Wiley - Color Space & Its Divisions
Wiley - Color Space & Its Divisions
Rolf G. Kuehni
Limit of Liability/Disclaimer of Warranty: While the publisher and author have used their best
efforts in preparing this book, they make no representations or warranties with respect to the
accuracy or completeness of the contents of this book and specifically disclaim any implied
warranties of merchantability or fitness for a particular purpose. No warranty may be created
or extended by sales representatives or written sales materials. The advice and strategies
contained herein may not be suitable for your situation. You should consult with a professional
where appropriate. Neither the publisher nor author shall be liable for any loss of profit or any
other commercial damages, including but not limited to special, incidental, consequential, or
other damages.
For general information on our other products and services please contact our Customer
Care Department within the U.S. at 877-762-2974, outside the U.S. at 317-572-3993 or
fax 317-572-4002.
Wiley also publishes its books in a variety of electronic formats. Some content that appears in
print, however, may not be available in electronic format.
Kuehni, Rolf G.
Color space and its divisions : color order from antiquity to the present / Rolf G. Kuehni.
p. cm.
Includes index.
ISBN 0-471-32670-4 (cloth : acid-free paper)
1. Color vision. 2. Colors. I. Title.
QP483 .K84 2003
152.14¢5—dc21 2002014045
10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
To the memory of Dorothy Nickerson and David L. MacAdam, and to
Andreas Brockes for their encouragement to continue the pursuit of color
order and color difference
Preface xii
vii
viii CONTENTS
Notes 361
Glossary 366
References 375
Credits 399
Index 403
Preface
Fig. 2-12 C.B.’s hand-painted color circles, 1708. Left: The seven-color circle; right: the
twelve-color circle. Note that the pigments used for some of the colors have deteriorated.
Color Space and Its Divisions: Color Order from Antiquity to the Present, by Rolf G. Kuehni
ISBN 0-471-32670-4 Copyright © 2003 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Fig. 2-13 Continuous color circle of Ignaz Schiffermüller, with twelve classes of colors, 1771.
Fig. 2-14 Schiffermüller’s tonal scales of three blues, from his 1771 work. Some of the col-
orations have deteriorated.
Fig. 2-18 Depiction of Lambert’s color pyramid, 1772. The lowest level contains the 45 colors
identified in Fig. 2-17. The higher levels contain reduced sets at higher lightness, ending in
white on top of the pyramid. Black is located on the lowest level. The colors displayed on the
front of the model represent well-known artist’s pigments of the time.
Fig. 2-19 Prismatic version of Moses Harris’s color circle of 1786. Some deterioration of col-
orants is evident.
Fig. 2-22 Hand-colored copperplate of Runge’s Farben-Kugel (1810). Views toward the white
and black poles are on top. The equatorial cross section is on bottom left and the polar cross
section on the right. There are four saturation steps between the full color on the surface and
the middle gray in the center of the sphere.
Fig. 2-25 One of twelve chromatic scales by Chevreul, with local discoloration. The colors
range from white through the full color (grade 11) to black. These scales are located on the
base plane of the hemisphere.
Fig. 2-32 Hering’s diagram illustrating the composition of mixed hue perceptions from the
unique hues located on the main axes. The fractions of blue and red of three mixed hues are
shown bottom right, 1905–1911.
Fig. 2-34 View toward the top, white corner of Benson’s tilted color cube, 1868. The gray
scale is hidden behind the white sphere.
Fig. 2-41 Artist’s rendition of Munsell’s balanced color sphere, patented in 1900. The sphere
was rotatable to achieve additive color mixture to gray and thereby show the “balance” of the
colors on the sphere. The mirror in the back discloses the blue region of the sphere.
Fig. 2-42 Constant hue page from a modern version of the Munsell Book of Color. The gray
scale is not shown. The chroma scale begins at 1 and continues from 2 at two-grade intervals
to chroma 14. Value grades are shown from 2.5 to 9. Courtesy Gretag-Macbeth Company.
Fig. 2-43 Page from Ridgway’s color atlas showing three reddish blue hues lightened from
the central color in three steps toward white and darkened in four steps toward black, 1912.
Fig. 2-45 Vertical cross section through Ostwald’s double-cone color solid illustrating constant hue colors 1 and 13, with veiling toward white
and black. The achromatic scale is at the center. From Farbkörper, undated.
Fig. 2-51 View of MacAdam’s model of the OSA-UCS color solid illustrating the existence of
several cleavage planes. See text for more detail. Slide courtesy D. L. MacAdam.
Fig. 2-53 NCS constant hue triangle of hue Y90R with full color C, white W, and black
S. Colors of constant blackness s are located on lines parallel to W–C, colors of constant
chromaticness c fall on lines parallel to W–S. Courtesy NCS.
Fig. 2-54 Color Picker screen from Adobe® Photoshop showing specification of a given reddish blue
color in four systems: HSB, Lab (=L*, a*, b*), RGB, and CMYK.
Fig. 7-16 Image of the 43 hexagonal enamel color plates used by the OSA-UCS committee
to establish the fundamental perceptual data for the system. Note that there is no achromatic
color among them.
Chapter 1
The Concept of Color
Space and Color Solid
1.1 INTRODUCTION
Attempting to understand our place in the world and classifying things and
experiences is a well-known human trait. Already ancient Greek philosophers
thought about the multitude of color perceptions, but they despaired of finding
a system in which to place them. First, colors were logically sorted according
to lightness, regardless of hue. Early in the second millennium we begin to find
descriptions of tonal scales of individual hues or mixed tones, like flesh color.
They were achieved by adding lighter or darker pigments of similar hue, even
black or white, to saturated chromatic pigments. Systematic hue circles began
to appear in the late seventeenth century. The concept of a three-dimensional
logical arrangement of color perceptions began to take shape only in the eigh-
teenth century.
Color space is a three-dimensional geometric space with axes appropriately
defined so that symbols for all possible color perceptions of humans or other
animals fit into it in an order corresponding to the psychological order. In this
space each color perception is represented as a point. The symbolic represen-
tations of color perceptions in this space form the color solid. The earliest pro-
posals for color solids had simple geometrical forms: triangular double
pyramid, sphere, cone, and so forth. There is, of course, no a priori reason why
a systematic arrangement of color perceptions should fit into a simple geo-
metrical solid. What controls the form of the solid is the definition of the axes
of the space and their divisions.
Color Space and Its Divisions: Color Order from Antiquity to the Present, by Rolf G. Kuehni
ISBN 0-471-32670-4 Copyright © 2003 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
1
2 THE CONCEPT OF COLOR SPACE AND COLOR SOLID
place in the world, the quest was shaped by technical and economic issues of
color control of manufactured colored goods.
A color space belongs in the domain of psychology. The description of
stimuli that under standard conditions result in perception of colors in that
space is an aspect of physics. Together they form the uneasy domain of psy-
chophysics that attempts to connect stimuli with perceptions (see Chapter 3).
The stimuli are messages to us from the outside world. An alternative view is
that we actively search for them when viewing the world. They enter through
the pupils of the eyes and are absorbed by the retinal layer. There they trigger
a complex chain of events that result in our perceptions. These events
belong into the domain of neuroscience and are part of the conundrum of
consciousness.
The number of different color experiences we can have is unknown, but
large. Given a particular starting point in color space the finest perceptual divi-
sion of color space is represented by visual threshold increments deviating
from that point in all directions. A color space of given definition can only be
expressed in terms of differences within the related color solid against a
chosen surround because it is only applicable to those conditions. The small-
est difference in a color solid as related to a given starting point, therefore,
consists of a pair of different color stimuli displayed against a particular
(usually neutral) surround and seen as having a just perceptible difference.
Generally, a color space and the related color solid may be defined as an
economic systematic description of subjective color experiences, and as such
it is not subject to engineering precision. It is indicative of our visual strate-
gies vis-à-vis the world.
of unique hues and, to less extent, from color perceptions judged to be inter-
mediate between unique hues. Unique hues are those four primary hues that
do not contain perceptual components of other hues. A unique red hue is
neither yellowish nor bluish: it is just red. Color stimuli resulting in unique hue
perception vary among color normal observers.3 This variation depends on the
hue in question. It ranges approximately from 5% to 12% of the total hue vari-
ation in a hue circle experienced under standard viewing conditions (i.e.,
approximately two to five Munsell 40-hue steps; see Chapters 2 and 7 for infor-
mation on the Munsell system). Because of the absence of unambiguous cri-
teria, it is not possible to meaningfully assess the stimulus variability for other
hues. It is quite evident that there is also variability in the experience of gray
scale steps, in adaptation and constancy response and other visual mechanisms,
resulting in considerable variability of individual experience when looking at
a given scene of color stimuli. Persons with impaired color vision have implicit
color spaces significantly different from those of color normal observers.
Their nature cannot be conveyed with certainty. Theoretical considerations
of the genetics of color vision indicate that as much as 50% of the female pop-
ulation have the potential for four rather than the normal three cone types
even though none has so far been identified as having four cone types.4 Richer
color experiences than those had by standard trichromatic observers have
recently been determined for females with the genetic potential for four cone
types. In how many ways their color experiences are richer remains to be
determined.
province of color appearance modeling, evolving rapidly in the last ten years
and having developed several mathematical models that are still compara-
tively simple and correspondingly only modestly accurate. This is expressed to
some extent by the fact that several different modeling approaches can result
in about the same level of prediction accuracy. An excellent survey of color
appearance modeling has recently been provided by MD. Fairchild (1998). As
indicated, the present text is concerned with color appearance under limited
conditions only.
Color spaces and solids are always expressed in terms of differences of some
kind between color perceptions. As will be shown, there are various kinds of
differences that have been proposed for color spaces. A kind of color space of
particular interest is one in which distances in the solid in all directions are
proportional to the magnitude of perceived differences between the related
color experiences. Such a space can be built from (or divided into) threshold
differences or larger differences. Differences imply scales, and there are
several different kinds of scales possible. The primary scales are psychological
or perceptual. Such scales are built on the basis of perceptual attributes. A
logical expectation is that the perceptual attributes form the axes of the space.
For simple observation situations (uniform achromatic surround and defined
light source) three attributes are sufficient to define the perceived color of an
object. If its dependence on surround and illumination is to be considered
quantitatively, additional attributes are required (see Chapter 4). We will find,
however, that all possible hue perceptions are best ordered in a circle and that
the hue attribute, therefore, is a function of two dimensions of the space.
A psychological color solid and the space into which it fits can be built from
a very large number of color samples. It requires picking the appropriate
samples to represent the chosen type and size of difference. Once the task is
complete, the selected samples represent the solid and the space. This is not a
generally satisfactory solution because producing many copies of the solid
requires large sheets of uniformly colored materials. Our inability to define
color experiences from an object verbally or by some other subjective means
with a high degree of accuracy and precision makes it desirable to use objec-
tive means of defining the color samples. Weight of colorants in a mixture has
been used in the earliest attempts at illustrating color scales, (e.g., F. Glisson,
Chapter 2). With the development of photometry in the eighteenth century
and colorimetry in the late nineteenth century, physical and psychophysical
means of specifying color stimuli became available. It quickly was learned that
in a given set of conditions the relationship between measured stimuli and per-
ceptions is not linear, and the next task was to develop models of the rela-
tionship between physical properties of stimuli and resulting perceived color.
This is the domain of psychophysics. This branch of psychology developed
DIVISIONS OF COLOR SPACES AND SOLIDS 7
Fig. 1-1 Images of chips of the OSA-UCS system. Left: Color 000; center: color 00-4; right:
color 00-8. (See color plate.)
red or twice as black as another color. The difficulties involved can be visual-
ized by comparing, say, OSA-UCS chips 000, 00-4, and 00-8 (Fig. 1-1). If the
OSA-UCS greenness–redness scale could be considered a ratio scale, the
statement describing the g-8 chip as twice as red as the g-4 chip should apply.
But many observers, including the author, are not prepared to agree with such
a statement.
Once a color solid has been perceptually developed for a specific set of con-
ditions, the selected stimuli/samples can be defined physically by spectral
power or reflectance measurements. The next step is to build a mathematical
model connecting the physical with the psychological data in a manner result-
ing in perfect or near perfect agreement between the two sets. As will be seen
in Chapter 6, much effort has been devoted to finding the mathematical defi-
nition of a uniform psychophysical color space. There are a number of prob-
lems and difficulties with such efforts. They begin with the difficulties or
impossibility of creating an euclidean geometrical model of a uniform psy-
chological color space. In addition the physical definition of samples and spec-
tral power distributions, representative of the observed objects, is not without
problems.
redness. In the latter case the space is uniform only in terms of the chosen
attributes but not in terms of perceived differences. It is useful to reserve
the term “uniform” for the former situation and use another term, perhaps
“regular color space” (Hering space for the Hering-inspired version), for the
latter.
As will be seen in Chapter 2, the concept of uniform color solid has a long
history. In the seventeenth century Glisson attempted to develop a gray (light-
ness) scale and three tonal color scales with visually equidistant steps with
which to specify the colors of objects. T. Mayer, J. H. Lambert, and P. O. Runge
in the eighteenth and nineteenth centuries were already thinking in terms of
visually uniform steps between the scale points of their color solids. Mayer
appears to have been the first to propose a three-dimensional color solid. H.
von Helmholtz was the first to attempt to find the relationship between phys-
ical measurements of the stimulus and a perceptually uniform space.
W. Ostwald, apparently through a misunderstanding of Helmholtz’s concept
of brightness, decided to use Hering’s blackness and whiteness as the two
attributes that together with chromatic color, form the color perception. He
used Hering’s equilateral triangular template to arrange all color perceptions
of a given hue. In this template, in the tradition of Runge, W. Wundt, and
Hering, lightness is not an attribute and chromaticness of all full colors (pure
pigment or maximal color) is considered perceptually equal. In regard to chro-
maticness the result is that the perceptual magnitude of chromaticness steps
depends on hue. In addition Ostwald decided that the Weber-Fechner law was
applicable regardless of size of color difference, and he scaled the grades in
the hue triangle accordingly.
Munsell introduced a radical philosophical departure from the German
school by using the three attributes lightness, hue, and chroma. His chromat-
icness measure, the chroma unit, is in principle of equal perceptual magnitude
regardless of hue, and it is defined in terms of (imperfectly defined) constant
perceived lightness. While he originally constrained his color solid into the
form of a sphere, Munsell soon learned from experiments that when building
his system from the central gray midpoint perceptual uniformity was not com-
patible with the complete color solid having a spherical form. The result was
the irregular shape of the Munsell “color tree.” Munsell’s successors continued
to refine the scaling of the three attributes, the last accepted revision being the
Munsell Renotations. In the Renotations perceptual data were “smoothed” to
some degree in terms of psychophysical data.
A major reason for the development of early forms of color solids, as
Chapter 2 will show, was to have a basis for discovering systematic rules of
color harmony. This desire was behind the efforts of Runge, O. N. Rood,
Munsell, Ostwald, and others (Schwarz, 1999). An American version of
Ostwald’s system was called Color Harmony Manual. Even though claims of
having discovered universal rules of color harmony have been discredited,
there has been a continuing discussion on the usefulness of the various systems
10 THE CONCEPT OF COLOR SPACE AND COLOR SOLID
for the purposes of art and design. Ostwald strove to make his system deriv-
able from additive color mixture data, thus developing a system that attempted
to combine perceptual psychology, psychophysics, and harmony.
The Swedish Natural Color System (NCS) is a modern interpretation of
Hering’s ideas. It was derived on a purely psychological basis using presumed
innate concepts of Hering type Vollfarben (full colors) with unique hues, black-
ness, and whiteness. Psychophysical measures were used only to specify color
grade samples exemplifying the system under a specific set of conditions. The
attributes of this Hering or Ostwald type of system are hue, expressed by quad-
rant in terms of one or two unique hues, blackness, and whiteness (or hue,
blackness, and chromaticness). The double-cone geometrical form of systems
such as Ostwald’s and NCS’s appears to imply conventional definitions of the
geometrical dimensions. But by placing all full colors on the periphery of
the central disk of the double cone and a perceptually uniform gray scale on
the central vertical axis, the meaning of the vertical dimension in these systems
is not defined. As a result the steps are not uniform in the sense defined above
but regular. The practical value of such systems must be found in principles
other than uniformity of difference.
The Munsell system on the other hand, as mentioned, is based on the psy-
chological attributes hue, as expressed in terms of five primary hues, value
(lightness), and chroma (saturation). Munsell’s original intent was to repre-
sent a uniform color space. However, by concentrating on planes of constant
hue, he neglected the changes in hue difference as a function of chroma and
lightness between adjacent constant hue planes. A uniform version of the
Munsell system is impossible to fit into a euclidean system as will be shown.
By disregarding the issue of relative perceptual size of hue and chroma dif-
ference steps, the Munsell system is simply accepted as fitting a polar system.
In this system the polar angle, radial distance, and distance from the origin in
the third dimension have defined meanings: hue, chroma, and lightness; but
the units are of different perceptual size (in the case of hue also as a function
of chroma). This was experimentally determined in the 1930s. According to D.
Nickerson’s index of fading formula (1936), one unit of value difference is
equal to two units of chroma difference and, at chroma 5, to three 100-step
units of hue difference.
In 1943, based on the then newly available calculations by D. L. MacAdam
of the object color limits, Nickerson and S. Newhall calculated two three-
dimensional models of the psychological Munsells solid (Fig. 1-2).8 They are
approximations of a uniform psychological solid under two different observa-
tional conditions without among other things, considering the matter of the
relationship between unit hue and chroma differences. They were described
as fulfilling the following requirements: “Dimensional scales . . . calibrated in
perceptually uniform steps; units of the several scales . . . equated; the surface
of the solid . . . represents all colors of maximum saturation; the difference and
volume . . . representative of all colors which are perceptibly different; condi-
tions of stimulation or viewing . . . described; and, finally, the scales . . . stan-
UNIFORM AND REGULAR COLOR SPACES 11
Fig. 1-2 Models of Nickerson and Newhall’s psychological color solid, based on the Munsell
system. The two figures represent color solids based on large and small perceived differences.
Left: At the level of Munsell Book of Color differences. Right: At the just noticeable difference
level.
Fig. 1-3 Vertical sections in five different hue planes through the color solid that represents
the Munsell system extrapolated to the optimal color limits. The inner borders delineate the
space filled by samples of the system.
imately 720 degrees. Judd attributed this result to experimental error, and
Godlove (1951) wrote a formula that reduces the magnitude of hue differ-
ences so that the Munsell equal lightness psychological data map onto a plane.
Toward the end of his life Judd reconsidered his view, and there is now sig-
nificant additional evidence indicating that the Nickerson formula is approx-
imately correct. Full clarification requires further psychological scaling.
UNIFORM AND REGULAR COLOR SPACES 13
1. Evidence for curvature of color space from the MacAdam ellipse data.
2. Superimportance of hue as indicated in the Nickerson formula.
3. Diminishing returns in color difference perception.
4. Influence of surround color.
One of the definitions in the Oxford English Dictionary, not without its diffi-
culties, of the word color is “The quality or attribute in virtue of which objects
present different appearances to the eye, when considered with regard only to
the kind of light reflected from their surfaces.”The definition is symptomatic of
the age-old problems of clearly describing this psychological quality. But we all
(those of us with normal color vision) know colors when we encounter them.
In 1912 the philosopher B. Russell expressed himself on the subject as follows
“. . . truths about the colour do not make me know the colour itself better than
I did before . . . I know the color perfectly and completely when I see it. . . .”
The conventional view of colors is that they are the result of standard
stimuli associated with them. I. Newton’s seven primary hues (R O Y G B I
V) are the result of spectral stimuli of particular wavelengths (as seen in a
dark surround). In color technology certain pigment or dye combinations
result in certain color perceptions when viewed under a standard light in a
light booth. That the appearance may change significantly if we change the
light source or the surround is described in terms of color inconstancy. The
idea that there is a standard stimulus for a given color perception has often
been questioned (independent of the issue of metamerism). In the 1960s the
inventor of the Polaroid photographic process, E. Land, sharply delineated the
limitations of such views. In natural scenes the colors assigned to a given spec-
tral power distribution entering the eye depend on the spectral structure of
the total visual field according to rules that are not yet understood. It does not
Color Space and Its Divisions: Color Order from Antiquity to the Present, by Rolf G. Kuehni
ISBN 0-471-32670-4 Copyright © 2003 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
19
20 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
seem appropriate to talk about color illusions when the implied stimulus/per-
ception association is violated. Colors perceived in any given situation are the
real colors. Our mind constructs them on the basis of its interpretation of the
total visual field as well as of items in this field that appear to belong together.
This view causes severe problems for the idea of a color order system in which
the specimens are defined by their reflectances. On the other hand, there
appears to be a finite limit in number of color perceptions that the color
normal observer can experience when seen as colors of objects.There is a legit-
imate question as to how one can systematically demonstrate these percep-
tions. The answer is that we have to select a standard set of conditions in which
to view the objects in order to be able to more closely relate stimuli and per-
ceptions. This is the approach taken implicitly or explicitly by all developers
of physical color order systems. After the materials and the observation situ-
ation have been fixed the variability of color normal observers comes into
focus. As a result more recent efforts in development of color order systems
always involved average data from many observers. From this perspective,
extensive relativization of the viewing circumstances to tightly controlled con-
ditions, relating color perceptions to particular spectral stimuli has a degree of
validity. Any claim to general validity is erroneous, however.
There are two situations in which it is meaningful to associate a color per-
ception with a specific stimulus:
Pre-Platonists
Among the oldest extant uses of color words are those by the poet, theolo-
gian, and natural philosopher Xenophanes, active in the sixth century bc. He
explained all things to have come from water and earth and commented on
the rainbow: “And she whom they call Iris, she too is actually a cloud, purple
and flame-red and yellow to behold.”2
Pythagoras (ca. 582–507 bc) philosopher and founder of a religious broth-
erhood is credited by his followers as having discovered that the relationship
between musical notes could be expressed with numbers, as could any other
relationship. Of considerable importance to the Pythagoreans was the tetrac-
tys, their name for the sum of the first four numbers, regarded as the source
of all things. According to Philolaus, a disciple of Pythagoras, he equated colors
with the number 5. Plutarch remarks on the views of the Pythagoreans on
color: “[They] called the surface of a solid chroma, that means color. Addi-
tionally, they named the species of color, white, black, red and yellow. They
looked for the cause of the differences in color in various mixtures of the ele-
ments, the manifold colors of animals, however, in their nutrients as well as
the climatic regions.”3
Empedocles (ca. 492–432 bc), a Sicilian philosopher and politician, is cred-
ited with the view that everything in existence is composed of four indestruc-
tible elements: earth, fire, water, and air. On color Empedocles is quoted as
follows: (after Simplicius, 28) “As when painters decorate temple-offerings
with colors—men who, following their intelligence, are well-skilled in their
craft—these, when they take many-colored pigments in their hands, and have
mixed them in harmony, taking more of some, less of another, create from
them forms like all things. . . .” (after Aetios, 132) “Empedokles explained
color as that fitting into the pores of the visual organ. The multiplicity of colors,
so he said, derive from the fixed mixtures of the elements. That there are four
colors, exactly as many as elements: white [leukhyn], black [melan], red
[erydron], yellow-green [ochron].”4
Democritus (ca. 460–? bc) expanded the atomistic theory of his predeces-
sor Leucippus. Among many other works he is reported to have written a text
22 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
Plato
Plato (427?–347 bc), philosopher and mathematician, friend and pupil of
Socrates, founder of the Academy, was one of the preeminent figures in Greek
philosophy. His influence remains active today. Plato made several statements
about colors in his dialogues. Concerning primary colors, and their mixtures
his key statement is found in Timaeus (67, 68):
. . . we ought to term white that which dilates the visual ray, and the opposite of
this black. . . . in (the eye) the fire, mingling with the ray of the moisture, pro-
duces a color like blood, to which we give the name red. A bright hue mingled
with red and white gives the color auburn [xandon]. The law of proportion,
however, according to which the several colors are formed, even if a man knew
he would be foolish in telling, for he could not give any necessary reason, nor
indeed any tolerable or probable explanation of them. Again, red when mingled
with black and white, becomes purple, but it becomes umber [orphninon] when
the colors are burned as well as mingled and the black more thoroughly mixed
with them. Flame color [pyrron] is produced by a union of auburn and dun
[phaion], dun by an admixture of black and white; and pale yellow [ochron] by
an admixture of white and auburn. White and bright meeting and falling upon a
full black, become dark blue [kyanoyn], and when the dark blue mingles with
white a light blue [glaykon] color is formed as flame color with black makes leek-
green [prasion]. There will be no difficulty in seeing how and by what mixtures
the colors derived from these are made according to the rules of probability.
Plato’s color mixture scheme is shown graphically in Fig. 2-1. Note that the
classification into primary, and so on, colors is by the author.
Aristotle
Aristotle (384–322 bc) was a student of Plato at the Academy. After teaching
Alexander the Great he founded a school in the Lyceum in Athens. Together
with Plato, Aristotle had an enormous influence on philosophical thinking
FROM ANCIENT GREECE TO THE MIDDLE AGES 23
Fig. 2-1 Plato’s color mixture scheme. The four primary experiences are bright, white, red and
black. Additions of these in various combinations form the secondary and later stage mixture
colors.
during the next 2000 years. In regard to basic colors and color scales
Aristotle expressed himself on more than one occasion and not always in the
same way.
In Sense and Sensibilia (442a20 ff): “Savors and colors contain respectively
about the same number of species. For there are seven species of each, if, as
is reasonable, we regard gray as a variety of black [melanon] (for the alterna-
tive is that yellow [xandon] should be classed with white [leukhon], as rich with
sweet); while crimson [phoinikoyn], violet [aloyrgon], leek-green [prasinon]
and deep blue [kyanoyn], come between white and black, and from these all
others are derived by mixture.”
Earlier he stated: (439b20 ff):
Publisher's Note:
Permission to reproduce this quote
was not granted by the copyright
holder. Readers are kindly requested
to refer to the printed version
of this article.
24 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
Publisher's Note:
Permission to reproduce this quote
was not granted by the copyright
holder. Readers are kindly requested
to refer to the printed version
of this article.
Publisher's Note:
Permission to reproduce this quote
was not granted by the copyright
holder. Readers are kindly requested
to refer to the printed version
of this article.
Ptolemy
This Greco-Egyptian mathematician and astronomer (second century ad) is
reported to have mentioned in the lost first book of his Optics a list of twelve
FROM ANCIENT GREECE TO THE MIDDLE AGES 25
simply seen colors. Eleven of these are listed in a biography of Ptolemy found
in the library of the ninth-century Byzantine scholar and patriarch of Con-
stantinople Photius.6 They are, in no particular order: black, white, orange
[xandos], dun, gray [phaion], yellow [ochros], red [erythros], blue [kyanos],
purple [halurgos], shining or bright [lampron], and dark brown [orphinon].
to have painted women in light robes and their jewelry in various bright colors.
However, the idea of light and shadow was not yet understood. Pliny puts the
invention of cinnabar pigment into the same time period. Light and shade
appear to have become common in Greek painting one or two generations
later. Invention of further new pigments in the fifth to second centuries bc
made more and more naturalistic painting possible and required more exten-
sive practical knowledge in pigment mixture.
Of considerable and ongoing controversy is Pliny’s statement that the most
refined classical Greek painters only used four colors in their work: white,
black, yellow, and red.7 Blue pigments must have been known to the Greeks
through trade with Egypt and other nations in the region. Traces of blue pig-
ments have been discovered on statuary and on temple friezes. Works of the
celebrated classical painters, like Apelles, have not survived. What is reputed
to be a copy in mosaic of a painting by Philoxenos (4th c. bc) was found in the
ruins of the House of the Faun (estimated to have been made ca. 100 bc) in
Pompeii. It represents Alexander the Great confronting Darius at the battle
of Issus. The colors of its tesserae are muted, beiges, browns, and grays, without
any blues or greens, thus being in agreement with a four-color painting theory.
The sacral colors of the ancient Greeks are white, the color of festivities,
black, the color of mourning, and various reddish shades from scarlet to violet.8
White bulls or lambs were often used in sacrifices. Red, as the color of blood,
invoked both death and life.
E. Veckenstedt has shown that the classical Greek epic writers used some
140 color words, while the philosophers of the same time period only used
approximately 50 words. He also showed that of the words used by the epic
writers and the philosophers nearly twice as many tended toward white than
toward black (Veckenstedt, 1888). Clearly, in the practical world many color
experiences were distinguished by name even though philosophers mentioned
only relatively few. At the same time there was considerable confusion as to
the exact meaning of given color names, well understandable in the absence
of color standards. B. Berlin and P. Kay (1969) have classified Homeric Greek
as representing stage IIIb in their scheme of development of basic color terms,
namely having the basic terms of black, white yellow, red, and blue. By the
time of the later classical epic writers the color palette had become quite
extensive, with many terms indicating mixed hues and toned colors. There is
no record, however, of a comprehensive effort to sort and order the multitude
of colors.
A perennial problem is that of interpreting the meaning of color names.
Without having specific examples for the meaning of various names, later
interpretations vary considerably. It is evident that throughout the classical
history of Greece what developed into color names had originally more
general meanings. The situation became more complicated in Roman times as
different translators of Greek texts sometimes used different Latin words for
a given Greek color word. A particularly confusing example is the word
glaukos, variously translated as light blue, grayish green, or gray, in one case
FROM ANCIENT GREECE TO THE MIDDLE AGES 27
as flashy. As seen later, in the late Middle Ages it briefly assumed also the
meaning of yellow.
The prefix “sub-” has the meaning of below, and the first two scales refer to
perhaps grays or browns to yellow, respectively wine red to full red. The third
scale travels across an expanse of hue as well as lightness, from a medium
green to a dark reddish blue. These appear to be the first instance of a color
arrangement that requires at least two dimensions to represent it.
The Spanish-Arabic philosopher and commentator ibn-Rushd
(1126?–1198?), known under the Latin name of Averroës, wrote extensive
28 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
Eraclius
Eraclius is believed to have been an Italian monk. Nothing is known about his
life dates or his activities. There are several manuscripts in Latin in existence
that are copies of two books in rhymes and one in prose, less certainly by him,
describing the manufacture of colorants for artistic purposes. Stylistic and
content comparisons have resulted in estimates that he lived in the tenth
century. In section 50 of the prose manuscript Eraclius10 discusses the “various
kinds and names of the principle and intermediate colors . . .”:
Of colours, some are white and some are black. . . . The intermediate colours are
red, green, yellow, purple, prasinus [leek green], azure and indicus [indigo], which
are each of them, in themselves, beautiful; but are more so when mixed, because,
by their variety, they give beauty to one another. And then, in composition, they
have a different hue, . . . colours of different kinds are mixed together, in order
that they may partake of the nature of the others as well as their own, and make
as many, and beautiful, and pleasing, varieties as possible. In this mixture, and in
the order in which one is laid over another in painting, great skill is excercised.
In section 58 he describes how shading and highlights are best achieved for
various colors: “And note that you must shade azure with black; and lay on
the lights with white lead. . . . If you wish to make a colour like lily green, mix
azure with white lead; shade it with azure; lay on the lights with white lead;
and when it is dry, cover it over with clear saffron.”
It is evident from this that Eraclius was well acquainted with color mixing
and painting technology. Mixing of pigments, lasing, and toning in black and
white directions were standard procedures. However, his color sequence does
not indicate a concern with systematic color arrangement.
Theophilus
Explicit tonal scales going from the full color of a bright pigment in both direc-
tions toward white and black were first described by the German Benedictine
monk Theophilus (1080?–1125?), also known under the name Rugerus. In
approximately 1122 he wrote a treatise, De diversis artibus (The various arts)
in which he describes for the benefit of brother monks at some length techni-
FROM ANCIENT GREECE TO THE MIDDLE AGES 29
cal details for painting, glass making, and metalworking as practiced in his
time. It seems likely that Theophilus was acquainted with the works of Era-
clius. In Book I of his manuscript he describes how to mix colorants for paint-
ing flesh tones with their shadows and highlights as well as facial and head
hair. He also describes how to mix colorants for the painting of draperies in
wall and ceiling painting and how to imitate the rainbow in painting. For flesh
color he begins with heated lead white, having turned yellowish, to which
unheated lead white and vermilion (mercuric sulfide, red pigment) is added
until flesh color is attained. More white is added for light faces, for pallid ones
green earth instead of vermilion. Theophilus proceeds with first and second
shadow colors, progressively darker, with first and second rose colors and a
dark red, first and second highlight colors as well as a dark gray used for paint-
ing eyes. Thus Theophilus describes scales of colors centered on average flesh
color and going into lighter, darker, redder, and greener directions. For
example, the second shadow color for flesh is described as follows:
Afterwards take the (first) shadow color for flesh which has been referred to
above, and mix with it more green earth and burnt ochre so that it is a darker
shade of the former color. Then fill the middle space between the eyebrows and
eyes, under the middle of the eyes, near the nose, between the mouth and chin,
on the down or beards of young men, on the half-palms toward the thumb, on
the feet above the smaller areas of relief, and on the faces of children and women
from the chin right up to the temples.
Similarly, for painting drapery various tonal scales are described, for example,
for a greenish yellow hue:
Mix pure viridian (green) with yellow ochre so that the yellow ochre predomi-
nates, and fills the drapery. Add to this color a little sap green and a little burnt
ochre and make the drawing. Mix white with the ground-color and paint the first
light areas. Add more white, and paint the lighter areas on top. Mix with the
above shadow-color more sap green and burnt ochre and a little viridian and
make the shadow on the outside. . . . Mix dark blue with white in the above way.
Similarly mix black with white. In the same way mix yellow ochre with white and
for its shadow add a little burnt ochre.
The band which looks like a rainbow is composed of various colors: namely ver-
milion and viridian, also vermilion and dark blue, viridian and yellow ochre, and
also vermilion and folium [a vegetable red lake]. . . . Then mix from vermilion
and white whatever tones you please so that the first contains a little vermilion,
the second more, the third still more, the fourth yet more, until you reach pure
vermilion. Then mix with this a little burnt ochre, then burnt ochre mixed with
black and finally black. . . . You can never have more than twelve of these strokes
in each color range. And if you want these many so arrange your combinations
that you place a plain color in the seventh row.
30 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
Here Theophilus clearly describes twelve-tone tonal scales for various pure
color pigments that represent the most intense color available for a given hue.
Presumably, the scale colors are to be mixed so that the steps appear approx-
imately even. In this fashion he extends Avicenna’s scales in a systematic
manner, however, without proposing a formal arrangement to place these
in.
and darkness. He used the specific Latin term gradus (step, gradation) to
describe color variations representing difference in the Porphyry sequence.
In De sensu et sensato Bacon included a list of twenty colors, sorted in
approximately tonal sequence, he considered important and explained their
meaning as derived from older sources (interpretations of Bacon’s meaning
are in parentheses).
Despite the flowering of the Renaissance on both sides of the Alps and the
writing of books on painting (Leon Battista Alberti, 1335, and Cennino
Cennini, ca. 1400), there was only modest progress in thinking on the subject
of color scales until the sixteenth century. Cennini mentions a list of seven
colors (but not as a scale) that appear to derive from Aristotle: “Know that
there are seven natural colors, or rather four actually mineral in character,
namely, black, red, yellow and green; there are natural colors but need to be
helped artificially, as lime-white, blue-ultramarine, azzurite, giallorino”
(Cennini, ch. 36).14 Cennini described and recommended a style of painting in
which the full pigment colors represent the darkest colors in a picture and gra-
dations are made exclusively with the addition of white (known as the Cennini
style; Hall, 1992). Alberti offered a painter’s view on colors. While he discussed
(in the Latin version of his manuscript) various views on color of philosophers
and experts, in the Italian version he said: “I speak here as a painter. . . .
Through the mixing of colors infinite other colors are born, but there are only
four true colors—as there are four elements—from which more and more other
kinds of colors may be thus created. Red is the color of fire, blue of the air,
green of water, and of the earth gray and ash [bigio et cenericio]. . . . Therefore,
there are four genera of colors, and these make their species according to
the addition of dark and light, black or white. They are thus almost innu-
merable. Therefore the mixing of white does not change the genus of colors
but forms the species. Black contains a similar force in its mixing to make
almost infinite species of color.” The interpretation of Alberti’s choice of the
earth-related fourth primary chromatic color is difficult. It has been described
as a dull yellowish gray (Gavel, 1979). It appears that the association of his
primary chromatic colors with the four elements was for Alberti more
important than a system that would recognize yellow as primary. However, he
clearly envisaged the systematic toning of the primary colors toward black and
white.
The painter and scientist Leonardo da Vinci (1452–1519) proposed a sort
of tonal scale of six colors: “The simple colors are six, of which the first is white,
COLOR ORDER IN THE RENAISSANCE 33
The list is remarkable for its focus on desaturated, natural colors and the
absence, perhaps influenced by Alberti, of saturated yellow. It is apparent that
34 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
Telesio understood rufus to be a yellowish red and ruber a bluish one. It also
shows that Telesio’s meaning of glaucus has returned to bluish gray.
Hieronymus Cardanus (1501–1576) Italian physician and mathematician
offered a further step in a color value scale.18 The subject of color appears in
more than one of his works, primarily in De gemmis et coloribus (On gems
and colors, 1563). Here Cardanus described an Aristotelian seven-step color
scale as follows: “white, yellow, red, green, wine color, blue, and black.” White
and black are primordial colors, the other five “intermediate.” Additional
mixed colors fall between these primary colors. What is new in Cardanus’s list
is that he assigns numbers of brightness to these colors, thus: “we assume that
white contains a hundred parts of light, scarlet fifty, black nothing.” These
colors fix the beginning, middle, and end of the scale. Yellow is described as
containing 65 to 78 parts of light, green 62, deep green 40, wine color 30, blue
25, and blackish gray 20. This scale is not influenced by concerns of painters
or by issues of color mixing. It is the first-known “quantitative” assessment of
the intrinsic brightness/lightness of object colors.
But if you want right to consider the origin and relations of the colors, you should
start from the five principle middle colors which are red, blue, green, gold, and
gray of white and black. And their gradings, they rise either closer to white by
their paleness or to black by their darkness; albeit they are (as above has been
made known) related to one another as previously shown. Because red rises to
white through pale red (pink) and skin color; to black through purple, brown,
violet brown and black brown. Similarly gold relates toward white through pale
gold, wooden and wheat color; to black through burnt gold and blackish brown.
COLOR ORDER IN THE RENAISSANCE 35
Equally blue rises to white through sky blue and pale blue, like Dutch cloth; and
to black through dark blue like indigo color that has some brownish to it. So rises
also green toward white through verdigris and pale green; to black through black-
ish green. Gray approaches white by the color of light gray, dapple gray and lime:
to black by mouse gray, black gray and pale black. And this is the correct rela-
tionship of colors that in their number agree with that of the planets as do the
lower colors with the five membranes of the eye, and with the five senses. All this
can be seen from the accompanying figure. (Fig. 2-2b)
Fig. 2-2b Forsius’ proposal of tonal scales ending in white and black.
form a circle, and vice versa. How to properly draw a transparent sphere was
well known in the seventeenth century from several sixteenth-century and
earlier books on perspective. On the other hand, Forsius certainly knew that
there are many intermediate hues between his primary hues and with them
many more curved lines. How he would have arranged these is not clear. It is
evident that Forsius’ second figure does not represent a color sphere but is
rather a figure that illustrates four chromatic and an achromatic value scale
beginning and ending at the same points. His manuscript was not published in
book form and thus did not become known outside Sweden.
Fig. 2-3 Graphical interpretation of a musical octave (or double diapason) split into dual-tone
cords, after Boethius (fifth c.). From a twelfth-century manuscript of Boethius’ De institutione
musica. This type of diagram was used to represent several kinds of connections in the first
half of the second millennium.
the relationship between the five types of classification of things (see Parkhurst,
1990). The use of such diagrams was thought to show the simplicity of natural
laws as expressed in the classical four areas of study: arithmetic, geometry,
music, and astronomy. It is not surprising that classically schooled thinkers also
used diagrams of this kind to show relationships between colors.
Fig. 2-4 François d’Aguilon’s basic color scale and mixture diagram of 1613. In the upper
portion the tonal scales of white, the full colors, and black are shown, and in the lower section
important secondary colors resulting from mixtures of the primary chromatic colors yellow, red,
and blue.
Fig. 2-5 Color circle of Robert Fludd of 1629, perhaps the first in print.
COLOR ORDER IN THE RENAISSANCE 39
colors is described as follows: black: no light; blue: more blackness, less light;
green: equality of light and blackness; red: middle between whiteness and
blackness; orange: more redness, less whiteness; yellow: equality of whiteness
and redness; white: no blackness. The Aristotelian number of seven colors and
the designation of red as halfway between black and white are conventional.
What is unusual is the description of colors toward black in terms of light and
toward white in terms of whiteness.
Athanasius Kircher (ca. 1601–1680), German Jesuit with wide interests in
the sciences, author of 44 volumes of writing, professor at the University of
Würzburg and later at the College of Rome, wrote a text on light and color:
Ars magna lucis et umbrae (All there is to know about light and shadow), pub-
lished in 1646. He also considered, in the Greek tradition, colors to be the
result of activity of light and darkness: “Since color is the property of a dark
body or, as some say, a shadowed light, the true offspring of light and shadow,
we must treat thereof. . . .” In the second chapter, On the multitudinous variety
of colors, he used a modified version of d’Aguilon’s color diagram to illustrate
the arrangement of colors (Fig. 2-6). In the cusps formed by semicircles the
mixture colors of the two chromatic colors involved are placed: aureus, viridis,
and purpureus. Below the peaks of the semicircles are located important tonal
Fig. 2-6 Athanasius Kircher’s color diagram in the Boethius style, 1646. Below are his ana-
logues in other qualities and ideas. Tonal scales are shown as by d’Aguilon, with intermediate
tonal colors identified (e.g., cinereus, ash colored, between white and blue). The secondary
mixtures are shown at the intersections of the corresponding tonal arches.
40 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
colors resulting from mixture of the chromatic color with white or black. Thus
subalbum is a mixture of yellow and white while fuscus is a mixture of yellow
and black. The diagram is interesting because of the attached analogies
between colors and various other qualities, properties, and things: light and
shade, taste, the four classical elements, human age, intellect, a scale from God
to plants, and finally, the strings of the Greek lyra.
Fig. 2-7 Pigment mixture chart for Francis Glisson’s gray scale. First column: grade of scale;
second column: weight of lead white (in grains); third column: weight of carbon black; fourth
column: reduced pigment ratio.
Fig. 2-8 Glisson’s sketch of the arrangement of tonal scales. The blue, red, and yellow scales
arch over the horizontal gray scale according to their lightness. The vertical dashes denote the
location of the pure pigment.
42 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
Fig. 2-9 CIE L* lightness values of reconstructed Glisson gray scale, beginning with white on
the left and ending with black on the right. The scale is only approximately uniform in terms of
CIE lightness. There is a very large step between white and the first mixture.
Glisson’s gray and red scales have been reconstructed using the specified
pigments and weights (Kuehni and Stanziola, 2002). Reflectance curves were
measured and tristimulus values, as well as CIE lightness values L*, calculated.
In Fig. 2-9 the differences in CIELAB (see Chapter 6) L* between the steps
of the gray scale are shown. Near both ends the step sizes increase strongly.
In between, they gradually decline in size toward black. Only extensive visual
scaling would have disclosed this fact. Similar results were obtained for the
red scale with, again, the first and last steps being too large and toward full
red the steps gradually becoming smaller.
of the Simple Yellows and Reds with each of the simple Blews, and these
Mixtures give most of the mean Colours, viz. Greens, Purples, &c.” (italics in
the original). The column header colorants are Spanish white, azurite, ultra-
marine, smalt, litmus, indigo, ink black. The row header yellow colorants are
(lead white), Naples yellow, gamboge, ochre, orpiment, and umbra, the red col-
orants minium, burnt ochre, vermilion, carmine, red lake, dragon’s blood, red
ochre (carbon black). One-to-one mixtures were made to fill in the intersect-
ing 98 fields. Waller’s expressed idea was to have a scientific systematic
arrangement of colors. He defended mixtures in single weight ratios by indi-
cating that the possible number of mixture ratios was infinite and therefore
not practically doable. In some copies of the printed paper the colorant mix-
tures were dabbed in.
It is remarkable that the first explicit, if incomplete, hue circle was the work
of Newton. Isaac Newton (1642–1727), celebrated mathematician and physi-
cist, clarified the composition of white light as a mixture of lights of different
wavelengths.When these are viewed individually they create various hue expe-
riences, the spectral colors. In his early lectures on optics, deposited as a matter
of his job responsibilities as Lucasian professor of mathematics in the library
of Cambridge University (Optica, 1670–1672) Newton was primarily con-
cerned with the refrangeability of light rays and had little to say about color
mixture and logical arrangements of colors. Proposition 3 states: “The colors
white and black together with intermediate ashens or grays are generated from
rays of every sort confusedly mixed.” In proposition 2 Newton described a
scale of eleven hues that he considered “prominent primitives.” He experi-
mented with overlaid mixtures of prismatic lights by appropriately arranging
three prisms and experienced the well-known difficulties in obtaining white
from mixture of two prismatic colors. He found, and expressed in proposition
4, that “[p]rimitive colors can be exhibited by the composition of the neigh-
boring colors on each side of them.”
In his mature reflections on colors, Opticks (1704), Newton introduced a
partly scientifically based color circle and color mixture diagram (Fig. 11 of
Plate II, Part II, Book I) (Fig. 2-10). He described it (in part) as follows:
With the Center O and Radius OD describe a Circle ADF and distinguish its cir-
cumference into seven parts . . . proportional to the seven musical Tones or Inter-
vals of the eight Sounds, contained in an Eight. . . . Let the first part DE represent
a red Colour, the second EF orange, the third FG yellow, the fourth GH green,
the fifth AB blue, the sixth BC indico, and the seventh CD violet, And conceive
that these are all the Colours of uncompounded Light gradually passing into one
another, as they do when made by Prisms. . . . Let p be the center of gravity of
the Arch DE [comparably for q, r, s, t, v, x] and about those centers of gravity let
44 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
Circles proportional to the number of rays of each Colour in the given mixture
be described. . . . Find the common center of gravity of all those Circles p, q, r, s,
t, v, x. Let that center be Z; and from the center of the Circle ADF, through Z to
the circumference, drawing the right line OY, the place of the point Y in the cir-
cumference shall shew the Colour arising from the composition of all the Colours
in the given mixture, and the line OZ shall be proportional to the fullness or
intenseness of the Colour, that is, to its distance from whiteness. As if Y fall in
the middle between F and G, the compounded Colour shall be the best yellow;
if Y verge from the middle toward F or G, the compounded Colour shall accord-
ingly be a yellow, verging toward orange or green. If Z fall upon the circumfer-
ence the Colour shall be intense and florid in the highest degree; if it fall in the
mid way between the circumference and center it shall be but half so intense,
that is, it shall be such a Colour as would be made by diluting the intensest yellow
with an equal quantity of whiteness; and if it fall upon the center O, the Colour
shall have lost all its intenseness and become a white. . . . if the point Z fall in or
near the line OD, the main ingredient being the red and violet, the Colour com-
pounded shall not be any of the prismatic Colours, but a purple, inclining to red
or violet. . . .
Newton describes with his figure a diagram that is a complete spectral hue
circle as well as an additive mixture diagram. At the same time he uses the
figure to give a specific example of additive color mixture:
Fig. 2-11 Descartes’s circular diapason, 1650. It may have served as a model for Newton’s
color circle.
6 parts, and of red 10 parts. Proportional to these parts I describe the Circles x,
v, t, s, r, q, p respectively, that is, so that if the Circle x be 1, the Circle s 3. . . .
Then I find Z, the common center of gravity of these Circles, and through Z
drawing the line OY the point Y falls upon the circumference between E and F,
. . . and thence I conclude, that the Colour compounded of these ingredients will
be an orange, verging a little more to red than to yellow. Also I find that OZ is
a little less than one half of OY, and thence I conclude, that this orange hath a
little less than half the fullness or intenseness of an uncompounded orange . . .
this proportion being not of the quantities of mixed orange and white powders,
but of the quantities of the lights reflected from them.23
Fig. 2-12 C.B.’s hand-painted color circles, 1708. Left: The seven-color circle; right: the
twelve-color circle. Note that the pigments used for some of the colors have deteriorated. (See
color plate.)
DEVELOPMENT OF THE COLOR CIRCLE 47
Fig. 2-13 Continuous color circle of Ignaz Schiffermüller, with twelve classes of colors, 1771.
(See color plate.)
but was interested in a system that could be used, among other things, to
deduce rules of color harmony. This required a physical expression of the
system in considerable detail. Castel’s central color spiral segment was flat-
tened into a color circle. To specify the twelve hues Schiffermüller used more
descriptive hue terms. Again, they were arranged in attempted perceptually
equal steps. Between yellow and red there are two intermediate steps: orange
and fire red; between red and blue there are four steps: crimson red, violet red,
violet blue, and fire blue; between blue and yellow there are three steps: sea
green, green, and olive green. Each of the twelve hues is the key representa-
tive of a “color class,” and Schiffermüller used Roman numerals to identify
them, starting with blue (Fig. 2-13). Next Schiffermüller began to create steps
toward white and black but did not get beyond three examples in the blue
region (Fig. 2-14). His book Versuch eines Farbensystems (Attempt to construct
a color system) was published in Vienna in 1771, thirteen years after Tobias
DEVELOPMENT OF THE COLOR CIRCLE 49
Fig. 2-14 Schiffermüller’s tonal scales of three blues, from his 1771 work. Some of the col-
orations have deteriorated. (See color plate.)
axis of the colors of a gray scale, and he placed tonal scales of four primary
hues, red, yellow, green, and blue, on circular segments on either side of the
gray scale. It is not obvious that the circle has a clear meaning. In case of Fludd
it is not unlikely that mystical, perhaps alchemical reasons produced his circle,
which begins with white and ends in black next to it.
Newton’s choice of a circle for his spectral colors may have been influenced
by Descartes’s circular diapason, but it was also based on his knowledge of
desaturation of spectral light by white light, common for all spectral colors.
Thus the radius length is an indicator of the saturation of a color. Newton’s
placing of the spectral hues on the periphery of the circle in proportion to a
musical scale meant that opposite colors were not exactly complementary
colors (if close to them). For this reason, and the varying chromatic strength
of different hues, opposite colors “when mixed in an equal proportion . . . the
Colour compounded of those two shall not be perfectly white, but some faint
anonymous Colour.” Newton also was aware of compounded purple colors
that belong near line D–O in his circle and that, thus, spectral hues and com-
pounded purples can form a closed series. Newton chose a distribution of hues
according to a musical scale on his color circle that, with its common white
center, also represents geometrical parsimony.
C.B.’s color circle, as shown, is a pigment-mixing diagram based on the idea
of yellow, red, and blue as primaries. The circular form, unlikely to have been
influenced by Newton, may have been the result of independent realization
by a painter that, perceptually, a series of hues derived from three primary pig-
ments and produced by mixing neighboring pigments can return upon itself.
As an entomologist Schiffermüller had interest in systematic color arrange-
ment as a means for classifying butterflies and other colored insects. He also
had contact with many artists and was aware of issues in finding harmonious
color combinations, devoting a chapter of his book to this question. In addi-
tion he thought that the time had arrived to create a complete system of colors.
His circle was a first attempt in this direction. Its form was likely derived from
the spiral arrangement of hues of different tonal values of Castel and his pre-
sumed knowledge of Newton’s work and C.B.’s book. By this time the hue
circle had become a convention that made intuitive sense. Many future systems
would be based on it.
The arguments offered so far for the color circle consist of rationalizations
based on likely reasoning and insight available in the seventeenth century.
There is a further and perhaps stronger argument. As will be shown later and
in Chapter 6, a form of data analysis called multidimensional scaling (see
Chapter 3), when applied to spectral colors, results in a geometrical distribu-
tion of data points best fit with a circle. In addition mathematical analysis of
large collections of spectra of colored objects by various methods, but without
consideration of human color vision properties, locates the spectra in spaces
where a series of hues approximately forms a circle. A hue circle therefore is
a pattern that may have become subconsciously apparent and is the result of
one of the strategies of our visual system.
