The Genetic Approach in Psychoanalysis 1945 Hartmann
The Genetic Approach in Psychoanalysis 1945 Hartmann
The Genetic Approach in Psychoanalysis 1945 Hartmann
To cite this article: Heinz Hartmann & Ernst Kris (1945) The Genetic Approach in Psychoanalysis,
The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 1:1, 11-30, DOI: 10.1080/00797308.1945.11823124
11
12 HEINZ HARTMANN and ERNST KRIS
1. See in this connection the misapprehension of Lewin (1937) who assumed that,
in psychoanalysis behavior is merely traced back to similar earlier situations.
See bibliography for references.
GENETIC APPROACH IN PSYCHOANALYSIS 13
speaking, one might say that the propositions concerned with psy-
chological dynamics are more fully elaborated and more widely
accepted. During the last decade they have gained some considerable
influence upon the total field of medical therapy, partly through the
studies in psychosomatic medicine, in which the dynamics of the "body"
were correlated to those of the "mind". At the same time the verifica-
tion of these propositions has acquired so independent a standing in
experimental psychology that one tends to speak of "experimental
psychoanalysis" as of a field of its own. Partly under the impression
of this expansive activity in neighboring disciplines, some of the
dynamic propositions of psychoanalysis are finding respectful con-
sideration in practices of social control, in welfare, and in the social
sciences. Briefly, they have deeply penetrated psychiatry and enlarged
its scope and influence.
In many practices of child care and education genetic propositions
have been hardly less influential. However, their influence in the
practical fields up to the present has outweighed by far their im-
portance in organized research. The academic study of child psy-
chology and child development has not sufficiently taken notice of
the genetic approach in psychoanalysis," Psychoanalysts, on the other
hand, have failed in many respects to take into account the data that
child psychology has assembled; an omission that has led to many
incongruities.
With this state of affairs in mind we here shall discuss two
problems: first the relation of dynamic and genetic propositions of
psychoanalysis, and second, the present stage of the development
of the genetic propositions themselves.
the view that the genetic propositions are unduly stressed in psycho-
analysis.
4, /I. sucvey of these reformulations Initiated by Dollard and Miller Was recently
made by Mowrer and Kluckhohn.
GENETIC APPR.OACH IN PSYCHOANALYSIS 17
5. See for similar views, Bernfeld, Hartmann (1943) and for slightly different
.arguments, Rapaport.
18 HEINZ HARTMANN and ERNST KRIS
grown out of the same root, and may justify the same prognosis.
Pacifism may in one case be a reaction formation to the wish to attack
and in the other an expression of fear of being attacked by a superior
enemy. Extreme aggressiveness may be in one case the reaction to
fear and its concealment, in the other, the direct expression of sadistic
wishes. These are distinctions that the genetic methods permit us to
establish. What appears to be similar behavior with the individuals
when seen in the cross-section can be differentiated when we take
account of its genesis. If we are able to indicate the position
of such behavior in the longitudinal section what appeared as similar
behavior gains in each individual case a very different meaning. It
is here that we rely upon the genetic propositions especially when
dealing with what has been called the central areas of personality;
only the genetic propositions permit us to make perceivable the drives
that a behavior detail represents, their direction, their intensity and
their structural interconnection.
True, many elements of the past may actually be visible in be-
havior in a given field. But on the other hand, many elements of the
past upon which the application of the genetic proposition has to be
based are not contained as memories in "the field". We here refer
to what psychoanalysis calls the repressed and to the unconscious
parts of ego-defense. But they may appear in the field if a specific
technique, that of the psychoanalytic interview, is being used. And every
application of this method forcibly leads to a restructuring of the field.
