Dialogue Memorization in Language Learning
Dialogue Memorization in Language Learning
Timothy M. HARRIS
Introduction
There are three dichotomies often discussed and debated in the field of second
language acquisition. One of these is the role of input vs. output, with much theory and
research concerning the Input Hypothesis (Krashen, 1981) and Output Hypothesis (Swain,
1985). Another dichotomy, or distinction, is the notion of focus on form (Long, 1991) vs.
exclusive focus on meaning (i.e. no overt attention to form), the latter sometimes found
in certain implementations of communicative language teaching. A third dichotomy
can be found in the debate over whether language learning is primarily conscious or
unconscious, the former exemplified by such concepts as noticing and paying attention
(Schmidt, 1990). The purpose of this article is to explore the usefulness of memorization
of stories and dialogue by second language learners, particularly as it relates to the
three theoretical issues mentioned above. Also discussed is the acquisition of formulaic
speech and its role in the language learning process.
Second language learners are sometimes required to memorize dialogues (or even
narrative passages), either from a textbook or self-written. This author has had personal
experience with this (in university Japanese language courses), and was required to
memorize a number of dialogues, including several co-written and performed in class
with a classmate. In addition, while studying abroad in Germany as an undergraduate,
the experience of enrolling in a theater course and having to memorize lines for a role
in a play, performed over two successive evenings for the local community, proved to be
a very powerful and effective method of language acquisition. In addition to memorizing
and delivering lines, merely participating in the production and interacting with the
other actors contributed noticeably to acquisition and development of fluency.
145
大阪産業大学論集 人文・社会科学編 23
The impetus for this article came from the recent realization, while engaging in self-
directed extensive reading in French in order to improve proficiency, that even in cases
of almost 100% comprehension (and therefore ample comprehensible input), it became
increasingly clear that there were specific forms and structures (preposition choice,
phraseology, etc.) that I did not seem to be "absorbing" adequately for use in subsequent
production. One test of this is that in attempting to retell (aloud to myself ) a story I had
previously read, I was aware that many of the forms to which I had been exposed, even
those well within my developmental level, did not appear in my production. Clearly,
such forms had not yet made the transition from passive recognition ability to active
productive ability. Of course, active ability invariably lags behind passive ability in
second language learning, but in this case, it seemed that there ought to be some way
to speed up the process. Recalling my experiences with memorization years earlier
while learning Japanese and German, the thought occurred to me that I might attempt
to memorize verbatim one of the stories I had read, and thereby "store" the forms in
my memory, within the context of the story. I had been reading Le Petit Nicolas, a book
of short stories from the popular series by René Goscinny, and I selected one story
( Je fréquente Agnan) that I found particularly humorous and which seemed especially
conducive to memorization. It consisted of approximately 1700 words, of which there
were less than twenty that had been entirely unfamiliar to me prior to reading the story.
(There were also a few words whose meaning was made clear by the context, but which
I might not have recognized or remembered out of context.) High school students in
many countries are often required to memorize textual passages in their own language,
and I was no exception, having been required in English class to commit poems to
memory, as well as monologues from Shakespeare's plays. Interestingly, however, few
second language learners are required to memorize passages of such length, and I had
never before attempted to memorize such a large "chunk" of text in a second language.
To my surprise, I was able to accurately memorize the entire story quite quickly and
enjoyably, and I believe that my success was partly due to the manner in which I
approached the task of memorization, making use of a variety of strategies (some overtly
and some unconsciously), which will be explained in detail later. Memorizing the text did
indeed serve the purpose that was intended: in the process of memorizing, I was aware
of noticing (and retaining) specific forms to a much greater degree than when merely
146
Text and Dialogue Memorization in English Language Learning(Timothy M. HARRIS)
One specific example concerned the use of the preposition de ("of ") in the clause
il m'a dit de le suivre ("he told me to follow him"), which is the only French word that
does not have a counterpart in the English translation. (The actual word order of the
French is: "he me has told of him to-follow." The morphological form "has told" equates
semantically to the single English word told, and the French infinitive form of the verb is
a single word encompassing the two-word English infinitive beginning with to.) I realized
that when merely reading, I had not fully noticed this de, probably for two reasons. First,
it was not necessary to the meaning and, as mentioned above, has no counterpart in the
corresponding English. Indeed, there is evidence that, for adult learners, paying attention
to form may be not only helpful but even necessary for acquisition of "redundant and
communicatively less important grammatical features" (Schmidt 1990: 145, 149). Second,
my proficiency in Spanish is much higher than in French, and in Spanish a conjunction
would be used here instead of a preposition: (Él) me dijo que lo siguiera (literally, "he
told me that I [should] follow him"). If I had tried to produce this sentence in French,
I would likely have begun the sentence with il m'a dit que je, with obvious language
transfer from Spanish.