MAYER AND LAMBERT’S COLOR SOLIDS 51
A full-fledged color order system based on a hue circle was delayed because
of the growing importance of the idea of three primary chromatic colors, pre-
sented as a triangle, that could be used to create all other hues by mixture. The
source of the idea of yellow, red, and blue as three primary colors is unknown,
and it might have developed from dyeing and painting technology. We have
seen these three chromatic primary colors in d’Aguilon’s diagram of 1613.
The same primary chromatic colors are mentioned by Robert Boyle in his
Experiments and Considerations Touching Colours (1664). Using “pigments”
as generic word for colorants he wrote: “. . . there are but few Simple and
Primary Colours (if I may so call them) from whose Various Compositions all
the rest do as it were Result. . . . I have not yet found, that to exhibit this strange
Variety they [painters] need imploy any more than White, and Black, and Red,
and Blew, and Yellow; these five, Variously Compounded, . . . being sufficient
to exhibit a Variety and Number of Colours, such as those that are altogether
Strangers to the Painters Pallets, can hardly imagine. . . . by these simple com-
positions again Compounded among themselves, the Skilfull Painter can
produce what kind of Colour he pleases, and a great many more than we have
yet Names for.” Yellow, red, and blue had been celebrated as key colors in
several works of the contemporary French painter Nicholas Poussin
(1594–1665). Francis Glisson was a supporter of these colors as primary chro-
matic colors. The inventor of four-color printing (yellow, red, blue, and black),
Johann Christoffel Le Blon (1667–1741), published his book on this subject,
Coloritto, in 1725. As a result, in the first explicit steps toward a three-
dimensional color system, the form of a triangular pyramid (tetrahedron) was
used with the primary colors yellow, red, blue, and white at the vertices.
Fig. 2-15 Arrangement of 91 colors of the central plane of Tobias Mayer’s double triangular
pyramid, with the color designation scheme based on the three primary colors R, G, and B,
1758.
Fig. 2-16 Schematic representation of Tobias Mayer’s double triangular pyramid color solid.
each, and with eleven divisions per side, obtained 78 colors. Pursuing this
approach he ended up with 364 colors above the middle plane and the same
number below, a total of 819 color samples for his double tetrahedron (Fig. 2-
16), each with its own designation. Mayer did not envisage an explicit central
gray scale made from mixtures of white and black but expected that mixture
of identical parts of the three primary colors (colorants) in the central plane
would produce neutral grays. In the published paper Mayer did not assign spe-
cific pigments to his primary colors. However, in the newspaper report of his
presentation in Göttingen the “Mayer coordinates” of several pigments were
identified, among them, with the identifier 12 (i.e., primary colors), orpiment
(king’s yellow), vermilion, and azurite (Bergblau). (Note that these are the
same pigments as those selected by Glisson.)
It is apparent that Mayer’s proposal was one of theory, applicable also to
mixture of lights. It is known that he made some experiments with pigment
mixture but not to what extent he investigated visually equally spaced scales.
There is no indication that he was fully aware of the nonlinear relationship
between pigment mixture ratio and the resulting visual experiences, as already
Johann Wolfgang von Goethe in his comments indicated (Goethe, 1810).
Mayer’s publisher, Georg Christoph Lichtenberg, added a commentary where
he applied Newton’s center of gravity principle to Mayer’s proposal. He also
printed a reproduction of a color triangle hand painted by Mayer and com-
mented extensively on the difficulties in obtaining a good result. In 1772, three
years before Mayer’s previously unpublished works appeared in print, the
Alsatian mathematician, physicist, and astronomer Johann Heinrich Lambert
(1728–1777) issued a small book under the title Beschreibung einer mit dem
Calauischen Wachse ausgemalten Farbenpyramide wo die Mischung jeder Farbe
aus Weiss und den drey Grundfarben angeordnet, dargelegt und derselben
Berechnung und vielfachen Gebrauch gewiesen wird (Description of a color
pyramid painted with Caulau wax where the mixture of each color from white
54 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
and the three basic colors is arranged, explained, and its calculation and
various uses indicated). He had read the report about Mayer’s lecture and
cited it in his book Photometria (Lambert, 1760). Using Mayer’s proposal as
a basis Lambert developed his own system. He also employed colorant mix-
tures and the same identification scheme. Lambert justified the triangular
scheme from a presumably visually equidistant color circle in which yellow
occupies the 12 o’clock position, the border of red against violet the 4 o’clock
and blue the 8 o’clock positions. He searched among the available pigments
for a yellow that was neither reddish nor greenish and, correspondingly, for a
neutral red and blue and on that basis selected gamboge, carmine (cochineal),
and Berlinerblau (prussian blue, potassium ferric ferrocyanide).30 A condition
Lambert required, beside neutrality of hue, was that his colorants should
approach as nearly as possible the intensity of spectral colors. He commented:
“I leave it undecided if in the future colorants will be found which approach
the spectral colors even closer than the mentioned carmine, gamboge and
Prussian blue.” Realizing from practical experience that the coloristic
strengths of his three colorants was significantly different, he established them
by finding (as Glisson did) middle colors between the three primaries and
reported them as two parts carmine to three parts prussian blue to twelve parts
gamboge. Mixing his primary colorants in a corresponding ratio (12 parts
prussian blue, 12 parts gamboge, and 2 parts carmine) produced a near black
when applied to white paper and Lambert did not see a need for mixtures of
his colorants with black and discarded the lower half of Mayer’s pyramid.
Lambert expressed dissatisfaction with Mayer’s uniform twelve steps. He had
the Prussian court painter Calau experiment to develop originally a six-step,
later a nine-step visually equidistant triangle. Mixing the colorants with a near
water-soluble wax of Calau’s invention and gum resulted in colorations of high
chroma and good stability. Since all three colorants had a high degree of trans-
parency, Lambert believed he could avoid adding a white pigment and instead
used the white of the paper together with increasing dilutions of the colorant
mixtures to achieve the tonal declines toward white. Nevertheless, Lambert
found that he had to produce a total of 67 mixtures to color his pyramid of
108 colors so that the result met his visual criteria. Interestingly, near black
colors are pushed close to basic blue (colors 11, 12, 19, 20; see Fig. 2-17) at the
lowest level rather than at 21, 27, 28 where, based on the gravimetric rule, one
would expect them.
Lambert proposed for the grades along the sides of the triangle a color
naming system that is only moderately intuitive. He used a system borrowed
from the methodology for designating directions around the compass, for
example: 1, blue; 10, blue toward red; 18, bluish reddish blue; 25, bluish red
toward blue; 31, blue red or red blue; 36, red blue toward red; 40, reddish bluish
red; 43, red toward blue; 45, red. For the interior of the triangle he used pri-
marily descriptive color terms such as “chestnut red brown.”
Lambert saw his pyramid as a general color atlas of use to merchants,
for example, to determine if they had fabrics in stock in the desirable colors.
Consumers could use the atlas to decide what color clothing to buy and what
COLOR CIRCLES FROM HARRIS TO HENRY 55
Fig. 2-17 Identification scheme of primary colors (1, 9, and 45) and color mixes used in the
basis plane of Lambert’s triangular pyramid color solid, 1772.
colors to combine: “Caroline wants to have a dress like Selinda’s. She memo-
rizes the color number from the pyramid and will be sure to have the same
color. Should the color need to be darker or go more in the direction of
another color, this will not pose a problem.” Lambert believed the pyramid to
be of particular use for dyers. If they could find three dyes approximating his
three primary colorants, and after having determined their coloristic strength
relative to Lambert’s primaries, dyers could calculate how much of each dye
to use to achieve a given shade in the pyramid. Other potential users were
artists who, after sketching, for example, flowers in pencil and writing their
color numbers next to them, could reproduce the natural colors in the studio
by referring to the corresponding colors in the pyramid.
Lambert built a wooden pyramidal structure, and included an image of it
in his book (Fig. 2-18). In this display the 108 selected colors could be prop-
erly displayed. For comparison purposes he had Calau paint chips with twelve
common artist’s colorants along the bottom border: naples yellow, king’s
yellow, orpiment, azurite, smalt, indigo, lamp black, sap green, chrysocolla,
verdigris, vermilion, and florentine lake.
Lambert went about the task with a considerable amount of scientific zeal.
Aside from developing an arithmetic of colorant mixture, he explained the
result of black from a mixture of his three primary colorants logically as the
simultaneous prevention of the activity of each of the three colorants by
the other two. His color pyramid is the first attempt to create a geometrical,
physical model of object color experiences achievable with his primary col-
orants, a color solid. The shortcomings of Mayer and Lambert’s proposals will
become apparent as we proceed.
Moses Harris
Color circles continued to be proposed in various formats. In 1766 Moses
Harris (1731–1785), an English entomologist and illustrator of insects, dedi-
56 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
Fig. 2-18 Depiction of Lambert’s color pyramid, 1772. The lowest level contains the 45 colors
identified in Fig. 2-17. The higher levels contain reduced sets at higher lightness, ending in
white on top of the pyramid. Black is located on the lowest level. The colors displayed on the
front of the model represent well-known artist’s pigments of the time. (See color plate.)
cated a small volume titled The Natural System of Colours to the then presi-
dent of the Royal Academy, the painter Joshua Reynolds. It contains copper-
plates illustrating two color circles, one based on “prismatic” primaries yellow,
red, and blue (whose mixture is illustrated as black, however; see Fig. 2-19)
and one based on “compound” primaries orange, green, and purple. There are
eighteen hues per circle and each hue is illustrated in twenty gradations. The
first circle is said to contain only those colors “shewn by the prism.”The second
one contains “all other colours in nature, not found in the prismatic part.”
Harris provides specific examples of what he means when using color names,
for example: red–vermilion, wild poppy; blue–ultramarine, cornbottle-flower;
green–sap-green, leaves of the lime-tree.
COLOR CIRCLES FROM HARRIS TO HENRY 57
Fig. 2-19 Prismatic version of Moses Harris’s color circle of 1786. Some deterioration of col-
orants is evident. (See color plate.)
required what colour is most opposite or contrary in hue to red, look directly
opposite to that colour in the system and it will be found to be green . . . of
every colour or teint in the system no one of them contain in their composi-
tions any of the colours of which those on the opposite side are formed. . . .”
Harris remarked on the problems of coloring his system properly. Certain col-
orants (indigo, gamboge, carmine, sap green) are said by him to contain twenty
degrees of power while others do not.
Frisch
A color circle with tonal scales in direction of both black and white was offered
in 1788 by the Prussian painter Johann Christoph Frisch (1738–1815).31 Frisch’s
immediate purpose was to demonstrate tonal colors in much detail as an aid
for painters at a time when subdued coloration was fashionable. Frisch’s color
circle, presented only in outline, consists of 40 concentric rings, beginning with
black in the center. Only two blackish colors are found on the second ring:
blackish red and blue. On ring 3 the range is expanded to eight hues with
reduced blackness: yellow, orange, red, purple, violet, blue, sea green, and leaf
green. On ring 4 the hues swell to sixteen, with further reduced blackess and
on ring 5 to 32, the maximum number. Here the explicit description ends and
rings 6 to 10 were to contain tonal values toward white. Rings 11 to 20 are
even less well described but apparently were to contain further tonal values
ending up in black again at the periphery. Frisch expressed the opinion that
there are more equal-sized hue steps between yellow and blue and between
red and blue than between yellow and red, and he selected the size of his hue
segments accordingly.
Goethe
Because of its importance in aesthetics the color circle of the German poet
and natural philosopher Johann Wolfgang von Goethe (1749–1832), intro-
duced in his Zur Farbenlehre (On the doctrine of colors) of 1810, should be
mentioned here. Goethe’s six-color circle is based on two triangles: the fun-
damental three colors with red on top, and the intermediary colors. The posi-
tion of red results from a process Goethe called Steigerung (intensification) in
which red represents the peak. At the same time it reflects successive contrast:
“To recognize quickly which colors are called forth by this contrast one can
use the illuminated color circle of our tables that is altogether organized
according to natural principles and offers here good service because its oppos-
ing colors are those that are demanding each other in the eye. In this manner
yellow demands violet, orange blue, purple green and vice versa.” Goethe
interpreted his color circle also in regard to principles of color harmony.
Color circles for the primary purpose of demonstrating rules of color harmony
have also been developed by the German painter Matthias Klotz (1748–1821)
in 1816 (see Section 2.10), the English colorant producer and dealer George
RUNGE’S COLOR SPHERE 59
The idea of three fundamental colors received an important boost in 1801 from
the English physicist and physician Thomas Young (1773–1829). He restated
an hypothesis first mentioned by the English glassmaker George Palmer in his
Theory of Colours and Vision (Palmer, 1777).32 Palmer believed that “The
surface of the retina is compounded of particles of three different kinds, anal-
ogous to the three rays of light [necessary to mix all colors]; and each of these
particles is moved by its own ray.” In Young’s mature version the eye is pro-
vided with distinct sets of nervous fibers: “. . . if we seek for the simplest
arrangement, which would enable [the eye] to receive and discriminate the
impressions of the different parts of the spectrum, we may suppose three dis-
tinct sensations only to be excited by the rays of the three principal pure
colours, falling on any given point of the retina, the red, the green, and the
violet, while the rays occupying the intermediate spaces are capable of pro-
ducing mixed sensations, the yellow those which belong to the red and the
green” (Young, 1824). However, Young’s three primary colors were not those
of Boyle, Le Blon, or the dyers and painters, a situation that continued to cause
confusion until resolved by Maxwell and Helmholtz (see glossary entry
primary colors).
In 1809 the English painter and conchologist James Sowerby (1757–1822) pub-
lished A new elucidation of colours, original, prismatic, and material: showing
their concordance in three primitives, yellow, red and blue, and the means of pro-
ducing, measuring, and mixing them: with some observations on the accuracy of
Sir Isaac Newton. In this treatise he attempted a synthesis of ideas about colors
from lights and from colorants. Sowerby experimented with additive color
mixture by placing narrow strips of paintings of his three primary colorants
gamboge, carmine, and prussian blue next to each other and viewing them from
a distance. As a painter he was interested in the effects of adding white and
black pigments to his primary colorants. In this work a two-dimensional color
order system progresses from white in the center to the most intense chromatic
colors and from there toward black on the periphery.
Since all three colors, blue, yellow and red stand at the same distance from white
and black, therefore, the center of the color disk in which those three have lost
their individuality through equal activity must be in the same relationship and
Fig. 2-20 Runge’s basic color triangle (R for red, G for yellow, B for blue) combined with the
triangle of secondary colors (orange, violet, green), extended horizontally to a circle and verti-
cally in cone shape toward white and black, 1810.
RUNGE’S COLOR SPHERE 61
Fig. 2-21 Runge’s double-cone color solid rounded to final spherical form, with white at the
top and black at the bottom of the sphere, 1810.
in the same distance to white as to black as those three. Since both of these points
(the center point between white and black and that of the triangle Blue, Yellow,
Red) mathematically coincide it follows that both must be one and the same
. . . and that from the identical difference a complete indifference results into
which all individual qualities have dissolved. . . . This point, since it is in equal
distance to all five elements, is therefore to be seen as the general central point
of them all. . . . All mixtures that result from the inclination of a point on the
complete color circle toward white or black (a tendency which is common to all
these points) will slowly loose themselves toward white and toward black. . . . as
the differences of all points of the inclination toward white or black from the
central point are radii the points form nothing but circle segments ending in the
poles white and black. . . . Thereby, the complete relationship of all five elements,
through its differences and inclinations, forms a perfect sphere. Its surface con-
tains all five elements and those of their mixtures which are generated in friendly
inclination of their qualities and toward its center all colors of the surface dis-
solve in equal steps into a balanced gray. . . . Every color is placed in its proper
relationship to all pure elements as well as all mixtures and in this manner the
sphere is to be seen as a general table by which he who requires various tables
in his business, can always find the relationship of the totality of all colors. It now
must be evident to the attentive reader that it is not possible to find a plane figure
that is a complete table of all mixtures; the relationship can only be presented
as a solid.
In a hand-colored copperplate figure views of the sphere from the white and
the black poles, a horizontal cross section along the equator and a vertical cross
section through the two poles are given (Fig. 2-22). Unlike Lambert’s, Runge’s
idea was not to offer just a color atlas but to present an idealized theoretical
construct that not only was meant to represent color relations for the painter
but also presumed deeper psychological and mystical relationships gleaned in
part from Goethe. As indicated in the title of the publication, Runge used the
sphere geometry to also develop his ideas about color harmony.
62 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
Fig. 2-22 Hand-colored copperplate of Runge’s Farben-Kugel (1810). Views toward the white
and black poles are on top. The equatorial cross section is on bottom left and the polar cross
section on the right. There are four saturation steps between the full color on the surface and
the middle gray in the center of the sphere. (See color plate.)
While representing the most complete color order system so far, Runge’s
proposal still suffers from unresolved issues. The central axis from pole to pole
represents a gray scale, but the full colors of the equatorial circle are of various
lightnesses so that the meaning of the vertical axis is not explicitly defined.
Colorants that in equal mixture produce a neutral gray do not exist, but Runge
did not address the problems materializing when mixing colorants. These and
other issues have only been resolved in the twentieth century.
THE EARLY DEVELOPMENT OF PSYCHOPHYSICS 63
Uniformity of spacing of colors in color scales, an atlas, or color solid had been
a clear but difficult to implement goal for Glisson, Castel, Mayer, Lambert,
Runge, and the just mentioned Klotz. It began to be approached from a
different angle as part of general psychological investigations. The idea of a
differential threshold for illumination was introduced by the French mathe-
matician Pierre Bouguer (1698–1758) in the posthumous Traité d’optique sur
la gradation de la lumière (Optical treatise on the gradation of light, 1760). He
64 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
sation. Fechner expressed Weber’s findings, meanwhile known to him, for the
JND increments as follows:
db
dg = K , (2-1)
b
g = log b . (2-2)
Fechner saw this formula as a differential formula and integrated it with the
result
Êbˆ (2-4)
g = K log .
Ë b¯
Fechner termed the value b/b the fundamental stimulus value. “The magni-
tude of the sensation g is . . . proportional to the logarithm of the fundamental
stimulus value.” Fechner named this formula the Maasformel (measurement
formula). It expresses the number of JNDs the stimulus is above threshold.
“In the measurement formula one has a general dependent relation between
the size of the fundamental stimulus and the size of the corresponding sensa-
tion. . . . This permits the amount of sensation to be calculated from the rela-
tive amounts of the fundamental stimulus and thus we have a measurement
of sensation.”
Fechner’s work resulted in three types of experimental methods applied in
psychology: just noticeable differences or the method of limits, the method of
right and wrong cases or method of constant stimuli, and the method of
average error. Each of these methods found champions in succeeding years
and is still employed today.
Fechner introduced the idea of “inner” and “outer” psychophysics. Outer
psychophysics considers the relationship between the measurable physical
stimulus and the reported psychological response. Fechner understood inner
psychophysics to mean the relationship between the excitation of nerve
fibers and the mind. Thus he saw the entire process as consisting of physical
66 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
Design of a hemispheric color order system was offered in 1839 in his book
De la loi du contrast simultané des couleurs by Michel-Eugène Chevreul
CHEVREUL’S HEMISPHERIC SYSTEM 67
Fig. 2-23 Chevreul’s concept for a 72-hue circle with twenty grades each. White is in the
center and black at the circumference of the circle, 1839.
In each of the scales . . . there is one tone which, when pure, represents in its
purity the colour of the scale to which it belongs: therefore I name it the normal
tone of this scale. . . . If the tone 15 of the Red scale is the normal tone, the
normal tone of the Yellow scale will be a lower number, while the normal tone
of the Blue scale will be of a higher number. This depends on the unequal degree
of brilliancy and luminousness of the colours.
68 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
Fig. 2-24 Schematic depiction of the tonal hemisphere raised above Chevreul’s color circle.
Black covers the hemisphere, with white at its origin.
However, later Chevreul placed all full colors on the same grade, a fact that
resulted in considerable confusion about his system by readers of different
book versions. Colors toward the center from the “normal tone” are mixed
with white, and those toward the periphery with black in appropriate amounts
to make the constant hue ladder visually equidistant. Chevreul realized that
there are many other colors of a given hue that he had not represented in his
plane (the plane being in essence a flat representation of the surface of a color
solid). To represent the missing colors Chevreul chose to erect a hemisphere
above the plane (Fig. 2-24). The apex of the hemisphere is occupied by black,
the line between white at the center of the plane and the vertical axis is formed
by a 20-step gray scale. Chevreul then proposed to mix each of the colors of
the base plane in ten steps with increasing amounts of black but comments:
“It is understood that these proportions relate to the effect of the mixtures
upon the eye and not to material quantities of the Red and Black substances.”
In this manner as the angle of the radial lines increases toward 90° the colors
become increasingly blackish. In such a system there would be large visual
steps between the ninth line of colors, nearing the vertical center line, and the
gray scale of the center line, particularly near the core of the hemisphere. This
can be avoided, as Schwarz has pointed out (Schwarz, 1997), if one assumes
that Chevreul had in mind mixtures of the base plane colors with the appro-
priate gray rather than with black so that a smooth transition to the gray scale
would result. Calculations show that in neither case explicit redundancy of
colors occurs, thus negating later criticism of Ostwald and others in this
respect. Assuming the mixtures to be with gray would have the advantage that
all colors in a hemispherical layer would have the same lightness throughout,
a result that Chevreul likely intended. Chevreul’s system was never fully illus-
trated.Ten 72-hue circles, from the full colors with increasing amounts of black,
and twelve constant hue ladders representing the key colors of the base plane
YELLOW, RED, AND BLUE, FOR A TIME FIRMLY ESTABLISHED AS PRIMARY COLORS 69
At the points R, B, G, M are located pure red, blue, yellow and gray (white) of
intensity AB = AR = AG = AM, the quarter circles BR, BG and GR contain all
binary mixtures of violet, green and orange, therefore not yet mixed with gray
(white). At the points a, b and g neutral violet, orange and green are located.—
Colors tending toward B, R and G are more bluish, reddish, or greenish. . . . The
circle segments MR, MB and MG are the loci for all mixtures of red with gray,
blue with gray and yellow with gray. . . . There is an infinite number of concen-
tric spherical sections [Ma, Mb and Mg] or at least as many as there are grada-
tions of white light from black via gray to the most intense white.
Doppler was the first to develop a three-dimensional color system not based
on object colors. As a mathematician he was able to describe the relationships
between colors in correct mathematical terms (see also Schwarz, 1991).
The speculations of Palmer, the work of Le Blon and his competitors, and the
choices of Mayer, Lambert, Runge, and others, helped to establish yellow, red,
and blue as the generally accepted primary colors. Young, possibly influenced
by Palmer, also had concluded that there are three primary colors but his
informed considerations made him eventually select red, green, and violet as
the additive primaries.
70 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
Fig. 2-25 One of twelve chromatic scales by Chevreul, with local discoloration. The colors
range from white through the full color (grade 11) to black. These scales are located on the
base plane of the hemisphere. (See color plate.)
YELLOW, RED, AND BLUE, FOR A TIME FIRMLY ESTABLISHED AS PRIMARY COLORS 71
Fig. 2-26 Sphere octant color space of Christian Doppler, 1847. The axes represent the three
primary colors, yellow (G), red (R), and blue (B).
Hayter
In 1826 the English architect and painter Charles Hayter (1761–1835) self-
published a book titled A new practical treatise on the three primitive colours
assumed as a perfect system of rudimentary information. Hayter’s choice of
primary colors were yellow, red, and blue, and he referred to Leonardo,
Newton, and Young as his predecessors. At the same time Hayter understood
that there is a difference “. . . between the properties of such materials as give
their colours to substances suitable to the purposes of art, and the transient
effects of light, which must not be considered as belonging to a system of
mixing colours for the purpose of painting.” Hayter offered six proofs for the
primacy of yellow, red, and blue:
First—That Yellow, Red, and Blue, are entire colours of themselves, and cannot
be produced by the mixture of any other colours. . . . Secondly—Yellow, Red, and
72 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
Blue, contain the sole properties of producing all other colours whatsoever, as
to colour . . . ; Thirdly—Because, by mixing proper portions of the Three Primi-
tives together, Black is obtained, providing for every possible degree of shadow.
Fourthly—And every practical degree of light is obtained by diluting any of the
colours . . . by the mixture of white paint. Fifthly—All transient or prismatic
effects can be imitated with the Three Primitive Colours, as permanently con-
sidered, but only to the same degree of compensation as white bears to light.
Sixthly—There are no other materials, in which colour is found, that are pos-
sessed of any of the foregoing perfections.”
Yellow, red, and blue as universal primaries received strong support for a time
from the Scottish physicist David Brewster (1781–1868), best known for his
work on light polarization. In his 1831 book A treatise on optics, Brewster
stated: “Red, yellow, and blue light exist at every point of the solar spectrum.
As a certain position of red, yellow, and blue constitute white light, the colour
of every point of the spectrum may be considered as consisting of the pre-
dominating colour at any point mixed with white light. . . .” He offered spec-
tral curves representing the content of red, yellow, and blue in spectral colors.
Brewster’s theory of three universal primary colors was widely accepted for
some thirty years until Helmholtz and Maxwell demonstrated its errors.
Fig. 2-27 Grassmann’s color circle, derived from Newton’s circle, 1853. The beginning
and end of the spectrum have been moved to the 12 o’clock position. The sector widths are
identical to Newton’s.
facts I would conclude that every ray of the spectrum is capable of stimulat-
ing all three pure sensations [related to Young’s three sensors], though in dif-
ferent degrees. The curve, therefore, which we have supposed to represent the
spectrum will be entirely within the triangle of colour. All natural or artificial
colours, being compounded of the colours of the spectrum, must lie within this
curve. . . .” In 1855 he had produced a schematic sketch of such an arrange-
ment. The triangle has at its vertices the three primary colors red, violet, and
green. One to one mixtures of these produce intermediate hues carmine, blue,
and yellow. The spectral colors are shown on the circle, with white at its center.
All real colors must fall on or within the circle (Fig. 2-28).
In the first edition of his Handbuch der physiologischen Optik (Treatise on
physiological optics) of 1867 Helmholtz offered estimated spectral sensitivity
curves for the three Youngian sensors (see Fig. 5-3) and a Maxwell-type tri-
angle with the resulting trace of spectral colors (Fig. 2-29). These excitation
curves were later measured first by Maxwell (Fig. 5-4) and later by Helmholtz’s
assistant Artur König (Fig. 5-5). Here Helmholtz also presented a view of the
basis plane, with opposing colors being complementary colors, as well as a view
from the top of his color cone which he compared to Lambert’s tetrahedron.
(In Lambert’s “subtractive” pyramid, black is in the center of the basis plane
and white is at the top, while in Helmholtz’s “additive” cone, white is in the
center of the basis plane and black on top; Figs. 2-30 and 2-31.)
In an 1872 paper on color vision Maxwell stated:
74 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
Fig. 2-28 Maxwell’s sketch of the fundamental color triangle based on the primaries red, green,
and violet at the apices, and white in the center. The prismatic and extraspectral purple colors
are located on a circle, with object colors falling within the circle, 1856.
Fig. 2-29 Helmholtz’s estimate of the location of the spectral curve (from the right: red,
yellow (G), green, cyan, indigo, violet, with white at the intersection point) in Maxwell’s primary
triangle, 1867.
Fig. 2-30 Basis plane of Helmholtz’s color cone with white in the center and the saturated
colors on the periphery, 1867.
Fig. 2-31 View from the top of Helmholtz’s cone, with black on top and tonal colors from black
to the saturated colors.
With these statements Maxwell provided the basis for all future mathematical/
geometrical models of psychological color space.
2.16 HERING
Helmholtz and his theory of color vision was opposed, at times bitterly, by
the German physician and physiologist Karl Ewald Konstantin Hering
(1834–1918) (e.g., see Turner, 1994). In the mid-1870s Hering proposed a seem-
ingly much different idea of color vision based not on three but on four
primary hues: yellow, red, blue, and green, which he called the natural color
76 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
All hues can be arranged in the circle so that these primary hues divide it into
its four quadrants. All colors in (one) half of this circle are clearly yellowish . . .
all those (in the other) have more or less blueness in common. . . . If one
imagines this color circle halved so that the dividing line passes . . . through two
intermediate colors that are opposite each other, for example, through a violet
and one of the greenish yellows, and if the hues in either half are compared, then
one finds no one chromatic quality common to all these hues. . . . If the dividing
line does not pass through two primary colors we always encounter colors that
have no chromatic property in common with certain other colors of the same
half and thus they do not have the slightest similarity of hue. In this way we
recognize . . . that a rational division of the color circle or grouping of color hues
in terms of their internal relations is possible only by using the four specified
primary colors. (Fig. 2-32)
Adjacent primary hues (but not those opposed) can form hue mixtures. The
hues in their most intense form (C) can be “nuanced” by admixture of white-
ness (W) and/or blackness (S). Hering presented as an example an equilateral
triangle with the full color, white and black at the corners (Fig. 2-33). He devel-
oped arithmetic principles that guide the composition of mixed colors. They
are composed of relative proportions of C, W, and S. Reinheit (purity) of color
can be expressed by C/(C + W + S). Hering was fully aware of the intrinsic
lightness of colors:
If one has a primary blue that is as clear as possible and finds a primary yellow
that one cannot say is either lighter or darker than the blue, then anyone with
good color vision who has even a little practice in color analysis will also observe
that the yellow is less clear than the blue or that it is more or less grayish or
blackish. On the other hand, if he has next to the clearest possible yellow a blue
that does not look decidedly darker than the yellow, then he will see that the
blue is whitish. . . . Moreover, I find a good primary red, that is, the clearest
possible, lighter than the clearest primary green available.
Hering did not construct an explicit color order system, and it is not clear how
he would have done so, that is, how he would have resolved the issue of the
different “brightness” of his primary colors.
HERING 77
Fig. 2-32 Hering’s diagram illustrating the composition of mixed hue perceptions from the
unique hues located on the main axes. The fractions of blue and red of three mixed hues are
shown bottom right, 1905–1911. (See color plate.)
Fig. 2-33 Schematical view of Hering’s constant hue triangle, with full color r, white w, and
black s on the vertices. Colors along the line parallel to that connecting w and s have constant
chromaticness. On the line r to g are located all colors derived from r by veiling with various
amounts of g, 1905–1911.
78 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
Hering was not satisfied with the Weber-Fechner law, which he found not
to apply in his investigations into gray scales. Instead, he proposed a relation-
ship between stimulus intensity and lightness response that equals a hyper-
bolic function. When the luminous reflectance is plotted on a log scale, this
relationship results in an S-shaped function, later found to also apply to cone
response kinetics.
Aubert was the first to recognize that Hering’s opponent color theory does
not have to be contradictory to that of Young and Helmholtz: “. . . if one
strictly distinguishes between the process of excitation and the process of
sensation.”38 This idea was pursued by Helmholtz and further developed by
Schrödinger and Luther (see below). It was the Dutch physiologist Franciscus
Donders (1818–1889) and the German physiologist Johannes von Kries
(1853–1928) who proposed a combination of the two theories in a “zone”
theory, assigning the Young-Helmholtz theory to the cone level and the Hering
theory to a later zone on the visual pathway.39 Despite Donders’s and Kries’s
proposals, the Young-Helmholtz theory of color vision became the leading
paradigm until Hering’s opponent-color theory was resuscitated in the second
half of the twentieth century.
Cubic Systems
In 1868 William Benson, an English architect, was, in his book Principles of
the science of colors, concisely stated to aid and promote their useful applica-
tion in the decorative arts, the first to propose a cubic system. He placed white
and black on two opposed corners of the tilted cube with yellow, pink, and sea
green on the upper three intermediate corners and red, blue, and green on the
lower three (Fig. 2-34). The center of the cube is occupied by a medium gray.
Variations of a cubic system were proposed by E. A. Hickethier (1940), and
others.
Fig. 2-34 View toward the top, white corner of Benson’s tilted color cube, 1868. The gray
scale is hidden behind the white sphere. (See color plate.)
Fig. 2-35 Bezold’s decagonal color pyramid, with black on top and white at the origin, 1874.
80 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
Fig. 2-36 Höfler’s double pyramid based on Hering’s unique hues, with the gray scale on the
vertical axis, 1897.
Fig. 2-37 Tilted double cone of Kirschmann, 1895. Zero and infinite brightness are at the
apices. Colors of constant brightness are on horizontal circular planes.
Fig. 2-38 Ebbinghaus’s tilted double pyramid with rounded corners, 1902. The gray scale is
located on the vertical axis.
object colors. His system was to be applicable to both, and he did not put
“white” and “black” at the two apices of his tilted double cone but the signs
0 and ∞, explaining: “It is an error to set “white” or “black” at the ends of the
axis of a color sphere or a double cone because these expressions do not
correctly designate the extremes of the achromatic series of light sensations
but instead are ideas of a rather complex nature.”
The German psychologist Hermann Ebbinghaus (1850–1909) offered in
1902 a color system in form of a tilted double pyramid. The neutral axis of
Ebbinghaus’s system is vertical, but the central opponent color plane is tilted
along the red–green axis with yellow being closer to white. All surface edges
of the solid are rounded because, as Ebbinghaus argued, the transitions
between colors are fluid (Fig. 2-38). In 1909 the English-American psycholo-
gist Edward Bradford Titchener (1867–1927) offered detailed instructions for
building a square tilted double-pyramid model (Fig. 2-39). Neither Höfler,
Kirschmann, Ebbinghaus, nor Titchener provided quantitative details of the
color arrangements (a painted version of Titchener’s pyramid, colored by the
82 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
Fig. 2-39 Titchener’s model of the tilted double pyramid. The rhombic central cross section is
shown on the right, 1909.
Fig. 2-40 Wundt’s cone with black on top and white on the origin, 1893.
occupy four, bluish five, and yellowish colors three. In 1892, in the second
edition of Vorlesungen über die Menschen- und Thierseele (Lectures on the
human and animal soul), Wundt offered a Helmholtzian cone with six basic
hues: red, orange, yellow, green, blue, and violet (Fig. 2-40). White was located
in the center of the base circle and black at the apex of the cone. In Grundriss
der Psychologie (1896), Wundt offered a double cone.
method of constant stimuli, making use of the error law (Fullerton and Cattell,
1892).
Psychophysics faced fundamental criticism by Kries who argued that
numbers applied to sensations are not quantities but merely convenient labels,
an issue not yet fully resolved. In the midtwentieth century the American psy-
chologist S. S. Stevens (1906–1973) developed a theory of sensory magnitude
based on extensive experimental data. He found that such data could usually
be modeled well with power functions. Chapter 3 offers a more detailed view
of psychophysics.
Fig. 2-41 Artist’s rendition of Munsell’s balanced color sphere, patented in 1900. The sphere
was rotatable to achieve additive color mixture to gray and thereby show the “balance” of the
colors on the sphere. The mirror in the back discloses the blue region of the sphere. (See color
plate.)
Munsell met with Ostwald (see below) and exchanged views on color order,
but both were to pursue their own views on what such a system was to look
like. In 1907 the first version of his Atlas of the Munsell Color System, a port-
folio of eight plates with painted paper samples, was published, followed in
1915 by an extended version with fifteen plates and a total of 880 color samples
(Munsell, 1907, 1915). The two versions offered for the first time detailed inter-
nal views of a color solid.
In 1918, shortly before Munsell’s death, the Munsell Color Company was
formed by Munsell’s son and friends. It produced painted paper chips of
Munsell colors and in 1929 issued the first version of the Munsell Book of
86 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
Color, a collection of forty pages, each containing chips of various value and
chroma of one given hue. The system is open ended in that the chips repre-
sent what is possible with available pigments. Identification of the samples is
by hue identifier, value level, and chroma level, for example, 2.5BG5/10
meaning a sample of hue 2.5 blue-green, value 5, and chroma 10. The units of
the three visual scales are not identical in perceived magnitude.
Modifications to the system (the so-called Munsell Renotations) were pro-
posed in 1943 by the Optical Society of America Subcommittee on the Spacing
of Munsell Colors (Newhall, Nickerson, and Judd, 1943) in form of colori-
metric values of the aim colors. They are the basis of the modern system (Fig.
2-42). Expanded editions, based on the Renotations, have been issued in Japan
(Chroma Cosmos 5000 and a condensed version thereof: Chromaton 707), and
the system has also been implemented in the form of textile samples. For more
details and an analysis of the Munsell system, see Chapter 7.
In 1912, before the publication of Munsell’s 15 plate color Atlas, the Ameri-
can ornithologist and botanist Robert Ridgway (1850–1925) published Color
standards and color nomenclature, an extensive proposal for what amounts to
a double-cone color order system. It consists of fifty-three plates each with
twenty-seven pigment painted color chips, a total of 1115 chips (excluding mul-
tiple whites and blacks). In several respects Ridgway’s system is a forerunner
of Ostwald’s. It is based on Maxwellian disk mixture. His primary disks were
produced using the brightest then available synthetic-organic dyes. His central
color plane was divided into 71 hue steps, taken in double steps, resulting in
thirty-six full color hues. Ridgway considered six hues to be fundamental: red,
orange, yellow, green, blue and violet. His fundamental red was located at 644
nm (as he had been assured it should be by the then associate physicist of the
U.S. National Bureau of Standards, P. G. Nutting), therefore being a quite yel-
lowish red. His yellow, green, and blue are located near the corresponding gen-
erally accepted unique hues. There are 22 steps containing redness, 17 with
yellowness, 13 with greenness, and 19 with blueness. The hue steps were meant
to be visually equidistant. There are 13 steps between unique red and blue but
only six between unique yellow and unique green. Disk mixtures toward white
and black for all hues were made in three grades each in constant increments
(Fig. 2-43). As Lambert already did Ridgway remarked that the admixtures to
yellow have a much different lightness scaling than those to blue. From the
central neutral gray scale of nine grades to the full color there are grades of
decreasing but always identical admixture of gray. Recent measurements of
an atlas have shown the gray scale to be essentially a Fechner type scale and
the color circle to be less than uniform. (Aside from possible colorant deteri-
oration it is also necessary to keep in mind the limited color measurement
capabilities when Ridgway developed his Atlas.)
RIDGWAY’S COLOR ATLAS 87
Fig. 2-42 Constant hue page from a modern version of the Munsell Book of Color. The gray
scale is not shown. The chroma scale begins at 1 and continues from 2 at two-grade intervals
to chroma 14. Value grades are shown from 2.5 to 9. Courtesy Gretag-Macbeth Company. (See
color plate.)
Fig. 2-43 Page from Ridgway’s color atlas showing three reddish blue hues lightened from
the central color in three steps toward white and darkened in four steps toward black, 1912.
(See color plate.)
OSTWALD’S FARBKÖRPER (COLOR SOLID) 89
The German chemist and Nobel prize winner Wilhelm Ostwald (1853–1932)
began in 1917 to publish a series of texts on color science, the first of which is
called Mathetische Farbenlehre (Theory of logical ordering of colors). Here
Ostwald described a double-cone color solid of related colors based on addi-
tive disk mixture. He scaled the resulting pychophysical solid using the Weber-
Fechner law to approximate the implied psychological color order. Ostwald
used Hering’s color equation: r + s + w = 1, where r is the amount of the Voll-
farbe (full color, colors of highest intensity), s that of black, and w that of white.
He interpreted the equation in terms of reflectance data in the visible portion
of the spectrum. However, the real colors he used in the disk mixture had, of
necessity, in all cases reflectances different from those idealized. Ostwald
based his system on three fundamental hues: yellow, red, and blue. The hue
circle begins with yellow occupying the 12 o’clock position, proceeding clock-
wise to red, blue, and green (unlike Munsell who placed red at 12 o’clock and
proceeded clockwise toward yellow). Opposing hues are complementary; that
is, when optically mixed they result in an achromatic color. Full colors are
located conventionally on the periphery of the central plane. All colors of a
given hue are placed on an equilateral triangle that is half of a vertical section
through the center of the double cone (Fig. 2-44). The central vertical axis is
a gray scale. As in Hering’s constant hue triangles, lines parallel to the line
connecting the full color and white are lines of equal blackness, lines parallel
to the line connecting full color and black are lines of equal whiteness. Lines
parallel to the line connecting black and white are lines of equal purity. Tonal
colors (tints) toward white are located in the upper half of the double cone
and tonal colors toward black (shades) in the bottom half.
To bring his psychophysical solid into agreement with psychological scaling,
Ostwald applied logarithmic scaling so that 16 grades from black to white and
black to full color, as well as white to full color, resulted in 15 visually equidis-
tant steps according to the Weber-Fechner law. In such an arrangement there
are 120 chromatic samples in a triangle and Ostwald applied a double-letter
system to identify them, in addition to the hue number. The system reveals
slices through the color solid in four different directions: equal hue, equal
whiteness and blackness, and equal purity. In Europe, the abridged Farbkör-
per illustrating on 12 plates 680 samples (24 hues and 8-step gray scale), when
first published in 1919 was a revelation of the multitude of colors, presented
systematically (see Fig. 2-45).
Ostwald’s system was analyzed extensively by Foss, Nickerson, and
Granville in 1944.They demonstrated that, unsurprisingly, it represents a series
of compromises and that it fills only a portion of the available gamut of the
Rösch-MacAdam limits (see Section 2.23). Perceptual differences between
grades in different areas of the solid were found to be of varying size.
Ostwald developed several sets of colorations as atlases of his system, the
most elaborate published as Der Farbenatlas, with 2500 samples (Ostwald,
90 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
Fig. 2-44 Color identification scheme for colors in a constant hue plane of Ostwald’s double-
cone color solid (1917). The grey scale is represented on the extreme right by colors aa to pp.
The full color is represented by pa.
1917). An atlas in two different sizes implemented with dyed wool samples
was also offered (Der Wollatlas). Ostwald used his color solid to demonstrate
his rules of color harmony (Ostwald, 1918). Implementations of the Ostwald
system after the Second World War are those by Aemilius Müller, who gave
up complementarity for improved uniform hue spacing (Müller, 1953) and the
Color Harmony Manual of the Container Corp. of America (Jacobson, 1942;
Granville, 1994), issued in three editions.
After Munsell and Ostwald several new color order systems were proposed,
all built on the ideas of double cones, double pyramids, or cubes. All of these
RÖSCH-MACADAM COLOR SOLID 91
Fig. 2-45 Vertical cross section through Ostwald’s double-cone color solid illustrating constant
hue colors 1 and 13, with veiling toward white and black. The achromatic scale is at the center.
From Farbkörper, undated. (See color plate.)
systems formed more or less idealized geometrical solids and did not offer any
fundamentally new ideas. Among them are:
• Rounded double cone with tilted central plane from 1920 by the Ameri-
can art educator Arthur Pope (1880–1974). Similar systems were subse-
quently developed in Sweden by Tryggve Johansson (1939) and Sven
Hesselgren (1953), and by Frans Gerritsen (1975).
• Modified double cone by C. Villalobos-Dominguez and J. Villalobos in
1947, developed in Argentina and with an atlas consisting of 7000 samples
produced by halftone printing.
• Double pyramid of 1929 by the American psychologist Edwin G. Boring.
• Tilted cube systems by E. Alfred Hickethier, 1940, issued with 1000
printed samples, and Harald Küppers, 1972, issued with 1400 printed
samples.
Fig. 2-46 Projective view of the Rösch-MacAdam color solid containing all object colors as
viewed by the CIE standard observer in standard daylight D65. The axes represent CIE chro-
maticity coordinates x and y and luminous reflectance Y. From Wyszecki and Stiles (1982).
Fig. 2-47 Luther-Nyberg color solid for object colors viewed in standard daylight D65, based
on the color moments M1 and M2 and the color weight S. From Wyszecki and Stiles (1982).
Fig. 2-48 Schematic depiction of the DIN6164 color solid based on attributes hue (T), satu-
ration (S), and darkness degree (D). W denotes the white point. From Richter (1976).
to white much shorter than to black. A schematical view is shown in Fig. 2-48.
The three color attributes are hue number T, saturation degree S, and dark-
ness degree D. Colors of constant hue as defined in the system have constant
dominant wavelength regardless of degree of saturation, and the perceived
hue therefore generally varies slightly along these lines. The 24 simple hues of
the system, modified from the Ostwald system, begin with greenish yellow at
12 o’clock and proceed clockwise toward red. They are intended to be visu-
ally equidistant. Between unique yellow (approximately DIN color 2) and
unique red (DIN 9) there are seven steps, between red and blue (DIN 17)
eight steps, between blue and green (DIN 21) four steps, and from green to
yellow five steps. Six saturation steps were visually scaled at one lightness level
only, and the results were extrapolated to other levels. Colors of equal degree
of saturation are located on roughly elliptical contours in the chromaticity
diagram, not unlike the equal chroma contours of the Munsell Renotations.
The darkness degree D is on a scale of 0 to 10, with twenty steps illustrated.
It is based on a formula recommended by Delboeuf in 1872, with experimen-
tally determined values for the constants:
where h is the relative lightness A/A0, with A0 representing the lightness of the
optimal color of the same hue (at the Rösch-MacAdam limit). White has the
value D = 0 and black D = 10.
The DIN system is based on limited visual data (scaling of hue at one level
of saturation, scaling of saturation for eight hues at one level of lightness,
scaling of darkness degree to find the applicable constants in the Delboeuf
OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA UNIFORM COLOR SCALES 95
The system was described by K. Witt (1981), K. Richter and Witt (1986), and
G. Derefeldt (1991).
Intensive study of uniform color scales during and after the Second World War
resulted in new insights about difficulties in representing hue, chroma, and
lightness in a euclidean system. The Munsell Renotations were completed in
1943, but there was an obvious, considerable discrepancy between those data
and the MacAdam color-matching error data of 1942 (see Chapter 6). In 1947
the Optical Society of America formed a research committee to develop as
uniform an atlas with samples of a uniform color solid as possible and a
formula to express the implicit space, usable for calculation of color differ-
ences from colorimetric data. It was headed from 1947 to 1972 by Dean B.
Judd and then by MacAdam. The committee very early on decided to present
its results in form of a crystalline internal structure of color space, thus to
abandon the Munsellian attributes. In the chosen system twelve equal dis-
tances around a center color form a cubo-octahedron (Fig. 2-49). The result is
the Optical Society of America Uniform Color Scales (MacAdam, 1974).
Overlapping cubo-octahedra make it possible to fill color space with this crys-
talline infrastructure. The cubo-octahedra are arranged in a manner that
results in a grid of squares in a given lightness plane, with all differences along
the horizontal and vertical directions of the grid implicitly of equal perceptual
size (Fig. 2-50). Grids at the next higher and next lower lightness increment
level are offset. This arrangement does not result in explicit lines of equal hue
or circles of equal chroma but allows seven different kinds of cleavage planes
that provide new vistas within the color solid (Fig. 2-51). In 1974 an atlas with
558 painted samples was made commercially available and the colorimetric
aim values for these samples have been published (MacAdam, 1978).
The system is open ended, and samples are identified by three rectangular
coordinates L, j, g, where L denotes lightness, j approximately yellowness or
blueness, and g approximately greenness or redness. Formulas have been
96 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
Fig. 2-49 A central color (black dot) surrounded by twelve visually equidistant colors located
in the center of spheres and forming a cubo-octahedral structure. From Gerstner (1986).
Fig. 2-50 Square grid constant lightness plane of the L, g, j space with the rotated half
cubo-octahedron placed over it. Grids at the next higher and lower lightness levels are
offset by one half chromatic step, as illustrated by the portion of the cubo-octahedron. From
Gerstner (1986).
A system called, after Hering, Natural Color System was developed in Sweden
in the 1970s. It traces its sources to Hering and Ostwald, and more recently to
SWEDISH NATURAL COLOR SYSTEM 97
Fig. 2-51 View of MacAdam’s model of the OSA-UCS color solid illustrating the existence of
several cleavage planes. See text for more detail. Slide courtesy D. L. MacAdam. (See color
plate.)
efforts by Johansson (1949) and Hesselgren (1954). The fundamental idea was
that any color normal observer is able to determine in any object color the
“content” of one or two fundamental full colors and of black and white. The
three, respectively four, percentages always add up to 100%. In a manner com-
parable to Ostwald’s, the Natural Color System is represented by a double
cone. A forty-hue circle of full colors based on experimentally determined
unique hues is located at the edge of the double cone (Fig. 2-52). The central
axis is represented by a gray scale with ten steps, with white on top. Mixtures
of full colors with white or black are located on the surface of the double cone.
Mixtures of full colors with white and black are located in the interior. Colors
of equal hue are located on triangles bounded by white, black, and the full
color (Fig. 2-53). Colors are identified by hue number, NCS chromaticness c,
and NCS blackness s. An atlas representing the system has been developed,
containing 1750 matt color chips arranged in triangles on forty equal hue
planes. Every other plane illustrates colors from c = 10 while those between
98 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
Fig. 2-52 Hue circle of the NCS color solid with hue designation scheme, identifying ten hue
steps between unique hues. Courtesy NCS.
In 1931 the then just formed (American) Inter-Society Color Council (ISCC)
began work under Godlove on a standardized method (in English) to desig-
nate colors. An initial list was issued in 1939, and Kelly and Judd issued in 1955
NBS Circular 553 The ISCC-NBS Method of Designating Colors and a Dic-
tionary of Color Names. In 1976 the NBS Special Publication 440 Color: Uni-
versal Language and Dictionary of Names was issued (Kelly and Judd, 1976).
The Munsell color solid was subdivided into regions that can be designated by
common names. The system is applicable to opaque, translucent, and trans-
parent colored materials. Twenty-nine major color regions contain a total of
267 subregions around centroid colors. Among the twenty-six major regions
are those of the primary colors black, white, yellow, red, blue, and green, as
well as secondary and tertiary colors such as orange, brown, olive, purplish
pink, and violet. The subregions are identified by modifiers, such as vivid,
strong, deep, dark, light, and grayish. A supplement contains on eighteen pages
a sample of the centroid color of the 267 subregions, with its ISCC-NBS name
and Munsell Renotation.
In the 1976 publication Kelly describes six levels of precision of designat-
UNIVERSAL COLOR LANGUAGE 99
Fig. 2-53 NCS constant hue triangle of hue Y90R with full color C, white W, and black
S. Colors of constant blackness s are located on lines parallel to W–C, colors of constant
chromaticness c fall on lines parallel to W–S. Courtesy NCS. (See color plate.)
ing colors. Level 1 consists of thirteen generic color names: white, gray and
black, yellow, orange, brown, red, pink, purple, blue, green, olive, yellow green.
On level 2 sixteen intermediate hue names are added, such as reddish brown,
bluish green, and purplish pink. Level 3 contains the 267 subregions. On level
4 the space is further subdivided into more than 1000 colors, as in the Munsell
Book of Color. Some 100,000 colors can be specified on level 5 by visually
interpolating between Munsell chips, such as a color specified as 7.8PB
100 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS
Fig. 2-54 Color Picker screen from Adobe® Photoshop showing specification of a given
reddish blue color in four systems: HSB, Lab (=L*, a*, b*), RGB, and CMYK. (See color plate.)
taneously in terms of HSB, RGB, CMYK, and CIELAB L*a*b*, and they
allow input values from any of these systems to display (with a degree of accu-
racy that depends on many factors) the corresponding color. There are
complex issues of color management in reproducibility of such CRT colors in
different media (color transparency or opaque color photos, printing on dif-
ferent types of printers, from computer printers to commercial printers).
a point. For an extensive set of samples, such as those of the Munsell system
or of images of natural scenes, about 90% of the variation in the spectral func-
tions is explained by three basis functions (Lenz et al., 1996; Wachtler et al.,
2001). There is an all-positive nearly horizontal function that can be seen as
being the spectral equivalent of a lightness function. The next two functions
have positive and negative lobes and can be seen as rough approximations
of opponent color functions (see Section 6.18). They extract information in
spectral functions more efficiently than functions describing the spectral
sensitivities of human cones. On the other hand, while both kinds of
functions place the reflectances of a Munsell hue circle into ordinal order,
(with some exceptions) only the cone and color-matching functions place them
approximately in interval order in regard both to hue difference and chroma.
