Temporal Geometries of An African Music: A Preliminary Sketch
Temporal Geometries of An African Music: A Preliminary Sketch
Temporal Geometries of An African Music: A Preliminary Sketch
NOTE: The examples for the (text-only) PDF version of this item are available online at:
http://www.mtosmt.org/issues/mto.10.16.4/mto.10.16.4.scherzinger.php
[1] This paper is a brief and preliminary sketch culled from a much larger study that describes and defines geometric
perspectives of temporal patterning in various musical genres found in southern Africa. Of particular interest for this study is
the music of the lamellaphone-type, including that of the njari, the mbira, the matepe, and the kalimba, found in Zimbabwe and
Mozambique. This paper will focus on one of these lamellaphones, the mbira dza vadzimu, played by the Zezuru people of
Mashonaland East. First, I will introduce and demonstrate some elementary ambiguities of meter-formation in the rhythmic
figures of mbira music; second, describe the basic kinesthetic processes that underlie these figures; and third, suggest a
conceptual affinity these rhythmic processes have with the cross-penetrating symmetries and near-symmetries that
characterize harmonic patterning in these musics. By focusing on rhythmic patterns and their relationship to both kinesthetic
motor movements and abstract harmonic ones—effectively oscillating between an understanding of this music as embodied
material practice no less than formal conceptualization—this intervention hopes to expand the coordinates of the field of
African music theory. While the analyses are largely grounded in a well-established “Western” theory of rhythm and meter,
this theory is frequently expanded or revised to capture otherwise unassimilable details of the music. In the final analysis, the
point is as much to challenge as to embrace the theory in the context of non-Western modes of music-making. Most music-
theoretical literature on mbira music (with a few notable exceptions) limits its analytic findings to general observations. To the
extent that music theory is engaged at all, most analyses of traditional African music examine aspects of rhythm alone, such as
interlocking performance techniques, asymmetric melodic lines, polyrhythmic interlacing of parts, shifting metric downbeats,
and inherent patterns. Indeed, there is an impressive literature on African rhythmic processes; including technical analytic
excursions (Locke 1987 and Anku 1992), theories grounded in anthropological narrative (Jones 1959 and Chernoff 1979) and
even political critique of the very elevation of rhythmic complexity as a peculiarly African musical trait (Agawu 2003,
Scherzinger 2003). For all this methodological richness, the overarching preoccupation with rhythm—figured as an
autonomous musical parameter—simultaneously marks a kind of deaf spot in the literature. For example, there is very little
scholarly work involving non-rhythmic aspects of African music, such as pitch spaces or pitch processes (melody, harmony,
counterpoint, etc.), and still less work on the relation between pitch processes and rhythm.
[2] It would be easy to claim, in a quick pseudo-Orientalist way, that this one-sided textual production is simply the
ideological legitimization of a kind of racialist ‘Africanism’; an exotic invention of rhythmic complexity that maps onto
geopolitical zones of economic exclusion and cultural difference. Kofi Agawu, for example, has amply described the ways
African rhythm is invented in scholarly discourse, linking this default perspective with an a priori projection of Africa’s
cultural difference from the West (Agawu 2003, 55–70). Resisting the tendency to keep the African aboriginal in a state of
excluded cultural conformity, this position recommends de-exoticizing African cultural practice by emphasizing its points of
affinity (instead of difference) with Western practice. This is an important critique, but it bears the marks of another kind of
limit in the current scholarly context, which has substantially expanded its horizons. In the last two decades non-Western
musics have been demonstrably mainstreamed and canonized in both academic and popular cultural circles in the United
States.
1 of 10
[3] Instead of adding to the critique of the ideological valences in Western discourse about African rhythm, this paper aims to
recognize, retrieve, and rehabilitate the dimension of rhythm in African music without disavowing the political concerns
raised by the critique. The reason is twofold. First, no matter how tilted this scholarly scene is and how contradicted by
concrete musical examples and genuine cross-cultural comparisons, it nonetheless provides scholarship with important
standards by which African musical expression is normatively measured. These standards should be engaged and debated
rather than dismissed. Second, if analyses of African music irreducibly risk becoming exotic projections of difference simply
by addressing rhythmic aspects of the music, then their contribution to a global debate about general theories of rhythm and
meter may become foreclosed once more. A framework grounded in cultural sameness, one that selects only those features
that render the commonplace banality of African rhythm, will thereby hyperbolically domesticate what was elsewhere all too
exoticized. In other words, a detailed close engagement with rhythmic phenomena need not recapitulate generalized exotic
topoi about African music just because it demonstrates unique modalities for patterning time. The point is neither to
aprioristically celebrate African rhythmic complexity nor to recoil from it in alarm.
[4] Relatedly, it should be noted that dethroning social inventions alone proffers no social alternatives. For example,
construing certain Western theories of rhythm and meter as Eurocentric impositions risks dichotomizing African and
European musical perceptions on false grounds. This construal simultaneously naturalizes these theories, as if they had
already accurately captured the experience of Western music, and then opposes them to the experience of African music. In
contrast, I hope to suggest that the close formal analysis of the African case may contribute to an understanding of
perceptions of meter and rhythm in general, that the analysis may inform the way Africans hear African music (or indeed the
way Africans hear Western music or vice versa), and, above all, that the African case may contribute to an understanding of
the way Westerners hear Western music. Therefore, the aim of this paper is not so much to localize the reach of these
ostensibly cognitive or universal theories (demonstrating their limited historical and geographical scope, for example), as it is
to Africanize those theories that are cognitive or universal. Again, it is my contention that the actual perceptions Westerners have of
rhythm and meter in general is more African than some of the West’s general theories describing those perceptions would
permit.
