Cognitive Benefits of Language Learning
Cognitive Benefits of Language Learning
Selected Articles
Cognitive Benefits of Bilingualism/Second Language Learning
▪ Foster, K. M., & Reeves, C. K. (1989). Foreign Language in the Elementary School
(FLES) improves cognitive skills. FLES News, 2(3), 4.
This study looks at the effects of an elementary school foreign language program on
basic skills by looking at the relationship between months of elementary foreign
language instruction in French and scores on instruments designed to measure
cognitive and metacognitive processes. The study included 67 sixth-grade students
who were divided into four groups that differed by lengths of time in the foreign
language program. There was a control group of 25 students who had no French
instruction and three groups of students who had participated in the program for
different lengths of time (6.5 months, 15.5 months, and 24.5 months). The students
who did receive foreign language instruction had received 30 minutes of French
instruction daily after 30 minutes of basal reading in English. The control group
received an additional 30 minutes of reading instruction in place of foreign language
instruction. The results of the analysis showed that the groups who received foreign
language instruction scored significantly higher in three areas (evaluation on the
Ross test, total score of all cognitive functions on Ross test, and total score on
Butterfly and Moths test) than the control group. In particular, the students who
had received foreign language instruction scored higher on tasks involving
evaluation which is the highest cognitive skill according to Bloom's
taxonomy. The linear trend analysis showed that the students who had
studied French the longest performed the best.
The main hypothesis of this study is that the experience of learning a second
language at the elementary school level is positively correlated to divergent thinking
in figural tasks. This study is concerned with flexibility in thinking through experience
with a foreign language. Comparisons are made between second language learners
and single language learners. The second language learners score significantly
higher than do the monolingual students. Second language learning
appears, therefore, not only to provide children with the ability to depart
from the traditional approaches to a problem, but also to supply them with
possible rich resources for new and different ideas.
It was the primary purpose of this study to investigate the effects of bilingualism on
the cognitive development and linguistic performance of children at various ages
living in the same cultural environment. It also studied the relationship between
formal operational thought and a prerequisite cognitive style as typified by field
independence/field dependence for both bilingual and monolingual subjects. The
bilingual subjects were tested for both language dominance and language
proficiency. To investigate the interrelationships between bilingualism and cognitive
function, it was necessary to include both verbal and non-verbal tests of cognition.
No significant differences in performance could be attributed to lingualism, grade, or
age with the exception of language proficiency correlated with cognitive level on
analytical reasoning. The childrens' overall cognitive level indicated some justification
for the theoretical relationship between verbal and non-verbal measures of abstract
thinking. The bilingual children used higher order rules more frequently than
the monolingual children. The evidence seems to suggest that bilingualism
may scaffold concept formation and general mental flexibility.
The fact that H.M. and other people with amnesia show Another important model for the study of memory is the
deficits in some types of memories and not others indicates that phenomenon of long-term potentiation (LTP), a long-lasting increase
the brain has multiple memory systems supported by distinct brain in the strength of a synaptic response following stimulation. LTP
regions. Nondeclarative knowledge, the knowledge of how to do occurs prominently in the hippocampus, as well as in the cerebral
something, is expressed in skilled behavior and learned habits and cortex and other brain areas involved in various forms of memory.
requires processing by the basal ganglia and cerebellum. The cer- LTP occurs through changes in the strength of synapses at contacts
ebellum is specifically involved in motor tasks that are time-depen- involving N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) receptors.
dent. The amygdala appears to play an important role in emotional Subsequently, a series of molecular reactions plays a vital
aspects of memory attaching emotional significance to otherwise role in stabilizing the changes in synaptic function that occur in
neutral stimuli and events. The expression of emotional memories LTP. These molecular events begin with the entry of calcium ions
involves the hypothalamus and sympathetic nervous system, which into the synapse, which activates the cyclic adenosine monophos-
support emotional reactions and feelings. Thus, the brain appears to phate (cAMP) molecule. This molecule activates several kinds of
process different kinds of information in separate ways. enzymes, some of which increase the number of synaptic receptors,
How exactly are memories stored in brain cells? After years making the synapse more sensitive to neurotransmitters. In addi-
of study, much evidence supports the idea that memory involves a tion, cAMP activates another molecule, called cAMP-response ele-
persistent change in synapses, the connections between neurons. In ment binding protein (CREB). CREB operates within the nucleus
animal studies, researchers found that this occurs in the short term of the neuron to activate a series of genes, many of which direct
through biochemical events that affect the strength of the relevant protein synthesis. Among the proteins produced are neurotrophins,
synapses. Turning on certain genes may lead to modifications within which activate growth of the synapse and increase the neuron’s
neurons that change the strength and number of synapses, stabilizing responsiveness to stimulation.
new memories. Researchers studying the sea slug Aplysia californica, for Many studies have shown that the molecular cascade lead-
example, can correlate specific chemical and structural changes in rel ing to protein synthesis is not essential to initial learning or to
evant cells with several simple forms of memory that the animal shows. maintaining short-term memory; however, this cascade is essential
NEW YORK, July 09 (Reuters) -- Researchers have discovered that the age at
which a second language is acquired determines just where that language is
'stored' within the brain.
