3 Madden Chase Joyce 1998
3 Madden Chase Joyce 1998
3 Madden Chase Joyce 1998
1 (Spring)
A great deal has been written about Lowe, 1996; Lowe, 1979). Frequently
differences between human and non- the new principles called for are those
human sensitivity to schedules of re- related to verbal behavior. For exam-
inforcement (e.g., Baron & Galizio, ple, Lowe and Home (1996) concluded
1983; Cerutti, 1989; Galizio, 1979; that "(a) the performance of verbally
Kaufman, Baron, & Kopp, 1966; able humans on schedules of reinforce-
Lowe, 1979; Shimoff, Catania, & Mat- ment, including concurrent schedules,
thews, 1981; Skinner, 1966). Some differed greatly from that observed in
have argued that because human and nonhuman species, and that (b) a key
nonhuman behavior is affected in dif- variable in bringing about these differ-
ferent ways by seemingly comparable ences was human subjects' ability to
schedule contingencies, different prin- specify the contingencies verbally and
ciples are required for accurate ac- to formulate their own rules for re-
counts of human and nonhuman behav- sponding" (p. 315). Consistent with
ior (e.g., Brewer, 1974; Home & this argument, a number of definitions
of rule-governed behavior have been
This work was supported by NIDA Grant 5- presented that include schedule insen-
ROI-DA06526-08. sitivity as a critical feature (Catania,
Portions of this article were written as the first Shimoff, & Matthews, 1989; Cerutti,
author's conceptual preliminary examination 1989; Shimoff et al., 1981).
while a doctoral student at West Virginia Uni- These arguments are important be-
versity. We are grateful to Margaret Vaughan cause they suggest that human behav-
and several anonymous reviewers who provided
insightful comments on the manuscript. James ior, at least that which is rule governed,
Joyce is now at the Continuous Learning Group, is not sensitive to changes in schedules
Jamesville, Virginia. of reinforcement. We find it difficult to
Address correspondence and reprint requests agree with this conclusion. Our review
to Gregory J. Madden, Human Behavioral Phar-
macology Laboratory, University of Vermont, of the human operant literature reveals
38 Fletcher Place, Burlington, Vermont 05401- two potentially conflicting definitions
1419. of the term sensitivity (we assume
1
2 GREGORY J. MADDEN et al.
O0MINUTESA B C D EF
tal procedures, and that this unexplored performance. Journal of the Experimental
status renders premature many conclu- Analysis of Behavior, 59, 501-520.
Baron, A., & Galizio, M. (1983). Instructional
sions about the generality (or the lack control of human operant behavior. The Psy-
of generality) of the processes govern- chological Record, 33, 495-520.
ing nonhuman behavior to the behavior Baron, A., & Kaufman, A. (1966). Human free-
of humans. Thus, we recommend using operant avoidance of "time out" from mon-
etary reinforcement. Journal of the Experi-
a within-subject definition of schedule mental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 557-565.
sensitivity, and separately describing Baum, W. M. (1974). On two types of deviation
the extent to which interspecies repli- from the matching law: Bias and undermatch-
cations are demonstrated. ing. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
Recognizing that insensitivity is Behavior, 22, 231-242.
Baum, W. M. (1975). Time allocation in human
synonymous with failure of experimen- vigilance. Journal of the Experimental Anal-
tal control suggests that when there is ysis of Behavior, 23, 45-53.
reason to believe that the behavior of Baum, W. M. (1979). Matching, undermatch-
nonhuman organisms is sensitive to a ing, and overmatching in studies of choice.
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Be-
particular independent variable and havior, 32, 269-281.
within-session manipulations reveal Baxter, G. A., & Schlinger, H. (1990). Perfor-
that human behavior is insensitive to mance of children under a multiple random-
this variable, then in these cases the ex- ratio random-interval schedule of reinforce-
ment. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
perimenter should focus on procedural Behavior, 54, 263-271.
factors that may have led to insensitiv- Bentall, R. P., Lowe, C. F, & Beasty, A. (1985).
ity. Only after these factors have been The role of verbal behavior in human learn-
ruled out would a finding of insensitiv- ing: II. Developmental differences. Journal of
the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43,
ity be interpreted as having theoretical 165-181.
significance, and then only within a Bickel, W. K., DeGrandpre, R. J., & Higgins, S.
carefully argued network of findings. T. (1995). The behavioral economics of con-
Although we recommend within- current drug reinforcers: A review and reanal-
subject experiments for assessing sen- ysis of drug self-administration research. Psy-
chopharmacology, 118, 250-259.
sitivity to the effects of an independent Bradshaw, C. M., & Szabadi, E. (1988). Quan-
variable, we recognize the importance titative analysis of human operant behavior. In
of between-species and between-sub- G. Davey & C. Cullen (Eds.), Human operant
jects comparisons. Between-species conditioning and behavior modification (pp.
