Child Language Acquisition Lecture Notes
Child Language Acquisition Lecture Notes
Common one is the rising intonation at end of utterance, as they are used to the
caregiver asking them questions. Even for children who can’t understand or appreciate
them
Other variations in rhythm/emphasis may suggest greeting or calling
Gesture
Although they do not yet have the power of speech, desire to communicate indicated
through gesture
Example: point to object and use facial expression ‘What’s that?’
Beginnings of pragmatic development – regulating the behaviour of others and getting
what you really want.
Understanding
Although the child may not begin to speak, they may understand meanings of certain
words.
Understanding precedes production - Piaget
Word recognition: is usually evident by the end of the first year.
Commonly understood: names, ‘no’ and ‘bye-bye’
The First Word
They say it at approximately 1 years old
They then learn 10 new words each day
First recognisable word – has to be used consistently and phonologically recognisable
New Vocabulary
Acclimatised
Instinctive
Cooing
Babbling
Bilabial
Reduplicated monosyllable
Phonemic expansion
Phonemic contraction
Network Building – Learning the connection between that word and other words,
e.g. saying one item is little but the other one is even smaller, using words
comparatively. Synonyms and antonyms, echoing someone else by giving another
example.
New Vocabulary:
GRAMMATICAL FUNCTION WORDS
UNDEREXTENSION
OVEREXTENSION
LABELLING
PACKAGING
NETWORK BUILDING
Lecture 4: Grammatical
Development 1
Syntactic development: development of a child’s ability to create grammatical constructions
by arranging words in an appropriate order.
One word (holophrastic) stage:
12-18 months: child speaks in single word utterances.
‘milk’
‘mummy’
Groups of words may be used as a single unit.
‘allgone’
In many situations, the words simply serve a naming function. – Labelling, Aitichison
Holophrases: single words which convey more complex messages than just a labelling
function.
Depends on delivery and the context
‘juice’
‘I want some juice’
‘I’ve spilt some juice’
Context, gesture (paralinguistic features) and intonation (prosodic) : enable
parent/carer to understand what child means.
Understanding:
Although the child’s utterances are limited, their understanding of syntax is (predictably)
more advanced.
Evidence: children at the one-word stage can respond to two-word instructions: ‘kiss
mummy’.
Two-word stage:
18 months: two-word utterances begin to appear.
Usually: grammatically correct sequence. – This could have prompted Skinner’s
imitation theory. Learnt behaviour.
Common constructions:
S+V ‘Daddy sleep’
V+O ‘Draw birdie’
S+O ‘Suzy juice’
S+C ‘Daddy busy’
Cognitive development
When repeating an adult, children at this stage commonly omit elements, but retain the
correct order:
Look, Ben’s playing in the garden.
Play garden.
Ambiguity
The scope for ambiguity at this stage arises because of the omission of inflectional
affixes.
Commonly possessive and plural ‘s’ and past tense ‘ed’ are absent.
Telegraphic Stage
Age 2: 3 and 4-word utterances begin to be produced.
Some will be grammatically complete:
S+V+O
‘Lucy likes tea’
S+V+C
‘Teddy is tired’
S+V+A
‘Mummy sleeps upstairs’
Predictable S V pattern/ standard syntax
Other utterances will have grammatical elements missing:
‘Daddy home now’ – main verb ‘is’ missing
‘Where Josh going?’ – Auxiliary verb ‘is’ missing, if they do put the auxiliary is present it
involves syntactic inversion (where josh is going?)
Interrogative formation
Hence the name ‘telegraphic’.
Like a telegram, they include key words, but omit elements such as:
Determiners
Auxiliary verbs
Prepositions
Wider range of structures will be used:
Questions (interrogatives)
Commands (imperatives)
Simple statements
Rapid Progress:
3 years: items such as determiners begin to be used regularly.
More than one clause appears
Coordinating conjunctions
Inflectional affixes (see next lecture)
5 years: many of most basic grammatical rules have been learned, though some (e.g. the
passive) have yet to be mastered.
Vocabulary
HOLOPHRASE
TELEGRAPHIC
COORDINATING CONJUNCTIONS
INFLECTIONAL AFFIXES
PASSIVE
Lecture 5: Grammatical
Development 2
Acquisition of inflections:
Predictable patterns – revealed by research in the acquisition of inflections
Grammatical function words – also seem to be acquired in a predictable order
Brown (1973):
Study: 20 – 36 month olds exhibited the sequence shown below:
-ing
plural –s
possessive –s
articles - ‘the’, ‘a’
past tense –ed
third person singular verb ending –s
auxiliary ‘be’
Cruttenden (1979)
1. Memorise words individually. No regard for rules
2. Awareness of general principles, governing inflections
3. Overgeneralisation
4. Correct inflections are used, including irregular forms
Understanding of grammatical rules
Researchers: how do children produce grammatically accurate constructions so early in
their development?
Rules or Imitation
Berko (1958)
Supports Chomsky
Showed a picture of a made up creature – wug
‘Wug’
‘This is a Wug’
‘Now there is another one; there are two of them’
Complete the sentence: ‘There are two …’
3-4 years old: ‘wugs’
Grammatical rule for plural ‘s’ was clearly being applied.