147
大阪産業大学論集 人文・社会科学編 23
The concept of focus on form has been the subject of considerable confusion,
misunderstanding, and controversy, so it is of paramount importance to adhere to a
precise definition. Focus on form, according to Long (1991: 45-46), "overtly draws students'
attention to linguistic elements as they arise incidentally in lessons whose overriding
focus is on meaning or communication" and this incorporates a "fundamental assumption
. . . that meaning and use must already be evident to the learner" prior to attention
being drawn to the form (Doughty & Williams, 1998: 4). Since memorization of a text or
dialogue is generally a solitary task completed by learners outside the classroom and
away from teacher supervision, the word lessons in the definition above can be changed
to activities, and in this case it is not the teacher that draws learners' attention; rather,
it is the task of focusing on the material that performs this function. The French story I
memorized was one that I already understood in its entirety, and the task of memorizing
it was first and foremost a meaning-based activity (albeit not communicative). My
noticing of the use of the preposition de was indeed the result of it arising incidentally in
the course of memorizing the meaning-based content in which it appeared. Memorizing
a text verbatim could be regarded as a form-based activity, but the meaning is of course
primary and the form secondary. In contrast, committing a list of verb conjugations
to memory would be an example of primarily form-based memorization. As will be
elaborated below, one of the most useful strategies for memorizing a story or dialogue is
to first analyze it from a top-down perspective, visualizing and internalizing the general
content and meaning before focusing on the specific words and forms themselves.
148
Text and Dialogue Memorization in English Language Learning(Timothy M. HARRIS)
One potential concern for some learners or educators might be the perception of
"rote" memorization as tedious, mindless, or boring. However, this need not be the case.
A typical approach to rote memorization might be described something like this: learner
looks at text on paper (or computer screen), then away (often while repeating aloud or
silently the chunk of information to be remembered), then back at paper or screen again,
then away, ad infinitum, with little or no variation, in an attempt to gradually get the
text to "stick" in memory. A common activity found in puzzle books requires the solver
to stare at a page for one minute before turning the page and trying to remember as
much as possible about what was there. In the absence of any specific techniques for
memorization, a person will likely stare at the page with furrowed brow, hoping that the
words or images will stick in his or her mind. The "looking at the paper, then away, then
back" approach described above amounts to a repetitious cycle of storing a manageable
amount of information in short-term memory, testing for recall, then confirming accuracy
and storing the next chunk of information. This is repeated until the information
makes the transition to relatively long-term memory. This attempt to mechanically
"put" the material into one's head through sheer sweat, concentration, and repetition
could be considered the "brute force" approach to memorization because it involves
no actual "thinking" or processing—no creativity, no imagination, no analysis, no logic,
no thinking about the material. It consists only of conscious exposure to the material,
mental concentration, and of course repetition, the very nature of which is often tedious.
Individuals have varied success with this approach, and some find memorizing a text
extremely difficult.
A far better investment of time and energy is to use the complementary approaches
of logic/analysis and imagination/visualization to "process" the text before even
beginning to try to remember it. Learners will vary in their preference for one or the
other depending on learning style, but using both is generally preferable, and when one
approach proves insufficient for successful memorization, learners can then rely more on
the other. The basic idea is to not approach the material from the standpoint of trying
to "memorize" it, but instead to focus on leisurely and enjoyably analyzing and examining
149
大阪産業大学論集 人文・社会科学編 23
the content from every conceivable angle without the thought (and accompanying feeling
of pressure) that it has to be remembered. The very process of engaging with the
material, analyzing it, visualizing it (and imagining it with any of the other four senses,
as well as with emotion), literally bathing oneself in it, naturally leads to remembering it.
Some of the material will end up in memory without even actually trying to memorize
it, and when actively trying to store the remainder in memory, the process will be
easier and will require less time and effort. The idea is to enjoy the process of becoming
intimately acquainted with the material and making it into a familiar friend. Fortunately,
it is generally much easier to memorize a story or dialogue than a list of individual
vocabulary words or phrases, due to the cohesive nature of a narrative or conversation.
Each part of the content provides context for other parts of the content, creating what
could be called mutually reinforcing context.
