Secular Group Affiliates

Download as doc, pdf, or txt
Download as doc, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 36

Patterns of thought and attitude among secular group affiliates in the Pacific Northwest

Paper delivered at the annual meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of Religion
Tampa, Florida, November 4, 2007

© Frank L. Pasquale
Research Associate, ISSSC, Trinity College, Hartford, CT
[email protected]

Abstract. Distinctions among types of religiosity and religious identification are many and
detailed. Research that compares and contrasts individuals who consider themselves secular or
nonreligious, or who hold affirmatively or substantially naturalistic worldviews, however, is
largely uncharted territory. These are customarily treated as a homogeneous group with which
to compare the religious. Based on a survey of 911 members of secularist, humanist, atheist,
skeptic, and freethought groups in Oregon, Washington, and British Columbia, varieties of
“secularity,” from “hard” to “soft,” are explored with respect to metaphysical beliefs, attitudes
toward “religion,” self-descriptions, and use and meanings of “spiritual/ity.”

Despite considerable attention devoted to the question of secularization in the past half
century or more, the study of forms of “secularism” or “secularity” as a coherent focus of
inquiry has barely begun (Kosmin and Keysar, 2007). Social scientific study of people’s
existential and metaphysical worldviews, or “meaning systems”1 (Hood, Hill, and Williamson,
2005), has customarily been approached from the vantage of religion and religiosity. This has
resulted in a detailed picture of religious individuals and forms, but comparatively little detail
in our understanding of people at the other end of the metaphysical spectrum, such as those
who may be characterized as substantially or affirmatively nonreligious, irreligious, secular,
non-transcendental,2 or philosophically naturalistic (Pasquale, 2007a, b). They have generally
been treated, as J. Russell Hale once said of the unchurched, “as if. . .they were one
homogeneous category” (1980: 97). Glenn Vernon’s observation, nearly forty years ago that
the image of the “atheist, agnostic, and nonbeliever. . .is blurry and indistinct” (1968: 220)
remains true today.
There are signs of growing interest in secularism, atheism, and related subjects, as
evidenced by Bainbridge, 2005, Beit-Hallahmi, 2007, Hunsberger and Altemeyer, 2006,
Kosmin and Keysar, 2007, and Zuckerman, 2007. But the differences, as well as shared
characteristics, among self-described secularists, (religious) skeptics, and philosophical
naturalists have remained largely unexplored.

Given the lack of coherent attention to the “secular” end the metaphysical spectrum,

1
we are faced with a swarm of terms and concepts with variable clarity or consistency of
meaning and use. There are many topics in the research literature that bear, directly or
indirectly, on the subject, such as the unchurched, “nones,” and religious “disaffiliation”
(“apostates,” “defection,” “outswitchers,” “dropouts”). Relevant descriptive terms are many:
skeptic, unbeliever, nonbeliever, irreligious, nonreligious, secularist, humanist, agnostic,
rationalist, freethinker, atheist, naturalist, nontheist, non-supernaturalist, non-transcendentalist,
monist, and materialist, among others. Order may come to this array of terms as the nuances of
their use and the characteristics they represent become clearer as research proceeds.
For the sake of clarity here, (philosophical) “naturalism”/“naturalist(ic)” and “this-
worldly” will be used to denote ways of thinking that substantially or affirmatively reject or
avoid “other-worldly” (i.e., supernatural or ontologically transcendental) ideas or phenomena.
“Transcendent” and “transcendental” (and their negation) will, unless otherwise noted, be used
in an ontological sense, and so, correspond to “other-worldly” (or “this-worldly”). “Secular”
refers more broadly to individuals and organizations that distance themselves from established
religious traditions or institutions, or that dismiss or de-emphasize references to theistic or
supernatural ideas (but may be naturalistic to varying degrees). “Secularity” will refer to the
degree to which such a tendency may be said to describe individuals’ meaning systems and
related behavior. Following Kosmin (2007) “secularism” will be reserved for reference to
political ideology concerning the relationship—and institutional separation—between religion
and government.
Since this is largely terra incognita, the approach taken in this study was exploratory
and deliberately a-theoretical. The aim was to cast a net that was wide enough in scope, but
fine enough in weave, to identify both shared and distinctive characteristics in a sample of
individuals who are substantially or affirmatively “secular” (for lack of a better summary
term). Nonetheless, a distinction suggested by Steinfels (2006) and Kosmin (2007) is useful—
between “soft” and “hard” secularity and secularism. As Kosmin writes (2007: 6-7):
The soft secularist. . .is neither a convinced Atheist nor a principled materialist,
and may not be hostile to religious beliefs and institutions. In fact, the majority
are liberal religionists.
Hard secularity, on the other hand,
refers to a worldview, a system of beliefs, or a modality of sense-making
that is determinedly non-religious. A disenchanted universe is a purely
physical and material one.

2
The question posed in the present research is: what do “soft” and “hard” forms look
like among affiliated, self-described “secularists” with respect to several indicators:
> views of metaphysical concepts or phenomena
> self-descriptions
> attitude toward or about “religion”
> attitude about religion-government or church-state relations (and separation)
> use or avoidance of the notion of “spirituality” and meanings assigned to this.
The first four of these are fairly straightforward. With regard to “spiritual” and
“spirituality,” based on in-depth interviews with 50 self-described nonreligious individuals and
a small-scale survey of one secular humanist group, Pasquale (2006, 2007a) found that self-
described secularists—both affiliated and unaffiliated—may use these terms. Their meanings,
however, were decidedly ‘this-worldly’ in character.
Prior work has tended to focus on religious samples and on teasing out the distinction
between “religiousness” and “spirituality,” as well as “spiritual but not religious,” among them
(Zinnbauer, et al., 1999; Zinnbauer and Pargament, 2000). Summarizing findings reported by
Unruh, Versnel, and Kerr (2002), Bregman (2006) gave six categories culled from 92
distinguishable definitions of “spirituality”:
1. “Relationship to God, spiritual being, higher power”
2. “Not of the self”
3. “Transcendence or connectedness, unrelated to belief in a higher being”
4. “Existential”
5. “Meaning and purpose in life”
6. “Life force of the person”

At a certain level, much like broadly functional definitions of “religion”—such as


“ultimate concern” (Emmons, 1999, or Tillich, 1957) or “intensive concerns” (Bailey, 1998)—
definitions of “spirituality” (as “meaning and purpose in life”) may refer to intrinsically human
(and this-worldly) activities and sensibilities. As such, they may be indicators of secularity
rather than alternative or non-institutional religiousness.
An expanded survey of affiliates in a range of secularist groups offers the opportunity
to probe the range of meanings of these terms, prevalence of usage, and other aspects of such
individuals’ worldviews as a means of exploring distinguishable varieties of secularity.

Methods and sample

3
An effort was made to identify all free-standing “secularist” groups in Oregon,
Washington, and southern British Columbia.3 This included groups that described themselves
as “secular,” “humanist” (secular, Jewish, or Unitarian), “atheist,” “freethought,” “rationalist,”
and/or “skeptic.” Questionnaires were distributed by postal mail or by hand to 1,638
individuals on the mailing lists of 22 such groups from September of 2006 through January of
2007.4 These were distributed in manila envelopes containing a cover letter, the questionnaire,
and a self-addressed, post-paid return envelope addressed to “OCHCP Survey [number for
each participating group]” at a post-office box in Portland, Oregon. Cover letters asked for
group-members’ participation in a study of people in the Pacific Northwest whose ways of
thinking are substantially secular, nonreligious, skeptical, non-supernatural, or naturalistic.
Anonymity was assured and it was indicated that “survey findings will not be reported for any
particular group or organization by name.” The letter was signed by the investigator as a
Research Associate with the Institute for the Study of Secularism, Trinity College, Hartford,
Connecticut.
Forty-one questionnaires were returned by the postal service as undeliverable.
Questionnaires were returned by 951 individuals, but due to membership in multiple groups
some individuals received more than one copy. To avoid data duplication (i.e., more than one
completed survey from anyone), recipients were asked to complete a cover sheet for each
duplicate questionnaire received, indicating the group for which they had already completed
and returned the survey, and the group(s) from which they received the duplicate(s). Twenty-
eight respondents did so, yielding 922 completed surveys for analysis, or an effective response
rate of 58.76 percent (of a non-duplicative distribution of 1,569). Ten questionnaires were
partially completed, but since they provided usable data in some sections they were not
excluded from the sample for analysis.
Anecdotal reports (as well as written notes on completed forms) indicated that some of
those receiving more than one survey did not return duplicate surveys with completed cover
sheets, as requested. Another indication of multiple group membership (and so, likely receipt
of more than one questionnaire) was provided by a survey item on respondent membership in
local religious, philosophical, or nonreligious groups. Sixty-six respondents indicated
affiliation with two or more of the targeted groups. The resulting response rate is thus a
conservative estimate, since the actual (net) number of individuals in the distribution sample

