Thoughts About Research On Mathematical Problem - Solving Instruct
Thoughts About Research On Mathematical Problem - Solving Instruct
Volume 10 Article 12
Number 1 Numbers 1 & 2
1-2013
Recommended Citation
Lester, Frank K. Jr. (2013) "Thoughts About Research On Mathematical Problem- Solving Instruction," The
Mathematics Enthusiast: Vol. 10 : No. 1 , Article 12.
Available at: https://scholarworks.umt.edu/tme/vol10/iss1/12
This Article is brought to you for free and open access by ScholarWorks at University of Montana. It has been
accepted for inclusion in The Mathematics Enthusiast by an authorized editor of ScholarWorks at University of
Montana. For more information, please contact [email protected].
TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p.245
Abstract: In this article, the author, who has written extensively about mathematical
problem solving over the past 40 years, discusses some of his current thinking about the
nature of problem-solving and its relation to other forms of mathematical activity. He
also suggests several proficiencies teachers should acquire in order for them to be
successful in helping students become better problem solvers and presents a framework
for research on problem-solving instruction. He closes the article with a list of principles
about problem-solving instruction that have emerged since the early 1970s.
Introduction
began more than 40 years ago as I was beginning to think seriously about a topic for my
doctoral dissertation. Since that time, my interest in and enthusiasm for problem solving,
in particular problem-solving instruction, has not waned but some of my thinking about it
has changed considerably. In this article I share some of my current thinking about a
variety of ideas associated with this complex and elusive area of study, giving special
much elaboration on these ideas and careful readers may be put off by such a cursory
discussion. My hope is that some readers will be stimulated by my ideas to think a bit
solving abilities is a primary objective of instruction and how this goal is to be reached
involves consideration by the teacher of a wide range of factors and decisions. For
example, teachers must decide on the problems and problem-solving experiences to use,
when to give problem solving particular attention, how much guidance to give students,
and how to assess students’ progress. Furthermore, there is the issue of whether problem
solving is intended as the end result of instruction or the means through which
mathematical concepts, processes, and procedures are learned. Or, to put it another way,
should teachers adopt “teaching for problem solving, ”—an ends approach—or “teaching
1
via problem solving” —a means approach? (I say more about means and ends later in
this article.) In my view, the answer to this question is that both approaches have merit;
problem solving should be both an end result of learning mathematics and the means
through which mathematics is learned (DiMatteo & Lester, 2010; Stein, Boaler, & Silver,
research is needed that focuses on the factors that influence student learning.
sort has been undertaken and, as a result, the accumulation of knowledge has been very
slow. Moreover, the present intense interest in research on teachers’ knowledge and
1
It has become more common to refer to the “means” approach to teaching as teaching through
problem solving. In Schroeder and Lester (1989) we discuss three approaches to problem-solving
instruction: teaching about, for, and via problem solving. Teaching “via” problem solving is
essentially the same as teaching through problem solving. Today, teaching about problem
solving is not generally regarded as a legitimate instructional method, although I suspect that
some (many?) teachers and curriculum writers subscribe to this approach.
TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .247
proficiencies demands that future problem-solving research pay close attention to the
mathematical and pedagogical knowledge and proficiencies a teacher should possess (cf.,
Ball, Thames, & Phelps, 2008; Hill, Sleep, Lewis, & Ball, 2007; Moreira & David, 2008;
But before discussing problem-solving instruction, let me first say a few things
about mathematical problem solving. This short discussion will highlight how my
thinking has changed about the nature of problem solving and other forms of
mathematical activity.
