The Halliday Centre Series in Appliable
The Halliday Centre Series in Appliable
The Halliday Centre Series in Appliable
J R Martin
Professor of Linguistics, University of Sydney
Christian M I M Matthiessen
Chair Professor of English, The Hong Kong Polytechnic University
Clare Painter
Honorary Research Associate, University of Sydney
ISBN 978-7-100-06851-2
功 能 语 法 教 程
〔澳〕 詹姆斯·R·马丁
〔瑞典〕克里斯琴·M I M·马西森
〔澳〕克莱尔·佩因特 著
商 务 印 书 馆
2010 年·北京
图书在版编目(CIP)数据
功能语法教程:英文/(澳)马丁等著.—北京: 商务印书馆,
2009
ISBN 978-7-100-06851-2
I.功… Ⅱ.马… Ⅲ.语法学-教材-英文 Ⅳ.H04
中国版本图书馆 CIP 数据核字(2009)第 207520 号
所有权利保留。
未经许可,不得以任何方式使用。
功 能 语 法 教 程
〔澳〕马丁 〔瑞典〕马西森 〔澳〕佩因特 著
商 务 印 书 馆 出 版
(北京王府井大街 36 号 邮政编码 100710)
商 务 印 书 馆 发 行
印刷
ISBN 978-7-100-06851-2
This workbook is the culmination of a project initiated in the early 1980s by Jim Martin
and Clare Painter, who were joined in the endeavour by Christian Matthiessen in 1988.
We three were all concerned to provide support materials for students and colleagues
who were learning the analyses proposed in Halliday's Introduction to Functional
Grammar. In particular we shared the experience of guiding students through familiar
kinds of difficulties year after year – and wanted to prepare materials which would help
learners through and around these experiences. We also wanted to provide some
support for people learning the grammar on their own, or without the dialogue
provided by a critical mass of systemic linguists such as we enjoy in the Sydney
metropolitan region.
Since the publication of the first workbook titled Working with Functional
Grammar in 1997, we have had the opportunity to use it with students, and to receive
feedback from many colleagues around the world. It became clear to us that we needed
to make a number of corrections and in some cases provide less confusing examples.
We were also keen to include a chapter on group and phrase analysis, which for
reasons of space was excluded from the first edition; and we also wanted to interface
the grammar analyses considered here with discourse semantic systems, so that their
complementary roles in text analysis could be exemplified.
When negotiations with the original publisher along these lines broke down, we
secured the rights for the volume, and began exploring publication elsewhere. The
Commercial Press has generously allowed us the additional scope we needed to amend
the first edition with two additional chapters. We are delighted to be publishing this
second edition in China, where so much interest in functional grammar has grown.
Since IFG2 is still in print in China, we have accordingly cross-referenced this edition to
both the second and third editions of IFG.
We are of course much indebted to the many colleagues who have discussed
aspects of these materials with us, to the many tutors who have dealt first-hand with
problems, and to the now thousands of students who have committed at least a hundred
hours of their lives each to learning functional grammar in our undergraduate
linguistics and MA applied linguistics programs at the University of Sydney, Macquarie
University, the University of Technology Sydney and the University of New South Wales.
Halliday's presentation of the grammar in IFG was designed for use in text analysis,
and this is the main context in which we have taught the material. Our experience of
this context is that nothing is more challenging to one's understanding of grammar and
the theory from which it derives than helping students apply it to texts of their choosing.
So much happens that intuitions don't intuit, that argumentation doesn't argue about,
that explicitness in computer generation elides! It's certainly driven us crazy over the
years and has been just wonderful too. We hope this workbook will help keep you and
your students somewhat sane as you explore the wonders of the language people really
use.
ii PREFACE
The authors are grateful to David Caldwell, Shooshi Dreyfus, Sue Hood, Sally
Humphrey and Michele Zappavigna for help with some final proofing. In addition we
are indebted to Joey Wong and Housheng Qian for their tremendous support in editing.
Finally, to disengage the 'we' of this preface, Christian and Jim would like to thank
Clare, who alongside her fair share of writing and editing had to take a set of more
rough than ready notes, and a pair of more distracted than reliable colleagues, and
re/organise them so that this project could be completed. Scaffolding this joint
construction above and beyond the call of duty – as ever. Bravo Clare!
Contents
Preface i
Chapter 1 Introduction 1
1. What is functional grammar? 1
2. Why this functional grammar? 2
3. Using the workbook 3
4. Getting started with text analysis 5
5. Constituency: functional and class units 7
6. Reasoning in a functional grammar 10
7. Paradigms as system networks 13
8. Choice and constituency 16
9. Learning to use IFG 19
References 299
Index 303
Chapter 1
Introduction
1 Gee includes an appendix comprising a very concise 11-page synopsis of Halliday's functional grammar.
2 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
analysis of culturally significant texts (e.g. Thibault 1991). It has been used in
institutions of healthcare for purposes of diagnosis and therapy in speech pathology
(Armstrong et al. 2005) and other language disorders (Fine 2006) and for the role
language plays in other healthcare practices such as nursing (Kealley 2007),
counselling (Butt et al. 2003, Muntigl 2004) and emergency care (Slade et al. in press).
It has also been used for text analysis in forensic settings (Martin et al. in press), for
interpretation of classroom discourse (Christie 2002), and in translation studies
(Steiner & Yallop 2001, Steiner 2005). The range of applications is growing all the time,
as illustrated in Hasan et al. 2005, 2007; see also Matthiessen (in press). One way to
get in touch with people using functional grammar in your area of interest would be to
make use of e-mail, and join one of the SFL discussion lists. For information on these
go to www.isfla.org/systemics.
There are many possible purposes for writing functional grammars, and different
contexts will have somewhat different requirements. For example, a grammar for
teachers and school students would not assume the same shared technical
understandings as a grammar for the professional linguist, a grammar for
computational contexts would need to be particularly explicit about the relation
between system and structure to facilitate automatic text generation and parsing,
while a grammar for critical discourse analysis would need to include considerable
social contextual information to facilitate informed text deconstruction. Halliday’s
main purpose in writing the IFG grammars, however, was not to orient the grammar to
any single defined area of application, but to provide a general grammar for purposes
of text analysis and interpretation. It is therefore a grammar which provides a basic
‘lingua franca’ for text analysts working in a wide range of differing contexts; and it is
very effectively organised as a tool of this kind.
You will find the IFG grammar much richer semantically than either formal
grammar or traditional school grammar. This makes the analyses you undertake more
insightful when it comes to interpreting a text. It will tell you more about a student’s
writing abilities, and more about what is semantically at stake in text generation, and
more about discourse and subjectivity than any other grammar available today. The
grammar has also been designed to be relatively easy to apply to texts. We have found
that between 26 and 39 hours of lectures 2, plus tutorial support, are enough to give
most students in undergraduate or MA programs a working knowledge of the
grammar. This workbook is designed to support introductory programs of this kind,
and to provide some scope for users to work more deeply into the grammar and its
application to texts.
Like traditional school grammar and formal grammar, you will find that the IFG
grammar makes use of class labels like noun, verb or adjective, with which you will
have some familiarity. Beyond this, the IFG grammar makes extensive use of function
labels like Actor, Process, Goal, Theme, Rheme, Deictic, Numerative, Classifier, Thing
and so on, many of which will be new to you. The function labels are there to make the
grammatical analysis semantically revealing – to show how the clauses, groups and
phrases constituting a text map its meanings. Without the function labels, the
2 We have found it possible to compress these lectures into a single week of full-time intensive study, or to
spread them out over one semester or two semester's work.
CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION 3
This workbook has largely been organised to reflect the organisation of Halliday’s IFG,
both the second and third editions (it can also be used with more basic introductions
such as Thompson 2004). Chapters in Deploying Functional Grammar support IFG
chapters as follows:
IFG CHAPTER IFG CHAPTER
WORKBOOK CHAPTER
(2ND EDITION) (3RD EDITION)
1 Introduction Introduction
1 Constituency 1 The architecture of language
2 Towards a functional grammar 2 Towards a functional grammar
2 Theme 3 Clause as message 3 Clause as message
3 Mood 4 Clause as exchange 4 Clause as exchange
4 Transitivity 5 Clause as representation 5 Clause as representation
5 Group and Phrase 6 Below the clause: groups and 6 Below the clause: groups and
phrases phrases
8 Group and phrase complexes
6 Clause complex 7 Above the clause: the clause 7 Above the clause: the clause
complex complex
7 Beyond the clause 9 Around the clause: cohesion 9 Around the clause: cohesion and
and discourse discourse
4 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
We have, however, written the workbook in such a way that the chapters need not
necessarily be used in this order. Moreover the final chapter indicates some of the
limits to a text analysis based simply on grammar and explores the role of discourse
semantics in text analysis and interpretation. This chapter is intended to serve only as
an introduction and more detailed discussion and explanation can be found in Martin
and Rose (2003/2007).
Each of Chapters 2-6 contains the following sections: Orientation, Survey of
Options, Troubleshooting, Exercises, Texts for Analysis, Review & Contextualisation and
Further Reading. Below we outline very briefly the nature of each of these sections.
Orientation: We begin each Chapter by referencing the relevant sections of the
most recent editions of IFG and providing a brief characterisation of the aspect of
grammar to be covered.
Survey of Options: This section provides an outline summary of the principal
grammatical options described in detail in IFG. No knowledge of the metalanguage
built up in other chapters is assumed in this opening description.
Troubleshooting: In this part we aim to sort out misunderstandings, queries and
issues that may arise when you apply the particular analysis being learned. We intend
it to be used as a reference that you can access when you have difficulty with any of
the exercises or analysis tasks, rather as than preliminary material to read straight
through before you start.
Exercises: Exercises provide practice in working with individual clauses before
complete texts are attempted, and at various points, you are referred to the relevant
part of the Troubleshooting section for help. The exercises, like the texts for analysis,
are organised into three phases in increasing order of difficulty. Phase 1 is for getting
started; Phase 2 is designed to get you to the point where you can have a go at
analysing a text on your own; Phase 3 takes you further into some more problematic
issues, points that arise less frequently, or tend to occur in more difficult texts.
Texts for Analysis: Like the exercises, these are presented in three phases. The
Phase 1 texts can be attempted after Phase 1 exercises and are quite simple, having
been edited where necessary to remove problems. By Phase 3 the texts are more
difficult and have not been adapted in any way.
Review & Contextualisation: This section provides a summary of the relevant
aspect of the grammar, drawing on the metalanguage learned in previous chapters and
showing where that part of the grammar fits into the overall picture. This section puts
the various aspects of the grammar into relation with one another and is partly
designed for those who have some knowledge or experience of the IFG grammar. It can
therefore be used as the principal summary for those revising their understanding of
IFG or as an extension for those moving through the Workbook, taking the chapters in
order.
Further Reading: At the end of Chapters 2-6, we provide a short bibliography of
further reading related to the relevant aspect of the grammar.
Key: Users can download the keys to the exercises in this book from:
http://www.hallidaycentre.cityu.edu.hk/Collection/DFG/KeysToExercises.zip
In designing the workbook we have aimed for enough flexibility to serve the needs
of a range of readers, whether working alone or as part of a formal program of study.
For example, if you are not a linguist and you are currently undertaking only a brief
introductory study in functional grammar, you may go through the workbook doing
only Phase 1 Exercises and Texts. On the other hand, if you have some experience with
grammatical analysis or are involved in a course of study that aims at independent text
CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION 5
analysis, you may focus your attention on Phase 2 Exercises and Texts. The book also
aims to cater for those who are extending or revising their understandings of IFG and
who may prefer to begin with the Review & Contextualisation section of each chapter
and concentrate on analysing Phase 2 and Phase 3 texts. At whatever level you are
working, you will find some parts of the Troubleshooting section helpful.
The first problem you will face when using functional grammar for text analysis is the
problem of what to analyse. How do you divide the text into pieces that the IFG
analyses can be applied to? Thankfully, most texts offer some help.
If you are dealing with a written text (like this one you are reading), then it will
probably have been divided into orthographic sentences for you. These units
(sentences) are orthographic or graphological – units of the writing system of English.
They begin with a capital letter and end with a major punctuation mark (a full stop, a
question mark or an exclamation mark). Even though they are graphological units
rather than grammatical ones, they are a good starting point since there is a
reasonable correspondence between the orthographic sentence in the graphology and
the clause complex in the grammar (see IFG 3, p. 16). There is a useful description of
punctuation in relation to functional grammar in Halliday 1985/1989: 34-39. If you
are dealing with a spoken text, then you will need to transcribe it before you start
analysing it (unless you already have a transcription of it). Transcribing a spoken text
is time-consuming but quite fascinating and instructive; the transcription itself
involves analytical decisions, including decisions about how to punctuate the text.
While there are a number of technical transcription systems that have been designed
for particular tasks, you will often find that the best starting point is to transcribe a
spoken text in the way speech is represented by novelists in dramatic dialogue. If the
spoken text is a dialogue, then it will involve a number of interactants, who take turns
at speaking. These units (turns), beginning where one speaker starts talking and
ending where they stop, are another good starting point. Of course, neither of these
strategies will work for spoken monologue – in which case you could try punctuating
the text on your own, dividing it into sentences as you go.
The most important unit for a functional grammar analysis is the clause rather
than the sentence, however, and to identify clauses, you have to start using what you
know about grammar to divide up the text. Basically, there are three ways in.
One way is what Halliday would call ideational. This involves looking for the
processes in a text – processes name events taking place (‘go’, ‘cook’, ‘think’, ‘sleep’ and
so on) or relationships among things (‘is’, ‘seems’, ‘has’ and so on). Then you divide the
text up into processes and whatever ‘goes with’ them (who did what to whom, where,
when, how, why etc.). If you have some idea what a verb is, you can think of looking for
processes as looking for verbs. Some of you may remember from traditional school
grammar the notion that verbs are ‘action’ words. This can be helpful, as long as you
keep in mind that lots of verbs (e.g. be and have) refer to relationships, not actions.
For example, here’s part of a report on whales3, with processes highlighted in bold
face:
3 As with many of the texts in this volume, we have simplified the text slightly so that things we don't want
to get in the way yet are avoided.
6 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
There are fewer species of the larger baleen whales that filter krill and small fish through
their baleen plates. The largest is the Blue whale, which is seen frequently in the Gulf of St
Lawrence. It reaches a length of 100 feet and a weight of 200 tons. The young are 25 feet
long at birth and gain about 200 lbs. a day on their milk diet.
We can divide the text into clauses by grouping each process with its dramatis
personae (who’s who) and scenery (where, when, how, why etc.):
There are fewer species of the larger baleen whales,
that filter krill and small fish through their baleen plates.
The largest is the Blue whale,
which is seen frequently in the Gulf of St Lawrence.
It reaches a length of 100 feet and a weight of 200 tons.
The young are 25 feet long at birth
and gain about 200 lbs. a day on their milk diet.
You can see that the punctuation helps – 4 of the 7 clauses end with a full stop and two
with a comma, and 4 of the clauses begin with a capital letter.
Another way in is what Halliday would call interpersonal. This involves treating
the text as a dialogue (even if it has a silent partner, as with monologue). Basically this
means dividing the text into things you can argue with. Here’s the whales report again,
but with arguing added:
There are fewer species of the larger baleen whales, [– Are there?] that filter krill and
small fish through their baleen plates. [– They do not.] The largest is the Blue whale,
[– Is it?] which is seen frequently in the Gulf of St Lawrence. [– It isn’t.] It reaches a length
of 100 feet and a weight of 200 tons. [– Does it?] The young are 25 feet long at birth
[– They aren’t always.] and gain about 200 lbs. a day on their milk diet. [– Do they?]
This approach also works to divide the text into clauses, but this time round with the
clause defined as something you can interact with:
There are fewer species of the larger baleen whales, [– Are there?]
that filter krill and small fish through their baleen plates. [– They do not.]
The largest is the Blue whale, [– Is it?]
which is seen frequently in the Gulf of St Lawrence. [– It isn’t.]
It reaches a length of 100 feet and a weight of 200 tons. [– Does it?]
The young are 25 feet long at birth [– They aren’t always.]
and gain about 200 lbs. a day on their milk diet. [– Do they?]
A third way in is what Halliday would call textual. We’ll use another small text to
illustrate this – part of a recount about whaling:
For one thousand years, whales have been of commercial interest for meat, oil, meal and
whalebone. About 1000 A.D., whaling started with the Basques using sailing vessels and
row boats. Over the next few centuries, whaling shifted to Humpbacks, Grays, Sperms and
Bowheads. By 1500, they were whaling off Greenland; by the 1700s, off Atlantic America;
and by the 1800s, in the south Pacific, Antarctic and Bering Sea. Early in this century,
whaling shifted to the larger and faster baleen whales.
This approach takes advantage of the fact that texts may tend to return to closely
related starting points at the beginning of successive clauses. In the whaling recount,
for example, the text keeps coming back to setting in time, in order to move the history
of whaling along:
For one thousand years, whales have been of commercial interest for meat, oil, meal and
whalebone. About 1000 A.D., whaling started with the Basques using sailing vessels and
row boats. Over the next few centuries, whaling shifted to Humpbacks, Grays, Sperms
CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION 7
and Bowheads. By 1500, they were whaling off Greenland; by the 1700s, off Atlantic
America; and by the 1800s, in the south Pacific, Antarctic and Bering Sea. Early in this
century, whaling shifted to the larger and faster baleen whales.
These temporal expressions organise the text into units as follows:
For one thousand years, whales have been of commercial interest for meat, oil…
About 1000 A.D., whaling started with the Basques using sailing vessels and row boats.
Over the next few centuries, whaling shifted to Humpbacks, Grays, Sperms and …
By 1500, they were whaling off Greenland;
by the 1700s, off Atlantic America;
and by the 1800s, in the south Pacific, Antarctic and Bering Sea.
Early in this century, whaling shifted to the larger and faster baleen whales.
This way into dividing up a text into clauses is less reliable than the other two, since
few texts have such a consistent pattern of starting points. But it can be helpful when a
text leaves out processes, as in two of the clauses above – since a text which leaves
some processes implicit makes the first two ways in harder to apply.
Ideally, in the best of possible worlds, the results of all three approaches coincide.
So, the units you get by looking for processes are the same as the ones you get by
arguing and looking for similar beginnings. Procedural (instructional) texts are about
as near to this ideal as you can find. Try out the three approaches outlined above on
the following instructions from a secondary school science classroom:
Collect 2 petri dishes. Place a thin layer of soil in one dish and some cotton wool in the
other dish. Label the dish with soil ‘soil’ and the other dish ‘no soil’. Next, place about 20
seeds in each 7 petri dish. Spray each dish with water. Finally, put the dishes in a warm
sunny spot in the classroom.
Once you have divided your text into clauses, you can begin to analyse each clause as
outlined in Chapters 3, 4 and 5 of the IFG. This will involve identifying the functional
parts of the clause from each of the three perspectives just outlined: ideational,
interpersonal and textual. For example, if we take an ideational approach (clause as
representation) to the clause Quite recently the Norwegians were whaling off Greenland,
we can identify four functional parts:
clause
In Figure 1.1, the labels in bold print provide functional names for the parts of the
clause when viewed as an ideational structure. As you can see, this kind of labelling is
semantically oriented, which will prove helpful when you are using grammar to
interpret a text.
8 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
As Figure 1 illustrates, the functional parts of any clause take the form of phrases
or groups of words of various kinds. For example, the Process part takes the form of,
or is realised by, a verb or a group of verbs (technically, a verbal group), as shown in
Figure 1.2:
clause
Figure 1.2 Clause structure picking out verbal group functioning as Process
A functional role like Actor, on the other hand, telling you who or what is involved
in the Process, takes the form of a noun or pronoun or else a group of words (such as
articles, adjectives, numerals) which are associated with a noun. As shown in Figure
1.3, we will call this a nominal group.
clause
Figure 1.3 Clause structure picking out a nominal group functioning as Actor
clause
Figure 1.4 Clause structure – picking out adverbial group and prepositional phrase
Just as a clause can be broken into its functional parts, so groups and phrases can
be analysed into their functional parts. For example, the short nominal group this red
flower has three functional parts as shown in Figure 1.5. Here the Deictic part
4 In the example, Greenland is a nominal group, even though it consists of just one noun; in general groups
consist of one or more items; they are still called groups when they consist of a single word.
CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION 9
functions to pick out and identify, the Epithet describes and the Thing tells you what it
is being picked out and described:
nominal group
The functional parts of a group or phrase are realised by different classes of word.
This is shown below in Figure 1.6 for the nominal group we have been looking at.
nominal group
Our discussion so far has shown that a clause can be analysed into its constituent
parts, which are groups and phrases; and these groups and phrases can be analysed
into their constituent parts, which are words. (It would also be possible to analyse
words into their constituent parts – morphemes, but IFG does not take this step.) The
scale of units from clause to word (or morpheme) is known as the rank scale.
As you can see from the examples, when we analyse a structure at clause rank or
at group/phrase rank into its constituent parts, every part can be labelled in two ways:
according to the function of the part and according to class of unit performing that
function. In Figures 1.1 to 1.6 the functions of each part of the structure have been
written in bold. They are also conventionally written with an upper case letter to
remind you that they are function labels – labels which tell you what job that unit is
doing, what role it is playing, what meaning it has in the structure.
For all the functions we have identified so far, we have also identified the class of
unit which realises or expresses it. In Figures 1.1 to 1.6 the class labels have been
written in italics. A class label tells you what kind or category the unit is by virtue of its
form, without reference to its role in any particular context. 5 Class labels may be all
that is necessary when analysing clauses using a formal grammar (as in ‘part of speech
tagging’), but if we are interested in analysing and interpreting texts, then functional
labelling of the kind provided in IFG will prove a much more useful tool.
In addition, there is important grammatical work for function labels to do. For
one thing, they help distinguish ambiguous sequences of classes. We can exemplify
this briefly by looking at the nominal group a running shoe. This word sequence
consists of a determiner, followed by a verb, followed by a noun. But it can mean more
than one thing. Probably the first meaning that comes to mind is that of a shoe made
5 In formal grammars the term 'category' is often used for class and 'relation' for function.
10 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
specially for running. However, it might mean, say in a children’s cartoon context, a
shoe that is running (with little legs protruding from the sole). In a functional
grammar we can say that on the first reading running functions as a Classifier, telling
us what type of shoe (Figure 1.7); on the second reading running is an Epithet,
describing what the shoe is doing (Figure 1.8).
nominal group nominal group
Figure 1.7 A classifying nominal group Figure 1.8 A descriptive nominal group
In a similar way, the structure of the nominal group this red flower can be
distinguished from that of this red wine through the function labelling. Both have the
same sequence of word classes, but the adjective red does not have the same function
in each case. In one case it is an Epithet functioning to describe the flower; in the other
it is a Classifier, telling us what type of wine.
If you have followed the discussion this far, you will have a basis for beginning to
learn to use a functional grammar to analyse text. One key point we have made in this
introduction is that there are three possible angles on analysis – the ideational (clause
as representation), the interpersonal (clause as exchange) and textual (clause as
message) – and that these can ultimately be brought together into a synthesis. We
have also discussed the part-whole, or constituency, structure of the clause, showing
how it is made up of groups and phrases which in turn are made up of words. In the
discussion of constituency we have emphasised that these smaller units have
particular functions to play in the larger structure and that analysis using a functional
grammar involves identifying these functional parts.
In the following sections of this chapter we will develop these initial points, taking
you further in understanding the theoretical orientation of the IFG and exploring some
more complex aspects of constituency. Particularly if you have no previous experience
of grammatical analysis, you may prefer to delay reading these sections until you have
gained some familiarity with the IFG, or perhaps return again to these points later
once your study of the IFG is under way.
This in turn affects the way in which the nominal groups can be expanded. For
example, with running as a Classifier, the group can be expanded through further
classification: the Adidas running shoes. With running as an Epithet on the other hand,
the group can be expanded by grading the description: the quickly running shoes. At
the same time, our functional description explains why we can’t say the quickly
running shoes when we are classifying, any more than we can say the quickly gym shoes;
in English Classifiers are categorical – we don’t intensify them. Similarly, we can’t say
the Adidas running shoes when we are describing, because in English Epithets always
come before, not after, Classifiers. So when we label running as Epithet or Classifier
according to its function, we are showing what kinds of nominal group the one we are
analysing is related to.
Relevant related structures are called agnate structures, and reasoning about an
optimum analysis in any instance involves looking at the patterns of agnation. So, for
example, if you were unsure whether red wine in a clause like she likes red wine was a
Classifier + Thing or an Epithet + Thing structure, you would need to consider agnate
groups. This would lead you to ask whether red wine relates to white wine, rose wine
and sparkling wine (open to extension as Hunter red wine or Italian sparkling wine) or
to pale wine and red medicine (open to extension through intensifiers, as in very pale
wine, bright red medicine). If the former are more plausible in the context, then you
will have grounds for a Classifier + Thing analysis. 6 Reasoning about a specific
instance therefore involves putting the example into a set of similar examples to find
out where it fits.
Ultimately, the purpose of function labels is to show how the items you are
analysing are related to the grammatical choices they embody. Traditionally, the way of
focussing on grammatical choices has been by means of a paradigm of related
grammatical items. (Hence the term ‘paradigmatic relations’.) The representation of
grammatical choice as a paradigm is often used in materials designed for students
learning noun or verb inflexions in a second language. Here’s an example for the
simple present tense forms of the English verb be, plotting choices of person (the rows)
against choices of number (the columns).
Table 1.1 A paradigm for present tense English be (person and number)
singular plural
Paradigms can be used to display relations not only among words but among
units of any size. Table 1.2 shows a simple paradigm for MOOD in the English clause
(with just one dimension of alternatives arranged by row):
6 Can you think of a context in which red would be interpretable as an Epithet in the nominal group the red
wine?
12 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
clause
clause
Below, we have included the relevant function labels for Subject and Finite into the
paradigm we’ve been working with. The analysis suggests that every time you analyse
a clause as Finite followed by Subject, with a tag having Finite followed by Subject, you
are analysing an example that fits into the interrogative, tagged box in the paradigm.
The paradigm as a whole shows how the clause is related to others. So an analysis of
an individual clause always involves implicitly relating that clause to others in the
language.
Table 1.4 A paradigm for MOOD and TAGGING including function structure
Once more than two dimensions of choice are brought into play, paradigms become
rather cumbersome. You have to add a third dimension out the back and the three
dimensional cube you create in this way gets hard to read. A fourth dimension is pretty
much out of the question. In systemic functional linguistics – the linguistic theory
behind the IFG – system networks are used to overcome this limitation. The two
dimensions of choice outlined in the paradigm above can be represented in a system
network as in Figure 1.11.
tagged
not tagged
clause
declarative
interrogative
imperative
Figure 1.11 A system network for MOOD and TAGGING
7 Australians use a tag in this context, although most speakers of English don't have the option of a tag in
interrogatives.
14 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
The network in Figure 1.118 says that as far as choices for MOOD go, speakers of
Australian English can choose among declarative, interrogative and imperative, and at
the same time between having a tag or not. The square brackets set up the choices as
alternatives: you have to choose tagged or not tagged, and you have to choose
declarative, interrogative or imperative. The brace means that speakers must choose
from both systems: from the choice of tagged or not tagged and from the choice of
declarative, interrogative or imperative. The square bracket and its set of choices is
referred to as a system. A set of systems is referred to as a system network. Figure
1.12 identifies the different parts of a system network for you.
system network
system
features (terms, options)
tagged
entry condition
not tagged
clause
declarative
interrogative
imperative
logical ‘and’
logical ‘or’
Figure 1.12 Naming the parts of a system network
A system network of this kind can be derived from any paradigm (including three
dimensional cubes) by turning names of rows and columns into sets of choices –
systems of features. This translation process is outlined in Figure 1.13 for the example
we’ve been considering.
tagged
not tagged
clause
declarative
interrogative
imperative
8 For the actual MOOD network underlying the analyses presented in IFG, see Chapter 3 below.
CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION 15
In this kind of display (the system network), there is no limit to the number of
choices that can be considered. In considering MOOD and TAGGING, we have been
considering interpersonal choices. Earlier on, we looked at the ideational structure of
the clause Quite recently the Norwegians were whaling off Greenland, identifying
functions like Actor, Process, Time and Place. These ideational functions embody
choices from an ideational paradigm where the options relate to the kind of
experience of the world being constructed: doing (material process), thinking (mental
process) or being (relational process). Choices for MOOD (IFG Chapter 4) and choices
for PROCESS TYPE (IFG Chapter 5) combine freely in English, so we represent the choices
as simultaneous, as in Figure 1.14:
material
mental
relational
clause
declarative
interrogative
imperative
Figure 1.14 A simple network for English PROCESS TYPE and MOOD
Just as the MOOD choice will be realised by a particular function structure, such as
Subject followed by Finite (Subject ^ Finite), so the choice from PROCESS TYPE will be
realised by the presence in the structure of particular functions like Actor and Process.
An interpersonal function like Subject may therefore map onto an ideational function
like Actor (as well as on to a textual function like Theme). And just as the configuration
of Subject and Finite tells you the mood of a clause, the presence of an Actor role tells
you what kind of process the clause is (material rather than mental or relational). The
function labelling, in other words, is all about relating clauses and groups to one
another according to the system networks on which IFG is based. (While most of these
networks are not presented in IFG2, they can be found in IFG3 as well as in
Matthiessen 1995.)
And as we extend our description of choices in any language, we may find that
choices start to depend on each other. For example, if we kept working on MOOD in
English, we’d find we needed to include exclamative clauses (What a fool he felt).
These could be grouped with declaratives on the basis that they share the sequence of
the Subject and Finite functions (Subject ^ Finite for declaratives (both exclamative
and non-exclamative), Finite ^ Subject9 for interrogatives). Then interrogatives could
be divided into the ‘yes/no’ (polar) type (Is he a fool?) and the wh type (Why is he a
fool?). Having got this far we’d realise that the possibility of adding a tag is actually
sensitive to mood. In Australian English you do not tag exclamatives or wh
interrogatives (*What a fool he is, isn’t he?, *Why is he a fool, is he?10), but you can tag
non-exclamative declaratives, polar interrogatives and imperatives. So TAGGING in fact
depends on MOOD; it’s not independent of it as implied by the simplified MOOD
networks in Figures 1.11-1.13. The more complex picture of MOOD we’ve developed to
this point is outlined in Figure 1.15 below.