[5] The mbira is an instrument with 22 to 24 metal keys (lamellae) fixed to a wooden soundboard that is then wedged to a
gourd resonator, which is used to amplify its resonance. Rattling pieces of metal, bottle caps, or shells are attached to the
instrument, which in turn produce buzzing sounds when the keys are plucked. Plucking the metal tongues produces a range
of sounds. Along with the vibrations of the bottle caps, the keys of the mbira tend to produce very prominent overtones
resulting in various layers of rhythmic accents and inherent melodies. Mbira players often report that the mbira sounds like
more than one instrument being played at once, or that implicit sounds emerge on close listening to the mbira (see, for
example, Berliner 1981, 23). As suggested by the full name of the instrument, mbira dza vadzimu (“mbira of the ancestral
spirits”), the principal role of the mbira involves conjuring ancestral spirits, which play a central role in the religious cosmos
of the Shona. In all-night ceremonies of spirit possession (known as mapira), the sound of the mbira has the power to entice
the spirits to come and to participate in events. Indeed, the buzzing creates acoustic illusions (sounding like high-pitched
instruments, such as the flute or the human voice) to materialize within the musical texture during the possession rites (see,
for example, Gwanzura Gwenzi’s testimony in Tracey 1970; and Hakurotwi Mude’s testimony in Berliner 1981, 260). The
participants dance, sing and drink home brewed sorghum beer until the music reaches an intense climax and the ancestral
spirit takes possession of the medium (homwe). The medium, whose sudden possession leads to heightened physical states
(such as bending over backwards in a perfect arc or dancing in convulsive movements), behaves like the awakened ancestor
who in turn provides insight about (and offers advice on) particular social and political difficulties that beset the participating
group. Thus, the mbira music provides a passage to negotiations with ancestors. These negotiations help participants to
break through an accustomed everyday way of thinking by opening up new dimensions of meaning.
[6] When two mbiras are played together the different paths they take (sometimes referred to as kushaura and kutsinhira parts)
are in an interlocking relationship with one another. One player sounds in the silence of the other, or one player sounds one
pulse after the other, thus forming figures of intricacy and variety exceeding the movements of the fingers alone. The
sustained hocketing is offset by a hosho (a pair of rattles made from dried gourds), which sounds in repeating three-pulse
groups. Sometimes performers give ‘voice’ to their dancing by attaching maghavu (gourds filled with seeds) to their legs, thus
adding rhythmic complexity to the hosho’s beating. The dancers provide an additional layer of rhythmic activity with
characteristic handclapping patterns that accompany the ensemble. Often one or both players also sing melodies and recite
words to the music. These can range from lengthy poems to personal comments, from ancient metaphoric wisdom and
social commentary to funny stories and sounds, and from criticism to nonsense syllables. These texts, often improvised, are
frequently both funny and tragic and are laced with double meaning.
[7] The tuning of the mbira is not fixed across time and space, and so the relationship between the intervals in any sequence
of keys is variable. Some of these tuning systems are given a general formal designation such as gandanga, nyamaropa, or
dambatsoko tunings, while others seem to be unique to a particular performer or group of performers. Usually cycling in 12-,
36- or 48-pulse patterns, mbira songs elaborate a seemingly endless number of variations around a basic harmonic shape,
shifting rhythms and melodies at ever-changing places in the cycle. Examples 1 through 3 below illustrate five typical
2 of 10
instances of various traditional 48-pulse tunes of the mbira dza vadzimu.
[8] The music’s elevated social importance, spiritual value, and social prestige has a lengthy historical genealogy. During the
period of the Mutapa dynasty, partly derived from a branch of the Zimbabwe culture that flourished between 1300–1450,
mbira music was prevalent in the Shona courts. Some European explorers and missionaries documented the character of the
mbira in strikingly complimentary terms. In 1589, for example, the missionary Father José Dos Santos described mbira music
with reference to the music of the harpsichord and emphasized its status in Shona culture:
Quiteve [the then-current ruler of the Mutapa]...makes use of [a]...class of [Africans], great musicians and
dancers, who have no other office than to sit in the first room of the king’s palace, at the outer door, and
round his dwelling, playing many different musical instruments, and singing to them a great variety of songs
and discourses in praise of the king, in very high and sonorous voices.... [The Africans] play upon [the
mabira] by striking the loose ends of the rods with their thumb-nails...and they strike the keys as lightly as a
good player strikes those of a harpsichord. Thus the iron rods are shaken and the blows resounding after the
fashion of a jew’s harp, they produce an altogether sweet and gentle harmony of accordant sounds (in McCall
1901, 203).
It is likely that the musical tradition of the mbira precedes these first European accounts of it by several centuries. For
example, the Zimbabwe culture, founded on the trade route between the Leopard’s Kopje culture to the west and the Sofala
Coast to the east, became very prosperous during the fourteenth century. By increasing its grip on the gold trade (via
taxation), the rulers of the Zimbabwe state were able to finance skilled builders for the purpose of cutting, dressing,
transporting, and laying stone for massive stone enclosures and conical towers. The lifestyle of the rulers became increasingly
elaborate as conditions in the valley became urbanized. Rulers imported cloth, silks, embroidered materials, beads and iron
gongs from Sofala, while a proportion of gold en route to Sofala was not exported but instead forged into ornaments by local
goldsmiths. Various ‘functionless’ objects, such as carved soapstone birds, stone monoliths, female figurines and phalli, or
abstract designs, such as the chevron patterns in the outer wall of the great building complex, indicate the considerable
luxury and wealth of the city of Zimbabwe. In light of the capacity of the Zimbabwean rulers to employ independent
builders as well as a military, it is highly likely that quasi-professional music-making played an important role in the
fourteenth and fifteenth-century courts of Zimbabwe. Archeological findings suggest that the mbira was commonly used in
the fifteenth century and, in all probability, was played as early as the tenth century. Iron ore nodules, for example, have been
found in a cave on the hill at Great Zimbabwe, which may indicate the use of iron for instrument building (See Beach 1980).