instruction I have worried about during my career, several have endured over time. In
this section I make five claims related to these enduring issues and offer brief
Although there have been at least four distinct problem-solving research traditions
Psychometric/Component Analysis), they all agree that a problem is a task for which an
individual does not know (immediately) what to do to get an answer (cf., Frensch &
Funke, 1995; Holth, 2008). Some representative definitions illustrate this fundamental
agreement:
A problem arises when a living creature has a goal but does not know how this
1966, p. 225)
A person is confronted with a problem when he wants something and does not
know immediately what series of actions he can perform to get it. (Newell &
Whenever you have a goal which is blocked for whatever reason . . . you have a
These definitions have two common ingredients: there is a goal and the
individual (i.e., the problem solver) is not immediately able to attain the goal. Moreover,
researchers irrespective of tradition, view problem solving simply as what one does to
achieve the goal. Unfortunately, these definitions and descriptions, like most of those
that have been given of mathematical problem solving – including those I and other
mathematics educators have proposed – are unhelpful for thinking about how to teach
students to solve problems or to identify the proficiencies needed to teach for or via
each of which requires some knowledge and skill, and some of which are not routine.
And, although it is difficult to define problem solving, the following statement – which
Paul Kehle and I devised a few years ago– comes much closer to capturing what it
The advantage of this description of problem solving over the others lies in its
successful problem solver, an individual must have ample relevant experience in learning
the possible exception of intuition, each of these ingredients should be attended to any
program aimed at equipping prospective teachers with the proficiencies needed to teach
mathematics either for or via problem solving. I say more about the implications of this
description for the education of mathematics teachers later in this article. But first, let me
continue with a few more observations about the nature of problem solving.
Claim 2. We know very little about how to improve students’ metacognitive abilities.
So much has been written about metacognition and its place both in the teaching
and learning of mathematics that a few comments about this elusive construct seem
warranted. I remain convinced that metacognition is one of the driving forces behind
2
The research perspective on the role of representation in doing mathematics provided by Goldin
(2003) is particularly relevant to this discussion.
3
A reviewer pointed out to me that intuition is itself a very subtle notion and as such the
definition we propose is unhelpful. To be sure, intuition is a subtle idea, but I think it essential to
include it in any description of what problem solving entails because it serves to point out just
how subtle the act of problem solving can be and, consequently, how difficult it has been to make
progress in learning how to teach students to be better problem solvers.
Lester
successful problem solving (Garofalo & Lester, 1984), but we really know almost
be sure, it is essential that successful problem solvers be able to monitor and regulate
their cognitive behaviors. But, almost no research has been done that demonstrates that
students’ can be taught these behaviors. Within the mathematics education community
both Schoenfeld (1992) and I (Lester, Garofalo, & Kroll, 1989), among others, have
has identified the proficiencies teachers need to do this. Instead, we have offered
suggestions, with too little evidence to support them. So, any program designed to
so only after acknowledging that there is no conclusive research evidence to support any
Claim 3. Mathematics teachers needn’t be expert problem solvers; they must be serious
before they are to be considered proficient mathematics teachers. But, I think this is
asking too much of them! George Polya was an expert (and, hence, proficient) problem
solver as well as an expert teacher of mathematics, but to expect all teachers to be experts
is both unreasonable and unnecessary. After all, expert basketball coaches needn’t have
been expert basketball players and expert violin teachers needn’t have been
have a firm grasp of what successful problem solving involves, but care should be taken
TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .251
problem solvers.
A fourth observation is that the description of problem solving I have given above
blurs the distinction between problem solving and other types of mathematical activity—I
have more to say about this blurring later. The distinctions that led historically to the
isolation of mathematical problem solving as a research focus from other areas of study
and the subsequent distinctions that resulted from this isolation are due in part to strong
traditions of disciplinary boundaries (Lester & Kehle, 2003).4 This isolation led to
subsuming mathematical understanding under problem solving. But, the inverse makes
more sense; that is, to subsume problem solving (and problem posing) under
emphasis is placed on several other constructs that are important in being able to do
of inference — that too often are not considered when problem solving is isolated from
Claim 5. Research tells us something about problem-solving instruction, but not nearly
enough.
see also the very last section of this article). In a paper I co-authored about 20 years ago,
4
Indeed, some years ago, one mathematics education researcher asked me why I (and most other
problem-solving researchers) studied problem solving in isolation from learning specific
mathematics concepts and processes. I had no good answer; she was correct and I couldn’t
provide a compelling reason why we did so. Today, I think the reason stems from our reliance on
cognitive science for guidance in developing our research agendas and methods.