The field theory as formulated by Lewin (who is inclined to speak
of a method rather than a theory, 1943) has produced the sharpest
and most logical formulation of the non-historical tendency in psy-
chology (Brown, 1937). It nevertheless has much in common with
psychoanalysis: the consideration of a large number of interdependent
factors, the assumption that every event results out of a variety of fac-
tors (over-determination), are derived from psychoanalysis. One of the
basic statements of the field theory, that "any behavior or any other
change in a psychological field depends only upon the psychological
field at that time", does not appear to be irreconcilable with psycho-
analysis, and it seems possible that if the field theory or other cross-
sectional approaches should develop new methods of investigation
their scope may be considerably enlarged." However, it seems essential
6. Thus Lewin in one of his latest papers seems to assume that those events of the past
that are of immediate relevance for the present can be investigated as parts of the field
and that such investigations may extend over "days and weeks" (1943). The analytic
approach de.6nitely postulates a reconstruction including the total life span.
20 HEINZ HARTMANN and ERNST KRIS
not to overlook the pragmatic side: the field theory has as yet not sug-
gested any definite answer to the question with which we are con-
cerned in this paper: under what conditions is "testing the properties
of a situation at a given time" the most productive and reliable method
for the understanding of the dynamic and structural properties of
psychological phenomena; and how far must such understanding be
based upon what to field theorists may appear a detour via genetic
investigation.
According to Lewin the postulates for an ideal topological investi-
gation of the field will consider what is psycho-biologically relevant
both in a phenotypical and genotypical sense (1935). No other
observational method seems fit to establish this relevance except the
psychoanalytic interview itself. It can reasonably be described as a
field situation in which two people react to each other within conditions
established by rules of procedure. The field situation is changed
from day to day not only by changes in the experience of the patient-
in his daily life but also by the interpretation given by the analyst.
The patient whose mechanism of retreat from danger has been mentioned
comes for analysis with no other complaint than that of lack of interest in his
work. The analyst's first impression is that the lack of interest may not
be genuine; a detail, the patient's affect when discussing events in his office
supplies the cue. A first interpretation draws the patient's attention to the
contradiction between lack of interest and intensity of emotional reaction.
The structure of the field is changed since the patient has been stimulated to
observe similar contradictions; for a time he has become allied to the ana-
lyst in observing under what conditions emotions of considerable intensity
arise. From this first step a way leads to the insight that the first set of condi-
tions is related to the second and that lack of interest occurs when continued
participation might lead to a clash with competitors. In the course of the
gradual elaboration of this pattern the following incident may take place: the
patient's lack of interest may shift from his work to the treatment, which he
may wish to discontinue. He has "suddenly" noticed other patients in the
analyst's waiting room and reacts to this observation with a desire to'
retreat. At this point the field is restructured by a transference interpretation.
He is told that the other patients have suddenly been noticed because he was
predisposed to discover rivals and that this rivalry is related to the growing
attachment to the analyst's person. The sequence attachment-rivalry-retreat
is discussed as one that has shifted from professional life to the treatment room.
When memory material supplies further cues a rivalry situation in childhood
in relation to siblings and parents may emerge. This, as a rule, does not come
about without the reexperiencing of repressed emotions. This in turn may
lead to a reconstruction of the "original situation", in which, for example,
the wish to attack was directed against sibling or parent, and in which attempts
in this direction had been undertaken; the reconstruction may then include the
dangers with which thought or action was fraught at the time, their suppression
GENETIC APPROACH IN PSYCHOANALYSIS 21
by the parents or by the patient's conscience and a large variety of other details.
In many cases the reconstruction may then be supplemented by a recollection
of a formerly repressed memory. Such reconstructions based upon traces in
dream and fantasy life which supplement actual behavior may well be called
predicting the past; predictions of this kind have by "objective verification"
proved to be correct 10 astonishing details."
the phallic phase; only the memory of the race will explain it," To
this we are inclined to reply with Freud's own arguments. While in
many cases the child in our civilization is no longer being threatened
with castration, the intensity of the veiled aggression of the adult
against the child may still produce the same effect. One might say
that there always is "castration" in the air. Adults who restrict the
little boy act according to patterns rooted in their own upbringing.