150
Text and Dialogue Memorization in English Language Learning(Timothy M. HARRIS)
bathed in moonlight). They cannot be killed in the conventional sense or even wounded
in the same way as humans (which works to their advantage in battle), but they are
also unable to satisfy their hunger, thirst, or other physical desires, and this insatiability
is a torment to them. Elizabeth also discovers why she was abducted: her blood is one
of the things the pirates need in order to lift the curse, or so they believe at this point.
The scene includes a great deal of dialogue, accounting for over half (59%) of the text
(246 of 415 words), and the remaining 169 words consist of narrative. (Barbossa has
the preponderance of lines, at 204 words to only 42 for Elizabeth.) The content of the
dialogue is concrete, dramatic and interesting, often describing action, and there is also
actual action in the scene. The following example illustrates the degree to which the
language has been simplified from the actual screenplay of the film. Compare this with
the actual lines delivered by Geoffrey Rush in his role as Captain Barbossa in the film.
(Particularly difficult words and expressions are shown in bold, along with non-standard
English usage like the "pirate-speak" use of the verb be.)
graded reader
"We found the gold on the Isla de Muerta," said Barbossa. "We took all of it.
We bought food and drink with it. But then, suddenly, we couldn't eat and
we couldn't drink. When we took the money, Miss Turner, the curse came
with it."
original film
"Find it, we did. There be the chest. Inside be the gold. And we took 'em
all. We spent 'em and traded 'em and frittered 'em away on drink and food
and pleasurable company. The more we gave 'em away, the more we came
to realize . . . the drink would not satisfy, food turned to ash in our mouths,
and all the pleasurable company in the world could not slake our lust. We
are cursed men, Miss Turner. Compelled by greed, we were, but now . . .
we are consumed by it." (transcribed during a viewing of the film)
151
大阪産業大学論集 人文・社会科学編 23
(analogous to cutting food into smaller, more manageable bites), and allows one to see
at a glance which sentences are longer and which are shorter. Of course, this is easily
accomplished if the text is stored in a computer document. If not, then scanning it into
electronic form using optical character recognition software (and "cleaning up" the text
by correcting any scan-related errors) may be faster than retyping the text, depending
on its length.
In this case, there are also three distinct types of sentences: narration, lines uttered
by Barbossa, and lines uttered by Elizabeth. A simple method for making these three
types easier to distinguish at a glance is to make them different colors by, for example,
marking them with highlighter pens. (If the text is stored in a computer document, the
color of the actual text can be changed directly on the computer.) Since this article is not
printed in color, the three types will be distinguished here as follows: Barbossa's lines in
bold with underlining, Elizabeth's lines in bold italics, and narration as normal text.
Here is how the first fifteen sentences appear in their original format in the book:
Elizabeth sat at a table on the Black Pearl. There was a lot of food
on the table—bread, fruit, and meat. Captain Barbossa sat at the
other end of the table.
"Are you hungry?" he said. "Please eat."
Elizabeth was very hungry. She took some bread and some meat
and started to eat.
"Have a drink," said Barbossa.
Elizabeth drank. Then she looked at the captain.
"You're not eating!" she said. "Is something wrong with the
food? Are you trying to kill me? You eat it!"
She gave the captain some bread, but he didn't take it.
152
Text and Dialogue Memorization in English Language Learning(Timothy M. HARRIS)
General tips for memorization enumerated by Winter & Winter (1997: 102-105)
include (1) those concerning the material to be remembered, such as: organization of
information, breaking things into smaller chunks, dividing into categories of related
items, making associations, and use of cadence (e.g. music, rhyme), as well as (2) one's
approach to memorization: intention to remember, paying attention (entailing sufficient
study breaks to maintain concentration), using all five senses (including visualization),
speaking information to be remembered aloud, and exercising memorization ability
on a daily basis in order to improve one's memory over time. Regarding "intention to
remember," we tend to remember what we are interested in and what is important
to us, so cultivating these feelings is helpful. As many of us have likely observed,
"the driver who must concentrate on the . . . road will remember [the route] better
than the passenger . . ." (p.116). Winter & Winter do not specifically mention rhythm
in their reference to cadence, but as Crystal (2008: 129) observes, in his discussion of
Shakespeare's writing, a "steady recurring rhythm makes it easy to memorise . . . lines."
In particular, "a line of iambic pentameter is within an easy memory span." Like a good
filing system, there are two goals of successful memorization, involving the two aspects
of memory (i.e. storage and recall): storing information in memory as reliably and
unshakably as possible, and making it as easy as possible to retrieve quickly.