Lester
my co-author and I identified four reasons for this unfortunate state of affairs: (1)
relatively little attention to the role of the teacher in instruction; (2) too little concern for
what happens in real classrooms; (3) a focus on individuals rather than small groups or
whole classes; and (4) the largely atheoretical in nature of problem-solving research
(Lester & Charles, 1992). I have discussed the fourth reason elsewhere (Lester, 2005), so
will not discuss it here. Instead, let me comment on the other three reasons. (Interested
readers may wish to read the provocative analysis of the state of mathematical problem-
solving research written by Lesh and Zawojewski (2007). In their analysis they take
issue with the nature and direction of nearly all the research over the past 50 years.)
The role of the teacher. More than twenty-five years ago, Silver (1985) pointed
out that the typical research report might have described in a general way the
instructional method employed, but rarely was any mention made of the teacher's specific
role. Some progress has been made since then (see, e.g., the edited volumes by Lester
and Charles (2003) and Schoen and Charles (2003) and the review by Schoenfeld
(1992)). But, as useful as these efforts have been, they fall short of what is needed.
Instead of simply considering teachers as agents to effect certain student outcomes, their
dimensions of a system involving: the role of the teacher, the nature of classroom tasks,
the social culture of the classroom, the use of mathematical tools as learning supports,
and issues of equity and accessibility. Changing any of the dimensions of this system
requires parallel changes in each of the other dimensions (Hiebert et al., 1997).
problem-solving instruction based on ten specific teaching actions (Charles & Lester,
1984). The research involved several hundred fifth and seventh grade students in more
than 40 classrooms over the period of one full school year. The results were
several key components of the problem-solving process. However, despite the promise
of our instructional approach, the conditions under which the study was conducted did
atmosphere that exists. It is vital that such descriptions be compiled if there is to be any
hope of deriving sound prescriptions for teaching problem solving. In the final section of
this article I present a framework for research that, if used, might provide the sorts of
the history research in mathematical problem solving (dating back about 50 years) the
focus has been on the thinking processes used by individuals as they solve problems or as
they reflect back on their work solving problems. When the goal of research is to
individual performance seems appropriate. However, when our concerns are with
classroom instruction, we should give attention to groups and whole classes. To be sure,
small groups can serve as an appropriate environment for research on teaching problem
solving, but the research on problem-solving instruction cannot be limited to the study of
small groups. In order for the field to move forward, research on teaching problem
Lester
solving needs to examine teaching and learning processes for individuals, small groups,
5
A Model of Complex Mathematical Activity
In addition to the lack of attention to the role of the teacher in real classrooms and
the focus on individuals rather than whole classes, the relative ineffectiveness of
instruction to improve students' ability to solve problems can be attributed to the fact that
problem solving has often been conceptualized in a simplistic way. This naive
perspective has two levels or "worlds": the everyday world of things, problems, and
concepts, and operations. In this naive perspective, the problem-solving process typically
has three steps. Beginning with a problem posed in terms of physical reality, the problem
solver first translates the problem into abstract mathematical terms, and then operates on
problem. This solution is then translated into the terms of the original problem.
According to this view, mathematics may be, and often is, learned separately from its
applications and (too often) with no attempt to connect new mathematics concepts to old
ones. Teachers who adhere to this perspective are very concerned about developing
representations and vice versa. However, these teachers tend to deal with problems and
applications of mathematics only after the mathematical concepts and skills have been
introduced, developed, and practiced. Many of the “problems” found in textbooks often
5
The discussion in this section is excerpted with only minor revision from Lester and Kehle
(2003).
TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .255
can be solved exactly as this naive perspective indicates. But for more challenging,
substantive problems, the problem solver cannot simply apply a previously learned
problems also demand more complex processes such as planning, selecting strategies,
verifying that a solution has been found. For non-routine tasks, a different type of
perspective is required, one that emphasizes the making of new meanings through
construction of new representations and inferential moves (refer back to the description
The new perspective, like the previous one, also contains two levels representing
the everyday world of problems and the abstract world of mathematical concepts,
symbols and operations. In this perspective, however, the mathematical processes in the
upper level are "under construction" (i.e., being learned, as opposed to already learned;
features are the relationships between steps in the mathematical process (in the
mathematics world) and actions on particular elements in the problem (in the everyday
world). It is in the forging of these relationships that results in the meaning making that
is central to mathematical activity of all kinds. At times the problem solver is learning to
make abstract written records of the actions that are understood in a concrete setting.