4
was likely closer to 1,500 than 1,600. It is reasonable to conclude that a response rate of
roughly 60 percent was achieved.5
A commitment was made to all participating groups that data would not be reported for
any individual group by name, but rather, for distinguishable group or philosophical types
based on self-descriptions (in group names, in materials or websites, and by group members
and representatives). Assignment of groups to categories was straightforward in most cases,
based on self-descriptive distinctions made by the groups themselves. Groups were assigned to
the Unitarian humanist category on the basis of an indicated relationship with a Unitarian
church or fellowship, substantial numbers of Unitarian church/fellowship members among
respondents, and/or explicit self-description as Unitarian humanists.6 Seven respondents each
who were affiliated with Ethical Culture and “Brights” groups, who described themselves as
“atheist(ic)” and/or indicated dual membership in a regional atheist group, were included in the
Atheist group category. Comparatively low response variability for the Atheist group category
on many items reinforced confidence in the appropriateness of this allocation.
One small group (n=11) that labeled itself “freethought” was excluded from the
analyses presented here. Analysis of responses suggested that its members were markedly and
consistently different from all other groups in the distribution sample. Most indicated
profoundly “religious,” “spiritual,” supernatural, and/or “mystical” views. Aggregation with
affiliates of other freethought groups yielded results with high response variability that did not
fairly represent either of two apparently very different “takes” on “freethought.” This points
up an apparent distinction in popular use and meaning of “freethought” and “freethinker.”
One widely cited dictionary definition of freethought/freethinker refers to “one who
forms opinions on the basis of reason independently of authority; especially one who doubts or
denies religious dogma.” Two of three groups in the distribution sample use these terms in
what may be called the (majority or standard) “rationalist” sense (as in, for example,
Robertson, 1936, or more recently, Jacoby, 2004). Here, emphasis is on reason (or rigorous
application of logic and empirical evidence to the evaluation of truth claims) and denial of
religious doctrine or supernatural ideas. The response patterns of the outlier group, however,
suggested a very different interpretation. Here, thinking may be set free from the perceived
constraints of “traditional” or institutional religious adherence, belief, or doctrine, but also
from those of rationalism, skepticism, naturalism, or non-transcendentalism. (This might best
be called an “unfettered” sense of “freethought” and “freethinker.”) Such variation in usage is

5
not restricted to the groups sampled here. A check of the Worldwide Web indicates that while
the rationalist usage seems most common, there are those (such as an “Oregon Freethought
Church,” not represented in the present sample) for whom these terms mean freedom from
either religious or rationalist/empiricist limitation (with consequent inclusion of pagan, gnostic,
mystical, magical, “neopagan polytheistic,” and “bohemian” references).
Following these adjustments, the final sample for analysis included 911 respondents in
six categories:

Group type ---Oregon/Washington--- ----British Columbia---- Total %


Number of groups n Number of groups n

Secular humanist 4 256 3 129 385 42.3


Jewish humanist 2 110 110 12.1
Unitarian humanist 3 129 129 14.2
Atheist 2 104 104 11.4
Skeptic, rationalist 3 153 153 16.8
Freethought _2 30 30 3.3
16 782 3 129 911 100.1

Respondents were overwhelmingly white and of European descent. Among 899


individuals supplying this information, 79.8 percent described themselves as “Euro/
Caucasian,” with another 16.3 percent “Jewish” or “Jewish”+“Euro-Caucasian” and 3.9
percent, other (including [East] Asian descent, Indian [South Asian], Middle Eastern [Persian,
Iraqi], African American, and Native American).
Average age of respondents was 62.68 and median age was 64, with a range of 15 to
92. Roughly 56 percent of respondents were male and 44 percent were female. (Ten
respondents did not provide this information.)
Gender distribution varied among group types as indicated in Table 1. The pattern
among survey respondents was a fair reflection of the actual gender distributions in the groups
(based on mailing list counts or group reports; indicated in brackets).

6
Table 1. Gender distribution by group type

Gender
male female Total
Group Humanist – Secular Count 210 169 379
type/affiliation % in group type 55.4% 44.6% 100.0%

[Est. actual] [58.5%] [41.5%]


Humanist - Jewish Count 33 75 108
% in group type 30.6% 69.4% 100.0%

[Est. actual] [40.5%] [59.5%]


Humanist - Unitarian Count 63 66 129
% in group type 48.8% 51.2% 100.0%

[Est. actual] [48.0%] [52.0%]


Atheist Count 69 34 103
% in group type 67.0% 33.0% 100.0%

[Est. actual] [69.3%] [30.7%]


Skeptic, rationalist Count 113 39 152
% in group type 74.3% 25.7% 100.0%

[Est. actual] [70.0%] [30.0%]


Freethought Count 18 12 30
% in group type 60.0% 40.0% 100.0%

[Est. actual] [N/A] [N/A]


Total N 506 399 901
56.2% 43.8% 100.0%

This is a comparatively well-educated group, with more than three-quarters holding


college degrees and nearly half holding advanced degrees, far higher than among all U. S.
citizens 25 years of age or older (Table 2).

Table 2. Respondents’ educational attainment

Total sample U. S. sample* U. S. population**


% % %

High school only 4.4 3.4 31.7


Some college, 16.0 15.5 17.0
no degree
Specialist/ 5.1 4.2 8.7
associate degree
Baccalaureate 28.8 30.3 18.3
Master’s degrees 28.2 28.7 6.8
Doctoral/professional 17.4 17.7 2.9
(J.D., M. D., Ph. D.)

* Excluding British Columbia groups. ** Among individuals 25 years of age and older (= 191,884,000) U. S.
Census, 2006: http://www.census.gov/population/socdemo/education/cps2006/tab01-01.xls

7
Among the U. S.-based groups (n=782), 17.5 percent indicated Jewish backgrounds.
In all U. S. groups except the Jewish humanists, 7.1 percent of respondents indicated Jewish
backgrounds, an over-representation compared with an estimated 2 to 3 percent of individuals
reporting Jewish descent in the U. S. population at large.
The distribution of ethnic/cultural backgrounds for the entire sample is given in Table
3. Notably, not all Jewish humanist affiliates indicated Jewish cultural/ethnic background, a
reflection of the fact that some member households consist of a Jewish and non-Jewish spouse.

Table 3. Group affiliation by ethnic/cultural background

Group type/affiliation Total


Cultural/ethnic Humanist - Humanist - Humanist - Skeptic,
background Secular Jewish Unitarian Atheist rationalist Freethought
Euro/Caucasian Count 329 18 116 89 136 29 717
%
87.0% 16.5% 89.9% 88.1% 90.1% 96.7% 79.8%
Jewish or Count
Jewish+Euro 34 89 11 7 5 0 146
%
9.0% 81.7% 8.5% 6.9% 3.3% .0% 16.3%
All other (African Count
Am, Asian, 15 2 2 5 10 1 35
Middle Eastern)
%
4.0% 1.8% 1.6% 5.0% 6.6% 3.3% 3.9%
Total N 378 109 129 101 151 30 898
100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0%

8
Selected findings

Survey components afforded several ways of probing degrees and types of secularity
among respondents:
a) views of specific metaphysical questions or ideas
b) applicability of a range of self-descriptive terms
c) attitudes toward something-called-religion
d) attitude about religion-government, or church-state, relations
e) use and specific meanings of “spiritual” or “spirituality.”

Metaphysical concepts. Five items assessed respondents’ views on existential or


metaphysical issues, one of which focused on explanations for perceived order in nature:
Do you think that the degree of order or patterning we perceive in nature is most likely
attributable to:
1. Properties that are intrinsic to the nature of the physical universe or all that exists,
and nothing more
2. A coherent organizing principle that we cannot, or do not yet, fully comprehend,
that is pervasive throughout the physical universe or all that exists
3. An organizing principle or force that in some way transcends the physical universe
or all that exists
4. An impersonal, but in some sense intelligent, creative force that has brought
all that exists into being
5. A personal, and in some sense intelligence creative force, or “God,” with which
human beings can make contact or have a communicative relationship
6. An omniscient, omnipotent, intelligent God that designed and created all that exists

On the basis of responses to this item alone (n=886), this is an overwhelmingly


“naturalistic” group, with 96.4 percent choosing response 1, 2, or both 1 and 2. Three percent
more chose responses 3 or 4 (which could be considered alternative expressions of “deism”).
Only one individual chose option 5 and none chose 6. Comparatively greater numbers of
Jewish (32 percent) affiliates chose option 2 rather than 1 (with other groups ranging from 8 to
22 percent). In related texts, fourteen individuals made reference to the human tendency to
perceive or impose pattern whether or not it is “really there.” (One stated definitively that
“there is no order or patterning in nature!”) Had there been a response option attributing
“order” to human perception (or human perceptual bias, alone) rather than to nature, more
individuals might well have indicated this view.
Four additional metaphysical concepts were presented. These focused on:
> A transcendent entity: “a being, entity, or higher power beyond, apart from, or transcending nature
(call this “God” if you wish)
> A “personal essence, spirit, or soul apart from our physical bodies or continuing beyond our physical
lives (or through multiple lifetimes)”