153
大阪産業大学論集 人文・社会科学編 23
The following systematic approach for analyzing a text for memorization was
created on the basis of insights gained from my small experiment with the French story,
as well as an analysis of the English text used here for illustrative purposes. Although
I do not find the systematic application of most of these techniques necessary when
memorizing, I am aware of employing some of them some of the time, even on a more
or less unconscious, "instinctive" level, and my goal was to develop an example of a
comprehensive approach that could be taught to students and from which they could
pick and choose what works for them.
The system outlined here involves approaching the text from two opposing angles
or perspectives: (1) top-down, involving examination of the overall content, transcending
the specific words or language used, and (2) bottom-up, involving careful analysis of the
lexical level and how the individual words combine to create meaning. Beginning from a
top-down perspective, it is helpful to put oneself in the position of the screenwriter and
each of the actors (or the author and each of the protagonists, in the case of a narrative),
and to ask the following key questions:
In the original film (and therefore in this adaptation), there is a need to explain to
the audience about the gold, the curse, and why the pirates have abducted Elizabeth.
This could be done through a narrative voiceover, but that would be unimaginative and
less interesting. Doing it this way is more dramatic, and involves a typical narrative
device: by Barbossa explaining it to Elizabeth, he is indirectly explaining it to the
audience. Another point of interest is how he begins his explanation. He could just sit
Elizabeth down and say, "Okay, here's the situation," but again, that is far less dramatic
and interesting. The whole purpose of the food scene is to give Barbossa a reason to
explain about the curse, and if we keep that in mind, it makes the memorization much
easier. The food scene naturally, logically, and inexorably leads into the explanation
of the gold and accompanying curse. Barbossa offers food to Elizabeth, who eats, but
Barbossa himself does not eat, to which the suspicious Elizabeth emotionally demands
154
Text and Dialogue Memorization in English Language Learning(Timothy M. HARRIS)
an explanation. Rather than immediately explaining himself and resolving the suspense,
Barbossa mysteriously pulls a gold medallion from his coat and starts the dramatic
delivery of his story with this prop, only eventually explaining why Elizabeth was taken
prisoner, thereby maintaining the suspense (and the interest of the audience) until then.
The final "reveal" (that the entire crew of the Black Pearl are "undead" who appear as
skeletons when bathed in moonlight) is saved for the very end. Regarding the actors'
motivations and emotions, Elizabeth is angry, defiant, frightened, suspicious, hungry,
curious, and wants to escape the pirates' clutches if she can. Barbossa is alternately
unhappy then happy (as explicitly stated in the text), menacing, chronically deprived of
the pleasure of eating and drinking (and bitter regarding the curse on himself and his
men), triumphant and gloating at having captured Elizabeth (and lording this over her),
and he ultimately wants Elizabeth's blood in order to free himself and his crew of the
curse. It is also helpful to vividly visualize the scene from the actors' points of view,
incorporating imagined sensations of the other four of the five senses whenever possible
—in other words, to actually be in the scene.
The following is one way to systematically approach a text from both top-down and
bottom-up perspectives, respectively.
TOP-DOWN
First, read through the entire text, noticing three things, each of which facilitates
memorization:
1 . Main idea of text, purpose of scene (already discussed above)
2 . Sequence: in what order are events (or topics of dialogue) presented, and more
importantly, why?
3 . Flow: this goes beyond sequence—a shopping list has a sequence, but not normally a
connected flow. How are the events (or topics of dialogue) connected?
The next three steps involve the concept of "divide and conquer," starting at the
highest level and chunking down into smaller parts. The goal here is to treat the
overall structure of the text or dialogue as a framework onto which to "hang" the actual
sentences, and to establish "quantifiable parameters" (i.e. number of sections, number of
155
大阪産業大学論集 人文・社会科学編 23
1 . Start with the main idea of the entire text (as above), which transcends the specific
words, meaning that it can be paraphrased in different words, and can even be
expressed in the L1. The focus here, as mentioned earlier, is solely on meaning, not
on form.
2 . Establish the structure: break the text into manageable sections and determine
the purpose or main idea of each. Remembering the number of sections creates a
quantified target: each section is like a box waiting to be filled with content from
memory, but to do this one must first know how many boxes there are. It is a good
idea to memorize the general content of each section (again, independent of the
actual wording) and quiz oneself on it. Remembering the number of sentences in
each section is not normally necessary or realistic, but can be helpful in particular
cases. However, noticing and having a general awareness of the varying length of
sections (in terms of number and length of sentences) can be helpful.