This involves the processes of abstraction and generalization. And, at other times the
problem solver attempts to connect a mathematical process to the real-world actions that
the mathematical process represents. Also, a problem solver who had forgotten the
imagining the corresponding concrete steps in the world in which the problem was posed.
As a result, typically the problem solver moves back and forth between the two worlds—
the everyday problem world and the mathematical world—as the need arises.
But, although this perspective is an improvement over the original, it too falls
short of what is needed because it does not account for many of the most important
actions (both cognitive and non-cognitive) involved during real problem solving. Even
the modified perspective regards problem solving as somehow being different from other
solving within a much broader category, "mathematical activity,” and to give a prominent
6
role to the metacognitive activity engaged in by the individual or group.
multiphase process that begins when an individual, working in a complex context (Box
A), poses (or is given) a specific task to solve (the solid arrow between A and B). To
start solving the task, the individual simplifies the complex setting by identifying those
concepts and processes that seem to bear most directly on the problem. This simplifying
and problem posing phase involves making decisions about what should be attended to
and what can be ignored, developing a sense about how the essential concepts are
connected, and results in a realistic representation of the original situation. This realistic
representation is a model of the original context from which the problem was drawn
because it is easier to examine, manipulate, and understand than the original situation.
Next comes the abstraction phase (solid arrow from B to C), which introduces
phase involves the selection of mathematical concepts to represent the essential features
of the realistic model. Often the abstraction phase is guided by a sense of what a given
situation, the realistic problem now becomes a specific mathematical problem related to
the representation. This mathematical problem acquires a meaning all its own, becoming
the figure by the “computing” arrow. During this phase, the person draws upon her or his
store of mathematical facts, skills, mathematical reasoning abilities, and so forth. For
example, the problem might call for a solution of a system of equations and solving this
system of equations does not depend on the original context of the initial problem. The
final phase (from D to A, D to B, and D to C), then, should involve the individual in
comparing the conclusions/results obtained with the original context and problem, as well
as with the mathematical representation (refer to the dashed arrows between boxes). But,
the act of comparing does not occur only after conclusions are drawn and a solution is
obtained. Rather, it might take place at any time and at any point during the entire
work is a key feature of success on complex mathematical tasks. In general, the act of
comparing the current state of one's work, thinking, and decisions denotes how complex
Lester
mathematical activity can be. The degree to which the individual chooses to compare her
or his current state with earlier states can be considered a determinant of task complexity
and, in fact, is the primary way to distinguish “routine” from “non-routine” tasks (i.e.,
problems). For example, performing routine calculations using whole numbers typically
requires little comparing, whereas work on more complex tasks might necessitate quite a
lot of comparing throughout ones work on it. In brief, then, the degree to which a task
involved.
To sum up, what Kehle and I have proposed is a blurring of the distinction
between problem solving and other mathematical activity emerging from research on
activity, learning, applying what has been learned, and other features of mathematical
about the proficiencies mathematics teachers need. In the next section I discuss these
The debate over the merits of direct (explicit) instruction versus constructivist
instruction has been raging for at least 50 years and any consideration of the
this debate. More specifically, the identification of such proficiencies must take into
account the assumptions that are being made about the nature of mathematics learning
and instruction, as well as about instructional goals. For example, a proponent of direct
instruction (e.g., Kirschner, Sweller & Clark, 2006) might view learning as simply a
involves much more. Constructivist teachers are concerned with (among other things)
how to help students select and use good procedures for solving problems (Gresalfi &
Lester, 2009). Clearly, these quite different perspectives on what mathematics learning
involves will have a tremendous influence on what teachers must be able to do in their
classrooms (i.e., what proficiencies they need). Furthermore, there is the matter of the
teacher’s goals. If problem solving is intended as the end result of instruction, one set of
proficiencies for teaching is needed, but if problem solving is the means through which
mathematical concepts, processes, and procedures are learned, then a different set of
Lester
proficiencies may be called for. For example, the teacher for whom problem solving is a
means, would likely need to be very proficient at listening to and observing students as
they work on mathematical tasks (Davis, 1997; Yackel, 2003). And, quite naturally,
listening to students would play a much less important role for a teacher who mostly
assumptions the teacher makes about the nature of mathematics learning and the goals of
instruction.