[9] Examples 1 through 3 illustrate typical passages of three characteristic mbira tunes: Nhemamusasa, Mutamba, and
Nyamaropa. Nhemamusasa, which refers to the building of temporary shelter, and Nyamaropa, which literally means “bloody
meat” and refers to the blood of sacrifice, are some of the oldest songs in the mbira repertoire. They are widespread today
and well documented. Mutamba, though less known, refers to the strychnos spinosa tree, which gives the mutamba fruit. It was
the song played by Zhanje for Pasipamire, the legendary spirit medium for Chaminuka, during the time of the nineteenth-
century mfecane (Shona/Ndebele wars). Legend has it that the song endowed Pasipamire with super-human strength in the
face of certain death. Examples 1a and 3a, elementary parts (or kushaura, meaning, broadly speaking, “to lead”) for
Nhemamusasa and Nyamaropa respectively, exemplify a basic interlocking structure between the two hands, with each hand
playing in the silence left by the other. In the transcriptions each eighth-note pitch event (or eighth-note silence) refers to a
pulse. A beat, in contrast, refers to a pulse that is an inmate of a particular metric situation. The grouping structure in
Examples 1a and 3a is straightforward, falling into periods of two- or four-pulses. This plain grouping, indicated by square
brackets below Example 1a, is offset by the entrance of a second part (sometimes called kutsinhira, meaning “to follow,” but
also implying being in-between and contrasting), which falls one pulse behind the first. The second part can comprise
identical music as the first; or, more often than not, some kind of variation. In other words, the pattern in Example 1a could
be interwoven with itself, or with the pattern in Example 1b (or, of course, some other variation). By performing these parts
one pulse apart, the mbirists effectively interlock bass and treble parts between the two players, producing thereby a new
pattern whose grouping structure is considerably more complex than that implied by the constituent parts alone.
[10] In contrast, the music’s grouping structure in Examples 1b, 2, and 3 implies different meters within the same pattern. In
Example 1b, the left-hand figures tend toward periodicity in three-pulse groupings, while the right-hand figures tend toward
periodicity in two- or four-pulse groupings. These groupings are indicated by square brackets. Examples 2 and 3b, in
contrast, elaborate the opposite grouping patterns—groups of three-pulsed figures in the right hand (in Example 2 square
brackets indicate an upper line interlocking with the bass in twos) and groups of two-pulsed figures in the left hand. In sum,
the situation in each case is polymetric, with different points of metric emphasis in each hand. This ambiguity is present even
before the entrance of the second part, which, by entering a pulse behind the first part, considerably enriches the
polyrhythmic complexity of the overall pattern.
[11] In the context of two mbirists, there are at least three ways of perceiving (or parsing) the overall musical texture, each of
which generates a unique metric organization for the listener. First, the small timbral differences between mbirists allow each
3 of 10
player to be heard as separately interweaving parts. Each part constitutes what I call kinesthetic patterns of the music, where the
sounding image is aligned with the motor image—the performers’ respective thumb movements. The second way to invest
the overall texture with metric characteristics is to track the counterpoint produced by the interlocking parts. In this hearing,
contrapuntal lines are formed by their registral proximity (rather than the timbral cohesion of their underlying kinesthetic
motion), which (following Kubik 1962) I will call the music’s inherent patterns. Finally, a third way to invest the music with
metric characteristics is achieved by considering the pattern as a unified Gestalt, which takes into account the overall pattern.
[12] What follows is a brief description of the perplexing ambiguities of meter-formation in basic mbira musical
constructions. It is important to note that these observations are limited to the most elementary level of rhythmic activity in the
music. Using the bassline alone in the basic patterns presented in Examples 1a and 3a as a reference, we find that, when
placed in an interlocking relationship with another equally basic pattern, there is a prominent mismatch between the overall
sounding pattern and the kinesthetic actions that produce this pattern. This mismatch, in turn, has significant implications
for the metric profile of the rhythmic melodic lines that emerge from the interlocking relationship. The bass pattern in 1a,
for example, could be described as a series of simple octave leaps (and occasionally leaps by 5th or 12th) from one manual of
the mbira to another in a high-low-high-low arrangement (with a pitch event every two pulses). Example 4 represents the
two interlocking bass parts by way of arrows. The first player is represented with arrows down and the second player with
arrows up. When such a pattern is combined with an identical pattern beginning one pulse later, we find a high-high-
low-low-high-high-low-low arrangement (with a pitch event on every consecutive pulse). The resulting patterns in the upper
and lower bass parts are depicted in Example 4 above and below the arrows respectively with an “x” for a sound and a “.”
for a silence. Despite the basic character of each individual pattern, their temporal alignment now produces a variety of
possible metric interpretations.
[13] The metric ambiguity can be demonstrated with reference to metric preference rules (MPRs) elaborated by Fred Lerdahl
and Ray Jackendoff in A Generative Theory of Tonal Music (1983). Most pertinent in this regard are probably MPRs 3, 5a, 5d, 5f,
and 6. These rules are defined as follows:
1. “MPR 3 (Event) Prefer a metrical structure in which beats of level L i that coincide with the inception of pitch-events
are strong beats of L i ” (76).
2. “MPR 5 (Length) Prefer a metrical structure in which a relatively strong beat occurs at the inception of either: a. a
relatively long pitch-event,...d. a relatively long pattern of articulation,...f. a relatively long duration of a harmony in the
relevant levels of the time-span reduction (harmonic rhythm)” (84).
3. “MPR 6 (Bass) Prefer a metrically stable bass” (88).
