0% found this document useful (0 votes)
47 views35 pages

Thoughts About Research On Mathematical Problem - Solving Instruct

Problem Solving

Uploaded by

fernandoutfcp
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
47 views35 pages

Thoughts About Research On Mathematical Problem - Solving Instruct

Problem Solving

Uploaded by

fernandoutfcp
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 35

The Mathematics Enthusiast

Volume 10 Article 12
Number 1 Numbers 1 & 2

1-2013

Thoughts About Research On Mathematical Problem- Solving


Instruction
Frank K. Lester Jr.

Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umt.edu/tme

Part of the Mathematics Commons


Let us know how access to this document benefits you.

Recommended Citation
Lester, Frank K. Jr. (2013) "Thoughts About Research On Mathematical Problem- Solving Instruction," The
Mathematics Enthusiast: Vol. 10 : No. 1 , Article 12.
Available at: https://scholarworks.umt.edu/tme/vol10/iss1/12

This Article is brought to you for free and open access by ScholarWorks at University of Montana. It has been
accepted for inclusion in The Mathematics Enthusiast by an authorized editor of ScholarWorks at University of
Montana. For more information, please contact [email protected].
TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p.245

Thoughts About Research On Mathematical Problem- Solving


Instruction
Frank K. Lester, Jr.
Indiana University, Bloomington, USA

Abstract: In this article, the author, who has written extensively about mathematical
problem solving over the past 40 years, discusses some of his current thinking about the
nature of problem-solving and its relation to other forms of mathematical activity. He
also suggests several proficiencies teachers should acquire in order for them to be
successful in helping students become better problem solvers and presents a framework
for research on problem-solving instruction. He closes the article with a list of principles
about problem-solving instruction that have emerged since the early 1970s.

Keywords: mathematical activity, problem solving, problem-solving instruction,


proficiencies for teaching, craft knowledge, research design, teaching as a craft, teacher
planning, metacognition..

Introduction

My interest in problem solving as an area of study within mathematics education

began more than 40 years ago as I was beginning to think seriously about a topic for my

doctoral dissertation. Since that time, my interest in and enthusiasm for problem solving,

in particular problem-solving instruction, has not waned but some of my thinking about it

has changed considerably. In this article I share some of my current thinking about a

variety of ideas associated with this complex and elusive area of study, giving special

attention to problem-solving instruction. To be sure, in this article I will not provide

much elaboration on these ideas and careful readers may be put off by such a cursory

discussion. My hope is that some readers will be stimulated by my ideas to think a bit

differently about how mathematical problem solving, and in particular problem-solving

instruction, might be studied.

The Mathematics Enthusiast, ISSN 1551-3440, Vol. 10, nos.1&2, pp.245-278


2013©The Author(s) & Dept of Mathematical Sciences-The University of Montana
Lester

Setting the stage

Most mathematics educators agree that the development of students’ problem-

solving abilities is a primary objective of instruction and how this goal is to be reached

involves consideration by the teacher of a wide range of factors and decisions. For

example, teachers must decide on the problems and problem-solving experiences to use,

when to give problem solving particular attention, how much guidance to give students,

and how to assess students’ progress. Furthermore, there is the issue of whether problem

solving is intended as the end result of instruction or the means through which

mathematical concepts, processes, and procedures are learned. Or, to put it another way,

should teachers adopt “teaching for problem solving, ”—an ends approach—or “teaching
1
via problem solving” —a means approach? (I say more about means and ends later in

this article.) In my view, the answer to this question is that both approaches have merit;

problem solving should be both an end result of learning mathematics and the means

through which mathematics is learned (DiMatteo & Lester, 2010; Stein, Boaler, & Silver,

2003). Whichever approach is adopted, or if some combination of approaches is used,

research is needed that focuses on the factors that influence student learning.

Unfortunately, as far as I know, no prolonged, in-depth, programmatic research of this

sort has been undertaken and, as a result, the accumulation of knowledge has been very

slow. Moreover, the present intense interest in research on teachers’ knowledge and

1
It has become more common to refer to the “means” approach to teaching as teaching through
problem solving. In Schroeder and Lester (1989) we discuss three approaches to problem-solving
instruction: teaching about, for, and via problem solving. Teaching “via” problem solving is
essentially the same as teaching through problem solving. Today, teaching about problem
solving is not generally regarded as a legitimate instructional method, although I suspect that
some (many?) teachers and curriculum writers subscribe to this approach.
TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .247

proficiencies demands that future problem-solving research pay close attention to the

mathematical and pedagogical knowledge and proficiencies a teacher should possess (cf.,

Ball, Thames, & Phelps, 2008; Hill, Sleep, Lewis, & Ball, 2007; Moreira & David, 2008;

Zazkis & Leikin, 2010).

But before discussing problem-solving instruction, let me first say a few things

about mathematical problem solving. This short discussion will highlight how my

thinking has changed about the nature of problem solving and other forms of

mathematical activity.

Some claims about Problem Solving

Among the many issues and questions associated with problem-solving

instruction I have worried about during my career, several have endured over time. In

this section I make five claims related to these enduring issues and offer brief

discussions of my current thinking about them.

Claim 1. We need to rethink what we mean by “Problem” and “Problem Solving”

Although there have been at least four distinct problem-solving research traditions

within (namely, Gestalt/Cognitive, Learning/S-R, Computer/Information Processing, and

Psychometric/Component Analysis), they all agree that a problem is a task for which an

individual does not know (immediately) what to do to get an answer (cf., Frensch &

Funke, 1995; Holth, 2008). Some representative definitions illustrate this fundamental

agreement:

A problem arises when a living creature has a goal but does not know how this

goal is to be reached. (Duncker, 1945, p. 1)


Lester

A question for which there is at the moment no answer is a problem. (Skinner,

1966, p. 225)

A person is confronted with a problem when he wants something and does not

know immediately what series of actions he can perform to get it. (Newell &

Simon, 1972, p. 72)

Whenever you have a goal which is blocked for whatever reason . . . you have a

problem. (Kahney, 1993, p. 15)

These definitions have two common ingredients: there is a goal and the

individual (i.e., the problem solver) is not immediately able to attain the goal. Moreover,

researchers irrespective of tradition, view problem solving simply as what one does to

achieve the goal. Unfortunately, these definitions and descriptions, like most of those

that have been given of mathematical problem solving – including those I and other

mathematics educators have proposed – are unhelpful for thinking about how to teach

students to solve problems or to identify the proficiencies needed to teach for or via

problem solving. A useful description should acknowledge that problem solving is an

activity requiring an individual (or group) to engage in a variety of cognitive actions,

each of which requires some knowledge and skill, and some of which are not routine.