9
> An “impersonal force or energy that courses through and connects all living things or all that exists”
(call this “spiritual” if you wish)
> An “ultimate purpose or direction in human life or all of existence”

Modified Glock and Stark response sets were provided for each of these items.
Modifications were suggested by prior interviews, particularly the addition of a “metaphorical”
approach to metaphysical concepts or phenomena. These were:

> Meaningless/nonexistent: “this makes no sense to me; I don’t think there is any such thing”
> Unknowable: “I don’t know and don’t think this is something human beings can know”
> Maybe/unsure: “this may be; I’m just not sure”
> Sometimes: “sometimes I think this is so and sometimes I do not”
> Metaphorical: “even though I doubt or reject the reality of this, or view this as a human construction,
I like to think as though there is”
> Probably: “there is probably something like this, but I have no idea about its actual nature”
> Definitely: “this is something I definitely think exists or is the case”

Strong majorities rejected notions of a transcendent entity and continuing personal


essence as meaningless/non-existent or unknowable. Weaker majorities rejected the notions of
ultimate purpose or an impersonal force.
Atheist affiliates were predictably most likely to reject the concept of a transcendent
entity, followed by Secular humanists, Skeptics, and Freethinkers (Figure 1). More Jewish

10
and Unitarian humanists viewed this as unknowable, with slightly greater numbers allowing its
possibility to varying degrees.
Responses concerning a personal essence (soul) were similar, except that fewer Jewish
humanists flatly reject the concept and more considered this unknowable or were unsure
(Figure 2).

Response patterns shifted markedly with regard to ultimate purpose (Figure 3) and an
impersonal force (Figure 4). A substantial number of respondents (12.3 percent, overall)
viewed ultimate purpose metaphorically. A slightly greater percentage of males (13.3 percent)
than females (10.7) indicated this view.
The concept of an impersonal connecting force or energy elicited the greatest response
variation among groups. Jewish humanists were most likely to allow the possibility or reality
of such a phenomenon, followed by Unitarian humanists and Freethinkers. Females more
often accepted an impersonal force maybe/sometimes (17.5 percent of women; 11.8 percent of
men), or probably/definitely (22.2 percent of women; 11.4 percent of men). But both male and
female Jewish humanists accepted the concept more often than those in other groups.

11
12
The most frequent single pattern of responses to all four concepts was “straight-ticket”
rejection (as meaningless or nonexistent). One-third of the full sample responded this way
(Table 4). Males (42.9 percent) were more likely to be straight-ticket rejecters than females
(27.4 percent; n=2987). One-half of the full sample considered all concepts meaningless/non-
existent or unknowable.

Table 4. Group affiliates rejecting a transcendental entity, personal essence, ultimate purpose,
and impersonal force as meaningless or non-existent

Group type/affiliation Total


Humanist - Humanist - Humanist - Skeptic, Freethough
Secular Judaic Unitarian Atheist rationalist t
All four worldview items Count 136 12 24 51 69 7 299
meaningless/nonexistent %
35.4% 11.1% 18.6% 49.5% 45.4% 23.3% 33.0%
N 384 108 129 103 152 30 906

Religious affiliation. Individuals were asked to indicate any local philosophical or


religious organizations with which they were affiliated (as active members, occasional
participants, or monetary contributors). Apart from affiliation with Unitarian churches or
fellowships, reported affiliation with religious organizations was negligible (Table 6). Most of
the Unitarians were found in the Unitarian humanist groups (60 percent), with 10.5 percent or
less in each of the other groups.

Table 6. Reported involvement in local religious institutions

Frequency Percent Valid Percent


Valid None indicated 739 81.1 82.7
Unitarian Universalism 139 15.3 15.5
Church, synagogue,
religious congregation 11 1.2 1.2
Pagan 1 .1 .1
Buddhist, meditation 4 .4 .4
Total 894 98.1 100.0
Missing No response/missing data 17 1.9
Total 911 100.0

Self-descriptions. A majority of respondents described themselves as not religious


(“if defined in supernatural or transcendental terms”), with 86.5 percent circling “not at all
religious” or “1” and 96.4 percent responding below the scale mid-point. More Jewish and

13
Unitarian humanists were willing to describe themselves as “religious” in some degree than
others (Table 5; response scale compressed).

Table 5. …currently consider yourself religious (in supernatural or transcendental terms)?

Group type/affiliation Total


Humanist - Humanist - Humanist - Skeptic,
Religious? Secular Jewish Unitarian Atheist rationalist Freethought
Not at all (1) Count 352 69 92 97 139 27 776
% 92.4% 64.5% 73.6% 94.2% 92.1% 90.0% 86.5%
A little (2-3) Count 26 29 23 6 7 2 93
% 6.8% 27.1% 18.4% 5.8% 4.6% 6.7% 10.4%
Mid-scale (4) Count 0 5 2 0 3 0 10
% .0% 4.7% 1.6% .0% 2.0% .0% 1.1%
Religious (5-7) Count 3 4 8 0 2 1 18
% .8% 3.7% 6.4% .0% 1.3% 3.3% 2.0%
Total Count 381 107 125 103 151 30 897
% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0%

A weaker majority described themselves as “not spiritual,” again “if defined in


supernatural or transcendental terms” (Table 7), with 73.4 percent choosing “Not at all
spiritual” and 87.8 percent responding below the scale mid-point. Judaic humanists were
noticeably more likely to apply “spiritual” to themselves in some degree than other affiliates,
followed by Unitarian humanist and freethought group affiliates. The numbers for Judaic
humanists were not attributable to the comparatively greater number of female affiliates, since
both males and females were more likely to apply “spiritual” than in other groups.

Table 7. …currently consider yourself spiritual (in supernatural or transcendental terms)?

Humanist - Humanist - Humanist - Skeptic,


Spiritual? Secular Jewish Unitarian Atheist rationalist Freethought
Not at all (1) Count 299 40 71 89 120 20 639
%
80.4% 38.8% 60.2% 89.0% 81.1% 66.7% 73.4%
A little (2-3) Count 41 26 25 10 17 6 125
% 11.0% 25.2% 21.2% 10.0% 11.5% 20.0% 14.4%
Mid-scale (4) Count 9 11 10 1 3 0 34
% 2.4% 10.7% 8.5% 1.0% 2.0% .0% 3.9%
Spiritual (5-7) Count 23 26 12 0 8 4 73
% 6.2% 25.2% 10.2% .0% 5.4% 13.3% 8.4%
Total N 372 103 118 100 148 30 871
% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0%

Respondents were also asked to indicate whether or not they “would. . .or do use any of

14
[several] terms to describe [their] way of thinking” (Table 8). Predictably (given the large
number of humanist affiliates in the sample), “humanist(ic)” was the most frequently chosen
descriptor. While chosen by large majorities of the humanist group types, substantial numbers
in all other groups applied it to themselves, as well, including 60 percent of Atheist affiliates.

Table 8. Self-descriptive terms applied by respondents to themselves

Group type/affiliation Total


Self-description Humanist - Humanist Humanist - Skeptic, (% of all
Secular – Jewish Unitarian Atheist rationalist Freethought respondents)
agnostic Count 130 40 61 25 59 12 327
% within
34.2% 36.4% 48.0% 24.5% 38.8% 41.4% 36.3%
Grouptype
atheist(ic) Count 243 45 55 94 103 22 562
% within
63.9% 40.9% 43.3% 92.2% 67.8% 75.9% 62.4%
Grouptype
anti-religious Count 115 19 29 37 55 11 266
% within
30.3% 17.3% 22.8% 36.3% 36.2% 37.9% 29.6%
Grouptype
humanist(ic) Count 315 90 96 61 81 13 656
% within
82.9% 81.8% 75.6% 59.8% 53.3% 44.8% 72.9%
Grouptype
naturalistic Count 130 23 44 33 53 12 295
% within
34.2% 20.9% 34.6% 32.4% 34.9% 41.4% 32.8%
Grouptype
scientific Count 217 41 72 61 115 18 524
% within
57.1% 37.3% 56.7% 59.8% 75.7% 62.1% 58.2%
Grouptype
secular(ist) Count 232 64 59 61 73 14 503
% within
61.1% 58.2% 46.5% 59.8% 48.0% 48.3% 55.9%
Grouptype
nontheist Count 159 37 40 36 47 12 331
% within
41.8% 33.6% 31.5% 35.3% 30.9% 41.4% 36.8%
Grouptype
skeptical Count 186 23 59 54 118 16 456
% within
48.9% 20.9% 46.5% 52.9% 77.6% 55.2% 50.7%
Grouptype
non/unbeliever Count 163 15 41 59 59 19 356
% within
42.9% 13.6% 32.3% 57.8% 38.8% 65.5% 39.6%
Grouptype
Total Count 380 110 127 102 152 29 900

“Atheist(ic)” was chosen by majorities of Atheist, Secular humanist, Skeptic, and


Freethought affiliates. Noticeably fewer Jewish and Unitarian humanists applied the term to
themselves.
“Agnostic” was chosen by little more than a third of all respondents. Nearly half of
Unitarian humanists, but only a quarter of Atheists, applied it to themselves.