9 Except in wh interrogatives which ask about the Subject (See IFG 3: 136).
10 We follow here the convention in linguistics of placing an asterisk before an ‘impossible’ example.
16 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
material
mental
exclamative
relational
clause
declarative
non-exclamative
indicative
yes/no
interrogative
tagged
imperative
wh-
not tagged
Figure 1.15 A more developed network for English MOOD
The network now includes an ‘or’ bracket that faces left, to account for the fact
that tags can be added to non-exclamative declaratives or polar interrogatives or
imperatives. Note that our discussion hasn’t had any repercussions for our PROCESS
TYPE options, which freely combine with all the MOOD variations we’ve outlined.
This grouping together of systems of interrelated options is the basis for
Halliday’s functional interpretation of English grammar. Options related to PROCESS
TYPE (clause as representation) are handled in IFG Chapter 5; options related to MOOD
are handled in IFG Chapter 4 (clause as exchange); and options related to the
distribution of information (what comes first and last in the clause – clause as
message) are handled in IFG Chapter 3. These bundles of options reflect the
‘metafunctional’ organisation of the grammar into experiential, interpersonal and
textual components respectively.
We can see in this example that the layering in the tree diagram reflects the
organisation of grammatical choices. The top two layers are oriented to clause choices
and the functional structures resulting from them (clause: Actor + Process + Goal); the
next two layers are oriented to group/phrase choices and the functional structures
resulting from them (e.g. nominal group: Deictic^ Thing); the final layer is oriented to
word choices, whose functional structure is not represented in the diagram. This way
of organising constituency with respect to a hierarchy of choices we have already
referred to as organisation by rank – so we speak of clause rank, group/phrase rank,
and word rank, with a clause consisting or one or more groups and phrases (minimally
a clause like Help! ), and with a group consisting of one or more words (minimally a
verbal group like help ); and ultimately, if we continued beyond the IFG coverage, with
a word consisting of one or more morphemes (e.g. one morpheme: my; two
morphemes: shoe + s, wreck + ing). The relation of ranked units ranging from clause to
word is outlined in Figure 1.17.
clause
clause rank choice
resulting in clause function structures
Actor Process Goal
| | |
nominal group verbal group nominal group
group rank choices
resulting in group function structures
Deictic Thing Finite Event Deictic Thing
| | | | | |
determiner verb verb verb pronoun noun
| | | | | | word rank choices
(realising group functions)
those shoes are wrecking my feet
Figure 1.17 Ranked units: underlying systems of choice and function structures
In bringing this discussion to a close, we’ll take two more steps. Both have to do
with opening up choices again at places inside the clause hierarchy where we wouldn’t
normally expect them. Consider the (ideational) analysis of the clause Wearing those
shoes is wrecking my feet. As we work through the analysis of this clause we find that
the Actor isn’t a nominal group as we’d normally expect, but is in fact another clause11.
So, at the point where we might expect to have run out of clause choices and be making
nominal group choices, we find the clause choices opened up again. See Figure 1.18.
When choices are opened up again in this way, at a rank lower than we’d normally
expect them, we say that one unit has been embedded in another – in this case, a
clause has been embedded inside another clause. This disturbs the normal layering we
find in clause structure – instead of a clause consisting or one or more group/phrases
and group/phrases consisting of one or more words, we have a clause consisting of
another clause among the group/phrases. For more detail on embedding (also known
as rankshift or downranking), see IFG Chapter 6, and Chapters 5 and 6 below.
Alongside embedding, the other way in which clause analysis gets more
complicated as choices for meaning are opened up again is through complexing
(discussed in IFG2 Chapter 7, including 7 Additional and IFG3 Chapters 7 and 8).
11 To simplify the discussion we will not deal here with the analysis that interprets the embedded clause as
embedded as the Head of a nominal group functioning as Actor; see IFG Chapter 6 for discussion.
18 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
clause
Figure 1.18 Clause structure with one element being realised by an embedded
clause instead of an ordinary nominal group
For example, if we adjust the example we’re working on slightly to read Those
shoes are wrecking my feet and ankles, then it turns out that the element representing
what is being wrecked is realised by two nominal groups12, not just one – two groups
which are coordinated with each other. The second group expands the information in
the first one, but is serving the same clause function. It’s not that the clause has two
Goals, but rather that the clause’s Goal is realised by a nominal group complex. An
outline of this structure is provided in Figure 1.19, where we have drawn an
interdependency arrow between the first nominal group and the second. Note that
with complexing, we get more choices of the same kind of meaning – in this case,
nominal group choices followed by more nominal group choices; this contrasts with
embedding, where in the example above we had clause choices where we were
expecting nominal group ones – choices of a different kind, rather than more of the
same.
clause
Figure 1.19 Clause structure with an element (Goal) realised by a nominal group complex
12 An alternative analysis might treat feet and ankles as a word complex (see below), realising the Thing; the
analysis proposed here allows for expansion of the second nominal group (e.g. my feet and my ankles, my
tired feet and my sore ankles).
CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION 19
Complexing occurs at all ranks in the grammar. Suppose we adjust our example to
read those shoes are hurting my bruised and battered feet. In this case, we have an
example of word complexing inside the nominal group describing my feet. Here, the
description of my feet, its Epithet, consists of two adjectives, coordinated with each
other. As with complexing in general, instead of one round of choices we have more of
the same – two rounds of foot description instead of one. An outline of this clause
structure, including word complexing is shown in Figure 1.20.
clause
1 2
adjective linker adjective
| | |
those shoes are hurting my bruised and battered feet
The description of the language provided by IFG is a very rich one and can only be
learned step by step. For example, you will need to focus on one of the three
perspectives – ideational, interpersonal, textual – at a time, even though the goal of
your study will be to bring all three perspectives to bear on any text. You will also need
20 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
Theme
clause as message
1. Orientation
Theme: Rheme:
Point of departure of clause as Non-Theme – where the presentation
message; moves after the point of departure;
local context of clause as a piece of what is presented in the local context set
text. up by Theme.
initial position in the clause position following initial position
THEME is one of two systems that organise the information presented in the
clause, the other being that of INFORMATION.1 And, although our concern in this
chapter is with THEME, it will be useful to distinguish it from that of INFORMATION and, at
various points, to make links between the two systems.
While THEME uses position within the clause to organise information into an initial
orientation followed by the Rheme, the system of INFORMATION uses intonation to
highlight what is particularly newsworthy in the message (see also Halliday & Greaves,
2008). The New element in the clause is foregrounded by being ‘stressed’ as we speak
(more technically, it contains a tonic syllable). For example:
A: Which one is Lindy’s boy?
B: He ’s the tall one.
Theme New
The information structure of the clause is discussed in IFG2 Chapter 8 and in IFG3
Chapter 3.5.
2. Survey of options
We have described the Theme as providing the local context for the information in the
rest of the clause. An important aspect to this is that the clause can be contextualised
in terms of all three of its metafunctional perspectives – textually, interpersonally, and
ideationally. The Theme of a clause can thus have textual, interpersonal, and ideational
phases, as shown in Figure 2.1. (The ideational stage to the Theme is known as topical
Theme.)
1 The domain of INFORMATION is really the information unit; see further IFG2 Ch.8; IFG3: Ch.3.5.
CHAPTER 2 THEME 23
contextualisation: textual
clause as message
continuative or interpersonal
conjunctive relating
clause to previous clause as exchange
messages in text ideational (topical)
modal aspect
indicating role of
clause as
move in exchange
representation
or
element of
representation
perhaps (participant,
oh circumstance or
process)
you
honestly
oh
it ’ve become
darling
oh
saved my life
it in hospital
why
’s fun watching
people digging
(why) bigger pits
shouldn’t she
have an admirer
Figure 2.1 illustrates the way the Theme of a clause may unfold from one
metafunctional phase to the next. The Theme starts textually, relating the clause to its
textual environment – to the method of development from which the clause emerges
as a new message, or quantum information. It then continues interpersonally, relating
the clause to its interpersonal environment – to the sequence of exchanges from which
the clause emerges as a new move, or quantum of interaction. Finally, it continues
ideationally, relating the clause to its ideational environment – to the episodic
structure that the clause expands on as a new figure, or quantum of change in the flow
of events.
As the examples in Figure 2.1 show, the Theme of the clause always concludes
with a ‘topical’ element, and indeed there may only be a topical Theme, as in Reptiles
were the first animals with backbones that could live on land all the time. The textual
and interpersonal stages to the Theme may or may not be present. The next section
will illustrate in turn each different type of Theme.
24 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
However, when the topical Theme of a declarative clause is not the Subject, it
gains a greater textual prominence. Non-Subject Themes are ‘marked’ Themes and are
often important in structuring the larger discourse. Here are some examples of
marked Themes:
2 A Wh- Theme is not exclusively interpersonal. It is simultaneously a topical Theme since, as well as its
function as a ‘question word’, it is also a participant or circumstance in the clause.
26 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
3 Relative elements serve both as textual and topical Theme since, as well as relating the relative clause to a
preceding one, they serve to specify a participant or a circumstance.
CHAPTER 2 THEME 27
and, secondly, unfortunately, in spring, the house is far too cold.
element of transitivity =
can be focus of theme
predication (it is … that…)
Section 3.7 below.) In a similar way, the initial clause complex of a paragraph may be
seen as functioning as a kind of ‘paragraph Theme’ (the Topic Sentence of traditional
accounts), while the introductory paragraph itself has a thematic status vis-a-vis the
text as a whole. (See Chapter 7.)
3. Troubleshooting
Such non-finite clauses have been backgrounded for interpersonal reasons (See
Chapter 3) and this has affected their texture, depriving them of topical Themes, so
that the whole clause can be counted as Rheme. In other words the Process which
begins the non-finite clause does not serve as topical Theme (IFG2: 62; IFG3: 100):
Old Mother Hubbard went to the cupboard || to fetch her poor dog a bone
Theme Rheme Rheme
Little Jack Horner sat in the corner || eating his Christmas pie
Theme Rheme Rheme
5 A tag like won’t you? counts as part of the clause and not as a separate unit.
CHAPTER 2 THEME 29
Theme begins and stops. The principle to remember is that everything up to and
including the first ‘topical’ (experiential) element will count as the Theme.
6 Where the Process is realised by a phrasal verb, as in Look at that car! or Open up the house to visitors,
only the lexical part of the verbal group (the Predicator) need be included as topical Theme.
7 An alternative analysis of this structure treats Let as part of a causative verbal group complex realising
the Process, in which case let will be topical Theme and me part of the Rheme.
30 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
these cases they will be phonologically weak and are textual Themes, linking the new
move to what went before. (See IFG2: 92; IFG3: 145). Examples:
A: Bad news about Maxine’s job.
B: Yes it is. Yes is textual Theme
A: We won’t go to the beach today.
B: No it’s too windy. No is textual Theme
Oh, well, now and okay are other textual continuatives, which similarly mark a
response move in dialogue or a fresh initiation in monologue.
(ii) Adjuncts
It is easy to confuse initial Adjuncts which have a textual, linking function and those
which have an interpersonal, modal function (see further Chapter 3). Some of the
Adjuncts most frequently confused are given below (See IFG2:49, IFG3: 82 for fuller
lists):
Table 2.1 Examples of textual and interpersonal Adjuncts
Textual Interpersonal
in fact evidently
anyway broadly speaking
at least obviously
in conclusion provisionally
The Theme predication test is a useful way to discriminate topical Themes. In the
following examples, the second version has the element in doubt as a predicated
Theme (see IFG 3.7). Only a topical element can successfully function as a predicated
Theme:
On Saturdays we used to go jogging.
It was on Saturdays we used to go jogging.
– On Saturdays is topical Theme.
CHAPTER 2 THEME 31
Usually Jeremy is too drunk (to drive home).
* It is usually that Jeremy is too drunk.
– Usually is not topical Theme.
8 Embedded clauses introduced by Wh items, such as [[whatever he did]] was wrong are discussed in
Section 3.6(iii).
32 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
First I put my head on top of the gate – then the head’s high enough – then I stand on my
head – then the feet are high enough, you see – then I’m over, you see.
As he sat in the barn he watched Bern coming and going about his work. At last the beggar
said...
In 1925, the playwright was awarded the Nobel Prize for Literature. Between the ages of
fifty-seven and sixty-seven, Shaw wrote such dramas as Heartbreak House, Back to
Methuselah, Androcles and the Lion, St. Joan. During his lifetime he was besieged by offers
to film his plays...
In all these cases, the item it serves as the Subject of the clause. This is shown by
the fact that it appears in the tag (the ‘Moodtag’, see Chapter 3): it’s hot, isn’t it?; it’s
raining, isn’t it?; it is said that..., isn’t it?; it seems that..., doesn’t it?; it worries us that...,
doesn’t it?; it is irrelevant that..., isn’t it?; it’s possible that…, isn’t it?; it was the dog that
died, wasn’t it? Since the principle of THEME SELECTION in English is that the unmarked
Theme keys into the mood of the clause, the Subject it will also be the unmarked
topical Theme in declarative and yes/no interrogative clauses:
It is hot.
topical
Theme Rheme
Is it hot?
interpersonal topical
Theme Rheme
That is, whether or not the item it has any representational value, it will always have
the potential for serving as unmarked Theme and when it does, it will give thematic
status to the choice of mood – just as any other unmarked topical Theme does in a
declarative or yes/no interrogative clause. (This is in fact part of the explanation why
English uses it in the constructions we are dealing with here. Many languages have no
equivalent item for some of these uses of it. For example, the Japanese for It’s hot is
Atsui, with an ‘i-adjective’ serving as Process and no Theme: the Japanese THEME
system is not oriented towards the MOOD system in the way the English THEME system
is.)
(i) Ambient it
Theme Rheme
9 Ambient it is typically a Carrier in an attributive relational clause with an Attribute expressing some
feature of the environment such as temperature (e.g. it’s hot), or else it appears as Actor in a material
clause of qualitative change (e.g. it’s cooling down).
34 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
(ii) Non-representational it
However, there are uses of it where the item does not represent some phenomenon of
experience and does not serve a participant role. This occurs in ‘meteorological’
clauses and in the ‘impersonal’ use of it.
In clauses construing precipitation, such as it’s raining/snowing/hailing, the item
it might at first glance look like ambient it. However, it differs from ambient it in that it
does not represent some aspect of the environment and does not serve a participant
role. Thus we cannot say (except jocularly) what is it doing? – It’s raining, nor can we
ask: what’s raining? Nor are there any lexicalised alternatives: we cannot say the sky’s
raining, the clouds are raining or the like. The same considerations apply to the other
non-representational use of it, the use of it in impersonal projections such as it is said
that he is very wealthy, it seems that he is very wealthy. (See IFG2: 266; IFG3: 472.)
From a textual point of view, clauses such as it is raining or it seems that he is very
wealthy have it as unmarked topical Theme. This topical Theme is not prototypical:
while it is quite regular in its orientation to the mood selection of the clause (it [is]: is
it), it does not in fact serve a role in the experiential structure of the clause.
Consequently, it is not open to special more marked thematic strategies – it cannot be
theme-predicated (we cannot say what it did was rain all day) and it cannot be marked
as an absolute Theme (we cannot say as for it, it is said that he is very wealthy. Further,
it cannot be picked up textually by reference: if we say it rained yesterday and it’s
snowing today, the second it does not refer back to the first.
So why do we still interpret this non-representational it as topical Theme? On the
one hand, as we have already noted, it is quite regular in its orientation to the mood
selection of the clause: as unmarked topical Theme, it serves to give the mood
selection thematic status –
It is raining today.
It is said that he’s wealthy.
topical
Theme Rheme
Is it raining today?
Is it said that he’s wealthy?
interpersonal topical
Theme Rheme
On the other hand, it contrasts with marked topical Themes in the same way as
prototypical, representational unmarked topical Themes do:
Henry is swimming today : Today Henry is swimming ::
It is raining today : Today it is raining
It is said nowadays that this substance is harmless :
Nowadays it is said that this substance is harmless
(iii) Anticipatory it (mental and relational clauses)
The anticipatory it serves to anticipate an embedded clause occurring later in the
structure. Unlike the impersonal projections, these structures have a variant where the
embedded clause is Theme. For example:
CHAPTER 2 THEME 35
it worries me that he’s not doing his homework :
that he’s not doing his homework worries me ::
it is clear that he’s not doing his homework :
that he’s not doing his homework is clear
(i) Nominal group in Theme position extends beyond the main noun:
A large black wolf with yellow fangs spoke to Red Riding Hood.
The teacher who understood him best was Marianne Fawley.
For explanation of this structure, see Chapter 5, IFG2 6.2.
(ii) Group and Phrase complexes in Theme position. Sometimes more than one
nominal group functions as the Theme:
Jack and Jill went up the hill.
Nelson Mandela, the newly elected President of South Africa, was invited.
(iv) Embedded non-finite clause as Theme. It is not only Wh- clauses that can be
embedded into Theme position. Non-finite clauses are also common as topical
Themes:
Doing twenty sit-ups a day will improve your tummy muscles
To err is human || to forgive (is) divine
It is also possible for a complex of clauses to be embedded in this way:
Getting plenty of exercise and eating the right food is important.
(v) Embedded ‘that’ clause as Theme. ‘Fact’ clauses such as (The fact) [[that the
food might not be fresh]] are more commonly found in the Rheme, but do occur in
Theme position:
That the food might not be fresh didn’t occur to them.
10 Square brackets [[ ]] signal that a clause is functioning in an embedded way as part of another clause.
CHAPTER 2 THEME 37
They are some of the animals we know.
[[Doing twenty sit-ups a day]] will flatten your tummy.
It will flatten your tummy.
(ii) Make the expression non-thematic, i.e. consider a version of the clause with a
different thematic structure. For example:
From house to house I went my way : I went my way from house to house
The alternation involves from house to house as a whole, not only say from house;
that is, you can’t normally get:11
From house to house I went my way : From house I went my way to house
At the same time, each individual clause has its own Theme-Rheme structure, so a
double analysis is possible:
Theme Rheme
Note that it is only ‘hypotactic’ (i.e. dependent, modifying) clauses which have the
possibility of occurring in this initial position as a marked Theme of the clause
complex. Non-finite clauses are one such type of hypotactic clause. Here are some
examples of non-finite clauses functioning as marked Theme of a clause complex:
To strengthen his knee, he did the exercise routine twice daily.
Blinking nervously, he tried to think of something to say.
11 As always in language, you have to treat analytical rules of thumb with care: note that you can have from
Athens to Madrid we flew with Olympic: From Athens we flew to Madrid with Olympic.
38 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
4. Analysis practice
4.1 Phase I
4.1.1 Exercises
Examples from M. Williams, The Velveteen Rabbit; P. Theroux, The Great Railway
Bazaar; A. A. Milne, The House at Pooh Corner; W. Russell, Educating Rita.
Exercise 1 Identifying topical Theme (declaratives)
Follow the models provided and underline the Topical Theme in each clause:
He was fat and bunchy.
I am real || said the little rabbit.
The clock was still saying five minutes to eleven...
1. He’s out.
2. You’re just in time for a little smackerel of something.
3. One mustn’t complain.
4. That’s a very good idea... See 3.3.3 (b).
5. Christopher Robin had spent the morning indoors...
6. The wind had dropped...
7. My mind’s full of junk, isn’t it?
8. This room does not need air.
9. A peaceful smile came over his face...
10. His argument just crumbled.
Theme Rheme
textual topical
So he sang it again.
And then we ’ll go out.
Well, poor Eeyore has nowhere to live.
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
Text 2 Explanation
After flash floods, desert streams from upland areas carry heavy loads of silt, sand and
rock fragments. || As they reach the flatter area of desert basins || they slow down || and
their waters may soak quickly into the basin floor. || Then the streams drop their loads; ||
first they drop the heaviest material – the stones, || then [they drop] the sand || and finally
[they drop] the silt. || Soon these short lived streams become choked by their own
deposits || and they spread their load in all directions. || After some time, fan or
cone-shaped deposits of gravel, sand, silt and clay are formed around each valley or
canyon outlet. || These are called alluvial fans.||
[Adapted from Sale, C., Wilson, G & Friedman, B. Our changing world Bk 1. Melbourne: Longman
Cheshire, 1980:54.]
4.2 Phase II
4.2.1 Exercises
Exercise 1 Identifying interpersonal Theme (declaratives)
Follow the models to identify interpersonal and topical components to the Theme in
each clause. Fill out the chart below:
Maybe you call them sparrows.
Of course he does.
Theme Rheme
interpersonal topical
Maybe you call them sparrows.
Of course he does.
1
2
3
4
5
Theme Rheme
interpersonal topical
Wh... ...ere shall we build it?
H... ...ow are you?
1
2
3
4
5
Theme Rheme
interpersonal topical
Look at it.
Take your old bunny.
Don’t mention it Pooh.
1
2
3
4
5
Theme Rheme
textual interpersonal marked topical unmarked topical
and here it is as good as ever
when Pooh and Piglet set out on their...
perhaps I ’ll put a muffler...
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
Text 1 Procedure
Paprika-Garlic Roast Chicken
1 medium whole frying chicken (2 1/2 - 2 3/4 pounds)
1 1/2 teaspoon garlic powder
1 1/2 teaspoons paprika
3 tablespoons salad oil
salt and pepper
Preheat oven to 325°.
Remove giblets from inside chicken and save for chicken stew or soup. Wash chicken
inside and outside. (Don’t use soap!)
Rub chicken inside and out with garlic powder. Using a pastry brush or paper towel,
coat chicken with oil and sprinkle with paprika to give a nice red tint. Salt and pepper
lightly.
Place chicken in a deep oven pan, breast down, and cover tightly with aluminium foil.
44 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
The juices will seep into the breast and keep the chicken moist.
Roast in oven for 40 minutes. At the end of this time, turn chicken breast up and
cover again with foil. Return to oven for another 30 minutes. Then remove foil and
continue roasting for 20-30 minutes more, or until chicken is fork tender and golden
brown. Serve with Brown Rice or Bulgur Wheat.
[Cadwallader, S. 1974: Cookup adventures for kids. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.]
Text 3 Exposition
The following text is analysed below. A rewritten version is then presented. Complete
the analysis of the second version and compare their methods of development.
3a)
a Although the United States participated heavily in World War I,
b the nature of that participation was fundamentally different from
what it became in World War II.
c The earlier conflict was a one-ocean war for the Navy and a one-theatre war for the
Army;
d the latter was a two-ocean war for the Navy and one of five major theatres for the
Army.
e In both wars a vital responsibility of the Navy was escort-of-convoy and
anti-submarine work,
f but in the 1917-1918 conflict it never clashed with the enemy on the surface;
g whilst between 1941 and 1945 it fought some twenty major and countless minor
engagements with the Japanese Navy.
h American soldiers who engaged in World War I were taken overseas in transports
i and landed on docks or in protected harbours;
j in World War II the art of amphibious warfare had to be revived and developed,
k since assault troops were forced to fight their way ashore.
l Airpower, in the earlier conflict, was still inchoate and almost negligible;
m in the latter it was a determining factor.
n In World War I the battleship still reigned queen of the sea,
o as she had in changing forms, since the age of Drake.
p And Battle Line fought with tactics inherited from the age of sail;
q but in World War II the capital naval force was the aircraft carrier task group,
r for which completely new tactics had to be devised.
CHAPTER 2 THEME 45
Theme Rheme
q but in World War II the capital naval force was the aircraft
carrier task group
12 Relative pronouns function for Halliday as conflations of textual and topical Theme.
46 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
Theme Rheme
textual marked topical unmarked topical
a Although the United States participated heavily in World War I
the nature of that was fundamentally different from
b
participation [[what it became in World War II]]
f but
Theme Rheme
textual marked topical unmarked topical
j the art of amphibious had to be revived and developed in
warfare World War II
4. The bored, lazy and unruly are not the only kind of problem students that
teachers have to tackle.
5. Matter, in all its forms, is endowed by the figurative genius of every language
with the functions which pertain to intellect.
6. ‘Fault’, ‘responsibility’, and ‘cause’ are explicit references to causation.
7. Relative genericness, non-specificity, and emptiness is commonplace in noun
phrases.
8. Mesoamerica, with its rich genetic and typological diversity, deserves attention...
8. It excited him, too, that many men had already loved Daisy.
9. In fact, it was the husband and not the wife who got custody in that case.
10. It happened that we were there at the same time.
Sultan Iskander Shah, the last ruler of the ancient kingdom of Singapura. At the mouth of
the river, or at least what used to be the mouth before the most recent bout of land
reclamation, stands Singapore’s symbol, the Merlion.
Change Alley, Singapore’s most famous place for bargains has survived or rather
adapted to modernisation. It still cuts through from Collyer Quay to Raffles Place, but has
become a pedestrian bridge and is known as ‘Aerial Change Alley’. It’s still lined with
shops and money changers although now it’s air-conditioned! The older alley runs below.
Further along the waterfront you’ll find large office blocks, airline offices and more
shops. Here too is the popular Telok Ayer Transit Food Centre by the waterfront.
Singapore’s disappearing Chinatown is inland from this modern city centre.
[T. Wheeler et al. 1991. Malaysia, Singapore and Brunei: Lonely Planet travel survival kit.
Hawthorn, Vic: Lonely Planet Publications.]
Text 3 Advertisement
Be seen in all the right places.
From New York to Los Angeles to Atlanta to Dallas/Ft. Worth, the Delta system flies to all
the top business centres in America. That’s over 4200 flights a day to more than 260 cities
around the U.S. and around the world. So next time business takes you to the States, book
Delta. We’ll make sure you’re seen in all the right places. At all the right times.
Delta Air Lines.
We love to fly and it shows.
Text 6 Exposition
... The claims for the educational value of Show and Tell and Morning News sessions are at
best rather questionable, however, for several reasons. In the first place, the commitment
to promoting oral language as something independent of other areas of language
development is itself very dubious. The notion of language development must involve
development both in speech and in literacy, and no very useful distinction can be drawn
between the two. They are necessarily very closely related.
Furthermore, even the children who are successful in Showing and Telling or
Newsgiving will benefit from being given opportunities to use spoken language in other
ways in schools. The particular activity and genre are not uniquely suitable for the
development of oral language abilities.
On the contrary, since the particular genre used at any time is itself dependent upon
the activity concerned, it should be clear that the need is to generate a range of differing
activities in schools, to enable children to master the associated range of genre types.
Regrettably, Morning News and Show and Tell sessions frequently feature as the only
concessions made to the development of oral language in daily school programs. Where
this is the case, the language program is impoverished indeed. In a good language program
children move easily through many learning activities of a kind designed to stimulate and
extend abilities to speak, to read and to write...
[F. Christie 1985 Language education. Geelong: Deakin University Press: 20-1.]
13 Note that the modal Adjunct apparently is not an interpersonal Theme here, since it does not precede the
topical Theme.
52 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
cables. A non-metal which conducts well is carbon. Salt water is an example of a liquid
conductor.
A material which does not easily release electrons is called an insulator. Rubber,
nylon, porcelain are all insulators. There are no perfect insulators. All insulators will allow
some flow of electrons, however this can usually be ignored because the flow they permit
is so small.
Semiconductors are midway between conductors and insulators. Under certain
conditions they allow a current to flow easily but under others they behave as insulators.
Germanium and silicon are semiconductors. Mixtures of certain metallic oxides also act as
semiconductors. These are known as thermistors. The resistance of thermistors falls
rapidly as their temperature rises. They are therefore used in temperature-sensing
devices.
[E. H. Glendinning 1980. English in Electrical Engineering and Electronics. London:
Oxford University Press (English in Focus).]
With penetrating he illustrates how such psychological entities such as memories, beliefs,
analyses values, and meanings are not formed in isolation but in a social or
cultural context that is inherently ideological or political.
Theme Rheme
The point of departure of the clause as message, its local environment – what is
called Theme – is realised by initial position in the clause, and is followed by the
non-Theme, the Rheme. The Rheme provides the information to be processed within
the local environment specified by the Theme.
phrase/ prep.
MINOR TRANSITIVITY MINOR MOOD MINOR THEME
group phrase
THING TYPE
nominal PERSON, ATTITUDE DETERMINATION
MODIFICATION
topical Theme. The first such element in the clause realises the topical theme while
any preceding textual or interpersonal elements constitute textual and interpersonal
Themes. For example:
Note that exclamative clauses are like wh- interrogatives in that the Wh- element is
thematic:
What an impetuous boy he is!
In addition, there are further types of imperative clause that are also thematically
distinct (e.g., with Theme underlined, Let’s wake up late today!, Lemme wake up late
today!)
Further reading
See especially Thompson 2007 for a recent survey of IFG based studies of Theme and
issues arising. Caffarel et al. 2004 interprets theme from a functional typological
perspective across a range of languages.
Micro and Macro Connexity of Discourse. 116-152. Hamburg: Buske (Papers in Text
Linguistics 45).]
Fries, Peter H 1995 Patterns of information ininitial position in English. P H Fries & M
Gregory (eds) Discourse in Society: functional perspectives. Norwood, NJ: Ablex
(Meaning and choice in language: studies for Michael Halliday). 47-66.
Ghadessy, Mohsen (ed) 1995 Thematic Development in English Texts. London: Pinter
(Open Linguistics Series).
Halliday, M A K 1985 It’s a fixed word order language is English. International Review
of Applied Linguistics, 67-68: 91-116. [Reprinted in M A K Halliday 2005 Studies in
English Language. London: Continuum (Volume 7 in the Collected Works of M A K
Halliday). 213-231.]
Hasan, Ruqaiya & Fries, Peter H (eds) 1995 On Subject and Theme: a discourse
functional perspective. Amsterdam: Benjamins (Current Issues in Linguistic
Theory 118).