3 . Analyze each sentence for general content and main idea, optionally represented
by one or more appropriate keywords from each sentence, which establishes a
connection with the lexical level. How does each sentence contribute to the section
as a whole? Quantifying can sometimes be helpful here, as well: How many words in
a sentence? Are words long or short? Again, the key is actively noticing.
BOTTOM-UP
Here, sections and sentences are analyzed at the lexical (and grammatical) level.
1 . Examine each section and sentence for any patterns, common threads, coincidences,
symmetries, asymmetries, "sandwiches" (explained later), lists, rhymes, rhythms, and
any other serendipitously "built-in" mnemonic aids.
2 . As with the top-down level, look at sequence and flow, this time on a "micro" level:
How does each sentence connect to the next? How does one topic segue to the
156
Text and Dialogue Memorization in English Language Learning(Timothy M. HARRIS)
next? The idea here is to identify or create logical connections and associations
between each sentence. Ideally, through a chain of associations, each sentence
should automatically stimulate recall of the next, greatly reducing the burden of
remembering: "Speaking of X, what logically or naturally comes next?"
4 . For each sentence, identify concrete core words (not necessarily the same "keywords"
as earlier, however). For example, this is the 24th sentence in the text: We bought
food and drink with it. Probably the best keyword from a top-down perspective is
bought, which expresses the general idea that the pirates spent the gold, but the
most concrete core words from a bottom-up perspective are food and drink (i.e.
what was bought), or more appropriately, the common and useful single collocation
food and drink. Of course, pronouns, conjunctions, prepositions, and interjections will
generally be excluded when identifying the most concrete words.
5 . These "core" words should then ideally stimulate recall of prepositions, articles, and
other "filler" words (the "mortar" between the "bricks"), with syntactic relationships
logically dictating word choice as much as possible, again reducing the burden on
memory.
6 . As an optional step, it can also be helpful to note the first word(s) of each sentence,
which can serve to jog the memory, analogous to the first bars or notes of a song.
7 . Fine-tuning: extra attention should be given to any parts that are not intuitive,
logical, or natural, and are therefore harder to remember. Here again, quantifying
can be helpful: counting words provides a target number against which to check
recall. Acronyms can even be created from the respective first letters of a sequence
of words in order to create a more compact bundle for memorization, which can then
serve as a prompt for the full sequence. As with counting words, the letters of the
157
大阪産業大学論集 人文・社会科学編 23
Regarding step 3 above, grouping words in this way provides an ideal opportunity to
notice and memorize formulaic language, referred to by a bewildering variety of names:
formulae, prefabs, patterns, sequences, lexical strings, chunks, and clusters. Included
in this more general category of recurring word combinations are more specific types,
such as collocations, idioms, and frames. There is a broad consensus (Ellis, 1996; Cowie,
1992; Howarth, 1998) that such formulaic language is one of the primary factors that
distinguishes native-like usage. While cautioning against spending a disproportionate
amount of classroom time on formulaic chunks and expecting all students to strive
for native-like proficiency, Swan (2006) agrees that it is advisable for learners to "pay
attention to and memorise instances of formulaic language," and this is precisely
what memorization of texts allows for. Moreover, all of the formulaic sequences are
memorized in meaningful context, which is certainly far more effective than learning
them in isolation, say, in a list. Sequences like "a lot (of )" are best treated as if they
were actually one word, and memorized accordingly. Regarding the notion of a "word,"
Pinker (1994: 148) observes that in addition to "a linguistic object that . . . behaves as
the indivisible, smallest unit with respect to the rules of syntax" (i.e. the conventional
notion of a "word"), there is also a very different definition, namely a "rote-memorized
chunk: a string of linguistic stuff that is arbitrarily associated with a particular meaning,
one item from the long list we call the mental dictionary." This is echoed by Swan
(2006): "Researchers differ in their analysis and classification of formulaic language, and
the storage and processing models they propose . . . . It is, however, generally agreed
that these chunks behave more like individual words than like separately constructed
sequences."