15
“Anti-religious” and “naturalist(ic)” were the least frequently chosen self-descriptions
overall, with less than a third of respondents choosing each. Jewish humanists were least likely
to choose “anti-religious,” followed by Unitarian humanists. (In fact, fewer Jewish humanists
chose “anti-religious,” “atheist[ic],” “naturalist(ic),” “scientific,” “skeptical,” or
“non/unbeliever” than other affiliates.) The limited number of affiliates of any kind who
applied “anti-religious” to themselves is notable in view of fairly critical attitudes about
something-called-religion indicated by many.

Views of “religion.” Attitudes about “religion” were assessed several ways. When
asked to what extent they would say they are “angry about the role, dominance, and/or effects
of religion in the world,” nearly 80 percent indicated anger affirmatively (scaled responses
from 5 to 7; 1 = not at all angry; 7 = very angry; Table 9). Comments provided by 79
individuals suggested that responses might have been more affirmative regarding words other
than anger, such as “concerned,” “distressed,” “disappointed,” “distraught,” “frustrated,”
“puzzled,” “sad,” “troubled,” “upset,” “worried,” or “aghast,” among others. While some
writers made it clear that “anger” was apt and warranted, others said that there are more
reasonable or productive sentiments.

Table 9. Reported anger about the role and effects of religion in the world

Humanist - Humanist - Humanist - Skeptic,


Secular Jewish Unitarian Atheist rationalist Freethought
Not angry Count
42 11 17 6 31 3 110
(1-3)
% 11.3% 10.2% 13.7% 6.0% 20.9% 10.3% 12.5%
Scale Count
32 14 9 8 11 2 76
midpoint (4.0)
% 8.6% 13.0% 7.3% 8.0% 7.4% 6.9% 8.6%
Angry Count
298 83 98 86 106 24 695
(5-7)
% 80.1% 76.9% 79.0% 86.0% 71.6% 82.8% 78.9%
Total N 372 108 124 100 148 29 881
% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0%

Respondents were also asked to what extent they considered religion a harmful or
positive force in human affairs (1=harmful; 7=positive). Roughly three-quarters of
respondents considered religion more harmful than positive, but again there were considerable
differences among groups (Table 10). Jewish humanists were least likely to consider religion a

16
harmful force followed by Unitarian humanists; Freethought and Atheist affiliates were most
likely to be of this opinion.

Table 10. Religion as a harmful or positive force in human affairs

Humanist - Humanist - Humanist - Skeptic,


Secular Judaic Unitarian Atheist rationalist Freethought
A harmful force (1-3) Count 302 61 81 90 110 29 673
%
78.6% 56.0% 63.8% 87.4% 72.4% 96.7% 74.4%
Scale midpoint (4.0) Count 19 10 21 5 15 0 70
%
4.9% 9.2% 16.5% 4.9% 9.9% .0% 7.7%
A positive force (4-7) Count 6 7 5 3 13 1 35
% 1.6% 6.4% 3.9% 2.9% 8.6% 3.3% 3.9%
Too complex a Count
phenomenon (no 50 29 19 5 14 0 117
scaled response)
% 13.0% 26.6% 15.0% 4.9% 9.2% .0% 12.9%
Harmful + positive Count
7 2 1 0 0 0 10
(multiple checked)
% 1.8% 1.8% .8% .0% .0% .0% 1.1%
Total N 384 109 127 103 152 30 905

100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0%

Respondents were offered the opportunity to indicate that “religion is too complex a
phenomenon to generalize about in this way” in addition to, or instead of, a scaled response.
In Table 10, those who chose this option instead of a scaled response are reported. In Table 11,
those who chose this option whether or not they provided a scaled response are reported.
In both cases, Jewish humanists were most likely to feel that “religion” is too complex to
generalize about in such a manner, followed by Unitarian humanists; Freethought and Atheist
affiliates were least likely to do so.

Table11 . Religion too complex to evaluate generally as a harmful or positive force in the world

Overall, more than a third of respondents felt that “religion” was too complex to
Group type/affiliation Total
Humanist - Humanist - Humanist - Skeptic,
Secular Jewish Unitarian Atheist rationalist Freethought
“Religion” Count
126 53 55 30 48 7 319
too complex
% of group N 32.8% 48.6% 43.3% 29.1% 31.6% 23.3% 35.2%
Total N 384 109 127 103 152 30 905

17
Additional items provided further insight into respondents’ attitudes about “religion”
and related issues.8 In general, Jewish humanists were less critical of “religion” and more
critical of “skeptical and nonreligious people” than others, followed by Unitarian humanists.
Close inspection of the data indicated that this was true of both males and females in
comparison with other affiliates. Atheist and Freethought affiliates tended to be most critical
of religion. Skeptic and Secular humanist affiliates generally held the middle ground, with
Skeptics leaning toward leniency and Secular humanists, toward criticism.
Taking another look at (mean responses for) anger about religion (Appendix I, Table
A), Skeptic affiliates registered the least, on average; Freethought and Atheist affiliates, the
most. Only the Skeptics and Unitarian humanists were significantly less angry than Atheist
affiliates (F 5, 875 = 4.34, p<.019).
Looking again at (mean responses concerning) religion as a harmful or positive force in
human affairs, and keeping in mind that 127 individuals did not provide (unequivocal) scaled
responses due to the complexity of “religion,” Jewish humanists were least likely to consider
religion a harmful force, followed closely by Skeptics and Unitarian humanists. These three
groups were significantly less likely to view religion as a harmful force than the Secular
humanist and Atheist affiliates (F 5, 772 = 10.04, p<.01).
The Jewish humanists were most likely to feel, on average, that everyone benefits from
a mix of religious and nonreligious people in the world (F 5, 802 = 11.19, p<.01) and that there
are valuable pieces of religious wisdom in religious texts/traditions (F 5, 795 = 15.31, p<.01).
They were significantly more likely to feel this way than all but the Unitarian humanists, who
in turn felt significantly more than Secular humanists that we benefit from a mix of religious
and nonreligious people, and more than Secular humanist, Freethought, and Atheist affiliates
that there is wisdom in religious texts and traditions. Mean responses among Skeptics, Secular
humanists, Freethought, and Atheist affiliates did not differ significantly.
Although respondents generally tended not to condone ridicule of supernatural or
superstitious beliefs, the Jewish humanists did so significantly less, on average, than other
affiliates, who did not differ significantly among one another (F 5, 791 = 9.46, p<.01). They
were also significantly less likely than others to feel that humans would be better off without
superstition, supernaturalism, or the will to believe without evidence (F 5, 791 = 10.98, p<.01),
and that religion fosters excessive group devotion (except in comparison with Skeptics, which
did not reach significance; F 5, 792 = 9.88, p<.01).

18
The Jewish humanists were also most likely, on average, to feel that it is important in
general to cultivate acceptance of other ways of thinking and behaving (significantly more than
Skeptic and Secular humanist affiliates, F 5, 788 = 3.27, p<.01; with no significant differences
among other affiliates; Appendix I, Table B). They were least likely to feel that skeptical or
nonreligious people were more consistently ethical, considerate, responsible, or reasonable
than religious people (significantly so compared with all other affiliates; F 5, 877 = 8.76,
p<.01). And they were significantly more likely than Secular humanists, Skeptics, and Atheists
to feel that whether one is religious or nonreligious is not as important as being a decent person
(F 5, 795 = 4.48, p<.01).
Skeptics were least likely to indicate that they would be unhappy if a friend or family
member became a religious fundamentalist—significantly less than Judaic and Secular
humanist affiliates (F 5, 796 = 3.95, p<.01).
Atheist and Freethought affiliates were most likely, on average, to feel that their ways
of thinking are discriminated against in society. Judaic humanists were least likely to feel this
way—significantly less than Atheist, Freethought, and Secular humanist affiliates (F 5, 800 =
6.92, p<.01).

