Meyer 2000 Four Functions Humor Communication
Meyer 2000 Four Functions Humor Communication
Theory
Ten:
~~ Three
John C. Meyer August
2000
Panes
3 10-33 1
Humor as a Double-Edged Sword: Four
Functions of Humor in Communication
310
Humor as Dou ble-Edged
sion show alone than during the same show with a group all laughing
hilariously. Persons who are perceived to appreciate humor readily are
generally more popular with others (Wanzer, Booth-Butterfield, & Booth-
Butterfield, 1996). Such social properties make humor a natural focus
for communication study. Central to all communication is the audience-
those to whom a message may be directed. The audience gives attempts
at humor their success or failure. This receiver-centered nature of hu-
311
Communication
Theory
serving. Humor has been claimed to emerge in three basic ways in hu-
man thought: through perceptions of relief, incongruity, and superiority
(Berger, 1993; Raskin, 1985).
312
Humor as Double-Edged
Incongruity
From the perspective of the incongruity theory, people laugh at what
surprises them, is unexpected, or is odd in a nonthreatening way (Berger,
1976; Deckers & Divine, 1981; McGhee, 1979). An accepted pattern is
violated, or a difference is noted-close enough to the norm to be non-
threatening, but different enough from the norm to be remarkable. It is
this difference, neither too shocking nor too mundane, that provokes
313
Communication
Theory
314
Humor as Double-Edged
315
Communication
Theory
316
Humor as Double-Edged
317
Communication
Theory
Table 2. Humor function Target person’s position on issue Target’s familiarity with issue
Degrees of
Agreement
on Position
and Identification ++ ++
+ +
- +
Key Humor’
Function Differentiation ++
318
H u m o r as Double-Edged
319
Communication
Theory
gesting that those presumably already having low self-esteem should use
a more discreet entrance than “normal.” Many similar examples exist,
including an announcement that “the Rev. Merriwether spoke briefly,
much to the delight of the audience,” and another account that revealed
that “during the absence of our Pastor, we enjoyed the rare privilege of
hearing a good sermon when J. F. Stubbs preached last Sunday.” The
social norm in such messages is to praise the minister or show apprecia-
320
Humor as Double-Edged
32 1
Communication
Theory
leaders may use humor to distinguish their own group from others.
Goldstein (1976) noted that such use of humor can help speakers tran-
scend the immediate situation and objectify it, promoting the use of
reason and thereby making these differences clearer and less colored by
previous experience and emotion. One can criticize with humor by ridi-
culing the opposition through laughter rather than through indignation,
anger, or violence (Volpe, 1977). Comic ridicule can also maintain iden-
For the government cannot direct the people, the people must direct the government. This
is not the outlook of my opponent, and he is my opponent, not my enemy. Though he has
tried to be a good Republican, there are certain distinctions between the two great parties
that will be debated, and must be debated in the next 82 days. (Dole, 1996, p. 679)
By pointing out in a humorous way that his opponent was trying to act
like a Republican, but was not really one, Dole (1996)differentiated his
opponent (and supporters) from his own supporters. Of course, such
differentiation can also serve as identification for one in agreement with
Dole on the issue, causing the rhetorical identification through differen-
tiation noted by Kenneth Burke (1984).
This is the harshest function of humor in rhetoric, as often no quarter
is given to the opposing group. The audience is very familiar with the
subject, but is in complete disagreement with the humor’s target. In-
deed, harsh comments about disliked groups are often perceived in them-
selves as humorous. One type of differentiation humor that is pervasive
in our society attacks attorneys. Several collections of lawyer jokes make
the rounds, including comments such as this: Q: Why don’t snakes bite
attorneys? A: Professional courtesy. Even more cruel is the one that asks:
How can you tell that an attorney is about to lie? A: His lips begin to
move. Both of the above are as insulting and venomous as they are hu-
morous-a superiority theorist’s touchstone. Another dart at the legal
system noted that “a jury is a collection of people banded together to
decide who hired the better lawyer.” Here was a shot at those who were
swayed by “good lawyers” instead of by the socially desired norms of
truth and justice. Finally, one asked: What do lawyers use for birth con-
trol? A: Their personalities. All of these attack and differentiate an op-
posing or disliked group, and it is the creative attacks on that group that
spark the humor. This function of humor is clearly effective at dividing
one group (those who communicate and appreciate the humor) from
322
Humor as Double-Edged