With brand new words they use rules they already know independently
Overgeneralisation
2 ½ - 5 years: grammatical errors show an awareness of rules. (Chomsky)
They ‘overgeneralise/over regularise’, trying to make the language more consistent than
it is:
‘sheeps’
‘wented’
‘mouses’
Although children apply grammatical rules in this way, they are not conscious that they
have acquired them and would not be able to explain them = NO METALINGUISTIC
AWARENESS
Interrogatives
Asking questions involves complex constructions.
Research: suggests they are three stages involved in acquiring this skill
Two-word stage: questions rely on rising intonation only.
Second year: question words acquired: first ‘what’ and ‘where’, then ‘why’, ‘who’ and
‘how’= ‘Where daddy gone?’
Third year: begin to use auxiliary verbs and inversion
Therefore: ‘Joe is here’ becomes ‘Is Joe here?’
However: questions involving –wh words are not always correctly inverted: ‘Why Joe
isn’t here?’
Negation
It also appears that the accurate expression of negative (stereotypically characterised by
the ‘terrible twos’) occurs in three stages …
Single dependence on the words ‘no’ and ‘not’ used independently or in front of
expressions: ‘no want’ and ‘no go bed’.
Third year: ‘don’t’ and ‘can’t’ appear. Begin to appear after the subject and before the
verb of the sentence:
‘I don’t want it’ and ‘Sammy can’t play’
More negative forms are acquired: ‘didn’t’ and ‘isn’t’. Negative constructions are not
generally more accurate.
Vocabulary:
OVERGENERALISATION
IRREGULAR FORMS
METALINGUISTIC AWARENESS
Lecture 6 Pragmatic
Development
Pragmatics = the part played by language in social situations and relationships.
Del Hymes: ‘communicative competence’
When to speak
How to respond to others
Appropriate registers
Language Functions:
Bottom line: children are motivated to acquire language because it serves certain
purposes/functions for them.
Halliday’ Taxonomy (1975): seven functions (It Really Is Pants Here In Rotheram)
1. INSTRUMENTAL – where the child is trying to fulfil a need (e.g. asking for food or
drink) ‘Need juice now’
2. REGULATORY – Used to control the behaviour of someone (e.g. telling a caregiver
where to sit) ‘Don’t do that’
3. INTERACTIONAL – Used to develop relationships with others (e.g. telling a sibling
you love them, saying hello (salutation) or goodbye (valediction), or you miss them)
‘Love you Ruthie’
4. PERSONAL – Used to express views and preferences (e.g. me no like it) ‘Me like
sausages Mummy’
5. HEURISTIC – Inquisitive function. Used to explore the world around them (e.g. what
are you doing mummy?) ‘What Daddy doing there?’
6. IMAGINATIVE – Used to explore something creatively or during play, role playing or
making their toys talk ‘Then teddy said he wants to go shopping’
7. REPRESENTATIONAL – Used to exchange information – to give or receive
information. They recognise that they know information that other people don’t
know. Announcing stage ‘Got new shoes Granny’
As with other elements of language acquisition, remember that pragmatic development
begins before a child starts to speak.
Early Years
Children are introduced to important role of language in everyday life.
Adults speak to them a great deal.
Routine events are accompanied by regularly repeated utterances.
Conversation Preparation
Later Development:
Phonology
Separate phrases more distinctly (longer pauses).
Speak more s-l-o-w-l-y.
Exaggerated ‘singsong’ intonation.
Exaggerated difference between questions, statements and commands.
Higher and wider range of pitch.
Lexis and semantics:
Use of concrete nouns (e.g. train, cat) and dynamic verbs (e.g. give, put).
Adopt child’s own words for things (e.g. wickle babbit).
Frequent use of child’s name and absence of pronouns
Grammar:
Repeated sentence frames: ‘That’s a …’
More simple sentences.
Fewer complex sentence and passives.
Omission of past tense and inflections.
More command, questions and tag questions.
Use of EXPANSIONS: where the adult ‘fills out’ the child’s utterance.
Use of RECASTINGS: where the child’s vocabulary is put into a new utterance.
Pragmatics:
Lots of gesture and body language.
Stopping frequently for child to respond.
Supportive language.
Clarke-Stewart (1973) - Children whose mothers talk to them more have larger
vocabularies.
Nelson (1973) - Holophrastic stage. Children whose mothers corrected them on word choice
and pronunciation actually advanced more slowly than those with mothers who were
generally accepting.
Kuhl (1992) - Studied exaggerated vowel sounds used by parents when speaking to 6-month
olds (in English, Swedish and Russian). Babies turn towards adults who speak in sing-song
voice, ignoring regular conversation. Mothers in all three countries exaggerated the
important vowels.
NOT ALL CULTURES USE CHILD-DIRECTED SPEECH.
In some (non-western) cultures babies are expected to ‘blend in’ with adult
interaction and no special accommodation is made. Papua New Guinea
These children still go through same developmental stages at roughly the same time,
as long as there is EXPOSURE to language.