There are several mathematical treatments that can be used for PCA and
similar operations. Some of these have found use in color constancy and color
appearance modeling (Maloney, 1999). However, for the reasons given and
others it is not justified to call spectral spaces based on reflectance functions
color spaces.
Color Space and Its Divisions: Color Order from Antiquity to the Present, by Rolf G. Kuehni
ISBN 0-471-32670-4 Copyright © 2003 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
104
FUNDAMENTS OF PSYCHOPHYSICS 105
S = F1 (I ). (3-1)
R = F2 (I ). (3-2)
R = F (I ). (3-3)
tion theory. More intense stimuli have greater information content than less
intense ones. According to Norwich sensation provides a measure of the infor-
mation content in the stimuli:
S = kH , (3-4)
possible reasons are changes in the adaptation level based on the previously
seen stimulus. The range of stimuli displayed and the level of stimulus are also
known to affect the result, particularly in case of magnitude estimates. Among
midgets and basketball players the meaning of small, medium, and large
involves absolute values that are much different.
Context effects are well known. How the lightness of a surface is assessed
depends not only on the adaptation level but also on other contextual infor-
mation such as is it seen in shadow or in direct illumination. Similar effects
apply to chromatic surfaces.
Despite its lack of a solid foundation psychophysics persists because it has
considerable pragmatic value. The continuing interest in psychophysics may
also be due to what L. E. Marks called “the metaphorical imperative” (1978).
Accordingly we have a need to express our inner world in terms of metaphors
that are more easily comprehended by other humans. Scaling and magnitude
determinations of perceptions can be seen as an expression of the metaphori-
cal imperative.
When viewed from the perspective of Fechner’s outer and inner psy-
chophysics, mentioned in Chapter 2, the conventional psychophysical enter-
prise can be represented in the schematic sketch of Fig. 3-1. The relationship
between stimulus and conscious experience represents classical psychophysics.
The relationship between nerve excitation and inner sensation is in the domain
of neurophysiology and as such subject to process variability that can be
Fig. 3-1 Conventional psychophysical enterprise as viewed from the perspective of Fechner’s
inner and outer psychophysics (modified from Murray, 1993). Outer psychophysics attempts to
connect the stimulus magnitude with the response magnitude. Inner psychophysics attempts
to connect nerve excitation with nonconscious “sensation.” It is quantified by recording from
visual system cells. Statistical variability and signal detection are taken to be aspects of inner
psychophysics.
108 PSYCHOPHYSICS
3.2 CATEGORIES
Visual classification is believed to take data from the visual area at the back
of the brain to the inferior temporal cortex of the brain located near the
temples. This region appears to analyze the data for certain features. It
exchanges data with the prefrontal cortex area in the front of the brain an area
that may contain category border codes. Categorization is achieved jointly
between the latter two areas (Hasegawa and Miyashita, 2002).
In regard to color classification the fundamental question is what process
results in setting classification boundaries. If given a thousand randomly dif-
ferent Munsell color chips and asked to categorize them, how would we do it
if we had not been exposed to theories and examples before? An obvious
attribute for categorization is hue. But it has taken until the seventeenth
century to recognize that hue forms a closed circular continuum. Newton has
separated spectral colors into seven categories and saw that bluish red and
purple colors, not found in the spectrum, can close the spectral array into a
circle. Rational classification of hued colors according to whiteness and black-
ness or lightness and chroma only took place in the nineteenth and early twen-
tieth centuries.
There is the question of the cause of human color categorization in lan-
guage. As briefly mentioned in Chapter 2, there is a theory by Berlin and Kay
that basic color terminology in human languages has followed the same
pattern. They identified eleven basic color terms, aside from the six Hering
fundamental colors: brown, purple, pink, orange, and gray. The list is notable
for its idiosyncrasy: all four chromatic colors are in the yellow-red region of a
color circle. In addition the English term pink had in the sixteenth and
seventeenth centuries the words yellow or brown attached to it (Merrifield,
1967), and the color of English hunting jackets is called pink. There are no
terms for yellow-green, blue-green, and their dark, desaturated versions olive
and navy. While there should have been a good number of such colors in
natural surroundings in many environments, it has taken systematic catego-
rization of the hue circle to recognize these colors as categories. In C.D.’s
extended color circle they are listed as yellowish green and sea blue (see Fig.
2-12). A convincing theory of color categorization that explains the historical
development of basic color terms seems to be a long way off.
The Weber-Fechner law assumes that just noticeable differences can be con-
sidered units of perception. It states that the increment required to result in a
just noticeable perceived difference is a constant percentage or fraction of the
stimulus magnitude (see Chapter 2). The Weber-Fechner law has been found
to be context sensitive.Typical experimental Weber fractions are given in Table
3-1 and an example of the Weber-Fechner law is illustrated in Fig. 3-2.
On a parallel but initially less conspicuous track runs the idea of a power
relationship between stimulus and response. Power relationships were
110 PSYCHOPHYSICS
Fig. 3-2 Relation between stimulus and sensation according to the Weber-Fechner law. The
logarithmic function illustrates the relationship between an arithmetic (sensation) and a geo-
metric (stimulus) scale.
more steps. The results were in better agreement with the Weber-Fechner
law, and the power law was largely forgotten until it found its champion in
Stevens.
Before Stevens it was J. P. Guilford who proposed in 1932 the first general
power law of psychophysics:
DI = ca I n , (3-5)
V = bx p , (3-6)
Fig. 3-3 Relation between stimulus and sensation based on three different power modulations.
From analysis of his own work and that of other researchers, Stevens con-
cluded that magnitude estimates of sensory experiences tended to have an
exponent near 1. In magnitude judgments the observer is free to assign any
chosen number to express the magnitude of the perceived stimulus. His own
results of magnitude judgments of the steps of a gray scale resulted in a power
exponent of 1.2 (Stevens and Galanter, 1957). When making category judg-
ments and difference judgments well above threshold, the power exponent
declined in size, and when making difference judgments near threshold, it
approached zero and the Weber-Fechner law.
Category scales have set numbers or adjectives and the results of category
judgments are usually nonlinearly related to those of magnitude judgments. A
gray scale based on equisection or category judgments, namely on differences,
results typically in an exponent of 0.25 to 0.5. In his own multiple bisection
experiment of brightness Stevens (1953) found an exponent of 0.26. Thresh-
olds are category measurements related to the uncertainty of the response,
with a relationship to the stimulus difference most accurately expressed as a
logarithmic function. The power function can also be closely matched with an
hyperbolic function, first implicitly used by Hering (1874):
ax
V= , (3-7)
1 + kx
where a and k are constants and x is the intensity of the stimulus. In a plot
with a linear ordinate and a log abscissa scale the resulting function has an S
shape. Thresholds, and the Weber-Fechner law approximating them represent
PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING: LEVELS OF MEASUREMENT 113
TABLE 3-3 Effect of judgment type and size of difference on applicable stimulus
modulation
Type of Evaluation Applicable Power Example
Magnitude 0.8 and larger Lightness of gray papers
Paired comparison 0.05–0.75 Small color differences, Munsell chroma scales
Thresholds 0.01–0.33 Color thresholds
(in Fechnerian terms) local psychophysics; the power law is, in terms of
differences or magnitudes, representative of more global psychophysics.
Table 3-3 provides a comparison.
It is apparent that neither the logarithmic nor the power law provides an
explanation for the causes of the psychophysical relationships.They are merely
mathematical models. In the case of visual scaling the explanation for the
apparent signal compression is buried in the complexity of the visual system
and the possible impact of empiricism. While some details about the visual
system are known at this time, they are not nearly enough to develop a fully
detailed model predictive of all known effects. J. C. Baird has developed a
general simulation model that attempts to explain the effects described by the
laws from the aggregate action of neural cells with varying thresholds (sensory
aggregate model; Baird, 1997). It is not evident that this model applies to visual
scaling. K. Richter (1996) has proposed a model that attempts to explain visual
psychophysics only in terms of cone receptor saturation. It seems more likely
that post-receptor effects also play a role.
Psychophysics in the classical sense depends on so-called linking proposi-
tions.These are propositions of how neurophysiology and psychophysics might
be connected. Linking propositions can be strong or weak, depending on the
level of evidence supporting the proposition. A typical linking proposition is
that chromatic perception is supported by neurons with opponent response in
the visual area of the brain. Linking propositions related to the concept of a
uniform color space are still weak. The modern view of vision is that the brain
constructs what we consciously experience from the output of several of its
visual modules and using many (and probably many as yet unknown) decision
rules to construct the experience from generally ambiguous input at the retinal
level. As S. E. Palmer expressed it: “The job of visual perception is to combine
external and internal information to make meaningful facts about the envi-
ronment available to the organism” (Palmer, 1999). Simple links between
neural sensitivities to stimuli and psychological measurements or judgments
are therefore unlikely except in highly relativized situations.
may be placed into a hierarchy with each subsequent type having greater
explanatory power than the previous one. This hierarchy is called levels of
measurement.
Nominal Scales
They are at the lowest level and refer to names or identifications of items only.
The same symbol is assigned to two things if they have the same value of the
attribute. An applicable sample for color is names. Colors can be grouped, for
example, into blues, browns, pinks, purples, and grays. An appropriate statistic
is the number of cases.
Ordinal Scales
Numbers are assigned to things in a way that the order of the numbers reflects
the order of the attribute. Items are placed into ascending or descending rank
order depending on some kind of magnitude. For colors a typical example is
a series of grays that can be ordered according to the concept of blackness.
The color nearest to black (sample E in Fig. 3-4) has ordinal scale value 1, that
closest to it but lighter (sample A) has value 2, and so on. A series of chro-
matic colors of varied chromaticness can also be placed into an ordinal chroma
scale. Ordinal scales do not contain any information about the sensory mag-
nitude of the steps between the grades on the scale. In Fig. 3-4 a random
Fig. 3-4 Twelve random samples of gray papers (A–K) are placed into an ordinal scale based
on their luminous reflectance value Y and on an interval scale based on lightness L*.
PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING: LEVELS OF MEASUREMENT 115
Interval Scales
At the next level interval scales provide quantitative information concerning
the distances or differences between grades. Here things are assigned numbers
so that differences between numbers reflect differences of the attribute. In an
interval scale two grades differing by three interval units at the lower end of
the scale, and two grades at the higher end of the scale also differing by three
interval units are equally distant (e.g., the Celsius and Fahrenheit temperature
scales). In interval scales the distance between two percepts is represented
with a number according to
a = mb + c0 , (3-8)
Ratio Scales
These are interval scales that have a natural origin. In this case things are
assigned numbers such that differences and ratios between the numbers reflect
differences and ratios of the attribute. While in interval scales two numbers
could be applied arbitrarily to the scale (m and c0) only one number can be
arbitrarily assigned:
a = mb. (3-9)
Absolute Scales
Here things are assigned numbers such that all properties of the numbers
reflect analogous properties of the attribute. These measurement levels are
part of a continuum of order, with the level of order being weakest for nominal
scales and strongest for absolute scales. In practice, depending on the mea-
surement technique used, the same scale can have properties of two mea-
surement levels.
Matching Method
A well-known experiment, believed related to color thresholds, is the deter-
mination of color matching error by MacAdam (1942). He constructed an
apparatus in which color stimuli could be varied along selected lines in the
CIE chromaticity diagram at constant luminosity. A standard stimulus was dis-
played and the observer adjusted a test field until the two fields matched, that
is, had identical appearance. From the variability of repeated tests MacAdam
calculated the matching error that he thought to be related to the difference
threshold.
These methods are known under the general rubric of confusability scaling.
Thurstone stated a law of comparative judgment in 1927. This law considers
every perceptual magnitude judgment as a variable datum from a discrim-
inable process that he took to be normally distributed. In this manner the full
power of statistics of normal distribution can be applied. Under specific con-
ditions such statistical treatment results in confirmation of the Weber-Fechner
law, other conditions result in applicability of Steven’s power law.
Partition Scaling
First used in the form of the equisection method by Plateau, partition scaling
is a direct estimation method. In the equisection method the perceptual dis-
tance between two different stimuli may be halved in several steps. Another
version determines equal-appearing intervals.
number. Thus a certain sound may be said to represent the value 10, and the
observer is asked to modify the sound stimulus so that the resulting loudness
of the sound represents the value 15, say. In magnitude estimation the observer
estimates the perceived magnitude of experiences from stimuli and assigns
numbers to them. For color scaling a large number of stimuli or the capabil-
ity of stepwise or continuous stimulus adjustment is required.
Category Scaling
Here the observer is asked to separate large numbers of experiences into
categories. The corresponding samples must be similar enough so different
observers arrive at different categorization. The variability in judgment by dif-
ferent observers is assumed to follow a standard normal distribution from
which an interval scale can be constructed. A typical example is acceptability
or pass-fail judgments of small color differences in which the observer is asked
to determine if a given sample meets or fails a criterion of acceptability.
Paired Comparison
In this method all samples are presented to the observer in all possible pairs
or in all pairs of test against a reference sample. The proportion of times a
given sample is judged greater in magnitude of a given attribute is determined.
Interval scales are derived from the results under the assumption of statisti-
cally normal distribution.
In recent years a form of interval judgment for the purpose of suprathresh-
old color scaling has been in wide use. In this method sample pairs exhibiting
small differences are compared against a reference difference, usually in form
of an achromatic pair with a perceptual difference of similar magnitude to
those of the sample pairs under estimation. Alternately, an International Stan-
dards Organization (ISO) type gray scale that displays achromatic pairs with
varying perceptual differences has been used. In such situations what are mul-
tidimensional color differences (usually involving hue, chroma, and lightness
differences at the same time) are evaluated as if they were unidimensional.
The surround lightness and chromaticness affects the perceptual magnitude of
the reference pair(s), as well as of the test pairs making the result a function
of the surround.
Fig. 3-5 Example of a psychometric function. Samples contain different amounts of a red and
a gray stimulus (by spinning disk mixture). Observers judged if a sample contained more red
than the one shown previously. Circles indicate results when a reference sample containing
80% red was shown immediately before the test sample, triangles where no reference was
shown. From Stevens (1975).
Fig. 3-6 Configuration of MDS analysis of judged differences between fourteen spectral colors
(circles with numbers and identified by spectral wavelength in nanometers. A circle segment
(closed to a full circle) has been fitted to the points. Presumably on the segment between spec-
tral colors 1 and 14 extraspectral red and purple colors mixed from various ratios of 1 and 14
would be located. From Shepard (1964).
ent MDS techniques often find several different models, all with comparable
accuracy of fit. A widely used methodology of MDS is INDSCAL (individual
difference scaling; Carroll and Chang, 1970). It is based on euclidean geome-
try, but evidence has been found that it can “adequately” recover sample con-
figurations even if the true metric is non-euclidean.
In one version of MDS the pairwise distance (in terms of a visual estimate
or any other distance measurement) between all items used in the test is estab-
lished. From these data a similarity or dissimilarity matrix is created as input
to the MDS module. Several parameters or assumptions can be changed in
MDS and the results are subject to interpretation.
An interesting finding of MDS is that when perceptual distances between
fourteen spectral colors are judged by observers the result of the MDS analy-
sis of the data is a two-dimensional structure that is fit best by a circle segment
(Fig. 3-6; Shepard, 1994).
MDS has been applied extensively to Munsell data by T. Indow and co-
workers (Indow, 1988) and others. Using judgments of magnitude of color dif-
ference between many samples of the Munsell system as input into the MDS
analysis, Indow recovered the psychological diagram of the Munsell system
with good accuracy (see Fig. 4-4 in the next chapter). In principle, scaling of
PSYCHOLOGICAL AND PSYCHOPHYSICAL SPACES 121
small complex color differences (involving more than one attribute) might be
analyzed by MDS to determine the implicit dimensions. This does not appear
to have been done so far, perhaps because the dimensionality of the color
experience is believed known.
The MDS method is not without its critics (see e.g., Saunders and van
Brakel, 2002). Some criticism harks back to Kries original doubts about psy-
chophysics, namely are magnitudes and differences distances that can mean-
ingfully be expressed in numerical or geometrical form.
Vision is defined as the sense, mediated by the eyes, by which the positions,
qualities, and movements of objects are perceived. The generally accepted
view is that patterns of light energy are absorbed by light sensitive cells in the
eye. The resulting electrochemical signals are passed into the brain where they
cause activation of other cell types eventually resulting in sensations and per-
ceptions of form, color, and movement. A representation of the outside world
is generated in the retina and eventually in the brain. There are standard rela-
tionships between stimulus and resulting perception for which, presumably,
neural correlates exist in the brain. Exceptions to these standard relationships
are just that. They are caused by limitations in the neural apparatus. Much of
the efforts of visual science in the 1980s and 1990s have been attempts at elu-
cidation of these standard relationships and exceptions and finding their
neural correlates (e.g., see De Valois, 2000). But parts of this view face serious
problems from a slew of perceptual responses that are not explained by
standard models.
Already Helmholtz thought that perception is the result of the interaction
of the neural messages from the eyes with stored memories from past experi-
ences (Helmholtz, 1866). With the wider acceptance of Darwin’s ideas about
evolution in the twentieth century, some researchers began to look at senses
Color Space and Its Divisions: Color Order from Antiquity to the Present, by Rolf G. Kuehni
ISBN 0-471-32670-4 Copyright © 2003 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
123
124 COLOR ATTRIBUTES AND PERCEPTUAL ATTRIBUTE SCALING
As has been discussed earlier, it is reasonably certain that Forsius and Glisson
did not have a concept of a third attribute, beyond hue and lightness.
The concept of saturation as an attribute of prismatic color makes its defin-
itive appearance with Newton. It is clearly based on his experiments with over-
lays of various wavebands of light. He described points on a radial line in his
diagram (see Fig. 2-10) as indicating colors “proportional to the fullness or
intenseness of the [prismatic] Colour, that is, to its distance from whiteness.”
Newton recognized seven primary and an indefinite number of intermediate
hues. He described lightness in connection with colorants as follows: “Now
considering that these grey and dun Colours may also be produced by mixing
whites and blacks, and by consequence differ from perfect whites not in
Species of Colours but only in degree of luminousness, it is manifest that there
is nothing more requisite to make them perfectly white than to increase their
Light sufficiently . . .” (Newton, 1704, p. 112). Here we see for the first time (if
not in the same place in the book) mention of three attributes describing color
perceptions.
Mayer, who in the mideighteenth century developed the first plan for a
three-dimensional color order system, did not distinguish color series from the
most saturated colors on the surface of his double triangular pyramid through
the interior of the triangle. A possible reason is that in his own efforts with
pigments he ended up with a neutral gray quite removed from the gravimet-
ric center of the triangle, according to his own information at r3 g2 b7, indicat-
ing that his scheme of determining relative strength of pigments was less than
perfect.
Also Lambert did not recognize saturation or chroma as a separate color
attribute, even though it can be seen as implicit in his single pyramid. The
reason is similar to the one applicable in the case of Mayer. His three primary
colors are placed on the same level regardless of their lightness, and his choice
of colorants and mixture ratios did not result in black falling onto the gravi-
metric center of his basis triangle but, again, not far from the blue corner.
The painter Runge provided clear and explicit discussion of the general idea
of saturation, but without using such a term. Because he placed the full colors
on the circumference of the central horizontal plane of his color sphere, its
vertical dimension does not refer to brightness or lightness. But Runge under-
stood clearly the desaturation of a full color, by the admixture of appropri-
ately selected complementary colors or combinations: “When we add to pure
green, a product of yellow and blue, the smallest amount of red as the third
color, we learn that it simply destroys and dirties the pleasant appearance of
green without adding the appearance of redness. Therefore, through a stronger
admixture of red, green is dissolved into completely colorless smut, or gray,
and assumes a reddish hue only through an even stronger admixture. . . . All
diametrically opposed colors and mixtures on the circle are (in the center point
of the circle) dissolved (into gray)” (Runge, 1810). In the colored figure of
the equatorial cross section of his sphere (see Fig. 2-22) Runge shows four
desaturation steps toward the central gray for each full color.1
Grassmann, as we have seen in Chapter 2, supplied Newton’s diagram with
126 COLOR ATTRIBUTES AND PERCEPTUAL ATTRIBUTE SCALING
Every impression on the eye made by an arbitrarily mixed light can always be
represented as a function of three variables that can be expressed in numbers,
that is,
1. Quantity of saturated colored light,
2. Quantity of white light that when admixed results in the same color
impression,
3. Wavelength of the colored light. (Helmholtz 1860)
In the Handbook he described the three attributes in the form still in use:
Fig. 4-1 Brücke’s vertical section through the color sphere with white on top and black on the
bottom, 1866. The full interior lines are meant to be lines of constant saturation.
color). The shapes of B and C are designed to produce from black and white
the appropriate level of gray to be added to the full color so that all resulting
saturation steps have identical lightness (thus representing a chroma scale
when seen as object colors). The exact shape of B and C depends on the pho-
tometrically measured luminosity of the color of A. With this method
Kirschmann investigated the validity of the Weber-Fechner law in regard to
saturation.
In Hering’s system full colors occupy one corner of equilateral triangles.
The remaining colors in a triangle consist of those of the gray scale and the
veiled (verhüllte) colors with the same hue as the full color. As mentioned in
Chapter 2, Hering was fully aware of the varying lightness of full colors but
never explicitly described how lightness as an attribute was to fit into his
“natural color system.” Any color could be described as the sum of one or two
primary colors, white and black. Thus, in the majority of cases, colors had four
attributes, for example, for a brown yellowness, redness, whiteness, and black-
ness. Hering abandoned use of the term saturation because he believed its
meaning to be “contaminated” by Helmholtz. He defined colors of “equally
strong veiling” as those falling on lines parallel to the line connecting white
and black in his triangle,4 those having the same ratio of full color to total color
128 COLOR ATTRIBUTES AND PERCEPTUAL ATTRIBUTE SCALING
Fig. 4-2 Construction of a disk arrangement for the spinning disk mixture resulting in fifteen
concentric circles at equal luminance with perceptually equally (according to the Weber-Fechner
law) different steps of saturation of the color displayed on shape A. From Kirschmann
(1895).
ment with those of Helmholtz, were thus named hue, value, and chroma.
Munsell, intrigued with the decimal system, selected a 100-part hue circle and
five primary colors rather than Hering’s four (he thought Hering to be wrong
because Hering’s claims for the operation of his opponent-color system were
not in accord with contemporary knowledge about the retinal structure). His
value scale consisted of ten steps. The chroma scale was open ended because
Munsell soon found that he could not fit all colors with uniform chroma
spacing into a sphere form. As described below and in Chapter 7, scales for
all three attributes were intensively investigated before finding their final form
in the Munsell Renotations.
Ostwald, working in isolation during the First World War, erroneously
believed Helmholtz had made no distinction between what we now call bright-
ness and lightness.6 For this reason he rejected lightness as an attribute for his
system and followed Hering in using full color content, whiteness and black-
ness. But unlike Hering, he based his hue circle on three primary colors, yellow,
red, and blue. Object colors were defined as the sum of full color perceptions
and perceptions of white and black. Hering’s colors of equally strong veiling
were termed by Ostwald Reingleiche (colors of equal purity). Analysis of
Ostwald’s system in 1944 indicated that:
1. The system did not cover the full range of object colors as defined by
the Rösch-MacAdam limits.
2. Ostwald’s claims for complementarity and visual uniformity in terms of
hue differences around the hue circle have not been met and cannot
be met.
3. Various compromises have been made by Ostwald in physically imple-
menting his system (Foss, Nickerson, and Granville, 1944).
Basic ideas of Hering were also used by Johannsson and Hesselgren and
resulted in the development of the Swedish Natural Color System (see Chap-
ters 2 and 7).
In Chapter 2 it was mentioned that white and black have been considered
colors for some 3000 years or more. But there has been a certain amount of
controversy about this subject. Non-hued visual percepts are often called
achromatic colors, that is, colorless colors. Truly achromatic colors differ from
chromatic colors in the lack of the key perceptual attribute of hue. While hued
colors are well defined, unique blue is a bluish color that is neither greenish
nor reddish, neither black nor white are well defined in a similar sense. This
becomes apparent if one looks, for example, at a collection of white and
black textiles. Generally preferred white and black both have a slightly bluish
130 COLOR ATTRIBUTES AND PERCEPTUAL ATTRIBUTE SCALING
tinge. But the preferences differ among individuals and cultures. In addition,
while not any chromatic stimulus can appear unique red, any neutrally reflect-
ing surface can be made to look white or black or any gray in between depend-
ing on illumination and surround. Perceptions of white and black as the
achromatic extremes are outputs of our color vision system, just as percep-
tions of hue and chroma are. It appears entirely justified to consider achro-
matic colors to be on an equivalent level with chromatic colors.
Helmholtz defined black as the absence of light energy, while Hering,
correctly, defined it as an indirect manifestation of light in terms of contrast.
When looking at reflectance data of pigmented black and white paint layers,
it is obvious that while the white layer reflects most light the black layer
reflects very little. But the small amount it reflects is responsible for the fact
that a black pigment layer, selectively illuminated with high intensity white
light, can be made to look white (as demonstrated by A. Gelb in 1929). Con-
trast plays a key role in the perception of black, as is well known from the
demonstrations of the effect of surround brightness by Evans and from
the fact that an unpowered television screen is gray and not black in appear-
ance. The blackness we see in images on the screen must come from contrast
effects.
The perception of blackness has been studied in considerable detail (e.g.,
see Volbrecht and Kliegl 1998). The blackness induction curve is somewhat
similar to the heterochromatic flicker brightness curve (see Chapter 5).
However, it varies significantly depending on the design of the experiment. If
a monochromatic ring surrounds a white center, the agreement with the flicker
brightness curve is relatively high. If center and ring are reversed, the func-
tion appears to have a chromatic opponent color component added, reminis-
cent of the Helmholtz-Kohlrausch effect. A detailed model of blackness
perception based on cone sensitivities and assumptions about the physiology
of color vision has been developed by K. Shinomori and co-workers
(Shinomori, 1997).
As we have seen, whiteness as a psychological attribute has been defined
in terms of a perceptually uniform gray scale by Hering and Ostwald. The psy-
chophysical description of whiteness has been of interest since the mid-1930s.
The definition of the portion of color space that most observers call white or
near white, and its description in terms of a psychophysical formula, have been
driven by industrial interest in measuring whiteness of papers or textiles.
Several formulas were developed around 1960, and in 1979 the CIE recom-
mended a whiteness formula to be used in the interest of uniformity (CIE
1979; for a bibliography until 1976, see Sève, 1979). An improved formula is
under development. It is of interest to note that the preferred psychological
optimal white is not one representing perfect 100% reflectance across the
spectrum, nor an equal energy distribution, but one with a slight bluish tint
and achieved with the addition of fluorescent whitening agents. This indicates
that whiteness and lightness are different psychological and psychophysical
concepts.
EVANS’S FIVE COLOR ATTRIBUTES 131
Fig. 4-3 Zero grayness (G0) of object colors as a function of dominant wavelength. From Evans
(1974).
132 COLOR ATTRIBUTES AND PERCEPTUAL ATTRIBUTE SCALING
described the problems with Judd’s original uniform color space definition as
due to the need for four dimensions of such a space. When defining uniform
color space in terms of surrounds that are always intermediate to the two
colors compared, as Judd did in his re-definition, the four-dimensional space
can be reduced to three dimensions. This minimizes the required stimulus
increment or decrement for color differences. Evans believed the spacing of
the Munsell system to be a good approximation of Judd’s re-defined uniform
color space. The usual approach, however, is to base an attempted uniform
color space on a single achromatic surround of a given lightness.
Hue
The term hue is defined as “attribute of visual perception according to which
an area appears to be similar to one of the colors red, yellow, green, and blue,
or to a combination of adjacent pairs of colors considered in a closed ring”
(CIE, 1987). Essentially this definition indicates that hues represent the vari-
able one experiences when looking at a Munsell (or other system) hue circle.
As mentioned earlier, such hue circles derive their legitimacy from the
arrangement of hues in the spectrum, as well as systematic mixtures of stimuli
from the beginning and end of the visible spectrum (see Fig. 3-6).
Brightness, Lightness
Lightness is an attribute related to brightness, the definition of which is:
“attribute of a visual perception according to which an area appears to emit,
or reflect, more or less light” (CIE, 1987). Brightness is generally taken to
apply to light sources. Lightness is defined as: “the brightness of an area judged
relative to the brightness of a similar area that appears to be white.” Lightness
thus can be said to be relative brightness and is generally taken to apply to
object colors. Brightness and lightness have been controversial since their def-
initions because they have been defined based on additive functions using an
experimental method that is far from natural. As will be shown in Chapter 5,
brightness and lightness of chromatic lights, respectively objects, when viewed
in natural or strongly relativized conditions have additional components, not
contained in the additive function.
COMMON COLOR ATTRIBUTE DEFINITIONS 133
Saturation, Chroma
Over the years there has been considerable discussion concerning the term
used for the third attribute. Helmholtz’s Sättigung, or saturation, is now an
“attribute of a visual sensation that permits a judgment to be made of the pro-
portion of pure chromatic color in the total sensation” (CIE, 1987). The term
chroma is closely related to Munsell chroma and refers to an “attribute of
visual sensation that permits a judgment to be made of the amount of pure
chromatic color present, regardless of the amount of achromatic color.” A
differently worded definition is: “attribute of color used to indicate the
degree of departure of the color from the gray of the same lightness” (ASTM
E284). This definition relates directly to the Munsell system. As discussed
next, chroma can be seen as related to an attribute termed “colorfulness,” as
lightness is related to brightness. Chroma and saturation are identical for two
colors having the same hue and lightness. Saturation remains constant regard-
less of brightness or lightness. Chroma, on the other hand, increases as
lightness increases. Saturation relates to an inverted cone arrangement of
colors, for example, the DIN system, while chroma refers to a cylindrical
arrangement.
The question as to how intuitive the concept of chroma is to the average
observer is an interesting one. We have seen that as a well-defined concept, as
introduced by Munsell, it is only approximately 100 years old. Seemingly there
are no terms of folk psychology that directly refer to it. Empirical evidence of
the author with more than 100 untrained observers in experiments where they
had to sort Munsell chips of constant hue into a Munsell value/chroma tem-
plate indicates that many had considerable difficulty to do so because of uncer-
tainty about the concepts of lightness and chroma. A recent investigation has
confirmed the higher uncertainty of judgments of assessing color differences
as chroma differences, compared to lightness and hue differences (Melgosa
et al., 2000).
1. Perceptual (psychological)
2. Psychophysical (related to stimulus)
3. Psychometric (interval scales)
4. Psychoquantitative (ratio scales)
rounding them. Hunt also proposed revised definitions for chroma and a new
attribute: colorfulness. It is defined as the “attribute of a visual sensation
according to which an area appears to exhibit more or less chromatic color.”
Colorfulness refers to chromatic power perceived regardless of the magnitude
of the stimulus. Hunt has used, for example, theatrical lights of varying inten-
sity to demonstrate the meaning of colorfulness. Hunt proposed the definition
of perceived chroma as amount of chromatic color in related colors judged in
proportion to the average brightness of the surroundings. His proposals for
metric hue and chroma are as, for example, defined in the CIELAB formula
(see Chapter 6).
The attributes hue, lightness, and chroma for surface colors in simplified
viewing conditions have received confirmation from multidimensional scaling
experiments. In 1988 Indow reported on the results of nineteen multidimen-
sional difference scaling studies using Munsell color chips. Because of diffi-
culties in comparing chromatic differences of colors with greatly differing hue
scaling was only done in overlapping, comparatively small regions of the
Munsell chromatic plane. Indow drew the following key conclusions:
Publisher's Note:
Permission to reproduce this image
online was not granted by the
copyright holder. Readers are kindly
asked to refer to the printed version
of this chapter.
Fig. 4-4 Multidimensional scaling analysis of Munsell colors involving samples at four differ-
ent value levels (From Indow, 1988). The results are shown in an elliptic space. Colors of
constant hue are connected with approximately radial lines, colors of equal chroma fall on
approximate circles.
Fig. 4-5 Reconstruction of the Munsell color solid from 1280 reflectance functions of Munsell
chips with a five-layer neural network (From Usui et al., 1992). Colors of constant value fall
approximately on planes of constant unit 3 values. Constant hue colors fall on approximately
radial lines and colors of constant chroma are connected by jagged ellipsoidal contours. The
arrangement is similar to one of Munsell colors in the CIE X, Y, Z space.
the other hand, the number of modifiers added in level 3 that describe light-
ness and chroma (e.g., light, strong, vivid, dark, and deep) is only 13. These,
together with the 26 hue terms of level 2 and the achromatic terms form the
267 terms of level 3. From this and other facts it is evident that hue is the
primary attribute of our normal color experiences. In evolutionary terms light
and dark are older, but they encompass one dimension. Hue, on the other
hand, has, in a manner of speaking, four dimensions and therefore provides a
much larger range of experiences. The general idea of a third attribute of
object colors, now named chroma, developed beginning in the nineteenth
century with people professionally involved with color, namely painters. In the
general population today, even in well-educated segments, it is not yet a com-
monly understood concept.
The attributes of the Hering-NCS systems are claimed to be intuitive by
their creators and to be introspectively determinable with a high degree of
precision. The author is not aware of an extensive independent test of this
claim. The clear expectation is that in a multidimensional scaling experiment
based on uniformity of perceived differences NCS color chips would be placed
into a Munsell type color solid.
NEURAL CORRELATES OF COLOR ATTRIBUTES 137
Color attributes are phenomenological entities that have as yet no firm basis
in neurology. Current answers to the question of neural correlates of color
attributes are speculative. According to the opponent color theory, there are
six primary object color perceptions: unique yellow, red, blue, green, and white
and black. A chromatic object color stimulus invokes one or two of the unique
138 COLOR ATTRIBUTES AND PERCEPTUAL ATTRIBUTE SCALING
hues, and perhaps white and/or black. It is not yet known what the neural cor-
relates of the unique hue perceptions are. Known opponent color cells in the
lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) and beyond in the cortical visual system do
not have outputs correlated with unique hue perceptions, as discussed in
Chapter 5 (see also Webster et al., 2000; Valberg, 2001). It appears less and less
likely that there are two simple subtractive chromatic opponent color systems
as proposed by Hering. Multiple hue detection and discrimination systems
have been proposed as an alternative to a two-component opponent color
process (e.g., see Krauskopf, 1999). There is a large amount of literature relat-
ing to the operation of a neural opponent color system, a subject outside the
scope of this text (relevant citations are found in Gegenfurtner and Sharpe,
1999).
Brightness-related signals are transferred from the retina through at least
two different neural pathways. Brightness and lightness perception is very
complex, for example, as the work by A. L. Gilchrist and co-workers shows
(Gilchrist, 1994; Gilchrist et al., 1999), and the Munsell value and similar light-
ness functions are only simple approximations. Recently Lotto and Purves
(1999) have argued that brightness/lightness perception of fields may be
guided by our past experiences as a species and as individuals, and interpre-
tation of clues in an image in regard to its likely illumination.
Hue and brightness/lightness perceived in contrast to a surround are
affected by a complex set of adaptations whose neural machinery is not well
known. As will be seen in Chapter 5, the neural machinery of hue and chroma
detection and discrimination is also not known, but signs point to a complex
system.
This section contains a discussion of various scaling attempts of the three fun-
damental color attributes hue, chroma, and lightness. The section on hue
includes the issue of the number of uniform hue difference steps between
unique hues at a given level of chroma. The chroma section includes a dis-
cussion of the determination of the magnitude of the first steps from gray, pre-
sumably an indication of the coloring power of the unique hues and their
combinations. Lightness scaling is briefly touched upon, but most of the dis-
cussion of lightness scaling is postponed until Chapter 5.
Hue
accuracy by a circle, with a considerable space between spectral violet and red,
as one would expect (see Fig. 3-6). With availability of purple hues in object
colors the hue circle is naturally closed, as Hering has pointed out.
Unlike lightness and chroma, hue cannot easily be envisaged by reference
to simple numbers. To have a concept of hues based on hue angles as deter-
mined in a given system is too abstract to be of practical use. One can have a
mental image of what a value 6 gray scale chip looks like, and it may even be
possible to have a mental image of a given hue identified by Munsell hue name
at chroma 8, but this is no longer possible with any reasonable accuracy for a
hue with a hue angle of 165° unless intensive training has taken place.
In general, it is easier to assign unique hue, chroma, and lightness values to
a given color stimulus than to mentally synthesize it based on such numbers.
Without extensive practical experience I can more easily estimate the content
of yellow, red, and gray in a brown than create a mental image of a brown
from numbers of yellow, red, and gray given to me. Our color names are rel-
atively vague and most have considerable extension around focal points. Only
the unique hues give us a comparatively solid mental reference point (however
individually variable as to the required stimulus, as will be seen below). Mid-
points between unique hues also can be fixed relatively well, though less well
than unique hues. On the other hand, “purplish pink” or “turquoise” can apply
to five or more (individually varying) Munsell 40-hue steps. In general, color
names are least well defined in the yellow-green segment, probably because
of the lack of widely present exemplars during the time period when color
terms developed.
As mentioned, division of Newton’s hue circle is proportional to the inter-
vals between the musical sounds of an octave, thereby implying a relationship
between sound and color (Newton, 1704). Newton’s circle did not explicitly
include extraspectral (purple) hues. Grassmann’s circle is essentially identical
to Newton’s. Helmholtz’s circle, at highest saturation, includes extraspectral
hues and is divided into ten equal segments with the middle color of each
segment diametrically opposed to its complementary color. Hue circles based
on complementary hues being diametrically opposed were common after
Helmholtz. However, there is no simple information on which to base the
placing of complementary diametrical lines relative to each other. In both his
earlier sphere and later cone models Wundt placed complementary hues oppo-
site. In case of the sphere model they were placed in such a manner that the
largest segments are occupied by yellow and blue. Rood studied this matter
and concluded that there was no objective way of placing the diametrical hue
lines relative to each other. His ten-hue chromatic circle has uneven division
between the hue lines and is apparently based on a placement using distances
along the spectrum scale with arbitrary placement of the nonspectral reds and
purples (Rood, 1879). There seem to be only two methods by which to place
hues with perceptually meaningful distances into a circle.
Hering was, as mentioned, the first basing a hue diagram on four unique
hues. His psychological hue circle (Fig. 2-32) has conceptually equal percep-
140 COLOR ATTRIBUTES AND PERCEPTUAL ATTRIBUTE SCALING
Fig. 4-6 Idealized uniform chroma circle with forty colors representing identical hue differences
in an a, b opponent color diagram.
angle, sine wave curves are obtained (Fig. 4-7).When these are converted to rel-
ative values (percentages, called hue coefficients) ¥ pattern functions result
(Fig. 4-8). An ¥ pattern in agreement with Fig. 4-8 is also obtained when plot-
ting the forty NCS hue steps, conceptually based on uniform changes in unique
hue components. In its psychological diagram the NCS atlas hue circle plots as
forty equal hue angle increments of 9°. The Munsell atlas hue steps, involving
constant hue differences, plot in the same way. However, this diagram is not
based on the four unique hues and has no preferred axis system. In the stan-
dard presentation of the Munsell hue circle the x axis is formed by hues
10PB and 10Y, the y axis by 5BG and 5R (as can be seen in Fig. 4-11 below).All
four are, to a greater or lesser extent, hues that are mixed in terms of unique
hues. But while a step along the NCS hue circle means a 10% change in unique
hue content, a step along the Munsell hue circle means a constant hue differ-
ence 1/40th of the total hue circuit. As a result the two diagrams are quite
different.
The hue scales of the Munsell system up to and including the Munsell Re-
renotations are briefly described in Chapter 7.Various hue scaling experiments
142 COLOR ATTRIBUTES AND PERCEPTUAL ATTRIBUTE SCALING
Fig. 4-7 Plot of a and b values of the ideal color circle of Fig. 4-6 as a function of hue angle.
The points form sinusoidal curves.
Fig. 4-8 Hue coefficients (relative a and b values) as a function of hue angle, calculated from
the absolute values of Fig. 4-6.
PSYCHOLOGICAL (PERCEPTUAL) SCALING OF COLOR ATTRIBUTES 143
using Munsell Book of Color chips have been performed by Indow and his
colleagues. In 1972 Indow and Ohsumi published the results of multidimen-
sional scaling involving 60 samples. The corresponding hue circle was consid-
erably distorted compared to the Munsell circle. The hue angle between 5PB
and 5P, 36° in the psychological diagram, was found to be 80°. In a later more
extensive multidimensional scaling experiment involving 176 Munsell chips, a
hue circle much more in geometrical agreement with the conceptual Munsell
circle was found (Indow and Aoki, 1983). The difference, in the most recent
analysis of the results (Indow, 1988), between 5PB and 5P at the highest
chroma level is 45°. In a later study Indow and co-workers had observers assess
the principal hue components in Munsell chips at different chroma and value
levels (Indow, 1999b). They had observers mark on a paper scale the estimated
amount of achromatic (gray) color in the total color. Next observers deter-
mined the content of one or two principal hues in the color and marked them
separately. Average absolute principal hue components at chroma 8 and value
6 are illustrated in Fig. 4-9; There is a reasonable resemblance to Fig. 4-8;
however, yellowness is undervalued compared to the other three principal hue
components (presumably because observers disagreed on what represents
100% yellow). The resulting implicit, considerably distorted, hue circle is illus-
trated in Fig. 4-10. Implied chroma varies substantially, as do the hue angle dif-
ferences. Somewhat different results have been obtained at other levels of
chroma and value. The changes in absolute hue components are more linear
Fig. 4-9 Average absolute principal hue components of Munsell colors at chroma 8 and value
6 (after Indow, 1999b). The curves have a resemblance to those of Fig. 4-8. The biggest devi-
ation is for yellow, presumably because the observers could not agree on which color repre-
sents unique yellow.
144 COLOR ATTRIBUTES AND PERCEPTUAL ATTRIBUTE SCALING
Fig. 4-10 Hue circle derived from the absolute hue components of Fig. 4-9.
than sinusoidal between unique hues, indicating that the observers judged the
chromatic content of mixed hue colors lower than that of unique hue colors,
most severely so for 1 : 1 mixes (compare Fig. 4-10 and Fig. 4-6).
Fig. 4-11 Ideal Munsell hue circle with the average unique hues determined by the author
(and an additional experiment for unique green) represented by heavy full lines.
arate the hue circle into four equal segments. Rounded to the nearest half-
step on the 40-step Munsell hue scale the number of steps in each sector is as
follows: R–Y: 8.0; Y–G: 9.5; G–B: 10.5; B-R: 12.0. If there are four primary hues
and all other hues are mixtures of two of them, one might, sensibly and logi-
cally expect the primary hues to fall on the axes of a Cartesian diagram. Only
then, as we saw above, can the content of primary hues in any given hue be
expressed in terms of the axis coordinates. The Munsell system indicates,
however, that in such a diagram there is a nonproportional relationship
between hue angle and perceived hue difference. For reasons presented in the
next chapter we cannot absolutely rely on the Munsell hue differences at a
given chroma level having all exactly identical perceived magnitude and addi-
tional constant hue difference scaling work is desirable.
A nearly identical figure results when locating the same unique hues in
Indow and Aoki’s multidimensional euclidean scaling grid of the Munsell hue
plane (fig. 6 in Indow 1988). The conclusion here also is that the number of
unit-sized hue differences in the four sectors between unique hues differs.
In addition to the experimental results above the following relevant data
are available:
uniform hue steps between the unique hues in the DIN system is R–Y:
6; Y–G: 5; G–B: 4; B–R: 9.
2. As part of a preliminary effort during the development of OSA-UCS
Newhall scaled a constant chroma circle at value 6 into 40 visually
equally sized hue differences (102 observers, 142 pairs, 14,484 judgments;
Judd, 1965). Unique hues have been plotted into an optimized model of
the Newhall results.
3. They also have been plotted into the g, j diagram of the OSA-UCS
system, regarding it as an optimized psychophysical model of the psy-
chological diagram.
4. A hue difference scaling experiment for the purposes of establishing a
hue difference weighting function has been performed by Y. Qiao et al.
in 1998. The resulting CIELAB weighting function can be integrated and
the number of equal hue steps between the unique hues determined.
5. R. S. Berns has proposed a somewhat modified hue function, taking into
account suprathreshold small color difference data (Berns, 2001).
In Table 4-1 the hue angle segments in the psychological chromatic diagram
between the four unique hues of the various experiments are compared
(Kuehni, 2001e). As Table 4-1 shows, there is considerable variation in the
results, but the trend is uniform (except for the yellow sector in the Hessel-
gren and DIN systems). The variation coefficients (CV) indicate greatest
agreement for the R–B sector and significant disagreement in the R–Y and
G–B sectors. Additional high-reliability hue-scaling experiments to provide a
clearer picture of this important issue are very desirable.
Chroma
While hue has four comparatively easy to locate psychological markers, the
unique hues, chroma has none. Different chroma scales (the results of differ-
ent experiments) can therefore only be compared in terms of physical or
psychophysical scales. Such comparisons will be made in Chapter 6.
Chroma steps have been investigated extensively as part of the develop-
ment of the Munsell system as described in Chapter 7 and the results codified
in the Munsell Renotations. The Renotations represent a considerable change
of chroma scaling compared to the 1915 Atlas and the 1929 Book of Color.
Few explicit studies of chroma scaling have appeared since then. In 1957, in
preparation for the development of the Optical Society of America Uniform
Color Scales (OSA-UCS), Judd, Nickerson, and Nimeroff determined a
constant chroma circle at value 6 and chroma 8 (384 chroma differences, 60
observers, 23,040 observations; Judd 1965). As Chapter 5 will show, it differs
significantly from the Renotation constant chroma circle. Implicit chroma
scales along the chromatic axes were also developed for the OSA-UCS system.
The four chroma scales, when compared in a common psychophysical system,
are found to differ considerably, as shown in Chapter 5.
In the DIN system saturation rather than chroma was scaled. The constant
saturation contours of that system in the CIE chromaticity diagram differ
significantly from the constant chroma contours of the Munsell system. DIN
saturation is independent of lightness and the relationship between chroma
and DIN saturation differs as a function of lightness.
NCS chromaticness is different from Munsell chroma. There has been no
recognition in that system of the varying chromatic power of optimal colors
as all implied full colors arbitrarily have been set at chromaticness 100. When
transforming Munsell colors of constant chroma to NCS notation the result-
ing chromaticness varies with hue in roughly sinusoidal fashion, as one would
expect from the definition of NCS chromaticness.
148 COLOR ATTRIBUTES AND PERCEPTUAL ATTRIBUTE SCALING
Chroma scales are also an implied result of Indow’s principal hue and chro-
matic versus achromatic color component determination as well as of his mul-
tidimensional scaling work. The scales from the two experimental efforts differ
significantly. In the principal hue component work the implied chroma scales
are often severely compressed at higher chroma levels, indicating the difficulty
observers had in assessing the relative amount of achromatic and chromatic
components (see Fig. 4-9). There was also some compression of the chroma
scale at high chroma levels in the multidimensional scaling experiment, when
compared to the Munsell system. However, when chroma scales of the Book
of Color atlas are viewed, the higher chroma steps do not generally appear
somewhat smaller than those at lower chroma. Indow also appeared to accept
the Munsell chroma scales as essentially valid since he fitted a Riemannian
model to his MDS data to improve the evenness in regard to chroma. The
results raise doubt that chroma can be accurately assessed by determination
of principal hue and achromatic components in a color or by multidimensional
scaling.
Chromaticness scaling used in all cases, for sensible reasons, an achromatic
surround. Chromatic surrounds change chromaticness scaling. However, this
subject is outside the scope of this text and an issue for general appearance
modeling. As will be argued later in this text, chromaticness depends on the
magnitude of the unit chromaticness difference used to scale it (see also the
related comment in the next section).
Chromatic thresholds, the first steps from the neutral point, have been
studied since the 1920s. Typical saturation thresholds (amount of spectral light
to be added to white light to result in a just perceptible color) as determined
in 1938 by I. G. Priest and F. G. Brickwedde are shown in Fig. 4-12. Results by
other investigators differ in detail but agree in the general features. Such
thresholds are, presumably, indicators of chromatic strength of spectral colors.
Evans defined spectral chromatic strength as the factor by which the lumi-
nance of a stimulus has to be multiplied in a brilliance match to equal the bril-
liance of the achromatic surround (Evans, 1974). Recall that a brilliance match
against the surround is achieved when the stimulus field looks neither grayish
nor fluorent against the surround. In 1968 Evans and Swenholt investigated
chromatic strength using the Munsell system. In plotting the reciprocal CIE
colorimetric purity against the dominant (or complementary) wavelength for
chroma circles 2, 4, 6, 8, and 16 at value 5, they obtained curves essentially
parallel to each other and reciprocal to the saturation threshold curve (see
Fig. 4-13). These curves provide another kind of support for the salience of
the chroma attribute.
In an experiment with 35 observers Kuehni investigated the relative sizes
of the first steps from gray of the colors nearest to the respective axes in the
a*, b* diagram in the Munsell as well as the OSA-UCS systems (Kuehni,
2000c). The results indicated that the average observer judged, in the Munsell
system at value 6/chroma 8, the first step toward yellow to be about 2.5 times
the magnitude of the first step toward blue. In the red/green direction the
PSYCHOLOGICAL (PERCEPTUAL) SCALING OF COLOR ATTRIBUTES 149
Fig. 4-12 Spectral saturation thresholds determined in 1938 by Priest and Brickwedde. From
Osgood (1953).
green step was seen as about 10% larger than the red step. For OSA-UCS
the first step toward yellow was seen as about 20% larger than the first step
toward blue. In that system the first steps toward red and green were seen as
of about equal size. Somewhat different results have been obtained by Indow
(2001).
The results of Priest and Brickwedde and many later researchers were
obtained monocularly with optical apparatus. M. De Matiello and co-workers
investigated the difference between monocular and binocular saturation dis-
crimination at the threshold level as well as for suprathreshold saturation dif-
ferences (2001). They found the changes in colorimetric purity for threshold
steps to be considerably larger for the first step from gray than for the first
step from the spectral color. For suprathreshold saturation differences they
found power relationships varying by wavelength with exponents from 0.61 to
0.97. Interestingly they found exponents of binocularly determined data to be
lower than those obtained for monocular data.
Fig. 4-13 Reciprocal CIE colorimetric purity as a function of dominant wavelength for Munsell
colors at five levels of chroma (represented by different symbols) and value 6. From Evans and
Swenholdt (1968).
Ê C ˆ(
I= 2 DH ) + 6 DV + 3DC , (4-1)
Ë 5¯
0.5
DE = {[ f f (C C )
g h 1 2
0.5
]
2 2
DH + (DC ) + (4 DV )
2
} , (4-2)
where C1 and C2 are the Munsell chromas of the two colors compared, DH is
the hue difference in Munsell 100-hue steps, DV is the difference in Munsell
value, and
0.5
[2( 1 - cos 3.6 ∞ DH )]
fg = ,
DH
2 - k + 4(k - 1)
fh = ,
3 - cos 3.6 ∞ DH
where k is the hue superweight factor with a value from 1 to 2. If the value is
1, then fh = 1. For optimal correlation the value of k was selected as 1.7 (Judd
and Nickerson, 1967), this being indicative of hue superimportance.This exper-
iment supported the idea of a relationship between perceptual hue differences
and chroma differences that does not fit into a euclidean system. This matter
is further discussed in Chapters 7 and 8.
Lightness
The first “qualitative” verbal gray scale, seemingly, is the one by Forsius, with
seven grades including black and white (see Chapter 2). Glisson described
quantitatively (in terms of weights of black and white pigment to be used) a
152 COLOR ATTRIBUTES AND PERCEPTUAL ATTRIBUTE SCALING
24-grade gray scale that is reasonably uniform through the middle portion.
Mayer envisaged 23 grades of different lightness in his double pyramid, while
Lambert reduced that to 8 grades. Runge’s color sphere has a 10-grade gray
scale.