The meanings of “spiritual” and “spirituality.” While in response to a scaled item


(reported earlier) self-descriptions as “spiritual” were low overall, observation, interviews, and
a prior pretest survey of a secular humanist group indicated a willingness, even among
affirmative naturalists and secularist affiliates, to use “spiritual” or “spirituality” in particular
ways. An item was devised to assess usage and meaning of these terms:

With regard to “spiritual” and “spirituality” . . . (check any that apply to you)

__ I (tend to) avoid these terms; they do not apply to me or my experience

__ I (may) use these terms, but only in psychological or experiential terms


__ as a special state of being at peace or equilibrium or harmony
__ as a process or experience of greater awareness or higher consciousness
__ as a general feeling or experience of connection with others or nature

__ I use these terms to refer to something that exists beyond physical nature and its properties
(a force, energy, or entity, or multiple forces or entities), with which I/we can make
contact

19
Among 896 respondents to this item, 58 percent indicated “avoid/do not apply” (Table
12). Thirty-eight percent indicated they may use the terms in a psychological/experiential
sense. Only 3.5 percent indicated transcendental usage. While Jewish humanists were least
likely to avoid the terms entirely, the majority of respondents indicated non-transcendental
meanings.

Table 12. Use and meaning of “spiritual/ity” among group affiliates

Group type/affiliation Total


Humanist - Humanist - Humanist - Skeptic,
Secular Judaic Unitarian Atheist rationalist Freethought
(Tend to) avoid; don't apply Count
to me or my experience 238 33 60 76 99 15 521
% 62.5% 30.8% 47.6% 75.2% 65.6% 50.0% 58.1%
(May) use in psychological Count
127 64 60 24 45 15 335
/experiential sense
% 33.3% 59.8% 47.6% 23.8% 29.8% 50.0% 37.4%
(Tend to) avoid + (may) Count
4 0 1 0 4 0 9
use in psychological sense
% 1.0% .0% .8% .0% 2.6% .0% 1.0%
Use to refer to Count
9 9 4 0 3 0 25
transcendent force(s)
% 2.4% 8.4% 3.2% .0% 2.0% .0% 2.8%
Psychological sense + Count
3 1 1 1 0 0 6
transcendent contact
% .8% .9% .8% 1.0% .0% .0% .7%
Total N 381 107 126 101 151 30 896

Among the respondents who specified psychological usage (Table 13), most indicated a
feeling or experience of connection with others or with nature. Fewest indicated a process or
experience of higher awareness or consciousness. Fittingly, being at peace, harmony, or
equilibrium was in between.

Table 13. Use of “spiritual/ity” in specifically psychological/experiential senses

Frequency Valid Percent


Valid 1. as being at peace, harmony,
equilibrium 46 14.5
2. as process/experience of higher
awareness, consciousness 18 5.7
3. as feeling/experience of
connection with others/nature
146 46.1
1+3 35 11.0
2+3 14 4.4
1+2+3 58 18.3
Total10 317 100.0

20
Non-transcendental usage was not reported among the “softer secularists” alone.
Among those who applied the term “atheist(ic)” to themselves (n = 556) 30.8 percent indicated
such usage, as well as 23.8 percent of Atheist group affiliates (n = 104), and 23.5 percent of
those who consider a transcendent entity, personal essence, or impersonal force meaningless/
nonexistent or unknowable (n = 535). Even 15.7 percent of those who flatly rejected notions
of God, soul, force, or ultimate purpose as meaningless or nonexistent (n = 299)—the “straight-
ticket rejecters”—indicated a willingness to use these terms non-transcendentally.
Written texts were provided by 250 respondents (in response to an “Other” prompt
following the scaled item). The great majority of these conveyed naturalistic or non-
transcendental uses and meaning of “spiritual/ity.” (Sample texts are provided in Appendix II.)
The most frequent meanings were:
> Experience of/in nature
> Awe, wonder, gratitude, or appreciation (about existence, nature, the universe):
> A sense of social connection or generalized connectedness
> Emotion(al) experience
> Reaction to art, literature, music, aesthetic beauty
> Psychological process
> Sense of something greater than oneself (but not ontologically transcendental):

Forty individuals supplied texts that suggest openness to or affirmation of


transcendental ideas or phenomena. Some described themselves as “seekers” or “open” to
transcendental possibilities, and others gave more specific transcendental, religious, spiritual,
or supernatural references.

Political secularism. With regard to views on the relationship between religion and
government, respondents were overwhelmingly in agreement. When asked, “What is your
attitude about keeping church and state, or religion and government, separate,” 95.9 percent of
respondents (n=903) indicated that “we can never be too vigilant about this” (scaled responses
from 5 to 7). Only 1.6 percent chose the scale midpoint and 2.6 percent indicated that “strict
separationists take this too far” (scaled responses from 1 to 3). Responses were consistent
across groups, with no significant difference among group means.

21
Discussion

The groups identified for participation in this research characterize themselves as


“secular,” “non-theist,” “non-supernatural,” and/or (religiously) “skeptical” in their public
utterances and self-descriptions. This does not, of course, ensure that everyone who appears on
their mailing (or email) lists is equally committed to such self-descriptions. Nonetheless, but
for those indicating affiliation with Unitarian Universalism (particularly among the Unitarian
humanist affiliates) these individuals indicated virtually no affiliation or identification with
(mainstream) churches, temples, or other religious institutions. They are substantially non-
theistic with respect to a customary conception of a transcendental entity (“God”). With 89
percent of respondents considering this meaningless/non-existent, unknowable, or
metaphorical, this is a mirror image of many national survey samples in the United States
(Bishop, 1999). Harris Interactive (2006), for example, found that 11 percent of respondents
“believe [somewhat or absolutely certain] there is no God.” Green (2005) identifies 11 percent
of U. S. Americans as “secular” (no religious identity or affiliation, minimal or no “religious
beliefs,” or self-described “atheist” or “agnostic”).
While these individuals may be substantially non-theist (in a narrow, customary sense)
and nonreligious (with regard to identification or affiliation with mainstream religious
traditions in North American culture), they are not equally irreligious (in Campbell’s sense of
hostile rejection of dominant religious beliefs, traditions, or institutions; 1972, 1977). Nor are
they equally naturalistic or non-transcendental (with respect, for example, to acceptance or
rejection of an impersonal connecting force or energy in the world).
Jewish humanists. Perhaps the most notable and distinctive “story” to emerge from the
data is that of the Jewish humanist affiliates. U. S. American Jews have been characterized as
an exceptional or distinctive subculture, and so it is here—but in a very different way.
Whereas the great majority of American Jews are said to be exceptionally “secular” in
comparison with the mainstream population (Kosmin, 2002; Smith, 2005), in the company of
affiliated “secularists” their secularity—at least in the small sample assessed here—is
distinctively “soft.” On most measures this applies to both males and females in comparison
with other groups, so these findings are not attributable simply to the greater number of
females in the Jewish humanist group.

22
The “softness” of the Jewish humanists’ secularity, as a group, is attitudinal as well as
metaphysical, and again, this is generally true of both males and females in comparison with
other affiliates. They are most likely to feel that everyone benefits from a mix of both religious
and nonreligious people in the world, and that there are pieces of wisdom in religious texts or
traditions (due, no doubt in part, to the fact that they themselves draw—sans supernatural
references—from the well of Judaic thought and ritual). They are least likely to feel that
religion is a harmful force in the world or, indeed, that religion can be evaluated in such
general terms. They are least likely to think that religious and supernatural beliefs foster
excessive group devotion or that supernatural and superstitious ideas deserve to be ridiculed.
In general, they are comparatively less judgmental and most likely to feel that it is important to
cultivate acceptance of ways of thinking and behavior other than our own. They are most
likely to feel that skeptical or nonreligious people can be as closed-minded as the religious, and
least likely to think that skeptical or nonreligious people are more consistently ethical,
responsible, or reasonable than the religious.
They are also, however, most likely—shoulder to shoulder with Atheist affiliates—
to indicate that they would be unhappy if close friends or family members were to become
religious fundamentalists. And they are not lowest in reported anger about the roles and effects
of “religion” in the world. This finding, coupled with moderate attitudes about “religion,” may
suggest wariness of perceived “extremism” of any kind.
Unitarian humanists. The Unitarian humanists display a pattern of beliefs and attitudes
similar to, but somewhat less “soft,” overall, than the Jewish humanists. This may be
attributable, at least in part, to the historical meaning and role of “Humanism” within
Unitarianism—as an affirmatively this-worldly, non-theistic, non-supernatural, and (to some)
non-“spiritualistic” meaning system and manner. It may also reflect an important development
(Lee, 1995) within Unitarian Universalism in recent years—a deliberate shift from a
“Humanist” emphasis toward what is called, in Unitarian circles, “God-talk” (increased
reference to “spirits” and “spirituality,” “transcendence,” or “God” and accommodation of
pagan, pantheistic, or “New Age” worldviews). While some “Humanists” have remained
within UU ranks, despite criticism of this shift and of perceived marginalization within the UU
fold, some have shifted affiliation to (secular) humanist groups. It is quite likely that the
Unitarian humanist affiliates sampled here fit this description—some still “in the fold” (as
indicated by the 60 percent who indicated UU membership), some “on the line,” and some “on