Martin, J R 1992 English text: system and structure. Amsterdam: Benjamins. [Chapter 6:
‘Texture’.]
Martin, J R 1992 Theme, method of development and existentiality – the price of reply.
Occasional Papers in Systemic Linguistics 6. 147-184.
Martin, J R 1993 Life as a noun. M A K Halliday & J R Martin Writing science. London:
Falmer Press. 221-267.
Martin, J R 1995 More than what the message is about: English theme. M Ghadessy
(ed) 223-258.
Matthiessen, C M I M 1992 Interpreting the textual metafunction. M Davies & L Ravelli
(eds), New advances in systemic linguistics. London: Pinter. 37-81.
Matthiessen, C M I M 1995 Lexicogrammatical cartography: English systems. Tokyo:
International Language Sciences Publishers. [Chapter 6 in particular relationship
to VOICE: Section 6.3.2; textual resources: Section 2.3.6]
Matthiessen, C M I M 1995 THEME as a resource in ideational ‘knowledge’
construction. M Ghadessy (ed) 20-54.
Thompson, G 2007 Unfolding theme: the development of clausal and textual
perspectives on theme. R Hasan, C M I M Matthiessen & J Webster (eds)
Continuing Discourse on Language: a functional perspective Vol. 2. London:
Equinox. 671-698.
Chapter 3
Mood
clause as exchange
1. Orientation
(i) role taken on by an interactant in the exchange and (ii) the nature of the
commodity being exchanged – goods-&-services vs. information. These are set out in
Table 3.1 below. As you can see from the table, (i) and (ii) combine to define the
traditional speech functional categories of statement, question, offer and command.
Statements and questions involve exchanges of information and are called
propositions, while offers and commands are exchanges of goods-&-services and are
called proposals. These semantic categories are realised by grammatical MOOD
options.
goods-&-services information
proposal proposition
In Table 3.1 the arrow represents the realisational move from semantic category
(statement, question, command, offer) to grammatical one (declarative, interrogative,
imperative). The grammatical realisations shown are congruent ones. Alternatively of
course, the speech functional categories may be realised incongruently by means of
interpersonal metaphors, as when a command 'Get me another beer!' is realised
metaphorically as if it were a question by an interrogative clause – Would you get me
another beer? – instead of congruently by an imperative one – Get me another beer!
Such interpersonal metaphors expand the range of speech function options, providing
interactants with richer resources to calibrate and negotiate the tenor of the
relationship between them, for example in the area of politeness. (See Section 2.5.1
below and IFG2 Section 10.4; IFG3 Sections 10.3, 10.4 for a discussion of interpersonal
metaphor.)
So far we have been looking at single clause examples, but to understand
interpersonal grammar, it is important to focus on its role in realising dialogue, which
is essentially an interactive, collaborative process. To explore the role of the MOOD
system further, then, let us look more closely at a minimal example of dialogue.
In the following dialogic exchange (extracted from Svartvik & Quirk, A corpus of
English conversation [CEC], p. 623), A demands information and B gives on demand:
The most central aspect of this passage of dialogue is that it is acted out as an
exchange between A and B. The example above is semantically a demand for
information (question) followed by a gift of information (statement), realised
58 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
A: B:
What the hell is structuralism ?
Residue Mood
Finite Subject
Mood Residue
Subject Finite
The example above also illustrates other interpersonal choices. For example, B
chooses to respond it's a form of analysis, dear rather than it's a form of analysis, thus
naming the addressee with a Vocative embodying an interpersonal attitude, an
endearment (dear).
Some further examples of exchanges revolving around the Mood element are
given in Figure 3.2:
1 Textually, the pattern is as follows (Theme underlined, focus of New in bold): What the hell is
structuralism? – It's a form of analysis, my dear. This is typical of an exchange revolving around a
Wh-element: in the interrogative clause, the Theme is its information demanding status in the exchange,
plus the point of demand; the New is the element whose characterisation is being sought, structuralism. In
the declarative clause, the Theme is information giving status in the exchange, plus the element whose
characterisation is being given, and the New is the characterisation.
CHAPTER 3 MOOD 59
A: B:
A: B:
A: B:
It ’s incredibly funny.
Is it ____ ?
In each example in Figure 3.2, the arrow connects the Mood element of A's
initiating declarative with the Mood element of B's response. You can also see that the
part of a responding clause which is continuous with the clause it is a response to can
be 'left out'. (More technically the Residue is presumed through ellipsis). This use of
ellipsis in responses effectively foregrounds the Mood element, the part of the clause
embodying its dialogic contribution.
2. Survey of options
In this section, the different options within MOOD will be outlined, together with the
clause structures that realise them. In addition, we will mention a number of related
interpersonal systems such as POLARITY, MODALITY, COMMENT and VOCATION.
60 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
imperative
Each of the three mood types – interrogative, declarative and imperative – can be
further subclassified. For example, interrogatives may be of the yes/no type (Was it
cool?) or the Wh- type (What was cool?), declaratives may be exclamative (How cool it
was!) or non-exclamative (It was cool), and there are various kinds of imperative (Stay
cool; Let me stay cool; Let's be cool).
Each MOOD category is realised in English by a particular interpersonal structure,
with one part – the Mood element – being fundamental in discriminating the different
mood types.
One of the reasons for recognising this kind of interpersonal clause structure is
that if a speaker wants to get a verbal response to a declarative or imperative clause,
s/he can add a tag (Moodtag) that picks up only part of the clause – viz. the Mood
2 Note that the term 'mood' is used by Halliday both for the interpersonal system in the grammar realising
speech function – written as MOOD – and for the interpersonal element of clause structure consisting of
Subject and Finite – written as Mood.
CHAPTER 3 MOOD 61
element, but not the Residue. That is, if the Moodtag is present, it picks up the Mood
element of the clause: you didn't expect that, did you; they shoot horses, don't they.
Another reason for recognising Mood and Residue as the major functions of the clause,
when viewed interpersonally, is that the different MOOD categories (declarative,
interrogative, etc) are realised in English by the Mood element, while the Residue may
in part or whole be involved in ELLIPSIS in responding moves. To explore this further
requires taking the second step in analysis, which is to identify the functional elements
of the Mood, Residue and Moodtag.
For example:
Mood Residue
To probe for the Subject role, add a Moodtag. In the tag the Subject will be
repeated as a pronoun, as in The girl has already done it, hasn't she? In a similar way,
the Finite can be identified as the verb which (re-) appears in the Moodtag, as in The
3There is a principled distinction between two types of modal Adjunct: mood Adjuncts and comment
Adjuncts.
62 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
indicative present
declarative
non-exclam. Subject^Finite They^will build the house.
exclamative Wh^Subject^Finite How quickly^ they^will build the house?
interrogative
yes/no Finite^Subject Will^they build the house?
wh- Wh^Finite^Subject5 What^ will^they build?
For instance:
Su Fi Su Fi Adjunct
(Note also the fusions of negative polarity and modality – as Finite: won't, can't, etc.; and
as Adjunct: rarely, seldom, [negative + usuality] etc.)
4 Halliday (1985) treated Comment Adjuncts as outside Mood-Residue structure, because they do not
necessarily form part of the proposition being negotiated.
5 Unless Wh = Subject, as Who will build the house?
6 In addition, not may also occur as a non-salient variant of n’t, in which case it is just part of the Finite: see
IFG2: 90; IFG3: 145.
CHAPTER 3 MOOD 63
There are four main kinds of modality: probability, usuality, obligation and
readiness. Halliday refers to probability and usuality together as modalisation,
which he associates with propositions (statements and questions); he refers to
obligation and readiness as modulation, which is associated with proposals (offers
and commands). See Table 3.4 below and IFG2 4.5: 88-92, 10.4: 354-362; IFG3 4.5:
143-50, 10.3: 613-625.
Here the Subject is responsible for the success of the proposal – it has to comply,
etc. But you can also get a sense of Subject as the modally responsible element in
propositions – indicative clauses negotiating information – when you consider the
dialogic potential (see also the example in IFG2:78; IFG3:120):
Meg: The two gentlemen had the last of the fry this morning.
Petey: Oh, did they?
Petey: You slept like a log list night.
Meg: Did I?
Meg: I was the belle of the ball.
Petey: Were you?
Meg: Oh yes, they all said I was.
Petey: I bet you were, too.
Meg: Oh, it's true. I was.
I know I was.
Meg: Well, I bet you don't know what it is.
Stanley: Oh yes, I do.
Stanley: They've got a wheelbarrow in that van.
Meg: They haven't.
Stanley: Oh yes, they have.
Meg: You're a liar.
Stanley: A big wheelbarrow. And when the van stops they wheel it out, and they wheel it
up the garden path, and then they knock at the front door.
Meg: They don't.
Stanley: They're looking for someone.
Meg: They're not.
shouldn't say that word, 'you' (Subject) rather than 'that word' (Complement) is vested
with the obligation.
(ii) When a clause is negative, the negation normally starts with Finite and affects
the whole of Residue, but Subject is not negated: it is the element with respect to
which something is affirmed or denied, etc. – he – has bought something for you/
hasn't bought anything for you.
For example:
Mood Residue
Su Fi Predicator
10 Traditionally, Complement was called Object (direct and indirect Object); but Complement also includes
the 'predicative complement' of traditional grammar (as in Cary Grant was a male war bride). The term
‘Complement’ is preferable to ‘Object’ since it indicates the complementarity with Subject while ‘object’
suggests some kind of inappropriate transitivity interpretation such as 'object of the action, acted upon'.
Adjunct was called Adverbial, but the strict association with the verb is misleading, so the term Adjunct is
preferable. Note the distinction between the term Predicator used in IFG and the traditional term
Predicate. The Predicator is just those verbal elements of the clause that do not serve as Finite, whereas
the ‘Predicate’ extends to cover Complements and Adjuncts as well (the Verb Phrase, VP, of many modern
formal grammars). Thus in these flowers have just been given my aunt, the Predicator is been given,
whereas the Predicate would be have just been given my aunt. The Predicate is not a functionally
motivated category in the description of English; it derives from an orientation towards logic in
traditional grammar.
66 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
Here the primary tense in the Finite establishes arguability in terms of past (was)
in relation to 'now', while the secondary tense in the Predicator specifies future (going
to) in relation to that past. See Chapter 5, 2.2.3.
The difference between Complements and Adjuncts is that Complements are
potential Subjects, whereas Adjuncts are not. Thus alongside she gave my aunt these
flowers out of pity, we have my aunt was given these flowers (by her) out of pity and
these flowers were given my aunt (by her) out of pity, but not out of pity was given these
flowers my aunt.
11 Note that attitudinal words will often occur as constituents of a group rather than a clause, in which case
they will not have the function of Expletive within the clause. Thus bloody in That bloody woman looks like
a flower is not an Expletive.
CHAPTER 3 MOOD 67
Structural conjunction, such as and, but, or; when, while, if (cf. IFG2: 100;
IFG3:160-1).
Semantically, the fact that all these fall outside the Mood-Residue structure means
that these elements are not part of the proposition or proposal being negotiated. They
relate to it by indicating its textual relevance as a message (by means of continuatives
and conjunctions) and by indicating the addressee (by means of the Vocative) and by
indicating speaker stance (by means of an Expletive), but they do not form part of
what is being negotiated.
Metaphorical realisations are also found for modality, expanding the range of modal
meanings beyond those realised by modal verbs as Finite and Mood Adjuncts.
As far as probability is concerned, one kind of metaphorical realisation involves
first person12, present tense 'mental' processes of cognition (e.g. I think, I reckon, I
suspect) or 'relational' processes of cognitive state (e.g. I'm sure, I'm convinced, I'm
uncertain). Here, the modal assessment of probability is construed as a clause – as a
proposition in its own right. This form of realisation, known as explicitly subjective,
makes the speaker explicitly responsible for the assessment. The contrast between
what Halliday's refers to as the congruent and the metaphorical realisation of
modalisation is outlined in Figure 3.5.
mean mean
certain Greiner must be corrupt certain Greiner must be corrupt
= stands for
say say
Greiner must be corrupt I’m sure Greiner is corrupt
congruent metaphorical
[say = mean] [say stands for mean]
mean mean
keen I would sack him keen I would sack him
= stands for
say say
I would sack him I’d like to sack him
congruent metaphorical
[say = mean] [say stands for mean]
3. Troubleshooting
(ii) Minor clauses are a second kind of moodless clause. They are 'minor' because
they are not open to any of the major systems of THEME, MOOD or TRANSITIVITY. Such
clauses as oh, yuk, hi, thanks may however fulfill a minor speech functional meaning
and thus serve an interpersonal function as a greeting, or acknowledgement, or the
like.13 They thus have a semantic role in dialogue but can simply be ignored when
analysing grammatically for MOOD.
Note that for purposes of emphasis, negation and tagging, the Finite is realised
separately from the Predicator, through the auxiliary verb do.
13 See IFG2:95-6; IFG3:153-4. For full analysis and discussion of speech function see Martin (1992); Martin
and Rose (2007, Chapter 7).
CHAPTER 3 MOOD 71
Note also that when the main verb in the clause is be, there is no Predicator
function, but only a Finite (also with have in some dialects). Thus Are they happy?, not
do they be happy? and they aren't happy, not they don't be happy.
The first Complement in the above examples also has the experiential role of
Beneficiary (see Chapter 4). This particular experiential role can also be realised later
in the clause in a prepositional form, as shown below:
This suggests a difference from other kinds of Adjuncts where if the nominal
element becomes Subject, then the preposition is left as an Adjunct by itself:
Predicator Complement
However a Finite is present in the emphatic form with do, or the negative with
don’t:
There may be textual reasons for placing the 'content' of the Subject role in a position
in the clause where it will be 'news' 14, particularly when an embedded clause
functions as Subject. To accommodate this, a 'contentless' it may hold the typical
Subject position in the clause, letting us know if the clause is interrogative or
declarative. For example:
It will upset her that they didn't give the job to Max.
Was it a good thing to lose Max?
Was it fair Max getting overlooked?
There are agnate variants to these clauses where the embedded clause itself
occurs first in the normal Subject position and no anticipatory it is needed:
That they didn't give the job to Max will upset her.
Was to lose Max a good thing?
Was Max getting overlooked fair?
14 See Chapter 2.
CHAPTER 3 MOOD 75
discontinuous constituent as Subject. The it forms the first part of the Subject and the
'postposed' embedded clause forms the second part:
it will upset her that they didn't give the job to Max
Sub… Finite Predicator Complement ...ject............................................................
For various reasons, a given example may be structurally ambiguous in terms of its
mood analysis. Here we will just point to a few examples.
(1) When the clause is negative and the Subject is 'you', yes/no interrogative
clauses and imperative ones may be structurally the same – Finite: don't ^ Subject: you,
as in:
They are distinct in their TONE selections so if you speak the examples you can
hear the difference (cf. IFG Section 8.9). They also have different variants. The
imperative clause is agnate with Don't ever go out you!, Don't you ever go out, will you!;
but the interrogative clause is agnate with e.g. Doesn't she ever go out? (with
non-interactant rather than interactant subject-person), Didn't you ever go out? (with
past rather than present tense).
(2) When the clause is positive and the Subject is 'you', a yes/no interrogative
clause may presume the Mood element (cf. IFG2: 94; IFG3: 152) and in such cases it
may be structurally the same as an imperative clause:
Declarative clauses normally select a falling tone; but they may also choose a rising
one. If they do, they are likely to be interpreted as questions at the semantic stratum,
as indicated by the question marks in the examples below:
You find a resemblance?
You got your paper?
question our decision. Since polarity and finiteness are closely bonded in English, the
Finite immediately follows the negative Theme and so comes before the Subject:
Although the order Finite ^ Subject suggests a yes/no interrogative clause, these
clauses are declarative. This is clear from the version with unmarked Theme – He
didn't question our decision at any time – and from the possibility (in all varieties of
English) of a Moodtag – At no time did he question our decision, did he?
The two examples above also illustrate a possible ambiguity between 'declarative' and
'imperative', more specifically between declarative clauses with addressee (you) as
Subject and (simple) present tense and 'jussive' imperative clauses with explicit
Subject:
You can probe these in various ways, for instance by checking the nature of the
Moodtag (declarative: don't you?; imperative: won't you? ) and the possibility of
leaving the Subject implicit in the imperative clause (Find a resemblance).
[i] The negative polarity value combines with usuality or degree. The clause is still
negative, even though you don't find a not or n't and even though the negative is
scaled down from definitely 'no': they seldom come here nowaydays, do they?; he is
hardly civil, is he?
[ii] The negative is a feature of the Subject: nothing upsets her, does it?; nobody believes
him, do they?
[iii] The negative polarity has been 'transferred' to a mental projecting clause serving as a
metaphorical modality: I don't believe this is the right move for us, is it? Here the
clause this is... looks positive, but it is not; the negative polarity is realised within the
metaphorical modality I don't believe.
4. Analysis practice
4.1 Phase I
4.1.1 Exercises (examples based on Roddy Doyle’s Snapper)
4. Dawn has had her baby for Paddy Bell. – She has of course.
4.2 Phase II
4.2.1 Exercises
1. Stop that.
2. How did it happen?
3. Don't start that.
4. Why won't you tell us?
5. What do you mean?
6. When's it due?
7. What a house this is!
4. Apparently she'd barely seen any of the sights in any quarter of the town.
She plugged the kettle in. She plugged the kettle in.
Subject Fi. Predicator Complement Adjunct
Mood Residue
2. Tracy came back in with the pup clinging to the front of her
jumper.
3. Drawn by only two horses, the coach moved slowly down the road.
5. He quickly took the King's part, wielding his flail against the attackers.
6. Given my aunt by the Duke the teapot remained a family heirloom for
generations.
7. He would attempt to repay him by showing him through the royal apartments.
8. To give him cause for rejoicing, he granted him sole possession of the farm.
1. Gosh, I forgot.
Kane: Clothesline.
Teacher: And what with it?
Kane: A wheel.
Teacher: A wheel. (repeats out loud to the class and writes on the board) Yeah, no you’re
right. Clotheslines. That was a… (interrupted) what did she use on the
clothesline?
Several: Pulley.
Teacher: A pulley, which is a type of (pause) lever. Except of course, you’ve got also a
what with it? A (pause) wind (prompting children) lass. Anything else that
wasn’t mentioned that possibly uses the principles of a lever?
Steven: Door handle.
Teacher: A door handle, good one, hey.
…..........
Teacher: Yep. (writes on board) Righto, let’s have a look at an inclined plane one (pause)
well actually that’s a type of tool which you have seen in action, come to think
of it. Maybe we can get six uses of an inclined plane. Um Aranthi?
Aranthi: Stairs.
Teacher: Stairs, right. Great answer. (writes on the board)…
– Okay, I will.
9. She said she was pregnant. 9. ………………………………………………..
– Did she really?
10. I still think you should tell us who 10. ………………………………………………..
the dad is.
– You can think away then.
Analyse the following extract from Pinter's Old Times (opening of Act One). With
reference to figure 3.8, show MOOD selections in the columns to the right, and
underline the Mood element of structure. We have supplied elided elements in square
brackets.
Compare the types of interpersonal selections Kate and Deeley make in this
passage. Comment on and interpret the division of dialogic labour between them.
CHAPTER 3 MOOD 93
give } statement
polar
move
demand
} question
elemental
information
(proposition) } offer
semantics ―
SPEECH declarative
lexicogrammar ―
MOOD falling
rising
tone level
group
falling-rising
rising-falling
phonology
― TONE
The basic options in the system of MOOD are set out as a system network in Figure 3.8.
94
tagged
↘ +Moodtag
non-exclamative
MOOD (Tagfinite ^
TAGGING
Tagsubject)
DECLARATIVE
declarative
↘ Subject ^ Finite TYPE untagged
exclamative
↘ +Wh; Wh ^ Subject
complement
indicative ↘ Complement/Wh
WH-
↘ + Mood
(+Finite, yes/no interrogative SELECTION
+Subject) ↘ Finite ^ Subject
non- subject adjunct
INTERROGATIVE
interrogative ↘ Adjunct/Wh
TYPE
WH-
MOOD wh- interrogative
free SELECTION
TYPE
↘ +Wh; Wh ^ Finite
FREEDOM subject
major ↘ Subject/Wh
↘ + Residue
STATUS bound
clause oblative
imperative ↘ +Subject: ‘let me’
minor IMPERATIVE
suggestive
TYPE ↘ +Subject: ‘let’s’ explicit
DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
↘ +Subject ^ ‘you’
IMPERATIVE
jussive
SUBJECT
PRESUMPTION
implicit
The options in the system of MOOD gain their realisation in structures of the clause
(see 5.2 below) and in selections in TONE (see IFG2 Section 8.9; IFG3 Section 4.4.4).
Figure 3.8 displays the options and shows the element of structure (and/or ordering
of elements) which realises each option, together with the unmarked tone realisations.
Note that to differentiate between the different contrasts in meaning made available
by the system of TONE, we need to extend the description of MOOD in delicacy (showing
finer choices). The more delicate MOOD systems realised by selections in tone (rather
than by the interpersonal structure of the clause) are known as the KEY systems (see
IFG2 Section 8.9 IFG3: 142; for more detail see Halliday and Greaves 2008).
We can now add to our consideration of any text an analysis of the interpersonal
organisation of the clause as a move in an exchange, as a contribution to the
development of dialogue. This means that there is an element for enacting a speech
function, the Mood element, potentially an element for eliciting a response to the
speech function, the Moodtag, and an element that expresses the rest of the
proposition or proposal being negotiated through the speech function, the Residue.
The interpersonal structure of the clause is thus Mood + Residue (+ Moodtag).
The Mood element grounds the proposition or proposal by providing a 'modally
responsible' element – the Subject – and by providing terms for negotiation in choices
of TENSE/MODALITY and POLARITY carried by the Finite and/or Mood Adjunct(s). See
Figure 3.9.
In addition, there may be interpersonal elements that fall outside this modal
structure: the Vocative element, which addresses the listener, and the Expletive (see
Section 2.4).
verbal
verbal group operator pronoun
nominal groups
The difference between the two functional perspectives is shown in the different
set of variants each displays. Textually, we find related variants such as:
Madam you'll look like a tulip : You'll look like a tulip Madam : Like a tulip you'll look
Madam
Interpersonally, these are all declarative clauses and thus do not contrast.
Interpersonal variants differ in MOOD; for example:
Madam you'll look like a tulip : Madam will you look like a tulip? : Madam who'll look like a
tulip? : Madam look like a tulip!
Textually, these clauses are all alike in having unmarked [topical] Theme. 15 Thus
we need to recognise different patterns of meaning relating to different metafunctions,
but simultaneously present in any clause.
Further reading
See especially Teruya 2007 for a recent survey of IFG based studies of MOOD and issues
arising; Caffarel et al. 2004 further interprets MOOD from a functional typological
perspective across a range of languages.
Caffarel, Alice 1995 Approaching the French clause as a move in a dialogue. In Hasan &
Fries (eds) 1-50.
Caffarel, A, J R Martin & C M I M Matthiessen 2004 Language Typology: a functional
perspective. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Halliday, M A K 1970 Functional diversity in language, as seen from a consideration of
modality and mood in English. Foundations of Language 6.3: 322-361 [Reprinted
in M A K Halliday 2005 Studies in English Language. London: Continuum (Volume
7 in the Collected Works of M A K Halliday). 164-204]
Halliday, M A K 1982 The de-automatisation of grammar: from Priestley's 'An
Inspector Calls'. J M Anderson (ed) Language Form and Linguistic Variation:
papers dedicated to Angus MacIntosh. Amsterdam: Benjamins. 129-159. [Reprinted
in M A K Halliday 2002 Linguistic Studies of Text and Discourse. London:
Continuum (Volume 2 in the Collected Works of M A K Halliday). 126-148]
15 We can add one further set of examples to bring out the difference between textual and interpersonal
variation (‘agnation’, to use the technical term): interpersonally, Madam you'll look like a tulip is related
to Madam you'll look like a tulip, won't you?; textually, Madam you'll look like a tulip is related to Madam
you'll look like a tulip, you will. The first variant, a question tag, is concerned with eliciting an indication of
the listener's state of agreement with the proposition in the dialogic interaction (e.g. No, I won't!),
whereas the second variant serves to present a reminder of the Theme at the end of the clause (a
strategy used in certain varieties of English).
CHAPTER 3 MOOD 97
Transitivity
clause as representation
1. Orientation
1 The third metafunction is the ideational one; but it has two modes for construing experience, the
experiential and the logical. Within the clause, we find the experiential mode, manifested in the system
of TRANSITIVITY (discussed in this chapter). The logical mode provides the resources for forming various
kinds of complexes – clause complexes, group complexes, and so on (IFG Ch.7), and it plays a role
together with the experiential mode in the organisation of groups (nominal groups, verbal groups etc.
(IFG Ch. 6)).
CHAPTER 4 TRANSITIVITY 99
experience of flux
construed as
2 It is easy to fall into the trap of thinking that there is some mode of objective phenomenal world that can
be reflected in different ways in language (especially given our somewhat simplistic diagram, where we
actually move from one semiotic representation to another, the first one standing for the outside world
experienced); but there is not. We construe our experience actively by imposing organisation. An
important aspect of this is treating things, events, etc. that are unlike one another as alike. It is perhaps
easier to realise this if we consider more abstract domains of experience – social value systems, religious
systems, scientific models.
3 Motion is in fact one domain of experience where there is a good deal of variation in how languages model
it and in how the process of motion is construed lexicogrammatically. This can be seen, for example, when
one translates a narrative of motion from one language into another language where it is treated in a
different way. There is now a significant body of literature on this topic.
100 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
DIRECTED MOTION she swam across she swam crossing she crossed the
the river the river river by swimming
POSSESSION she has a car with her is a car s/he (location)
car be: exist
PRECIPITATION it's raining water is falling rain is falling the sky's dropping
water
When we look at the different ways different languages construe the same
semantic domain, we find variation along the various dimensions of transitivity, e.g. in
whether some phenomenon is construed as a process or a participant. However, such
choices are also available within a given language. For example, we have alternatives
such as in May people bought more and more TVs, VCRs, etc. and May saw a sharp rise in
consumer electronic purchases. These are complementary; they are both part of the
overall system, but the second example depends on the system being expanded
through grammatical metaphor (IFG Chapter 10), where (among other things)
processes are construed as if they were participants.
2. Survey of options
Each of the six types of process has its own small set of participant roles. For
example, a material clause involves an Actor and possibly a Goal:
She stirred the coffee
Actor Process: material Goal
Table 4.3 shows the central, or 'nuclear', participants involved in each process
type together with an example clause. Additionally, each process type has further
participants less centrally involved in the process, often being closer to circumstances.
In IFG2 Section 5.6 these are generalised across process types as the two roles of
Beneficiary and Range. In IFG2: 166, Table 5 (18) and IFG3: 260, Table 5 (27), they are
differentiated and labeled according to process type. These additional roles are also
shown in Table 4.3 in the final column and will be introduced in Section 2 below.
material Actor, Goal she made the coffee Initiator, Recipient, Client,
Scope**, Attribute
* Found only in a sub-type of behavioural/verbal clause (see further Sections 2.4 and 2.5 below).
** Referred to only in terms of the ergative role of ‘Range’ in IFG2.
(ii) CIRCUMSTANTIATION is the resource for ‘augmenting’ the configuration of
process + participants by means of elements that are less directly involved in the
process, circumstances. Circumstances are discussed in Section 5.7 of IFG2 and
Section 6.6 of IFG3. They are general across process types (precisely because they are less
centrally involved in the process than participants). Examples are given in Table 4.4:
Table 4.4 Types of circumstance in English
The various process types differ according to the criteria summarised in IFG2:
173 and IFG3: 301. (See Troubleshooting section for further discussion.) A brief
outline of characteristics of each type is given below.
addition, there may also be a Goal – a participant impacted by a doing (the one done
to/with), and sometimes a Client or Recipient4 – a participant benefitting from the
doing (the one given to or done for), or else (in clauses without a Goal) a Scope5 – a
participant specifying the scope of a happening. Examples are shown in Table 4.5:
Table 4.5 Participant roles in material clauses
The Goal is either actually brought into existence by the doing (build a house, bake
a cake, write a letter), or it exists prior to the doing, but is impacted in some way. In the
latter case, the Goal can be probed with do to/with, as in what did she do with the chair?
or what she did with the chair was move it. Also, it can often be accompanied by a
representation of the result of the impact, such as a new location (she moved the chair
into the corner), a new possessor (she gave the chair to the duke) or a new quality (she
scrubbed the chair clean). In this, it differs from the Scope, which cannot be probed
with do to/with; thus we do not get: what she did with [Scope:] the mountain was climb
it, she climbed [Scope:] the mountain into fame. This is because the Scope is not
impacted by the performance of the Process; it construes the scope or domain over
which the process takes place.
The Client or Recipient can be probed by looking at that version of a clause
where it follows the Goal. In this case, it is realised with a preposition – either to,
marking it as a Recipient type of Beneficiary, or for, marking it as a Client type of
Beneficiary:
She gave his aunt a teapot : She gave a teapot to his aunt.
She built his aunt a gazebo : She built a gazebo for his aunt.
The Recipient appears when the clause construes a transfer in possession of
existing goods, while the Client is involved when the clause construes a provision of
service (including creation of new goods).
6 See Troubleshooting Section 3.5 (iv) below for guidance in discriminating a projected clause and an
embedded clause functioning as Phenomenon within the mental process clause.
7 In addition to the roles of Token and Value, participants in identifying clauses construe the roles of
Identified and Identifier (see IFG2:122ff ; IFG3: 227-9 )
CHAPTER 4 TRANSITIVITY 105
name referent name, christen, call [:causative] [They] named the baby John
function filler function as, serve as, act as The reservoir functions as the
city’s emergency water supply
holder of position vote, elect [causative] [The people] voted him President
position
actor role play, act as Jenny played Ophelia
Table 4.7 shows some contrasting examples of attributive and identifying clauses.
Table 4.7 Attributive and identifying clauses
8 This is clear from the oddness of passive and theme-predicated variants such as to leave at once was said
to me by them, it was to leave at once that they said to me.