Furthermore, these cognitive actions are influenced by a number of non-cognitive factors.

And, although it is difficult to define problem solving, the following statement – which

Paul Kehle and I devised a few years ago– comes much closer to capturing what it

involves than most of those that have appeared in the literature.

Successful problem solving involves coordinating previous experiences,

knowledge, familiar representations and patterns of inference, and intuition in an


TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .249

effort to generate new representations and related patterns of inference that

resolve some tension or ambiguity (i.e., lack of meaningful representations and

supporting inferential moves) that prompted the original problem-solving activity.

(Lester & Kehle, 2003, p. 510)

The advantage of this description of problem solving over the others lies in its

identification of several key ingredients of success: coordination of experience,

knowledge, familiar representations, patterns of inference, and intuition. So, to be a

successful problem solver, an individual must have ample relevant experience in learning

how to solve problems, strong content knowledge, proficiency in using a variety of


2
representations and a solid grasp of how to recognize and construct patterns of inference.

Moreover, it recognizes the importance of intuition in successful problem solving3. With

the possible exception of intuition, each of these ingredients should be attended to any

program aimed at equipping prospective teachers with the proficiencies needed to teach

mathematics either for or via problem solving. I say more about the implications of this

description for the education of mathematics teachers later in this article. But first, let me

continue with a few more observations about the nature of problem solving.

Claim 2. We know very little about how to improve students’ metacognitive abilities.

So much has been written about metacognition and its place both in the teaching

and learning of mathematics that a few comments about this elusive construct seem

warranted. I remain convinced that metacognition is one of the driving forces behind

2
The research perspective on the role of representation in doing mathematics provided by Goldin
(2003) is particularly relevant to this discussion.
3
A reviewer pointed out to me that intuition is itself a very subtle notion and as such the
definition we propose is unhelpful. To be sure, intuition is a subtle idea, but I think it essential to
include it in any description of what problem solving entails because it serves to point out just
how subtle the act of problem solving can be and, consequently, how difficult it has been to make
progress in learning how to teach students to be better problem solvers.
Lester

successful problem solving (Garofalo & Lester, 1984), but we really know almost

nothing about what teachers should do to develop students’ metacognitive abilities. To

be sure, it is essential that successful problem solvers be able to monitor and regulate

their cognitive behaviors. But, almost no research has been done that demonstrates that

students’ can be taught these behaviors. Within the mathematics education community

both Schoenfeld (1992) and I (Lester, Garofalo, & Kroll, 1989), among others, have

conducted research aimed at enhancing students’ metacognitive abilities, but neither of us

has identified the proficiencies teachers need to do this. Instead, we have offered

suggestions, with too little evidence to support them. So, any program designed to

enhance mathematics teachers’ proficiencies that pays heed to metacognition should do

so only after acknowledging that there is no conclusive research evidence to support any

particular method of metacognition instruction over another.

Claim 3. Mathematics teachers needn’t be expert problem solvers; they must be serious

students of problem solving.

It is natural to suggest that teachers must themselves be expert problem solvers

before they are to be considered proficient mathematics teachers. But, I think this is

asking too much of them! George Polya was an expert (and, hence, proficient) problem

solver as well as an expert teacher of mathematics, but to expect all teachers to be experts

is both unreasonable and unnecessary. After all, expert basketball coaches needn’t have

been expert basketball players and expert violin teachers needn’t have been

concertmasters. Of course, teachers should be experienced problem solvers and should

have a firm grasp of what successful problem solving involves, but care should be taken
TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .251

not to confuse proficiency in teaching students to solve problems with expertise as

problem solvers.

Claim 4. Problem solving isn’t always a high-level cognitive activity.

A fourth observation is that the description of problem solving I have given above

blurs the distinction between problem solving and other types of mathematical activity—I

have more to say about this blurring later. The distinctions that led historically to the

isolation of mathematical problem solving as a research focus from other areas of study

and the subsequent distinctions that resulted from this isolation are due in part to strong

traditions of disciplinary boundaries (Lester & Kehle, 2003).4 This isolation led to

subsuming mathematical understanding under problem solving. But, the inverse makes

more sense; that is, to subsume problem solving (and problem posing) under

mathematical understanding and, hence, under mathematical activity. By so doing,

emphasis is placed on several other constructs that are important in being able to do

mathematics — e.g., model building, generation of representations, constructing patterns

of inference — that too often are not considered when problem solving is isolated from

other forms of mathematical activity.

Claim 5. Research tells us something about problem-solving instruction, but not nearly

enough.

Although research on mathematical problem solving has provided some valuable

information about problem-solving instruction, we haven’t learned nearly enough (but

see also the very last section of this article). In a paper I co-authored about 20 years ago,

4
Indeed, some years ago, one mathematics education researcher asked me why I (and most other
problem-solving researchers) studied problem solving in isolation from learning specific
mathematics concepts and processes. I had no good answer; she was correct and I couldn’t
provide a compelling reason why we did so. Today, I think the reason stems from our reliance on
cognitive science for guidance in developing our research agendas and methods.
Lester

my co-author and I identified four reasons for this unfortunate state of affairs: (1)

relatively little attention to the role of the teacher in instruction; (2) too little concern for

what happens in real classrooms; (3) a focus on individuals rather than small groups or

whole classes; and (4) the largely atheoretical in nature of problem-solving research

(Lester & Charles, 1992). I have discussed the fourth reason elsewhere (Lester, 2005), so

will not discuss it here. Instead, let me comment on the other three reasons. (Interested

readers may wish to read the provocative analysis of the state of mathematical problem-

solving research written by Lesh and Zawojewski (2007). In their analysis they take

issue with the nature and direction of nearly all the research over the past 50 years.)