In 1729 the French mathematician Bouguer published Essai d’optique sur
la gradation de la lumière (Optical treatise on the gradation of light) in which
he described experiments using shadows of rods made by the light of two
candles on a white screen. He found that the distance ratio of the two candles
needed to be about 8 : 1 to result in a just noticeable difference between the
two shadows. Thus he initiated the study of brightness thresholds. In 1845 his
compatriot V. Masson described a white disk on which a black radial line
segment was inscribed. When spinning the disk the line segment darkened a
ring of the disk (the spinning disk method was invented by Pieter van Muss-
chenbroek in 17688). The width of the line segment as a fraction of the total
disk circumference is an indicator of the fractional change in luminosity
required to see a difference. Masson found that the ratio, depending on cir-
cumstances of illumination and viewing distance, was between 1/50 to 1/120.
In 1850 the French astronomer D. F. Arago (1786–1853) repeated the shadow
experiment with improved equipment and found a ratio of 1/133. Masson’s
experiment was repeated in 1858 by Fechner and Volkmann, who found a ratio
of 1/100 and by Helmholtz in 1860 who, under optimal conditions, could see a
ratio of 1 : 167. In 1888/9 König and Brodhun used Masson’s technique to
determine brightness as well as chromatic thresholds.
Attacking the problem of a uniform gray scale from another angle Plateau
in 1872 asked several painters to paint a color halfway between black and
white (as described in Chapter 2), thus performing an equisection. Indepen-
dently Delboeuf developed a detailed uniform gray scale using successive
equisection, as well as by adjusting black and white disk segments to obtain
perceptually equal steps.
In 1874 Hering, as part of his psychological color triangle, described a
“nuanced” series of grays from black to white that was to have uniform incre-
ments/decrements of blackness and whiteness. Ebbinhaus described in 1887
an eight-grade gray scale (see Chapter 5). In 1899 Munsell began to develop
his color order system of which an eleven-grade (ten-step) value scale was an
integral part. The Munsell value scale underwent development until the 1930s
and was finalized in the Munsell Renotations. In the final experiments gray
scales were visually measured against a white, gray, and black background.
They were found to differ, and the results were averaged into the value scale
(see Chapter 5, Section 5.7). Ostwald selected a logarithmic relationship
between the stimulus and the response for his gray scale. In the 1960s Kaneko
and Takasaki confirmed and further quantified W. Schönfelder’s finding that
color differences between two stimuli are perceived best if the surround is
intermediate between the two stimuli. This makes a gray scale and its steps
dependent on the surround. Surround dependence had been found in 1922 by
Adams and Cobb and was also investigated by Evans. The effect was included
PSYCHOLOGICAL (PERCEPTUAL) SCALING OF COLOR ATTRIBUTES 153
into the definition of the OSA-UCS lightness scale, otherwise based on the
Munsell value scale. The NCS whiteness/blackness scale is derived from
average judgments of the content of whiteness and blackness in gray samples.
The atlas scale has eleven grades (including black and white) resulting in ten
steps, the numerical scale has 100 steps.
From the beginning with Bouguer the development of lightness scales has
run parallel with the development in photometry. While work with the Masson
disk or Delboeuf’s equisection method and others did not explicitly require
photometry, their results were best interpretable in quantitative terms when
compared to the corresponding stimulus strength. The development of light-
ness scales is therefore further discussed in Chapter 5.
A fundamental issue involving lightness is how brightness and lightness are
connected. As has been demonstrated by many researchers over the last 100
years, a source of achromatic color with any given luminance value can,
depending on the conditions it is seen in, be perceived as any grade of gray
from white to black. The question arises how the human visual system decides
to assign a given lightness value to a given luminance value. Among the early
researchers investigating this matter was D. Katz who was a student of G. E.
Müller. In 1909 he published a book-length paper, Die Erscheinungsweisen der
Farben und ihre Beinflussung durch die individuelle Erfahrung (translated in
1935 as The world of colour) in which he discussed his investigations of dif-
ferent appearance modes of colors, including the relationship of brightness,
lightness, and luminance. In a dramatic experiment Gelb showed in 1929 that
if a black piece of paper is suspended in midair and illuminated with a strong
beam of white light, its appearance is white. If a paper of higher luminous
reflectance than that of the black paper is placed next to it, the original paper
no longer appears white but a shade of gray or black, depending on the lumi-
nous reflectance of the second paper. Thus lightness perception is dependent
on the relative luminous reflectance of adjacent fields. A few years later gestalt
psychology theorists postulated that the visual system compares the luminance
of a target against the weighted average of the luminance of the total scene.
In 1948 H. Wallach proposed that lightness is decided by the ratio of the lumi-
nances of two adjacent fields. The so-called intrinsic image model of lightness
was developed in the last twenty years. It had been found that when a gray
paper is successively viewed against backgrounds of different lightness, the
ratio of luminous reflectances changes significantly but the perceived lightness
of the paper changes little. Similarly there is considerable lightness constancy
when viewing the arrangement of paper and background under different kinds
of illumination. The intrinsic image model analyzes the image as it appears on
the retina according to three components: surface reflectance, illumination,
and three-dimensional form clues. However, significant disagreements
between the predictions of theory and the experimental results remained in
certain situations. As Gilchrist and colleagues (1999) describe it, a fundamen-
tal question is that of the anchoring problem. It relates to the question “where
to locate the range of luminance values (in a given situation) on the scale of
154 COLOR ATTRIBUTES AND PERCEPTUAL ATTRIBUTE SCALING
perceived gray shades.” Once it is solved, the next question is “how the range
of luminance values is distributed on the scale of perceived gray shades.” Many
researchers have contributed to the current answers: in simple images (two
fields of different luminance filling the entire visual space) the highest lumi-
nance is seen as white unless the relative area of the darker field is more than
half of the total field. Then an area rule applies. As the darker side grows in
relative size, it appears lighter and lighter, making the smaller, lighter area
appear more and more white, eventually fluorent and then self-luminous.
In more complex images Gilchrist and his group propose to separate the
image into components, called frameworks, that belong together according to
gestalt principles (an exact definition of framework is difficult, so coplanarity
of the elements appears to be a good start). For each local framework the
anchoring and area rules of the simple situation approximately apply. The
entire visual field is called the global framework. Within a local framework a
veridical, or 1 : 1 scaling between lightness and luminance, is found to apply if
the luminance ratio between white and black is less than 30 : 1. If it is larger,
some luminance scale compression occurs. Gilchrist et al. have tested their
model on many classical lightness test results and “illusions,” such as the
Benary effect (1924) or White’s illusion (White, 1981) and found good explana-
tory power. In cases where the information from local frameworks appears
contradictory, Gilchrist et al. believe that our visual system makes a compro-
mise between the results or alternately displays one or the other. It is evident
that much work remains to be done.
space uniform in color differences, hue, lightness, and chroma appear to be the
essential color attributes. Other attributes can replace lightness and chroma if
uniformity in terms of difference is not the guiding principle. Historically two
sets of fundamental attributes of color perceptions have been proposed. Hue
is common to both. In Hering’s approach veiling is described by the relative
amounts of blackness and whiteness in the perception. But veiling can also be
described in terms of lightness and chroma. It has not been determined how
under comparable conditions the two sets of veiling attributes compare.
Various experimental scales of constant chroma and equal-sized hue differ-
ences vary considerably for unclear reasons. It has also been shown that a psy-
chological color space with the unique hues on the chromatic axes is not
uniform because there are varying numbers of equal-sized hue differences in
the four quadrants. The relationship between chroma and hue differences
appears to change significantly as a function of the size of the differences. In
the next chapter these and other issues are investigated in terms of quantita-
tive stimulus differences.
Chapter 5
Psychophysical Scaling of
Color Attributes: Stimulus
and Perception
Color Space and Its Divisions: Color Order from Antiquity to the Present, by Rolf G. Kuehni
ISBN 0-471-32670-4 Copyright © 2003 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
157
158 PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING OF COLOR ATTRIBUTES: STIMULUS AND PERCEPTION
degree of validation from the correlation with visual data, as we will see. To
bring such models into close agreement with psychological results requires
extensive fine-tuning and application of auxiliary functions, as Chapter 6 will
show. The following sections discuss the development of these implied rela-
tionships in some detail.
For the visual sense the earliest quantitative studies comparing stimulus to
response involved brightness. Photometry, the quantitative measurement of
light, has been of interest since the Renaissance. In its more modern form it
was developed by Bouguer and advanced by Lambert and Benjamin Thomp-
son, the Count of Rumford (1753–1814). The initial equipment consisted of
simple wedges that allowed visual comparison of two different light sources
under controlled conditions. A curious, very early illustration of what appears
to be a photometric measurement1 is that by the celebrated painter Rubens
used as the frontispiece for Book V of d’Aguilon’s text of 1613 Opticorum
Libri Sex (Fig. 5-1). Here the effect of distance of light source on the perceived
brightness of the patches on the screen is being studied.
Bouguer made quantitative measurements by comparing a test light source
against a candle as the reference source. The first step toward making mea-
surements more reliable was to use an artificial reference light source other
than a candle. This resulted, beginning in the late eighteenth century, in the
Fig. 5-1 Rubens’s, frontispiece illustration to Book V of d’Aguilon’s Opticorum libri sex depict-
ing an experiment in measuring light intensity as a function of distance of the lights, 1613.
160 PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING OF COLOR ATTRIBUTES: STIMULUS AND PERCEPTION
development of oil lamps, pentane lamps, and the Hefner lamp, burning
various hydrocarbons (used until the 1940s).2 The human observer began to
be replaced by photoelectric cells beginning in the early twentieth century.
Lambert had already envisaged this development by describing in Photome-
tria in concept a piece of equipment that could measure light analogous to
temperature measurement with a thermometer.3 For such measurements to be
in agreement with visual measurements the correspondence between the spec-
tral sensitivity of photoelectric cells and that of the average human had to be
established.
The discovery of rods and cones of the human visual system in 1828 by the
German physiologist G. R. Treviranus (1776–1837) and the discovery of visual
purple in 1877 by F. Boll resulted in extended efforts to determine their
purpose and activity.4 The duplicity theory of the double function of the retina,
according to which the cones mediate daylight vision and the rods night vision,
was proposed by the German anatomist M. J. S. Schultze (1825–1874) in 1866
and expanded by Helmholtz’s student Kries. Measurements of the luminosity
of spectral colors at different levels of illumination were made in Helmholtz’s
laboratory by König and C. Dieterici (1884, 1892). König was able to show
the close agreement among an absorption curve he had measured for visual
purple, the luminosity curve of a person with monochromatic vision estab-
lished under daylight conditions, and the luminosity curve for an observer with
normal vision determined at very low light levels. This was a clear indication
of the existence of the postulated second visual system based on rods and
operational for night vision in observers with normal color vision. König and
Dieterici obtained a different spectral sensitivity curve for daylight vision.
Comparison of the sensitivity of photoelectric cells and of humans indicated
large discrepancies, and in order to bring measurements into agreement with
average visual results, filters had to be interposed between light source and
the photoelectric cell.
A new type of photometer, the flicker photometer, appeared in the early
twentieth century and was intensively investigated by H. E. Ives (1912). The
original method of obtaining flicker was to rotate a partial disk in front of a
light. Depending on the disk sector and the speed of the disk, more or less
flicker is observed. If the occluding portion is half of the disk, at the critical
flicker frequency at which the flicker disappears the perceived intensity of light
is exactly half the intensity when viewing it unobstructed. By matching the
brightness of chromatic flickering fields against a steady field of white light,
the luminous efficiency of chromatic lights can be measured. Ives’s studies
showed that brightness measured in this manner is additive. Additivity is not
observed when brightness of lights of different color is measured by compar-
ing steady fields. Given the state of computing at the time, additive functions
were very desirable. Important determinations of spectral brightness using
flicker photometry were reported in 1923 by K. S. Gibson and E. P. T. Tyndall,
working at the U.S. National Bureau of Standards. An average sensitivity curve
was recommended by them to the CIE and standardized largely unchanged
THE COLORIMETRIC SYSTEM 161
Fig. 5-2 CIE standard scotopic (low light, V¢l) and photopic (daylight level, Vl) luminosity
curves. The wavelengths of maximum sensitivity are indicated. From Wyszecki and Stiles
(1982).
The idea of trichromatic color vision was originally put forth by Palmer in 1777
and revived by Young in 1802. Important support was later provided by the
work of Helmholtz, Grassmann, and Maxwell, as discussed earlier. In the 1850s
Helmholtz estimated the spectral sensitivity of the human color vision system
in agreement with Young’s theory (Fig. 5-3; Helmholtz, 1860). In 1860 Maxwell
provided the first set of measured functions derived using a visual colorime-
ter (Fig. 5-4). Detailed measurements were made by König and Dieterici in
1892, using an advanced König-Helmholtz spectral colorimeter (Fig. 5-5).
Similar measurements were made also by Abney and reported in 1914.
162 PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING OF COLOR ATTRIBUTES: STIMULUS AND PERCEPTION
Fig. 5-3 Helmholtz’s sketch of estimated spectral sensitivity of three fundamental color vision
processes, 1860. The letters refer to major hues of the spectrum, from red ( R) on the left to
violet on the right.
Fig. 5-4 Spectral curves of the three fundamental processes of color vision R, G, and B, and
the sum curve S as determined by Maxwell, observer K. The wavelength scale is in arbitrary
units; C–G identify Fraunhofer lines in the spectrum, 1860.
Between 1920 and 1930 J. Guild (1930) and W. D. Wright (1928–29) in England
independently built improved equipment to measure these functions, and the
color-matching functions of various observers were determined. These data
were considered by the CIE for standardization. Following a suggestion by
Judd, linear transformations were calculated so that two of the implied pri-
maries were located on a line with zero luminance (the alychne). This resulted
in the third primary having a spectral function identical to the CIE luminance
function. In this form the functions were standardized as the CIE 1931 2° stan-
dard observer color-matching functions (applicable to a field of view of 2°, Fig.
5-6; CIE, 1931).5 In 1964 the CIE 1964 10° standard observer color-matching
functions were added, applying to a field of view of 10° (CIE, 1964) and based
THE COLORIMETRIC SYSTEM 163
Fig. 5-5 Measurements of their own fundamental color vision processes R, G, and V (violet)
by König and Dieterici. The dashed line represents the G function of an observer with anom-
alous color vision, 1886. Note that the wavelength scale is in reverse. Letters along the wave-
length scale denote major Fraunhofer lines.
Fig. 5-6 CIE spectral color matching functions of the 2° standard observer. They have been
linearly transformed from the measured functions. From Wyszecki and Stiles (1982).
identical to the luminous reflectance for object colors (luminance for lights).
The X, Y, Z space is a nonuniform psychophysical color space, taken to be
euclidean, relating measured reflectance of a sample viewed in a standard light
to a specific point in the space. Tristimulus values in case of object colors are
normalized sums of the reflectance function weighted by the spectral power
distribution of the light source and the three color matching functions x̄, ȳ, z̄.
With one dimension in this space representing (flicker) luminance, the other
two must represent in some fashion the chromatic components from which
hue and chroma are derived. In the CIE colorimetric system the psychophys-
ical definition of hue is the dominant, or complementary wavelength, of satu-
ration colorimetric purity as expressed in the CIE chromaticity diagram, a
particular version of chromatic plane defined by the chromaticity coordinates
x and y. Their definition is as follows:
X Y (5-1)
x= and y= .
X +Y + Z X +Y + Z
Physiology has identified three cone types in the human retina, in agreement
with the postulates of trichromatic vision (but recall the comment about
females with the genetic potential for four cone types in Chapter 1). These
three cone types are presumably the only transducers of information respon-
sible for color vision (rods may contribute to color vision under certain limited
circumstances). Along the visual path in the brain many transformations of
CONE RESPONSE SPACE 165
Fig. 5-7 CIE chromaticity diagram with spectral trace and the nonspectral purple colors for
the 2° standard observer. E represents the equal energy illuminant. From Wyszecki and Stiles
(1982).
the information generated in the retina take place, all derived from cone
responses. Cone sensitivity and cone response to specific light energy are
therefore important pieces of information in an attempt to link physical stim-
ulus and psychological response. Attempts to measure spectral absorption
functions of cone pigments began in the middle of the twentieth century, with
refinement continuing into the present. However, light radiation undergoes
changes in the eye before being absorbed by the cones of the retina, changes
that need to be accounted for. They are due to the media in the eye from the
outer surface to the cones. For a comparison between stimulus and percep-
tion, cone response functions taking into account intraocular absorptions are
therefore essential. In vivo measurements of human cone response are not
practical, and the data currently accepted are based on absorption measure-
ments of excised eyes, on measurements made on primates, and on the as-
sumption that cone response functions are linearly related to color-matching
functions. Proposals for standard cone sensitivity functions have been made
166 PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING OF COLOR ATTRIBUTES: STIMULUS AND PERCEPTION
Fig. 5-8 Locus of spectral colors in the CIE X, Y, Z space, beginning at 380 nm and ending
at 750 nm (wavelengths 500 and 600 nm are identified).
Fig. 5-9a Spectral cone sensitivity functions according to Smith and Pokorny, calculated for
the CIE 2° standard observer, with identical area under the curves, linear sensitivity scale.
Fig. 5-9b Spectral cone sensitivities from Fig. 5-9 a in logarithmic sensitivity scale.
168 PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING OF COLOR ATTRIBUTES: STIMULUS AND PERCEPTION
Fig. 5-10 Spectral trace in the L, M, S space based on the cone sensitivity functions of Fig.
5-9a. The wavelengths 500 and 600 nm are identified.
for four cone types. So far no individual actually having four cone types has
been identified. All these issues touch on the question of a statistically mean-
ingful average observer and how different sets of data may be influenced by
the genetic variability in its observer pool. Such observer pools are likely
to also differ in the degree of ocular yellowing and distribution of macular
pigment, both affecting in particular the S cone sensitivity function. Another
issue in connection with cone functions is the question of their intrinsic
smoothness. CIE color-matching functions are smooth, and cone functions
calculated from these by linear transformation are also smooth. Directly
measured individual cone sensitivity functions differ somewhat in shape and
appear to have in some cases more or less pronounced “dents” (see Fig. 5-5
for an example). Some of these appear to be due to the degree of macular
absorption in individuals, as already König surmised.
Fig. 5-11 Schrödinger’s opponent color and brightness functions calculated from König and
Dieterici’s fundamental functions (Fig. 5-5). Note that the wavelength is reversed from the cus-
tomary presentation, 1926. Hell refers to the brightness function. The other two functions rep-
resent yellowness-blueness ( Gelb, Blau) and greenness-redness ( Grün, Rot).
form) linear opponent color space with two color moments and a color
weight.
In 1923 the American physicist E. Q. Adams proposed a zone theory in
which the output from three cone types is modulated and subtracted to result
in opponent color signals. Adams expanded on his proposal in 1942 where he
showed, using CIE tristimulus data, that a theory such as he had proposed
could offer a good model for the Munsell system (Fig. 5-12). There he specif-
ically proposed that the output of the three cone types should be considered
identical to the CIE tristimulus values and the modulation of the output iden-
tical to that of the Munsell value function. He constructed a “chromance”
diagram by subtracting modulated Y values from modulated X and Z values.
In 1944 Nickerson and K. Stultz offered a color space and color difference
formula based on Adams’s proposals which became influential in technology
(more details in Chapter 6).
At the same time support for an opponent color step in color vision began
to develop on a different front. Around 1940 physiologists began to measure
signals by inserting microelectrodes into retinal cells. The Swedish physiolo-
gist R. Granit measured spectral response curves in ganglion cells in the retina
OPPONENT COLOR SPACE 171
Fig. 5-12 Munsell colors at value 5 of the 1929 Book of Color in Adams’s 1942 opponent color
chromatic diagram. Lines represent the smoothing proposal by Newhall (1940).
Fig. 5-13 Opponent color functions of the CIE 2° standard observer calculated by Jameson
and Hurvich (1955).
Fig. 5-14 Spectral trace in the Y, a*, b* space, 10° observer. The wavelengths of 500 and
600 nm are identified.
As Chapter 6 will show, most color vision models developed since the mid-
nineteenth century are zone models and a simple version of a zone model
color space was recommended in 1976 by the CIE as the CIELAB formula
(CIE 1976; see Chapter 6). In this space, considered euclidean and with a carte-
sian chromatic diagram, colors are identified by chromatic coordinates a* and
b* and a lightness coordinate L*. There is also a polar coordinate interpreta-
tion where chroma is defined as the radial distance from the origin and hue
by the hue angle, thus bringing the system conceptually in agreement with a
psychological system of the Munsell type. Experimental work before and since
then has indicated that there are surround effects requiring consideration and
that CIELAB space is at best a rough approximation of a large difference
uniform color space. Figure 5-14 illustrates the spectral colors in the Y, a*, b*
space.
An important matter usually not considered in models until very recently
is that of the interaction of cone responses between test field(s) and surround.
Four types of opponent cells involving L and M cone output comparison have
been identified (Wässle et al., 1994). All four involve comparison of output of
cones from the center of the receptive field of the opponent cell with those
from the surround. A similar situation applies to the opponent cells compar-
174 PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING OF COLOR ATTRIBUTES: STIMULUS AND PERCEPTION
ing output from S cones with that of L plus M cones.These mechanisms appear
to be responsible for test field/surround effects, such as contrast and assimila-
tion, possibly crispening, and others. Correct treatment of these effects is
essential for a uniform color space model.
In recent years experimental data have begun to raise doubts about a
simple subtractive opponent color system such as proposed by Hurvich and
Jameson. New experimental work points to four independent chromatic
dimensions and perhaps to a multiplicity of hue detection mechanisms (see
below).
s = 1.00000z , (5-2)
where x̄, ȳ, z̄ are CIE color-matching functions, originally the Judd and Vos
modified 2° observer functions. In this transformation l̄ + m–– add up to equal
ȳ (equivalent of the luminosity function). As mentioned, the same equations
have been applied to both CIE 2° and 10° observer data. Slightly different
equations have been proposed by other authors.
The inverse relationship is as follows:
y = l + m,
z = 1.000 s . (5-3)
Both the L, M, S space and the CIE tristimulus space are viewed as orthogo-
nal. It is evident that the two spaces cannot be orthogonal in the same refer-
ence frame, but a case can be made that they represent different realities and
each may be orthogonal by itself. There is, however, the unanswered basic
question if the assumption of orthogonality is valid for either space.
Based on their recordings from parvocellular neurons in the LGN of
macaques A. M. Derrington, J. Krauskopf, and P. Lennie (DKL) in 1984 pro-
posed a color space (nonuniform) representative of the signal output of dif-
ferent cell types (see Chapter 6). They located opponent cells that report in
terms of L–M or the reverse and in terms of (L + M) - S or the reverse. When
they plotted the corresponding axes in the CIE chromaticity diagram, they
HOW ARE THE L, M, S, AND X, Y, Z, COLOR SPACES RELATED? 175
Fig. 5-15 Balanced opponent functions a and b calculated from CIE color-matching functions.
These functions are supported by recordings in the lateral geniculate nuclei of macaques.
found that the axes do not represent average unique hues. This can be demon-
strated by calculating balanced (equal areas under the four curve segments)
opponent functions based on cell input reported by DKL:
a = 5.673(l - 1.98842 m ),
b = l + m - s. (5-4)
The resulting two functions a and b are illustrated in Fig. 5-15. The b function
is identical to ȳ - z̄ and therefore has the same form as the Hurvich-Jameson
yellowness-blueness opponent color function. The a function is different from
the greenness-redness function because it lacks positive values in the short-
wave range, considered indicative of the reappearance of redness at short
wavelengths. The a function can therefore not be a greenness-redness express-
ing function (in the sense that green and red are represented by the unique
hues), and the DKL space is not a space in alignment with the psychological
space (e.g., see Lee 1999). It is important to be aware that the measured func-
tions apply to macaques and that they are from the lateral geniculate nucleus:
details in the human visual system may differ and additional transformations
in the cortex are known to take place. Recently it has been found that in the
primary visual cortex at the back of the brain, most cells receive input from
all the different opponent cell types in the lateral geniculate nuclei (De Valois
176 PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING OF COLOR ATTRIBUTES: STIMULUS AND PERCEPTION
et al., 1997). On that basis a mechanism for S cone input into the greenness-
redness opponent system appears to exist. Equation (5-3) indicates that the
definition in terms of cone sensitivities of the x̄ color-matching function
includes S cone input. It is interesting to note that axis rotation required to
provide a result equal to a balanced linear opponent chromatic diagram (based
on CIE 1931 tristimulus values) can be achieved by the following equation
(Kuehni, 2000a):
a = f (a - 0.56b ),
b = b, (5-5)
Historically the X, Y, Z space (and its predecessors) has been used as a basis
to express psychological scales in a psychophysical space. The fact that one of
its dimensions is aligned with brightness and the other two therefore with
chromaticness seemed a good reason to expect reasonable agreement if the
functions expressing x̄ and z̄ are correctly selected. They were selected so that
they had only positive values (the experimentally determined color-matching
functions have stretches of negative values). There is, of course, no a priori
reason why a color-matching space should be in agreement with, or easily
modifiable into, a color appearance space, and as will be seen later, there are
important discrepancies.
Ê Rb + 100 ˆ
V =R , (5-6)
Ë R + Rb ¯
Fig. 5-16 Results of visual determination of Munsell value of Munsell value scale samples
viewed against a white, mid-level gray, and black background. After Newhall et al. (1943).
V 2 = mR + nR 2 , (5-7)
where V is the Munsell value, m and n are constants, and R is luminous
reflectance.
In the studies leading up to the Munsell Renotations the value scale was
evaluated against a black, a middle gray, and a white background. The results
differed significantly (Fig. 5-16). The optimal power for the results against the
black background is cube root against the white background square root. As
expected, the results against the gray background show two branches, indi-
cating the lightness crispening effect. The committee working on the Renota-
tions decided to average the results in some fashion, and the final, smoothed
curve was mathematically described with a quintic function developed by
Judd:
V = bx p - V0 , (5-9)
where V0 is the subtractive constant. In the same year T. Kaneko investigated
lightness of achromatic colors in form of color chips as a function of surround
lightness. He found, in agreement with Schönfelder, that differences between
gray scale grades were perceived largest if the surround lightness falls between
the lightnesses of the samples being compared. When the sample lightness is
higher or lower than the surround lightness, a larger increment in luminous
reflectance is required for a perceived difference of the same magnitude. He
also found that the effect depends to some degree on the size of the test fields
compared to the surround. With larger test fields the crispening effect was
reduced; that is to say, the magnitude of the lightness crispening effect appears
180 PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING OF COLOR ATTRIBUTES: STIMULUS AND PERCEPTION
to be a function of the test field size, the effect being strongest for small fields.
Kaneko constructed gray scales valid for a given surround lightness (Kaneko,
1964). Adams and Cobb had also noticed the described effect, but they
believed it to be limited to threshold level differences. Kaneko found it also
to apply for much larger lightness differences. It was H. Takasaki who gave the
effect the name lightness crispening. Kaneko developed a formula consisting
of two simultaneous equations to model his results.
Takasaki conducted in 1966 extensive investigations of the effect of
surround lightness on perceived lightness of test fields. He confirmed contrast
as well as crispening effects. His complex formula was shown by C. C.
Semmelroth in 1970 to be reducible to
n
V = Rm + k R - Rb , (5-10)
Fig. 5-18 Graph developed by Judd and Wyszecki from a table by Semmelroth (1971)
depicting the effect of lightness crispening on perception of value differences in the Munsell
Renotation value scale. From Judd and Wyszecki (1975). Vs is the nominal Munsell value, Va
is the background adjusted Munsell value.
Fig. 5-19 Average settings expressed in L* values of 22 observers of the luminance of eight
grades of achromatic colors against three surrounds so that steps between the grades appear
equidistant.
Heterochromatic Brightness/Lightness
Fig. 5-20 Iso-brightness to luminance (B/L) contours in the CIE chromaticity diagram as deter-
mined by Wyszecki and Sanders (1964), the result of the Helmholtz-Kohlrausch effect. W iden-
tifies the white point. The effect is weakest for yellow and strongest for purple colors.
YA = YC + 0.23 a if b is positive,
0.5
YA = YC + 0.20 (a 2 + b 2 ) if a is negative and b is negative,
This formula predicts the committee data with a correlation coefficient of 0.88.
The corresponding effective luminosity function is compared with the CIE
luminosity function in Fig. 5-21. When inverted for comparison to the chro-
Fig. 5-21 Spectral effective luminous reflectance for the OSA-UCS Helmholtz-Kohlrausch
effect data, as modeled by the author (2000d), compared to the 10° observer ȳ function.
186 PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING OF COLOR ATTRIBUTES: STIMULUS AND PERCEPTION
matic strength function, its long wave trough is found to be shifted toward
lower wavelengths. The reason for the difference of the apparent contribution
of opponent color signals to perceived brightness in the two experiments is
not clear. However, these results provide implicit support for the idea that the
opponent color system makes a contribution to perceived brightness/lightness
of chromatic colors.
TABLE 5-1 Cone sensitivity based opponent color models fitted to “uniform” chroma
circles
Data a b
Munsell Renotations a = 0.926 [5.67(L - 1.9884M) - 0.50b] b+ = L + M - S
COV = 5.6% b- = 0.90(L + M - S)
Munsell Re-renotations a+ = 0.646 [5.67(L - 1.9884M) - 0.25b] b+ = 1.09(L + M - S)
a- = 0.807 [5.67(L - 1.9884M) - 0.25b] b- = L + M - S
COV = 1.6%
Nickerson et al. a+ = 0.953 [5.67(L - 1.9884M) - 0.20b] b =L+M-S
a- = 1.030 [5.67(L - 1.9884M) - 0.20b]
COV = 4.4%
OSA-UCS a+ = 0.908 [5.67(L - 1.9884M) - 0.40b] b+ = 1.33(L + M - S)
a- = 0.976 [5.67(L - 1.9884M) - 0.40b] b- = 0.75(L + M - S)
COV = 4.7%
CIELAB a+ = 0.676 [5.67(L - 1.9884M) - 0.25b] b+ = 1.33(L + M - S)
a- = 5.67(L - 1.9884M) - 0.25b b- = 0.65(L + M - S)
COV = 8.8%
188 PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING OF COLOR ATTRIBUTES: STIMULUS AND PERCEPTION
Fig. 5-22a Constant chroma contours determined in three different experiments in the bal-
anced cone opponent diagram: Nickerson et al. (Judd, 1965), Munsell Renotations, and Munsell
Re-renotations.
Fig. 5-22b Constant chroma contour implicit in the OSA-UCS system, in the balanced cone
opponent diagram.
EXPRESSING PSYCHOLOGICAL SCALES IN PSYCHOPHYSICAL SPACES 189
Fig. 5-23 Schematic depiction of quadrant 1 of an opponent color diagram with constant
chroma contours calculated from a formula and determined by an experiment.
experimental conditions often different contours provided the best fit to data
of different observers (e.g., see Sankeralli and Mullen, 1996).
Fig. 5-24a Munsell Renotation colors of value 6, chroma 8 in the a*, b* diagram of the CIE
2° standard observer and the equal energy illuminant. The letters indicate the locations of the
average unique hues determined in the experiment by the author (2001a).
192 PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING OF COLOR ATTRIBUTES: STIMULUS AND PERCEPTION
Fig. 5-24b Colors of Fig. 5-24a illustrated in the a*, b* diagram of the CIE 10° standard
observer, with locations of average unique hues.
chapter). These functions not only embody the fundamental variability (as
related to uniform hue difference) in hue angle difference but also correction
for the error added by the CIELAB formula.
We can surmise from Table 4-1 that the various described experiments have
somewhat different hue scales. We find this to be the case when comparing
hue angle differences in the optimized cone sensitivity based opponent dia-
grams of Table 5-1 around the hue circuit for the Munsell Renotations, the Re-
renotations and the Newhall data. These are the only data sets where we have
continuous consistent hue scaling around a psychologically scaled constant
chroma circle (see Fig. 5-25 for the Newhall data). The results indicate little
or no discernible agreement except for the region of hue angles 0 to 100 for
the Newhall and Re-renotation data. While the changes in hue angle differ-
ence appear to be random in the Renotations, there are systematic minima
near hue angles 75° and 275° in the Re-renotations. The Newhall data have a
systematic development in roughly the first half of the hue circuit. Overall, the
lowest level of disagreement is between the Renotations and the Newhall data.
All three sets of hue angle differences have slightly positive slopes relative to
the hue angle—Renotations 0.0099, Newhall 0.0076, Re-renotations 0.0048—
indicating a tendency for the increment to become larger as the hue angle
increases.
COLOR MATCHING AND APPEARANCE SCALING 193
Fig. 5-25 Hue angle differences in degrees as a function of hue angle between uniform hue
steps on a constant chroma contour in the optimized cone sensitivity based opponent diagram,
Newhall data. The straight line is a linear regression line.
Partial scaling of hue circles has been done by Qiao et al. (1998) and several
color vision scientists (e.g., see Krauskopf and Gegenfurtner, 1992). It is also
implicit in small color difference data (but assuming that a total color differ-
ence can be split into components based on the euclidean model and that the
CIELAB formula describes visually uniform chroma circles). Differences in
stimulus increments needed at different hue angles may be the result of dif-
ferent specific mechanisms being responsible for hue identification and
discrimination.
of the more central area of the retina by absorbing short wavelength light. The
macula has the form of an irregular spot centered on the fovea, the most sensi-
tive region of the retina. However, the central region of the fovea is free of
macular material.There is the interesting situation of the central fovea believed
to have no S cones and being macula free.10 Informal tests, with rare exceptions,
have not resulted in any discernible hue differences when simultaneously
viewing the same colored paper with a gray mask with 2° and 10° openings.
Small hue changes have been noted in case of some complex grays and some
purples. Chroma is often slightly enhanced for small fields compared to larger
ones, perhaps as a result of increased contrast at the lower field size.11 The 2°
observer geometry results in the image being focused to a good extent on the
macula-free central fovea.The 10° observer geometry, on the other hand, results
in a considerable filtering effect by the macula expressed in the different func-
tions. Nevertheless, appearance of colored materials against a neutral surround
seems largely unaffected by field size from 2° to 10°.
From the differences between the two sets of observer functions one can
predict that metameric matches viewed in a 2° field generally will not match
in a 10° field. In regard to appearance, the “blue” system axis falls on domi-
nant wavelength approximately 477 nm for the 2° observer and approximately
470 nm for the 10° observer. It means that if unique hue for the 2° observer
falls on the negative b axis, that axis for the 10° observer represents a slightly
reddish blue. For the “yellow” axis the values are 578 nm, respectively 573 nm,
implying in case of the 10° observer a slightly greenish yellow (as demon-
strated in the OSA-UCS system). The 10° blue-yellow axis can be rotated by
a linear transformation so that it is much closer to the perceptual unique blue
and yellow. The x̄ modification required is approximately as follows:
x10 ,mod = 0.9 x10 + 0.1z10 . (5-13).
However, such a change also results in significant changes in predicted chroma
of a hue circle. The difference between the two CIE standard observers there-
fore is not just a matter of axis rotation but represents a more fundamental
change. It is evident that very specific forms of color-matching functions would
be required to reduce reflectances to geometric patterns that closely resem-
ble the geometrical pattern of the psychological space. However, it is ques-
tionable if such a transformation exists. There is evidence, to be discussed later,
that such a simple approach of combining facts of color matching with facts
of color appearance is insufficient. Thus appearance is not in good agreement
with the changes implied by the color-matching functions.
The dominant wavelengths of objects seen as having unique red hue are not
spectral; their complementary wavelengths fall for most observers on the
PLACEMENT OF THE RED AND GREEN UNIQUE HUES 195
Fig. 5-26 Spectral reflectance functions of a Munsell color chip (2.5R6/8) nearly representing
average unique red hue and a modified red. Open circles represent a simplified metamer of
2.5R6/8. Inverted triangles show the short waveband adjustment required to place the color on
the positive a* axis of the CIELAB chromatic diagram.
purple line in the CIE chromaticity diagram. This is indicated by the fact that
objects with red appearance have dominant wavelengths not only in the long
wave but also in the shortwave region. Conceptually unique red is located
where blueness and yellowness are in balance. The reflectance function of an
object resulting in unique red hue appearance in standard daylight against an
achromatic surround has a small short wave and a larger long wave loop. The
real and an idealized reflectance curve representing Munsell color 2.5R6/8,
closest in hue at that level of chroma and value to average unique red, is illus-
trated in Fig. 5-26. While for the 2° observer the tristimulus values are identi-
cal, there is a calculated color difference of 0.9 CIELAB units between them
for the 10° observer. When calculating the position of this hue in the a*, b*
opponent color diagram, we find that its hue angle is not near 0° but at 18°
(15° for the 10° observer). The Munsell hue located at 0° for both observers
is 5RP, for most observers a distinctly bluish red. Instead of near zero, the b*
value for 2.5R6/8is 12.1, indicating that there is too much implied yellowness
or not enough blueness present to balance the two. One can determine how
much higher the shortwave reflectance would have to be (illustrated in Fig. 5-
26) to result in a b* value of zero. It is relatively a 40% increase. It is possible
to shift the weight of the b+ opponent function on the spectral scale while
maintaining its total weight in such a way that b* has a considerably smaller
value. But as a result dramatic imbalances of the chromas of a Munsell hue
circle are obtained. A similar situation applies to unique green. In fact there
196 PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING OF COLOR ATTRIBUTES: STIMULUS AND PERCEPTION
xmod = ( x + mx ) - mz , (5-14)
where m is a factor with a value of 0.15 or less and depends on the visual angle
occupied by the sample; that is, the optimal value for m differs for the two CIE
standard observers (Kuehni, 1999). The effect of this equation is to adjust the
relative size of the two loops of the x̄ function by slightly reducing the short-
wave and enlarging the long wave loop. We have seen above that adjustments
of this type (expressed in terms of cone sensitivity) are also important for
optimal fitting of constant chroma data. The effect is also evident in the short-
wave red loop of the OSA-UCS g function (see Fig. 7-17b). P. W. Trezona and
R. P. Parkins (1998) pointed out that the size of the two loops in the CIE color-
matching functions is somewhat arbitrary. Application of the revised function
rotates not only bluish but also (if less so) yellowish colors in the a*, b*
diagram.
For a better understanding of how the three psychophysical color spaces are
related, it is instructive to compare a selection of Munsell colors (Kuehni,
MUNSELL COLORS IN THE L, M, S AND X, Y, Z SPACES AND THE A, B DIAGRAM 197
Fig. 5-27 Ellipses in the a*, b* diagram fitted to selected Luo and Rigg ( solid) and to the RIT-
DuPont (dashed) small color difference data. Note the rotation of ellipses near the negative b*
axis. From Melgosa et al. (1997).
2001c). For this purpose a “Celtic cross” figure is selected consisting of a hue
circle at chroma 8 and colors at increasing chroma from the neutral point of
hues falling nearest the axes of the a, b diagram at values 3, 6, and 8. It is under-
stood that the hue and chroma scaling of these colors cannot be taken as
perfect, but there is no doubt that the corresponding samples are a reasonable
approximation of a uniform hue circle and uniform chroma scales. In the psy-
chological chromatic diagram the three Celtic crosses coincide forming a
single cross. Figure 5-28 illustrates these colors in the three-dimensional psy-
chological diagram. After converting the CIE tristimulus values (adjusted to
reflect an equal energy illuminant) to L, M, S values using the Smith-Pokorny
transformation, the colors are plotted in the L, M, S space in Fig. 5-29. The
three crosses form sections through an elliptical funnel. Circles in the percep-
tual diagram are strongly elongated along the S axis in the L, S and M, S planes.
The chroma steps along the S axis become increasingly larger as S increases.
Because of the definition of Y in terms of L and M the slices of the elliptical
198 PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING OF COLOR ATTRIBUTES: STIMULUS AND PERCEPTION
Fig. 5-28 Representation of Munsell colors at values 3, 6, and 8 and chroma 8, and chroma
scales on the axes (Celtic crosses) in the conceptual Munsell color space where hue, value,
and chroma scales are not matched.
funnel are not perpendicular to the equal energy axis but slanted. It is evident
that a series of constant value/constant chroma Munsell hue circles has a
complex form in the L, M, S space without a simple relationship to any of the
three dimensions.
The situation improves when plotting the same colors in the X, Y, Z space
(Fig. 5-30). Since in this space one of the dimensions is luminous reflectance,
colors of a given Munsell value fall on planes perpendicular to the Y axis. The
chroma axes of the cross are well aligned with the X and Z axes of the space.
Scaling of X against Z is required to make the contours circular. This is evident
if we plot the three crosses in a normalized linear opponent color diagram
where a = 2.272 (X - Y) and b = Y - Z (Fig. 5-31). The funnel of hue circles is
now parallel to the Y axis. In order to convert the funnel to a cylinder in agree-
ment with the psychological cylinder and at the same time create uniform
chroma scales, we must determine the applicable psychophysical function(s).
As mentioned above, in 1942 Adams proposed that the Munsell value func-
tion was applicable to all three tristimulus values, and this proposal has been
enshrined in the CIELAB formula. However, the three Munsell Celtic crosses
require power modulation that is different for the four semi axes (Table 5-2),
and additional modulation to convert the cone in tristimulus space with good
accuracy into a cylinder (more on this subject in Chapter 7)
SUPRATHRESHOLD SMALL COLOR DIFFERENCES 199
Fig. 5-29 Actual Munsell colors approximating the Celtic crosses of Fig. 5-28 in the L, M, S
cone sensitivity space; 2° observer, equal energy illuminant. The straight line represents the
equal energy locus.
Differences in all sets of such data, as will be seen in the next chapter, have
been judged as total differences. In nearly all cases the total differences, as cal-
culated by the CIELAB formula, involve differences in all three attributes.
The historical process of fitting psychophysical formulas to such data is
described in Chapter 6. CIELAB did not result in good correlation against
visual judgments in various data sets, and efforts since the mid-1970s usually
involved modification of CIELAB to improve correlation. The most widely
used formulas today (in the United States) are CMC and CIELAB. The CIE
has issued recommendations of new formulas in 1994 and 2001. As in the case
of hue and chroma scaling data sets, different sets of small color difference
judgments vary considerably. A formula fitted to one set of data often does
not fit another set well. Some of the reasons may have to do with
different experimental conditions, often inadequately described. Other
reasons may involve the composition of the observer pools used in the exper-
iments and, possibly, cognitive components in the judgments of the observers.
Clarification of the reasons behind the psychological variability is important
for accurate model building.
200 PSYCHOPHYSICAL SCALING OF COLOR ATTRIBUTES: STIMULUS AND PERCEPTION
Fig. 5-30 Colors of Fig. 5-29 in the 2° observer X, Y, Z tristimulus space, equal energy illu-
minant. The straight line represents the equal energy locus.
Fig. 5-31 Colors of Fig. 5-29 projected onto a balanced linear opponent color diagram based
on tristimulus values (2° observer, equal energy illuminant).
DIFFERENCE THRESHOLD MEASUREMENTS 201
such clear orientation of cell response is absent, even though there is a slight
tendency in this direction (Lennie, 1999). The same applies to areas V2 to V4.
De Valois et al. (1997) found that LGN cells of all types have input into later
cells with color sensitivity. This may explain the differences in threshold mea-
surement results along the cardinal axes compared to away from them. These
results point to the possibility of a rather complex hue detection mechanism
that may depend on averaging computations among many cells with specific
hue tuning.
This is an interesting question for which several answers have been given in
the past. The first determinations of this kind addressed the limited issue of
the number of discriminable steps in the spectrum. Initial experimental
attempts to answer this question were made by E. Mandelstamm, reported in
1867. Measurements considered valid for many years were those by W.
Dobrowolsky (1872). Kries computed in 1882 from Dobrowolsky’s data 208
discriminably different steps. Based on König and Dieterici’s experiments of
1884 the number increased to 235. However, there is the issue of conflation of
hue, saturation, and brightness differences when looking at spectral colors. In
addition the results depend on the experimental setup. When correcting for
brightness differences, L. A. Jones in 1917 found 128 hue steps. Many deter-
minations of wavelength discrimination have been made since then, notably
by Wright and Pitt in 1934 and Bedford and Wyszecki in 1958, with quite
similar results. MacAdam (1947), on basis of his color-matching error data, cal-
culated a number of 250 just noticeable differences in the spectrum, not far
different from that of König and Dieterici.
But color, as we know, is not just hue but also brightness/lightness and sat-
uration/chroma. In 1896 Titchener estimated the total number of perceptibly
different colors at about 33,000. By 1939 the number, as estimated by Boring,
had risen to 300,000. Both Titchener and Boring did not distinguish between
light colors and object colors. In the same year Judd (1939) estimated the
number of perceptible object colors as 10 million.This figure has become much
quoted. In 1943, in connection with their development of a psychological color
solid (see Figs. 1-2 and 1-3), Nickerson and Newhall calculated a rounded
number of 7,500,000 at the just noticeable difference level (applicable to the
model on the right side of Fig. 1-2) and a number of 1,875,000 under less favor-
able viewing conditions (applicable to the model on the left side). This number
applies to a color solid reaching out to the object color limits.
MacAdam in 1947, based on his color matching error data of 1942, limited
himself to determining the number of perceptually different colors in a con-
stant luminance plane. Using three standard deviations of the matching error
as the JND limit, he calculated the number of colors distinguishable in a con-
stant luminance plane, determined under the conditions of his color-matching
error experiment, in a rounded number, as 17,000.
HOW MANY COLORS CAN WE DISTINGUISH? 203
More recently the question has appeared again. In 1981 A. Hård and L.
Sivik (in contrast to the 10 million colors Judd mentioned as discriminable)
estimated the number of distinct colors that can be identified with a degree of
certainty as 10 to 20,000. In 1995 Indow estimated the number of colors that
can be discriminated from each other as approximately 7 million without pro-
viding an explanation for the estimate. In 1998, using the CIELAB formula as
a basis and assuming the limit of object color distinction to be 1 CIELAB unit
of total color difference, M. R. Pointer and G. G. Attridge calculated the
number of distinguishable object colors to be 2.28 million. They also calcu-
lated the theoretical limit of different object colors displayable on a video
monitor to be 16.78 million.
Based on a single medium gray background/surround the number must be
significantly smaller than 2.3 million because of lightness and chromatic
crispening effects. At the same time the JND limit near the surround is likely
smaller than 1 CIELAB unit. The CIE94 color difference formula predicts that
one CIELAB chroma unit at metric chroma zero is only approximately 0.2
chroma units at metric chroma 100. Unless there is a widely variable surround
the number of perceptibly different object colors is more likely about 1
million. Nevertheless, it is a remarkable number of different percepts based
on the output from three cone types.
There is, however, an additional issue.The number of 1 million or 2.3 million
applies to the situation of placing a net defining JNDs over a much finer net
defining visible stimuli. Once a starting position is defined, one can perceive 1
million other colors compared to the reference color. But the reference color
can be varied within the just noticeable difference limen around it. Applying
the JND net to a new reference point, we experience 1 million slightly differ-
ent colors. The number of colors we can experience may, after all, be more in
the range of what can be produced on a video monitor.
In this chapter it has been shown that the fitting of psychophysical models to
uncertain psychological data has added another layer of problems. In the
absence of full understanding of the human visual mechanism, such modeling
is mainly empirical. The currently neurophysiologically supported (in
macaque) opponent space at the LGN level is not in agreement with the
human psychological opponent color space. But a comparatively simple
adjustment can improve the fit. Interestingly different experimental psycho-
logical constant chroma circles can be modeled closely with only changes in
the level of this adjustment and modifications in scaling of the semi axes. It is
necessary to investigate the causes of differences in constant chroma evalua-
tions: uncontrolled surround effects, variability in the personal color-matching
functions of the observer pool, perhaps other reasons. Different sets of hue
scaling data around a constant chroma circle also have provided significantly
different results. In both cases development of reliable, replicated data seems
urgent.
Chapter 6
Historical Development
of Color Space and
Color Difference
Formulas
The line element is the so-called first fundamental form of a regular surface.1
For small color differences it is given explicitly by the Riemannian metric
Color Space and Its Divisions: Color Order from Antiquity to the Present, by Rolf G. Kuehni
ISBN 0-471-32670-4 Copyright © 2003 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
204
LINE ELEMENTS 205
which determines the arc length of a curve on a surface. Connected with the
line element is the geodesic: the curve that locally minimizes the length of a
curve. It depends on the geometry of the space to which it applies: in plane
geometry the geodesic is a straight line, on a sphere a segment of a great circle
(e.g., the equator). In connection with color Wyszecki and Stiles (1982) defined
the line element as “a measure of distance in a postulated space in which per-
ceived colors are represented by points or vectors.” Historically the spaces
related to given line elements have been based on color fundamentals R, G,
and B or cone sensitivities.The differences between colors in these spaces have
been defined with increments, originally based on the Weber-Fechner law and
later on more complex relationships.
2 2 2
2 1 È l R (dR) lG (dG) l B (dB) ˘
(ds) = Í + + ˙, (6-3)
l R R + lGG + l B B Î R G B ˚
Stiles
In 1946 Stiles offered a version of a line element based on extensive investi-
gation of two-color thresholds. Importantly Stiles found that different Weber
fractions apply to the three visual processes rather than the common fraction
assumed by Helmholtz and Schrödinger. Stiles’s line element has the follow-
ing form:
2 2
z (R) ˘ Èz (G) ˘ Èz (B) ˘
(ds) = ÈÍ
2
dR˙ + Í dG˙ + Í , (6-4)
Î r ˚ Î g ˚ Î b ˙˚
d a 2 + d b 2 + dg 2
ds 2 = 2
(6-5)
È 1 ˘
ÍÎ1 + 4 K (a 2 + b 2 + g 2 ) ˙˚
number of quanta for which supersaturation occurs. The model thus can
account for various levels of saturation of the cone system and in this manner
can predict, in addition to color discrimination at the threshold level, the
Bezold-Brücke and the Stiles-Crawford effects. While the model is a modified
version of the Helmholtz line element, it considers the ratio of cones that gives
rise to different Weber fractions for the three cone types as well as saturation
and supersaturation effects, making it not just applicable to a middle range of
luminance but to the complete range.
The Helmholtz, Schrödinger, Stiles, and Vos-Walraven line elements were
believed to be more or less uniform in terms of thresholds. However, as will
be shown in Chapter 8, they are regular color spaces. In his paper Schrödinger
suggested that geodesics, the lines indicating the paths of smallest numbers of
threshold differences, are lines of constant hue. MacAdam originally deter-
mined geodesics based on his color-matching error data mechanically, by
stretching threads across a model representing his ellipses (MacAdam, 1981).
Geodesics can be calculated, for example, by using linear programming (Jain,
1972). For an example of constant hue and saturation geodesic lines in the CIE
chromaticity diagram calculated from MacAdam’s 1965 geodesic chromatic-
ity diagram (MacAdam, 1981), see Fig. 6-1. With known hue, chroma, and light-
ness geodesics, it is possible to develop formulas transforming nonuniform
cone sensitivity or tristimulus spaces into a space uniform in terms of the
assumptions of the model involved.
Another approach to a uniform color space was based on the idea that the
CIE 1931 x, y chromaticity diagram could be linearly transformed to result in
a modified diagram in which distances were proportional to visual distances.
In 1932 D. B. Judd offered an early version representing his own threshold and
other published data (Fig. 6-2) and based on color-matching functions recom-
mended by the Optical Society of America in 1922. The diagram contains
radial lines of constant dominant wavelength and ovoids of constant colori-
metric purity. In 1935 Judd published a modified version of a uniform chro-
maticity diagram in the form of a Maxwell triangle (Fig. 6-3). In a different
reference frame the diagram became in 1960 the basis for the CIE u, v
diagram. In 1935 Judd also introduced the symbol DE to denote a color dif-
ference. In the following year Judd published a graph of the CIE 1931 chro-
maticity diagram with ellipses that represent circles of equal size in his 1935
diagram (Fig. 6-4). These ellipses are intended to represent uniform threshold
color differences, enlarged 100 times. They illustrated for the first time explic-
itly the perceptual nonuniformity of the CIE chromaticity diagram.
In 1937 MacAdam modified Judd’s 1935 diagram with simplified coeffi-
cients resulting in a rectangular coordinate diagram. In 1939 F. C.
Breckenridge and W. R. Schaub developed the rectangular uniform chro-
maticity scale diagram (RUCS), a transformed version of Judd’s 1935 diagram.