But, what of the proficiencies needed to help students learn how to solve
problems? One consequence of subsuming problem solving under the broader heading
proficiencies teachers need. To illustrate, consider the task “Which is more 2/3 or 2/5?”
Does this task involve any problem solving on the part of the student? Maybe, maybe
not! Of course, one can “cross multiply” to determine that 2/3 is more (or use some other
previously learned procedure), or one may have had sufficient experience with fractions
to simply “know” that 2/3 is more. In these instances, one could argue that no problem
7
solving is going on. But suppose you are a 3rd grader who does not know of any
procedures to decide which is more. Without a prescribed method of attack, this task
might be used to help you better understand the meanings of numerator and denominator
and also help you see how useful one half can be as a fraction benchmark (Van de Walle,
2003). This is at the heart of what it means to teach via problem solving. But, what
7
One needn’t be a 3rd grader to find this task problematic. Over the past 4 years I have been
tutoring unemployed adults who hope one day to pass the US high school equivalency exam
(GED). Almost to a person, they do not know how to solve this task when they begin to study
with me.
TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .261
proficiencies must teachers have who subscribe to a teaching via problem solving
approach? Of course, they must be adept at selecting good problems, at listening and
observing, at asking the right questions, at knowing when to prod and when to withhold
8
comment, as well as a host of other actions . These actions make up what Moore (1995)
has called the “craft of teaching.” Moore’s “image of a [proficient] teacher is that of a
skilled craftworker, a master machinist say, who knows exactly what she must do, brings
the tools she needs, does the work with straightforward competence, and takes pleasure in
a job well done. She does her work right every day, and every day's work fits the larger
plan of her project” (p. 5). For Moore a craft is a “collection of learned skills
accompanied by experienced judgment” (p. 5). So, the question is “How does one
become a craftsman?”
Thirty years ago, Randy Charles and I wrote a book in which we laid out an
instructional plan for teachers to follow in order to be effective in teaching students how
to solve mathematics problems (Charles & Lester, 1982). The plan focused on three
“teaching actions.” Since then, the three phases have appeared in different guises in
various American elementary and middle school textbook series (e.g., the middle grades
Summarize (Pearson Education Inc. 2011)). The features of our plan that most clearly
distinguish it from more “traditional” instructional plans have to do with the teacher’s
role and the nature of the classroom environment. However, this is far from sufficient;
knowing about the teaching actions is simply not enough! In addition to knowing what to
8
I, and various collaborators over the years, have used the word (teaching) “actions” to refer to
what the teacher does during the act of teaching.
Lester
do, the teacher must also know when to do it and what the implications might be of the
action taken. In particular, teachers must be adept at: (1) designing and selecting tasks
and activities, (2) listening to and observing students as they engage with problem-
solving activities, (3) making sure that instructional activities remain problematic for
students, (4) focusing on the methods students use to solve problems and being familiar
with problem-solving methods (e.g., heuristics, strategies) that are accessible to students,
and (5) being able to tell the right thing at the right time (cf., Cai, 2010; DiMatteo &
Lester, 2010; Hiebert, 2003). Moreover, teachers and students share responsibility for
sharing ideas, cooperating with each other, and risk taking (Stephan & Whitenack, 2003).