I publicly declared that this is a depression and the President before the day was out
went to the press to say, “That shows how little he knows. This is a recession.” If the
323
Communication
Theory
President wants a definition, I’ll give him one. Recession is when your neighbor loses his
job, depression is when you lose yours, and [here he paused as the laughs began] recov-
ery will be when Jimmy Carter loses his. (Boller, 1981, p. 354)
Just before President Carter’s helicopter landed in Justin, Texas, about forty miles north-
east of the drought-stricken Dallas-Fort Worth area in July 1980, there was a sudden
rainfall lasting for about ten minutes. Carter stepped onto slippery clay soil that only an
hour earlier had been hard and rough. “Well,” he smiled at the farmers who had gath-
ered to greet him, “you asked for either money or rain. I couldn’t get the money so I
brought the rain.” (Boller, 1981, p. 346)
324
Humor as Double-Edged
reality or truth; the latter, “humor,” which was artistic, creative, and
often concerned with fantasy. Years earlier, Dahlberg (1945) had char-
acterized a wit as one who laughed at you, whereas a humorist laughed
with you. Gruner’s distinction seems to be a case of using more broad
terms for the humor duality explored above: Wit referred to the differ-
entiation or enforcement functions, whereas humor for him referred to
the relaxing identification or clarification functions of humor in com-
325
Communication
Theory
form into the audience, he let it be known that he had simply decided to
try break dancing. This reassured his audience that, although it was
unusual for a candidate to fall off the platform, things were really all
right and he was still in control. The norm (doing crazy things in the
course of campaigning) was stressed in the humor over and above the
violation (being so clumsy or unsteady that he fell off a platform).
Humor also can divide through the enforcement and differentiation
326
Humor as Double-Edged
Question: Mr. President, have you ever considered contributing to a sperm bank?
Clinton’s answer: No, I already gave at the office.
These jokes, which make verbal jabs at their target, separate audiences
who laugh at the violations of social norms illustrated in them from the
327
Communication
Theory
328
Humor as Double-Edged
John C. Meyer (PhD, University of Kansas, 1991) is associate professor of speech communication Author
at the University of Southern Mississippi, Hattiesburg. The author would like to thank students in
his Humor in Communication seminars, as well as the editor of Communication Theory and two
reviewers for their contributions to this essay. An earlier version of this essay was presented at the
National Communication Association annual convention, New York, NY, November 1998.
Apte, M. (1985).Humor and laughter: An anthropological approach. Ithaca, NY: Cornell Univer- References
sity Press.
Alberts, J. (1990).The use of humor in managing couples’ conflict interactions. In D. Cahn (Ed.),
Intimates in conflict (pp. 110-120). Hillside, NJ: Erlbaum.
Alberts, J. K., Kellar-Guenther, Y., & Corman, J. R. (1996).That’s not funny: Understanding re-
cipients’ responses to teasing. Westem Journal of Communication, 60, 337-357.
Berger, A. A. (1993).An anatomy ofhumor. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.
Berger, A. A. (1976).Anatomy of the joke. Journal of Communication, 26(3), 113-115.
Bergson, H. (1911).Laughter: An essay on the meaning ofthe comic. New York: MacMillan.
Berlyne, D. E. (1972).Humor and its kin. In J. H. Goldstein & P. E. McGhee (Eds.) The psychology
ofhumor (pp. 43-60). New York: Academic Press.
329
Communication
Theory
Bishop, B., & Brand, C. (1998). Mr. President. . . Everything you wanted to know from President
Bill Clinton but were afraid to ask. [self-published]
Boland, R. J., & Hoffman, R. (1983). Humor in a machine shop: An interpretation of symbolic
action. In 1.. R. Pondy, P. J. Frost, G. Morgan, & T. C. Dandridge (Eds.), Organizational sym-
bolism (pp. 187-198). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Boller, P. F. (1981).Presidential anecdotes. New York: Penguin Books.
Bormann, E. G. (1982).Fantasy and rhetorical vision: Ten years later. Quarterly journal ofspeech,
68,288-305.
Bormann, E. G. (1972). Fantasy and rhetorical vision: The rhetorical criticism of social reality.
330
Humor as Double-Edged
Gruner, C. R. (1965).Is wit to humor what rhetoric is to the poetic? Central States Speech/ou~nal,
16,17-22.
Hackman, M. Z., & Barthel-Hackman, T. A. (1993).Communication apprehension, willingness to
communicate, and sense of humor: United States and New Zealand perspectives. Communica-
tion Quarterly, 41,282-291.
Levasseur, D. G., & Dean, K. W. (1996).The Dole humor myth and the risks of recontextualizing
rhetoric. Southern Communication Journal, 62, 56-72.
Levanthal, H., & Cupchik, G . (1976).A process model of humor judgment.]ournal of Communi-
cation, 26(3), 190-205.
331