The role of the teacher. More than twenty-five years ago, Silver (1985) pointed

out that the typical research report might have described in a general way the

instructional method employed, but rarely was any mention made of the teacher's specific

role. Some progress has been made since then (see, e.g., the edited volumes by Lester

and Charles (2003) and Schoen and Charles (2003) and the review by Schoenfeld

(1992)). But, as useful as these efforts have been, they fall short of what is needed.

Instead of simply considering teachers as agents to effect certain student outcomes, their

role should be viewed as one dimension of a dynamic interaction among several

dimensions of a system involving: the role of the teacher, the nature of classroom tasks,

the social culture of the classroom, the use of mathematical tools as learning supports,

and issues of equity and accessibility. Changing any of the dimensions of this system

requires parallel changes in each of the other dimensions (Hiebert et al., 1997).

Observations of real classrooms. Several years ago, my colleague, Randy

Charles, and I conducted a large-scale study of the effectiveness of an approach to


TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .253

problem-solving instruction based on ten specific teaching actions (Charles & Lester,

1984). The research involved several hundred fifth and seventh grade students in more

than 40 classrooms over the period of one full school year. The results were

encouraging: students receiving the instruction benefited tremendously with respect to

several key components of the problem-solving process. However, despite the promise

of our instructional approach, the conditions under which the study was conducted did

not allow us to make extensive, systematic observations of classrooms. Ours is not an

isolated instance. In particular, there has been a lack of descriptions of teachers'

behaviors, teacher-student and student-student interactions, and the type of classroom

atmosphere that exists. It is vital that such descriptions be compiled if there is to be any

hope of deriving sound prescriptions for teaching problem solving. In the final section of

this article I present a framework for research that, if used, might provide the sorts of

rich, detailed descriptions I think we need.

Focus on individuals rather than groups or whole classes. Throughout most of

the history research in mathematical problem solving (dating back about 50 years) the

focus has been on the thinking processes used by individuals as they solve problems or as

they reflect back on their work solving problems. When the goal of research is to

characterize the thinking involved in a process like problem solving, a microanalysis of

individual performance seems appropriate. However, when our concerns are with

classroom instruction, we should give attention to groups and whole classes. To be sure,

small groups can serve as an appropriate environment for research on teaching problem

solving, but the research on problem-solving instruction cannot be limited to the study of

small groups. In order for the field to move forward, research on teaching problem
Lester

solving needs to examine teaching and learning processes for individuals, small groups,

and whole classes.

5
A Model of Complex Mathematical Activity

In addition to the lack of attention to the role of the teacher in real classrooms and

the focus on individuals rather than whole classes, the relative ineffectiveness of

instruction to improve students' ability to solve problems can be attributed to the fact that

problem solving has often been conceptualized in a simplistic way. This naive

perspective has two levels or "worlds": the everyday world of things, problems, and

applications of mathematics and the idealized, abstract world of mathematical symbols,

concepts, and operations. In this naive perspective, the problem-solving process typically

has three steps. Beginning with a problem posed in terms of physical reality, the problem

solver first translates the problem into abstract mathematical terms, and then operates on

this mathematical representation in order to come to a mathematical solution of the

problem. This solution is then translated into the terms of the original problem.

According to this view, mathematics may be, and often is, learned separately from its

applications and (too often) with no attempt to connect new mathematics concepts to old

ones. Teachers who adhere to this perspective are very concerned about developing

skillfulness in translating (so-called) real-world problems into mathematical

representations and vice versa. However, these teachers tend to deal with problems and

applications of mathematics only after the mathematical concepts and skills have been

introduced, developed, and practiced. Many of the “problems” found in textbooks often

5
The discussion in this section is excerpted with only minor revision from Lester and Kehle
(2003).
TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .255

can be solved exactly as this naive perspective indicates. But for more challenging,

substantive problems, the problem solver cannot simply apply a previously learned

procedure to solve the problem. In addition to translation and interpretation, these

problems also demand more complex processes such as planning, selecting strategies,

identifying sub-goals, choosing or creating appropriate representations, conjecturing, and

verifying that a solution has been found. For non-routine tasks, a different type of

perspective is required, one that emphasizes the making of new meanings through

construction of new representations and inferential moves (refer back to the description

of problem solving Kehle and I (2003) have proposed).

The new perspective, like the previous one, also contains two levels representing

the everyday world of problems and the abstract world of mathematical concepts,

symbols and operations. In this perspective, however, the mathematical processes in the

upper level are "under construction" (i.e., being learned, as opposed to already learned;

coming to be understood, as opposed to being understood) and the most important

features are the relationships between steps in the mathematical process (in the

mathematics world) and actions on particular elements in the problem (in the everyday

world). It is in the forging of these relationships that results in the meaning making that

is central to mathematical activity of all kinds. At times the problem solver is learning to

make abstract written records of the actions that are understood in a concrete setting.

This involves the processes of abstraction and generalization. And, at other times the

problem solver attempts to connect a mathematical process to the real-world actions that

the mathematical process represents. Also, a problem solver who had forgotten the

details of a mathematical procedure would attempt to reconstruct that procedure by


Lester

imagining the corresponding concrete steps in the world in which the problem was posed.

As a result, typically the problem solver moves back and forth between the two worlds—

the everyday problem world and the mathematical world—as the need arises.

But, although this perspective is an improvement over the original, it too falls

short of what is needed because it does not account for many of the most important

actions (both cognitive and non-cognitive) involved during real problem solving. Even

the modified perspective regards problem solving as somehow being different from other

sorts of mathematical activity. In my view, what is needed is to subsume problem

solving within a much broader category, "mathematical activity,” and to give a prominent
6
role to the metacognitive activity engaged in by the individual or group.