23
the outs,” but many having been affected by the shift away from a “harder (secular)
Humanism” in Unitarian Universalism. In-depth interviews with two current and two ex-
Unitarian humanists (now Secular humanist affiliates) support this view, as do reports and
observations of former Unitarian church or fellowship affiliates joining (secular) humanist
groups in the region in the past several years.
Skeptics/rationalists. Unlike members of The Skeptic Society reported by Shermer
(2000), among whom one fifth to one third (in two surveys) indicated some degree of belief in
a “purposeful higher intelligence that created the universe,” only 4 percent of the current
sample of Skeptic affiliates probably or definitely accept the existence of a transcendent entity,
with 5 percent more who are unsure or waver between belief and disbelief. These tend to be
“thoroughgoing” rather than “selective” skeptics (Pasquale, 2006) regarding metaphysical, as
well as “paranormal” or “pseudo-scientific” claims, and as such, they are similar to the Secular
humanist, Atheist, and Freethought affiliates. Attitudinally, however, there are signs of more
muted criticism regarding something-called-religion in comparison with those groups. In
addition to registering the least amount of anger about “religion,” on average, they are more
likely than Atheist affiliates to think that there is some wisdom to be found in religious texts or
traditions. And they are least likely to say they would be unhappy if a close friend or family
member were to become a religious fundamentalist.
Secular humanist, atheist, and freethought affiliates. Secular humanists frequently
acknowledge kinship with Atheists concerning rejection of theistic or supernatural thinking,
criticism of established or institutional religion, and support of church-state separation. But
they also tend to distinguish themselves from self-described Atheists with respect to
comparatively greater focus on positive social values and less on religious criticism. Whatever
may transpire in their respective group meetings and private communications, the data here
suggest more substantial kinship than distinctiveness between Secular humanist and Atheist
affiliates with respect to attitudes about metaphysical ideas as well as something-called-
religion. Certainly, the Atheists tend to be more consistently resolute in their rejection of
metaphysical ideas and criticism of “religion,” but this is by degrees.
The Atheist and Secular humanist affiliates are both, on average, equally and
comparatively more likely than others to consider “religion” a harmful force in human affairs
and to feel least that we all benefit from a mix of religious and nonreligious people in the
world. On other measures, while differences did not prove statistically significant, the pattern

24
of responses suggests that Atheist affiliates are somewhat more severe in their views of
religion and supernatural ideas than the Secular humanists. Moreover, the data suggest that the
Secular humanists are somewhat more likely to be (self-)critical (about “skeptical or
nonreligious people”). The Atheist affiliates are least likely, on average, to feel that skeptical
or nonreligious people can be as close-minded or dogmatic as religious people, for example.
Little can be said with confidence, based on these data, about Freethought group
affiliates due to small samples and widely divergent meanings of “freethought” and
“freethinker.” While the small sample of (rationalist) Freethought affiliates analyzed here
generally exhibit views resembling Skeptics, Secular humanists, and Atheists, very wide
differences of opinion between male and female affiliates on several measures complicate the
picture further. Public self-descriptions among groups that choose the (rationalist)
“freethought” label suggest that they tend to be generalists—welcoming a wide range of
secularists (including non-theists, atheists, secular humanists, unbelievers, skeptics, agnostics,
rationalists, and freethinkers). The primary focus in some groups, particularly those indicating
affiliation with the Freedom from Religion Foundation, is on separation of church and state.
As such, they may tend to attract strong supporters of that cause whose personal worldviews
vary more than in, for example, Atheist groups. But again, more representative data are
required before anything can be said with confidence.
Soft and hard secularism. It becomes clear that characterizations of secularity from
“soft” to “hard” must be qualified. With respect to political secularism, or insistence on
separation between religion/church and government/state, there is broad unanimity. In this
sense, all of these individuals and group types may be said to be hard secularists.
With regard to attitudes about something-called-religion, groups varied as follows:

“Soft” (Less critical) -------------------------------------------------------- “Hard”(More critical)


Jewish humanists Atheists
Unitarian humanists (Freethinkers)
Skeptics Secular humanists

It would be of value in future research to ascertain the conceptions of “religion”


associated with use of the term by various secularists. Long observation among affiliated
secularists, and text responses in the present research on attitudes about “religion,” suggest that
while “religion” is typically used without qualification, when probed it most often refers to
“monotheistic religion(s),” generally Christianity (and now Islam), and particularly the forms

25
of these phenomena said to be “fundamentalist” or “extremist.” The conception of “religion”
in the minds of many Jewish or Unitarian humanists may well be something different (e.g.,
more “functional”) compared with what many Atheist or Secular humanist affiliates have in
mind.
With regard to naturalistic thinking, or views of metaphysical or other-worldly
concepts, the continuum shifts somewhat:

“Soft” (Less rejection) -------------------------------------------------- “Hard”(Stronger rejection)

Jewish humanists Skeptics/rationalists


Unitarian humanists Atheists
(Freethinkers)
Secular humanists

In particular, it was something of a surprise that the response pattern concerning an


impersonal force did not resemble that for a transcendent entity or a personal essence (broad
dismissal as non-existent or unknowable) or an ultimate purpose in existence (either dismissal
or a metaphorical view of the notion). Since ultimate purpose is the most abstract of these
ideas, it was expected that this would be dismissed least often, simply on this basis.
Willingness to entertain the possibility or reality of an impersonal connecting force suggests
the relevance of some notion of “spirituality” among a substantial minority of these self-
described secularists, and, indeed, a substantial minority indicated willingness to use these
terms as a description of self or personal experience. But what, precisely, does this mean?
Meanings and meaninglessness of ‘spiritual/ity.’ The majority (58 percent) of all
respondents indicate that they prefer to avoid these terms since they do not apply to their
experience or (based on written texts) are too ambiguous to be meaningful. Most of those who
are willing to use “spiritual/ity” indicate that they do so in purely this-worldly, psychological
or experiential rather than transcendental, terms. This most often refers to a sense of
connection with others or with nature, or experiences of peace, beauty, art, awe, or appreciation
for sentient existence. With regard to the six categories of “spiritual/ity” suggested by Unruh,
Versnel, and Kerr (2002), virtually none of these individuals use these terms to mean “a
relationship with God, a spiritual being, or higher power.” If used at all, they are most often
associated with “transcendence or connectedness, unrelated to belief in a higher being” or
“meaning and purpose in life.”

26
A noticeable minority, among the Jewish humanists in particular, entertains the reality
or possibility of an impersonal force or energy that connects all living (or existing) things.
This is not in a metaphorical sense, since this option was available but not chosen. With regard
to the basis for perceived order in nature, there is a hint of (call this small-“t”) transcendental
(but not “theistic”) thinking among the 17 percent of respondents who view this as a coherent
organizing force pervasive in the universe (but do not see this as an organizing principle that
“transcends” the universe). For this minority, something that knits everything together in some
“coherent” fashion, but which we do not fully comprehend, is brought within nature rather than
residing outside it. It is, one might say, something more than animism but less than pantheism.
Thematic emphasis on “secular spirituality” can be found in material on secular and
humanistic Judaism. Yakov Malkin (2004) devotes a chapter to “The spirituality of secular
Jews” in Secular Judaism: Faith, Values, and Spirituality (with an approving introduction by
Sherwin Wine, founder of the Society for Humanistic Judaism). He writes that “[o]ne of the
grave dangers facing secularism in general, and Jewish secularism in particular, is the apparent
loss of spirituality or even the perception of its need, as if spirituality somehow needed to be
‘religious’” (p. 37). He proceeds to describe “secular spirituality” in exclusively this-worldly
terms—with reference to social and communal connection, awe and appreciation for existence,
uplifting aesthetic experience, a larger sense of purpose, focus on values and experience
beyond the mundane or everyday, and dedication to matters beyond self and self-interest.
Many of the Jewish humanists,’ and most other affiliates,’ use of “spiritual/ity” would
seem to conform to such a this-worldly meaning, but by no means all. It certainly cannot be
assumed, as often seems to be the case, that use of these terms signals alternate forms of
(transcendental) religiosity. Depending upon the worldviews and intended meanings of those
who use them, they may reflect (“hard”) naturalistic secularity. To quote one resolutely
naturalistic respondent, experience of “the paradoxes of existence” or of “the mystery of the
human condition” is not unknown to these people. Use of “spiritual/ity” to describe such
experience by substantially naturalistic individuals would seem to reflect a limitation of
language when it comes to capturing an affirmatively this-worldly sensation or sensibility that
human beings experience (regardless of their ontological interpretation of it).
The polysemy of “spiritual” and “spirituality” presents challenges not only for these
respondents, but for productive and consistently meaningful empirical investigation of human