PROJECTIVE TRANSFORMATIONS 209
Publisher's Note:
Permission to reproduce this image
online was not granted by the
copyright holder. Readers are kindly
asked to refer to the printed version
of this chapter.
Fig. 6-1 Geodesic lines of constant hue and chroma in the CIE chromaticity diagram, calcu-
lated from MacAdam’s 1965 geodesic chromaticity diagram (Fig. 6-9). Solid lines: constant hue;
dashed lines: constant saturation. From MacAdam (1981).
In the same year Judd, Scofield, and Hunter proposed the a, b diagram and
a color space related to it. In 1942 this linear transformation of the CIE
chromaticity diagram became the basis of the National Bureau of Standards
(NBS) formula, with the difference units designated as NBS units or judds
(after D. B. Judd).2 The formula is as follows:
0.5
{[
DE = fg 221Y 0.25 (Da 2 + Db 2 )
0.5 2
] + [kD(Y 0.5 )]
2
} , (6-7)
Fig. 6-2 Judd’s projective transformation of the CIE chromaticity diagram of 1932 meant to
result in proportionality of distances with visual distances. The numbers on the spectral trace
represent wavelength in nanometers.
PROJECTIVE TRANSFORMATIONS 211
Fig. 6-3 Judd’s 1935 uniform chromaticity diagram embedded in the Maxwell triangle. On the
dashed curve are colors of a black body at the indicated absolute temperatures in Kelvin.
Compare with Fig. 2-29.
U * = 13 W * (u - u0 ),
V * = 13 W * (v - v0 ),
W * = 25 Y 1 3 - 17, (6-9)
212 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR SPACE AND COLOR DIFFERENCE FORMULAS
Fig. 6-4 CIE chromaticity diagram with ellipses that represent circles of equal size in Judd’s
1935 diagram. From Judd (1936).
Fig. 6-5 Munsell Renotation colors at value 5 as represented in the CIELUV projective trans-
formation formula. From Mahy et al. (1994).
where x¢, y¢ are the transformed chromaticity coordinates and x, y are the CIE
chromaticity coordinates; axx are transformation coefficients. Several of the
formulas are in a simplified form requiring only five coefficients. The coeffi-
cients of the above and other linear transformation formulas are given in Table
I (6.4) of Wyszecki and Stiles (1982). Linear transformations have the advan-
tage, and are thus of interest to optical and lighting engineers, that the system
remains additive and mixtures of two lights fall on a straight line connecting
their locations in the diagram. The CIELUV formula will be further discussed
below.
tation colors. In this form the Munsell system was taken to represent a good
approximation of a visually uniform psychological color space.
In 1936 Nickerson proposed the first formula with the goal of predicting
perceptually equal color differences, the Nickerson Index of Fading (see equa-
tion 4-1). The formula has a purely psychological basis, since no physical mea-
surements are involved in its application. Its immediate purpose was to
provide a measure of fading by light of dyed textiles. Component differences
were combined by simple addition. The formula recognized the dependence
in a polar system of the hue difference on chroma and also indicated that in
the Munsell system unit differences in the three attributes are of different per-
ceptual magnitude. As a result such a system is not uniform, as it doesn’t rep-
resent distances in all directions proportionally to differences in its euclidean
space (see Chapter 7).
In 1942 Adams plotted Munsell colors at several values in what he called a
“chromance” diagram, representing a linear opponent color diagram norma-
lized to the equal energy point. The two dimensions were defined as X - Y and
Z - Y. By applying the Munsell-Sloan-Godlove lightness formula (a power 0.5
modulation; see Chapter 5) not only to the luminous reflectance value Y but
also to the other two tristimulus values, Adams converted the chromance
diagram to a chromatic value diagram. Adams interpreted the subtractions as
representations of his 1923 theory of color vision that took the color-matching
functions to be cone responses. In this theory Adams had assumed an inhibitory
effect of the output of the postulated Y cone on the outputs of the X, respec-
tively Z cones. He represented this inhibitory effect with subtractions. He called
the outputs VX, VY, and VZ.Adams calculated chromatic values of a high chroma
Munsell hue circle and found those related to Z to be larger than those related
to X and Y.To have scales of maximum value 10 in all three dimensions he pro-
posed to multiply the VX values by 1.90 and the Vz values by 0.72. The result of
these operations showed reasonably circular contours in the chromatic value
diagram for Munsell colors of the 1929 Book of Color and the preliminary
smoothed values of the committee (see Fig. 5-12). Adams did not propose a
color difference formula based on his VX ,VY, VZ space, but assuming that it is
a euclidean space, such a formula is obvious. Adams’s chromatic value system
proved to be influential in coming years.
Aside from the Munsell data, in flux until the release of the Renotations
in 1943, there were few object color sample sets with statistically supported
visual data. In 1941 Balinkin reported on a set of five pale green tiles. One pair
of tiles was designated as the standard difference, and sixty observers estimated
the differences of all possible pairs against the reference pair. The data were
used in several studies of color difference formulas. In 1944 Nickerson and her
co-worker Stultz investigated the usefulness of color difference calculation for
color quality control work. The visual data consisted of category judgments
(five categories) by twelve observers of painted textile samples, primarily in
two color regions: yellowish brown and olive green. In this study they used,
among others, a formula based on Adams’s chromatic value space as follows:
JUDD’S MODEL OF MÜLLER’S THEORY OF COLOR VISION 215
2 0.5
[ 2 2
DE A = (DVY ) + {D(VX - VY )} + {0.4 D(VZ - VY )} ] , (6-11)
where VX ,VY, VZ are the Adams chromatic values of the samples, now inter-
preted in terms of the revised Munsell value function. This formula became
known as the Adams-Nickerson color difference formula and proved of endur-
ing value. In their evaluations Nickerson and Stultz found the formula only
marginally better than formulas derived by Judd but considerably easier to
calculate. The experiments also indicated considerable individual variability in
judgment.
In 1946 J. L. Saunderson and B. L. Milner proposed a modification of the
Adams chromatic value system to obtain closer agreement with the Munsell
system. Contours of constant chroma are somewhat eccentric in the Adams
chromatic diagram, and the Saunderson-Milner solution corrected for the
eccentricity using an empirical trigonometric method. The Saunderson-Milner
color space model was described as the “Zeta” space, based on their use of the
Greek letter. It is defined as
z 2 = kVY ,
where Q is the angle calculated from tanQ = 0.4(VZ - VY)/(VZ - VY), and k is
a constant depending on the observation conditions. Color differences are
calculated as the square root of the sum of the squares of the differences in
the three z values. A somewhat different procedure with a comparable effect
was proposed in 1952 by Godlove.
Assuming that two concentric circles of five equally spaced Munsell hues
are a good representation of psychologically uniform space, Burnham in 1949
investigated the performance of ten formulas (including the CIE x, y and x, z
diagrams) and found the Saunderson-Milner Zeta space to perform best.
However, all of the formulas resulted in deviations that were in visual terms
statistically significant.
Threshold differences were first investigated along the spectrum because exact
setting of spectral differences was technically relatively easy. However, spec-
tral differences are complex in visual terms since every difference is composed
of hue, saturation, and brightness components. Wavelength discrimination
began to be investigated at the turn of the twentieth century. A classical inves-
tigation is that by W. D. Wright and F. H. G. Pitt (1934). Wright subsequently
also investigated threshold differences along straight lines in the CIE chro-
maticity diagram. The results of both investigations are illustrated in Fig. 6-6
(Wright, 1941). Wright’s important data were overshadowed by MacAdam’s
extensive color matching error data of a single observer, published a year later
(1942). As discussed in Chapter 3, in MacAdam’s work two hemi-fields were
displayed against a dark surround in a specially constructed colorimeter. In
one-half a standard color was displayed. The color of the second half could be
adjusted by the observer along straight lines passing through the standard
color in the CIE chromaticity diagram, in a constant luminance plane. Using
a single knob, the observer adjusted the color of the test field by the method
of adjustment until a visual match between the two hemi-fields was achieved.
The match was approached along a given line from both sides. Color matches
along several lines were repeatedly set for each of 25 standard colors. From
the visual data (some 20,000 observations) MacAdam calculated the standard
error of color matching for his single observer. The result, fitted with ellipses,
is illustrated in Fig. 6-7. MacAdam also determined that the threshold differ-
ences around his standard colors were approximately twice the standard
deviation of the color-matching errors. Subsequently comparable but three-
dimensional contours involving also brightness differences were determined
for additional observers, and the results indicated that observers vary signifi-
cantly in this task (Brown and MacAdam, 1949; Brown, 1957). The MacAdam
ellipses rapidly became a key set of data used as test data for line elements
and color difference formulas. Color-matching error was explained in 1949 in
terms of cone activity by Y. LeGrand (see Chapter 8). The ellipses obtained
good confirmation in an experiment by R. M. Boynton and N. Kambe (1980).
Additional determinations of color-matching error were made by G. Wyszecki
and G. Fielder (1971). Determinations of achromatic and chromatic thresholds
using industrially relevant conditions were performed by K. Richter (1985)
and by Witt (1987, 1990). For a comparison of such data, see Chapter 8. As
mentioned in the previous chapter, thresholds and color-matching error data
of limited groups of colors and using various methodologies have been per-
formed in recent years by several researchers.
Fig. 6-6 Perceptually equal steps between spectral colors as well as colors in the interior of
the chromaticity diagram, as determined by Wright (1969).
MacAdam calculated gik values from the following equation that describes his
ellipses in geometrical terms:
2 2 2
(ds) = g11 (dx) + 2 g12 dxdy + g 22 (dy) = 1, (6-13)
218 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR SPACE AND COLOR DIFFERENCE FORMULAS
Publisher's Note:
Permission to reproduce this image
online was not granted by the
copyright holder. Readers are kindly
asked to refer to the printed version
of this chapter.
Fig. 6-7 Color matching error ellipses of observer PGN for 25 colors at the center of the
ellipses, enlarged 10 times, in the CIE 2° chromaticity diagram. From MacAdam (1942).
where ds is the distance between the center of an ellipse and a point on its
contour and x, y are CIE chromaticity coordinates. MacAdam drew interpo-
lating lines connecting his data points. Results are shown in Fig. 6-8a–c.
Knowing the gik values for a particular location in the chromaticity diagram
by reading them from tables or graphs with interpolation between neighbor-
ing values makes possible the calculation of color differences using eq. 6-13
and applying the square root. Alternately, the chromaticity diagram could be
modified locally to convert the ellipses to circles of equal size. Sets of charts
that simplified the calculation of color differences by this method have been
available in the 1950s and 1960s. From the results of such calculation it was
apparent that linear transformation of the CIE chromaticity diagram does not
yield a uniform chromaticity diagram in which the MacAdam ellipses form
circles of equal size.
By 1950 the situation presented itself as follows: On the one hand, there were
line elements, either derived from theoretical considerations using best esti-
COLOR DIFFERENCE THRESHOLDS AND MATCHING ERROR 219
Publisher's Note:
Permission to reproduce this image
online was not granted by the
copyright holder. Readers are kindly
asked to refer to the printed version
of this chapter.
(a)
Fig. 6-8a–c Contours of the g11, 2g12, and g22 ellipse parameter functions in the CIE chro-
maticity diagram for the ellipses of Fig. 6-7.
Publisher's Note:
Permission to reproduce this image
online was not granted by the
copyright holder. Readers are kindly
asked to refer to the printed version
of this chapter.
(b)
Fig. 6-8a–c (Continued)
The Roman numeral distinguished it from a later version, FMC II, with a dif-
ferent treatment of lightness. (For an informative discussion of the develop-
ment of these formulas see MacAdam 1981.) Friele based his approach on the
three-stage color vision theory by G. E. Müller (1930). The CIE tristimulus
values were converted to cone sensitivity functions P, Q, and S. Opponent
color differences and lightness differences were calculated from these in two
steps.
FMC II formula
P = 0.724 X + 0.382Y - 0.098Z ,
S = 0.686Z ,
(QDP - PDQ)
DC rg = 0.5
,
(P 2 + Q 2 )
SDL1
DC yb = 0.5
- DS,
(P + Q 2 )
2
COLOR DIFFERENCE THRESHOLDS AND MATCHING ERROR 221
Publisher's Note:
Permission to reproduce this image
online was not granted by the
copyright holder. Readers are kindly
asked to refer to the printed version
of this chapter.
(PDP + QDQ)
DL1 = 0.5
,
(P 2 + Q 2 )
0.279 DL1
DL2 = ,
a
2 2 0.5
ÈÊ DC rg ˆ Ê DC yb ˆ ˘
DC1 = Í + ˙ ,
ÎË a ¯ Ë b ¯ ˚
0.0000173(P 2 + Q 2 )
a2 = ,
[1 + 2.73P 2Q 2 (P 4Q 4 )]
b 2 = 0.0003098(S 2 + 0.2015Y 2 ),
DC = K1 DC1 ,
DL = K 2 DL2 ,
222 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR SPACE AND COLOR DIFFERENCE FORMULAS
K1 = 0.054 + 0.46Y 1 3 ,
The total color difference is calculated from the chromatic and the lightness
difference as
0.5
[ 2
DE = (DC ) + (DL2 ) ] . (6-15)
The two K functions have the purpose of adjusting the size of the implied
ellipse as a function of luminous reflectance and to adjust the lightness value
to be in reasonable agreement with Munsell lightness. They are shown above
in the simplified form proposed by MacAdam.
A uniform (in terms of color-matching error) color space model is
implicit in this difference formula, but it has not been stated explicitly.
The formula is an elaborate fitting of the MacAdam ellipses in a cone-based
opponent color framework. It represents a compromise in regard to size of
the ellipse as a function of luminous reflectance. MacAdam had found that
the color-matching error ellipses were affected only to a small extent (less
than 20%) by changes within reasonable levels in luminosity. The K1 function
made them change in size in line with cube root compression. In his work
with Brown, MacAdam had determined that ellipsoids generated from
threshold determinations including luminance differences had one of
their three axes parallel to the luminance axis. In the Adams chromatic
value space, on the other hand, corresponding ellipsoids are tilted toward
the neutral point of the chromatic diagram. One of the advantages of the
FMC formula is that it implicitly adjusts for the Helmholtz-Kohlrausch
effect.
Publisher's Note:
Permission to reproduce this image
online was not granted by the
copyright holder. Readers are kindly
asked to refer to the printed version
of this chapter.
Fig. 6-9 MacAdam’s geodesic uniform chromaticity diagram, 1965. Lines of constant value of
chromaticity coordinates x and y are drawn in the interior.
where
a1 = 10 x (2.4 x + 34 y + 1) ,
a2 = 10 x (4.2 y - x + 1) ,
b1 = 10 y (2.4 x + 34 y + 1) ,
b2 = 10 y (4.2 y - x + 1) .
L = 10Y 0.5 ,
17.5(1.02 X - Y )
aL = ,
Y 0.5
7.0(Y - 0.847Z )
bL = , (6-17)
Y 0.5
where X, Y, Z are the CIE tristimulus values for illuminant C taken as per-
centages. The total color difference was given as the square root of the sum of
the squares of the differences in L, aL, and bL. These instruments were quite
popular and continued to be used into the 1980s. Kuehni showed that unit
chromatic contours derived from the formula were not in good agreement with
ellipses fitted to small color difference data (1982).
The calculation of the Munsell value function from luminous reflectance
with the quintic formula was troublesome, and efforts were made to find a
simpler solution. It was found that properly scaled cube roots of luminous
reflectance resulted in good agreement with the quintic formula. In 1958 L. G.
Glasser, A. H. McKinney, C. D. Reilley, and P. D. Schnelle proposed a cube
root model of color space:
L* = 25.29G1 3 - 18.38,
a* = Ka (R1 3 - G1 3 ),
in industry to assess quality of coloration. Among these were the FMC II, the
Adams-Nickerson or the cube root, Saunderson-Milner, CIE 1964, and the
Hunter L,a,b formulas.
Strocka’s results with the circular formula were puzzling but indicated that the
actual unit difference contours in the CIE chromaticity diagram could not be
circles, since the correlation with the circular formula would have been greater.
The implication was that the ellipsoidal contours implicit in a formula such as
Adams-Nickerson did not match the ellipsoidal contours implicit in the visual
data well, perhaps in direction as well as in size. In 1971 Kuehni graphically
fitted unit ellipses in the CIE chromaticity diagram to various sets of visual
data (Fig. 6-10; Kuehni 1971a). The results indicated that the major axes of
the resulting ellipses were usually tilted 20° to 30° clockwise compared to
the MacAdam ellipses and that ellipses tended to increase in size as chroma
increased (as McLaren had found).
Kuehni asked MacAdam to fit the parameters of his xi-eta equation (see
above) to fitted ellipses. The resulting, then unpublished, equations are as follows:
Fig. 6-10 Ellipses graphically fitted to small suprathreshold color difference data by Robinson
1962 (Rob), and by Davidson and Friede 1953 (A–T). From Kuehni (1971a).
The lightness scale used together with the formulas above was the cube root
scale. The combined formulas resulted in a correlation with visual data sig-
nificantly higher than the cube root and ANLAB or the circle formula.
In 1972 Kuehni refined the ellipse fitting method by systematically vary-
ing ellipse parameters until highest correlation for the samples around a
standard were obtained. In 1975 R. M. Rich, F. W. Billmeyer, and W. G. Howe
employed a computer algorithm using the maximum likelihood function to fit
ellipses to visual data with greater statistical validity. Mathematical ellipse or
ellipsoid fitting has since become a standard tool of investigation of color
differences.
As shown in Section 8.7 the final answer to this question has not been found
yet.
Similarly Brown and MacAdam had found that the third axis of color-
matching error ellipsoids was aligned parallel with the luminance axis. For-
mulas such as Adams-Nickerson have implied third axes that are tilted toward
the white point of the color space. Work based on small suprathreshold dif-
ferences reported by W. Schultze and L. Gall in 1971 indicated no tilt of the
ellipsoids. The comparative success of power modulation-based formulas indi-
cated that tilt is appropriate.
The dependence of the ellipse’s size in a constant luminous reflectance
plane on metric chroma has already been mentioned. In addition there was
the controversy of perceptibility judgments versus acceptability judgments.
While in perceptibility experiments presumably purely psychophysical judg-
ments are obtained (but see Chapter 3), acceptability experiments can, and
sometimes do, include additional cognitive overlays. Perceptibility experi-
ments are usually difference magnitude estimations against a reference pair.
Acceptability experiments also involve difference magnitude estimation,
however, against an internal standard of acceptability in a commercial situa-
tion. Biases in the latter case are possible based on specific situations; that is,
in a chromatic diagram the limit contour of acceptable color differences may
not be positioned symmetrically around the standard. E. Allen and B. Yuhas
(1984) and, more recently, Berns (1996) have shown how this situation can be
mathematically treated. Another issue is that acceptability tolerances for iden-
tically colored materials may vary significantly depending on the context.
However, experiments have shown that tolerance contours from acceptability
judgments and unit difference contours from perceptibility judgments of the
same sample pairs, when determined in the absence of a specific context, are
symmetrical; that is to say, acceptability judgment in that situation is guided
by perceptibility (Kuehni, 1975; McLaren, 1976; Mahy, Van Eycken, and
Oosterlinck, 1994).
Multiple linear regression as well as fitting of ellipses and ellipsoids had clearly
indicated a dependence of the chromatic differences (in the equal luminance
plane) on metric chroma. In 1972 Kuehni proposed a modification to the
Glasser et al. (see above) cube root color difference formula that adjusted the
size of the calculated total color difference as a function of the radial differ-
ence of the standard from the illuminant point in the CIE chromaticity
diagram:
2 0.5
[ 2
DE = (DC ) + (DL) ] , (6-20)
THE CIE 1976 L*a*b* AND L*u*v* SPACES 229
where
2 0.5
[ 2
DC = (Da) + (Db) ] F,
F = 1.0 + 6S ,
2 0.5
[ 2
S = (x - 0.3100) + ( y - 0.3162) ] .
S is applicable for illuminant C. The need for such an adjustment was ascribed
to Takasaki’s chromatic crispening effect. For a combined set of data the
formula provided modest improvement over the FMC II or the unmodified
Glasser formula.
McLaren’s work with multiple linear regression had shown that it was
useful to consider separately three components of the total color difference,
assumed to be related in a euclidean manner: metric lightness, metric chroma,
and metric hue differences. Metric chroma was calculated as the square root
of the sum of the squares of the cartesian coordinates in the Adams-
Nickerson chromatic diagram and metric lightness and metric chroma were
subtracted in the euclidean manner from total color difference to result in
metric hue difference:
2 0.5
[ 2
DH = (DE ) - (DL) - (DC )
2
] . (6-21)
While the L, a, b view of the system is the cartesian coordinate view of the
euclidean space, that of L, C, H is the polar coordinate view.
Using new experimental data, and expanding on McLaren’s multiple linear
regression, R. McDonald in 1974 found that he could obtain significant
improvement in correlation between visual and calculated data by adjusting
the total color difference as a function of metric chroma as follows:
DE
DE a = , (6-22)
1 + 0.022C
where DEa is the equivalent color difference at the achromatic point and C is
the chroma value.
CIELUV
0.333
ÊYˆ
L* = 116 - 16,
Ë Yn ¯
4X
u' = ,
X + 15Y + 3Z
9Y
v' = ,
X + 15Y + 3Z (6-23)
where
4Xn
un¢ = ,
X n + 15Yn + 3Z n
9Yn
vn¢ = ,
X n + 15Yn + 3Z n
and Xn, Yn, Zn are the tristimulus values of the nominally white object color
stimulus. The optimal object color solid and the spectral trace derived from
this formula are illustrated in Fig. 6-11.
CIELAB
0.333
Y
L* = 116Ê ˆ - 16,
Ë Yn ¯
È X ˆ 0.333 Ê Y ˆ 0.333 ˘
a* = 500 ÍÊ - ˙,
ÎË X n ¯ Ë Yn ¯ ˚
È Y 0.333 Ê Z ˆ 0.333 ˘
b* = 200 ÍÊ ˆ - ˙,
ÎË Yn ¯ Ë Zn ¯ ˚
2 0.5
[ 2
DEab * = (DL *) + (Da *) + (Db *)
2
] , (6-24)
THE CIE 1976 L*a*b* AND L*u*v* SPACES 231
Fig. 6-11 Projective view of the L*, u*, v* object color space for the CIE 10° standard observer
and illuminant D65 (inner contour) and the spectrum locus. From Judd and Wyszecki (1975).
where Xn, Yn, Zn are the tristimulus values of the nominally white object color
stimulus. A slightly different calculation for L*, a*, and b* applies for low tris-
timulus value ratios. For X/Xn, Y/Yn, and Z/Zn > 0.01, instead of cube roots,
factors f are applied to the ratios, determined as follows: f(Y/Yn) = 7.787 (Y/Yn)
+ 16/116 and comparably for Y and Z. This adjustment is valid for the L*, a*,
and b* scales.
Differences can also be calculated in a polar coordinate version, where
From lightness and metric chroma differences, the total color difference DE is
calculated as follows:
2 0.5
[ 2 2
DE = (DL *) + (DC *) + (DH *) ] , (6-25)
where DH* = [(DE)2 - (DL*)2 - (DC*)2]0.5 and DC* = [(Da*)2 + (Db*)2]0.5. The
optimal object color solid and the spectral trace derived from this formula are
illustrated in Fig. 6-12.
As mentioned before, the former formula is primarily of interest to light-
ing engineers as it provides for additivity of light mixtures. The latter was rec-
ommended for use with object colors. CIELAB is a simplification of, but no
advancement over, the Adams-Nickerson formula, and it was apparent at the
232 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR SPACE AND COLOR DIFFERENCE FORMULAS
Fig. 6-12 Projective view of the L*, a*, b* object color space for the CIE 10° standard observer
and illuminant D65 (inner contour) and the spectrum locus. From Judd and Wyszecki (1975).
In 1978 Friele published a new formula optimized against various sets of visual
data. It was a modification of his earlier effort resulting in the FMC metric.
Friele had created psychometric scales for the various sets of acceptability data
that he used in addition to perceptibility data. The formula provided signifi-
cant improvements in correlation for these data, compared to CIELAB.
FCM
B = 0.847Z . (6-26)
For R > G,
13
Ê 1-Y ˆ
T = 125Y 1 3 .
Ë R1 3 ¯
FRIELE’S FCM FORMULA 233
For R < G,
13
0.760 Ê 1-Y ˆ
T= 125Y 1 3 .
-0.484 Ë G1 3 ¯
2 -1 2
Y ÈÊ 0.085 x 4 3 ˆ 2˘
D=Ú ÍË + (0.055Y 2 3 ) ˙ dx. (6-27)
B
Î Y 2 3 ¯ ˚
L = 18Y 1 3 ,
a = T - 0.3D,
b = D - 0.3 T ,
1 2
Chroma = (a 2 + b 2 ) ,
0.024R 4 3
t= ,
Y2 3
1 2
ÈÊ 0.085B4 3 ˆ 2 ˘
d =Í + (0.055Y 2 3 )˙ .
ÎË Y ¯
2 3
˚
c 2 = T 2 + D2 ,
2
ÊD ˆ
a = arctan 2 ,
ËT ¯
DL = 6Y -2 3 DY
Ê X ˆ Ê Z ˆ
tDT = 0.760 DX - DY - 0.124 DZ - DY ,
Ë Y ¯ Ë Y ¯
Ê Z ˆ
tDD = -0.847 DZ - DY ,
Ë Y ¯
2.5 0.5
DE =
1 + 0.01Y
[ 2 2
( fDL1 ) + (DT 2 ) + (DD) - fDTDD . ] (6-28)
In the late 1970s K. Richter proposed a series of formulas as good models for
the Munsell and the OSA-UCS systems (Richter, 1980). Of these only the non-
linear LABHNU2 formula will be mentioned:
2 3
[(x y) + (1 6)]
a¢ = ,
15
13
[(z y) + (1 6)]
b¢ = , (6-29)
12
JPC79 Formula
As mentioned above, in 1976 McLaren optimized weights for the three color
difference components based on the Adams-Nickerson formula. He found that
the optimum weights varied by set of visual data. McDonald continued to
pursue individual adjustment of the three color difference components and
developed new industrial visual data. In 1980 he proposed a formula employ-
ing continuous weight adjustment for all three metric components. It was
based on an extensive set of 640 samples around 55 standards distributed over
a significant portion of the object color solid. The samples consisted of dyed
spun polyester sewing thread and the visual evaluations (acceptability judg-
ments) were performed by eight industrial color matchers. An additional set
of some 8500 judgments against 600 color standards by a single observer in an
WEIGHTING OF METRIC LIGHTNESS, CHROMA, AND HUE DIFFERENCES 235
where DL, DC, and DH are, respectively, the lightness, chroma, and hue differ-
ences calculated from ANLAB,
S L = 0.08195L (1 *0.01765L),
S H = SCT ,
T = 1 if C = 0.38, otherwise
T = 0.38 + 0.4 cos(h + 35) , unless h is between 164∞ and 345∞, then
CMC (l : c) Formula
This formula was slightly modified from JPC79 and based on CIELAB com-
ponent differences by the Color Measurement Committee of the Society of
Dyers and Colourists in England (Clark, 1984). It has been standardized in
England and in the United States, and is recommended by the International
Standards Organization (ISO). It is defined as follows:
2 2 2 0.5
ÈÊ DL* ˆ Ê DC* ˆ Ê DH* ˆ ˘
DECMC =Í + + ˙ , (6-31)
ÎË lSL ¯ Ë cSC ¯ Ë SH ¯ ˚
where
S L = 0.04097L* (1 + 0.01765L*) unless L* < 16, then S L = 0.511,
S H = SC (TF + 1 - F ),
0.5
{
F = (C*)
4
[(C*) 4
+ 1900 ]} ,
and l and c are additional weights adjusting the relative weight of lightness
and chroma differences. Analysis has shown that for flat surface materials
(paints, plastics) viewed in sharp juxtaposition values of 1 in both cases are
appropriate, while for textile materials, the value l = 2 was found to improve
correlation. CMC found wide international usage in industry and continues to
be used in many firms.
In 1978 the CIE issued a set of guidelines for coordinated research on color
difference equations (Robertson, 1978). In these guidelines particular empha-
sis was placed on five color centers: gray, yellow, red, blue, and green. As a
result several experimenters provided data for these centers but new data for
many other centers were also established.
Fig. 6-13 Ellipses fitted to unadjusted data of the Luo and Rigg data set, enlarged 1.5 times,
in a portion of the CIE chromaticity diagram. From Luo and Rigg (1986).
Witt (1987–1990)
In 1987 K. Witt reported on the results of a threshold experiment using 50 to
64 sample pairs in four of the five CIE color regions prepared with high gloss
acrylic paint. Some 24 observers determined at least four times (some
observers 10 times) under simulated daylight and against a surround of Y =
20 if they could perceive a difference between the pairs. Ellipses were calcu-
lated from the resulting thresholds for individual observers and for sets of 4
to 22 observers with from 22 to 118 repetitions. Considerable intra- and inter-
238 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR SPACE AND COLOR DIFFERENCE FORMULAS
RIT-DuPont Data
In an effort to develop a set of high-reliability small color difference percep-
tibility data D. Alman of DuPont and Berns of RIT (Rochester Institute of
Technology) collaborated in 1990 and published the RIT-DuPont data (Berns
et al., 1991). They are based on 156 acrylic lacquer spray-painted sample pairs
on primed aluminum panels representing 19 color centers. The sample pairs
did not have a common reference and were arranged so as to represent spe-
cific vector directions in CIELAB color space. The sample pairs, presented
edge-to-edge on unprimed aluminum panels, were evaluated by 50 observers
against a near gray standard pair presented identically. Probit analysis was
used to establish psychometric scales, and the colorimetric values were set so
that visual differences equal to that caused by a 1.0 DE CIELAB lightness dif-
ference resulted. Ellipsoids in CIELAB space have later been optimized to
the visual data. The ellipses in the a*, b* plane show fair agreement with those
of Luo and Rigg (see Fig. 5-29). Both show ellipses more or less pointed toward
the origin of the diagram, except for blue colors. The data have been published
and are also available on the University of Derby Web site.
Leeds Data
Two sets of data were established by D. H. Kim and J. H. Nobbs at Leeds Uni-
versity in 1997. They consist of matte painted samples. Sample set PC consists
of 152 pairs evaluated juxtaposed by 15 observers against a near neutral ref-
erence pair. Sample set GS consists of 204 sample pairs evaluated against a
gray scale by 12 observers. Color differences ranged from 0.4 to 3.7 CIELAB
units in the two sets. The data are available on the University of Derby Web
site.
Witt (1999)
In 1999 Witt published a set of data involving the five CIE standard color
regions. For each region there are approximately 30 painted samples. These
have been evaluated by from 10 to 13 observers in approximately 85 different
combinations of two each, thus providing a detailed evaluation in each region
of color difference vectors in two and three dimensions. Comparisons were
made against a specially prepared gray scale (near logarithmic) found to be
visually uniform, with the result expressed in steps and third or quarter frac-
tion steps of the reference scale. Color differences ranged from approximately
0.3 to 8 CIELAB units. Means and standard deviations of the visual judgments
were calculated. The coefficient of variation of the visual results ranged from
15% to 60%. The data have been published in Witt’s paper and are available
on the University of Derby Web site.
BFD (l:c)
Using the above-described composite data set, Luo and Rigg (1987) optimized
a formula within the general CMC framework as follows:
2 2 2 0.5
ÈÊ DL ˆ Ê DC* ˆ Ê DH* ˆ Ê DC* ˆ Ê DH* ˆ ˘
DEBFD =Í + + + RT , (6-32)
ÎË l ¯ Ë cDC
¯ Ë DH
¯ Ë DC ¯ Ë DH ¯ ˙˚
240 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR SPACE AND COLOR DIFFERENCE FORMULAS
where
DH = DC (GT ¢ + 1 - G),
0.5
{
G = (C *)
4
[(C ) 4
+ 14000 ]} ,
RT = RH RC ,
DC* and DH* are CIELAB chroma and hue differences. The overbar indicates
the mean value between standard and sample. Constants l and c are compa-
rable to the corresponding constants in the CMC equation.
While DC is a slight modification of SC in CMC DH is a complex function
that corrects for experimental variation in the relationship between hue angle
difference and visual hue difference around the hue circle. An additional term
is added to accommodate ellipses of bluish colors not directed toward the
origin.
SVF Formula
Also in 1986, a different approach was taken by T. Seim and A. Valberg.
They fitted a formula to the Munsell Renotation data based on cone sensi-
tivities. In the first step tristimulus values are converted to cone sensitivities
that are then centered on white (S1 comparable to L, S2 comparable to M and
S3 comparable to S). The lightness value V is obtained with a hyperbolic
formula that represents a close fit of the polynomial Munsell value formula:
VY = 40v1 (Y ), (6-33)
where
0.51
(Y - 0.43)
v1 (Y ) = 0.51
.
(Y - 0.43) + 31.75
NEW FORMULAS 241
Following Adams, the same model is applied to the two chromatic dimensions.
Opponent color signals are calculated in two steps:
p1 = v1 (S1 ) - v1 (Y ),
p2 = v1 (Y ) - v1 (S3 ) if S3 £ Y ,
F1 = 700 p1 - 54 p2 ,
F2 = 96.5 p2 .
Color differences are calculated as
2 0.5
[ 2 2
DESVF = (DF1 ) + (DF2 ) + 2.3(DVY ) ] .
Unlike in the Müller-Judd approach the p level does not involve the
subtraction of cone responses but the subtraction of Y from the response of
L in one case and the response of S in the other. As a result the yellow-blue
axis is established in the final form (except for scaling) at the p level. The
green-red axis needs to be rotated; thus semicolon the subtractive form of F1.
A different hyperbolic function is applied to S > Y to account for the appar-
ent change in the modulation in the Munsell system of the S function of yel-
lowish and bluish colors, which we will encounter again later.
The formula provides a reasonably good fit to the Munsell system even
though the yellowish and bluish constant hue lines are significantly curved.
The formula was also applied to the OSA-UCS data where it is less success-
ful because the implicit modulations in this system are different from those
implicit in the Munsell system. Seim and Valberg applied the formula also to
small suprathreshold color difference data with reportedly good results.
CIE94
In the early 1990s CIE technical committee 1–29 investigated three data sets
considered reliable in regard to the relationship between DL* and L*, DC*, and
C*, and DH* and hue angle h (Berns, 1993). Considerable scatter was found in
all three cases.As a result the committee optimized simple weights for CIELAB
lightness, chroma, and hue differences and issued the formula known as CIE94:
242 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR SPACE AND COLOR DIFFERENCE FORMULAS
2 2 2 0.5
ÈÊ DL * ˆ Ê DC * ˆ Ê DH * ˆ ˘
DE *94 = Í + + ˙ , (6-35)
ÎË kL SL ¯ Ë kC SC ¯ Ë kh Sh ¯ ˚
where
SL = 1,
SC = 1 + 0.045C *,
SH = 1 + 0.015C * .
The k constants are additional weights on the lightness, chroma, and hue dif-
ferences. Under reference conditions they all equal 1. For textile applications
kL typically has a value of 2. No weighting of lightness differences was made
because the scatter in the combined experimental data (without consideration
of surround lightness) was too large for a statistically meaningful weight.
SC = (1 + 0.045C *)SCH ,
S H = (1 + 0.015C *)S HH ,
[ 3
]
S R = -C * (2 + 0.07C ) sin(2DQ),
{
DQ = 30 exp -[ h - 275 25] ,
2
}
K L = KCH = 1.0 for nontextile samples, K L = 1.5 for textile samples.
Complex sinusoidal SCH and SHH functions have been developed that improved
the correlation between calculated and visual data for some data sets but not
others:
SCH = 1 + 0.07 sin(h) - 0.16 cos(2 h + 250) - 0.05 cos(3h) - 0.03 cos(4 h),
S HH = 1 + 0.03 cos(h + 60) + 0.12 cos(2 h) + 0.12 cos(3h) - 0.07 cos(4 h - 45).
NEW FORMULAS 243
Integrating Weights
Proposals to integrate the weights on chroma and hue differences in CIE94
directly into the calculation of modified a* and b* values were made by E.
Rohner and D. C. Rich in 1996, by H. G. Völz in 1998 and 1999, and by K.
Thomsen in 2000 (see also DIN99 below). In Thomsen’s version,
a * ¢ = a *f (C *),
b * ¢ = b *f (C *), (6-37)
where
ln(1 + 0.0531301C *)
f (C *) = .
0.0500951C *
The two numerical factors have been optimized against CIE94 from 200,000
positions at constant L* in the a*, b* diagram. The maximum discrepancy
found by this method, compared to conventional calculation using CIE94, is
10.5%. Given the variability of the visual data behind the SX weights, this is
not problematical. The only direct advantage of such an approach is that
euclidean relations are maintained. However, the additional optimization
parameters added in CIEDE2000 (see below) would require new, more
complex integration efforts to maintain euclidean relationship with question-
able ultimate value, as all are based on the uncertain fundament of CIELAB.
In addition CIEDE2000 is only applicable from threshold to 6 to 8 units of
total difference.
depending on the lightness of the test and the surround fields. It results in S-
shaped functions of the relationship between L* and L*¢.
DIN99 Formula
A new color space and difference formula was developed in 1999 in Germany
(DIN 6176 2000). Its purpose is to apply the integration method proposed by
Rohner and Rich (1996) to produce an euclidean space and difference formula
with good performance against various sets of small color difference data. The
formula went through several stages of development, and presently there are
four versions in existence. Only the latest (and best performing) formula
DIN99d is given here. It is based on the CIELAB formula and the X tristim-
ulus values of reference, and test colors have been adjusted by the procedure
proposed by Kuehni (1999).
X ¢ = 1.12 X - 0.12Z ,
L99d = 325.22 ln(1 + 0.0036L *),
e = a * cos(50 ∞) + b * sin(50 ∞),
f = 1.14[-a * sin(50 ∞) + b * cos(50 ∞)],
0.5
G = (e 2 + f 2 ) ,
C99d = 22.5 ln(1 + 0.06G),
Ê fˆ
h99d = arctan + 50 ∞,
Ë e¯
a99d = C99d cos(h99d ),
b99d = C99d sin(h99d ),
1 0.5
DE99d = (DL299d + Da992 d + Db992d ) . (6-38)
kE
The formula has been tested against the same data used in testing CIEDE2000
(see below) and performs somewhat better than CMC and CIE94 but
slightly inferior to CIEDE2000. Its error against the combined test data set
is 35% (compared to 38% for CIE94 and 33% for CIEDE2000) (Cui et al.,
2002).
The factor 1.1 is applicable to the CIE 10° observer data. It has a value of 1.06
for the 2° observer data. This adjustment results in a rotation of the b* axis
that better aligns the unit ellipses of blue colors with lines of constant hue
angle.
The lightness scale is revised to make it dependent on surround lightness,
resulting in a true opponent color scale. An SC type adjustment for the light-
ness crispening effect is introduced:
0.333 0.333
ÈÊ Y ˆ Ê Ys ˆ ˘
LŸ = 116 Í - ˙,
ÎË Y0 ¯ Ë Y0 ¯ ˚
DLŸ
DL = , (6-40)
SL
SL = 1 + 0.010LŸ .
CIEDE2000
In 2001 Luo, Cui, and Rigg, based on extensive analysis of several sets of per-
ceptual color difference data, proposed a modified BFD formula they initially
called M2b. It is based on CIELAB with the following analytically arrived
modifications:
246 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR SPACE AND COLOR DIFFERENCE FORMULAS
L¢ = L*,
a¢ = (1 + G)a*,
b¢ = b*,
2 0.5
[
¢ = (a¢) + (b¢)
Cab
2
] ,
Ê b¢ ˆ
¢ = tan -1
hab ,
Ë a¢ ¯
0.5
Ï È 7 ¸
Ô Í
G = 0.5Ì1 -
(Cab* ) ˘
˙ Ô
(6-41)
7 ˝,
Ô ÍÎ (Cab* ) + (25) ˙˚
7
Ô
Ó ˛
where C *ab is the arithmetic mean of the C*ab values for a pair of samples.
DL ¢ = Lb¢ - L¢s ,
¢ = C ab
DC ab ¢ ,b - C ab
¢ ,s ,
È 0.5 ¢ ˆ˘
Ê Dhab
¢ = Í2 C ab
DH ab (
¢ ,bC ab
¢ ,s ) sinÁ
Ë 2 ¯˚
˜ ˙,
Î
¢ = hab
where Dhab ¢ ,b - hab
¢ ,s .
The subscripts b and s refer to the comparison sample and the standard
sample, respectively.
2 2 0.5
ÈÊ DL¢ ˆ 2 Ê DCab ¢ ˆ
¢ ˆ Ê DH ab Ê DCab ¢ ˆ˘
¢ ˆ Ê DH ab
DE = Í +Á ˜ +Á ˜ + RT Á ˜ Á ˜˙ , (6-42)
ÍÎË kL SL ¯ Ë kC SC ¯ Ë kH SH ¯ Ë kC SC ¯ Ë kH SH ¯ ˙˚
where
2
0.015(L ¢ - 50)
SL = 1 + ,
2 0.5
[ 20 + (L ¢ - 50) ]
SC = 1 + 0.045Cab¢ ,
SH = 1 + 0.015Cab¢ T ,
with T = 1 - 0.17 cos(h a¢b - 30°) + 0.24 cos(2h¢ab) + 0.32 cos(3h a¢b + 6°) - 0.20
cos(4h a¢b - 63°). RT = -sin(2DQ)RC and
NEW FORMULAS 247
2
Ô
DQ = 30 expÌ- Í
( )
Ï È hab¢ - 275 ∞ ˘ ¸
˙ ˝,
Ô
ÔÓ ÍÎ 25 ˙˚ Ô˛
7 0.5
Ê Cab¢ ˆ
RC = 2Á 7 ˜ .
Ë Cab¢ + 25 7 ¯
L¢, C ab
¢ , and h ab
¢ are the arithmetic means for a pair of samples of the respec-
tive individual values.
This formulation corrects for the slanted ellipses near the negative b*
axis, contracts the a* axis near the neutral point so that ellipses located in
that area can be treated as such, introduces a new lightness weighting func-
tion, and uses the hue difference weighting function T proposed by Berns
at the Warsaw CIE meeting in 1999 (Berns, 2000). The new lightness dif-
ference weight is adjusted, without explicitly stating so, for a surround with
L* = 50. For the combined set of all data the new formula has an error of
prediction of 33% compared to 38% for CIE 94 and CMC. The formula per-
forms about equally well as CIE 94 for the RIT-DuPont data set and inferior
to BFD for the Luo-Rigg (now BFD) set. It is marginally better with the Witt
set and distinctly better with the Leeds and a new set of data, BIT, involving
CRT display colors. The formula was adopted as CIEDE2000. Unit difference
ellipses in the a*, b* diagram generated by this formula are illustrated in
Fig. 6-14.
Fig. 6-14 Unit ellipses in the CIELAB a*, b* diagram as calculated from the CIEDE2000
formula. From Luo, Cui, and Rigg (2001).
Since the 1950s several comprehensive models of color vision have been pro-
posed, some of which have had an impact on mathematical formulations of
color space and color differences. A detailed discussion of this subject is
outside the scope of this text and only a brief overview will be presented.
Müller-Judd
G. E. Müller (1850–1934) was a German psychophysicist much interested in
color vision. He adopted Hering’s theory of three reversible photochemical
substances and attempted to bring an opponent color model in line with
psychological facts. Müller developed the concept of cortical gray as repre-
senting the neutral state of color vision. In a series of articles published in
1896–97 titled Zur Psychophysik der Gesichtsempfindungen (On the psy-
chophysics of the visual sense) he described a three-stage process of color
vision and later expanded on the subject in a book-length treatise Über die
COLOR SPACE FORMULAS AND COMPREHENSIVE MODELS OF COLOR VISION 249
P3 = 5.0000Z . (6-43)
In the second stage the cone signals are converted to intermediate opponent
color signals as follows:
a1 = P1 - P2 ,
In the third stage these signals are converted to the final chromatic opponent
color signals as follows:
b1 = a1 - 0.6265a 2 ,
b 2 = a 2 + 0.1622a1 . (6-45)
The achromatic final signal is equal to the CIE tristimulus value Y. The chro-
matic signal calculation can be reduced to expressions of tristimulus values
with the following result:
b1 = 6.325(X - Y ),
b 2 = 2.004(Y - Z ),
b3 = Y . (6-46)
At the third stage this model bears similarity to the Adams zone theory model
and the Jameson-Hurvich model to be presented below. Judd used his imple-
mentation of the Müller theory to predict results of color vision impairment
and good agreement with wavelength discrimination data of protanopes, tri-
tanopes and normal dichromats was obtained (Judd and Yonemura, 1970). The
efforts by Friele to use the Müller framework to develop color difference for-
mulas were discussed above. The Müller framework has also been used by
S. L. Guth (see section below).
250 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR SPACE AND COLOR DIFFERENCE FORMULAS
Adams
As mentioned earlier, Adams offered a zone theory of color vision in 1923 and
a chromatic value diagram based on it in 1942. He equated cone sensitivity
with tristimulus values and applied the Munsell value function, representing
the modulation of the output of the “cones,” to all three tristimulus values. The
chromatic signals were calculated as follows:
a = 1.9(VX - VY ),
b = 0.72(VZ - VY ), (6-47)
where VX, VY, and VZ are square root Munsell value type functions of the CIE
tristimulus values X, Y, and Z that Adams equated with cone responses. The
constants were arrived at by comparing the ranges of chromatic value result-
ing from the color-matching functions and by bringing them into agreement
with the Munsell value scale. The ratio is 2.64, larger than the theoretical value
for a balanced linear system and larger than the value of 2.5 of the CIELAB
space. As seen earlier, this model was used in modified form as a basis for the
Adams-Nickerson color space and difference formula and the various formu-
las derived from it.
where x, y, z, are the CIE 1931 color-matching functions. These functions can
be expressed in relative terms as hue coefficients (Fig. 6-16), and the latter
COLOR SPACE FORMULAS AND COMPREHENSIVE MODELS OF COLOR VISION 251
Fig. 6-15 Spectral opponent color (and lightness) functions determined from hue cancellation
experiments, of a single observer. From Hurvich (1981).
were found to be in good agreement with the results of spectral hue naming
experiments. Except for the weights, they are identical to the final Müller-Judd
functions and in basic agreement with the Adams functions. Jameson also
expressed the chromatic functions in terms of König type cone fundamentals
(1972).The Hurvich-Jameson functions provide good support, derived through
the paradigm of hue cancellation, for the basic ideas of Hering, Müller-Judd,
and Adams.
Hurvich and Jameson proposed a polar chromatic diagram they termed a
psychological diagram. It is derived by multiplying a spectral power distribu-
tion with their two chromatic functions a and b, based on their own experi-
mental data. The two functions form the axes of a polar diagram (see Fig. 6-17)
where saturation is calculated as the euclidean sum of the two chromatic
responses. Conceptually unique hues fall on the axes of this diagram. However,
their chromatic functions resemble the functions of equation (6-48), and
average experimental unique red and green have significant positive b values.
The diagram is essentially identical to a linear opponent diagram in polar coor-
dinate form based on CIE color-matching functions. For a description of the
work of Jameson and Hurvich related to the Hering system, see Hurvich
(1981).
252 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR SPACE AND COLOR DIFFERENCE FORMULAS
Fig. 6-16 Spectral hue coefficient functions for an average observer (Hurvich, 1981). Compare
with Figs. 4-8 and 4-9.
Fig. 6-17 The polar hue/saturation diagram of Hurvich and Jameson (Hurvich, 1981). Spec-
tral colors fall on the heavy line in the interior.
COLOR SPACE FORMULAS AND COMPREHENSIVE MODELS OF COLOR VISION 253
0.70
L = (0.16 X + 0.56Y - 0.034Z ) ,
0.70
M = (-0.40 X + 1.16Y + 0.084Z ) ,
0.70
Z = (0.017Y + 0.27Z ) . (6-49)
Output from the three cone types is subjected to gain control by multiplying
them with their attenuation factors:
s
,
s + L + k(La - L)
s
,
s + M + k ( Ma - M )
s
, (6-50)
s + S + k(Sa - S)
where s = 200 and k = 5.5, and subscript a denotes the surround (adapting
field). The equations apply only if the cone response of the surround is larger
than that of the test field; otherwise, the difference between the test and sur-
round cone responses in the calculation of the attenuation factors is taken
as zero. The resulting cone responses after gain control (subscript g) are used
to calculate uncompressed responses for stage 1 and stage 2 mechanisms as
follows:
where subscripts 1 and 2 refer to the mechanism stages and subscript i to the
initial uncompressed response. The uncompressed stage 2 opponent color
responses are illustrated in Fig. 6-18 for comparison to the Jameson-Hurvich
functions and other functions below.
In the next step the final compression for the second-stage ATD values are
calculated as follows (the compressed values for the stage 1 ATD values are
calculated in the same manner):
A2 = A2 i (200 + A2 i ),
T2 = T2 i (200 + T2 i ),
D2 = D2 i (200 + D2 i ). (6-52)
At stage 1 the brightness signal is equal to the vector sum of the compressed
achromatic and two chromatic components. Lightness is calculated as the
brightness compared to the brightness of the reference white (scale 100).
Colorfulness, chroma, and saturation are all calculated according to
0.50
Co = C = Sa = (T22 + D22 ) A2 .
are calculated as the square root of the sum of the squares of the three com-
ponent vector differences at the compressed stage 1 level, while large color
differences are calculated in the same manner at the compressed stage 2 level.
Many kinds of data have been predicted, with varying success, with differ-
ent versions of the model. It has not found industrial use as a uniform color
space formula. As a color appearance model the ATD95 version has received
faint praise by Fairchild (1998).
g = C (-13.7R1 3 + 17.7G1 3 - 4 B1 3 ),
È Y01 3 - 2 ˘
L = 5.9 Í 13 ˙
,
ÍÎ 3 + 0.042(Y0 - 30) ˙˚
where
13
1 + 0.042(Y0 - 30)
C= ,
Y01 3 - 2 3
Fig. 6-19 Colors of the 1929 Munsell Book of Color as viewed under CIE illuminant C in
Cohen’s RLV fundamental color space (Burns et al., 1990). The spectral trace is also shown
as are the vectors of the equal energy illuminant and illuminant C.
Fig. 6-20 Spectral functions representing the red, violet and luminosity axes of the RLV version
of Cohen’s fundamental color space (Burns et al., 1990).
258 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR SPACE AND COLOR DIFFERENCE FORMULAS
Fig. 6-21 Schematic view of the DKL color space based on cone sensitivity data. A color (black
dot) is represented by its azimuth, F, and its elevation, Q (Derrington et al., 1984.)
location of the chromatic axes in the CIE chromaticity diagram (Fig. 6-22).
This space has direct neurophysiological support from measured cell activity
in the LGN. The axes are neither in agreement with average unique hues nor
with those of the CIE-based opponent color chromatic diagram.
Color vision models that imply a color space have also been developed in con-
nection with color appearance models. An inclusion of two of these (Hunt and
Nayatani) in the Mahy et al. (1994) evaluation of uniform color spaces indi-
cates that they represent the Munsell value 5 plane and the OSA-UCS systems
with significant deviations. Such systems, as mentioned earlier, are considered
outside the scope of this text.
Fig. 6-22 Axes of constant R&G and constant B of the DKL space in the CIE chromaticity
diagram. The squares represent the chromaticities of the phosphors used in the experiment.
L0 = L - (L + M + S),
M0 = M - (L + M + S),
S0 = S - (L + M + S). (6-55)
These cells also have mirror image copies. The cells are considered to carry
both luminance and color information at different spatial frequencies.
In the third stage, in the parvocellular pathway, the authors’ proposal posits
combinations of signals in a way that separates luminance and color informa-
tion. Accordingly, for example, L0 - M0 sums color and cancels luminance,
while L0 + M0 sums luminance and cancels color. The response functions of the
third stage are illustrated in Fig. 6-24. They resemble somewhat the Hurvich-
Jameson opponent color functions but are not balanced. They are calculated,
in the indiscriminate version, according to
260 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR SPACE AND COLOR DIFFERENCE FORMULAS
Fig. 6-23 Three cone opponency signals of the second stage of the De Valois and De Valois
color model (indiscriminate surround). From De Valois and De Valois (1996).