If these rules reflect cognitive preferences for meter formation, the case of mbira’s elementary interlocking design produces a
variety of delicately precarious results. For example, the ‘harmonic rhythm’ of most mbira pieces (including those depicted in
Examples 1a and 3a above) is generally constituted by three harmonies (or harmonic dyads) for every twelve pulses (or one
quarter of the total pattern), with each harmony generally spanning four (eighth-note) pulses before changing. This steady
harmonic rhythm supports a metric preference for 4+4+4 pulses. Focusing on the harmonic metric preferences produced by
the kinesthetic patterns (that is, by the motor patterns of each individual bassline), we find two distinct downbeats and two sets
of metric accents are implied. These are depicted in Figure A1, which represents the down and up arrows (players one and
two) with black and white dots respectively. For the first mbira line (depicted by black dots) the downbeat falls on the first
pulse, with points of emphasis on the fifth and ninth pulses. In set-theoretical parlance, the metric structure is articulated by
timepoints 0, 4 and 8, yielding a kind of 3/2 meter. For the second mbira line (depicted by white dots), in contrast, the
downbeat implied by harmonic preferences falls on timepoint 1 (or t 1), with accents on t 5 and t 9, yielding 3/2 one pulse
behind that of the first mbira.
[14] Harmonic considerations aside, if we focus on the kinesthetic patterns in terms of MPR 6, which emphasizes the
importance of the bass note in downbeat-formation, the two mbira lines would produce different metric interpretations.
These are depicted in Figure A2. Under this hearing, the kinesthetic movements from the bass upward produce the
preferred rhythmic groupings. Thus, the first mbira line now implies a downbeat on t 2, with metric accents on t 6 and t 10
(or 3/2 beginning on t 2). In contrast, the second mbira line implies a downbeat on t 3, with metric accents on t 7 and t 11
(or 3/2 beginning on t 3). Notice that, with kinesthetic patterns in the ear, metric accent formation is maximally distributed
across all four possible pulse groupings for 3/2. In sum, under MPR 5f, 3/2 falls on t 0 or t 1, and for MPR 6, it falls on t 2
or t 3. Furthermore, since harmonic pitch spaces in mbira music, though grounded in fifths and thirds, are not defined by
tonal criteria (emphasizing harmonic locales—beginnings, departures, arrivals, returns, closures, etc.) but by symmetric and
near-symmetric dyad cycles, the very idea of a starting point is radically relativized. Hence, according to the relevant MPRs
discussed in relation to the two kinesthetic patterns, every one of the twelve pulses aspires not only to some kind of metric
accent, but also to equal validity to downbeat formation itself. Under the rubric of metric preference rules, therefore, the
kinesthetic patterns, while implying a generalized 3/2, can oscillate between twelve possibilities. Any overarching metric
determination is undecidable or, to put it differently, they are, metrically speaking, maximally ambiguous.
[15] If we now focus on the metric fallout produced by the inherent patterns of the basslines (that is, the contrapuntal lines
derived from tones sounded by both mbiras congealed into distinct lines by their registral proximity), we find slightly
4 of 10
differently elaborated, but equally ambiguous, projected fields of 3/2. For example, MPRs 5a and 5d, which indicate a
preference to identify relatively longer pitch events or patterns of articulation with metric accents, would imply downbeat
formation on ts 1, 5 and 9 for the upper inherent pattern (depicted by the top horizontal line, joining white dots to black
ones, in Figure A3), and on ts 3, 7 and 11 for the lower inherent pattern (depicted as the bottom horizontal line, joining
white dots to black ones, in Figure A3). In contrast, if we engage harmonic considerations (MPR 5f) in the context of the
inherent patterns, downbeat formation is implied on ts 0, 4 and 8 for the upper inherent pattern (depicted by the top
horizontal line, joining black dots to white ones, in Figure A4), and on ts 2, 6 and 10 for the lower inherent pattern
(depicted by the bottom horizontal line, joining black dots to white ones, in Figure A4). Once again, all twelve rotations of
3/2 are thereby suggested with equal validity. The implied meters of the inherent patterns are maximally ambiguous.
[16] Finally, if we focus on the overall resultant pattern produced by the two interweaving mbira lines the metric fallout is, once
again, suspended between various MPRs. Combining consideration for the length rule (MPR 5a/d) with that for the bass
rule (MPR 6), we find 3/2 implied with downbeats on either ts 3, 7 or 11. However, with the harmonic rhythm of the
resultant pattern in the ear, 3/2 is implied with downbeats on 0, 4 and 8. These possibilities are illustrated in Figures A5 and
A6 respectively. While the resultant pattern less readily proffers possible downbeats on ts 1, 2, 5, 6, 9, and 10, it nonetheless
oscillates between six other possibilities implied by the various criteria for downbeat formation.
[17] As can be seen, the metric structures afforded by the pattern, in its various perceptual guises, can readily migrate across
all twelve pulses of the pattern. However, the metric permutations presented so far are all incarnations of some kind of 3/2
(or perhaps 6/4) structure. There are additional metric permutations not grounded in this basically binary structure. As
mentioned above, in Shona practice the interlocking of mbira parts is frequently offset by the shaking of the gourd rattle
(hosho), which accompanies the ensemble. This pattern, which involves an up-down flicking movement in both the left and
right hands creates a richly textured ternary-time grouping, which could be notated as < x x . x x . x x . x x . >, and so on.
Because of the ricochet of the seeds, as well as the difference in wrist-flicking action between hands, the actual sound is
much more complex than the notation implies, filling each of the three pulses spanning the motor movements with
differently nuanced accentuation. In short, the hosho provides a layer of rhythmic activity that, while complex, generally
implies a kind of 12/8.