Figure 1 below depicts mathematical behavior as a complex, involved,

multiphase process that begins when an individual, working in a complex context (Box

A), poses (or is given) a specific task to solve (the solid arrow between A and B). To

start solving the task, the individual simplifies the complex setting by identifying those

concepts and processes that seem to bear most directly on the problem. This simplifying

and problem posing phase involves making decisions about what should be attended to

and what can be ignored, developing a sense about how the essential concepts are

connected, and results in a realistic representation of the original situation. This realistic

representation is a model of the original context from which the problem was drawn

because it is easier to examine, manipulate, and understand than the original situation.

Next comes the abstraction phase (solid arrow from B to C), which introduces

mathematical concepts and notations (albeit perhaps idiosyncratic). This abstraction


6
I should point out that this depiction is a representation of ideal, rather than typical, performance
during an individual's work on some mathematical task. It is ideal in the sense that it denotes key
actions in which the individual should engage in order to obtain acceptable results.
TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .257

phase involves the selection of mathematical concepts to represent the essential features

of the realistic model. Often the abstraction phase is guided by a sense of what a given

representation makes possible in the subsequent computation phase. The explicit

representation of the original setting and problem in mathematical symbolism constitutes

a mathematical representation of both the setting and the task/problem.

Once a problem solver has generated a mathematical representation of the original

situation, the realistic problem now becomes a specific mathematical problem related to

the representation. This mathematical problem acquires a meaning all its own, becoming

an isolated, well-defined mathematical problem (Box C).

The third phase of the process (from C to D) involves manipulating the

mathematical representation and deducing some mathematical conclusions—depicted in

the figure by the “computing” arrow. During this phase, the person draws upon her or his

store of mathematical facts, skills, mathematical reasoning abilities, and so forth. For

example, the problem might call for a solution of a system of equations and solving this

system of equations does not depend on the original context of the initial problem. The

final phase (from D to A, D to B, and D to C), then, should involve the individual in

comparing the conclusions/results obtained with the original context and problem, as well

as with the mathematical representation (refer to the dashed arrows between boxes). But,

the act of comparing does not occur only after conclusions are drawn and a solution is

obtained. Rather, it might take place at any time and at any point during the entire

process. Indeed, this regular and continual monitoring—metacognitive activity—of one's

work is a key feature of success on complex mathematical tasks. In general, the act of

comparing the current state of one's work, thinking, and decisions denotes how complex
Lester

mathematical activity can be. The degree to which the individual chooses to compare her

or his current state with earlier states can be considered a determinant of task complexity

and, in fact, is the primary way to distinguish “routine” from “non-routine” tasks (i.e.,

problems). For example, performing routine calculations using whole numbers typically

requires little comparing, whereas work on more complex tasks might necessitate quite a

lot of comparing throughout ones work on it. In brief, then, the degree to which a task

can be considered problematic can be determined by the amount of “comparing”

involved.

Figure 1. A model of complex mathematical activity

To sum up, what Kehle and I have proposed is a blurring of the distinction

between problem solving and other mathematical activity emerging from research on

mathematical problem solving and constructivist thinking about learning. Furthermore,

we have proposed a blurring of task, person, mathematical activity, non-mathematical


TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .259

activity, learning, applying what has been learned, and other features of mathematical

problem solving. A consequence of this blurring is that it necessitates some rethinking

about the proficiencies mathematics teachers need. In the next section I discuss these

proficiencies in light of the preceding discussions.

Proficiencies for Teaching Mathematics

The debate over the merits of direct (explicit) instruction versus constructivist

instruction has been raging for at least 50 years and any consideration of the

mathematical proficiencies needed for teaching mathematics must be made in view of

this debate. More specifically, the identification of such proficiencies must take into

account the assumptions that are being made about the nature of mathematics learning

and instruction, as well as about instructional goals. For example, a proponent of direct

instruction (e.g., Kirschner, Sweller & Clark, 2006) might view learning as simply a

matter of making a change in students’ long-term memories. But for a constructivist

teacher, in addition to making a change in students’ long-term memories, learning

involves much more. Constructivist teachers are concerned with (among other things)

how to help students select and use good procedures for solving problems (Gresalfi &

Lester, 2009). Clearly, these quite different perspectives on what mathematics learning

involves will have a tremendous influence on what teachers must be able to do in their

classrooms (i.e., what proficiencies they need). Furthermore, there is the matter of the

teacher’s goals. If problem solving is intended as the end result of instruction, one set of

proficiencies for teaching is needed, but if problem solving is the means through which

mathematical concepts, processes, and procedures are learned, then a different set of
Lester

proficiencies may be called for. For example, the teacher for whom problem solving is a

means, would likely need to be very proficient at listening to and observing students as

they work on mathematical tasks (Davis, 1997; Yackel, 2003). And, quite naturally,

listening to students would play a much less important role for a teacher who mostly

lectures. Put more directly, consideration of how to include problem solving in a

mathematical-proficiencies-for-teaching framework should be done in view of the

assumptions the teacher makes about the nature of mathematics learning and the goals of

instruction.

But, what of the proficiencies needed to help students learn how to solve

problems? One consequence of subsuming problem solving under the broader heading

“mathematical activity,” is that it becomes more difficult to specify a precise set of

proficiencies teachers need. To illustrate, consider the task “Which is more 2/3 or 2/5?”

Does this task involve any problem solving on the part of the student? Maybe, maybe

not! Of course, one can “cross multiply” to determine that 2/3 is more (or use some other

previously learned procedure), or one may have had sufficient experience with fractions

to simply “know” that 2/3 is more. In these instances, one could argue that no problem
7
solving is going on. But suppose you are a 3rd grader who does not know of any

procedures to decide which is more. Without a prescribed method of attack, this task

might be used to help you better understand the meanings of numerator and denominator

and also help you see how useful one half can be as a fraction benchmark (Van de Walle,

2003). This is at the heart of what it means to teach via problem solving. But, what

7
One needn’t be a 3rd grader to find this task problematic. Over the past 4 years I have been
tutoring unemployed adults who hope one day to pass the US high school equivalency exam
(GED). Almost to a person, they do not know how to solve this task when they begin to study
with me.
TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .261

proficiencies must teachers have who subscribe to a teaching via problem solving

approach? Of course, they must be adept at selecting good problems, at listening and

observing, at asking the right questions, at knowing when to prod and when to withhold
8
comment, as well as a host of other actions . These actions make up what Moore (1995)

has called the “craft of teaching.” Moore’s “image of a [proficient] teacher is that of a

skilled craftworker, a master machinist say, who knows exactly what she must do, brings

the tools she needs, does the work with straightforward competence, and takes pleasure in

a job well done. She does her work right every day, and every day's work fits the larger

plan of her project” (p. 5). For Moore a craft is a “collection of learned skills

accompanied by experienced judgment” (p. 5). So, the question is “How does one

become a craftsman?”