Fig. 6-24 Third-stage chromatic functions of the De Valois and De Valois color model
(indiscriminate surround). From De Valois and De Valois (1993).
For the fourth stage, the De Valois proposal has complex color selective cells
resulting from the summing of the responses of simpler cells earlier in the
COLOR SPACE FORMULAS AND COMPREHENSIVE MODELS OF COLOR VISION 261
pathway. The authors believe that simple opponency such as posited by Hering
and Hurvich and Jameson ends at the LGN level and that already in area V1
of the brain’s visual system there are cells that fire to stimulation from spe-
cific spectral regions and not to those of others. “The chromatic opponency at
this stage is between, not within, individual cells.” At this stage two cell types
each, such as “red” and “blue” or “red” and “yellow,” can fire at the same time
to the same stimulus, resulting in mixed hues. In the form presented here, the
De Valois model does not result in a close representation of the Munsell
system.
The De Valois model is of particular interest because it considers a wealth
of neurophysiological data accumulated over the last fifty years. It represents
informed assumptions in regard to how the visual system operates (up until
area V4) and is in this respect different from Guth’s early models that
optimized the Müller structure to psychophysical data, as the authors point
out in a commentary in 1996.
In 1997 De Valois et al. reported on a hue-naming experiment whose results
they explained in terms of a modified version of their model where in the third
stage they subtracted modified amounts of second stage S0 from L0 and M0
(roughly comparable to the change from the a, b diagram to the a, b diagram;
see Section 5.7) They also found the red and the green systems to likely receive
non-symmetrical inputs from cones or LGN cells. The De Valois proposal is a
recent example of a color vision model that assumes a relatively simple rela-
tionship between proposed outputs of cells in the visual area of the cortex and
color perceptions.
Fig. 6-26 Schematic view of the color vision model of Chichilniski and Wandell to account
for their hue classification results. Output from cones L, M, S is split into increment/decrement
portions that are separately modulated to form the intermediate cone signals L*, M*, S*. These
are linearly combined to form incremental and decremental pre-opponent signals. They, in turn,
are again linearly combined to form the opponent signals RG, BY, and WB. From Chichilnisky
and Wandell (1999).
schematic view of the model is found in Fig. 6-26. Linear cone signals L, M, S
are are treated separately as increments or decrements with respect to a neutral
point. These are scaled through gain control depending only on the surround.
The resulting intermediate cone signals L*, M*, and S* are combined linearly
and then separated into pairs of increment and decrement pre-opponent
IS THE OPPONENT COLOR SYSTEM “SOFT-WIRED”? 263
signals. These are combined linearly to create the final opponent color signals,
whose signs determine the opponent color invoked. Concerning the neural
location of opponent color null surfaces the authors comment: “The non-
planarity of opponent color classification boundaries raise the possibility that
either individual parvocellular LGN neurons are not the neural substrate of
perceptual opponency, or that significant nonlinearities in neural responses
were overlooked.” Increments and decrements have also been found to be
treated differently in spatial patterns (Bäuml, 2002).
The relevance of these findings for a model of a global object color space
viewed under conditions much different from those of the described experi-
ment remains to be determined. There is the likelihood that all models using
simple subtractions of cone absorptions or tristimulus values are much sim-
plified approximations of the real mechanisms of color vision. The issue of the
strategy pursued by evolution in the development of the primate visual system
assumes key importance as well as if perception ultimately can be modeled
from implicit cone responses.
Over the last hundred years color discrimination data have been established
at four different levels of difference:
PERFORMANCE COMPARISON OF VARIOUS FORMULAS
265
Fig. 6-27a–d Sequential plot of spectral reflectance functions of every second Munsell chip around the hue circle at value 6 and
chroma 8, indicating the comparatively regular changes in reflectance. Note the common crossovers between 550 and 600 nm.
266 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR SPACE AND COLOR DIFFERENCE FORMULAS
Fig. 6-28 First three eigenvectors representative of the first three principal components, fitted
to a database of reflectance functions of 1269 Munsell chips. Solid line: eigenvector 1; dashed
line: eigenvector 2; dotted line: eigenvector 3. From Lenz et al. (1996).
Ideally all these data sets would be describable with high accuracy using a
single model/formula. It is obvious from past discussions that this is impos-
sible. When comparing various models of color spacing with typical data sets,
significant variation in results is obtained. In a few cases formulas have been
optimized against a particular data set, and not surprisingly, these formulas
tend to perform better than others against these data. Typical examples are
the FMC formulas optimized against the MacAdam data, the OSA-UCS
formula optimized against the corresponding large difference data, and the
BFD formula optimized mainly against the Luo and Rigg modified data set.
PERFORMANCE COMPARISON OF VARIOUS FORMULAS 267
(a)
(b)
Fig. 6-29a and b Plots of the twenty Munsell colors of Fig. 6-27 in the space formed by the
three eigenvectors of Fig. 6-28 and in the diagram formed by the second and third eigenvec-
tors. Hue designations of four colors are shown for identification. It is apparent that the space
orders the Munsell colors regularly in an ordinal sense, but it is far from uniform.
268 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR SPACE AND COLOR DIFFERENCE FORMULAS
TABLE 6-1 Prediction accuracy of three color difference formulas for various sets of
color difference data
Data set Coefficient of Variation, %
CIELAB CMC BFD
MacAdam, 24 ellipses 34.5 32.2 27.7
Wyszecki-Fielder/GW, 28 ellipses 26.9 20.2 18.8
Witt threshold, 5 ellipses 39.3 39.1 42.6
Luo-Rigg, 131 ellipses 25.1 16.2 13.6
Cheung-Rigg, 5 ellipses 31.6 39.1 42.6
Munsell value, 232 differences 7.3 29.0 21.5
Munsell hue, 365 differences 64.6 50.7 38.4
Munsell chroma, 356 differences 20.5 35.5 41.8
Source: Adapted from Melgosa et al. (1992).
In 1992 Melgosa and co-workers compared a number of data sets using the
CIELAB, CIELUV, CMC, and BFD formulas. From their extensive data
only a few examples are shown in Table 4-1. Results, in form of coefficients of
variation for the difference between visual and calculated differences, are
bewildering. Some conclusions can be drawn from the table:
1. There are systematic differences between the MacAdam data, on the one
hand, and the Wyszecki-Fielder/observer GW data, on the other.
2. Witt threshold data are poorly predicted by any of the three formulas.
3. CMC and BFD are significant improvements over CIELAB for the
Luo-Rigg data but not for the Cheung-Rigg data.
4. CIELAB is a good predictor of Munsell value but a poor predictor of
Munsell hue differences, while CMC and BFD are poorer in predicting
value differences but better (if still poor) in predicting hue differences.
CIELAB is a better predictor of Munsell chroma differences than the
other two formulas (in agreement with findings in Chapter 8 of absence
of chromatic crispening in color differences of the size of Munsell system
differences).
Fig. 6-30 Munsell Renotation colors at value 5 plotted in the chromatic diagram of Richter’s
LABHNU formula. From Mahy et al. (1994).
Fig. 6-31 Munsell Renotation colors at value 5 plotted in the chromatic diagram of Hunt’s 1991
color appearance model. From Mahy et al. (1994).
270 HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF COLOR SPACE AND COLOR DIFFERENCE FORMULAS
variation for color differences calculated by various formulas for the set are
listed.
The variation in calculated differences for the data set selected is now
approximately half of that for the 1976 CIE formulas. The largest improve-
ment was achieved, as indicated earlier, by applying variable weights to the
implicit lightness, chroma, and hue differences to deal with chromatic crispen-
ing and the non-euclidean nature of uniform color space (in the absence of
euclidization procedure).
The Munsell system was selected for the purpose of comparison of the
performance of color space formulas. Constant value, respectively metric light-
ness, planes are illustrated. To be in perfect agreement with the perceptual
results, a color space formula should represent an equal value plane of the
Munsell system as a series of concentric circles with equal radial spacing and
radial lines that are equidistant from each other in terms of hue angle. In the
evaluation of Mahy et al. (1994) the formulas (then known) most accurate in
modeling the Munsell system were the LABHNU formula of Richter (a some-
what different version from the one documented above, but found in the same
reference), SVF, and the CIELAB formula (for OSA-UCS it is the OSA
formula and SVF). A significant further improvement will be shown in
Chapter 7 (compare Fig. 7-6). Formulas such as CIE94 cannot be used as color
space formulas because of the variable weights on the difference components,
unless the weights are integrated such as in the Rohner and Rich, Völz,
Thomsen, or DIN99 proposals (see above). It is evident, however, that they
would perform poorer than the formulas above against the Munsell and OSA-
UCS systems because most of the integrated weight of SC relates to chromatic
crispening, absent in the Munsell system and OSA-UCS.
We have seen a rich tapestry of efforts to develop psychophysical models
of color space and color differences applicable to differences of various size.
But as discussed in Chapter 4, we do not have extensive reliable, replicated
data of which we can be confident that they accurately describe color spacing
for a given situation of illumination, surround, size of differences, and the truly
average observer. Various data sets developed at different times vary consid-
erably for generally unknown reasons and are described optimally by differ-
ent formulas. It is important to develop data sets that can be reproduced in
different locations by observer groups of comparable and known relation to
a truly average standard observer (that may still need to be developed). If this
proves to be impossible, it may be due to irreproducible cognitive input into
the judgments. Truly reliable data sets and further knowledge of the human
color vision system may make it possible to develop a psychophysical basis
model, perhaps non-euclidean and certainly nonlinear, that can, with various
parameters, accurately describe the tiling of color space uniform under closely
defined observation conditions. There is a considerable way to get there and
it is evident that CIEDE2000 is a mere milestone on that path.
Chapter 7
Major Color Order
Systems and Their
Psychophysical Structure
In this chapter only the Munsell, OSA-UCS, and Swedish NCS systems will
be discussed. The former two are the most important attempts to create psy-
chologically uniform systems, the latter uses the presumed natural approach
of Hering (see Chapter 2) to create a color order system, having a regular
structure, but not one uniform in terms of size of perceived differences. There
are several other newer color order systems extant, but they neither make
claims for uniformity nor for regularity according to new, significant psycho-
logical attributes.
The issue of viewing conditions for these systems has been attended to in
different ways.As discussed previously, the Munsell system is illustrated as if the
chips at each value level would be viewed against an achromatic surround of the
same value. Chips of two adjacent value levels are illustrated as if viewed against
an achromatic surround of intermediate value. The actual atlas displays the
chips on white paper (historically of various degrees of whiteness), thus result-
ing in distortions of the value scale, particularly at lower values.The OSA-UCS
system is defined for an achromatic surround of luminous reflectance Y = 30
(L = 0).The atlas samples are in transparent jackets. NCS, finally, has been estab-
lished against an immediate achromatic surround of Y = 78 in a light booth
painted with an achromatic gray of Y = 54. The atlas displays samples on white
paper. Both latter systems only result in the intended color experiences when
viewed against the appropriate surrounds. Munsell and NCS are defined for
Color Space and Its Divisions: Color Order from Antiquity to the Present, by Rolf G. Kuehni
ISBN 0-471-32670-4 Copyright © 2003 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
271
272 MAJOR COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS AND THEIR PSYCHOPHYSICAL STRUCTURE
CIE daylight C and the 2° standard observer. OSA-UCS is defined for daylight
D65 and the 10° standard observer.
Albert H. Munsell was educated as an artist and art instructor. His initial inter-
est in color order (beginning in the 1880s) resulted from the need for an edu-
cational tool for instruction in color order for school children and art students
as well as a tool for objectively expressing harmonious color relations.1 His
initial concept employed a sphere.The inspiration for the sphere form (Munsell
became aware of Runge’s book only in 1899) came from a child’s ball with
colored segments and from plotting the colors of one of his paintings in form
of a double spiral, suggesting a sphere (Munsell, 1918). Munsell, having con-
vinced himself that Hering’s approach could not be correct, decided to base
his hue circle on five primary colors: red, yellow, green, blue, and purple. The
main motivation was to be able to express his system in a decimal framework.
Munsell, not burdened with detailed knowledge about earlier attempts
at systematic color order, unhesitatingly decided to make lightness (“value” in
his artistically influenced term) a key attribute. His early sphere models, for
which he had received a patent in 1900, placed hues of equal value on the
equator, that is, in addition to middle green and red, darkened yellow, and light-
ened blue and purple. Munsell invented the term “chroma” to designate the
radial dimension from the neutral gray center to the equatorial colors. After
plotting “intensity” against “luminosity” values of painted pigments, measured
by Abney, Munsell realized in the year 1900 that on the basis of attributes hue,
value, and chroma, a uniform color solid as represented by available pigments
could not fit into a sphere but would form an irregular “spheroid.” As a result
he abandoned the idea of his color solid fitting into an ideal geometrical solid.
Munsell named the irregularly shaped solid a “color tree.” He maintained the
color sphere as an educational tool until the end of his life, and it remained a
part of the descriptive literature of the system until the Second World War (Fig.
7-1). Munsell began preparing an equal value chart of painted paper chips in
1901 and proceeded with charts at other value levels. In 1902 he sketched a
model based on constant hue planes and began to assemble corresponding hue
charts (Fig. 7-2). During the same years Munsell decided that the system should
be visually uniform, that is, steps along the three attributes should, within an
attribute, be of equal perceptual size. Having differences of equal perceptual
magnitude within all three attributes was contemplated by Munsell but
somehow never implemented. In 1904, when preparing a publication describ-
ing the system and as part of a patent application, Munsell also had settled on
his hue, value, and chroma terminology and color identification scheme. Over
a span of five years Munsell had developed all key concepts of the system as
they stand today. In 1905 he published his conceptual description of the system
THE MUNSELL COLOR SYSTEM 273
Fig. 7-1 Munsell’s irregular “color tree” enveloping the original sphere. From Derefeldt (1991).
Fig. 7-2 Trace of Munsell’s sketch in his color Diary of an irregular “color tree” with constant
hue leafs, dated March 20, 1902.
274 MAJOR COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS AND THEIR PSYCHOPHYSICAL STRUCTURE
under the title A color notation (Munsell, 1905). He was granted in 1906 a patent
for the system, and he received copyright protection for the charts. In 1907 the
first version of the Atlas of the Munsell color system was published, containing
eight charts with painted samples (Munsell, 1907). A second, enlarged version
of the Atlas appeared in 1915 (Munsell, 1915). It consists of 15 charts, five con-
stant hue and seven constant value charts, as well as three charts of general
descriptive nature.2
In the year of Munsell’s death, 1918, the Munsell Color Company was
formed and continued operating under his son. Scientific support was obtained
from the National Bureau of Standards, and the company soon moved from
Boston to Baltimore to be closer to the Bureau. One of the young researchers
at the Bureau was D. B. Judd. Research on uniform spacing of hue, value, and
chroma was continued, and in 1929 the company issued the first version of
the Munsell Book of Color. The Munsell Book of Color is internationally today
perhaps the most widely known color order/appearance system and is com-
mercially available in a matte and a glossy chip edition, with a supplementary
collection of near-neutral color chips. There is also a textile fabric edition.
The history of the Munsell system has been described by Nickerson (1940,
1969, 1976), Berns and Billmeyer (1985), and Kuehni (2002). A modern
description of the system is that by Long and Luke (2001).
Fig. 7-3 Location in the aŸ, bŸ chromatic diagram of the fifty colors of chart 50 (value 5) of the
1915 Atlas, 2° observer and equal energy illuminant. Radial lines connect colors of constant
hue, circular lines colors of constant chroma.
pared in Fig. 7-4 against measurements of the value samples of the 1929 Book
of Color (see below). The scales are somewhat uneven and show breaks in
continuity: the 1915 Atlas value scale between value steps 4 and 5, the Book
of Color scale between steps 5 and 6. They are indicative of the lightness
crispening effect. The 1915 Atlas measurements are optimally linearized with
a single function by applying a power of 0.43 to the luminous reflectance
values.
Fig. 7-4 Luminous reflectance Y of the value scales of the 1915 Munsell Atlas (solid line) and
the 1929 Book of Color (dotted line), 2° observer.
Fig. 7-5 Location in the aŸ, bŸ chromatic diagram of the colors of the value 6 plane of the 1929
Munsell Book of Color, 2° observer and equal energy illuminant.
THE MUNSELL COLOR SYSTEM 277
Fig. 7-6 Munsell Renotation aim colors for the 2° observer and equal energy illuminant. Every
second hue (5 and 10) at value 6 and chroma 0–12 in the aŸ, bŸ chromatic diagram. Compare
with Figs. 6-30 and 6-31.
Fig. 7-7 Munsell Re-renotation aim colors for the 2° observer and equal energy illuminant.
Every second hue (5 and 10) at value 6 and chroma 0–12 in the aŸ, bŸ chromatic diagram.
Compare with the Renotation colors of Fig. 7-6.
Fig. 7-8 Schematic view of the cylindrical organization of the Munsell system, illustrating four
constant hue planes and the organization of samples on these planes. The chromatic plane is
not representative of the perceptual relations in the Munsell system. From Judd and Wyszecki
(1975).
psychological space as illustrated in Fig. 7-9 for the colors of Fig. 7-6. Among
other things, it also has the same weakness in regard to hue difference as the
psychological conceptual system of Fig. 7-8.
Among the assumptions implicit in most psychophysical models are: (1)
colors are defined by two opponent color coordinates a and b and luminous
reflectance and (2) unit color difference contours can be expressed in terms
of distances in a and b. The CIELAB formula makes the additional assump-
tion that the power modulation of the tristimulus values used to calculate a*,
b*, and L* is uniform and its value is cube root. As discussed before, this
assumption was made by Adams in 1942 and is still in use. The b* axis result-
ing from application of the 2° observer is a reasonable representation of
unique yellow and blue, but as we have seen, the a* axis does not coincide
with unique green and unique red. Disregarding this difficulty, we can take the
THE MUNSELL COLOR SYSTEM 281
Fig. 7-9 Representation of the colors of Fig. 7-6 in the CIELAB a*, b* diagram, 2° observer
and equal energy illuminant.
Fig. 7-10a Progression of X tristimulus values of Munsell colors nearest to the a opponent
color axis as a function of Munsell chroma, value 6, 2° observer, and equal energy illuminant.
Fig. 7-10b Progression of Z tristimulus values of Munsell colors nearest to the b opponent
color axis as a function of Munsell chroma, value 6, 2° observer, and equal energy illuminant.
THE MUNSELL COLOR SYSTEM 283
calculating the chromatic coordinates of the colors of Figs. 7-3, 7-5, 7-6, and
7-7 are as follows:
0.15 0.85
ÈÊ X ˆ ÊYˆ Ê Y ˆ˘
aŸ + = 3500 Í * - ,
ÎË X 0 ¯ Ë Y0 ¯ Ë Y0 ¯ ˙˚
0.19 0.81
Ÿ
ÈÊ X ˆ ÊYˆ Ê Y ˆ˘
a - = 2000 Í * - ,
ÎË X 0 ¯ Ë Y0 ¯ Ë Y0 ¯ ˙˚
0.06 0.94
ÈÊ Y ˆ Ê Z ˆ ÊYˆ ˘
bŸ + = 1400 Í - * ˙,
ÎË Y0 ¯ Ë Z0 ¯ Ë Y0 ¯ ˚
0.42 0.58
ÈÊ Y ˆ Ê Z ˆ ÊYˆ ˘
bŸ - = 485Í - * ˙, (7-1)
ÎË Y0 ¯ Ë Z0 ¯ Ë Y0 ¯ ˚
where X, Y, Z are the CIE tristimulus values of the Munsell colors for the 2°
observer and illuminant C and X0, Y0, Z0 are the tristimulus values of illumi-
nant C. The resulting aŸ and bŸ values are on average of the same size as those
obtained with the CIELAB formula. Without a further, nonlinear scaling
factor the formula is only applicable to value 6.
Fig. 7-11a Plot of Munsell colors nearest to the a axis at six value levels (from the top 9, 8,
6, 3, 2, 1) in the X, Y diagram, 2° observer and equal energy illuminant. Colors of constant
chroma at different value levels are connected by lines.
consisting primarily of higher value colors and the left half of the lower curve
of lower-value colors. As a result of the different powers required for chroma
linearization, a cylinder is not obtained without additional adjustment. Empir-
ically the application of a factor of 0.133Y0.59 was found to provide the required
correction for the Renotations (Kuehni, 1999).
Fig. 7-11b Plot of Munsell colors nearest to the b axis at six value levels (from the top 9, 8,
6, 3, 2, 1) in the Z, Y diagram, 2° observer and equal energy illuminant. Colors of constant
chroma at different value levels are connected by lines.
for optimization (aŸ+: power 0.75, aŸ-: power 0.5, bŸ+: power 0.25, bŸ-: power
0.333).
Calculating backward to a linear a, b diagram (Fig. 7-14b), we find the axis
colors spaced differently and the intermediate colors connected with lines of
different curvature. Curvature of the lines of intermediate color is also obtained
if the colors along an axis are optimally linearized by a single power. If the same
power is required for both axes, the line at 45° is straight while the one at 135°
has a curvature. When calculating the corresponding chromaticity coordinates
(Fig.7-14c) curvature is also obtained. Geometry requires that in order to have
straight lines after linearization with the appropriate power non-axis lines need
to be curved both in the linear a, b and the x, y diagrams in a way that depends
on the powers involved.Thus in the x, y diagram most lines connecting constant
hue colors of the Renotations are curved.
Fig. 7-12 Weber fractions DZ/ Z as a function of tristimulus value Z of Munsell colors nearest
to the b axis at six value levels, 2° observer and equal energy illuminant.
and that colors falling on two neighboring constant hue lines vary in hue
differences between them, we can make estimates of the shape of unit chro-
matic differences in the Munsell system. At the level of differences of the
magnitude of Munsell chroma steps, at chroma 5 three Munsell 100 hue steps
equal two chroma steps. On this basis most unit contours in the psychological
diagram are of a rectangular or oval nature (or perhaps trapezoidal) with
the major axis of the contours aligned with constant hue lines. Based on
Nickerson’s relationship at chroma 5 we can calculate a ratio between major
and minor axis of approximately 2 : 1. Using the ratio of Bellamy and Newhall
at threshold the ratio is approximately 2.8 : 1. This indicates that unit contours
at Munsell sample size differences are somewhat less elongated than at thresh-
old level, that is, smaller hue differences require a relatively smaller stimulus
increment than larger hue differences. Judd showed in 1968 that, as a result
of the unit difference contour, a uniform perceptual color space cannot
be mapped isomorphically into a geometric space. A euclidean model of a
uniform space at the level of Munsell differences is impossible. Judd’s answer
to this question was a “crinkled fan” (Fig. 7-15). While a sector of this fan can
be spread out a complete circular crinkled fan cannot. As a result it is appar-
ent that the Munsell system is but a regular approximation of a uniform color
solid.
OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA UNIFORM COLOR SCALES (OSA-UCS) 287
Fig. 7-13 Portion of the CIE chromaticity diagram with radial lines of constant Munsell hue
and ovals of constant chroma, 2° observer and illuminant C. From Agoston (1987).
Publisher's Note:
Permission to reproduce this image
online was not granted by the
copyright holder. Readers are kindly
asked to refer to the printed version
of this chapter.
Fig. 7-14a Hypothetical colors falling on the axis lines and intermediate angles in an aŸ, bŸ
diagram that by semi axis required different powers for linearization.
Publisher's Note:
Permission to reproduce this image
online was not granted by the
copyright holder. Readers are kindly
asked to refer to the printed version
of this chapter.
Fig. 7-14b Back-calculated linear opponent color diagram of the same colors showing
curvature of lines connecting intermediate angle colors.
tetrahedral tiling of a uniform color space, Foss made already in the first formal
meeting of the committee in 1947 a proposal to tile the committee color space
in this manner. As L. Silberstein had pointed out in 1942, if one makes the size
of color differences in a triangle equal in magnitude, there can only be six
nearest neighbors in the chromatic plane for the system to be euclidean. With
more than six neighbors the space assumes hyperbolic shape, with less than
six Riemannian. Silberstein recommended testing hexagonal arrays of colors
OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA UNIFORM COLOR SCALES (OSA-UCS) 289
Publisher's Note:
Permission to reproduce this image
online was not granted by the
copyright holder. Readers are kindly
asked to refer to the printed version
of this chapter.
Fig. 7-14c Colors from Fig. 7-14a in the CIE chromaticity diagram showing a different curva-
ture of intermediate angle color lines.
Fig. 7-15 Segment of Judd’s “crinkled fan” uniform color difference surface which considers
the unit uniform difference contour of the Munsell system as a ratio between major and minor
axes of 2 : 1. The straight line across the open fan is a secant. It is also shown on the crinkled
version. When the crinkled fan is circular (representative of the total Munsell hue circle), it
cannot be spread out (Judd, 1968).
290 MAJOR COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS AND THEIR PSYCHOPHYSICAL STRUCTURE
Fig. 7-16 Image of the 43 hexagonal enamel color plates used by the OSA-UCS committee
to establish the fundamental perceptual data for the system. Note that there is no achromatic
color among them. (See color plate.)
between visual and calculated color differences was a disappointing 0.45 and
was achieved with the Munsell formula. A series of iterative steps (12 revi-
sions, 33 steps) was subsequently taken to modify that formula as well as the
Munsell Renotation aim data to improve the correlation between formula and
committee data to 0.80. The resulting revised aim data are the Re-renotations
of the NBS report discussed above.
Reilly developed modifications of the cube root formula to improve cor-
relation with the visual data, and MacAdam proceeded from the MacAdam-
Friele space to a nonlinear transformation space based on geodesics. Reilly
found that correlation was significantly improved by applying a superhue
weight of 2.3. Key results obtained from analysis of the visual data and com-
parison with colorimetric data were as follows:
It had become evident that it was impossible to present the results of the eval-
uation of the 43 samples of the value 6 plane on a flat surface. Thus the results
were in this respect in agreement with the Munsell system and findings of the
MacAdam color-matching error data. The committee decided to proceed
along two lines:
Optimized Formula
With the visual results in hand several independent efforts to arrive at an opti-
mized mathematical formula were made. In 1970 two formulas were in con-
tention as the best: a version of MacAdam’s nonlinear transformation formula
(xi, eta formula; see Chapter 6) and Reilly’s modified cube root formula.
MacAdam’s formula was found not to extrapolate well beyond the experi-
mental data. The committee was interested in a formula that extends reason-
ably to the limits of object color perception (the MacAdam limits). Reilly’s
formula was found to do a better job in this respect. Various parameters of
OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA UNIFORM COLOR SCALES (OSA-UCS) 293
Reilly’s formula were now optimized to provide the best correlation obtain-
able with such a formula (see equation 6-54). The committee selected a set of
color fundamentals R, G, B to fit the visual data that imply as yet unknown
neurophysiological processes to achieve the fit, illustrated in Fig. 7-17a. Figure
7-17b shows the committee’s opponent color functions j and g. In Fig. 7-17c
the linear form of these functions is compared to balanced linear opponent
functions a and b derived from the 10° observer functions. For ease of com-
parison the conventional functions have been multiplied by a factor of 8. The
results indicate systematic deviations, particularly in higher implied redness
and lower blueness of bluish-reddish colors (short wavelengths).
The lightness scale is a modified Semmelroth scale based on a surround of
Y = 30 and is illustrated in Fig. 7-18. Lightnesses of chromatic colors are further
adjusted by a correction for the Helmholtz-Kohlrausch effect in the form of a
modified Sanders-Wyszecki formula with calculation from CIE chromaticity
coordinates (it should be noted that experimental determination of the HKE
had been made only at one level of luminous reflectance).
The fit of the formula to the visual data involving 43 samples is shown
graphically in Fig. 7-19 (MacAdam, 1974). The gaps represent cases where the
calculated difference exceeds the average perceived differences, and the rec-
tangles the opposite. It is evident that while there is a general impression of
inhomogeneity, there is a degree of systematic deviation. Individual differ-
ences have errors of the calculated difference of up to 70%. For a mean
observed difference of 2.483 units for all 174 differences included by the com-
mittee the root mean square error (RMS) using the committee’s final formula
Fig. 7-17a Spectral R, G, B functions selected by the Committee on Uniform Color Scales for
the formula fitted to the experimental data, 10° observer and illuminant D65.
294 MAJOR COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS AND THEIR PSYCHOPHYSICAL STRUCTURE
Fig. 7-17b Spectral opponent color functions j and g as defined by the Committee, 10°
observer and illuminant D65.
Fig. 7-17c Comparison of linear spectral opponent functions of the OSA-UCS system with
balanced linear functions a and b derived from 10° observer data. For ease of comparison
the linear g function has been inverted and the a and b functions multiplied with the factor 8.
OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA UNIFORM COLOR SCALES (OSA-UCS) 295
Fig. 7-18 OSA-UCS lightness L as a function of luminous reflectance Y, with lightness crispen-
ing for surround L = 0.
OSA-UCS Atlas
With the formula established, the committee proceeded to sample the implied
space according to the rules of regular rhombohedral sampling. Rather than
use triangular sampling of the constant lightness plane, it was decided to rotate
the unit cubo octahedron to obtain a square grid in the constant lightness
296 MAJOR COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS AND THEIR PSYCHOPHYSICAL STRUCTURE
Fig. 7-19 Plot of 102 chromatic differences between 43 color samples in the chromatic
diagram of the Committee on Uniform Color Scales. Numbers identify the samples. Lines with
arrows and bars indicate the size of the average visual judgments. The colorimetric difference
is too small in case of arrows, too large in case of bars. From MacAdam (1974).
plane. The twelve distances from the central point M to any of the points on
the surface of the cubo octahedron are the same (Fig. 7-20; see also Figs. 2-49
and 2-50). By implication, the distances between the four points each (I, J, K,
L and A, B, C, D) are also the same. The basic cubo octahedron of the system
is illustrated in Fig. 7-20 and the lattice, doubly expanded in all three dimen-
sions, is illustrated in Fig. 7-21. Preceding and succeeding constant lightness
planes are offset by one j, g unit. An illustrated description of the space lattice
used was offered by Foss in 1978 (see Fig. 7-21).
A three-dimensional model of the system was manufactured by MacAdam.
He mounted spheres, dipped in the formulated paints, in cubo octahedral con-
figuration as shown in Fig. 2-51. This figure, as well as Fig. 7-20, illustrates the
existence of seven cleavage planes that allow bisection of the color solid in
various directions. The cleavage planes are formed by the following points in
the figure:
Fig. 7-20 Cubo octahedral arrangement of twelve colors equally distant from central color M.
Colors I, J, K, and L represent the square forming the square grid pattern of the OSA-UCS
system at constant lightness. Compare with Figs. 2-49 and 2-50.
Fig. 7-21 Basic cubo octahedral unit lattice doubly expanded in all three dimensions. From
Foss (1978).
298 MAJOR COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS AND THEIR PSYCHOPHYSICAL STRUCTURE
Plane 6 I, A, D, L
Plane 7 J, B, C, K
A Revised OSA-UCS
In 1990 MacAdam published a revision of OSA-UCS based on 2000 judgments
by two young color normal observers. A major advantage claimed by
MacAdam for the new observations is that for the first time samples of all
twelve lightness planes were evaluated by the same observers. As a result of
regression calculations he proposed redefined R and B values as well as rede-
fined j and g values:
cate a superhue weight of 2.2 (close to the one obtained by Reilly, mentioned
earlier). The resulting average total color difference was nearly identical to the
committee’s value for these data. For the reduced data set (104 differences)
the committee’s RMS error is 20.2%, with a result of 12.7% after the described
optimization. The correlation coefficient is improved from a value of 0.45 to
0.81. The results indicate that due to superimportance of hue the unit chro-
matic difference contours in the OSA-UCS basis data in the j, g diagram are
also elongated in a radial direction, in agreement with the Munsell and small
color difference data.
More exactly the ratio of the multiplication factors in quadrants 1–3 is 2.17,
comparable to the ratio of 2.0 for Munsell colors at chroma 5 (2.8 at the JND
level). In quadrant 4 it is 1.38. Colors falling on diagonals at 45 and 135° are
chroma differences only. Colors such as j4, g0 and j0, g4 are essentially hue
differences from j2, g2, a color on the diagonal. If hue superimportance has
been suppressed in the physical system, one would expect the essentially hue
differences to be perceptually larger than the chroma differences. Informal
evaluations confirm this. A formal experiment is required. The situation
implies that in the final system the visual distances along diagonals in a square
anchored at the neutral point are not identical as they should be in a uniform
system.
As a result we find that the apparent paradox of circular unit chromatic
contours in OSA-UCS compared to the elongated unit contours in the Munsell
system and in small color difference data is due to the suppression of hue
superimportance in OSA-UCS. In reality the visual data in all three cases are
in general agreement. The suppression has also resulted in unequal visual
distances along diagonal lines in the j, g diagram. As a result of these nonuni-
formities the major value of OSA-UCS should today perhaps be seen in its
various types of more or less uniform scales and their aesthetics.
Fig. 7-22a OSA-UCS colors falling on the g axis at six levels of lightness L in the X, Y diagram.
Achromatic colors fall on the diagonal, 10° observer and illuminant D65. Colors of constant
chroma at different value levels are connected by lines. Compare with Fig. 7-11a.
the range as one would expect from partitioning of the scale by a formula.
Here the first steps from gray were found to be in good agreement with the
average judgment of 35 observers (Kuehni, 2000b). As mentioned earlier, the
optimized linearizing powers are 0.84 for the green colors and 0.70 for the red
colors. They are 0.58 for the yellow colors and 0.33 for the blue colors. The
average increments in X and Z have the same ratio of 1 : 2.4 in OSA-UCS and
Munsell, but the average increment is 30% larger in OSA-UCS than in
Munsell. Figure 7-23 illustrates the Weber fractions for Z (comparable to Fig.
7-12) at six levels of lightness for colors on the j axis. They have a greater
regularity than those for the Munsell system for reasons mentioned.
The designation “natural color system” has been used by Hering (Das
natürliche Farbsystem; Hering, 1905). To quote Hering: “For a systematic
grouping of colors the only thing that matters is color itself. Neither the qual-
itative (frequency) nor quantitative (amplitude) physical properties of the
radiations are relevant.”6 Thus Hering developed a system on perceptual basis
only. He postulated two achromatic percepts white and black and their ratio
W/B representing proportions. From this follows the expression for whiteness
302 MAJOR COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS AND THEIR PSYCHOPHYSICAL STRUCTURE
Fig. 7-22b OSA-UCS colors falling on the j axis at six levels of lightness L in the Z, Y diagram.
Achromatic colors fall on the diagonal, 10° observer and illuminant D65. Colors of constant
chroma at different value levels are connected by lines. Compare with Fig. 7-11b.
Fig. 7-23 Weber fractions of Z for OSA-UCS colors falling on the j axis, at six levels of
lightness L, 10° observer and equal energy illuminant. Compare with Fig. 7-12.
nents: “It can be said that in each clearly veiled chromatic color both a
chromatic and a black-white component can be distinguished. . . .” Regarding
lightness/brightness of chromatic colors Hering said: “It is not such a simple
matter for chromatic colors, whose brightness or darkness is determined not
only by their black-white components, but also in part by their chromatic
components. . . . I have . . . ascribed an intrinsic brightness to yellow and red
and an intrinsic darkness to blue and green.” Hering’s equilateral constant hue
triangle therefore does not express anything about lightness or darkness of its
colors but only about the degree of veiling of its full color with white and/or
black.
As discussed in Chapter 2, Ostwald departed from Hering in attempting a
psychophysical definition while maintaining the geometrical form. Restricting
himself to colors that could be achieved with colorants, his full colors of neces-
sity fell short of Hering’s full colors. The form of his color solid is based on an
arrangement of Hering type constant hue triangles in a hue circle, resulting in
a double cone solid. But his hue circle is not based on unique hues but on
yellow, red, and blue primaries.
In the same chapter mention is made of the efforts of two Swedish
researchers, Johannson and Hesselgren, who tried to find a compromise
between Hering’s double cone and a color solid based in its third dimension
on a lightness scale. Johansson accepted hue, saturation, and value or lightness
as the three properties uniquely characterizing any color perception. He
304 MAJOR COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS AND THEIR PSYCHOPHYSICAL STRUCTURE
Fig. 7-24 Schematic views of NCS. Left: double cone with circle of full colors. W: white; S:
black; F: location of a given color in the double cone. Right: Constant hue triangle with full color
C. The location of color F is determined by its blackness s and chromaticness c. From the two
values whiteness w can be derived. From Hård et al. (1996).
Fig. 7-25 Lines connecting colors of constant luminous reflectance for NCS hue R75B. YC
denotes the intrinsic blackness of full color C. From Hård et al. (1996).
plotted into constant hue NCS triangles and found to approximately intersect
at a point outside the triangle (Fig. 7-25). The position of this intersection point
varies widely by hue. Dominant wavelength of constant hue colors, as one
would expect, varies for most colors as a function of chromaticness and
lightness.
In the CIE chromaticity diagram or the a*, b* diagram NCS constant hue
lines are not straight, and constant chromaticness lines do not form circles,
pronouncedly so at higher chromaticness (Fig. 7-26). One of the reasons is
Hering’s arbitrary decision to value chromaticness of all full colors identically
at 100. In terms of chroma they vary significantly. Meaningful hue angle dif-
ferences between hue steps, such as presented in Fig. 5-25 for the Newhall data
set, cannot be calculated because of the very considerable variation in light-
ness of the full colors. The work of Billmeyer and Bencuya (1987) indicate the
considerable difference in apparent hue spacing of the Munsell Renotations
306 MAJOR COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS AND THEIR PSYCHOPHYSICAL STRUCTURE
Fig. 7-26 NCS colors of constant hue (radial lines) and constant chromaticness (ovoids) in
the CIELAB a*, b* diagram. Capital letters indicate the four NCS unique hues. From Derefeldt
and Hedin (1987).
Fig. 7-27a NCS colors falling closest to the a opponent axis in the X, Y diagram. Achromatic
colors fall on the diagonal. Lines converging on the diagram origin connect colors of constant
NCS chromaticness, curved lines connect colors of constant blackness, 2° observer and equal
energy illuminant. Compare with Figs.7-11a and 7-22a.
software that converts, by iterative methods CIE colorimetric values, into NCS
values, and vice versa.
Fig. 7-27b NCS colors falling closest to the b opponent axis in the Z, Y diagram. Achromatic
colors fall on the diagonal. Lines converging on the diagram origin connect colors of constant
NCS chromaticness, curved lines connect colors of constant blackness, 2° observer and equal
energy illuminant. Compare with Figs. 7-11b and 7-22b.
The NCS atlas is a material example of the NCS system instantiated for a
particular surround and lighting condition: achromatic surround of Y = 56 and
CIE illuminant C, with the samples of a size to be, at normal viewing distance,
in agreement with the CIE 2° standard observer. Strictly speaking, for other
situations the atlas is less or not valid.
Color atlases, such as those described above are fragile in a metaphorical sense
as well as in reality. The variability of observer responses to color stimuli and
the fact that less than ideal colorants are used to color atlas chips make color
atlases fragile in a metaphorical sense. The three described atlases all have
been prepared with the help of colorimetric tools. In the case of the Munsell
system the Renotations are defined in terms of colorimetric data from which
they can be instantiated by matching tristimulus values. The aim values have
been arrived at by plotting, smoothing, and interpolating results of visual
evaluations. In case of the OSA-UCS atlas a formula has been fitted to a set
of visual data, and that formula has been used to tile the space according to
the principle of interlocking cubo octahedra. The resulting L, j, g values have
been converted back to tristimulus values that are the aim points for the gen-
eration of atlas chips. A similar process of defining colors has taken place in
generating the NCS chips. A quality issue relatively easily definable and con-
trollable is that of the agreement between aim color and atlas color in terms
of colorimetric coordinates.
For more recent atlas editions an aim has been to select pigments that not
only are color fast but that result in colorations reasonably color constant
when the atlas is viewed in different phases of daylight or artificial daylight
sources. None of the three systems makes any but the most general claims in
this respect. The available selection of suitable pigments is not such that a sig-
nificant degree of demonstrated color constancy under different light sources
is possible. Atlases have been formulated for CIE daylight C or D65. There
are no artificial light sources that closely match these phases in spectral power
distribution. Artificial daylight sources are today usually based on fluorescent
lamps, and while they match the correlated color temperature of a particular
daylight phase, their spectral power distributions are often significantly dif-
ferent. So-called triband lamps (CIE illuminant F11) are particularly differ-
ent. As a result the appearance of the chips can change significantly, and the
original goal of uniformity or regularity of spacing is no longer met or met
less well.
The issue of surrounds has been discussed earlier, but a color atlas is valid
in its appearance strictly only for the surround color for which it has been
designed. Distortions in scaling take place if the surround is different in light-
ness or chromaticity from the design surround. Distortions due to surround
can be relatively large because of crispening effects.
310 MAJOR COLOR ORDER SYSTEMS AND THEIR PSYCHOPHYSICAL STRUCTURE
Since a numerical standard observer has been used in the design of atlases,
individuals with normal color vision viewing an atlas experience smaller or
greater distortions when viewing it, based on the difference between their
color vision apparatus and that implied for the standard observer, and likely
due to additional reasons. An idea of the magnitude of the changes in experi-
ence can be gleaned from the range of colors selected as unique hues by a
group of observers (see Chapter 1). In addition observers have varying
perceptions of equality of lightness, chroma, and hue steps to a degree that is
unknown. Different assessments of constant chroma circles by observer
groups in past extensive visual experiments, and different assessments of the
number of constant size hue steps between unique hues, as discussed in
Chapters 4 and 5, give an indication of the magnitude of these problems.
Aside from these conceptual fragilities there are also material fragilities. Of
particular importance here is the resistance of pigments to degradations of
various kind. Pigments, binders, and other materials are selected to make
modern atlases long lasting. However, much depends on the kinds and lengths
of exposures atlases suffer in use.
In this chapter three major color order systems have been compared, two
attempts at a uniform color solid and the third a Hering type color solid. The
considerable differences between the former two and the latter are evident
from views of the distribution of samples in the X, Y and Z, Y planes at the
system axes. Hue superimportance is implied in the Munsell system (as a result
of the unequal magnitude of hue and chroma scale intervals). It is nonuniform
in its euclidean form and non-euclidean when uniform. The system considers
neither the Helmholtz-Kohlrausch effect nor the surround lightness.The OSA-
UCS system, not arranged according to hue and chroma attributes (in fact, not
directly expressive of meaningful chromatic attributes), does not in its final
form indicate hue superimportance. However, analysis of the basis data clearly
shows the presence of this effect. Hue superimportance was suppressed in
the final formula used to tile color space according to this system to make
the space euclidean. As a result the system is not uniform. The suppression
explains the apparent difference in unit difference chromatic contours
between the Munsell system and suprathreshold small color difference data,
on the one hand (of generally oval shape), and OSA-UCS, on the other (cir-
cular). The circular unit contour is not in agreement with the perceptual facts
of the OSA-UCS basis data. OSA-UCS, however, is built under consideration
of the Helmholtz-Kohlrausch effect and of a specific achromatic surround. The
NCS solid has been developed based on Hering’s principles. Because the dis-
tances in the solid do not represent constant perceptual distances, there is no
problem fitting the solid into a double cone. With minor changes it could be
fitted into a sphere.We have arrived at an understanding that the three systems
in effect represent regular color solids, none of them being able to make a
strong claim for perceptual uniformity.
Chapter 8
From Color-Matching
Error to Large
Color Differences
Color differences have been scaled from the level of color-matching error,
through threshold and industrially important suprathreshold small color dif-
ferences, large differences of the size in OSA-UCS, all the way to quadrant-
sized differences. In all cases considered here the difference judgments have
been made against a simple achromatic surround. In previous chapters we
have seen that there are systematic changes in terms of stimulus increments,
depending on the magnitude of the difference. In this chapter color differences
of varying sizes are compared in terms of stimulus increments to assess com-
monalities and divergences.
Color Space and Its Divisions: Color Order from Antiquity to the Present, by Rolf G. Kuehni
ISBN 0-471-32670-4 Copyright © 2003 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
311
312 FROM COLOR-MATCHING ERROR TO LARGE COLOR DIFFERENCES
Fig. 8-1a Increments in S as a function of S for the MacAdam ellipses (excluding ellipse 1).
Open circles represent points calculated by linear regression. They are connected by linear
regression lines.
314 FROM COLOR-MATCHING ERROR TO LARGE COLOR DIFFERENCES
Fig. 8-1b Increments in L as a function of L for the MacAdam ellipses (excluding ellipse 1).
Open circles represent points calculated by linear regression. They are connected by linear
regression lines.
Fig. 8-2a Increments in S as a function of S for a series of ellipses according to the CIE94
formula in the a*, b* diagram.
316 FROM COLOR-MATCHING ERROR TO LARGE COLOR DIFFERENCES
Fig. 8-2b Increments in L as a function of L for a series of ellipses according to the CIE94
formula in the a*, b* diagram.
The results show that the V functions implied in CIE 94 are more sharply
angled than those of all experimental data sets. This indicates that the SC func-
tion of that formula adjusts for more than the chromatic crispening effect. The
implication is that unit small color difference contours in the a*, b* diagram
adjusted for chromatic crispening are oval in form. The additional effect of SC
is to convert the elongated contours to circles of equal size, as will be seen
later.
Using ellipses in the a*, b* diagram fitted to the Luo and Rigg and the RIT-
DuPont (R-D) data (Melgosa et al., 1994, 1997), one can determine the
average change in chromatic ellipse size at an approximately constant lumi-
nous reflectance. The average axis length (between L* = 56 and 64, n = 30)
increases by a factor 4.4 from chroma 0 to chroma 100 as a result of the chro-
matic crispening effect.
CHROMATIC AND LIGHTNESS CRISPENING EFFECTS 317
Fig. 8-3a Regression lines of DS versus S of the selected Luo-Rigg ( Y = 25–35) and OSA-
UCS data normalized at the neutral point to those of the MacAdam data.
Fig. 8-3b Regression lines of DL versus L of the selected Luo-Rigg and OSA-UCS data
normalized at the neutral point to those of the MacAdam data.
In Fig. 8-3a and b the regression lines of the DL versus L and the DS versus
S functions of the L-R and the OSA-UCS data sets have been normalized at
the neutral point to those of the MacAdam data. Interestingly, for both cone
types the L increments are larger for colors both greenish and yellowish for
318 FROM COLOR-MATCHING ERROR TO LARGE COLOR DIFFERENCES
Fig. 8-4 DY/Y as a function of Y for the RIT-DuPont data. Open circles represent points cal-
culated by linear regression. They are connected by linear regression lines. Surround luminous
reflectance Y = 11.
the L-R data compared to the MacAdam data, and vice versa for the colors
both reddish and bluish.
A generally comparable situation applies to lightness crispening. We have
seen in Chapters 5 and 7 that lightness crispening is implicit in all experi-
mentally determined lightness scales. When plotting DL versus L of the lumi-
nous reflectance scale (or DY versus Y) we can expect to find a similar V
shaped form as in the case of chroma crispening, if the surround luminous
reflectance falls between the extremes of that of the test colors. Since in all
CME data the surround luminosity was lower than that of the least luminous
test field a V-shaped function is not expected in the data with variable lumi-
nosity, as indeed is not the case. In the R-D data, where the surround lumi-
nous reflectance was Y = 11, the effect is present but small, best illustrated by
plotting the Weber fraction of Y versus Y (Fig. 8-4). In terms of DL versus L
we can find the effect when plotting this function for OSA-UCS lightness that
has lightness crispening for Y = 30 built into the formula.The crispening effects
indicate that the Weber-Fechner law is not applicable to color differences
throughout the range of size where the crispening effects are applicable.
When plotting DL versus L and DS versus S for colors of the Munsell and the
OSA-UCS systems along the axes in the a*, b* diagram, we find no chroma
crispening effect, and there is a continuous increase with a positive slope.
CHROMATIC CRISPENING FADES AS A FUNCTION OF THE SIZE OF THE DIFFERENCE 319
The same applies for constant size chroma differences as calculated by the
CIELAB formula. When optimizing the divisor in SC for these color series, it
is found to be near 1 (Kuehni, 2001c). It is 1 when optimizing it to the hue and
chroma difference optimized OSA-UCS formula for the basis data of that set
(see Chapter 7). Guan and Luo (1999) optimized a modified CIE94 formula
to various sets of large color difference data, and found different optimal SC
and SH factors. They recommended for large differences a formula in which
the SC divisor is 1 + 0.016C* and the SH divisor is 1 (compared to SC divisor
1 + 0.045C* and SH divisor 1 + 0.015C* in CIE 94, GLAB, see chapter 6). An
explanation for these findings is that chromatic crispening fades as the size of
the chromatic difference increases. This is surprising and different from light-
ness where lightness crispening is present from color-matching error to large
differences.
In the absence of detailed experimental data one can estimate the relative
magnitude of the adjustment for the chromatic crispening effect in small color
difference perceptual data and the effect of converting ellipses to circles in the
SC divisor. If we assume the length of the major axis at the neutral point to be
1.5 (the average of 7 ellipses with C* < 2 is 1.55) and the increase in axis length
as a function of the chromatic crispening effect (see above) a factor 4.4, the
total value of the SC divisor should be 6.6 at metric chroma 100. It is found to
be only 5.5 in CIE94, a reasonable result given the variability in the visual
data.
It seems likely that this change in implied SC is located on a continuous
function, as estimated in Fig. 8-5. Systematic experiments are required to
clarify the shape of the function.
TABLE 8-2 Comparison of unit L and S increments for the first step from the neutral
color toward higher L and S values and their ratio for various data sets based on
linear regressions, Y = 30
Data set L Increment S Increment Ratio
CME data
MacAdam 0.0168 0.40 23.8
Brown-MacAdam 0.0232 0.60 25.8
Wyszecki-Fielder/GW 0.0191 0.72 37.9
Wyszecki-Fielder/AR 0.0300 0.78 26.0
Brown 0.0410 0.21 5.1
Wyszecki-Fielder/CDM 0.076 1.53 19.7
Threshold and small color difference data
Richter (extrapolated to Y = 30) 0.036 0.38 10.6
Luo-Rigg data 0.045 0.63 14.0
Large difference data
Munsell 0.373 6.00 16.1
OSA-UCS 0.529 8.22 15.5
Color difference formulas
CIE94/DE = 1.0 0.043 0.31 7.2
CIELAB/DE = 1.0 0.103 0.66 6.4
SIZE AND RATIO OF UNIT INCREMENTS 321
TABLE 8-3 Comparison of unit increment values of X and Z for the first step from
neutral for selected data, based on regression calculation at Y = 30
Data set X Increment Z Increment Ratio
CME data average (excluding Brown) 0.136 0.625 4.6
Luo and Rigg 0.279 0.629 2.3
Color difference matching data 0.471 1.50 3.2
Munsell data 2.35 6.03 2.5
OSA-UCS 3.23 8.22 2.5
CIELAB formula, DE = 1.0 0.639 0.658 1.0
CIE94 formula, DE = 1.0 0.265 0.310 1.2
7. Munsell and OSA-UCS have similar ratios, and the OSA-UCS steps are
approximately 1.4 times larger than two Munsell chroma steps, as we
have seen in Chapter 7.
The average L increment of the CME data (excluding the Brown data) is
0.022, the S increment 0.625, for a ratio of 28.4. For the L-R data selection, the
ratio is 14.8. It is evident that a significant change is happening between imper-
ceptible color-matching errors and small color differences: while S increments
are nearly the same it is L increments that become larger in small color dif-
ferences by approximately a factor of 2. The ratio remains essentially the same
when the differences are large. The color difference matching error has a ratio
smaller than that of color-matching error but larger than that of color differ-
ences. The Richter threshold data with a ratio of 10.6 are an exception to the
picture, due to a very low S increment.
When data are viewed, in the CIE tristimulus system a more familiar picture
emerges. Table 8-3 contains a selection of the data of Table 8-2 expressed in
terms of X and Z (the latter identical to S).