However symbolic or distant from actual castration their threats might
be, they are likely to be interpreted by the little boy in terms of his
own experiences. The tumescent penis with which he responds in
erotic excitement, that strange phenomenon of a change in a part
of his body that proves to be largely independent of his control, leads
him to react not to the manifest content but rather to the latent mean-
ing of the restriction with which his strivings for mother, sister, or
girl-playmate meet. And then, what he may have. seen frequently
before, the genitals of the little girl, acquire a new meaning as evidence
and corroboration of that fear. However, the intensity of fear is
not only linked to his present experience, but also to similar experi-
ences in his past. The dreaded retaliation of the environment revives
memories of similar anxieties when desires for other gratifications were
predominant and when the supreme fear was not that of being cas-
trated but that of not being loved." In other words: pregenital experi-
ence is one of the factors determining the reaction in the phallic phase.
This simple formulation refers to a wealth of highly significant
experiences which form the nucleus of early childhood; to the total
attitude of the environment toward the child's anaclitic desires, when
the need for protection is paramount, and toward the child's later
erotic demands.
While phylogenetic speculation was suggested to Freud by the-
ories current in the 1880's, his insight into the relevance of ontogenetic
factors grew out of empirical material. When, in the quest for the
etiology of hysteria, clinical impressions led to the patient's childhood,
Freud attempted to solve what appeared to him then as an unexplained
difficulty; he made the assumption that one traumatic sexual experi-
ence, the seduction of the child by an adult, had been of decisive
etiological importance (1896). This assumption was soon dropped
and replaced by descriptions of regular phases in the development of
the child's instinctual needs.
which the fear of castration arises; and later those which lead to the
fear of conscience-terms that refer to situations of high complexity
and long duration.
In summarizing what is explicitly and implicitly contained in
Freud's concept as far as genetic propositions are concerned, we sug-
gest the following formulation. In the life of each individual crucial
situations occur. They may be due predominantly to external events
or they may be due predominantly to predispositions in the individual
which then may invest insignificant situations with high significance.
In order to assess the predispositions of an individual that meet those
crucial situations, the data in every case would have to refer to his
total past. For a considerable time the reference to the instinctual
demands dominated the discussions of these predispositions and the
functions of the ego were either incompletely described or the de-
scription was limited to that of mechanisms of defense at its disposal.
Though at the present it is generally realized that the realm of the
ego is wider, clinical and theoretical discussions are not conducted on
the same level. While there is no hesitation to refer in clinical de-
scription to the capacities with which an individual is equipped in
coping with pressures of many kinds at any stage of his development,
this point of view is comparatively new in theoretical discussions.
If we turn to the ego as the psychic system that controls perception
and motility, achieves solutions, and directs actions, we have to insist
on distinctions that seemed irrelevant when Freud first formulated his
genetic propositions. A number of functions of the ego related to the
apparatus at its disposal develop largely outside of the reach of psychic
conflict; Hartmann(1939) actually speaks of a sphere of the ego
free from conflict. These functions gain for our discussion a specific
importance since they exercise a considerable influence as independent
factors; they determine together with other factors what mechanism of
defense an individual adopts and with what results, or what sub-
stitute goals he adopts for his instinctual desires. However, this dis-
tinction between psychological processes predominantly dependent on
biological maturation, and others predominantly dependent on in-
fluences of the environment, to which we here refer as "development",
is not limited to ego psychology. The growth of the teeth and of the
muscular sphincter control are according to Freud influential in deter-
mining the progress from one phase of libidinal development to
the other; but these maturational sequences determine also the sequence
of experiences that owe their special character to one or the other
GENETIC APPROACH IN PSYCHOANALYSIS 25
10. A model of such coincidence in the regular normal development of the little girl
and her discovery of the Sex difference, is tentatively indicated by Rado, For similar
theoretical views see Erikson.
26 HEINZ HARTMANN and ERNST KRIS
BIBLIOGRAPHY