It will now be demonstrated how the general strategies described above can be
specifically applied to the text chosen here for illustrative purposes. In this case, the text
158
Text and Dialogue Memorization in English Language Learning(Timothy M. HARRIS)
Longer sections can be broken up into even smaller sections. For example, the
longest section (number 4) can be divided in half, the first five sentences discussing the
gold, Aztecs, and curse (less personal), and the last five sentences concerning the crew
of the Black Pearl (more personal: the subject of each sentence is "we"). The first half
provides general background information, and the second half clarifies how the pirates
are directly involved. Section 4 can even be further subdivided, into a total of five
subsections:
159
大阪産業大学論集 人文・社会科学編 23
Section 1 will be used here to illustrate the application of bottom-up strategies at the
lexical level. Even a section of text as short as this (the shortest of the eight sections) is
rich with possibilities for analysis and development of mnemonic aids.
A careful look at this first section immediately reveals a common thread running
through all three sentences and tying them together: the word table. Elizabeth is at one
end, Barbossa is at the other, and the food is between them. This provides a powerful
visual image for the imagination and the first step is to take a moment to vividly
internalize it. Moreover, there is a natural flow here from one end of the table to the
other, as though a movie camera is panning from Elizabeth to Barbossa, so this is how
the visual image should be committed to memory. If this "panning" image is vivid and
solid in the memory, the text will naturally and logically follow.
Carrying the analysis deeper, there is a natural symmetry present in the actual
wording employed in this group of three sentences. The first and last sentences begin
with parallel words and syntax, both involving people (the only two characters in the
scene): "Elizabeth sat at . . . table . . ." and "Captain Barbossa sat at . . . table." There is
exactly one sentence between these two, involving things rather than people, creating
what could be labeled a sentence "sandwich": two similar, parallel pieces of "bread"
with something different in between. Moreover, the middle sentence appropriately and
conveniently happens to describe the "middle" of the table, creating a link between text
and visual image. The fact that food is what comes in the middle of the sandwich is
160
Text and Dialogue Memorization in English Language Learning(Timothy M. HARRIS)
161
大阪産業大学論集 人文・社会科学編 23
"breadfruit meat."
Imagination and visualization are also especially effective when memorizing lists.
Rather than merely trying to remember the words, the key is to vividly picture the
items and to make them as concrete, specific, and detailed as possible. What kind of
bread? What shape? How much? What kind of fruit and meat? What kind of dishes are
they placed on? Taking a moment to decide these details and visualize them is a much
better time investment than repeatedly reviewing and quizzing oneself on the words
alone. A small, simple list like this one can usually be easily and quickly memorized
without resorting to any mnemonic techniques, but using detailed visualization promotes
faster recall and is likely to result in more enduring long-term memory. This can also
be combined with the acronym technique described above, by choosing a "backronym"
that provides more detailed, concrete images for visualization. For example, "baguettes,
figs, and mutton" is much more specific than "bread, fruit, and meat." When visualizing,
it is also helpful to make images exaggerated, grotesque, absurd, taboo, and otherwise
"unforgettable."
Returning to the three sentences, the next step in the analysis is to identify
meaningful "word groups," which are best memorized as chunks, enclosed in brackets
below and hereafter. There is obviously some degree of variation in the way the words
can be grouped, but collocations and common frames are best memorized as groups in
order to facilitate acquisition. Ultimately, however, the ideal criterion for grouping words
is whatever makes them easiest to memorize.
The list of food has already been discussed, and Captain Barbossa is of course
a name. The remaining word groups are primarily prepositional phrases, including
three contrasting phrases with table and at, on, of, respectively. There is/are/was/were
functions as a single lexical unit, and a lot of . . . and the other end are best treated as
162
Text and Dialogue Memorization in English Language Learning(Timothy M. HARRIS)
single chunks, rather than as individual words. Two of the chunks are further embedded
in a larger chunk (at the other end of the . . .), worth memorizing verbatim for its own
sake, since it is such a commonly occurring phrase. It is also important to account for
each occurrence of definite or indefinite article, as was done with the word table earlier.
Anything that does not make logical sense will be more arbitrary and therefore more
difficult to retain in memory. In this case, there is only one ship called the Black Pearl,
there is only one other end of the table, and the presence of both the ship and the table
have already been established in the story. Thus, the definite article is the logical choice.
In contrast, a lot (of) functions as a single formulaic unit ("a-lotta" in rapid speech), so the
use of the indefinite article is fixed. The nouns bread, fruit, and meat are all uncountable
here and mentioned for the first time, so of course no articles are used.