Thirty years ago, Randy Charles and I wrote a book in which we laid out an

instructional plan for teachers to follow in order to be effective in teaching students how

to solve mathematics problems (Charles & Lester, 1982). The plan focused on three

phases of instruction—Before, During, and After—and was organized around 10

“teaching actions.” Since then, the three phases have appeared in different guises in

various American elementary and middle school textbook series (e.g., the middle grades

Connected Mathematics series organizes activities around Launch, Explore, and

Summarize (Pearson Education Inc. 2011)). The features of our plan that most clearly

distinguish it from more “traditional” instructional plans have to do with the teacher’s

role and the nature of the classroom environment. However, this is far from sufficient;

knowing about the teaching actions is simply not enough! In addition to knowing what to

8
I, and various collaborators over the years, have used the word (teaching) “actions” to refer to
what the teacher does during the act of teaching.
Lester

do, the teacher must also know when to do it and what the implications might be of the

action taken. In particular, teachers must be adept at: (1) designing and selecting tasks

and activities, (2) listening to and observing students as they engage with problem-

solving activities, (3) making sure that instructional activities remain problematic for

students, (4) focusing on the methods students use to solve problems and being familiar

with problem-solving methods (e.g., heuristics, strategies) that are accessible to students,

and (5) being able to tell the right thing at the right time (cf., Cai, 2010; DiMatteo &

Lester, 2010; Hiebert, 2003). Moreover, teachers and students share responsibility for

creating and maintaining a classroom atmosphere that is conducive to exploring and

sharing ideas, cooperating with each other, and risk taking (Stephan & Whitenack, 2003).

Thus, for me, in addition to myriad other knowledge and skills, a proficient mathematics

teacher must be skillful at —

 Designing and selecting appropriate tasks for instruction

 Making sense of and taking appropriate actions after listening to and

observing students as they work on a task

 Keeping tasks appropriately problematic for students

 Paying attention to and being familiar with the methods students use to solve

problems

 Being able to take the appropriate action (or say the right thing) at the right

time

 Creating a classroom atmosphere that is conducive to exploring and sharing.

To be sure, teachers who have command of these and related teaching actions and

who also have considerable mathematics content knowledge appropriate for the level at
TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .263

which they are teaching might be considered craftsmen. But, I think what separates a

craftsman from others has to do with the amount of planning and reflection that he or she

has done prior to and after instruction. Unfortunately, even though it seems clear to me

that the type and amount of planning a teacher does have tremendous impact on what

happens during instruction, teacher planning has been largely ignored as a factor of

importance in research on problem-solving instruction. Indeed, in most studies teacher

planning has not even been considered because the teachers in these studies have simply

implemented a plan that had been predetermined by the researchers, not the teachers.

Furthermore, it is no longer warranted to assume that the planning decisions teachers

make are driven totally by the content and organization of the textbooks used and,

therefore, need not be considered as an object of research. The challenge, then, is to

determine ways to provide these teachers with opportunities to acquire the proficiencies

needed to become craftsmen; opportunities that in my view are best provided through

apprenticeship experiences in their real-world context and situation (Collins, Brown, &

Newmann, 1990). To date, too little attention has been paid to studying the design and

implementation of apprenticeship programs for teacher education. This lack of attention

is unfortunate because I think apprenticeship training is the approach most likely to result

in highly proficient teachers—that is, teacher craftsmen skilled at teaching mathematics

via problem solving.

A Framework for Research on Problem-solving Instruction

Twenty years ago, Randy Charles and I developed a framework for research on

problem –solving instruction that was a synthesis of previous conceptualizations of


Lester

teaching in general and mathematics teaching in particular (Lester & Charles, 1992).

Unfortunately, to my knowledge, other researchers have not adopted this framework. I

still think it could serve us well in designing research on problem-solving instruction and

I bring this article to a close by offering a slightly-modified sketch of what it consists of.

The framework is comprised of four broad categories of factors that we consider

essential in the conceptualization and design of research studies: (1) Non-classroom

factors, (2) Teacher planning, (3) Classroom processes, and (3) Instructional outcomes.

Of course, the categories overlap and the factors within each interact both within and

across categories.

Category 1: Non-classroom Factors

What goes on in a classroom is influenced by many things that exist or take place

apart from actual classroom instruction. For example, the teacher's and students'

knowledge, beliefs, attitudes, emotions, and dispositions all play a part in determining

what happens during instruction. Furthermore, the nature of the tasks used as well as the

contextual conditions present outside the classroom also affect instruction (e.g., course

schedules, school structures). There are six types of factors: teacher presage

characteristics, student presage characteristics, teacher knowledge and affects, student

knowledge and affects, tasks features, and contextual (situational) conditions.

Teacher and student presage characteristics. These are characteristics of the

teacher and students that are not amenable to change but which may be examined for

their effects on classroom instruction. In addition, presage characteristics serve to

describe the individuals involved. Typically, in experimental research these

characteristics have potential for control by the researcher. But, awareness of these
TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .265

characteristics can useful in non-experimental research as well by helping researchers

make sense of what they are observing. Among the more prominent presage

characteristics are age, sex, and previous experience (e.g., teaching experience, previous

experience with the topic of instruction). Factors such as previous experience may

indeed be of great importance as we learn more about the ways knowledge teachers glean

from experience influences practice.