Thus, for me, in addition to myriad other knowledge and skills, a proficient mathematics
Paying attention to and being familiar with the methods students use to solve
problems
Being able to take the appropriate action (or say the right thing) at the right
time
To be sure, teachers who have command of these and related teaching actions and
who also have considerable mathematics content knowledge appropriate for the level at
TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .263
which they are teaching might be considered craftsmen. But, I think what separates a
craftsman from others has to do with the amount of planning and reflection that he or she
has done prior to and after instruction. Unfortunately, even though it seems clear to me
that the type and amount of planning a teacher does have tremendous impact on what
happens during instruction, teacher planning has been largely ignored as a factor of
planning has not even been considered because the teachers in these studies have simply
implemented a plan that had been predetermined by the researchers, not the teachers.
make are driven totally by the content and organization of the textbooks used and,
determine ways to provide these teachers with opportunities to acquire the proficiencies
needed to become craftsmen; opportunities that in my view are best provided through
apprenticeship experiences in their real-world context and situation (Collins, Brown, &
Newmann, 1990). To date, too little attention has been paid to studying the design and
is unfortunate because I think apprenticeship training is the approach most likely to result
Twenty years ago, Randy Charles and I developed a framework for research on
teaching in general and mathematics teaching in particular (Lester & Charles, 1992).
still think it could serve us well in designing research on problem-solving instruction and
I bring this article to a close by offering a slightly-modified sketch of what it consists of.
factors, (2) Teacher planning, (3) Classroom processes, and (3) Instructional outcomes.
Of course, the categories overlap and the factors within each interact both within and
across categories.
What goes on in a classroom is influenced by many things that exist or take place
apart from actual classroom instruction. For example, the teacher's and students'
knowledge, beliefs, attitudes, emotions, and dispositions all play a part in determining
what happens during instruction. Furthermore, the nature of the tasks used as well as the
contextual conditions present outside the classroom also affect instruction (e.g., course
schedules, school structures). There are six types of factors: teacher presage
teacher and students that are not amenable to change but which may be examined for
characteristics have potential for control by the researcher. But, awareness of these
TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .265
make sense of what they are observing. Among the more prominent presage
characteristics are age, sex, and previous experience (e.g., teaching experience, previous
experience with the topic of instruction). Factors such as previous experience may
indeed be of great importance as we learn more about the ways knowledge teachers glean
Teacher and student knowledge and affects. The teacher's and students'
knowledge (both cognitive and metacognitive) and affects (including beliefs) can
strongly influence both the nature and effectiveness of instruction. As a category, these
teacher and student traits are similar to, but quite different from, presage characteristics.
The similarity lies in the potential for providing clear descriptions of the teacher and
students. The difference between the two is that affects and knowledge may change, in
Task features. Task features are the characteristics of the tasks used for
describe tasks: syntax, content, context, structure, and process (see Goldin & McClintock,
1984). Syntax features refer to the arrangement of and relationships among words and
symbols in a task. Content features deal with the mathematical meanings in the problem.
Two important categories of content features are the mathematical content area (e.g.,
geometry, probability) and linguistic content features (e.g., terms having special
mathematical meanings such as "less than," "function," "squared"). Context features are
describe the problem embodiment (representation), verbal setting, and the format of the
Lester
information given in the problem statement. Structure features can be described as the
particular representation that is chosen for a problem. For example, one student may
choose to represent a task in terms of a system of equations, while another student may
represent the same problem in terms of some sort of guessing process. Finally, process
features represent something of an interaction between task and student. That is,
characteristics of the student, it is reasonable to suggest that a problem may lend itself to
solution via particular processes. A consideration of task process features can be very
informative to the researcher in selecting tasks for both instruction and assessment.