9 Note that IFG also includes examples where the clause is not an exchange of information and the content
of saying is a non-linguistic commodity, e.g. 'She requested a dance'.
CHAPTER 4 TRANSITIVITY 107
I’m listening to John working I hear John working; I can hear John working
The cat’s sniffing the flower The cat smells the flower
I’m tasting the wine I taste the wine in this; I can taste the wine in the sauce
Behaviourals include categories reflecting the mental and verbal ones: see Table
4.9.
behavioural mental/verbal
perceptive look at, watch, stare, gawk, view, look over, see, observe
observe
listen to, eavesdrop hear, overhear
sniff, smell smell
taste taste
feel feel
108 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
behavioural mental/verbal
cognitive ponder, puzzle, solve, work out, meditate, think, know, believe
ruminate, think
emotive smile, frown, laugh, pout, gasp, grin, scowl, fear, enjoy, like ; frighten, scare, alarm,
shake, shudder, tremble disgust, please, amuse, upset
verbal10 whine, whinge, mumble, stammer, stutter, say; tell, ask
mutter, moan, chatter, gossip, talk, speak,
sing;
frown, grimace, snort, cough;
slander, insult, praise, flatter [These are
treated as verbal processes with a Target
in IFG. See IFG2:141; IFG3:255.]
Behaviourals also include more material-like subtypes. The border area (see
Section 3.1) between material processes and behavioural ones is covered by two main
types, physiological processes – twitch, shiver, tremble, sweat, etc. – and social
processes – kiss, hug, embrace, dance, play, etc. Both of these shade into the verbal type,
from different angles; physiologically: cough, gasp, stutter, etc. and socially: chat, talk,
gossip etc.
In an existential clause, the there signals the process type but does not function as
a Location circumstance and nor does it represent a participant. However existential
clauses frequently have a circumstance of Location and if it occurs in thematic (initial)
position, the existential there may be absent:
All around them was a silence.
Along this waterway toiled a string of wolfish dogs.
Location Process: Existent
existential
But the absent there will turn up when the clause is tagged, as in all around them
was silence, wasn’t there?; along this waterway toiled a string of wolfish dogs, didn’t there?
10 IFG3: 255 treats the verbal subcategory of behaviourals as a special class of verbal process. (It is only
members of this class that may involve a Target participant.) Unlike other verbal clauses, they cannot
project another clause, and a projection has to be added by means of a verb like say, as in Then the monk
praised Yang Shan saying: ‘I have come over to China in order to worship Manjucri, and met unexpectedly
with Minor Shakya’ (although there are natural examples such as ‘Nelson played magnificent football’,
Meek praised.). The closest to a representation of the content of praising, blaming, etc. in English is an
enhancing clause of reason, as in She praised him for having acted so quickly; in Japanese, such clauses can
in fact project (see Teruya, 2007). However, like verbal clauses, they can be construed with a Receiver, as
in She praised him to her boss for having acted so quickly.
CHAPTER 4 TRANSITIVITY 109
additional Agent
Process
intransitive
Actor
Process transitive
Goal
Actor
From the transitive perspective, the question we are asking is whether the action
carries over to affect an additional participant or not, and this provides the contrast
between transitive and intransitive clauses. See Table 4.11:
110 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
Process
middle
Medium
Process
Agent effective
Medium
The additional question we can ask from the ergative perspective is who or what
brought the happening about (the effective voice). Example clauses are shown in Table
4.12:
Halliday suggests that all English clauses can be analysed from either a transitive
or an ergative perspective, even though some clause types lend themselves to one
analysis or the other,11 as do some texts. The transitive categories can be ‘translated’
into ergative ones as in Table 4.13, which shows that in addition to the Agent and
Medium roles, the ergative model includes the Beneficiary and Range. (Note too that
all the differentiated ‘additional Agent’ roles exemplified in Section 2.7 above are
Agents in effective clauses in the ergative interpretation.)
11 For example, the police attacked the militia lends itself to a transitive analysis, pairing naturally with the
police attacked – and could thus be challenged no, the army attacked since it is the police or army that are
doing the attacking; the sergeant marched the troops on the other hand lends itself to an ergative analysis,
pairing naturally with the troops marched – and can't be challenged: no, the corporal marched, since it is
the troops rather than the sergeant or corporal that are doing the marching.
112 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
3. Troubleshooting
happening
and doing saying
material behavioural: ‘saying’ verbal
as activity
space as external
motion behavioural: symbolise as sensing
sensing as activity communication
space as internal
relation saying
[symbolisation
as identity]
relational mental
sensing as [sensing as
attribute inert process]
being and sensing
having
The clines are not all of the same type – some are more discrete than others, some
clearly involve grammatical metaphor, and so on; but it is useful to bring them
together for purposes of discussion and to explain why there may be different possible
interpretations of ‘borderline’ examples. (Sometimes transitivity has been discussed in
terms of a single scale running from high transitivity to low transitivity; but this is in
fact only one type of consideration.)
To avoid clutter in the figure we have left out some clines. For instance, there is,
arguably, a cline between receptive (passive) material (it was broken (by a stone)) and
attributive relational (it was broken and useless) and a similar one between receptive
(passive) mental (he was excited (by the prospect)) and, again, attributive relational (he
was happy and excited). (See IFG2: 121-2; IFG3: 224-6.)
Because the different process types ‘meet’ at certain points in the
semantic-grammatical ‘topology’ illustrated above, some indeterminacy in assigning a
particular text instance to a typological category is to be expected on occasion.
Nonetheless the ‘core’ instances can always be distinguished by checking for
differentiating criteria, as discussed further below in the sections indicated in Table
4.14:
Table 4.14 Troubleshooting sections for distinguishing process types
In all the above cases, there is a single clause, with a single Process element, but
this element is realised by a complex of verbal groups. For example the Process will
start to enjoy is realised by a complex of the two verbal groups will start and to enjoy.
In a TRANSITIVITY analysis, the second (non-finite) verbal group is the relevant one for
PROCESS TYPE. Thus, they will start to enjoy the camp next week is a mental clause, they
began asking questions is a verbal clause and they seem to have enough money is a
possessive relational one. (See IFG2 7A.4-6; IFG3 8.5.) In contrast, the first verbal
group elaborates the unfolding of the Process, for example in terms of phase (begin/
continue/stop doing). Note that these simple clauses with complex Processes are
12 We discuss cases with two lexical verbs here. But the potential for introducing additional expansions
exists; e.g. he seemed to begin to try to study harder. What we say here about the first verb applies
to all in such a series except the last; the second verb in our discussion corresponds to the last in the
series.
CHAPTER 4 TRANSITIVITY 115
different from cases where there are two clauses – and hence two distinct Processes –
such as the following (see further Chapter 6):
he got up || and ( ) turned on the light
he got up || yawning
he got up || to let in the cat
he got up || having told them his news
Process Process
Where there are two distinct Processes like this (i.e. two clauses), each can be
associated with its own circumstances, as in he left hurriedly at 3 pm, having told them
his news quietly before the meeting.
Less clear-cut is the analysis of examples similar to those in the first group above
but where the first lexical verb has a mental or verbal meaning:
they want to change their library books
they ’d like to bring their friends
they claim to know the Minister
IFG recommends that these be analysed as a single transitivity configuration, in
the same way as the earlier examples. Thus:
they want to change their library books
Actor Process: material Goal
This does not, however, foreground the fact that the choice of participant is
restricted by the lexical verb in the first verbal group (i.e. only a conscious or semiotic
entity can be construed as the major participant). An alternative analysis which gives
equal status to both verbal groups by taking them to be processes of separate clauses
may therefore be preferred:
they want to change their library books
Senser Process: mental Process: material Goal
The interpretation here is that there are two clauses forming a clause complex,
just as in cases where each process has its own set of participants and circumstances,
as in the following examples:
they want you to change their books tomorrow
they told us they would change their books tomorrow
See IFG2 7A.6 and IFG3 8.8 for discussion of alternate analyses of the inter-
mediate examples.
A second crucial probe is whether the clause can project (i.e., report or quote)
another clause or not. If it can, it is not material (but rather verbal or mental). Finally,
it is important to realise that in certain types of text a material process may in fact be
quite abstract in character. (See Section 2.1 above.)
(ii) Material or relational?
See Section 3.6 (i) below.
(iii) Material clauses with an Attribute
In addition to the participant roles discussed in the Survey section (Actor, Goal,
Client/Recipient, Scope, Initiator), we may also find an Attribute in a material clause
which has a Goal. The Attribute either specifies the state of a participant when
participating in the process or as a result of the process. For example:
They left the bedroom happy
Actor Process Goal Attribute (depictive)
Note the potential here for an interpretation according to the relational model of
Attributor + Process + Carrier + Attribute. However in a material clause the Attribute
can always be left out, which is not the case in an attributive relational clause. Thus we
folded the paper flat: we folded the paper but not we kept the paper flat: we kept the
paper. (See further IFG2: 157; IFG3: 194-5.)
(iv) Scope13 or Goal?
The difference between a Scope and a Goal in a material clause is very clear in the
prototypical case: the Goal is (as the name suggests) the participant that is impacted
by the performance of the Process by the Actor. For example:
he squashed the slug;
they felled the tree;
they threw the ball into a corner;
he mashed the potatoes;
she repaired the car;
he broke the glass.
The nature of the relationship between Actor-Process and Goal is thus very much
a material one.
In contrast, the Scope elaborates or enhances the Process.
(1) It elaborates the Process –
(a) by restatement (the traditional category of cognate object), as in:
he ran a tremendous race
(b) by specification (of the ‘lexical content’), as in:
they took a leisurely walk; she took a brisk shower
(c) or by exemplification (i.e., by giving a subtype of the process), as in:
13 Note (1) Scope is referred to by the ergative term Range in IFG2. (2) An equivalent role in a behavioural
clause is termed Behaviour (e.g. She sang four songs). (See IFG3: 294.)
CHAPTER 4 TRANSITIVITY 117
they played a game of tennis; they danced a polka
(2) Alternatively, the Scope may enhance the Process, typically, by a circumstantial
feature of space (location or extent):
they climbed the nearest peak; the horse jumped the fence; they roamed the streets
The Goal and the Scope thus differ in the degree and type of their involvement in
the Process: the Goal is a real participant, actually affected by the performance of the
process, whereas the Scope is a restatement or further specification of the Process
itself. From these differences follow differences in probes:
Goal: (1) it is impacted (affected), so it can often be followed by an Attribute, a
Role, a Recipient, or a Location of destination that represents the result of the impact 14
– drive the car [Attribute:] hot [the car is hot]; paint the wall [Attribute:] pink; cut the
onions [Role: product:] into cubes; kick the cat [Location:] down the stairs; give
[Recipient:] the kid a car, etc.; (2) it is impacted, so it can be probed by do to/with
(what did he do to the wall? – he painted it pink).15
Scope: it represents the scope of the performance of the Process, so it can often
be preceded by a preposition that indicates the nature of this scope (on, at, in, etc.) –
she climbed up the ladder. When it is a restatement of the Process, there is typically an
alternative wording with Process alone – have a walk ~ walk; take a shower ~ shower.
Since the Scope isn’t impacted, it can neither be construed with a result (Attribute, etc.)
nor be probed with do to/with. Scopes are contrasted with Goals in Table 4.15:
Table 4.15 Contrasting examples of Scope and Goal
As can be expected, there are a number of cases that are less clear than those
represented by the examples given above. From one point of view, Goal and Scope are
really points on a cline between highly affected material participant (i.e., Goal) and
non-affected material participant (i.e., Scope). Various factors determine to what
extent a particular participant approximates a prototypical Goal or a prototypical
14 This resultative Attribute may be 'incorporated' in the process itself – kill = 'cause to be dead'; break =
'cause to be broken'; flatten = 'cause to be flat'; etc. – in which case it cannot be configured as a separate
element.
15 This probe with 'do to/with' does not apply to Goals in creative material clauses, i.e. Goals that are
actually brought into existence by the performance of the process; but the nature of the impact on them
is clear enough – they actually result from the performance of the process: he'll bake a cake tomorrow,
Wren wants to build another gazebo, Danes design opera houses.
118 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
Note that the first example, like many other mental clauses, has an alternative
active version where the Senser is Subject in Our children respond to violent TV
programs.
Mental and relational clauses both construe emotion. And very often the same
emotion may be construed either as a mode of conscious processing or as an attribute.
For example:
Who fears Virginia Wolf? (mental)
Who’s afraid of Virginia Wolf? (relational)
Her story depressed me. (mental)
I felt unhappy. (relational)
Since both clause types construe emotion, it may be hard to know whether a
particular example is a mental or relational clause. The pairs above are clearly distinct.
For instance, relationally fear or misery can be intensified by very (as in he’s very
afraid of Virginia Wolf; I felt very unhappy), and mentally they can be passive (as in
Virginia Wolf is feared by him; I was depressed by her story). However, with clauses such
as he is scared, it is harder to tell:
He is scared
mental Senser Process: passive
The mental interpretation relates the example to he is scared by large dogs: large
dogs scare him, he got scared, and so on; the relational interpretation relates it to he is
very scared, he seems scared, he became scared, and so on.
(iv) Phenomenon or projected idea?
It is not always easy to judge whether a clause is embedded into the Phenomenon role
or whether it is ‘projected’ as a separate clause by the mental process. (See further
Chapter 6.) The first thing to consider is what category of mental clause is involved,
since perception clauses do not project and nor do emotion clauses, such as enjoy or
dislike: See Table 4.16:
Table 4.16 Mental process types
16 Note that a perception verb like see may also function as a (projecting) cognition process, as in he saw
that no amount of discussion would make any difference.
120 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
projected clause. One test is to see whether the element in question can function as
Subject in a passive variant of the clause. If it can, it is a participant (Phenomenon).
Consider the following examples:
The supervisor wanted them to leave.
Jem thought the idea was preposterous.
Did you find out who was responsible?
He accepted that he would not get sole custody.
They all understood that there would be a cancellation fee.
If we explore the possibility of passive variants we find that this is only possible
for the final two examples:
* (For) them to leave was wanted by the supervisor.
* (That) the idea was preposterous was thought by Jem.
* Was who was responsible found out by you?
(The fact) that he would not get sole custody was accepted by him.
(The fact) that there would be a cancellation fee was understood by them all.
Thus them to leave, that the idea was preposterous and who was responsible are
projected clauses not participants.
Another test that can be applied in doubtful cases is that of theme predication
(see Chapter 2). As a participant, a Phenomenon is available for a theme predicated
variant of the clause, whereas a projected clause is not. Thus:
* It was (for) them to leave that was wanted by him.
* It was (that) the idea was preposterous that was thought by Jem.
* Was it who was responsible that was found out by you?
It was (the fact) that he would not get sole custody that was accepted by him.
It was (the fact) that there would be a cancellation fee that was understood by them all.
(v) Projecting clause complex or verbal group complex?
See Section 3.3 above and IFG2 Section 7A.6; IFG3 Section 8.8.
back to Newton and Galileo. As usual, we can also probe by exploring agnate verbs.
Here we would have the idea of relativity dates from Newton and Galileo, which is
clearly relational (cf. the impossibility of the idea is dating from Newton and Galileo);
but we cannot relate the example to the idea of relativity moves back to Galileo and
Newton, which would be material; nor can a circumstance of Manner be added to
represent the rate of motion: we cannot say the idea of relativity is going back quickly
to Newton and Galileo.
Similarly contrast:
The road runs along the river (relational) :
The jogger is running along the river (material)
The fence surrounds the White House (relational) :
The protestors are surrounding the White House (material)
Possession
Although have, in its meaning of possess, is a relational process, the meaning of
possession may be construed materially where it involves some dynamic aspect such
as getting, taking, obtaining, giving, receiving. Thus:
The young man has high qualifications. (Carrier + Pro + Attribute)
The young man is obtaining high qualifications. (Actor + Pro + Scope)
(Note also the material use of have, as in the young man is having dinner.)
Change of state
A state of being is typically represented in a Carrier + Process + Attribute structure
such as in the porridge was cold. In addition, a change of state can be represented
relationally, as in the porridge became/got/turned cold. This latter set of attributives is
distinctive however, in selecting present-in-present as the unmarked choice for
present time: it’s getting late, it’s turning cold, etc. In this they reflect a greater
semantic proximity to the material process type. And in fact a change of state can also
be construed as a material process with no Attribute element, as in she paled, the sky
brightened, the water boiled.
(ii) Relational or mental?
See Section 3.5 (iii) above.
(iii) Relational or verbal?
See Section 3.7 (iii) below.
(iv) Attributive or identifying?
(1) The most important test is that of reversibility. Compare:
Tendulkar is short : *Short is Tendulkar [attributive]
Border is the captain : The captain is Border [identifying]
Attributive clauses really have only one participant that can serve as Subject – the
Carrier. Consequently, these clauses have no passive variants and are thus not
reversible.
The reversibility test distinguishes easily between ‘quality’ type attributive
clauses and identifying ones, but is not so successful if the clause is a nominal type
attributive. Compare:
122 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
(You can use represent as a substitute relational verb as here, or check IFG2: 120, 123
and IFG3: 238 Table 5(18) for other relational verbs.)
(vi) Relational clauses sometimes not recognised
There are some relational clause types that differ from the typical Carrier + Process +
Attribute or Token + Process + Value structure in some respect – (i) they have an
Agent (Assigner, Attributor), or (ii) a Beneficiary, or (iii) the Process itself embodies
the Attribute role. These types are sometimes not recognised in analysis so we are
drawing attention to them here with a few examples of each.
attributive
She made him a good citizen
This proves him wrong
Attributor Process: attributive Carrier Attribute
Note that these should not be confused with material processes which include a
‘resultative’ Attribute. See Section 3.4 (iii) above.
(2) Benefactive (attributive) relational clauses (i.e. with Beneficiary role)
He owes her three dollars
He makes her a good husband
The room cost me fifteen dollars
Note the ambiguity of an example such as she made him a good friend:
she made him a good friend
(a) Attributor Process Carrier Attribute
(b) Carrier Process Beneficiary Attribute
(c) Actor Process Client Goal
They can also, however, function to modify the meaning expressed by the verb in
examples where the process is not relational:
124 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
But as noted in IFG, they cannot project reported speech and are towards the
material end of a cline. Because they cannot project, these too could be taken as
behavioural and analysed in the following way:
They praised her to her parents
Behaver Process: behavioural Target Receiver
One final borderline case is when a sound is ‘quoted’, usually with go realising the
process. For example:
the tyres went ‘screech!’
the little engine went ‘wheeee!’
the car went ‘bang!’
18 IFG3: 251 Table 5(24) lists these as verbs functioning in behavioural clauses; however they also appear
on p. 255 as examples of verbs functioning in verbal clauses of ‘activity’. The inability of such clauses to
project is an argument for preferring the behavioural interpretation.
CHAPTER 4 TRANSITIVITY 125
It is not possible to have a Receiver in these clauses since the sound is simply a
sound and not a piece of language being addressed to anyone. They are therefore
moving towards the material end of a cline and are best treated as behavioural.
(ii) Verbal or mental?
See Section 3.5 (ii) above.
Note too that a small set of ‘verbal judgement’ verbs – see Section 3.7 (i) above –
such as condone, criticise, excuse, applaud may also occur with an embedded fact
clause: The press applauded the fact that no violence occurred; The press condoned the
fact that some violence occurred. In these contexts, no Receiver is possible and such
verbs are functioning as mental processes of emotion.
(iii) Verbal or relational?
There are a number of verbs (show, indicate, suggest) that can serve either in verbal
clauses or in identifying relational ones. Contrast:
She showed (told, convinced) us || that the substance was potassium (verbal)
The result showed (meant, was) [[that the substance was potassium]] (relational)
They emphasised/highlighted/underlined (said clearly) to the union || that there would
be no further negotiations on the pay offer (verbal)19
The stripes emphasise/highlight the width of the material (relational)
One difference between the two process types illustrated by these examples is
that a verbal clause will usually admit a Receiver whereas a relational one will not.
19 Note that he took the pen and highlighted/underlined the main points in the chapter is an example of these
verbs functioning in a material clause.
126 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
clause, you may choose to label the second participant as Phenomenon or Verbiage:
they solved the problem
they watched the cricket
Behaver Process: behavioural Phenomenon
Note that some behavioural clauses of the verbal subtype will also admit a Target
role, as in:
they insulted her (to her face)
Behaver Process: behavioural Target (Receiver)
They got off the old bus OR They got off the old bus
Actor Process : material Scope Actor Pro : mat Location
Often it will be obvious that the preposition must count as part of the process
realisation. For example:
CHAPTER 4 TRANSITIVITY 127
They ran down the boy They ran down the lane
Actor Process : material Goal Actor Pro : mater. Location
If in doubt, the best test is to check the ‘circumstance’ as the focus of theme
predication.
??It was off the old bus they got.
?It was at the snail we were looking.
It wasn’t about the strike (that) they were worrying.
It was for a chat they called.
Where the prepositional phrase is feasible as a predicated Theme, as in the last
two cases, it is likely to be functioning as a circumstance.
Circumstance or Recipient (or Client)?
Examples:
She sent the parcel to New York. Location
She sent the parcel to her aunt. Recipient
If an expression like to New York or to her Aunt is Recipient then it can also occur
in an alternative position in the clause without the preposition to. Thus:
*She sent New York the parcel.
She sent her aunt the parcel.
A similar test can discriminate a Client from a Cause:
She committed crimes for her children. Cause
She made a cake for her aunt. Client
*She committed her children crimes.
She made her aunt a cake. Client
Manner or Attribute?
Examples:
Last year, the term went by faster. Manner
Maybe it is a bit like reading a book. Attribute
He is luckier than most. Attribute
The Attribute is always inherent in an attributive clause, whereas Manner is not
inherent in any transitivity types. Consequently, you can probe the difference by
leaving out the element in question. Further, while you can’t tell with faster whether it
is adjectival or adverbial, you can check a related lexical item to see whether it is
adjectival (and hence Attribute) or adverbial (and hence Manner). For example:
Time goes more quickly when you’re having fun.
Manner: means or Agent?
Examples:
His jaw was broken by a particularly savage blow. Agent (Actor)
He was chosen for that task by chance. Manner
128 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
mistaken as circumstances. They are marked with β. Processes are picked out in bold:
α H. Hertz had demonstrated the truth in the theory ||
β by transmitting radio waves over a short distance.
α Newtonian physics used this argument ||
β in developing the special theory of relativity.
α As well as committing funds to this new highway
β we should be upgrading public transport in our cities...
(iv) Circumstance or textual conjunctive?
Circumstances and conjunctives are metafunctionally distinct, but there can be areas
of overlap within the semantic domain of space-time. First let’s look at a Conjunctive
example:
Earlier, several attempts had been made to cross the Blue Mountains.
Here earlier marks a temporal relation in construction of the narrative – a
flashback. Such relations in text are conjunctive and if they are made explicit, they are
marked by elements serving as linkers within the textual layer of the clause.
By contrast, circumstances of time construe time as an aspect of the process
configuration itself, representing a location in time, in terms of some time interval (in
the morning, on Monday, after midnight, before Easter, during the Middle Ages), in terms
of the now of speaking (now, then; yesterday, tomorrow), or in terms an event (after the
meeting, at breakfast, before the war).
In principle, you can probe the difference by checking whether the element in
question can be the focus of theme predication. For example, it was before 1820 that
several attempts had been made to cross the Blue Mountains is perfectly fine, whereas it
was earlier that several attempts had been made to cross the Blue Mountains is not so
good. This is clearer outside the domain of space-time. For instance, it was however/
nevertheless/consequently/furthermore that several attempts had been made to cross
the Blue Mountains are all impossible.
The border between conjunctives and circumstances is hazier in the domain of
space-time, as shown in Table 4.17:
Table 4.17 Temporal conjunctives and circumstances
There are unambiguously textual items with a clear anaphoric sense: earlier
means ‘earlier than this’, previously means ‘before this’, afterwards means ‘after this’;
all these relate the current clause to previous text. Then there are circumstances
which provide a time reference within the clause in which they occur, such as earlier
than 1942, before the war, after the meeting.
In between these, are those items which have a noun representing a period of
130 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
time (year, day, hour, minute, month, period, etc.) as the central (‘Head’) element and
which are modified by an item with conjunctive implications in the text: (next, previous,
subsequent, following, etc.) – The previous year they had made several attempts; the
following day they would try again. On the grounds that these can be the focus of
Theme predication – only available to ‘topical’ Themes – they can be analysed as
circumstances.
4. Analysis practice
4.1 Phase I
4.1.1 Exercises
Exercise 1 Practising the tense test
Write a clause agnate to each of the following examples which would be used to
comment on action or states concurrent with the moment of speaking:
A small boy stood bravely on the ramparts. A small boy is standing bravely on the
ramparts.
He had a crown on his head. He has a crown on his head.
Text 2 Explanation
1. After flash floods, desert streams from upland areas carry heavy loads of silt, sand and
rock fragments. || 2. As* they reach the flatter area of desert basins || 3. they slow down ||
4. and their waters may soak quickly into the basin floor.|| 5. Then the streams drop
their loads; || 6. first they drop the heaviest material – the stones, || 7. then [they drop]
the sand || 8. and finally [they drop] the silt. || 9. Soon these short lived streams become
choked by their own deposits ||10. and they spread their load in all directions.|| 11. After
some time, fan or con-shaped deposits of gravel, sand, silt and clay are formed around
each valley or canyon outlet. || 12.These are called alluvial fans.||
[Adapted from Sale, C., Wilson, G & Friedman, B. Our changing world Bk 1. Melbourne,
Longman Cheshire 1980:54.]
* Note that as, then, first, then, and finally have no role in transitivity. They are textual elements, linking
one clause or sentence to another, rather than being circumstances. See Section 3.10 (iv).
Text 2
PROCESS TYPE CIRCUMSTANTIATION
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
10.
11.
12.
136 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
Text 4
material verbal mental relational
CHAPTER 4 TRANSITIVITY 137
4.2 Phase II
4.2.1 Exercises
Exercise 1 Identifying participant roles
Go back to Phase 1, exercise 4 and label the Actor, Goal, Senser and Phenomenon
participants. For example:
The boy stood bravely on the ramparts. material
Actor
Only his closest attendants knew his true identity. mental
Senser Phenomenon
4. The basic mechanism is one where the teacher surveils the student.
For further practice in analysing texts for transitivity, texts 5 & 6 are provided.
The Key will include analyses for embedded clauses.
Text 5 Procedure
Blackbirds take the heaviest toll of raspberries. They are more troublesome in gardens
where there is plenty of cover to nest in, and fewer plants to feed from, than in large open
raspberry fields. Unless you can devise a really efficient way of scaring them, you will have
to net. To support the net, make a framework of wires or canes fixed to posts over the row.
Cover the tops of the posts with polythene or jam jars so that the net can be pulled
smoothly over them without snagging. Alternatively, buy more expensive supports which
slot together to form a rigid frame. The net should be high enough above the row to
prevent birds sitting on the top and pecking through; it must hang well clear of the sides
as fruit is carried on side shoots growing out from the canes.
[From Spiller, M. Growing fruit. Harmondsworth: Penguin Books: 155.]
146 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
4.3.1 Exercises
10. Dolphins are toothed whales with a clear dorsal fin, some cervical vertebrae
fused and spatular teeth.
CHAPTER 4 TRANSITIVITY 147
3. The last item on the program said Secondary School Choir – Carol.
He persuaded her (that the proposal was genuine.) i.e. ‘he made her believe’
Inducer Pro:mental Senser
4. She failed to teach them that Tokens were Subject in the active.
Exercise 10 Ergativity
Analyse the follwing clauses from both a transitive and ergative perspective:
He opened the box slowly.
Actor Material Goal Manner
Agent Process Medium Manner
Exercise 11 Ergativity
Analyse the following clauses from both a transitive and ergative perspective.
The argument convinced her later that evening.
Phenomenon Pro:mental Senser Location
Agent Process Medium Location
Text 1 Description
The following text was considered in Chapter 2 from the point of view of Theme
choices. Now analyse the TRANSITIVITY structures.
The trail of the meat
The land was cold and white and savage. Across it there ran a thread of frozen waterway,
with dark spruce forest looming on either side. Along this waterway toiled a string of
wolfish dogs, hauling a sled of birch-bark. On the sled, along with the camp-outfit, was
lashed a long and narrow oblong box. In front of the dogs, on wide snowshoes, toiled a
man. Behind the sled came a second man. On the sled in the box lay a third man, whose life
was at an end – a man whom the Wild had beaten down and conquered. The bodies of the
live men were covered with soft fur and leather. Their faces were blurred and shapeless
under a coating of crystals from their frozen breath. All around them was a silence which
seemed to press upon them as water does upon a diver.
[J. London White Fang. Abridged Edition N. Farr, Pendulum Press 1977.]
circumstances
associated with
participants
involved in
process
The major options in TRANSITIVITY as they are presented in IFG are summarised in
the system network in Figure 4.7, with references to sections in IFG2 followed by
references to IFG3 if different.
154 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
middle
AGENCY
behavioural
+ Behaver § 5.5.1
‘please’-type Senser Phenom.
+ Carrier/Subject;
+ Attribute
identifying Value Token
+Token; +Value;
relational +Identified; +Identifier
§ 5.4
intensive
possessive
circumstantial
existential Existent
+ Existent §5.5.3
CHAPTER 4 TRANSITIVITY 155
middle effective
material happening/ [happening] The sugar [doing] She dissolved the
doing dissolved in the glass. sugar in the glass.
behavioural She coughed. They criticised IFG.
mental perceptive She saw the kite. The kite caught her
attention.
cognitive She forgot his name. His name escaped her.
affective She enjoyed the chocolates. The chocolates pleased her.
verbal She told us a story.
relational intensive She was a leader. She was the leader.
possessive She had a VW bug. She owned the VW bug.
circumstantial The trees were around her. The trees surrounded her.
existential There was once a man
from Manchuria.
Viewed from this perspective, the core of the clause is the Process together with
the Medium participant. Where the process is something external to this, there will
also be an Agent present in the structure. See Figure 4.9 below.