[19] Generally speaking, whether we focus on the mbira pattern alone or if we combine that focus with the supplementary
hosho-effect, the metric situation can be characterized as indeterminate; a situation in which downbeat formation is
de-hierarchized to a maximum degree. This is not to say that all beats in this music are somehow equal and that metric
considerations are thereby suspended. On the contrary, there are strong tendencies toward meter formation within the
pattern. But these tendencies are various and contradictory, suspended between criteria that disperse metric assignations
across all pulses equally. Of course actual mbira patterns are mostly much more complex than this simple example, but for
the purposes of this paper it is important to note that even for such beginners’ patterns, mbira music is carefully crafted
around malleable metric axes, whose shifting downbeats encourage perceptual modulations of metric perspective.
[20] Let me give one more example of metric ambiguities that emerge in a slightly more complex interlocking situation. As
can be seen in Ephat Mujuru’s rendition of Mutamba (Example 2) as well as Gwanzura Gwenzi’s variation of Nyamaropa
(Example 3b), left-thumb movements between upper and lower manuals of the mbira are not restricted to alternating
(high-low) movements alone. In these renditions, Mujuru and Gwenzi pattern their left-hand movements in a way that
implies a slightly different rhythmic grouping than the binary type (back-and-forth) notated in Examples 1 or 3a. Mujuru
articulates a high-low-low ternary-type movement, while Gwenzi articulates a low-high-low ternary-type movement. In both
cases, the kinesthetic pattern implies a kind of 3/4 meter, with different downbeats (on the third pitch-event for Mujuru, and
on the first for Gwenzi). While this motor pattern is in itself regular, it produces distinctly irregular inherent patterns when it
is interwoven with a second pattern, such as the one described above (Figure A). Example 5 represents the two interlocking
bass parts notated in Examples 3a and 3b by way of arrows. Once again, the first player is represented with arrows down,
and the second with arrows up. When such a pattern is combined with an identical pattern beginning one pulse later, we find
a low-high-high-low-low-high-low-low-high-high-low-low arrangement (with a pitch event on every consecutive pulse). This
is once again depicted in Example 5 above and below the arrows respectively with an “x” for a sound and a “.” for a silence.
As can be seen, the smallest shift in motor pattern of one or other mbirist results in a striking metamorphoses of internal
patterns that ultimately emerge from the total pattern.
5 of 10
[21] Figure B illustrates the types of metric ambiguity implied by the nesting of a binary-type bass line motion (such as the
bass pattern depicted in Figure 3a) within the spaces of a ternary-type bass line motion (such as the pattern depicted in
Figure 3b). The inherent patterns resulting from the interlocking of these two parts is irregular if projected against an
underlying periodicity. In the lower part we find < x . . x x . x x . . x x x . . x x . x x . . x x >, and so on, and in the upper part
we find the complementary < . x x . . x . . x x . . . x x . . x . . x x . . >, and so on. (See Example 5). Once again, as far as the
harmonic metric implications are concerned, the kinesthetic patterns imply the same two distinct downbeats and two sets of
metric accents as found in Figure A1. Figure B1 illustrates how the first mbira articulates a metric structure at ts 0, 4 and 8
under a harmonic reading; and the second on ts 1, 5 and 9. Two articulations of 3/2 time thus coexist, one pulse apart. As
far as the overall resultant pattern is concerned, the harmonic rhythm implies a metric structure at ts 0, 4 and 8, potentially
domesticating the phased metric relationship between parts. (See Figure B3).
[22] However, under the bass rule (MPR6) the kinesthetic patterns once again diverge. These are depicted in Figure B2.
Now the first mbira articulates a 3/4-type structure with metric accents on ts 0, 2, 4, 6, 8 and 10; while the second mbira still
articulates 3/2, only this time with metric accents on ts 3, 7 and 11. Interestingly, if we combine the bass rule (MPR6) with
the event rule (MPR3), the implied 3/4 structure would likely have downbeats on t 0 and t 6. This is unlike the 3/2 structure
(or 3 times 2/4 [or 4/8] structure), where each metric accent could equally be experienced as a downbeat. In effect then, a
fairly secure 3/4 time (or 3+3) unfolds here within the spaces of a simultaneous, somewhat more slippery, 2/4 time (or
2+2+2). A complex texture of variously-shaded and variously nuanced metric accents results.
[23] Focusing now on the inherent patterns formed by this interlocking of meters, the event rule (MPR3) produces a 3/2
structure in the upper bass line on ts 1, 5 and 9, and a 3/2 structure in the lower line on ts 3, 7 and 11. These meters are
depicted in Figures B4 and B5 respectively. Combining the event rule with the length rule (MPR 5a/d), we would likely find
downbeats at t 9 for the upper part, and at t 7 for the lower part. Of course, this 3/2 structure can only emerge if we posit
3/2 in a highly particular, even constrained, way. For example, while ts 3, 7 and 11 in the lower line all coincide with metric
accent points making a compelling case for positing 3/2, the intervening beat structure (ts 5, 9 and 1) is strongly refuted by
the rhythmic pattern, which does not coincide with any of these timepoints. (Indeed, if the intervening beat structure were
taken into account, 3/2 time at ts 0, 4 and 8 or ts 2, 6 and 10 would be more likely than that depicted in B5.) Furthermore,
these 3/2 structures (or 3 times 4/8 structures) can only emerge if we posit a binary-type metric grid in the first place. In
other words, even without the supplementary shaking of the hosho, the inherent patterns here amply imply a host of
ternary-type metric interpretations as well.