"A second language acquired during the teenage years, which is late in
developmental life, is represented in the brain in a separate location from the
native language," says Dr. Joy Hirsch, a professor of neuroscience at New York's
Memorial Sloan-Kettering Cancer Center, and senior author of a study published in
the current issue of the journal Nature. "But when both languages are learned at
the same time early in life, they are represented in areas that have a considerable
amount of overlap."
Six of the subjects had learned two languages in early childhood, while the other
six had picked up a second language during their teenage years.
Each subject was placed within the fMRI scanner, and asked to engage in a silent,
internal monologue about the previous day's events, conducted in each of their
acquired languages. The researchers asked the subjects not to speak aloud, since
this can cause small head movements which obscure fMRI readings.
Looking at the fMRI pictures, the study authors discovered that "in both late and
early bilingual subjects, Wernicke's area... show(s) effectively little or no separation
of activity based on the age of language acquisition."
This meant that when a particular study subject thought in his mother tongue (for
example, English), the fMRI scan registered activity in a particular spot within
Broca's area. When asked to think in the French he acquired in high school, a
separate area stirred into action.
However, for those who became bilingual as toddlers, "native and second
languages tend to be represented in common... areas," of the Broca's area,
researchers say.
It's still not clear just why child and adult language acquisition take such divergent
neurological paths. But Hirsch and her colleagues speculate that infants, with
relatively 'plastic', still-developing brains, may easily adapt to two incoming
language patterns, incorporating both into the same storage space within Broca's
area. But after the passage of early childhood, that area may become less
malleable and, as the researchers say, "not subsequently modified." They believe
"this could necessitate the utilization of adjacent... areas for the second language
learned as an adult."
Hirsch believes her research could have a real benefit for Sloan-Kettering's cancer
patients. In fact, she initiated her research with those individuals in mind. Twenty-
five percent of all brain tumors occur in and around speech-sensitive areas of the
brain. Surgeons must be extremely careful in excising tumors, to try to minimize
damage to the patient's language capability.
"Based on the results of the current study, we always ask our patients whether they
speak more than one language," said Hirsch's colleague and study lead author Dr.
Karl Kim. "If they do, both languages need to be mapped to acquire a complete
picture of language-sensitive areas of the brain."
1) Check out books and audiotapes at your local public library or invest in books/audiotapes that are
written in Spanish. (Books are available at Barnes & Noble, through Scholastic and Troll book orders,
book fairs at school, and other sources.)
2) Designate a time for daily family reading and/or silent reading. Encourage your child to read
something in Spanish as well as English.
3) Invite a classmate/schoolmate over to have a special "Spanish evening" or "Spanish night over."
4) Let your child call a student from the immersion school to speak Spanish on the phone.
5) Host a teacher's assistant (T.A.) from Mexico or be an "Aunt and Uncle" family and invite a T.A.
over for dinner or a party, or include a T.A. in a family outing.
6) Give your child the necessary supplies to write letters to Spanish-speaking families and/or friends
from school. Get on the "super highway" by using the Internet to speak with others who can
communicate in Spanish - parent supervision is advised.
7) Use your child's Spanish writing skills to write to the embassies or offices of tourism of Spanish-
speaking countries. Request information, pamphlets, posters, postcards, booklets about the country.
Use the information to make your own travel guide, mural, poster, etc.
8) Visit a Latino market in your community. Cheer your child on as s/he helps you purchase your food
in Spanish.
9) Talk about the reasons why you want your child to learn another language and ask your child why
s/he thinks it is important to know a second language.
10) Recognize, praise and encourage any efforts your child makes to speak Spanish. Often, just the
vocalization of our pride in his or her efforts can make the difference.
11) Review the songs and poems that your child sings and recites at school. Your child can sing the
song or recite the poem alone by memory, then ask him or her to teach you a song or poem. Let your
child be the singing entertainment on your next road trip!
12) Purchase children's videos, music and software programs in Spanish. Call the company "Niños" at
1-800-634-3304 for a catalog. Blockbuster video stores will order Disney videos in Spanish for
purchase upon request.
Center for Advanced Research on Language Acquisition (CARLA) • 140 University International Center • 331 17th Ave SE • Minneapolis, MN