teacher and students that may affect the nature of instruction. For example, class size is a
condition that may directly influence the instructional process and with which both
teacher and students must contend. Other obvious contextual conditions include
political forces, and assessment programs. Also, since instructional method provides a
context within which teacher and student behaviors and interactions take place, it too can
at times be considered a factor within this category. I should add that that these six areas
of consideration do not necessarily cover all possible influences; it is likely that there are
other influences that may be at least as important as the ones I have mentioned. Rather,
my intent is to point out the importance of paying attention to the wide range of factors
Teacher planning is not clearly distinct from the other categories, in fact, it
overlaps each of them in various ways. Of particular interest for research are the various
decisions made before, during and as a result of instruction about student presage
activities and topics. Unfortunately, teacher planning has been given too little attention
teacher planning has not even been considered because the teachers in these studies have
simply implemented a plan that had been predetermined by the researchers, not the
teachers make are driven totally by the content and organization of the textbooks used
while teaching either for or via problem solving is certainly influenced by the teacher's
knowledge and affects. However, some of this behavior is likely to be determined by the
kinds of decisions the teacher makes prior to entering the classroom. For example, a
teacher may have planned to follow a specific sequence of teaching actions for delivering
a particular problem-solving lesson knowing that the exact ways in which these teaching
actions are implemented evolve situationally during the lesson. Or, if the knowledge
episodes, then we would search for those cases that significantly shape the craft
knowledge teachers use as a basis for planning and action. Future research should
consider how teachers go about planning for problem-solving instruction and how the
Classroom processes include the host of teacher and student actions and
interactions that take place during instruction. Four dimensions of classroom processes
are apparent: teacher knowledge and affects; teacher behaviors; student knowledge and
Both the teacher's and the students' thinking processes and behaviors during
instruction are almost always directed toward achieving a number of different goals,
sometimes simultaneously. For example, during a lesson the teacher may be assessing
the appropriateness of the small-group arrangement that was established prior to the
lesson, while at the same time trying to guide the students' thinking toward the solution to
a problem. Similarly, a student may be thinking about what her classmates will think if
she never contributes to discussions and at the same time be trying to understand what the
task confronting her is all about. In our framework, we have restricted consideration to
what the teacher thinks about and does to facilitate the students’ thinking and what the
student thinks about and does to solve a problem. We have not attempted to include a
complete menu of objects or goals a teacher might think about during instruction.
Teacher knowledge and affects. These processes include those attitudes, beliefs,
emotions, cognitions and metacognitions that influence, and are influenced by, the
multitude of teacher and student behaviors that occur in the classroom during instruction.
In particular, this dimension is concerned with the teacher's thinking and affects while
facilitating students’ attempts to understand a task, develop a plan for solving it, carry
out the plan to obtain an answer, and look back over the solution effort.
TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .269
instruction give rise to the teacher's behaviors, the overt actions taken by the teacher
regard to use (or non-use) as well as quality. The quality of a teacher behavior can
include, among other things, the correctness of the behavior (e.g., correct mathematically
or correct given the conditions of the problem), the clarity of the action (e.g., a clear
question or hint), and the manner in which the behavior was delivered (e.g., the verbal
Student knowledge and affects. Similar to the teacher, this subcategory refers to
the knowledge and affects that interact with teacher and student behaviors. The concern
here is with how students interpret the behavior of the teacher and how the students'
thinking about a problem, their affects, and their work on the problem affects their own
behavior. Also of concern here is how instructional influences such as task features or
Student behaviors. These behaviors include the overt actions of the student
solving phases mentioned earlier, we can identify several behaviors students might
research has been concerned with short-term effects only. Furthermore, transfer effects,
Lester
effects on attitudes, beliefs, and emotions, and changes in teacher behavior have been
Student outcomes. Both immediate and long-term effects on student learning are
included in this category, as are transfer effects (both near and far transfer). Illustrative
attitudes about problem solving or about themselves as problem solvers and the effect of
instructional efforts. In particular, their attitudes and beliefs, the nature and extent of
their planning, as well as their classroom behavior during subsequent instruction are all
his craft knowledge. Thus, it is reasonable to expect that experience affects the teacher's
area) and heightened parental interest in their children's school work are two examples of
and affects play in instruction. Questions such as the following need to be investigated :
teachers need to be effective as teachers of problem solving? How is that knowledge best
students, teaching mathematics, and problem solving influence the decisions they make
actually happens in the classroom during instruction. As I mentioned earlier, there may
be other important factors to be included in this framework and that certain of the factors
this framework could serve as a step in the direction of making research in the area more
I do not intend for my remarks to give the impression that I think mathematical
problem solving research has not amounted to much during the past 40 years or that
current research efforts are misguided. Indeed, quite the opposite is the case! Several
important principles have slowly emerged from the research since the early 1970s. I end
this article by listing these principles without comment: each principle could serve as the
Lester
basis for an article or monograph. My hope is that this list, like much of the rest of my
article, will stimulate discussion among those who are interested in pursuing a research
2. The task variety principle. Students will improve as problem solvers only if
they are given opportunities to solve a variety of types of problematic tasks (in
to solve problems involving those concepts. That is, heuristics, skills, control
processes, and awareness of one’s own thinking develop concurrently with the
tells us that problem- solving ability is best developed when it takes place in
teacher.