[24] Figures B6, B7 and B8 outline the three possibilities for ternary-type meter formation for the two inherent patterns. In
the upper part, 12/8 is thus readily posited at ts 2, 5, 8 and 11. Further rotations of 12/8 are implausible for this pattern. In
the lower part, 12/8 is equally posited at ts 0, 3, 6 and 9 (with a likely downbeat on t 0) and ts 1, 4, 7 and 10 (with a likely
downbeat on either t 7 or t 4). In all cases, the ternary-type structure is implied with equal validity, as all possibilities articulate
three pitch-event coincidences with four metric accents. The various metric possibilities are represented as a formalism in the
boxes in Figure B10. Finally, it should be noted that the pattern can be heard in more ways than simply those construed by
way of metric inflection. For example, instead of projecting an underlying regularity, the bass pattern can be heard as an
additive rhythm (groups of 5+3+4 pulses [see Figure B9] beginning on t 10 [in Example 5], namely < x x x . . x x . x x . . x x
x . . x x . x x . . >, and so on), or as an overlapped palindrome (pivoting on t 5 or t 11 [in Example 5] of each quarter: < . x x
. . x x x . . x x . x x . . x x x . . x x . >). The former kind of asymmetry, in this case a grouping of 5+7, is an important feature
of much African rhythm (see, for example Arom 1991), while the latter kind of symmetry is an important feature of
polymetric music in general.
[25] One of the interesting things about this kind of polyrhythm in general is that it always yields durational mirror images of
half of itself. These reflecting duration patterns contribute to an ambiguity regarding which is perceptually prior and also can
mean different things at once, because they involve a pivotal point that borders both reflecting patterns at opposite ends.
While it is by no means automatic, listening in this way can cycle the music in a startling and invigorating manner. Take the
extracted bass pattern above and listen to it as a palindrome. This is easily done because the patterns in this case are just
about the same (even when they run in the same direction) - < . x x . . x x > and < x x . . x x . > - sharing a silence on one
end and a sound on the other. Unlike the metrically inflected hearings of this pattern, where the irregular grouping
preconceives a periodic background (whether grouped in twos or threes), this last hearing preconceives a symmetrical
foreground, which then sounds beautifully syncopated no matter how we invest the background periodicitiy. Many of the
standard makwa (handclapping) patterns, such as < x . x . x . x x . x . x >, cycle similarly, so that in this textural layer our
hearing can shuttle between periodic asymmetry and aperiodic symmetry (or, of course, something else).
[26] As can be seen, the very attribution of metric (and other rhythmic) properties to this music involves a malleable mode of
listening that enables their emergence in various ways. Even in the context of the simplest patterns, such as those depicted in
Figures A and B, various metric and rhythmic interpretations jostle for syntax formation. Furthermore, by changing patterns
regularly using simple variation techniques, performers can intensify these metric ambiguities in surprising and unexpected
ways. One extremely important way such variation produces uncanny ambiguities is when resultant rhythmic patterns that
have the same basic structure emerge at shifting timepoints. For example, take the interweaving of the patterns of Figures 3a
and 3b. If a performer simply shifts the bass motion in Figure 3a from high-low leaping to low-high leaping—an elementary
6 of 10
variation technique for beginners—the inherent pattern formed in conjunction with the pattern in Figure 3b for the upper
line would shift from < . x x . . x . . x x . . > to < . . x x . . . x x . . x >, while the inherent pattern for the lower line would
shift from < x . . x x . x x . . x x > to < x x . . x x x . . x x . >. The rhythmic fallout produced by this subtle kinesthetic shift is
depicted in Example 6.
[27] Notice that the first pattern (in Example 5) and its metamorphosis (in Example 6) are in fact identical patterns shifted
by six timepoints. In other words, the upper and lower bass rhythms beginning at t 0 in Example 5 recapitulate the upper and
lower rhythms respectively beginning at t 6 in Example 6. A rhythmic motive is thereby set adrift from its temporal
coordinates; or, otherwise put, a transformation of rhythmic activity recoups an uncanny similitude. There are two points to be
made about this. First, a kind of rhythmic phase-shifting has occurred by way of a tiny shift in the motor actions of a single
thumb. Again, the motor actions and the sounding results are radically disaligned. Second, whereas rhythm in Western music
is casually understood as a flexible set of pitch-events framed within a fixed grid of hierarchical timepoints (or meter), the
rhythmic patterns in this African music have taken on the specter of fixed reference points while the meter has become
ephemeral, indeterminate, and flexible.
[28] The most immediately apparent factor in the formation of the rhythmic phenomena described above seems substantially
to involve the kinesthetic actions of mbirists. However, the thumb movements alone cannot explain the conceptual work
done by these unique ways of shaping time. This is where Shona harmonic practice suggests a richer explanation for the
aesthetics of this music than that suggested by bodily motion alone. Indeed, this kind of illusionism, whereby characteristic
rhythmic patterns reappear phantom-like and in unpredictable places along a temporal continuum, is a crucial dimension of
the temporal geometries of mbira music and of southern African music in general. To illustrate this point, I could show
many more examples of phantom pattern production, phase shifting and the concomitant metric ambiguities implied
thereby. For the purposes of this article, however, I will simply note that these rhythmic phenomena have important
conceptual affinities with modalities of harmonic practice as well. Let me conclude with a single example of this connection.