225-259). Chichester, England: Wiley.
comparisons are important in assessing Brewer, W. F. (1974). There is not convincing
the interspecies generality of behavior- evidence for operant or classical conditioning
al principles and in understanding how in adult humans. In W. B. Weimer & D. J.
phylogenic differences affect the be- Palermo (Eds.), Cognition and symbolic pro-
cesses (pp. 1-42). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
havior of different species. Likewise, Buskist, W. F, Bennett, R. H., & Miller, H. L.
between-subjects comparisons are im- (1981). Effects of instructional constraints on
portant in assessing the effects of sub- human fixed-interval performance. Journal of
ject variables and variables that are the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 35,
suspected of having irreversible ef- 217-225.
Buskist, W. F, & Miller, H. L. (1981). Concur-
fects. When using these methodolo- rent operant performance in humans: Match-
gies, however, the researcher must be ing when food is the reinforcer. The Psycho-
careful to avoid making statements logical Record, 31, 95-100.
about the sensitivity or insensitivity of Catania, A. C., Matthews, B. A., Silverman, P.
J., & Yohalem, R. (1977). Yoked variable-
a particular species' or subject's behav- ratio and variable-interval responding in pi-
ior based on these comparisons alone. geons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis
of Behavior, 28, 155-161.
REFERENCES Catania, A. C., Shimoff, E., & Matthews, B. A.
(1989). An experimental analysis of rule-gov-
emed behavior. In S. C. Hayes (Ed.), Rule-
Allison, J. (1983). Behavioral economics. New governed behavior: Cognition, contingencies,
York: Praeger. and instructional control, (pp. 119-150). New
Barnes, D., & Keenan, M. (1993). Concurrent York: Plenum Press.
activities and instructed human fixed-interval Cerutti, D. T. (1989). Discrimination theory of
SENSITIVITY 11
rule-governed behavior. Journal of the Exper- behavior. Psychonomic Monograph Supple-
imental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 259-276. ments, 1, 243-250.
Darcheville, J. C., Riviere, V., & Wearden, J. H. Kramer, T. J., & Rilling, M. (1970). Differen-
(1993). Fixed-interval performance and self- tial-reinforcement-of-low-rates: A selective
control in infants. Journal of the Experimental critique. Psychological Bulletin, 74, 225-256.
Analysis of Behavior, 60, 239-254. LeFrancois, J. R., Chase, P. N., & Joyce, J. H.
de Villiers, P. A. (1977). Choice in concurrent (1988). The effects of a variety of instructions
schedules and a quantitative formulation of on human fixed-interval performance. Journal
the law of effect. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 49,
Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behav- 383-393.
ior (pp. 233-287). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Logue, A. W., Forzano, L. B., & Tobin, H.
Prentice Hall. (1992). Independence of reinforcer amount
Dougherty, D. M., & Lewis, P. (1992). Match- and delay: The generalized matching law and
ing by horses on several concurrent variable- self-control in humans. Learning and Moti-
interval schedules. Behavioural Processes, 26, vation, 23, 326-342.
69-76. Lowe, C. F (1979). Determinants of human op-
Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Sched- erant behaviour. In M. D. Zeiler & P. Harzem
ules of reinforcement. New York: Appleton- (Eds.), Reinforcement and the organization of
Century-Crofts. behavior (pp. 159-192). New York: Wiley.
Foster, T. M., Temple, W., Robertson, B., Nair, Lowe, C. F, Beasty, A., & Bentall, R. P. (1983).
V., & Poling A. (1996). Concurrent-schedule The role of verbal behavior in human learn-
performance in dairy cows: Persistent under- ing: Infant performance on fixed-interval
matching. Journal of the Experimental Anal- schedules. Journal of the Experimental Anal-
ysis of Behavior, 65, 57-80. ysis of Behavior, 39, 157-164.
Galizio, M. (1979). Contingency-shaped and Lowe, C. F, Harzem, P., & Bagshaw, M. (1978).
rule-governed behavior: Instructional control Species differences in temporal control of be-
of human loss avoidance. Journal of the Ex- havior II: Human performance. Journal of the
perimental Analysis of Behavior, 31, 53-70. Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 29, 351-
Hayes, S. C., Brownstein, A. J., Zettle, R. D., 361.
Rosenfarb, I., & Korn, Z. (1986). Rule-gov- Lowe, C. F, & Home, P J. (1996). Reflections
erned behavior and sensitivity to changing on naming and other symbolic behavior. Jour-
consequences of responding. Journal of the nal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45, 237- 65, 315-340.