Small and large color difference data have comparable ratios of approxi-
mately 2.5. The CIELAB formula, by definition, has a ratio of 1, due to the
factor of 2.5 between the multipliers of a* and b* (500/200). CIE94, due to the
effect of SC, has a slightly larger ratio. Unsurprisingly, the key difference
remains the same: while in the CME and small color difference data the Z
increments are the same, the X increment increases by an approximate factor
of 2 in the small and large color difference data, compared to CME data. It
should be recalled that the multiplier balancing the linear opponent color
functions a and b is 2.3 to 2.4, depending on the standard observer. The impli-
cation is that color difference perception regards the balanced a and b systems
as equivalent.
On the other hand, color-matching errors appear based only on the sensi-
tivity limits of the cones. (Here the findings of Boynton and Kambe, 1980, of
thresholds along constant S and constant L/2M lines are not in agreement,
since they largely duplicated, if with larger Weber fractions, MacAdam’s
results.) Color-matching error data and color difference data are, it appears,
322 FROM COLOR-MATCHING ERROR TO LARGE COLOR DIFFERENCES
The difference between CME and color difference data is further clarified
when comparing the shape of their chromatic contours in L, M, S and X, Y,
Z spaces. For this purpose the intersection points of the major and minor axes
of the unit contours are translated into these spaces and views in certain direc-
tions are created. The exercise is limited to representative data: the MacAdam
ellipses and the Wyszecki-Fielder (observer GW) data as representative for
CME data and the Luo-Rigg data for small suprathreshold color differences.
Figures 8-6 to 8-8 illustrate the ellipses, each represented by five points (the
endpoints of the long and short ellipse axes in the a*, b* diagram and the
centerpoint), for the three cases in the L, S view. A number of observations
Fig. 8-6 MacAdam ellipses represented by the center point and the four points of the ellipse
intersections with the major and minor axes, in the L, S cone response diagram, equal energy
illuminant.
DIRECTION OF UNIT CHROMATIC CONTOURS IN THE L, M, S AND X, Y, Z SPACES 323
Fig. 8-7 Selected Luo-Rigg ellipses represented by the center point and the four points of the
ellipse intersections with the major and minor axes, in the L, S cone response diagram, equal
energy illuminant.
TABLE 8-4 Angles of major ellipse axis against abscissa in the L, S and X, Z
diagrams
Data set Number of Angles Mean Angle, Range, deg COV, %
deg
<90 >90
L, S diagram
MacAdam 16 9 89.8 86.2–96.0 0.1
Wyszecki-Fielder/GW 5 23 92.6 87.0–101.0 3.9
Wyszecki-Fielder/AR 8 20 91.0 87.9–99.9 2.1
Luo-Rigg (Y = 25–35) 5 26 93.1 86.1–99.4 3.5
X, Z diagram
MacAdam 22 3 77.4 57.4–115.0 16.8
Wyszecki-Fielder/AR 21 7 87.9 65.1–136.5 19.8
Luo-Rigg 9 22 96.8 56.9–130.6 19.2
can be made from the results. The MacAdam ellipses are well aligned with the
axes of the diagram; that is, they are well described by differences in L and S.
Table 8-4 contains data concerning the angles against the abscissa of the major
ellipse axis for some data sets in both the L, S and the X, Z diagrams.
The essentially vertical alignment of the MacAdam ellipses in the L, S
324 FROM COLOR-MATCHING ERROR TO LARGE COLOR DIFFERENCES
Fig. 8-8 Wyszecki-Fielder (observer GW) ellipses represented by the center point and the four
points of the ellipse intersections with the major and minor axes, in the L, S cone response
diagram, equal energy illuminant.
Fig. 8-9 MacAdam ellipses represented by the center point and the four points of the ellipse
intersections with the major and minor axes, in the X, Z diagram, equal energy illuminant.
the Munsell system the contours, as we have seen in Chapter 7, are uniformly
aligned along radial lines. In the X, Z diagram this is more or less the same.
The conclusions one can draw is that under the conditions of the MacAdam
experiment and/or as a result of the vision properties of that observer the
color-matching error is caused by limitations in cone sensitivity only. In the
conditions of the Wyszecki-Fielder experiment yellowish-reddish colors of
quadrant 4 point toward the neutral point, but the ratio between the two axes
remains average for CME data. An opponent color system appears active in
case of the L-R suprathreshold data. Here in addition the L, respectively X,
unit increment is increased compared to the S or Z increment in accordance
with global requirements for uniformity.
Pages of the Munsell or NCS atlases contain by design colors of constant hue
at different levels of lightness and chromaticness. One might assume that the
hue differences between corresponding colors on two adjacent pages are all of
identical size, since they all bear the same respective hue names (i.e., that hue
326 FROM COLOR-MATCHING ERROR TO LARGE COLOR DIFFERENCES
Fig. 8-10 Selected Luo-Rigg ellipses represented by the center point and the four points
of the ellipse intersections with the major and minor axes, in the X, Z diagram, equal energy
illuminant.
Fig. 8-11 Lines connecting two hypothetical series ( m and n) of colors of constant hue in a
perfect hue circle. Line o represents the colors of line n after application of the SH hue differ-
ence weight in the CIE94 color difference formula, normalized at metric chroma 25.
Fig. 8-12 Uniform chromatic Riemannian plane constructed from segments between lines m
and o in Fig. 8–11.
1. Constant hue angle difference between colors of two hues does not imply
constant perceived hue difference.
2. As a consequence a color space based on SH cannot be euclidean because
uniform slices created by SH do not add up to form a flat circular plane.
When forming a complete hue/chroma plane from segments between
lines m and o of Fig. 8-11, a Riemannian plane (Fig. 8-12) is obtained.
Fig. 8-13 Scatter diagram of angle of major ellipse axis (ellipse angle) versus hue angle for
the combined set of Luo and Rigg and RIT-DuPont ellipses with linear regression line.
was calculated as 0.95. As the scatter diagram of Fig. 8-13 indicates, significant
discrepancies appear to be random. When investigating the R-D data sepa-
rately (n = 16), the correlation coefficient is found to be 0.88. After deleting
two outliers that indicate longer axes in the hue direction than the chroma
direction, the correlation coefficient increases to 0.98. Angles of the 11 near-
neutral ellipses for the complete data set are found to range from 53 to 136.
While it is evident that CIELAB is not a good basis model for a uniform
chromatic diagram and there is the fact that transformation of ellipses fitted
in the CIE chromaticity diagram into ellipses in a*, b* involves a certain
amount of error (Melgosa et al., 1994), the surprisingly high correlation coef-
ficient appears to provide solid support for the conjecture of unit contours
being aligned with constant hue lines. The conjecture is found to apply also to
unit contours in the Munsell system and, as seen in Chapter 7, in OSA-UCS.
A tendency in this direction was also found in quadrant 4 of the Wyszecki-
Fielder color-matching error data. The alignment of fitted small color differ-
ence contours in the chromatic diagram has resulted in color difference
calculation being performed in a polar coordinate rather than a rectangular
system.
Additional data have in recent years been provided by J. Krauskopf and K.
R. Gegenfurtner (K-G, 1992) and by M. J. Sankeralli and K. T. Mullen (1999).
K-G determined visual thresholds around sixteen colors placed equidistant
330 FROM COLOR-MATCHING ERROR TO LARGE COLOR DIFFERENCES
Fig. 8-14 Visual thresholds around sixteen colors placed equidistant from the white point in
the center of the cone opponent diagram. From Krauskopf and Gegenfurtner (1992).
from the white point in a cone threshold diagram (see Fig. 8-14). In this
diagram the scales of the two axes have been adjusted so that a circular
contour is formed by the thresholds surrounding the white point. Most of the
resulting fitted threshold contours are ellipses more or less pointing toward
the achromatic point of the diagram. The fact that they do not closely do so
in all cases is an aspect of the controversy concerning the number of hue detec-
tion mechanisms in the human visual system (as briefly discussed in Chapter
5). Sankeralli and Mullen (1999) determined unit chromatic contours at the
45° and 135° angles of a normalized cone based diagram with the axes L - M
and S - (L + M)/2 in the observer’s personal isoluminant plane. The more or
less elliptical contours are aligned well with the corresponding hue angles.
Ellipse Shape, Size, and Relation to Hue Angle, Metric Chroma, and
Metric Lightness
In Chapter 7 it was shown that the ratio of contour axis length for the Munsell
colors at moderate chroma has been determined as approximately 2 : 1 (2.8 : 1
at JND level). On average, a ratio of about 2 : 1 applies to the OSA-UCS data
(see Section 7.2). Based on a regression of axis length versus metric chroma
of the combined Luo and Rigg and RIT-DuPont set, the average major and
minor axis lengths are found to be a = 1.7, b = 0.80 at C* = 0 and a = 4.1 and
b = 1.92 at C* = 100 (ratio of 2.14 : 1). For seven near-neutral colors the ratio
is found to be 1.55 : 1. The value of 2.14 : 1 is less than that found by Bellamy
and Newhall. The average ratio of the Krauskopf and Gegenfurtner contours
is found to be 1.7 : 1. The values for the Sankeralli and Mullen contours differ
UNIT DIFFERENCE CONTOURS AROUND THE HUE CIRCLE 331
TABLE 8-5 Correlation coefficients for the relationship between ellipse area and
metric lightness L*, Luo and Rigg and RIT-DuPont data
Metric Chroma C* Correlation Coefficient Number of Samples
10–20 0.06 21
20–30 -0.19 25
30–40 -0.64 27
31.00–34.99 -0.63 15
40–50 -0.10 21
60–70 0.19 11
I calculated these rusults from the L–R and R–D data.
by observer, with a ratio of 2.4 : 1 for one and 1.9 : 1 for the other. Undoubt-
edly, the experimental conditions also affect the ratio.
When determining the relationship between the length of the major axis
and metric chroma for the L-R and R-D data, a correlation coefficient of 0.77
is found (n = 150), indicating good correlation. An only slightly lower value is
found when comparing ellipse area against metric chroma (0.74). This level of
correlation is another expression of the activity of the chromatic crispening
effect. On the other hand, no correlation was found when comparing ellipse
area and hue angle. In the limited metric chroma range of C* = 30 - 40
(n = 27) the correlation coefficient is -0.01.
An unexpected result was obtained when comparing ellipse area against
metric lightness L* as a function of metric chroma. The results are found in
Table 8-5. They indicate a considerable negative correlation between lightness
and ellipse area for metric chroma values between approximately 25 and 45.
That is, in this chroma range the ellipses become smaller as lightness increases.
Based on the regression the average ellipse area in the metric chroma range
of 30 to 40 is 18.1 at metric lightness 20 and 8.7 at metric lightness 80, a ratio
of approximately 2 : 1. It is not evident what the cause of this finding is, and it
requires further investigation and formula fitting.
As discussed in Chapter 4, there is little doubt that lightness and hue
are fundamental color attributes. Chroma (contrast) is the necessary third
attribute to represent all possible object colors systematically. Regardless of
the mechanism we assume for the generation of hue (two process or multi-
process) it seems to be a matter of fact that in terms of a psychophysical
presentation, we are more sensitive to stimulus increments if they signal
hue changes than if they signal chroma changes. In a psychological or psy-
chophysical diagram the outcome is an elongated unit contour. In practical
terms, there appear to be two independent systems: one that assesses changes
in the ratio of two opponent color signals (assuming a two-process hue
detection system) and the other changes in the size of the vector sum of the
opponent color system (indicative of contrast). Both are affected by surround.
The two seemingly operate independently of each other and are not connected
in a euclidean sense. A euclidean space can only be achieved (and only for
332 FROM COLOR-MATCHING ERROR TO LARGE COLOR DIFFERENCES
small color differences) with the help of euclidization operations such as the
one by Thomsen.
In Chapter 5 we observed that there are different numbers of constant size
hue differences between unique hues. As a result, in a chromatic diagram in
which constant hue differences occupy equal hue angles (e.g., the Munsell
system), unique hues do not fall on the diagram axes. On the other hand, in a
diagram where the unique hues fall on the axes, equal hue angle differences
do not indicate equal hue differences. To understand this situation, we require
knowledge of the mechanism resulting in unique hues and of the hue
difference mechanism.
Differences of the Munsell or OSA-UCS step size are not the largest dif-
ferences that can be compared in a color space. It is possible to think of more
global kinds of differences by posing questions like:
1. Are the hue differences between the four unique hues the same?
2. Are the differences between colors falling on axes (however chosen) in
the Munsell psychological diagram and the central gray of the same mag-
nitude, and is gray in fact exactly in the middle between such colors?
The first question has already been answered in terms of the varying number
of hue differences between the unique hues. Questions of the second kind were
of interest to Judd and discussed by him in his article Ideal color space (1969).
Based on his experiments with Munsell chips, he concluded that the superim-
portance of hue gradually fades as hue differences become very large.
Of Judd’s observers a majority decided that the shortest path between
opposing axis colors was not through gray but through a desaturated, hued
color halfway between the opposing colors being compared. Judd worked out
a function that accomplishes this fade for the modified Godlove difference
formula (equation 4-2). But he indicated that there appears to be no geomet-
rical model corresponding to his formulation, “at least, I have not been able
to think of one.” He concluded that also from this viewpoint ideal color space
was impossible. Additional complications appear to arise when considering
very large color differences.
test stimuli tend to be very short to minimize changes in adaptation due to the
test stimulus.
Color-scaling experiments, on the other hand, usually involve unlimited
exposure times. Experimental conditions usually varied between different
experiments and the degree of adaptation (in experiments involving small or
large suprathreshold experiments) were not considered nor decisively con-
trolled. As a result it is unlikely that fundamental processes have been deter-
mined but rather that the results represent overlapping fundamental processes
(if it is possible to identify fundamental processes in this manner), however,
often representative of practice in visual color quality control and of more
natural viewing conditions.
In natural viewing conditions adaptation tends to change continuously as a
result of what is being viewed, and the observer controls exposure times. If I
harvest ripe strawberries in a strawberry patch, as I change my gaze, the visual
system is adapted in various degrees to, among other things, combinations of
the brownish-grayish color of soil, the green leaves of the strawberry plants,
and, if there are plenty of ripe strawberries, the redness of the fruit. It is known
that some adaptation processes proceed very rapidly in time while others are
slower. Perceived color differences between two adjoining strawberries are a
result of the total momentary adaptation situation.
It remains to be seen how large the differences are between the two kinds
of observation conditions. In addition the observer groups in different
experiments most always are different. Variations in the results of different
observers are well known. They can be due to variations in their cone response
functions or other components of their color vision system. The degree of
linear transformability of individual color-matching functions remains to be
determined as well if meaningful transformable means can be calculated
from individual results. As discussed earlier, there is the possibility that certain
judgments may be influenced by evolutionary experiences.
It is important to keep in mind that the following conclusions are valid only
for a general viewing condition where the surround is achromatic and in
luminance/lightness somewhere between the upper and lower extremes of the
targets used in testing. At the color-matching error level we have found
variation among observers and, possibly, experimental conditions. The
MacAdam data are well represented by elongated rectangles in the L, S
diagram. Both Wyszecki-Fielder observers here investigated (GW and AR)
produced ellipses pointing already to some degree in the direction of the
neutral point, most strongly so in the fourth quadrant (this applies also to
the third observer). The impression is created that under the conditions of the
MacAdam experiment, or at least for that observer, CME is not influenced by
specific properties of the opponent color system but is based only on cone
response limitations. Under the conditions of the Wyszecki-Fielder CME
experiment, rotation of unit ellipses in the direction of the neutral point begins
to be noticeable particularly in one quadrant. The same applies to the color
difference matching results. At the level of thresholds the opponent color
334 FROM COLOR-MATCHING ERROR TO LARGE COLOR DIFFERENCES
Data analysis in this chapter has been pursued based on a conventional view
of color vision. From the results we can draw a number of conclusions regard-
ing the various divisions of a common psychophysical color space (e.g.,
CIELAB), based on the magnitude of differences involved. Before going into
details it should be mentioned again that the experimental basis at the various
levels tends to be different. Color-matching errors and thresholds are usually
determined using visual colorimeters. Small suprathreshold, medium, and
large differences have usually been determined using color samples of some
kind viewed in varied surroundings or, more frequently, in a light booth,
usually in an achromatic surround.
The results of observer PGN in the MacAdam experiment indicate that hue
discrimination was not explicitly active but that they are strictly a result of the
sensitivity limitations of the three cone types. For both observers, in the
Wyszecki-Fielder experiment, hue discrimination appears partially engaged (a
number of ellipses point to the origin), but not all other aspects of the oppo-
nent color system (the average ratio between L and S increments is the same
as in the MacAdam experiment and twice as large as in the Luo-Rigg and large
color difference data). This implies engagement of different aspects of the
system under different conditions. Lightness and chromatic crispening effects
are engaged for CME data.
Based on the Richter data, at the threshold level all identified components
are active. Suprathreshold small color difference unit ellipses, as we have seen,
have a quite strong tendency to point to the origin. This effect is geometrically
enhanced in the X, Z diagram, and even more if the X axis scaling is expanded
HOW FUNDAMENTAL ARE THE VARIOUS KINDS OF DATA? 335
by a factor 2.3 for numerical balance with Z. Power modulation, lightness, and
chromatic crispening apply at this level. The L or X increments have doubled
compared to CME data, indicating increments in L or X that are not guided
by cone sensitivity but by another mechanism, an opponent color system.
In differences of Munsell and OSA-UCS magnitude the opponent color
system and hue discrimination are active, explicitly so in case of Munsell and
implicitly in case of OSA-UCS, as we have seen. Lightness crispening is active
as seen in the pre-1943 Munsell data and as recognized by inclusion of the
effect in OSA-UCS. Chromatic crispening, however, has now faded. The result
indicates that the processes guiding uniform tiling of color space are different
at different levels of size of differences.
Chapter 9
Conclusions and Outlook
Color Space and Its Divisions: Color Order from Antiquity to the Present, by Rolf G. Kuehni
ISBN 0-471-32670-4 Copyright © 2003 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
336
WHAT ARE COLOR SPACES AND HOW CAN THEY BE JUSTIFIED? 337
chophysical color space models that order colors and generally have no claims
of uniformity, such as the CIE tristimulus space, the x, y, Y space, the Luther-
Nyberg space, and many others. For those without claim to uniformity, the
general designation regular color space has been proposed. In addition there
are spectral spaces derived from mathematical dimensionality reduction of
spectral functions of color stimuli not involving cone or color-matching
functions.
For a period of some 150 years there was belief that all object color per-
ceptions, uniformly spaced, could fit into a simple geometrical solid. Munsell
was the first to show that if uniformity of differences is a goal this belief is mis-
placed. Since then it has become evident that a Munsell level uniform color
solid cannot fit into a euclidean space.
The following is a discussion of a number of significant issues related to
color spaces and solids with stated or proposed answers (depending on the
strength of current evidence).
Color spaces and color solids represent attempts to show a multitude of pos-
sible color perceptions in a systematic fashion. This is an effort that faces
several philosophical and scientific problems. We do not know how we per-
ceive colors as part of our consciousness. From an extreme point of view (see
Saunders and van Brakel 2002) this dooms the entire enterprise. We can all
agree that there is no generally valid direct relationship between spectral
power entering our eyes, and thereby between reflectance functions of objects,
and our experiences of color. Experimental work in color scaling under ele-
mentary conditions and work in color technology indicate, however, that for
such conditions there is a reasonably solid relationship between members of
a class of metamers and the resulting color experience as judged by an average
color normal observer. Without it, colorant formulation and color technolo-
gies of various kinds (color photography, television, color printing, etc.) would
not work as well as they do. Is viewing color fields under elementaristic con-
ditions a true reflection of natural color experiences? The answer, obviously,
is no. However, the evolutionary development of human color vision should
not be confused with its current average operation. Arguments can be made
that (in industrial countries) we experience color today more often in unnat-
ural as well as quasi elementaristic conditions (looking at a video screen or
color television) than in natural conditions (except when on vacation). Does
it mean our color experiences are degraded? Does the color vision system of
young children now develop differently from that of our grandparents? We do
not know but it is unlikely to be so.
The hue sequence of the generally accepted hue circle derives its justifica-
tion from the spectrum and the mixture in various ratios of narrow band
338 CONCLUSIONS AND OUTLOOK
energy from the two ends of the spectrum. For the reason that color experi-
ence is private we can only assume that one person experiences the colors of
spectral lights approximately the same way the next one does (assuming they
do not have impaired color vision). That photometry can produce an ordinal
scale of gray objects viewed under elementaristic conditions is without doubt.
Perceived brightness of chromatic objects, as was shown, is not in agreement
with photometric results, for reasons that we do not know. Speculative models
can, however, result in a high level of agreement with average visual data.
Quantitative relationship at the ordinal level for elementary conditions also
exists quite clearly between spectral power distributions of metameric sets
and the resulting perceived saturation or chroma. All these relationships have
high rank order correlations. Such information has provided the basis for
simplistic color models attempting to relate under elementaristic conditions
physical data of lights or materials with the resulting average perceived color.
For the models to be quantitatively useful they must have a higher degree of
complexity. There is no doubt, and the history of chapters 2 and 6 clearly indi-
cates it, that often (in an absolute sense always) large houses of cards are
erected in the building of these models. But as the history of science shows
this is how progress toward reaching goals of understanding is made. Will
humans understand their color vision system in 50 or 200 years, or never? Time
will tell.
The curious thing (or perhaps not) is the considerable practical level of
success that color science has gained in coloration technology, color device
technology and color management with the kind of house of cards models just
mentioned. None of these is perfect and perhaps never will be. One good
reason is that there is just too much variation in individual experience from a
given stimulus set. These relative successes have given color scientists and
technologists a level of confidence that the conjectured relationships might not
be totally coincidental. Coloration of textiles, paints and plastics can now be
controlled at levels approaching just noticeable differences. Metamerism for
a limited number of light sources can be avoided and it is even possible to
obtain a good idea if a matched material sample will or will not have a similar
appearance under different lights.
All existing color spaces and solids result in color experiences that are at
least in ordinal order of perceived color. (It is not known if this applies uni-
versally to color normal observers.) To produce an atlas that is an accurate
representation of a perceptually uniform color solid for a given set of condi-
tions is impossible at the level of large differences. But this does not seem to
be important as long as it is understood in what way the atlas configuration
deviates from the space implied by its samples (as we know we cannot produce
accurate large scale flat geographical maps). Such an atlas has not yet been
produced. The best color difference formulas for small color differences have
limited accuracy for reasons that have not yet been investigated in detail. The
models are likely too simplistic but, as discussed before, there are also many
issues of observer panels and observation conditions. The technical value of
improved formulas makes their pursuit worthwhile.
WHAT CAUSES THE PERCEPTION OF COLORS AND THEIR DIFFERENCES? 339
These are very fundamental questions in regard to color space. Our immedi-
ate experience of the world as expressed through visual and tactile clues is
BASIC COLOR EXPERIENCE THREE-DIMENSIONAL AND FOUR UNIQUE HUES? 341
somewhat accidental. If basic hues are a result of opponent signals, then three
cone types, in theory, make six basic hues possible. But because of the high
degree of spectral overlap of the M and L cones, there would be a consider-
able amount of redundancy in the information. Four basic hues were sufficient
to support the survival of our genus.
In the matter of the number of basic hues an intriguing conjecture was
recently offered by Lotto and Purves (2002). They claim that we have four
basic hues in response to the need “to solve the four-color [topological] map
problem” as well as “to order spectra according to their physical similarities
and differences.”
It is evident that broad band sensors can only provide effective discrimi-
nation with overlap of the sensitive regions. Thus, having a short wavelength
sensitive detector added with some overlap to a midwavelength sensitive
detector improved discrimination capabilities in the visible region. Dichromats
can discriminate more than monochromats. Adding a third, overlapping sensor
improved discrimination further. Information manipulation was now required
to avoid considerable redundancy of information and to normalize percep-
tions at different levels of lightness. Based on three cone types the minimal
opponent system has four basic hues. It can be considered evolutionarily suf-
ficient (since that is what we have), but it is certainly not optimal. There is a
large spectral range (from about 580 to 730 nm, nearly 45% of the total visual
spectral range) where there is comparatively little discrimination. On the short
wavelength end the discrimination ability has been significantly improved
(perhaps by up to 20%) by the reappearance of redness. Mathematical analy-
sis shows that filtering of spectral data through the color-matching functions
is not optimal in terms of extraction of the information from the spectral data
and ordering of spectra according to their similarities. Three principal com-
ponents and other techniques do a considerably better job. But cone absorp-
tion is a process with biological limitations, and our visual system represents
a compromise between what is desirable and what is biologically possible. The
question is which evolutionary pressure was larger, that of improving dis-
crimination capability while maintaining a high level of visual acuity or the
unambiguous solution of the four color map problem?
The conventional, but not uncontroversial, view is that they relate to lightness
and hue perception, as well as to intensity of coloration. In terms of evolu-
tionary development brightness/lightness is the oldest perceptual variable.
It antedates the development of color vision. Its current implementation in
humans appears to be of complex nature, fine-tuned to allow useful interpre-
tation of the visual field in terms of light, shadow, and forms. But today it is
not the most salient attribute. This position belongs to hue. Color is defined
HOW ARE HUE, CHROMA AND LIGHTNESS PERCEPTIONS COMBINED? 343
color system seems fully engaged, as evidenced by the lower S/L, respectively
Z/X, ratios. This is the case for larger color differences up to and, likely, ex-
ceeding the size of OSA-UCS differences. Here power relationships connect
stimulus and perceived difference. The applicable powers and the weighting
constants (different by semi axis) are found to vary widely in different exper-
iments for reasons that are not clear but may involve the design of the exper-
iments or the observer panels. Different optimal powers and weights appear
to indicate independence of at least four chromatic processes. The immediate
reasons for the power relationship are not known but assumed to be satura-
tion effects of various cell types.
The relative enlargement of unit L increments against unit S increments for
threshold and larger differences compared to CME data signals a sea change,
the change from the Helmholtzian cone level to the higher zone, opponency
level. The higher intrinsic sensitivity of the L and M cones compared to S is
suppressed. Regardless of the input from cones, here one unit of a is equal to
one unit of b, with the positive and negative functions in balance. The relative
number of L, M, and S cones per unit area in the retina does not seem to play
any role in this situation. The new ratio of implicit L versus S response of the
first step from gray is valid throughout the range of differences from thresh-
old to OSA-UCS sized (and very likely larger) differences.
test field size. Increments for a criterion response become increasingly larger
as the test field luminous reflectances differ from that of the surround. Color
space and difference formulas so far have generally not considered this
effect. If achromatic reference pairs are used the magnitude of difference per-
ceived between them is also subject to the lightness crispening effect, that is,
the psychological magnitude of the difference between two gray reference
samples depends on the surround and the relative size of the fields. This issue
affects comparability of different data sets based on different surrounds.
The lightness crispening effect is active from threshold size differences to
differences of the size found in the Munsell value scale and, likely, larger
differences.
The chromatic crispening effect is, on a relative basis, about equally distinct
for L and M compared to S. The L increment required for criterion response
quadruples approximately over a change in L of 10 units from the neutral
point in direction of increasing L, less so for decreasing L, as the increment is
a sum of the fundamental increment and the chromatic crispening increment.
As has been shown in Chapter 8, it is most strongly active at the level of CME
and threshold differences and appears to gradually fade as chromatic differ-
ences become larger. It is absent at the level of Munsell double-chroma steps
or OSA-UCS steps.
Chromatic crispening makes it possible to euclidize the Riemannian space
implicit in small color difference data. The approximate factor 2 of the ratio
of the longer (chroma-related) to the shorter (hue-related) diameter of the
unit contour has been included in the SC weight adjusting for chromatic
crispening. Since this weight is a continuous function of chroma it can be inte-
grated in the euclidization of formulas like CIE94.
Despite the findings of Plateau the question of increment magnitude for a cri-
terion response as a function of stimulus magnitude was dominated for the
second half of the nineteenth century by Fechner and the Weber-Fechner law
indicating the required increment to be a constant fraction of the stimulus.
Study of lightness scales from black to white in the early twentieth century
and additional investigations of other sensory magnitudes pointed to the
applicability of power modulation. Different experimental conditions resulted
in different optimal power modulations. The findings in regard to crispening
discussed in Chapter 8 indicate that the Weber-Fechner law is generally not
applicable to color differences. It is also apparent that simple power functions,
as proposed by Stevens, are not representative of color differences from sub-
threshold to medium size.
In global psychophysical color space scaling efforts the Munsell value scale,
HOW WELL DO FORMULAS PREDICT PERCEIVED COLOR DIFFERENCES? 347
with its suppression of the lightness crispening effect (and its fit with cube root
modulation) and the proposal by Adams in 1942 to apply cube root modula-
tion also to the X and Z tristimulus values, proved influential and continues
to be the official paradigm of the CIE. This situation has been influenced in
the past by computational difficulties and then by habit. The evidence derived
from the Munsell Renotations and the Re-renotations is that different powers
are optimal for the four chromatic semi axes. Conceptually it would be sur-
prising if the complex processes of the visual and related cognitive systems
could be accurately modeled with a single and simple power modulation. This
matter has not yet been thoroughly investigated in regard to threshold and
suprathreshold small color difference data.
Experimentally determined incremental and decremental stimulus
amounts lack a solid foundation in neurophysiology. The prevailing explana-
tion involves cone response saturation effects. It is unlikely to be the complete
story. The situation is complicated by the fact that psychophysical models need
to account for three effects in the chromatic plane:
In formulas such as CIE94 and CIEDE2000 the cube root formula addresses
point 1. The weight on chroma differences and the chroma-related weight on
hue differences addresses points 2 and 3.
(together with SH) eliminates elongation of the unit contour. It is not clear that
it actually does so because the complete hue circle has not been scaled rela-
tive to chroma in connection with color difference formulas that have such
weights.
Euclidean structure has been denied by Judd (1968) in case of a space based
on a constant achromatic surround with arguments involving the superimpor-
tance of hue and crispening effects. An additional argument was called “dimin-
ishing returns in color difference perception.” MacAdam had pointed out in
1963 that if there are three color samples A, B, and C selected along a geo-
desic so that samples A and C appear equally distant from sample B, samples
A and C are seen as having less than twice the difference between A and B
or B and C. In other words, the applicable scale is an interval scale but not a
ratio scale. This effect may be connected with the fading of chromatic crispen-
ing for larger differences and the changing of the unit contour elongation ratio
as a function of size of the difference.
In 1995 Wuerger and co-workers reported on tests of euclidean color geom-
etry. In one test they used proximity judgments to determine the angle
between intersecting lines in color space, and three kinds of tests for the addi-
tivity of such angles were made. All three failed to support a euclidean struc-
ture. In a second experiment the increase in variability of judgments of
similarity with the distance between test and reference stimulus was deter-
mined. Also this experiment failed to support euclidean structure.
The euclidean addition of hue, chroma, and lightness differences does so far
not seem to have been tested explicitly at the level of suprathreshold small or
large differences and under conditions representative of industrial practice (no
control of local adaptation). Among the reasons are the difficulty in preparing
accurate, well defined samples and the lack of replicated uniform chroma, hue,
and lightness (including the Helmholtz-Kohlrausch effect) scales.
As mentioned, euclidean structure with luminous reflectance as one dimen-
sion also can be expected to fail because visual lightness perception of chro-
matic samples involves not only luminous reflectance but also the presumed
contribution of the opponent color system in the Helmholtz-Kohlrausch
effect. Resulting constant psychological lightness planes are not orthogonal to
the achromatic lightness axis.
The question arises if it is useful and veridical to appropriately transform,
for small color differences, a* and b* scales by integration so that the result-
ing diagram is of euclidean nature, as Thomsen and others have shown and as
implemented in the DIN99 formula. Given the many remaining problems,
some addressed in CIEDE2000, the result is likely less than satisfactory and
not useful for large color differences. Another version of integration used in
the past is projective transformation, extensively employed by MacAdam.
However, his best effort in devising a nonlinear transformation for the OSA-
UCS basis data did not result in correlation improved over that obtained with
a cube root based euclidean formula.
350 CONCLUSIONS AND OUTLOOK
A psychological euclidean color space with the unique hues on the axes is not
uniform in terms of differences because hue differences representing identi-
cal hue angle differences are of different perceptual magnitude in the four
quadrants (Chapter 4). In addition there is the matter of the elongated unit
contours.
There is poor agreement between the cone contrast chromatic diagram axes
and unique hues. There is only slightly better agreement in case of a chromatic
diagram based on opponent functions derived from CIE color matching func-
tions. But there are two major issues here also:
1. The implied unique hues of the two standard observers, taken as the
crossover points of the opponent functions, are considerably different.
This implies that the primaries represented by the system axes are of
more or less mixed hues for both standard observers.
2. In terms of both standard observer opponent color functions objects
seen on average as unique red and unique green have a considerable
positive b (“yellowish”) component.
Aside from the neurophysiological evidence and the purely psychological evi-
dence in the Hering sense, there is considerable indirect psychophysical
evidence. In a cone-sensitivity space an approximately perceptually uniform
system such as the Munsell system is placed in alignment not in agreement with
any of its axes (see Fig. 5-29). Rough alignment with the axes is only obtained
in a tristimulus space and constant chroma circles at different lightness levels
only become concentric if the chromatic diagram is based on opponent func-
tions (Fig. 5-31). In other words, a reasonably accurate model that places the
information contained in reflectance functions of the Munsell system into a
corresponding psychophysical model requires opponent color functions.
Implicit evidence for an opponent system also comes from the fact that cone
increments required for the perception of threshold and suprathreshold
OPPONENT SIGNALS: THE SOURCE OF HUE AND CHROMA PERCEPTIONS? 351
differences are, in case of L and M cones, not related to their intrinsic sen-
sitivity as expressed in color-matching error but represent increments of
approximately double relative magnitude. Such enlarged increments are
consistent with the operation of an opponent system.
Perceived brightness/lightness of chromatic colors is not in agreement with
power-modulated flicker luminance/luminous reflectance. But it can be
modeled closely with addition of a portion of opponent color activation to
flicker luminance (Chapter 5), indicating that perceived brightness/lightness
of chromatic colors is not a dimension of tristimulus color-matching space.
Fig. 9-1 Values of an ideal, euclidean, constant chroma first quadrant with identical hue
differences between adjacent colors. Circles represent the reference color, and triangles the
test color.
tion) and that additional processing takes place beyond simple opponency. The
real hue and chroma perception and hue and chroma difference perception
processes may be more complex as hinted at by the results of Chichilnisky and
Wandell (Chapter 6).
Given the difficulties alluded to in earlier sections and the lack of sufficiently
detailed experimental data it is not possible to create an accurate geometrical
model of a uniform object color solid. An approximation of the surface of such
a space based on small color differences was calculated using simplifications
and assumptions. At eight different levels of luminous reflectance twenty
maximal object colors around the hue circle were determined. The luminous
reflectances were adjusted to reflect the Helmholtz-Kohlrausch effect using
the corresponding formula from OSA-UCS. These values were transformed
into the L*, a*, b* space and the a* and b* values euclidized using the Thomsen
formula. As was mentioned in Section 9.9, it is not clear if this calculation
adjusts also fully for the fundamental elongation of the unit contour. The
results are points on the surface of a color solid uniform according to the con-
ditions implied and are shown in three projections in Fig. 9-2a, b, and c. View
9-2a shows the solid facing the yellow to blue dimension and view 9-2b facing
A RESEARCH PROGRAM 353
Fig. 9-2 View of a color solid approximately uniform at the level of small color differences.
The vertical line connects achromatic colors from luminous reflectance 0 to 100. The different
symbols represent colors of a given luminous reflectance at the object color limit. They have
been adjusted for the Helmholtz-Kohlrausch effect. (a) View of the yellow-to-blue axis; ( b) view
of the green-to-red axis; (c) view from the top onto the chromatic plane.
the green to red dimension. Achromatic colors fall on the straight vertical line.
The third view shows the solid from the top. Its irregular nature is clearly
evident. It can be compared to Fig. 1-2.
Analysis of published data in regard to color attribute scaling and color dif-
ference scaling shows significant discrepancies in results between different
efforts, despite large numbers of samples, observers, and observations. There
are several potential reasons for such discrepancies: different observer panels,
different visual context of samples and surround, different light sources, and
354 CONCLUSIONS AND OUTLOOK
unrecognized contextual clues that can affect the results due to empirical inter-
pretation of the visual field.
Independent replication of experimental results is a standard requirement
in science. The degree to which experimental color scaling results can be inde-
pendently replicated needs to be established. Given the known and perhaps
some unknown sensitivities of results to experimental conditions, it seems
imperative that independent replication involve essentially identical viewing
conditions: the same samples, the same visual context, and the same or very
similar (implicit or explicit) light source. Today duplication of this kind is prob-
ably best achieved with simulated samples on a computer monitor. The same
program creating the displays can be used in different locations, and the
general surrounds can be duplicated with masks surrounding the monitor
screen. Illumination of the room in which the monitor is viewed may be impor-
tant for control of the complete adaptation situation. Monitor calibration is a
critical issue that needs to be satisfactorily resolved. Variability of results from
different observer panels in several locations can then be established. The
results may indicate if the variability we see in past results is normal or if, given
A RESEARCH PROGRAM 355
Lightness Scaling
Once an intermediate lightness for the achromatic surround and a reference
difference have been chosen, a 10- or 20-step gray scale can be established.
356 CONCLUSIONS AND OUTLOOK
Chroma Scaling
Once planes of constant lightness have been established chroma can be scaled
at different levels of chroma and different levels of perceived lightness. Again,
the work may be easiest to accomplish using monitor display colors that are
adjustable essentially in one attribute direction only. As the observer adjusts
fields for identical perceived chroma, the program should automatically make
adjustments for the previously established constant lightness and follow
approximate lines of constant hue as reported in the literature. Once 20 to 40
hues around the hue circle have been scaled for chroma, constant chroma
contours can be established by interpolation of data points. Such contours
need to be established minimally at 5 to 7 levels of chroma at several levels
of perceived lightness.
Hue Scaling
With chroma scaling complete, constant chroma contours can be scaled for
constant hue increments along the contours. Fine adjustments can be made to
result in an integer number of differences. Again, the program should make
automatic adjustments for perceived lightness and chroma as the observer
makes changes of the perceived hue of the test field.
Many issues need to be decided. For example, should all three attributes be
scaled separately for each observer, or should average perceived lightness and
then average perceived chroma be used for the subsequent step?
Once results of the best achievable level of replication have been obtained,
efforts to fit the visual data with psychophysical formulas can be undertaken.
It remains to be seen if formula fitting achieves satisfactory agreement or if,
in the end, lookup tables are required.
Robustness of Formula
The robustness of a formula developed in such a manner needs to be estab-
lished. We have already seen that without several adaptable parameters such
a formula is only strictly applicable to a narrow set of conditions in regard to
samples, surround, and lighting. The question to be resolved is how much con-
ditions can change before the accuracy of the formula is unacceptably reduced.
This may involve sample size and structure, separating line, surround quality
and structure, spectral power distribution, and intensity of the light source. Can
the formula be meaningfully adapted to apply for incandescent or triband
lamps?
This text has made clear that the idea of a single fundamental color space is mis-
placed. As found in practice there are different kinds of color spaces and solids
applicable with a degree of accuracy to different situations. Changes in the
applicable conditions can quickly further reduce the accuracy of a formula to a
significant degree.
Color spaces can be placed in a kind of hierarchy of complexity and
meaning. The structural requirements for these kinds of spaces differ.
Color-Matching Spaces
Color-matching spaces include many examples, such as the Rösch-MacAdam
space, the CIE tristimulus space, and the Cohen fundamental color space (with
many possible configurations). The last derives its name from the decomposi-
tion of spectral power distributions into the so-called fundamental of each
reflectance function, meaning the portion of each spectral function free of
metameric black. Each related color perception is represented as a vector, just
as in the other two spaces mentioned. Average visual metamers plot in the
same location in all three cases. These spaces make no claims for quantitative
systematicity in terms of appearance.
Spectral Spaces
Spectral spaces do not involve data related to human color vision and, there-
fore, should not be termed color spaces. They are based on finding components
representing a given reduced dimensionality implicit in the spectral functions,
such as by principal component analysis. Some three-dimensional spectral
spaces place spectra of the Munsell colors in correct ordinal order, but the
dimensions have no perceptual meaning and differences between colors in
such spaces have no meaningful relationship to perceived differences. Visual
metamers plot in different locations in such spaces.
Among the many kinds of color spaces the uniform space has the highest
degree of systematicity if equal sense distance has the importance that psy-
chophysics assigns to it. What degree of accuracy and repeatability is possible
for a uniform color space for the average observer and a specific set of exper-
imental conditions remains to be determined. Unless one wants to raise a
charge of bias of some kind for the RIT-DuPont data, they show that signifi-
cant improvement compared to many other data sets may be possible based
on careful experimentation. Additional improvement resulting from a fuller
understanding of the color vision process may also be feasible. Problems and
uncertainties increase the more complete the color vision model is to be in its
explanatory power, for example, as color appearance models indicate.
Given the view that vision is an animal type’s best way to deal with the
ambiguities of the information provided by the energy streams that we call
light, it is evident that finding the one color space representative of human
360 CONCLUSIONS AND OUTLOOK
Chapter 1
1. For empirical rules, see Purves and Lotto (2002). For an elementaristic approach,
see Mausfeld (1998). For relativized conditions, see McLaughlin (2002). On the
change in appearance of objects, consider the works of Impressionist painters
such as those of Monet who distinctly varied the coloration of his Haystack
series and views of the Cathedral of Rouen depending on time of day and/or
weather.
2. On the ordinal order of personal spaces, it is well known that only a small per-
centage of color normal observers perform error-free in the Farnsworth-Munsell
100-hue test where colored caps with small differences in hue must be sorted in
correct sequence.
3. For unique hue variation, see Kuehni (2001a). In the Munsell Book of Colors the
complete hue circle has been divided into 40 perceptually equally different hue
steps.
4. On the four cone types in women, tetrachromacy is well established in the animal
kingdom. The possibility of tetrachromacy in human females was raised by Jordan
and Mollon (1993). Jameson et al. (2001) have investigated the color experiences
of females with the genetic potential for having four cone types. The estimate is
from Neitz et al. (1998).
5. On adaptation, see, for example, Fairchild (1998). Current usage of the term in the
vision science community is limited to processes beginning in the retinal layer. On
retinal illumination, actual levels of illumination at the retina are extremely diffi-
cult to determine. Instead values based on luminance arriving at the eye, assumed
pupil size as well as other assumptions are calculated. The unit of retinal illumi-
nance is the troland.
Color Space and Its Divisions: Color Order from Antiquity to the Present, by Rolf G. Kuehni
ISBN 0-471-32670-4 Copyright © 2003 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
361
362 NOTES
6. Lux is the photometric unit of illuminance, 1 lux = 1 lumen per m2; lumen is the
unit of luminous flux. A lumen is equal to the flux emitted in a unit-solid angle
from a uniform point source of one (standard) candle.
7. CIE is the French acronym for International Commission on Illumination, an inter-
national body concerned with technical aspects of lighting and color. For more
details on the Munsell color system, see Chapters 2 and 7. The Munsell system is
perhaps the best-known color appearance system.
8. The Nickerson-Newhall psychological color solid models are located at the Hagley
Museum and Library in Wilmington, DE. The models have been manufactured by
Nickerson’s assistant K. F. Stultz.
9. CIELAB is a color space and difference formula recommended by the CIE; see
Chapter 6.
10. On Schönfelder’s law, see Schönfelder 1933.
11. The broad use of the word sensation in the historical psychophysical literature
makes it impossible to use uniform terminology for these two terms.
Chapter 2
1. A review of color appearance spaces was provided by G. Wyszecki in 1960 and
G. Derefeldt in 1991. Information is also provided on the Web site
www.colorsystem.com.
2. Xenophanes as quoted in Freeman (1957).
3. Pythagoras as quoted in Mansfeld (1986), translation by the author.
4. Quote from Empedocles in Mansfeld (1986), translation by the author.
5. Quote from Democritus in Mansfeld (1986), translation by the author.
6. Photius as quoted in Gage (1993).
7. Pliny as quoted by Heinrich Meyer in Goethe, Geschichte der Farbenlehre, 1810,
translation by the author.
8. Ancient Greek sacral colors, Oxford Classical Dictionary, 3d ed. New York: Oxford
University Press 1996.
9. On Avicenna scales, see Gage 1993.
10. On Eraclius, see Merrifield 1967.
11. On Urso de Salerno, see Gage (1993).
12. Albertus Magnus quoted in Gage (1993).
13. For an analysis of Bacon’s work on color, see Parkhurst (1990).
14. For Cennini quote, see Cennini (1933).
15. Leonardo, Codex Urbinas latinus 1270, see McMahon (1966).
16. For Ficino list, see Barasch (1978).
17. For Telesio list, see Goethe (1810), translation by the author.
18. For Cardanus list, see Barasch (1978).
19. Forsius translation and figures from Feller and Stenius (1970) and Parkhurst and
Feller (1982).
20. Boethius and figure, see Murdoch (1984).
NOTES 363
Chapter 3
1. On linking propositions, see, for example, Teller and Pugh (1983).
2. For the nine-dimensional universe, see, for example, Greene (1999).
364 NOTES
Chapter 4
1. Runge translations by the author.
2. Grassmann translation by the author.
3. Helmholtz (1909, Vol II, p. 130).
4. Hering definition of constant veiling from Hering (1964, pp. 51–52).
5. Chromo-luminarisme, a term invented by the French painter George Seurat
(1859–1891) to designate his early style of neoimpressionist painting. The term
chromolithographe was first mentioned in French literature in 1837.
6. On Ostwald’s view of Helmholtz’s brightness definition, see Schwarz (1995).
7. Godlove formula from Judd (1969).
8. Pieter van Musschenbroek (1692–1761) was the inventor of the Leyden flask, a
form of electrical capacitor. The law of disk mixture was developed by Plateau in
1853 and the technique perfected by Maxwell (Boring, 1942).
Chapter 5
1. For a short history of photometry, see Walsh (1958).
2. On the Hefner lamp, see Walsh (1958).
3. Lambert comment in Lambert (1760).
4. On Treviranus and Boll, see Polyak (1957).
5. For a lively description on the CIE standard observer development, see Wright
(1996).
6. The density and distribution of cone types varies throughout the retina. The
macular spot has an irregular distribution and is absent in the central area of
focus of the normal eyes optics. To account for the average observer, for these
differences two different standard observers have been specified by the CIE, one
applying to a visual field subtending 2° and the other 10°.
7. For a trenchant critique of the CIE colorimetric system, see Cohen 2001. For a jab
see the comment by the eminent visual physiologist W. A. H. Rushton: “The CIE
triangle is brilliantly ingenious as an aid to the calculation of chromaticities which
can be upheld in a court of law where colour specification is in dispute. But the tri-
angle is monstrous as an indication of what is going on in the mechanism of vision.
It displays all colours as a mixture of three primary lights, none of which have an
existence that can be easily imagined. One of the three primaries is bright: it is pure
green from which is subtracted a lot of red, which it does not contain. The other
primaries are quite dark; they have strong colour but zero luminance. These do not
seem to me ingredients that lead to clarity in our conception of colour mechanisms
and I am astonished that some physiologists and many psychologists employ
them to instruct the young and bewilder the old.” (Journal of Physiology 1972;
220:178)
8. Many people have contributed to the elucidation of the genetic basis of color
vision. Among the pioneers were J. Nathans, R. and S. Yokoyama, and others. For
a succinct history, see Sharpe et al. (1999).
9. On Granit and Svaetichin, see Polyak (1957).
NOTES 365
10. The notion of a central fovea free of S cones was first proposed by Artur
König in 1894. Since then it was confirmed in some experiments but remains
controversial.
11. The effect of field size on observed appearance in unpublished results by the
author.
Chapter 6
1. For an extended discussion on the concept of line element, see Wyszecki and Stiles
(1982, p. 654ff). See MacAdam (1981) on Schrödinger’s and Stiles’s line elements.
2. The terms NBS unit or judd have not gained widespread use.
Chapter 7
1. On Munsell system development, see Munsell (1918) and Kuehni (2002a).
2. The supplier of the Munsell Book of Colors is GretagMacbeth LLC, New Windsor,
NY.
3. On the committee experiments, see Judd and Nickerson (1967) and Judd (1955,
1957, 1965, 1967).
4. This was a forced choice experiment in which the observer could only answer in
one of two ways, “larger” or “smaller.”
5. MacAdam revision of OSA-UCS, personal communication by J. T. Luke.
6. Hering translations by Hurvich and Jameson; see Hering (1905–1911).
7. On the “beauty test for acceptance,” see Hård et al. (1996a).
Chapter 8
1. MacAdam’s ellipse 1 applies to a highly saturated reddish blue. It is more highly
saturated than any other color used in any color-matching error experiment. The
implicit S cone absorption value is very high and not in agreement with that of all
other ellipses. It has been left out of the analysis for that reason.
Glossary
Color Space and Its Divisions: Color Order from Antiquity to the Present, by Rolf G. Kuehni
ISBN 0-471-32670-4 Copyright © 2003 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
366
GLOSSARY 367
wave ends of the spectrum; usually illustrated with high chroma pigment
colorations.
Colorimeter Optical instrument for the investigation of color vision; in
technology also an instrument that measures the reflectance of materials
through three filters duplicating the color-matching functions of a standard
observer.
Colorimetry The branch of color science concerned with the numerical
specification of color stimuli.
Colorimetric purity A measure of saturation related to color stimuli and
expressed in the CIE chromaticity diagram. Its relationship to perceived
saturation in some standard conditions is complex.
Color difference The perceived difference between two non-identical fields
of color.
Color difference formula A mathematical formula that allows the calcula-
tion from stimuli of the difference between two color fields in a given
surround, as perceived by an average observer.
Color, full Translation of Hering’s term Vollfarbe, the mental image of a color
at its highest chromaticness; the color with a particular hue at the highest
level of chroma on the MacAdam limit.
Color harmony The combination of color elements in objects of art or craft
so that the effect is perceived as harmonious, in concord.
Color-matching error Stimulus variability in repeated matches of a standard
color.
Color-matching functions Three spectral functions describing the amounts
of three primary lights required to result in color perceptions matching
those obtained from spectral lights.
Color metric A metric describes the mathematical structure of a geometri-
cal space; a color metric applies to a color space, specifically a uniform color
space.
Color order Systematic arrangement of color perceptions in terms of
attributes and geometrical or mathematical models thereof.
Color, primary Colloquial term used in different circumstances: (1) One of
three lights whose color appearance cannot be matched by the other two
used with the other two to match the appearance of any other light; (2) one
of three colorants used in color order systems or in color reproduction, such
as yellow, red, and blue or yellow, magenta, and cyan; (3) one of the four
Hering Urfarben or fundamental hue perceptions of yellow, red, blue, and
green.
Color, related Color perception caused by light reflected from an object in
the presence of other objects. The perceived color depends on the perceived
color of surrounding objects.
Color, unrelated Color perceived to belong to an area seen in isolation from
other areas.
GLOSSARY 369
Hues, unique The four hues of the color circle that can not be matched with
colors other than themselves; the psychological primary hues yellow, red,
blue, and green. Unique red is a red hue that is neither yellowish nor bluish,
for example.
Hue superimportance Refers to the fact that a smaller stimulus increment is
required for a criterion difference response if it represents a hue difference
than if it represents a chroma or saturation difference of the same perceived
magnitude.
Illuminant An illuminating device; technically a set of numbers representing
the spectral power distribution of a light source.
Isomorphism A one-to-one correspondence between mathematical sets;
specifically, mapping of objects of color experience to objects in a geomet-
rical space so that a one-to-one correspondence is obtained.
Just noticeable difference (JND) Threshold difference; the initial perceptual
difference that can be seen when one of two originally identical fields of
color changes in any given direction.
Lateral geniculate nucleus A mass of cells in the brain along the visual pas-
sageway between the retina and the visual area at the back of the brain.
Lattice A regular geometrical arrangement of points over an area or in a
space; specifically related to the arrangement of colors in a color space.
Lightness Perceptual attribute of related colors according to which a
color field appears to emit equal or less light compared to a white field.
Lightness can be understood as relative brightness.
Line element The first fundamental form of a regular surface. It is defined
by the Riemannian metric. In connection with color the term is used to
describe a certain kind of color space defined by (weighted) increments of
color fundamentals.