Teacher and student knowledge and affects. The teacher's and students'

knowledge (both cognitive and metacognitive) and affects (including beliefs) can

strongly influence both the nature and effectiveness of instruction. As a category, these

teacher and student traits are similar to, but quite different from, presage characteristics.

The similarity lies in the potential for providing clear descriptions of the teacher and

students. The difference between the two is that affects and knowledge may change, in

particular as a result of instruction, whereas presage characteristics cannot.

Task features. Task features are the characteristics of the tasks used for

instructional or assessment purposes. Historically, at least five types of features serve to

describe tasks: syntax, content, context, structure, and process (see Goldin & McClintock,

1984). Syntax features refer to the arrangement of and relationships among words and

symbols in a task. Content features deal with the mathematical meanings in the problem.

Two important categories of content features are the mathematical content area (e.g.,

geometry, probability) and linguistic content features (e.g., terms having special

mathematical meanings such as "less than," "function," "squared"). Context features are

the non-mathematical meanings in the task statement. Furthermore, context features

describe the problem embodiment (representation), verbal setting, and the format of the
Lester

information given in the problem statement. Structure features can be described as the

logical-mathematical properties of a task. Structure features are determined by the

particular representation that is chosen for a problem. For example, one student may

choose to represent a task in terms of a system of equations, while another student may

represent the same problem in terms of some sort of guessing process. Finally, process

features represent something of an interaction between task and student. That is,

although problem-solving processes (e.g., heuristic reasoning) typically are considered

characteristics of the student, it is reasonable to suggest that a problem may lend itself to

solution via particular processes. A consideration of task process features can be very

informative to the researcher in selecting tasks for both instruction and assessment.

Contextual conditions. These factors concern the conditions external to the

teacher and students that may affect the nature of instruction. For example, class size is a

condition that may directly influence the instructional process and with which both

teacher and students must contend. Other obvious contextual conditions include

textbooks used, community ethnicity, type of administrative support, economic and

political forces, and assessment programs. Also, since instructional method provides a

context within which teacher and student behaviors and interactions take place, it too can

at times be considered a factor within this category. I should add that that these six areas

of consideration do not necessarily cover all possible influences; it is likely that there are

other influences that may be at least as important as the ones I have mentioned. Rather,

my intent is to point out the importance of paying attention to the wide range of factors

that can have an impact on what takes place during instruction.

Category 2: Teacher Planning


TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .267

Teacher planning is not clearly distinct from the other categories, in fact, it

overlaps each of them in various ways. Of particular interest for research are the various

decisions made before, during and as a result of instruction about student presage

characteristics, instructional materials, teaching methods, classroom management

procedures, evaluation of student performance, and amount of time to devote to particular

activities and topics. Unfortunately, teacher planning has been given too little attention

as a factor of importance in problem-solving instruction research. Indeed, in most studies

teacher planning has not even been considered because the teachers in these studies have

simply implemented a plan that had been predetermined by the researchers, not the

teachers. Furthermore, it is no longer warranted to assume that the planning decisions

teachers make are driven totally by the content and organization of the textbooks used

and, therefore, need not be considered as an object of research. A teacher's behavior

while teaching either for or via problem solving is certainly influenced by the teacher's

knowledge and affects. However, some of this behavior is likely to be determined by the

kinds of decisions the teacher makes prior to entering the classroom. For example, a

teacher may have planned to follow a specific sequence of teaching actions for delivering

a particular problem-solving lesson knowing that the exact ways in which these teaching

actions are implemented evolve situationally during the lesson. Or, if the knowledge

teachers use to plan instruction is knowledge gleaned from previous instructional

episodes, then we would search for those cases that significantly shape the craft

knowledge teachers use as a basis for planning and action. Future research should

consider how teachers go about planning for problem-solving instruction and how the

decisions made during planning influence actions during instruction.


Lester

Category 3: Classroom Processes

Classroom processes include the host of teacher and student actions and

interactions that take place during instruction. Four dimensions of classroom processes

are apparent: teacher knowledge and affects; teacher behaviors; student knowledge and

affects; and student behaviors.

Both the teacher's and the students' thinking processes and behaviors during

instruction are almost always directed toward achieving a number of different goals,

sometimes simultaneously. For example, during a lesson the teacher may be assessing

the appropriateness of the small-group arrangement that was established prior to the

lesson, while at the same time trying to guide the students' thinking toward the solution to

a problem. Similarly, a student may be thinking about what her classmates will think if

she never contributes to discussions and at the same time be trying to understand what the

task confronting her is all about. In our framework, we have restricted consideration to

what the teacher thinks about and does to facilitate the students’ thinking and what the

student thinks about and does to solve a problem. We have not attempted to include a

complete menu of objects or goals a teacher might think about during instruction.

Teacher knowledge and affects. These processes include those attitudes, beliefs,

emotions, cognitions and metacognitions that influence, and are influenced by, the

multitude of teacher and student behaviors that occur in the classroom during instruction.

In particular, this dimension is concerned with the teacher's thinking and affects while

facilitating students’ attempts to understand a task, develop a plan for solving it, carry

out the plan to obtain an answer, and look back over the solution effort.
TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .269

Teacher behaviors. A teacher's knowledge and affects that operate during

instruction give rise to the teacher's behaviors, the overt actions taken by the teacher

during problem-solving instruction. Specific teacher behaviors can be studied with

regard to use (or non-use) as well as quality. The quality of a teacher behavior can

include, among other things, the correctness of the behavior (e.g., correct mathematically

or correct given the conditions of the problem), the clarity of the action (e.g., a clear

question or hint), and the manner in which the behavior was delivered (e.g., the verbal

and nonverbal communication style of the teacher).

Student knowledge and affects. Similar to the teacher, this subcategory refers to

the knowledge and affects that interact with teacher and student behaviors. The concern

here is with how students interpret the behavior of the teacher and how the students'

thinking about a problem, their affects, and their work on the problem affects their own

behavior. Also of concern here is how instructional influences such as task features or

contextual conditions directly affect a student's knowledge, affects, and behaviors.