20 Note that from an ergative perspective the central core of the configuration is Process + Medium, rather
than Process alone.
156 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
This example shows that not every element that enters into a textual or
interpersonal structure will be part of the transitivity structure. Here the Vocative
Madam plays no role in the representation of the process. We can see this easily
enough if we probe the clause textually by means of theme predication: we can get
Madam it is you who’ll look like a tulip (focus on Carrier), Madam it is a tulip that you’ll
look like (focus on Attribute), but not it is Madam that you’ll look like a tulip (attempted
focus on Vocative). This difference is explained by the interpretation of the Vocative as
a purely interpersonal element.
The general correspondences between the experiential and interpersonal
functions are shown in Table 4.18:
Table 4.18 Correspondences between experiential and interpersonal functions
The general principle is that participants have the potential for being given the
interpersonal status of modal responsibility; thus they serve as Subject (actual modal
responsibility) or as Complement (non-actual but potential modal responsibility). For
example, all three participants in our earlier example Hermes gave my aunt these
flowers out of pity can serve as Subject. See Tables 4.19 - 4.21.
Table 4.19 Mapping of participant roles and interpersonal roles
(i) active clause: Hermes gave my aunt these flowers out of pity
Agent √ Hermes
Beneficiary √ my aunt
Agent √ Hermes
Beneficiary √ my aunt
Agent √ Hermes
Beneficiary √ my aunt
Note that in the passive clauses, the Agent does not in fact serve as Complement
although it is a participant. We can therefore make a simple yet powerful
generalisation about the hierarchy of participants in relation to the interpersonal
functions Subject, Complement and Adjunct. See Table 4.22:
Table 4.22 Hierarchy of participant functions in relation to interpersonal functions
In the active, unmarked voice, the Agent is Subject and the other participants are
Complements; in the passive, marked voice, either of the other participants can be
Subject and the one that is not serves as Complement. All of these are simply nominal
groups. The passive Agent is marked and its marked status is indicated by the
preposition by. It serves neither as Subject nor as Complement, but as Adjunct. It has
the characteristics of a circumstantial Adjunct: (i) it is realised by a prepositional
phrase, not by nominal group; and (ii) it is optional, not obligatory (in fact, the
majority of passive clauses have no Agent).
In fact, we can take one step further in our generalisation about the relation of
participants to interpersonal roles. There is a variant of our three-participant clause
where the Beneficiary (but not the Medium) is realised by a prepositional phrase
rather than by a nominal group; for example: Hermes gave these flowers to my aunt.
Here the Beneficiary is an Adjunct rather than a Complement (there is no passive
clause to my aunt was given these flowers (by Hermes)).
158 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
This suggests a view of transitivity which has the Medium as a nuclear participant,
at the centre of the configuration, as shown in Figure 4.9:
at the Peninsula
other participants:
politely Agent, Range, Beneficiary
because of tradition involved in
according to Mr.
Fodor
Process
bell boy open
nucleus
Medium
door
Further reading
For further interpretation of the semantics of TRANSITIVITY, see Halliday & Matthiessen
1999, Chapter 4.
Butt, David G, Annabelle Lukin & C M I M Matthiessen. 2004 ‘Grammar – The First
Covert Operation of War.’ Discourse & Society 15(2-3): 267-290.
Caffarel, A, J R Martin & C M I M Matthiessen 2004 Language Typology: a functional
perspective . Amsterdam: Benjamins.
[It further interprets TRANSITIVITY from a functional typological perspective across
a range of languages. Matthiessen 2007 surveys recent descriptive and theoretical
developments relating to lexicogrammar, including TRANSITIVITY, MOOD and THEME.]
Davidse, Kristin 1992 Transitive/ergative: the Janus-headed grammar of actions and
events. M Davies & Louise Ravelli (eds) Advances in systemic linguistics. London:
Pinter. 105-135.
[An intensive study of transitive and ergative motifs in English grammar.]
Davidse, Kristin 1996 The verbal/mental domain: prototypes and cryptotypes. In R
Hasan et al (eds) 85-144.
Fawcett, Robin P 1987 The semantics of clause and verb for relational processes in
English. In M A K Halliday & R.P. Fawcett (eds) New developments in systemic
linguistics Vol. 1: Theory and description. Amsterdam, Benjamins. 130-183.
Halliday, M A K 1971 Linguistic function and literary style: an enquiry into the
language of William Golding’s ‘The Inheritors’. In Seymor Chatman (ed), Literary
Style: a symposium. New York: Oxford University Press. [Reprinted in M A K
Halliday 2002 Linguistic Studies of Text and Discourse. London: Continuum (Vol. 2
in the Collected Works of M A K Halliday). 88-125.]
[This paper shows how transitivity is used to project different world views in
Golding’s novel.]
Halliday, M A K 1985 Dimensions of discourse analysis: grammar. In Teun A van Dijk
(ed), Handbook of Discourse Analysis, Vol. 1. New York: Academic Press. [Reprinted
in M A K Halliday 2002 On Grammar London: Continuum (Vol. 1 in the Collected
Works of M A K Halliday) 261-286.]
[For an example of a text analysed in terms of transitivity.]
Halliday, M A K 1992 Some lexicogrammatical features of the Zero Population Growth
text. In Sandra Thompson & William Mann (eds), Discourse Description: diverse
analyses of a fund-raising text. Amsterdam: Benjamins. [Reprinted in M A K
Halliday 2002 Linguistic Studies of Text and Discourse. London: Continuum (Vol. 2
in the Collected Works of M A K Halliday) 197-227.]
[For an example of a text analysed in terms of transitivity.]
Halliday, M A K & C M I M Matthiessen 1999 Construing experience through meaning: a
language based approach to cognition. London: Cassell. [Reprinted by Continuum
2006.]
Hasan, Ruqaiya 1987 The grammarian’s dream: lexis as most delicate grammar. In M
A K Halliday & R P Fawcett (eds) New developments in systemic linguistics v.1:
Theory and description. London, Pinter. 184-211. [Reprinted in C Cloran, D Butt &
160 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
1. Orientation
These three configurations are mapped onto one another; they are unified in a
single structural representation through a series of conflations of functions:
Theme/Adjunct/Location, Subject/Agent/Actor, Finite & Predicator/Process, and
Complement/Medium/Goal.
Such bundles of conflated functions are realised by grammatical units as follows:
Theme/Adjunct/Location – realised by: a prepositional phrase;
Subject/Agent/Actor – realised by: a nominal group;
Finite & Predicator/Process – realised by: a verbal group;
Complement/Medium/Goal – realised by: a nominal group.
To put this the other way around: the grammatical units of prepositional phrase,
nominal group and verbal group serve functions in the structure of the clause.
The example thus illustrates how the functional parts of a clause are realised by
groups/phrases, which are units of a lower ‘rank’ than that of the clause. In their turn,
groups are made up of words (they are groups of words), and words are made up of
morphemes. The rank scale of the grammar of English is thus:
clause
group/phrase
word
morpheme
We find the same rank-based principle of constituency elsewhere in English; for
example, phonological constituency is determined by the rank scale of tone group –
foot – syllable – phoneme (see IFG2 Chapters 1 and 8, IFG3 Chapter 1). We also find it
outside language in other kinds of system; for example, molecules are made up of
atoms, and atoms are made up of subatomic particles.
In this chapter, then, we are concerned with the rank immediately below clause
rank – with the units that make up clauses, i.e. with groups/phrases. They make their
contribution to the creation of meaning in the environment of the clause; and they are
differentiated according to the generalised functions they serve in the clause. We can
begin by considering this differentiation from the point of view of the generalised
transitivity functions of process + participants + circumstances:
process – realised by: verbal group
participants – realised by: nominal group
circumstances – realised by: adverbial group;
prepositional phrase
Let us exemplify from the text extract above.
CHAPTER 5 GROUP AND PHRASE 163
Table 5.2 Examples of groups and phrases
verbal group has decided to create; has been working; is; to being admitted;
will walk; covered; will have taken
nominal group this bare, drab hall; James Gordon; a physical interpretation of
the magazine; he; this space; – the Sydney Cove Authority’s
red-brick Victorian building; three days,...
adverbial group now
prepositional phrase in this bare, drab hall; in this space; for three days; for
someone...; from the front door,...
(The word classes of English are summarised in Section 6.6 of IFG – IFG2: 214, IFG3: 362.)
In the limiting case, a group may consist of a single word; but such groups are still
groups rather than simply words. One reason is that a one-word group is always
agnate with other groups of more than one word; it always has the potential for being
expanded. The verbal group covered is thus agnate with the verbal groups is being
covered, has been covered, and so on. Similarly, the adverbial group now is agnate with
right now, almost right now, now or tomorrow....
So while groups may consist of single words, they also have the potential for
being expanded indefinitely (at least in principle). Thus we can find a nominal group
1 Under certain conditions, this participant may even come to serve as Theme, Wh and/or Subject in the
clause: see IFG2: 169, IFG3: 297.
164 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
such as those two splendid old electric trains or a verbal group such as will have been
going to have been tested still with potential for further expansion: for example those
first two splendid old electric trains and will have been going to have been being tested.
We can develop these groups step by step:
trains > electric trains > old electric trains > splendid old electric trains > two splendid
old electric trains > first two splendid old electric trains > those first two splendid old electric
trains
will > will have > will have been going to > will have been going to have > will have been
going to have been > will have been going to have been being > will have been going to have
been being tested
These examples reveal that groups are structured serially as strings or chains of words.
There is one central or nuclear word, which serves the function of Head. In principle,
this is the only element of structure in the series that is required (trains and will in the
examples above). In addition, this Head may be expanded by other words; these serve
the general function of Modifier. In the most expanded examples above, there are
several instances of the Modifier function:
those first two splendid old electric trains
Modifier.6 Modifier.5 Modifier.4 Modifier.3 Modifier.2 Modifier.1 Head
Since the elements of groups form chains, each Modifier expands on the
preceding links in the chain. For example, electric expands trains, and old expands
electric trains. In other words, the Modifiers do not simply relate directly to the Head
of the group.
To show this step-wise modification, we can represent the structure as a series of
linked elements denoted by small Greek letters, with for the Head and ,,etc. for
successive Modifiers. For example:
will have been going to have been being tested
Head Modifier.1 Modifier.2 Modifier.3 Modifier.4 Modifier.5 Modifier.6
This serial kind of structure derives from the logical subtype of the ideational
metafunction2.
This logical type of structure is known as univariate structure because it
involves repeated instances of one and the same structural variable – the relation of
modification. In contrast, a structure such as Actor + Process + Goal or Theme +
Rheme is a multivariate structure: each element makes a different structural
contribution to the unit whole of the clause. Further, multivariate structures are all
modelled as constituency structures – as configurations of component parts of a unit
whole (see IFG2 Section 2.1, IFG3 Section 1.1.3 on grammatical constituency).
There are thus two basic types of grammatical structure whose characteristics
are summarised in Table 5.4.
2 As explained in Chapter 6, the Modifier-Head type of logical structure is the hypotactic mode, the other
type of logical structure is the paratactic.
CHAPTER 5 GROUP AND PHRASE 165
Table 5.4 Types of grammatical structure
multivariate univariate
type of different functions making unique contributions repeated instances of same
variable to unit whole – fixed number of elements (e.g. function expanding a series
Theme + Rheme) of elements – open number
of elements (indefinitely
expandable)
unit clause; group
phrase
composition constituency relational interdependency
(series)
examples Theme + Rheme;
Mood + Residue + Moodtag;
Process + Medium + Agent + Location + Manner;
Minor Process + Minirange
diagramming Actor Process Goal Location
(One major domain for univariate structure is that of groups. Chapter 6 will discuss
the other major domain, which is chaining (complexing) of units in general, including
complexing of clauses and groups/phrases.)
The concept of univariate structure allows us to show very explicitly how a group
is a group of words, which would seem to suggest that groups are organised only to
realise the logical meaning of modification. But what about the other metafunctions
– the experiential subtype of the ideational metafunction, the interpersonal
metafunction and the textual metafunction? To account for these other metafunctional
contributions, we also have to posit multivariate structures for groups – at least for the
most complex ones, i.e. nominal groups and verbal groups. For example, the nominal
group cited above can be interpreted both univariately and multivariately as follows:
those first two splendid old electric trains
Deictic Numerative: ordinal Numerative: cardinal Epithet: attitudinal Epithet: age Classifier Thing
The multivariate structure here is shown as one layer only (rather than the three
metafunctional layers of the clause), but it embodies contributions from all three
metafunctions. (See Section 2 below.)
2. Survey of options
Groups will be surveyed first and then prepositional phrases. We start with the
nominal group, which is the most elaborated of the groups.
textual non-specific
function Deictic Post-deictic Numerative Numerative Epithet* Epithet* Classifier* Thing Qualifier*
examples those famous first two dreadful long maths sums in the exam
word class determiner adjective ordinal cardinal adjective adjective noun noun prep. phrase
other a, the, every, typical, second, five, several, wretched, big, Pekinese, dog, boy, in the yard,
examples both, some, other, third... many, terrific, small, copper, star, [[that he gave
his, our, usual... much... spunky... green, Renaissance, mistake, us]]...
Peter’s... old... post-modern... loss...
you, she, it, who, somebody, something), there is no separate Deictic element; but since
the options realised by the Deictic are embodied in the pronoun, we might say that the
Deictic and the Thing are fused: Deictic/Thing3.
The Deictic provides the key to the textual status of the particular referent
presented by the nominal group. For example, his dog, that dog, the dog are all shown
to be identifiable or recoverable by the hearer, whereas a dog, some dogs are shown to
be non-recoverable. The Deictic thus ‘contextualises’ the nominal group (compare the
way a Theme ‘contextualises’ a clause). The choices associated with the Deictic all
concern particular referents at a given point in the discourse – their recoverability
status, their location relative to the speaker, etc. At the same time, the Deictic also
embodies the interpersonal orientation of the group: it indicates whether the group
gives information (e.g. this dog, a dog) or demands information (e.g. which dog?).
The Thing provides the basic general class in terms of which the participant is
construed. It thus locates it in some particular domain of experience, such as the
domain of conscious beings or the domain of abstractions. (In this respect, it is similar
to the Process of the clause: the Process locates the clause in some domain of
experience such as sensing or being-&-having.)
Thing:
general experiential class
in taxonomy of classes
bird/penguin/kite/
swallow…
frame of construal:
the general
experiential system
Deictic:
members of class:
particular referents
this/that/some/any…
frame of reference:
the here-&-now of
the speech situation
3 As the form of non-specific pronouns suggests: something, nothing, everything, somebody, nobody,
everybody, etc., with Deictic: some/no/every... and Thing: body/thing.
168 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
represent (complexes of) qualities that are long-term, inherent ones. They are
typically realised by nouns or else de-nominal adjectives (wooden, golden, etc.) or
participial forms of verbs (v-ing, v-en). Classifiers answer the question what kind/
subclass? and their meanings encompass those manifested in circumstances in the
clause (see Chapter 4). See Table 5.6:
Table 5.6 Meanings of Classifier
TYPE Examples
role guise a city state, a child star, a toy soldier
accompaniment part a two-legged creature, a yellow-crested bird
a Medieval treatise, postmodern architecture, a lunch
location time
date
place a country house, a desktop computer
extent duration a two-week vacation
frequency an annual report
distance a five-mile hike
manner means: material a plastic bag, a wooden house, a brick wall
means: agent a Hemingway novel, a Wenders movie
comparison Kafkaesque literature, Socratic dialogue
a delivery truck, a beer glass, a reference manual,
cause purpose
cleaning agents
matter a maths tutor, a weather report
In addition, where the noun construing the Thing is a nominalisation (see further
below), the Classifier and the Thing may stand in a participant-process relation. Thus
cancer treatment relates to treat cancer.
Epithets tend to represent experientially simpler and often more transitory
qualities than those represented by Classifiers; for example: age (old/young; old/new),
size (big/small), length (long/short), height (tall/short), loudness (loud/quiet), degree
(strong/weak), weight (heavy/light), behavioural propensity (skillful/clumsy), rating
(good/bad), emotion (happy/sad), dimension (round/square/rectangular/oval...),
colour (red/green/blue/yellow...), taste (salty/sour/sweet...). Many of these are in fact
locations on one-dimensional scales between polar opposites (old/new) and are
typically gradable, for example from a very new car to a very old car. Others form
taxonomic oppositions, e.g. those construing colour and shape. These latter are closer
to Classifiers which also form taxonomic oppositions and which are never gradable (*a
very delivery truck). The closer relation between taxonomic Epithets and Classifiers is
reflected in relative sequence: Epithets of colour, shape, etc. come after those Epithets
that construe polar oppositions. Thus we say a big blue wooden house rather than a
blue big wooden house, a large round wooden table rather than a round large wooden
table, a delicious sour Thai dish rather than a sour delicious Thai dish.
In contrast, at the other end of the cline, the Postdeictic is concerned with
particular referents; it serves to indicate how particular referents are selected from
some general class – for example, particular referents that are the same or different
from another set of referents (the same sedimentary rocks, some different sedimentary
rocks). Similarly, Numeratives are concerned with the numerative properties of
particular referents (their order in a series or the size of the set of referents) rather
CHAPTER 5 GROUP AND PHRASE 169
Or the response might simply be those, with the Head conflated with the Deictic.
The most common multivariate elements other than the Thing that serve as Head
are the Numerative and the Deictic, as illustrated above. Except in cases where
adjectival nominal groups serves as Attributes (as in her dress was yellow, the puppies
became boisterous), Epithets are not so common as Heads.
(ii) Full nominal groups
When the nominal group is full rather than elliptical, the Thing of the multivariate
structure and the Head of the univariate structure are conflated: these two structures
are in phase. However, there is one situation where the two types of structure are out
of phase with one another even though the nominal group is a full one (see IFG2:
195-196, IFG3: 332-5 for grammatical indications of the two structures being out of
phase). This happens when the Head is a noun that does not represent a thing in its
own right but rather an elaboration or extension of another thing, as in the side of the
house, two metres of fabric, another cup of coffee. In such cases the multivariate and
univariate structures are out of phase with one another:
two metres of fabric
multivariate: Thing
univariate: Head
170 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
Here the univariate Head has a kind of ‘numerative’ function, but in fact, the full
potential of the nominal group (including Deictic, Post-Deictic and Numerative) is still
available after this initial Numerative choice. We might have, for example, two metres
of the first fabric [[I looked at]]. Nominal group expansions of this kind are very
common, and we’ll suggest an analysis here that treats these structures as having an
embedded nominal group with the multivariate function Focus; in these nominal
groups of functions as a structure marker, signaling the embedding (there is no need
to label it separately in the analysis).
two metres of fabric
multivariate: Focus Thing
univariate: Head
The Focus element can have a range of functions, which are outlined in Table 5.7
below.
Table 5.7 Range of Focus nominal groups (where Thing and Head are out of phase)
Closely related to these ‘of’ expansions are groups that would be considered
appositive in traditional terms. Here there is a relation between a thing and its class
identity: the month of May, the state of Alabama, the city of Rome, the kingdom of
Norway, the house of Windsor, the science of cladistics, the title of Mayor (cf. the month is
4 Some selecting Focus items appear without the structure marker of (e.g. half the boys, all the problems).
CHAPTER 5 GROUP AND PHRASE 171
May, the state is Alabama, etc.). We could assimilate these with the classifying type
above5.
5 Some have variants without of: the name of James: the name James is very common in these parts. The
version without of is a nominal group complex and some classes can only be elaborated in this way; for
example: (the word) kataba means ‘write’, (the clause) ‘visiting generativists can be dangerous’ is
ambiguous.
6 Some verb forms have different uses as either lexical verbs or auxiliary verbs: do, be, have, will. They may
of course occur in one and the same verbal group, as in now you [auxiliary:] are [lexical verb:] being very
silly; he [auxiliary:] has [lexical verb:] had his car for two years.
172 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
serves as Finite + Predicator in the interpersonal (modal) structure of the clause and it
serves as Process in the experiential (transitivity) structure of the clause. Because it
serves the combination of the two interpersonal functions Finite and Predicator, the
verbal group is often split for interpersonal reasons as in Are they leaving? However,
as we will see, the structural unfolding of the verbal group is not disrupted, merely
‘delayed’.
Event:
general experiential class
in taxonomy of events
located at event time
building/producing/
baking…
temporal/modal bridge swallow…
frame of construal:
the experiential domain
of the clause
[experiential time]
Finite:
enactment in time
or modality relative
to the actual ‘now’
did/does/will‖
can/will/must…
frame of enactment:
the now of speaking
[interpersonal time]
The resources of the verbal group make it possible to construct longer bridges
between the interpersonal and the experiential, as in:
7 The term ‘auxiliary’ is thus used in two related but distinct senses: it is the name of a function in the
structure of the verbal group, in which case it is spelt with an initial capital, Auxiliary; it is also the name
of a class of verb, the class of auxiliary verbs, or auxiliaries. Auxiliary verbs are, as we have already noted,
grammatical (as opposed to lexical) verbs.
174 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
If the interpersonal time of speaking and the experiential time of the event are
not separated, the Finite and the Event are often conflated, e.g. Finite/Event: produces.
If the times are separated, this may be done modally rather than temporally, with a
modal auxiliary serving as Finite: may produce, must produce, should produce, etc.
Other elements not indicating time or modality may also enter into the sequence
between Finite and Event; in particular, negative polarity may be indicated by a
separate Polarity: not following the Finite and passive voice is indicated by an
Auxiliary: ‘be’ followed by the Event realised by the v-en form of the verb. For example:
will not have been going to be being produced
The logical structure of the verbal group is univariate (as it is in the nominal
group; see above); the same dependency relation is repeated again and again from
to . The logical structure construes a semantic ordering of a series of relations
between times: see Figure 5.4.
CHAPTER 5 GROUP AND PHRASE 175
t1
t3
δ be [v] be
2.2.4 TENSE
The English TENSE system construes the temporal relation between the interpersonal
speaking time, ‘now’, and the experiential time of the event, the event time, as a series
of one or more temporal relations. Our earlier example contains a series of four tense
selections:
Table 5.8 Example of 4 tense selections
will have been going to be producing
Finite Auxiliary Auxiliary Auxiliary Event
will have, v be going to, v-en be, v produce, v-ing
future past future present (event)
This is, of course, a fairly long series; but it does not represent the maximum
tense series in English (see IFG2: 200, IFG3 Table 6(11)). Verbal groups such as will
have been going to be producing, was going to have been producing, will have been
going to have been producing seem very complex and may be hard to follow in
isolation, out of their discourse environment, where they typically have a temporal
history that warrants the complexity. However, they are really made up of two very
176 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
simple choices:
(i) the system of the tense type – ‘past’/‘present’/‘future’ relative to some time;
and
(ii) the system of tense serialisation – the choice whether to introduce another
temporal relation or not.
In the simplest case, only one tense type is chosen and there are no further tense
selections (no tense serialisation): these are verbal groups with simple past, present
or future (e.g. did produce (produced), does produce (produces), will produce). Here the
time of the event is only at one remove from the time of speaking. However, the system
of tense serialisation makes it possible to choose again from the tense type system to
create a tense series of two to five tense choices 8. Thus the example above is built up
as follows:
Table 5.9 Tense choices in English
The first tense selection relates the time of speaking to some other time; if no
further tense selections are made, this will be the time of the event. However, if further
tense selections are made, additional times are set up. The time of the event is always
the last time in the series. The first tense selection, the one relating deictically to ‘now’,
is called primary tense. It is obligatory in finite verbal groups – unless the verbal
group is modal, in which case the primary tense is displaced by a modal auxiliary (as
in may be going to have...). Additional tense selections are called secondary tense.
They select from the same three tense types, ‘past’/‘present’/‘future’, but they are
realised in different ways.
To recognise the different tense choices being made, you need to be able to
recognise the five different forms of the English verb. These are shown below with
examples from be, have, take and lift.
Table 5.10 Five forms of the English verb
be have take lift modal
v (infinitive) (to) be (to) have (to) take (to) lift
v-s am, are, is has (have) take(s) lift(s) may/might
v-ed was, were had took lifted
v-en been had taken lifted
v-ing being having taking lifting
As noted above, tense is realised differently after the first (primary tense) choice.
Table 5.11 contrasts the realisation of past, present and future depending on whether
the choice is made with respect to the moment of speaking (primary tense) or with
respect to an already chosen tense (secondary tense).
8 There are restrictions on secondary tense selection as follows: (1) Apart from , future occurs only once.
(2) Apart from present occurs just once, at deepest level. (3) Apart from , the same tense doesn’t
occur twice consecutively.
CHAPTER 5 GROUP AND PHRASE 177
Table 5.11 Primary and secondary tense realisations
As is explained in IFG2: 198, IFG3: 338, tense selections are labelled from the
highest-order secondary tense selection downwards to the primary tense selection, so
the selection above is present-in-past.
A few further examples are given in the Table 5.12:
Table 5.12 Examples of English tense forms
This naming convention has the advantage that it makes explicit the tense frame
relative to which a tense selection has been made, such as future relative to past in
were going to produce.
Why has the English tense system evolved to embody this serial principle? The
basic reason is that it allows us to develop discourses where successive time frames
are constructed relative to earlier time frames. To supplement the discussion in IFG,
here are two text examples, analysed using the convention of indicating past, present
and future choices with the notation of -, ø, + respectively:
178 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
Here the discussion is concerned with the future. There are two predictions with
the simple future, clauses (1) and (6). Two clauses are concerned with present
possession of future work load, clauses (2) and (5). Clause (3) switches to the primary
future (after that I shall have...). Clause (4) continues this primary future perspective,
but it construes a secondary present time relative to that future time: will be doing.
Example (2) [from Halliday, 1992: 41]
9 The exception is the verb be, which has no passive form – thus, I’m the author / the author is me (not *the
author is been by me).
180 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
‘present passive’
[present] [passive]
is seen
2.2.6 Modality
Not all verbal groups in English begin by choosing tense. The alternative to choosing a
tense, relating the verbal group temporally the ‘now’ of speaking, is to choose a
modality. Modality relates the verbal group to the actuality of speaking in terms of
probability, usuality, obligation, inclination or potentiality. In the verbal group, these
assessments may be realised through the modal verbs will, would, can, could, may,
might, should, must, ought to.10 Modal verbs do not choose for tense (the difference
between Jane may go and Jane might go is not a difference in tense; might is not the
past of may11); rather, modal verbs function as an alternative to choosing primary
tense.
Although modals function as an alternative to primary tense, they may combine
with secondary tense and voice. Here are examples with secondary tenses:
Our children should have learned western knowledge in school.
[combined with secondary past]
Our children could be learning western knowledge in school.
[combined with secondary present]
10 In some dialects need and dare are arguably modal, since they invert with Subject in interrogatives
and/or have negative forms: Need you do it?, You needn’t do it?, Dare you do it?, You daren’t do it.
11 The exception to this is the modal verb can/could when it realises potentiality; thus I can do it now
contrasts in tense with I could do it then.
CHAPTER 5 GROUP AND PHRASE 181
[present perfective]
... to be asking again
2.2.8 Polarity
In general, negative polarity is realised through the modal adverb not. As we have
already noted, it may be fused with the Finite verb expressing primary tense or
modality: don’t (know), shouldn’t (have come). In such cases the verb in question can
be analysed as carrying both polarity and modality:
don’t know
ø/neg event
shouldn’t have come
have...en
modal/neg - event
When it is not fused with the Finite, it serves a special function in the structure of
the verbal group, that of Polarity (see IFG3: 337); and in the interpersonal structure of
the clause, it serves as a mood Adjunct.
The negative adverb not expresses pure negative polarity. However, in addition to
not, there are negative adverbs that combine the feature negative with another
interpersonal feature, either usuality (seldom, rarely, never) or intensity of the degree
type (hardly, scarcely). These serve as mood Adjuncts in the interpersonal structure of
the clause and are outside verbal group structure proper.
One very common type of hypotactic verbal group complex is used to show that
Agents have been added to the clause; note that these ‘causative’ complexes are
discontinuous (e.g. forced... to be below):
but he forced his face and his voice to be calm [discontinuous hypotactic verbal gp. complex12]
12 In some of these causative complexes the dependent non-finite verbal group appears without to: made
him go, let him go, had him go, helped him go.
182 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
Note also Range: play somebody at tennis, play beautifully on the flute, speak to somebody in
Chinese.
CHAPTER 5 GROUP AND PHRASE 185
3. Troubleshooting
Table 5.16 The functional potential of different primary and secondary classes of word
group/ function nominals verbals adverbial
phrase
determiner ordinal cardinal adjective noun modal other lexical preposition adverb conjunction
auxil. auxil.
nominal Deictic √
group
Numeral √ √
Classifier √ √ √ [v-ing,
(typically v-ed]
denominal (e.g.
) moving
pictures)
Thing √
Auxiliary √
(non-finite
form)
Modifier √
(various
intensi-
fiers)
conj. Head √
group
Modifier √
clause Conjunctive √
(conjunctive
prep. in
prep.
group)
(introducing
non-finite
clauses, e.g.
instead of
me doing
the work)
IFG2: 236,
IFG3: 416-7
In addition to these possibilities, note that words may also combine at word rank
(rather than group rank) through compounding. Noun compounding (e.g. blackbird,
gravy boat, airship) may look like a configuration of Classifier + Thing and the line
between the two is indeterminate: see IFG2: 185; IFG3: 320.
188 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
Sequence in nominal √ – –
group:
can always come before the identical three twins the two usual (ordinary, the three identical twins
a numerative everyday, festive) dishes
the usual (typical) two
dishes
Can (typically) be √ √ –
compared and
intensified (by a fast train : a very fast but not: a very fast (express)
submodifiers) train train
type examples
desideration be eager/determined/happy/keen/willing to do
probability be certain/likely to do
190 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
Sequences such as be able to finish, be hard to solve look like relational attributive
clauses where the Attribute has an adjectival Head postmodified by a non-finite clause
(as in [Carrier:] he [Process:] was [Attribute:] too scared [[to move]]) and for certain
cases this might be an alternative analysis. However, the constructions have different
properties and these can be used as probes in analysis.