The next step is to identify concrete core words, which of course entails eliminating
all pronouns, prepositions, articles, etc. Proper nouns can also be excluded, since they are
arbitrary labels, and we can therefore temporarily substitute the concrete noun ship for
the proper noun Black Pearl. Again, a lot of is treated here as one "word," the core of
which is lot. This reduces the three sentences to the following skeletal outline:
These core words are all captured in the visual image created. The scene logically
begins with the story's heroine, Elizabeth, and where she is: sitting at a table on a ship.
The camera pans across, showing food on the table—a lot. Finally, at the (other) end
of the table sits the only other character present in the scene: Barbossa. The other
"filler" words can be easily plugged in once the core words are remembered. Who sat?
Elizabeth. She sat at a table. Where? On a ship—the ship already named in the story:
on the Black Pearl. What does a table remind one of? Food. Our internal movie camera
continues to pan across the table: There was a lot of food on the table. The sentence
logically begins with there was, and the remaining words on the are logically determined
by the grammar of the sentence. Finally, the "sandwich" is completed with the only
163
大阪産業大学論集 人文・社会科学編 23
other person present, Captain Barbossa, who "sat at the other end of the table." The only
word not determined by the grammar of this sentence is other, but again, the other end
is most appropriately treated and memorized as a single lexical chunk. As discussed
earlier, the memorization of these formulaic sequences contributes to learners' language
acquisition and development of fluency, and memorizing them in context in this way is
easier, more meaningful, and likely more enjoyable than, for example, studying a list of
such expressions.
An optional step, in the interest of approaching the text from every conceivable
angle, even to the point of overkill, is to notice how each sentence begins. This can serve
as yet another way to jog the memory, analogous to the opening bars or lyrics of a song.
Elizabeth . . .
There was . . .
Captain Barbossa . . .
This recalls the "sandwich" described earlier. The first and last sentence both begin
with names (again, the only two people present), and the middle sentence announces the
existence of something, which turns out to be food.
Finally, a connection must be forged between this section and the next section of the
text, the theme of which is Elizabeth eating and drinking. The scene has been set, with
a great quantity of food on the table, so eating is the next logical step, and with eating
comes drinking. Always keeping in mind the purpose of the scene and each section in
it, as well as the motivation of the actors, supplies a momentum that propels the scene
naturally forward. The eating/drinking scene is vital in order to move on to the real
purpose of the scene, namely the explanation of the gold and the curse.
Conclusions
164
Text and Dialogue Memorization in English Language Learning(Timothy M. HARRIS)
165
大阪産業大学論集 人文・社会科学編 23
precisely because they are retrieved from memory as whole chunks. Wood (2007), in
particular, has found evidence for the use of formulaic sequences contributing to fluency
in the speech of Japanese learners of English.
The focus of this article is primarily on the act of memorizing (i.e. "input"), but there
is also the other side of the process: the act of recall or "output." Learners will of course
engage in ongoing self-quizzing as a natural part of the memorization process until the
entire content is memorized, after which each act of recall of part or all of the text
will serve as a method of reviewing the material and retaining it in long-term memory.
There are three forms this output can take: (1) reciting aloud, (2) writing out the text,
and (3) reciting it silently (i.e. in one's head). Reciting aloud is preferable, partly because
it exercises the speech organs, but also because it involves more of the brain. "When we
read aloud . . . we use very different brain circuits than when we read silently. One of
the earliest demonstrations of brain imaging clearly showed three distinct brain regions
lighting up when the same word was read, spoken, or heard." Specifically, it was found
that "speaking words activated the motor cortex on both sides of the brain as well as . . .
the cerebellum. Just looking at words activated only one area of the cortex in the left
hemisphere." (Katz & Rubin, 1999: 50). The advantage of writing out the text, even if
only once, is that this written "record" allows for better checking of accuracy through
comparison with the original text after the fact. The only practical option for confirming
accuracy when reciting (aside from being quizzed by someone, which requires a willing
partner) is to cover the original text and check one's recall against it while uncovering
it a bit at a time, which is cumbersome and carries the risk of prematurely seeing bits
of text that one has not yet tested recall of. It is also very easy to accidentally overlook
deviations from the original text. The least desirable option is "reciting" in one's head,
but of course the advantage of this method is that it can be done anytime, anywhere,
even in public.