27
meaning systems or worldviews. There are innumerable uses and meanings for these terms, as
Bregman (2006) observes. Here, three distinguishable meanings have been explored:
1. Supernatural (relationship with an autonomous entity apart from “nature”)
2. Immanent (a meta-empirical dimension pervasive within nature that is experienced)
3. This-worldly (a purely psychological or affective experience or state of being).
As respondents’ texts indicate, the last of these may be as simple as a sense awe or
wonder, or as complex as an intense, multi-dimensional state of being that (some) human
beings experience, report, and apparently enjoy (of connectedness, expansiveness, immersion,
letting go of a sense of self or self-consciousness, and so on). But for many this is interpreted
as a psychological, experiential, and wholly this-worldly phenomenon. This is distinct from
either an immanent/meta-empirical or a supernatural frame. The latter is overwhelmingly
dismissed by these individuals, but a minority seems willing to embrace an immanent
conception of their experience or the nature of reality. What would seem to be required are
different words for each of these phenomena and their attendant interpretive frames. The use
of “spiritual/ity” for all of them serves only to obfuscate distinctions—and subjective
experiences—that are of apparent importance to people.
Concluding observation. As others have found (e.g., Bishop, 1999; The Baylor
Institute for Studies of Religion, 2006), when more nuanced questions are put to people about
their existential or metaphysical thinking, customary categories erode. It becomes increasingly
difficult to decide where definitive lines may be drawn between “naturalistic,”
“transcendental,” “supernatural,” or “theistic” when we probe beyond conventional notions of
a personal transcendent entity (“God”) or personal essence (“soul”) to those, for example, of an
immanent connecting force or an ultimate purpose or directionality in existence—and
metaphorical conceptions, as well as acceptance or rejection, of such ideas.
We have barely begun to map varieties of human experience at “the other end of the
metaphysical spectrum,” whatever we may wish to call this domain. The closer we look, the
more we may find that customary categories dissolve into continua or complex mosaics of
thought, attitude, experience, and interpretation that defy simple categorization.

28
APPENDIX I: Table A. Attitudes about religion and supernatural beliefs

Jewish Unitarian Skeptic/ Secular Atheist Freethought Average


Humanist Humanist Rationalist humanist (rationalist) response
(JH) (UH) (SR) (SH) (A) (FR)
Favorable views

> “Religion is generally a 2.56 2.36 2.37 1.82 1.78 1.70 2.06
harmful (1)…positive (7) > SR, SH, A* > SH*,A** > SH*, A*
force in human affairs > FR***

> “We all benefit from… 4.75 4.07 3.52 3.34 3.30 3.54 3.67
and nonreligious people” > all* > SH**
but UH***

> “There are valuable 5.76 5.49 4.89 4.53 4.07 4.29 4.86
pieces of wisdom in > all* > SH*,A* > A***
religious texts/traditions” but UH > SR**
> FR***
Critical views

> “…Angry about the role, 5.38 5.33 5.09 5.62 5.94 5.97 5.51
dominance, or effects of < A*** < A** < A*
religion in the world.” < SH***

> “The human species will be 5.50 6.24 6.16 6.46 6.57 6.59 6.26
better off when it outgrows… < all*
superstition, supernaturalism, but SR***
and the will to believe
without evidence”
> “Religions & supernatural 5.36 6.10 5.86 6.18 6.32 6.25 6.02
beliefs…foster excessive < all*
devotion to particular groups, but SR**
ideologies, or lifestyles”
> “Superstitious and 2.53 3.39 3.92 3.79 4.33 4.00 3.64
supernatural beliefs < all* < A***
deserve to be ridiculed”

* p<.01 ** p<.05 ***p<.10

29
APPENDIX I: Table B. Measures of acceptance and self-criticism

Jewish Unitarian Skeptic/ Secular Atheist Freethought Average


Humanist Humanist Rationalist humanist (rationalist) response
(JH) (UH) (SR) (SH) (A) (FR)

> “It is important to cultivate 5.88 5.38 5.15 5.30 5.18 5.24 5.38
acceptance of ways of > SR,SH*
thinking and behaving > UH***
different from one’s own.”

> “Whether you are religious 6.58 6.32 6.07 6.07 5.84 6.10 6.16
or nonreligious…is of little > SH,SR,A*
concern. What is important
is [being] ethical, considerate,
responsible, and reasonable”

> “Skeptical or nonreligious 5.92 5.73 5.57 5.40 5.13 5.55 5.54
people can be as close- > SH,A**
minded or dogmatic as
religious people”

> “Skeptical or nonreligious 3.62 4.63 4.52 4.62 5.22 5.26 4.54
…are likely to be ethical, < all*
considerate, responsible and
reasonable more consistently
than religious people.”

> “I would be very unhappy 5.90 5.44 5.14 5.67 5.88 5.66 5.58
if a close friend or family < JH*
member became a < SH**
religious fundamentalist” < A***

> “People with my way of 4.54 4.88 4.76 5.12 5.89 5.83 5.02
thinking are generally < A,FR*
discriminated against < SH**
in society”

* p<.01 ** p<.05 ***p<.10

30
Appendix II. Written texts on use/meaning of “spiritual/ity”

> Experience of/in nature:


> Appreciate the wonders of the natural world
> If you remove religious connotations from it, I consider myself spiritual
and in need of relaxation via calm music or walks. . .on nature trails.
> . . .[A] sense of serenity in wilderness areas
> Love of the out of doors
> Awe, wonder, gratitude, or appreciation (about existence, nature, the universe):
> It is possible to be in awe of the universe as it is shown to us by science,
but I do not believe that there is any supernatural energy or personification
in the workings of the universe
> Ethical, aesthetic, empathy, open to wonder & the unexpected, positive & optimistic
> I look with wonder upon the universe without bringing in a deity
> I'm often in awe and full of gratitude which feels "spiritual" to me
> Awed by the amazing creative power of the universe
> I feel "inspired" by beauty, truth, and nature
> Emotional sense of wonder, awe, beauty, inspiration
> Similar to Carl Sagan
> A sense of social connection or generalized connectedness:
> Love of family and work, care for others, seems spiritual to me
> A sense of connection to and responsibility for the earth and humanity
> . . . [A] sense of oneness with the world when I am hiking, walking
> . . . [A] sense of shared responsibility
> "Interconnected" to all elements in universe.
> Identify with the interconnected web of all life.
> Emotion(al) experience:
> Can be emotional - attributed to biochemicals, beauty in nature
> I believe most people interpret human emotions as spirituality
> Emotional reaction to grand/beautiful aspects of nature
> I trust my intuition; I follow my feelings.
> Reaction to art, literature, music, aesthetic beauty:
> Music can be a "spiritual" experience where I feel connected to something
very powerful and timeless
> I do appreciate some non-material phenomena (e.g., sunsets, music)
> I do like music and resonate to natural beauty - but as a rationalist
> I can rise above mundane cares by viewing great art, reading great writers, etc.
> This is a very controversial term; most people associate it with religion, but it
isn’t necessarily. My human spirit is very affected by, say, a newborn baby,
a solo walk in the woods, a Beethoven sonata, a fine (in my opinion) piece of
poetry, etc.
> Psychological process:
> I avoid these words, dislike them a lot, but recognize as valid to me the three
"psychological or experiential" descriptions [given in the survey]
> Not the belief in a higher power, but happy to be conscious and thinking
> Power of subconscious to work out solutions (this is physical or spiritual)
> Sense of something greater than oneself (but not ontologically transcendental):
> Feel people can evolve to a higher sense and to work towards that
> Spiritual feeling of something greater than myself - part of non-
denominational group of like-minded people

31
Forty individuals supplied texts that suggest openness to or affirmation of transcendental ideas or
phenomena. Some described themselves as “seekers” or “open” to transcendental possibilities:

> The path from my parents' Atheism to mine included exploration of


Christianity, Depak Chopra's Quantum Physics, and New Age through and
transcendental meditation. I'm neither a strictly rational, logical, critical
thinker, nor a spiritual person. I don't see the world in black & whites or rights
and wrongs (regarding religious beliefs). I do have ethics and values, but am
completely non-theistic, though I'm open-minded.
> WE DON'T KNOW! The transcendental may be possible - so I'm open to it.
> There are some mysteries we cannot yet explain
> There could be something on the other side. WE DON'T KNOW!
> Am exploring my spirituality by trying. . .to realize what is spiritual for me.
> Still searching for answers
> Seeking

Others gave more specific transcendental, religious, spiritual, or supernatural references:


> I believe in a nonpersonal creative intelligence
> Believe there is collective intelligence
> Believe in unity of all nature and all life - evolution to higher states of
consciousness
> Belief in scientific studies of "near-death" experiences; also belief in "past
lives" as removed from Bible by Catholic (Vatican) and others
> Universal energy - I do energy and "light" work
> I believe in a superior spiritual process
> I consider myself spiritual, close to nature, believe in afterlife
> I do believe that all we have around us came from something more than
inanimate matter, but I have no idea what.
> Only in the sense that there could be something on the other side.
> Decided to believe in God, e.g., as Deist
> More associated to a Deistic philosophy or Emersonian view
> 12-Step recovery programs - Higher Power - . . .six years clean!
> The power that exists within the infinite universe, and is exemplified in the
natural laws that govern it, is way beyond our puny human intellect to
fathom. I pray (for enlightenment) to "the powers that be" in the
universe. Still think ghosts MAY be real.
> Spiritual in a kind of non-religious Buddhist, yoga kind of way
> Somewhat "spiritual" - very interested in Native American spiritual life
> Native American community building rituals for stewardship of land
> I feel that the concept of Kharma applies. Expecting and living for good
attracts it.
> Sense of wonder, amazement at the beauty of the universe. A bit pantheistic,
maybe.
> Quasi-Taoist tree-hugging dirt-worshipper
> I like Zen or Tao philosophy
> Buddhist atheist
> Taoist
> Pagan

32
References

Bailey, Edward. (1998) Implicit religion: An introduction. London: Middlesex University


Press.

Bainbridge, William Sims. (2005) Atheism. Interdisciplinary Journal of Research on


Religion, 1 (Article 2). http://www.religjournal.com

Baylor Institute for Studies in Religion, The (2006) American piety in the 21st century: New
insights to the depth and complexity of religion in the U.S. Waco, TX: Baylor
University. (www.baylor.edu/content/services/document.php/33304.pdf)

Beit-Hallahmi, Benjamin (2007). “Atheists: A psychological profile.” In M. Martin (Ed.),


The Cambridge Companion to Atheism, 300-313. New York: Cambridge University
Press.

Bishop, George. (1999) “The polls—trends: Americans’ belief in God,” Public Opinion
Quarterly, 63: 421-434
-------. (2005) The illusion of public opinion: Fact and artifact in American public opinion
polls. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers.

Bregman, Lucy. (2006) “Spirituality: A glowing and useful term in search of a meaning,”
Omega, 53 (1-2): 5-25.

Campbell, Colin. (1972) Toward a sociology of irreligion. NY: Herder and Herder.
-------. (1977) “Analysing the rejection of religion,” Social Compass, 24: 339-346.

Emmons, Robert A. (1999) The psychology of ultimate concerns: Motivation and


spirituality in personality. New York: Guilford Press.

Green, John C. (2005) The American religious landscape and political attitudes: A baseline
for 2004. Washington, D. C.: The Pew Forum on Religion & Public Life.
(http://pewforum.org/publications/surveys/green-full.pdf)

Hale, J. Russell. (1977) Who are the unchurched? An exploratory study. Washington, D. C.:
Glenmary Research Center.

Honderich, Ted (Ed.) (2005) The Oxford companion to philosophy: Second edition. Oxford,
UK: Oxford University Press.

Hood, Ralph W., Jr., Peter C. Hill, and W. Paul Williamson. (2005) The psychology of
religious fundamentalism. New York: Guilford Press.

Hunsberger, Bruce and Bob Altemeyer (2006) Atheists: A groundbreaking study of America’s
nonbelievers. Amherst, NY: Prometheus Press.

33
Jacoby, Susan (2004) Freethinkers: A history of American secularism. New York:
Metropolitan Books.

Kosmin, Barry A. (2002) “As secular as they come,” Moment Magazine.


(http://www.momentmag.com/archive/june02/feat1.html).

Kosmin, Barry A. and Ariela Keysar (2007) Secularism & secularity: Contemporary
International Perspectives. Hartford, CT: Institute for the Study of Secularism in
Society and Culture, Trinity College.

Lee, Richard Wayne. (1995) “Strained bedfellows: Pagans, New Agers, and “starchy
Humanists” in Unitarian Universalism,” Sociology of Religion, 56 (4): 379-396.

Malkin, Yakov (2004) Secular Judaism: Faith, values, and spirituality. London:
Vallentine Mitchell.

Pasquale, Frank L. (2006) “Neglecting the ‘nots’ in the Northwest: Irreligion as a facet of the
study of religion.” Paper presented at annual meeting of the Society for the Scientific
Study of Religion, Portland, OR, October 21.
-------. (2007a) “The ‘nonreligious’ in the American Northwest.” In Barry A. Kosmin and
Ariela Keysar, (Eds.), Secularism & secularity: Contemporary International
Perspectives. Hartford, CT: Institute for the Study of Secularism in Society and
Culture, Trinity College. Pages 41 to 58.
-------. (2007b) “Unbelief and irreligion, empirical study and neglect of.” In Tom Flynn
(Ed.), The New Encyclopedia of Unbelief. Amherst, NY: Prometheus Press.
Pages 760-766.

Robertson, J. M. (1936) A history of freethought: Ancient and modern to the periofd of the
French Revolution. London: Watts & Co.

Shermer, Michael. (2000) How we believe: The search for God in an age of science.
New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.

Smith, Tom W. (2005) Jewish distinctiveness in America: A statistical portrait. New York,
NY: The American Jewish Committee.

Tillich, Paul. (1957) The dynamics of faith. New York: Harper and Row.

Unruh, A., J. Versnel, and N. Kerr. (2002) “Spirituality unplugged: A review of


commonalities and contentions, and a resolution,” Canadian Journal of Occupational
Therapy, 69: 1-19.

Wong, P. T. P. (1998) “Spirituality, meaning, and successful aging.” In P. T. P. Wong and


P. S. Fry (Eds.), The human quest for meaning: A handbook of psychological research
and clinical applications (pages 11-32). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Zinnbauer, Brian J., Kenneth I. Pargament, Brenda Cole, Mark S. Rye, Eric M. Butter,

34
Timothy Belavich, Kathleen M Hipp, Allie B. Scott, and Jill L. Kadar. (1997)
“Religion and spirituality: Unfuzzying the fuzzy,” Journal for the Scientific Study of
Religion, 36 (4): 549-564.

Zinnbauer, Brian J., and Kenneth I. Pargament. (2000) “Capturing the meanings of
religiousness and spirituality: One way down from a definitional Tower of Babel.”
Research in the Social Scientific Study of Religion, 12: 23-54.

Zuckerman, Philip. (2005) “Atheism: Contemporary numbers and patterns.” In Michael


Martin (Ed.), The Cambridge companion to atheism, 47-65. New York City:
Cambridge University Press.

Notes

35
1
“A ‘meaning system’ can be thought of as a group of beliefs or theories about reality that includes both a world theory. . .
and a self theory” (Hood, Hill, and Williamson, 2005: 14) or, borrowing from Wong (1998: 368), “an individually
constructed cognitive system that endows life with personal significance.” Here, “worldview” and “meaning system” will
be used synonymously.
2
Unless specified otherwise, “transcendent” and “transcendental” are used here in an ontological sense, that is, “existence
beyond; independent existence” (Honderich, 2005: 922) or “existing apart from, not subject to the limitations of, the
material universe (The Oxford Dictionary and Thesaurus: American edition, 1996: 1623).
3
Atheist, skeptic, or rationalist student groups at some universities in the region were not sought out for inclusion in the
study. Membership is customarily small and shifts annually as students matriculate and graduate.
4
The investigator is grateful for a Shand Award from the Society for the Scientific Study of Religion which helped defray
distribution costs.

5
It should be noted, too, that the nature and quality of mailing lists varied noticeably among the groups. This reflects, in
part, differences in the purposes, activities, and nature of affiliation in these groups. Some exhibit formal organizational
structure with governing boards, committees, regular meetings, and clearly defined levels of membership. Others are less
structured networks of interested individuals and supporters who meet irregularly, distribute newsletters or other bulletins,
maintain continuing contact largely via email, and have less well-defined affiliation levels or requirements, As a result,
some were clearly regularly updated lists of active or dues-paying members, while others were less frequently updated
and/or more inclusive lists covering both active participants (in meetings, email correspondence, or other activities) as well
as people who had inquired about the group or asked to receive electronic or printed newsletters.

6
Nearly 60 percent of Unitarian humanist affiliates indicated affiliation with a Unitarian church or fellowship, while in
other groups 10.5 percent or less did so.

7
Does not total 299 since one respondent did not indicate gender.
8
These were among the appended questions distributed to all but the first two groups, resulting in samples of 353 for the
Secular humanist, and 59 for Atheist, affiliates.
9
Unless otherwise noted, Tamhane post-hoc analyses were performed due to unequal variances between groups.
10
Does not total 335, as in Table 12, since 18 respondents did not check particular psychological/experiential senses.

You might also like