256. Madden, G. J., & Perone, M. (in press). Human
Herrnstein, R. J. (1970). On the law of effect. sensitivity and insensitivity to concurrent
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Be- schedules of reinforcement: Effects of observ-
havior, 13, 243-266. ing schedule-correlated stimuli. Journal of the
Horne, P J., & Lowe, C. F (1993). Determi- Experimental Analysis of Behavior.
nants of human performance on concurrent Matthews, B. A., Shimoff, E., Catania, A. C., &
schedules. Journal of the Experimental Anal- Sagvolden, T. (1977). Uninstructed human
ysis of Behavior, 59, 29-60. responding: Sensitivity to ratio and interval
Horne, P J., & Lowe, C. F (1996). On the or- contingencies. Journal of the Experimental
igins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Analysis of Behavior, 27, 453-467.
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Be- Matthews, L. R., & Temple, W (1979). Con-
havior, 65, 185-241. current schedule assessment of food prefer-
Hursh, S. R. (1980). Economic concepts for the ence in cows. Journal of the Experimental
analysis of behavior. Journal of the Experi- Analysis of Behavior, 32, 245-254.
mental Analysis of Behavior, 34, 219-238.
Hyten, C., & Madden, G. J. (1993). The scallop Navarick, D. J., Bernstein, D. J., & Fantino, E.
in human fixed-interval research: A review of (1990). The experimental analysis of human
problems with data description. The Psycho- behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analy-
logical Record, 43, 471-500. sis of Behavior, 54, 159-162.
Hyten, C., Madden, G. J., & Field, D. P. (1994). Perone, M., Galizio, M., & Baron, A. (1988).
Exchange delays and impulsive choice in The relevance of animal-based principles in
adult humans. Journal of the Experimental the laboratory study of human operant con-
Analysis of Behavior, 62, 225-233. ditioning. In G. Davey & C. Cullen (Eds.),
Jackson, K., & Hackenberg, T. D. (1996). To- Human operant conditioning and behavior
ken reinforcement, choice, and self-control in modification (pp. 59-85). Chichester, En-
pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis gland: Wiley.
of Behavior, 66, 29-49. Pierce, W D., Epling, W. F, & Greer, S. M.
Joyce, J. H., & Chase, P N. (1990). Effects of (1981). Human communication and the
response variability on the sensitivity of rule- matching law. In C. M. Bradshaw, E. Szabadi,
governed behavior. Journal of the Experimen- & C. F Lowe (Eds.), Quantification ofsteady-
tal Analysis of Behavior, 54, 251-262. state operant behavior (pp. 345-348). Am-
Kaufman, A., Baron, A., & Kopp, R. E. (1966). sterdam: Elsevier.
Some effects of instructions on human operant Rachlin, H., Kagel, J. H., & Battalio, R. (1980).
12 GREGORY J. MADDEN et al.
Substitutability in time-allocation. Psycholog- Torgrud, L. J., & Holburn, S. W. (1990). The
ical Bulletin, 87, 355-374. effects of verbal performance descriptions on
Schroeder, S. R., & Holland, J. G. (1969). Re- nonverbal operant responding. Journal of the
inforcement of eye movement with concurrent Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54, 273-
schedules of reinforcement. Journal of the Ex- 291.
perimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 897-903. Trenholme, I. A., & Baron, A. (1975). Imme-
Shimoff, E., Catania, A. C., & Matthews, B. A. diate and delayed punishment of human be-
(1981). Uninstructed human responding: Sen- havior by loss of reinforcement. Learning and
sitivity of low-rate performances to schedule Motivation, 6, 62-79.
contingencies. Journal of the Experimental Wanchisen, B. A., Tatham, T. A., & Mooney, S.
Analysis of Behavior, 36, 207-220. E. (1989). Variable-ratio conditioning history
Shimoff, E., Matthews, B. A., & Catania, A. C. produces high- and low-rate fixed-interval
(1986). Human operant performance: Sensi- performance in rats. Journal of the Experi-
tivity and pseudosensitivity to contingencies. mental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 167-179.
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Be-
havior, 46, 149-157. Wearden, J. H., & Burgess, I. S. (1982). Match-
Skinner, B. F (1956). A case history in scien- ing since Baum (1979). Journal of the Ex-
tific method. American Psychologist, 11, 211- perimental Analysis of Behavior, 38, 339-
233. 348.
Skinner, B. F (1966). An operant analysis of Weiner, H. (1969). Controlling human fixed-in-
problem-solving. In B. Kleinmuntz (Ed.), terval performance. Journal of the Experimen-
Problem solving (pp. 225-257). New York: tal Analysis of Behavior, 12, 349-373.
Wiley.