[29] Consider the harmonic progression of Nhemamusasa (see Figure C1), from the fragments notated in Examples 1a and 1b
above. Aside from the formal play of its cross-referential harmonic symmetries and near-symmetries, which I have discussed
elsewhere in the context of Nyamaropa, these harmonic movements can also be heard in various ambiguous “contrapuntal”
combinations (See Scherzinger 2001). Consider listening to these harmonic movements in groupings that begin at different
points in the dyad sequence, as depicted in Figure C2. For example, listen to the sequence unfolding from dyad XI, and
notice the intriguing near-semblance of the harmonic shape that follows the first six dyads (illustrated in Figure C2 (a)). The
inversions in the figure aid in hearing the kinship between these harmonic shapes. That is, the harmonic moves from dyad V
onwards, for example, approximate the six-dyad shape beginning at dyad XI. Indeed, for four dyads, these moves seem to
amount to the same progression as those beginning at dyad XI transposed up a tone (marked by +T 1 above the square
bracket in the diagram). But then the sequence reiterates the last two dyads of the first shape at pitch (marked by T 0 above
the slur in the diagram), and so breaks, or mutates, the sequential hearing. On the other hand, listen to the harmonic shape
beginning on dyad III in Figure C2 (b). Here, the first six dyads are identical to those in Figure C2 (a), albeit transposed, thus
sounding as if they were in a kind of harmonic canon to the progression in Figure C2 (a). Now, following this identical
six-dyad harmonic movement (at a staggered time interval), the remaining six dyads begin by seemingly repeating the first
shape at pitch (marked by T 0 ), but then on its third dyad (dyad XI) the sequence mutates from this path by lowering
everything that follows by a tone (marked by -T 1 ). So, this time a sequential hearing follows an initial iteration.
[30] These relationships can be noted in other sequences as well. Consider, for example, the dyad sequence beginning on
dyad I (depicted in Figure C2 (c)). Once again, the first six dyads replicate the harmonic shapes outlined above and, once
again, the ensuing trajectory is strangely (dis)similar. The point is that when listeners follow these identical shapes at different
times, different things become of the respective trajectories as they mutate in different senses. Yet, once they have run their
course, they seem to have produced the same thing—in this case a partial (four dyad) sequence a tone apart and a partial
(two dyad) iteration at pitch—as if the mutations were some kind of trick promising dissimilarity and then remaining the
same in another way. As can be seen, mbira music should not be reduced to the simple logic of ‘theme and variation.’ Indeed,
in the hands of expert performers (such as Garikai Tirikoti, Forward Kwenda, Musekiwa Chingodza), the music’s cycles call
to question the very structural ground of their harmonic patterning.
[31] As found in the rhythmic phenomena described above, a kind of acoustic trick seems to be at work here. The promised
variation between canonic harmonic strands in the music yields to an uncanny repetition of transformation types. This
particular kind of acoustic illusionism—where variation is experienced as uncanny repetition (or, otherwise put, where
repetition is paradoxically issued by passages of variation)—is discussed very little but, in my view, is one of the central
aesthetic characteristics of traditional African music. Illusionism of this sort resonates with the more general idea that, just as
(kinesthetic) simplicity may issue forth (formal) complexity (as the rhythmic interweaving in Figures A and B amply attests),
(kinesthetic) complexity conversely may issue forth (formal) simplicity. As a general principle, it is worth noting that
instances of complex kinesthetic patterns producing (relatively) simpler acoustic patterns in southern African musical
practices abound. Of particular interest here are the so-called “silent” drumming patterns of the Wagogo women of
7 of 10
Tanganika or the Nankasa drummers within the Buganda xylophone ensemble in Uganda. In both cases complex sticking
patterns (sometimes hitting skin, sometimes air) produce audible results that are out of sync with the actual hand
movements. Again, the motor image differs from the heard image, producing what Bela Balazs (in a different context) might
call “asynchronous” sound (Balazs 1985, 120). In the case of the Vimbuza dance (from Malawi), we too find sticking patterns
that are more complex than the acoustic results. These inaudible movements are nonetheless mentally effective for they
permit the drummer to inflect the texture with a patterned cross-rhythm, which in turn proffers metric ambiguities.
Concluding Remarks
[32] The conceptual basis for the embodied mbira-performance practices (a quest for non-visible sound or sounding forms
severed from their visible production) resonates strikingly with the uncanny rhythmic and harmonic relations produced by
the music’s temporal geometries (a quest for non-varying sound or rhythmic and harmonic repetitions proffered in contexts of
metamorphosis). A conceptual resonance notwithstanding, it would be a mistake to finally ground these musical phenomena
in kinesthetics alone. Far from reading the harmonic and rhythmic phenomena described above as the acoustic fallout of an
embodied musical situation, they also represent conceptual work that is finely attuned to specific mathematical conditions of
possibility and impossibility for partitioning and patterning musical time. In particular, just as rhythmic motives are
kinesthetically set adrift from their temporal coordinates in a kind of subtle phase-shifting, thereby effecting what should
properly be called a metric modulation (if Elliot Carter had not appropriated this idea for another, less accurately modulatory,
musical situation!), the harmonic patterning too is set adrift from its coordinates to produce a kind of uncanny phase-shifting
experience in the context of an otherwise ‘monophonic’ harmonic span. This is a music in which listening by actively shifting
metric instants, shifting modalities of rhythmic grouping and shifting starting points for harmonic patterning encourages the
appearance of phantom tunes, rhythms and harmonies that come to resemble each other precisely in those passages where
they metamorphose into something else.
[33] These different modalities of acoustic illusionism—first, the general mismatch in the music between motor images and
heard images; second, the acoustically vivid, but non-visible, rhythmic phase-shifting processes; and third, the non-varying
variations of the music’s harmonic patterning—are carefully constructed audile conundra. These are questions or riddles
whose answers are imbricated in some kind of musical pun. First, the discrepancy between things heard and things seen can
impress upon close listeners an experience of phantom sound—a sound of unknown character or origin suddenly visited
upon the music, as if from afar. Second, the reappearance of things in curious and unexpected places in the music can
impress upon close listeners an experience of phantom musical logic—a sound of (seemingly) grasped character and origin
suddenly propelled elsewhere into the music, as if by an unknown force. Finally, the puzzling experience of similitude in
contexts of transformation can impress upon listeners an experience of underlying design—a sound of changing character
strangely doubling as unexpected mimesis, as if of unknown authorship.