Student behaviors. These behaviors include the overt actions of the student

during a classroom problem-solving episode. By restricting our attention to the problem-

solving phases mentioned earlier, we can identify several behaviors students might

exhibit as they work on a task.

Category 4: Instructional Outcomes

The fourth category of factors consists of three types of outcomes of instruction:

student outcomes, teacher outcomes, and incidental outcomes. Most instruction-related

research has been concerned with short-term effects only. Furthermore, transfer effects,
Lester

effects on attitudes, beliefs, and emotions, and changes in teacher behavior have been

considered only rarely.

Student outcomes. Both immediate and long-term effects on student learning are

included in this category, as are transfer effects (both near and far transfer). Illustrative

of a student outcome, either immediate or long-term, is a change in a student's skill in

implementing a particular problem-solving strategy (e.g., guess and check, working

backwards). An example of a transfer effect is a change in students' performance in

solving non-mathematics problems as a result of solving only mathematics problems.

Also, of special importance is the consideration of changes in students' beliefs and

attitudes about problem solving or about themselves as problem solvers and the effect of

problem-solving instruction on mathematical skill and concept learning; for example,

how is computational skill affected by increased emphasis on the thinking processes

involved in solving problems?

Teacher outcomes. Teachers, of course, also change as a result of their

instructional efforts. In particular, their attitudes and beliefs, the nature and extent of

their planning, as well as their classroom behavior during subsequent instruction are all

subject to change. Each problem-solving episode a teacher participates in changes her or

his craft knowledge. Thus, it is reasonable to expect that experience affects the teacher's

planning, thinking, affects, and actions in future situations.

Incidental outcomes. Increased performance in science (or some other subject

area) and heightened parental interest in their children's school work are two examples of

possible incidental outcomes. Although it is not possible to predetermine the relevant


TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .271

incidental effects of instruction, it is important to be mindful of the potential for

unexpected “side effects.”

Research on teaching in general points to the important role a teacher's knowledge

and affects play in instruction. Questions such as the following need to be investigated :

What knowledge (in particular, content, pedagogical, and curriculum knowledge) do

teachers need to be effective as teachers of problem solving? How is that knowledge best

structured to be useful to teachers? How do teachers' beliefs about themselves, their

students, teaching mathematics, and problem solving influence the decisions they make

prior to and during instruction?

The forgoing analysis of factors to be considered for research on problem-solving

instruction is intended as a general framework for designing investigations of what

actually happens in the classroom during instruction. As I mentioned earlier, there may

be other important factors to be included in this framework and that certain of the factors

may prove to be relatively unimportant. Notwithstanding these possible shortcomings,

this framework could serve as a step in the direction of making research in the area more

fruitful and relevant.

A Final, More Positive Note

I do not intend for my remarks to give the impression that I think mathematical

problem solving research has not amounted to much during the past 40 years or that

current research efforts are misguided. Indeed, quite the opposite is the case! Several

important principles have slowly emerged from the research since the early 1970s. I end

this article by listing these principles without comment: each principle could serve as the
Lester

basis for an article or monograph. My hope is that this list, like much of the rest of my

article, will stimulate discussion among those who are interested in pursuing a research

agenda that includes problem solving at its core.

1. The prolonged engagement principle. In order for students to improve their

ability to solve mathematics problems, they must engage in work on

problematic tasks on a regular basis, over a prolonged period of time.

2. The task variety principle. Students will improve as problem solvers only if

they are given opportunities to solve a variety of types of problematic tasks (in

my view, principles 1 and 2 are the most important of the seven).

3. The complexity principle. There is a dynamic interaction between

mathematical concepts and the processes (including metacognitive ones) used

to solve problems involving those concepts. That is, heuristics, skills, control

processes, and awareness of one’s own thinking develop concurrently with the

development of an understanding of mathematical concepts. (This principle

tells us that problem- solving ability is best developed when it takes place in

the context of learning important mathematics concepts.)

4. The systematic organization principle. Problem-solving instruction,

metacognition instruction in particular, is likely to be most effective when it is

provided in a systematically organized manner under the direction of the

teacher.

5. The multiple roles for the teacher principle. Problem-solving instruction that

emphasizes the development of metacognitive skills should involve the

teacher in three different, but related, roles: (a) as an external monitor, (b) as a
TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .273

facilitator of students' metacognitive awareness, and (c) as a model of a

metacognitively-adept problem solver.

6. The group interaction principle. The standard arrangement for classroom

instructional activities is for students to work in small groups (usually groups

of three or four). Small group work is especially appropriate for activities

involving new content (e.g., new mathematics topics, new problem-solving

strategies) or when the focus of the activity is on the process of solving

problems (e.g., planning, decision making, assessing progress) or exploring

mathematical ideas.

7. The assessment principle. The teacher's instructional plan should include

attention to how students' performance is to be assessed. In order for students

to become convinced of the importance of the sort of behaviors that a good

problem-solving program promotes, it is necessary to use assessment

techniques that reward such behaviors.

References

Ball, D. L., Thames, M. H., & Phelps, G. (2008). Content knowledge for teaching: What
makes it special? Journal of Teacher Education, 59, 389-408.

Cai, J. (2010). Helping elementary students become successful mathematical problem


solvers. In D. V. Lambdin & F. K. Lester (Eds.), Teaching and learning
Lester

mathematics: Translating research for elementary teachers (pp. 9-14). Reston, VA:
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

Charles, R., & Lester, F. (1982). Teaching problem solving: What, why and how. Palo
Alto, CA: Dale Seymour Publications.

Charles, R.I. & Lester, F. K. (1984). An evaluation of a process-oriented mathematical


problem-solving instructional program in grades five and seven. Journal for Research
in Mathematics Education, 15(1), 15-34.

Collins, A., Brown, J., & Newmann, S. (1990). Cognitive apprenticeship: Teaching
students the craft of reading, writing, and mathematics. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.),
Knowing, learning and instruction: Essays in honor of Robert Glaser (pp. 453-494).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Davis, B. (1997). Listening for differences: An evolving conception of mathematics


teaching. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 28, 355-376.