This type of output, in all three cases, clearly differs from the communicative type
described in the Output Hypothesis (Swain, 1985) in that the learner is not forming
original utterances and testing hypotheses. However, it is still language "production" and
performance, albeit in a different sense. Improvement in fluency, resulting from practice,
166
Text and Dialogue Memorization in English Language Learning(Timothy M. HARRIS)
is one distinct benefit of output (Swain and Lapkin, 1995), and surely this is not limited
solely to self-generated output. Fluency in speaking is clearly one skill that cannot be
developed through comprehensible input alone. Moreover, this "output" of memorized
text need not be confined to the original narration or lines of dialogue. Once the material
is successfully memorized and internalized, ideally to the point where it can be recited
with no effort and has become second nature, the learner can ad-lib and improvise freely,
just as actors often do, by plugging in different words, adapting the material in creative
ways, and personalizing it.
Most individuals outside the acting profession have never had the experience of
having to "learn one's lines," but imagine for a moment if learners were to memorize
even a fraction of the amount of text that many professional actors routinely do, and
were to do it in their target language. How much of that language is then processed by
the brain and truly acquired, and whether general rules are abstracted from the raw
data, are questions to be answered by much-needed further research, but surely having
all those chunks of authentic, prefabricated (and grammatically accurate) language stored
167
大阪産業大学論集 人文・社会科学編 23
in memory is preferable to not having them in one's head at all. Part of performance
in a second language is indeed performing, and there is broad agreement on the value
of acting and role-playing in language learning. Reciting memorized language is no
substitute for original, self-initiated output, but such "performing" may contribute to
language acquisition in ways we have yet to discover. Playing scales on a piano usually
involves no creative or original "output," in contrast to improvisational playing of music
("communicative" in a sense), but scales can play a vital role in development of piano-
playing ability, and in ways that go beyond merely exercising the physical muscles
involved. It is certainly true that text memorization is not a communicative activity, and
it is doubtful that anyone would recommend spending an inordinate amount of time on it,
especially at the expense of other activities. However, there would appear to be a useful
role for some memorization of stories and dialogues in language learning (particularly
self-learning), if approached in an interesting and creative way.
168
Text and Dialogue Memorization in English Language Learning(Timothy M. HARRIS)
Appendix A
SECTION 4:
EXPLANATION OF GOLD & CURSE (THE "BAD NEWS") (10 sentences)
He took the gold medallion from his coat. "This gold, Miss Turner, is very old. The
Aztecs gave it to Cortes when he arrived in the Americas. There are many, many
more of these. And the Aztecs put a curse on them." "We found the gold on the Isla
de Muerta," said Barbossa. "We took all of it. We bought food and drink with it. But
then, suddenly, we couldn't eat and we couldn't drink. When we took the money,
Miss Turner, the curse came with it."
169
大阪産業大学論集 人文・社会科学編 23
you have all of it," Elizabeth said. "Yes. With this gold medallion, we have all of it.
Thank you."
170
Text and Dialogue Memorization in English Language Learning(Timothy M. HARRIS)
REFERENCES
Doughty, C. and J. Williams (eds). (1998). Focus on Form in Classroom Second Language
Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Ellis N. (1996). 'Sequencing in SLA: Phonological memory, chunking, and points of order.'
Studies in Second Language Acquisition 18: 91-126.
Katz, L. C. and M. Rubin (1999). Keep Your Brain Alive. New York: Workman Publishing
Company.
Krashen, S. (1981). Second Language Acquisition and Second Language Learning. Oxford:
Pergamon Press.
Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct. New York: William Morrow and Co.
171
大阪産業大学論集 人文・社会科学編 23
Skehan, P. (1995). 'Analyzability, accessibility, and ability for use' in G. Cook and B.
Seidlhofer (eds.): Principles and Practice in Applied Linguistics. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Swain, M. (1991). 'French immersion and its offshoots: Getting two for one' in B. Freed:
Foreign language acquisition research and the classroom. Lexington, MA: Heath.
Swain, M. and S. Lapkin (1995). 'Problems in output and the cognitive processes they
generate: A step towards second language learning. Applied Linguistics 16: 371-391.
Swan, M. (2006). 'Chunks in the classroom: let's not go overboard.' The Teacher Trainer
20/3: 5-6.
Trimble, I. (adapted by), D. Eastment (retold by), T. Elliot and T. Rossio (based on
screenplay by), A. Hopkins and J. Potter (series editors) (2007). Pirates of the
Caribbean: The Curse of the Black Pearl (Penguin Readers, Level 2), (Based on
Walt Disney's Pirates of the Caribbean). Harlow/Essex: Pearson Education Limited.
Winter, A. and R. Winter (1997). Brain Workout: Easy Ways to Power Up Your Memory,
Sensory Perception, and Intelligence. New York: St. Martin's Press.
172