Linear model Of the first degree with respect to variables; having a graph
that is a straight line.
Linear regression A functional relationship between two or more variables
in which the variables are linearly related.
Linking proposition Postulated link between two sets of facts that are only
indirectly related.
Luminance Luminous flux of a light beam emanating from a surface in a
given direction, per unit solid angle.
Luminous reflectance Luminance of the surface of an object compared to
the luminance of the surface of a perfectly reflecting diffuser, illuminated
with the same light source and viewed at the same angle. Also known as
luminance factor Y.
Macula “Yellow spot,” an irregularly formed ring-like area of yellowish
pigment in the central region of the retina. The fovea is located in the
central area of the macular ring, free of macular pigment.
372 GLOSSARY
Value Munsell’s term for the grades of a perceptually uniform gray scale.
Vision Process by which the extended visual system extracts information
from light energy to help generate appropriate response behavior.
Weber fraction Proportionality constant between the stimulus increment
and the absolute value of the stimulus.
Weighting To apply a statistically or otherwise determined weighting factor
to a variable.
Whiteness Attribute of a diffusing surface permitting, when viewed under a
standard light source, the judgment of similarity to a standard white surface
viewed in the same light.
References
Abney, W. de W. 1910. On the change in hue of spectrum colors by dilution with white
light. Proceedings of the Royal Society (London) A83:120–127.
Abney, W. de W. 1914. Researches in Colour Vision and the Trichromatic Theory.
London: Longmans, Green.
Ackerman, J. S. 1980. On early Renaissance color theory and practice. In H. A. Millon
ed. Studies in Italian Art and Architecture, 15th through 18th Centuries. Cambridge:
MIT Press.
Adams, E. Q. 1923. A theory of color vision. Psychological Review 30:56–76.
Adams, E. Q. 1942. X-Z planes in the 1931 I.C.I. system of colorimetry. Journal of the
Optical Society of America 32:168–173.
Adams, E. Q., and Cobb, P. W. 1922. The effect on foveal vision of bright (and dark)
surroundings. Journal of Experimental Psychology 5:39–45.
Agoston, G. A. 1987. Color Theory and Its Application in Art and Design. Berlin:
Springer.
Aguilonius, F. 1613. Opticorum libri sex. Antwerp: Plantin (with illustrations by PP
Rubens).
Alberti, L. B. Leon Battista Alberti on painting, ed. J. Spenser. New Haven: New Haven
University Press, 1956.
Albertus Magnus. Opera omnia, 38 vols, ed. A. Borgnet. Paris: Vivès, 1890–99.
Allen, E., and Yuhas, B. 1984. Setting up acceptability tolerances: A case study. Color
Research and Application 9:37–48.
Anonymous (C.B.). 1708. Traité de la peinture en mignature. The Hague: van Dole.
Arago, D. F. J. 1850. Un movement modéré rend la visibilité des objets plus facile. In
Oeuvres complètes de François Arago, vol. 10. Paris: Gide, pp. 255–260.
Color Space and Its Divisions: Color Order from Antiquity to the Present, by Rolf G. Kuehni
ISBN 0-471-32670-4 Copyright © 2003 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
375
376 REFERENCES
Berns, R. S. 1996. Deriving instrumental tolerances from pass-fail and colorimetric data.
Color Research and Application 21:459–472.
Berns, R. S. 2000. Billmeyer and Saltzman’s Principles of Color Technology, 3d ed. New
York: Wiley.
Berns, R. S. 2001. Derivation of a hue-angle dependent, hue-difference weighting func-
tion for CIEDE2000. Proceedings AIC Color 01. Bellingham WA: SPIE.
Berns, R. S., and Billmeyer, F. W. 1985. Development of the 1929 Munsell Book of
Color: A historical review. Color Research and Application 10:246–250.
Berns, R. S., Alman, D. H., Reniff, L., Snyder, G. D., and Balonon-Rosen, M. R. 1991.
Visual determination of supra-threshold color-difference tolerances using probit
analysis. Color Research and Application 16:297–316.
Bezold, W. von. 1874. Die Farbenlehre im Hinblick auf Kunst und Kunstgewerbe. Braun-
schweig: Westermann. American edition: The Theory of Color in Its Relation to Art
and Art-Industry. Boston: Prang, 1876.
Billmeyer, F. W., and Bencuya, A. K. 1987. Interrelation of Natural Color System and
the Munsell color order system. Color Research and Application 12:243–255.
Billock, V.A., Gleason, G. A., and Tsou, B. H. 2001. Perception of forbidden colors in
retinally stabilized images: an indication of softwired cortical color opponency?
Journal of the Optical Society of America A18:2398–2403.
Birren, F. 1987. M. E. Chevreul, The Principles of Harmony and Contrast of Colors and
Their Application to the Arts. With a special introduction and newly revised com-
mentary by Faber Birren. West Chester, PA: Schiffer.
Boll, F. 1877. Zur Anatomie und Physiologie der Retina. Archiv für Physiologie, p.4.
Boring, E. G. 1929. A History of Experimental Psychology. New York: Century.
Boring, E. G. 1939. Introduction to Psychology. New York: Wiley.
Boring, E. G. 1942. Sensation and Perception in the History of Experimental Psychol-
ogy. New York: Appleton.
Bouguer, P. 1729. Essai d’optique. Paris: Jombert.
Bouguer, P. 1760. Traité d’optique sur la gradation de la lumière. Paris: Guerin et
Delatour.
Boyle, R. 1664. Experiments and Considerations Touching Colours. London:
Herringman.
Boynton, R. M. 1983. Ten years of research with the minimally distinct border. In J. D.
Mollon and L.T. Sharpe, eds., Colour vision: Physiology and Psychophysics. London:
Academic Press, pp. 193–206.
Boynton, R. M., and Kambe, N. 1980. Chromatic difference steps of moderate size along
theoretically critical axes. Color Research and Application 5:13–24.
Boynton, R. M., Nagy, A. L., Olson, C. X. 1983. A flaw in equations for predicting
chromatic differences. Color Research and Application 8:
Breckenridge, F. C., and Schaub, W. R. 1939. Rectangular uniform-chromaticity-scale
coordinates. Journal of the Optical Society of America 29:370–377.
Brenner, E. 1680. Nomenclatura et species colorum. Stockholm.
Brewster, D. 1831. A Treatise on Optics. London: Longman, Rees.
Brill, M. H., Fairman, H. S., Hemmendinger, H., and Ladson, J. A. 2002. The color-order
screen book: A softcopy simulated color atlas with selectable observer and illumi-
nant. Proceedings AIC Color 01. Bellingham, WA: SPIE.
378 REFERENCES
Brücke, E. 1866. Die Physiologie der Farben für die Zwecke der Kunstgewerbe. Leipzig:
Hirzel.
Brown,W. R. J., and MacAdam, D. L. 1949.Visual sensitivities to combined chromaticity
and luminance differences. Journal of the Optical Society of America 39:808–834.
Brown, W. R. J. 1952. The effect of field size and chromatic surroundings on color dis-
crimination. Journal of the Optical Society of America 47:137–142.
Brown, W. R. J. 1957. Color discrimination of twelve observers. Journal of the Optical
Society of America 47:137–143.
Burnham, R. W. 1949. Comparison of color systems with respect to uniform visual
spacing. Journal of the Optical Society of America 39:387–392.
Burns, S. A., Cohen, J. B., and Kuznetsov, E. N. 1990. The Munsell color cystem in fun-
damental color space. Color Research and Application 15:25–51.
Cardanus, H. 1563. Hieronimi Cardani opera omnia, 10 vols. Lyon (vol. 2, p. 552ff).
Carroll, J. D., and Chang, J. J. 1970. Analysis of individual differences in multidimen-
sional scaling via an N-way generalization of “Eckhardt-Young” decomposition.
Psychometrika 35:283–319.
Castel, L. B. 1739. Projet d’une nouvelle optique des couleurs fondée sur les observa-
tions et uniquement relative à la teinture et les autres arts coloristes. Journal de
Trévoux, April.
Chalcidius. Platonis Timaeus interprete Chalcidio, ed. J. Wrobel. Leipzig: Teubner, 1876.
Chalmers, D. J. 1996. The Conscious Mind. New York: Oxford University Press.
Charpentier, A. 1888. La lumière et les couleurs au point de vue physiologique. Paris:
Baillière.
Cheung, M., and Rigg, B. 1986. Colour-difference ellipsoids for five CIE colour centres.
Color Research and Application 11:185–195.
Cennini, C. 1400. The craftsman’s handbook: “Il libro dell’ arte,” transl. D. V. Thomp-
son. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1933.
Chevreul, M. E. 1839. De la Loi du contraste simultané des couleurs. Paris: Pitois-
Levraux.
Chichilnisky, E. J., and Wandell, B. A. 1999. Trichromatic opponent color classification.
Vision Research 39:3444–3458.
Chou, W., Lin, H., Luo, M. R., Westland, S., Rigg, B., and Nobbs, J. 2001. Performance
of lightness difference formulae. Coloration Technology 117:19–29.
Chroma Cosmos 5000 1978. Tokyo: Japan Color Research Institute.
Chromaton 707 1982. Tokyo: Japan Color Research Institute.
CIE 1924. CIE Proceedings. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1926.
CIE 1964 (U*V*W*)-System. 1963. CIE Proccedings. Vienna, Austria: Commission
Internationale de l’Éclairage.
CIE 1976 (L*u*v*)-System. 1986. No. 15.2, Colorimetry, 2nd ed. Vienna, Austria:
Commission Internationale de l’Éclairage.
CIE 1976 (L*a*b*)-System. 1986. No. 15.2, Colorimetry, 2nd ed. Vienna, Austria:
Commission Internationale de l’Éclairage.
CIE 1987. International lighting vocabulary, 4th ed. Vienna, Austria: Commission Inter-
nationale de l’Éclairage.
REFERENCES 379
CIE 1990. CIE 86, 2° Spectral Luminous Efficiency Function for Photopic Vision.
Vienna: Commission Internationale de l’Éclairage.
Clark, F. J. J., McDonald, R., and Rigg, B. 1984. Modification to the JPC79 colour-
difference formula. Journal of the Society of Dyers and Colourists 100:128–132.
Cohen, J. B. 1964. Dependency of the reflectance curves of the Munsell color chips.
Psychonomic Science 1:369–370.
Cohen, J. B. 2001. Visual Color and Color Mixture. Urbana: University of Illinois
Press.
Cohen, J. B., and Kappauf, W. E. 1982. Metameric color stimuli, fundamental metamers,
and Wyszecki’s metameric blacks. American Journal of Psychology 95:537–564.
Cohn, J. 1894. Experimentelle Untersuchungen über die Gefühlsbetonung der Farben,
Helligkeiten und ihrer Combinationen. Philosophische Studien 10:562–602.
Craik, K. J. W. 1938. The effect of adaptation on differential brightness discrimination.
Journal of Physiology 92:406–421.
Crane, H. D., and Piantanida, T. P. 1983. On seeing reddish green and yellowish blue.
Science 221:1078–1080.
Cronin, T. W., and Marshall, N. J. 1989. A retina with at least ten spectral types of
photoreceptors in a stomatopod crustacean. Nature 339:137.
Cui, G., Luo, M. R., Rigg, B., Roesler, G., and Witt, K. 2002. Uniform colour spaces
based on the DIN99 colour-difference formula. Color Research and Application
27:282–290.
Davidson, H. R., and Friede, E. 1953. The size of acceptable color differences. Journal
of the Optical Society of America 43:581–589.
Delboeuf, J. R. L. 1873. Étude psychophysique. Mémoires couronnés et autres mémoires
publiés par l’Academie Royales des Sciences, des Lettres, et des Beaux-Arts de
Belgique, vol. 23. Brussels: Hayez.
Delboeuf, J. R. L. 1883. Examen critique de la loi psychophysique: sa base et sa signifi-
cation. Paris: Baillière.
De Matiello, M. L. F., Salinas, H., and Benavente, C. 2001. Dichoptic discrimination of
saturation. Color Research and Application 26 (suppl):S53–S56.
Democritus of Abdera. In Die Vorsokratiker II. Stuttgart: Reclam, 1986.
Dennett, D. C. 1991. Consciousness Explained. Boston: Little, Brown.
Derby, 1999. Available at ftp//colour.derby.ac.uk.
Derefeldt, G. 1991. Colour appearance systems. In P. Gouras, ed., The Perception of
Colour. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Derefeldt, G., and Sahlin, C. 1986. Transformation of NCS data into CIELAB colour
space. Color Research and Application 11:146–152.
Derefeldt, G., and Hedin, C. E. 1987. A color atlas for graphical displays. In B. Knave
and P. G. Wiedebäck, eds., Work with Display Units 86. Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Derrington, A. M., Krauskopf, J. and Lennie, P. 1984. Chromatic mechanisms in lateral
geniculate nucleus of macaque. Journal of Physiology 357:241–265.
De Valois, K. K., ed. 2000. Seeing. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
De Valois, R. L. 1965. Analysis and coding of color vision in the primate visual system.
Cold Spring Harbor Symposium on Quantitative Biology 30:565–579.
380 REFERENCES
De Valois, R. L., and De Valois, K. K. 1993. A multi-stage color model. Vision Research
33:1053–1065.
De Valois, R. L., and De Valois, K. K. 1996. On “A three-stage color model.” Vision
Research 36:833–836.
De Valois, R. L., De Valois, K. K., Switkes, E., and Mahon, L. 1993. Hue scaling of
isoluminant and cone-specific lights. Vision Research 37:885–897.
De Valois, R. L., Cottaris, N. P., and Elfar, S. 1997. S-cone inputs to striate cortex cells.
Investigative Ophtalmology and Visual Science 38:S15.
DeVries, H. 1946. The luminosity curve of the eye as determined by measurements with
the flicker photometer. Physica 14:319–348.
Diderot, D. 1766. Essai sur la peinture. In Oeuvres complètes, 20 vols. Paris: P. Garnier
1875–1879.
DIN Farbenkarte, German Standard DIN 6164. 1962. Berlin: Beuth.
DIN 6176: Farbmetrische Bestimmung von Farbabständen bei Körperfarben nach der
DIN99 Formel. 2000. Berlin: Deutsches Institut für Normung.
Dobrowolsky, W. 1872. Beiträge zur physiologischen Optik. Archiv für Ophtalmologie
18:53–103.
Donders, F. C. 1881. Über Farbensysteme. Archiv für Ophtalmologie 26:155–223.
Doppler, C. 1848. Zwei weitere Abhandlungen aus dem Gebiete der Optik. 2. Versuch
einer systematischen Classification der Farben. Abhandlungen der königlichen
bömischen Gesellschaft der Wissenschaften 5(5):401–412. Reprinted in Schwarz, A.
1991/2. Christian Doopler’s Kugeloctant von 1847. Die Farbe 38:49–81.
Ebbinghaus, H. 1887. Die Gesetzmässigkeit des Helligkeitscontrastes. Sitzungsberichte
der Preussischen Akademie der Wissenschaften 2:995–1009.
Ebbinghaus, H. 1902. Grundzüge der Psychologie, Vol. 1. Leipzig: Viet.
Empedocles of Acragas. In Die Vorsokratiker II. Stuttgart: Reclam, 1986.
Eraclius. De coloribus et artibus romanorum. Original and translation in M. P. Merri-
field, de., Medieval and Renaissance Treatises on the Arts of Painting. New York:
Dover, 1967.
Eskew, R. T., McLellan, J. S., and Giulianini, F. 1999 Chromatic detection and discrim-
ination. In K. R. Gegenfurtner and L.T. Sharpe, eds., Color Vision. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Evans, R. M. 1974. The Perception of Color. New York: Wiley.
Evans, R. M., and Swenholt, B. K. 1968. Chromatic strengths of colors, Part II: The
Munsell System. Journal of the Optical Society of America 58:580–584.
Fechner, G. T. 1851. Zend-Avesta. Leipzig: Voss.
Fechner, G. T. 1860. Elemente der Psychophysik, transl. H.S. Langfeld. Leipzig:
Breitkopf und Härtel.
Farnsworth, D. 1944. The Farnsworth rectilinear uniform chromaticity diagram No. 38.
Memorandum 44-1. Medical Laboratory of the U.S. Submarine Base, New London,
CT.
Fairchild, M. D. 1998. Color Appearance Models. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Fairchild, M. D., and Pirotta, E. 1991. Predicting the lightness of chromatic object colors
using CIELAB. Color Research and Application 16:385–393.
REFERENCES 381
Fechner, G. T. 1860. Elemente der Psychophysik, Vols. 1 and 2. Leipzig: Breitkopf und
Härtel. Translation: Elements of Psychophysics. Vol. 1, eds. E. G. Boring and D. H.
Howes. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1966.
Feller, R. L., and Stenius, A. 1970. On the color space of Sigfrid Forsius, 1611. Color
Engineering 8:51–58.
Ficino M. 1519. Opera, I. Basel, pp. 825–826.
Field, G. 1835. Chromatography; or a Treatise on Colours and Pigments, and of their
Powers in Painting. London: Tilt.
Fiorentini, A., and Lee, B. B. 2000. Tobias Mayer’s On the relationship between colors.
Color Research and Application 25:66–74.
Fludd, R. 1629–31. Medicina catholica, 2 vols. Frankfurt: C. Rötelli.
Forsius, S. A. 1611. Physica, Codex Holmiensis D. 6, Royal Library, Stockholm.
Foss, C. E. 1947. Representations of color space and their applications. American
Ceramic Society Bulletin 27:55–63.
Foss, C. E. 1978. Space lattice used to sample the color space of the Committee on
Uniform Color Scales of the Optical Society of America. Journal of the Optical
Society of America 68:1616–1619.
Foss, C. E., Nickerson, D., and Granville, W. C. 1944. Analysis of the Ostwald color
system. Journal of the Optical Society of America 34:361–382.
Freeman, K. 1957. Ancilla to the pre-Socratic Philosophers. Cambridge: Harvard
University Press.
Friele, L. F. C. 1961. Analysis of the Brown and Brown-MacAdam colour discrimina-
tion data. Die Farbe 10:193–202.
Friele, L. F. C. 1965. Further analysis of colour discrimination data. Proceedings of the
International Colour Meeting (Lucerne 1965). Göttingen: Musterschmidt, 1966.
Friele, L. F. C. 1978. Fine color metric (FCM). Color Research and Application 3:53–64.
Frisch, J. C. 1788. Ueber eine harmonische Farben-Tonleiter, und die Wirkungen und
Verhältnisse der Farben im Colorite. Monatsschrift der Akademie der Künste und
mechanischen Wissenschaften zu Berlin 2(8):58–77.
Fullerton, G. S., and Cattell, J. K. 1892. On the Perception of Small Differences. Philadel-
phia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Gage, J. 1993. Color and Culture. Boston: Little, Brown.
Gage, J. 1999. Color and Meaning. Berkley: University of California Press.
Gavel, J. 1979. Colour, A Study of its Position in the Art Theory of the Quattro- &
Cinquecento. Stockholm: Almquist and Wiksell.
Gelb, A. 1929. Die “Farbenkonstanz” der Sehdinge. In W. A. von Bethe, ed., Handbuch
der normalen und pathologischen Sehphysiologie, Vol 12:594–678. Berlin: Springer.
Gerritsen, F. 1984. Entwicklung der Farbenlehre. Göttingen: Muster-Schmidt.
Gerstner, K. 1986. The forms of color. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Gilchrist, A. L., ed. 1994. Lightness, Brightness, and Transparency. Hillsdale NJ:
Erlbaum.
Gilchrist, A. L., Kossyfidis, C., Agostini, T., Bonato, F., Cataliotti, J., Li, X., Spehar, B.,
Annan, V., and Economou, E. 1999. An anchoring theory of lightness perception.
Psychological Review 106:795–834.
382 REFERENCES
Gibson, K. S., and Nickerson, D. 1940. An analysis of the Munsell Color System based
on measurements made in 1919 and 1926. Journal of the Optical Society of America
30:591–608.
Gibson, K. S., and Tyndall, E. P. T. 1923. Visibility of radiant energy. Bulletin Bureau of
Standards 19:131.
Giulianini, F., and Eskew Jr., R. T. 1998. Chromatic masking in the (DL/L, DM/M) plane
of constant-contrast space reveals only two detection mechanisms. Vision Research
38:3913–3926.
Glasser, L. G., McKinney, A. H., Reilly, C. D., and Schnelle, P. D. 1958. Cube-root color
coordinate system. Journal of the Optical Society of America 48:736–740.
Glenn, J. T., and Killian, J. T. 1940. Trichromatic analysis of the Munsell Book of Color.
Journal of the Optical Society of America 30:609–616.
Glisson, F. 1677. Tractatus de ventriculo et intestinis. London: Brome.
Godlove, I. H. 1933. Neutral value scales: II. Comparison of results and equations
describing value scales. Journal of the Optical Society of America 23:419–425.
Godlove, I. H. 1951. Improved color-difference formula, with applications to the
perceptibility and acceptability of fadings. Journal of the Optical Society of America
41:760–772.
Goethe, J. W. v. 1810. Zur Farbenlehre. Tübingen: Cotta.
Granville, W. C. 1994. The Color Harmony Manual. Color Research and Application
19:77–98.
Grassmann, H. G. 1853. Zur Theorie der Farbenmischung, Poggendorff’s Annalen der
Physik und Chemie 29:69–84.
Green, D. M., and Swets, J. A. 1966. Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics. New
York: Wiley.
Greene, B. 1999. The Elegant Universe. New York: Norton.
Guan, S. S., and Luo, M. R. 1999. A colour-difference formula for assessing large colour
differences. Color Research and Application 24: 344–355.
Guild, J. 1931. The colorimetric properties of the spectrum. Philosophical Transactions
of the Royal Society (London) A230:149–187.
Guilford, J. P. 1932. A generalized psychological law. Psychological Review 39:73–
85.
Guth, S. L. 1972. A new vector model. In J. J. Vos, L. F. C. Friele and P. L. Walraven,
eds., Color Metrics. Soesterberg: Institute for Perception TNO, pp. 82–98.
Guth, S. L. 1991. Model for color vision and light adaptation. Journal of the Optical
Society of America A8:976–993.
Guth, S. L. 2001. ATD01 model for color appearances and differences. Proceedings AIC
Color 01. Bellingham, WA: SPIE, 2002.
Hall, M. 1992. Color and Meaning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hård, A., and Sivik, L. 1981. NCS-Natural Color System: A Swedish standard for color
notation. Color Research and Application 6:129–138.
Hård, A., Sivik, L., and Tonnquist, G. 1996. NCS, Natural Color System—From concept
to research and applications. Part I. Color Research and Applications 21:180–205.
Part II. Color Research and Applications 21:206–220.
Hardin, C. L. 1988. Color for Philosophers. Indianapolis IN: Hackett.
REFERENCES 383
Indow, T. 1999b. Predictions based on Munsell notation: II. Principal hue components.
Color Research and Application 24:19–32.
Indow, T. 1999c. Principal hue curves and color difference. Color Research and Appli-
cation 24:266–279.
Indow, T. 2001. Uniformities in OSA-UCS and in NCS tested by color difference pre-
diction based on principal hue components. Proceedings AIC Color 01. Bellingham,
WA: SPIE, 2002.
Indow, T. 2002. Color differences predicted by color component differences. Color
Research and Application 27:425–429.
Indow, T., and Aoki, N. 1983. Multidimensional mapping of 178 Munsell colors. Color
Research and Application 8:145–152.
Indow, T., and Ohsumi, K. 1972. Multidimensional mapping of sixty Munsell colors by
non-metric procedure. In J. J. Vos, L. F. C. Friele and P. L. Walraven, eds., Color
Metrics. Soesterberg, Netherlands: Institute for Perception.
Indow, T., and Morrison, M. L. 1991. Construction of discrimination ellipsoids for
surface colors by the method of constant stimuli. Color Research and Application
16:42–56.
Ives, H. E. 1912. Studies in the photometry of lights of different colours: I. Spectral
luminosity curves obtained by the equality of brightness photometer and flicker
photometer under similar conditions. Philosophical Magazine Series 6, 24:149–188.
Jacobson, E., Granville, W. C., and Foss, C. E. 1942. Color Harmony Manual. Chicago:
Container Corporation of America.
Jaeckel, S. M. 1975. The HATRA data on colour differences and visual colour passing.
Journal of the Society of Dyers and Colourists 91:242.
Jain, A. K. 1972. Color distance and geodesics in color 3-space. Journal of the Optical
Society of America 62:1287–1291.
Jameson, D. 1972. Theoretical issues of color vision. In D. Jameson and L. M. Hurvich,
eds., Handbook of Sensory Physiology, Vol 7/4 Visual Psychophysics. Berlin:
Springer.
Jameson, D., and Hurvich, L. M. 1964. Theory of brightness and color contrast in human
vision. Vision Research 4:135–154.
Jameson, K. A., Highnote, M. S., and Wasserman, L. M. 2001. Richer color experience
in observers with multiple photopigment opsin genes. Psychonomic Bulletin and
Review 8:244–261.
Jastrow, J. 1888. Critique of psychophysical methods. American Journal of Psychology
1:271–309.
Johannsen, T. 1937. Färg. Stockholm: Lindfors.
Jones, L. A. 1917. The fundamental scale of pure hue and retinal sensibility to hue
differences. Journal of the Optical Society of America 1:63–77.
Jordan, C., and Mollon, J. D. 1993. A study of women heterozygous for color deficien-
cies. Vision Research 33:1495–1508.
Judd, D. B. 1932. Chromaticity sensibility to stimulus differences. Journal of the Optical
Society of America 22:72–108.
Judd, D. B. 1935. A Maxwell triangle yielding uniform chromaticity scales. Journal of
the Optical Society of America 25:24–35.
REFERENCES 385
König, A. 1886. Über die neuere Entwicklung von Thomas Young’s Farbentheorie (On
the more recent development of Thomas Young’s theory of color). In Gesammelte
Abhandlungen zur physiologischen Optik. Leipzig: Barth, 1903.
König, A. 1891. Ueber den Helligkeitswert der Spectralfarben bei verschiedener
absoluter Intensität. In Beiträge zur Psychologie und Physiologie der Sinnesorgane.
Hamburg: Voss, pp. 309–388.
König, A. 1892. Die Grundempfindungen in normalen und anomalen Farbensystemen
und ihre Intensitätsvertheilung im Spectrum (In Gemeinschaft mit Conrad
Dieterici). Zeitschrift für die Physiologie und Psychologie der Sinnesorgane
4:241–347.
König, A., and Dietrici, C. 1884, Über die Empfindlichkeit des normalen Auges für
Wellenlängenunterschiede des Lichtes. Annalen der Physik und Chemie 22:579–
589.
König, A., and Brodhun, E. 1888–89. Experimentelle Untersuchungen über die psy-
chophysische Fundamentalformel in Bezug auf den Gesichtssinn. Sitzungsberichte
der preussischen Akademie der Wissenschaften 1888:917–931; 1889:641–644.
König, A., and Dietrici, C. 1892. Die Grundempfindungen in normalen und anomalen
Farbsystemen und ihre Intensitätsvertheilung im Spectrum. Zeitschrift für Psy-
chologie und Physiologie der Sinnesorgane 4:241–347.
Krantz, D. H., Luce, R. D., Suppes, P., and Tversky, A. 1971. Foundations of Measure-
ment, Vol. 1: Additive and Polynomial Representations. New York: Academic Press,
1971. Vol. 2: Geometrical, Threshold, and Probabilistic Representations (Suppes,
Krantz, Luce, and Tversky. Academic Press, 1989). Vol. 3: Representation, Axiomati-
zation, and Invariance (Luce, Krantz, Suppes, and Tversky. Academic Press, 1990).
Krauskopf, J. 1999. Higher order color mechanisms. In K. R. Gegenfurtner and L. T.
Sharpe, eds. Color vision. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Krauskopf, J., and Gegenfurtner, K. 1992. Color discrimination and adaptation. Vision
Research 32:2165–2175.
Kries, J. von. 1882. Die Gesichtsempfindungen und ihre Analyse. Archiv für Anatomie,
Physiologie und wissenschaftliche Medicin (Suppl. Physiologische Abteilung): 1–178.
Kuehni, R. G. 1971a. Acceptability contours and small color difference formulas.
Journal of Color and Appearance 1(1):30–35,42.
Kuehni, R. G. (chairman). 1971b. Acceptability contours of selected textile matches in
color space. Textile Chemist and Colorist 3:248–255.
Kuehni, R. G. 1972. Color difference and objective acceptability evaluation. Journal of
Color and Appearance 1(3):4–10,15.
Kuehni, R. G. 1975. Visual and instrumental determination of small color differences.
Journal of the Society of Dyers and Colourists 91:68–71.
Kuehni, R. G. 1982. Advances in color difference formulas. Color Research and Appli-
cation 7:19–23.
Kuehni, R. G. 1999. Towards an improved uniform color space. Color Research and
Application 24:253–265.
Kuehni, R. G. 2000a. Uniform color space modeled with cone responses. Color Research
and Application 25:56–63.
Kuehni, R. G. 2000b. A comparison of five color order systems. Color Research and
Application 25:123–131.
REFERENCES 387
Kuehni, R. G. 2000c. A structural comparison of the Munsell Renotation and the OSA-
UCS uniform color systems. Color Research and Application 25:186–192.
Kuehni, R. G. 2000d. An opponent-color model for the Sanders-Wyszecki Helmholtz-
Kohlrausch effect data set. Color Research and Application 25:292–293.
Kuehni, R. G. 2001a. Determination of unique hues using Munsell color chips. Color
Research and Application 26:61–66.
Kuehni, R. G. 2001b. Analysis of five sets of color difference data. Color Research and
Application 26: 141–150.
Kuehni, R. G. 2001c. Color space and its divisions. Color Research and Application
26:209–222.
Kuehni, R. G. 2001d. From color matching error to large color differences. Color
Research and Application 26:384–393.
Kuehni, R. G. 2001e. “Uniform” color space is not homogeneous. Proceedings AIC
Color 01. Bellingham, WA: SPIE, 2002.
Kuehni, R. G. 2002. The early development of the Munsell system. Color Research and
Application 27:20–27.
Kuehni, R. G., and Marcus, R. T. 1979. An experiment in visual scaling of small color
differences. Color Research and Application 4:81–91.
Kuehni, R. G., and Stanziola, R. 2002. Francis Glisson’s color specification system of
1677. Color Research and Application 27:15–19.
Küppers, H. 1968. Das Grundgesetz der Farbenlehre. Köln: DuMont.
Lambert, J. H. 1760. Photometria, sive de mensura et gradibus luminis, colorum et umbra.
Augsburg.
Lambert, J. H. 1772. Beschreibung einer mit dem Caulauischen Wachse ausgemalten
Farbenpyramide. Berlin: Haude und Spener.
Lang, H. 1980. Tobias Mayers Abhandlung über die Verwandtschaft der Farben 1758.
Die Farbe 28:1–34.
Larimer, J., Krantz, D. H., and Cicerone, C. M. 1974. Opponent-process additivity. I.
Red/green equilibria. Vision Research 14:1127–1140.
Larimer, J., Krantz, D. H., and Cicerone, C. M. 1975. Opponent-process additivity. II.
Yellow/blue equilibria and nonlinear models. Vision Research 15:723–731.
Le Blon, J. C. 1725. Coloritto or the harmony of colouring in painting reduced to mechan-
ical practice. London.
Lee, B. B. 1998. Receptors, channels and color in primate retina. In W. G. K. Backhaus,
R. Kliegl, J. S. Werner, eds., Color Vision. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.
LeGrand, Y. 1949. Les seuils différentiels de couleurs dans la theorie de Young. Revue
d’Optique 28:261–278. Translated in: Color Research and Application 1994;19:
296–309.
Lennie, P. 1999. Color coding in the cortex. In K. R. Gegenfurtner and L. T. Sharpe,
eds., Color Vision. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lenz, R., Österberg, M., Hiltunen, J., Jaaskelainen, T., and Parkkinen, J. 1996. Unsu-
pervised filtering of color spectra. Journal of the Optical Society of America A13:
1315–1324.
Long, J., and Luke, J. T. 2001. The New Munsell Student Color Set. Lebanon, PA: Maple
Press.
388 REFERENCES
Lotto, R. B., and Purves, D. 1999. The effect of color on brightness. Nature Neuroscience
2:1010–1014.
Lotto, R. B., and Purves, D. 2002. A rationale for the structure of color space. Trends
in Neuroscience 25:84–89..
Lübbe, E. 1999. Empfindungsgemässe Farbbeschreibung unter Berücksichtigung des
Umfeldes. Dissertation. Aachen: Shaker Verlag.
Lucretius. 1968. De rerum natura (The way things are). Transl. R. Humphries.
Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Luke, J. T. 2002. Color Cleaver® software. Marketed by Michael Wilcox School of
Colour, Bristol, England.
Luneburg, R. K. 1950. The metric of binocular visual space. Journal of the Optical
Society of America 40:627–642.
Luther, R. 1927. Aus dem Gebiete der Farbreizmetrik. Zeitschrift für technische Physik
8:540–558.
Luo, M. R., and Rigg, B. 1986. Chromaticity-discrimination ellipses for surface colours.
Color Research and Application 11:25–42.
Luo, M. R., and Rigg, B. 1987. BFD (l:c) colour-difference formula, Part I – Develop-
ment of the formula. Journal of the Society of Dyers and Colourists 103:86–94. Part
II – Performance of the formula. Journal of the Society of Dyers and Colourists
103:126–132.
Luo, M. R., Cui, G., and Rigg, B. 2001. The development of the CIE 2000 colour-
difference formula: CIEDE2000. Color Research and Application 26:340–350.
MacAdam, D. L. 1935. Maximum visual efficiency of colored materials. Journal of the
Optical Society of America 25:361–367.
MacAdam, D. L. 1937. Projective transformations of I.C.I. color specifications. Journal
of the Optical Society of America 27:294–299.
MacAdam, D. L. 1942. Visual sensitivities to color differences in daylight. Journal of
the Optical Society of America 32:247–274.
MacAdam, D. L. 1947. Note on the number of distinct chromaticities. Journal of the
Optical Society of America 37:308–309.
MacAdam, D. L. 1965. Specification of color differences. Acta Chromatica 1:147–156.
MacAdam, D. L. 1974. Uniform color scales. Journal of the Optical Society of America
64:1691–1702.
MacAdam, D. L. 1978. Colorimetric data for samples of OSA uniform color scales.
Journal of the Optical Society of America 68:121–130.
MacAdam, D. L. 1981. Color Measurement. Berlin: Springer.
MacAdam, D. L. 1990. Redetermination of colors for uniform scales. Journal of the
Optical Society of America A7:113–115.
MacAdam, D. L. 1995. Letter to Joy Turner Luke of March 22, 1995, with permission
of the recipient.
Mahy, M., Van Eycken, L., and Oosterlinck, A. 1994. Evaluation of uniform color spaces
developed after the adoption of CIELAB and CIELUV. Color Research and
Application 19:105–121.
Maloney, L. T. 1999. Physics-based approaches to modeling surface color perception.
In K. R. Gegenfurtner and L. T. Sharpe, eds., Color Vision. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
REFERENCES 389
Mandelstamm, E. 1867. Beitrag zur Physiologie der Farben. Archiv für Ophtalmologie
13:399–406.
Mansfeld, J. 1986. Die Vorsokratiker I and II. Stuttgart: Reclam.
Marks, L. E. 1978. The Unity of the Senses: Interrelations among the Modalities. New
York: Academic Press.
Marks, L. E., and Algom, D. 1998. Psychophysical scaling. In M. H. Birnbaum, ed.,
Measurement, Judgment, and Decision Making. New York: Academic Press.
Masson, V. 1845. Étude de photométrie électrique. Annales de chimie et de physique
14:129–195.
Mausfeld, R. 1998. Color perception: from Grassmann codes to a dual code for object
and illumination colors. In W. G. K. Backhaus, R. Kliegl and J. S. Werner, eds., Color
Vision. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.
Mayer, T. 1775. Opera inedita Tobiae Mayeri; ed. G. C. Lichtenberg. Göttingen.
Maxwell, J. C. 1856. On the theory of colours in relation to colour-blindness. Transac-
tions of the Royal Scottish Society of Arts 4:394–400.
Maxwell, J. C. 1860. Theory of compound colours, and the relations of the colours of
the spectrum. Proceedings Royal Society (London) 10:57–84.
McDonald, R. 1974. The effect of non-uniformity in the ANLAB colour space on the
interpretation of visual colour differences. Journal of the Society of Dyers and
Colourists 90:189–198.
McDonald, R. 1980. Industrial pass-fail colour matching: Part I. Preparation of visual
colour-matching data. Journal of the Society of Dyers and Colourists 96: 372–376.
Part II. Methods of fitting tolerance ellipsoids. Ibid. 96:418–433. Part III. Develop-
ment of pass-fail formula for use with instrumental measurement of colour differ-
ence. Ibid. 96:486–495.
McLaren, K. 1971. Multiple linear regression: A new technique for improving colour-
difference formulae. Color Metrics. Soesterberg: AIC, pp. 296–307.
McLaren, K. 1976. An introduction to instrumental shade passing and sorting and a
review of recent developments. Journal of the Society of Dyers and Colourists
92:317–326.
McLaughlin, B. P. 2002. Color, consciousness, and color consciousness. In Q.
Smith and A. Jokic, eds., Aspects of Consciousness. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
McMahon, E. 1956. Leonardo da Vinci: Treatise on Painting Codex Urbinas Latinus
1270. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Melgosa, M., Hita, E., Romero, J., and Jiménez del Barco, L. 1992. Some classical color
differences calculated with new formulas. Journal of the Optical Society of America
A9:1247–1254.
Melgosa, M., Hita, E., Poza, A. J., Alman, D. H., and Berns, R. S. 1997. Suprathreshold
color-difference ellipsoids for surface colors. Color Research and Application
22:148–155.
Melgosa, M., Rivas, M. J., Hita, E., and Viénot, F. 2000. Are we able to distinguish color
attributes? Color Research and Application 25:356–367.
Merrifield, M. P. 1967. Medieval and Renaissance Treatises on the Art of Painting. New
York: Dover.
Müller, A. E. 1963. Swiss Color Atlas 2541. Winterthur: Chromos.
390 REFERENCES
Nickerson, D. 1940. History of the Munsell color system and its scientific application.
Journal of the Optical Society of America 30:575–586.
Nickerson, D. 1969. History of the Munsell color system. Color Engineering 7(5):42–51.
Nickerson, D. 1976. History of the Munsell color system, company, and foundation.
Color Research and Application 1:7–10.
Nickerson, D. 1977. History of the OSA Committee on Uniform Color Scales. Optics
News, Winter 1977:8–17.
Nickerson, D. 1978. Munsell Renotations for samples of OSA uniform color scales.
Journal of the Optical Society of America 68:1343–1347.
Nickerson, D., and Newhall, S. M. 1943. A psychological color solid. Journal of the
Optical Society of America 33:419–422.
Nickerson, D., and Stultz, K. F. 1944. Color tolerance specification. Journal of the Optical
Society of America 34:550–570.
Norwich, K. H. 1993. Information, Sensation, and Perception. New York:Academic Press.
Nyberg, N. D. 1929. Zum Aufbau des Farbkörpers im Raume aller Lichtempfindungen.
Zeitschrift für technische Physik 52:406–411.
Oleari, C. 1999. Inter-observer comparison of color-matching functions. Color Research
and Application 24:177–184.
Oleari, C. 2001. Comparisons between color space scales, uniform-color-scale atlases,
and color-difference formulas. Color Research and Application 26:351–361.
O’Regan, J. K., and Noë, A. 2001. A sensorimotor account of vision and visual con-
sciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24:912–1031.
OSA Uniform Color Scales. 1977. Washington: Optical Society of America.
Osgood, C. E. 1953. Method and Theory in Experimental Psychology. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Ostwald, W. 1918. Die Farbenlehre: Vol. 1. Mathetische Farbenlehre. Leipzig: Unesma.
Ostwald, W. 1919. Der Farbkörper. Leipzig: Unesma.
Palmer, G. 1777. Theory of Colours and Vision. London: Leacroft.
Palmer, S. E. 1999. Vision Science, Photons to Phenomenology. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Parkhurst, C. 1990. Roger Bacon on color. In K. L. Selig and E. Sears, eds., The Verbal
and the Visual, New York: Italica Press.
Parkhurst, C., and Feller, R. L. 1982. Who invented the color wheel? Color Research
and Application 7:217–230.
Photius. Bibliothèque, 8 vols., transl. R. Henry. Paris: Société d’éditions Les Belles
Lettres, 1959–1991.
Plateau, J. A. F. 1872. Sur la mesure des sensations physiques, et sur la loi qui lie
l’intensité et ces sensations a la cause excitante. Bulletins de l’Académie Royales des
Sciences, des Lettres et des Beaux-Arts de Belgique 33:376–388.
Plato. The Collected Dialogues, eds. E. Hamilton and H. Cairns. Princeton: Princeton
University Press, 1961.
Pointer, M. R., and Attridge, G. G. 1997. Some aspects of the visual scaling of large
colour differences. Color Research and Application 22:298–307.
Pointer, M. R., and Attridge, G. G. 1998. The number of discernible colours. Color
Research and Application 23:52–54.
392 REFERENCES
Pointer, M. R., and Attridge, G. G. 2000. Some aspects of the visual scaling of large
colour differences–II. Color Research and Application 25:116–122.
Pokorny, J., Smith, V. C., and Starr, S. J. 1976. Variability of color mixture data: II The
effect of viewing field size on the unit coordinates. Vision Research 16:1095–1098.
Polyak, S. 1957. The Vertebrate Visual System. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Pope, A. 1929. The Painter’s Terms. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Porphyry. Isagoge, Porphyry the Phoenician, trans. E. W. Warren. Toronto: Pontifical
Institute of Mediaeval Studies, 1975.
Priest, I. G., Gibson, K. S., and McNicholas, H. J. 1920. An examination of the Munsell
color system. I. Spectral and total reflection and the Munsell scale of value. U.S.
National Bureau of Standards Technical Paper 167.
Priest, I. G., and Brickwedde, F. G. 1938. The perceptible colorimetric purity as a
function of dominant wavelength. Journal of the Optical Society of America
28:133–139.
Purves, D., Lotto, B., and Polger, T. 2000. Color vision and the four-color-map problem.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12:233–237.
Purves, D., and Lotto, R. B. 2002. Why We See What We Do: Evidence for a Wholly
Probabilistic Strategy of Vision. Sunderland: Sinauer Associates.
Qiao, Y., Berns, R. S., Reniff, L., and Montag, E. 1998. Visual determination of hue
suprathreshold color-difference tolerances. Color Research and Application 23:302–
313.
Ramanath, R., Kuehni, R., Snyder, W. E., and Hinks, D. 2003. Spectral spaces and color
spaces. Color Research and Application 28: forthcoming.
Rich, R. M., Billmeyer, F. W., and Howe, W. G. 1975. Method for deriving color-
difference-perceptibility ellipses for surface-color samples. Journal of the Optical
Society of America 65:956–959.
Richter, K. 1985. Farbempfindungsmerkmal Elementarbuntton und Buntheitsabstände
als Funktion von Farbart und Leuchdichte von In- und Umfeld. Bremerhaven:
Wirtschaftsverlag.
Richter, K. 1980. Cube-root color spaces and chromatic adaptation. Color Research and
Application 5:25–43.
Richter, K. 1996. Computergrafik und Farbmetrik. Berlin: VDE Verlag.
Richter, M. 1976. Einführung in die Farbmetrik. Berlin: Walter de Gruiter.
Richter, M., and Witt, K. 1986. The story of the DIN color system. Color Research and
Application 11:138–145.
Ridgway, R. 1912. Color Standards and Color Nomenclature. Washington: published by
the author.
Robertson, A. R. 1978. CIE guidelines for coordinated research on colour-difference
evaluation. Color Research and Application 3:149–151.
Robinson, F. D. 1962. Acceptability of color matches. Journal of the Oil Colour Chemist
Association 52:15–45.
Rohner, E., and Rich, D. C. 1996. Eine angenähert gleichförmige Farbabstandsformel
für industrielle Farbtoleranzen von Körperfarben. Die Farbe 42:207–220.
Rood, N. O. 1879. Modern Chromatics. New York: Appleton.
Rösch, S. 1928. Zur Kennzeichnung der Farben. Physikalische Zeitschrift 29:83–91.
REFERENCES 393
Strocka, D. 1971. Color difference formulas and visual acceptability. Applied Optics 10:
1308–1313.
Strohmeyer, C. F., Chaparro, A., Rodriguez, C., Chen, D., Hu, E., and Kronauer, R. E.
1998. Short-wave cone signal in the red-green detection mechanism. Vision Research
38:813–826.
Stumpf, C. 1917. Die Attribute der Gesichtsempfindungen. Abhandlungen der königlich
preussischen Akademie der Wissenschaften. Philosophisch-historische Klasse 8.
Berlin: Verlag der Königlichen Akademie der Wissenschaften.
Sugiyama, Y., and Fukuda, T. 1959. Comparison of different U.C.S. by using MacAdam
ellipses and Munsell scales. Journal of Applied Physics (Japan) 28:757–766.
Svaetichin, G. 1956. Spectral response curves from single cones. Acta Physiologica Scan-
dinavica 134:17–46.
Swedish Standard SS 01 91 03. 1982. CIE tristimulus values and chromaticity
coordinates for the colour samples in SS 01 91 02. Stockholm: Swedish Standards
Institution.
Takasaki, H. 1966. Lightness change of grays induced by change in reflectance of gray
background. Journal of the Optical Society of America 56:504–509.
Takasaki, H. 1967. Chromatic changes induced by changes in chromaticity of
background of constant lightness. Journal of the Optical Society of America 57:93–
96.
Teller, D. A., and Pugh, E. N. 1983. Linking propositions in color vision. In J. D. Mollon
and L. T. Sharpe, eds., Colour Vision: Physiology and Psychophysics, London:
Academic Press, pp. 577–589.
Telesio, A. 1529. Antonii Thylesii Cosentini libellus de coloribus. Venice.
Teufel, H. J., and Wehrhahn, C. 2000. Evidence for the contribution of S cones to the
detection of flicker brightness and red-green. Journal of the Optical Society of
America A17:994–1006.
Theophilus, The Various Arts, ed. and transl. C. R. Dodwell. Oxford: Clarendon Press,
1986.
Thomsen, K. 2000. A euclidean color space in high agreement with the CIE94 color
difference formula. Color Research and Application 25:64–65.
Thurner, K., and Walther, V. 1970. Untersuchungen zur Korrelation von Farbabstands-
bewertungen auf visuellen Wegen und über Farbabstandsformeln. Proceedings
of the International Color Meeting “Color 69,” Vol. 2, pp. 671–687. Göttingen:
Musterschmidt.
Thurstone, L. L. 1927. Psychophysical analysis. American Journal of Psychology
38:368–389.
Titchener, E. B. 1896. Outline of Psychology. New York: Macmillan.
Tonnquist, G. 1966. A comparison between symmetrical and equi-spaced hue circles.
Die Farbe 15:376–388.
Torgerson, W. S. 1958. Theory and Methods of Scaling. New York: Wiley.
Treviranus, G. R. 1828. Beiträge zur Anatomie und Physiologie der Sinneswerkzeuge des
Menschen und der Thiere. Bremen: Heyse.
Trezona, P. W., and Parkins, R. P. 1998. Derivation of the 1964 colorimetric standards.
Color Research and Application 23:221–225.
396 REFERENCES
Webster, M. A., Miyahara, E., Malkoc, G., and Raker, V. E. 2000. Variations in normal
color vision. II. Unique hues. Journal of the Optical Society of America A17:1545–
1555.
White, M. 1981. The effect of the nature of the surround on the perceived lightness of
gray bars within square-wave test gratings. Perception 10:215–230.
Whittle, P. 1992. Whiteness, discriminability and the “crispening” effect. Vision Research
32:1493–1507.
Wiesel, T. N., and Hubel, D. H. 1966. Spatial and chromatic interactions in the lateral
geniculate body of the rhesus monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology 29:1115–1156.
Witt, K. 1981. Der Farbenraum nach DIN6164 in neuer Sicht. Die Farbe 29:17–52.
Witt, K. 1987. Three-dimensional threshold of color-difference perceptibility in painted
samples: variability of observers in four CIE color regions. Color Research and
Application 12:128–134.
Witt, K. 1990. Parametric effects on surface color-difference evaluation at threshold.
Color Research and Application 15:189–199.
Witt, K. 1999. Geometric relations between scales of small colour differences. Color
Research and Application 24:78–92.
Wright, W. D. 1928–29. A re-determination of the mixture curves of the spectrum.
Transactions of the Optical Society London 30:141–164.
Wright, W. D., and Pitt, F. H. G. 1934. Hue discrimination on normal colour-vision.
Proceedings of the Physical Society (London) 46:459–454.
Wright, W. D. 1941. The sensitivity of the eye to small colour differences. Proceedings
of the Physical Society (London) 53:93–112.
Wright, W. D. 1969. The Measurement of Colour, 4th ed. New York: Van Nostrand
Reinhold.
Wright, W. D. 1996. The origins of the 1939 CIE system. In P. K. Kaiser and
R. M. Boynton, Color Vision, second ed., Washington, DC: Optical Society of
America.
Wuerger, S. M., Maloney, L. T., and Krauskopf, J. 1995. Proximity judgments in color
space: Test of a euclidean color geometry. Vision Research 35:827–835.
Wuerschmidt, J. 1914. Dietrich von Freiberg, Ueber den Regenbogen. In C. Bauemker,
ed., Beiträge zur Geschichte der Philosophie des Mittelalters, Vol. 12. Münster:
Aschendorff.
Wundt, W. 1874. Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie. Leipzig: Engelmann.
Wundt, W. M. 1880. Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie. Leipzig: Engelmann.
Wundt, W. 1892. Vorlesungen ueber die Menschen- und Thierseele, zweite Auflage.
Leipzig: Voss.
Wundt, W. 1896. Grundriss der Psychologie. Leipzig: Alfred Körner.
Wyszecki, G. 1953. Valenzmetrische Untersuchung des Zusammenhanges zwischen
normaler und anomaler Trichromasie. Die Farbe 2:39–52.
Wyszecki, G. 1954. A regular rhombohedral lattice sampling of Munsell renotation
space. Journal of the Optical Society of America 44:725–734.
Wyszecki, G. 1960. Farbsysteme. Göttingen: Musterschmidt-Verlag.
Wyszecki, G. 1963. Proposal for a new color-difference formula. Journal of the Optical
Society of America 53:1318–1319.
398 REFERENCES
Permission to reproduce certain figures and text was obtained from the fol-
lowing copyright holders and is gratefully acknowledged:
Color Space and Its Divisions: Color Order from Antiquity to the Present, by Rolf G. Kuehni
ISBN 0-471-32670-4 Copyright © 2003 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc. 399
400 CREDITS
Figure 8-12: reprinted from Vision Research 32, 1992, p. 2171, J. Krauskopf and
K. R. Gegenfurtner, Color discrimination and adaptation. All materials used
with permission from Elsevier Science Inc.
Figure 6-12 reprinted from Color Research and Application 26 (2001) p. 346,
M. R. Luo, G. Cui, and B. Rigg, The development of the CIE 2000 colour-
difference formula.
Figures 6-19 and 6-20 reprinted from Color Research and Application 15
(1990) p. 33, S. A. Burns, J. B. Cohen, and E. N. Kuznetsov, The Munsell color
system in fundamental color space.
Figures 7-26 and 7-27 reprinted from Color Research and Application 21
(1996) pp. 189 and 194 respectively, A. Hård, L. Sivik, and G. Tonnquist, NCS
Natural Color System, Part I.
Figure 7-28 reprinted from Color Research and Application 11 (1986) p. 147.
G. Derefeldt and C. Sahlin, Transformation of NCS data into CIELAB colour
space. All materials used with permission of John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Index
Color Space and Its Divisions: Color Order from Antiquity to the Present, by Rolf G. Kuehni
ISBN 0-471-32670-4 Copyright © 2003 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc. 403
404 INDEX