DiMatteo, R. W. & Lester, F. K. (2010). The role of problem solving in the secondary
school mathematics classroom. In J. Lobato & F. Lester (Eds.). Teaching and
learning mathematics: Translating research for secondary teachers (pp. 7-12).
Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 58(5), Whole No.


270.

Frensch, P. A. & Funke, J. (Eds.). (1995). Complex problem solving: The European
perspective. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Garofalo, J. & Lester, F. K. (1985). Metacognition, cognitive monitoring, and


mathematical performance. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 16(3),
163-176.

Goldin, G. A. (2003). Representation in school mathematics: A unifying research


perspective. In J. Kilpatrick, W. G. Martin, & D. Schifter (Eds.), A research
TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .275

companion to Principles and Standards for School Mathematics (pp. 275-285).


Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

Goldin, G. A. & McClintock, C. E. (Eds.). (1984). Task variables in mathematical


problem solving. Philadelphia, PA: Franklin Institute Press.

Gresalfi, M. S. & Lester, F. (2009). What’s worth knowing in mathematics? In S.


Tobias & T. M. Duffy (Eds.), Constructivist instruction: Success or failure? (pp. 264-
290). New York: Routledge.

Hiebert, J. (2003). Signposts for teaching mathematics through problem solving. In F.


K. Lester & R. I. Charles (Eds.), Teaching mathematics through problem solving:
Prekindergarten – grade 6. (pp. 53-62). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers
of Mathematics.

Hiebert, J., Carpenter, T. P., Fennema, E., Fuson, K., Wearne, D., Murray, H., Olivier,
A., & Human, P. (1997). Making sense: Teaching and learning mathematics with
understanding. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

Hill, H. C., Sleep, L., Lewis, J. M., & Ball, D. L. (2007). Assessing teachers’
mathematical knowledge: What knowledge matters and what evidence counts? In F.
K. Lester (Ed.), Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning
(vol. 1, pp. 111-155). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.

Holth, P. (2008). What is a problem? Theoretical conceptions and methodological


approaches to the study of problem solving. European Journal of Behavior Analysis,
9(2), 157-172.

Kahney, H. (1993). Problem solving: Current issues (2nd edition). Buckingham, UK:
Open University Press.

Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during
instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery,
problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist,
41, 75-86.
Lester

Lesh, R. & Zawojewski, J. (2007). Problem solving and modeling. In F. K. Lester (Ed.),
Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp.763-804).
Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishers.

Lester, F. K. (2005). On the theoretical, conceptual, and philosophical foundations for


research in mathematics education. Zentralblatt fur Didaktik der Mathematik, 37(6),
457-467.

Lester, F. K. & Charles, R. I. (1992). A framework for research on mathematical problem


solving. In J. P. Ponte, J. F. Matos, J. M. Matos, & D. Fernandes (Eds.), Issues in
mathematical problem solving and new information technologies (pp. 1 – 15). Berlin:
Springer-Verlag.

Lester, F. K. & Charles, R. I. (Eds.). (2003). Teaching mathematical problem solving:


prekindergarten – grade 6. Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of
Mathematics.

Lester, F. K., Garofalo, J., & Kroll, D. (1989). The role of metacognition in
mathematical problem solving: A study of two grade seven classes. Final report to
the National Science Foundation of NSF project MDR 85-50346.

Lester, F. K. & Kehle, P. (2003). From problem solving to modeling: The evolution of
thinking about research on complex mathematical activity. In R. Lesh & H. M. Doerr
(Eds.), Beyond constructivism: Models and modeling perspectives on mathematics
problem solving, learning, and teaching (pp. 501-517). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Moore, D. S. (1995). The craft of teaching. MAA Focus, 15(2), 5-8.

Moreira, P. C., & David, M. M. (2008). Academic mathematics and mathematical


knowledge needed in school teaching practice: Some conflicting elements. Journal of
Mathematics Teacher Education, 11, 23-40.
TME, vol10, nos.1&2, p .277

Pearson Education, Inc. (2011). Connected Mathematics2. Retrieved July 28, 2011 from,
http://www.phschool.com/cmp2/implementing/

Schoen, H. L. & Charles, R. I. (Eds.). Teaching mathematical problem solving: Grades 6


– 12. Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

Schoenfeld, A. H. (1992). Learning to think mathematically: Problem solving,


metacognition, and sense-making in mathematics. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook
of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 334-370). New York:
MacMillan.

Schroeder, T. L. & Lester, F. K. (1989). Understanding mathematics via problem solving.


In P. Trafton (Ed.), New directions for elementary school mathematics (pp. 31-42).
Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

Skinner, B. F. (1966). An operant analysis of problem solving. In B. Kleinmetz (Ed.),


Problem solving: Research, method, and theory (pp. 225-257). New York: Wiley.

Stein, M. K., Boaler, J. & Silver, E. A. (2003). Teaching mathematics through problem
solving: Research perspectives. In H. L. Schoen & R. I. Charles (Eds.), Teaching
mathematics through problem solving: Grades 6-12 (pp. 245–256). Reston, VA:
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

Stephan, M. & Whitenack, J. (2003). Establishing classroom social and


sociomathematical norms for problem solving. In F. K. Lester & R. I. Charles (Eds.),
Teaching mathematics through problem solving: Prekindergarten – grade 6 (pp. 149-
162). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

Van de Walle, J. (2003). Designing and selecting problematic tasks. In F. K. Lester & R.
I. Charles (Eds.), Teaching mathematics through problem solving: Prekindergarten -
grade 6 (pp. 67-80). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.

Yackel, E. (2003). Listening to children: Informing us and guiding our instruction. In F.


K. Lester & R. I. Charles (Eds.), Teaching mathematics through problem solving:
Lester

Prekindergarten - grade 6 (pp. 107-121). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers


of Mathematics.

Zazkis, R., & Leikin, R. (2010). Advanced mathematical knowledge in teaching practice:
Perceptions of secondary mathematics teachers. Mathematical Thinking and
Learning, 12(4), 263-281.

You might also like