One test is to look for an agnate example consisting of the final verbal group of
the complex. For example, the verbal group complex were able to finish is agnate with
the simple verbal group finished (did finish).
Another is to see whether the example (e.g. were able to finish) is agnate with a
simple version of the first verbal group of the complex (e.g. were able). Here we see the
verbal group complex were able to finish is not agnate with were able. In the second
case, able can be co-ordinated with a range of regular adjectives (they were able, strong
and healthy), but it cannot be in the first case (there is no they were able, strong and
healthy to finish). In contrast, where an adjective functions as Attribute in a relational
clause, there is always an agnate without the Postmodifier. Thus the relational clause
he was too scared [[to move]] is agnate with he was too scared. (This is to be expected
since Postmodifiers are not obligatory elements of structure.)
13 The one exception is enough, which comes after the Head, as in they are not working fast enough (for us
to finish the job). See IFG2 p. 211.
CHAPTER 5 GROUP AND PHRASE 191
Table 5.19 The different functions of adverbials
Submodifier
Unit Modifier [grammatical] Head [lexical] Example
[grammatical]
Note that many adverbs of comparison and intensification may serve either as
(Sub)Modifiers in adverbial and nominal groups or as Heads in adverbial groups that
realise circumstances of Manner: degree. For example: totally.
I totally agree with you adverb as Head
It’s like totally awesome adverb as Submodifier
192 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
MODALITY
it was perfectly it is perfectly it’ll be perfectly that’ll be [median]
VALUE
all right all right all right the postman
14 In some varieties of (British) English, future will serves together with future shall in the realisation of
primary future: shall is used for speaker (plus), i.e. first person; for example: after that I shall have
another two weeks.
CHAPTER 5 GROUP AND PHRASE 193
buggered up in this kind of way. So you can probe examples with will to check to see
whether they are agnate with a variant with am/are/is going to. For example, the
example that’ll be the postman is not agnate with that’s going to be the postman.
If we move up one rank to that of the clause, we find that there is a wider pattern
of agnation for modal will, which you can again use in probes. Modal will is related to
forms such as probably, I think, and it’s probable/likely that. For example, that’ll be the
postman is agnate with that’s probably the postman and I think that’s the postman.
(Notice that the primary tense here is the present rather than the future.) At clause
rank, you can also probe by means of other temporal specifications; for example, it’ll
be perfectly all right this summer vs. that’ll be the postman now. (If you are trying to
choose between future will and the modal will of inclination [‘be keen to’], it may be
helpful to consider a variant of the clause where the Subject is not likely to exercise
inclination. So if the Subject is a conscious being, you can consider a variant with a
non-conscious being as Subject, possibly a passive version of the clause. For example,
is I won’t mend the fence temporal or modal? It can be either. If it is temporal, it is
agnate with the fence won’t be mended by me, since primary future is not related to the
choice of Subject; but if it is modal: modulation: inclination, it is not agnate with the
fence won’t be mended by me, since this can hardly mean ‘the fence is not keen (willing)
to be mended by me’.
15 The situation is somewhat different with projection: see IFG2: 288-291, IFG3: 515-9.
194 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
Table 5.20 Contrasting properties of verbal group, (hypotactic ) verbal group complex
and (hypotactic) clause complex
He has to eat the cake. He is trying to eat the cake. He is trying, to eat the cake.
time one process time one process time two process time
he has to eat the cake he is trying to eat the cake he is trying (fasting) now, to
now/tomorrow now eat the cake tomorrow
agnates agnate verbs grammatical verbs, lexical verbs: is lexical verbs: he is
i.e. auxiliaries: attempting/avoiding/ trying/working/making an
must/should/may... managing/failing to eat; effort to eat the cake
eat is succeeding in eating
form with ... to eat : in order to eat
explicit binder
finite second ... to eat : so that he may eat
lexical verb
thematic variant To eat the cake, he is trying
with second (making an effort).
lexical verb
Complement as What does he have to What is he trying to eat? *What is he trying, in order
Wh eat (What has he got to eat?
to eat)?
second part He has to eat the cake. He is trying to eat the cake. He is trying, to eat the cake.
presupposed by – Oh, does he have to? – Oh, is he trying to? – *Oh, is he trying (in order)
verbal ellipsis to?
In analytical practice, there is of course little difficulty in this area: you can always
treat modal have to as part of a simple verbal group.
The two easiest ways of probing a prepositional phrase to find out which sense it
has are:
(1) Check which Wh-element it answers or which Wh-element is used in a theme
nominalisation. For example, the preposition according to can indicate either Manner
or Angle; Manner can be probed by how, but Angle cannot: They built it according to
her instructions – How did they build it? (cf. how they built it was according to her
instructions) but not They built it according to her – How did they build it (cf. how they
built it was according to her).
(2) Check what other prepositions are agnate. For example, the preposition
according to is agnate with in accordance with as Manner but not as Angle: They built it
according to/in accordance with her instructions but not They built it according to/in
accordance with her. The table above indicates examples of agnate prepositions that
can be used in this probe.
With respect to the second probe, sometimes it will also be useful to check what
the agnate hypotactic clause would be. This can be particularly helpful in order to
identify circumstances of Cause since the causal link often has to be inferred but is
made explicit in the agnate enhancing clause. 16 So for example, purpose may be
probed by clauses which are marked by in order to and contain a Process that makes
explicit the purposive link, such as a Process of getting, or having: she worked hard for
her money: she worked hard in order to get/have her money.
16 In addition, prepositional phrases serving as Cause are often metaphorical versions of enhancing clauses
in clause complexes.
CHAPTER 5 GROUP AND PHRASE 199
(i) ranking
(ii) downranked
or
The first part of the nominal group, ‘things’, is clearly qualified by the postponed
part, ‘from which one averts the mind’: what things? – ones from which one averts the
mind. From a textual point of view, these two parts serve as Theme and New
respectively: Theme: things... New: from which one averts the mind. And this seems to
be the motivation behind such discontinuous nominal groups: they are split into two
parts to accommodate the swell of information within the clause: the movement from
a peak of thematic prominence (things) to a peak of prominence of news (from which
one averts the mind). As the discourse environment shows, Wilder is building up a
point in the discourse to illustrate instances of calamities, so from which one averts the
mind fits into this pattern, as the culmination of calamities – touched – burned down –
from which one averts the mind. If the nominal group had not been discontinuous, the
rhetorical effect would have been that their children would have been presented as
New, in the discourse company of calamities.
(ii) Qualifier or part of nominal group complex?
In addition to serving in its own right as an element of the clause, a ranking
prepositional phrase may be linked to a nominal group to form a nominal group
complex, as in The rain was pouring down when [:] Margaret, [:] in a yellow raincoat,
greeted us. Such prepositional phrases serve as elements of the clause together with
the nominal groups they expand: that is, the whole complex is treated as an element of
the clause. Thus Margaret, in a yellow raincoat is, in its entirety, the
participant/Theme/Subject of the clause.
Given this situation, it may not be obvious why the prepositional phrase is not
regarded as an embedded Qualifier. The difference is that a phrase linked to a nominal
group within a complex does not define the nominal group. For this reason it can be
used with a proper name; and there is a contrast in meaning between (a) [:]Margaret,
[:] in a yellow raincoat, greeted us and (b) the woman [in a yellow raincoat ] greeted us.
In (a), the referent of the nominal group has already been established and Margaret is
sufficient to identify her. In contrast, in (b), the prepositional phrase in a yellow
raincoat is needed to define the referent of the nominal group of which it is part. In
spoken English, the hypotactically related prepositional phrase in the nominal group
complex would normally be assigned a separate tone group, and in written English
this is indicated by separating it with commas.
CHAPTER 5 GROUP AND PHRASE 201
4. Analysis practice
This section will focus on analysis of nominal and verbal groups in Sections 4.1 and 4.2
respectively, with only brief exercises for adverbial group and prepositional phrase in
Section 4.3.
4.1.1 Phase I
4.1.1.1 Exercises
Exercise 1 Identifying nominal groups, types of nominal
Underline the nominal groups in the following text and label the class of the Head as
common, proper or pronoun.
‘We’ve absolutely got to stop them!’ Sophie cried. ‘Put me back in your pocket quick and
we’ll chase after them and warn people in England they’re coming.’ ‘I can’t,’ the big friendly
giant said. ‘They are going faster than me and they’ll get there before we are halfway.’
1. your pocket
4. many soldiers
6. a terrific idea
9. each one
4. a merry hiss
7. a greetings card
8. a female robin
9. a frilly apron
1. a greeting card
6. lining paper
9. a coloured woman
1. Dalgliesh
2. Sister Gearing
4. twenty-four hours
5. Mrs Wentworth
8. England
3. those poor girls and boys who are going to be eaten alive
*the bottom of the mug is best analysed as [Focus:] the bottom of + [Deictic:] the +
[Thing:] mug. (See 2.1.3 (ii).) This kind of structure will be practised in Phase II.
206 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
4.1.2 Phase II
4.1.2.1 Exercises
Exercise 1 Recognising Post-Deictic
Analyse the following nominal groups, including a Post-Deictic function where
required.
their four usual visitors
D N E T
the typical ten questions
D Post-D N T
4.1.3.1 Exercises
Exercise 1 Recognising discontinuous Qualifiers
Analyse each nominal group in the following clauses, including embedded groups. See
3.4.1 (i).
When would the felicity begin that she had always associated with social position?
the felicity [[that she had always associated with social position]]
D T Q
she social position
T C T
1. A story was soon published which dealt with their exploits.
2. A friend arrived who they hadn’t seen for years.
3. Did you realise who wrote it?
4. Who left that she wanted to meet?
5. Things had happened to their children from which one averts the mind.
6. Which story did they like?
7. But we can’t just sit here doing nothing!
8. Who was involved that we know?
9. Worse things were said of her, and petitions were afloat that she be looked up.
10. I call a person bad who lies and cheats and is unkind.
210 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
2. his death
4. her bed
5. tonight’s children
6. their children
3. a month of Mondays
4. a bugger of a job
7. the rest of it
8. a weighting which reflects the relative importance of the item in the budget
11. the near identity of these two criticisms of the governmental state
a responsibility [[for taking positive action to ensure that the work and study
environment remains free of harassment || and for dealing with complaints in a serious and
confidential manner]]
a responsibility for taking positive action to and for dealing with
ensure that the work and complaints in a serious and
study environment remains confidential manner
free of harassment
D T Q
1 +2
1. an increase in the general level of retail prices as measured by the consumer
price index
3. demand pull, wage push, external causes, inflationary expectations, public sector
causes, price shocks and excess money supply
5. advice such as this [i.e. be wary of approaching children before they are ‘ready’ to
learn mathematics]
7. eight groups: food, clothing, health and personal care, housing, household
equipment and operation, tobacco and alcohol, recreation and education, and
transportation
1. I hate them.
5. Come on!
6. Let’s hurry!
2. If I was a titchy little runty giant only twenty-four feet high then...
3. She and the big friendly giant sat quietly on the blue rock...
5. He thinks perhaps...
8. ... and there are very few people about in the witching hour.
2. If I wasn’t a titchy little runty giant only twenty-four feet high then...
4. If I do show myself...
Identify and analyse the verbal groups in the following text excerpt:
Northern Hairy-nosed Wombat
Lasiorhinus krefftii
Identification
A large wombat with silky grey fur and black eye-rings. Appearance is similar to the
Southern Hairy-nosed Wombat, but the northern species has a much broader muzzle and is
larger (body weight up to 38 kilograms and total length up to 1.1 metres). Both hairy-nosed
wombats differ from the Common Wombat in that they have finer fur, longer ears, longer
muzzles, and hair over the snout. Females are generally larger than males but males have
thicker necks. Both sexes have very powerful forearms and broad forepaws.
Biology
Nocturnal, feeds on grasses and sedges. Each individual uses 3-4 burrows, and burrow use
of different individuals overlaps. Social behaviour largely unknown. Individuals are mostly
solitary but burrows occur in groups, which whole families of wombats occupy. Females
more commonly move between groups than males. Females give birth mainly in late spring
and early summer.
[Adapted from Australian Natural History v.24 no.10, 1994:36.]
Text 2 Recount
Identify and analyse the verbal groups in the following text:
Wednesday 9th November 1994
On Friday, I will go on holidays to Mossy Point for ten days and last time I went up there we
saw a whale in the creek and some people and my little brother and me pushed it back in
the water but it came back out of the water. Then it died the next day. And on the weekend I
went to the beach. Next week my Grandad will come back from England.
216 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
Text 3 Conversation
Identify and analyse the verbal groups in the following conversation:
(Mother and child are discussing presentation of awards at local soccer club where child’s
brother, Harry, will get a trophy)
M: ... And everybody gets a little trophy.
C: Will I get one?
M: Well, when you join the soccer club and you play a game, you’ll get these things, darling.
Harry didn’t get them when he was four. You’ll have a shelf and have all your own
trophies on it when you are older.
C: But why did Harry get bigger before I did?
4.2.2 Phase II
4.2.2.1 Exercises
Exercise 1 Analysing verbal groups with secondary tenses
Analyse the verbal groups in the following clauses and name the tense:
They ’re coming.
-s be...-ing come
ø ø event [present in present]
1. How many girls and boys are they going to eat tonight?
3. Some people without genius have a remarkable power of being about to stimulate
it.
4. I meant that in noting your fallacies, I was occasionally guided towards the truth.
5. We have a fresh basis from which to be starting our construction of the visitor.
7. Is it, then, stretching our inference too far for it to be said that the presentation...
8. He had risen...
Indicate on the table the primary and (where appropriate) secondary tense choices in
the following conversation, as done for the first clause:
primary secondary
past pres fut past pres fut
- A: (1) No, the last time I saw you
(2) you were going to change your job.
B: (3) Yeah, I’d just finished my thesis, hadn’t I?
A: (4) That’s right.
B: (5) I’ve since started my job
(6) and am officially lecturer in East African
languages
(7) and I know one African language.
C: (8) English.
B: (9) Arabic, Arabic.
C: (10) That’s all right.
(11) You’re covered,
(12) you’re covered.
A: (13) You’re lecturing, are you?
B: (14) I’m a lecturer,
(15) but I haven’t got any students,
(16) so I don’t give any lectures.
A: (17) God!
Text 2 Narrative
‘What’s your interest?’ she asked. ‘Oh,’ I replied breezily, ‘I wondered if it had anything to do
with the National Bank robbery.’ She didn’t think it did. ‘You got anything to tell us on that
score?’ she asked. I didn’t. I cast my line again into the Chinatown killing. She said as usual
no-one had seen anything. Even in reasonably broad daylight. I said I might have. She found
that interesting. I asked her if the victim had been wearing a tie. She said no. I asked her if
he’d been wearing kung fu shoes. She said what is this, twenty questions? I said no. I told
her I’d seen a man acting suspiciously in the Chinese Gardens. She asked what I’d been
doing there but I said it was irrelevant. I said I’d seen the same man later that afternoon in
Cabramatta. She asked what I was doing in Cabramatta. I said it was irrelevant. I said it was
a long shot and probably had nothing to do with anything but it might be worth asking a
few discreet questions around the place. Check out the snooker hall. I asked her if she could
tell me any more about the victim. She gave me a lot of bland stuff which would be
appearing in the next press release.
[Marele Day, 1990:41-42, The Case of the Chinese Boxes. Sydney: Allen & Unwin.]
220 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
4.2.3.1 Exercises
1. A beautiful girl with white teeth had been sitting with her boyfriend in a sports
car.
6. (She gave me a lot of stuff) which is going to be appearing in the press release.
8. Because for the last few days I have been feeling rheumatic and old.
10. (You say) that you have had it (because you need a change).
CHAPTER 5 GROUP AND PHRASE 221
‘If I said I wanted to dream that I was in a flying bathtub with silver wings, could you make
me dream it?’
wanted to dream
-ed want
- event event/per
5. He is also saying he would like very much to gobble up one of the soldiers.
9. When are you claiming to have agreed to have mixed this special dream.
4. Do take care.
6. Help!
7. Do not fear.
Text 1
Analyse the verbal groups in the following text:
‘But why Turkish?’ asked Holmes, gazing fixedly at my boots. I was reclining in a
cane-backed chair at the moment, and my protruded feet had attracted his ever-active
attention.
‘English,’ I answered in some surprise. ‘I got them at Latimer’s, in Oxford Street.’
Holmes smiled with an expression of weary patience.
‘The bath!’ he said, ‘the bath! Why the relaxing and expensive Turkish rather than the
invigorating home-made article?’
‘Because for the last few days I have been feeling rheumatic and old. A Turkish bath is
what we call an alternative in medicine – a fresh starting-point, a cleanser of the system.
‘By the way, Holmes,’ I added, ‘I have no doubt the connection between my boots and a
Turkish bath is a perfectly self-evident one to a logical mind, and yet I should be obliged to
you if you would indicate it.’
‘The train of reasoning is not very obscure, Watson,’ said Holmes with a mischievous
twinkle. ‘It belongs to the same elementary class of deduction which I should illustrate if I
were to ask you who shared your cab in your drive this morning.’
‘I don’t admit that a fresh illustration is an explanation,’ said I with some asperity.
‘Bravo, Watson! A very dignified and logical remonstrance. Let me see, what were the
points? Take the last one first – the cab. You observe that you have some splashes on the left
sleeve and shoulder of your coat. Had you sat in the centre of a hansom you would probably
have had no splashes, and if you had they would certainly have been symmetrical.
Therefore it is clear that you sat at the side. Therefore it is equally clear that you had a
companion.’
‘That is very evident.’
‘Absurdly commonplace, is it not?’
‘But the boots and the bath.’
‘Equally childish. You are in the habit of doing up your boots in a certain way. I see them
on this occasion fastened with an elaborate double bow, which is not your usual method of
tying them. You have, therefore, had them off. Who has tied them? A bootmaker – or the boy
224 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
at the bath. It is unlikely that it is the bootmaker, since your boots are nearly new. Well,
what remains? The bath. Absurd, is it not?
[Doyle, A.C. The Penguin Complete Sherlock Holmes, Harmondsworth 1981: 942]
Text 2
Analyse the verbal groups and verbal group complexes in the following text:
Mrs Birling a – I think we’ve just about come to the end of this wretched business.
Gerald b – I don’t think so.
c Excuse me. (He goes out. They watch him go in silence. We hear the front door
slam.)
Sheila d (to Inspector) – You know, you never showed him that photograph of her.
Inspector e – No.
f It wasn’t necessary.
g And I thought it better not to.
Mrs Birling h – You have a photograph of this girl?
Inspector i – Yes.
j I think you’d better look at it.
Mrs Birling k – I don’t see any particular reason why I should.
Inspector l – Probably not.
m But you’d better look at it.
Mrs Birling n – Very well. (He produces a photograph and she looks hard at it.)
Inspector o (taking back the photograph) – You recognise her.
Mrs Birling p – No.
q Why should I?
Inspector r – Of course she might have changed lately.
s but I can’t believe she could have changed so much.
Mrs Birling t – I don’t understand you, Inspector.
Inspector u – You mean you don’t choose to, Mrs Birling.
Mrs Birling v (angrily) – I meant what I said.
Inspector w – You’re not telling the truth.
Mrs Birling x – I beg your pardon!
Birling y (angrily to Inspector) – Look here,
z I’m not going to have this, Inspector.
aa You’ll apologise at once.
Inspector bb – Apologise for what – doing my duty?
Birling cc – No,
dd for being so offensive about it.
ee I’m a public man.
Inspector ff (massively) – Public men, Mr Birling, have responsibilities as well as privileges.
Birling gg – Possibly.
hh But you weren’t asked to come here and talk to me about my responsibilities.
Sheila ii – Let’s hope not.
jj Though I’m beginning to wonder.
Mrs Birling kk – Does that mean anything, Sheila?
Sheila ll – It means that we’ve no excuse now for putting on airs and that if we’ve any
sense we won’t try.
mm Father threw this girl out
nn because she asked for decent wages.
oo I went and pushed her out further, right into the street,
pp just because I was angry
qq and she was pretty.
rr Gerald set her up as his mistress,
ss and then dropped her
tt when it suited him.
uu And now you’re pretending that you don’t recognise her from that photograph.
vv I admit I don’t know why you should,
ww but I know jolly well you did in fact recognise her, from the way you looked.
CHAPTER 5 GROUP AND PHRASE 225
xx And if you’re not telling the truth,
yy why should the Inspector apologise.
zz And can’t you see both of you, you’re making it worse.
[from Priestley, An Inspector Calls]
function
Deictic Post-Deictic Numerative Numerative Epithet* Epithet* Classifier* Thing Qualifier*
structure:
multivariate Premodifier Head Postmodifier
word class determiner; adjective ordinal, cardinal adjective adjective noun noun prep. phrase,
rankshifted superlative (verbal (adjective, clause
poss. nom. participle) verbal
gp. participle)
example these typical first two little darling black German sheepdogs with warm fur
active
passive ↘ be ^ v-en (to be taken)
circumstances. They may also realise textual elements, such as conjunctive linkers (e.g.
under the circumstances; in contrast) or interpersonal elements such as Modal
Adjuncts (e.g. at times). In addition, they are frequently downranked to serve as
Qualifier within a nominal group, as in the boy with green hair.
Further reading
Matthiessen 1996 develops the reasoning around IFG’s approach to English tense in
some detail; for an alternative perspective see Fawcett 2000a, b. Halliday & James
1993 adopt a quantitative perspective on English tense and polarity. Halliday 2008
touches on English tense at several points. For detailed explorations of the meanings
typically realised through the Epithet function in the English nominal group see
Martin & White 2005, Tucker 1997. For further interpretation of the semantics of
verbal and nominal groups see Halliday & Matthiessen 1999, Chapter 5.
Caffarel, Alice 1992 Interacting between a generalised tense semantics and register-
specific semantic tense systems: a bi-stratal exploration of the semantics of
French tense. Language Sciences. 14.4: 385-419.
Fawcett, R 2000a In place of Halliday’s ‘verbal group’: Part 1: evidence from the
problems of Halliday’s representations and the relative simplicity of the proposed
alternative. Word 51.2. 157-203.
Fawcett, R 2000b In place of Halliday’s ‘verbal group’: Part 2: evidence from
generation, semantics and interruptability. Word 51.3. 327-375.
Halliday, M A K 1979 On being teaching. In S Greenbaum, G Leech & J Svartvik (eds)
Studies in English Linguistics: for Randolph Quirk. London, Longman: 61-64.
[Reprinted in M A K Halliday 2005 Studies in English Language. London:
Continuum (Volume 7 in the Collected Works of M A K Halliday). 209-212]
Halliday, M A K 2008 Complementarities in Language. Beijing: The Commercial Press.
Halliday, M A K & Hasan, R 1984 Cohesion in English. London, Cambridge. Deals with
those group resources that are involved in the creation of cohesive links in
discourse.
Halliday, M A K & Z L James 1993 A quantitative study of polarity and primary tense in
the English finite clause. J M Sinclair, M Hoey & G Fox (eds) Techniques of
Description: spoken and written discourse. London: Routledge. [Reprinted in M A K
Halliday 2005 Computational and Quantitative Studies. London: Continuum
(Volume 6 in the Collected Works of M A K Halliday). 93-129]
Halliday, M A K & C M I M Matthiessen 1999 Construing experience through meaning: a
language based approach to cognition. London: Cassell. [Reprinted by Continuum
2006]
Martin, J R 1992 English Text. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
[This moves up one stratum from this grammatical account, presenting the details
of the discourse semantics of cohesive resources, including reference and
substitution/ellipsis which are realised in group structure.]
Martin, J R & P R R White 2005 The Language of Evaluation. London: Palgrave.
CHAPTER 5 GROUP AND PHRASE 229
1. Orientation
1 This position has also been taken by e.g. Michael Gregory within systemic linguistics/communication
linguistics and Robert Longacre within tagmemic linguistics.
230 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
(i) (ii)
sentence complexing
rank
group/ group/
phrase phrase
... ...
Figure 6.1 Sentence and clause complex
flow of events
The following examples are from procedural texts; here the clause complexes
construe sequences of operations that constitute subprocedures:
Heat remaining oil in same pan add salmon (cook until lightly browned)
remove from pan.
If no successors were generated in (3), (then label node no unsolvable, and (if the
unsolvability of n makes any of its ancestors unsolvable, label these ancestors
unsolvable), and (if the start node is labeled unsolvable, exit with failure), and
remove from OPEN any nodes with an unsolvable ancestor).
As the parentheses indicate, clause complexes may contain subcomplexes such as
if the start node is labeled unsolvable, exit with failure. In procedures, clause complexes
serve to guide the sequence of operations and relate conditions (if no successors were
generated in (3); if you want a more substantial stuffing) and temporal limits to them
(until it is lightly browned). Clause complexes are also an important resource in
reasoning, as in proofs and explanations.
CHAPTER 6 THE CLAUSE COMPLEX 231
All the introductory examples so far have been clause complexes where the
clauses are related by expansion. We will survey the subtypes of expanding relations
presently. But first we should introduce one other major kind of relation by which
clauses may be combined in a clause complex. This relation is projection. You have
already met it in the context of mental and verbal clauses (see Chapter 4 and IFG
Chapter 5), since it always involves a mental or verbal clause projecting its own
content of sensing or saying as a separate clause. For example:
Calypso knew that her aunt knew she knew how unwelcome Richard would be in
Enderby Street.
‘You will have to go to hospital’, said Dr. Smith.
Projection is deployed in narrative to construe the dialogic passages and to
represent the characters’ processes of consciousness; it is also important in news
reporting where it serves to specify the journalists’ sources, as with eye-witness
reports and comments by ‘important people’. Examples:
Greenpeace’s atmosphere and energy campaign co-ordinator Ms Lyn Goldsworthy said
both the tourism and fishing industries at W would be affected by the drilling... ‘The
world now knows it has a global warming problem,’ Ms Goldworthy said.... A
spokeswoman for BHP Petroleum said yesterday she did not know when drilling
would begin...
The difference and complementarity between expansion and projection can be
seen very clearly in the conventions of comic strips. Expansion is represented
pictorially by sequences of frames (sometimes with a conjunction added, if the
unmarked sequence in time is broken, e.g. meanwhile) or by split frames if the
expanding relation is one of addition. Projection, on the other hand, cannot be
represented simply by the pictures: the content of projection is represented
linguistically by text in ‘balloons’ or ‘clouds’.
2. Survey of options
2.1 TAXIS
TAXISis concerned with the interdependency relations between grammatical units
forming a complex, such as groups forming a group complex or clauses forming a
clause complex. Here are some examples of complexes at different ranks:
Table 6.1 Complexes at different ranks
The two options within the system of TAXIS are those of parataxis or hypotaxis,
categories which relate to (but are not identical with) traditional notions of
co-ordination and subordination, respectively. If the units in a complex are of equal
status, the relation between them is a paratactic one; if the units are of unequal status,
the relation is a hypotactic one.
Where two units within a complex are linked in a paratactic relation, each unit
could stand as an independent functioning whole, and neither is dependent on the
other. Here are some examples of paratactic clause complexes:
He looked terrible; || his clothes were crumpled and dirty.
Miriam was somewhat scared by the wind, || but the lads enjoyed it.
He hated her, || for she seemed in some way to make him
despise himself.
He replied quickly, || ‘I can’t complain.’
This paratactic relation is signalled in the notation by labelling the clauses numerically
in sequence. For example:
The rivers overflow, || the streets become rivers || and everything is soaked.
1 2 3
By contrast, where two units within a complex are linked in a hypotactic relation,
one unit modifies the other, one clause is a ‘principal’ or ‘dominant’ clause and the
other is dependent upon it. Examples are shown in Table 6.2:
Table 6.2 Examples of hypotactic clause complexes
complex example diagrammed in IFG2, where the first three clauses form a block with
the second two clauses dependent upon it.
α I don’t mind
if
α β you leave
as soon as
γ you’ve finished
as long as
α you’re back
β when
β I need you
While they have been exemplified separately here, paratactic and hypotactic
relationships may well be combined in a single clause complex. For example:
1 We’re going to Childworld
2 and we’re going to buy some diapers for Stephen and Emily
3 and we’re going to buy an intercom system
β so that we can hear Stephen in different parts of the house
whenever he cries.
or
1 Morel went to bed in misery,
2 and Mrs Morel felt
1 as if she were numbed by some drug,
2 as if her feelings were paralysed.
An alternative representation of the last example can also be given to show the
‘layering’ of the structure:
234 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
and
as if
2
1 she were numbed by some drug
β (i.e.)
parataxis
TAXIS
hypotaxis
2.2.1 Expansion
The various logico-semantic relations included as expanding relations are of three
main kinds: elaboration, extension and enhancement.
2 An embedded clause having the same form, such as who’d been expelled in We spoke to the boy [[who’d
been expelled]], does not have these characteristics.
236 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
2.2.2 Projection
Projection typically concerns the relation between a mental or verbal clause and the
content which it quotes or reports. A paratactic relation holds when one clause quotes
another (‘direct speech/thought’), and a hypotactic relation when one clause reports
another (‘indirect speech/thought’). See Table 6.3:
Table 6.3 Examples of projection
Projected clauses, i.e. quotes or reports, represent the wording or meaning of the
speaker represented in the projecting clause, not the speaker of the clause complex.
They are thus meta-representations, or metaphenomena. The metaphenomenon
projected by a verbal clause is a locution, symbolised by (‘) notation, while a mental
clause projects an idea, notated as (‘):
He told his aunt that he would write home regularly.
‘β (locution)
He decided that he would take a little time off first.
‘β (idea)
All the locutions and ideas illustrated so far have been of information clauses
(propositions), but it is also possible for commands and offers to be projected.
Projected clauses of this latter kind are called proposals, and if hypotactic, take the
form of a modalised declarative or a non-finite clause as shown in Table 6.4:
Table 6.4 Examples of projected proposals
parataxis hypotaxis
ELABORATION
1 Jogging on the streets is bad for you; Jogging stresses your feet,
=2 it damages your feet. =β which can cause serious problems.
