrets revealed by Southern vowel shifting, American
Speech 78: 150–16.
Trudgill, Peter and Elizabeth Gordon (2006), Predicting the past: dialect
archaeology and Australian English rhoticity, EWW 27: 235–46.
Ward, Nigel (2006), Non-lexical conversational sounds in American English,
Pragmatics and Cognition 14(1), 113–84.
Watson, Kevin (2007), Liverpool English, JIPA 37: 351–60.
Watt, Dominic and William Allen (2003), Tyneside English, JIPA 33: 267–71.
Wells, J. C. (1982), Accents of English, vols 1–3, Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Wells, J. C. (2006), English intonation: an introduction, Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Wolfram, Walt and Ralph W. Fasold (1974), The study of social dialects in American
English, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Wright, Melissa (2007), Clicks as markers of new sequences in English
conversation, Proceedings of the 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences,
Saarbrücken, 1069–72.
Zsiga, Elizabeth C. (1994), Acoustic evidence for gestural overlap in consonant
sequences, Journal of Phonetics 22: 121–40.
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 190
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 191
Index
Note: this index covers the main chapters of the book, not the Glossary, where many of the terms listed
in the Index can also be found.
191
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 192
192 INDEX
INDEX 193
fricative, 112, 118 phoneme, 4–5, 64, 66, 108, 146, 155, 132
nasal, 144 phrasing, 22, 46–9
plosive, 106–7 pitch, 23, 34, 44, 47, 48, 49, 53, 54, 151,
labiovelarisation, 85, 86, 92, 94, 102, 103 160, 162
larynx, 9, 15–16, 33, 40–1, 42, 43, 46, 154, place of articulation, 12, 14, 18, 22, 110,
162–3, 167 150
lateral plosive, 16, 21, 23, 27, 82, 83–4, 96–117,
airflow, 10–11 128, 132–5, 139, 147–8, 149, 150,
approximant, 83–9, 91, 95, 116, 128 163, 165–6, 167–8
click, 157 postalveolar, 13–14
fricative, 119 affricate, 17
release, 112–13 fricative, 128–30
length, 22 nasal, 145
consonants, 106 plosive, 107, 110–11
vowels, 65, 67, 68 preaspiration, 100, 104
linear scale, 44–5, 63 primary articulation, 86, 91
linking-r, 90 pulmonic airflow (airstream), 7–8, 22, 154
liquid, 18
loan words, 130 reduced vowel, 74–5
logarithmic scale, 44–5 resonance, 33, 97
secondary, 132, 144
manner of articulation, 16–18, 89 retroflexion, 14, 63–4, 92, 103, 127
meaning, 2, 29 rhotic (sound), 89–94, 95, 111, 115, 127
sentence, 46–9 rhotic (variety), see rhoticity
word, 4–5, 24, 25, 27 rhoticity, 63, 65, 68, 77, 90, 145
morpheme, 64, 65, 130, 131, 144, 146, rhythm, 146, 159
151–2 rounding, 23, 60, 61, 63, 75, 81, 82, 85, 86,
91, 104, 108, 111, 127, 129, 130, 132
nasal, 163
airflow, 11 schwa, 62
cavities, 10, 11, 15, 78, 96, 120, 156 secondary articulation, 84–6, 87, 91, 108,
click, 156, 162 127, 128, 129, 144, 170
consonant, 16, 128 segment, 23, 35–6, 78, 141–2
release, 109, 111–12 sociolinguistics, 23, 53, 76, 95, 127, 172
nasalisation, 11, 53, 139 spectrogram, 31–3, 35–6, 36–7, 63; see also
tap, 114 individual figures listed on pages viii–x
vowel, 27, 146–8, 150 spelling, 3, 5, 17, 20–1, 25–6, 29, 57, 64,
nasalised, see nasal, nasalisation 66, 79, 83, 90, 114, 120, 129–30, 132,
nasality, 28, 53 148
stance, 52–3
off-glide, 60, 90, 130, 131 stop, 106, 114, 133, 139, 155
on-glide, 86, 88–9, 147–8 stricture, 17, 18, 22
open approximation, 18, 80, 81, 83, 91, strident fricative, 119
92 suprasegmental, 22, 23–4
orthography, see spelling syllabic, 56, 79, 81
lateral, 86–7
palatalisation, 85, 94, 102 nasal, 148–52
percussive, 113, 146, 156, 162, 164 syllable
periodic, 31–2, 33, 34 final (position), 85, 86, 91, 104, 110,
pharynx (pharyngeal), 15, 58, 93, 167 138, 143–4, 147–8
02 pages 001-194:An Introduction to English Phonetics 18/11/09 07:53 Page 194
194 INDEX
initial (position), 84–5, 89, 103, 110, 111, 125, 130–1, 132, 150–1, 166
138, 168 voicing, 9, 27–8, 40–43, 170
stressed, 46, 47, 61, 65, 163 acoustics, 31–3
unstressed, 46–7, 62, 74, 75, 112, 149 clicks, 156
syntax, 49 fricatives, 120–6, 132
implosives, 167
tap, 17, 23, 89, 92, 114–15 laterals, 83–4
teeth, 12, 12–13, 91, 92, 118, 127, 128; plosives, 99–104, 111, 116
see also dental vowel, 23, 42, 86, 138–53
tempo, 23 acoustics, 30, 34
topic (in conversation), 44, 161 breathy voiced, 130
transcription, 24–5, 36 duration, 99, 104, 114, 116
allophonic, 25, 27, 36, 80, 182 frontness/backness, 58, 59, 61, 62, 63,
broad, 24, 25, 89 74
consonants, 25–6 height, 58–9, 61
impressionistic, 25, 28–9, 36, 72, 80 nasalisation, 27, 140–3, 146–8
narrow, 24, 25, 28, 80, 115, 129 rhoticity, 90
phonemic, 25, 27 rounding, 60
simple, 24, 67–8 transcription, 25
systematic, 25, 26, 27, 89 weak, 29
vowels, 66–8 vowel harmony, 74
transient, 31–2, 34–5, 98, 114, 146, 156–7, vowel quadrilateral, 59, 69, 72
168
trill, 16–17, 24, 89, 156 waveform, 30–1, 32, 33, 34, 35, 98–9, 104,
triphthong, 64, 77 122, 140, 143, 171; see also individual
figures listed on pages viii–x
undershoot, 134–5 weak form, 26–7, 29, 75, 112
uvula (uvular), 10, 15, 93, 134, 154 whisper, 51, 100, 116
words (as linguistic units with phonetic
velarisation, 85, 86, 92, 93, 94, 102–3 implications), 46, 49, 56, 84, 87, 90,
vocal cords, see vocal folds 106, 108, 109, 110, 111, 112, 114,
vocal folds, 9, 15, 28, 31–3, 40–4, 46, 50–3, 130, 144, 145, 146, 151, 155, 163, 170;
102, 104, 120, 167 see also function word
voice quality, 23, 29, 43, 50–3, 54, 108, writing, see spelling
116, 144, 145, 148, 171, 184
voicelessness, 75, 79–80, 81–2, 83–4, 104, yod-dropping, 79