[34] It is a small wonder that mbirists frequently report that their instruments issue sounds unplayed by them, as if inhabited
by spirits (Berliner 1981, 23). Forward Kwenda and Dumisani Maraire, for example, report that the mbira should be played in
a way that is directed by the ancestral spirits alone, encouraging a hands-off approach to performance, whereby the mbira
itself speaks back by creating variations beyond the imagination of a single human player or voice (Scherzinger 2001, 33).
From the perspective of traditionalists in Zimbabwe, it is evident that the Shona regard the music of the mbira with an
attitude of high seriousness, spirituality, and respect. Many mbira players suggest that the music is connected to ancestral
spirits who can be reached via profound contemplation. In the words of mbirist Luken Pasipamire: “Sometimes when you
are playing mbira nicely, you will cry, for the mbira makes you think too much” (Pasipamire, in Berliner 1981, 133). Likewise,
the singer Hakurotwi Mude insists that aesthetically good mbira music must produce a kind of deep thinking in the listener.
[35] The power of mbira’s sound can transport performers and listeners out of the commonplace. Forward Kwenda, one of
the most acclaimed mbirists of the younger generation, claims: “the music takes me higher and higher until I end up crying
because the music is so much greater than a human being can understand” (Kwenda 1997). For the traditionalist Tute
Chigamba, the religious and introspective attitude demanded by mbira music renders it unsuitable as an instrument of mere
entertainment (personal communication). Echoing this sentiment, Mude asserts, “The mbira dza vadzimu is not played for
pleasure” (Mude, in Berliner 1981, 134). Far from providing sensuous experiences alone, mbira music, especially for the
traditionalists, is central to the spiritual cosmology of the Shona. It “speaks back” to performers. Andrew Tracey reports that
the idea that one mbira sounds like more than is even more pronounced with other lamellaphone-types, like matepe, njari, and
nyonganyonga (personal communication). Matepe players actually boast of it. The perplexing ability of the music to elicit audile
condundra, issue forth asynchronous sounds, materialize phantom melodies and rhythms, and recoup similitude in contexts
of metamorphosis facilitates a listening experience that grows beyond the dimensions of the tactile performance alone,
touching instead upon something unguessed at.
Martin Scherzinger
New York University
Works Cited
8 of 10
Agawu, Kofi. 2003. Representing African Music: Postcolonial Notes, Queries, Positions. New York and London: Routledge.
Anku, Willie. 1992. Structural Set Analysis of African Music. Soundstage Production: Legon, Ghana.
Arom, Simha. 1991. African Polyrhythm and Polyphony. Translated by Martin Thom, Barbara Tuckett and Raymond Boyd.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Balazs, Bela. 1985. “Theory of the Film: Sound.“ In Film Sound: Theory and Practice, edited by Elisabeth Weiss and John Belton.
New York: Columbia University Press.
Berliner, Paul. 1981. The Soul of Mbira. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press.
Beach, David N. 1980. The Shona and Zimbabwe 900–1850: An Outline of Shona History. London, Ibadan, Nairobi, and
Johannesburg: Heinemann.
—————. 1986. War and Politics in Zimbabwe: 1840–1900. Gweru: Mambo Press.
Chernoff, John Miller. 1979. African Rhythm and African Sensibility: Aesthetics and Social Action in African Musical Idioms. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Jones, A.M.. 1959. Studies in African Music. 2 vols. London, New York, Toronto, Cape Town: Oxford University Press.
Kubik, Gerhard. 1962. “The Phenomenon of Inherent Rhythms in East and Central African Instrumental Music.” African
Music: Journal of the African Music Society 3, no. 1: 33–42.
Kwenda, Forward. 1997. Liner notes on Svikiro: Meditations of An Mbira Master. Shanachie.
Locke, David. 1987. Drum Gahu: A Systematic Method for an African Percussion Piece. Crown Point, IN: White Cliffs Media.
Scherzinger, Martin. 2001. “Negotiating the Music Theory/African Music Nexus: A Political Critique of Ethnomusicological
Anti-Formalism and a Strategic Analysis of the Harmonic Patterning of the Shona Mbira Song Nyamaropa.” Perspectives of
New Music 39, no. 1: 5–117.
—————. 2003. “Notes on a Postcolonial Musicology: Kofi Agawu and the Critique of Cultural Difference [Agawu,
Kofi. 2003. Representing African Music: Postcolonial Notes, Queries, Positions. New York and London: Routledge, 304.].” Current
Musicology 75: 223–50.
Tracey, Andrew. 1970. How to Play the Mbira (Dza Vadzimu). Roodepoort: South Africa.
Footnotes
* I would like to thank Matthew Butterfield, Matthew Shaftel, and above all, Andrew Tracy, for their helpful contributions to
this text.
Return to text
Copyright Statement
Copyright © 2010 by the Society for Music Theory. All rights reserved.
[1] Copyrights for individual items published in Music Theory Online (MTO) are held by their authors. Items appearing in MTO
may be saved and stored in electronic or paper form, and may be shared among individuals for purposes of scholarly
research or discussion, but may not be republished in any form, electronic or print, without prior, written permission from
the author(s), and advance notification of the editors of MTO.
[2] Any redistributed form of items published in MTO must include the following information in a form appropriate to the
medium in which the items are to appear:
This item appeared in Music Theory Online in [VOLUME #, ISSUE #] on [DAY/MONTH/YEAR]. It was
authored by [FULL NAME, EMAIL ADDRESS], with whose written permission it is reprinted here.
[3] Libraries may archive issues of MTO in electronic or paper form for public access so long as each issue is stored in its
entirety, and no access fee is charged. Exceptions to these requirements must be approved in writing by the editors of MTO,
who will act in accordance with the decisions of the Society for Music Theory.
This document and all portions thereof are protected by U.S. and international copyright laws. Material contained herein may
be copied and/or distributed for research purposes only.
9 of 10
Prepared by John Reef, Editorial Assistant
10 of 10