1 It was nothing serious; I met Granny,
E =2 it was just a small cut. =β who was looking very well.
X EXTENSION
P
A 1 They cleared the table +β As well as doing the shopping,
N +2 and washed the dishes. they cooked all the meals.
S
1 Did they complain They just kept quiet as usual
I
+2 or did they just keep quiet as usual? +β instead of complaining.
O
N ENHANCEMENT
1 She spent all her money on a car, She couldn’t afford to go on holiday
x2 so she couldn’t afford to go on holiday. xβ because she had spent all her money.
1 Heat the mixture to boiling point, xβ When the pudding has cooled,
x2 then add the milk very slowly. remove carefully from the container.
LOCUTION
P 1 The babysitter said: The babysitter told us
R '2 ‘don’t make a noise!’ 'β not to make a noise.
O
J '1 ‘That’s ridiculous,’ He said to us
E 2 he snapped. 'β that it was ridiculous.
C IDEA
T
I '1 ‘That’s funny,’ He thought
O 2 he thought. 'β we would enjoy it.
N '1 ‘Don’t stop now’, He expected
2 she thought. 'β everybody to help.
238 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
3. Troubleshooting
The ‘minimal length’ approach will allow for a consistency in the analysis, but has
the disadvantage that some logico-semantic relations (particularly of elaboration) are
likely to be missed. This may not be considered too serious a drawback if the text is
also being analysed at the discourse-semantic level, in terms of CONJUNCTION. (See
Chapter 7; also IFG2 9.4, IFG3 9.3 and Martin 1992: Chapter 4; Martin & Rose 2007:
Chapter 4.) Alternatively, an opposite, ‘maximal’ length approach may be taken
whereby a logico-semantic paratactic link is assumed wherever it is possible to argue
for one.
In a written text, it is best to allow punctuation to override grammatical
expectations concerning clause-complex boundaries. And in a similar way, whether a
minimal or maximal approach is favoured, intonation should be taken into account to
modify the general principles being used. IFG2: 306-7 and IFG3: 483 suggest that the
unmarked sequence of tones for two clauses in a cohesive relation is tone 1 (falling)
followed by tone 1; whereas the unmarked sequence for a paratactic sequence is tone
3 (low rise) followed by tone 1.
Below, some examples will be discussed in terms of the two general approaches
(i.e. minimal and maximal):
(i) Paratactic or cohesive elaboration?
Example 1
Teacher: ... his [=chicken embryo] head tucked up under his wing.
Child: He’s squashing.
Teacher: He’s squashing.
He’s filling up nearly the whole of that shell.
A minimal approach will regard the child’s He’s squashing as initiating a fresh complex
and the teacher’s He’s squashing as initiating a further complex. A maximal approach
might take each clause as a paratactic elaboration of the previous one. Given a tone 1
contour on the first three clauses, there would be additional grounds for favouring the
minimal analysis.
Example 2
Child: What does Daddy do
when he’s sick?
Mother: When he’s sick
he goes to bed
and lies down.
When you’re not very well
you, etc.
A minimal approach will regard the Mother’s β clause when you’re not very well as
initiating a new complex on the grounds that this new βα sequence is not linked to the
preceding speech by any explicit conjunction. A maximal approach would see this
section as a (paratactic) elaboration by the mother of her sequence beginning when
he’s sick.
(ii) Paratactic or cohesive extension and enhancement?
Example 1
a ... he said
b he thought
c he knew
d where there was one [dog] in a yard
240 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
e so my husband went up
f and looked over the fence
g and there was our dog, only about a block away
h and they also had a male corgi there
i and the two were running together
j so he brought ours home
k and he went back up later...
In this part of an oral recount, clauses e – k might constitute a single complex, or might
be taken as several different clause complexes, depending on whether any of the
conjunctions are interpreted as cohesive rather than paratactic. We can reason about
the boundaries in the following way:
Clause e can be taken as initiating a new complex with a cohesive so, on the
ideational grounds that it is clearly not part of what is projected by a, b or c; and on the
textual grounds that it serves to initiate a new sequence of actions in the story.
Clauses f and g are paratactic links. This is clear from the omission of the Subject
in f and the impossibility of substituting an unequivocally cohesive conjunction for and
at g. It would, however be feasible to have Furthermore at h and/or i and Consequently
at j. It is at these points, then, that phonological considerations will determine the
boundaries. A tone 3 + tone 1 sequence for two clauses ambiguously related will
suggest that the two form a single complex, whereas a series of tone 1s will indicate a
cohesive relation. Other signals of cohesion rather than taxis would be a pause before
the conjunction and/or a lengthening of the syllable before the conjunction and/or a
change of pitch level on the conjunction.
Example 2
A: Can you do that?
B: He can,
I can’t.
In this example, there is a contrastive relation signalled by the opposed polarity and
the intonation (a tone 4, tone 1 sequence). There is however, no extending conjunction.
A minimal clause complex analysis might therefore wish to treat these as lacking any
grammatical relation of extension. A maximal analysis will allow for the relation,
which can be recognised as paratactic rather than cohesive on the grounds that but
could more readily be used than however between the two clauses.
using. The essential difference is that a prepositional phrase only has a potential for a
two-element configuration, Minor Process ^ Minor Range, whereas a clause is always
expandable and will have some related finite variants. For example:
prepositional phrase (part of clause) functioning as circumstance
What do you intend to do regarding Pamela’s situation?
non-finite clause
He laid out his offer, regarding his opposite number with some hostility.
Including the entire committee in her criticism, the woman made an emotional speech.
The boy didn’t wait; The boy ran away, The boy who ran away
elaboration
5 See Chapter 5. In general terms a nominal group can be thought of as a noun together with any modifying
elements, such as articles, numerals, adjectives preceding the noun and/or an embedded prepositional
phrase (or embedded clause) following the noun, as in the two little boys with red hair or that old green
Ford [[he used to drive]].
244 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
Fact (meta)
* [[That he ran the marathon]] killed him.
* It killed him [[that he ran the marathon]].
(Cf. note in IFG2: 249-50, IFG3: 441 including the interpretation of the example the
fact/knowledge that the experiment had failed destroyed his life.)
Mental clauses
Embedded clauses can function as the Phenomenon of a mental clause under certain
conditions. Embedded clauses representing acts can occur as the Phenomenon of
perception in perceptive clauses.
Act (macro)
The driver didn’t see [[them crossing the street]].
He felt [[the ant crawling up his leg]].
I just heard [[him come in]].
Embedded clauses representing facts can occur as the Phenomenon of emotive
clauses – meta-phenomena: such facts exist independently of the Senser’s conscious
processing and impinge on his/her consciousness:
Fact (meta)
I regret (the fact) [[that we have been unable to succeed]]. :
(The fact) [[That we have been unable to succeed]] annoys me. :
It annoys me [[that we have been unable to succeed]]. :
The fact annoys me [[that we have been unable to succeed]].
As shown, these clauses may function as the postmodifying Qualifier in a nominal
group where the Head is a noun such as fact, case, news, idea, etc. Where there is no
such Head noun, the embedded clause itself can be regarded as serving as the Head of
the nominal group. See IFG 7.5.7.
Cognitive mental clauses usually project ideas. That is, they bring ideas into
existence through the cognitive processing (he thought/believed/guessed/dreamed/
imagined that the earth was flat); they don’t usually involve pre-existing facts as
Phenomenon. However, there are a few examples of facts in a cognitive context – for
instance: He accepted (the fact) that the earth is round (see IFG3: 478).
Verbal clauses
Verbal clauses project locutions in clause complexes but they do not typically occur
with embedded clauses of the act or fact type. There are a couple of exceptions with an
embedded clause as Verbiage:
He acknowledged/admitted to the press (the fact) [[that he had made a serious mistake]].
Relational clauses
Clauses representing both acts and facts can occur as Carrier in attributive clauses and
as Token or Value in identifying ones.
Act (macro)
[[Leaving early]] is better.:
My preference is [[to leave early]].
Fact (meta)
[[That he has already left]] is unlikely. :
It’s unlikely [[that he has already left]].
CHAPTER 6 THE CLAUSE COMPLEX 245
The problem is [[that he has already left]]. :
[[That he has already left]] is the problem.
(ii) Within groups
Embedded clauses only occur within nominal and adverbial groups, not within verbal
groups. Within nominal groups, they may serve as Head, which means that the
embedded clause functions as participant in the clause. In both nominal and adverbial
groups an embedded clause may serve as Postmodifier (see Chapter 5). Table 6.9
provides examples of embedded clauses in nominal groups:
Table 6.9 Embedded clause in nominal group
projection (the problem is) [[that he is (the problem is) the fact [[that he is
negligent]] negligent]]
elaboration [[whatever Wren built]] (was the gazebo [[that Wren built]] (was
admired) admired)
frying green tomatoes (can be fun) the act of [[frying green tomatoes]]
(can be fun)
expansion extension the gazebo [[whose architect later
became famous]] (was admired)
enhancement [[wherever Wren built the gazebo]] the gazebo [[where they used to
(is now a famous place) meet]] (was admired)
(there were) more flowers [[than
they had expected to find]]
projection -- --
elaboration -- --
expansion extension -- --
enhancement -- (he ran) more quickly [[than a man
his age could be expected to]]
ranking embedded
[hypotactic] in nominal group in adverbial group
projection idea: fact (as Phenomenon):
• typically projected by processes • typically configured with
of cognition [proposition] and processes of emotion
affection: desideration [proposal] • not agnate with paratactic
• agnate with paratactic projection, projection
i.e. can be quoted (although • can be the Subject in the agnate
unlikely) passive variant of the clause (since
• cannot be the Subject (since it is it is a participant)
not a participant) • can be the focus of theme
• cannot be the focus of theme predication (since it is a
predication (since it is not a participant)
participant) • cannot be presumed by
• can be presumed by substitute substitute so/not, only by reference
so/not such as that, it
• cannot follow explicit fact noun • can follow explicit fact noun as
as Head/Thing in nominal group Head/Thing in nominal group
she thought ||that she had painted she regretted (the fact) [[that she
the house : had painted the house]] :
she thought || ‘I painted the house’ : it was (the fact) [[that she had
she thought || so painted the house ]] that she
regretted:
she regretted it
elaboration non-defining relative clause: defining relative clause (as
• spoken on separate tone group, Qualifier):
with tone concord (IFG2: 238, 306, • not on separate tone group
IFG3:402) • not separated by punctuation
• written with separating • not agnate with paratactic
punctuation symbols, usually elaboration
commas (IFG2: 238) • full range of relative forms,
• agnate with paratactic including relative that and ‘contact’
elaboration relative (if Complement)
• range of relative items typically
excluding relative that and
‘contact’ relative
Hyde Park, <<which they used to the park [[which/that/they used
like>>, has been turned into a to like ]] has been turned into a
shopping complex : shopping complex
Hyde Park has been turned into a
shopping complex; ||they used to
like it
extension instead of/as well as going fishing, [*]
they took a stroll on the beach
enhancement serves as qualifying clause in serves as Qualifier in nominal enhancing clauses
clause complex group: embedded Postmodifiers
• cannot follow explicit • can follow explicit circumstantial in adverbial groups are
circumstantial noun noun as Head/Thing in nominal only comparative:
• only certain subtypes can be the group than..., as (if)..., (for x) to...
focus of theme predication • nominal group serves in [IFG2: 210, IFG3: 357-8]
• is agnate with a paratactic relational clause they proceeded more
enhancement • can always be the focus of theme cautiously [[than the other
they went fishing || just before the predication team]]
sun set : • is not agnate with a paratactic
they went fishing || then the sun set enhancement
(the period) [[just before the sun
sets]] is the best time to go fishing
[*] Non-defining relative clauses with whose, of which can be taken as extending (see IFG2: 245,
IFG3: 432).
CHAPTER 6 THE CLAUSE COMPLEX 247
You can probe which sense is relevant in a given example by considering what the
set of agnate items is: each sense of the item in question will correspond to a different
agnation set. When you are trying to identify the sense of a structural conjunction, you
can also probe it by checking what a variant with an added cohesive conjunction (a
conjunctive Adjunct) would be.
CHAPTER 6 THE CLAUSE COMPLEX 249
3.8 Projection
1 She said
‘2 ‘That would be wonderful!’
If the projected clause happens to be the initiating clause in a paratactic complex,
the quote symbol is written against the 1 to indicate this:
‘1 ‘Don’t flatter yourself, chum,’
2 she thought.
expanding the initial embedded clause several times. By the time a new clause
complex is initiated, there is a sense that it is conjunctively related to the previous
‘embedded’ clause, as though by then the structure has surfaced from the initial
embedding. In such a case it is probably most realistic to let the analysis reflect this
‘surfacing’ by displaying a ‘ranking’ clause complex at the initial embedding:
What you have to do is
1 make up two teams
+2 and get someone from Team A to read a question to Team B,
=β 1 who have a set amount of time to answer,
+2 and who – << >> go on and roll the dice.
xβ <<if they get it right>>
Alternatively, treat the second (or some later clause) within the embedding as
linked by taxis with the initial non-embedded part:
1 What you have to do is [[make up two teams
+2 and get someone from Team A to read a question to Team B,
=β who have a set amount of time to answer,
4. Analysis practice
4.1 Phase I
4.1.1 Exercises
Examples are taken or adapted from various sources including The Wentworth Courier,
William Golding’s Lord of the Flies, D.H. Lawrence’s Sons and Lovers, Aldous Huxley’s
Antic Hay, George Orwell’s Animal Farm and F. Scott Fitzgerald’s The Great Gatsby.
4. The Thai do not kill animals, so the meat butchering is left to the Muslims and
Chinese.
5. It was a dismal affair, || which might have belonged to Maurice Barres in youth.
7. I looked back at my cousin, || who began to ask me questions in her low, thrilling
voice.
2. He sent ten shillings once or twice, || but he had many things to pay for at first.
4. In a few minutes she put on her coat,|| to walk the two and a half miles to the
station.
5. Mrs Morel talked again to Paul, || who was helping her with her housework.
7. Folding him in her arms, || she swayed slightly from side to side with love.
9. Whilst Morel was progressing favourably in the hospital, || the family was
extraordinarily happy and peaceful.
10. The Thai do not kill animals, || so the meat butchering is left to the Muslims and
Chinese.
3. The three children realised || that it was very bad for their father.
8. Paul knew || that this girl, Louise Travers, was now Dawes’s woman.
256 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
4. Whilst Morel was progressing favourably in the hospital, || the family was
extraordinarily happy
and peaceful.
β
5. She dared not look at him, || but sat with her head bowed.
1 2
6. To Miriam he more or less condescended, || because she seemed so humble.
β
7. His face quivered || as he looked at his mother.
β
8. The clouds empty their watery load || and are then dispelled by the sun.
1 2
9. Mrs. Morel was not anxious to move into the Bottoms, || which was already 12
years old.
β
10. The women did not spare her, at first; || for she was superior.
1 2
There are many species of whales. They are conveniently divided into toothed and baleen
categories. The toothed whales are found worldwide in great numbers. The largest is the
Sperm whale, which grows to about the size of a boxcar. Other species familiar to
Canadians are the Beluga or white whale, the Narwhal with its unicorn-like tusk, the Killer
whale or Orca, the Pilot or Pothead whale, << which is commonly stranded on beaches>>,
the Spotted and Spinner Dolphins, << that create a problem for tuna seiners>>, and the
Porpoises, which we commonly see along our shores.
There are fewer species of the larger baleen whales, that filter krill and small fish
through their baleen plates. The largest is the Blue whale, which is seen frequently in the
Gulf of St Lawrence. It reaches a length of 100 feet and a weight of 200 tons. The young are
25 feet long at birth and put on about 200 lbs. a day on their milk diet. Other species are:
the Fins << which at a length of 75 ft. blow spouts of 20 ft.>>, the fast swimming Seis, the
Grays, <<(which are) so commonly seen on migrations along our Pacific coast between
Baja California and the Bering Sea>>, the Bowheads of Alaskan waters, the Rights,
<<(which are) so seriously threatened>>, the Humpbacks, <<(which are) enjoyed by
tourists in such places as Hawaii and Alaska>>, the smaller Bryde’s whales, and the
smallest Minke whales, which continue to be abundant worldwide.
[From Martin, W.R. 1989 ‘Innovative Fisheries management: International Whaling.’ A.T. Bielak
(ed) Innovative Fisheries Management Initiatives. Ottawa, Canadian Wildlife Federation: 1-4.]
2. Count the paratactic links, then the hypotactic. How do the numbers compare?
3. Are there examples of both projection and expansion?
4. What kind of projection is found? What kind of expansion is most frequent?
5. How does this text contrast with Texts 1 and 2 in its use of clause complexing
resources?
Are governments necessary?
I think governments like the Federal Government are necessary because they help to keep
our economic system in order and if any problems occur the Federal Government will more
or less straighten it out.
I also think that the State Government isn’t necessary because there is the local
government, which is known as a shire or municipality, and so there is hardly any use for
the State Government because the local governments do all the work.
[Text courtesy of Bill Crowley]
4.2 Phase II
4.2.1 Exercises
2. Mrs. Blackmore’s car, which can’t be more than two years old, is already full of rust.
3. Meg and Paul, although they despised the others, remained with the group and
took part in their schemes.
4. The older man, who had been sitting in the corner and seemed uninterested,
suddenly got up and approached them.
5. Marco Polo, journeying through Yunnan in the thirteenth century, after it had been
conquered by the Mongols, observed that the people ate their meat raw.
260 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
2. Paul always examined the big grass bank, because in it grew tiny pansies.
3. He knew the order of the names – they went according to stall number.
7. ‘Well, you can’t be stuck in the house for ever,’ Annie agreed.
We supported the French Revolution at first, || but after the fall of the monarchy fled to
England. enhancing
Text 2 Recount
This text is a written version of a spoken text where the speaker is recalling a period
from his youth. Divide the following text into clauses and analyse the clause complex
relations.
If I would be too late in the morning, the station owner would pour cold water over me.
Sometimes I would watch the cattle while they had their lunch or got ready in the morning,
and if a calf would walk away he would yell out from the camp, ‘Hey, what do you think,
don’t you want that calf, eh?’ I wouldn’t know, because I was looking in another direction.
He would come up and say, ‘You’ve got to start watching those cattle.’ I couldn’t talk back
to him because I was too small. That bloke always did that to me, and he would give me a
good beating with a whip. When I was working there I really had a terrible life with that
man. And if I didn’t do my job, he would starve me for two days and just give me dry
damper. And if I didn’t do my job well, he would next time just give me a piece of meat.
[Adapted from Paddy Patrick Jangala 1985 Stories from Lajamanu. Darwin NT: Dept. of Education.]
Text 4 Exposition
Here we return to a text from Phase I. This time notate the clause complex relations in
the left hand margin.
I think
governments like the Federal Government are necessary
because they help to keep our economic system in order
and if any problems occur
the Federal Government will more or less straighten it out.
I also think
that the State Government isn’t necessary
because there is the local government,
which is known as a shire or municipality,
and so there is hardly any use for the State Government
because the local governments do all the work.
4.3.1 Exercises
Exercise 1 Identifying embedded clauses (facts)
Enclose in square brackets any embedded clauses in the following examples.
It is important to understand the strategy adopted by the developers.
It is important [[to understand the strategy adopted by the developers.]]
1. It infuriated him that she could not give him an answer.
2. It is instructive to recall the invitation for expressions of interest.
3. That it’s likely market value had diminished had certainly occurred to him.
4. It is good to be seen as the great reformer.
5. It wasn’t at Christmas that he had seen her.
6. To understand the natural order is a good thing.
Text 2 Expository
Divide the following text into ranking clauses and analyse the clause complex
relations.
Advice such as this (i.e. be wary of approaching children before they are ‘ready’ to learn
mathematics) has produced teachers who are likely to be wary about interference into
children’s play in case they are not ‘ready’ or they spoil something spontaneous which is
going on, and when they do so interfere they expect that they will have to provide children
with experiences which will lead them to make the appropriate discriminations, which
they will not expect the children already to have mastered. The framework for the
interpretation of errors is therefore given by the pedagogy: that is, that children will fail
because they are not ‘ready’ for that kind of learning, which will in turn lead the teacher to
feel implicitly at fault because (by implication) she was also not sensitive enough. This
provides one aspect of a ‘regime of meaning’, a regime of ‘truth’ in which the teacher
268 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
herself is positioned. This creates her insertion as subject into the practice and provides
both the basis of her readings and the aspect of the regulation to which she is subjected.
The regime is not one which is provided by the teacher, but which itself produces her
consciousness of what teaching means: thus it produces not only her assessment of the
children but is bound up with her assessment of herself. The teacher can in no way be
judged as standing outside the practices in which she as teacher is positioned and which
delimit, define, and evaluate her work.
[Walkerdine, V. 1988 The Mastery of Reason: cognitive development and the production
of rationality. London: Routledge (Critical Psychology).]
Text 3 Teaser
Try puzzling out the clause complex relations here. (No key provided.)
Jack thinks
he does not know
what he thinks
Jill thinks
he does not know
But Jill thinks Jack does know it.
So Jill does not know
she does not know
that Jack does not know
that Jill thinks
that Jack does know
and Jack does not know he does not know
that Jill does not know she does not know
that Jack does not know
that Jill thinks Jack knows
what Jack thinks he does not know
Jack doesn’t know he knows
and he doesn’t know
Jill does not know.
Jill doesn’t know she doesn’t know,
and doesn’t know
that Jack doesn’t know Jill does not know.
They have no problems.
[Knots by R. D. Laing, quoted in H.H. Clark Arenas of Language. Chicago & Stanford:
University of Chicago Press and Center for the Study of Language and Information. 1992: 9.]
5.1 Review
In Chapters of IFG and this workbook concerned with ‘Clause as message’, ‘Clause as
exchange’ and ‘Clause as representation’, you have seen how the three metafunctions
come together to define the clause as a plurifunctional grammatical unit; it is
internally organised according to the textual (thematic), interpersonal (modal) and
ideational: experiential (transitivity) metafunctions. See Figure 5.6:
CHAPTER 6 THE CLAUSE COMPLEX 269
In this Chapter we have been concerned with how any clause can be combined
with another to form a clause complex, using the resources of the logical metafunction.
These resources also serve to combine smaller grammatical units such as words,
groups and phrases into complexes; however when clauses are combined, no higher
grammatical unit with constituent parts is created. A clause complex is just a complex
of units – clauses; it is not itself a new kind of higher-ranking grammatical unit.
A clause complex would arise if, after having said Madam, you’ll look like a tulip,
the speaker decides to expand this clause by another clause; and this option of
expansion could then be selected again:
Madam, you’ll look like a tulip
Madam, you’ll look like a tulip so the other guests will laugh at you
Madam, you’ll look like a tulip so the other guests will laugh at you and then you’ll
get angry
This expansion is shown diagrammatically in Figure 6.7.
In fact, there is no theoretical limit to the number of times a clause complex can
be expanded by another clause. The reason is that the structure of the clause complex
270 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
is relational: one clause is related or linked to another step by step. A clause complex
is thus like a chain or series – it can be expanded indefinitely. Compare TV serials like
Days of our Lives, Coronation Street or Neighbours that just go on and on. A clause
complex is thus structurally quite different from a simple clause. A simple clause is, as
we have seen, on organic whole. Just like other organic wholes, such as biological
organisms or atoms, it has a fixed number of component parts with particular values
relative to the whole: Theme + Rheme (textual), Mood + Residue [+ Moodtag +
Vocative] (interpersonal), and Participants + Process + Circumstances (experiential).
That is, the organisation of the clause is configurational rather than serial or relational.
hypotaxis parataxis
Further reading
For further interpretation of the semantics of clause complexing see Halliday &
Matthiessen 1999, Chapter 3.
Text analysis
1. Orientation
1 For an alternative extension see Halliday & Hasan's 1976 Cohesion in English, which elaborates Chapter 9
of IFG (3rd edition).
CHAPTER 7 TEXT ANALYSIS 273
As a first step we might consider the layout of the text – its multimodal
'graphology' if you will. To the left we have two photos, Bondi beach below, and Peter
FitzSimons above; then to the right we have a 'kicker', with the main text below. And
below the main text we have two separated paragraphs, one thanking a reader for
drawing Fitzsimons' attention to the 'Black Sunday' rescue, followed by another in
bold soliciting further suggestions for his regular 'placeintime' column. This general
layout is outlined in Figure 7.2, including placement of the photographer's byline
274 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
('Photography Marco Del Grande') and the column's heading ('placeintime – bondi
beach') to the left of FitzSimons' photograph.
photographer
byline
Peter kicker
FitzSimons
head- photo
ing
Bondi text
Beach
photo
thanks
solicitation
To get started with any linguistically informed text description, we need some units of
analysis. The clause complex is as good a place to start as any, because it makes us
read a text very closely for meaning at the same time as it forces us out of the clause to
consider inter-clause relations. It orients us to text structure in other words.
FitzSimons' text can be divided into clauses using the conventions developed in
this workbook (i.e. double slashes between ranking clauses, triple slashes between
clause complexes and double square brackets for embedded clauses). For this analysis
it is clearer to start each ranking clause on a new line, as illustrated for FitzSimon's
kicker below:
Big waves and Bondi Beach have always gone together, ||
writes Peter FitzSimons, ||
but no one had ever seen [[the ocean rise up with a strength such as this]] … |||
For the analysis of the main body of the text we have in addition skipped a line
between paragraphs (marked ¶), which as we can see are shorter in print media
discourse than print discourse of other kinds (this book for example). For ranking
CHAPTER 7 TEXT ANALYSIS 275
2 This detail can be pursued at a later stage of analysis as required; but a comprehensive analysis of
conjunctive relations, both within and between clause complexes, makes a great deal of this delicacy
redundant.
3 An alternative analysis would treat including those standing on the beach as a prepositional phrase,
realising a circumstance of accompaniment.
276 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
Now, the boiling surf, <<…>>, was filled with distressed folk ||
<<with yet more large waves continuing to roll over>>, ||
waving for help. |||
¶
In their long and glorious history, this still stands as the finest hour of the Australian surf
lifesaving movement. |||
For,<<…>>, the Bondi Boys now charged into the surf, ||
<<ignoring their own possible peril>>, ||
some attached to one of the seven reels available, ||
some relying only on their own strength. |||
As one, they began pulling the people out. |||
¶
On the shore, many survivors were resuscitated, ||
as the Bondi clubhouse was turned into a kind of emergency clearing house, ||
and ambulances from all over Sydney town descended ||
and carried the victims away. ||
¶
4
Finally, just half an hour after the waves hit, ||
the water was cleared of bobbing heads and waving arms, ||
and it was time to take stock: ||
250 people had needed the lifesavers to pull them out, ||
of whom 210 were OK once back on land. |||
¶
Thirty-five needed mouth to mouth ||
to be restored to consciousness, ||
while five people perished. |||
The final two paragraphs of the verbal text parse as follows:
With thanks to Waverly Library and reader Diane Touzell, ||
whose grandfather, Bill Jenkins, was one of the lifesavers. |||
Do you have a historical anecdote about a place in Sydney? |||
Write to Peter FitzSimons at [email protected] |||
Clause analysis of this kind places us in a good position to pursue relations either
within or between clause complexes. Looking within, each ranking or embedded
clause can be analysed for transitivity; as for mood and theme, ranking finite clauses
should probably be our focus of attention, since non-finite hypotactically dependent
clauses and embedded clauses have been demoted from both the arguability of the
discourse and the main thrust of its information flow. Looking between clause
complexes we can move to a consideration of conjunctive relations (WWD Chapter 4),
and explore the rhetorical and logical structure of the text as it unfolds.
4 Taken here as a dependent clause, with just half an hour modifying after; could alternatively be read as a
prepositional phrase with [[after the waves hit]] postmodifying hour.
CHAPTER 7 TEXT ANALYSIS 277
The incursion proper begins with a marked Theme highlighting the sound of the
first wave, and an unmarked Theme introducing the wave itself. Subsequently, the
waves are unmarked Theme in every ranking clause. Alongside explicit unmarked
Themes we've added boxes for Themes that have been ellipsed in non-finite and
paratactic extending clauses; these missing Subjects all refer to the waves as well. In
this respect, English is operating here like many languages that use an unmarked
Theme to establish an orientation to a field and then don't mention Theme again until
this orientation needs to be reinforced or changed (Caffarel et al. 2004). In terms of
the gaze sustaining function of unmarked Theme it would be quite misleading to treat
the Predicators as Theme in these 'elliptical' clauses.
With a roar like [[a Bondi tram running amok]], an enormous wave suddenly rolled over the
thousands in the surf, ||
including those many [[standing on the large sandbank just out from the shore]] ||
– knocking them all over ||
as it went. |||
And then another wave hit, ||
and then another. |||
¶
Three huge waves, just like that, piggy-backed their way further and further up the beach ||
and grabbed everything [[they could]] along the way – from babies to toddlers to
adolescents to beach umbrellas, to old blokes and young sheilas alike, ||
and then made a mad dash for the open sea again, ||
carrying all before it ||
and sweeping everyone off the sandbank and into the deep channel next to it in the process.
|||
Subsequently another marked theme shifts our attention to the waves' impact:
¶
In no more than 20 seconds, that peaceful scene had been tragically transformed into utter
chaos. |||
Now, the boiling surf, <<…>>, was filled with distressed folk ||
<<with yet more large waves continuing to roll over>>, ||
waving for help. |||
We then shift via a marked Theme to the Bondi Boys, who are unmarked Theme in
every clause involving the rescue; in addition we find another marked Theme, as one,
emphasising the intensity of their cooperation.