5. The multiple roles for the teacher principle. Problem-solving instruction that
teacher in three different, but related, roles: (a) as an external monitor, (b) as a
TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .273
mathematical ideas.
References
Ball, D. L., Thames, M. H., & Phelps, G. (2008). Content knowledge for teaching: What
makes it special? Journal of Teacher Education, 59, 389-408.
mathematics: Translating research for elementary teachers (pp. 9-14). Reston, VA:
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Charles, R., & Lester, F. (1982). Teaching problem solving: What, why and how. Palo
Alto, CA: Dale Seymour Publications.
Collins, A., Brown, J., & Newmann, S. (1990). Cognitive apprenticeship: Teaching
students the craft of reading, writing, and mathematics. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.),
Knowing, learning and instruction: Essays in honor of Robert Glaser (pp. 453-494).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
DiMatteo, R. W. & Lester, F. K. (2010). The role of problem solving in the secondary
school mathematics classroom. In J. Lobato & F. Lester (Eds.). Teaching and
learning mathematics: Translating research for secondary teachers (pp. 7-12).
Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Frensch, P. A. & Funke, J. (Eds.). (1995). Complex problem solving: The European
perspective. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Hiebert, J., Carpenter, T. P., Fennema, E., Fuson, K., Wearne, D., Murray, H., Olivier,
A., & Human, P. (1997). Making sense: Teaching and learning mathematics with
understanding. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Hill, H. C., Sleep, L., Lewis, J. M., & Ball, D. L. (2007). Assessing teachers’
mathematical knowledge: What knowledge matters and what evidence counts? In F.
K. Lester (Ed.), Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning
(vol. 1, pp. 111-155). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.
Kahney, H. (1993). Problem solving: Current issues (2nd edition). Buckingham, UK:
Open University Press.
Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during
instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery,
problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist,
41, 75-86.
Lester
Lesh, R. & Zawojewski, J. (2007). Problem solving and modeling. In F. K. Lester (Ed.),
Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp.763-804).
Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishers.
Lester, F. K., Garofalo, J., & Kroll, D. (1989). The role of metacognition in
mathematical problem solving: A study of two grade seven classes. Final report to
the National Science Foundation of NSF project MDR 85-50346.
Lester, F. K. & Kehle, P. (2003). From problem solving to modeling: The evolution of
thinking about research on complex mathematical activity. In R. Lesh & H. M. Doerr
(Eds.), Beyond constructivism: Models and modeling perspectives on mathematics
problem solving, learning, and teaching (pp. 501-517). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Pearson Education, Inc. (2011). Connected Mathematics2. Retrieved July 28, 2011 from,
http://www.phschool.com/cmp2/implementing/
Stein, M. K., Boaler, J. & Silver, E. A. (2003). Teaching mathematics through problem
solving: Research perspectives. In H. L. Schoen & R. I. Charles (Eds.), Teaching
mathematics through problem solving: Grades 6-12 (pp. 245–256). Reston, VA:
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Van de Walle, J. (2003). Designing and selecting problematic tasks. In F. K. Lester & R.
I. Charles (Eds.), Teaching mathematics through problem solving: Prekindergarten -
grade 6 (pp. 67-80). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Zazkis, R., & Leikin, R. (2010). Advanced mathematical knowledge in teaching practice:
Perceptions of secondary mathematics teachers. Mathematical Thinking and
Learning, 12(4), 263-281.