¶
In their long and glorious history, this still stands as the finest hour of the Australian surf
lifesaving movement. |||
For,<<…>>, the Bondi Boys now charged into the surf, ||
<<ignoring their own possible peril>>, ||
some attached to one of the seven reels available, ||
some relying only on their own strength. |||
As one, they began pulling the people out. |||
And then we shift focus again, via another marked Theme, to the rescue on shore:
¶
On the shore, many survivors were resuscitated, ||
as the Bondi clubhouse was turned into a kind of emergency clearing house, ||
and ambulances from all over Sydney town descended ||
278 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
1 Three huge waves, just like that, piggy-backed their way further and further up
the beach ||
+
2 and grabbed everything [[they could]] along the way – from babies to toddlers to
adolescents to beach umbrellas, to old blokes and young sheilas alike, ||
x
3 and then made a mad dash for the open sea again, ||
x
1 carrying all before it ||
+
2 and sweeping everyone off the sandbank and into the deep channel next to it in
the process. |||
In no more than 20 seconds, that peaceful scene had been tragically transformed
into utter chaos. |||
Now, the boiling surf, <<…>>, was filled with distressed folk ||
+
<<with yet more large waves continuing to roll over>>, ||
=
waving for help. |||
In their long and glorious history, this still stands as the finest hour of the
Australian surf lifesaving movement. |||
For,<<…>>, the Bondi Boys now charged into the surf, ||
=
<<ignoring their own possible peril>>, ||
=
1 some attached to one of the seven reels available, ||
+
2 some relying only on their own strength. |||
As one, they began pulling the people out. |||
On the shore, many survivors were resuscitated, ||
x
1 as the Bondi clubhouse was turned into a kind of emergency clearing house, ||
+
2 1 and ambulances from all over Sydney town descended ||
+
2 and carried the victims away. |||
1 1 x
Finally, just half an hour after the waves hit, ||
=
of whom 210 were OK once back on land. |||
5 Contrast IFG 3rd edition 542-3 which focuses grammatically on simple, complex and simple internal
temporal relations, with several subdivisions within each of these; the opposition of successive to
simultaneous events is not a major parameter.
CHAPTER 7 TEXT ANALYSIS 281
^ In their long and glorious history, this still stands as the finest hour of the Australian surf
lifesaving movement. |||
For,<<…>>, the Bondi Boys now charged into the surf, ||
& <<ignoring their own possible peril>>, ||
& some attached to one of the seven reels available, ||
& some relying only on their own strength. ||
^ As one, they began pulling the people out. |||
On the shore, many survivors were resuscitated, ||
& as the Bondi clubhouse was turned into a kind of emergency clearing house, ||
^and ambulances from all over Sydney town descended ||
^and carried the victims away. ||
^ Finally, just half an hour after the waves hit, ||
^ the water was cleared of bobbing heads and waving arms, ||
& and it was time to take stock: ||
250 people had needed the lifesavers to pull them out, ||
^ of whom 210 were OK once back on land. |||
& Thirty-five needed mouth to mouth ||
^ to be restored to consciousness, ||
while five people perished. |||
As we might expect, successive (then, now, finally, after) and simultaneous (as,
with) temporal conjunctions are used to time events; but there are many implicit
temporal links with events connected 'neutrally' by simple juxtaposition, and, or the
dependency of non-finite clauses on finite ones. The text, as do stories in general, thus
takes advantage of field based understandings, in this case of what happens when big
waves hit a beach and a rescue operation gets underway – a reasonable expectation in
an article composed for beach-savvy Sydneysiders.
We can also note that many of these relations obtain between clauses inside
clause complexes, and others between clause complexes – since successive and
simultaneous temporal relations can be realised in either way. For example, the same
phrase and then functions both within and between clause complexes during the
waves' attack above, highlighting the borders of a purely grammatical approach to
logical relations:
^ And then another wave hit, ||
^ and then another. |||
A further limitation of the grammatical approach outlined in IFG 3rd edition is its
backgrounding of the important distinction between what Halliday & Hasan (1976)
call internal and external conjunction. External conjunctions construct logical
relations between events in the field of discourse; internal relations construct
rhetorical relations among phases of unfolding text. FitzSimons for example begins his
text with And so, even though he hasn't mentioned any events which could be
construed as causing people to be lying on the beach. The causal relation here is a
rhetorical one, related to us settling down with text and image and starting to read
because we have the feature in front of us.
And so there they lie, ||
happily sweltering in the summer sun on Australia's most famous beach, ||
just as they have for so many generations past. |||
It is such a wonderfully peaceful scene – of people and nature as a happy whole – [[that it
is simply unimaginable [[that in a few seconds nature could ever rear up|| and savage the
lot of them]].]] |||
282 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
Similarly Ah, but marks the resigned counterexpectation of shifting from viewing
the image in the present to reconstructing events from the past – the meaning is
something like ‘although we've been looking at the picture for a bit, it's time now to
begin our tale’:
Ah, but those 35,000 Sydneysiders [[who were lying in those very spots on the afternoon
of February 6, 1938]], surely felt equally at peace. |||
Likewise For explains why FitzSimons is claiming the rescue to be the Australian
surf lifesaving movement's finest hour, not why one event resulted in another:
In their long and glorious history, this still stands as the finest hour of the Australian surf
lifesaving movement. |||
For,<<…>>, the Bondi Boys now charged into the surf, ||
<<ignoring their own possible peril>>, ||
some attached to one of the seven reels available, ||
some relying only on their own strength. ||
These internal relations, as we can see, tend to be realised between rather than
within clause complexes and are oriented to phases of the generic structure of texts
rather than what happened per se. Working with Discourse introduces conventions for
analysing internal relations alongside external ones, rather than ignoring them
(because they are between clause complexes) or subsuming some of them as delicate
sub-types of expansion (inside clause complexes).
As far as mood is concerned, FitzSimons' text is overwhelmingly concerned with
giving information, and uses declarative clauses to do so. It is only in the final two
clauses that we find an interrogative (Do you...) and then an imperative (Write...) clause,
signalling the shift from chronicling to solicitation:
Do you have a historical anecdote about a place in Sydney? |||
Write to Peter FitzSimons at [email protected] |||
Working with Discourse, Chapter 7, introduces a framework for dealing with the
function of mood in dialogue; see also Eggins & Slade 1997.
As noted above however, the text does feature a number of non-finite clauses,
many with an implicit Subject/unmarked Theme. All except one (to be restored...) are
imperfective and construct action that is underway and overlapping with the alpha
clause on which they depend for temporal deixis (past) and to which they defer in
terms of arguability (declarative). These clauses in effect help the text accelerate as a
number of synchronous events are canvassed, thereby foregrounding the
encompassing chaos of the waves' incursion and the cooperative intensity of the Bondi
Boys' rescue.
With a roar like [[a Bondi tram running amok]], an enormous wave suddenly rolled over
the thousands in the surf, ||
& including those many [[standing on the large sandbank just out from the shore]] ||
& – knocking them all over ||
& as it went. |||
^ And then another wave hit, ||
^ and then another. |||
^ Three huge waves, just like that, piggy-backed their way further … up the beach ||
& and grabbed everything [[they could]] along the way – from babies to toddlers to
adolescents to beach umbrellas, to old blokes and young sheilas alike, ||
^ and then made a mad dash for the open sea again, ||
& carrying all before it ||
CHAPTER 7 TEXT ANALYSIS 283
& and sweeping everyone off the sandbank and into the deep channel next to it in the
process. |||
In no more than 20 seconds, that peaceful scene had been tragically transformed into utter
chaos. |||
^ Now, the boiling surf, <<…>>, was filled with distressed folk ||
& <<with yet more large waves continuing to roll over>>, ||
& waving for help. |||
^ In their long and glorious history, this still stands as the finest hour of the Australian surf
lifesaving movement. |||
For,<<…>>, the Bondi Boys now charged into the surf, ||
& <<ignoring their own possible peril>>, ||
& some attached to one of the seven reels available, ||
& some relying only on their own strength. ||
^ As one, they began pulling the people out. |||
Both effective and middle clauses interact with the temporal relations discussed
above to construct the activity sequences comprising a disaster and rescue operation
6 For this analysis some attached to one of the seven reels available has been treated as a passive material
clause, in line with its treatment as a non-finite process above; it could arguably be taken as relational
attributive, with attached as Attribute.
284 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
of this kind. A transitive perspective on what happened is presented in Table 7.3 below.
The experiential grammar builds the figures (participant, process and circumstance
configurations) which constitute the predictable sequences the story recounts. Once
the waves roll in, there are no surprises, which accounts for the absence of external
concessive relations in FitzSimon's article. For more on ideational semantics in
relation to field, see Working with Discourse Chapter 3.
Table 7.3 Activity sequencing – a transitive perspective
an enormous wave
it
another wave
another [wave]
three huge waves
their
they
it
the boiling surf
more large waves
the surf
286 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
the waves
the water
The experiential construction of the Bondi Boys and closely related participants is
outlined in Table 7.6. The boys themselves involve very little description (bronzed) or
classification (Bondi). Their environs and activities are more developed (described
popular, possible; and classified Bondi Surf Bathers' Life Saving, surf, Australian,
lifesaving, Bondi).
Table 7.6 Nominal construal of the Bondi Boys
The Bondi Boys are a little easier to keep track of, since they are treated as a
group (except for partitioning into those attached to reels and those not). The boys are
introduced esphorically8, with Deictic those resolved in the Qualifier of the Bondi Surf
Bather's Life Saving Club. Thereafter they are tracked through the, their, they (and
elided them). Two related participants, the Australian surf lifesaving movement and
the Bondi clubhouse, are bridged from these lifesavers:
Table 7.7 Identity chains for the Bondi Boys
those bronzed boys of the Bondi Surf Bathers' Life Saving Club
their popular surf competitions.
– the Australian surf lifesaving movement
their own possible peril
the Bondi Boys
some (of them)
some (of them)
their own strength
they
7 Treated as elliptical for the partitive some of the boys here; could alternatively be treated as Deictic, with
ellipsis expanded as some Bondi Boys; in which case the reference would be non-phoric (i.e. presenting a
cohort of the boys, not tracking a subset of them).
8 With esphoric reference, deixis is resolved inside the same nominal group, typically from Deictic to
Qualifier.
CHAPTER 7 TEXT ANALYSIS 287
– the Bondi clubhouse
the lifesavers
The people in the contemporary image of Bondi Beach are minimally construed
by pronouns and ellipsis (letting the picture speak for itself). The people there in 1935
however are variously modified – both situated (at the beach, in the surf and on a
sandbank) through Qualifiers, and quantified via both Numerative (35,000, many, 250,
210, thirty-five, five) and Thing (numbers, thousands, everyone) functions.
Table 7.8 Nominal construal of people on the beach
As far as tracking these people is concerned the text is a much more stop and
start affair. It begins by referring exophorically9 to the people in the image:
they (people on beach in image)
(they)
them
It then turns its attention to the people on the beach in 1935 (with bridging for the
numbers).
those 35,000 Sydneysiders [[who were lying on those very spot on the afternoon of
Feburary 6, 1935]]
– the numbers
the thousands [in the surf]
those many [[standing on the large sandbank just out from the shore]]
them
Later on, those stranded in the surf are introduced:
distressed folk
the people
And further on the survivors are introduced (providing bridging support for the
victims).
many survivors
– the victims
These different guises reflect the impact that the agentive waves and Bondi Boys have
at the beach that day, transforming Sydneysiders on the beach into distressed folk in
the water and then survivors on shore.
FitzSimons' text also features an important interaction between identification
and periodicity. Note that in three of the text's higher level Themes we find text
reference10 used to point forward to the discourse these Themes dominate (this, it
and this as boxed below):
[macro-Theme] Big waves and Bondi Beach have always gone together, writes
Peter FitzSimons, but no one had ever seen the ocean rise up with a strength such
as this …
[hyper-Theme] At three o'clock there was still not the slightest clue that this
afternoon would forever be known as ‘Black Sunday’ in the annals of Sydney.
Then it happened.
[hyper-Theme] In their long and glorious history, this still stands as the finest
hour of the Australian surf lifesaving movement.
Text reference is also used retrospectively to consolidate the waves’ attack and
withdrawal:
With a roar like a Bondi tram running amok, an enormous wave suddenly rolled over the
thousands in the surf, including those many standing on the large sandbank just out from
the shore – knocking them all over as it went. And then another wave hit, and then another.
Three huge waves, just like that, piggy-backed their way further and further up the beach
and grabbed everything they could along the way – from babies to toddlers to adolescents
to beach umbrellas, to old blokes and young sheilas alike, and then made a mad dash for
the open sea again, carrying all before it and sweeping everyone off the sandbank and into
the deep channel next to it in the process.
10 Text reference refers to the use of deixis, sometimes in conjunction with a metadiscursive lexical item
(e.g. the process), to treat text itself as if it was a participant, and track it accordingly; it is much more
frequently cataphoric (i.e. pointing forward) than identification of other kinds.
CHAPTER 7 TEXT ANALYSIS 289
White 2005), these evaluations appreciate the value of things – the strength of the
ocean, the notoriety of the day, the glory of the lifesaving movement's history and the
splendour of this moment in their past:
[macro-Theme] ocean... strength
[hyper-Theme] this afternoon... ‘Black Sunday’
[hyper-Theme] glorious history, finest hour
So instead of highlighting the emotional reactions of people, or their character,
FitzSimons invites us to share attitudes to things (both concrete, like the ocean, and
abstract, like history). His appreciations of the lifesaving movement can of course be
taken as tokens of judgement (of the Bondi Boys' strength and courage). But it is
significant that the text uses almost no inscribed 11 judgement to evaluate the
lifesavers (referring just once to their strength), and employs just four instances of
inscribed affect12 (happily, happy, felt equally at peace, distressed). The main type of
evaluation inscribed throughout the text is in fact appreciation:
Australia's most famous beach
such a wonderfully peaceful scene
The day was, in the vernacular of the time, a ‘stinker’
a record turnout on the beach
one of their popular surf competitions
‘Black Sunday’
that peaceful scene
tragically transformed
utter chaos.
long and glorious history
the finest hour of the Australian surf lifesaving movement
ignoring their own possible peril
For FitzSimons then it is the significance of the moment that matters, not directly
the character or emotional reactions of the lifesavers or the people they rescue. In this
respect, his stance is more that of a historian than narrator, since in narrative proper
we typically use both inscribed affect and judgement to engage reader/listeners in our
tale.
Beyond attitude there are other dimensions of appraisal analysis to consider.
Turning to resources for graduation, as we saw in the nominal group analysis above,
size matters – both with respect to the enormity of the waves and the numbers of
people involved:
big, enormous, huge, large;
35,000, many, many, 250, 210, thirty-five, five;
thousands
In addition, FitzSimons turns up the volume of his article through various types of
grammatical intensification which grade Epithet and Numerative functions:
simply unimaginable; such a wonderful peaceful scene;
most famous, the slightest clue, finest hour;
so many
11 Attitude is inscribed when explicitly evaluative lexis is deployed (e.g. their own strength) and invoked
when the selection of experiential meaning implies an evaluation (e.g. the implied bravery of the Bondi
Boys now charged into the surf).
12 FitzSimons in fact writes of people and nature as a happy whole, with happy arguably inscribing
appreciation here.
290 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
13 Non-core enormous for example is more forceful than the more neutral large.
CHAPTER 7 TEXT ANALYSIS 291
In no more than 20 seconds, that peaceful scene had been tragically transformed into
utter chaos.
For the rescue and taking stock phases, quantification is the main strategy deployed to
sustain the Bondi Boys' finest hour, a prosody which arguably extends over both these
phases (with the periodicity adjusted accordingly below).
In their long and glorious history, this still stands as the finest hour of the Australian surf
lifesaving movement.
For, ignoring their own possible peril, the Bondi Boys now charged into the surf, some
attached to one of the seven reels available, some relying only on their own strength.
As one, they began pulling the people out.
On the shore, many survivors were resuscitated, as the Bondi clubhouse was turned
into a kind of emergency clearing house, and ambulances from all over Sydney town
descended and carried the victims away.
Finally, just half an hour after the waves hit, the water was cleared of bobbing heads
and waving arms, and it was time to take stock:
250 people had needed the lifesavers to pull them out, of whom 210 were OK
once back on land.
Thirty-five needed mouth to mouth to be restored to consciousness, while five
people perished.
To summarise then, FitzSimons foregrounds appreciation over judgement and affect,
and makes liberal use of force to dramatise events. So the text is about the kind of day
it was, not in the first instance about how people reacted emotionally or how well or
badly they behaved. The point of the story thus seems to involve the historical
significance of the day – its value as a piece of history. And this stance is foregrounded
in trite phrasing verging on lexicalisation as far as history making is concerned.
And so there they lie, happily sweltering in the summer sun on Australia's most famous
beach, just as they have for so many generations past.
Ah, but those 35,000 Sydneysiders who were lying in those very spots on the afternoon of
February 6, 1938, surely felt equally at peace.
The day was, in the vernacular of the time, a ‘stinker’,
At three o'clock there was still not the slightest clue that this afternoon would forever be
known as ‘Black Sunday’ in the annals of Sydney.
In their long and glorious history, this still stands as the finest hour of the Australian surf
lifesaving movement. For…
Even the connectives (And so…, Ah but…, For…) seem to flag a long term elegiac view,
particularly FitzSimons' sigh of resignation (Ah) as he moves from his description of
the image of the beach to Black Sunday 1935.
G N
I
kicker
Peter
FitzSimons
photo
sky
I
text
Bondi
Beach
photo
R
R beach
G N
Where have we been? In this chapter we've tried to build some bridges from the
grammar analyses developed in Chapters 2-6 towards the analysis of discourse. An
outline of these and related resources is presented in Table 7.9 below, which aligns
columns by metafunction and rows by levels of abstraction (stratification). IFG
14 FitzSimons is also the biographer of the federal opposition Labor leader Kim Beazley, and a writer of
popular history (focussing on WWII).
15 The notion of hold and release vectors is taken from Arnheim 1982.
294 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
provides rich tools for the analysis of clauses, groups and phrases across
metafunctions; and these analyses inspired Kress & van Leeuwen's tools for analysing
images, as touched on above. Turning our attention to phonology, the key resource is
Halliday & Greaves (2008), which deals with rhythm and intonation in spoken
discourse16.
Martin & Rose (2003) interface with grammar on the discourse semantic side,
and we've illustrated how to move from ideational, interpersonal and textual meaning
in grammar to the discourse structuring resources which they realise. Working with
Discourse in turn interfaces with models of social context, specifically the model of
register (field, tenor and mode) outlined in Martin (1992) and the work on genre
consolidated in Martin & Rose (2008). Each of the sources cited in the left hand
column of Table 7.9 includes extensive references to relevant work in functional
linguistics and related models of language in social life.
As we have tried to illustrate above, analysing a text involves shunting around
Table 9, doing what we can to bring out as many semantic motifs as we can. Basically
we have to be selective and do what time allows, and fine-tune our gaze in terms of
what we are doing the analysis for. Working through FitzSimons' text for this chapter,
for example, is a different goal from analysing it as a piece of media discourse or
critiquing the way it and comparable monthly features by FitzSimons' naturalise
European or Asian Australians’ relationship with their country at the expense of
indigenous belonging (Read 2000). The only generalised advice we feel confident to
give has to do with remembering to look right to textual meaning and look up to
higher levels of contextualisation, whatever analysis we are working on.
Looking up means moving beyond discourse semantics into a model of social
context – the register and genre rows in Table 7.9. Limitations of space preclude too
lengthy an excursion here. We'll make just a few comments on register, before turning
to genre.
Field focuses our attention on the institutional context of a text – including
domestic, work and leisure activities. A field can be defined as a set of activity
sequences defining participation in a specific walk of life – for example doing the
rounds, writing stories, attending staff meetings, reading newspapers, viewing current
affairs programs on TV etc. as a print media journalist. Each step in each of these
activity sequences consists of configurations of participants, processes and
circumstances. And each participating entity, human or non-human, abstract or
concrete, is organised within each field into distinctive taxonomies – of both
subclassification ('is a kind of') and composition ('is a part of').
As we have illustrated above, analysing clause complexes (grammar) in tandem
with conjunctive relations (discourse semantics) is a good way of exploring activity
sequences. To analyse each step in these sequences more closely we need to look at
transitivity (from both transitive and ergative perspectives). And to understand
participant relations we need to look at nominal group structure (Epithet, Classifier,
Thing in particular), relational clauses (identifying and attributive), and relationships
of hyponymy (class/subclass) and meronymy (part/whole) between nominal groups.
We looked at FitzSimons' field in all these ways, in order to bring more fully to
consciousness the senses in which he was concerned with the leisure activity of going
to the beach in relation to the semi-professional activities of the Australian surf
lifesaving movement.
16 Halliday 1985 has some useful discussion of the evolution of the English writing system (graphology).
CHAPTER 7 TEXT ANALYSIS 295
Table 7.9 Key resources for text analysis (by strata and metafunction)
Tenor is concerned with the kind of relationship FitzSimons constructs with his
readers. Its key dimensions are power and solidarity. Power is concerned with
hierarchical social relations, solidarity with horizontal ones. As far as power is
concerned, FitzSimons' position is an authoritative one – he writes for us to read and
we read what he writes. The only real say we have is a yet to be realised one, solicited
in his invitation to e-mail him with suggestions for historical anecdotes he might use.
As we have seen, FitzSimons' authority is enhanced inter-modally – verbally through
his blue coloured by-line in the text's macro-Theme and imagically through his
Ideal/New position above the image of the beach.
Solidarity is also inter-modally construed – imagically, mateship is emphasised, as
he faces us directly, in his casual dress, looking us in the eye with a smile. Verbally the
bonds have more to do with regional pride in the accomplishments of the Bondi Boys;
graduation dramatises events, positioning us to appreciate all the more the historical
significance of the rescue. FitzSimons' elegiac tones confirm us in the heritage he
shares.
Mode deals with the role language is playing in relation to other modalities of
communication and the concreteness of its rendering of the field. As we have seen, the
text is an inter-modal one, with explicit exophoric connections between verbiage and
image early on. In addition, there are many experiential connections between the
beachside lexis of the verbiage and the image of people on the beach. Blue colour
cohesion also ties the graphology explicitly to the ambience of the image.
As far as abstraction is concerned, the text shifts back and forth from more
abstract to more concrete, partly in relation to periodicity. The higher level Themes
and New reviewed above draw on nominalisation and text reference to preview and
296 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
consolidate events (a strength, the slightest clue17, utter chaos; this, it, that, this):
[macro-Theme] Big waves and Bondi Beach have always gone together, writes Peter
FitzSimons, but no one had ever seen the ocean rise up with a strength such as this …
[hyper-Theme] At three o'clock there was still not the slightest clue [[that this afternoon
would forever be known as ‘Black Sunday’ in the annals of Sydney]]. Then it happened.
[hyper-New] In no more than 20 seconds, that peaceful scene had been tragically
transformed into utter chaos.
[hyper-Theme] In their long and glorious history, this still stands as the finest hour of the
Australian surf lifesaving movement.
And the marked Themes flagging the waves' attack and the rescue involve key
nominalisations (roar, peril).
With a roar like [[a Bondi tram running amok]]
<<ignoring their own possible peril>>
Once the incursion and rescue get underway, however, events unfold
chronologically with text time matching field time (and thus temporal connections
realised between clauses); and figures by and large 18 are realised congruently (with
events as verbs, qualities as adjectives, entities as nouns).
The other factor interacting with mode is genre staging, since one of the story's
early stages is more abstract than the rest of the text. To explore this, we'll have to
move from register (field, tenor and mode) to genre. Martin & Rose (2008) treat genre
as a recurrent configuration of meanings, which phases field, mode and tenor
variables together into stages which enact its social purpose. As far as the family of
story genres is concerned, several stages of FitzSimons' text unfold as a canonical
narrative (along the lines outlined by Labov & Waletzky 1967). There is an Orientation
stage, introducing the story's key participants, setting them in time and place, and
foreshadowing what's to come.
Orientation
Ah, but those 35,000 Sydneysiders who were lying in those very spots on the afternoon of
February 6, 1938, surely felt equally at peace. The day was, in the vernacular of the time, a
‘stinker’, and some thought it was in fact a record turnout on the beach, with the numbers
perhaps swelled by the fact that those bronzed boys of the Bondi Surf Bathers' Life Saving
Club had turned up in force to have one of their popular surf competitions.
This stage is the most lexically dense phase of the story (cf. Then it happened. in
the next stage). It features two embedded clauses, the second of which acts agentively
on the numbers on the beach (in active voice – the fact that [[...]] swelled the numbers
on the beach).
[[who were lying in those very spots on the afternoon of February 6, 1938]]
[[that those bronzed boys of the Bondi Surf Bathers' Life Saving Club had turned up in
force to have one of their popular surf competitions.]]
Some less abstract reworking of several phrases are listed below by way of
17 Taken as grammatically metaphorical here, in relation to concrete mental processing along the lines of
No-one predicted at all.
18 The exceptions to this are the colloquial Process + Scope structure, made a mad dash, which affords
appropriate attitude and graduation; the process, which consolidates the incursion; and their own
strength, which facilitates the comparison of lifesavers attached to reels and those on their own.
CHAPTER 7 TEXT ANALYSIS 297
19 This sentence is taken as part of the Complication here, in line with its treatment as hyper-Theme above;
it is however arguably transitional between Orientation (was) and Complication (would be).
298 DEPLOYING FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR
the images which make this text a multimodal one. But FitzSimons has more than a
short mediating caption linking the photograph to his text. He further recontextualises
his story by devoting an initial phase to the photo of Bondi Beach. This explicitly
intermodal phase is an important one for FitzSimons' 'placeintime' column, which is
designed to draw readers' attention to the historical significance of more and less
familiar Sydney landmarks (e.g. The Harbour Bridge, The GPO Clock Tower, Canada
Bay, Coogee Beach, Botany Bay in other issues). It recurs often enough in these
columns to warrant reification as a generic stage, which we'll refer to as Localisation
here.
Localisation
And so there they lie, happily sweltering in the summer sun on Australia's most famous
beach, just as they have for so many generations past. It is such a wonderfully peaceful
scene – of people and nature as a happy whole – that it is simply unimaginable that in a
few seconds nature could ever rear up and savage the lot of them.
FitzSimons' final phase further recontextualises the narrative with the interactive
Thanks and Solicitation stages which construct FitzSimons' readers as participants in
his authoring process.
Thanks
With thanks to Waverly Library and reader Diane Touzell, ||
whose grandfather, Bill Jenkins, was one of the lifesavers. |||
Solicitation
Do you have a historical anecdote about a place in Sydney? |||
Write to Peter FitzSimons at [email protected] |||
Taken together, the Kicker, Localisation, Thanks and Solicitation underscore the way in
which well established genres can be adapted to new environments. In this case, the
recontextualisation of narrative to fit into FitzSimons' ‘placeintime’ monthly feature
column in a Sydney lifestyle magazine.
Clearly there is more to say about a story of this kind. Our tour of the levels
outlined in Table 9 is far from complete. Our main point here is to give some indication
of the theoretical map within which Halliday's grammar of English has been proposed.
As far as text analysis is concerned, every cell in the Table affords a glimpse of what is
going on; and we have to take care in privileging one cell over another – for reasons of
time, or interest, or familiarity with analysis of one kind or another. Halliday has given
us a far richer grammar than any other grammarian of ours or any other time, and
thus a sound foundation, and a resounding mandate, to push on.
References
Gee, J P 2005 An Introduction to Discourse Analysis: theory and method. 2nd edition.
London: Routledge.
Hage, G 2003 Against Paranoid Nationalism: searching for hope in a shrinking world.
Sydney: Pluto Press.
Halliday, M A K 1985 Spoken and Written Language. Geelong: Deakin University Press.
[Reprinted by Oxford University Press 1989.]
Halliday, M A K 1994 Introduction to Functional Grammar. 2nd edition. London: Arnold
Halliday, M A K 2004 Introduction to Functional Grammar. 3rd edition. Revised by C M I
M Matthiessen. London: Arnold.
Halliday, M A K & W Greaves 2008 Intonation in the Grammar of English. London:
Equinox.
Halliday, M A K & E McDonald 2004 Metafunctional profile of the grammar of Chinese.
In Caffarel, Martin & Matthiessen (eds). 305-396.
Halliday, M A K & C M I M Matthiessen 1999 Construing Experience through Language:
a language-based approach to cognition. London: Cassell.
Halliday, M A K & C M I M Matthiessen 2004 An Introduction to Functional Grammar.
3rd edition. London: Arnold.
Hasan, R 1984 The nursery tale as a genre. Nottingham Linguistic Circular 13 (Special
Issue on Systemic Linguistics). 71-102.
Hasan, R, C M I M Matthiessen & J J Webster (eds) 2005 Continuing Discourse on
Language: a functional perspective. Vol. 1. London: Equinox.
Hasan, R, C M I M Matthiessen & J J Webster (eds) 2007 Continuing Discourse on
Language: a functional perspective. Vol. 2. London: Equinox.
Hasan, R & G Williams (eds) 1996 Literacy in Society. London: Longman (Language
and Social Life).
Hood, S & J R Martin 2005 Invoking attitude: the play of graduation in appraising
discourse. R Hasan, C M I M Matthiessen & J Webster (eds) Continuing Discourse on
Language. London: Equinox. [Spanish translation 'Invocación de actitudes: El
juego de la gradación de la valoración en el discurso.' Revista Signos 2005 38.58.
195-220.]
Jordens, C & M Little 2004 ‘In this scenario, I do this, for these reasons’: narrative,
genre and ethical reasoning in the clinic. Social Science & Medicine 58. 1635-1645.
Kealley, D J 2007 ‘I can’t find a pulse but that’s OK’: Nursing in Context: A Systemic
Functional Linguistic Examination of Nursing Practice. Unpublished PhD
dissertation. University of South Australia.
Kress, G & T van Leeuwen 2006 Reading Images 2nd edition. London & New York:
Routledge.
Labov, W and J Waletzky 1967 Narrative analysis: oral versions of personal experience.
In J Helm (ed) Essays on the Verbal and Visual Arts (Proceedings of the 1966 Spring
Meeting of the American Ethnological Society). Seattle: University of Washington
Press, 12-44. [Republished in Bamberg. 3-38.]
Li, E 2007 Systemic functional grammar of Chinese: a text-based analysis. London &
New York: Continuum.
Martin, J R 1986 Grammaticalising ecology: the politics of baby seals and kangaroos. T
REFERENCES 301